Abstract
BACKGROUND
The effects of clinical-trial funding on the interpretation of trial results are poorly understood. We examined how such support affects physicians’ reactions to trials with a high, medium, or low level of methodologic rigor.
METHODS
We presented 503 board-certified internists with abstracts that we designed describing clinical trials of three hypothetical drugs. The trials had high, medium, or low methodologic rigor, and each report included one of three support disclosures: funding from a pharmaceutical company, NIH funding, or none. For both factors studied (rigor and funding), one of the three possible variations was randomly selected for inclusion in the abstracts. Follow-up questions assessed the physicians’ impressions of the trials’ rigor, their confidence in the results, and their willingness to prescribe the drugs.
RESULTS
The 269 respondents (53.5% response rate) perceived the level of study rigor accurately. Physicians reported that they would be less willing to prescribe drugs tested in low-rigor trials than those tested in medium-rigor trials (odds ratio, 0.64; 95% confidence interval [CI], 0.46 to 0.89; P = 0.008) and would be more willing to prescribe drugs tested in high-rigor trials than those tested in medium-rigor trials (odds ratio, 3.07; 95% CI, 2.18 to 4.32; P<0.001). Disclosure of industry funding, as compared with no disclosure of funding, led physicians to downgrade the rigor of a trial (odds ratio, 0.63; 95% CI, 0.46 to 0.87; P = 0.006), their confidence in the results (odds ratio, 0.71; 95% CI, 0.51 to 0.98; P = 0.04), and their willingness to prescribe the hypothetical drugs (odds ratio, 0.68; 95% CI, 0.49 to 0.94; P = 0.02). Physicians were half as willing to prescribe drugs studied in industry-funded trials as they were to prescribe drugs studied in NIH-funded trials (odds ratio, 0.52; 95% CI, 0.37 to 0.71; P<0.001). These effects were consistent across all levels of methodologic rigor.
CONCLUSIONS
Physicians discriminate among trials of varying degrees of rigor, but industry sponsorship negatively influences their perception of methodologic quality and reduces their willingness to believe and act on trial findings, independently of the trial’s quality. These effects may influence the translation of clinical research into practice.
A large proportion of clinical trials of new treatments is funded by the pharmaceutical industry.1,2 There is increasing concern over how such support can affect the design of these trials3 and the reporting of the results.4–9 Some practitioners may be swayed by potential bias in industry-supported publications to use certain medications more widely than would be appropriate, whereas other practitioners may be skeptical about the validity of company-funded research.10 In the past two decades, to ensure full transparency in the reporting of clinical trials, disclosure of financial support has become the norm in medical research publications.11–13 Some biomedical journals include disclosures in the abstracts themselves, where their effect on readers might be greatest.14
Financial disclosure may not fully address the problem of potential bias in published research results.15,16 One study showed that physicians ignored disclosure statements,17 whereas other studies have shown that disclosure of substantial conflicts of interest can lead clinicians to discount the reported results of a trial.18–20 Some reports suggest that the disclosure of conflicts of interest can have a paradoxical effect,21 such as enhancing the trustworthiness of the discloser.22
The methodologic rigor of a trial, not its funding disclosure, should be a primary determinant of its credibility. Skepticism about results that is based on a trial’s funding sources may be less appropriate for well-controlled, double-blind, randomized trials than for poorly controlled or unblinded trials, in which conflicts of interest may have a stronger effect on interpretation of the data. However, little is known about how information concerning study design interacts with information concerning funding sources to influence physicians’ interpretation of research. We therefore conducted a randomized study involving simulated research abstracts to assess the role that disclosure plays in physicians’ interpretation of the results of medical research.
