Are Reading and Behavior Problems Risk
Factors for Each Other?
Journal of Learning Disabilities
Volume 41 Number 5
September/October 2008 417-436
© 2008 Hammill Institute on
Disabilities
10.1177/0022219408321123
http://journaloflearningdisabilities
.sagepub.com
hosted at
http://online.sagepub.com
Paul L. Morgan
George Farkas
Paula A. Tufis
Rayne A. Sperling
The Pennsylvania State University, University Park
Two questions were investigated. First, are children with reading problems in first grade more likely to experience behavior problems in third grade? Second, are children with behavior problems in first grade more likely to experience reading
problems in third grade? The authors explored both questions by using multilevel logistic regression modeling to analyze
data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study–Kindergarten Class (ECLS-K). After statistically controlling for a wide
range of potential confounds, they found that children with reading problems in first grade were significantly more likely
to display poor task engagement, poor self-control, externalizing behavior problems, and internalizing behavior problems
in third grade. They also found that children displaying poor task engagement in first grade were more likely to experience
reading problems in third grade. Collectively, these findings suggest that the most effective types of interventions are likely
to be those that target problems with reading and task-focused behaviors simultaneously.
Keywords: reading problems; reading disabilities; behavior problems; emotional and behavioral disorders; at-risk students
A
cademic underachievement and problem behaviors
frequently co-occur (e.g., Hinshaw, 1992; Reid,
Gonzalez, Nordness, Trout, & Epstein, 2004; Rutter &
Yule, 1970; Trout, Nordness, Pierce, & Epstein, 2003).
The link with reading difficulties is particularly well
established (e.g., Arnold et al., 2005; Kauffman,
Cullinan, & Epstein, 1987; McGee, Williams, Share,
Anderson, & Silva, 1986). For example, Greenbaum et
al. (1996) found that the percentage of children with
emotional and behavioral disorders (EBD) reading
below grade level increased from 54% to 85% across the
study’s 7-year span. Nelson, Benner, Lane, and Smith
(2004) reported that 83% of their study’s sample of
children with EBD scored below the norm group on a
standardized measure of reading skill.
One of four causal models explains this co-occurrence
(for reviews, see Hinshaw, 1992; Spira & Fischel, 2005).
First, it may be that “common cause” variables (e.g.,
poor attention) lead to problems in both reading and
behavior. This model implies that the relation between
reading and behavior problems is spurious. Second, it
may be that reading problems result in behavior
problems. Reading difficulties might trigger frustration,
agitation, acting out, avoidance, and withdrawal from
learning tasks (e.g., Fleming, Harachi, Cortes, Abbott, &
Catalano, 2004; Kellam, Mayer, Rebok, & Hawkins,
1998; Lane, Beebe-Frankenberger, Lambros, & Pierson,
2001; Walker, Colvin, & Ramsey, 1995; Wehby, Falk,
Barton-Arwood, Lane, & Cooley, 2003). If so, then
instruction that improves a child’s reading skills should
help decrease his or her problem behaviors. This should
occur because the behaviors were being maintained by a
desire to escape an aversive task.
Third, it may be that behavior problems lead to reading problems. Off-task and disruptive behaviors might
decrease attending to instruction and activities, thereby
worsening a child’s school performance (Coie, 1996;
Ialongo et al., 1999; Kellam et al., 1991; Lane, 1999;
Rabiner, Coie, & the Conduct Problems Prevention
Research Group, 2000; Reid, 1993; Reid, Eddy, Fetrow,
Authors’ Note: Please address correspondence to Dr. Paul L.
Morgan, The Pennsylvania State University, Department of
Educational Psychology, School Psychology, and Special Education,
211 CEDAR Building, University Park, PA 16802; e-mail: paulmorgan
@psu.edu.
417
418 Journal of Learning Disabilities
& Stoolmiller, 1999; Walker et al., 1995). Consequently,
reducing those behaviors that are interfering with the
child’s learning should help improve his or her reading
ability.
Fourth, it may be that reading and behavior problems
cause each other. Both factors might be reciprocally
causative over time, leading to a negative feedback cycle
of increasing problem behaviors, school disengagement,
and academic failure (McGee et al., 1986). Such a cycle
would complicate intervention efforts. Depending on the
timing of the cycle’s feedback effects, effectively remediating poor reading ability may require interventions
that target both reading and behavioral deficits.
Theoretical Explanations of the
Bidirectional Model
Why might reading and behavior problems cause each
other? One possibility is that the negative feedback cycle
is set in motion by a child’s early reading failure. For
example, Stanovich (1986) hypothesizes that early reading failure, which itself results from cognitive deficits in
phonological processing, should initiate a “causal chain
of escalating negative side effects” (p. 364). Specifically,
“the combination of lack of practice, deficient decoding
skills, and difficult materials results in unrewarding early
reading experiences that lead to less involvement in reading-related activities” (p. 364). The reading disabled
child’s increasing feelings of frustration and anxiety,
learned helplessness, or difficulties in self-regulating emotions should, in turn, maintain his or her reading failure
(e.g., Aunola, Leskinen, Onatsu-Arvilommi, & Nurmi,
2002; Chapman, Tunmer, & Prochnow, 2000). This
occurs because the child begins to avoid both reading
activities in the classroom and reading practice in the
home (Stanovich, 1986). This avoidance of reading tasks
should constrain further growth in the child’s basic reading skills, comprehension strategies, and later, cognitive
capacities (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1991; Echols,
West, Stanovich, & Zehr, 1996; Griffiths & Snowling,
2002; Guthrie, Schafer, & Huang, 2001; Senechal,
LeFevre, Hudson, & Lawson, 1996). Prolonged reading
failure should lead to increasingly more generalized
deficits in cognition, motivation, and behavior. Early
reading failure should, therefore, contribute to a range of
later behavior problems.
Another possibility is that the cycle is initiated by the
young child’s lack of higher order skills in planning, initiation, and self-regulation of goal-directed behavior. The
lack of such skills may itself be the result of deficits in
executive functioning (Stevens, Kaplan, & Hesselbrock,
2003). Executive functions are “general-purpose control
mechanics that modulate the operation of various cognitive subprocesses and thereby regulate the dynamics of
human cognition” (Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki,
& Howerter, 2000, p. 50). These self-regulatory processes
include “planning, organizational skills, selective attention, and inhibitory control” (Morgan & Lilienfeld,
2000, p. 114). The disabled child’s limited ability to selfregulate his or her behavior should limit his or her capacity to manage the classroom’s learning environment.
Because of the child’s resulting frustration and anxiety
(Elliott & Mirsky, 2002), these behaviors should
contribute to heightened aggression or withdrawal
(Giancola, Mezzich, & Tarter, 1998; Riggs, Greenberg,
Kusche, & Pentz, 2006). The child’s skills growth in
reading should be uneven, at best, because time spent
engaging in this second set of problem behaviors should
further interfere with his or her task engagement
(Johnson, McGue, & Iacono, 2005). Consequently,
behavior problems related to planning, organization, and
task persistence, as well as subsequent behaviors like
acting out and withdrawal, should contribute to later
reading problems.
Which of the four causal models explains the cooccurrence between reading and behavior problems? The
answer to this question is important in order to better
understand the etiology of learning disabilities (LD). Most
children with LD are poor readers (e.g., Snow, Burns, &
Griffin, 1998). Many children with LD also display task
avoidant behaviors (e.g., Beitchman & Young, 1997; Fulk,
Brigham, & Lohman, 1998). Indeed, the potential interaction between poor reading ability and such behaviors is
viewed as a primary reason that children with LD often
underachieve academically (e.g., McDermott, Goldberg,
Watkins, Stanley, & Glutting, 2006; Stanovich, 1986;
Torgesen, 1982; Torgesen et al., 1999).
The answer to this question also has important practical implications. Schools face a difficult choice when
deciding which type of deficit to remediate. Significant
deficits in either reading (e.g., Adams, 1990; Snow et al.,
1998) or behavior (e.g., Schaeffer, Petras, Ialongo,
Poduska, & Kellam, 2003; Sprague & Walker, 2000)
place children at great risk for negative long-term outcomes (e.g., delinquency, dropout, poverty, unemployment, incarceration). Deficits in either reading (e.g.,
Torgesen et al., 2001; Torgesen et al., 1999) or behavior
(e.g., Kazdin, 1987; Walker et al., 1995) also quickly
become resistant to intervention. Thus, schools have a
narrow window of opportunity to effectively provide
either type of intervention. Yet, schools typically have
only limited resources available to deliver special services. Which deficit, then, should they target? If, for
Morgan et al. / Reading and Behavior Problems
example, reading problems cause behavior problems,
then school staff could devote more of their scarce
resources toward reading skills interventions and still
expect to see improvements in children’s behaviors. In
contrast, if reading and behavior problems cause each
other, then interventions targeting only reading problems,
without simultaneously attending to a child’s behavior
problems, may ultimately prove ineffective.
Methodological Approaches to Evaluating
the Causal Nature of the Co-Occurrence
To date, two different types of methodologies have
been used to determine the causal nature of co-occurring
reading and behavior problems. First, some researchers
have used experimental or quasi-experimental designs.
Most of these researchers have attempted to remediate
children’s reading deficits to determine whether there is
a corresponding decrease in the occurrence of problem
behavior. Results from these intervention studies are
mixed (Rivera, Al-Otaiba, & Koorland, 2006). Whereas
some researchers report declines in problem behavior
after improving a child’s reading skill (e.g., Allyon &
Roberts, 1974; Coie & Krehbiel, 1984; Kellam et al.,
1998), others do not (e.g., Barton-Arwood, Wehby, &
Falk, 2005; Lane, 1999; Nelson, Stage, Epstein, &
Pierce, 2005; Wehby et al., 2003). The intervention studies are rare (Levy & Chard, 2001). Coleman and Vaughn
(2000) identified only three published reading intervention
studies using samples of young children with EBD. Rivera
et al.’s (2006) more recent review identified only six such
studies. Few studies have attempted to boost children’s
reading skills by improving their social skills (Allyon &
Roberts, 1974; Kellam et al., 1998; Wooster, 1986;
Wooster & Carson, 1982). The intervention studies are also
methodologically limited (Lane, 2004). The limited and
mixed experimental and quasi-experimental literature
makes any causal inferences tenuous (Hinshaw, 1992).
