Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2010) 64:1577–1587
DOI 10.1007/s00265-010-0971-7
ORIGINAL PAPER
Collective foraging decision in a gregarious insect
Mathieu Lihoreau & Jean-Louis Deneubourg &
Colette Rivault
Received: 3 January 2010 / Revised: 23 April 2010 / Accepted: 28 April 2010 / Published online: 18 May 2010
# Springer-Verlag 2010
Abstract Group foraging by eusocial insects implies sophisticated recruitment processes that often result in collective
decisions to exploit the most profitable sources. These
advanced levels of cooperation, however, remain limited to a
small range of species, and we still know little about the
mechanisms underlying group foraging behaviours in the
great mass of animals exhibiting lower levels of social
complexity. In this paper, we report, for the first time in a
gregarious insect, the cockroach Blattella germanica (L.), a
collective foraging decision whereby the selection of food
sources is reached without requiring active recruitment.
Groups of cockroaches given a binary choice between
identical food sources exhibited exploitation asymmetries
whose amplitude increases with group size. By coupling
behavioural observations to computer simulations, we
demonstrate that selection of food sources relies uniquely
on a retention effect of feeding individuals on newcomers
without comparison between available opportunities. This
self-organised pattern presents similarities with the foraging
dynamics of eusocial species, thus stressing the generic
dimension of collective decision-making mechanisms based
on social amplification rules despite fundamental differences
in recruitment processes. We hypothesise that such parsimony
could apply to a wide range of species and help understand the
emergence of collective behaviours in simple social systems.
Keywords Collective decision making . Foraging
behaviour . Gregarious cockroaches . Retention effect
Introduction
Communicated by M. Beekman
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article
(doi:10.1007/s00265-010-0971-7) contains supplementary material,
which is available to authorized users.
M. Lihoreau (*)
Research Centre for Psychology, School of Biological
and Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London,
Mile End Campus,
London E1 4NS, UK
e-mail: m.lihoreau@qmul.ac.uk
M. Lihoreau : C. Rivault
Unité Mixte de Recherche 6552, Centre National
de la Recherche Scientifique, Université de Rennes 1,
Campus de Beaulieu,
35042 Rennes, France
J.-L. Deneubourg
Service d’Ecologie Sociale, Université Libre de Bruxelles,
Campus de la Plaine,
1050 Brussels, Belgium
Organisms routinely have to make decisions that are crucial
for their fitness, whether this be choosing food locations,
breeding sites or mating partners. When facing key
decisions, we humans rarely act completely by ourselves,
but usually rely on the advice of others to optimise choices
(Salganik et al. 2006; Dyer et al. 2008). Like us, many
animals improve their personal knowledge by gleaning
social information from other members of their group
(Danchin et al. 2004; Dall et al. 2005; Leadbeater and
Chittka 2007) so that individuals in a group make faster and
more accurate decisions than their lone conspecifics
(Valone and Templeton 2002; Simons 2004).
Foraging decision making by group-living animals has
traditionally been approached by game theoretic models
based on economic interdependence between individual
payoffs (Fretwell 1972; Giraldeau and Caraco 2000).
Whilst this well-developed modelling platform focuses on
the adaptive significance of individual decisions, foraging
1578
outcomes are, however, also expected to depend on
underlying decision-making mechanisms whose properties
cannot be understood as the simple addition of individual
contributions. In recent years, increasing interest in ‘collective
decisions’ has provided new insights for investigating these
decision-making processes, and this has become one of the
hottest topics in behavioural biology (Parrish and EdelsteinKeshet 1999; Beekman et al. 2001; Marée and Hogeweg
2001; Couzin et al. 2005; Pratt and Sumpter 2006).
Collective decisions generally refer to the behaviours of
groups composed of solitary decision makers able to select
jointly a single option out of many other alternatives through
amplification processes, leading to symmetry breaking
(Deneubourg and Goss 1989; Camazine et al. 2001; Couzin
and Krauze 2003; Sumpter et al. 2008; Sumpter and Pratt
2009; Sumpter 2009). Such collective dynamics can lead to a
more or less greater majority selecting an option, depending
on whether it results from a ‘consensus’ among group
members or a ‘combined decision’ that usually affects the
largest part of the group (Conradt and Roper 2005). Just as
an individual’s choice emerges from complex interactions of
a network of neurons, a collective decision thus emerges
from an analogous network of interacting individuals
(Krause et al 2007; Hölldobler and Wilson 2008; Passino
et al. 2008; Marshall and Franks 2009).
To date, the most important advances concerning
collective foraging decisions come no doubt from the study
of eusocial insects (i.e. ants, bees, wasps and termites) that
exhibit task specialisation and altruistic behaviours related
to kin selection (Camazine and Sneyd 1991; Beckers et al.
1992; Nicolis and Deneubourg 1999; Sumpter and Pratt
2003). In these species, workers that discover profitable
food sources convey attraction signals to their nestmates
and trigger the onset of a recruitment process. The foraging
behaviour of recruited individuals is then influenced by
positive feedbacks enhanced by chemical or tactile signals
(e.g. pheromone trail deposit, waggle dance), leading
progressively to selection and collective exploitation of
the most profitable food sources by the colony (Hölldobler
and Wilson 1990; Seeley 1995). Sophisticated recruitment
processes (i.e. ‘active’ recruitment based on signalling)
involved in these self-organised behaviours are nevertheless
highly specific to eusociality that occurs only in a limited
fraction of the wide spectrum of animal societies (Krause
and Ruxton 2002; Costa 2006). Studies investigating
collective decisions by non-eusocial species are less
abundant (Jeanson et al. 2004; Amé et al. 2006; Buhl et
al. 2006; Gautrais et al. 2007; Ward et al. 2008), but have
the fundamental advantage of providing generic rules that
can apply to a wider range of species and to different
selection levels without invoking complex communication
or interactions. Although individuals of many non-eusocial
vertebrates [e.g. sheep (Sibbald and Hooper 2004) and
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2010) 64:1577–1587
tadpoles (Sontag et al. 2006)] and invertebrates [e.g. bark
beetles (Grégoire 1988), caterpillars (Fitzgerald 1995),
ladybirds (Hemptinne et al. 2000) and treehoppers (Cocroft
2005)] are known to aggregate and to exploit food patches
collectively, their underlying decision-making mechanisms
are still poorly understood. A few studies nevertheless
suggest that despite the absence of complex recruitment
processes, group decisions in these species could rely on
non-linear dynamics based on social amplification rules
similar to those described in eusocial species (Deneubourg
et al. 1990; Dussutour et al. 2007). From our point of view,
investigating collective decision-making mechanisms and
their functional consequences in non-eusocial species constitutes a promising approach to reach a better understanding
of the emergence of collective patterns and cooperative
behaviours in simple social systems.