METHODS
DEVELOPMENT OF THE SURVEY INSTRUMENT AND STUDY OVERSIGHT
We developed hypothetical scenarios in which three new drugs were being evaluated for the treatment of disorders commonly encountered in primary care: hyperlipidemia, diabetes, and angina. “Lampytinib” would be used for dyslipidemia in patients who had unacceptable side effects from statins, “bondaglutaraz” would be used for diabetes and low levels of high-density lipoprotein cholesterol in patients who were taking metformin and a sulfonylurea and who were unwilling or unable to add insulin, and “provasinab” would be used for angina in patients with untreatable multivessel coronary disease who were taking maximal doses of beta-blockers. Since internists report that they frequently read only the abstracts when reviewing the medical literature,23 we created abstracts describing trials of these drugs in which we varied the drug being tested, the trial’s methodologic rigor, and the funding source. For each drug, one trial had a high level of rigor, one had a medium level of rigor, and one had a low level of rigor. The features defining these levels were based on guidelines24,25 and on our experience in conducting randomized trials26,27 and in studying evidence-based drug-evaluation and prescribing practices28,29 (Table 1). Differences in methodologic rigor among the trials were consistent across drugs. All the trials had similar effect sizes, and statistically significant results.
Table 1.
Characteristic | Level of Rigor | ||
---|---|---|---|
High | Medium | Low | |
Randomization | Randomized | Randomized | Randomized |
Blinding | Double-blind | Single-blind | Open-label |
Comparator | Active | Active | Usual care |
Dropout rate (%) | <9 | 13 | 19 |
Sample size (no. of patients) | 5322 | 964 | 483 |
End point | Mortality | Surrogate | Surrogate |
Patient population | Representative | Representative | Not representative |
Length of follow-up (mo) | 36 | 12 | 4 |
Report of safety | Drug appears to be safe | Drug appears to be safe | No report |
We then added a variable describing the trial’s funding status. Each abstract included one of three variations: no funding source mentioned, funding by the National Institutes of Health (NIH), or funding by a pharmaceutical company, with financial involvement in that company on the part of the lead author. (Companies named in the disclosure statements were randomly selected from the top 12 global pharmaceutical enterprises.30)
Assigning one of the three conditions to each of the three variables (drug, study design, and funding source), we created 27 different abstracts describing a hypothetical new-drug trial. The abstracts, along with the study protocol, are provided in the Supplementary Appendix, available with the full text of this article at NEJM.org. The survey was pretested among 12 physicians certified in internal medicine.
The study was approved by the institutional review board at Brigham and Women’s Hospital, with written informed consent implied by the participant’s completion of the survey. There were no agreements concerning confidentiality of the data between the authors and the institutions providing financial support for the study.
SURVEY SAMPLE
The American Board of Internal Medicine (ABIM) maintains a list of diplomates with active certification and maintenance of certification. The data set included demographic characteristics and information about medical training and responses to the ABIM Practice Characteristics Survey.31,32 From a potential sample of 45,398 physicians, we randomly identified 514 who reported spending 40% or more of their time and 21 hours or more per month in activities related to patient care and spending 50% or less of their time in the intensive care unit, the emergency department, or the cardiac catheterization laboratory. Of these possible participants, 11 had noncurrent contact information, resulting in a potential sample of 503 physicians.
SURVEY ADMINISTRATION
Between July 2011 and October 2011, physicians in the sample received two postcards and three e-mail messages from ABIM indicating that they had been randomly selected to participate in a study investigating how physicians make prescribing decisions. These communications included the names of the sponsoring institutions and the lead investigators, a link to the online survey, an opportunity to opt out, and an offer of a $50 honorarium on completion of the survey (see the Supplementary Appendix). Physicians who did not respond were mailed a printed version of the invitation along with a $5 bill and an offer of $45 on completion of the survey. Those who still did not respond received a telephone reminder.
Participants were presented with three abstracts, each of which described a trial of a different hypothetical new drug. Participants were told to assume that the hypothetical drug had recently been approved by the Food and Drug Administration and was covered by insurance and that the abstract was from a “high-impact” biomedical journal and written by academic physicians at established U.S. universities. We randomly varied the level of methodologic rigor and the disclosure so that the three abstracts that the physicians received described trials at each level of methodologic rigor and with each disclosure variation.
DEPENDENT MEASURES AND VARIABLES
Each abstract was followed by the same set of questions, with the choice of answers based on a 7-point Likert scale. Physicians were asked about their likelihood of prescribing the new drug (ranging from a score of 1 for “very unlikely to prescribe” to a score of 7 for “very likely to prescribe”), the level of methodologic rigor of the trial (ranging from a score of 1 for “not at all rigorous” [low rigor] to a score of 7 for “very rigorous” [high rigor]), and their confidence in the study investigators’ conclusions (ranging from a score of 1 for “not confident at all” to a score of 7 for “very confident”). Secondary outcomes were participants’ rating of the importance of the trial20 and their level of interest in reading the full article. (These questionnaires are provided in the Supplementary Appendix.)