A second group of studies has used “causal modeling”
statistical techniques. These studies have sought to statistically control for confounding variables (e.g., socioeconomic status [SES]) while testing one of the
aforementioned models (for methodological discussions,
see Aneshensel, 2002; Kenny, 1979; Shadish, Cook, &
Campbell, 2002). If the tested model is indeed “true”
(i.e., accurately specified and estimated without undue
measurement error), then findings from a modeling
study can help provide an estimate of fit, or the strength
of the interrelations between a set of hypothesized causal
factors (Shadish et al., 2002). When the outcome is a disease, disorder, or condition, as is the case here, modeling
419
studies provide an estimate of risk, or the likelihood of
experiencing the outcome (e.g., being diagnosed with cancer) given another condition (e.g., being a smoker). If a
relation remains between one condition and another after
accounting for multiple confounding factors, then the relation is more likely to be causal (see Thun, Apicella, &
Henley, 2000, for an epidemiological example).
Unlike the intervention studies, the modeling studies
have appeared fairly frequently. For instance, Hinshaw
(1992) summarized findings from 17 such studies.
Collectively, this literature suggests a bidirectional
causal model between reading and behavior problems. In
some studies, reading problems lead to behavior problems (e.g., Bennett, Brown, Boyle, Racine, & Offord,
2003; Carroll, Maughan, Goodman, & Meltzer, 2005). In
other studies, behavior problems lead to reading problems
(e.g., Jorm, Share, Matthews, & Maclean, 1986; Spira,
Bracken, & Fischel, 2005). Studies directly testing for an
early interplay between reading and behavior also find
support for a bidirectional model (e.g., Lepola, Poskiparta,
Laakkonen, & Niemi, 2005; Onatsu-Arvilommi &
Nurmi, 2000).
Methodological and Substantive
Limitations of the Modeling Literature
However, methodological limitations have been noted
in much of the modeling literature. Hinshaw (1992)
points out that many of these studies fail to control for
either earlier reading or behavior problems as a predictor. Some of the modeling studies do not include likely
common cause variables in the analyses (Fleming et al.,
2004). This is problematic because at least one such variable (i.e., poor attention) has already been identified as
accounting for most of the association between academic
underachievement and behavior problems (Maguin &
Loeber, 1996). Factors such as gender, race/ethnicity, and
social class also act as consistent predictors of differences
in reading and behavior (e.g., Feil et al., 2005; Kaplan &
Walpole, 2005; Landgren, Kjellman, & Gillberg, 2003;
Lepola, 2004; Riordan, 2002; Sanchez, Bledsoe,
Sumabat, & Ye, 2004) and, thus, should be statistically
controlled as confounds. Relatively few studies are longitudinal. Collectively, the modeling studies’ aforementioned limitations “severely constrain inferences regarding
causal precedence” (Hinshaw, 1992, p. 146).
There is also an important substantive limitation of the
extant literature. Investigations of the relation between
reading and behavior problems focus primarily on externalizing (e.g., being disruptive, oppositional-defiant, or
aggressive) problem behavior (Fleming et al., 2004;
420 Journal of Learning Disabilities
Hinshaw, 1992). Yet, such behavior problems are just
one type of problem behavior that teachers confront. For
example, teachers work with students who are frequently
(a) task avoidant or socially dependent (i.e., exhibiting
poor task engagement), (b) socially avoidant, depressed,
phobic, or anxious (i.e., exhibiting internalizing problem
behaviors), or (c) argumentative or inappropriate with
their peers (i.e., exhibiting poor self-control or interpersonal skills). By concentrating on whether reading problems cause or are caused by externalizing problem
behaviors, researchers may have inadvertently ignored
whether reading problems also affect or are affected by
other types of problem behaviors.
Purposes of This Study
This study had two purposes. First, we tested whether,
after controlling for earlier problem behaviors and other
antecedent variables, children’s reading problems predict
their later behavior problems. Here, we investigated a set
of interrelated questions. Does being a poor reader in
first grade increase the odds that a child will display
problem behavior in third grade? Does this relation hold
after controlling for whether the children were already
displaying these problem behaviors in first grade? Does
this relation continue to hold after controlling for a large
number of antecedent variables, including poor attention,
family- and school-level poverty, gender, and race? We
tested for this relation by examining how reading difficulties affect each of five types of problem behaviors:
poor task engagement, poor self-control, poor interpersonal skills, internalizing problem behaviors, and externalizing problem behaviors.
Second, we investigated whether, after controlling for
both prior reading problems and a range of antecedent variables, early manifestations of problem behavior predict
later relative reading failure. Again, we asked a set of interrelated questions. Does displaying any of the five types of
problem behaviors increase the odds that a child will be a
poor reader in third grade? Does this relation hold after
controlling for whether the child was already a poor reader
in first grade? Does this relation continue to hold after controlling for a large number of potential confounds?
We sought to determine the strength of the interrelations between reading and behavior problems. Thus, we
investigated the aforementioned questions using multilevel logistic regression modeling. This analytical
approach, which quantifies whether and to what degree
one condition acts as a risk factor for another condition,
is commonly used in epidemiological studies of diseases
and disorders (e.g., Buzi et al., 2003; Feil et al., 2005;
Gould, Herrchen, Pham, Bera, & Brindis, 1998;
Marshall et al., 1999). Here, we dichotomized children
as having or not having reading or behavior problems.
Analyses based on dichotomization are sometimes criticized as coarse (e.g., Kachigan, 1991). However, in this
study, we were not seeking to estimate the strength of the
relationship between reading and behavior per se. This is
because it is problems in reading or behavior—whether
defined as performing worse than 90% of one’s peers or
by some other criterion—that are hypothesized to act as
risk factors for the aforementioned negative outcomes
(e.g., Stanovich, 1986). When the goal is information
that can be used to more effectively target experimental
and quasi-experimental intervention work, as is the case
here, it is important to focus analyses on the interplay
between known risk factors, especially those that may be
amenable to treatment (e.g., Bennett et al., 2003; Catts,
Fey, Zhang, & Tomblin, 2001; Maxwell, Bastani, &
Warda, 2000; Nash & Bowen, 2002).
Our analyses estimate the strength of relations
between reading and behavior problems in a hypothesized bidirectional causal model. The methodological
richness of the study’s database (e.g., large sample size,
multiple time points, measures of many different possible
confounds, individually administered reading achievement tests, measures of different categories of problem
behavior) is the type called for when testing such a
model (Hinshaw, 1992). The nature of our statistical
analyses meets the conditions necessary for making preliminary (but not conclusive) inferences about causality
(Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003; Hinshaw, 1992;
Kenny, 1979; Shadish et al., 2002). Collectively, the size
and quality of the database, combined with the wide
range of covariates accounted for in our statistical analyses, should provide researchers, policy makers, and practitioners with a relatively accurate estimate of the nature
and strength of the hypothesized model’s interrelations
and, in so doing, help guide subsequent experimental and
quasi-experimental intervention efforts that can then be
used to confirm causal inferences (e.g., Cohen et al.,
2003; Hinshaw, 1992; National Center for Education
Statistics, 2004).
Method
Study’s Database
The study’s data set was the Early Childhood
Longitudinal Study–Kindergarten Class (ECLS-K).
The ECLS-K is maintained by the U.S. Department of
Education’s National Center for Education Statistics
(NCES). The ECLS-K is the first large-scale nationally
Morgan et al. / Reading and Behavior Problems
representative sample of children as they age through the
elementary school years. The sample was selected to be
representative of all students in kindergarten in fall 1998.
Children were recruited from both public and private
kindergartens offering full- or half-day classes. The
sample was constructed to support separate analyses of
kindergarteners in public and private schools, as well as
Black, Hispanic, White, and Asian children and those of
varied SES. The NCES used sample freshening to help
make the ECLS-K representative of all first graders in
fall 1999. Data from the sampled children were collected
at the beginning and end of kindergarten, in the fall and
spring of first grade (with a random subsample in the
fall), and again in the spring of third grade. Data continue to be collected as the children advance further
through the grade levels.
Study’s Analytical Sample
Table 1 displays descriptive statistics for the study’s
analytical sample. This sample included 11,515 students
attending 1,471 public and private elementary schools.
The sample was 50% male. Age averaged 65.6 months
when data were first collected in fall of kindergarten. The
remaining variables in Table 1 are the key independent
and dependent variables for the study’s multilevel logistic
regression modeling analyses. Table 2 displays descriptive statistics using scale scores for the groups of
children classified as at risk for reading or behavioral
disabilities in the spring of first or third grade.
Measures
The Reading Test. The questions on the Reading Test
seek to assess basic skills (e.g., print familiarity, letter
recognition, beginning and ending sounds, rhyming
words, phonemic awareness, decoding, sight word
recognition), vocabulary (receptive vocabulary), and
comprehension (i.e., demonstrating an understanding of
the text, making interpretations, using personal background knowledge, and taking a critical stance). The
measure was administered individually. Most of the
Reading Test’s items used a multiple-choice format; a
small number were open-ended questions or called for a
constructed response. The Reading Test’s content
emphasis changes over time to reflect children’s growth
as readers. For first graders, 40%, 10%, and 50% of the
measure’s testing time is devoted to assessing basic
skills, vocabulary, and comprehension, respectively. For
third graders, these percentages change to 15%, 10%,
and 75%, respectively.
The Reading Test was created through a multistage
panel review. Some items were borrowed or adapted from
421
published tests (e.g., the Peabody Picture Vocabulary
Test–Revised, the Woodcock Johnson Tests of Achievement–
Revised). The Educational Testing Service, elementary
school curriculum specialists, and practicing teachers
supplied other items. All items were field tested. Items
were included in the Reading Test’s final form if they
displayed (a) acceptable item-level statistics, (b) good fit
with maximum likelihood item response theory (IRT)
parameters, and (c) no differential item functioning
across gender or race (NCES, 2004). The ECLS-K uses
a routing procedure (i.e., a child is given a different battery of test items depending on the accuracy of his or her
initial responses) and IRT methods to derive scale scores
that are comparable across grade levels. The NCES considers reliabilities of the Reading Test’s IRT theta scores
(i.e., estimates of a child’s ability) to be the most appropriate internal consistency estimate. These reliabilities
were .96 and .94 for the end of first and third grade, respectively (NCES, 2004). First graders’ Reading Test scores
correlated .85 or above with the Kaufman Test of
Educational Achievement reading test (NCES, 2002); third
graders’ scores correlated .83 with the Woodcock-McGrewWerder Mini-Battery of Achievement (NCES, 2005a).
