INFLUENCE
The Psychology
of
Persuasion
ROBERT B. CIALDINI PH.D.
This book is dedicated to Chris,
who glows in his father’s eye
Contents
Introduction
v
1
Weapons of Influence
1
2
Reciprocation: The Old Give and Take…and Take
13
3
Commitment and Consistency: Hobgoblins of the Mind
43
4
Social Proof: Truths Are Us
87
5
Liking: The Friendly Thief
126
6
Authority: Directed Deference
157
7
Scarcity: The Rule of the Few
178
Epilogue Instant Influence:
Primitive Consent for an Automatic Age
205
Notes
211
Bibliography
225
Index
241
Acknowledgments
About the Author
Cover
Copyright
About the Publisher
INTRODUCTION
I can admit it freely now. All my life I’ve been a patsy. For as long as I
can recall, I’ve been an easy mark for the pitches of peddlers, fundraisers, and operators of one sort or another. True, only some of these
people have had dishonorable motives. The others—representatives of
certain charitable agencies, for instance—have had the best of intentions.
No matter. With personally disquieting frequency, I have always found
myself in possession of unwanted magazine subscriptions or tickets to
the sanitation workers’ ball. Probably this long-standing status as
sucker accounts for my interest in the study of compliance: Just what
are the factors that cause one person to say yes to another person? And
which techniques most effectively use these factors to bring about such
compliance? I wondered why it is that a request stated in a certain way
will be rejected, while a request that asks for the same favor in a slightly
different fashion will be successful.
So in my role as an experimental social psychologist, I began to do
research into the psychology of compliance. At first the research
vi / Influence
took the form of experiments performed, for the most part, in my
laboratory and on college students. I wanted to find out which psychological principles influence the tendency to comply with a request. Right
now, psychologists know quite a bit about these principles—what they
are and how they work. I have characterized such principles as weapons
of influence and will report on some of the most important in the upcoming chapters.
After a time, though, I began to realize that the experimental work,
while necessary, wasn’t enough. It didn’t allow me to judge the importance of the principles in the world beyond the psychology building and
the campus where I was examining them. It became clear that if I was
to understand fully the psychològy of compliance, I would need to
broaden my scope of investigation. I would need to look to the compliance professionals—the people who had been using the principles on
me all my life. They know what works and what doesn’t; the law of
survival of the fittest assures it. Their business is to make us comply,
and their livelihoods depend on it. Those who don’t know how to get
people to say yes soon fall away; those who do, stay and flourish.
Of course, the compliance professionals aren’t the only ones who
know about and use these principles to help them get their way. We
all employ them and fall victim to them, to some degree, in our daily
interactions with neighbors, friends, lovers, and offspring. But the
compliance practitioners have much more than the vague and amateurish understanding of what works than the rest of us have. As I thought
about it, I knew that they represented the richest vein of information
about compliance available to me. For nearly three years, then, I combined my experimental studies with a decidedly more entertaining
program of systematic immersion into the world of compliance professionals—sales operators, fund-raisers, recruiters, advertisers, and others.
The purpose was to observe, from the inside, the techniques and
strategies most commonly and effectively used by a broad range of
compliance practitioners. That program of observation sometimes took
the form of interviews with the practitioners themselves and sometimes
with the natural enemies (for example, police buncosquad officers,
consumer agencies) of certain of the practitioners. At other times it involved an intensive examination of the written materials by which
compliance techniques are passed down from one generation to another—sales manuals and the like.
Most frequently, though, it has taken the form of participant observation. Participant observation is a research approach in which the researcher becomes a spy of sorts. With disguised identity and intent, the
investigator infiltrates the setting of interest and becomes a full-fledged
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / vii
participant in the group to be studied. So when I wanted to learn about
the compliance tactics of encyclopedia (or vacuum-cleaner, or portraitphotography, or dance-lesson) sales organizations, I would answer a
newspaper ad for sales trainees and have them teach me their methods.
Using similar but not identical approaches, I was able to penetrate advertising, public-relations, and fund-raising agencies to examine their
techniques. Much of the evidence presented in this book, then, comes
from my experience posing as a compliance professional, or aspiring
professional, in a large variety of organizations dedicated to getting us
to say yes.
One aspect of what I learned in this three-year period of participant
observation was most instructive. Although there are thousands of
different tactics that compliance practitioners employ to produce yes,
the majority fall within six basic categories. Each of these categories is
governed by a fundamental psychological principle that directs human
behavior and, in so doing, gives the tactics their power. The book is
organized around these six principles, one to a chapter. The principles—consistency, reciprocation, social proof, authority, liking, and
scarcity—are each discussed in terms of their function in the society
and in terms of how their enormous force can be commissioned by a
compliance professional who deftly incorporates them into requests
for purchases, donations, concessions, votes, assent, etc. It is worthy of
note that I have not included among the six principles the simple rule
of material self-interest—that people want to get the most and pay the
least for their choices. This omission does not stem from any perception
on my part that the desire to maximize benefits and minimize costs is
unimportant in driving our decisions. Nor does it come from any
evidence I have that compliance professionals ignore the power of this
rule. Quite the opposite: In my investigations, I frequently saw practitioners use (sometimes honestly, sometimes not) the compelling “I can
give you a good deal” approach. I choose not to treat the material selfinterest rule separately in this book because I see it as a motivational
given, as a goes-without-saying factor that deserves acknowledgment
but not extensive description.
Finally, each principle is examined as to its ability to produce a distinct
kind of automatic, mindless compliance from people, that is, a willingness to say yes without thinking first. The evidence suggests that the
ever-accelerating pace and informational crush of modern life will make
this particular form of unthinking compliance more and more prevalent
in the future. It will be increasingly important for the society, therefore,
to understand the how and why of automatic influence.
It has been some time since the first edition of Influence was published.
viii / Influence
In the interim, some things have happened that I feel deserve a place
in this new edition. First, we now know more about the influence
process than before. The study of persuasion, compliance, and change
has advanced, and the pages that follow have been adapted to reflect
that progress. In addition to an overall update of the material, I have
included a new feature that was stimulated by the responses of prior
readers.
That new feature highlights the experiences of individuals who have
read Influence, recognized how one of the principles worked on (or for)
them in a particular instance, and wrote to me describing the event.
Their descriptions, which appear in the Reader’s Reports at the end of
each chapter, illustrate how easily and frequently we can fall victim to
the pull of the influence process in our everyday lives.
I wish to thank the following individuals who—either directly or
through their course instructors—contributed the Reader’s Reports
used in this edition: Pat Bobbs, Mark Hastings, James Michaels, Paul
R. Nail, Alan J. Resnik, Daryl Retzlaff, Dan Swift, and Karla Vasks. In
addition, I would like to invite new readers to submit similar reports
for possible publication in a future edition. They may be sent to me at
the Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ
85287-1104.
—ROBERT B. CIALDINI
Chapter 1
WEAPONS OF
INFLUENCE
Everything should be made as simple as possible, but not simpler.
—ALBERT EINSTEIN
I opened an Indian jewelry store in Arizona. She was giddy with a
GOT A PHONE CALL ONE DAY FROM A FRIEND WHO HAD RECENTLY
curious piece of news. Something fascinating had just happened, and
she thought that, as a psychologist, I might be able to explain it to her.
The story involved a certain allotment of turquoise jewelry she had
been having trouble selling. It was the peak of the tourist season, the
store was unusually full of customers, the turquoise pieces were of good
quality for the prices she was asking; yet they had not sold. My friend
had attempted a couple of standard sales tricks to get them moving.
She tried calling attention to them by shifting their location to a more
central display area; no luck. She even told her sales staff to “push” the
items hard, again without success.
Finally, the night before leaving on an out-of-town buying trip, she
scribbled an exasperated note to her head saleswoman, “Everything in
this display case, price × ½,” hoping just to be rid of the offending pieces,
even if at a loss. When she returned a few days later, she was not surprised to find that every article had been sold. She was shocked, though,
to discover that, because the employee had read the “½” in her scrawled
message as a “2,” the entire allotment had sold out at twice the original
price!
2 / Influence
That’s when she called me. I thought I knew what had happened but
told her that, if I were to explain things properly, she would have to
listen to a story of mine. Actually, it isn’t my story; it’s about mother
turkeys, and it belongs to the relatively new science of ethology—the
study of animals in their natural settings. Turkey mothers are good
mothers—loving, watchful, and protective. They spend much of their
time tending, warming, cleaning, and huddling the young beneath
them. But there is something odd about their method. Virtually all of
this mothering is triggered by one thing: the “cheep-cheep” sound of
young turkey chicks. Other identifying features of the chicks, such as
their smell, touch, or appearance, seem to play minor roles in the
mothering process. If a chick makes the “cheep-cheep” noise, its
mother will care for it; if not, the mother will ignore or sometimes kill
it.
The extreme reliance of maternal turkeys upon this one sound was
dramatically illustrated by animal behaviorist M. W. Fox in his description of an experiment involving a mother turkey and a stuffed polecat.1
For a mother turkey, a polecat is a natural enemy whose approach is
to be greeted with squawking, pecking, clawing rage. Indeed, the experimenters found that even a stuffed model of a polecat, when drawn by
a string toward a mother turkey, received an immediate and furious
attack. When, however, the same stuffed replica carried inside it a small
recorder that played the “cheep-cheep” sound of baby turkeys, the
mother not only accepted the oncoming polecat but gathered it underneath her. When the machine was turned off, the polecat model again
drew a vicious attack.
How ridiculous a female turkey seems under these circumstances:
She will embrace a natural enemy just because it goes “cheep-cheep,”
and she will mistreat or murder one of her own chicks just because it
does not. She looks like an automaton whose maternal instincts are
under the automatic control of that single sound. The ethologists tell
us that this sort of thing is far from unique to the turkey. They have
begun to identify regular, blindly mechanical patterns of action in a
wide variety of species.
Called fixed-action patterns, they can involve intricate sequences of
behavior, such as entire courtship or mating rituals. A fundamental
characteristic of these patterns is that the behaviors that compose them
occur in virtually the same fashion and in the same order every time.
It is almost as if the patterns were recorded on tapes within the animals.
When the situation calls for courtship, the courtship tape gets played;
when the situation calls for mothering, the maternal-behavior tape gets
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 3
played. Click and the appropriate tape is activated; whirr and out rolls
the standard sequence of behaviors.
The most interesting thing about all this is the way the tapes are activated. When a male animal acts to defend his territory, for instance,
it is the intrusion of another male of the same species that cues the territorial-defense tape of rigid vigilance, threat, and, if need be, combat
behaviors. But there is a quirk in the system. It is not the rival male as
a whole that is the trigger; it is some specific feature of him, the trigger
feature. Often the trigger feature will be just one tiny aspect of the totality
that is the approaching intruder. Sometimes a shade of color is the
trigger feature. The experiments of ethologists have shown, for instance,
that a male robin, acting as if a rival robin had entered its territory, will
vigorously attack nothing more than a clump of robin-redbreast feathers
placed there. At the same time, it will virtually ignore a perfect stuffed
replica of a male robin without red breast feathers; similar results have
been found in another species of bird, the bluethroat, where it appears
that the trigger for territorial defense is a specific shade of blue breast
feathers.2
Before we enjoy too smugly the ease with which lower animals can
be tricked by trigger features into reacting in ways wholly inappropriate
to the situation, we might realize two things. First, the automatic, fixedaction patterns of these animals work very well the great majority of
the time. For example, because only healthy, normal turkey chicks make
the peculiar sound of baby turkeys, it makes sense for mother turkeys
to respond maternally to that single “cheep-cheep” noise. By reacting
to just that one stimulus, the average mother turkey will nearly always
behave correctly. It takes a trickster like a scientist to make her tapelike
response seem silly. The second important thing to understand is that
we, too, have our preprogrammed tapes; and, although they usually
work to our advantage, the trigger features that activate them can be
used to dupe us into playing them at the wrong times.3
This parallel form of human automatic action is aptly demonstrated
in an experiment by Harvard social psychologist Ellen Langer. A wellknown principle of human behavior says that when we ask someone
to do us a favor we will be more successful if we provide a reason.
People simply like to have reasons for what they do. Langer demonstrated this unsurprising fact by asking a small favor of people waiting
in line to use a library copying machine: Excuse me, I have five pages. May
I use the Xerox machine because I’m in a rush? The effectiveness of this
request-plus-reason was nearly total: Ninety-four percent of those asked
let her skip ahead of them in line. Compare this success rate to the results
when she made the request only: Excuse me, I have five pages. May I use
4 / Influence
the Xerox machine? Under those circumstances, only 60 percent of those
asked complied. At first glance, it appears that the crucial difference
between the two requests was the additional information provided by
the words “because I’m in a rush.” But a third type of request tried by
Langer showed that this was not the case. It seems that it was not the
whole series of words, but the first one, “because,” that made the difference. Instead of including a real reason for compliance, Langer’s third
type of request used the word “because” and then, adding nothing new,
merely restated the obvious: Excuse me, I have five pages. May I use the
Xerox machine because I have to make some copies? The result was that once
again nearly all (93 percent) agreed, even though no real reason, no
new information, was added to justify their compliance. Just as the
“cheep-cheep” sound of turkey chicks triggered an automatic mothering
response from maternal turkeys—even when it emanated from a stuffed
polecat—so, too, did the word “because” trigger an automatic compliance response from Langer’s subjects, even when they were given no
subsequent reason to comply. Click, whirr!4
Although some of Langer’s additional findings show that there are
many situations in which human behavior does not work in a mechanical, tape-activated way, what is astonishing is how often it does. For
instance, consider the strange behavior of those jewelry-store customers
who swooped down on an allotment of turquoise pieces only after the
items had been mistakenly offered at double their original price. I can
make no sense of their behavior, unless it is viewed in click, whirr terms.
The customers, mostly well-to-do vacationers with little knowledge
of turquoise, were using a standard principle—a stereotype—to guide
their buying: “expensive = good.” Thus the vacationers, who wanted
“good” jewelry, saw the turquoise pieces as decidedly more valuable
and desirable when nothing about them was enhanced but the price.
Price alone had become a trigger feature for quality; and a dramatic
increase in price alone had led to a dramatic increase in sales among
the quality-hungry buyers. Click, whirr!
It is easy to fault the tourists for their foolish purchase decisions. But
a close look offers a kinder view. These were people who had been
brought up on the rule “You get what you pay for” and who had seen
that rule borne out over and over in their lives. Before long, they had
translated the rule to mean “expensive = good.” The “expensive = good”
stereotype had worked quite well for them in the past, since normally
the price of an item increases along with its worth; a higher price typically reflects higher quality. So when they found themselves in the position of wanting good turquoise jewelry without much knowledge of
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 5
turquoise, they understandably relied on the old standby feature of
cost to determine the jewelry’s merits.
Although they probably did not realize it, by reacting solely to the
price feature of the turquoise, they were playing a shortcut version of
betting the odds. Instead of stacking all the odds in their favor by trying
painstakingly to master each of the things that indicate the worth of
turquoise jewelry, they were counting on just one—the one they knew
to be usually associated with the quality of any item. They were betting
that price alone would tell them all they needed to know. This time,
because someone mistook a “½” for a “2,” they bet wrong. But in the
long run, over all the past and future situations of their lives, betting
those shortcut odds may represent the most rational approach possible.
In fact, automatic, stereotyped behavior is prevalent in much of human action, because in many cases it is the most efficient form of behaving, and in other cases it is simply necessary. You and I exist in an extraordinarily complicated stimulus environment, easily the most rapidly
moving and complex that has ever existed on this planet. To deal with
it, we need shortcuts. We can’t be expected to recognize and analyze all
the aspects in each person, event, and situation we encounter in even
one day. We haven’t the time, energy, or capacity for it. Instead, we
must very often use our stereotypes, our rules of thumb to classify
things according to a few key features and then to respond mindlessly
when one or another of these trigger features is present.
Sometimes the behavior that unrolls will not be appropriate for the
situation, because not even the best stereotypes and trigger features
work every time. But we accept their imperfection, since there is really
no other choice. Without them we would stand frozen—cataloging,
appraising, and calibrating—as the time for action sped by and away.
And from all indications, we will be relying on them to an even greater
extent in the future. As the stimuli saturating our lives continue to grow
more intricate and variable, we will have to depend increasingly on
our shortcuts to handle them all.
The renowned British philosopher Alfred North Whitehead recognized this inescapable quality of modern life when he asserted that
“civilization advances by extending the number of operations we can
perform without thinking about them.” Take, for example, the “advance” offered to civilization by the discount coupon, which allows
consumers to assume that they will receive a reduced purchase price
by presenting the coupon. The extent to which we have learned to operate mechanically on that assumption is illustrated in the experience
of one automobile-tire company. Mailed-out coupons that—because of
a printing error—offered no savings to recipients produced just as much
customer response as did error-free coupons that offered substantial
6 / Influence
savings. The obvious but instructive point here is that we expect discount coupons to do double duty. Not only do we expect them to save
us money, we also expect them to save us the time and mental energy
required to think about how to do it. In today’s world, we need the first
advantage to handle pocketbook strain; but we need the second advantage to handle something potentially more important—brain strain.
It is odd that despite their current widespread use and looming future
importance, most of us know very little about our automatic behavior
patterns. Perhaps that is so precisely because of the mechanistic, unthinking manner in which they occur. Whatever the reason, it is vital
that we clearly recognize one of their properties: They make us terribly
vulnerable to anyone who does know how they work.
To understand fully the nature of our vulnerability, another glance
at the work of the ethologists is in order. It turns out that these animal
behaviorists with their recorded “cheep-cheeps” and their clumps of
colored breast feathers are not the only ones who have discovered how
to activate the behavior tapes of various species. There is a group of
organisms, often termed mimics, that copy the trigger features of other
animals in an attempt to trick these animals into mistakenly playing
the right behavior tapes at the wrong times. The mimic will then exploit
this altogether inappropriate action for its own benefit.
Take, for example, the deadly trick played by the killer females of
one genus of firefly (Photuris) on the males of another firefly genus
(Photinus). Understandably, the Photinus males scrupulously avoid
contact with the bloodthirsty Photuris females. But through centuries
of experience, the female hunters have located a weakness in their
prey—a special blinking courtship code by which members of the victims’ species tell one another they are ready to mate. Somehow, the
Photuris female has cracked the Photinus courtship code. By mimicking
the flashing mating signals of her prey, the murderess is able to feast
on the bodies of males whose triggered courtship tapes cause them to
fly mechanically into death’s, not love’s, embrace.
Insects seem to be the most severe exploiters of the automaticity of
their prey; it is not uncommon to find their victims duped to death. But
less uncompromising forms of exploitation occur as well. There is, for
instance, a little fish, the saber-toothed blenny, that takes advantage of
an unusual program of cooperation worked out by members of two
other species of fish. The cooperating fish form a Mutt and Jeff team
consisting of a large grouper fish on the one hand and a much smaller
type of fish on the other. The smaller fish serves as a cleaner to the larger
one, which allows the cleaner to approach it and even enter its mouth
to pick off fungus and other parasites that have attached themselves to
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 7
the big fish’s teeth or gills. It is a beautiful arrangement: The big grouper
gets cleaned of harmful pests, and the cleaner fish gets an easy dinner.
The larger fish normally devours any other small fish foolish enough
to come close to it. But when the cleaner approaches, the big fish suddenly stops all movement and floats open-mouthed and nearly immobile
in response to an undulating dance that the cleaner performs. This
dance appears to be the trigger feature of the cleaner that activates the
dramatic passivity of the big fish. It also provides the saber-toothed
blenny with an angle—a chance to take advantage of the cleaning ritual
of the cooperators. The blenny will approach the large predator, copying
the undulations of the cleaner’s dance and automatically producing the
tranquil, unmoving posture of the big fish. Then, true to its name, it
will quickly rip a mouthful from the larger fish’s flesh and dart away
before its startled victim can recover.5
There is a strong but sad parallel in the human jungle. We too have
exploiters who mimic trigger features for our own brand of automatic
responding. Unlike the mostly instinctive response sequences of nonhumans, our automatic tapes usually develop from psychological
principles or stereotypes we have learned to accept. Although they vary
in their force, some of these principles possess a tremendous ability to
direct human action. We have been subjected to them from such an
early point in our lives, and they have moved us about so pervasively
since then, that you and I rarely perceive their power. In the eyes of
others, though, each such principle is a detectable and ready weapon—a
weapon of automatic influence.
There is a group of people who know very well where the weapons
of automatic influence lie and who employ them regularly and expertly
to get what they want. They go from social encounter to social encounter
requesting others to comply with their wishes; their frequency of success
is dazzling. The secret of their effectiveness lies in the way they structure
their requests, the way they arm themselves with one or another of the
weapons of influence that exist within the social environment. To do
this may take no more than one correctly chosen word that engages a
strong psychological principle and sets an automatic behavior tape
rolling within us. And trust the human exploiters to learn quickly exactly
how to profit from our tendency to respond mechanically according to
these principles.
Remember my friend the jewelry-store owner? Although she benefited
by accident the first time, it did not take her long to begin exploiting
the “expensive = good” stereotype regularly and intentionally. Now,
during the tourist season, she first tries to speed the sale of an item that
has been difficult to move by increasing its price substantially. She
claims that this is marvelously cost-effective. When it works on the
8 / Influence
unsuspecting vacationers—as it frequently does—it results in an
enormous profit margin. And even when it is not initially successful,
she can mark the article “Reduced from _____” and sell it at its original
price while still taking advantage of the “expensive = good” reaction
to the inflated figure.
By no means is my friend original in this last use of the “expensive
= good” rule to snare those seeking a bargain. Culturist and author Leo
Rosten gives the example of the Drubeck brothers, Sid and Harry, who
owned a men’s tailor shop in Rosten’s neighborhood while he was
growing up in the 1930s. Whenever the salesman, Sid, had a new customer trying on suits in front of the shop’s three-sided mirror, he would
admit to a hearing problem, and, as they talked, he would repeatedly
request that the man speak more loudly to him. Once the customer had
found a suit he liked and had asked for the price, Sid would call to his
brother, the head tailor, at the back of the room, “Harry, how much for
this suit?” Looking up from his work—and greatly exaggerating the
suit’s true price—Harry would call back, “For that beautiful all-wool
suit, forty-two dollars.” Pretending not to have heard and cupping his
hand to his ear, Sid would ask again. Once more Harry would reply,
“Forty-two dollars.” At this point, Sid would turn to the customer and
report, “He says twenty-two dollars.” Many a man would hurry to buy
the suit and scramble out of the shop with his “expensive = good”
bargain before Poor Sid discovered the “mistake.”
There are several components shared by most of the weapons of
automatic influence to be described in this book. We have already discussed two of them—the nearly mechanical process by which the power
within these weapons can be activated, and the consequent exploitability
of this power by anyone who knows how to trigger them. A third
component involves the way that the weapons of automatic influence
lend their force to those who use them. It’s not that the weapons, like
a set of heavy clubs, provide a conspicuous arsenal to be used by one
person to bludgeon another into submission.
The process is much more sophisticated and subtle. With proper execution, the exploiters need hardly strain a muscle to get their way. All
that is required is to trigger the great stores of influence that already
exist in the situation and direct them toward the intended target. In this
sense, the approach is not unlike that of the Japanese martial-art form
called jujitsu. A woman employing jujitsu would utilize her own
strength only minimally against an opponent. Instead, she would exploit
the power inherent in such naturally present principles as gravity,
leverage, momentum, and inertia. If she knows how and where to engage the action of these principles, she can easily defeat a physically
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 9
stronger rival. And so it is for the exploiters of the weapons of automatic
influence that exist naturally around us. The exploiters can commission
the power of these weapons for use against their targets while exerting
little personal force. This last feature of the process allows the exploiters
an enormous additional benefit—the ability to manipulate without the
appearance of manipulation. Even the victims themselves tend to see
their compliance as determined by the action of natural forces rather
than by the designs of the person who profits from that compliance.
An example is in order. There is a principle in human perception, the
contrast principle, that affects the way we see the difference between
two things that are presented one after another. Simply put, if the second
item is fairly different from the first, we will tend to see it as more different than it actually is. So if we lift a light object first and then lift a
heavy object, we will estimate the second object to be heavier than if
we had lifted it without first trying the light one. The contrast principle
is well established in the field of psychophysics and applies to all sorts
of perceptions besides weight. If we are talking to a beautiful woman
at a cocktail party and are then joined by an unattractive one, the second
woman will strike us as less attractive than she actually is.
In fact, studies done on the contrast principle at Arizona State and
Montana State universities suggest that we may be less satisfied with
the physical attractiveness of our own lovers because of the way the
popular media bombard us with examples of unrealistically attractive
models. In one study college students rated a picture of an averagelooking member of the opposite sex as less attractive if they had first
looked through the ads in some popular magazines. In another study,
male college-dormitory residents rated the photo of a potential blind
date. Those who did so while watching an episode of the Charlie’s Angels
TV series viewed the blind date as a less attractive woman than those
who rated her while watching a different show. Apparently it was the
uncommon beauty of the Angels female stars that made the blind date
seem less attractive.6
A nice demonstration of perceptual contrast is sometimes employed
in psychophysics laboratories to introduce students to the principle
firsthand. Each student takes a turn sitting in front of three pails of
water—one cold, one at room temperature, and one hot. After placing
one hand in the cold water and one in the hot water, the student is told
to place both in the lukewarm water simultaneously. The look of amused
bewilderment that immediately registers tells the story: Even though
both hands are in the same bucket, the hand that has been in the cold
water feels as if it is now in hot water, while the one that was in the hot
water feels as if it is now in cold water. The point is that the same
10 / Influence
thing—in this instance, room-temperature water—can be made to seem
very different, depending on the nature of the event that precedes it.