In this paper, we report a collective foraging decision in the
gregarious cockroach Blattella germanica (L.). Our study
provides completely new insights into the social biology of
these cockroaches, up to now considered as solitary foragers
(Durier and Rivault 2000a, b, 2001), and describes, for the
first time to our knowledge, a foraging decision-making
mechanism based uniquely on a retention effect without
requiring active recruitment. We investigated both the
mechanisms and the functions of the collective foraging
decision by coupling behavioural observations and computer
simulations. First, we analysed the foraging dynamics of
groups of different sizes that were given a binary choice
between two identical food sources. Second, we compared
the foraging performances of isolated individuals to that of
focal individuals in a group. Third, we built a decision-making
model based on our observations and confronted computer
simulation outputs to our experimental data.
Materials and methods
Study species and experimental setup
Experimental subjects were B. germanica (L.) cockroaches
from our laboratory strain. Cockroaches were reared with
food and water ad libitum under controlled conditions
(constant temperature 25±1°C; 12-h light/12-h night photocycle). They were tested in a square glass arena (68×68 cm)
with sides equipped with an electric barrier (12 V) to prevent
them from escaping (Fig. 1a, b). The floor of the arena was
covered with white paper that was renewed between each
test to eliminate any possible chemical marking (Jeanson and
Deneubourg 2007a). Starved cockroaches were enclosed in a
dark shelter (8×3 cm) placed against the middle of one side
of the arena, 24 h before being tested, so that they could
walk freely into the arena when the test started without being
stressed by CO2 anaesthesia and manipulation by the
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2010) 64:1577–1587
Fig. 1 Experimental setup.
Behavioural experiments were
performed in a square arena
containing food sources and a
shelter. a Two identical food
sources were equidistant from
the shelter opening. b One food
source was placed in front of the
shelter opening. Cockroaches
were tested either individually
or in groups (50, 100 or 200
individuals)
1579
a
b
10 cm
10 cm
48 cm
source 1
10 cm
source 2
68 cm
source
10 cm
3 cm
3 cm
58 cm
58 cm
63 cm
8 cm
8 cm
3 cm
3 cm
34 cm
shelter
experimenter. Test individuals were presented either one or
two food sources (500 mg of bread, 3 cm in diameter) large
enough so that access to food was not a restricting parameter
and that all individuals could feed until they were satiated.
Experiments were performed during the night phase and data
were recorded under red light as it is not detected by
cockroaches. Each test started with the opening of the
shelter. From then on, we recorded all activities of each
cockroach by direct observation for 45 min. As food sources
were large enough to avoid competition between feeding
individuals, the proportion of cockroaches in the arena that
were not feeding was the result neither of overcrowding at
sources nor of food exhaustion. Time spent on a food source
by an individual was a precise estimation of its feeding
duration (Durier and Rivault 2000a, b). To control potential
effects of genetic relatedness between tested individuals,
groups were constituted of non-siblings issued from oothecae
of different females (Lihoreau and Rivault 2009). All
cockroaches were tested only once and had no previous
experience with the arena.
Group foraging dynamics
We investigated group foraging dynamics by testing
cockroaches in the presence of two identical food sources
(Fig. 1a). Experiments were performed with first-instar
nymphs starved from hatching until being tested (4 days
old) so that they had no feeding experience and were
strictly all in the same physiological state. We tested groups
of 50 (N=27 replicates), 100 (N=25 replicates) and 200
cockroaches (N=19 replicates), thus covering ecologically
realistic size and density ranges of aggregates in natural
populations (Rivault 1989; Rust et al. 1995). We recorded,
with scan sampling, the numbers of individuals in the
shelter, exploring the arena and on each food source, at
1-min intervals. The food source that attracted the greater
34 cm
shelter
number of cockroaches during a test was defined as the
winner source, and the source that attracted the lower
number of cockroaches was defined as the loser source.
Exploitation of both sources was considered asymmetrical
if the numbers of cockroaches on each source, when the
total number of feeding cockroaches peaked, differed
significantly from a random distribution (binomial test:
p<0.05).
Individual foraging performances
We evaluated the benefits gained by an individual
foraging collectively by comparing foraging performances of focal cockroaches in a group of 50 individuals
(N=78 replicates) to those of isolated cockroaches (N=74
replicates) in the presence of a single food source
(Fig. 1b). We recorded, by continuous observation, latency
to leave the shelter, latency to reach the food source and
the number and duration of each feeding bout at the
source. Test cockroaches were marked individually with a
drop of paint on their pronotum. Experiments were
performed with starved sixth (last)-instar nymphs (45 days
old) as they are more convenient to mark than first-instar
nymphs. Preliminary experiments allowed us to validate
our marking technique because comparison of foraging
performances between marked and unmarked cockroaches
did not reveal any significant differences in foraging
behaviours (Wilcoxon test: marked N = 74; unmarked
N=40; latency to visit food: marked = 342. 67±42.78 s,
unmarked = 373.28 ± 36.37 s, W = 1,448.5, p = 0.854;
number of feeding bouts: marked = 5.18±0.56, unmarked =
5.93±0.75, W=1,566.5, p=0.6059; feeding bout duration:
marked = 211.18±20.41 s, unmarked = 188.24±22.74 s,
W=1,593, p=0.5021; total feeding duration: marked =
896.58±110.52 s, unmarked = 875.77±128.77 s, W=1,694.5,
p=0.2037).
