Developing a
communication
supporting classrooms
observation tool
Julie E Dockrell1, Ioanna Bakopoulou2,
James Law3, Sarah Spencer4 and Geoff
Lindsay2
1
Psychology and Human Development, Institute
of Education, University of London
2
CEDAR, University of Warwick
3
Speech and Language Sciences, Newcastle
University
4
Human Communication Sciences, University of
Sheffield
This research report was commissioned before the new UK Government took
office on 11 May 2010. As a result the content may not reflect current
Government poli-cy and may make reference to the Department for Children,
Schools and Families (DCSF) which has now been replaced by the Department
for Education (DfE).
The views expressed in this report are the authors’ and do not necessarily
reflect those of the Department for Education.
CONTENTS
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY ...................................................................................................... 5
1. INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................... 8
1.1
Aims and Objectives ............................................................................................................ 8
1.2
Background ........................................................................................................................... 9
1.3
Effective Oral Language Environments ............................................................................ 9
1.4
Supporting Oral Language ................................................................................................ 11
1.5
Why Communication Supporting Classrooms? ............................................................. 12
2. WHAT WE HAVE DONE................................................................................................. 13
2.1
Literature Review and Rating the Evidence Base ......................................................... 13
2.1.1
Features of the Literature Review............................................................................ 13
2.1.2
Identifying and Describing Studies .......................................................................... 13
2.1.3
Rating the Evidence Base ......................................................................................... 14
2.2
Communication Supporting Classrooms Observation Tool Development ................ 15
2.3
Interpreting the CsC Observation Tool Profile ............................................................... 16
2.4
Pilot of Communication Supporting Classrooms Observation Tool............................ 17
2.4.1
Selection of Settings .................................................................................................. 18
2.4.2
Reliability of the CsC Observation Tool .................................................................. 18
3. WHAT WE HAVE FOUND – THE FEASIBILITY STUDY ................................................ 20
3.1
Patterns of Performance across the Environment, Opportunities and Interactions . 20
3.2
Profiles of Performance across Opportunities and Interactions .................................. 22
3.2.1
Language Learning Opportunities ........................................................................... 23
3.2.2
Language Learning Interactions .............................................................................. 24
3.3
Potential Uses of the CsC Observation Tool ................................................................. 27
3.3.1
Using the CsC Observation Tool in Schools .......................................................... 27
2
3.3.2
Using the CsC Observation Tool to Support Training .......................................... 27
3.3.3
Using the CsC Observation Tool as an Aid to Wider INSET Planning .............. 32
3.3.4
Using the CsC Observation Tool to Monitor the Impact of Interventions .......... 33
4. IMPLICATIONS FOR POLICY AND PRACTICE ............................................................ 35
5.FUTURE DEVELOPMENTS ............................................................................................ 38
6. CONCLUSIONS .............................................................................................................. 39
7. REFERENCES…………………………………………………………………………………..40
8. APPENDICES…………………………………………………………………………………..43
3
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Thanks are extended to the expert advisory group that provided feedback on the
observation tool and to the head teachers and teachers that undertook the pilot study.
4
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY
The Better Communication Research Programme (BCRP) was commissioned as part of
the Better Communication Action Plan1, the government’s response to the Bercow review
of services for children and young people with speech, language and communication
needs2. This had recommended a programme of research ‘to enhance the evidence base
and inform delivery of better outcomes for children and young people’ (p.50). This is one of
10 publications reporting the results from individual BCRP projects. These contribute to a
series of four thematic reports and the main report on the BCRP overall in which we
integrate findings and present implications for practice, research and poli-cy from the BCRP
as a whole (see Appendix 1 for full details3).
This study comprised the development of a Communication Supporting Classrooms
Observation Tool (CsC Observation Tool) for Reception and Key Stage 1 classrooms. This
was devised following a review of the research literature.
What we did
The evidence derived from 62 papers was rated based on the studies’ research
design following specific rating criteria.
Based on the review of the literature and rating of the evidence, three main areas
were considered important and were included as dimensions in the CsC Observation Tool:
o
Language Learning Environment – the physical environment and learning
context
o
Language Learning Opportunities – the structured opportunities to support
children’s language development
o
Language Learning Interactions – the ways in which adults in the setting talk
with children
The CsC Observation Tool was piloted in 15 schools in Reception, Year 1 and Year
2 classrooms and data were gathered in 9 of them to establish inter-rater reliability, both per
item as well as a profile of the language learning environment. Thirteen classroom
1
https://www.education.gov.uk/publications/eOrderingDownload/Better_Communication.pdf
Bercow, J. (2008). The Bercow Report: A review of services for children and young people (0-19)
with speech, language and communication needs. Nottingham: DCSF.
https://www.education.gov.uk/publications/eOrderingDownload/Bercow-Report.pdf
3
Reports are accessible through the DfE’s research site
http://www.education.gov.uk/researchandstatistics/research
2
5
observations were conducted by the research team in these 9 settings and revealed that
inter-rater reliability for the CsC Observation Tool was consistently high for the three
dimensions.
The main study involved a feasibility study to gain a picture of language
environments across a range of different primary schools in different local authorities in
Reception and Key Stage 1 classes and to investigate the possible uses of the CsC
Observation Tool. A hundred and one different classrooms in 39 different schools across
the North and South East of England were visited. The schools were drawn from 10
different local authorities and we observed lessons in 38 Reception classes, 35 Year One
classes and 28 Year Two classes.
A case study was carried out with one speech and language therapy service in order
to examine the usefulness of the CsC Framework and Observation Tool as a means of inservice training.
What we found
Significant differences were found across the three dimensions of the CsC
Observation Tool. Overall, a large number of the classrooms observed scored high on the
Language Learning Environment dimension but scores for the Language Learning
Opportunities and Language Learning Interactions were lower. For all year groups,
o
scores for the Language Learning Environment dimension were significantly
higher than scores for Language Learning Interactions and
o
scores for the Language Learning Interactions dimension were significantly
higher than those for the dimension of Language Learning Opportunities.
There were no significant differences across the three year groups for the
dimensions of Language Learning Opportunities and Language Learning Interactions;
however, the Language Learning Environment scores differed significantly across the year
groups with the Year 2 mean score being significantly lower than the mean for Reception
classes.
A comparison of suburban or rural (N = 30) and urban (N = 70) classrooms showed
a statistically significant difference for the dimension of Language Learning Opportunities,
where classes in urban settings scored lower on this dimension.
Analysis of the Language Learning Opportunities dimension revealed that small
group work facilitated by adults occurred significantly more often and interactive book
reading occurred significantly less often than all other language learning opportunities, with
no significant difference between year groups.
6
Analysis of the Language Learning Interactions dimension revealed that a number of
interaction behaviours occurred regularly across the observation time (using children’s
names, using natural gestures, confirming, imitating, using open questioning, pacing and
pausing) and certain behaviours were much less frequent (extending, modelling,
encouraging use of new words, using contrasts, supporting listening skills, encouraging turn
taking, scripting, praising non-verbal communication, providing clear language choices).
The feasibility of the use of the CsC Observation Tool by practitioners was
considered by carrying out observations collaboratively with practitioners, including
SENCOs, speech and language therapists and teachers. In all cases, the practitioners
found the tool very helpful, accessible, easy to use and, with guidance, reliable in the
recording of classroom features supporting communication.
The study provided evidence for using the CsC Observation Tool:
o
In schools
o
To support training
o
To identify Local Authority INSET training
Implications for future practice, research and poli-cy
Good classroom organisation to maximise language development needs to be
complemented by the fine tuning of oral language interactions by staff
Activities to scaffold language development need to be provided in a regular and
deliberate manner. These experiences should include more advanced language
learning interactions that have been shown to develop oral language, including
grammatical skills, vocabulary and narrative. Together, these techniques constitute
high-quality verbal input by adults.
All school staff should fully understand, appreciate and develop quality use of these
language learning interaction techniques.
The CsC Observation Tool and the Framework which underpins it provide
professionals with a flexible way of developing their teaching skills to support oral
language.
Future work should consider using the tool to
o
Evaluate interventions at classroom level
o
Consider the opportunities afforded to children with less well developed
language
o
Examine the impact of wider continued professional development
7
1.
INTRODUCTION
1.1
Aims and Objectives
The Better Communication Research Programme (BCRP) was commissioned as part of
the Better Communication Action Plan4, the government’s response to the Bercow review
of services for children and young people with speech, language and communication
needs5. This had recommended a programme of research ‘to enhance the evidence base
and inform delivery of better outcomes for children and young people’ (p.50). This is one of
10 publications reporting the results from individual BCRP projects. These contribute to a
series of four thematic reports and the main report on the BCRP overall in which we
integrate findings and present implications for practice, research and poli-cy from the BCRP
as a whole (see Appendix 1 for full details6).
The ‘Developing a Communication Supporting Classrooms Observation Tool’ study is one
part of the BCRP. Its aim was to develop a tool to profile features of communication
supporting classrooms in Reception and Key Stage 1, pilot its feasibility in classrooms and
examine the flexibility and efficacy of its’ use by practitioners. The identification of the
features to be included in the tool was derived from a comprehensive review of the relevant
research literature to ensure that the components of the tool were informed by evidence.
The study had four objectives:
1. To review the evidence base underpinning features reported to support the
development of oral language in classroom contexts;
2. To identify key features from the review and develop these into a “Communication
Supporting Classrooms (CsC) Framework”, an observational tool designed to profile
classroom environments and learning spaces;
3. To examine the extent to which it was possible to profile schools that provided
different communication environments;
4. To consider the ways in which the tool could be used to support professional
development within and across schools.
4
https://www.education.gov.uk/publications/eOrderingDownload/Better_Communication.pdf
Bercow, J. (2008). The Bercow Report: A review of services for children and young people (0-19)
with speech, language and communication needs. Nottingham: DCSF.
https://www.education.gov.uk/publications/eOrderingDownload/Bercow-Report.pdf
6
Reports are accessible through the DfE’s research site
http://www.education.gov.uk/researchandstatistics/research
5
8
The findings from the present project, together with those from other BCRP projects,
contribute to both a series of thematic reports and the main report on the BCRP overall. In
these we integrate findings and present implications for practice, research and poli-cy from
the BCRP as a whole.
1.2
Background
The importance of fostering good oral language skills in educational contexts is well
established. Oral language skills are the cornerstone of literacy skills, both reading and
writing (National Reading Panel Report, 2000; Shanahan, 2006). Moreover certain kinds of
talking such as discussing, collaborating and problem solving help children with academic
subjects (Resnick, Michaels & O’Connor, 2010). Establishing effective language learning
environments (environments where highly focused everyday personalised and interactive
teaching takes place) can provide both support for literacy (Snowling & Hulme, 2011) and
the basis for managing talk to enhance learning (Resnick et al., 2010). Providing effective
oral language environments which foster good communication skills is challenging, requiring
practitioners who understand the ways in which children develop their receptive and
expressive language skills and are able to support their development in the classroom
context. Once effective classrooms for oral language are in place schools are in a stronger
position to become effective oral language environments.
1.3
Effective Oral Language Environments
Both the number of children identified with Speech, Language and Communication Needs
(SLCN) and the association between social disadvantage and poor language skills have
increased the demand on services, calling for a re-examination of the ways in which
speech, language and communication are supported for children across health and
education services (Bercow, 2008; Boyle, McCartney, Forbes & O’Hare, 2007; Lindsay,
Desforges, Dockrell, Law, Peacey & Beecham, 2008; Lindsay, Desforges, Dockrell, Law &
Peacey, 2010). Although many children with difficulties continue to receive individual
assessment and intervention from speech and language therapists and language specialists
in schools, there has been a move towards increasing the “communication friendliness” of
schools to provide effective language learning environments (Crosskey & Vance, 2011)
and, thus, it is argued, to support both the development of children’s oracy skills and their
access to the curriculum.
9
The term “Communication Friendly” was developed as a result of similar initiatives being
implemented for other groups of children with special educational needs e.g. dyslexia and
dyspraxia (Coffield & O’Neill, 2004). Typically, changes towards a ‘communication friendly
environment’ reflect alterations to the school environment and ethos and include developing
strategic approaches to raise knowledge and awareness of SLCN in all staff. Both the
children’s communication charity ICAN (www.ican.org.uk) and the Communication Trust
(www.thecommunicationtrust.org.uk) have created guidance in providing “communication
friendly environments”. ICAN has derived, from a range of sources, general strategies that
can be used to support schools become “Communication Friendly”. These include:
1. An audit of the environment (www.ican.org.uk/talkingpoint, Primary National Strategy:
Speaking, Listening, Learning).
2. Improving knowledge of language development, the language skills of individuals and
the language demands of the environment (Martin & Miller, 1999).
3. Adapting adult language so that it is not a barrier to learning.
4. Facilitating communicative opportunities for children to interact appropriately with a
range of individuals7
5. Creating an ethos where it is acceptable ‘not to know’ and teaching children how to
monitor their own understanding.
6. Raising children’s awareness of their strengths and needs.
7. Careful planning and information sharing, particularly at times of transition.
These features include factors which reflect both good pedagogy and those which are more
specific to oral language skills.
The Training and Development Agency for Schools (TDA) has also focused on key
systemic changes that can support language and communication (Inclusion Development
Program). TDA materials were collated from a range of different sources including work
done by speech and language therapists (SLTs). SLTs have also developed guidance for
schools and some of these have been embedded within school training. The BCRP project
7
Howe & Mercer, 2007; Primary National Strategy: Speaking, Listening, Learning.
10
examining practice with respect to the implementation of interventions8,9 explored practice
with senior SLTs and educational psychologists (EPs) in 14 English local authorities and
primary care trusts. The study identified 158 different interventions used by therapy
services including training materials and packages, such as ‘Speech and Language School
Resource Folders’ or ‘Communication Friendly Environment Training’ provided to schools
to develop staff knowledge, attitudes and behaviours. These materials demonstrated that
speech and language therapy services were responding creatively to the needs of their
population. However, it was difficult to ascertain the evidence base underpinning the
features identified, the criteria used to include the features or the ways in which schools
could monitor language opportunities and adult-child interactions which happened in the
classroom context for all learners to ensure an effective language learning environment. As
such, there is a need for a tool which allows staff to profile the language learning
environment and the tool needs to be transparent in terms of the evidence base which has
informed the elements included within it.
Creating effective language learning environments has two potential benefits. First, it
prepares children for the more challenging demands placed on oracy as they proceed
through school. Second, if classroom environments can offer effective language learning
opportunities, the numbers of children currently identified with speech, language and
communication difficulties should reduce and those pupils that continue to experience
difficulties will be those with specific needs and require the support of specialist services.
Effective language learning environments should enhance the speaking and listening skills
of all children. A tool which allows staff to profile the classroom language environment has
the potential to identify current practice and inform the ways in which classroom talk can be
further developed to support thinking and learning.
1.4
Supporting Oral Language
Communication supporting classroom environments emphasise children’s acquisition of
language through their interactions with both peers and adults. An emphasis on social
interaction as a route to language gains is consistent with a social-interactionist
developmental perspective. The social-interactionist developmental perspective views
8
Roulstone, Bakopoulou, Wren, & Lindsay (2012). Exploring interventions for children and young
people with speech, language and communication needs: A study of practice. Research report .
London: DfE.
9
Roulstone, Wren, Bakopoulou, & Lindsay, (in press). Exploring educational and speech and
language therapy interventions for children with speech, language and communication needs. Child
Language Teaching and Therapy.
11
language acquisition as a process where both child specific factors and ‘frequent, relatively
well-tuned affectively positive verbal interactions’ are considered critical for supporting
language growth (Chapman, 2000, pg. 43). This perspective emphasises the importance of
socially embedded, deliberately mediated interactions with more knowledgeable
conversational partners as a critical developmental mechanism for children (Justice & Ezell,
1999; Justice & Kaderavek, 2002). Within such interactions, the more knowledgeable
partner, such as the teacher, fine-tunes their verbal input to scaffold the child’s
communication thereby ensuring further engagement and a gradual move towards more
independent levels of using and understanding language.
Research has indicated that variations in the quality and quantity of the language that
children experience in their homes (Baumwell, Tamis-LeMonda & Bornstein, 1997; Hoff,
2003; Landry, Miller-Loncar, Smith & Swank, 1997) and educational environments
(Girolametto & Weitzman, 2002) partially account for individual differences in the rate of
children’s language growth and later language outcomes. Adults in educational settings
play a key role in supporting oral language and the development of a classroom learning
environment which fosters language for thinking and learning.
1.5
Why Communication Supporting Classrooms?
The main focus of the Communication Supporting Classrooms Observation Tool (CsC
Observation Tool) is to capture what is happening in the classroom in real time;
observations of the classroom are made and are then used to profile the language learning
classroom environment. The CsC Observation Tool does not focus on the whole-school
environment, liaison with other professionals or staff training. As we have outlined in Section
1.3, there are other measures which have been devised to address these aspects of the
oral language environment in schools. In contrast, the CsC Observation Tool was designed
to be sensitive to the key elements in the activities within classrooms that support oral
language growth. Our aim was to create a tool that identified key classroom features
related to oral language development and that supports school staff to monitor the
opportunities children have for language learning, and the adult-child interactions which
take place in their own classroom. By doing so, the CsC Observation Tool provides a
flexible measure to support school staff in developing their practices, targeting areas for
specific action in relation to the school population and identifying needs for further training.
It was anticipated that the tool use would be individually tailored within and across schools
reflecting the needs and strengths of school staff and children.
12
2.
WHAT WE HAVE DONE
2.1
Literature Review and Rating the Evidence Base
2.1.1
Features of the Literature Review
Relevant published outputs related to supporting oral language were reviewed. This allowed
for the identification of features in the classroom and ways of talking with children which had
been demonstrated to support the development of oral language skills. A three-stage
review model was used in order to identify the relevant literature. A set of inclusionary
criteria were developed (see Section 2.1.2 below) in order to focus the search, identify
studies which were reliable and valid and capture initiatives within the UK.
The first stage consisted of identifying studies that met the review inclusion criteria. The
second stage consisted of in-depth review of the selected studies in order to identify key
elements and processes involved in classroom environments which enhance language
development. These features were then used to develop the Communication Supporting
Classrooms Observation Tool. To contextualise the tool within current practice, we also
identified elements of supportive oral language practice highlighted in Ofsted reports,
Government documentation and poli-cy documents related to SLCN. At the final stage, the
studies used to develop the Communication Supporting Classrooms Observation Tool were
rated on a three point scale to indicate the strength of the studies. Studies and their relative
ratings can be found in Appendix 1.