After reading the third abstract, participants were asked to estimate how many abstracts describing trials of medications they had read in biomedical journals in the past month and to respond to the following question: “Do you think that pharmaceutical company funding is likely to influence the outcome of scientific studies about the efficacy and safety of pharmaceuticals in favor of the drug in question?” Finally, participants were asked about any financial support they had received from drug, device, or other medically related companies in the previous year.33
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
For each question, we estimated a hierarchical proportional-odds regression model, using the appropriate Likert-scale response as the outcome. This model included a random intercept for each physician to account for within-physician correlation of responses across abstracts, as well as fixed effects for methodologic rigor (low, medium, or high), disclosure statement (industry funding, NIH funding, or no statement), and drug. A second model included interactions between the indicators for funding source and methodologic rigor.34 Because we developed the three hypothetical drugs solely to obtain data about the effects of the level of methodologic rigor and variation of disclosure, we did not separately analyze differences among the drugs.
To investigate the association between characteristics of the physicians and survey responses, we created proportional-odds models that included random intercepts for physicians; indicators for the variables describing methodologic rigor, type of disclosure statement, and drug; and terms for physician characteristics (i.e., age, sex, location of medical school [U.S. vs. non-U.S.], type of practice [general internal medicine vs. subspecialty], proportion of time spent in clinical care [≥80% vs. <80%], hours per month spent in clinical care [≥80 vs. <80], acceptance of gifts from industry [any vs. none], and opinion about whether industry funding influences the outcome of trials in favor of the drug being tested [a score of 6 or higher vs. a score of 5 or lower on the 7-point scale, with higher numbers indicating a perception of a stronger influence]).
RESULTS
CHARACTERISTICS OF THE RESPONDENTS
Of the 503 physicians who were invited to participate in the survey, 269 responded (a 53.5% response rate); 192 (71.4%) responded online (spending a median of 14 minutes), and 77 (28.6%) responded by returning a paper copy of the survey. We evaluated the responses of the 263 physicians who answered all the questions for one or more abstracts.
The demographic and practice characteristics of the respondents did not differ significantly from those of the nonrespondents (Table 2). Most of the respondents (75.5% [188 of 249]) reported accepting at least one type of industry support. Respondents generally agreed with the statement that industry funding could influence the outcome of clinical trials of pharmaceutical agents in favor of the drug in question (mean score, 5.4 [out of a possible 7.0]; 95% confidence interval [CI], 4.0 to 6.9). Responses to other questions are provided in the Supplementary Appendix.
Table 2.
Characteristic | Respondents† (N = 241) |
Nonrespondents (N = 262) |
P Value |
---|---|---|---|
Age — yr | 0.82 | ||
Median | 48 | 48 | |
Interquartile range | 45–53 | 45–53 | |
Male sex — no. of physicians (%) | 162 (67.2) | 183 (69.8) | 0.53 |
Medical-school location — no. of physicians (%) | |||
United States | 136 (56.4) | 140 (53.4) | 0.50 |
Other than United States | 105 (43.6) | 122 (46.6) | |
Time spent in clinical care activities — no. of physicians (%)‡ | |||
>80 hr/mo | 78 (32.4) | 86 (32.8) | 0.94 |
≤80 hr/mo | 162 (67.2) | 176 (67.2) | |
Clinical time devoted to primary care — % | 0.35 | ||
Median | 80 | 70 | |
Interquartile range | 20–91 | 6–95 | |
No. of journal abstracts read in past month relating to prescription drugs§ | NA | ||
Median | 4 | ||
Interquartile range | 2–8 | ||
Industry support received — no. of physicians/total no. (%)¶ | 188/249 (75.5) | NA |
NA denotes not available.
Demographic data could not be matched to 22 of 263 respondents (8.4%). These data therefore remain in the group of nonrespondents.