Be assured that the nice little weapon of influence provided by the
contrast principle does not go unexploited. The great advantage of this
principle is not only that it works but also that it is virtually undetectable. Those who employ it can cash in on its influence without any appearance of having structured the situation in their favor. Retail clothiers
are a good example. Suppose a man enters a fashionable men’s store
and says that he wants to buy a three-piece suit and a sweater. If you
were the salesperson, which would you show him first to make him
likely to spend the most money? Clothing stores instruct their sales
personnel to sell the costly item first. Common sense might suggest the
reverse: If a man has just spent a lot of money to purchase a suit, he
may be reluctant to spend very much more on the purchase of a
sweater. But the clothiers know better. They behave in accordance with
what the contrast principle would suggest: Sell the suit first, because
when it comes time to look at sweaters, even expensive ones, their prices
will not seem as high in comparison. A man might balk at the idea of
spending $95 for a sweater, but if he has just bought a $495 suit, a $95
sweater does not seem excessive. The same principle applies to a man
who wishes to buy the accessories (shirt, shoes, belt) to go along with
his new suit. Contrary to the commonsense view, the evidence supports
the contrast-principle prediction. As sales motivation analysts Whitney,
Hubin, and Murphy state, “The interesting thing is that even when a
man enters a clothing store with the express purpose of purchasing a
suit, he will almost always pay more for whatever accessories he buys
if he buys them after the suit purchase than before.”
It is much more profitable for salespeople to present the expensive
item first, not only because to fail to do so will lose the influence of the
contrast principle; to fail to do so will also cause the principle to work
actively against them. Presenting an inexpensive product first and following it with an expensive one will cause the expensive item to seem
even more costly as a result—hardly a desirable consequence for most
sales organizations. So, just as it is possible to make the same bucket of
water appear to be hotter or colder, depending on the temperature of
previously presented water, it is possible to make the price of the same
item seem higher or lower, depending on the price of a previously
presented item.
Clever use of perceptual contrast is by no means confined to clothiers.
I came across a technique that engaged the contrast principle while I
was investigating, undercover, the compliance tactics of real-estate
companies. To “learn the ropes,” I was accompanying a company realty
salesman on a weekend of showing houses to prospective home buyers.
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 11
The salesman—we can call him Phil—was to give me tips to help me
through my break-in period. One thing I quickly noticed was that
whenever Phil began showing a new set of customers potential buys,
he would start with a couple of undesirable houses. I asked him about
it, and he laughed. They were what he called “setup” properties. The
company maintained a run-down house or two on its lists at inflated
prices. These houses were not intended to be sold to customers but to
be shown to them, so that the genuine properties in the company’s inventory would benefit from the comparison. Not all the sales staff made
use of the setup houses, but Phil did. He said he liked to watch his
prospects’ “eyes light up” when he showed the place he really wanted
to sell them after they had seen the run-down houses. “The house I got
them spotted for looks really great after they’ve first looked at a couple
of dumps.”
Automobile dealers use the contrast principle by waiting until the
price for a new car has been negotiated before suggesting one option
after another that might be added. In the wake of a fifteen-thousanddollar deal, the hundred or so dollars required for a nicety like an FM
radio seems almost trivial in comparison. The same will be true of the
added expense of accessories like tinted windows, dual side-view mirrors, whitewall tires, or special trim that the salesman might suggest
in sequence. The trick is to bring up the extras independently of one
another, so that each small price will seem petty when compared to the
already-determined much larger one. As the veteran car buyer can attest,
many a budget-sized final price figure has ballooned from the addition
of all those seemingly little options. While the customer stands, signed
contract in hand, wondering what happened and finding no one to
blame but himself, the car dealer stands smiling the knowing smile of
the jujitsu master.
READER’S REPORT
From the Parent of a College Coed
Dear Mother and Dad:
Since I left for college I have been remiss in writing and
I am sorry for my thoughtlessness in not having written before. I will bring you up to date now, but before you read on,
please sit down. You are not to read any further unless you
are sitting down, okay?
Well, then, I am getting along pretty well now. The skull
fracture and the concussion I got when I jumped out the
window of my dormitory when it caught on fire shortly after
my arrival here is pretty well healed now. I only spent two
12 / Influence
weeks in the hospital and now I can see almost normally and
only get those sick headaches once a day. Fortunately, the
fire in the dormitory, and my jump, was witnessed by an attendant at the gas station near the dorm, and he was the one
who called the Fire Department and the ambulance. He also
visited me in the hospital and since I had nowhere to live
because of the burntout dormitory, he was kind enough to
invite me to share his apartment with him. It’s really a basement room, but it’s kind of cute. He is a very fine boy and
we have fallen deeply in love and are planning to get married.
We haven’t got the exact date yet, but it will be before my
pregnancy begins to show.
Yes, Mother and Dad, I am pregnant. I know how much
you are looking forward to being grandparents and I know
you will welcome the baby and give it the same love and
devotion and tender care you gave me when I was a child.
The reason for the delay in our marriage is that my boyfriend
has a minor infection which prevents us from passing our
pre-marital blood tests and I carelessly caught it from him.
Now that I have brought you up to date, I want to tell you
that there was no dormitory fire, I did not have a concussion
or skull fracture, I was not in the hospital, I am not pregnant,
I am not engaged, I am not infected, and there is no boyfriend.
However, I am getting a “D” in American History, and an
“F” in Chemistry and I want you to see those marks in their
proper perspective.
Your loving daughter,
Sharon
Sharon may be failing chemistry, but she gets an “A” in psychology.
Chapter 2
RECIPROCATION
The Old Give and Take…and Take
Pay every debt, as if God wrote the bill.
—RALPH WALDO EMERSON
experiA ment. He sent Christmas cards to a sample of perfect strangers.
FEW YEARS AGO, A UNIVERSITY PROFESSOR TRIED A LITTLE
Although he expected some reaction, the response he received was
amazing—holiday cards addressed to him came pouring back from the
people who had never met nor heard of him. The great majority of those
who returned a card never inquired into the identity of the unknown
professor. They received his holiday greeting card, click, and, whirr,
they automatically sent one in return. While small in scope, this study
nicely shows the action of one of the most potent of the weapons of influence around us—the rule for reciprocation.1 The rule says that we
should try to repay, in kind, what another person has provided us. If
a woman does us a favor, we should do her one in return; if a man
sends us a birthday present, we should remember his birthday with a
gift of our own; if a couple invites us to a party, we should be sure to
invite them to one of ours. By virtue of the reciprocity rule, then, we
are obligated to the future repayment of favors, gifts, invitations, and
the like. So typical is it for indebtedness to accompany the receipt of
such things that a term like “much obliged” has become a synonym for
“thank you,” not only in the English language but in others as well.
The impressive aspect of the rule for reciprocation and the sense of
obligation that goes with it is its pervasiveness in human culture. It is
so widespread that after intensive study, sociologists such as Alvin
14 / Influence
Gouldner can report that there is no human society that does not subscribe to the rule.2 And within each society it seems pervasive also; it
permeates exchanges of every kind. Indeed, it may well be that a developed system of indebtedness flowing from the rule for reciprocation
is a unique property of human culture. The noted archaeologist Richard
Leakey ascribes the essence of what makes us human to the reciprocity
system: “We are human because our ancestors learned to share their
food and their skills in an honored network of obligation,”3 he says.
Cultural anthropologists Lionel Tiger and Robin Fox view this “web of
indebtedness” as a unique adaptive mechanism of human beings, allowing for the division of labor, the exchange of diverse forms of goods,
the exchange of different services (making it possible for experts to
develop), and the creation of a cluster of interdependencies that bind
individuals together into highly efficient units.4
It is the future orientation inherent in a sense of obligation that is
critical to its ability to produce social advances of the sort described by
Tiger and Fox. A widely shared and strongly held feeling of future obligation made an enormous difference in human social evolution, because it meant that one person could give something (for example, food,
energy, care) to another with confidence that it was not being lost. For
the first time in evolutionary history, one individual could give away
any of a variety of resources without actually giving them away. The
result was the lowering of the natural inhibitions against transactions
that must be begun by one person’s providing personal resources to
another. Sophisticated and coordinated systems of aid, gift giving, defense, and trade became possible, bringing immense benefit to the societies that possessed them. With such clearly adaptive consequences for
the culture, it is not surprising that the rule for reciprocation is so deeply
implanted in us by the process of socialization we all undergo.
I know of no better illustration of how reciprocal obligations can
reach long and powerfully into the future than the perplexing story of
five thousand dollars of relief aid that was sent in 1985 between Mexico
and the impoverished people of Ethiopia. In 1985 Ethiopia could justly
lay claim to the greatest suffering and privation in the world. Its economy was in ruin. Its food supply had been ravaged by years of drought
and internal war. Its inhabitants were dying by the thousands from
disease and starvation. Under these circumstances, I would not have
been surprised to learn of a five-thousand-dollar relief donation from
Mexico to that wrenchingly needy country. I remember my chin hitting
my chest, though, when a brief newspaper item I was reading insisted
that the aid had gone in the opposite direction. Native officials of the
Ethiopian Red Cross had decided to send the money to help the victims
of that year’s earthquakes in Mexico City.
It is both a personal bane and a professional blessing that whenever
I am confused by some aspect of human behavior, I feel driven to investigate further. In this instance, I was able to track down a fuller account of the story. Fortunately a journalist who had been as bewildered
as I was by the Ethiopians’ action had asked for an explanation. The
answer he received offers eloquent validation of the reciprocity rule:
Despite the enormous needs prevailing in Ethiopia, the money was
being sent because Mexico had sent aid to Ethiopia in 1935, when it was
invaded by Italy. So informed, I remained awed, but I was no longer
puzzled. The need to reciprocate had transcended great cultural differences, long distances, acute famine, and immediate self-interest. Quite
simply, a half century later, against all countervailing forces, obligation
triumphed.
Make no mistake, human societies derive a truly significant competitive advantage from the reciprocity rule, and consequently they make
sure their members are trained to comply with and believe in it. Each
of us has been taught to live up to the rule, and each of us knows about
the social sanctions and derision applied to anyone who violates it. The
labels we assign to such a person are loaded with negativity—moocher,
ingrate, welsher. Because there is general distaste for those who take
and make no effort to give in return, we will often go to great lengths
to avoid being considered one of their number. It is to those lengths
that we will often be taken and, in the process, be “taken” by individuals
who stand to gain from our indebtedness.
To understand how the rule for reciprocation can be exploited by
one who recognizes it as the source of influence it certainly is, we might
closely examine an experiment performed by Professor Dennis Regan
of Cornell University.
one who recognizes it as the source of influence it certainly is, we might
closely examine an experiment performed by Professor Dennis Regan
of Cornell University.
one who recognizes it as the source of influence it certainly is, we might
closely examine an experiment performed by Professor Dennis Regan
of Cornell University.
one who recognizes it as the source of influence it certainly is, we might
closely examine an experiment performed by Professor Dennis Regan
of Cornell University.
ne who recognizes it as the source of influence it certainly is, we might
losely examine an experiment performed by Professor Dennis Regan
f Cornell University.
who recognizes it as the source of influence it certainly is, we might
ely examine an experiment performed by Professor Dennis Regan
Cornell University.
ho recognizes it as the source of influence it certainly is, we might
y examine an experiment performed by Professor Dennis Regan
nell University.
60 / Influence
of kind treatment, and statements sympathetic to communism. The
hope was that the Chinese would want such letters to surface and
would, therefore, allow their delivery. Of course, the Chinese were
happy to cooperate because those letters served their interests marvelously. First, their worldwide propaganda effort benefited greatly
from the appearance of pro-Communist statements by American servicemen. Second, in the service of prisoner indoctrination, they had,
without raising a finger of physical force, gotten many men to go on
record as supporting the Chinese cause.
A similar technique involved political essay contests that were regularly held in camp. The prizes for winning were invariably small—a
few cigarettes or a bit of fruit—but were sufficiently scarce that they
generated a lot of interest from the men. Usually the winning essay was
one that took a solidly pro-Communist stand…but not always. The
Chinese were wise enough to realize that most of the prisoners would
not enter a contest that they could win only by writing a Communist
tract. And the Chinese were clever enough to know how to plant small
commitments to communism in the men that could be nurtured into
later bloom. So the prize was occasionally given to an essay that generally supported the United States but that bowed once or twice to the
Chinese view. The effects of this strategy were exactly what the Chinese
wanted. The men continued to participate voluntarily in the contests
because they saw that they could win with an essay highly favorable
to their own country. But perhaps without realizing it, they began to
shade their essays a bit toward communism in order to have a better
chance of winning. The Chinese were ready to pounce on any concession
to Communist dogma and to bring consistency pressures to bear upon
it. In the case of a written declaration within a voluntary essay, they
had a perfect commitment from which to build toward collaboration
and conversion.
Other compliance professionals also know about the committing
power of written statements. The enormously successful Amway Corporation, for instance, has hit upon a way to spur their sales personnel
to greater and greater accomplishments. Members of the staff are asked
to set individual sales goals and commit themselves to those goals by
personally recording them on paper:
One final tip before you get started: Set a goal and .
Whatever the goal, the important thing is that you set it, so you’ve
got something for which to aim—and that you write it down. There
is something magical about writing things down. So set a goal and
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 61
write it down. When you reach that goal, set another and write
that down. You’ll be off and running.10
If the Amway people have found “something magical about writing
things down,” so have other business organizations. Some door-to-door
sales companies use the magic of written commitments to battle the
“cooling-off” laws recently passed in many states. The laws are designed
to allow customers a few days after purchasing an item to cancel the
sale and receive a full refund. At first this legislation hurt the hard-sell
companies deeply. Because they emphasize high-pressure tactics, their
customers often buy, not because they want the product but because
they are duped or intimidated into the sale. When the new laws went
into effect, these customers began canceling in droves.
The companies have since learned a beautifully simple trick that cuts
the number of such cancellations drastically. They merely have the
customer, rather than the salesman, fill out the sales agreement. According to the sales-training program of a prominent encyclopedia company,
that personal commitment alone has proved to be “a very important
psychological aid in preventing customers from backing out of their
contracts.” Like the Amway Corporation, then, these organizations
have found that something special happens when people personally
put their commitments on paper: They live up to what they have written
down.
Another common way for businesses to cash in on the “magic” of
written declarations occurs through the use of an innocent-looking
promotional device. Before I began to study weapons of social influence,
I used to wonder why big companies such as Procter & Gamble and
General Foods are always running those “25-, 50-, or 100 words or less”
testimonial contests. They all seem to be alike. The contestant is to
compose a short personal statement that begins with the words, “Why
I like…” and goes on to laud the features of whatever cake mix or floor
wax happens to be at issue. The company judges the entries and awards
some stunningly large prizes to the winners. What had puzzled me was
what the companies got out of the deal. Often the contest requires no
purchase; anyone submitting an entry is eligible. Yet, the companies
appear to be strangely willing to incur the huge costs of contest after
contest.
I am no longer puzzled. The purpose behind the testimonial contests
is the same as the purpose behind the political essay contests of the
Chinese Communists. In both instances, the aim is to get as many people
as possible to go on record as liking the product. In Korea, the product
was a brand of Chinese communism; in the United States, it might be
a brand of cuticle remover. The type of product doesn’t matter; the
62 / Influence
process is the same. Participants voluntarily write essays for attractive
prizes that they have only a small chance to win. But they know that
for an essay to have any chance of winning at all, it must include praise
for the product. So they find praiseworthy features of the product and
describe them in their essays. The result is hundreds of men in Korea
or hundreds of thousands of people in America who testify in writing
to the product’s appeal and who, consequently, experience that “magical” pull to believe what they have written.
The Public Eye
One reason that written testaments are effective in bringing about
genuine personal change is that they can so easily be made public. The
prisoner experience in Korea showed the Chinese to be quite aware of
an important psychological principle: Public commitments tend to be
lasting commitments. The Chinese constantly arranged to have the proCommunist statements of their captives seen by others. A man who
had written a political essay the Chinese liked, for example, might find
copies of it posted around camp, or might be asked to read it to a prisoner discussion group, or even to read it on the camp radio broadcast.
As far as the Chinese were concerned, the more public the better. Why?
Whenever one takes a stand that is visible to others, there arises a
drive to maintain that stand in order to look like a consistent person.
Remember that earlier in this chapter we described how desirable good
personal consistency is as a trait; how someone without it could be
judged as fickle, uncertain, pliant, scatterbrained, or unstable; how
someone with it is viewed as rational, assured, trustworthy, and sound.
Given this context, it is hardly surprising that people try to avoid the
look of inconsistency. For appearances’ sake, then, the more public a
stand, the more reluctant we will be to change it.
An illustration of how public commitments can lead to doggedly
consistent further action is provided in a famous experiment performed
by a pair of prominent social psychologists, Morton Deutsch and Harold
Gerard. The basic procedure was to have college students first estimate
in their own minds the length of lines they were shown. At this point,
one sample of the students had to commit themselves publicly to their
initial judgments by writing them down, signing their names to them,
and turning them in to the experimenter. A second sample of students
also committed themselves to their first estimates, but they did so
privately by putting them on a Magic Writing Pad and then erasing
them by lifting the Magic Pad’s plastic cover before anyone could see
what they had written. A third set of students did not commit them-
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 63
selves to their initial estimates at all; they just kept the estimates in mind
privately.
In these ways, Deutsch and Gerard had cleverly arranged for some
students to commit themselves publicly, some privately, and some not
at all to their initial decisions. What Deutsch and Gerard wanted to find
out was which of the three types of students would be most inclined
to stick with their first judgments after receiving information that those
judgments were incorrect. So all of the students were given new evidence suggesting that their initial estimates were wrong, and they were
then given the chance to change their estimates.
The results were quite clear. The students who had never written
down their first choices were the least loyal to those choices. When new
evidence was presented that questioned the wisdom of decisions that
had never left their heads, these students were the most influenced by
the new information to change what they had viewed as the “correct”
decision. Compared to these uncommitted students, those who had
merely written their decisions for a moment on a Magic Pad were significantly less willing to change their minds when given the chance.
Even though they had committed themselves under the most anonymous of circumstances, the act of writing down their first judgments
caused them to resist the influence of contradictory new data and to
remain consistent with the preliminary choices. But Deutsch and Gerard
found that, by far, it was the students who had publicly recorded their
initial positions who most resolutely refused to shift from those positions
later. Public commitment had hardened them into the most stubborn
of all.
This sort of stubbornness can occur even in situations where accuracy
should be more important than consistency. In one study, when six- or
twelve-person experimental juries were deciding a close case, hung
juries were significantly more frequent if the jurors had to express their
opinions with a visible show of hands rather than by secret ballot. Once
jurors had stated their initial views publicly, they were reluctant to allow
themselves to change publicly, either. Should you ever find yourself as
the foreperson of a jury under these conditions, then, you could reduce
the risk of a hung jury by choosing a secret rather than public balloting
technique.11
The Deutsch and Gerard finding that we are truest to our decisions
if we have bound ourselves to them publicly can be put to good use.
Consider the organizations dedicated to helping people rid themselves
of bad habits. Many weight-reduction clinics, for instance, understand
that often a person’s private decision to lose weight will be too weak
to withstand the blandishments of bakery windows, wafting cooking
scents, and late-night Sara Lee commercials. So they see to it that the
64 / Influence
decision is buttressed by the pillars of public commitment. They require
their clients to write down an immediate weight-loss goal and show that
goal to as many friends, relatives, and neighbors as possible. Clinic
operators report that frequently this simple technique works where all
else has failed.
Of course, there’s no need to pay a special clinic in order to engage
a visible commitment as an ally. One San Diego woman described to
me how she employed a public promise to help herself finally stop
smoking:
I remember it was after I heard about another scientific study
showing that smoking causes cancer. Every time one of those
things came out, I used to get determined to quit, but I never could.
This time, though, I decided I had to do something. I’m a proud
person. It matters to me if other people see me in a bad light. So I
thought, “Maybe I can use that pride to help me dump this damn
habit.” So I made a list of all the people who I really wanted to
respect me. Then I went out and got some blank business cards
and I wrote on the back of each card, “I promise you that I will
never smoke another cigarette.”
Within a week, I had given or sent a signed card to everybody on the
list—my dad, my brother back East, my boss, my best girlfriend, my
ex-husband, everybody but one—the guy I was dating then. I was just
crazy about him, and I really wanted him to value me as a person. Believe me, I thought twice about giving him a card because I knew that
if I couldn’t keep my promise to him I’d die. But one day at the office—he worked in the same building as I did—I just walked up to him,
handed him the card, and walked away without saying anything.
Quitting “cold turkey” was the hardest thing I’ve ever done. There
must have been a thousand times when I thought I had to have a smoke.
But whenever that happened, I’d just picture how all of the people on
my list, especially this one guy, would think less of me if I couldn’t stick
to my guns. And that’s all it took. I’ve never taken another puff.
You know, the interesting thing is the guy turned out to be a real
schmuck. I can’t figure out what I saw in him back then. But at the time,
without knowing it, he helped me get through the toughest part of the
toughest thing I’ve ever had to do. I don’t even like him anymore. Still,
I do feel grateful in a way because I think he saved my life.
The Effort Extra
Yet another reason that written commitments are so effective is that
they require more work than verbal ones. And the evidence is clear that
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 65
the more effort that goes into a commitment, the greater is its ability to
influence the attitudes of the person who made it. We can find that
evidence quite close to home or as far away as the back regions of the
primitive world. For example, there is a tribe in southern Africa, the
Thonga, that requires each of its boys to go through an elaborate initiation ceremony before he can be counted a man of the tribe. As with
many other primitive peoples, a Thonga boy endures a great deal before
he is admitted to adult membership in the group. Anthropologists
Whiting, Kluckhohn, and Anthony have described this three-month
ordeal in brief but vivid terms:
When a boy is somewhere between 10 and 16 years of age, he is
sent by his parents to “circumcision school,” which is held every
4 or 5 years. Here in company with his age-mates he undergoes
severe hazing by the adult males of the society. The initiation begins when each boy runs the gauntlet between two rows of men
who beat him with clubs. At the end of this experience he is
stripped of his clothes and his hair is cut. He is next met by a man
covered with lion manes and is seated upon a stone facing this
“lion man.” Someone then strikes him from behind and when he
turns his head to see who has struck him, his foreskin is seized
and in two movements cut off by the “lion man.” Afterward he is
secluded for three months in the “yard of mysteries,” where he
can be seen only by the initiated.
During the course of his initiation, the boy undergoes six major
trials: beatings, exposure to cold, thirst, eating of unsavory foods,
punishment, and the threat of death. On the slightest pretext, he
may be beaten by one of the newly initiated men, who is assigned
to the task by the older men of the tribe. He sleeps without covering and suffers bitterly from the winter cold. He is forbidden to
drink a drop of water during the whole three months. Meals are
often made nauseating by the half-digested grass from the stomach
of an antelope, which is poured over his food. If he is caught
breaking any important rule governing the ceremony, he is severely punished. For example, in one of these punishments, sticks are
placed between the fingers of the offender, then a strong man
closes his hand around that of the novice, practically crushing his
fingers. He is frightened into submission by being told that in
former times boys who had tried to escape or who had revealed
the secrets to women or to the uninitiated were hanged and their
bodies burned to ashes.12
On the face of it, these rites seem extraordinary and bizarre. Yet, at
the same time, they can be seen to be remarkably similar in principle
66 / Influence
and even in detail to the common initiation ceremonies of school fraternities. During the traditional “Hell Week” held yearly on college
campuses, fraternity pledges must persevere through a variety of
activities designed by the older members to test the limits of physical
exertion, psychological strain, and social embarrassment. At week’s
end, the boys who have persisted through the ordeal are accepted for
full group membership. Mostly their tribulations have left them no
more than greatly tired and a bit shaky, although sometimes the negative
effects are more serious.
What is interesting is how closely the particular features of Hell Week
tasks match those of the tribal initiation rites. Recall that anthropologists
identified six major trials to be endured by a Thonga initiate during his
stay in the “yard of mysteries.” A scan of newspaper reports shows
that each trial also has its place in the hazing rituals of Greek-letter societies:
• Beatings. Fourteen-year-old Michael Kalogris spent three weeks in a
Long Island hospital recovering from internal injuries suffered during
a Hell Night initiation ceremony of his high-school fraternity, Omega
Gamma Delta. He had been administered the “atomic bomb” by his
prospective brothers, who told him to hold his hands over his head
and keep them there while they gathered around to slam fists into
his stomach and back simultaneously and repeatedly.
• Exposure to cold. On a winter night, Frederick Bronner, a California
junior-college student, was taken three thousand feet up and ten
miles into the hills of a national forest by his prospective fraternity
brothers. Left to find his way home wearing only a thin sweatshirt
and slacks, Fat Freddy, as he was called, shivered in a frigid wind
until he tumbled down a steep ravine, fracturing bones and hurting
his head. Prevented by his injuries from going on, he huddled there
against the cold until he died of exposure.
• Thirst. Two Ohio State University freshmen found themselves in the
“dungeon” of their prospective fraternity house after breaking the
rule requiring all pledges to crawl into the dining area prior to Hell
Week meals. Once locked in the house storage closet, they were given
only salty foods to eat for nearly two days. Nothing was provided
for drinking purposes except a pair of plastic cups in which they
could catch their own urine.
• Eating of unsavory foods. At Kappa Sigma house on the campus of the
University of Southern California, the eyes of eleven pledges bulged
when they saw the sickening task before them. Eleven quarter-pound
slabs of raw liver lay on a tray. Cut thick and soaked in oil, each was
to be swallowed whole, one to a boy. Gagging and choking re-
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 67
peatedly, young Richard Swanson failed three times to down his
piece. Determined to succeed, he finally got the oil-soaked meat into
his throat where it lodged and, despite all efforts to remove it, killed
him.
• Punishment. In Wisconsin, a pledge who forgot one section of a ritual
incantation to be memorized by all initiates was punished for his error. He was required to keep his feet under the rear legs of a folding
chair while the heaviest of his fraternity brothers sat down and drank
a beer. Although the pledge did not cry out during the punishment,
a bone in each of his feet was broken.
• Threats of death. A pledge of Zeta Beta Tau fraternity was taken to a
beach area of New Jersey and told to dig his “own grave.” Seconds
after he complied with orders to lie flat in the finished hole, the sides
collapsed, suffocating him before his prospective fraternity brothers
could dig him out.