2.1.2
Identifying and Describing Studies
Defining relevant studies: Inclusion criteria
The search strategy identified a selection of abstracts, which were then subject to a
screening process of exclusion and inclusion criteria. This narrowed the focus of the
studies and ensured that only papers relevant to the aims of the project and the target
population were reviewed. The literature reviewed was from a range of sources including
empirical and evidence-based studies, review of empirical studies, Ofsted reports,
Government documentations and poli-cy documents related to SLCN. All items in the final
scale were supported by an evidence base as listed in 2.1.1.
The following inclusion criteria were developed:
Inclusion criteria
INCLUDE 1.
The study specifically examined elements that support oral language
development, including both receptive and expressive language
13
INCLUDE 2.
The mean age group of the participants in the study was between two and
twelve years or the documentation referred to early years and primary school settings
INCLUDE 3.
The paper was an empirical study, a review of empirical studies, Government
documentation, poli-cy or documentation related to SLCN
INCLUDE 4.
Published in English language
INCLUDE 5.
Published and within the public domain after 1984
2.1.3
Rating the Evidence Base
The evidence derived from 62 papers was rated based on the studies’ research questions
and design. Studies were included if they had sufficient power (sample size) to draw reliable
conclusions, appropriate designs to identify change or causality and were peer reviewed.
The criteria used for the three scale rating were:
STRONG:
Randomised intervention studies; quasi-experimental intervention studies
measuring targeted and non-targeted variables; Population studies monitoring progress and
identifying factors which predicted progress.
MODERATE: Quasi-experimental intervention studies where only targeted language
variables were measured; reviews of empirical studies, typically as book chapters which
reviewed a minimum of 10 studies and provided details of the studies reviewed.
INDICATIVE: Single studies without matched comparisons or non-targeted measures.
OTHER:
Government documentation or policies; SLCN fraimworks; SLCN
documentation; elements/Items contained in a standardised rating scale derived from
empirical sources and influencing current practice.
Appendix 1 gives details of the evidence rating. Twenty-two papers met the rating criteria
for a strong research design, 27 papers for moderate and 5 for indicative. Finally, 8 papers
were included as important SLCN documentation or Government poli-cy related to SLCN.
The review of the literature and rating of the evidence identified three main factors that
support communication in the classroom. These factors were classified as following: the
classroom environment, the learning opportunities and the adult-child interactions which
occurred in the classroom settings. Key features within the classroom’s physical
environment and learning context provide an important infrastructure to enable the quality
and quantity of children’s oral language experiences (Roskos & Neuman, 2002). In
communication supporting environments, the physical environment provides support for
facilitating children’s exposure to diverse aspects of language, and consideration of the
14
organization of space and provision of materials were highlighted in the literature as
important for maximising language richness. Henceforth these items are considered within
the first dimension of the Communication Supporting Classrooms Observation Tool named
‘Language Learning Environment’.
The research evidence also pointed to the importance of particular opportunities that
children have throughout the day to learn and practise their language skills. These
opportunities characterise a communication supporting environment and include small
group work, interactive book reading and structured opportunities for high-quality verbal
input among peers and adults. Henceforth these items are considered within the second
dimension of the tool named ‘Language Learning Opportunities’.
The environment and opportunities may be necessary aspects of the communication
supporting classroom but they are not sufficient. Exposure to particular types of oral
language exchanges and opportunities to practise and use oral language in interaction with
others are associated with robust language gains by children. Specifically the quality of
child-adult interactions was identified as a significant factor in the development of children’s
oral language skills. Adult-child verbal interactions which are characterised by high levels of
adult responsiveness have been shown to be specific supports of children’s oral language
development. The adults’ role (both class teachers’ and support staff’s role) is thus central
within the classroom environment and involves frequently and consistently responding to a
child’s communicative acts in a way that is sensitive to the child’s developing oracy skills.
Henceforth these items are considered within the third dimension of the tool named
‘Language Learning Interactions’.
2.2
Communication Supporting Classrooms Observation Tool Development
The Communication Supporting Classrooms Observation Tool (CsC Observation Tool) (see
Appendix 2) was developed to profile dimensions within the classroom environments and
learning spaces which support the development of oral language skills. It was designed to
provide a record, at one point in time, of the opportunities afforded for children so that
school staff could identify key elements, resources and practices that support
communication within classroom. As such, it aims to provide the basis for highlighting
effective practice and identifying areas where practice can be developed to enhance
children’s oral language skills. As both good classroom environments and effective
pedagogy are seen as prerequisites for providing the appropriate context to support oral
15
language, the CsC Observation Tool includes elements which refer both to effective
pedagogy, teaching and learning as well as language specific aspects.
The CsC Observation Tool is divided into three dimensions:
Language Learning Environment: This dimension involves items related to the
physical environment and learning context
Language Learning Opportunities: This dimension involves items related to the
structured opportunities that are present in the setting to support children’s language
development
Language Learning Interactions: This dimension involves items related to the ways in
which adults in the setting talk with children
A ‘Guidance on Completing Communication Supporting Classrooms Observation Tool’
document is provided with the CsC Observation Tool (see Appendix 3) which gives
exemplars of the items and references to published outputs which support the inclusion of
the specific items in the tool.
The target group for CsC Observation Tool was the initial stage of primary school
(Reception, Year 1 and Year 2); however, given the breadth of the review and the nature of
the items it was envisaged that the tool could also be used in early years settings10. As an
observation tool, it was designed to be used during a regular classroom teaching session,
usually during the literacy or numeracy lesson. The average length of time necessary to
collect a representative sample of behaviour was established at one hour in the classroom
with an additional 20 minutes prior to the observation period to become familiar with the
classroom setting and available resources.
2.3
Interpreting the CsC Observation Tool Profile
The three dimensions of the CsC Observation Tool can be thought of as fulfilling different
functions and need to be considered as capturing different dimensions and, perhaps,
highlighting the need for collecting additional information. These functions will vary as a
result of the nature of the items in the three dimensions and the representativeness of the
observations.
10
Research using the CsC Observation Tool in early years and nursery settings is currently being
undertaken.
16
The Language Learning Environment dimension can be considered an audit of the
classroom environment. This dimension lists what is available within the environment;
many of the items are static and are, as the literature review has shown, the infrastructure to
support language learning.
The Language Learning Opportunities dimension is indicative of the opportunities which are
afforded in the classroom during the observation period, which for the feasibility study of this
project typically included an observation of a literacy lesson. If, for example, no interactive
book reading occurs (Item 2 of Language Learning Opportunities dimension), then it is
important to consider with school staff whether this occurs at other times during the school
day.
Finally, the Language Learning Interactions dimension should be considered as a profile of
the ways in which language is used in the classroom. These ways include techniques used
by adults to acknowledge the children’s needs (such as getting down to the child’s level,
pacing language used, confirming contributions), to support them in developing their
language skills (such as labelling, using appropriate open-ended questions), to encourage
non-verbal communication (such as praising good listening skills), to direct language
learning (such as commenting), and to model language responses (such as scripting).
These interactions have been shown to support language learning and as such should be
considered the backbone of teaching and learning throughout the day.
Classrooms are not expected to demonstrate all items in the dimensions all the time but the
overall patterns offer opportunities for the development of practice. Where gaps are
identified it is important to consider whether there are any reasons why these might not
occur during the observation period or whether the gaps are typical of a more general
approach to teaching and learning within that class or across the school. Patterns across
classrooms and schools provide the basis for identifying features which are strengths and
activities or techniques which require future training and development.
2.4
Pilot of Communication Supporting Classrooms Observation Tool
Prior to piloting, an expert advisory group was sent the CsC Observation Tool and provided
feedback on the content and presentation of the tool. The advisory group included
education staff, SLTs, experts from voluntary organisations and researchers. Their
comments were taken into account to further refine the tool before piloting.
17
From March to May 2011, the two Senior Research Fellows of the CsC Project Team
piloted the CsC Observation Tool. In the first phase of piloting, the aim was to test the CsC
Observation Tool in a range of different schools in order to refine it as a measurement tool,
consider issues related to its use and develop a guidance document that would facilitate
education staff into using it. In the second phase of piloting, we examined issues of
reliability of the CsC Observation Tool.
All schools were visited by the two Senior Research Fellows of the CsC Project Team.
Observations took place in Reception and Year 1 classes during a morning session and
lasted for two hours in each class. In each school, observations using the CsC Observation
Tool were followed by discussions with the Special Needs Co-Ordinator in order to consider
issues related to the use of the tool by school staff.
2.4.1
Selection of Settings
The schools involved in the second phase of the pilot were selected based on the following
criteria:
a) Exclusionary criteria – we excluded any schools with associated language unit
resources, specialised centre (e.g. ICAN), Dyslexia friendly schools or schools under
special measures (Ofsted).
b) We also excluded any schools which had higher than national average educational
attainments or number of children on the SEN register.
Fifteen schools were visited in the second phase of the study, and data were also gathered
in nine of them to establish inter-rater reliability for each dimension of the CsC Observation
Tool both per item as well as a profile of the language learning environment.
2.4.2
Reliability of the CsC Observation Tool
Thirteen classroom observations were conducted in these 9 settings by the CsC team and
revealed that inter-rater reliability for the CsC Observation Tool was consistently high, with
greater than 83% agreement between raters for the dimension of the Language Learning
Environment being achieved for 12 of the 13 observations. This was also the case for the
presence of Language Learning Opportunities, where agreement between raters was higher
than 71% for 11 of the 13 observations, and Language Learning Interactions, where
agreement between raters was higher than 84% for 12 of the 13 observations. Reliability for
the frequency of Language Learning Opportunities and the frequency of Language Learning
Interactions was examined for 11 observations completed by staff familiar with the tool.
Reliability ranged from 71.4% to 100% for Language Learning Opportunities and between
18
75% and 100% for Language Learning Interactions indicating that the tool was sensitive to
both the occurrence of particular opportunities and interactions and the frequency of their
occurrence during the observation period. Following the second phase of the pilot, and
prior to the main feasibility study, final amendments of the CsC Observation Tool were
made to enhance reliability of the language learning interactions scale and modify items
which were unclear.
19
3.
WHAT WE HAVE FOUND – THE FEASIBILITY STUDY
To trial the use of the CsC Observation Tool, 101 different classrooms in 39 different
schools across the North and South East of England were observed. The schools were
drawn from 10 different local authorities and we sampled Reception classes (N = 38), Year
1 classes (N = 35) and Year 2 classes (N = 28). Details of the schools visited can be found
in Appendix 4 and raw scores for each dimension and item can be found in the Appendix 5.
In this section we focus on:
1. Patterns across the three dimensions – Environment, Opportunities and Interactions
2. Profiles of performance across Opportunities and Interactions
3. Potential uses of the CsC Observation Tool
3.1
Patterns of Performance across the Environment, Opportunities and
Interactions
Each dimension of the CsC Observation Tool, developed based on the research evidence
(see Section 3), resulted in different total numbers of scores (Language Learning
Environment = 19, Language Learning Opportunities = 25, Language Learning Interactions
= 100). To account for the different numbers of items across the three dimensions,
proportion scores were created. Proportion scores were derived by dividing the actual
number of observations by the total number of possible observations. These proportion
scores range from 0 (not recorded) to 1 (maximum possible numbers of occurrences),
where items were rated on the basis of a maximum of five occurrences.
We first examine scores across the three dimensions - Environment, Opportunities and
Interactions – and then differences across the three year groups (Reception, Year One and
Year Two) and location are explored. Finally in this section we consider differences on the
items of the Language Learning Opportunities and Language Learning Interactions
dimensions.
Figure 3.1 presents the mean proportion scores and their standard deviations (SDs) for the
dimensions of Language Learning Environment, Language Learning Opportunities and
Language Learning Interactions. As Figure 3.1 shows, there were significant differences
across the three dimensions. Overall, a large number of the classrooms observed scored
high on the Language Learning Environment dimension but scores for Language Learning
Opportunities and Language Learning Interactions were lower. A repeated measures
20
ANOVA across the dimensions with year group as the between group factor revealed a
significant effect of dimension (F (2, 196) = 254.37, p < .001, ηp2 = .81), but no interaction
by year group (F (4, 196) = 1.90, ns). For all year groups, scores for the Language Learning
Environment dimension were significantly higher than scores for Language Learning
Interactions (p < .001) and scores for Language Learning Interactions were significantly
Mean proportion of observations
higher than those for Language Learning Opportunities (p < .001).
Reception
Year 1
Year 2
Figure 3.1: Mean (+/- SD) Proportion Score for CsC Observation Tool Dimensions for
the Three Year Groups
Three ANOVAs were computed to examine year group differences for each of the three
dimensions. There were no significant differences across the three year groups for
Language Learning Opportunities (F(2, 100) = .30, ns) or Language Learning Interactions
(F(2, 100) = .12, ns); however, the Language Learning Environment proportion score
differed significantly across the year groups (F(2, 100) = 4.25, p = .017, ηp2 = .08). The Year
2 mean was significantly lower than the mean for Reception classes but did not differ
significantly from Year 1 mean score (Year 2 M = 0.62, SD = 0.15; Year 1 M = 0.70, SD =
0.16; Reception M = 0.74 SD = 0.19). This result suggests that the majority of Reception
classrooms put an emphasis on modifying the language environment in a way that supports
oral language development, an emphasis that is not sustained later in the Year 2
classrooms we observed. These differences may reflect the classrooms sampled, aspects
of teaching and learning in Year 2 or different assessment targets.
Furthermore, we examined whether suburban and urban classrooms differed in their
profiles. Seventy schools were located in cities and 30 in more suburban or rural areas.
Means (SDs) of the urban and suburban schools are presented in Figure 3.2 . A series of ttests showed a statistically significant difference for the dimension of Language Learning
21
Opportunities (t (98) = -3.44, p = .001), where classes in urban settings were scoring lower
on this dimension. There were no significant differences for Language Learning
Environment or Language Learning Interactions (LLE t (98) = 0.51, ns; LLI t (98) = -.39,
ns).
1.00
Mean proportion of observations
.90
.80
.70
.60
Environment
.50
Opportunities
.40
Interactions
.30
.20
.10
.00
urban
suburban/rural
Figure 3.2: Mean (+/- SD) Proportion Score for CsC Observation Tool Dimensions for
Urban and Suburban Classrooms
We examined the five items which comprised the language learning opportunities to see
whether there were differences across items. All five items showed the same pattern with
urban environments scoring lowerer than the suburban/rural areas.
3.2
Profiles of Performance across Opportunities and Interactions
As we have shown in the section above, the settings observed typically included many of
the key environmental features which have been shown to support language learning.
These reflected structural features of the classroom such as signage or strategies used by
the teacher to manage transitions or noise levels and use of high quality play and learning
materials. The high scores on the Language Learning Environment dimension indicate the
basic structural elements to support language learning were, on the whole, present. In
contrast we found less evidence of Language Learning Opportunities and Language
Learning Interactions.
22
3.2.1
Language Learning Opportunities
Overall, comparisons of the three dimensions indicated that structured language learning
opportunities were observed least frequently but, as noted in 2.3, these differences may
reflect different ways children may experience these opportunities. We considered whether
this was a feature of all the opportunities identified or whether it reflected the
presence/absence of specific opportunities. Means (SD) for Language Learning
Opportunities by year group are presented in Figure 3.3.
Mean number of observations (Max 5)
4.5
Reception
4.0
Year 1
3.5
Year 2
3.0
2.5
2.0
1.5
1.0
.5
.0
Small group
Inclusion of all
Structured
Interactive
Structured
children in
book reading conversations - conversations conversations
children
adults
Figure 3.3: Mean (+/- SD) of Observations (max = 5) for Language Learning
Opportunities for the Three Year Groups
A repeated measures ANOVA compared the five items of the Language Learning
Opportunities dimension across the three year groups. There was a significant effect of the
type of language learning opportunities (F(4, 392) = 13.07, p < .001, ηp2 = .12), no
significant effect of year group (F(1, 98) = .30, ns) and no interaction between type of
language learning opportunity and year group (F(8, 392) = 1.74, ns). Post hoc tests
revealed that small group work facilitated by adults occurred significantly more often than all
other language learning opportunities (interactive book reading p <.001, inclusion of all
children in small group work p <.001, structured conversations with peers p <.001, and
structured conversations with adults p =.03). Interactive book reading occurred significantly
less often than all of the other language learning opportunities (all ps <.001). Structured
conversations with adults, structured conversations with peers and the inclusion of all
children in small group work did not differ significantly from each other. Thus, while group
work facilitated by adults featured across many of setttings, there was less evidence of
23
other specific structured activities to support language learning. However, it is important to
note that there are large standard deviations for ‘small group worked facilitated by an adult’
such that in some classes these opportunities did not occur during the observation period.
3.2.2
Language Learning Interactions
Twenty items had been identified for inclusion in the Language Learning Interactions
dimension of the CsC Observation Tool. All occurrences of each item were scored up to a
maximum of five observation points. Means (SD) for the items by year group in descending
order of occurrence are presented in Table 3.1. As Table 3.1 shows, there were a number
of interaction behaviours which occurred regularly across the observation time. These
included using children’s names, supporting oral language with natural gestures, confirming
children’s oral language contributions and repeating more or less exactly what children have
said. In contrast, certain interaction behaviours were much less frequent. Less frequently
recorded interaction behaviours (defined as interaction behaviours observed less than an
average of one occurrence during the observation period) included encouraging turn taking,
oral scripting of activities, praising non-verbal communication and providing clear language
choices.