Data were missing for 2 physicians.
Results are based on data for 248 physicians.
Industry support included drug samples, meals, educational support, honoraria, and support for travel to meetings.
Data were available for 249 physicians.
EFFECT OF METHODOLOGIC RIGOR
We found strong associations between the methodologic rigor of the hypothetical trials and physicians’ perceptions of rigor. In comparisons with trials having a medium level of rigor, physicians were less likely to identify low-rigor trials as rigorous (odds ratio, 0.55; 95% CI, 0.40 to 0.76; P<0.001) (Fig. 1A) and were more likely to report high-rigor trials as rigorous (odds ratio, 3.95; 95% CI, 2.81 to 5.55; P<0.001). Similarly, in comparisons with medium-rigor trials, physicians had less confidence in the results of low-rigor trials (odds ratio, 0.48; 95% CI, 0.35 to 0.66; P<0.001) and more confidence in the results of high-rigor trials (odds ratio, 2.73; 95% CI, 1.95 to 3.82; P<0.001) (Fig. 1B) and would be less likely to prescribe drugs described in low-rigor trials (odds ratio, 0.64; 95% CI, 0.46 to 0.89; P=0.008) and more willing to prescribe drugs described in high-rigor trials (odds ratio, 3.07; 95% CI, 2.18 to 4.32; P<0.001) (Fig. 1C).
EFFECT OF DISCLOSURE VARIATIONS
We found clear associations between the funding disclosure variations and physicians’ perceptions of a trial’s rigor and results. Regardless of the actual study design, physicians were less likely to view a trial as having a high level of rigor if funding by a pharmaceutical company was disclosed than if no disclosure statement was included (odds ratio, 0.63; 95% CI, 0.46 to 0.87; P = 0.006) (Fig. 2A). Similarly, in comparisons with trials for which no funding was listed and regardless of the study design, physicians were less likely to have confidence in the results of trials funded by industry (odds ratio, 0.71; 95% CI, 0.51 to 0.98; P = 0.04) (Fig. 2B) and were less willing to prescribe drugs described in such trials (odds ratio, 0.68; 95% CI, 0.49 to 0.94; P = 0.02) (Fig. 2C). These effects were even greater when industry-funded trials were compared with trials described as having NIH support.
In comparisons with NIH-funded trials, respondents were also less likely to describe industry-funded trials as “important” (odds ratio, 0.59; 95% CI, 0.42 to 0.82; P = 0.002), and were less likely to want to read the full article (odds ratio, 0.67; 95% CI, 0.47 to 0.95; P = 0.03). In comparisons between industry-funded trials and trials with no funding statement, the differences were smaller with respect to respondents’ ratings of the importance of the trial (odds ratio, 0.88; 95% CI, 0.63 to 1.22; P = 0.43) and their desire to read the full article (odds ratio, 0.88; 95% CI, 0.63 to 1.23; P = 0.46).
INTERACTION BETWEEN METHODOLOGIC RIGOR AND DISCLOSURE VARIATIONS
The reduction in physicians’ willingness to prescribe drugs studied in industry-funded trials as compared with drugs studied in trials without funding disclosures was consistent across levels of methodologic rigor (P = 0.87 for high-rigor trials vs. medium-rigor trials and P = 0.83 for low-rigor trials vs. medium-rigor trials). The greater willingness to prescribe drugs described in NIH-funded trials was also constant across rigor levels (P = 0.81 for high-rigor trials vs. medium-rigor trials and P = 0.56 for low-rigor trials vs. medium-rigor trials). We found no interaction between methodologic rigor and disclosure with respect to variations in physicians’ perceptions of the rigor of a trial or their confidence in the results (data not shown).