There is another striking similarity between the initiation rites of tribal
and fraternal societies: They simply will not die. Resisting all attempts
to eliminate or suppress them, such hazing practices have been phenomenally resilient. Authorities, in the form of colonial governments or
university administrations, have tried threats, social pressures, legal
actions, banishments, bribes, and bans to persuade the groups to remove
the hazards and humiliations from their initiation ceremonies. None
has been successful. Oh, there may be a change while the authority is
watching closely. But this is usually more apparent than real, the
harsher trials occurring under more secret circumstances until the
pressure is off and they can surface again.
On some college campuses, officials have tried to eliminate dangerous
hazing practices by substituting a “Help Week” of civic service or by
taking direct control of the initiation rituals. When such attempts are
not slyly circumvented by fraternities, they are met with outright
physical resistance. For example, in the aftermath of Richard Swanson’s
choking death at USC, the university president issued new rules requiring that all pledging activities be reviewed by school authorities before
going into effect and that adult advisers be present during initiation
ceremonies. According to one national magazine, “The new ‘code’ set
off a riot so violent that city police and fire detachments were afraid to
enter campus.”
Resigning themselves to the inevitable, other college representatives
have given up on the possibility of abolishing the degradations of Hell
Week. “If hazing is a universal human activity, and every bit of evidence
points to this conclusion, you most likely won’t be able to ban it effectively. Refuse to allow it openly and it will go underground. You can’t
68 / Influence
ban sex, you can’t prohibit alcohol, and you probably can’t eliminate
hazing!”13
What is it about hazing practices that make them so precious to these
societies? What could make the groups want to evade, undermine, or
contest any effort to ban the degrading and perilous features of their
initiation rites? Some have argued that the groups themselves are
composed of psychological or social miscreants whose twisted needs
demand that others be harmed and humiliated. But the evidence does
not support such a view. Studies done on the personality traits of fraternity members, for instance, show them to be, if anything, slightly
healthier than other college students in their psychological adjustment.
Similarly, fraternities are known for their willingness to engage in beneficial community projects for the general social good. What they are
not willing to do, however, is substitute these projects for their initiation
ceremonies. One survey at the University of Washington found that,
of the fraternity chapters examined, most had a type of Help Week
tradition but that this community service was in addition to Hell Week.
In only one case was such service directly related to initiation procedures.14
The picture that emerges of the perpetrators of hazing practices is of
normal individuals who tend to be psychologically stable and socially
concerned but who become aberrantly harsh as a group at only one
time—immediately before the admission of new members to the society.
The evidence, then, points to the ceremony as the culprit. There must
be something about its rigors that is vital to the group. There must be
some function to its harshness that the group will fight relentlessly to
maintain. What?
My own view is that the answer appeared in 1959 in the results of a
study little known outside of social psychology. A pair of young researchers, Elliot Aronson and Judson Mills, decided to test their observation that “persons who go through a great deal of trouble or pain to
attain something tend to value it more highly than persons who attain
the same thing with a minimum of effort.” The real stroke of inspiration
came in their choice of the initiation ceremony as the best place to examine this possibility. They found that college women who had to endure a severely embarrassing initiation ceremony in order to gain access
to a sex discussion group convinced themselves that their new group
and its discussions were extremely valuable, even though Aronson and
Mills had previously rehearsed the other group members to be as
“worthless and uninteresting” as possible. Different coeds, who went
through a much milder initiation ceremony or went through no initiation
at all, were decidedly less positive about the “worthless” new group
they had joined. Additional research showed the same results when
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 69
coeds were required to endure pain rather than embarrassment to get
into a group. The more electric shock a woman received as part of the
initiation ceremony, the more she later persuaded herself that her new
group and its activities were interesting, intelligent, and desirable.15
Now the harassments, the exertions, even the beatings of initiation
rituals begin to make sense. The Thonga tribesman watching, with tears
in his eyes, his ten-year-old son tremble through a night on the cold
ground of the “yard of mysteries,” the college sophomore punctuating
his Hell Night paddling of his fraternity “little brother” with bursts of
nervous laughter—these are not acts of sadism. They are acts of group
survival. They function, oddly enough, to spur future society members
to find the group more attractive and worthwhile. As long as it is the
case that people like and believe in what they have struggled to get,
these groups will continue to arrange effortful and troublesome initiation
rites. The loyalty and dedication of those who emerge will increase to
a great degree the chances of group cohesiveness and survival. Indeed,
one study of fifty-four tribal cultures found that those with the most
dramatic and stringent initiation ceremonies were those with the greatest
group solidarity.16 Given Aronson and Mills’s demonstration that the
severity of an initiation ceremony significantly heightens the newcomer’s
commitment to the group, it is hardly surprising that groups will oppose
all attempts to eliminate this crucial link to their future strength.
Military groups and organizations are by no means exempt from
these same processes. The agonies of “boot camp” initiations to the
armed services are legendary. The novelist William Styron, a former
Marine, catalogs his own experiences in language we could easily apply
to the Thongas (or, for that matter, to the Kappas or Betas or Alphas):
“the remorseless close-order drill hour after hour in the burning sun,
the mental and physical abuse, the humiliations, the frequent sadism
at the hands of drill sergeants, all the claustrophobic and terrifying insults to the spirit which can make an outpost like Quantico or Parris
Island one of the closest things in the free world to a concentration
camp.” But, in his commentary, Styron does more than recount the
misery of this “training nightmare”—he recognizes its intended outcome: “There is no ex-Marine of my acquaintance, regardless of what
direction he may have taken spiritually or politically after those callow
gung-ho days, who does not view the training as a crucible out of which
he emerged in some way more resilient, simply braver and better for
the wear.”
But why should we believe William Styron, the writer, in such matters? After all, for professional storytellers, the line between truth and
fiction is often blurred. Indeed, why should we believe him when he
alleges that the “infernal” character of his military training was not only
70 / Influence
successful, it was specifically intended, intended to create desired levels
of pride and camaraderie among those who endured and survived it?
At least one reason to accept his assessment comes from unfictionalized
reality—the case of West Point cadet John Edwards, who was expelled
from the U.S. Military Academy in 1988 on charges involving the authorized hazing that all first-year cadets experience at the hands of
upperclassmen to ensure that the newcomers can withstand the rigors
of West Point training. It was not that Mr. Edwards, who ranked academically near the top of his eleven-hundred-member class, had been
unable to bear up under the ritual when he was subjected to it. Nor was
he expelled because he had been aberrantly cruel in his treatment of
the younger cadets. His offense was that he would not expose the
newcomers to what he felt was “absurd and dehumanizing” treatment.
Once again, then, it appears that, for groups concerned about creating
a lasting sense of solidarity and distinction, the hardship of demanding
initiation activities provides a valuable advantage that they will not
easily surrender—either to aspiring members who are unwilling to take
the harshness or to give it out.
The Inner Choice
Examination of such diverse activities as the indoctrination practices
of the Chinese Communists and the initiation rituals of college fraternities has provided some valuable information about commitment. It
appears that commitments are most effective in changing a person’s
self-image and future behavior when they are active, public, and effortful. But there is another property of effective commitment that is more
important than the other three combined. To understand what it is, we
first need to solve a pair of puzzles in the actions of Communist interrogators and fraternity brothers.
The first puzzle comes from the refusal of fraternity chapters to allow
public-service activities to be part of their initiation ceremonies. Recall
that one survey showed that community projects, though frequent,
were nearly always separated from the membership-induction program.
But why? If an effortful commitment is what fraternities are after in
their initiation rites, surely they could structure enough distasteful and
strenuous civic activities for their pledges; there is plenty of exertion
and unpleasantness to be had in the world of old-age-home repairs,
mental-health-center yard work, and hospital bedpan duty. Besides,
community-spirited endeavors of this sort would do much to improve
the highly unfavorable public and media image of fraternity Hell Week
rites; a survey showed that for every positive newspaper story concerning Hell Week, there were five negative stories. If only for public-rela-
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 71
tions reasons, then, fraternities should want to incorporate communityservice efforts into their initiation practices. But they don’t.
To examine the second puzzle, we need to return to the Chinese
prison camps of Korea and the regular political essay contests held for
American captives. The Chinese wanted as many Americans as possible
to enter these contests so that, in the process, they might write things
favorable to the Communist view. If, however, the idea was to attract
large numbers of entrants, why were the prizes so small? A few extra
cigarettes or a little fresh fruit were often all that a contest winner could
expect. In the setting, even these prizes were valuable, but still there
were much larger rewards—warm clothing, special mail privileges,
increased freedom of movement in camp—that the Chinese could have
used to increase the number of essay writers. Yet they specifically chose
to employ the smaller rather than the larger, more motivating rewards.
Although the settings are quite different, the surveyed fraternities
refused to allow civic activities into their initiation ceremonies for the
same reason that the Chinese withheld large prizes in favor of less
powerful inducements: They wanted the men to own what they had
done. No excuses, no ways out were allowed. A man who suffered
through an arduous hazing could not be given the chance to believe he
did so for charitable purposes. A prisoner who salted his political essay
with a few anti-American comments could not be permitted to shrug
it off as motivated by a big reward. No, the fraternity chapters and
Chinese Communists were playing for keeps. It was not enough to
wring commitments out of their men; those men had to be made to take
inner responsibility for their actions.
Given the Chinese Communist government’s affinity for the politicalessay contest as a commitment device, it should come as no surprise
that a wave of such contests appeared in the aftermath of the 1989
massacre in Tiananmen Square, where pro-democracy protesters were
gunned down by government soldiers. In Beijing alone, nine state-run
newspapers and television stations sponsored essay competitions on
the “quelling of the counterrevolutionary rebellion.” Still acting in accord
with its long-standing and insightful de-emphasis of rewards for public
commitments, the Beijing government left the contest prizes unspecified.
Social scientists have determined that we accept inner responsibility
for a behavior when we think we have chosen to perform it in the absence of strong outside pressures. A large reward is one such external
pressure. It may get us to perform a certain action, but it won’t get us
to accept inner responsibility for the act. Consequently, we won’t feel
committed to it. The same is true of a strong threat; it may motivate
immediate compliance, but it is unlikely to produce long-term commitment.
72 / Influence
All this has important implications for rearing children. It suggests
that we should never heavily bribe or threaten our children to do the
things we want them truly to believe in. Such pressures will probably
produce temporary compliance with our wishes. However, if we want
more than just that, if we want the children to believe in the correctness
of what they have done, if we want them to continue to perform the
desired behavior when we are not present to apply those outside pressures, then we must somehow arrange for them to accept inner responsibility for the actions we want them to take. An experiment by Jonathan
Freedman gives us some hints about what to do and what not to do in
this regard.
Freedman wanted to see if he could prevent second- to fourth-grade
boys from playing with a fascinating toy, just because he had said that
it was wrong to do so some six weeks earlier. Anyone familiar with
seven-to-nine-year-old boys must realize the enormity of the task. But
Freedman had a plan. If he could first get the boys to convince themselves that it was wrong to play with the forbidden toy, perhaps that
belief would keep them from playing with it thereafter. The difficult
thing was making the boys believe that it was wrong to amuse themselves with the toy—an extremely expensive, battery-controlled robot.
Freedman knew it would be easy enough to have a boy obey temporarily. All he had to do was threaten the boy with severe consequences
should he be caught playing with the toy. As long as he was nearby to
deal out stiff punishment, Freedman figured that few boys would risk
operating the robot. He was right. After showing a boy an array of five
toys and warning him, “It is wrong to play with the robot. If you play
with the robot, I’ll be very angry and will have to do something about
it,” Freedman left the room for a few minutes. During that time, the
boy was observed secretly through a one-way mirror. Freedman tried
this threat procedure on twenty-two different boys, and twenty-one of
them never touched the robot while he was gone.
So a strong threat was successful while the boys thought they might
be caught and punished. But Freedman had already guessed that. He
was really interested in the effectiveness of the threat in guiding the
boys’ behavior later on, when he was no longer around. To find out
what would happen then, he sent a young woman back to the boys’
school about six weeks after he had been there. She took the boys out
of the class one at a time to participate in an experiment. Without ever
mentioning any connection with Freedman, she escorted each boy back
to the room with the five toys and gave him a drawing test. While she
was scoring the test, she told the boy that he was free to play with any
toy in the room. Of course, almost all the boys played with a toy. The
interesting result was that, of the boys playing with a toy, 77 percent
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 73
chose to play with the robot that had been forbidden to them earlier.
Freedman’s severe threat, which had been so successful six weeks before,
was almost totally unsuccessful when he was no longer able to back it
up with punishment.
But Freedman wasn’t finished yet. He changed his procedure slightly
with a second sample of boys. These boys, too, were initially shown the
array of five toys by Freedman and warned not to play with the robot
while he was briefly out of the room because “It is wrong to play with
the robot.” But this time, Freedman provided no strong threat to
frighten a boy into obedience. He simply left the room and observed
through the one-way mirror to see if his instruction against playing
with the forbidden toy was enough. It was. Just as with the other sample,
only one of the twenty-two boys touched the robot during the short
time Freedman was gone.
The real difference between the two samples of boys came six weeks
later, when they had a chance to play with the toys while Freedman
was no longer around. An astonishing thing happened with the boys
who had earlier been given no strong threat against playing with the
robot: When given the freedom to play with any toy they wished, most
avoided the robot, even though it was by far the most attractive of the
five toys available (the others were a cheap plastic submarine, a child’s
baseball glove without a ball, an unloaded toy rifle, and a toy tractor).
When these boys played with one of the five toys, only 33 percent chose
the robot.
Something dramatic had happened to both groups of boys. For the
first group, it was the severe threat they heard from Freedman to back
up his statement that playing with the robot was “wrong.” It had been
quite effective at first when Freedman could catch them should they
violate his rule. Later, though, when he was no longer present to observe
the boys’ behavior, his threat was impotent and his rule was, consequently, ignored. It seems clear that the threat had not taught the
boys that operating the robot was wrong, only that it was unwise to do
so when the possibility of punishment existed.
For the other boys, the dramatic event had come from the inside, not
the outside. Freedman had instructed them, too, that playing with the
robot was wrong, but he had added no threat of punishment should
they disobey him. There were two important results. First, Freedman’s
instruction alone was enough to prevent the boys from operating the
robot while he was briefly out of the room. Second, the boys took personal responsibility for their choice to stay away from the robot during
that time. They decided that they hadn’t played with it because they
didn’t want to. After all, there were no strong punishments associated
with the toy to explain their behavior otherwise. Thus, weeks later,
74 / Influence
when Freedman was nowhere around, they still ignored the robot because they had been changed inside to believe that they did not want
to play with it.17
Adults facing the child-rearing experience can take a cue from the
Freedman study. Suppose a couple wants to impress upon their
daughter that lying is wrong. A strong, clear threat (“It’s bad to lie,
honey; so if I catch you at it, I’ll cut your tongue out”) might well be
effective when the parents are present or when the girl thinks she can
be discovered. But it will not achieve the larger goal of convincing her
that she does not want to lie because she thinks it’s wrong. To do that,
a much subtler approach is required. A reason must be given that is
just strong enough to get her to be truthful most of the time but is not
so strong that she sees it as the obvious reason for her truthfulness. It’s
a tricky business, because exactly what this barely sufficient reason will
be changes from child to child. For one little girl, a simple appeal may
be enough (“It’s bad to lie, honey; so I hope you won’t do it”); for another child, it may be necessary to add a somewhat stronger reason
(“…because if you do, I’ll be disappointed in you”); for a third child, a
mild form of warning may be required as well (“…and I’ll probably
have to do something I don’t want to do”). Wise parents will know
which kind of reason will work on their own children. The important
thing is to use a reason that will initially produce the desired behavior
and will, at the same time, allow a child to take personal responsibility
for that behavior. Thus, the less detectable outside pressure such a
reason contains, the better. Selecting just the right reason is not an easy
task for parents. But the effort should pay off. It is likely to mean the
difference between short-lived compliance and long-term commitment.
For a pair of reasons we have already talked about, compliance professionals love commitments that produce inner change. First, that
change is not just specific to the situation where it first occurred; it
covers a whole range of related situations, too. Second, the effects of
the change are lasting. So, once a man has been induced to take action
that shifts his self-image to that of, let’s say, a public-spirited citizen,
he is likely to be public-spirited in a variety of other circumstances
where his compliance may also be desired, and he is likely to continue
his public-spirited behavior for as long as his new self-image holds.
There is yet another attraction in commitments that lead to inner
change—they grow their own legs. There is no need for the compliance
professional to undertake a costly and continuing effort to reinforce the
change; the pressure for consistency will take care of all that. After our
friend comes to view himself as a public-spirited citizen, he will automatically begin to see things differently. He will convince himself that
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 75
it is the correct way to be. He will begin to pay attention to facts he
hadn’t noticed before about the value of community service. He will
make himself available to hear arguments he hadn’t heard before favoring civic action. And he will find such arguments more persuasive than
before. In general, because of the need to be consistent within his system
of beliefs, he will assure himself that his choice to take public-spirited
action was right. What is important about this process of generating
additional reasons to justify the commitment is that the reasons are new.
Thus, even if the original reason for the civic-minded behavior was
taken away, these newly discovered reasons might be enough by
themselves to support his perception that he had behaved correctly.
The advantage to an unscrupulous compliance professional is tremendous. Because we build new struts to undergird choices we have
committed ourselves to, an exploitative individual can offer us an inducement for making such a choice, and after the decision has been
made, can remove that inducement, knowing that our decision will
probably stand on its own newly created legs. New-car dealers frequently try to benefit from this process through a trick they call
“throwing a lowball.” I first encountered the tactic while posing as a
sales trainee at a local Chevrolet dealership. After a week of basic instruction, I was allowed to watch the regular salesmen perform. One
practice that caught my attention right away was the lowball.
For certain customers, a very good price is offered on a car, perhaps
as much as four hundred dollars below competitors’ prices. The good
deal, however, is not genuine; the dealer never intends it to go through.
Its only purpose is to cause a prospect to decide to buy one of the dealership’s cars. Once the decision is made, a number of activities develop
the customer’s sense of personal commitment to the car—a raft of purchase forms are filled out, extensive financing terms are arranged,
sometimes the customer is encouraged to drive the car for a day before
signing the contract “so you can get the feel of it and show it around
in the neighborhood and at work.” During this time, the dealer knows,
customers automatically develop a range of new reasons to support the
choice they have now made.
Then something happens. Occasionally an “error” in the calculations
is discovered—maybe the salesman forgot to add in the cost of the air
conditioner, and if the buyer still requires air conditioning, four hundred
dollars must be added to the price. To keep from being suspected of
gouging by the customer, some dealers let the bank handling the financing find the mistake. At other times, the deal is disallowed at the last
moment when the salesman checks with his boss, who cancels it because
“We’d be losing money.” For only another four hundred dollars the
car can be had, which, in the context of a multithousand-dollar deal,
76 / Influence
doesn’t seem too steep since, as the salesman emphasizes, the cost is
equal to competitors’ and “This is the car you chose, right?” Another,
even more insidious form of lowballing occurs when the salesman
makes an inflated trade-in offer on the prospect’s old car as part of the
buy/trade package. The customer recognizes the offer as overly generous and jumps at the deal. Later, before the contract is signed, the usedcar manager says that the salesman’s estimate was four hundred dollars
too high and reduces the trade-in allowance to its actual, blue-book
level. The customer, realizing that the reduced offer is the fair one, accepts it as appropriate and sometimes feels guilty about trying to take
advantage of the salesman’s high estimate. I once witnessed a woman
provide an embarrassed apology to a salesman who had used the last
version of lowballing on her—this while she was signing a new-car
contract giving him a huge commission. He looked hurt but managed
a forgiving smile.
No matter which variety of lowballing is used, the sequence is the
same: An advantage is offered that induces a favorable purchase decision; then, sometime after the decision has been made but before the
bargain is sealed, the original purchase advantage is deftly removed.
It seems almost incredible that a customer would buy a car under these
circumstances. Yet it works—not on everybody, of course, but it is effective enough to be a staple compliance procedure in many, many car
showrooms. Automobile dealers have come to understand the ability
of a personal commitment to build its own support system, a support
system of new justifications for the commitment. Often these justifications provide so many strong legs for the decision to stand on that when
the dealer pulls away only one leg, the original one, there is no collapse.
The loss can be shrugged off by the customer who is consoled, even
made happy, by the array of other good reasons favoring the choice. It
never occurs to the buyer that those additional reasons might never
have existed had the choice not been made in the first place.18
The impressive thing about the lowball tactic is its ability to make a
person feel pleased with a poor choice. Those who have only poor
choices to offer us, then, are especially fond of the technique. We can
find them throwing lowballs in business, social, and personal situations.
For instance, there’s my neighbor Tim, a true lowball aficionado. Recall
that he’s the one who, by promising to change his ways, got his girlfriend, Sara, to cancel her impending marriage to another and to take
him back. Since her decision for Tim, Sara has become more devoted
to him than ever, even though he has not fulfilled his promises. She
explains this by saying that she has allowed herself to see all sorts of
positive qualities in Tim she had never recognized before.
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 77
I know full well that Sara is a lowball victim. Just as sure as I had
watched buyers fall for the give-it-and-take-it-away-later strategy in
the car showroom, I watched her fall for the same trick with Tim. For
his part, Tim remains the guy he has always been. But because the new
attractions Sara has discovered (or created) in him are quite real for her,
she now seems satisfied with the same arrangement that was unacceptable before her enormous commitment. The decision to choose Tim,
poor as it may have been objectively, has grown its own supports and
appears to have made Sara genuinely happy. I have never mentioned
to Sara what I know about lowballing. The reason for my silence is not
that I think her better off in the dark on the issue. As a general guiding
principle, more information is always better than less information. It’s
just that, if I said a word, I am confident she would hate me for it.
Depending on the motives of the person wishing to use them, any of
the compliance techniques discussed in this book can be employed for
good or for ill. It should not be surprising, then, that the lowball tactic
can be used for more socially beneficial purposes than selling new cars
or reestablishing relationships with former lovers. One research project
done in Iowa, for example, shows how the lowball procedure can influence homeowners to conserve energy.19 The project, headed by Dr.
Michael Pallak, began at the start of the Iowa winter when residents
who heated their homes with natural gas were contacted by an interviewer. The interviewer gave them some energy-conservation tips and
asked them to try to save fuel in the future. Although they all agreed
to try, when the researchers examined the utility records of these families after a month and again at winter’s end, it was clear that no real
savings had occurred. The residents who had promised to make a
conservation attempt used just as much natural gas as a random sample
of their neighbors who had not been contacted by an interviewer. Just
good intentions coupled with information about saving fuel, then, were
not enough to change habits.
Even before the project began, Pallak and his research team had recognized that something more would be needed to shift long-standing
energy patterns. So they tried a slightly different procedure on a comparable sample of Iowa natural-gas users. These people, too, were
contacted by an interviewer, who provided energy-saving hints and
asked them to conserve. But for these families, the interviewer offered
something else: Those residents agreeing to save energy would have
their names publicized in newspaper articles as public-spirited, fuelconserving citizens. The effect was immediate. One month later, when
the utility companies checked their meters, the homeowners in this
sample had saved an average of 422 cubic feet of natural gas apiece.
The chance to have their names in the paper had motivated these residents to substantial conservation efforts for a period of a month.
Then the rug was pulled out. The researchers extracted the reason
that had initially caused these people to save fuel. Each family that had
been promised publicity received a letter saying it would not be possible
to publicize their names after all.
At the end of the winter, the research team examined the effect that
letter had had on the natural-gas usage of the families. Did they return
to their old, wasteful habits when the chance to be in the newspaper
was removed? Hardly. For each of the remaining winter months, they
actually conserved more fuel than they had during the time they thought
they would be publicly celebrated for it! In terms of percentage of energy
savings, they had managed a 12.2 percent first-month gas savings be-
84 / Influence
clearly recognizing it, without having a massive heart of hearts attack?
There is no telling. One thing is certain, however: As time passes, the
various alternatives to Tim are disappearing. She had better determine
soon whether she is making a mistake.
Easier said than done, of course. She must answer an extremely intricate question: “Knowing what I now know, if I could go back in time,
would I make the same choice?” The problem lies in the “Knowing
what I now know” part of the question. Just what does she now know,
accurately, about Tim? How much of what she thinks of him is the
result of a desperate attempt to justify the commitment she made? She
claims that since her decision to take him back, he cares for her more,
is trying hard to stop his excessive drinking, has learned to make a
wonderful omelet, etc. Having tasted a couple of his omelets, I have
my doubts. The important issue, though, is whether she believes these
things, not just intellectually—we can play such mind games on
ourselves—but in her heart of hearts.
There may be a little device Sara can use to find out how much of her
current satisfaction with Tim is real and how much is foolish consistency. Accumulating psychological evidence indicates that we experience
our feelings toward something a split second before we can intellectualize about it.21 My suspicion is that the message sent by the heart of
hearts is a pure, basic feeling. Therefore, if we train ourselves to be attentive, we should register it ever so slightly before our cognitive apparatus engages. According to this approach, were Sara to ask herself the
crucial “Would I make the same choice again?” question, she would be
well advised to look for and trust the first flash of feeling she experienced in response. It would likely be the signal from her heart of hearts,
slipping through undistorted just before the means by which she could
kid herself flooded in.22
I have begun using the same device myself whenever I even suspect
I might be acting in a foolishly consistent manner. One time, for instance,
I had stopped at the self-service pump of a filling station advertising a
price per gallon a couple of cents below the rate of other stations in the
area. But with pump nozzle in hand, I noticed that the price listed on
the pump was two cents higher than the display sign price. When I
mentioned the difference to a passing attendant, who I later learned
was the owner, he mumbled uncon-vincingly that the rates had changed
a few days ago but there hadn’t been time to correct the display. I tried
to decide what to do. Some reasons for staying came to mind—“I really
do need gasoline badly.” “This pump is available, and I am in sort of
a hurry.” “I think I remember that my car runs better on this brand of
gas.”
I needed to determine whether those reasons were genuine or mere
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 85
justifications for my decision to stop there. So I asked myself the crucial
question, “Knowing what I know about the real price of this gasoline,
if I could go back in time, would I make the same choice again?” Concentrating on the first burst of impression I sensed, the answer was
clear and unqualified. I would have driven right past. I wouldn’t even
have slowed down. I knew then that without the price advantage, those
other reasons would not have brought me there. They hadn’t created
the decision; the decision had created them.