Teacher Social Rating Scale. The ECLS-K uses
an adapted version of the Social Skills Rating System
(SSRS; Gresham & Elliott, 1990) to measure children’s
behavior. The original psychometric data of the SSRS
were based on 4,170 K-12 students (Gresham & Elliott,
1990). Of these, 83% and 17% attended general education and special education classes, respectively. The
test–retest correlation over 4 weeks was .85 for the
teacher ratings (Gresham & Elliott, 1990). Both correlational and factor analyses support the measures’ construct validity (Feng & Cartledge, 1996; Furlong &
Karno, 1995).
The NCES adapted the SSRS for use with the ECLSK sample. These changes included (a) the addition of
items measuring the child’s frequency of positive affect,
behavior, and approaches to learning, (b) expanding the
response format from a 3-point to a 4-point scale and
including a “not observed” response, and (c) rewording
some items to reduce cultural bias (e.g., changing
“Responds appropriately when pushed or hit by other
children” to “Firmly tells an aggressive peer to stop hurtful acts,” e.g., “Stop hitting,” “No pushing”). Meisels,
Atkins-Burnett, and Nicholson (1996) provide additional
details of the adaptations to the SSRS.
The ECLS-K’s Teacher Social Rating Scale includes
five subscales: (a) Approaches to Learning, (b) SelfControl, (c) Interpersonal Skills, (d) Externalizing
422 Journal of Learning Disabilities
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics of the Study’s Analytical Sample
Variable
Gender—male (T1)
Age at K entry in months (T1)
Mother’s educational level (T4)
Less than high school
High school diploma or equivalent
Some college (including vocational/technical training)
Bachelor’s degree or higher
Father’s educational level (T4)
Less than high school
High school diploma or equivalent
Some college (including vocational/technical training)
Bachelor’s degree or higher
Family living below federal poverty level (T4)
Family received AFDC (T4)
Family received food stamps (past 12 months) (T4)
Received WIC (T1)
During both pregnancy and childhood
During either pregnancy or childhood
During neither pregnancy nor childhood
Head Start participation (T1)
Race (T1)
Black non-Hispanic
Hispanic
Asian
Other
White non-Hispanic
Household structure (T4)
Single-parent family
Other structures
Two parents, both biological
Number of siblings in household (T4)
Primary home language—not English (T4)
Current mom teenager at first birth—younger than 19 (T1)
Census region (T4)
Northeast
Midwest
South
West
Urbanicity (T4)
Large and mid-size cities
Large and mid-size suburb and large town
Small town and rural
More than 25% Black students in school—Level 2 (T4)
More than 25% Hispanic students in school—Level 2 (T4)
% eligible for free lunch—Level 2 (T4)
Reading in bottom 10% (IRT scale) (T4)
Reading in bottom 10% (IRT scale) (T5)
Approaches to learning—bottom 10% (T4)
Approaches to learning—bottom 10% (T5)
Self-control—bottom 10% (T4)
Self-control—bottom 10% (T5)
Interpersonal skills—bottom 10% (T4)
Interpersonal skills—bottom 10% (T5)
Externalizing behavior problems—upper 10% (T4)
Externalizing behavior problems—upper 10% (T5)
Internalizing behavior problems—upper 10% (T4)
Internalizing behavior problems—upper 10% (T5)
SD
Min
Max
0.50
65.59
0.50
4.27
0.00
51.90
1.00
80.32
0.12
0.29
0.34
0.25
0.32
0.45
0.47
0.43
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
0.13
0.32
0.29
0.26
0.17
0.04
0.12
0.34
0.47
0.45
0.44
0.38
0.21
0.32
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
0.35
0.07
0.58
0.15
0.48
0.25
0.49
0.35
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
0.12
0.17
0.07
0.05
0.59
0.32
0.38
0.25
0.23
0.49
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
0.18
0.12
0.70
1.54
0.14
0.21
0.39
0.32
0.46
1.19
0.35
0.41
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
10.40
1.00
1.00
0.19
0.26
0.32
0.23
0.39
0.44
0.47
0.42
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
0.38
0.39
0.23
0.17
0.15
31.18
0.09
0.09
0.10
0.09
0.08
0.11
0.07
0.08
0.09
0.09
0.08
0.09
0.48
0.49
0.42
0.35
0.33
22.46
0.28
0.29
0.30
0.29
0.26
0.31
0.26
0.27
0.29
0.28
0.26
0.29
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
95.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.00
M
Note: Level 1 n = 11,515. Level 2 n = 1,471. T1 = fall of kindergarten; T4 = spring of first grade; T5 = spring of third grade; AFDC = Aid to Families with
Dependent Children; WIC = Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children; IRT = item response theory.
Table 2
Descriptive Statistics of the Groups of Children Classified as At Risk or Not At Risk for Reading or Behavioral Disabilities
Bottom 10% (T4)
Reading score
Approaches to learning
Self-control
Interpersonal skills
Upper 90% (T4)
Upper 90% (T5)
Min
Max
M
Min
Max
M
Min
Max
M
Min
Max
M
16.54
1.00
1.00
1.00
41.61
2.00
2.25
2.20
34.74
1.78
1.94
1.88
41.62
2.00
2.25
2.20
141.36
4.00
4.00
4.00
71.60
3.19
3.28
3.20
42.36
1.00
1.00
1.00
79.38
2.12
2.25
2.20
67.57
1.82
2.04
1.87
79.39
2.12
2.25
2.20
148.95
4.00
4.00
4.00
111.87
3.15
3.31
3.17
Bottom 90% (T4)
Externalizing problems
Internalizing problems
Bottom 10% (T5)
Upper 10% (T4)
Bottom 90% (T5)
Upper 10% (T5)
Min
Max
M
Min
Max
M
Min
Max
M
Min
1.00
1.00
2.60
2.25
1.51
1.50
2.60
2.25
4.00
4.00
3.02
2.75
1.00
1.00
2.50
2.26
1.59
1.53
2.50
2.26
Note: T1 = fall of kindergarten; T4 = spring of first grade; T5 = spring of third grade.
Max
4.00
4.00
M
3.01
2.74
423
424 Journal of Learning Disabilities
Problem Behaviors, and (e) Internalizing Problem
Behaviors (NCES, 2004). Teachers use a frequency scale
to rate how often the child displays a particular social
skill or behavior (i.e., 1 = never to 4 = very often). The
NCES included two additional items on the third-grade
version of the scale (i.e., one additional item on the
Approaches to Learning scale, and one additional item
on the Externalizing Problem Behaviors scale). The
Approaches to Learning scale’s six to seven items measure behaviors that affect how well a child benefits from
the classroom environment (e.g., displays attentiveness,
task persistence, eagerness to learn, learning independence, easily adapts to changes in routine, and organization). The Self-Control scale’s four items rate a child’s
ability to control his or her behavior (i.e., respecting the
property rights of others, controlling his or her temper,
accepting a peer’s ideas for group activities, responding
appropriately to peer pressure). The Interpersonal Skills
scale includes five items that measure the child’s ability
to initiate and maintain friendships (i.e., get along with
people who are different; comfort or help peers; express
his or her feelings, ideas, and opinions appropriately; and
show sensitivity to the feelings of others). The five to six
items of the Externalizing Problem Behaviors scale measure acting out behaviors (e.g., arguing, fighting, showing anger, acting impulsively, disturbing the classroom’s
ongoing activities). The four items on the Internalizing
Problem Behavior scale ask whether the child appears
anxious, lonely, or sad or has low self-esteem.
The NCES (2005a) reports that the split-half reliabilities for the five scales for first-grade and third-grade
children were, respectively, .89 and .91 (Approaches
to Learning), .80 and .79 (Self-Control), .89 and
.89 (Interpersonal Skills), .86 and .89 (Externalizing
Problem Behaviors), and .77 and .76 (Internalizing
Problem Behaviors). Exploratory and confirmatory
factor analyses confirmed the full scale’s structure
(NCES, 2005a). Correlations among the Approaches to
Learning, Self-Control, Interpersonal Skills, and
Externalizing Problem Behaviors scales ranged from
.59 to .81 for third graders. Correlations with the
Internalizing Problem Behaviors scale and the other
scales ranged between .32 and .41.
Controlled confounds. We included many antecedent
variables that might act as potential confounds of the
relationship. These belonged to one of two blocks of
confounds. The first was a block of variables indexing
family resources (i.e., mother’s and father’s education
level; whether the family’s income was below the federal
poverty level; whether the family participated in federal
assistance programs, such as Aid to Families with
Dependent Children or Head Start; the percentage of the
school’s students eligible for free lunch). The second was
a block of variables indexing demographic differences
(i.e., the child’s race and ethnicity, the child’s gender,
whether the language spoken at home was English,
whether the racial composition of the child’s school was
more than 25% Black or Hispanic, the child’s household
structure and number of siblings, the mother’s age at first
birth, the child’s age at kindergarten entry, whether the
child’s school was located in an urban or rural location).
In addition, we included both the “autoregressor” (e.g.,
whether the child was already displaying either poor
reading ability or abnormal levels of the specific behavior
type in first grade) and a control for task-focused attention (i.e., scoring in the bottom 10% of the Approaches to
Learning or, when predicting Approaches, the bottom
10% of the Self-Control scale in first grade). The former
is a strong confound (e.g., Badian, 2001; Torgesen,
Wagner, Rashotte, Burgess, & Hecht, 1997); the latter is
considered a common cause variable to the relation
(Fleming et al., 2004; Hinshaw, 1992).
Data Collection Procedures
Children completed the Reading Test during oneto-one, untimed sessions with a trained assessor. The
ECLS-K assessor was trained to administer the Reading
Test through a multistage process including 5 days of
interactive lectures, scripted role-plays, interactive exercises, and self-administered exercises (see NCES,
2005b, for additional training details). To be certified to
administer the Reading Test, the adult assessor had to be
able to (a) accurately score responses on the test (e.g.,
read questions verbatim) and (b) display appropriate testadministrating behaviors (e.g., build rapport, use neutral
praise, avoid coaching) while working with an actual
child respondent. The NCES reports that 98.2% of the
third-grade assessors scored an 85% or above on its
Reading Test certification test. The 1.8% of assessors
scoring below 85% completed remedial training.
Teachers completed the self-administered Teacher
Social Rating Scale each time children were assessed.
Teachers were mailed a copy of the scale, which
included a cover sheet and a summary of the study and
its goals. Teachers were monetarily compensated for
their time. ECLS-K field staff called and visited teachers
to assist them and to prompt return of the completed
assessments (NCES, 2005b).