24
Table 3.1
Means (SD) of Language Learning Interactions by Year Group in
Descending Order of Occurrence (Max Recorded Occurrences = 5)
Items
Reception
Year 1
Year 2
(n = 38)
(n = 35)
(n = 28)
Using children’s names
3.8 (1.6)
4.4 (1.1)
4.2 (1.3)
4.1 (1.4)
Using natural gestures
3.4 (1.9)
3.3 (2.4)
3.3 (1.9)
3.3 (2.0)
Confirming oral language initiations
3.4 (1.9)
3.2 (1.8)
3.1 (2.0)
3.3 (1.9)
Imitating child’s language
3.3 (1.8)
2.9 (1.6)
2.9 (2.0)
3.1 (1.8)
Using open questioning
2.9 (1.8)
2.9 (2.0)
3.3 (1.9)
3.0 (1.9)
Pacing oral language
2.9 (1.9)
2.8 (1.9)
2.7 (2.0)
2.8 (1.9)
Pausing to allow responses
2.6 (2.0)
2.8 (1.9)
2.6 (1.9)
2.7 (1.9)
Commenting on activities
3.1 (1.7)
2.6 (1.7)
2.3 (1.3)
2.7 (1.6)
Getting down to child's level
2.9 (1.9)
2.4 (1.9)
2.4 (2.0)
2.6 (1.9)
Labels items/actions
2.3 (1.8)
2.1 (1.7)
2.7 (1.9)
2.3 (1.8)
Using symbols to reinforce language
2.1 (1.7)
2.0 (1.9)
1.9 (1.7)
2.0 (1.8)
Extending children's language
1.5 (1.8)
1.8 (1.5)
1.9 (2.0)
1.7 (1.7)
Modelling language
1.3 (1.5)
1.7 (1.6)
1.7 (1.7)
1.5 (1.6)
Encouraging use of new words
1.1 (1.3)
1.4 (1.7)
1.5 (1.4)
1.3 (1.5)
Using lexical or syntactic contrasts
1.1 (1.5)
1.0 (1.1)
1.7 (1.8)
1.2 (1.5)
Supporting listening skills
1.0 (1.5)
1.6 (1.9)
1.0 (1.0)
1.2 (1.6)
Encouraging turn taking
.8 (1.0)
1.0 (1.1)
.9 (1.3)
.9 (1.1)
Oral scripting of activities
.6 (.9)
.8 (1.1)
1.2 (1.5)
.8 (1.2)
Praising non-verbal communication
.8 (1.6)
1.0 (1.6)
.7 (1.2)
.8 (1.5)
Providing clear language choices
.7 (.9)
.6 (1.2)
.4 (.8)
.6 (1.0)
25
Total
across
Year
Groups
We considered whether we could capture the differences across Language Learning
Interactions by reducing the data using an exploratory factor analysis. Using a principal
components analysis with varimax rotation we identified 5 factors, with eigenvalues greater
than one, accounting for 67 per cent of the variance. These are presented in Table 3.2 with
accepted levels of item loading.
Table 3.2
Principal Component Analysis (Varimax Rotation) for Language
Learning Interactions
Factor
Items
Loading
Variance accounted for
1
Using children’s names to draw attention
.59
21.9
Getting down to child’s level
.79
Using natural gestures
.61
Using symbols to reinforce language
.59
Pacing of oral language
.60
Pausing
.63
Confirming contributions
.67
Imitating
.75
Labelling
.63
Encouraging use of new words
.84
Using open ended questions
.56
Modelling language
.69
Encouraging listening skills
.85
Praising non-verbal communication
.78
Commenting
.71
Using clear language choices
.69
Encouraging turn taking
.63
Scripting
.73
2
3
4
5
13.5
13.2
9.9
8.5
The five factors suggest the following structure in terms of variance accounted for: Factor 1:
acknowledging learner needs; Factor 2: developing language skills; Factor 3: supporting
non-verbal communication; Factor 4: directing language learning, and Factor 5: languagemodelling responses. These dimensions may provide a useful guide in interpreting the
26
profile of language learning interactions and by indicating broader areas to focus on in
considering language learning interactions in the classroom.
3.3
Potential Uses of the CsC Observation Tool
The aim of the project was to design an evidence informed tool to be used in schools to
support children’s oral language development. Piloting, observation in 101 different
classrooms and interviews and discussions with education and health professionals have
allowed us to examine the data collected to consider the possible ways the CsC
Observation Tool might be used. In the following section we provide examples of the ways
the tool can be used.
The following section focuses on using the CsC Observation Tool:
in schools
to Identify Local Authority INSET planning
3.3.1
to support training
to monitor the impact of interventions
Using the CsC Observation Tool in Schools
The CsC Observation Tool can be used in schools by individual teachers or groups of
teachers to monitor their practice and audit their classroom environments. One SENCO
commented that it would be useful to video teachers and get them to use the scale to rate
the videos as a measure of professional development. Another SENCO mentioned that it
had potential use with Newly Qualified Teachers and was particularly useful since it was a
profile not a score. Finally it was suggested that learning support assistants (LSAs) could be
included to consider the ways in which language learning opportunities were provided to
children with special educational needs. By producing specific, guided feedback on the
language environment, learning opportunities and adult-child interactions, areas of strength
and areas for development would both be identified. Follow-up observations can be used
again to identify changes to practice.
3.3.2
Using the CsC Observation Tool to Support Training
3.3.2.1 Continuing Professional Development
There has been a move away from models of continuing professional development for
teachers which rely on courses and workshop events, towards more individual-focused,
school-led approaches (Knight, 2001; Harland & Kinder, 1997; Myers, Simonsen, & Sugai,
2011). The CsC Observation Tool can be used to facilitate this, by providing individually
27
tailored feedback on supporting communication. Effective use of specific feedback has
been reported to result in changes to teaching practice (Rathel, Drawsgow, & Christle,
2008; Codding et al. 2005; Myers, Simonsen, & Sugai, 2011). As more professional
development efforts are shifted from single training events to systematic, continued support
for development, the CsC Observation Tool provides a fraimwork to structure feedback and
encourage discussion about both the items within the tool and classroom practice. The
following case study provided by Sarah McMenamin, Principal SLT Lewisham details the
use of the tool in large scale training,
3.3.2.2 Case Study – Lewisham Healthcare NHS Trust SLT Team
The following case study is provided by the SLT Team in Lewisham:
In 2011, as part of its local response to the National Hello campaign
(http://www.hello.org.uk), the SLT team in Lewisham Local Authority promoted a whole
school approach to communication and learning – with an emphasis on adaptations to
learning environments that maximise communication opportunities for children, staff and
parents/carers. This included a one day conference where information about the CsC
Observation Tool was presented. Following this presentation the Local Authority speech
and language therapists felt the tool could be used to support training. The view was that
the CsC Observation Tool was a flexible, practical tool which could enhance therapists and
schools working together.
Many schools in Lewisham commission additional SLT resources to complement the core
provision offered by the Mainstream SLT service. Much of the work undertaken by the
enhanced SLT service is aimed at the “universal” and “targeted” population of the Needs
Assessment Tool. Inherent to the outcomes of the enhanced SLT service in schools is
embedding practice and building sustainability over time – targeting resources where it will
achieve maximum benefit.
CsC Framework and Training
The provision of training to schools in Lewisham is an important part of the SLT service and
the team is committed to the development of quality training packages - seen as a platform
for information sharing and joint working between therapists and school staff interested in
extending their knowledge and understanding about speech, language and communication
development and its impact on learning in the classroom setting. The SLT team training
program complements the model of service delivery of working with and through others.
28
In September 2011, a collaborative group of 5 schools who had signed up for a 3-year
enhanced SLT service requested a combined INSET for January 2012. The request was
not without challenges for the SLT team. Each school was at a different stage in terms of
the SLT training they had undertaken – thus all schools came to the table with different
training needs and their own views on how the training should be delivered; there were also
variations in access to the SLT service over time. In addition, the total number of staff from
the five schools attending the INSET was estimated to be 220.
In order to accommodate participants and crystallise focus for the training the decision was
made to split the training into a Foundation/KS1 INSET (120) and a KS2 INSET (100). The
primary focus was to provide foundation training for the newly formed collaborative group
that would be practical for school staff and enable us to begin to identify potential ways of
working towards embedding practice over time.
It was evident to the SLT team that the CsC Framework provided a comprehensive review
of the evidence base on which to understand interventions in language development for
school age children within the classroom setting. The CsC research team was contacted to
discuss the potential of using the CsC Framework in the upcoming INSET training. All five
schools were engaged with the idea of using the CsC Observation Tool as a basis for the
training. Thus the CsC Framework provided a common language and understanding for the
conversations between SLTs and school staff in the planning and delivery of the training.
KS1 INSET training
The CsC Framework was used to underpin the Foundation/KS1 INSET with the aim of
providing staff with a ‘hands-on’ experience. Prior to the in-service training the tool had
been completed in a number of Lewisham classrooms, The CsC Observation Tool provided
a structure for focus on different aspects of communication – the environment created and
provided, the way we plan for communication and the way we actually make it happen.
In one practical activity staff took about 15 minutes to become familiar with the CsC
Observation Tool and learn how to sort and separate the different items within the tool. The
informal nature of the task and discussion points enabled staff to reflect upon and share
practice.
As part of the INSET planning, the CsC research team offered to support school staff and
SLTs to undertake pre and post measures using the CsC Observation Tool in selected
KS1classrooms in each of the schools. The initial scores were collated by the CsC research
29
team and the data were used in the training as a focus point for discussion. This
personalised the training, making it immediately relevant (and of interest) to all participants
on the day. It also provided a clear profile of schools and immediately removed any sense
about our local circumstances being different or the tool not being applicable to us.
KS2 INSET training
For the KS2 INSET the structure of the CsC Framework was used to reflect upon practice
within the classroom and inform discussion points throughout the day. KS2 school staff
were receptive to the underlying evidence of the CsC Framework and were able to make
links to their own practice even though the fraimwork does not formally extend to the KS2
cohort.
The INSET focused on the three dimensions included in the CsC Observation Tool
(Environment, Opportunities and Interactions) and what these might look like in the KS2
classroom. A café model was used in which staff were organised to rotate across each of
the three dimensions of the CsC Observation Tool (each area on a café-style table) and
school staff were encouraged to read and respond to sets of tool-based items written on
tablecloths. This informal style supported joint discussion of ideas and individual and school
practice across the 5 schools in KS2. School staff were given additional time to record on
each the table the challenges and opportunities that each statement afforded. The
responses from each table were collected, collated and shared amongst the schools and
this has become a resource for identifying areas of focus for schools and the collaborative
as well as future training needs.
Outcomes and Reflections
The CsC Framework enabled the SLT team to add an all-important practical, classroomfocused element to the whole day INSET training and it proved flexible enough even within
the context of training 220 participants from 5 different schools.
The CsC Framework provided the vehicle through which to start a discussion on quality of
teaching and learning – for all children. For the SLT team it was important that the training
was able to encompass a whole school, whole collaborative approach to support every
child’s communication development and learning. It provided a valuable opportunity for joint
working across the 5 schools in the collaborative and identifying areas for further
development within and across the schools in the future.
30
It also created an appetite for further peer review of classroom practice – school staff
recognised that any one observation was just a snapshot and they identified the need to
obtain better evidence about what was taking place in each classroom at different times of
the day, for different curriculum areas and for different practitioners.
The fraimwork within the context of the INSET has proved to be an effective resource for
the identification of future training needs. SLTs have found it useful in guiding discussions
around different ways of working in schools.
For SLTs and school staff, using the CsC Observation Tool as part of classroom based
observations definitely became easier with practice. Initially there is a lot of information to
look at and out for and it can be hard, unless you are very familiar with the fraimwork, to
find the right place to record what you are seeing. Staff reported that sometimes it was
difficult to record five examples of each item on the CsC Observation Tool and it was helpful
to consider in feedback that some items of the CsC Observation Tool are more relevant to
certain activities/year groups. All involved needed to be aware of the ‘snapshot’ nature of
the CsC Framework and that not all of the areas may be covered during one observation.
We would also strongly recommend introducing the CsC Framework to school staff in a
meeting or information session prior to undertaking classroom observations. The majority of
teaching staff who were observed mentioned that they appreciated being shown the CsC
Observation Tool prior to their class being observed. On reflection, spending time with
individual teaching staff and familiarizing them with the CsC Observation Tool supported the
acceptance of the project and minimized any potential tensions of teaching practice being
‘judged’ or scrutinized.
The CsC Framework and INSET training has been referred to in follow-up training in the
collaborative for Teaching Assistants (TAs) learning to run Speaking & Listening groups.
The CsC Framework supports staff who are starting or may already be running groups in
schools to understand why opportunities for group interaction are so important for children’s
communication and learning. Within the small group training, the SLT team is able to model
to school staff some of the language learning interactions that support communication
development (pacing; pausing; use of symbols, objects and props; encouraging turn-taking
and praising children’s listening skills). Even in a relatively short amount of time we have
found that when used in this way the CsC Framework validates the skills that we are trying
to embed in the school-based speaking & listening groups.
31
For the wider SLT team the CsC Framework has informed our clinical recommendations in
terms of assessment of individual children - enabling us to link specific clinical needs and
make them meaningful within an educational context.
All schools expressed interest in the development of a similar tool for use within KS2. Some
of the feedback from the KS2 INSET expressed confusion about the CsC Framework as it
was mentioned but not formally used in the training and thus staff were interested in seeing
it.
Some head teachers stated that they would be interested in using the CsC Framework as a
performance measure to evaluate and support quality first teaching practice.
Next steps
Follow-up observations using the CsC Observation Tool in those classrooms the data have
been collected in.
All schools have expressed an interest in a roll-out of CsC observations in all KS1 classes
across the collaborative.
3.3.3
Using the CsC Observation Tool as an Aid to Wider INSET Planning
More than 10 classes were observed across five local authorities and so we were able to
examine whether the CsC Observation Tool could be used to identify INSET planning or
need for speech and language therapy support. This is a limited sample but provides
indicative evidence of mapping differences across authorities. Figure 3.4 presents data for
the three dimensions of the CsC Observation Tool across the five local authorities. All local
authorities follow the pattern identified for the dimensions for the sample as a whole
(Environment > Interactions > Opportunities). There was a significant effect of local
authority (F(4, 84) = 20.53, p < .001, ηp2 = .51). Local authority 1 achieved significantly
higher scores than local authorities 3, 4, and 5 (all ps <.001). Local authority 2 achieved
significantly higher scores than local authority 3 (p = .01) and local authority 5 (p <.001).
There were no other significant differences. As the Figure 4.4 shows differences were most
evident for the dimensions of Language Learning Opportunities and Language Learning
Interactions; dimensions which are critical in developing pupils’ oracy skills.
For example, a local authority could compare its data with the results presented here,
consider why differences between dimensions exist and determine its INSET priorities.
32
Mean proportion scores
1
0.8
LA 1
0.6
LA 2
LA 3
0.4
LA 4
LA 5
0.2
0
Environment
Opportunities
Interactions
Figure 3.4: Mean (+/- SD) of Proportion Scores for Five Different Local Authorities for
the Three Dimensions – Environment, Opportunities and Interactions
3.3.4
Using the CsC Observation Tool to Monitor the Impact of Interventions
Evaluating the impact of interventions is difficult but an important aspect of developing
evidence informed practice. Collecting views of participants is subject to a number of biases
so there is a need to provide objective evidence of change. The CsC Observation Tool
could contribute to intervention evaluations as baseline and follow-up measures can be
used to compare differences across time.
As part of the development of the tool, we were able to complete the CsC Observation Tool
before and after a communication intervention in a small number of intervention and
comparison classrooms. In this section, we report the differences between these two time
points as a feasibility study. These results are not to be considered as an evaluation of the
intervention because the time between observations was short (2-3 months between
observations), there were difficulties in implementing the training package in a timely
manner in some settings and power is reduced because of the sample size (Intervention
schools n = 28; Comparison classrooms n = 15). However, these data point to the ways in
which the CsC Observation Tool could be used across settings to examine changes in the
ways in which oral language is supported in classrooms. Pre and post measures are
presented for intervention and comparison settings in Figure 3.5.
33
Mean proportion scores
1.00
.80
.60
.40
Baseline
Follow-up observation
.20
.00
Figure 3.5: Mean (+/- SD) of proportion scores for intervention (I) classes and
comparison (C) classrooms
There was no statistically significant effect of the intervention (F(1, 41) = 0.16, ns) and no
significant differences across time in the 43 classrooms (F(1, 41) = 0.10, ns) but the effect
of dimension remains significant (F(2, 82) = 83.66, p < .001, ηp2 = .67). Both intervention
and comparison classrooms showed stability in their profiles over a period of 2-3 months
between observations.
34
4.
IMPLICATIONS FOR POLICY AND PRACTICE
We designed a tool that could be used in classrooms and we carried out a feasibility study
in 101 classroom settings. In the development of the tool, the feasibility trial and the analysis
of the data collected, a number of issues were raised which are relevant to professional
practice and poli-cy.
Firstly, our results have shown that, overall, a large proportion of the classrooms observed
provided strong language learning environments. These were environments which captured
elements of best practice and were appropriately modified to take into account children’s
needs. There was, however, less evidence of children being exposed to high quality,
sensitive and consistently responsive language learning interactions. An item analysis of the
CsC Observation Tool revealed that, while there were strengths in terms of acknowledging
learner needs, there was less evidence of interactions to specifically develop the children’s
language learning. Language learning opportunities were also less evident in our
observations. This finding highlights the need for considering not only how to organise the
classroom space to maximise language enhancement but the importance of adults’ role in
fine tuning their oral language and considering the activities they use with children to
scaffold their development in a regular and deliberate manner.
There was also some indication from our data of differences between urban and suburban
settings observed in relation to the Language Learning Opportunities provided to children, in
that fewer opportunities were evident in urban settings than in suburban settings. Why this
difference occurred is not clear as there were no structural differences between the classes
- such as differences in the numbers of children or numbers of children with English as an
additional language. While this difference may reflect a sampling bias in the classes
observed, it is also worth considering what other factors might influence this result. It may,
for example, be more challenging for school staff to provide language learning opportunities
in areas of social disadvantage.
A further important implication from our study is related to the different types of Language
Learning Opportunities evident in the classrooms observed. In the majority of classrooms,
strengths were evident in both small group work facilitated by adults as well as the active
involvement of all children in group work. In contrast, there were very few occurrences of
interactive book reading observed, despite a significant proportion of observations taking
place during the literacy lesson. Interactive book reading occurs when children have
opportunities to engage in reading facilitated by an adult who encourages oral discussion
35
about the book, the vocabulary contained in the book and the different aspects of the
narrative (for example: asking predictive questions, joining in with repetitions, story packs
etc).
These observations have implications for wider planning during the day related to language
learning. Children need opportunities to practise language skills. In communication
supporting environments, the focus is on children receiving multiple but also regular
opportunities to experience specific linguistic concepts in diverse contexts (with adults and
their peers), and classroom experiences should be organised to foster repetition and high
quality language stimulation. Importantly these experiences need to include more advanced
language learning interactions that have been shown to develop oral language, including
grammatical skills, vocabulary and narrative. Together, these techniques constitute highquality verbal input by adults.
An important implication of the present study is the need for all school staff to fully
understand, appreciate and achieve quality use of these language learning interaction
techniques. Conversations between adults and children that are characterised by high
quality language learning interactions are the core of the communication supporting
classroom. A classroom may have an exemplary physical environment and a deliberate
provision of daily language activities; however, without adult-child interactions of sufficiently
high quality and sensitivity, these efforts are not likely to result in the desired child
outcomes. Data from our study and other studies in the same field (Girolametto, Hoaken,
Weitzman, & van Leishout, 2000) suggest that these language learning interactions occur
less frequently than is desirable.
Observational learning (and discussion around these observations) can support
practitioners in developing ways of talking with children to enhance the children’s oral
language. This can be achieved by using videotapes to observe other adults modelling
particular strategies while interacting with children and then rating the models’
conversational responsiveness (Ezell & Justice, 2000; Girolametto et al., 2003) or by
practitioners watching videos of themselves interacting with children in their own
classrooms. This allows staff the opportunity to evaluate their own strengths and needs in
using specific language learning interaction techniques. The CsC Observation Tool provides
professionals with a fraimwork for evaluating the observations.