That settled, there was another decision to be faced, though. Since I
was already there holding the hose, wouldn’t it be better to use it than
to suffer the inconvenience of going elsewhere to pay the same price?
Fortunately, the station attendant-owner came over and helped me
make up my mind. He asked why I wasn’t pumping any gas. I told him
I didn’t like the price discrepancy, and he said with a snarl, “Listen,
nobody’s gonna tell me how to run my business. If you think I’m
cheating you, just put that hose down right now and get off my property
as fast as you can do it, bud.” Already certain he was a cheat, I was
happy to act consistently with my belief and his wishes. I dropped the
hose on the spot…and drove over it on my way to the closest exit.
Sometimes consistency can be a marvelously rewarding thing.
READER’S REPORT
From a Woman Living in Portland, Oregon
“I was walking through downtown Portland on my way to a lunch
appointment when a young, attractive man stopped me with a friendly
smile and a powerful line: ‘Excuse me, I’m involved in a contest and I
need a good-looking woman like yourself to help me win.’ I was truly
skeptical, since I know there are many more attractive women than
myself running around; however, I was caught off guard and was
curious to find out what he wanted. He explained that he would receive
points for a contest by getting total strangers to give him a kiss. Now I
consider myself a fairly level-headed person who shouldn’t have believed his line, but he was quite persistent, and since I was almost late
for my lunch appointment, I thought, ‘What the heck, I’ll give the guy
a kiss and get out of here.’ So I did something totally against my common sense and pecked this total stranger on the cheek in the middle of
downtown Portland!
“I thought that would be the end of it, but I soon learned that it was
just the beginning. Much to my distress, he followed the kiss with the
line ‘You are a great kisser, but the real contest I am involved in is to
sell magazine subscriptions. You must be an active person. Would any
of these magazines interest you?’ At this point I should have slugged
86 / Influence
the guy and walked away; but somehow, because I had complied with
his initial request, I felt a need to be consistent, and I complied with his
second request. Yes, much to my own disbelief, I actually subscribed
to SKI magazine (which I occasionally enjoy reading, but had no intention of subscribing to), gave him a five-dollar initial-subscription fee
and left as quickly as possible, feeling quite frustrated with what I had
just done and not understanding why I had done it.
“Although it still pains me to think about it, in reflecting on the incident after reading your book, I’ve now figured out what happened. The
reason this tactic worked so effectively is because once small commitments have been made (in this case, giving a kiss), people tend to add
justifications to support the commitment and then are willing to commit
themselves further. In this situation, I justified complying with the
second request because it was consistent with my initial action. If I had
only listened to my ‘stomach signs,’ I could have saved myself a lot of
humiliation.”
By extracting a kiss, the salesman exploited the consistency principle
in two ways. First, by the time he asked for her aid in the magazine
contest, his prospect had already gone on record—with that kiss—as
agreeing to help him win a contest. Second, it seems only natural (i.e.,
congruent) that if a woman feels positively enough toward a man to
kiss him, she should feel positively toward helping him out.
Chapter 4
SOCIAL PROOF
Truths Are Us
Where all think alike, no one thinks very much.
—WALTER LIPPMANN
, when
I I surveyed the people who came into my office one day—several
DON’T KNOW ANYONE WHO LIKES CANNED LAUGHTER. IN FACT
students, two telephone repairmen, a number of university professors,
and the janitor—the reaction was invariably critical. Television, with
its incessant system of laugh tracks and technically augmented mirth,
received the most heat. The people I questioned hated canned laughter.
They called it stupid, phony, and obvious. Although my sample was
small, I would bet that it closely reflects the negative feelings of most
of the American public toward laugh tracks.
Why, then, is canned laughter so popular with television executives?
They have won their exalted positions and splendid salaries by knowing
how to give the public what it wants. Yet they religiously employ the
laugh tracks that their audiences find distasteful. And they do so over
the objections of many of their most talented artists. It is not uncommon
for acclaimed directors, writers, or actors to demand the elimination of
canned responses from the television projects they undertake. These
demands are only sometimes successful, and when they are, it is not
without a battle.
What could it be about canned laughter that is so attractive to television executives? Why would these shrewd and tested businessmen
champion a practice that their potential watchers find disagreeable and
their most creative talents find personally insulting? The answer is at
88 / Influence
once simple and intriguing: They know what the research says. Experiments have found that the use of canned merriment causes an audience
to laugh longer and more often when humorous material is presented
and to rate the material as funnier. In addition, some evidence indicates
that canned laughter is most effective for poor jokes.1
In the light of these data, the actions of television executives make
perfect sense. The introduction of laugh tracks into their comic programming will increase the humorous and appreciative responses of an
audience, even—and especially—when the material is of poor quality.
Is it any surprise, then, that television, glutted as it is with artless situation-comedy attempts, should be saturated with canned laughter?
Those executives know precisely what they are doing.
But with the mystery of the widespread use of laugh tracks solved,
we are left with a more perplexing question: Why does canned laughter
work on us the way it does? It is no longer the television executives
who appear peculiar; they are acting logically and in their own interests.
Instead, it is the behavior of the audience, of you and me, that seems
strange. Why should we laugh more at comedy material afloat in a sea
of mechanically fabricated merriment? And why should we think that
comic flotsam funnier? The executives aren’t really fooling us. Anyone
can recognize dubbed laughter. It is so blatant, so clearly counterfeit,
that there could be no confusing it with the real thing. We know full
well that the hilarity we hear is irrelevant to the humorous quality of
the joke it follows, that it is created not spontaneously by a genuine
audience, but artificially by a technician at a control board. Yet, transparent forgery that it is, it works on us!
To discover why canned laughter is so effective, we first need to understand the nature of yet another potent weapon of influence: the
principle of social proof. It states that one means we use to determine
what is correct is to find out what other people think is correct. The
principle applies especially to the way we decide what constitutes correct behavior. We view a behavior as more correct in a given situation
to the degree that we see others performing it. Whether the question is
what to do with an empty popcorn box in a movie theater, how fast to
drive on a certain stretch of highway, or how to eat the chicken at a
dinner party, the actions of those around us will be important in defining
the answer.
The tendency to see an action as more appropriate when others are
doing it normally works quite well. As a rule, we will make fewer
mistakes by acting in accord with social evidence than contrary to it.
Usually, when a lot of people are doing something, it is the right thing
to do. This feature of the principle of social proof is simultaneously its
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 89
major strength and its major weakness. Like the other weapons of influence, it provides a convenient shortcut for determining how to behave
but, at the same time, makes one who uses the shortcut vulnerable to
the attacks of profiteers who lie in wait along its path.
In the case of canned laughter, the problem comes when we begin
responding to social proof in such a mindless and reflexive fashion that
we can be fooled by partial or fake evidence. Our folly is not that we
use others’ laughter to help decide what is humorous and when mirth
is appropriate; that is in keeping with the well-founded principle of
social proof. The folly is that we do so in response to patently fraudulent
laughter. Somehow, one disembodied feature of humor—a
sound—works like the essence of humor. The example from Chapter
1 of the turkey and the polecat is instructive here. Remember that because the particular “cheep-cheep” of turkey chicks is normally associated with newborn turkeys, their mothers will display or withhold
maternal care solely on the basis of that sound? And remember how,
consequently, it was possible to trick a female turkey into mothering a
stuffed polecat as long as the replica played the recorded “cheep-cheep”
of a baby turkey? The simulated chick sound was enough to start the
female’s mothering tape whirring.
The lesson of the turkey and the polecat illustrates uncomfortably
well the relationship between the average viewer and the laugh-trackplaying television executive. We have become so accustomed to taking
the humorous reactions of others as evidence of what deserves laughter
that we, too, can be made to respond to the sound and not to the substance of the real thing. Much as a “cheep-cheep” noise removed from
the reality of a chick can stimulate a female turkey to mother, so can a
recorded “ha-ha” removed from the reality of a genuine audience
stimulate us to laugh. The television executives are exploiting our
preference for shortcuts, our tendency to react automatically on the
basis of partial evidence. They know that their tapes will cue our tapes.
Click, whirr.
Television executives are hardly alone in their use of social evidence
for profit. Our tendency to assume that an action is more correct if
others are doing it is exploited in a variety of settings. Bartenders often
“salt” their tip jars with a few dollar bills at the beginning of the evening
to simulate tips left by prior customers and thereby to give the impression that tipping with folding money is proper barroom behavior.
Church ushers sometimes salt collection baskets for the same reason
and with the same positive effect on proceeds. Evangelical preachers
are known to seed their audience with “ringers,” who are rehearsed to
come forward at a specified time to give witness and donations. For
90 / Influence
example, an Arizona State University research team that infiltrated the
Billy Graham organization reported on such advance preparations
prior to one of his Crusade visits. “By the time Graham arrives in town
and makes his altar call, an army of six thousand wait with instructions
on when to come forth at varying intervals to create the impression of
a spontaneous mass outpouring.”2
Advertisers love to inform us when a product is the “fastest-growing”
or “largest-selling” because they don’t have to convince us directly that
the product is good, they need only say that many others think so,
which seems proof enough. The producers of charity telethons devote
inordinate amounts of time to the incessant listing of viewers who have
already pledged contributions. The message being communicated to
the holdouts is clear: “Look at all the people who have decided to give.
It must be the correct thing to do.” At the height of the disco craze, certain discotheque owners manufactured a brand of visible social proof
for their clubs’ quality by creating long waiting lines outside when there
was plenty of room inside. Salesmen are taught to spice their pitches
with numerous accounts of individuals who have purchased the
product. Sales and motivation consultant Cavett Robert captures the
principle nicely in his advice to sales trainees: “Since 95 percent of the
people are imitators and only 5 percent initiators, people are persuaded
more by the actions of others than by any proof we can offer.”
Researchers, too, have employed procedures based on the principle
of social proof—sometimes with astounding results. One psychologist
in particular, Albert Bandura, has led the way in developing such procedures for the elimination of undesirable behavior. Bandura and his
colleagues have shown how people suffering from phobias can be rid
of these extreme fears in an amazingly simple fashion. For instance, in
an early study nursery-school-age children chosen because they were
terrified of dogs merely watched a little boy playing happily with a dog
for twenty minutes a day. This exhibition produced such marked
changes in the reactions of the fearful children that after only four days,
67 percent of them were willing to climb into a playpen with a dog and
remain confined there, petting and scratching it while everyone else
left the room. Moreover, when the researchers tested the children’s fear
levels again one month later, they found that the improvement had not
evaporated during that time; in fact, the children were more willing
than ever to interact with dogs.
An important practical discovery was made in a second study of
children who were exceptionally afraid of dogs: To reduce their fears,
it was not necessary to provide live demonstrations of another child
playing with a dog; film clips had the same effect. And the most effective
type of clips were those depicting not one but a variety of other children
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 91
interacting with their dogs; apparently the principle of social proof
works best when the proof is provided by the actions of a lot of other
people.3
The powerful influence of filmed examples in changing the behavior
of children can be used as therapy for various problems. Some striking
evidence is available in the research of psychologist Robert O’Connor
on socially withdrawn preschool children. We have all seen children
of this sort, terribly shy, standing alone at the fringes of the games and
groupings of their peers. O’Connor worried that a long-term pattern
of isolation was forming, even at an early age, that would create persistent difficulties in social comfort and adjustment through adulthood. In
an attempt to reverse the pattern, O’Connor made a film containing
eleven different scenes in a nursery-school setting. Each scene began
by showing a different solitary child watching some ongoing social
activity and then actively joining the activity, to everyone’s enjoyment.
O’Connor selected a group of the most severely withdrawn children
from four preschools and showed them his film. The impact was impressive. The isolates immediately began to interact with their peers at
a level equal to that of the normal children in the schools. Even more
astonishing was what O’Connor found when he returned to observe
six weeks later. While the withdrawn children who had not seen
O’Connor’s film remained as isolated as ever, those who had viewed it
were now leading their schools in amount of social activity. It seems
that this twenty-three-minute movie, viewed just once, was enough to
reverse a potential pattern of lifelong maladaptive behavior. Such is
the potency of the principle of social proof.4
When it comes to illustrations of the strength of social proof, there is
one that is far and away my favorite. Several features account for its
appeal: It offers a superb example of the much underused method of
participant observation, in which a scientist studies a process by becoming immersed in its natural occurrence; it provides information of interest to such diverse groups as historians, psychologists, and theologians; and, most important, it shows how social evidence can be used
on us—not by others, but by ourselves—to assure us that what we
prefer to be true will seem to be true.
The story is an old one, requiring an examination of ancient data, for
the past is dotted with millennial religious movements. Various sects
and cults have prophesied that on one or another particular date there
would arrive a period of redemption and great happiness for those who
believed in the group’s teachings. In each instance it has been predicted
that the beginning of the time of salvation would be marked by an important and undeniable event, usually the cataclysmic end of the world.
92 / Influence
Of course, these predictions have invariably proved false. To the acute
dismay of the members of such groups, the end has never appeared as
scheduled.
But immediately following the obvious failure of the prophecy, history
records an enigmatic pattern. Rather than disbanding in disillusion, the
cultists often become strengthened in their convictions. Risking the ridicule of the populace, they take to the streets, publicly asserting their
dogma and seeking converts with a fervor that is intensified, not diminished, by the clear disconfirmation of a central belief. So it was with the
Montanists of second-century Turkey, with the Anabaptists of sixteenthcentury Holland, with the Sabbataists of seventeenth-century Izmir,
with the Millerites of nineteenth-century America. And, thought a trio
of interested social scientists, so it might be with a doomsday cult based
in modern-day Chicago. The scientists—Leon Festinger, Henry Riecken,
and Stanley Schachter—who were then colleagues at the University of
Minnesota, heard about the Chicago group and felt it worthy of close
study. Their decision to investigate by joining the group, incognito, as
new believers and by placing additional paid observers among its ranks
resulted in a remarkably rich firsthand account of the goings-on before
and after the day of predicted catastrophe.5
The cult of believers was small, never numbering more than thirty
members. Its leaders were a middle-aged man and woman, whom the
researchers renamed, for purposes of publication, Dr. Thomas Armstrong and Mrs. Marian Keech. Dr. Armstrong, a physician on the staff
of a college student health service, had a long-held interest in mysticism,
the occult, and flying saucers; as such he served as a respected authority
on these subjects for the group. Mrs. Keech, though, was the center of
attention and activity. Earlier in the year she had begun to receive
messages from spiritual beings, whom she called the Guardians, located
on other planets. It was these messages, flowing through Marian Keech’s
hand via the device of “automatic writing,” that were to form the bulk
of the cult’s religious belief system. The teachings of the Guardians
were loosely linked to traditional Christian thought. No wonder that
one of the Guardians, Sananda, eventually “revealed” himself as the
current embodiment of Jesus.
The transmissions from the Guardians, always the subjects of much
discussion and interpretation among the group, gained new significance
when they began to foretell a great impending disaster—a flood that
would begin in the Western Hemisphere and eventually engulf the
world. Although the cultists were understandably alarmed at first,
further messages assured them that they and all those who believed in
the Lessons sent through Mrs. Keech would survive. Before the calamity,
spacemen were to arrive and carry off the believers in flying saucers to
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 93
a place of safety, presumably on another planet. Very little detail was
provided about the rescue except that the believers were to make
themselves ready for pickup by rehearsing certain passwords to be exchanged (“I left my hat at home.” “What is your question?” “I am my
own porter.”) and by removing all metal from their clothes—because
the wearing or carrying of metal made saucer travel “extremely dangerous.”
As Festinger, Riecken, and Schachter observed the preparations
during the weeks prior to the flood date, they noted with special interest
two significant aspects of the members’ behavior. First, the level of
commitment to the cult’s belief system was very high. In anticipation
of their departure from doomed Earth, irrevocable steps were taken by
the group members. Most had incurred the opposition of family and
friends to their beliefs but had persisted nonetheless in their convictions,
often when it meant losing the affections of these others. In fact, several
of the members were threatened by neighbors or family with legal actions designed to have them declared insane. In Dr. Armstrong’s case,
a motion was filed by his sister to have his two younger children taken
away. Many believers quit their jobs or neglected their studies to devote
full time to the movement. Some even gave or threw away their personal
belongings, expecting them shortly to be of no use. These were people
whose certainty that they had the truth allowed them to withstand
enormous social, economic, and legal pressures and whose commitment
to their dogma grew as each pressure was resisted.
The second significant aspect of the believers’ preflood actions was
a curious form of inaction. For individuals so clearly convinced of the
validity of their creed, they did surprisingly little to spread the word.
Although they did initially make public the news of the coming disaster,
there was no attempt to seek converts, to proselyte actively. They were
willing to sound the alarm and to counsel those who voluntarily responded to it, but that was all.
The group’s distaste for recruitment efforts was evident in various
ways besides the lack of personal persuasion attempts. Secrecy was
maintained in many matters—extra copies of the Lessons were burned,
passwords and secret signs were instituted, the contents of certain
private tape recordings were not to be discussed with outsiders (so
secret were the tapes that even longtime believers were prohibited from
taking notes of them). Publicity was avoided. As the day of disaster
approached, increasing numbers of newspaper, television, and radio
reporters converged on the group’s headquarters in the Keech house.
For the most part, these people were turned away or ignored. The most
frequent answer to their questions was, “No comment.” Although discouraged for a time, the media representatives returned with a ven-
94 / Influence
geance when Dr. Armstrong’s religious activities caused him to be fired
from his post on the college health service staff; one especially persistent
newsman had to be threatened with a lawsuit. A similar siege was repelled on the eve of the flood when a swarm of reporters pushed and
pestered the believers for information. Afterward, the researchers
summarized the group’s preflood stance on public exposure and recruitment in respectful tones: “Exposed to a tremendous burst of publicity,
they had made every attempt to dodge fame; given dozens of opportunities to proselyte, they had remained evasive and secretive and behaved
with an almost superior indifference.”
Eventually, when all the reporters and would-be converts had been
cleared from the house, the believers began making their final preparations for the arrival of the spaceship scheduled for midnight that night.
The scene, as viewed by Festinger, Riecken, and Schachter, must have
seemed like absurdist theater. Otherwise ordinary people—housewives,
college students, a high-school boy, a publisher, an M.D., a hardwarestore clerk and his mother—were participating earnestly in tragic
comedy. They took direction from a pair of members who were periodically in touch with the Guardians; Marian Keech’s written messages
from Sananda were being supplemented that evening by “the Bertha,”
a former beautician through whose tongue the “Creator” gave instruction. They rehearsed their lines diligently, calling out in chorus the responses to be made before entering the rescue saucer, “I am my own
porter.” “I am my own pointer.” They discussed seriously whether the
message from a caller identifying himself as Captain Video—a TV space
character of the time—was properly interpreted as a prank or a coded
communication from their rescuers. And they performed in costume.
In keeping with the admonition to carry nothing metallic aboard the
saucer, the believers wore clothing that had been cut open to allow the
metal pieces to be torn out. The metal eyelets in their shoes had been
ripped away. The women were braless or wore brassieres whose metal
stays had been removed. The men had yanked the zippers out of their
pants, which were supported by lengths of rope in place of belts.
The group’s fanaticism concerning the removal of all metal was
vividly experienced by one of the researchers who remarked, twentyfive minutes before midnight, that he had forgotten to extract the zipper
from his trousers. As the observers tell it, “this knowledge produced a
near panic reaction. He was rushed into the bedroom where Dr. Armstrong, his hands trembling and his eyes darting to the clock every few
seconds, slashed out the zipper with a razor blade and wrenched its
clasps free with wire-cutters.” The hurried operation finished, the researcher was returned to the living room a slightly less metallic but,
one supposes, much paler man.
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 95
As the time appointed for their departure grew very close, the believers settled into a lull of soundless anticipation. With trained scientists
on site, we are afforded a detailed account of the events that transpired
during this momentous period in the life of the group:
The last ten minutes were tense ones for the group in the living
room. They had nothing to do but sit and wait, their coats in their
laps. In the tense silence two clocks ticked loudly, one about ten
minutes faster than the other. When the faster of the two pointed
to twelve-five, one of the observers remarked aloud on the fact. A
chorus of people replied that midnight had not yet come. Bob
Eastman affirmed that the slower clock was correct; he had set it
himself only that afternoon. It showed only four minutes before
midnight.
These four minutes passed in complete silence except for a single
utterance. When the [slower] clock on the mantel showed only one
minute remaining before the guide to the saucer was due, Marian
exclaimed in a strained, high-pitched voice: “And not a plan has
gone astray!” The clock chimed twelve, each stroke painfully clear
in the expectant hush. The believers sat motionless.
One might have expected some visible reaction. Midnight had
passed and nothing had happened. The cataclysm itself was less
than seven hours away. But there was little to see in the reactions
of the people in that room. There was no talking, no sound. People
sat stock-still, their faces seemingly frozen and expressionless. Mark
Post was the only person who even moved. He lay down on the
sofa and closed his eyes but did not sleep. Later, when spoken to,
he answered monosyllabically but otherwise lay immobile. The
others showed nothing on the surface, although it became clear
later that they had been hit hard.
Gradually, painfully, an atmosphere of despair and confusion
settled over the group. They reexamined the prediction and the
accompanying messages. Dr. Armstrong and Mrs. Keech reiterated
their faith. The believers mulled over their predicament and discarded explanation after explanation as unsatisfactory. At one point,
toward 4 A.M., Mrs. Keech broke down and cried bitterly. She knew,
she sobbed, that there were some who were beginning to doubt
but that the group must beam light to those who needed it most
and that the group must hold together. The rest of the believers
were losing their composure, too. They were all visibly shaken and
many were close to tears. It was now almost 4:30 A.M., and still no
way of handling the disconfirmation had been found. By now, too,
96 / Influence
most of the group were talking openly about the failure of the escort
to come at midnight. The group seemed near dissolution.
In the midst of this gathering doubt, as cracks crawled through the
believers’ confidence, the researchers witnessed a pair of remarkable
incidents, one after another. The first occurred at about 4:45 A.M., when
Marian Keech’s hand suddenly leapt to the task of transcribing through
“automatic writing” the text of a holy message from above. When read
aloud, the communication proved to be an elegant explanation for the
events of that night. “The little group, sitting alone all night long, had
spread so much light that God had saved the world from destruction.”
Although neat and efficient, this explanation was not wholly satisfying
by itself; for example, after hearing it, one member simply rose, put on
his hat and coat, and left. Something additional was needed to restore
the believers to their previous levels of faith.
It was at this point that the second notable incident occurred to meet
that need. Once again, the words of those who were present offer a
vivid description:
The atmosphere in the group changed abruptly and so did their
behavior. Within minutes after she had read the message explaining
the disconfirmation, Mrs. Keech received another message instructing her to publicize the explanation. She reached for the telephone
and began dialing the number of a newspaper. While she was
waiting to be connected, someone asked: “Marian, is this the first
time you have called the newspaper yourself?” Her reply was immediate: “Oh, yes, this is the first time I have ever called them. I
have never had anything to tell them before, but now I feel it is
urgent.” The whole group could have echoed her feelings, for they
all felt a sense of urgency. As soon as Marian had finished her call,
the other members took turns telephoning newspapers, wire services, radio stations, and national magazines to spread the explanation of the failure of the flood. In their desire to spread the word
quickly and resoundingly, the believers now opened for public attention matters that had been thus far utterly secret. Where only
hours earlier they had shunned newspaper reporters and felt that
the attention they were getting in the press was painful, they now
became avid seekers for publicity.
Not only had the long-standing policies concerning secrecy and
publicity done an about-face, so, too, had the group’s attitude toward
potential converts. Whereas likely recruits who previously visited the
house had been mostly ignored, turned away, or treated with casual
attention, the day following the disconfirmation saw a different story.
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 97
All callers were admitted, all questions were answered, attempts were
made to proselyte all such visitors. The members’ unprecedented willingness to accommodate possible new recruits was perhaps best demonstrated when nine high-school students arrived on the following
night to speak with Mrs. Keech.
They found her at the telephone deep in a discussion of flying
saucers with a caller whom, it later turned out, she believed to be
a spaceman. Eager to continue talking to him and at the same time
anxious to keep her new guests, Marian simply included them in
the conversation and, for more than an hour, chatted alternately
with her guests in the living room and the “spaceman” on the
other end of the telephone. So intent was she on proselyting that
she seemed unable to let any opportunity go by.
To what can we attribute the believers’ radical turnabout? In the space
of a few hours, they went from clannish and taciturn hoarders of the
Word to expansive and eager disseminators of it. And what could have
possessed them to choose such an ill-timed instant—when the failure
of the flood was likely to cause nonbelievers to view the group and its
dogma as laughable?
The crucial event occurred sometime during “the night of the flood,”
when it became increasingly clear that the prophecy would not be fulfilled. Oddly, it was not their prior certainty that drove the members
to propagate the faith; it was an encroaching sense of uncertainty. It
was the dawning realization that if the spaceship and flood predictions
were wrong, so might be the entire belief system on which they rested.
For those huddled in the Keech living room, that growing possibility
must have seemed hideous.
The group members had gone too far, given up too much for their
beliefs to see them destroyed; the shame, the economic cost, the mockery
would be too great to bear. The overarching need of the cultists to cling
to those beliefs seeps poignantly from their own words: From a young
woman with a three-year-old child:
I have to believe the flood is coming on the twenty-first because
I’ve spent all my money. I quit my job, I quit computer school…. I
have to believe.
And from Dr. Armstrong to one of the researchers four hours after
the failure of the saucermen to arrive:
I’ve had to go a long way. I’ve given up just about everything. I’ve
cut every tie. I’ve burned every bridge. I’ve turned my back on the
98 / Influence
world. I can’t afford to doubt. I have to believe. And there isn’t any
other truth.