Analytical Strategy
Because our theoretical focus was on whether students
fall into the “problem” group in reading or behavior, we
Morgan et al. / Reading and Behavior Problems
dichotomized these variables at the 10% cutoff at the
“worst” end of their distribution in first and third grades
(i.e., bottom 10% on the Reading Test, Approaches to
Learning, Self-Control, and Interpersonal Skills scales,
or the top 10% on the Internalizing Behavior Problems
or Externalizing Behavior Problems scales for the full
sample) as displaying abnormal levels of that specific
behavior. Those children with scores in the remaining
90% of the full sample for each scale were considered
not to be displaying abnormally poor levels of that skill
or behavior. The 10% cutoff was applied separately for
all interviewed first graders and all interviewed third
graders. We based this 10% cutoff on previous empirical
work on the prevalence of clinically significant reading
(Catts et al., 2001; Konold, Juel, & McKinnon, 1999)
and behavior (Feil et al., 2005; Roberts, Attkisson, &
Rosenblatt, 1998) problems.
We used multilevel logistic regression modeling to
determine whether reading and behavior problems acted
as risk factors for each other. Logistic regression is a frequently used analytical tool to identify risk factors for diseases, disorders, or conditions (Ely, Dawson, Mehr, &
Burns, 1996) such as severe reading or behavioral difficulties (e.g., Bennett et al., 2003; Carroll et al., 2005; Catts
et al., 2001). Logistic regression produces odds ratios as
an estimate of effect size. An odds ratio is the odds [i.e.,
(the probability of an event)/(1 – the probability of an
event)] of experiencing an event for Group A relative to
that of Group B (Case, Kimmick, Paskett, Lohman, &
Tucker, 2002). We used Hierarchical Linear Modeling
(HLM) to perform regressions that statistically adjusted
for the spatially clustered nature of the sample design (i.e.,
students within schools) and the wide variety of potentially confounding variables (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002).
Missing Data
Missing data are common in longitudinal data sets.
This is particularly likely to occur in data sets that
attempt to administer a wide range of measures across
several years to a large and varied (e.g., children, parents,
teachers, administrators) sample. Here, missing data
accounted for between 0% and 23% of the child-level
predictors and between 23% and 38% of the school-level
predictors. Analysts of longitudinal data sets sometimes
restrict their attention to sample members for whom
complete data are available (e.g., Buhs, Ladd, & Herald,
2006; Kaplan & Walpole, 2005). In contrast, we
responded to the ECLS-K’s amount of missing data by
using multiple imputation procedures (Allison, 2002;
R. Little & Rubin, 1987; Rubin, 1987; Schafer, 1997).
Use of multiple imputation results in parameter estimates
425
and standard errors that take into account uncertainty
due to missing data (Sinharay, Stern, & Russell, 2001).
Recent examples of the utility of this method in the
behavioral and health sciences include Taylor et al.
(2002), Barzi and Woodward (2004), and Moskowitz,
Laraque, Doucette, and Shelov (2005).
We used the IVEware software (Raghunathan,
Solenberger, & Van Hoewyk, 2002) to impute missing
values, resulting in five imputed data sets. The software
uses a sequence of multiple regressions to compute predicted values for each individual and then, for each predicted value, adds an error term, which is selected as a
random draw from the residual distribution for that variable. The type of regression model used varies according
to the type of variable that is imputed. The procedure
uses a normal regression model to impute missing values
for both ordinal (e.g., urbanicity) and continuous (e.g.,
age at kindergarten entry, mother’s age at first birth, percentage eligible for free lunch) variables, a logistic or
generalized logistic model for missing categorical variables (e.g., living below the poverty level, primary home
language, census region), and a Poisson regression
model to estimate missing count variable (e.g., number
of siblings; see Raghunathan et al., 2002).
The sequential regression multivariate imputation
(SRMI) method is based on the assumptions that the
sample is a simple random sample and the missing data
mechanisms are ignorable (Raghunathan et al., 2002).
The SRMI method does not account for the nesting of
students within schools. Therefore, we imputed both
individual-level and school-level variables at the individual level and then used school means to construct the
school-level variables. We also assumed that the data
were missing at random (MAR; Allison, 2002).
Specifically, we considered it a plausible assumption
that, given the presence of all the included control and
auxiliary variables, the probability of missing data on a
variable was unrelated to the value of that variable.
Collins, Schafer, and Kam (2001) recommend the inclusion of auxiliary variables that might be correlates of
missingness in the imputation process to achieve a situation closer to MAR. At the same time, their simulations
suggest that, in the typical case, an erroneous assumption
of MAR will have little effect on estimates and standard
errors in the substantive model.
We used an imputation model that included all of the
variables present in the HLM analyses and several other
auxiliary variables (reading and behavior scores during
fall and spring of kindergarten) that were correlated with
variables in the substantive model. The inclusion of auxiliary variables in the imputation model is designed to
provide controls for additional missing data mechanisms
426 Journal of Learning Disabilities
(Acock, 2005; Collins et al., 2001) to improve prediction
of missing values in each variable, in particular in our
endogenous variables, and has been shown to improve
the reliability and efficiency of estimates (Allison, 2002;
Raghunathan et al., 2002). We chose to include our
endogenous variables in the imputation model because
this produces unbiased estimates of regression coefficients. We also used the imputed endogenous variables
in the substantive analyses because cases with missing
data on endogenous variables also have missing data on
some of the independent variables (Allison, 2002).
The hierarchical linear model assumes that students
are nested within the same schools at each of the two
time points. Thus, we restricted attention to students who
did not change schools between these survey rounds (see
Note 1). The multiple imputation technique will produce
unbiased coefficient estimates on this sample as long as,
conditional on control variables in the regressions, data
are MAR. Our data likely approximate this situation
because we have used an unusually extensive and
detailed set of control variables and allowed a number of
them (e.g., mother’s and father’s education) to have nonlinear effects by entering them as dummy variables for
multiple categories. We used the HLM software to (a) conduct a separate HLM analysis for each of the five complete (with imputed values) data sets, (b) average
parameter estimates across the five resulting complete
data sets, and (c) compute the standard errors of these estimates (Raudenbush, Bryk, Cheong, & Congdon, 2004).
Results
Are Reading Problems a Risk Factor
for Behavior Problems?
The first independent variable of interest for this
study’s regressions was the dummy variable for reading
problems in the spring of first grade. Table 3 displays the
results of these regressions. (Coefficients and standard
errors in this table result from appropriately averaging
the estimates for each of the five data sets containing
imputed values.) Regressions predicting the different
dependent variables are shown in the columns of this
table. The first five are the different behavior problem
variables; the final column is for reading problems. The
key independent variables are reading problems (the second row of the table) and different types of behavior
problems (the third through seventh rows). The remaining
rows of the table show the effects of the control variables.
Table 3’s first column displays the results for
approaches to learning (measured by the teacher’s judgments of the student’s attentiveness, task persistence,
eagerness to learn, learning independence, flexibility,
and organization). This variable is the ECLS-K’s best
measure of learning-related or task-focused behaviors,
and it is also the variable that, after controlling prior test
performance, best predicts future test performance (Tach
& Farkas, 2006). Here, we found strong support for the
hypothesized model’s causal relation. Statistically controlling for (a) poor task engagement in spring of first
grade, (b) self-control problem behaviors (as a measure
of attention-related problems), and (c) a wide range of
SES- and demographic-related control variables, we
found that being a poor reader elevated a child’s odds by
2.17 (p < .001) of displaying poor task engagement in the
spring of third grade.
Table 3’s second column displays results of these
analyses for self-control behavior problems. Once again,
the analyses yielded preliminary evidence for the hypothesized causal relation. Controlling for a wide range of
confounds, including both poor task engagement (as a
control for attention-related problem behaviors) and poor
self-control in spring of first grade, the odds that a poor
reader displayed poor self-control in the spring of third
grade were 1.33 times higher (p < .05) than the odds for
an average-to-good reader. The effect was smaller than
that for predicting the approaches-indexed behavior problems, but still statistically significant.
Table 3’s next column presents the analysis for interpersonal behavior problems in spring of third grade.
Here and subsequently, to retain comparability across
these analyses, we included two first-grade behavior
problems variables as controls—the variable used as the
dependent variable (in this case, interpersonal behavior
problems) and the approaches to learning variable
(again, as a control for attention problems). In this case,
our results do not support a hypothesized relation
between reading and behavior problems; net of the control variables, the estimated effect does not achieve statistical significance. Hence, being a poor reader in first
grade was not a risk factor for displaying poor interpersonal skills in the third grade.
Table 3’s fourth column displays the analyses for externalizing behavior problems. Here, the odds ratio for the
effect of reading problems in first grade was a statistically
significant 1.39 (p < .05), supporting a relation between
reading and externalizing behavior problems. A similar
result is found in the fifth column for the prediction of internalizing behavior problems. Net of statistical controls, the
odds that a child displayed internalizing problem behaviors
in the spring of third grade were 1.66 times higher (p <
.001) for poor readers than for average-to-good readers.