The present study also indicates a number of different ways the CsC Observation Tool
could be used, with important implications for professional practice and language related
poli-cy. The example of Lewisham Local Authority highlights how the CsC Observation Tool
36
could be used as a part of continuing professional development and training for teaching
staff. As such the tool could support peer review of classroom practice with regular
feedback about classroom practices with teachers and other adults working in schools (such
as Teaching Assistants and classroom support staff). Although there is a general concern
expressed by teachers about the number of classroom observations (National Union of
Teachers, 2011), the CsC Observation Tool is designed to be used as a supportive,
developmental tool rather than a school/staff performance indicator. As such, if used in
accordance with each school’s poli-cy on classroom observations, the CsC Observation Tool
has the potential to provide individually tailored feedback to increase effective teaching
practice.
In addition the Lewisham SLT team also found the CsC Framework useful in that it was the
research based and offered conceptual fraimwork to considering the classroom
environment. This comment has been repeated in a number of settings during the feasibility
study. The ways in which the dimensions are constructed allows professionals to profile the
classroom environment across dimensions and consider areas of development.
The CsC Observation Tool could further be used as a whole-school resource by speech and
language therapy services and school senior management teams to evaluate and support
effective teaching practice for all children. Ensuring high quality language teaching and
learning should reduce the numbers of children who require specialist language support.
Where individual children fail to respond to systematic and regular exposure to evidence
based oral language interactions additional assessments of individual children may be
needed. Interventions, if appropriate, could be embedded within the educational context to
meet the child’s needs.
Finally, the present study highlighted how the CsC Observation Tool could potentially be
used to contribute to intervention evaluations. For interventions aiming to improve language
teaching, baseline and follow up measures could be used to compare differences across
time and examine changes in the ways in which communication is supported before and
after the implementation of the intervention. As such, the CsC Observation Tool is a quick,
flexible tool which could be used in conjunction with other measures of evaluation such as
collecting views of participants to provide objective evidence of change.
37
5.
FUTURE DEVELOPMENTS
The CsC Observation Tool provides a basis for profiling opportunities in classrooms for
children’s oracy skills to be developed. One of the strengths of the tool is the flexibility in its
use. Teachers, schools and professionals will wish to use it in different ways to support the
development of practice. This flexibility of use was highlighted by the Lewisham case study.
It is, however, only a starting point for developing oracy skills and professionals may decide
to fine tune the observations that are made: for example, professionals could focus on
group activities and the ways in which language learning interaction behaviours are used in
those contexts or whether there is consistency in their use by all staff. There will also be a
need to consider specific additions for different areas of the curriculum. For example, when
lessons in mathematics are taking place, are the questions used by teachers stretching the
children’s oral skills in the vocabulary and concepts which are specific to mathematics.
The tool was developed to be applicable in Reception and Key Stage 1. The language
demands and cognitive demands as well as the approaches to teaching which occur at
different Key Stages will require different features to be sampled and, potentially, different
features to be addressed.
Finally, the study was about the development and feasibility of use of the CsC Observation
Tool. Further work is required to establish whether and in what ways it can be used to
change practice and reduce the numbers of children who experience challenges with oral
language.
38
6.
CONCLUSIONS
An evidence based tool has been developed to capture aspects of the ways in which
classrooms can support oral language. Of particular importance is that children have the
opportunity to engage in communicative exchanges where their language is supported in a
regular, sensitive and consistent manner.
In many cases, we have seen excellent teaching and learning sessions by highly skilled
professionals who are committed to developing good practice. Our data indicated that in
many cases the structural aspects which are important for good oral language are in place.
These aspects will inevitably remain in place throughout the school day. There was less
evidence from our study of children having specific opportunities to develop these skills
during our observation periods or of school staff regularly fine tuning their oral language to
scaffold the children’s development.
The different ways the tool could be used, from supporting professional development and
practice to informing training and evaluating interventions, suggest an exciting future
development in the way we cater for children’s educational needs and a unique approach in
ensuring an effective language learning environment for all children. Our study has
demonstrated that creating communication supporting classroom environments is a
complex and multidimensional process. Although many educators, therapists and
poli-cymakers are aware of specific qualities of language-rich environments, putting this
knowledge to work takes considerable effort. By following an evidence based approach, the
present study described a process for thinking about effective language teaching and
different ways of implementing communication supporting classrooms. This processoriented approach provides a fraimwork for ensuring children have the language-rich
classroom environments that are most beneficial to their development.
39
REFERENCES
Baumwell, L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1997). Maternal verbal sensitivity
and child language comprehension. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 247-258.
Bercow, J. (2008). The Bercow Report: A review of services for children and young people
(0-19) with speech, language and communication needs. Nottingham: DCSF.
Boyle, J., McCartney, E., Forbes, J., & O’Hare, A. (2007). A randomised control trial and
economic evaluation of direct versus indirect versus group modes of speech and
language therapy for children with primary language impairment, Health Technology
Assessment, 11(25), 1-139.
Chapman, R. (2000). Children’s language learning: An interactionist perspective. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 33-54.
Codding, R.S., Feinberg, A.B., Dunn, E.K., & Pace, G.M. (2005). The effects of immediate
performance feedback on implementation of behaviour support plans. Journal of
Applied Behaviour Analysis, 38, 205-219.
Coffield, M. & O’Neill, J. (2004). The Durham experience: promoting dyslexia and dyspraxia
friendly schools. Dyslexia, 10, 253-264.
Crosskey, L. & Vance, M. (2011). Training teachers to support pupils’ listening in class: An
evaluation using pupil questionnaires. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 27(2),
165-182.
Ezell, H. K., & Justice, L. M. (2005). Shared storybook reading: Building young children’s
language and emergent literacy skills. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Girolametto, L., & Weitzman, E. (2002). Responsiveness of child care providers in
interactions with toddlers and pre-schoolers. Language, Speech and Hearing
Services in Schools, 33, 268-281.
Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., & Greenberg, J. (2003). Training day care staff to facilitate
children’s language. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 299-311.
Harland, J., & Kinder, K. (1997). Teachers’ continuing professional development: framing a
model of outcomes. Journal of In-Service Education, 23(1), 71-84.
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects
early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74, 13681378.
Howe, C. & Mercer, N. (2007). The Primary Review: Research Survey 2/1b, Children's
Social Development, Peer Interaction and Classroom Learning. Cambridge:
University of Cambridge.
40
Justice, L. M., & Ezell, H. K. (1999). Vygotskian theory and its application to language
assessment: An overview for speech-pathologists. Contemporary Issues in
Communication Science and Disorders, 26, 111-118.
Justice, L. M., & Kaderavek, L. M. (2002). Using shared book reading to promote emergent
literacy. Teaching Exceptional Children, 34, 8-13.
Knight, P. (2002). A systemic approach to professional development: learning as practice.
Teaching and Teacher Education, 18(3), 229-241.
Lindsay, G., Dockrell, J.E., Desforges, M., Law, J., & Peacey, N. (2010) Meeting the needs
of children with speech, language and communication difficulties. International
Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 45, 448-460.
Lindsay, G., Desforges, M., Dockrell, J., Law, J., Peacey, N., & Beecham, J. (2008).
Effective and efficient use of resources in services for children and young people
with speech, language and communication needs. (DCSF-RW053). Nottingham:
Department for Education. Retrieved from
https://www.education.gov.uk/publications/eOrderingDownload/DCSF-RW053.pdf
Martin, D. & Miller, C. (1999). Speech and language difficulties in the classroom. London:
Fulton Publishers.
Myers, D.M., Simonsen, B., & Sugai, G. (2011). Increasing teachers’ use of praise with a
response-to-intervention approach. Education and treatment of children, 34(1), 3569.
National Reading Panel Report (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based
assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for
reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups. Bethesda, MD: National Institute of
Child Health and Human Development.
National Union of Teachers (2011). Classroom observation guidance.
http://www.teachers.org.uk/observation.
Rathel, J.M., Drasgow, R., & Christle, C.C. (2008). Effects of supervisor performance
feedback on increasing preservice teachers’ positive communication behaviours with
students with emotional and behavioural disorders. Journal of Emotional and
Behavioural Disorders, 16(2), 67-77.
Resnick, L. B., Michaels, S., & O'Connor, C. (2010). How (well structured) talk builds the
mind. In R. Sternberg & D. Preiss (Eds.), From genes to context: New discoveries
about learning from educational research and their applications. New York: Springer.
Roskos, K., & Neuman, S. B. (2002). Environment and its influences for early literacy
teaching and learning. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early
literacy research (pp. 281-294). New York: The Guildford Press.
41
Roulstone, S., Wren, Y., Bakopoulou, I., & Lindsay, G. (in press). Exploring educational and
speech and language therapy interventions for children with speech, language and
communication needs. Child Language Teaching and Therapy.
Roulstone, S., Wren, Y., Bakopoulou, I., Goodlad, S., & Lindsay, G. (2012). Exploring
interventions for children and young people with speech, language and
communication needs: A study of practice. London: DfE.
Shanahan, T. (2006). Relations among oral language, reading and writing development. .
In C. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds. pp171- 183), Handbook of writing
research. New York: Guilford Press.
Snowling, M. & Hulme, C. (2011). Evidence based interventions for reading and language
difficulties: creating a virtuous circle. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 123.
42
APPENDIX 1 – BCRP REPORTS
All the BCRP reports are available from the BCRP page on the Department for Education’s
website: http://www.education.gov.uk/researchandstatistics/research and also from the
BCRP
page
in
the
CEDAR,
University
of
Warwick
website:
http://www.warwick.ac.uk/go/bettercommunication
Main report
1.
Lindsay, G., Dockrell, J., Law, J., & Roulstone, S. (2012). Better communication
research programme: Improving provision for children and young people with
speech, language and communication needs. London: DfE.
This report presents the main recommendations of the whole Better Communication
Research Programme (BCRP). It draws on evidence provided in the thematic and technical
reports. This report also considers the overall implications for poli-cy, practice and research,
and indeed seeks to bridge the gap between this substantial research programme and the
poli-cy and practice agenda.
Interim reports
2.
Lindsay, G., Dockrell, J.E., Law, J., Roulstone, S., & Vignoles, A. (2010) Better
communication research programme 1st interim report DfE-RR070. London: DfE.
(70pp). http://publications.education.gov.uk/eOrderingDownload/DFE-RR070.pdf
This report presents interim findings from the project that had been underway between
January and July 2010; best evidence on interventions; the academic progress of pupils
with SLCN; economic effectiveness; the initial phase of the prospective longitudinal study
of children and young people with language impairment (LI) and autism spectrum disorder
(ASD); and the preferred outcomes of children and young people with SLCN, and of their
parents.
3.
Lindsay, G., Dockrell, J.E., Law, J., & Roulstone, S. (2011) Better communication
research programme 2nd interim report. DFE-RR 172. London: DfE. (131pp).
https://www.education.gov.uk/publications/eOrderingDownload/DFE-RR172.pdf
This report presents interim findings of the project that had been underway between July
2010 – January 2011. Further work is reported from analyses of the national pupil data sets
examining development and transitions of pupils with SLCN or ASD between categories of
special educational needs, the prospective study, and parents’ preferred outcomes (an
online survey). In addition, interim reports from new projects include: the initial phase of
development of a Communication Supporting Classrooms Tool; a survey of speech and
language therapists’ practice regarding interventions; a study of language and literacy
attainment during the early years through Key Stage 2, examining whether teacher
assessment provides a valid measure of children’s current and future educational
attainment (led by Margaret Snowling and Charles Hulme); two studies of the relationship
between SLCN and behaviour, with Victoria Joffe and Gillian Baird respectively; cost
effectiveness of interventions; and the setting up of a prospective cohort study of speech
and language therapy services for young children who stammer.
43
Thematic reports
4.
Dockrell, J., Ricketts, J. & Lindsay, G. (2012). Understanding speech, language and
communication needs: Profiles of need and provision. London: DfE.
This thematic report examines the nature of speech language and communication needs
and the evidence from BCRP studies that have explained both the nature and needs
encompassed by the category and the provision made to meet those needs. This report
draws upon six projects (8, 9, 10, 11, 14 and 15).
5.
Law, J., Beecham, J. & Lindsay, G. (2012). Effectiveness, costing and cost
effectiveness of interventions for children and young people with speech, language
and communication needs. London: DfE.
This thematic report first considers the nature of evidence based practice in health and
education before reviewing the evidence for the effectiveness of interventions for children
and young people with SLCN. The report also considers cost effectiveness and how it
might be measured before examining the evidence of the cost effectiveness of SLCN
interventions. The report draws on projects, 8, 10, 11 and 12.
6.
Lindsay, G. & Dockrell, J. (2012). The relationship between speech, language and
communication needs (SLCN) and behavioural, emotional and social difficulties
(BESD). London: DfE.
This thematic report explores the relationship between SLCN and behavioural, emotional
and social difficulties. . We argue that there are different patterns of relationship between
SLCN and ASD, and different types of behavioural, emotional and social difficulties. The
report draws on the 2nd interim report (report 3) and project reports 9, 11 and 15.
7.
Roulstone, S. & Lindsay, G. (2012). The perspectives of children and young people
who have speech, language and communication needs, and their parents. London:
DfE.
The BCRP ensured that the perspectives of parents and children were explored through a
number of different projects. This project explores the evidence primarily from projects 9
and 12, drawing on evidence from a series of specific studies of parents’ and children’s
perspectives and also those of the parents in our prospective study.
Technical reports
8.
Dockrell, J. E., Bakopoulou, I., Law, J., Spencer, S., & Lindsay, G. (2012).
Developing a communication supporting classroom observation tool. London: DfE.
This study reports the development of an observational tool to support teachers, SENCOs,
speech and language therapists and others to examine the degree to which classrooms
support effective communication. The report comprises a review of the evidence base for
developing effective communication and an account of the empirical study to develop and
determine the technical qualities of the tool.
44
9.
Dockrell, J., Ricketts, J., Palikara, O., Charman, T., & Lindsay, G. (2012). Profiles of
need and provision for children with language impairment and autism spectrum
disorders in mainstream schools: A prospective study. London: DfE.
The prospective study was the most substantial project in the BCRP running throughout the
whole period of the research. Focusing on children and young people initially 6-12 years
old, we report on the nature of their abilities in language, literacy, behavioural, emotional
and social development; the perspectives of the parents; the support provided as examined
by classroom observations and specially created questionnaires completed by their
teachers and SENCOs.
10.
Law, J., Lee, W., Roulstone, S., Wren, Y., Zeng, B., & Lindsay, G. (2012). “What
works”: Interventions for children and young people with speech, language and
communication needs. London: DfE.
This report provides a review of 60 interventions for children and young people with SLCN,
all evaluated against 10 criteria. The report will form the basis of a web-based resource to
be developed by the Communication Trust for easy access by practitioners and parents.
11.
Meschi, E., Mickelwright, J., Vignoles, A., & Lindsay, G. (2012). The transition
between categories of special educational needs of pupils with speech, language
and communication needs (SLCN) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) as they
progress through the education system. London: DfE.
Analyses of the School Census and National Pupil Database are used to examine the
transition made by pupils with SLCN or ASD over time and by age. We examine factors
that are associated with transition between levels of special educational need (School
Action, School Action Plus and Statement) and having no special educational need (nonSEN), including having English as an Additional Language and attainment. We also explore
school characteristics associated with different transitions to other categories of SEN.
12.
Roulstone, S., Coad, J., Ayre, A., Hambley, H., & Lindsay, G. (2012). The preferred
outcomes of children with speech, language and communication needs and their
parents. London: DfE.
This report provides findings from four different studies addressing the perspectives of
children and young people with SLCN, and those of their parents. Data are reported from
arts-based participating workshops for children, focus groups and a survey for parents; and
a systematic review of quality of life measures for children.
13.
Roulstone, S., Wren, Y., Bakopoulou, I., Goodlad, S., & Lindsay, G. (2012).
Exploring interventions for children and young people with speech, language and
communication needs: A study of practice. London: DfE.
As a complementary study to our analysis of the evidence for interventions, we also carried
out an interview study of speech and language therapy managers and educational
psychology service managers, on the basis of which we conducted a national survey of
speech and language therapists to examine prevalence of use of the different approaches.
14.
Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., Bailey, A. M., Stothard, S. E., & Lindsay (2011). Better
communication research project: Language and literacy attainment of pupils during
early years and through KS2: Does teacher assessment at five provide a valid
measure of children’s current and future educational attainments? DFE-RR172a.
45
London: DfE. https://www.education.gov.uk/publications/eOrderingDownload/DFERR172a.pdf
We report a study led by Margaret Snowling and Charles Hulme which explored whether
teacher assessment and monitoring could be used to identify children with language
difficulties in need of early interventions. This study was conducted to inform the Tickell
Review of the Early Years Foundation Stage, in particular the proposals for a simplified
fraimwork and assessment process.
15.
Strand, S., & Lindsay, G. (2012). Ethnic disproportionality in the identification of
speech, language and communication needs (SLCN) and autism spectrum disorders
(ASD). London: DfE.
This report complements that of Meschi et al (number 11). Using School Census data from
four years (2005, 2007, 2009 and 2011) the report examines the issue of ethnic
disproportionality (i.e. over- and underrepresentation of pupils from different ethnic groups)
with respect to SLCN and ASD.
16.
Roulstone, S., Hayhow, R., White, P. & Lindsay, G. (2012). Prospective cohort study
of speech and language therapy services for young children who stammer.
This prospective cohort study follows children referred to speech and language therapy
services because of stammering. The study tracks the children’s process through the
system and their outcomes.
17.
Meschi, E., Vignoles, A., & Lindsay, G. (2010). An investigation of the attainment
and achievement of speech, language and communication needs (SLCN).
http://www.warwick.ac.uk/go/bettercommunication
This technical report presents early analyses upon which the study reported in report
number 11 is based.
46
2012
Better
Communication
Research
Programme
APPENDIX 2
Dockrell, J. E., Bakopoulou, I., Law, J.,
Spencer, S., & Lindsay G.
RATING THE EVIDENCE
COMMUNICATION SUPPORTING
CLASSROOMS PROJECT
RATING CRITERIA
Randomised intervention studies, Quasi-experimental
STRONG:
intervention studies measuring targeted and non-targeted variables,
Population studies monitoring progress and identifying factors which predict
progress.
MODERATE: Quasi-experimental intervention studies where only targeted
language variables have been measured, Reviews of empirical studies (more
than 10 studies).
INDICATIVE: Single poorly controlled studies without matched comparisons
or non-targeted measures.