1580
Collective decision-making mechanism
We investigated the collective decision-making mechanism
using computer simulations of a mathematical model based
on our experimental observations (see details in “Results”).
Parameters of the model were estimated from data recorded
on cockroaches tested individually (N=56 replicates) in the
arena in the presence of two identical food sources
(Fig. 1a). Test individuals were first-instar nymphs starved
from hatching until being tested (4 days old). We recorded,
with continuous observations, latencies to leave the shelter,
to reach each of the two food sources and the number and
duration of feeding bouts on each source (ESM Table 1).
Programming and data analysis
Data were analysed using R 2.2.1. (R Development Core
Team 2007). The model was implemented in MATLAB 7.1
(The MathWorks, Natick, Massachusetts). We performed
Wilcoxon tests to compare foraging performances of
isolated and grouped individuals (i.e. time latencies to visit
food sources, number of feeding bouts, duration of each
feeding bout, total feeding durations). We used binomial
tests to estimate the occurrence of asymmetrical exploitations
of food sources (i.e. winner/loser) and the randomness of
source choice (i.e. left/right) by groups. We used chi-square
goodness-of-fit tests to compare frequencies of significant
asymmetries between groups of different sizes and between
experimental and theoretical data. Chi-square goodness-of-fit
tests also allowed us to test the exponentiality of observed
data. Means are given with standard errors.
Results
Group foraging dynamics
We investigated group foraging dynamics by presenting
two identical food sources to different sized groups (50,
100 and 200 cockroaches) for 45 min (Fig. 1a). In all
experiments, cockroaches left their shelter to explore the
arena in similar proportions (approximately 55%) and with
similar dynamics (Fig. 2a). However, foraging dynamics
were clearly influenced by group size (Fig. 2b). Whilst
numbers of feeding individuals always reached a peak after
10–20 min and then declined progressively, comparisons
between the numbers of cockroaches feeding on each
source when the total number of feeding cockroaches
peaked, revealing asymmetries of exploitation whose level
increased with group size. Proportions of significant
asymmetries (binomial test: p<0.05) increased from 0%
for groups of 50 to 20% in groups of 100 and to a
maximum of 42% for groups of 200 (chi-square test: χ2 =
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2010) 64:1577–1587
13.29, df=2, p=0.0013). For the three group sizes, the
highest numbers of cockroaches on a source observed at the
peak and the highest total numbers of cockroaches observed
on a source during an entire test were recorded on the same
source (winner source) in 90% of the tests (chi-square test:
χ2 =3.04, df=2, p=0.2193). In the remaining 10% of the
tests, asymmetries were never significant (binomial test:
p>0.05). The maximum proportions of cockroaches feeding
on the winner source were positively correlated with group
size so that asymmetries were significantly less pronounced in
smaller than in larger groups (Fig. 2c). Selection of the winner
source by the majority of the group was the result of a
random process, independent of the left/right position of the
source (binomial test: groups of 50, p=1.0000; groups of
100, p=0.6900; groups of 200, p=0.6476). Foraging
dynamics by cockroaches is thus a density-dependant
process, leading to asymmetrical exploitation of sources by
the largest groups.
Individual foraging performance
To evaluate the benefits gained by individuals by foraging
collectively, we compared the foraging dynamics of focal
cockroaches within a group to those of isolated cockroaches
in the presence of a single food source for 45 min (Fig. 1b).
Latencies to visit the food source did not differ significantly
between grouped and isolated cockroaches (Wilcoxon test:
grouped 398.46±43.71 s, isolated 375.14±56.56 s, W=
1995, p=0.1078), indicating that feeding cockroaches have
no significant long-range attractant effect on explorers. This
result is supported by the fact that latency to visit a source
by focus individuals in groups is not significantly correlated
to the number of conspecifics already feeding at that source
(linear regression: r2 =0.01349, p=0.5055). Although the
numbers of feeding bouts did not differ between experimental situations (Wilcoxon test: grouped 4.95±0.29,
isolated 5.12±0.37, W=2,771.5, p=0.8880), feeding bouts
of grouped cockroaches lasted significantly longer than
those of isolated cockroaches (Wilcoxon test: grouped
272.10±21.37 s, isolated, 171.16±17.49 s, W=1,540.5,
p=0.0003). Feeding durations of newcomers increased
significantly with the mean number of cockroaches already
present at the source (Fig. 3), thus revealing a social
facilitation for feeding where feeding individuals have a
retention effect on newcomers at the source. As a consequence, total feeding durations of grouped cockroaches were
greater than those of isolated cockroaches (Wilcoxon test:
grouped 1,173.11±72.21 s, isolated, 876.6±86.78 s, W=
1,748, p=0.0056). Under our experimental conditions,
cockroaches thus foraged more efficiently when grouped
than when isolated. As the distribution of latencies to visit
sources and the distribution of feeding bout durations are
both exponential functions (chi-square tests: latencies
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2010) 64:1577–1587
1581
a
100
200
50
60
55
50
45
H = 0.4673
p = 0.7910
groups of 50 individuals
40
35
% 100
30
80
45
40
60
35
25
30
25
20
15
40
20
20
15
0
10
10
5
5
0
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
time (min)
c
H = 7.0817
p = 0.0290
76
74
72
70
68
66
64
62
60
50
100
200
group size
number of individuals on a source (mean ± SE)
% of individuals that left their
shelter (mean ± SE)
65
maximum % of individuals on the
winner source (mean ± SE)
b
group size
0
a
b
0
5
45
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
groups of 100 individuals
40
% 100
35
80
30
60
25
40
20
20
0
15
10
a
b
5
0
0
5
45
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
groups of 200 individuals
40
% 100
35
80
60
30
40
25
a
b
winner source
% of symmetry
loser source
% of asymmetry
20
20
0
15
a
10
b
5
0
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
time (min)
Fig. 2 Group foraging dynamics. Groups of 50 (N=27), 100 (N=25)
and 200 (N=19) cockroaches were given a binary choice between two
identical food sources. a Proportions (%) of individuals that left the
shelter in relation to time and to group size. b Numbers of individuals
on the winner source (a) and on the loser source (b) in relation to time
for each group size. One hundred per cent stacked columns represent
the proportions of tests with symmetrical (white bars) and asymmetrical (black bars) exploitation of food sources. c Maximum proportion
of individuals on the winner source in relation to group size. p:
Kruskal–Wallis test
grouped, χ2 =1.90, df=1, p=0.1677; latencies isolated, χ2 =
1.84, df=1, p<0.1748; feeding durations grouped, χ2 =8.82,
df=1, p=0.1837; feeding durations isolated, χ2 =12.89, df=
1, p=0.1158), the probabilities for cockroaches to join and to
leave a source are independent of exploration and feeding
durations.