Overall, these regressions provide consistent evidence
that being a poor reader in first grade increases a child’s
(text continues on page 430)
Table 3
Multilevel Logistic Regression Analyses of Behavior and Reading Problems in Spring of Third Grade
T5 Approaches
Problems
Intercept
T4 reading problems
T4 approaches problems
T4 self-control problems
T5 Self-Control
Problems
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
p
–3.937
(0.211)
0.774
(0.113)
1.201
(0.100)
0.583
(0.135)
0.019
***
2.168
***
3.322
***
1.791
***
T5 Interpersonal
Problems
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
p
–3.128
(0.155)
0.288
(0.115)
0.406
(0.162)
1.337
(0.154)
0.044
***
1.334
*
1.501
*
3.809
***
T4 interpersonal problems
Log
Odds
–3.534
(0.197)
0.178
(0.141)
0.552
(0.110)
1.203
(0.116)
Odds
Ratio
0.029
T5 Externalizing
Problems
p
***
1.195
1.736
3.332
***
T5 Internalizing
Problems
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
p
–3.851
(0.176)
0.331
(0.127)
0.304
(0.115)
0.021
***
1.392
*
1.356
**
1.873
(0.091)
6.509
More than 25% Hispanic students
% eligible for free lunch
Gender (male)
Age at K entry
Mother’s education
Less than high school
High school diploma
Some college
Father’s education
Less than high school
High school diploma
Some college
0.462
(0.184)
0.453
(0.148)
0.349
(0.127)
0.269
(0.178)
0.202
(0.151)
0.123
(0.135)
0.703
**
1.012
1.001
2.274
***
0.979
*
1.588
*
1.573
**
1.418
**
1.309
1.224
1.131
–0.211
(0.138)
–0.122
(0.145)
0.003
(0.002)
0.490
(0.073)
–0.007
(0.010)
0.204
(0.150)
0.120
(0.119)
0.191
(0.114)
0.014
(0.148)
–0.021
(0.120)
0.035
(0.113)
0.810
0.885
1.003
1.633
0.993
1.226
1.128
1.210
1.014
0.979
1.036
***
–0.381
(0.153)
–0.019
(0.137)
0.004
(0.002)
0.506
(0.076)
–0.014
(0.013)
0.317
(0.198)
0.146
(0.128)
0.133
(0.130)
–0.046
(0.210)
0.091
(0.151)
0.101
(0.129)
0.683
*
0.982
1.004
1.659
0.986
1.372
1.157
1.142
0.955
1.095
1.106
Odds
Ratio
p
–2.819
(0.154)
0.508
(0.117)
0.660
(0.115)
0.060
***
1.662
***
1.935
***
***
T4 internalizing problems
–0.352
(0.132)
0.012
(0.163)
0.001
(0.002)
0.822
(0.080)
–0.021
(0.010)
Log
Odds
***
T4 externalizing problems
More than 25% Black students
T5 Reading
Problems
***
–0.189
(0.146)
0.046
(0.178)
–0.001
(0.002)
0.614
(0.085)
–0.017
(0.011)
0.186
(0.172)
0.128
(0.137)
0.210
(0.122)
0.248
(0.218)
0.093
(0.150)
0.109
(0.127)
0.828
1.047
0.999
1.848
0.983
1.204
1.137
1.234
1.282
1.097
1.115
***
0.930
(0.118)
0.056
(0.132)
0.061
(0.159)
0.001
(0.002)
0.074
(0.073)
0.001
(0.010)
2.534
0.196
(0.210)
0.159
(0.140)
0.115
(0.139)
1.216
0.034
(0.199)
0.063
(0.144)
0.089
(0.117)
1.057
1.063
1.001
1.077
1.001
1.172
1.122
1.035
1.065
1.093
***
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
p
–5.424
(0.229)
2.485
(0.101)
1.123
(0.125)
–0.102
(0.278)
–0.179
(0.173)
0.066
(0.159)
0.274
(0.141)
0.015
(0.131)
0.132
(0.154)
0.008
(0.002)
0.282
(0.086)
–0.012
(0.010)
0.004
***
12.005
***
3.074
***
0.898
(0.227)
0.651
(0.199)
0.587
(0.186)
0.634
(0.220)
0.460
(0.208)
0.362
(0.191)
0.903
0.836
1.069
1.315
1.015
1.142
1.008
***
1.325
***
0.988
2.455
***
1.918
**
1.798
**
1.886
**
1.584
*
1.436
(continued)
427
428
Table 3 (continued)
T5 Approaches
Problems
Family below poverty level
Federal programs
Family received AFDC
Family received food stamps
WIC during pregnancy and
childhood
WIC during pregnancy
or childhood
Head Start participation
Race
Black non-Hispanic
Hispanic
Asian
Other
Household structure
Single-parent family
Other structures
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
–0.008
(0.126)
0.209
(0.214)
0.180
(0.131)
0.216
(0.109)
0.098
(0.150)
0.109
(0.108)
0.381
(0.137)
–0.202
(0.164)
–0.771
(0.335)
–0.128
(0.167)
0.317
(0.118)
0.465
(0.142)
T5 Self-Control
Problems
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
0.992
0.033
(0.112)
1.233
0.210
(0.147)
0.123
(0.124)
0.224
(0.095)
0.222
(0.143)
0.020
(0.098)
p
1.197
1.241
1.103
1.115
1.464
**
0.817
0.463
*
0.880
1.373
*
1.593
**
0.560
(0.132)
–0.103
(0.124)
–0.371
(0.202)
–0.046
(0.152)
0.245
(0.092)
0.463
(0.105)
T5 Interpersonal
Problems
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
1.033
0.139
(0.114)
1.234
0.108
(0.249)
0.217
(0.131)
0.344
(0.110)
0.253
(0.155)
–0.175
(0.119)
p
1.131
1.250
*
1.248
1.020
1.751
***
0.902
0.690
0.955
1.277
**
1.589
***
0.560
(0.150)
0.010
(0.140)
–0.463
(0.259)
0.188
(0.205)
0.255
(0.110)
0.443
(0.116)
T5 Externalizing
Problems
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
1.149
0.032
(0.138)
1.114
0.080
(0.213)
0.041
(0.170)
0.207
(0.112)
0.187
(0.167)
0.013
(0.116)
p
1.242
1.411
**
1.288
0.839
1.751
***
1.010
0.630
1.206
1.290
*
1.558
***
0.819
(0.121)
–0.059
(0.159)
–0.604
(0.272)
0.000
(0.191)
0.329
(0.126)
0.521
(0.117)
T5 Internalizing
Problems
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
1.033
0.179
(0.146)
1.083
–0.061
(0.228)
0.146
(0.133)
0.285
(0.104)
0.187
(0.160)
0.033
(0.116)
p
1.041
1.230
1.206
1.013
2.267
***
0.943
0.546
*
1.000
1.390
*
1.684
***
–0.149
(0.136)
–0.144
(0.145)
–0.720
(0.309)
0.028
(0.185)
0.270
(0.112)
0.361
(0.146)
T5 Reading
Problems
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
p
1.196
0.283
(0.120)
1.327
*
0.941
0.248
(0.164)
0.197
(0.140)
0.336
(0.119)
0.172
(0.183)
–0.011
(0.116)
1.282
p
1.157
1.330
**
1.206
1.034
0.862
0.866
0.487
*
1.029
1.310
*
1.435
*
0.489
(0.135)
0.279
(0.146)
–0.447
(0.260)
0.327
(0.177)
0.037
(0.133)
–0.051
(0.123)
1.218
1.399
**
1.188
0.989
1.630
***
1.322
0.639
1.386
1.038
0.951
(continued)
Table 3 (continued)
T5 Approaches
Problems
Number of siblings
Home language not English
Mother’s age at first birth
Region
Midwest
South
West
Urbanicity
Large and mid-size cities
Small town and rural
Level 2 variance
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
–0.021
(0.037)
–0.115
(0.168)
0.174
(0.089)
0.979
–0.003
(0.148)
–0.022
(0.128)
0.074
(0.127)
0.997
–0.004
(0.095)
–0.094
(0.110)
T5 Self-Control
Problems
p
0.891
1.190
0.978
1.077
0.996
0.242
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
p
–0.067
(0.031)
–0.111
(0.151)
0.171
(0.085)
0.935
*
0.165
(0.123)
0.003
(0.120)
0.038
(0.148)
1.179
–0.084
(0.095)
0.016
(0.100)
0.910
*
T5 Interpersonal
Problems
0.895
1.187
1.003
1.038
0.919
1.016
0.296
*
T5 Externalizing
Problems
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
p
–0.091
(0.042)
–0.190
(0.223)
0.068
(0.104)
0.913
*
0.209
(0.148)
–0.033
(0.147)
–0.004
(0.140)
1.232
–0.064
(0.109)
0.003
(0.115)
0.827
1.070
0.968
0.996
0.938
1.003
0.322
Log
Odds
Odds
Ratio
–0.040
(0.042)
–0.215
(0.185)
0.204
(0.127)
0.961
0.173
(0.128)
0.029
(0.134)
0.203
(0.163)
1.189
0.020
(0.100)
0.124
(0.109)
T5 Internalizing
Problems
p
0.806
1.227
1.030
1.225
1.020
Odds
Ratio
–0.053
(0.031)
–0.102
(0.184)
–0.052
(0.115)
0.949
–0.109
(0.140)
–0.177
(0.128)
0.010
(0.136)
0.897
–0.053
(0.116)
–0.085
(0.116)
1.133
0.202
Log
Odds
***
T5 Reading
Problems
p
0.903
0.950
0.838
1.010
0.949
Odds
Ratio
p
0.136
(0.035)
0.377
(0.152)
0.191
(0.095)
1.145
***
1.458
*
1.210
*
–0.171
(0.140)
–0.056
(0.131)
0.033
(0.149)
0.843
0.058
(0.111)
0.343
(0.118)
0.919
0.282
Log
Odds
**
0.945
1.033
1.059
1.409
**
0.293
Note: Level 1 n = 11,515. Level 2 n = 1,471. Standard errors are in parentheses. Random intercept, Bernoulli. T4 = spring of first grade; T5 = spring of third grade; AFDC = Aid to Families with Dependent
Children; WIC = Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
429
430 Journal of Learning Disabilities
likelihood of displaying behavior problems in third grade.
Statistically significant effects are found for the prediction of four of five types of behavior problems. The
largest effect is for problems in approaches to learning, a
measure of task-focused and learning-related behaviors.
Are Behavior Problems a Risk Factor for
Reading Problems?
Table 3’s sixth column displays the results of regressions predicting reading problems in the spring of third
grade. The independent variables of interest for these
regressions were the dummy variables for problem
behaviors in the spring of first grade. Despite the autoregressor’s strength (an extraordinarily high odds ratio of
12.01; p < .001), one type of behavior problem remained
a statistically significant predictor. The odds of being
a poor reader in the spring of third grade were 3.07
(p < .001) higher if a child was displaying low levels
of task-focused behaviors in the spring of first grade.
Again, this was the strength of association after statistically controlling for both the autoregressor and a wide
range of SES- and demographic-related variables. It is
clear that task-related behavior problems in first grade
strongly predicted reading problems in third grade.
Discussion
We first tested whether being a poor reader by the
spring of first grade increased the likelihood that a child
would engage in problem behaviors by the spring of
third grade. We estimated the predictive strength of this
relation after controlling for prior problem behavior,
attention, and other potential confounds (e.g., parent
education, family structure, poverty, gender, and race).
By taking into account a large set of potential confounds
using a nationally representative data set, we attempted
to better isolate the hypothesized strength of the relation
between reading and behavior problems. We were especially interested in whether the strength of relation
varied by behavior problem type.
We found that reading problems indeed elevated
a child’s odds of engaging in problem behaviors.