Imagine the corner in which Dr. Armstrong and his followers found
themselves as morning approached. So massive was the commitment
to their beliefs that no other truth was tolerable. Yet that set of beliefs
had just taken a merciless pounding from physical reality: No saucer
had landed, no spacemen had knocked, no flood had come, nothing
had happened as prophesied. Since the only acceptable form of truth
had been undercut by physical proof, there was but one way out of the
corner for the group. They had to establish another type of proof for
the validity of their beliefs: social proof.
This, then, explains their sudden shift from secretive conspirators to
zealous missionaries. And it explains the curious timing of the
shift—precisely when a direct disconfirmation of their beliefs had
rendered them least convincing to outsiders. It was necessary to risk
the scorn and derision of the nonbelievers because publicity and recruitment efforts provided the only remaining hope. If they could spread
the Word, if they could inform the uninformed, if they could persuade
the skeptics, and if, by so doing, they could win new converts, their
threatened but treasured beliefs would become truer. The principle of
social proof says so: The greater the number of people who find any
idea correct, the more the idea will be correct. The group’s assignment
was clear; since the physical evidence could not be changed, the social
evidence had to be. Convince and ye shall be convinced!6
CAUSE OF DEATH: UNCERTAIN(TY)
All the weapons of influence discussed in this book work better under
some conditions than under others. If we are to defend ourselves adequately against any such weapon, it is vital that we know its optimal
operating conditions in order to recognize when we are most vulnerable
to its influence. In the case of the principle of social proof, we have
already had a hint of one time when it works best. Among the Chicago
believers, it was a sense of shaken confidence that triggered their craving
for converts. In general, when we are unsure of ourselves, when the
situation is unclear or ambiguous, when uncertainty reigns, we are
most likely to look to and accept the actions of others as correct.
In the process of examining the reactions of other people to resolve
our uncertainty, however, we are likely to overlook a subtle but important fact. Those people are probably examining the social evidence, too.
Especially in an ambiguous situation, the tendency for everyone to be
looking to see what everyone else is doing can lead to a fascinating
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 99
phenomenon called “pluralistic ignorance.” A thorough understanding
of the pluralistic ignorance phenomenon helps immeasurably to explain
a regular occurrence in our country that has been termed both a riddle
and a national disgrace: the failure of entire groups of bystanders to
aid victims in agonizing need of help.
The classic example of such bystander inaction and the one that has
produced the most debate in journalistic, political, and scientific circles
began as an ordinary homicide case in the borough of Queens in New
York City. A woman in her late twenties, Catherine Genovese, was
killed in a late-night attack on her home street as she returned from
work. Murder is never an act to be passed off lightly, but in a city the
size and tenor of New York, the Genovese incident warranted no more
space than a fraction of a column in The New York Times. Catherine
Genovese’s story would have died with her on that day in March 1964
if it hadn’t been for a mistake.
The metropolitan editor of the Times, A. M. Rosenthal, happened to
be having lunch with the city police commissioner a week later.
Rosenthal asked the commissioner about a different Queens-based
homicide, and the commissioner, thinking he was being questioned
about the Genovese case, revealed something staggering that had been
uncovered by the police investigation. It was something that left
everyone who heard it, the commissioner included, aghast and grasping
for explanations. Catherine Genovese had not experienced a quick,
muffled death. It had been a long, loud, tortured, public event. Her assailant had chased and attacked her in the street three times over a
period of thirty-five minutes before his knife finally silenced her cries
for help. Incredibly, thirty-eight of her neighbors watched the events
of her death unfold from the safety of their apartment windows without
so much as lifting a finger to call the police.
Rosenthal, a former Pulitzer Prize—winning reporter, knew a story
when he heard one. On the day of his lunch with the commissioner, he
assigned a reporter to investigate the “bystander angle” of the Genovese
incident. Within a week, the Times published a long, page 1 article that
was to create a swirl of controversy and speculation. The first few
paragraphs of that report provide the tone and focus of the burgeoning
story:
For more than half an hour thirty-eight respectable, law-abiding
citizens in Queens watched a killer stalk and stab a woman in three
separate attacks in Kew Gardens.
Twice the sound of their voices and the sudden glow of their
bedroom lights interrupted him and frightened him off. Each time
he returned, sought her out, and stabbed her again. Not one person
146 / Influence
ative has had nothing to do with advancing the project or has, in some
cases, voted against it.
While politicians have long strained to associate themselves with the
values of motherhood, country, and apple pie, it may be in the last of
these connections—to food—that they have been most clever. For instance, it is White House tradition to try to sway the votes of balking
legislators over a meal. It can be a picnic lunch, a sumptuous breakfast,
or an elegant dinner; but when an important bill is up for grabs, out
comes the silverware. And political fund-raising these days regularly
involves the presentation of food. Notice, too, that at the typical fundraising dinner the speeches, the appeals for further contributions and
heightened effort never come before the meal is served, only during or
after. The advantages to this pairing of the affairs of the table with those
of the state are several: For example, time is saved and the reciprocity
rule is engaged. The least recognized benefit, however, may be the one
uncovered in research conducted in the 1930s by the distinguished
psychologist Gregory Razran.
Using what he termed the “luncheon technique,” he found that his
subjects became fonder of the people and things they experienced while
they were eating. In the example most relevant for our purposes,
Razran’s subjects were presented with some political statements they
had rated once before. At the end of the experiment, after all the political statements had been presented, Razran found that only certain of
them had gained in approval—those that had been shown while food
was being eaten. And these changes in liking seem to have occurred
unconsciously, since the subjects could not remember which of the
statements they had seen during the food service.
How did Razran come up with the luncheon technique? What made
him think it would work? The answer may lie in the dual scholarly
roles he played during his career. Not only was he a respected independent researcher, he was also one of the earliest translators into English
of the pioneering psychological literature of Russia. It was a literature
dedicated to the study of the association principle and dominated by
the thinking of a brilliant man, Ivan Pavlov.
Although a scientist of varied and elaborated talent—he had, for instance, won a Nobel Prize years earlier for his work on the digestive
system—Pavlov’s most important experimental demonstration was
simplicity itself. He showed that he could get an animal’s typical response to food (salivation) to be directed toward something irrelevant
to food (a bell) merely by connecting the two things in the animal’s
mind. If the presentation of food to a dog was always accompanied by
the sound of a bell, soon the dog would salivate to the bell alone, even
when there was no food to be had.
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 147
It is not a long step from Pavlov’s classic demonstration to Razran’s
luncheon technique. Obviously, a normal reaction to food can be
transferred to some other thing through the process of raw association.
Razran’s insight was that there are many normal responses to food besides salivation, one of them being a good and favorable feeling.
Therefore, it is possible to attach this pleasant feeling, this positive attitude, to anything (political statements being only an example) that is
closely associated with good food.
Nor is there a long step from the luncheon technique to the compliance professionals’ realization that all kinds of desirable things can
substitute for food in lending their likable qualities to the ideas,
products, and people artificially linked to them. In the final analysis,
then, that is why those good-looking models are standing around in
the magazine ads. And that is why radio programmers are instructed
to insert the station’s call-letters jingle immediately before a big hit song
is played. And that is even why the women playing Barnyard Bingo at
a Tupperware party must yell the word “Tupperware” rather than
“Bingo” before they can rush to the center of the floor for a prize. It
may be “Tupperware” for the women, but it’s “Bingo” for the company.
Just because we are often the unaware victims of compliance practitioners’ use of the association principle doesn’t mean that we don’t
understand how it works or don’t use it ourselves. There is ample
evidence, for instance, that we understand fully the predicament of a
Persian imperial messenger or modern-day weatherman announcing
bad news. In fact, we can be counted on to take steps to avoid putting
ourselves in any similar positions. Research done at the University of
Georgia shows just how we operate when faced with the task of communicating good or bad news. Students waiting for an experiment to
begin were given the job of informing a fellow student that an important
phone call had come in for him. Half the time the call was supposed to
bring good news and half the time, bad news. The researchers found
that the students conveyed the information very differently, depending
on its quality. When the news was positive, the tellers were sure to
mention that feature: “You just got a phone call with great news. Better
see the experimenter for the details.” But when the news was unfavorable, they kept themselves apart from it: “You just got a phone call.
Better see the experimenter for the details.” Obviously, the students
had previously learned that, to be liked, they should connect themselves
to good news but not bad news.25
A lot of strange behavior can be explained by the fact that people
understand the association principle well enough to strive to link
148 / Influence
themselves to positive events and separate themselves from negative
events—even when they have not caused the events. Some of the
strangest of such behavior takes place in the great arena of sports. The
actions of the athletes are not the issue here, though. After all, in the
heated contact of the game, they are entitled to an occasional eccentric
outburst. Instead, it is the often raging, irrational, boundless fervor of
the sports fan that seems, on its face, so puzzling. How can we account
for wild sports riots in Europe, or the murder of players and referees
by South American soccer crowds gone berserk, or the unnecessary
lavishness of the gifts provided by local fans to already wealthy
American ballplayers on the special “day” set aside to honor them?
Rationally, none of this makes sense. It’s just a game! Isn’t it?
Hardly. The relationship between sport and the earnest fan is anything
but gamelike. It is serious, intense, and highly personal. An apt illustration comes from one of my favorite anecdotes. It concerns a World War
II soldier who returned to his home in the Balkans after the war and
shortly thereafter stopped speaking. Medical examinations could find
no physical cause for the problem. There was no wound, no brain
damage, no vocal impairment. He could read, write, understand a
conversation, and follow orders. Yet he would not talk—not for his
doctors, not for his friends, not even for his pleading family.
Perplexed and exasperated, his doctors moved him to another city
and placed him in a veterans’ hospital where he remained for thirty
years, never breaking his self-imposed silence and sinking into a life of
social isolation. Then one day, a radio in his ward happened to be tuned
to a soccer match between his hometown team and a traditional rival.
When at a crucial point of play the referee called a foul against a player
from the man’s home team, the mute veteran jumped from his chair,
glared at the radio, and spoke his first words in more than three decades:
“You dumb ass!” he cried. “Are you trying to give them the match?”
With that, he returned to his chair and to a silence he never again violated.
There are two important lessons to be derived from this true story.
The first concerns the sheer power of the phenomenon. The veteran’s
desire to have his hometown team succeed was so strong that it alone
produced a deviation from his solidly entrenched way of life. Similar
effects of sports events on the long-standing habits of fans are far from
unique to the back wards of veterans’ hospitals. During the 1980 Winter
Olympics, after the U.S. hockey team had upset the vastly favored Soviet
team, the teetotaling father of the American goaltender, Jim Craig, was
offered a flask. “I’ve never had a drink in my life,” he reported later,
“but someone behind me handed me cognac. I drank it. Yes, I did.” Nor
was such unusual behavior unique to parents of the players. Fans out-
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 149
side the hockey arena were described in news accounts as delirious:
“They hugged, sang, and turned somersaults in the snow.” Even those
fans not present at Lake Placid exulted in the victory and displayed
their pride with bizarre behavior. In Raleigh, North Carolina, a swim
meet had to be halted when, after the hockey score was announced, the
competitors and audience alike chanted “U.S.A.! U.S.A.!” until they
were hoarse. In Cambridge, Massachusetts, a quiet supermarket erupted
at the news into a riot of flying toilet tissue and paper towel streamers.
The customers were joined in their spree—and soon led—by the market
employees and manager.
Without question, the force is deep and sweeping. But if we return
to the account of the silent veteran, we can see that something else is
revealed about the nature of the union of sports and sports fan, something crucial to its basic character: It is a personal thing. Whatever
fragment of an identity that ravaged, mute man still possessed was
engaged by soccer play. No matter how weakened his ego may have
become after thirty years of wordless stagnation in a hospital ward, it
was involved in the outcome of the match. Why? Because he, personally,
would be diminished by a hometown defeat. How? Through the principle of association. The mere connection of birthplace hooked him,
wrapped him, tied him to the approaching triumph or failure. As distinguished author Isaac Asimov put it in describing our reactions to
the contests we view, “All things being equal, you root for your own
sex, your own culture, your own locality…and what you want to prove
is that you are better than the other person. Whomever you root for
represents you; and when he wins, you win.”26
When viewed in this light, the passion of the sports fan begins to
make sense. The game is no light diversion to be enjoyed for its inherent
form and artistry. The self is at stake. That is why hometown crowds
are so adoring and, more tellingly, so grateful toward those regularly
responsible for home-team victories. That is also why the same crowds
are often ferocious in their treatment of players, coaches, and officials
implicated in athletic failures.
Fans’ intolerance of defeat can shorten the careers of even successful
players and coaches. Take the case of Frank Layden, who abruptly quit
as coach of the NBA’s Utah Jazz while the team was leading the league’s
Midwest Division. Layden’s relative success, warm humor, and widely
known charitable activities in the Salt Lake City area were not enough
to shield him from the ire of some Jazz supporters after team losses.
Citing a brace of incidents with abusive fans, including one in which
people waited around for an hour to curse at him following a defeat,
Layden explained his decision: “Sometimes in the NBA, you feel like
a dog. I’ve had people spit on me. I had a guy come up to me and say,
150 / Influence
‘I’m a lawyer. Hit me, hit me, so I can sue you.’ I think America takes
all sports too seriously.”
So we want our affiliated sports teams to win to prove our own superiority. But to whom are we trying to prove it? Ourselves, certainly;
but to everyone else, too. According to the association principle, if we
can surround ourselves with success that we are connected with in even
a superficial way (for example, place of residence), our public prestige
will rise.
Are sports fans right to think that without ever throwing a block,
catching a ball, scoring a goal, or perhaps even attending a game, they
will receive some of the glory from a hometown championship? I believe
so. The evidence is in their favor. Recall that Persia’s messengers did
not have to cause the news, my weatherman did not have to cause the
weather, and Pavlov’s bell did not have to cause the food for powerful
effects to occur. The association was enough.
It is for this reason that, were the University of Southern California
to win the Rose Bowl, we could expect people with a Southern Cal
connection to try to increase the visibility of that connection in any of
a variety of ways. In one experiment showing how wearing apparel
can serve to proclaim such an association, researchers counted the
number of school sweatshirts worn on Monday mornings by students
on the campuses of seven prominent football universities: Arizona State,
Louisiana State, Notre Dame, Michigan, Ohio State, Pittsburgh, and
Southern California. The results showed that many more home-school
shirts were worn if the football team had won its game on the prior
Saturday. What’s more, the larger the margin of victory, the more such
shirts appeared. It wasn’t a close, hard-fought game that caused the
students to dress themselves, literally, in success; instead, it was a clear,
crushing conquest smacking of indisputable superiority.
This tendency to try to bask in reflected glory by publicly trumpeting
our connections to successful others has its mirror image in our attempt
to avoid being darkened by the shadow of others’ defeat. In an amazing
display during the luckless 1980 season, season-ticket-holding fans of
the New Orleans Saints football team began to appear at the stadium
wearing paper bags to conceal their faces. As their team suffered loss
after loss, more and more fans donned the bags until TV cameras were
regularly able to record the extraordinary image of gathered masses of
people shrouded in brown paper with nothing to identify them but the
tips of their noses. I find it instructive that during a late-season contest,
when it was clear that the Saints were at last going to win one, the fans
discarded their bags and went public once more.
All this tells me that we purposefully manipulate the visibility of our
connections with winners and losers in order to make ourselves look
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 151
good to anyone who could view these connections. By showcasing the
positive associations and burying the negative ones, we are trying to
get observers to think more highly of us and to like us more. There are
many ways we go about this, but one of the simplest and most pervasive
is in the pronouns we use. Have you noticed, for example, how often
after a home-team victory fans crowd into the range of a TV camera,
thrust their index fingers high, and shout, “We’re number one! We’re
number one!” Note that the call is not “They’re number one” or even
“Our team is number one.” The pronoun is “we,” designed to imply
the closest possible identity with the team.
Note also that nothing similar occurs in the case of failure. No television viewer will ever hear the chant, “We’re in last place! We’re in last
place!” Home-team defeats are the times for distancing oneself. Here
“we” is not nearly as preferred as the insulating pronoun “they.” To
prove the point, I once did a small experiment in which students at
Arizona State University were phoned and asked to describe the outcome of a football game their school team had played a few weeks
earlier. Some of the students were asked the outcome of a certain game
their team had lost; the other students were asked the outcome of a
different game—one their team had won. My fellow researcher, Avril
Thorne, and I simply listened to what was said and recorded the percentage of students who used the word “we” in their descriptions.
When the results were tabulated, it was obvious that the students had
tried to connect themselves to success by using the pronoun “we” to
describe their school-team victory—“We beat Houston, seventeen to
fourteen,” or “We won.” In the case of the lost game, however, “we”
was rarely used. Instead, the students used terms designed to keep
themselves separate from their vanquished team—“They lost to Missouri, thirty to twenty,” or “I don’t know the score, but Arizona State
got beat.” Perhaps the twin desires to connect ourselves to winners and
to distance ourselves from losers were combined consummately in the
remarks of one particular student. After dryly recounting the score of
the home-team defeat—“Arizona State lost it, thirty to twenty”—he
blurted in anguish, “They threw away our chance for a national championship!”27
If it is true that, to make ourselves look good, we try to bask in the
reflected glory of the successes we are even remotely associated with,
a provocative implication emerges: We will be most likely to use this
approach when we feel that we don’t look so good. When-ever our
public image is damaged, we will experience an increased desire to restore that image by trumpeting our ties to successful others. At the same
time, we will most scrupulously avoid publicizing our ties to failing
152 / Influence
others. Support for these ideas comes from the telephone study of Arizona State University students. Before being asked about the hometeam victory or loss, they were given a test of their general knowledge.
The test was rigged so that some of the students would fail badly while
the others would do quite well.
So at the time they were asked to describe the football score, half of
the students had experienced recent image damage from their failure
of the test. These students later showed the greatest need to manipulate
their connections with the football team to salvage their prestige. If they
were asked to describe the team defeat, only 17 percent used the pronoun “we” in so doing. If, however, they were asked to describe the
win, 41 percent said “we.”
The story was very different, though, for the students who had done
well on the general knowledge test. They later used “we” about equally,
whether they were describing a home-team victory (25 percent) or defeat
(24 percent). These students had bolstered their images through their
own achievement and didn’t need to do so through the achievement
of others. This finding tells me that it is not when we have a strong
feeling of recognized personal accomplishment that we will seek to
bask in reflected glory. Instead, it will be when prestige (both public
and private) is low that we will be intent upon using the successes of
associated others to help restore image.
I think it revealing that the remarkable hubbub following the American hockey team victory in the 1980 Olympics came at a time of recently
diminished American prestige. The U.S. government had been helpless
to prevent both the holding of American hostages in Iran and the Soviet
invasion of Afghanistan. It was a time when, as a citizenry, we needed
the triumph of that hockey team and we needed to display or even
manufacture our connections to it. We should not be surprised to learn,
for instance, that outside the hockey arena, in the aftermath of the win
over the Soviet team, scalpers were getting a hundred dollars a pair for
ticket stubs.
Although the desire to bask in reflected glory exists to a degree in all
of us, there seems to be something special about people who would
wait in the snow to spend fifty dollars apiece for the shreds of tickets
to a game they had not attended, presumably to “prove” to friends back
home that they had been present at the big victory. Just what kind of
people are they? Unless I miss my guess, they are not merely great
sports aficionados; they are individuals with a hidden personality
flaw—a poor self-concept. Deep inside is a sense of low personal worth
that directs them to seek prestige not from the generation or promotion
of their own attainments, but from the generation or promotion of their
associations with others of attainment. There are several varieties of
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 153
this species that bloom throughout our culture. The persistent namedropper is a classic example. So, too, is the rock-music groupie, who
trades sexual favors for the right to tell girlfriends that she was “with”
a famous musician for a time. No matter which form it takes, the behavior of such individuals shares a similar theme—the rather tragic view
of accomplishment as deriving from outside the self.
Certain of these people work the association principle in a slightly
different way. Instead of striving to inflate their visible connections to
others of success, they strive to inflate the success of others they are
visibly connected to. The clearest illustration is the notorious “stage
mother,” obsessed with securing stardom for her child. Of course, women are not alone in this regard. In 1991 a Davenport, Iowa, obstetrician
cut off service to the wives of three school officials, reportedly because
his son had not been given enough playing time in school basketball
games. One of the wives was eight months’ pregnant at the time.
Physicians’ wives often speak of the pressures to obtain personal
prestige by association with their husband’s professional stature. John
Pekkanen, who authored the book The Best Doctors in the U.S., reports
that many enraged protests to his list came not from the physicians
who were omitted but from their wives. In one instance that reveals
the extent to which the principle of association dominates the thinking
of some of these women, Pekkanen received a letter from a frantic wife
along with her proof that her husband deserved to be on the list of best
doctors. It was a photograph of the man with Merv Griffin.
HOW TO SAY NO
Because liking can be increased by many means, a proper consideration
of defenses against compliance professionals who employ the liking
rule must, oddly enough, be a short one. It would be pointless to construct a horde of specific countertactics to combat each of the myriad
versions of the various ways to influence liking. There are simply too
many routes to be blocked effectively with such a one-on-one strategy.
Besides, several of the factors leading to liking—physical attractiveness,
familiarity, association—have been shown to work unconsciously to
produce their effects on us, making it unlikely that we could muster a
timely protection against them.
Instead we need to consider a general approach, one that can be applied to any of the liking-related factors to neutralize their unwelcome
influence on our compliance decisions. The secret to such an approach
may lie in its timing. Rather than trying to recognize and prevent the
action of liking factors before they have a chance to work on us, we
might be well advised to let them work. Our vigilance should be directed
154 / Influence
not toward the things that may produce undue liking for a compliance
practitioner, but toward the fact that undue liking has been produced.
The time to react protectively is when we feel ourselves liking the
practitioner more than we should under the circumstances.
By concentrating our attention on the effect rather than the causes,
we can avoid the laborious, nearly impossible task of trying to detect
and deflect the many psychological influences on liking. Instead, we
have to be sensitive to only one thing related to liking in our contacts
with compliance practitioners: the feeling that we have come to like the
practitioner more quickly or more deeply than we would have expected.
Once we notice this feeling, we will have been tipped off that there is
probably some tactic being used, and we can start taking the necessary
countermeasures. Note that the strategy I am suggesting borrows much
from the jujitsu style favored by the compliance professionals themselves. We don’t attempt to restrain the influence of the factors that
cause liking. Quite the contrary. We allow these factors to exert their
force, and then we use that force in our campaign against them. The
stronger the force, the more conspicuous it becomes and, consequently,
the more subject to our alerted defenses.
Suppose, for example, we find ourselves bargaining on the price of
a new car with Dealin’ Dan, a candidate for Joe Girard’s vacated
“greatest car salesman” title. After talking a while and negotiating a
bit, Dan wants to close the deal; he wants us to decide to buy the car.
Before any such decision is made, it would be important to ask ourselves
a crucial question: “In the twenty-five minutes I’ve known this guy,
have I come to like him more than I would have expected?” If the answer
is yes, we might want to reflect upon whether Dan behaved during
those few minutes in ways that we know affect liking. We might recall
that he had fed us (coffee and doughnuts) before launching into his
pitch, that he had complimented us on our choice of options and color
combinations, that he had made us laugh, that he had cooperated with
us against the sales manager to get us a better deal.
Although such a review of events might be informative, it is not a
necessary step in protecting ourselves from the liking rule. Once we
discover that we have come to like Dan more than we would have expected to, we don’t have to know why. The simple recognition of unwarranted liking should be enough to get us to react against it. One
possible reaction would be to reverse the process and actively dislike
Dan. But that might be unfair to him and contrary to our own interests.
After all, some individuals are naturally likable, and Dan might just be
one of them. It wouldn’t be right to turn automatically against those
compliance professionals who happen to be most likable. Besides, for
our own sakes, we wouldn’t want to shut ourselves off from business
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 155
interactions with such nice people, especially when they may be offering
us the best available deal.
I would recommend a different reaction. If our answer to the crucial
question is “Yes, under the circumstances, I like this guy peculiarly
well,” this should be the signal that the time has come for a quick
countermaneuver: Mentally separate Dan from that Chevy or Toyota
he’s trying to sell. It is vital to remember at this point that, should we
decide for Dan’s car, we will be driving it, not him, off the dealership
lot. It is irrelevant to a wise automobile purchase that we find Dan
likable because he is good-looking, claims an interest in our favorite
hobby, is funny, or has relatives back where we grew up.
Our proper response, then, is a conscious effort to concentrate exclusively on the merits of the deal and car Dan has for us. Of course, in
making a compliance decision, it is always a good idea to keep separate
our feelings about the requester and the request. But once immersed in
even a brief personal and sociable contact with a requester, that distinction is easy to forget. In those instances when we don’t care one way
or the other about a requester, forgetting to make the distinction won’t
steer us very far wrong. The big mistakes are likely to come when we
are fond of the person making a request.
That’s why it is so important to be alert to a sense of undue liking for
a compliance practitioner. The recognition of that feeling can serve as
our reminder to separate the dealer from the merits of the deal and to
make our decision based on considerations related only to the latter.
Were we all to follow this procedure, I am certain we would be much
more pleased with the results of our exchanges with compliance professionals—though I suspect that Dealin’ Dan would not.
READER’S REPORT
From a Chicago Man
“Although I’ve never been to a Tupperware party, I recognized the
same kind of friendship pressures recently when I got a call from a
long-distance-phone-company saleswoman. She told me that one of
my buddies had placed my name on something called the MCI Friends
and Family Calling Circle.
“This friend of mine, Brad, is a guy I grew up with but who moved
to New Jersey last year for a job. He still calls me pretty regularly to get
the news on the guys we used to hang out with from the neighborhood.
The saleswoman told me that he can save twenty percent on all the calls
he makes to the people on his Calling Circle list, provided that they are
MCI-phone-company subscribers. Then she asked me if I wanted to
156 / Influence
switch to MCI to get all the blah, blah, blah benefits of MCI service, and
so that Brad could save twenty percent on his calls to me.
“Well, I couldn’t have cared less about the benefits of MCI service; I
was perfectly happy with the long-distance company I had. But the
part about wanting to save Brad money on our calls really got to me.
For me to say that I didn’t want to be in his Calling Circle and didn’t
care about saving him money would have sounded like a real affront
to our friendship when he learned of it. So, to avoid insulting him, I
told her to switch me to MCI.