EFFECT OF PHYSICIANS’ CHARACTERISTICS
Irrespective of the disclosure and level of methodologic rigor, physicians who strongly agreed that funding by pharmaceutical companies can influence the results of a trial (score of 6 or 7 on the Likert scale) were less likely to prescribe the hypothetical drugs than were physicians with lower scores (odds ratio, 0.58; 95% CI, 0.37 to 0.91; P = 0.02). For all the abstracts presented, U.S.-trained physicians were substantially less likely to say they would be willing to prescribe the hypothetical drugs than were physicians who were trained elsewhere (odds ratio, 0.44; 95% CI, 0.27 to 0.70; P = 0.001). Physicians who were older than the median age of 48 years for the sample were more likely than the younger physicians to report that they would prescribe the simulated drugs (odds ratio, 1.65; 95% CI, 1.04 to 2.61; P = 0.03). Adjustment for physicians’ characteristics did not alter the effect estimates of methodologic rigor or disclosure statement, indicating that the randomization effectively balanced these characteristics.
DISCUSSION
In this randomized study, we found that practicing internists understood and appreciated methodologic differences when they read abstracts describing hypothetical studies of new drugs. They discounted small, poorly designed trials and assigned greater validity to large trials that tested clinical end points. We also found that respondents downgraded the credibility of industry-funded trials, as compared with the same trials randomly characterized as having NIH funding or having no source of support listed. The magnitude of this reduction in perceived methodologic rigor was about the same as that for low-rigor trials as compared with medium-rigor trials. Physicians’ skepticism of industry-funded research affected their responses to high-rigor and low-rigor trials similarly.
Well-publicized controversies related to industry-funded research may help explain these findings. Reports have emerged of trials that with-held critical data35–37 or that presented positive results while withholding negative results.38,39 Other concerns stem from reports of industry-financed articles that were ghostwritten40 or published primarily as instruments of marketing.41 Physicians’ skepticism of industry-funded research may be a response to such trends.
These findings have important implications. Despite the occasional scientific and ethical lapses in trials funded by pharmaceutical companies, it is also true that the pharmaceutical industry has supported many major drug trials that have been of particular clinical importance.42 Excessive skepticism concerning trials supported by industry could hinder the appropriate translation of the results into practice. For example, after publishing the results of a large, well-designed trial describing a new use for a widely prescribed class of drugs,43 a leading biomedical journal noted that many of its readers believed that the results of the trial did not justify a change in clinical management, citing industry funding as a key reason for this conclusion.44
The results of our study were based on physicians’ responses to descriptions of hypothetical trials of three new drugs, each of which was described in a single trial with varying attributes, 45 whereas actual prescribing behavior integrates drug information from many sources. Nevertheless, prescribing decisions made when a drug is first approved may rely principally on a single published study, as presented in these scenarios. Our response rate was similar to the mean rate in published surveys of physicians,46 and our respondents were similar to other internists in terms of the characteristics we measured.47 However, unmeasured variables may have differed between respondents and nonrespondents, contributing to bias in our sample. Finally, the findings from this survey of board-certified internists may not be generalizable to other specialties.48
Pharmaceutical companies seeking to enhance the appropriate use of important new products or to expand the appropriate uses of existing products must address the attitudes that our survey revealed,49,50 so that the credibility of the results of industry-supported trials is more likely to be based on methodologic rigor than on funding sources. Exactly how to change such attitudes was not the subject of this research. Currently, journal reviewers and editors, those who conduct systematic reviews, or even interested physicians can refer to the ClinicalTrials.gov database to see whether trial data as reported reflect the planned study design.51 This retrospective check could alleviate concerns about the possibility that trial outcomes were changed after the data were gathered and analyzed. However, the information provided to this database may have missing values or may be of poor quality.51,52 We do not have empirical data that address whether concordance between the study design and the reporting of results influences physicians’ perceptions of methodologic rigor.
We found that physicians assigned the highest level of credibility to NIH-funded trials. Thus, an increase in the number of clinical trials funded by the NIH or by the new Patient-Centered Outcomes Research Institute might reduce clinicians’ skepticism and lead to more data-driven changes in practice.53 Despite the initial financial outlay, such publicly funded trials are likely to save more than they cost.54 Partnerships between the NIH and industry55 may also serve this purpose if their jointly funded trials feature characteristics that are a routine part of NIH-funded trials, including data and safety monitoring boards and public reporting of protocols.