Specifically, we found that, after statistically controlling
for prior problem behavior, poor attention, and both
SES- and demographic-related confounds, poor reading
ability in first grade consistently acted as a statistically and
clinically significant predictor of problem behavior in
third grade. The odds of displaying poor task engagement,
poor self-control, externalizing problem behaviors, or
internalizing problem behaviors in third grade were 2.17,
1.33, 1.39, and 1.66 times higher, respectively, for poor
readers than for average-to-good readers. Collectively,
these odds ratios indicate a modest-to-moderate relation
between reading problems and certain types of problem
behaviors (Haddock, Rindskopf, & Shadish, 1998).
Whereas the predictive strength of poor reading varied by
behavior type, it almost always remained a highly statistically (despite the substantial number of confounds entered
into the analyses) and practically (given the negative outcomes associated with displaying problem behaviors) significant predictor. The only exception to this pattern was
the relation with interpersonal problem behavior. In this
case, although the odds ratio was above 1, it failed to
achieve statistical significance.
We then tested whether displaying abnormal levels of
one of five types of behavior in first grade increased the
likelihood that a child would be a poor reader in third
grade. We estimated the predictive strength of this relation after controlling for prior reading problems and
both SES- and demographic-related confounds. Here,
we found that abnormal levels of only one of the five
types of behavior (i.e., poor task engagement) elevated
a child’s odds of being a poor reader in third grade. This
odds ratio was a high 3.07. We also found that poor
readers in first grade were almost always poor readers in
third grade. These odds were an extraordinary high
12.01 to 1.
Taken together, these two sets of analyses provide
preliminary support for a bidirectional causal model
between reading and behavior problems. Our analyses
support four pathways of risk: (a) Early reading problems strongly predict later reading problems; (b) early
behavior problems strongly predict later behavior
problems; (c) early reading problems modestly-tomoderately predict a general set of behavior problems;
and (d) one type of early behavior problem (i.e., the
type most directly related to self-regulation of learning) strongly predicts later reading problems. We
believe it to be unlikely that these relations are spurious because of the large number of confounds controlled for in the analyses.
Contributions to the Extant Literature
Our findings extend work from previous investigations. To date, few experimental studies in this research
area have been conducted. Moreover, methodological
limitations have typified much of the causal modeling
work on the link between reading and behavior problems
(Hinshaw, 1992; Spira & Fischel, 2005). Specifically, few
of the modeling studies have (a) controlled for prior problem behavior as a predictor, (b) included common cause
variables such as attention problems or socioeconomic
Morgan et al. / Reading and Behavior Problems
background, (c) used longitudinal data sets, or (d) tested
alternative causal models. In contrast, our study’s analyses statistically controlled for prior problem behavior,
prior reading problems, attention problems, and many
demographic- and SES-related variables while also testing a cross-lagged model of over-time effects. These
analyses were based on a large data set. If the hypothesized bidirectional model is indeed “true” (i.e., accurately
specified and estimated without undue measurement
error; Shadish et al., 2002), then our findings should
accurately describe the nature and strength of the interrelations between reading and behavior problems in the
primary grades. However, systematic randomized experiments are needed to confirm that this bidirectional
model can indeed be characterized as causal.
Our results also seem to support elements of
Stanovich’s (1986) Matthew effects model. We found
that, as predicted, early reading failure negatively
affected children’s later behavior. The effect seemed to
generalize. That is, not only were poor readers in first
grade more likely to be task avoidant in third grade, they
were also more likely to act out, withdraw from classroom activities, and display poor self-control. That the
effect was strongest for task engagement is consistent
with the Matthew effects model in that this behavior is
the one most closely related to the classroom’s learning
demands. Only the behavior that was, arguably, least
related to these demands (i.e., interpersonal skills, or the
ability to make and keep friends) was relatively resistant
to early but severe reading difficulties.
Our results offer mixed support for the hypothesis that
poor reading ability results from a young child’s behavior problems. Neither poor self-control nor poor interpersonal skills nor externalizing problem behaviors nor
internalizing problem behaviors in first grade predicted
reading problems in third grade. Instead, we found that
only poor task engagement in first grade strongly predicted poor reading in third grade. Thus, it seems that
only those behaviors that might be considered proximally related to deficits in executive functioning acted as
a risk factor for reading failure. This finding is consistent
with McDermott et al.’s (2006) results, in which frequently engaging in learning-related behaviors acted to
decrease children’s risk for each of the measured types of
LD (i.e., reading, spelling, and mathematics disabilities).
In contrast, aggression, defiance, and other types of
problem behaviors had inconsistent effects on children’s
risk for LD identification.
Our results, therefore, might be characterized as consistent with a restricted executive function model, in
which poor self-regulation of behavior constrains the
child’s ability to meet the classroom’s learning demands.
431
However, a full model, in which this second set of problem
behaviors also undermined children’s reading growth,
was not supported. Indirect evidence for this distinction
is reported in two other recent studies. Johnson et al.
(2005) found that most of the association between
children’s disruptive behavior and school grades could
be accounted for by poor attention and ability. Clark,
Prior, and Kinsella (2002) found that children with both
attention and externalizing behavior problems scored
lower on a reading test than typical peers, but not those
displaying only externalizing behavior problems.
The Study’s Limitations
Several limitations characterize this study. First, our
analyses are based on a limited number of time points
(see Raudenbush, 2001) as well as on a sample of
children who did not change schools between these time
points. Second, our analyses are based in part on multiply imputed values replacing varying amounts of missing data. Third, we did not manipulate a hypothesized
causal agent (e.g., pronounced difficulty learning to
read), which is the “gold standard” in demonstrating a
causal relation (e.g., Shadish et al., 2002; Tabachnick &
Fidel, 1989). Instead, we attempted to test the degree to
which reading and behavior problems interrelated in a
hypothesized bidirectional causal model. Because we did
so after taking into account a large set of potential confounds, we characterize our results as providing relatively accurate estimates of these interrelations in the
hypothesized model (i.e., that poor reading causes poor
behavior and that poor behavior causes poor reading).
There is a persuasive methodological rationale for considering these estimates as accurate (e.g., Aneshensel,
2002; Kenny, 1979) if the model is indeed accurately
specified. It is also important to note that our study’s
odds ratios estimate a child’s risk of experiencing the
negative outcome and, thus, are inherently probabilistic
rather than deterministic (D. Little, 1991). As with any
type of causal modeling analysis, it is also possible that
we did not include an important confound into the analyses (whether hitherto identified or not) that could explain
the effects we attribute to reading or behavior problems.
Implications for Further Research and Practice
Our study has both theoretical and practical implications. Theoretically, the findings highlight the need for
continued investigations into the links between academic
performance problems and behavior problems. A diverse
set of questions remains to be explored. For example,
why are reading problems more likely to lead to internalizing problem behaviors than to interpersonal problem
432 Journal of Learning Disabilities
behaviors? Are the negative effects on behavior unique to
whether a child does poorly in reading, or does poor performance in other academic subjects (e.g., math) also
lead to behavior problems? How do these relationships
evolve as children move to higher grade levels?
Practically, our findings suggest the need for a multifaceted approach toward preventing the later occurrence
of either reading or behavior problems. It is interesting
that this agrees with findings from at least two intervention studies, which indicate that resistance to intervention is associated with executive functioning-related
behaviors such as inattention and poor inhibitory control
(Riggs et al., 2006; Torgesen et al., 1999). Here, children
appeared more likely to become poor readers in third
grade if they left first grade as poor readers or as taskavoidant learners. Children also appeared more likely
to become task-avoidant learners in the third grade if
they left first grade as task-avoidant or as poor readers.
Collectively, then, others’ and our findings (e.g., Rabiner,
Malone, & the Conduct Problems Prevention Research
Group, 2004; Spira et al., 2005) point to the need to
ensure that children in the primary grades move on to
subsequent grades with the requisite reading skills and
self-regulatory, task-focused behaviors. The most effective types of interventions are likely to be those that target both reading and behavior problems simultaneously.
Note
1. The Early Childhood Longitudinal Study–Kindergarten Class
(ECLS-K) includes 13,964 children who were interviewed during the
spring of first and third grades. Of these, 2,449 children (17.54%)
changed schools some time between the two time periods. Those
children who did and who did not change schools displayed different
sociodemographic characteristics. For example, and compared with
children who did not change schools, children who changed schools
were more likely to (a) come from poor, less educated, and singleparent families and (b) attend schools where a higher percentage of
children lived in poverty. Children who changed schools also displayed somewhat higher means on all of our study’s dependent variables, suggesting that these children were more likely to display
reading and behavior problems. However, although most of the aforementioned mean or percentage differences were statistically significant (due in part to the relatively large number of children in each
sample), the magnitudes of these differences were not typically substantial. For instance, the differences between those who changed and
those who did not change schools on our study’s dependent variables
ranged between 1.93 and 4.22 percentage points.
References
Acock, A. (2005). Working with missing values. Journal of Marriage
and Family, 67, 1012-1028.
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about
print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Allison, P. D. (2002). Missing data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Allyon, T., & Roberts, M. D. (1974). Eliminating discipline problems
by strengthening academic performance. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 7, 71-76.
Aneshensel, C. S. (2002). Theory-based data analysis for the social
sciences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Pine Forge Press.
Arnold, E. M., Goldston, D. B., Walsh, A. K., Reboussin, B. A.,
Daniel, S. S., Hickman, E., et al. (2005). Severity of emotional and
behavioral problems among poor and typical readers. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 205-217.
Aunola, K., Leskinen, E., Onatsu-Arvilommi, T., & Nurmi, J. (2002).
Three methods for studying developmental change: A case of
reading skills and self-concept. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 72, 343-364.
Badian, N. A. (2001). Phonological and orthographic processing:
Their roles in reading prediction. Annals of Dyslexia, 51, 179-202.
Barton-Arwood, S. M., Wehby, J. H., & Falk, K. B. (2005). Reading
instruction for elementary-age students with emotional and behavioral disorders: Academic and behavioral outcomes. Exceptional
Children, 72, 7-27.
Barzi, F., & Woodward, M. (2004). Imputations of missing values
in practice: Results from imputations of serum cholesterol in 28
cohort studies. American Journal of Epidemiology, 160, 34-45.
Beitchman, J. H., & Young, A. R. (1997). Learning disorders with a
special emphasis on reading disorders: A review of the past 10
years. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 36, 1020-1032.
Bennett, K. J., Brown, K. S., Boyle, M., Racine, Y., & Offord, D.
(2003). Does low reading achievement at school entry cause conduct problems? Social Science and Medicine, 56, 2443-2448.