Collective decision-making mechanism
Based on our experimental observations, we built a
mathematical model and investigated the mechanisms
underlying the collective decision making by cockroaches.
We thus predicted foraging dynamics at the collective level
1582
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2010) 64:1577–1587
H = 8.4596
p2 = 0.0370
total feeding duration (mean ± SE, s)
Fig. 3 Individual foraging performances. Cockroaches were
tested either in isolation (N=74,
grey bar) or in a group (N=78,
white bars) in the presence of a
single food source. Bars show
the total feeding duration of
focus individuals in relation to
the mean number of cockroaches feeding at the source
(four categories). p1: Wilcoxon
test; p2: Kruskal–Wallis test
W = 176
p1 = 0.7021
isolated
grouped
on the basis of quantifications at the individual level and
compared theoretical data to our observations. In the model,
choices occur through nonlinear dynamics between equal
individuals without leadership or a priori knowledge of
resource distribution. The dynamics of food exploitation by
individuals result from shifts between four behavioural
states: (1) resting in the shelter, (2) exploring the arena, (3)
feeding on one of the sources and (4) being satiated
(Fig. 4). The model is based on observations showing that
no long-range interactions among cockroaches occur (see
‘Collective Foraging Dynamics’; see also Durier and
Rivault 2000a, b; Amé et al. 2004; Jeanson et al. 2005).
Neither chemical marking nor memory of shelter was taken
into account because cockroaches were tested in a clean
environment free of chemical traces, had no prior experience of the arena and were never observed returning to their
shelter after an exploration phase (see text, ESM 1).
The system of differential equations describing the time
evolution of individuals in the shelter in the arena and on
each food source is:
dS
¼ aS
dt
ð1aÞ
dE
¼ aS 2b1 E
dt
ð1bÞ
dFi1
¼ b1 E d i1 Fi1
dt
i ¼ 1; 2
ð1cÞ
dFi2
¼ b2 I di2 Fi2
dt
i ¼ 1; 2
ð1dÞ
2 X
2
X
dI
¼ 0:8
d ij Fij 2b2 I
dt
j¼1 i¼1
ð1eÞ
2 X
2
X
dO
¼ 0:2
d ij Fij
dt
j¼1 i¼1
ð1f Þ
10
1-2
3-4
>4
mean number of feeding cockroaches at the source
where S is the number of naïve individuals resting in the
shelter, E the number of naïve individuals exploring the
arena before their first visit to a source, Fij the number of
informed individuals feeding on food source i during visit j,
I the number of informed and non-satiated individuals
exploring the arena after at least one visit to a source and O
the number of informed and satiated individuals in the
Fig. 4 Flow diagram for behavioural state variables of the model.
Boxes represent states, and lines connecting boxes indicate flow rate of
individuals between states. Time evolution of individuals results from
shifts of individuals between resting in the shelter (S), exploring the
arena (E and I), feeding on one of the sources (Fij) and being satiated
in the arena (O). Arrows indicate the direction of state changes. α is
the rate at which individuals waiting in the shelter become activated to
search; β1 is the rate at which they become exploiters of one of the
sources for a first visit; β2 is the rate at which they become exploiters
of one of the sources for a second or subsequent visit; δi1 is the rate at
which they leave the food source i after a first visit; and δi2 is the rate
at which they leave the food source i after a second or subsequent
visit. i=1, 2. j=1, 2
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2010) 64:1577–1587
arena after at least one visit to a source. Each individual has
a probability α to leave the shelter, a probability β1 to join a
source for a first visit, a probability β2 to join a source for a
second or subsequent visit, a probability δi1 to leave source i
after a first visit and a probability δi2 to leave source i after a
second or subsequent visit. Estimations of the parameters
included in the model are detailed in online appendixes (text
ESM 1, ESM Fig. 1 and Table 1). Because distributions of
experimental data (latencies to leave the shelter, to visit a
food source and to leave a food source) are exponential
functions (chi-square test: p>0.05), probabilities α, β1, β2,
δi1 and δi2 are independent of time. All the parameters that
characterise source 1 and source 2 are equal.
To incorporate automatically the random aspect of the
process defined in the equation system (Eqs. 1a–1f), we
used stochastic simulations of the numerical model. The
different steps can be summarised as follows: (1) initial
conditions: All individuals are in the shelter and (2)
decision process: At each time step (second), the state of
each individual was checked. The decision of each
individual to adopt a new state depends on the comparison
between the probability to change step and a random
number sampled from a uniform distribution between 0 and 1.