“I used to wonder why women would go to a Tupperware party just
because a friend was holding it, and then buy stuff they didn’t want
once they were there. I don’t wonder anymore.”
This reader is not alone in being able to testify to the power of the
pressures embodied in MCI’s Calling Circle idea. When Consumer
Reports magazine inquired into the practice, the MCI salesperson they
interviewed was quite succinct: “It works nine out of ten times,” he
said.
Chapter 6
AUTHORITY
Directed Deference
Follow an expert.
—VIRGIL
S an ad for volunteers to take part in a “study of memory” beingnotice
done
UPPOSE THAT WHILE LEAFING THROUGH THE NEWSPAPER, YOU
in the psychology department of a nearby university. Let’s suppose
further that, finding the idea of such an experiment intriguing, you
contact the director of the study, a Professor Stanley Milgram, and make
arrangements to participate in an hour-long session. When you arrive
at the laboratory suite, you meet two men. One is the researcher in
charge of the experiment, as is clearly evidenced by the gray lab coat
he wears and the clipboard he carries. The other is a volunteer like
yourself who seems average in all respects.
After initial greetings and pleasantries are exchanged, the researcher
begins to explain the procedures to be followed. He says that the experiment is a study of how punishment affects learning and memory.
Therefore, one participant will have the task of learning pairs of words
in a long list until each pair can be recalled perfectly; this person is to
be called the Learner. The other partici-pant’s job will be to test the
Learner’s memory and to deliver increasingly strong electric shocks for
every mistake; this person will be designated the Teacher.
Naturally, you get a bit nervous at this news. And your apprehension
increases when, after drawing lots with your partner, you find that you
are assigned the Learner role. You hadn’t expected the possibility of
pain as part of the study, so you briefly consider leaving. But no, you
158 / Influence
think, there’s plenty of time for that if need be and, besides, how strong
a shock could it be?
After you have had a chance to study the list of word pairs, the researcher straps you into a chair and, with the Teacher looking on, attaches electrodes to your arm. More worried now about the effect of
the shock, you inquire into its severity. The researcher’s response is
hardly comforting; he says that although the shocks can be extremely
painful, they will cause you “no permanent tissue damage.” With that,
the researcher and the Teacher leave you alone and go to the next room,
where the Teacher asks you the test questions through an intercom
system and delivers electric punishment for every wrong response.
As the test proceeds, you quickly recognize the pattern that the
Teacher follows: He asks the question and waits for your answer over
the intercom. Whenever you err, he announces the voltage of the shock
you are about to receive and pulls a level to deliver the punishment.
The most troubling thing is that with each error you make, the shock
increases by 15 volts.
The first part of the test progresses smoothly. The shocks are annoying
but tolerable. Later on, though, as your mistakes accumulate and the
shock voltages climb, the punishment begins to hurt enough to disrupt
your concentration, which leads to more errors and ever more disruptive
shocks. At the 75-, 90-, and 105-volt levels, the pain makes you grunt
audibly. At 120 volts, you exclaim into the intercom that the shocks are
really starting to hurt. You take one more punishment with a groan and
decide that you can’t take much more pain. After the Teacher delivers
the 150-volt shock, you shout back into the intercom, “That’s all! Get
me out of here! Get me out of here, please! Let me out!”
But instead of the assurance you expect from the Teacher that he and
the researcher are coming to release you, the Teacher merely gives you
the next test question to answer. Surprised and confused, you mumble
the first answer to come into your head. It’s wrong, of course, and the
Teacher delivers a 165-volt shock. You scream at the Teacher to stop,
to let you out. But he responds only with the next test question—and
with the next slashing shock when your frenzied answer is incorrect.
You can’t hold down the panic any longer; the shocks are so strong
now they make you writhe and shriek. You kick the wall, demand to
be released, beg the Teacher to help you. But the test questions continue
as before and so do the dreaded shocks—in searing jolts of 195, 210,
225, 240, 255, 270, 285, and 300 volts. You realize that you can’t possibly
answer the test correctly now, so you shout to the Teacher that you
won’t answer his questions any longer. Nothing changes; the Teacher
interprets your failure to respond as an incorrect response and sends
another bolt. The ordeal continues in this way until, finally, the power
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 159
of the shocks stuns you into near paralysis. You can no longer cry out,
no longer struggle. You can only feel each terrible electric bite. Perhaps,
you think, this total inactivity will cause the Teacher to stop. There can
be no reason to continue this experiment. But he proceeds relentlessly,
calling out the test questions, announcing the horrid shock levels (about
400 volts now), and pulling the levers. What must this man be like? you
wonder in confusion. Why doesn’t he help me? Why won’t he stop?
For most of us, the above scenario reads like a bad dream. To recognize how nightmarish it is, though, we should understand that in most
respects it is real. There was such an experiment—actually, a whole
series—run by a psychology professor named Milgram in which participants in the Teacher role were willing to deliver continued, intense,
and dangerous levels of shock to a kicking, screeching, pleading other
person. Only one major aspect of the experiment was not genuine. No
real shock was delivered; the Learner, the victim who repeatedly cried
out in agony for mercy and release, was not a true subject but an actor
who only pretended to be shocked. The actual purpose of Milgram’s
study, then, had nothing to do with the effects of punishment on
learning and memory. Rather, it involved an entirely different question:
When it is their job, how much suffering will ordinary people be willing
to inflict on an entirely innocent other person?
The answer is most unsettling. Under circumstances mirroring precisely the features of the “bad dream,” the typical Teacher was willing
to deliver as much pain as was available to give. Rather than yield to
the pleas of the victim, about two thirds of the subjects in Milgram’s
experiment pulled every one of the thirty shock switches in front of
them and continued to engage the last switch (450 volts) until the researcher ended the experiment. More alarming still, not one of the forty
subjects in this study quit his job as Teacher when the victim first began
to demand his release; nor later, when he began to beg for it; nor even
later, when his reaction to each shock had become, in Milgram’s words,
“definitely an agonized scream.” Not until the 300-volt shock had been
sent and the victim had “shouted in desperation that he would no longer
provide answers to the memory test” did anyone stop—and even then,
it was a distinct minority who did.
These results surprised everyone associated with the project, Milgram
included. In fact, before the study began, he asked groups of colleagues,
graduate students, and psychology majors at Yale University (where
the experiment was performed) to read a copy of the experimental
procedures and estimate how many subjects would go all the way to
the last (450-volt) shock. Invariably, the answers fell in the 1 to 2 percent
range. A separate group of thirty-nine psychiatrists predicted that only
160 / Influence
about one person in a thousand would be willing to continue to the
end. No one, then, was prepared for the behavior patterns that the experiment actually produced.
How can we explain those alarming patterns? Perhaps, as some have
argued, it has to do with the fact that the subjects were all males who
are known as a group for their aggressive tendencies, or that the subjects
didn’t recognize the potential harm that such high shock voltages could
cause, or that the subjects were a freakish collection of moral cretins
who enjoyed the chance to inflict misery. But there is good evidence
against each of these possibilities. First, the subjects’ sex was shown by
a later experiment to be irrelevant to their willingness to give all the
shocks to the victim; female Teachers were just as likely to do so as the
males in Milgram’s initial study.
The explanation that subjects weren’t aware of the potential physical
danger to the victim was also examined in a subsequent experiment
and found to be wanting. In that version, when the victim was instructed
to announce that he had a heart condition and to declare that his heart
was being affected by the shock—“That’s all. Get me out of here. I told
you I had heart trouble. My heart’s starting to bother me. I refuse to go
on. Let me out”—the results were the same as before; 65 percent of the
subjects carried out their duties faithfully through the maximum shock.
Finally, the explanation that Milgram’s subjects were a twisted, sadistic bunch not at all representative of the average citizen has proven
unsatisfactory as well. The people who answered Milgram’s newspaper
ad to participate in his “memory” experiment represented a standard
cross section of ages, occupations, and educational levels within our
society. What’s more, later on, a battery of personality scales showed
these people to be quite normal psychologically, with not a hint of
psychosis as a group. They were, in fact, just like you and me; or, as
Milgram likes to term it, they are you and me. If he is right that his
studies implicate us in their grisly findings, the unanswered question
becomes an uncomfortably personal one: What could make us do such
things?
Milgram is sure he knows the answer. It has to do, he says, with a
deep-seated sense of duty to authority within us all. According to Milgram, the real culprit in the experiments was his subject’s inability to
defy the wishes of the boss of the study—the lab-coated researcher who
urged and, if need be, directed the subjects to perform their duties,
despite the emotional and physical mayhem they were causing.
The evidence supporting Milgram’s obedience to authority explanation is strong. First, it is clear that, without the researcher’s directives
to continue, the subjects would have ended the experiment quickly.
They hated what they were doing and agonized over their victim’s
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 161
agony. They implored the researcher to let them stop. When he refused,
they went on, but in the process they trembled, they perspired, they
shook, they stammered protests and additional pleas for the victim’s
release. Their fingernails dug into their own flesh; they bit their lips
until they bled; they held their heads in their hands; some fell into fits
of uncontrollable nervous laughter. As one outside observer to the experiment wrote:
I observed a mature and initially poised businessman enter the
laboratory smiling and confident. Within twenty minutes he was
reduced to a twitching, stuttering wreck who was rapidly approaching a point of nervous collapse. He constantly pulled on his earlobe
and twisted his hands. At one point he pushed his fist into his
forehead and muttered: “Oh, God, let’s stop it.” And yet he continued to respond to every word of the experimenter and obeyed
to the end.1
In addition to these observations, Milgram has provided even more
convincing evidence for the obedience-to-authority interpretation of
his subjects’ behavior. In a later study, for instance, he had the researcher
and the victim switch scripts so that the researcher told the Teacher to
stop delivering shocks to the victim, while the victim insisted bravely
that the Teacher continue. The result couldn’t have been clearer; 100
percent of the subjects refused to give one additional shock when it was
merely the fellow subject who demanded it. The identical finding appeared in another version of the experiment in which the researcher
and fellow subject switched roles so that it was the researcher who was
strapped into the chair and the fellow subject who ordered the Teacher
to continue—over the protests of the researcher. Again, not one subject
touched another shock lever.
The extreme degree to which subjects in Milgram’s situation were
attentive to the wishes of authority was documented in yet another
variation of the basic study. In this case, Milgram presented the
Teacher with two researchers, who issued contradictory orders; one
ordered the Teacher to terminate the shocks when the victim cried out
for release, while the other maintained that the experiment should go
on. These conflicting instructions reliably produced what may have
been the project’s only humor: In tragicomic befuddlement and with
eyes darting from one researcher to another, subjects would beseech
the pair to agree on a single command they could follow: “Wait, wait.
Which is it going to be? One says stop, one says go. Which is it!?” When
the researchers remained at loggerheads, the subjects tried frantically
to determine who was the bigger boss. Failing this route to obedience
with the authority, every subject finally followed his better instincts and
Chapter 7
SCARCITY
The Rule of the Few
The way to love anything is to realize that it might be lost.
—G. K. CHESTERTON
where
T I live. Perhaps the most notable features of Mesa are its sizable
HE CITY OF MESA, ARIZONA, IS A SUBURB IN THE PHOENIX AREA
Mormon population—next to that of Salt Lake City, the largest in the
world—and a huge Mormon temple located on exquisitely kept grounds
in the center of the city. Although I had appreciated the landscaping
and architecture from a distance, I had never been interested enough
in the temple to go inside until the day I read a newspaper article that
told of a special inner sector of Mormon temples to which no one has
access but faithful members of the Church. Even potential converts
must not see it. There is one exception to the rule, however. For a few
days immediately after a temple is newly constructed, nonmembers
are allowed to tour the entire structure, including the otherwise restricted section.
The newspaper story reported that the Mesa temple had recently
been refurbished and that the renovations had been extensive enough
to classify it as “new” by Church standards. Thus, for the next several
days only, non-Mormon visitors could see the temple area traditionally
banned to them. I remember quite well the effect of the article on me:
I immediately resolved to take a tour. But when I phoned a friend to
ask if he wanted to come along, I came to understand something that
changed my decision just as quickly.
After declining the invitation, my friend wondered why I seemed so
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 179
intent on a visit. I was forced to admit that, no, I had never been inclined
toward the idea of a temple tour before, that I had no questions about
the Mormon religion I wanted answered, that I had no general interest
in the architecture of houses of worship, and that I expected to find
nothing more spectacular or stirring than I might see at a number of
other temples, churches, or cathedrals in the area. It became clear as I
spoke that the special lure of the temple had a sole cause: If I did not
experience the restricted sector shortly, I would never again have the
chance. Something that, on its own merits, held little appeal for me had
become decidedly more attractive merely because it would soon become
unavailable.
Since that encounter with the scarcity principle—that opportunities
seem more valuable to us when their availability is limited—I have
begun to notice its influence over a whole range of my actions. For instance, I routinely will interrupt an interesting face-to-face conversation
to answer the ring of an unknown caller. In such a situation, the caller
has a compelling feature that my face-to-face partner does not: potential
unavailability. If I don’t take the call, I might miss it (and the information
it carries) for good. Never mind that the ongoing conversation may be
highly engaging or important—much more than I could reasonably
expect an average phone call to be. With each unanswered ring, the
phone interaction becomes less retrievable. For that reason and for that
moment, I want it more than the other.
The idea of potential loss plays a large role in human decision making.
In fact, people seem to be more motivated by the thought of losing
something than by the thought of gaining something of equal value.
For instance, homeowners told how much money they could lose from
inadequate insulation are more likely to insulate their homes than those
told how much money they could save. Similar results have been obtained by health researchers: Pamphlets urging young women to check
for breast cancer through self-examinations are significantly more successful if they state their case in terms of what stands to be lost (e.g.,
“You can lose several potential health benefits by failing to spend only
five minutes each month doing breast self-examination”) rather than
gained (e.g., “You can gain several potential health benefits by spending
only five minutes each month doing breast self-examination”).1
Collectors of everything from baseball cards to antiques are keenly
aware of the influence of the scarcity principle in determining the worth
of an item. As a rule, if it is rare or becoming rare, it is more valuable.
Especially enlightening as to the importance of scarcity in the collectibles
market is the phenomenon of the “precious mistake.” Flawed items—a
blurred stamp or a double-struck coin—are sometimes the most valued
180 / Influence
of all. Thus a stamp carrying a three-eyed likeness of George Washington
is anatomically incorrect, aesthetically unappealing, and yet highly
sought after. There is instructive irony here: Imperfections that would
otherwise make for rubbish make for prized possessions when they
bring along an abiding scarcity.
With the scarcity principle operating so powerfully on the worth we
assign things, it is natural that compliance professionals will do some
related operating of their own. Probably the most straightforward use
of the scarcity principle occurs in the “limited-number” tactic, when
the customer is informed that a certain product is in short supply that
cannot be guaranteed to last long. During the time I was researching
compliance strategies by infiltrating various organizations, I saw the
limited-number tactic employed repeatedly in a range of situations:
“There aren’t more than five convertibles with this engine left in the
state. And when they’re gone, that’s it, ’cause we’re not making ’em
anymore.” “This is one of only two unsold corner lots in the entire development. You wouldn’t want the other one; it’s got a nasty east-west
exposure.” “You may want to think seriously about buying more than
one case today because production is backed way up and there’s no
telling when we’ll get any more in.”
Sometimes the limited-number information was true, sometimes it
was wholly false. But in each instance, the intent was to convince customers of an item’s scarcity and thereby increase its immediate value
in their eyes. I admit to developing a grudging admiration for the
practitioners who made this simple device work in a multitude of ways
and styles. I was most impressed, however, with a particular version
that extended the basic approach to its logical extreme by selling a piece
of merchandise at its scarcest point—when it seemingly could no longer
be had. The tactic was played to perfection in one appliance store I investigated, where 30 to 50 percent of the stock was regularly listed as
on sale. Suppose a couple in the store seemed from a distance to be
moderately interested in a certain sale item. There are all sorts of cues
that tip off such interest—closer-than-normal examination of the appliance, a casual look at any instruction booklets associated with the appliance, discussions held in front of the appliance, but no attempt to
seek out a salesperson for further information. After observing the
couple so engaged, a salesperson might approach and say, “I see you’re
interested in this model here, and I can understand why; it’s a great
machine at a great price. But, unfortunately, I sold it to another couple
not more than twenty minutes ago. And, if I’m not mistaken, it was the
last one we had.”
The customers’ disappointment registers unmistakably. Because of
its lost availability, the appliance jumps suddenly in attractiveness.
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 181
Typically, one of the customers asks if there is any chance that an unsold
model still exists in the store’s back room, warehouse, or other location.
“Well,” the salesperson allows, “that is possible, and I’d be willing to
check. But do I understand that this is the model you want and if I can
get it for you at this price, you’ll take it?” Therein lies the beauty of the
technique. In accord with the scarcity principle, the customers are asked
to commit to buying the appliance when it looks least available—and
therefore most desirable. Many customers do agree to a purchase at
this singularly vulnerable time. Thus, when the salesperson (invariably)
returns with the news that an additional supply of the appliance has
been found, it is also with a pen and sales contract in hand. The information that the desired model is in good supply may actually make some
customers find it less attractive again.2 But by then, the business transaction has progressed too far for most people to renege. The purchase
decision made and committed to publicly at an earlier, crucial point
still holds. They buy.
Related to the limited-number technique is the “deadline” tactic, in
which some official time limit is placed on the customer’s opportunity
to get what the compliance professional is offering. Much like my experience with the Mormon temple’s inner sanctum, people frequently
find themselves doing what they wouldn’t particularly care to do simply
because the time to do so is shrinking. The adept merchandiser makes
this tendency pay off by arranging and publicizing customer deadlines—witness the collage of such newspaper ads in Figure 7–3—that
generate interest where none may have existed before. Concentrated
instances of this approach often occur in movie advertising. In fact, I
recently noticed that one theater owner, with remarkable singleness of
purpose, had managed to invoke the scarcity principle three separate
times in just five words that read, “Exclusive, limited engagement ends
soon!”
Swindled
By Peter Kerr
New York Times
NEW YORK—Daniel Gulban doesn’t remember how his life savings disappeared.
He remembers the smooth voice of a salesman on the telephone. He remembers
dreaming of a fortune in oil and silver futures. But to this day, the 81-year-old retired
utility worker does not understand how swindlers convinced him to part with
$18,000.
“I just wanted to better my life in my waning days,” said Gulban, a resident of
182 / Influence
Holder, Fla. “But when I found out the truth, I couldn’t eat or sleep. I lost 30 pounds.
I still can’t believe I would do anything like that.”
Gulban was the victim of a what law enforcement officials call a “boiler-room
operation,” a ruse that often involves dozens of fast-talking telephone salesmen
crammed into a small room where they call thousands of customers each day. The
companies snare hundreds of millions of dollars each year from unsuspecting
customers, according to a U.S. Senate subcommittee on investigations, which issued
a report on the subject last year.
“They use an impressive Wall Street address, lies and deception to get individuals
to sink their money into various glamorous-sounding schemes,” said Robert Abrams,
the New York State attorney general, who has pursued more than a dozen boilerroom cases in the past four years. “The victims are sometimes persuaded to invest
the savings of a lifetime.”
Orestes J. Mihaly, the New York assistant attorney general in charge of the bureau
of investor protection and securities, said the companies often operate in three
stages. First, Mihaly said, comes the “opening call,” in which a salesman identifies
himself as representing a company with an impressive-sounding name and address.
He will simply ask the potential customer to receive the company’s literature.
A second call involves a sales pitch, Mihaly said. The salesman first describes
the great profits to be made and then tells the customer that it is no longer possible
to invest. The third call gives the customer a chance to get in on the deal, he said,
and is offered with a great deal of urgency.
“The idea is to dangle a carrot in front of the buyer’s face and then take it away,”
Mihaly said. “The aim is to get someone to want to buy quickly, without thinking
too much about it.” Sometimes, Mihaly said, the salesman will be out of breath on
the third call and will tell the customer that he “just came off the trading floor.”
Such tactics convinced Gulban to part with his life savings. In 1979, a stranger
called him repeatedly and convinced Gulban to wire $1,756 to New York to purchase
silver, Gulban said. After another series of telephone calls the salesman cajoled
Gulban into wiring more than $6,000 for crude oil. He eventually wired an additional $9,740, but his profits never arrived.
“My heart sank,” Gulban recalled. “I was not greedy. I just hoped I would see
better days.” Gulban never recouped his losses.
FIGURE 7-2
The Scarcity Scam
Note how the scarcity principle was employed during the second and
third phone calls
to cause Mr. Gulban to “buy quickly without thinking too much about
it.” Click, blur.
(PETER KERR, THE NEW YORK TIMES)
A variant of the deadline tactic is much favored by some face-to-face,
high-pressure sellers because it carries the purest form of decision
deadline: right now. Customers are often told that unless they make an
immediate decision to buy, they will have to purchase the item at a
higher price or they will be unable to purchase it at all. A prospective
health-club member or automobile buyer might learn that the deal
offered by the salesperson is good only for that one time; should the
customer leave the premises, the deal is off. One large child-portrait
photography company urges parents to buy as many poses and copies
as they can afford because “stocking limitations force us to burn the
unsold pictures of your children within twenty-four hours.” A door-todoor magazine solicitor might say that salespeople are in the customer’s
area for just a day; after that, they—and the customer’s chance to buy
their magazine package—will be long gone. A home vacuum-cleaner
operation I infiltrated instructed its sales trainees to claim, “I have so
many other people to see that I have the time to visit a family only once.
It’s company policy that even if you decide later that you want this
machine, I can’t come back and sell it to you.” This, of course, is nonsense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
sense; the company and its representatives are in the business of make; 272 This,
208 / Influence
quickly; we contact more people and have shorter relationships with
them; in the supermarket, car showroom, and shopping mall, we are
faced with an array of choices among styles and products that were
unheard of the previous year and may well be obsolete or forgotten by
the next. Novelty, transience, diversity, and acceleration are acknowledged as prime descriptors of civilized existence.
This avalanche of information and choices is made possible by burgeoning technological progress. Leading the way are developments in
our ability to collect, store, retrieve, and communicate information. At
first, the fruits of such advances were limited to large organizations—government agencies or powerful corporations. For example,
speaking as chairman of Citicorp, Walter Wriston could say of his
company, “We have tied together a data base in the world that is capable
of telling almost anyone in the world, almost anything, immediately.”2
But now, with further developments in telecommunication and computer technology, access to such staggering amounts of information is
falling within the reach of individual citizens. Extensive cable and
satellite television systems provide one route for that information into
the average home.
The other major route is the personal computer. In 1972, Norman
Macrae, an editor of The Economist, speculated prophetically about a
time in the future:
The prospect is, after all, that we are going to enter an age when
any duffer sitting at a computer terminal in his laboratory or office
or public library or home can delve through unimaginable increased mountains of information in mass-assembly data banks
with mechanical powers of concentration and calculation that will
be greater by a factor of tens of thousands than was ever available
to the human brain of even an Einstein.
One short decade later, Time magazine signaled that Macrae’s future
age had arrived by naming a machine, the personal computer, as its
Man of the Year. Time’s editors defended their choice by citing the
consumer “stampede” to purchase small computers and by arguing
that “America [and], in a larger perspective, the entire world will never
be the same.” Macrae’s vision is now being realized. Millions of ordinary
“duffers” are sitting at machines with the potential to present and
analyze enough data to bury an Einstein.
Because technology can evolve much faster than we can, our natural
capacity to process information is likely to be increasingly inadequate
to handle the surfeit of change, choice, and challenge that is characteristic of modern life. More and more frequently, we will find ourselves
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 209
in the position of the lower animals—with a mental apparatus that is
unequipped to deal thoroughly with the intricacy and richness of the
outside environment. Unlike the animals, whose cognitive powers have
always been relatively deficient, we have created our own deficiency
by constructing a radically more complex world. But the consequence
of our new deficiency is the same as that of the animals’ long-standing
one. When making a decision, we will less frequently enjoy the luxury
of a fully consid-ered analysis of the total situation but will revert increasingly to a focus on a single, usually reliable feature of it.
When those single features are truly reliable, there is nothing inherently wrong with the shortcut approach of narrowed attention and
automatic response to a particular piece of information. The problem
comes when something causes the normally trustworthy cues to counsel
us poorly, to lead us to erroneous actions and wrongheaded decisions.
As we have seen, one such cause is the trickery of certain compliance
practitioners who seek to profit from the rather mindless and mechanical nature of shortcut response. If, as seems true, the frequency of
shortcut response is increasing with the pace and form of modern life,
we can be sure that the frequency of this trickery is destined to increase
as well.
What can we do about the expected intensified attack on our system
of shortcuts? More than evasive action, I would urge forceful counterassault. There is an important qualification, however. Compliance
professionals who play fairly by the rules of shortcut response are not
to be considered the enemy; on the contrary, they are our allies in an
efficient and adaptive process of exchange. The proper targets for
counteraggression are only those individuals who falsify, counterfeit,
or misrepresent the evidence that naturally cues our shortcut responses.
Let’s take an illustration from what is perhaps our most frequently
used shortcut. According to the principle of social proof, we often decide
to do what other people like us are doing. It makes all kinds of sense
since, most of the time, an action that is popular in a given situation is
also functional and appropriate. Thus, an advertiser who, without using
deceptive statistics, provides information that a brand of toothpaste is
the largest selling or fastest growing has offered us valuable evidence
about the quality of the product and the probability that we will like
it. Provided that we are in the market for a tube of good toothpaste, we
might want to rely on that single piece of information, popularity, to
decide to try it. This strategy will likely steer us right, will unlikely steer
us far wrong, and will conserve our cognitive energies for dealing with
the rest of our increasingly information-laden, decision-overloaded
environment. The advertiser who allows us to use effectively this effi-
210 / Influence
cient strategy is hardly our antagonist but rather must be considered a
cooperating partner.