Buhs, E. S., Ladd, G. W., & Herald, S. L. (2006). Peer exclusion and
victimization: Processes that mediate the relation between peer
group rejection and children’s classroom engagement and
achievement? Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 1-13.
Buzi, R. S., Tortolero, S. R., Roberts, R. E., Ross, M. W., Addy, R. C.,
& Markham, C. M. (2003). The impact of a history of sexual
abuse on high-risk sexual behaviors among females attending
alternative schools. Adolescence, 38, 595-605.
Carroll, J. M., Maughan, B., Goodman, R., & Meltzer, H. (2005).
Literacy difficulties and psychiatric disorders: Evidence for comorbidity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 524-532.
Case, L. D., Kimmick, G., Paskett, E. D., Lohman, K., & Tucker, R.
(2002). Interpreting measures of treatment effect in cancer clinical
trials. Oncologist, 7, 181-187.
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (2001).
Estimating the risk of future reading difficulties in kindergarten
children: A research-based model and its clinical implementation.
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 38-50.
Chapman, J. W., Tunmer, W. E., & Prochnow, J. E. (2000). Early
reading-related skills and performance, reading self-concept, and
the development of academic self-concept: A longitudinal study.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 703-708.
Clark, C., Prior, M., & Kinsella, G. (2002). The relationship between
executive function abilities, adaptive behaviour, and academic
achievement in children with externalising behaviour problems.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 785-796.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences
(3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Coie, J. D. (1996). Prevention of violence and antisocial behavior. In
R. Peters & R. McMahon (Eds.), Preventing childhood disorders,
Morgan et al. / Reading and Behavior Problems
substance abuse, and delinquency (pp. 1-18). Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Coie, J. D., & Krehbiel, G. (1984). Effects of academic tutoring on
the social status of low-achieving, socially rejected children. Child
Development, 55, 1465-1478.
Coleman, M., & Vaughn, S. (2000). Reading interventions for
students with emotional/behavioral disorders. Behavioral
Disorders, 25, 93-104.
Collins, L. M., Schafer, J. L., & Kam, C. M. (2001). A comparison of
inclusive and restrictive strategies in modern missing data procedures. Psychological Methods, 6, 330-351.
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Tracking the unique
effects of print exposure in children: Associations with vocabulary, general knowledge, and spelling. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 83, 264-274.
Echols, L. D., West, R. W., Stanovich, K. E., & Zehr, K. S. (1996).
Using children’s literacy activities to predict growth in verbal cognitive skills: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 88, 296-304.
Elliott, A. K., & Mirsky, A. F. (2002). Cognitive antecedents of violence and aggression. In J. Glicksohn (Ed.), The neurobiology of
criminal behavior (pp. 111-136). Boston: Kluwer Academic.
Ely, J. W., Dawson, J. D., Mehr, D. R., & Burns, T. L. (1996).
Understanding logistic regression through example. Family
Medicine, 28, 134-140.
Feil, E. G., Small, J. W., Forness, S. R., Serna, L. A., Kaiser, A.,
Hancock, T. B., et al. (2005). Using different measures, informants, and clinical cut-off points to estimate prevalence of emotional or behavioral disorders in preschoolers: Effects on age,
gender, and ethnicity. Behavioral Disorders, 30, 375-391.
Feng, H., & Cartledge, G. (1996). Social skill assessment of inner city
Asian, African, and European American students. School
Psychology Review, 25, 228-239.
Fleming, C. B., Harachi, T. W., Cortes, R. C., Abbott, R. D., &
Catalano, R. F. (2004). Level and change in reading scores and
attention problems during elementary school as predictors of
problem behavior in middle school. Journal of Emotional and
Behavioral Disorders, 12, 130-144.
Fulk, B. M., Brigham, F. J., & Lohman, D. A. (1998). Motivation
and self-regulation: A comparison of students with learning and
behavior problems. Remedial and Special Education, 19,
300-309.
Furlong, M. J., & Karno, M. (1995). Review of the Social Skills
Rating System. In J. C. Conoley & J. C. Impara (Eds.), Twelfth
mental measurements yearbook (pp. 697-969). Lincoln, NE:
Buros Institute of Mental Measurement.
Giancola, P. R., Mezzich, A. C., & Tarter, R. E. (1998). Executive
cognitive functioning, temperament, and antisocial behavior in
conduct-disordered adolescent females. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 107, 629-641.
Gould, J. B., Herrchen, B., Pham, T., Bera, S., & Brindis, C. (1998).
Small-area analysis: Targeting high-risk areas for adolescent pregnancy prevention programs. Family Planning Perspectives, 30,
173-176.
Greenbaum, P. E., Dedrick, R. F., Freidman, R. M., Kutash, K.,
Brown, E. C., Lardieri, S. P., et al. (1996). National Adolescent
and Child Treatment Study (NACTS): Outcomes for children with
serious emotional and behavioral disturbance. Journal of
Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 4, 130-146.
Gresham, P. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social Skills Rating System.
Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
433
Griffiths, Y. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2002). Predictors of exception
word and nonword reading in dyslexic children: The severity
hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 34-43.
Guthrie, J. T., Schafer, W. D., & Huang, C. (2001). Benefits of opportunity to read and balanced instruction on the NAEP. Journal of
Educational Research, 94, 145-162.
Haddock, C. K., Rindskopf, D., & Shadish, W. R. (1998). Using odds
ratios as effect sizes for meta-analysis of dichotomous data: A
primer on methods and issues. Psychological Methods, 3, 339-353.
Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Externalizing behavior problems and academic
underachievement in childhood and adolescence: Causal relationships and underlying mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 111,
127-155.
Ialongo, N. S., Werthamer, L., Kellam, S. G., Brown, C. H., Wang, S.,
& Lin, Y. (1999). Proximal impact of two first-grade preventive
interventions on the early risk behaviors for later substance abuse,
depression, and antisocial behavior. American Journal of
Community Psychology, 27, 599-641.
Johnson, W., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2005). Disruptive behavior and school grades: Genetic and environmental relations in
11-year-olds. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 391-405.
Jorm, A. F., Share, D. L., Matthews, R., & Maclean, R. (1986).
Behaviour problems in specific reading retarded and general reading backward children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 33-43.
Kachigan, S. K. (1991). Multivariate statistical analysis: A conceptual introduction (2nd ed.). New York: Radius Press.
Kaplan, D., & Walpole, S. (2005). A stage-sequential model of reading transitions: Evidence from the Early Childhood Longitudinal
Study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 551-563.
Kauffman, J. M., Cullinan, D., & Epstein, M. H. (1987).
Characteristics of students placed in special programs for the seriously emotionally disturbed. Behavioral Disorders, 12, 175-184.
Kazdin, A. E. (1987). Treatment of antisocial behavior in children:
Current status and future directions. Psychological Bulletin, 102,
187-203.
Kellam, S. G., Mayer, L. S., Rebok, G. W., & Hawkins, W. E. (1998).
Effects of improving achievement on aggressive behavior and of
improving aggressive behavior on achievement through two preventative interventions. An investigation of causal paths. In
B. P. Dohrenwend (Ed.), Adversity, stress, and psychopathology
(pp. 486-505). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Kellam, S. G., Werthamer-Larsson, L., Dolan, L. J., Brown, C.C.H.,
Mayer, L. S., Rebok, G. W., et al. (1991). Developmental epidemiologically based preventive trials: Baseline modeling of early
target behaviors and depressive symptoms. American Journal of
Community Psychology, 19, 563-584.
Kenny, D. (1979). Correlation and causality. New York: John Wiley.
Konold, T. R., Juel, C., & McKinnon, M. (1999). Building an integrated model of early reading acquisition (Tech. Rep. No. 11003). Retrieved March 22, 2006, from http://www.ciera.org/
library/reports/inquiry-1/1-003/1-003.html
Landgren, M., Kjellman, B., & Gillberg, C. (2003). A school for all
kinds of minds: The impact of neuropsychiatric disorders, gender
and ethnicity on school-related tasks administered to 9-10 year old
children. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 12, 162-171.
Lane, K. L. (1999). Young students at risk for antisocial behavior: The
utility of academic and social skills interventions. Journal of
Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 7, 211-223.
Lane, K. L. (2004). Academic instruction and tutoring interventions
for students with emotional and behavioral disorders: 1990 to the
434 Journal of Learning Disabilities
present. In R. B. Rutherford, M. M. Quinn, & S. R. Mathur (Eds.),
Handbook of research on emotional and behavioral disorders
(pp. 462-486). New York: Guilford.
Lane, K. L., Beebe-Frankenberger, M., Lambros, K. L., & Pierson,
M. E. (2001). Designing effective interventions for children atrisk for antisocial behavior: An integrated model of components
necessary for making valid inferences. Psychology in the Schools,
38, 365-379.
Lepola, J. (2004). The role of gender and reading competence in the
development of motivational orientations from kindergarten to
grade 1. Early Education and Development, 15, 215-240.
Lepola, J., Poskiparta, E., Laakkonen, E., & Niemi, P. (2005).
Development of and relationship between phonological and motivational processes and naming speed in predicting word recognition in grade 1. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 367-399.
Levy, S., & Chard, D. J. (2001). Research on reading instruction for
students with emotional and behavioral disorders. International
Journal of Disability, Development, and Education, 48, 429-444.
Little, D. (1991). Varieties of social explanation: An introduction to
the philosophy of social science. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Little, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1987). Statistical analysis with missing
data. New York: John Wiley.
Maguin, E., & Loeber, R. (1996). Academic performance and delinquency. Crime and Justice: A Review of Research, 20, 145-264.
Marshall, J. A., Lopez, T. K., Shetterly, S. M., Morgenstern, N. E.,
Baer, K., Swenson, C., et al. (1999). Indicators of nutritional risk
in a rural elderly Hispanic and non-Hispanic White population:
San Luis Valley Health and Aging Study. Journal of the American
Dietetic Association, 99, 315-322.
Maxwell, A. E., Bastani, R., & Warda, U. S. (2000). Demographic
predictors of cancer screening among Filipino and Korean immigrants in the United States. American Journal of Preventative
Medicine, 18, 62-68.
McDermott, P. A., Goldberg, M. M., Watkins, M. W., Stanley, J. L.,
& Glutting, J. J. (2006). A nationwide epidemiologic modeling
study of LD: Risk, protection, and unintended impact. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 39, 230-251.
McGee, R., Williams, S., Share, D. L., Anderson, J., & Silva, P. A.