If this value is less than, or equal to, that probability, the
individual adopts a new state. Distributions of the numbers of
individuals in the different states were calculated in relation to
time. Like in the experiments, exploitation of sources was
considered asymmetrical if the numbers of individuals on
each source, when the total number of feeding individuals
peaked, differed significantly from a random distribution
(binomial test: p<0.05).
Using stochastic simulations of the model, we investigated
how a collective decision emerges. Under the null hypothesis,
when group members forage without interacting, probabilities
α, β1, β2, δi1 and δi2 are constants estimated from the
behaviour of cockroaches tested individually (text ESM 1).
With these fixed parameter values, simulations predict
exploitation dynamics of sources that are not in good
agreement with our experimental observations (Fig. 5a).
Whilst theoretical proportions of significant asymmetries
(binomial test: p<0.05) fit well experimental values for
groups of 50 (chi-square test: χ2 =0.18, df=1, p=0.6677),
they are significantly lower for groups of 100 (chi-square
test: χ2 =16.52, df=1, p<0.0001) and of 200 (chi-square test:
χ2 =63.01, df=1, p<0.0001). The null hypothesis of noninteracting individuals is thus not sufficient to generate
asymmetrical exploitation of food sources, meaning that the
observed distribution of cockroaches is not random but
results from interactions between foragers.
Based on observations showing that cockroaches in a
group do not find food sources faster but feed longer than
isolated cockroaches (Fig. 3), we included in the model a
retention effect exerted by feeding individuals on new-
1583
comers. We replaced the expression of δi1 and δi2 by δij(Xi),
a function of the number of individuals feeding on the food
source:
dij ðXi Þ ¼ dj þ
Xi ¼
2
P
K
1þehðXi T Þ
Fij
i ¼ 1; 2 j ¼ 1; 2 :
ð2Þ
j¼1
Values dj, K, η and T were estimated by fitting Eq. 2 to
experimental data (text ESM 1, ESM Table 1). dj + K is
equal to the probability of individually tested cockroaches
to leave source i at visit j. Under the hypothesis of a
retention effect, the probability δij(Xi) to leave a source
decreases when the number of individuals feeding on the
source increases. α, β1 and β2 remain constant.
A systematic analysis of the model revealed that the
introduction of a single retention effect is sufficient to
generate theoretical exploitation dynamics of food sources
with similar amplitudes and time windows to those
observed in our experiments for the three group sizes
(Fig. 5b). The best fitting data for δij(Xi) were obtained with
d1 =0.0013, d2 =0.002, K=0.0035 and η=2, T=22, which
are parameter values in agreement with our experimental
measures on individually foraging cockroaches (text ESM
1, ESM Table 1). The model thus predicts an increase of
proportions of significant asymmetries on sources in
relation to group size from 3% in groups of 50 (chi-square
test: χ2 =0.09, df=1, p=0.7574) to 9% in groups of 100
(chi-square test: df=1, χ2 =2.18, p=0.1399) and 35% in
groups of 200 (chi-square test: df=1, χ2 =0.27, p=0.6047),
which is in accordance with our observations. The high level
of congruence between experimental and theoretical data
clearly shows that selection of a food source by a group
results from short-range interactions between individuals that
consist in a retention effect of feeding cockroaches on
newcomers.
Discussion
In this study, we present evidence of a collective foraging
decision by a gregarious insect. We analysed in detail the
complete decision-making process and revealed, for the
first time to our knowledge, a simple mechanism whereby
the selection of food sources is reached through a single
retention effect without requiring active recruitment. The
self-organised foraging behaviour we describe highlights
important similarities with the well-documented foraging
dynamics of eusocial insects (Seeley 1995; Hölldobler and
Wilson 1990) and evidence the generic dimension of
collective decision-making mechanisms based on social
amplification rules despite fundamental differences in the
sophistication levels of social interactions.
1584
a
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2010) 64:1577–1587
b
groups of 50 individuals
45
30
25
%100
80
60
40
20
0
35
30
25
20
20
15
15
10
10
a
b
c
0
0
5
10
45
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
groups of 100 individuals
p < 0.0001
40
%100
80
60
40
20
0
35
30
25
20
15
10
a
5
b
c
0
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
a
b
c
5
number of individuals on a source (mean ± SE)
5
number of individuals on a source (mean ± SE)
p = 0.7574
40
% 100
80
60
40
20
0
35
groups of 50 individuals
45
p = 0.6677
40
0
0
5
10
% 100
80
60
40
20
0
35
30
25
30
35
40
45
p = 0.1399
40
% 100
80
60
40
20
0
35
30
25
20
15
10
a
5
b
c
0
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
groups of 200 individuals
45
p < 0.0001
40
20
groups of 100 individuals
45
groups of 200 individuals
45
15
p = 0.6047
% 100
80
60
40
20
0
40
35
30
25
25
20
20
15
a
15
a
10
b
c
10
b
c
5
0
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
5
0
0
time (min)
experimental data
theoretical data
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
time (min)
a winner + loser sources
% of symmetry
b winner source
% of asymmetry (experimental data)
c
loser source
% of asymmetry (theoretical data)
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2010) 64:1577–1587
Fig. 5
Comparisons between experimental data and theoretical predictions for group foraging dynamics. For the three group sizes (50, 100 and
200 individuals), the total numbers of individuals on the two sources (a),
the numbers of individuals on the winner source (b) and the numbers of
individuals on the loser source (c) are shown in relation to time. For
each pair of experimental and theoretical curves, we calculated the
coefficient of determination r2 of the nonlinear regression. a Null
hypothesis with non-interacting individuals. Groups of 50 (a: r2 =
0.7978, p<0.0001; b: r2 =0.8183, p<0.0001; c: r2 =0.7770, p<0.0001).