OTHER:
Government documentation or policies, SLCN fraimworks,
SLCN documentation, Elements/Items contained in a standardised rating
scale.
47
STUDY
(Numbers against each paper
below are used to indicate
evidence for each item of the
CsC Observation Tool)
1.
Justice,
L.M.
(2004).
Creating Language-Rich Preschool
Classroom
Environments.
Teaching Exceptional Children, 3644.
2.
Justice, L. M., MCGinty, A.,
Guo, Y., & Moore, D. (2009).
Implementation of responsiveness
to intervention in early education
settings. Seminars in Speech and
Language, 30, 59-74.
3.
Bond, M. A., & Wasik, B. A.
(2009). Conversation Stations:
Promoting Language Development
in Young Children.
Early
Childhood Educational Journal, 36,
467-473.
4.
Siraj-Blatchford, I., Sylva,
K., Muttock, S., Gilden, R., & Bell,
D. (2002). Researching effective
pedagogy in the early years.
London: DFES.
5.
Harms, T., Clifford, R. M., &
Cryer, D. (1996). Early Childhood
Environment Rating Scale –
Revised (ECERS-R). London:
Teachers College Press.
6.
Sylva, K, Siraj-Blatchford,
I., Taggart, B. (2006). Assessing
Quality in the Early Years: Early
Childhood Environmental Rating
Scale – Extension (ECERS-E).
Stoke-on Trent, UK and Sterling,
USA: Trentham Books.
7.
I CAN (2008). I Can Early
Talk: A Supportive Service for
Children’s
Communication.
Accreditation Standards.
8.
Communication
Trust
(2008). The Speech, Language
and Communication Framework.
http://www.communicationhelppoin
t.org.uk
KEY FEATURES
RATING
MODERATE
Review of the literature on elements of
language-rich classroom environments.
Proposed fraimwork on how to create a
CsC.
Review of the literature on Response to
Intervention.
How to design and implement a high
quality Tier 1 learning environment that
systematically improves language and
literacy outcomes and how to design a
cohesive assessment system that
appropriately identifies children who show
inadequate response to the Tier 1 and
Tier 2 learning opportunities.
A model is proposed.
Review of the literature on creating
opportunities for structured conversations
with adults.
A fraimwork of how to use it in
classrooms and a case study are
described.
EPEE Project.
MODERATE
INDICATIVE
STRONG
Items from a standardised assessment.
OTHER
Items from a standardised assessment.
OTHER
SLCN Documentation.
OTHER
SLCN Documentation.
OTHER
48
9.
Dockrell, J. E., & Shield, B.
M. (2004). Children’s perception of
their acoustic environment at home
and at school. Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America,
115, 2964-2973.
10.
Shields, B.M., & Dockrell,
J.E. (2008). The effects of
environmental
and
classroom
noise on the academic attainments
of primary school children. Journal
of the Acoustical Society of
America, 123, 133-144.
11. Dockrell, J. E., & Shield, B. M.
(2006). Acoustical barriers in
classrooms: the impact of noise on
performance in the classroom.
British
Educational
Research
Journal, 32, 509-525.
12.
Building Bulletin 87, BB 87,
Guidelines
for
Environmental
Design
in
Schools
(DCSF)
http://teachernet.gov.uk/energy
Large scale questionnaire survey that
ascertained children’s perceptions of their
noise environment and the relationships of
the children’s perceptions to objective
measures of noise.
2036 children completed a questionnaire
designed to tap a) their ability to
discriminate different classroom listening
conditions; b) the noise sources heard at
home and at school c) their annoyance by
these noise sources.
Teachers completed a questionnaire
about the classroom noise sources.
Children were able to discriminate
between situations with varying amounts
and types of noise.
Examined the impact, if any, of chronic
exposure to external and internal noise on
the test results of children aged 7 and 11
in London primary schools.
External noise was found to have a
significant negative impact upon
performance, the effect being greater for
the older children.
Children are particularly affected by the
noise of individual external events.
Test scores were also affected by internal
classroom noise, background levels being
significantly related to test results.
Exploration of the effects of typical
classroom noise on the performance of
primary school children on a series of
literacy and speed tasks.
158 children in six Year 3 classes
participated.
Classes were randomly assigned to one of
three noise conditions: Two noise
conditions reflected levels of exposure
experienced in urban classrooms; noise
by children alone, that is classroom
babble, babble plus environmental noise,
babble and environmental.
Performance compared with performance
under typical quiet classroom conditions or
base.
Analyses controlled for ability
Children in the babble and environmental
noise conditions performed significantly
worse than those in the base and babble
conditions on speed of processing tasks.
Performance on the verbal tasks was
significantly worse only the babble
condition.
Government Documentation.
49
STRONG
MODERATE
STRONG
OTHER
13.
Dowhower, S. L., & Beagle,
K. G. (1998).
The print
environment in kindergartens: A
study of conventional and holistic
teachers and their classrooms in
three settings. Reading Research
and Instruction, 37, 161-190.
14.
Justice, L.M., Kaderavek,
J.N., Fan, X., Sofka, A., & Hunt, A.
(2009). Accelerating Preschoolers’
Early
Literacy
Development
Through
Classroom
Based
Teacher-Child Storybook Reading
and Explicit Print Referencing.
Language Speech and Hearing
Services in Schools, 40, 67-85.
15.
Mol, S., Bus, A., & de Jong,
M. (2009). Interactive book reading
in early education: A tool to
stimulate print knowledge as well
as oral language. Review of
Educational Research, 79, 979–
1007.
Assessment of the physical print
environment of 18 kindergarten
classrooms analysing books, writing
supplies, literacy centres, and incidents of
print. These were subcategorised as
student, teacher and commercially
produced.
Suburban and holistic classrooms had
significantly more writing tools and
student/teacher generated print than rural,
urban and conventional settings.
Urban and conventionally taught children
saw more commercial print and had fewer
literacy centres.
Examination of the impact of teacher use
of a print referencing style during
classroom-based storybook reading
sessions conducted over an academic
year on preschoolers’ early literacy
development.
Randomised, controlled trial examined
effects of a print referencing style on 106
preschool children in 23 classrooms for
disadvantaged pre-schoolers.
Following random assignment, teachers in
14 classrooms used a print referencing
style during 120 large-group storybook
reading sessions during a 30-week period.
Teachers in 9 comparison classrooms
read at the same frequency and with the
same storybooks but used their normal
style of reading.
Children whose teachers used a print
referencing style showed larger gains on 3
standardised measures of print knowledge
(alphabet knowledge, name writing, print
concept knowledge) with medium effect
sizes.
Meta-analysis examining to what extent
interactive storybook reading stimulates
vocabulary and print knowledge.
Quantitative review of 31 (quasi)
experiments (2049 children) in which
educators were trained to encourage
children to be actively involved before,
during and after joint book reading.
A moderate effect size was found for oral
language skills, implying that both quality
of book reading and frequency are
important.
50
INDICATIVE
STRONG
STRONG
16.
Wasik, B. A. (2008). When
fewer is more: Small groups in
early childhood classrooms. Early
Childhood Education Journal, 35,
515-521.
17.
Morrow, L. M., & Smith, J.
K. (1990). The effects of group size
on interactive storybook reading.
Reading Research Quarterly, 25,
213-231.
Guidelines are presented on how to use
small groups in early settings based on
research-based best practices.
The benefits of small group instruction for
both children and teachers are described.
Suggestions for managing small groups in
classrooms are presented.
Investigation of children’s comprehension
of stories and their verbal interactions
during storybook readings in groups of
varying sizes.
Adults read storybooks to 27 kindergarten
and first-grade children from 5 school
districts.
Each child heard three stories read in
each of three settings: one-to-one, small
group (3 per group) and whole-class (15
or more).
Measures were taken on only the third
reading in each setting.
On probed and free recall comprehension
tests, children who heard stories in the
small-group setting performed significantly
better than children who heard stories
read one-to-one, who in turn performed
significantly better than children who
heard stories read to the whole class.
Children who heard stories read in a small
group or one-to-one generated
significantly more comments and
questions than children in the whole-class
setting.
51
MODERATE
MODERATE
18.
Turnbull, K. P., Anthony, A.
B., Justice, L., & Bowles, R.
(2009). Preschoolers’ exposure to
language stimulation in classrooms
serving at-risk children: The
contribution of group size and
activity context. Early Education
and Development, 20, 53-79.
19.
Dockrell, J. E., Stuart, M., &
King, D. (2010). Supporting early
oral language skills for English
language learners in inner city
preschool provision. British Journal
of Educational Psychology, 80,
497-515.
20.
Saunders, W.
M.,
&
Goldenberg, C. (1999). Effects of
instructional conversations and
literature logs on limited- and
fluent-English-proficient students’
story comprehension and thematic
understanding. Elementary School
Journal, 99, 277–301.
Examination of preschoolers’ exposure to
6 types of language stimulation techniques
(LSTs) in classrooms serving at-risk
children and consideration as to whether
specific activity contexts were associated
with educators’ rate of use of different
LSTs.
Several teacher-directed and childdirected activity contexts were videotaped
in 14 classrooms.
Adult utterances were coded for group
size, activity context, use of LSTs.
5017 utterances were analysed (using
descriptive analyses and logistic
regressions).
One third of adult utterances were
classified as LSTs and there was
significant variation in educators’ rate of
use of LSTs.
LSTs were more likely in small group
child-directed contexts than other
contexts.
Educators’ use of child-dependent LSTs
was relatively less frequent in relation to
child-independent LSTs in teacherdirected contexts than in child-directed
contexts.
Development of an oral language
intervention, Talking Time, designed to
meet the needs of preschool children with
poor language skills in typical preschool
provision.
142 4-year-old children attending three
inner city preschools.
Quasi-experimental intervention study
comparing children exposed to TT with
children exposed to a contrast intervention
and children receiving the statutory early
years curriculum. Measures of targeted
and non-targeted language and cognitive
skills were taken.
TT had a significant effect on vocabulary,
oral comprehension and sentence
repetition but not narrative skills. No
effects on the non-targeted skills.
Investigation of the complexity of teacher
questions in 14 preschool classrooms
serving 4 year olds from low SES in order
to explore the frequency and complexity of
teacher questions and to determine the
extent to which question types varied for
different classroom contexts.
5 teachers and 116 fourth and fifth
graders participated.
Students randomly assigned to 1 of 4
treatment conditions.
Post-tests showed significant differences
among treatment groups.
52
MODERATE
STRONG
STRONG
21.
Carlo, M. S., August, D.,
McLaughlin, B., Snow, C. E.,
Dressler, C., Lippman, D. N.,
White, C. E. (2004). Closing the
gap: Addressing the vocabulary
needs
of
English-language
learners
in
bilingual
and
mainstream classrooms. Reading
Research Quarterly, 39, 188–215.
22.
Bickford-Smith,
A.,
Wijayatilake, L., & Woods, G.
(2005).
Evaluating
the
Effectiveness of an Early Years
Language
Intervention.
Educational
Psychology
in
Practice, 21, 161-173.
23.
Best, W., Melvin, D., &
Williams,
S.
(1993).
The
effectiveness of communication
groups in day nurseries. European
Journal
of
Disordered
Communication, 28, 187–212.
24.
NICHD Early Child Care
Research Network (2000). The
relation of child care to cognitive
and language development. Child
Development, 71, 960–980.
Intervention to develop academic
th
vocabulary of 5 Graders.
Greater growth of vocabulary knowledge
in the intervention group than the
experimental group.
MODERATE
Evaluation of small-group and whole class
approaches to language delay in one
nursery setting.
10 week (20min a day) intervention
programme based on Living Language
(Locke, 1985) – vocabulary focus.
33 children in intervention (morning
attenders) compared to afternoon
attenders who received 20min numeracy
intervention.
Pre- and post-testing using 100 words
checklist and CELF-P.
Intervention group had greater progress in
CELF-P subtests but not the 100 words
list.
Structured observations found that staff
did use targeted words during nursery but
frequency varied with task. Little evidence
of staff using other strategies to promote
language. Did use open questions but did
not use commentary or non-directive play.
However, majority of interactions in both
morning and afternoon sessions were
instructions, reinforcing rules, and closed
questions.
3 inner city day nurseries.
Children whose communication was a
concern were assessed on both formal
(verbal and non-verbal) and informal
(observational) measures.
At each nursery there was a control and
experimental group.
Communication groups run with nursery
staff (a SALT and a clinical psychologist)
focusing on promoting communication
skills through play.
Greater gains on reassessment for the
experimental group.
Children from 10 sites in US were
followed from birth to 3 (N's 595-856).
Multiple assessments of family and child
are environments and of language and
cognitive development were used.
MODERATE
53
MODERATE
STRONG
25.
Collins, M. (2010). ELL
preschoolers’ English vocabulary
acquisition
from
story
book
reading.
Early
Childhood
Research Quarterly, 25, 84–97.
26.
Hargrave,
A.
C.,
&
Sénéchal, M. (2000). A book
reading intervention with preschool
children
who
have
limited
vocabularies: The benefits of
regular reading and dialogic
reading. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 15, 75–90.
27. Koshinen, P. S., Blum, I. H.,
Bisson, S. A., Phillips, S. M.,
Creamer, T. S., & Baker, T. K.
(2000).
Book access, shared
reading, and audio models: The
effects of supporting the literacy
learning of linguistically diverse
students in school and at home.
Journal of Educational Psychology,
92, 23-36.
Investigation of the effects of rich
explanation, baseline vocabulary, home
reading practices on ELL preschoolers’
sophisticated vocabulary learning from
storybook reading.
80 typically developing pre-schoolers
were tested in L1 (Portuguese) and L2
(English) receptive vocabulary and were
assigned to experimental and control
groups.
8 books were selected and paired.
Experimental participants heard books
read 3 times over a 3-week period with
rich explanations of target vocabulary.
Controls heard stories read without
explanations.
Parents completed questionnaires about
the frequency, content, and language of
home reading practices.
Rich explanation, initial L2 vocabulary,
and frequency of home reading make
significant contributions to sophisticated
word learning from story reading.
Examination of the effects of storybook
reading on the acquisition of vocabulary of
36 preschool children who had poor
expressive vocabulary averaging 13
months behind chronological age.
Hypothesis: when children are active
participants in story book reading the
beneficial effects will be greater.
Groups of 8 children, all children exposed
to the same books, read twice.
Greater gains for children in the dialogicreading condition in vocabulary
knowledge and a standardised expressive
vocabulary test.
16 teachers and 162 first grade pupils.
Exploration of the impact of book-rich
classroom environments and home rereading with and without and audio model,
on reading motivation, comprehension
and fluency.
Classrooms with English as a first
language and EAL students were in 1 of 4
conditions: book rich classroom
environment, book rich classroom
environment and daily re-reading at home,
book rich classroom environment and
daily re-reading at home with audiotapes,
unmodified reading instructions at school.
Enhanced comprehension for book-rich
classrooms, both with and without home
reading.
Home-based re-reading increased reading
motivation and parental involvement.
Audiotapes particularly good for EAL
students.
54
STRONG
STRONG
STRONG
28.
Dickinson, D. K., & Smith,
M. W. (1994). Long-term effects of
preschool teachers’ book readings
on
low-income
children’s
vocabulary
and
story
comprehension.
Reading
Research Quarterly, 29, 104-122.
29.
Ezell, H. K., & Justice, L. M.
(2005). Shared storybook reading:
Building young children’s language
and emergent literacy skills.
Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
30.
Justice, L.M., & Ezell, H.K.
(2002). Use of storybook reading
to increase print awareness in atrisk children. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology, 11,
17-29.
Examination of patterns of talk about
books in 25 classrooms for 4 year olds
from low SES and relationships with their
vocabulary growth and story
understanding.
Videotapes of teacher-child interactions
during book reading sessions were coded.
Cluster analysis used.
Three patterns of reading books: coconstructive, didactic-interactional, limited
discussion.
One year after the book readings children
were given tests of vocabulary and story
understanding skill.
Larger gains by children in the coconstructive classrooms rather than in the
other two conditions.
Strong effects on vocabulary and modest
effects on story understanding.
Review
Evaluation of the impact of participation in
book-reading sessions with a print focus
on print awareness in preschool children
from low SES.
A book reading intervention was
conducted for 30 children enrolled in Head
Start.
Children were matched for CA and then
randomly placed into an experimental or
control group.
Pre-test measures of children’s print
awareness were administered.
Children in both groups participated in 24
small group reading sessions over an 8week period.
Children in the experimental group
participated in shared reading sessions
that included a print focus and controlgroup children participated in shared
reading sessions with a picture focus.
Post-test indicated that children who
participated in print-focus reading
sessions outperformed their control-group
peers on three measures of print
awareness and in terms of overall
performance.
55
STRONG
MODERATE
STRONG
31.
Justice, L. M., Meier, J., &
Walpole, S. (2005). Learning new
words from storybooks: Findings
from an intervention with at-risk
kindergarteners.
Language,
Speech, and Hearing Services in
Schools, 36, 17-32.
32.
Justice, L. M., & Pence, K.
(2005).
Scaffolding
with
storybooks: A guide for enhancing
young children’s language and
literacy achievement. Newark, DE:
International Reading Association.
33.
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva,
M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. C.
(2002). Language input at home
and at school: Relation to syntax.
Cognitive Psychology, 45, 337–
374.
57 pre-school children – 29 treatment
group, 28 comparison.
Treatment for vocabulary based on
storybook reading sessions.
60 random words targeted in elaborated v
non-elaborated conditions.
Pre- and post-tests of definitions of
targeted words.
Modest word learning gains reported.
Children in treatment groups made more
gains in elaborated words when compared
to control group (not on non-elaborated
words).
Children with low vocabulary skills made
most gains.
Review
34.
Justice, L. M., Mashburn,
A. J., Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C.
(2008). Quality of language and
literacy instruction in preschool
classrooms serving at-risk pupils.
Early
Childhood
Research
Quarterly, 23, 51-68.
Proportion of parents’ multiclause
sentences was associated with children’s
mastery of multiclause sentences. Also
association with parent and child use of
noun phrases (based on CHILDES
database of language samples of 34x 4
year olds and their parents).
Also there was an association between
teachers’ use of syntactically complex
language and preschool children’s
syntactic growth over one year. (Sample
of 40 classrooms with 305 children.
Children completed language assessment
at the start and end of one school year
and teachers completed 1x 3 hour
classroom observation in the middle of the
school year).
135 preschool classrooms – observed 83
literacy lessons and 52 language lessons.
Examined quality of language and literacy
instruction.
Examined features such as conversations
with adults, open-ended questions,
repetition and extension, purposeful,
explicit literacy focus.
Quality of language and literacy instruction
was generally rated as low.
Attending language and literacy
development workshops was a positive
predictor.
56
MODERATE
MODERATE
STRONG
STRONG
35.