It is reassuring that the physicians in our study were attentive to the level of methodologic rigor. They also clearly took notice of funding sources for trials, according greater credibility and import to NIH-funded research than to industry-funded research. Although attention to potential sources of bias is necessary, such skepticism apparently can also reduce the credibility and acceptance of even high-quality research that is industry-supported. Financial disclosure is important, but more fundamental strategies, such as avoiding selective reporting of results in reports of industry-sponsored trials, ensuring protocol and data transparency, and providing an independent review of end points, will be needed to more effectively promote the translation of high-quality clinical trials — whatever their funding source — into practice.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Supported by a grant from the Edmond J. Safra Center for Ethics at Harvard University, a career development award from the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (K08HS18465-01) and a Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Investigator Award in Health Policy Research (both to Dr. Kesselheim), a fellowship at the Petrie–Flom Center for Health Law Policy, Biotechnology, and Bioethics at Harvard Law School (to Dr. Robertson), and a grant from the National Cancer Institute (2R25CA092203-08, to Dr. Rose).
We thank the American Board of Internal Medicine Foundation for help with administrative support; Adam Licurse, Maureen Garmon, and David Yokum for their help in the research process; Lawrence Lessig and Christine Cassel for their comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript; and members of the Research Ethics Program of the Harvard Catalyst.
Footnotes
Disclosure forms provided by the authors are available with the full text of this article at NEJM.org.
REFERENCES
- 1.Angell M. Industry-sponsored clinical research: a broken system. JAMA. 2008;300:1069–1071. doi: 10.1001/jama.300.9.1069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Zinner DE, Bolcic-Jankovic D, Clarridge B, Blumenthal D, Campbell EG. Participation of academic scientists in relationships with industry. Health Aff (Millwood) 2009;28:1814–1825. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.28.6.1814. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Lexchin J, Bero LA, Djulbegovic B, Clark O. Pharmaceutical industry sponsorship and research outcome and quality: systematic review. BMJ. 2003;326:1167–1170. doi: 10.1136/bmj.326.7400.1167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Als-Nielsen B, Chen W, Gluud C, Kjaergard LL. Association of funding and conclusions in randomized drug trials: a reflection of treatment effect or adverse events? JAMA. 2003;290:921–928. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.7.921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Bekelman JE, Li Y, Gross CP. Scope and impact of financial conflicts of interest in biomedical research: a systematic review. JAMA. 2003;289:454–465. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.4.454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ridker PM, Torres J. Reported outcomes in major cardiovascular clinical trials funded by for-profit and not-forprofit organizations: 2000–2005. JAMA. 2006;295:2270–2274. doi: 10.1001/jama.295.19.2270. [Erratum, JAMA 2006; 295:2726.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Irwin RS. The role of conflict of interest in reporting of scientific information. Chest. 2009;136:253–259. doi: 10.1378/chest.09-0890. [Erratum, Chest 2009; 136:653.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Golder S, Loke YK. Is there evidence for biased reporting of published adverse effects data in pharmaceutical industry-funded studies? Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2008;66:767–773. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2125.2008.03272.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rose SL, Krzyzanowska MK, Joffe S. Relationships between authorship contributions and authors’ industry financial ties among oncology clinical trials. J Clin Oncol. 2010;28:1316–1321. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2008.21.6606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.DeAngelis CD, Fontanarosa PB. Impugning the integrity of medical science: the adverse effects of industry influence. JAMA. 2008;299:1833–1835. doi: 10.1001/jama.299.15.1833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Conflict of interest in medical research, education, and practice. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2009. Institute of Medicine, Board on Health Sciences Policy. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Sage WM. Some principles require principals: why banning ‘conflicts of interest’ won’t solve incentive problems in biomedical research. Tex Law Rev. 2007;85:1413–1463. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Drazen JM, de Leeuw PW, Laine C, et al. Toward more uniform conflict disclosures — the updated ICMJE conflict of interest reporting form. N Engl J Med. 2010;363:188–189. doi: 10.1056/NEJMe1006030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Information for authors. Lancet. 2011 Nov; ( http://download.thelancet.com/flatcontentassets/authors/lancet-information-for-authors.pdf). [Google Scholar]