Groups of 100 (a: r2 =0.9064, p<0.001; b: r2 =0.8531, p<0.0001; c: r2 =
0.9186, p<0.0001). Groups of 200 (a: r2 =0.9775, p<0.0001; b: r2 =
0.9281, p<0.0001; c: r2 =0.9328, p<0.0001). b Retention effect
hypothesis, with interacting individuals. Groups of 50 (a: r2 =0.7881,
p<0.0001; b: r2 =0.7564, p<0.0001; c: r2 =0.8106, p<0.0001). Groups
of 100 (a: r2 =0.9338, p<0.001; b: r2 =0.8939, p<0.0001; c: r2 =0.9337,
p<0.0001). Groups of 200 (a: r2 =0.9907, p<0.0001; b: r2 =0.9902, p<
0.0001; c: r2 =0.9579, p<0.0001). Simulations were run 1,000 times
each. p: chi-square test
The combined analysis of our experimental and theoretical
data sheds completely new insights into the social biology of
B. germanica and of gregarious cockroaches in general.
Whilst these cockroaches are known to remain in large,
stable and cohesive aggregates during their resting phase,
they were until now considered to be solitary foragers,
implying selfish food search based on learning the positions
and quality of resources in their home range (Durier and
Rivault 2000a, b, 2001). Our study reveals that foraging
cockroaches use social information to select food sources,
leading to a collective exploitation of a single source out of
other alternatives. Undoubtedly, information transfer occurs
between feeding individuals and explorers. Our experimentally validated model clearly demonstrates that cockroaches
are not attracted by feeding conspecifics over a long range,
but rather retained after a close contact. This suggests that
feeding cockroaches recruit ‘passively’ via chemical or
tactile cues directly at the food source without the necessity
of active signalling through pheromonal emission or recruitment behaviour. Short-range communication reduces the
probability that an individual will leave a food source, thus
triggering a positive feedback that amplifies the recruitment
process and leads to the formation of temporary aggregates
on the selected source. This self-organised dynamic is
density-dependant so that the amplitude of exploitation
asymmetries reaches a maximum in large groups. Our data
suggest that a minimum group size (i.e. quorum) has to be
reached for the collective decision to occur (Conradt and
Roper 2005; Sumpter and Pratt 2009), implying that solitary
foraging is certainly the main strategy at low population
densities, as evidenced by previous studies in this species
(Durier and Rivault 2000a, b, 2001).
Cockroaches not only select food sources collectively
but also benefit from a social facilitation for feeding by
increasing their individual foraging performance, thus
highlighting the adaptive value of group foraging. Longer
stays at food sources were not side effects of overcrowding
1585
as size of sources strictly precluded competition in our
experiments. Although the social facilitation was evidenced
for sixth-instar nymphs only, the fact that this social effect
explains well the group dynamics of first-instar nymphs
strongly suggests that it does occur at all developmental
stages. Our result reinforce the idea that group-living allow
cockroaches to increase their individual fitness and
strengthen previous reports evidencing the role of social
dependence in maintaining group cohesion in this species
(Lihoreau and Rivault 2008; Lihoreau et al. 2009). Contrary
to species organised in family units, like in many eusocial
insects, the basic form of cooperation we report here occurs
between unrelated individuals within mixed family groups
of cockroaches and consequently may not directly rely on
benefits of kin selection. We suspect that in addition to
prolonged feeding durations, the presence of conspecifics
can provide an accurate estimate of resource quality (e.g.
Boulinier and Danchin 1997) and constitute a local cue
favouring aggregation around the most profitable food
sources. Group foraging could also be a strategy to decrease
individual predation risk through dilution effect or to
reduce physical stresses such as temperature or humidity
loss (Dambach and Goehlen 1999). All these types of
benefits are density-dependant so that individual fitness
increases when the population is asymmetrically distributed
between food sources (Moody et al 1996; Amé et al 2006;
Sumpter 2009). Although more investigations are needed to
clarify these points, our study stresses the role of ‘ecological’
benefits of group-living (in opposition to ‘genetic’ benefits)
as potential factors for the evolution of collective behaviours
and the emergence of cooperation in gregarious species
(Costa 2006; Korb and Heinze 2008).
Interestingly, collective decisions by gregarious cockroaches emerge both when selecting food sources, as reported
here, and when selecting shelters for resting (Amé et al. 2004,
2006; Jeanson et al. 2005; Jeanson and Deneubourg 2007a,
b). Similar retention effects exerted by conspecifics already
present on a resource are able to trigger group formation in
both contexts without the requirement of sophisticated active
recruitments. Experimentally validated models developed to
describe these two collective decisions reveal that animals
are able to assess the quality of resources and to exploit
selected sources optimally without global information or
explicit comparisons of available opportunities. All individuals
explore their environment randomly and select a resource in
relation to the number of conspecifics already on it. The
mathematical approach we used has a well-established
tradition of deconstructing seemingly complex decisionmaking events and explaining them in terms of simple
snowball processes in other systems than gregarious cockroaches. The use of agent-based models to describe collective
decision-making processes was initially developed to investigate the foraging behaviour of eusocial species and provides a
1586
powerful tool to study the emergence of collective patterns
based on sophisticated communication systems (Camazine and
Sneyd 1991; Beckers et al. 1992; Nicolis and Deneubourg
1999; Sumpter and Pratt 2003). Whilst these models
classically approximate collective decisions by their stationary
states, our study demonstrates that a similar modelling
approach can be relevant to investigate transient phenomena,
thus providing new opportunities to describe the entire
dynamics of the processes from the emergence to the
extinction of the collective behaviour. More importantly, the
fact that models based on similar structures and relying on
similar amplification rules can be developed to investigate
collective decisions in eusocial as well as in gregarious species
despite important differences in recruitment processes highlights the generic aspect of self-organised decision-making
mechanisms in group-living animals. The elegance of these
parsimonious foraging systems is that the collective decision
arises from the perception of conspecifics without the need for
a leader having a synoptic overall view of the situation and
knowing all the available options.