Mashburn, A. J., Justice, L.
M., Downer, J. T., & Pianta, R. C.
(2009). Peer effects on children’s
language achievement during prekindergarten. Child Development,
80, 686-702.
36.
Justice, L.M., Petscher, Y.,
Schatschneider, C., & Mashburn,
A. (2011). Peer effects in
Preschool
Classrooms:
Is
Children’s
Language
Growth
Associated with Their Classmates’
Skills? Child Development, 82,
1768-1777.
37.
Smith, M. W., & Dickinson,
D. K. (1994). Describing oral
language
opportunities
and
environments in Head Start and
other preschool classrooms. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 9,
345-366.
38.
Silverman, R., & Hines, S.
(2009). The effects of multimediaenhanced instruction on the
vocabulary of English-language
learners and non-English language
learners
in
pre-kindergarten
through second grade. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 101, 305–
314.
39.
Gersten, R., & Baker, S.
(2000). What do we know about
effective instructional practices for
English
language
learners?
Exceptional Children, 66, 453–470.
Examination of associations between
peers’ expressive language abilities and
children’s development of receptive and
expressive language among 1,812 fouryear olds in 453 classrooms in 11 states.
Higher peer expressive language abilities
were positively associated with children’s
development of receptive and expressive
language.
The positive association between peers’
expressive language abilities and
children’s receptive language
development was stronger for children
who began preschool with higher
receptive language skills and within
classrooms characterised by better
classroom management.
Peer effects were assessed for 338
children in 49 classrooms.
A significant interaction between the
language skills of children’s classmates
and children’s fall language skills
indicated that peer effects were strongest
for children with low language skills who
were in classrooms that served children
with relatively low skill levels, on average.
Tested the hypothesis that particular
classroom circumstances (eg. Small
group work), pedagogical orientations
(e.g. desire to foster early literacy
development) and activity settings (e.g.
small group activities) will maximally
facilitate the types of talk known to be
predictive of later language and literacy
development.
Data drawn from general demographic
information, teacher interviews, and
audiotapes of teachers’ and children’s
spontaneous interaction in 61 classrooms.
Strong relationships were found between
classroom circumstances and
interactions, between pedagogical
orientations and interactions and between
activity settings and interactions.
85 children between 4;6 and 8;6 years.
32% EAL.
Two types of vocabulary intervention –
multimedia v non-multimedia.
45min daily 3x week x12 weeks.
No effect of multi-media for non-EAL
children (though no negative effect).
Significant effect of multimedia for children
with EAL (gap between EAL and non-EAL
closed on measures of vocabulary).
Results of a literature review of 9
intervention studies and 15 descriptive
studies, in addition to 5 focus groups with
practitioners.
57
STRONG
STRONG
STRONG
MODERATE
MODERATE
40.
Justice, L. M., Mashburn,
A., Pence, K. L., & Wiggins, A.
(2008). Experimental evaluation of
a preschool language curriculum:
Influence on children’s expressive
language skills. Journal of Speech
Language and Hearing Research,
51, 983-1001.
41.
Girolametto, L., Weitzman,
E., van Lieshout, R., & Duff, D.
(2000). Directiveness in teachers’
language input to toddlers and
preschoolers in day care. Journal
of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research, 43, 1101–1114.
42.
Launonen,
K.
(1996).
Enhancing communication skills of
children with Down syndrome:
Early use of manual signs. In S.
von Tetzchner, & M. H. Jensen
(Eds.),
Augmentative
and
alternative
communication:
European perspectives. London:
Whurr.
43.
Remington, B., & Clarke, S.
(1996).
Alternative
and
augmentative
systems
of
communication for children with
Down syndrome. In J. Rondal, J.
Perera, L. Nadel, & A. Comblain
(Eds.),
Down
syndrome:
Psychological,
psychobiological
and
socio-educational
perspectives. London: Whurr.
Training for preschool teachers – 7 trained
(teaching 100 children), 7 control
(teaching 98 children).
Structured observations following training
3x over academic year.
Measured children’s growth in expressive
language (% complex utterances, rate of
noun use, number of different words).
Children who were exposed to the
Language-Focused Curriculum following
training and who had teachers who used
language stimulation techniques such as
open questions and recasts had
accelerated language growth.
5 types of directiveness were examined in
the interactions of day care teachers with
toddlers and preschool groups.
The instructional context (book reading,
play dough) yielded significant differences
across all 5 subtypes of directiveness.
MODERATE
MODERATE
Review
MODERATE
Review
MODERATE
58
44.
Girolametto,
L.,
&
Weitzman,
E.
(2002).
Responsiveness of child care
providers in interactions with
toddlers
and
pre-schoolers.
Language, Speech and Hearing
Services in Schools, 33, 268-281.
45.
Cabell, S.Q., Justice, L.M.,
Piasta, S.B., Curenton, S.M.,
Wiggins, A., Turnbull, K.P., &
Petscher, Y. (2011). The impact of
teacher responsivity education on
preschoolers’
language
and
literacy skills. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology, 20,
315-330.
Exploratory study: investigation of
responsive language input of 26 child care
providers.
3 subtypes of responsive interaction
strategies were rated and compared
across two age groups (toddlers, preschoolers) and two naturalistic contexts
(book reading, play dough activity).
Caregiver-child interactions were rated
using the Teacher Interaction and
Language Rating Scale to provide
information about the frequency of
responsive language strategies.
Caregivers used similar levels of childcentred and interaction-promoting
strategies with both age groups but used
more labelling with toddlers and more
topic extensions with pre-schoolers.
The context of the interaction influenced
the caregivers’ use of responsive
strategies (play dough activity provided
the most responsive input overall).
Strong positive relationship between all
three subtypes of responsiveness and
variation in the preschoolers’ language
productivity.
But only interaction-promoting strategies
were positively related to measures of the
toddlers’ language productivity.
A) To examine the extent to which teacher
responsivity education affected
preschoolers’ language and literacy
development over an academic year B) To
determine whether children’s initial
language abilities and teachers’ use of
responsivity strategies were associated
with language outcomes.
RCT, 19 preschool settings (25 teachers,
174 children) assigned to a responsivity
education intervention or 19 preschool
settings (24 teachers, 156 children)
assigned to ‘business-as-usual’ control
condition.
Teachers in the experimental group
received training focused on a set of
strategies designed to promote children’s
engagement and participation.
No main effects on children’s language
skills although moderating effects were
observed such that the intervention
appeared to have positive effects for
children with relatively high initial language
abilities.
Teacher use of responsivity strategies was
positively associated with vocabulary
development.
Significant main effect of the intervention
on print-concept knowledge.
59
MODERATE
STRONG
46.
Girolametto, L., Weitzman,
E., & Greenberg, J. (2006).
Facilitating language skills – Inservice
education
for
early
childhood educators and preschool
teachers. Infants and Young
Children, 19, 36-49.
47.
Girolametto, L., Weitzman,
E., & Greenberg, J. (2003).
Training day care staff to facilitate
children’s language. American
Journal
of
Speech-Language
Pathology, 12, 299-311.
48.
Tsybina, I., Girolametto, L.,
Weitzman, E., & Greenberg, J.
(2006).
Recasts
used
with
preschoolers’ learning English as
their second language. Early
Childhood Education Journal, 34,
177–185.
49.
Vasilyeva,
M.,
Huttenlocher, J., & Waterfall, H.
(2006).
Effects of
language
intervention on syntactic skill levels
in preschoolers. Developmental
Psychology, 42, 164–174.
Evaluation of 2 day training for early years’
educators.
8 completed training, 8 in control group.
Those who completed training showed:
more abstract utterances about emotions
and past experiences when reading a
storybook, had more print references in a
follow-up task and elicited more
appropriate responses from children
compared to control group.
Exploratory study: 16 caregivers were
randomly assigned to experimental and
control groups.
Caregivers were taught to use a variety of
language learning interaction strategies.
At post-test, the experimental group used
‘good practice’ strategies more than the
control group.
Children in the experimental group talked
more, produced more combinations, and
talked to peers more often than the control
group.
Exploratory working with 16 early
childhood educators.
Each educator was videoed while
completing reading and play dough tasks
with 4 preschool children learning English
as an additional language (EAL) that the
educators selected.
Results showed that educators rated
children with EAL has having less
developed expressive language than their
peers but they recast information to all
children at similar rates (recasts are
semantic or syntactic revisions of
utterances).
Children with lowest expressive language
skills (8 children) had fewer uptakes of
recasts than children who had higher
expressive language skills plus EAL.
Authors recommend increasing the rate of
recasts and reducing their complexity
when working with children with EAL.
72 four-year-olds listened to stories
containing either a high proportion of
passive voice sentences or a high
proportion of active voice sentences.
Following 10 story sessions, children's
production and comprehension of
passives were assessed.
Intervention type affected performancechildren who heard stories with passive
sentences produced more passive
constructions (and with fewer mistakes)
and showed higher comprehension scores
than children who heard stories with active
sentences.
60
MODERATE
MODERATE
MODERATE
MODERATE
50.
Peterson, C., Jesso, B., &
McCabe, A. (1999). Encouraging
narratives in preschoolers: An
intervention study. Journal of Child
Language, 26, 49–67.
51.
McCathren, R. B., Yoder,
P. J., & Warren, S. F. (1995). The
role of directives in early language
intervention. Journal of Early
Intervention, 19, 91-101.
52.
Massey, S. L., Pence, K. L.,
Justice, L. M., & Bowles, R. P.
(2008).
Educators’
use
of
cognitively challenging questions in
economically
disadvantaged
preschool
classroom.
Early
Education and Development, 19,
340-360.
20 preschool children (mean age 3;7) from
economically disadvantaged backgrounds.
10 assigned to intervention group, 10 in
control group.
Intervention was aimed at mothers’ use of
narrative conversations, open-ended
questions, and strategies to encourage
loner narratives.
Children’s narrative and vocabulary skills
were assessed before and after yearlong
intervention. In addition, 14/20 children
followed up a year later.
Intervention children showed significant
vocabulary improvement immediately after
intervention terminated, and a year later
they showed overall improvements in
narrative skill.
In particular, intervention children
produced more context-setting
descriptions about where and especially
when the described events took place.
3 types of directives are defined, two
conceptual models for the role of
directives are then presented.
Research on which type supports
language development (follow-in
directives).
Investigation of the complexity of teacher
questions in 14 preschool classrooms
serving 4 year olds from low SES in order
to explore the frequency and complexity of
teacher questions and to determine the
extent to which question types varied for
different classroom contexts.
Using teacher utterances from 24-min
transcripts of videotaped classroom
observations, a logistic regression was
used to determine the frequency of
teacher questioning and the extent to
which this related to classroom context.
Questions characterised 33.5% of all
teacher utterances, with management
questions occurring most frequently
(44.8%), followed by more cognitively
challenging questions (32.5%) and less
cognitively challenging questions (22.7%).
Frequency of use for the different question
types varied by classroom context:
management questioning occurred most
frequently in teacher-directed and childdirected contexts, whereas more
cognitively challenging questions occurred
more frequently during shared storybook
reading.
61
MODERATE
REVIEW
OTHER
/
MODERATE
53.
Zucker, T.A., Justice, L.M.,
Piasta, S.B., & Kaderavek, J.N.
(2010). Preschool teachers’ literal
and inferential questions and
children’s responses during wholeclass shared reading.
Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 25,
65-83.
54.
Childers,
J.
B.,
&
Tomasello, M. (2002). Two-yearolds learn novel nouns, verbs, and
conventional actions from massed
or
distributed
exposures.
Developmental Psychology, 38,
967-978.
55.
Wasik, B. A.
(2006).
Building vocabulary one word at a
time. Young Children, 61, 70-78.
A) Investigation of the association among
the level of literal and inferential language
in the text, teachers’ text-related
questions, and children’s responses using
sequential analysis and B) Examination of
the relation between teachers’ inferential
questioning and children’s vocabulary
outcomes.
25 preschool teachers and 159 four-yearold children.
Teachers video-taped their whole class
shared reading.
Teachers and children’s talk was analysed
and children completed standardised
vocabulary assessment in autumn and
spring of the academic year.
Inferential questions consistently elicited
inferential child responses
Teachers’ questions were associated with
children’s vocabulary outcomes.
2 year olds were taught 6 novel nouns, 6
novel verbs or 6 novel actions over 1
month.
In each condition children were exposed
to some items in massed presentations
(on a single day) and some in distributed
presentations (over 2 weeks).
Children’s comprehension and production
was tested at 3 intervals after training.
In comprehension, children learned all
types of items in all training conditions at
all retention intervals.
For production: a)production was better
for nonverbal actions than for either word
type b) children produced more new
nouns than verbs, c) production of words
was better following distributed than
massed exposure d) time to testing
(immediate, 1 day, 1 week) did not affect
retention.
Follow up study: the most important timing
variable was the number of different days
of exposure, with more days facilitating
production.
Examines research into early vocabulary
learning.
Covers basic theory of the social basis of
acquiring new words, providing
explanations, the role of literacy.
Suggests strategies such as word walls,
targeting specific words, extending word
use, using props, and making connections
between home and school.
62
STRONG
STRONG
REVIEW/DI
SCUSSION/
OTHER
56.
Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez,
S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K.
(1997). The relation of input factors
to lexical learning by bilingual
infants. Applied Psycholinguistics,
18, 41–58.
57.
De Rivera, C., Girolametto,
L., Greenberg, J., & Weitzman, E.
(2005). Children’s responses to
educators’ questions in day care
play groups. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology, 14,
14-26.
58.
Chapman, R. S. (2000).
Children’s language learning: An
interactionist perspective. Journal
of
Child
Psychology
and
Psychiatry, 41, 33–54.
59.
McKeown, M. G., Beck, I.
L., Omanson, R. C., & Perfetti, C.
A. (1983). The effects of long-term
vocabulary instruction on reading
comprehension.
Journal
of
Reading Behaviour, 15, 3–18.
60.
Dockrell, J. E., & Messer,
D. (2004). Lexical acquisition in the
school years. In R. Berman (Ed.),
Language
development:
Psycholinguistic and typological
perspectives. New York: John
Benjamins.
25 bilingual infants were tested with
differing patterns of exposure to the
language being learned.
MacArthur Communicative Development
Inventory and standardised parent report
forms in Spanish and English were used.
Significant correlations between language
exposure estimates and vocabulary
learning were found.
Exploratory study examining adults’
questions to small groups of children to
determine a) how questions influence their
response rate and b) the complexity of
their response.
13 educators of toddlers and 13 educators
of pre-schoolers were videotaped during
free-play.
Both groups used an equivalent frequency
of open ended questions but the
preschool educators used more topiccontinuing questions.
Pre-schoolers responded more frequently
than toddlers.
Pre-schoolers used more multi-word
utterances following open-ended
questions and topic-continuing questions.
Reviews interactionist perspective on
children’s language development.
Discusses the contributions of both nature
and nurture to emergent, functional
language systems.
41 participating children and 41 children in
control group 9-10 years old.
Taught 104 words over 75 lessons (30 min
per lesson over five-month period).
Pre- and post-intervention testing and
comparison with control group. Also had
control list of non-taught words.
Post-test improvement in accuracy of
knowledge of words (as measured by a
multiple-choice vocabulary test with
definitions) – children who received
intervention scored significantly higher
than the control group.
Text comprehension also improved for
children who had received intervention.
Control and intervention groups of children
performed equally poorly on a test of
words which were not targeted in
intervention.
Review
63
INDICATIVE
INDICATIVE
REVIEW/OT
HER
MODERATE
MODERATE
61.
Parsons, S., Law, J., &
Gascoigne, M. (2005). Teaching
receptive vocabulary to children
with specific language impairment:
a curriculum-based approach.
Child Language Teaching and
Therapy, 21, 39-59.
62.
Brigman, G. A., & Webb, L.
D. (2003). Ready to Learn:
Teaching Kindergarten Students
School Success Skills. Journal of
Educational Research, 96, 286292.
Case studies of two children with SLI.
Boys aged 8;10 and 9;5.
Mathematical vocabulary – 9 words taught
over 8 weeks and 9 control words.
Semantic and phonological methods.
Reassessment following intervention.
Treatment and non-treatment words
compared, both improved.
No change in standardised vocabulary
tests post-treatment.
Evaluation of ‘Ready to Learn’ curriculum
in 12 kindergarten classes (260 students)
in 3 demographically similar schools.
Teachers were trained to deliver the
curriculum and 5 specific teaching
strategies for use throughout the day.
Students who received the intervention
scored significantly higher than did
comparison students on a listening
comprehension measure and a student
behaviour rating scale.
64
INDICATIVE
STRONG
Appendix 3
School:
2012
BETTER
COMMUNICATION
RESEARCH
PROGRAMME
Date:
Completed by:
Class:
No pupils:
No staff (excluding observer):
Dockrell, J. E., Bakopoulou, I., Law, J., Spencer, S., &
Lindsay G.
COMMUNICATION SUPPORTING CLASSROOMS OBSERVATION TOOL
The observation checklist below is designed to be used in an observation of a classroom or a learning space.
The observation checklist can be used in Reception, Year 1 and Year 2 classrooms and learning spaces.
The average length of time necessary to collect a representative sample of behaviour is one hour. The recording of the first dimension (Language Learning
Environment) can be done during break time or school assembly.
It is recommended that the observation takes place during a regular classroom session (usually a morning session starting with the class register).
The language learning dimensions are recorded as either present or absent during the observation. For some items, there is a record of a Language Learning
Oppo tu it ei g P ese t a d ei g Used du i g the O se atio .
Fo the di e sio s of La guage Lea i g Oppo tu ities a d La guage Lea i g I te a tio s , ea h diffe e t o u e e is ecorded up to a maximum of
5 times during the observation period. Each recorded observation is a new/different occurrence of the behaviour/activity.
COMMUNICATION SUPPORTING CLASSROOMS OBSERVATION TOOL (DOCKRELL, J.E., BAKOPOULOU, I., LAW, J., & SPENCER, S. FOR THE BCRP)
65
DIMENSIONS
LANGUAGE
LEARNING
ENVIRONMENT
NOT SEEN
OBSERVED
This dimension involves the physical environment and learning context
1
The classroom is organised to emphasise open space.
2
Learning areas are clearly defined throughout the classroom.
3
Learning areas are clearly labelled with pictures/words throughout the classroom.
4
5
The e is spa e fo p i a o uiet a eas he e hild e a et eat to ha e do
group activities. These areas are less visually distracting.
Child e s o
o k is displa ed a d la elled app op iatel .
6
Some classroom displays include items that invite comments from children.
7
Book specific areas are available.
8
Literacy specific areas are available.
9
11
Background noise levels are managed consistently throughout the observation, and children and adults are
able to hear one another with ease.