The story becomes quite different, however, should a compliance
practitioner try to stimulate a shortcut response by giving us a fraudulent signal for it. The enemy is the advertiser who seeks to create an
image of popularity for a brand of toothpaste by, say, constructing a
series of staged “unrehearsed-interview” commercials in which an array
of actors posing as ordinary citizens praise the product. Here, where
the evidence of popularity is counterfeit, we, the principle of social
proof, and our shortcut response to it, are all being exploited. In an
earlier chapter, I recommended against the purchase of any product
featured in a faked “unrehearsed-interview” ad, and I urged that we
send the product manufacturers letters detailing the reason and suggesting that they dismiss their advertising agency. I would recommend
extending this aggressive stance to any situation in which a compliance
professional abuses the principle of social proof (or any other weapon
of influence) in this manner. We should refuse to watch TV programs
that use canned laughter. If we see a bartender beginning a shift by
salting his tip jar with a bill or two of his own, he should get none from
us. If, after waiting in line outside a nightclub, we discover from the
amount of available space that the wait was designed to impress passersby with false evidence of the club’s popularity, we should leave immediately and announce our reason to those still in line. In short, we
should be willing to use boycott, threat, confrontation, censure, tirade,
nearly anything, to retaliate.
I don’t consider myself pugnacious by nature, but I actively advocate
such belligerent actions because in a way I am at war with the exploiters—we all are. It is important to recognize, however, that their
motive for profit is not the cause for hostilities; that motive, after all, is
something we each share to an extent. The real treachery, and the thing
we cannot tolerate, is any attempt to make their profit in a way that
threatens the reliability of our shortcuts. The blitz of modern daily life
demands that we have faithful shortcuts, sound rules of thumb to handle
it all. These are not luxuries any longer; they are out-and-out necessities
that figure to become increasingly vital as the pulse of daily life quickens. That is why we should want to retaliate whenever we see someone
betraying one of our rules of thumb for profit. We want that rule to be
as effective as possible. But to the degree that its fitness for duty is
regularly undercut by the tricks of a profiteer, we naturally will use it
less and will be less able to cope efficiently with the decisional burdens
of our day. We cannot allow that without a fight. The stakes have gotten
too high.
NOTES
CHAPTER
1 (PAGES 1–16)
1. Honest, this animal researcher’s name is Fox. See his 1974 monograph for a complete description of the turkey and polecat experiment.
2. Sources for the robin and bluethroat information are Lack (1943)
and Peiponen (1960), respectively.
3. Although several important similarities exist between this kind of
automatic responding in humans and lower animals, there are some
important differences as well. The automatic behavior sequences of
humans tend to be learned rather than inborn, more flexible than the
lock-step patterns of the lower animals, and responsive to a larger
number of triggers.
4. Perhaps the common “because…just because” response of children
asked to explain their behavior can be traced to their shrewd recognition
of the unusual amount of power adults appear to assign to the raw
word because.
The reader who wishes to find a more systematic treatment of Langer’s
Xerox study and her conceptualization of it can do so in Langer (1989).
5. Sources for the Photuris and the blenny information are Lloyd (1965)
and Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1958), respectively. As exploitative as these creatures
seem, they are topped in this respect by an insect known as the rove
beetle. By using a variety of triggers involving smell and touch, the rove
beetles get two species of ants to protect, groom, and feed them as larvae
and to harbor them for the winter as adults. Responding mechanically
to the beetles’ trick trigger features, the ants treat the beetles as though
they were fellow ants. Inside the ant nests, the beetles respond to their
hosts’ hospitality by eating ant eggs and young, yet they are never
harmed (Hölldobler, 1971).
6. These studies are reported by Kenrick and Gutierres (1980), who
warn that the unrealistically attractive people portrayed in the popular
media (for example, actors, actresses, models) may cause us to be less
212 / Influence
satisfied with the looks of the genuinely available romantic possibilities
around us. More recent work by these authors takes their argument a
step farther, showing that exposure to the exaggerated sexual attractiveness of nude pinup bodies (in such magazines as Playboy and Playgirl)
causes people to become less pleased with the sexual desirability of
their current spouse or live-in mate (Kenrick, Gutierres, and Goldberg,
1989).
CHAPTER
2 (PAGES 17–56)
1. A formal description of the greeting-card study is provided in Kunz
and Woolcott (1976).
2. Certain societies have formalized the rule into ritual. Consider for
example the “Vartan Bhanji,” an institutionalized custom of the gift
exchange common to parts of Pakistan and India. In commenting upon
the “Vartan Bhanji,” Gouldner (1960) remarks:
It is…notable that the system painstakingly prevents the total
elimination of outstanding obligations. Thus, on the occasion of
a marriage, departing guests are given gifts of sweets. In weighing
them out, the hostess may say, “These five are yours,” meaning
“These are a repayment for what you formerly gave me,” and then
she adds an extra measure, saying, “These are mine.” On the next
occasion, she will receive these back along with an additional
measure which she later returns, and so on.
3. The quote is from Leakey and Lewin (1978).
4. For a fuller discussion, see Tiger and Fox (1971).
5. The experiment is reported formally in Regan (1971).
6. The statement appears in Mauss (1954).
7. Surprise is an effective compliance producer in its own right. People
who are surprised by a request will often comply because they are
momentarily unsure of themselves and, consequently, influenced easily.
For example, the social psychologists Stanley Milgram and John Sabini
(1975) have shown that people riding on the New York subway were
twice as likely to give up their seats to a person who surprised them
with the request “Excuse me. May I have your seat?” than to one who
forewarned them first by mentioning to a fellow passenger that he was
thinking of asking for someone’s seat (56 percent vs. 28 percent).
8. It is interesting that a cross-cultural study has shown that those
who break the reciprocity rule in the reverse direction—by giving
without allowing the recipient an opportunity to repay—are also disliked for it. This result was found to hold for each of the three national-
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 213
ities investigated—Americans, Swedes, and Japanese. See Gergen et al.
(1975) for an account of the study.
9. The Pittsburgh study was done by Greenberg and Shapiro. The
data on women’s sexual obligations were collected by George, Gournic,
and McAfee (1988).
10. To convince ourselves that this result was no fluke, we conducted
two more experiments testing the effectiveness of the rejection-thenretreat trick. Both showed results similar to the first experiment. See
Cialdini et al. (1975) for the details of all three.
11. The Israeli study was conducted in 1979 by Schwartzwald, Raz,
and Zvibel.
12. The TV Guide article appeared in December 1978.
13. The source for the quotes is Magruder (1974).
14. Consumer Reports, January 1975, p. 62.
15. Another way of gauging the effectiveness of a request technique
is to examine the bottom-line proportion of individuals who, after being
asked, complied with the request. Using such a measure, the rejectionthen-retreat procedure was more than four times more effective than
the procedure of asking for the smaller request only. See Miller et al.
(1976) for a complete description of the study.
16. The blood-donation study was reported by Cialdini and Ascani
(1976).
17. The UCLA study was performed by Benton, Kelley, and Liebling
in 1972.
18. A variety of other business operations use the no-cost information
offer extensively. Pest-exterminator companies, for instance, have found
that most people who agree to a free home examination give the extermination job to the examining company, provided they are convinced
that it is needed. They apparently feel an obligation to give their business
to the firm that rendered the initial, complimentary service. Knowing
that such customers are unlikely to comparisonshop for this reason,
unscrupulous pest-control operators will take advantage of the situation
by citing higher-than-competitive prices for work commissioned in this
way.
CHAPTER
3 (PAGES 57–113)
1. The racetrack study was done twice, with the same results, by Knox
and Inkster (1968). See Rosenfeld, Kennedy, and Giacalone (1986) for
evidence that the tendency to believe more strongly in choices, once
made, applies to guesses in a lottery game, too.
2. It is important to note that the collaboration was not always intentional. The American investigators defined collaboration as “any kind
214 / Influence
of behavior which helped the enemy,” and it thus included such diverse
activities as signing peace petitions, running errands, making radio
appeals, accepting special favors, making false confessions, informing
on fellow prisoners, or divulging military information.
3. The Schein quote comes from his 1956 article “The Chinese Indoctrination Program for Prisoners of War: A Study of Attempted Brainwashing.”
4. See Greene (1965) for the source of this advice.
5. Freedman and Fraser published their data in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, in 1966.
6. The quote comes from Freedman and Fraser (1966).
7. See Segal (1954) for the article from which this quote originates.
8. See Jones and Harris (1967).
9. It is noteworthy that the housewives in this study (Kraut, 1973)
heard that they were considered charitable at least a full week before
they were asked to donate to the Multiple Sclerosis Association.
10. From “How to Begin Retailing,” Amway Corporation.
11. See Deutsch and Gerard (1955) and Kerr and MacCoun (1985) for
the details of these studies.
12. From Whiting, Kluckhohn, and Anthony (1958).
13. From Gordon and Gordon (1963).
14. The survey was conducted by Walker (1967).
15. The electric-shock experiment was published seven years after
the Aronson and Mills (1959) study by Gerard and Mathewson (1966).
16. Young (1965) conducted this research.
17. The robot study is reported fully in Freedman (1965).
18. The reader who wishes stronger evidence for the action of the
lowball tactic than my subjective observations in the car showroom
may refer to articles that attest to its effectiveness under controlled,
experimental conditions: Cialdini et al. (1978), Burger and Petty (1981),
Brownstein and Katzev (1985), and Joule (1987).
19. A formal report of the energy-conservation project appears in
Pallak et al. (1980).
20. It is not altogether unusual for even some of our most familiar
quotations to be truncated by time in ways that greatly modify their
character. For example, it is not money that the Bible claims as the root
of all evil, it is the love of money. So as not to be guilty of the same sort
of error myself, I should note that the Emerson quote from “Self-Reliance” is somewhat longer and substantially more textured than I have
reported. In full, it reads, “A foolish consistency is the hobgoblin of
little minds adored by little statesmen, and philosophers, and divines.”
21. See Zajonc (1980) for a summary of this evidence.
22. This is not to say that what we feel about an issue is always differ-
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 215
ent from or always to be trusted more than what we think about it.
However, the data are clear that our emotions and beliefs often do not
point in the same direction. Therefore, in situations involving a decisional commitment likely to have generated supporting rationalizations,
feelings may well provide the truer counsel. This would be especially
so when, as in the question of Sara’s happiness, the fundamental issue
at hand concerns an emotion (Wilson, 1989).
CHAPTER
4 (PAGES 114–166)
1. The general evidence regarding the facilitative effect of canned
laughter on responses to humor comes from such studies as Smyth and
Fuller (1972), Fuller and Sheehy-Skeffinton (1974), and Nosanchuk and
Lightstone the last of which contains the indication that canned laughter
is most effective for poor material.
2. The researchers who infiltrated the Graham Crusade and who
provided the quote are Altheide and Johnson (1977).
3. See Bandura, Grusec, and Menlove (1967) and Bandura and Menlove (1968) for full descriptions of the dog-phobia treatment.
Any reader who doubts that the seeming appropriateness of an action
is importantly influenced by the number of others performing it might
try a small experiment. Stand on a busy sidewalk, pick out an empty
spot in the sky or on a tall building, and stare at it for a full minute.
Very little will happen around you during that time—most people will
walk past without glancing up, and virtually no one will stop to stare
with you. Now, on the next day, go to the same place and bring along
four friends to look upward too. Within sixty seconds, a crowd of
passersby will have stopped to crane their necks skyward with the
group. For those pedestrians who do not join you, the pressure to look
up at least briefly will be nearly irresistible; if your experiment brings
the same results as the one performed by three New York social psychologists, you and your friends will cause 80 percent of all passersby to
lift their gaze to your empty spot (Milgram, Bickman, and Berkowitz,
1967).
4. Other research besides O’Connor’s (1972) suggests that there are
two sides to the filmed-social-proof coin, however. The dramatic effect
of filmed depictions on what children find appropriate has been a source
of great distress for those concerned with frequent instances of violence
and aggression on television. Although the consequences of televised
violence on the aggressive actions of children are far from simple, the
data from a well-controlled experiment by psychologists Robert Liebert
and Robert Baron (1972) have an ominous look. Some children were
shown excerpts from a television program in which people intentionally
216 / Influence
harmed another. Afterward, these children were significantly more
harmful toward another child than were children who had watched a
nonviolent television program (a horserace). The finding that seeing
others perform aggressively led to more aggression on the part of the
young viewers held true for the two age groups tested (five-to-six-yearolds and eight-to-nine-year-olds) and for both girls and boys.
5. An engagingly written report of their complete findings is
presented in Festinger, Riecken, and Schachter’s (1956) book When
Prophecy Fails.
6. Perhaps because of the quality of ragged desperation with which
they approached their task, the believers were wholly unsuccessful at
enlarging their number. Not a single convert was gained. At that point,
in the face of the twin failures of physical and social proof, the cult
quickly disintegrated. Less than three weeks after the date of the predicted flood, group members were scattered and maintaining only
sporadic communication with one another. In one final—and ironic—disconfirmation of prediction, it was the movement that perished in the
flood.
Ruin has not always been the fate of doomsday groups whose predictions proved unsound, however. When such groups have been able to
build social proof for their beliefs through effective recruitment efforts,
they have grown and prospered. For example, when the Dutch Anabaptists saw their prophesied year of destruction, 1533, pass uneventfully, they became rabid seekers after converts, pouring unprecedented
amounts of energy into the cause. One extraordinarily eloquent missionary, Jakob van Kampen, is reported to have baptized one hundred
persons in a single day. So powerful was the snowballing social evidence
in support of the Anabaptist position that it rapidly overwhelmed the
disconfirming physical evidence and turned two thirds of the population
of Holland’s great cities into adherents.
7. From Rosenthal’s Thirty-eight Witnesses, 1964.
8. This quote comes from Latané and Darley’s award-winning book
(1968), where they introduced the concept of pluralistic ignorance.
The potentially tragic consequences of the pluralistic ignorance phenomenon are starkly illustrated in a UPI news release from Chicago:
A university coed was beaten and strangled in daylight hours near
one of the most popular tourist attractions in the city, police said
Saturday.
The nude body of Lee Alexis Wilson, 23, was found Friday in
dense shrubbery alongside the wall of the Art Institute by a 12year-old boy playing in the bushes.
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 217
Police theorized she may have been sitting or standing by a
fountain in the Art Institute’s south plaza when she was attacked.
The assailant apparently then dragged her into the bushes. She
apparently was sexually assaulted, police said.
Police said thousands of persons must have passed the site and
one man told them he heard a scream about 2 P.M. but did not investigate because no one else seemed to be paying attention.
9. The New York “seizure” and “smoke” emergency studies are reported by Darley and Latané (1968) and Latané and Darley (1968), respectively. The Toronto experiment was performed by Ross (1971). The
Florida studies were published by Clark and Word in 1972 and 1974.
10. See a study by Latané and Rodin (1969) showing that groups of
strangers help less in an emergency than groups of acquaintances.
11. The wallet study was conducted by Hornstein et al. (1968), the
antismoking study by Murray et al. (1984), and the dental anxiety study
by Melamed et al. (1978).
12. The sources of these statistics are articles by Phillips in 1979 and
1980.
13. The newspaper story data are reported by Phillips (1974), while
the TV story data come from Bollen and Phillips (1982), Gould and
Schaffer (1986), Phillips and Carstensen (1986), and Schmidtke and
Hafner (1988).
14. These new data appear in Phillips (1983).
15. The quote is from The International Cyclopedia of Music and Musicians, 1964, which Sabin edited.
16. From Hornaday (1887).
CHAPTER
5 (PAGES 167–207)
1. The Canadian election study was reported by Efran and Patterson
(1976). Data of this sort give credence to the claim of some Richard
Nixon backers that the failure that contributed most to the loss of the
1960 TV debates with John F. Kennedy—and thereby to the election—was the poor performance of Nixon’s makeup man.
2. See Mack and Rainey (1990).
3. This finding—that attractive defendants, even when they are found
guilty, are less likely to be sentenced to prison—helps explain one of
the more fascinating experiments in criminology I have heard of (Kurtzburg et al., 1968). Some New York City jail inmates with facial disfigurements were given plastic surgery while incarcerated; others with
similar disfigurements were not. Furthermore, some of each of these
two groups of criminals were given services (for example, counseling
218 / Influence
and training) designed to rehabilitate them to society. One year after
their release, a check of the records revealed that (except for heroin
addicts) those given the cosmetic surgery were significantly less likely
to have returned to jail. The most interesting feature of this finding was
that it was equally true for those criminals who had not received the
traditional rehabilitative services as for those who had. Apparently,
some criminologists then argued, when it comes to ugly inmates, prisons
would be better off to abandon the costly rehabilitation treatments they
typically provide and offer plastic surgery instead; the surgery seems
to be at least as effective and decidedly less expensive.
The importance of the newer, Pennsylvania data (Stewart, 1980) is
its suggestion that the argument for surgery as a means of rehabilitation
may be faulty. Making an ugly criminal more attractive may not reduce
the chances that he will commit another crime; it may only reduce his
chances of being sent to jail for it.
4. The negligence-award study was done by Kulka and Kessler (1978),
the helping study by Benson et al. (1976), and the persuasion study by
Chaiken (1979).
5. An excellent review of this research is provided by Eagly et al.
(1991).
6. The dime-request experiment was conducted by Emswiller et al.
(1971), while the petition-signing experiment was done by Suedfeld et
al. (1971).
7. The insurance sales data were reported by Evans (1963). The
“mirroring and matching” evidence comes from work by LaFrance
(1985), Locke and Horowitz (1990), and Woodside and Davenport (1974).
Additional work suggests yet another reason for caution when dealing
with similar requesters: We typically underestimate the degree to which
similarity affects our liking for another (Gonzales et al., 1983).
8. See Drachman et al. (1978) for a complete description of the findings.
9. Bornstein (1989) summarizes much of this evidence.
10. The mirror study was performed by Mita et al. (1977).
11. For general evidence regarding the positive effect of familiarity
on attraction, see Zajonc (1968). For more specific evidence of this effect
on our response to politicians, the research of Joseph Grush is enlightening and sobering (Grush et al., 1978; Grush, 1980), in documenting a
strong connection between amount of media exposure and a candidate’s
chances of winning an election.
12. See Bornstein, Leone, and Galley (1987).
13. For an especially thorough examination of this issue, see Stephan
(1978).
14. The evidence of the tendency of ethnic groups to stay with their
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 219
own in school comes from Gerard and Miller (1975). The evidence for
the dislike of things repeatedly presented under unpleasant conditions
comes from such studies as Burgess and Sales (1971), Zajonc et al. (1974),
and Swap (1977).
15. From Aronson (1975).
16. A fascinating description of the entire boys’-camp project, called
the “Robbers’ Cave Experiment,” can be found in Sherif et al. (1961).
17. The Carlos example comes once again from Aronson’s initial report
in his 1975 article. However, additional reports by Aronson and by
others have shown similarly encouraging results. A representative list
would include Johnson and Johnson (1983), DeVries and Slavin (1978),
Cook (1990), and Aronson, Bridgeman, and Geffner (1978a, b).
18. For a careful examination of the possible pitfalls of cooperative
learning approaches, see Rosenfield and Stephan (1981).
19. In truth, little in the way of combat takes place when the salesman
enters the manager’s office under such circumstances. Often, because
the salesman knows exactly the price below which he cannot go, he
and the boss don’t even speak. In one car dealership I infiltrated while
researching this book, it was common for a salesman to have a soft
drink or cigarette in silence while the boss continued working at his
desk. After a seemly time, the salesman would loosen his tie and return
to his customers, looking weary but carrying the deal he had just
“hammered out” for them—the same deal he had in mind before entering the boss’s office.
20. For experimental evidence of the validity of Shakespeare’s observation, see Manis et al. (1974).
21. A review of research supporting this statement is provided by
Lott and Lott (1965).
22. See the study by Miller et al. (1966) for evidence.
23. The study was done by Smith and Engel (1968).
24. The rights to such associations don’t come cheaply. Corporate
sponsors spend millions to secure Olympic sponsorships, and they
spend many millions more to advertise their connections to the event.
Yet it may all be worth the expense. An Advertising Age survey found
that one third of all consumers said they would be more likely to purchase a product if it were linked to the Olympics.
25. The Georgia study was done by Rosen and Tesser (1970).
26. From Asimov (1975).
27. Both the sweatshirt and the pronoun experiments are reported
fully in Cialdini et al. (1976).
220 / Influence
CHAPTER
6 (PAGES 208–236)
1. The quote is from Milgram’s 1963 article in the Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology.
2. All of these variations on the basic experiment, as well as several
others, are presented in Milgram’s highly readable book Obedience to
Authority, 1974. A review of much of the subsequent research on obedience can be found in Blass (1991).
3. In fact, Milgram first began his investigations in an attempt to understand how the German citizenry could have participated in the
concentration-camp destruction of millions of innocents during the
years of Nazi ascendancy. After testing his experimental procedures in
the United States, he had planned to take them to Germany, a country
whose populace he was sure would provide enough obedience for a
full-blown scientific analysis of the concept. That first eye-opening experiment in New Haven, Connecticut, however, made it clear that he
could save his money and stay close to home. “I found so much obedience,” he has said, “I hardly saw the need of taking the experiment to
Germany.”
More telling evidence, perhaps, of a willingness within the American
character to submit to authorized command comes from a national
survey taken after the trial of Lieutenant William Calley, who ordered
his soldiers to kill the inhabitants—from the infants and toddlers
through their parents and grandparents—of My Lai, Vietnam (Kelman
and Hamilton, 1989). A majority of Americans (51 percent) responded
that, if so ordered, in a similar context, they too would shoot all the
residents of a Vietnamese village. But Americans have no monopoly
on the need to obey. When Milgram’s basic procedure has been repeated
in Holland, Germany, Spain, Italy, Australia, and Jordan, the results
have been similar. See Meeus and Raaijmakers for a review.
4. We are not the only species to give sometimes wrongheaded deference to those in authority positions. In monkey colonies, where rigid
dominance hierarchies exist, beneficial innovations (like learning how
to use a stick to bring food into the cage area) do not spread quickly
through the group unless they are taught first to a dominant animal.
When a lower animal is taught the new concept first, the rest of the
colony remains mostly oblivious to its value. One study, cited by Ardry
(1970), on the introduction of new food tastes to Japanese monkeys
provides a nice illustration. In one troop, a taste for caramels was developed by introducing this new food into the diet of young peripherals,
low on the status ladder. The taste for caramels inched slowly up the
ranks: A year and a half later, only 51 percent of the colony had acquired
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 221
it, and still none of the leaders. Contrast this with what happened in a
second troop where wheat was introduced first to the leader: Wheat
eating—to this point unknown to these monkeys—spread through the
whole colony within four hours.
5. The experiment was performed by Wilson (1968).
6. The study on children’s judgments of coins was done by Bruner
and Goodman (1947). The study on college students’ judgments was
done by Dukes and Bevan (1952). In addition to the relationship between
importance (status) and perceived size that both of these experiments
show, there is even some evidence that the importance we assign to
our identity is reflected in the size of a frequent symbol of that identity:
our signature. The psychologist Richard Zweigenhaft (1970) has collected data suggesting that as a man’s sense of his own status grows, so
does the size of his signature. This finding may give us a secret way of
discovering how the people around us view their own status and importance: Simply compare the size of their signature to that of their
other handwriting.
7. Subhumans are not alone in this regard, even in modern times. For
example, since 1900 the U.S. presidency has been won by the taller of
the major-party candidates in twenty-one of the twenty-four elections.
8. From Hofling et al. (1966).
9. Additional data collected in the same study suggest that nurses
may not be conscious of the extent to which the title Doctor sways their
judgments and actions. A separate group of thirty-three nurses and
student nurses were asked what they would have done in the experimental situation. Contrary to the actual findings, only two predicted
that they would have given the medication as ordered.
10. See Bickman (1974) for a complete account of this research. Similar
results have been obtained when the requester was female (Bushman,
1988).
11. This experiment was conducted by Lefkowitz, Blake, and Mouton
(1955).
12. The horn-honking study was published in 1968 by Anthony Doob
and Alan Gross.
13. For evidence, see Choo (1964), and McGuinnies and Ward (1980).
14. See Settle and Gorden (1974), Smith and Hunt (1978), and Hunt,
Domzal, and Kernan (1981).
CHAPTER
7 (PAGES 237–272)
1. The home-insulation study was done by Gonzales, Costanzo, and
Aronson (1988) in northern California; the breast-examination work
was conducted by Meyerwitz and Chaiken (1987) in New York City.
222 / Influence
2. See Schwartz (1980) for evidence of such a process.
3. See Lynn (1989). Without wishing to minimize the advantages of
this type of shortcut or the dangers associated with it, I should note
that these advantages and dangers are essentially the same ones we
have examined in previous chapters. Accordingly, I will not focus on
this theme in the remainder of the present chapter, except to say at this
point that the key to using properly the shortcut feature of scarcity is
to be alert to the distinction between naturally occurring, honest scarcity
and the fabricated variety favored by certain compliance practitioners.
4. The original reactance-theory formulation appeared in Brehm
(1966); a subsequent version appears in Brehm and Brehm (1981).
5. Brehm and Weintraub (1977) did the barrier experiment. It should
be noted that two-year-old girls in the study did not show the same
resistant response to the large barrier as did the boys. This does not
seem to be because girls don’t oppose attempts to limit their freedoms.
Instead, it appears that they are primarily reactant to restrictions that
come from other people rather than from physical barriers (Brehm,
1983).
6. For descriptions of the two-year-old’s change in self-perception,
see Mahler et al. (1975), Lewis and Brooks-Gunn (1979), Brooks-Gun
and Lewis (1982), and Levine (1983).
7. The occurrence of the Romeo and Juliet effect should not be interpreted as a warning to parents to be always accepting of their teenagers’
romantic choices. New players at this delicate game are likely to err
often and, consequently, would benefit from the direction of an adult
with greater perspective and experience. In providing such direction,
parents should recognize that teenagers, who see themselves as young
adults, will not respond well to control attempts that are typical of
parent-child relationships. Especially in the clearly adult arena of mating, adult tools of influence (preference and persuasion) will be more
effective than traditional forms of parental control (prohibitions and
punishments). Although the experience of the Montague and Capulet
families is an extreme example, heavy-handed restrictions on a young
romantic alliance may well turn it clandestine, torrid, and sad.