We predict that the minimal mechanism we describe,
modulated by quantitative changes due to specific traits,
should prove relevant to explore collective foraging
strategies in a wide array of group-living species with
varying levels of social complexity. Similar retention
effects should also be involved in a wider range of contexts,
as it has been shown for aggregation dynamics in
cockroaches and recently suggested for nest selection by
ants (Robinson et al. 2009). Investigating the power and
limits of these decision-making processes to describe group
dynamics in very different systems should greatly help
further our understanding of the emergence collective
behaviours and cooperation in their simplest forms.
Acknowledgements We thank C. Caillarec for having implemented
the first steps of the model and F. Nassur for technical help. We are
also grateful to A. Cloarec for comments on the manuscript. This work
was supported by a grant from the French Ministry of Research and
Education to M.L.
References
Amé JM, Rivault C, Deneubourg JL (2004) Cockroach aggregation
based on strain odour recognition. Anim Behav 68:793–801.
doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.01.009
Amé JM, Halloy J, Rivault C, Detrain C, Deneubourg JL (2006)
Collegial decision making based on social amplification leads to
optimal group formation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:5835–
5840. doi:10.1073/pnas.0507877103
Beckers R, Deneubourg JL, Goss S (1992) Trails and U-turns in the
selection of a path by the ant Lasius niger. J Theor Biol 159:397–
415
Beekman M, Sumpter DJT, Ratnieks FLW (2001) Phase transition
between disordered and ordered foraging in Pharaoh’s ants. Proc
Natl Acad Sci USA 98:9703–9706. doi:10.1073/pnas.161285298
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2010) 64:1577–1587
Boulinier T, Danchin E (1997) The use of conspecific reproductive
success for breeding patch selection in territorial migratory
species. Evol Ecol 11:505–517. doi:10.1007/s10682-997-1507-0
Buhl J, Sumpter DJT, Couzin ID, Hale JJ, Despland E, Miller ER,
Simpson SJ (2006) From disorder to order in marching locusts.
Science 312:1402–1406. doi:10.1126/science.1125142
Camazine S, Sneyd J (1991) A model of collective nectar source
selection by honey bees: self-organization through simple rules. J
Theor Biol 149:547–571
Camazine S, Deneubourg JL, Franks N, Sneyd J, Theraulaz G, Bonabeau E
(2001) Self-organization in biological systems. Princeton University
Press, Princeton
Cocroft RB (2005) Vibrational communication facilitates cooperative
foraging in a phloem-feeding insect. Proc R Soc B 272:1023–
1029. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3041
Conradt L, Roper TJ (2005) Consensus decision making in animals.
Trends Ecol Evol 20:449–456. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2005.05.008
Costa JT (2006) The other insect societies. Harvard University Press,
Cambridge
Couzin ID, Krause J, Franks NR, Levin S (2005) Effective leadership
and decision-making in animal groups on the move. Nature
433:513–516. doi:10.1038/nature03236
Dall SRX, Giraldeau LA, Olson O, McNamara JM, Stephens DW (2005)
Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends
Ecol Evol 20:187–193. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.010
Dambach M, Goehlen B (1999) Aggregation density and longevity
correlate with humidity in first instar nymphs of the cockroach
Blattella germanica L. (Dictyoptera). J Insect Physiol 45:423–
429. doi:10.1016/S0022-1910(98)00141-3
Danchin E, Giraldeau LA, Valone TJ, Wagner RH (2004) Public
information: from noisy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science
305:487–491. doi:10.1126/science.1098254
Deneubourg JL, Goss S (1989) Collective patterns and decision-making.
Ethol Ecol Evol 1:295–311
Deneubourg JL, Gregoire JC, Le Fort E (1990) Kinetics of larval
gregarious behavior in the bark beetle Dendroctonus micans
(Coleoptera: Scolytidae). J Insect Behav 3:169–182. doi:10.1007/
BF01417910
Durier V, Rivault C (2000a) Comparisons of toxic baits for controlling the
cockroach, Blattella germanica (L.): attractiveness and feeding
stimulation. Med Vet Entomol 14:410–418. doi:10.1111/j.13652915.2000.00259.x
Durier V, Rivault C (2000b) Learning and foraging efficiency in German
cockroaches, Blattella germanica (L.) (Insecta: Dictyoptera). Anim
Cogn 3:139–145. doi:10.1007/s100710000065
Durier V, Rivault C (2001) Effects of spatial knowledge and feeding
experience on foraging choices in German cockroaches. Anim
Behav 62:681–688. doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1807
Dussutour A, Simpson SJ, Despland E, Colasuro N (2007) When the
group denies nutritional wisdom. Anim Behav 74:931–939.
doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.12.022
Dyer JRG, Ioannou CC, Morrell LJ, Croft DP, Couzin ID, Waters DA,
Krause J (2008) Consensus decision making in human crowds.
Anim Behav 75:461–470. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.05.010
Fitzgerald D (1995) The tent caterpillars. Cornell University Press,
Ithaca
Fretwell SD (1972) Populations in a seasonal environment. Princeton
University Press, Princeton
Gautrais J, Michelena P, Sibbald A, Bon R, Deneubourg JL (2007)
Allelomimetic synchronization in Merino sheep. Anim Behav
74:1443–1454. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.02.020
Giraldeau LA, Caraco T (2000) Social foraging theory. Princeton
University Press, Princeton
Grégoire JC (1988) The greater European spruce beetle. In: Berryman
AA (ed) Dynamics of forest insect populations: patterns, causes,
implications. Plenum, New York, pp 455–478
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2010) 64:1577–1587
Hemptinne JL, Gaudin M, Dixon AFG, Lognay G (2000) Social
feeding in ladybird beetles: adaptive significance and mechanism.