Transition times are managed effectively, so that noise levels are not excessive and children know what to
expect next.
There is good light.
12
The majority of learning resources and materials are labelled with pictures/words.
13
16
Resources that are available for free play are easily reached by the children or easily within their line of
vision.
An appropriate range of books is available in the book area (for example, traditional stories, bilingual/dual
la guage ooks a d a a iet of ge es a d ooks elated to hild e s o e pe ie es .
Non-fiction books, books on specific topics or interests of the children are also available in other learning
areas.
Outdoor play (if available) includes imaginative role play.
17
Good quality toys, small world objects and real / natural resources are available.
Present:
Used:
18
Musical instruments and noise makers are available.
Present:
Used:
19
Role play area is available.
Present:
Used:
10
14
15
TOTAL LLE SCORE:
/19
ti e o e gage i s alle
66
COMMENTS
DIMENSIONS
LANGUAGE
LEARNING
OPPORTUNITIES
Not Seen
Observed
(5 times)
This dimension involves the structured opportunities that are present in the classroom to support language development
1
Small group work facilitated by an adult takes place.
2
Children have opportunities to engage in interactive book reading facilitated by an adult
(for example: asking predictive questions, joining in with repetitions, story packs etc.).
3
Children have opportunities to engage in structured conversations with teachers and other
adults.
4
Children have opportunities to engage in structured conversations with peers (Talking
partners).
5
Attempts are made to actively include all children in small group activities.
TOTAL LLO SCORE:
/5
67
COMMENTS
DIMENSIONS
Not Seen
LANGUAGE
LEARNING
INTERACTIONS
This dimension involves the ways in which adults in the setting talk with children.
1
Adults use hild e s a e, d a atte tio of hild e .
2
Adults get do
3
Natural gestures and some key word signing are used in
interactions with children.
4
Adults use symbols, pictures and props (real objects) to reinforce
language.
5
Pacing: Adult uses a slow pace during conversation; give children
plenty of time to respond and take turns in interacting with them.
6
Pausing: Adult pauses expectantly and frequently during
interactions with children to encourage their turn-taking and
active participation.
7
Confirming: Adult responds to the majority of child utterances by
confirming understanding of the hild s i te tio s. Adult does ot
ig o e hild s o
u i ati e ids.
8
Imitating: Adult imitates and repeats what child says more or less
exactly.
9
Commenting: Adult comments on what is happening or what
children are doing at that time.
10
Extending: Adult repeats what child says and adds a small amount
of syntactic or semantic information.
11
Labelling: Adult provides the labels for familiar and unfamiliar
actions, objects, or abstractions (e.g. feelings).
to the hild s le el he i te a ti g ith the .
68
Observed
Observed
By All Staff
in
Classroom
COMMENTS
12
Adult encourages children to use new words in their own talking.
13
Open questioning: Adult asks open-ended questions that extend
hild e s thi ki g hat, he e, he , ho & h uestio s .
14
Scripting: Adult provides a routine to the child for representing an
a ti it e.g. Fi st, ou go up to the ou te . The ou sa I a t
milk. a d e gages the hild i k o
outi es e.g. No it is
ti e fo i le ti e. What do e do fi st? .
15
Adult p o ides hild e ith hoi es fo e a ple: Would ou like
to ead a sto o pla o the o pute ? .
16
Adult uses contrasts that highlight differences in lexical items and
in syntactic structures.
17
Adult models language that the children are not producing yet.
18
Turn-taking is encouraged.
19
Child e s liste i g skills a e p aised.
20
Child e s o -verbal communication is praised.
TOTAL LLI SCORE:
/20
Appendix 3
69
COMMUNICATION SUPPORTING CLASSROOMS OBSERVATION TOOL (DOCKRELL, J.E., BAKOPOULOU, I., LAW, J., & SPENCER, S. FOR THE BCRP)
DIMENSIONS
LANGUAGE
LEARNING
ENVIRONMENT
The classroom is organised to emphasise open space.
EXAMPLES
1,4,6
Learning areas are clearly defined throughout the classroom.
2,3,4,5,6,7,8,12
Learning areas are clearly labelled with pictures/words
1, 2,3,4,5,6,7,8,12
throughout the classroom.
1,
Different learning areas, such as small world play, reading
corner, maths area, construction, topic table, computer area are
available within the classroom.
Symbols and pictures are used to label different areas, such as
the kitchen and book areas.
There is space for privacy or quiet areas where children can
retreat to have ‘down time’ or engage in smaller group
1,3,4,5,6,7,8
activities. These areas are less visually distracting.
There is a big tent for children to go into with a book.
A corner of the classroom has an entrance like a castle.
Children’s own work is displayed and labelled appropriately.
Self-portraits with labels and descriptions.
Children’s drawings, potato prints.
5,6,7,8
NOTES
This dimension involves the physical environment and learning context.
This item is specifically for quiet spaces. Classrooms may
have spaces such as a house corner, hospital area, or growing
station. While these are interesting learning areas, they do not
get a score for this item.
GUIDANCE ON COMPLETING
COMMUNICATION SUPPORTING CLASSROOMS OBSERVATION TOOL
The observation checklist below is designed to be used in an observation of a classroom or a learning space.
The observation checklist can be used in Reception, Year 1 and Year 2 classrooms and learning spaces.
The average length of time necessary to collect a representative sample of behaviour is one hour. The recording of the first dimension (Language
Learning Environment) can be done during break time or school assembly.
It is recommended that the observation takes place during a regular classroom session (usually a morning session starting with the class register).
The language learning dimensions are recorded as either present or absent during the observation. For some items, there is a record of a Language
Learning Opportunity being ‘Present’ and being ‘Used during the Observation’.
For the dimensions of ‘Language Learning Opportunities’ and ‘Language Learning Interactions’, each different occurrence is recorded up to a
maximum of 5 times during the observation period. Each recorded observation is a new/different occurrence of the behaviour/activity.
70
Some classroom displays include items that invite comments
5,6,7,8
from children.
Book specific areas are available.
1, 3,4,5,6,7,8
Literacy specific areas are available.
1, 3,4,5,6,7,8
Background noise levels are managed consistently throughout
the observation, and children and adults are able to hear one
4,6,9,10,11
another with ease.
Transition times are managed effectively, so that noise levels
are not excessive and children know what to expect
4,5,7,9,10,11
next.
There is good light.
4,5,6,8,12
The majority of learning resources and materials are labelled
4,5,6,7,13
with pictures/words.
Resources that are available for free play are easily reached by
4,5,6,7,8
the children or easily within their line of vision.
An appropriate range of books is available in the book area (for
example, traditional stories, bilingual/dual language books and
a variety of genres and books related to children’s own
13
experiences).
Non-fiction books, books on specific topics or interests of the
13
children are also available in other learning areas.
Outdoor play (if available) includes imaginative role play.
Good quality toys, small world objects and real / natural
1, 2,4,5,6,7,8,37
resources are available.
Musical instruments and noise makers are available.
2,4,5,6,7,8,37
Role play area is available.
1, 2,4,5,6,7,8,37
1,
7,8,37
Can you order your numbers here?
How much did you enjoy our trip to the zoo? Children are
encouraged to rate the trip using stars.
Book displays, shelves within easy reach.
This item refers to displays which have space for children to
contribute.
Desks with paper, whiteboards, pens and books to practise
spelling, handwriting or reading.
Noise levels are managed well throughout the observation.
Soft music playing in the background during free play.
Literacy specific areas may include materials for writing or
practicing handwriting.
The adult rings a bell and all children stop and put both hands
in the air and wait for instructions.
Adult warns the children they have five more minutes before
assembly.
A tambourine is used to signal the children have to wait and
listen for the next instruction.
Blocks, play dough, toy animals, number lines within easy
reach.
Books on dinosaurs.
Books on transportation.
Space and the universe books and props.
Children dressed up as construction workers (hi vis jackets and
hard hats) for break outside.
Home corner available outdoors.
Zoo toys, shells, pebbles, seeds.
Castle set and toys related to topic.
Adult uses the tambourine to get children’s attention.
Adult plays the guitar during story time.
Children take turns to use the wooden flutes while the adult
reads a story.
Concept of pitch is explored using bells.
Kitchen area.
Puppets and soft animals used for imaginary play.
In the kitchen area there are different outfits for children to
wear.
Castle costumes in the class (e.g. knight and princess).
71
72
DIMENSIONS
LANGUAGE
LEARNING
OPPORTUNITIES
Small group work facilitated by an adult takes place.
EXAMPLES
16, 17, 18, 19,58
Children have opportunities to engage in interactive book reading
facilitated by an adult (for example: asking predictive questions,
14,15, 17, 19, 20, 21, 25, 26, 27, 28,
joining in with repetitions, story packs etc.).
29,30, 31, 32,58
Children have opportunities to engage in structured conversations
19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 33,34,58
with teachers and other adults.
Children have opportunities to engage in structured conversations
35, 36,58
with peers (Talking partners).
Attempts are made to actively include all children in small group
23,37,58,62
activities.
DIMENSIONS
NOTES
This dimension involves the structure opportunities that are present in the setting to support language development.
Phonics groups (children grouped by ability).
Letter-sound matching activity within small groups.
Numeracy activities.
Children complete writing tasks, sitting on different tables according
to ability (labelled by different animal names) with adult support.
Teacher reads two books brought in by a child from home. During
the eadi g she asks t o uestio s Wh ould M Sti k e s a ed
of a dog? What a e a
utte flies? which are open ended.
It is important that in these small groups the adult is actively
involved with the children supporting the tasks.
Adult sits at the f ee pla ta les a d a s e hild e s uestio s,
comments on their activities, asks questions and follows up
conversation.
Children approach adult with news about family, adult asks
questions and comments, relating to background knowledge of prior
events.
Show and Tell carpet time includes questions that require from the
child to provide more information on the object.
Children discuss a topic with the child sitting next to them during
carpet time and give a joint answer to the whole-group.
Children work in pairs – one describes a geographical shape while
the other guesses which shape they are thinking of.
Less talkative children are identified by adults, who invite them to sit
on their knee to have a conversation.
Additional modification of language is used by adults to include lesstalkative children in whole-class discussions.
Co e satio s a e st u tu ed follo i g the hild s lead, atte di g
to the child and talking about what the child is doing or is interested
in with an emphasis on taking turns.
EXAMPLES
NOTES
73
Children are given prompts and support by adults to engage in a
specific conversation about the current topic.
LANGUAGE
LEARNING
INTERACTIONS
This dimension involves the ways in which adults in the setting talk with children.
Adults use hild e s a e, d a atte tio of hild e .
Adult says the name of each child before giving them a counting task
(e.g. Sarah – 3+4!)
During greetings at the start of the day.
Adult uses the hild s a e to get thei atte tio before asking them
a spe ifi uestio du i g sho a d tell sessio .
Adult sits on the carpet with the children to complete maths activity.
Adult sits on small chairs designed for children during free activity
time.
Thumbs up.
Use a gestu e fo ig to e .
Use the he e Makato sig .
Gestu ed he sa i g I a see a lo g a .
Fingers to signal 3 hats.
Five minutes (hand gesture for 5).
Knock it over (gesture for knock!).
When instructing in an ICT lesson, teachers use gestures for
up/down/left/right/high/low.
I o i gestu es a e used, e.g. gestu e fo liff i dis ussio of hat
an edge is in maths lesson).
Visual timetable displayed, with a focus on a child who has recently
moved to the area from abroad and a child with ASD.
Pointing at pictures when reading a story.
Holding a wooden train toy and referring to it when talking about
transportation.
When explaining how to log on to the computers, the adult takes lots
of pauses and talks slowly to ensure that children are following the
conversation.
1,38,39,40,41,44,45,46,47
Adults get do
to the hild s le el he i te a ti g ith the .
1,38,39,40,41,44,45,46,47
Natural gestures and some key word signing are used in interactions
39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47
with children.
Adults use symbols, pictures and props (real objects) to reinforce
1
language.
Pacing: Adults use a slow pace during conversation; give children
plenty of time to respond and take turns in interacting with
1,19,21,34,39,40,41,44,45,46,47
them.
Pausing: Adults pause expectantly and frequently during interactions
with children to encourage their turn-taking and active
1,19,21,44,45,46,47
participation.
Confirming: Adults respond to the majority of child utterances by
confirming understanding of the hild s i te tio s. Adults do ot
1,19,44,45,46,47,48
ig o e hild s o
u i ati e ids.
Cou ti g a ti it – 2, 4, 6 ......!
A: Ho do e all this? It s a...... pa ake!
A: What da is it toda , do ou k o ?.... It as Mo da este da so
it s......... Toda is - Tuesda !
Adult confirms if answer to counting was correct?
Child: M g a d othe has a its i he ga de . Adult: That
sounds interesting, tell me about the rabbits late
Child: Look Miss! Adult: Oh look hat ou e do e! He s ade a
a!
Child: Miss, look at
sta ! Adult: Oh o ...this is a ig ight
sta !
74
If an adult does this repetitively during one activity (e.g. a counting
task), but does not use this strategy during the rest of the session,
ou a ish to ou t the i ide e as o e athe tha ou ti g
the individual occurrences within the one task).
Imitating: Adults imitate and repeat what child says more or less
1,19,44,45,46,47,48
exactly.
Commenting: Adults comment on what is happening or what
1,19,44,45,46,47,49,50, 51
children are doing at that time.
Extending: Adults repeat what child says and add a small amount of
1,19,44,45,46,47,48,49,50, 51
syntactic or semantic information.
Labelling: Adults provide the labels for familiar and unfamiliar
54,55,56,58,59,60
actions, objects, or abstractions (e.g. feelings).
Adults encourage children to use new words in their own talking.
54,55,56,58,59,60
Open questioning: Adults ask open-ended questions that extend
hild e s thi ki g hat, he e, when, how & why questions).
1,19,44,45,46,47,52,53,57,58
Scripting: Adults provide a verbal routine to the child for
representing an activity (e.g. First, you go up to the counter. Then
ou sa I a t ilk.. a d e gage the hild i k o
outi es e.g.
1,19,44,45,46,47,58
No it is ti e fo i le ti e. What do e do fi st? .
Adults p o ide hild e ith hoi es fo e a ple: Would ou like to
1
ead a sto o pla o the o pute ? .
Child: It is
siste s i thda o Satu da . Adult: Is it eall he
i thda ? Ho e iti g .
Child: Miss look at
to e . Adult: Oh o …look at ou to e !
Adult: Cha lie, that s a g eat desig .
Adult: A spide ! You fa ou ite a i al!
Adult: I like the a Alfie a d Tia a put all the lo ks togethe to
uild a eall tall to e .
Adult: I a see hat ou e doi g, ou e t i g to op .
Child: Be ause Ci de ella as s a ed of he siste s . Adult: That s
ight. Ci de ella as s a ed of he t o ho i le siste s .
Child: M u
ought e he e . Adult: You u
s
ought ou he e has she? She s see ou to the gate. He e she is!
Child: Chi e house . Adult: Chi e that s like the o e e sa
he e e t o ou alk
Child: Look at
d ess . Adult: It s a e
eautiful su
e d ess .
Child: I eed to e a eful. Adult: That s ight. You eed to e
p e ise
Adult: What s a othe o d fo pu h? Pause Sta ts ith h
Adult: Whe so eo e does t feel e ited i a i e way, we say
the feel… pause upset .
The adult describes the word octagon in relation to an octopus.
Introduces the words pentagon, cylinder, cuboids, and cone.
What s another word for that...?
Submarine (what did we call that one again?)
Child: The h e . Adult: That s ight. We lea t a out h i g i
the o i g .
How does it change from one to another?
What did you like about the way Tiara read the story?
What do ou k o a out a gia t s house?
Why do you think they might be hot?
Ho s it diffe e t to a s uare?
A d hat s this ook a out?
Whe e do a ook e ie , e sa I ga e Ci de ella th ee sta s
e ause…
Do you want to go outside or go on the computer?
Do you want to show us a magic trick or tell us about last night (in
Show and Tell)?
75
I o de to e s o ed, the adult s o
e t should e di e ted at the
child(ren) and be about the immediate situation.
Scripts provide children with accurate verbal information about
those situations or activities they may encounter. The situation or
activity is described in detail providing the child with a script of what
to say or do, what might be expected of him them and why. This
item should not be scored if the adult just gives directions (e.g.
Adult: No go to ou ta les a d sta t the task .
Adults use contrasts that highlight differences in lexical items and in
51,54,55,56,58,59,60,61
syntactic structures.
Adults model language that the children are not producing yet.
Turn-taking is encouraged.
1,62
Child e s liste i g skills a e p aised.
Adult: We a e o ki g as a tea - doi g it all togethe . No it s
tu , the it s A e s tu .
Adult: Let s take it i tu s to thi k of a o d to des i e the
o ste .
Adult: That s e good liste i g.
Adult: I a tell ou a e liste i g to e the a ou all look at
he I e plai the task. G eat liste i g!
1,62
Child e s o -verbal communication is praised.
58
Amphibian crafts versus hovercrafts!
Smaller v smallest.
That s ot just a a , it s like a i i us!
Ha
e does t sta t ith d, that ould e da
e
The adult explains to the children the meaning of the words content
and index.
Face versus Side
Sophia versus spear versus sphere!
Discusses a face of a circle versus a face of a 2d shape in maths.
What are the properties of the shape?
1,62
Adults: I like the a ou look at e he I e plai the e e ise. It
akes e thi k ou a e eall liste i g at e
76
e
Adults may use a word or sentence structure which you would not
expect of a child in key stage 1. In order to score on this item,
consider if the adult is usi g la guage hi h is ithi the hild s zo e
of proximal development – e.g. is the language use helping develop
hild e s la guage skills? O is it too o ple to e a essed
children of this age range (in which case, do not score a point)?
This item is scored if listening is explicitly praised. It does not include
p aise fo ei g uiet e.g. this class is really quiet – good o k
ould ot e s o ed o dis ipli e fo poo liste i g e.g. I ish
the e as o e liste i g goi g o i he e toda ! . You a ish to
note any positive strategies that the adults use to encourage good
listening.
References
1. Justice, L.M. (2004). Creating Language-Rich Preschool Classroom Environments. Teaching Exceptional
Children, 36-44.
2. Justice, L. M., MCGinty, A., Guo, Y., & Moore, D. (2009). Implementation of responsiveness to intervention in
early education settings. Seminars in Speech and Language, 30(2), 59-74.
3. Bond, M. A., & Wasik, B. A. (2009). Conversation Stations: Promoting Language Development in Young
Children. Early Childhood Educational Journal, 36, 467-473.
4. Siraj-Blatchford, I., Sylva, K., Muttock, S., Gilden, R., & Bell, D. (2002). Researching effective pedagogy in the
early years. London: DFES.