- 15.Boozang KM, Coleman CH, Greenwood K, Handler-Hutchinson S, Finizio C. The limits of disclosure as a response to financial conflicts of interest in clinical research. Newark, NJ: Seton Hall University School of Law; 2010. Dec, ( http://law.shu.edu/ProgramsCenters/HealthTechIP/HealthCenter/upload/health-center-whitepaper-dec2010.pdf). [Google Scholar]
- 16.Glaser BE, Bero LA. Attitudes of academic and clinical researchers toward financial ties in research: a systematic review. Sci Eng Ethics. 2005;11:553–573. doi: 10.1007/s11948-005-0026-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Silverman GK, Loewenstein GF, Anderson BL, Ubel PA, Zinberg S, Schulkin J. Failure to discount for conflict of interest when evaluating medical literature: a randomised trial of physicians. J Med Ethics. 2010;36:265–270. doi: 10.1136/jme.2009.034496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Lacasse JR, Leo J. Knowledge of ghostwriting and financial conflicts-ofinterest reduces the perceived credibility of biomedical research. BMC Res Notes. 2011;4:27. doi: 10.1186/1756-0500-4-27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Chaudhry S, Schroter S, Smith R, Morris J. Does declaration of competing interests affect readers’ perceptions? A randomised trial. BMJ. 2002;325:1391–1392. doi: 10.1136/bmj.325.7377.1391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Schroter S, Morris J, Chaudhry S, Smith R, Barratt H. Does the type of competing interest statement affect readers’ perceptions of the credibility of research? Randomised trial. BMJ. 2004;328:742–743. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38035.705185.F6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cain D, Lowenstein G, Moore D. The dirt on coming clean: perverse effects of disclosing conflicts of interest. J Legal Studies. 2005;34:1–25. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Pearson SD, Kleinman K, Rusinak D, Levinson W. A trial of disclosing physicians’ financial incentives to patients. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166:623–628. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.6.623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Saint S, Christakis DA, Saha S, et al. Journal reading habits of internists. J Gen Intern Med. 2000;15:881–884. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2000.00202.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.GRADE Working Group. Grading quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. BMJ. 2004;328:1490. doi: 10.1136/bmj.328.7454.1490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Guyatt G, Oxman AD, Akl EA, et al. GRADE guidelines 1. Introduction — GRADE evidence profiles and summary of findings tables. J Clin Epidemiol. 2011;64:383–394. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.04.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Glynn RJ, Danielson E, Fonseca FA, et al. A randomized trial of rosuvastatin in the prevention of venous thromboembolism. N Engl J Med. 2009;360:1851–1861. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0900241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Glynn RJ, Ridker PM, Goldhaber SZ, Buring JE. Effect of low-dose aspirin on the occurrence of venous thromboembolism: a randomized trial. Ann Intern Med. 2007;147:525–533. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-147-8-200710160-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Avorn J, Chen M, Hartley R. Scientific versus commercial sources of influence on the prescribing behavior of physicians. Am J Med. 1982;73:4–8. doi: 10.1016/0002-9343(82)90911-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fischer MA, Avorn J. Economic implications of evidence-based prescribing for hypertension: can better care cost less? JAMA. 2004;291:1850–1856. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.15.1850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Contract Pharma. 2009 Top 20 pharmaceutical companies report. 2009 Jul 15; ( http://www.contractpharma.com/issues/2009-07/view_features/2009-top-20-pharmaceutical-companies-report). [Google Scholar]
- 31.Zhang Y, Guille R. A longitudinal analysis of certification trend and performance on dual certification in hematology and medical oncology. Presented at the American Educational Research Association Meeting; New Orleans. April 8–12, 2011; abstract. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Weng W, Gray B, Lipner RS. Racial disparity in the quality of diabetes care: do physicians treat minority patients differently or do minority patients receive care from different types of physicians?. Presented at the AcademyHealth Annual Research Meeting; Boston. June 27–29, 2010; abstract. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Campbell EG, Gruen RL, Mountford J, Miller LG, Cleary PD, Blumenthal D. A national survey of physician–industry relationships. N Engl J Med. 2007;356:1742–1750. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa064508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Peterson B, Harrell FE. Partial proportional odds models for ordinal response variables. Appl Stat. 1990;39:205–217. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Curfman GD, Morrissey S, Drazen JK. Expression of concern: Bombardier et al., “Comparison of upper gastrointestinal toxicity of rofecoxib and naproxen in patients with rheumatoid arthritis,”. N Engl J Med. 2000;343:1520–1528. doi: 10.1056/NEJMe058314. N Engl J Med 2005;353:2813-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Turner EH, Matthews AM, Linardatos E, Tell RA, Rosenthal R. Selective publication of antidepressant trials and its influence on apparent efficacy. N Engl J Med. 2008;358:252–260. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa065779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Fontanarosa PB, Rennie D, DeAngelis CD. Postmarketing surveillance — lack of vigilance. JAMA. 2004;292:2647–2650. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.21.2647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Psaty BM, Kronmal RA. Reporting mortality findings in trials of rofecoxib for Alzheimer disease or cognitive impairment: a case study based on documents from rofecoxib litigation. JAMA. 2008;299:1813–1817. doi: 10.1001/jama.299.15.1813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Madigan D, Sigelman DW, Mayer JW, Furberg CD, Avorn J. Under-reporting of cardiovascular events in the rofecoxib Alzheimer studies. Am Heart J. 2012;164:186–193. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2012.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ross JS, Hill KP, Egilman DS, Krumholz HM. Guest authorship and ghostwriting in publications related to rofecoxib: a case study of industry documents from rofecoxib litigation. JAMA. 2008;299:1800–1812. doi: 10.1001/jama.299.15.1800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Steinman MA, Bero LA, Chren MM, Landefeld CS. Narrative review: the promotion of gabapentin: an analysis of internal industry documents. Ann Intern Med. 2006;145:284–293. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-145-4-200608150-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Dorsey ER, de Roulet J, Thompson JP, et al. Funding of US biomedical research 2003–2008. JAMA. 2010;303:137–143. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ridker PM, Danielson E, Fonseca FA, et al. Rosuvastatin to prevent vascular events in men and women with elevated C-reactive protein. N Engl J Med. 2008;359:2195–2207. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0807646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kritek P, Campion EW. JUPITER clinical directions — polling results. N Engl J Med. 2009;360(10):e14. doi: 10.1056/NEJMe086509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Eccles MP, Hrisos S, Francis J, et al. Do self-reported intentions predict clinicians’ behaviour: a systematic review. Implement Sci. 2006;1:28. doi: 10.1186/1748-5908-1-28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Asch DA, Jedrziewski MK, Christakis NA. Response rates to mail surveys published in medical journals. J Clin Epidemiol. 1997;50:1129–1136. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(97)00126-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Association of American Medical Colleges. 2008 Physician specialty data. 2008 Nov; ( https://www.aamc.org/download/47352/data/specialtydata.pdf). [Google Scholar]
- 48.Campbell EG, Rao SR, DesRoches CM, et al. Physician professionalism and changes in physician-industry relationships from 2004 to 2009. Arch Intern Med. 2010;170:1820–1826. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2010.383. [Erratum, Arch Intern Med 2010;170:1966.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Krumholz HM, Ross JS. A model for dissemination and independent analysis of industry data. JAMA. 2011;306:1593–1594. doi: 10.1001/jama.2011.1459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Robertson CT. The money blind: how to stop industry bias in biomedical science, without violating the First Amendment. Am J Law Med. 2011;37:358–387. doi: 10.1177/009885881103700207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Califf RM, Zarin DA, Kramer JM, Sherman RE, Aberle LH, Tasneem A. Characteristics of clinical trials registered in ClinicalTrials.gov, 2007–2010. JAMA. 2012;307:1838–1847. doi: 10.1001/jama.2012.3424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Zarin DA, Tse T, Williams RJ, Califf RM, Ide NC. The ClinicalTrials.gov results database — update and key issues. N Engl J Med. 2011;364:852–860. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa1012065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Clancy C, Collins FS. Patient-Centered Outcomes Research Institute: the intersection of science and health care. Sci Transl Med. 2010;2 doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3001235. 37cm18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Avorn J. Powerful medicines: the benefits, risks, and costs of prescription drugs. New York: Alfred A. Knopf; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Collins FS. Reengineering translational science: the time is right. Sci Transl Med. 2011;3 doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3002747. 90cm17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.