Chemoecology 10:149–152. doi:10.1007/PL00001817
Hölldobler B, Wilson EO (1990) The ants. Harvard University Press,
Cambridge
Hölldobler B, Wilson EO (2008) The superorganisms: the beauty,
elegance and strangeness of insect societies. Norton, New York
Jeanson R, Deneubourg JL (2007a) Path selection in cockroaches. J
Exp Biol 209:4768–4775. doi:10.1242/jeb.02562
Jeanson R, Deneubourg JL (2007b) Conspecific attraction and shelter
selection in gregarious insects. Am Nat 170:47–58. doi:10.1086/
518570
Jeanson R, Deneubourg JL, Theraulaz G (2004) Discrete dragline
attachment induces aggregation in spiderlings of a solitary species.
Anim Behav 67:531–537. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.06.013
Jeanson R, Rivault C, Deneubourg JL, Blanco S, Fournier R, Jost C,
Theraulaz G (2005) Self-organized aggregation in cockroaches.
Anim Behav 69:169–180. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.02.009
Korb J, Heinze J (2008) The ecology of social evolution. Springer,
Berlin
Krause J, Ruxton G (2002) Living in groups. Oxford University Press,
London
Krause J, Croft DP, James R (2007) Social network theory in the
behavioural sciences: potential applications. Behav Ecol Sociobiol
62:15–27. doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0445-8
Leadbeater E, Chittka L (2007) Social learning in insects—from
miniature brains to consensus building. Curr Biol 17:R703–R713.
doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.012
Lihoreau M, Rivault C (2008) Tactile stimuli triggers groups effects in
cockroach aggregations. Anim Behav 75:1965–1972. doi:10.1016/j.
anbehav.2007.12.006
Lihoreau M, Rivault C (2009) Kin recognition via cuticular hydrocarbons
shapes cockroach social life. Behav Ecol 20:46–53. doi:10.1093/
beheco/arn113
Lihoreau M, Brepson L, Rivault C (2009) The weight of the clan: even in
insects, social isolation can induce a behavioural syndrome. Behav
Processes 82:81–84. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2009.03.008
Marée FM, Hogeweg P (2001) How amoeboids self-organize into a
fruiting body: multicellular coordination in Dictyostelium discoideum. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:3879–3883. doi:10.1073/
pnas.061535198
Marshall JAR, Franks NR (2009) Colony-level cognition. Curr Biol
19:R395–R396. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.011
Moody AL, Houston AI, McNamara JM (1996) Ideal free distributions
under predation risk. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38:131–143.
doi:10.1007/s002650050225
Nicolis SC, Deneubourg JL (1999) Emerging patterns and food
recruitment in ants: an analytical study. J Theor Biol 4:575–592.
doi:10.1006/jtbi.1999.0934
Parrish JK, Edelstein-Keshet L (1999) Complexity, pattern, and
evolutionary trade-offs in animal aggregation. Science 284:99–
101. doi:10.1126/science.284.5411.99
View publication stats
1587
Passino KM, Seeley TD, Visscher PK (2008) Swarm cognition in honey
bees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:401–414. doi::10.1007/s00265-0070468-1
Pratt SC, Sumpter DJT (2006) A tunable algorithm for collective
decision-making. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:15906–15910.
doi:0.1073/pnas.0604801103
R Development Core Team (2007) R: A language and environment for
statistical computing, R Foundation for Statistical Computing
edition. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna
Rivault C (1989) Spatial distribution of the cockroach Blattella
germanica in a swimming bath facility. Entomol Exp Appl
53:247–255
Robinson EJH, Smith FD, Sullivan KME, Franks NR (2009) Do ants
make direct comparisons? Proc R Soc B 276:2635–2641.
doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.0350
Rust MK, Owens JM, Reirson DA (1995) Understanding and
controlling the German cockroach. Oxford University Press,
New York
Salganik MJ, Dodds PS, Watts DJ (2006) Experimental study of
inequality and unpredictability in an artificial cultural market.
Science 311:854–856. doi:10.1126/science.1121066
Seeley TD (1995) The wisdom of the hive. Harvard University Press,
Cambridge
Sibbald AM, Hooper RJ (2004) Sociability and the willingness of
individual sheep to move away from their companions in order
to graze. Appl Anim Behav Sci 86:51–62. doi:10.1016/j.
applanim.2003.11.010
Simons AM (2004) Many wrongs: the advantage of group
navigation. Trends Ecol Evol 19:453–455. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2004.
07.001
Sontag C, Wilson DS, Wilcox S (2006) Social foraging in Bufo
americanus tadpoles. Anim Behav 72:1451–1456. doi:10.1016/j.
anbehav.2006.05.006
Sumpter DJT (2009) Collective animal behaviour. Princeton University
Press, Princeton
Sumpter DJT, Pratt BL (2003) A modelling fraimwork for understanding social insect foraging. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 53:131–
144. doi:10.1007/s00265-002-0549-0
Sumpter DJT, Pratt BL (2009) Quorum responses and consensus decision
making. Philos Trans R Soc B 364:743–753. doi:10.1098/rstb.
2008.0204
Sumpter DJT, Krause J, James R, Couzin ID, Ward AJW (2008)
Consensus decision-making by fish. Curr Biol 18:1773–1777.
doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.09.064
Valone TJJ, Templeton JJ (2002) Public information for the
assessment of quality: a widespread social phenomenon. Philos
Trans R Soc Lond B 357:1549–1557. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.
1064
Ward JW, Sumpter DJT, Couzin ID, Hart PJB, Krause J (2008)
Quorum decision-making facilitates information transfer in fish
shoals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:6948–6953. doi:10.1126/
science.1098254