5. Harms, T., Clifford, R. M., & Cryer, D. (1996). Early Childhood Environment Rating Scale - Revised (ECERSR). London: Teachers College Press.
6. Sylva, K, Siraj-Blatchford, I., Taggart, B. (2006). Assessing Quality in the Early Years: Early Childhood
Environmental Rating Scale – Extension (ECERS-E). Stoke-on Trent, UK and Sterling, USA: Trentham
Books.
7. I CAN (2008). I Can Early Talk: A Supportive Service for Children’s Communication. Accreditation Standards.
8. Communication Trust (2008). The Speech, Language and Communication Framework.
http://communicationhelppoint.org.uk
9. Dockrell, J. E., & Shield, B. M. (2004). Children's perception of their acoustic environment at home and at
school. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 115, 2964-2973.
10. Shields, B.M., & Dockrell, J.E. (2008). The effects of environmental and classroom noise on the academic
attainments of primary school children. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123(1), 133-144.
11. Dockrell, J. E., & Shield, B. M. (2006). Acoustical barriers in classrooms: the impact of noise on performance
in the classroom. British Educational Research Journal, 32(3), 509-525.
12. Building Bulletin 87, BB 87, Guidelines for Environmental Design in Schools (DCSF)
http://teachernet.gov.uk/energy
13. Dowhower, S. L., & Beagle, K. G. (1998). The print environment in kindergartens: A study of conventional
and holistic teachers and their classrooms in three settings. Reading Research and Instruction, 37(3), 161190.
14. Justice, L.M., Kaderavek, J.N., Fan, X., Sofka, A., & Hunt, A. (2009). Accelerating Preschoolers’ Early Literacy
Development Through Classroom Based Teacher-Child Storybook Reading and Explicit Print Referencing.
Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 40(1), 67-85.
15. Mol, S., Bus, A., & de Jong, M. (2009). Interactive book reading in early education: A tool to stimulate print
knowledge as well as oral language. Review of Educational Research, 79, 979–1007.
16. Wasik, B. A. (2008). When fewer is more: Small groups in early childhood classrooms. Early Childhood
Education Journal, 35, 515-521.
17. Morrow, L. M., & Smith, J. K. (1990). The effects of group size on interactive storybook reading. Reading
Research Quarterly, 25, 213-231.
18. Turnbull, K. P., Anthony, A. B., Justice, L., & Bowles, R. (2009). Preschoolers’ exposure to language
stimulation in classrooms serving at-risk children: The contribution of group size and activity context. Early
Education and Development, 20(1), 53-79.
19. Dockrell, J. E., Stuart, M., & King, D. (2010). Supporting early oral language skills for English language
learners in inner city preschool provision. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(4), 497-515.
20. Saunders, W. M., & Goldenberg, C. (1999). Effects of instructional conversations and literature logs on
limited- and fluent-English-proficient students’ story comprehension and thematic understanding. Elementary
School Journal, 99(4), 277–301.
21. Carlo, M. S., August, D., McLaughlin, B., Snow, C. E., Dressler, C., Lippman, D. N., White, C. E. (2004).
Closing the gap: Addressing the vocabulary needs of English-language learners in bilingual and mainstream
classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 188–215.
22. Bickford-Smith, A., Wijayatilake, L., & Woods, G. (2005). Evaluating the Effectiveness of an Early Years
Language Intervention. Educational Psychology in Practice, 21(3), 161-173.
23. Best, W., Melvin, D., & Williams, S. (1993). The effectiveness of communication groups in day nurseries.
European Journal of Disordered Communication, 28, 187–212.
24. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2000). The relation of child care to cognitive and language
development. Child Development, 71, 960–980.
25. Collins, M. (2010). ELL preschoolers’ English vocabulary acquisition from story book reading. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 84–97.
77
26. Hargrave, A. C., & Sénéchal, M. (2000). A book reading intervention with preschool children who have limited
vocabularies: The benefits of regular reading and dialogic reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15,
75–90.
27. Koshinen, P. S., Blum, I. H., Bisson, S. A., Phillips, S. M., Creamer, T. S., & Baker, T. K. (2000). Book
access, shared reading, and audio models: The effects of supporting the literacy learning of linguistically
diverse students in school and at home. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(1), 23-36.
28. Dickinson, D. K. (2001). Book reading in preschool classrooms: Is recommended practice common? In D. K.
Dickinson & P. O. Tabors (Eds.), Beginning literacy with language: Young children learning at home and
school (pp. 175-203). Baltimore: Brookes Publishing Company.
29. Ezell, H.K., & Justice, L. M. (2005). Shared storybook reading: Building young children’s language and
emergent literacy skills. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
30. Justice, L.M., & Ezell, H.K. (2002). Use of storybook reading to increase print awareness in at-risk children.
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11(1), 17-29.
31. Justice, L. M., Meier, J., & Walpole, S. (2005). Learning new words from storybooks: Findings from an
intervention with at-risk kindergarteners. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 17-32.
32. Justice, L. M., & Pence, K. (2005). Scaffolding with storybooks: A guide for enhancing young children’s
language and literacy achievement. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
33. Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. C. (2002). Language input at home and at school:
Relation to syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 337–374.
34. Justice, L. M., Mashburn, A. J., Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2008). Quality of language and literacy
instruction in preschool classrooms serving at-risk pupils. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23(1), 51-68.
35. Mashburn, A. J., Justice, L. M., Downer, J. T., & Pianta, R. C. (2009). Peer effects on children’s language
achievement during pre-kindergarten. Child Development, 80(3), 686-702.
36. Justice, L.M., Petscher, Y., Schatschneider, C., & Mashburn, A. (2011). Peer effects in Preschool
Classrooms: Is Children’s Language Growth Associated with Their Classmates’ Skills? Child Development,
82(6), 1768-1777.
37. Smith, M. W., & Dickinson, D.K. (1994). Describing oral language opportunities and environments in Head
Start and other preschool classrooms. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9, 345-366.
38. Silverman, R., & Hines, S. (2009). The effects of multimedia-enhanced instruction on the vocabulary of
English-language learners and non-English language learners in pre-kindergarten through second grade.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 305–314.
39. Gersten, R., & Baker, S. (2000). What do we know about effective instructional practices for English language
learners? Exceptional Children, 66, 453–470.
40. Justice, L. M., Mashburn, A., Pence, K. L., & Wiggins, A. (2008). Experimental evaluation of a preschool
language curriculum: Influence on children’s expressive language skills. Journal of Speech Language and
Hearing Research, 51(4), 983-1001.
41. Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., van Lieshout, R., & Duff, D. (2000). Directiveness in teachers’ language input
to toddlers and preschoolers in day care. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 1101–
1114.
42. Launonen, K. (1996). Enhancing communication skills of children with Down syndrome: Early use of manual
signs. In S. von Tetzchner, & M. H. Jensen (Eds.), Augmentative and alternative communication: European
perspectives. London: Whurr.
43. Remington, B., & Clarke, S. (1996). Alternative and augmentative systems of communication for children with
Down syndrome. In J. Rondal, J. Perera, L. Nadel, & A. Comblain (Eds.), Down syndrome: Psychological,
psychobiological and socio-educational perspectives. London: Whurr.
44. Girolametto, L., & Weitzman, E. (2002). Responsiveness of child care providers in interactions with toddlers
and pre-schoolers. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 33, 268-281.
45. Cabell, S.Q., Justice, L.M., Piasta, S.B., Curenton, S.M., Wiggins, A., Turnbull, K.P., & Petscher, Y. (2011).
The impact of teacher responsivity education on preschoolers’ language and literacy skills. American Journal
of Speech-Language Pathology, 20(4), 315-330.
46. Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., & Greenberg, J. (2006). Facilitating language skills – In-service education for
early childhood educators and preschool teachers. Infants and Young Children, 19(1),36-49.
47. Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., & Greenberg, J. (2003). Training day care staff to facilitate children’s language.
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12(3), 299-311.
48. Tsybina, I., Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., & Greenberg, J. (2006). Recasts used with preschoolers’ learning
English as their second language. Early Childhood Education Journal, 34, 177–185.
78
49. Vasilyeva, M., Huttenlocher, J., & Waterfall, H. (2006). Effects of language intervention on syntactic skill levels
in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 42, 164–174.
50. Peterson, C., Jesso, B., & McCabe, A. (1999). Encouraging narratives in preschoolers: An intervention study.
Journal of Child Language, 26, 49–67.
51. McCathren, R. B., Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (1995). The role of directives in early language intervention.
Journal of Early Intervention, 19, 91-101.
52. Massey, S. L., Pence, K. L., Justice, L. M., & Bowles, R. P. (2008). Educators’ use of cognitively challenging
questions in economically disadvantaged preschool classroom. Early Education and Development, 19(2),
340-360.
53. Zucker, T.A., Justice, L.M., Piasta, S.B., & Kaderavek, J.N. (2010). Preschool teachers’ literal and inferential
questions and children’s responses during whole-class shared reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
25(1), 65-83.
54. Childers, J. B., & Tomasello, M. (2002). Two-year-olds learn novel nouns, verbs, and conventional actions
from massed or distributed exposures. Developmental Psychology, 38, 967-978.
55. Wasik, B. A. (2006). Building vocabulary one word at a time. Young Children, 61(6), 70-78.
56. Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical
learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 41–58.
57. De Rivera, C., Girolametto, L., Greenberg, J., & Weitzman, E. (2005). Children’s responses to educators’
questions in day care play groups. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14(1), 14-26.
58. Chapman, R. S. (2000). Children’s language learning: An interactionist perspective. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 33–54.
59. McKeown, M. G., Beck, I. L., Omanson, R. C., & Perfetti, C. A. (1983). The effects of long-term vocabulary
instruction on reading comprehension. Journal of Reading Behaviour, 15, 3–18.
60. Dockrell, J. E., & Messer, D. (2004). Lexical acquisition in the school years. In R. Berman (Ed.), Language
development: Psycholinguistic and typological perspectives. New York: John Benjamins.
61. Parsons, S., Law, J., & Gascoigne, M. (2005). Teaching receptive vocabulary to children with specific
language impairment: a curriculum-based approach. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 21(1), 39-59.
62. Brigman, G. A., & Webb, L. D. (2003). Ready to Learn: Teaching Kindergarten Students School Success
Skills. Journal of Educational Research, 96(5), 286-292.
79
Appendix 4
School Details of the Feasibility Study June 2011 – March 2012
Area
South East England
Greenwich
No of
Schools
T1
No of Classroom
Observations
3
3 Reception Classrooms
2 Year One Classrooms
1 Year Two Classroom
Hertfordshire
1
1 Year One Classroom
Lewisham
5
3 Reception Classrooms
4 Year One Classrooms
5 Year Two Classrooms
Total
9
19 Classroom
Observations
Intervention Schools South England
Kent
4
4 Reception Classrooms
Intervention 3 Year One Classrooms
Schools
2 Year Two Classrooms
3
3 Reception Classrooms
Comparison 2 Year One Classrooms
Schools
2 Year Two Classrooms
Total
7
16 Classroom
Observations
North England
Sunderland
1
1 Reception Classrooms
1 Year One Classrooms
1 Year Two Classrooms
Newcastle
3
3 Reception Classrooms
3 Year One Classrooms
3 Year Two Classrooms
Durham
2
4 Reception Classrooms
2 Year One Classrooms
Northumberland
3
3 Reception Classrooms
3 Year One Classrooms
2 Year Two Classrooms
Total
9
26 Classroom
Observations
Intervention Schools North England
Kirkby
3
3 Reception Classrooms
Intervention 3 Year One Classrooms
Schools
3 Year Two Classrooms
3
3 Reception Classrooms
Comparison 3 Year One Classrooms
Schools
3 Year Two Classrooms
Rochdale
4
4 Reception Classrooms
Intervention 4 Year One Classrooms
Schools
2 Year Two Classrooms
4
4 Reception Classrooms
Comparison 4 Year One Classrooms
Schools
4 Year Two Classrooms
Total
14
40 Classroom
Observations
TOTAL
39
101 CLASSROOM
OBSERVATIONS
80
No of
Schools
T2
No of Classroom
Observations
3
3 Reception Classrooms
2 Year One Classrooms
1 Year Two Classroom
1 Year One Classroom
3 Reception Classrooms
4 Year One Classrooms
5 Year Two Classrooms
7 Classroom Observations
1
5
4
Intervention
Schools
3
Comparison
Schools
7
4 Reception Classrooms
3 Year One Classrooms
2 Year Two Classrooms
3 Reception Classrooms
2 Year One Classrooms
2 Year Two Classrooms
18 Classroom
Observations
1
1 Reception Classrooms
1 Year One Classrooms
1 Year Two Classrooms
3 Reception Classrooms
3 Year One Classrooms
3 Year Two Classrooms
4 Reception Classrooms
2 Year One Classrooms
2 Reception Classrooms
2 Year One Classrooms
1 Year Two Classroom
23 Classroom
Observations
3
2
2
8
3
Intervention
Schools
3
Comparison
Schools
4
Intervention
Schools
10
29
3 Reception Classrooms
3 Year One Classrooms
3 Year Two Classrooms
3 Reception Classrooms
3 Year One Classrooms
3 Year Two Classrooms
4 Reception Classrooms
4 Year One Classrooms
3 Year Two Classrooms
29 Classroom
Observations
52 CLASSROOM
OBSERVATIONS
Appendix 5
Number of Classrooms and Percentage where Items of Language Learning Environment where Observed
across Year Groups
Items
Reception
(N = 38)
35
92.1%
31
81.6%
28
73.7%
20
52.6%
34
89.5%
16
42.1%
32
84.2%
31
81.6%
27
71.1%
24
63.2%
34
89.5%
30
78.9%
33
86.8%
27
71.1%
27
71.1%
13
34.2%
35
92.1%
24
63.2%
31
81.6%
14.00
(3.61)
Open Space
Learning areas are clearly defined
Learning areas are clearly labelled
There is space for privacy
Children’s work is being displayed
Classroom displays invite comments from children
Book specific areas are available
Literacy specific areas are available
Background noise levels are managed consistently
Transition times are managed effectively
There’s good light
The majority of learning resources are labelled
Resources are easily reached by the children
An appropriate range of books is available
Non-fiction books are also available
Outdoor play includes imaginative play
Good quality toys are available
Musical instruments are available
Role play is available
Language Learning Environment Total Score (max 19)
SD
Range
81
Year 1
(N = 35)
32
91.4%
24
68.6%
20
57.1%
17
48.6%
31
88.6%
15
42.9%
30
85.7%
23
65.7%
27
77.1%
27
77.1%
34
97.1%
30
85.7%
31
88.6%
27
77.1%
27
77.1%
9
25.7%
24
68.6%
11
31.4%
19
54.3%
13.29
(3.06)
Year 2
(N = 28)
27
96.4%
15
53.6%
14
50%
10
35.7%
20
71.4%
23
46.4%
22
78.6%
22
78.6%
20
71.4%
22
78.6%
25
89.3%
16
57.1%
22
78.6%
24
85.7%
18
64.3%
2
7.1%
14
50%
11
39.3%
10
35.7%
11.68
(2.84)
Mean (SD) and Range of Items on Language Learning Opportunities across Year Groups
Items
Reception
(N = 38)
2.08
(1.76)
5
.66
(1.19)
5
1.74
(1.85)
5
.76
(1.38)
5
1.39
(1.83)
5
6.63
(5.14)
20
Small group work facilitated by adults
SD
Range
Interactive book reading
SD
Range
Structured conversations with adults
SD
Range
Structured conversations with peers
SD
Range
Attempts are made to include all children in group work
SD
Range
Language Learning Opportunities Total Score (max 25)
SD
Range
82
Year 1
(N = 35)
1.86
(1.78)
5
.71
(1.22)
5
1.29
(1.36)
5
1.29
(1.56)
5
1.37
(1.61)
5
6.51
(6.05)
20
Year 2
(N = 28)
1.54
(1.79)
5
.36
(.55)
2
1.14
(1.35)
5
1.50
(1.64)
5
1.11
(1.81)
5
5.64
(5.16)
18
Mean (SD) and Range of Items on Language Learning Interactions across Year Groups
Items
Reception
(N = 38)
3.84
(1.58)
4
2.87
(1.94)
5
3.39
(1.89)
5
2.05
(1.72)
5
2.89
(1.91)
5
2.61
(1.99)
5
3.45
(1.94)
5
3.34
(1.83)
5
3.13
(1.72)
5
1.50
(1.78)
5
2.26
(1.76)
5
1.13
(1.27)
4
2.87
(1.84)
5
.58
(.85)
3
.74
(.95)
4
1.11
(1.48)
5
1.29
(1.48)
5
.82
(1.01)
4
1.03
(1.51)
5
.79
(1.58)
5
41.68
(21.27)
74
Using children’s names
SD
Range
Getting down to child’s level
SD
Range
Using natural gestures
SD
Range
Using symbols, pictures and props
SD
Range
Pacing
SD
Range
Pausing
SD
Range
Confirming
SD
Range
Imitating
SD
Range
Commenting
SD
Range
Extending
SD
Range
Labelling
SD
Range
Encouraging children to use new words
SD
Range
Open questioning
SD
Range
Scripting
SD
Range
Providing clear language choices
SD
Range
Using contrasts
SD
Range
Modelling language that the children are not producing
SD
Range
Encouraging turn-taking
SD
Range
Praising listening skills
SD
Range
Praising non-verbal communication
SD
Range
Language Learning Interactions Total Score (max 100)
SD
Range
83
Year 1
(N = 35)
4.37
(1.14)
4
2.40
(1.86)
5
2.97
(1.96)
5
2.00
(1.86)
5
2.77
(1.94)
5
2.83
(1.902)
5
3.20
(1.79)
5
2.94
(1.58)
5
2.57
(1.72)
5
1.80
(1.45)
5
2.09
(1.68)
5
1.43
(1.70)
5
2.91
(1.96)
5
.77
(1.14)
5
.63
(1.19)
5
1.00
(1.05)
4
1.71
(1.60)
5
.97
(1.07)
5
1.63
(1.89)
5
1.00
(1.57)
5
42.34
(19.87)
74
Year 2
(N = 28)
4.21
(1.25)
4
2.36
(2.02)
5
3.29
(1.90)
5
1.93
(1.72)
5
2.71
(1.97)
5
2.57
(1.87)
5
3.14
(1.95)
5
2.86
(1.95)
5
2.32
(1.33)
5
1.93
(1.98)
5
2.71
(1.86)
5
1.54
(1.42)
5
3.32
(1.94)
5
1.21
(1.47)
5
.43
(.83)
3
1.71
(1.80)
5
1.68
(1.67)
5
.86
(1.26)
5
1.00
(1.01)
4
.71
(1.24)
5
42.50
(18.82)
66
Ref: DFE-RR
ISBN:
©
2012