(1986). The relationship between specific reading retardation,
general reading backwardness and behavioral problems in a large
sample of Dunedin boys: A longitudinal study from five to eleven
years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 597-610.
Meisels, S. J., Atkins-Burnett, S., & Nicholson, J. (1996). Assessment
of social competence, adaptive behaviors, and approaches to
learning with young children. Washington, DC: U.S. Department
of Education, Office of Educational Research and Improvement.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., &
Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A
latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100.
Morgan, A. B., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2000). A meta-analytic review of
the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological
measures of executive function. Clinical Psychology Review, 20,
113-136.
Moskowitz, H., Laraque, D., Doucette, J., & Shelov, E. (2005).
Relationships of U.S. youth homicide victims and their offenders,
1976-1999. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 159,
356-361.
Nash, J. K., & Bowen, G. L. (2002). Defining and estimating risk and
protection: An illustration from the School Success Profile. Child
and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 19, 247-261.
National Center for Education Statistics. (2002). Early Childhood
Longitudinal Study–Kindergarten class of 1998–1999 (ECLS-K):
Psychometric report for kindergarten through first grade (NCES
2002-05). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Office
of Educational Research and Improvement.
National Center for Education Statistics. (2004). User’s manual for
the ECLS-K third grade public-use data file and electronic code
book (NCES 2004-001). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of
Education, Institute for Education Sciences.
National Center for Education Statistics. (2005a). Early Childhood
Longitudinal Study–Kindergarten class of 1998-1999 (ECLS-K):
Psychometric report for the third grade (NCES 2005-062).
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Institute of
Education Sciences.
National Center for Education Statistics. (2005b). Early Childhood
Longitudinal Study–Kindergarten class of 1998-1999 (ECLS-K):
Third grade methodology report (NCES 2005-018). Washington,
DC: U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences.
Nelson, J. R., Benner, G. J., Lane, K., & Smith, B. W. (2004).
Academic achievement of K-12 students with emotional and
behavioral disorders. Exceptional Children, 71, 59-73.
Nelson, J. R., Stage, S. A., Epstein, M. H., & Pierce, C. D. (2005).
Effects of a prereading intervention on the literacy and social
skills of children. Exceptional Children, 72, 29-45.
Onatsu-Arvilommi, T., & Nurmi, J. E. (2000). The role of taskavoidant and task-focused behaviors in the development of reading and mathematical skills during the first school year: A
cross-lagged longitudinal study. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 92, 478-491.
Rabiner, D., Coie, J. D., & the Conduct Problems Prevention Research
Group. (2000). Early attention problems and children’s reading
achievement: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 859-867.
Rabiner, D. L., Malone, P. S., & the Conduct Problems Prevention
Research Group. (2004). The impact of tutoring on early reading
achievement for children with and without attention problems.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 273-284.
Raghunathan, T. E., Solenberger, P. W., & Van Hoewyk, J. (2002).
IVEware: Imputation and variance estimation software: User
guide. Retrieved July 12, 2005, from ftp://ftp.isr.umich.edu/pub/
src/smp/ive/ive_user.pdf
Raudenbush, S. W. (2001). Comparing personal trajectories and
drawing causal inferences from longitudinal data. Annual Review
of Psychology, 52, 501-525.
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., Cheong, Y. F., & Congdon, R.
(2004). HLM6: Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling.
Chicago: Scientific Software International.
Reid, J. (1993). Prevention of conduct disorder before and after
school entry: Relating interventions to developmental findings.
Development and Psychopathology, 5, 243-262.
Reid, J. B., Eddy, J. M., Fetrow, R. A., & Stoolmiller, M. (1999).
Description and immediate impacts of a preventive intervention for
conduct problems. American Journal of Community Psychology,
27, 483-517.
Reid, R., Gonzalez, J. E., Nordness, P. D., Trout, A., & Epstein, M.
H. (2004). A meta-analysis of the academic status of students with
emotional/behavioral disturbance. Journal of Special Education,
38, 130-143.
Morgan et al. / Reading and Behavior Problems
Riggs, N. R., Greenberg, M. T., Kusche, C. A., & Pentz, M. A. (2006).
The mediational role of neurocognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social-emotional prevention program in elementary
school students: Effects of the PATHS curriculum. Prevention
Science, 7, 91-102.
Riordan, C. (2002, April). Male/female early achievement and development: Gender gaps among first time kindergarten children in
the public schools. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA.
Rivera, M. O., Al-Otaiba, S., & Koorland, M. A. (2006). Reading
instruction for students with emotional and behavioral disorders
and at risk for antisocial behaviors in primary grades: Review of
the literature. Behavioral Disorders, 31, 323-337.
Roberts, R. E., Attkisson, C. C., & Rosenblatt, A. (1998). Prevalence
of psychopathology among children and adolescents. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 715-725.
Rubin, D. B. (1987). Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys.
New York: John Wiley.
Rutter, M., & Yule, W. (1970). Reading retardation and antisocial behaviour—The nature of the association. In M. Rutter, J. Tizard, & K.
Whitmore (Eds.), Education, health and behaviour (pp. 240-255).
London: Longmans.
Sanchez, K. S., Bledsoe, L. M., Sumabat, C., & Ye, R. (2004).
Hispanic students’ reading situations and problems. Journal of
Hispanic Higher Education, 3, 50-63.
Schaeffer, C. M., Petras, H., Ialongo, N., Poduska, J., & Kellam, S.
(2003). Modeling growth in boys’ aggressive behavior across elementary school: Links to later criminal involvement, conduct disorder, and antisocial personality disorder. Developmental Psychology,
39, 1020-1035.
Schafer, J. (1997). Analysis of incomplete multivariate data. London:
Chapman & Hall.
Senechal, M., LeFevre, J., Hudson, E., & Lawson, E. P. (1996).
Knowledge of storybooks as a predictor of young children’s
vocabulary. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 520-536.
Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental
and quasi-experimental design for generalized causal inference.
Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
Sinharay, S., Stern, H. S., & Russell, D. (2001). The use of multiple
imputation for the analysis of missing data. Psychological
Methods, 6, 317-329.
Snow, C. E., Burns, S. M., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing
reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National
Academy Press.
Spira, E. G., Bracken, S. S., & Fischel, J. E. (2005). Predicting improvement after first grade reading difficulties: The effects of early language, emergent literacy, and behavior skills. Developmental
Psychology, 41, 225-234.
Spira, E. G., & Fischel, J. E. (2005). The impact of preschool attention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity on social and academic development: A review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
46, 755-773.
Sprague, J. R., & Walker, H. (2000). Early identification and
intervention for youth with antisocial and violent behavior.
Exceptional Children, 66, 367-379.
Stanovich, K. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy.
Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360-407.
Stevens, M. C., Kaplan, R. F., & Hesselbrock, V. M. (2003).
Executive-cognitive functioning in the development of antisocial
personality disorder. Addictive Behaviors, 28, 285-300.
435
Tabachnick, B. A., & Fidel, L. S. (1989). Using multivariate statistics
(2nd ed.). New York: Harper & Row.
Tach, L. M., & Farkas, G. (2006). Learning-related behaviors, cognitive skills, and ability grouping when schooling begins. Social
Science Research, 35, 1048-1079.
Taylor, J., Cooper, K. L., Wei, J. T., Sarma, A. V., Raghunathan, T. E., &
Heeringa, S. G. (2002). Use of multiple imputation to correct for
nonresponse bias in a survey of urologic symptoms among AfricanAmerican men. American Journal of Epidemiology, 156, 774-782.
Thun, M. J., Apicella, L. F., & Henley, S. J. (2000). Smoking vs. other
risk factors as the cause of smoking-attributable deaths:
Confounding in the courtroom. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 284, 706-712.
Torgesen, J. K. (1982). The learning disabled child as an inactive
learner: Educational implications. Topics in Learning and
Learning Disabilities, 2, 45-52.
Torgesen, J. K., Alexander, A. W., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A.,
Voeller, K. K., Conway, T., et al. (2001). Intensive remedial
instruction for children with severe reading disabilities:
Immediate and long-term outcomes from two instructional
approaches. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 33-58.
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C., Burgess, S. R., & Hecht,
S. A. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid
automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills
in second- to fifth-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading,
1, 161-185.
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Rose, E., Lindamood,
P., Conway, T., et al. (1999). Preventing reading failure in young
children with phonological processing disabilities: Group and
individual response to instruction. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 91, 579-593.
Trout, A. L., Nordness, P. D., Pierce, C. D., & Epstein, M. H. (2003).
Research on the academic status of children with emotional and
behavioral disorders: A review of the literature from 1961 to 2000.
Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 11, 198-210.
Walker, H. M., Colvin, G., & Ramsey, E. (1995). Antisocial behavior
in school: Strategies and best practices. Pacific Grove, CA:
Brooks/Cole.
Wehby, J. H., Falk, K. F., Barton-Arwood, S., Lane, K. L., & Cooley,
C. (2003). The impact of comprehensive reading instruction on
academic and social behavior of students with emotional and
behavioral disorders. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral
Disorders, 11, 225-238.
Wooster, A. (1986). Social skills training and reading gain.
Educational Research, 28, 68-71.
Wooster, A., & Carson, A. (1982). Improving reading and self-concept
through communication and social skills training. British Journal
of Guidance and Counseling, 10, 83-87.
Paul L. Morgan, PhD, is an assistant professor in the
Department of Educational Psychology, School Psychology,
and Special Education at The Pennsylvania State University
and a research affiliate of the university’s Population Research
Institute. His work investigates the etiologies of learning and
behavioral disabilities, as well as the effects of interventions
designed to help children with such disabilities.
George Farkas, PhD, was a professor of sociology, demography, and education at The Pennsylvania State University and
is now a professor in the Department of Education at the
436 Journal of Learning Disabilities
University of California, Irvine. His research focuses on
inequality in educational achievement and attainment and how
it can be reduced.
is currently investigating status attainment patterns in postcommunist countries and different types of capitalist
societies.
Paula A. Tufis, PhD, completed her doctorate in sociology at
The Pennsylvania State University and is now a principal
research fellow at the Research Institute for the Quality of
Life, Romanian Academy of Science. Her research interests
include social stratification and statistical methods, and she
Rayne A. Sperling, PhD, is an associate professor in the
Department of Educational Psychology, School Psychology,
and Special Education at The Pennsylvania State University.
Her primary research interests examine reading comprehension,
self-regulation, and academic achievement outcomes.