A full description of the Colorado couples study can be found in
Driscoll et al. (1972).
8. See Mazis (1975) and Mazis et al. (1973) for formal reports of the
phosphate study.
9. For evidence, see Ashmore et al. (1971), Wicklund and Brehm
(1974), Worchel and Arnold (1973), Worchel et al. (1975), and Worchel
(1991).
10. The Purdue study was done by Zellinger et al. (1974).
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 223
11. The University of Chicago jury experiment on inadmissible
evidence was reported by Broeder (1959).
12. The initial statements of commodity theory appeared in Brock
(1968) and Fromkin and Brock (1971). For an updated statement, see
Brock and Bannon (1992).
13. For ethical reasons, the information provided to the customers
was always true. There was an impending beef shortage and this news
had, indeed, come to the company through its exclusive sources. See
Knishinsky (1982) for full details of the project.
14. Worchel et al. (1975).
15. See Davies (1962, 1969).
16. See Lytton (1979), and Rosenthal and Robertson (1959).
17. The quote comes from MacKenzie (1974).
EPILOGUE (PAGES
273–280)
1. For evidence of such perceptual and decisional narrowing see
Berkowitz (1967), Bodenhausen (1990), Cohen (1978), Easterbrook (1959),
Gilbert and Osborn (1989), Hockey and Hamilton (1970), Mackworth
(1965), Milgram (1970), and Tversky and Kahnemann (1974).
2. Quoted in the PBS-TV documentary The Information Society.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Altheide, D. L., and J. M. Johnson. “Counting Souls: A Study of Counseling at Evangelical Crusades.” Pacific Sociological Review 20 (1977):
323–48.
Ardry, R. The Social Contract. New York: Atheneum, 1970.
Aronson, E. “The Jigsaw Route to Learning and Liking.” Psychology
Today, Feb. 1975.
——, D. L. Bridgeman, and R. Geffner. “The Effects of a Cooperative
Classroom Structure on Students’ Behavior and Attitudes.” In Social
Psychology of Education: Theory and Research, edited by D. Bar-Tal and
L. Saxe. New York: Halstead Press, 1978.
——. “Interdependent Interactions and Prosocial Behavior.” Journal of
Research and Development in Education 12 (1978): 16–27.
Aronson, E., and D. L. Linder. “Gain and Loss of Esteem as Determinants
of Interpersonal Attractiveness.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 1 (1965): 156–71.
Aronson, E., and J. Mills. “The Effect of Severity of Initiation on Liking
for a Group.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 59 (1959):
177–81.
Aronson, E. et al. The Jigsaw Classroom. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications,
1978.
Ashmore, R. D., V. Ramchandra, and R. A. Jones. “Censorship as an
Attitude Change Induction.” Paper presented at Eastern Psychological
Association meetings, New York, April 1971.
Asimov, I. “The Miss America Pageant.” TV Guide, Aug. 30, 1975.
Bandura, A., J. E. Grusec, and F. L. Menlove. “Vicarious Extinction of
Avoidance Behavior.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 5
(1967): 16–23.
Bandura, A., and F. L. Menlove. “Factors Determining Vicarious Extinction of Avoidance Behavior Through Symbolic Modeling.” Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 8 (1968): 99–108.
Benson, P. L., S. A. Karabenic, and R. M. Lerner. “Pretty Pleases: The
226 / Influence
Effects of Physical Attractiveness on Race, Sex, and Receiving Help.”
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 12 (1976): 409–15.
Benton, A. A., H. H. Kelley, and B. Liebling. “Effects of Extremity of
Offers and Concession Rate on the Outcomes of Bargaining.” Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology 24 (1972): 73–83.
Berkowitz, L., and R. W. Buck. “Impulsive Aggression: Reactivity to
Aggressive Cues Under Emotional Arousal.” Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology 35 (1967): 415–24.
Berry, S. H., and D. E. Kanouse. “Physician Response to a Mailed Survey:
An Experiment in Timing of Payment.” Public Opinion Quarterly 51
(1987): 102–14.
Bickman, L. “The Social Power of a Uniform.” Journal of Applied Social
Psychology 4 (1974): 47–61.
Blass, T. “Understanding Behavior in the Milgram Obedience Experiment.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60 (1991): 398–413.
Bodenhausen, G. V. “Stereotypes as Judgmental Heuristics: Evidence
of Circadian Variations in Discrimination.” Psychological Science 1
(1990): 319–22.
Bollen, K. A., and D. P. Phillips. “Imitative Suicides: A National Study
of the Effects of Television News Stories.” American Sociological Review
47 (1982): 802–09.
Bornstein, R. F. “Exposure and Affect.” Psychological Bulletin 106 (1989):
265–89.
——, D. R. Leone, and D. J. Galley. “The Generalizability of Subliminal
Mere Exposure Effects.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53
(1987): 1070–79.
Brehm, J. W. A Theory of Psychological Reactance. New York: Academic
Press, 1966.
Brehm, S. S. “Psychological Reactance and the Attractiveness of Unattainable Objects: Sex Differences in Children’s Responses to an
Elimination of Freedom.” Sex Roles 7 (1981): 937–49.
——, and J. W. Brehm. Psychological Reactance. New York: Academic
Press, 1981.
Brehm, S. S., and M. Weintraub. “Physical Barriers and Psychological
Reactance: Two-year-olds’ Responses to Threats to Freedom.” Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology 35 (1977): 830–36.
Brock, T. C. “Implications of Commodity Theory for Value Change.”
In Psychological Foundations of Attitudes, edited by A. G. Greenwald,
T. C. Brock, and T. M. Ostrom. New York: Academic Press, 1968.
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 227
——, and L. A. Brannon. “Liberalization of Commodity Theory.” Basic
and Applied Social Psychology 13 (1992): 135–43.
Broeder, D. “The University of Chicago Jury Project.” Nebraska Law Review 38 (1959): 760–74.
Brooks-Gunn, J., and M. Lewis. “The Development of Self-Knowledge.”
In The Child, edited by C. B. Kopp and J. B. Krakow. Reading, Mass.:
Addison-Wesley, 1982.
Brownstein, R., and R. Katzev. “The Relative Effectiveness of Three
Compliance Techniques in Eliciting Donations to a Cultural Organization.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology 15 (1985): 564–74.
Bruner, J. S., and C. C. Goodman. “Value and Need as Organizing
Factors in Perception.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 42
(1947): 33–44.
Burger, J. M., and R. E. Petty. “The Low-Ball Compliance Technique:
Task or Person Commitment?” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 40 (1981): 492–500.
Burgess, T., and S. Sales. “Attitudinal Effects of ‘Mere Exposure’: A
Reevaluation.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 7 (1971):
461–72.
Bushman, B. A. “The Effects of Apparel on Compliance.” Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin 14 (1988): 459–67.
Chaiken, S. “Communicator Physical Attractiveness and Persuasion.”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37 (1979): 1387–97.
——, and C. Stangor. “Attitudes and Attitude Change.” Annual Review
of Psychology 38 (1987): 575–630.
Choo, T. “Communicator Credibility and Communication Discrepancy
as Determinants of Opinion Change.” Journal of Social Psychology 64
(1964): 1–20.
Cialdini, R. B. “Full Cycle Social Psychology,” Applied Social Psychology
Annual, Vol. 1, ed. L. Beckman. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage, 1980.
——, and K. Ascani. “Test of a Concession Procedure for Inducing
Verbal, Behavioral, and Further Compliance with a Request to Give
Blood.” Journal of Applied Psychology 61 (1976): 295–300.
Cialdini, R. B., R. R. Reno, and C. A. Kallgren. “A Focus Theory of
Normative Conduct: Recycling the Concept of Norms to Reduce
Littering in Public Places.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
58 (1990): 1015–26.
Cialdini, R. B. et al. “Basking in Reflected Glory: Three (Football) Field
Studies.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 34 (1976): 366–75.
228 / Influence
Cialdini, R. B. et al. “The Low-Ball Procedure for Producing Compliance:
Commitment, Then Cost.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
36 (1978): 463–76.
Cialdini, R. B. et al. “Reciprocal Concessions Procedure for Inducing
Compliance: The Door-in-the-Face Technique.” Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology 31 (1975): 206–15.
——. “Why Don’t Bystanders Help? Because of Ambiguity?” Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 24 (1972): 392–400.
Clark, M. S., J. R. Mills, and D. M. Corcoran. “Keeping Track of Needs
and Inputs of Friends and Strangers.” Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin 15 (1989): 533–42.
Clark, R. D. III, and L. E. Word. “Where Is the Apathetic Bystander?
Situational Characteristics of the Emergency.” Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology 29 (1974): 279–87.
Cohen, M., and N. Davis. Medication Errors: Causes and Prevention.
Philadelphia: G. F. Stickley, 1981.
Cohen, S. “Environmental Load and the Allocation of Attention,” Advances in Environmental Psychology, Vol. 1, ed. A. Baum, J. E. Singer,
and S. Valins. New York: Halstead Press, 1978.
Cook, S. W. “Interpersonal and Attitudinal Outcomes in Cooperating
Interracial Groups.” Journal of Research and Development in Education
12 (1978): 97–113.
——. “Toward a Psychology of Improving Justice.” Journal of Social Issues
46 (1990): 147–61.
Crane, D. Invisible Colleges. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1972.
Darley, J. M., and B. Latané. “Bystander Intervention in Emergencies:
Diffusion of Responsibility.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
8 (1968): 377–83.
Davies, J. C. “The J-Curve of Rising and Declining Satisfactions as a
Cause of Some Great Revolutions and a Contained Rebellion.” In
Violence in America, edited by H. D. Graham and T. R. Gurr. New
York: Signet Books, 1969.
——. “Toward a Theory of Revolution.” American Sociological Review
27 (1962): 5–19.
Deutsch, M., and H. B. Gerard, “A Study of Normative and Informational Social Influences upon Individual Judgment.” Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology 51 (1955): 629–36.
De Vries, D. L., and R. E. Slavin. “Teams-Games-Tournaments (TGT):
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 229
Review of Ten Classroom Experiments.” Journal of Research and Development in Education 12 (1978): 28–38.
Dion, K. K. “Physical Attractiveness and Evaluation of Children’s
Transgressions.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 24 (1972):
207–13.
Doob, A. N., and A. E. Gross. “Status of Frustrator as an Inhibitor of
Horn-Honking Responses.” Journal of Social Psychology 76 (1968):
213–18.
Drachman, D., A. deCarufel, and C. A. Inkso. “The Extra Credit Effect
in Interpersonal Attraction.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
14 (1978): 458–67.
Driscoll, R., K. E. Davies, and M. E. Lipetz. “Parental Interference and
Romantic Love: The Romeo and Juliet Effect.” Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology 24 (1972): 1–10.
Dukes, W. F., and W. Bevan. “Accentuation and Response Variability
in the Perception of Personally Relevant Objects.” Journal of Personality
20 (1952): 457–65.
Eagly, A. H., et al. “What Is Beautiful Is Good, but…: A Meta-Analytic
Review of Research on the Physical Attractiveness Stereotype.” Psychological Bulletin 110 (1990): 109–28.
Easterbrook, J. A. “The Effects of Emotion on Cue Utilization and the
Organization of Behavior.” Psychological Review 66 (1959): 183–201.
Efran, M. G., and E.W.J. Patterson. “The Politics of Appearance.” Unpublished manuscript, University of Toronto, 1976.
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. “Der Fisch Aspidontus taeniatus als Machahmer des
Putzers Labroides dimidiatus.” Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie 16 (1959):
19–25.
Emswiller, T., K. Deaux, and J. E. Willits. “Similarity, Sex, and Requests
for Small Favors.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology (1971): 284–91.
Evans, F. B. American Behavioral Scientist 6:7 (1963): 76–79.
Festinger, L., H. W. Riecken, and S. Schachter. When Prophecy Fails.
Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1956.
Fox, M. W. Concepts in Ethology: Animal and Human Behavior. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1974.
Freedman, J. L. “Long-term Behavioral Effects of Cognitive Dissonance.”
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 4 (1966): 195–203.
——, and S. C. Fraser. “Compliance Without Pressure: The Foot-in-theDoor Technique.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 4 (1966):
195–203.
230 / Influence
Fromkin, H. L., and T. C. Brock. “A Commodity Theory Analysis of
Persuasion.” Representative Research in Social Psychology 2 (1971): 47–57.
Fuller, R.G.C., and A. Sheehy-Skeffinton. “Effects of Group Laughter
on Responses to Humorous Materials: A Replication and Extension.”
Psychological Reports 35 (1974): 531–34.
George, W. H., S. J. Gournic, and M. P. McAfee “Perceptions of
Postdrinking Female Sexuality.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology
18 (1988): 1295–1317.
Gerard, H. B., and G. C. Mathewson. “The Effects of Severity of Initiation
on Liking for a Group: A Replication.” Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology 2 (1966): 278–87.
Gerard, H. B., and N. Miller. School Desegregation. New York: Plenum,
1975.
Gergen, K. et al. “Obligation, Donor Resources, and Reactions to Aid
in Three Cultures.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 31 (1975):
390–400.
Gilbert, D. T., and R. E. Osborne. “Thinking Backward: Some Curable
and Incurable Consequences of Cognitive Busyness.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57 (1989): 940–49.
Gonzales, M. H., E. Aronson, and M. Costanzo. “Increasing the Effectiveness of Energy Auditors: A Field Experiment.” Journal of Applied
Social Psychology 18 (1988): 1046–66.
Gonzales, M. H. et al. “Interactional Approach to Interpersonal Attraction.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44 (1983): 1192–97.
Gordon, R. E., and K. Gordon. The Blight on the Ivy. Englewood Cliffs,
N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1963.
Gould, M. S., and D. Schaffer. “The Impact of Suicide in Television
Movies.” The New England Journal of Medicine 315 (1986): 690–94.
Gouldner, A. W. “The Norm of Reciprocity: A Preliminary Statement.”
American Sociological Review 25 (1960): 161–78.
Green, F. “The ‘Foot-in-the-Door’ Technique.” American Salesman 10
(1965): 14–16.
Greenberg, M. S., and S. P. Shapiro. “Indebtedness: An Adverse Aspect
of Asking for and Receiving Help.” Sociometry 34 (1971): 290–301.
Greenwald, A. F. et al. “Increasing Voting Behavior by Asking People
if They Expect to Vote.” Journal of Applied Psychology 72 (1987):
315–318.
Grush, J. E. “Impact of Candidate Expenditures, Regionality, and Prior
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 231
Outcomes on the 1976 Democratic Presidential Primaries.” Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 38 (1980): 337–47.
——, K. L. McKeough, and R. F. Ahlering. “Extrapolating Laboratory
Exposure Experiments to Actual Political Elections.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 36 (1978): 257–70.
Hockey, G.R.J., and P. Hamilton. “Arousal and Information Selection
in Short-term Memory.” Nature 226 (1970): 866–67.
Hofling, C. K. et al. “An Experimental Study of Nurse-Physician Relationships.” Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 143 (1966): 171–80.
Hölldobler, B. “Communication Between Ants and Their Guests.” Scientific American 198 (Jan.) (1971): 68–76.
Hornaday, W. T. “The Extermination of the American Bison, with a
Sketch of Its Discovery and Life History.” Smithsonian Report, 1887,
Part II, 367–548.
Hornstein, H. A., E. Fisch, and M. Holmes. “Influence of a Model’s
Feeling About His Behavior and His Relevance as a Comparison
Other on Observers’ Helping Behavior.” Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology 10 (1968): 222–26.
Howard, D. J. “The Influence of Verbal Responses to Common Greetings
on Compliance Behavior: The Foot-in-the-Mouth Effect.” Journal of
Applied Social Psychology 20 (1990): 1185–96.
Hunt, J. M., T.J. Domzal, and J. B. Kernan. “Causal Attribution and
Persuasion: The Case of Disconfirmed Expectancies,” Advances in
Consumer Research, Vol. 9, ed. A. Mitchell. Ann Arbor, Mich.: Association for Consumer Research, 1981.
Johnson, D. W., and R. T. Johnson. Learning Together and Learning Alone.
Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1975.
——. “The Socialization and Achievement Crisis: Are Cooperative
Learning Experiences the Solution?” Applied Social Psychology Annual,
Vol. 4, ed. L. Bickman. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage, 1983.
Jones, E. E., and V. E. Harris. “The Attribution of Attitudes.” Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology 3 (1967): 1–24.
Jones, E. E., and C. Wortman. Ingratiation: An Attributional Approach.
Morristown, N.J.: General Learning Corp., 1973.
Joule, R. V. “Tobacco Deprivation: The Foot-in-the-Door Technique
Versus the Low-Ball Technique.” European Journal of Social Psychology
17 (1987): 361–65.
Kelman, H. C., and V. L. Hamilton. Crimes of Obedience. New Haven,
Conn.: Yale University Press, 1989.
232 / Influence
Kelman, J., and L. Lawrence. “Assignment of Responsibility in the Case
of Lt. Calley: Preliminary Report on a National Survey.” Journal of
Social Issues 28:1 (1978).
Kenrick, D. T., and S. E. Gutierres. “Contrast Effects in Judgments of
Attractiveness: When Beauty Becomes a Social Problem.” Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 38 (1980): 131–40.
——, and L. L. Goldberg. “Influence of Popular Erotica on Judgments
of Strangers and Mates.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 25
(1989): 159–67.
Kerr, N. L., and R. J. MacCoun. “The Effects of Jury Size and Polling
Method on the Process and Product of Jury Deliberation.” Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 48 (1985): 349–63.
Knishinsky, A. “The Effects of Scarcity of Material and Exclusivity of
Information on Industrial Buyer Perceived Risk in Provoking a Purchase Decision.” Doctoral dissertation, Arizona State University, 1982.
Knox, R. E., and J. A. Inkster. “Postdecisional Dissonance at Post Time.”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 8 (1968): 319–23.
Kraut, R. E. “Effects of Social Labeling on Giving to Charity.” Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology 9 (1973): 551–62.
Kulka, R. A., and J. R. Kessler. “Is Justice Really Blind? The Effect of
Litigant Physical Attractiveness on Judicial Judgment.” Journal of
Applied Social Psychology 4 (1978): 336–81.
Kunz, P. R., and M. Woolcott. “Season’s Greetings: From My Status to
Yours.” Social Science Research, 5 (1976): 269–78.
Kurtzburg, R. L., H. Safar, and N. Cavior. “Surgical and Social Rehabilitation of Adult Offenders.” Proceedings of the 76th Annual Convention
of the American Psychological Association 3 (1968): 649–50.
Lack, D. The Life of the Robin. London: Cambridge University Press, 1943.
LaFrance, M. “Postural Mirroring and Intergroup Relations.” Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin 11 (1985): 207–17.
Langer, E. J. “Rethinking the Role of Thought in Social Interaction,”
New Directions in Attribution Research, Vol. 2., ed., Harvey, Ickes, and
Kidd. Potomac, Md.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1978.
——. “Minding Matters,” Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,
Vol. 22, ed. L. Berkowitz. New York: Academic Press, 1989.
Latané, B., and J. M. Darley. “Group Inhibition of Bystander Intervention
in Emergencies.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 10 (1968):
215–21.
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 233
——. The Unresponsive Bystander: Why Doesn’t He Help? New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1968.
Latané, B., and J. Rodin. “A Lady in Distress: Inhibiting Effects of Friends
and Strangers on Bystander Intervention.” Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology 5 (1969): 189–202.
Leakey, R., and R. Lewin. People of the Lake. New York: Anchor
Press/Doubleday, 1978.
Lefkowitz, M., R. R. Blake, and J. S. Mouton. “Status Factors in Pedestrian Violation of Traffic Signals.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 51 (1955): 704–06.
Levine, L. E. “Mine: Self-Definition in Two-Year-Old Boys.” Developmental Psychology 19 (1983): 544–49.
Lewis, M., and J. Brooks-Gunn. Social Cognition and the Acquisition of
Self. New York: Plenum, 1979.
Liebert, R., and R. A. Baron. “Some Immediate Effects of Televised Violence on Children’s Behavior.” Developmental Psychology 6 (1972):
469–75.
Lloyd, J. E. “Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris: Firefly Femme Fatales.”
Science 149 (1965): 653–54.
Locke, K. S., and L. M. Horowitz. “Satisfaction in Interpersonal Interactions as a Function of Similarity in Level of Dysphoria. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 58 (1990): 823–31.
Lott, A. J., and B. E. Lott. “Group Cohesiveness as Interpersonal Attraction: A Review of Relationships with Antecedent and Consequent
Variables.” Psychological Bulletin 64 (1965): 259–309.
Lytton, J. “Correlates of Compliance and the Rudiments of Conscience
in Two-Year-Old Boys.” Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science 9 (1979):
242–51.
McGuinnies, E., and C. D. Ward. “Better Liked Than Right: Trustworthiness and Expertise as Factors in Credibility.” Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin 6 (1980): 467–72.
Mack, D., and D. Rainey. “Female Applicants’ Grooming and Personnel
Selection.” Journal of Social Behavior and Personality 5 (1990): 399–407.
MacKenzie, B. “When Sober Executives Went on a Bidding Binge.” TV
Guide, June 22, 1974.
Mackworth, N. H. “Visual Noise Causes Tunnel Vision.” Psychonomic
Science 3 (1965): 67–68.
Macrae, N. “Multinational Business.” The Economist (London), Jan. 22,
1972.
234 / Influence
Magruder, J. S. An American Life: One Man’s Road to Watergate. New
York: Atheneum, 1974.
Mahler, M. S., F. Pine, and A. Bergman. The Psychological Birth of the Infant. New York: Basic Books, 1975.
Manis, M., S. D. Cornell, and J. C. Moore. “Transmission of Attitude
Relevant Information Through a Communication Chain.” Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 30 (1974): 81–94.
Mauss, Marcel. The Gift, trans. I. G. Cunnison. London: Cohen and West,
1954.
Mazis, M. B. “Antipollution Measures and Psychological Reactance
Theory: A Field Experiment.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 31 (1975): 654–66.
——, R. B. Settle, and D. C. Leslie. “Elimination of Phosphate Detergents
and Psychological Reactance.” Journal of Marketing Research 10 (1973):
390–95.
Meeus, W.H.J., and Q.A.W. Raaijmakers. “Administrative Obedience:
Carrying Out Orders to Use Psychological-Administrative Violence.”
European Journal of Social Psychology 16 (1986): 311–24.
Melamed, B. F. et al. “The Effects of Film Modeling on the Reduction
of Anxiety-related Behaviors.” Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 46 (1978): 1357–74.
Milgram, S. “Behavorial Study of Obedience.” Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology 67 (1963): 371–78.
——. “The Experience of Living in Cities.” Science 13 (1970): 1461–68.
——. Obedience to Authority. New York: Harper & Row, 1974.
——, L. Bickman, and O. Berkowitz. “Note on the Drawing Power of
Crowds of Different Size.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
13 (1969): 79–82.
Milgram, S., and J. Sabini. “On Maintaining Norms: A Field Experiment
in the Subway.” Unpublished manuscript, City University of New
York, 1975.
Miller, N. et al. “Similarity, Contrast, and Complementarity in Friendship Choice.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 3 (1966): 3–12.
Miller, R. L. et al. “Perceptual Contrast Versus Reciprocal Concession
as Mediators of Induced Compliance.” Canadian Journal of Behavioral
Science 8 (1976): 401–09.
Mita, T. H., M. Dermer, and J. Knight. “Reversed Facial Images and the
Mere Exposure Hypothesis.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
35 (1977): 597–601.
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 235
Monahan, F. Women in Crime. New York: Ives Washburn, 1941.
Moriarty, T. “Crime, Commitment, and the Responsive Bystander.”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 31 (1975): 370–76.
Murray, D. A. et al. “The Prevention of Cigarette Smoking in Children:
A Comparison of Four Strategies.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology
14 (1984): 274–88.
Nosanchuk, T. A., and J. Lightstone. “Canned Laughter and Public and
Private Conformity.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 29
(1974): 153–56.
O’Connor, R. D. “Relative Efficacy of Modeling, Shaping, and the
Combined Procedures for Modification of Social Withdrawal.”
Journal of Abnormal Psychology 79 (1972): 327–34.
Packard, V. The Hidden Persuaders. New York: D. McKay Co., 1957.
Pallak, M. S., D. A. Cook, and J. J. Sullivan. “Commitment and Energy
Conservation.” Applied Social Psychology Annual 1 (1980): 235–53.
Peiponen, V. A. “Verhaltensstudien am blaukehlchen.” Ornis Fennica
37 (1960): 69–83.
Pekkanen, J. The Best Doctors in the U.S. New York: Seaview Books, 1971.
Pennebaker, J. W. et al. “Don’t the Girls Get Prettier at Closing Time.”
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 5 (1979): 122–25.
Phillips, D. P. “Airplane Accidents, Murder, and the Mass Media: Towards a Theory of Imitation and Suggestion.” Social Forces 58 (1980):
1001–24.
——. “The Impact of Mass Media Violence on U.S. Homicides.” American Sociological Review 48 (1983): 560–68.
——. “The Influence of Suggestion on Suicide: Substantive and Theoretical Implications of the Werther Effect.” American Sociological Review
39 (1974): 340–54.
——. “Suicide, Motor Vehicle Fatalities, and the Mass Media: Evidence
Toward a Theory of Suggestion.” American Journal of Sociology 84
(1979): 1150–74.
——, and L. L. Carstensen. “The Effect of Suicide Stories on Various
Demographic Groups, 1968–1985.” Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior 18 (1988): 100–14.
Razran, G.H.S. “Conditioned Response Changes in Rating and Appraising Sociopolitical Slogans.” Psychological Bulletin 37 (1940): 481.
——. “Conditioning Away Social Bias by the Luncheon Technique.”
Psychological Bulletin 35 (1938): 693.
236 / Influence
Regan, D. T. “Effects of a Favor and Liking on Compliance.” Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology 7 (1971): 627–39.
Rich, J. “Effects of Children’s Physical Attractiveness on Teachers’
Evaluations.” Journal of Educational Psychology 67 (1975): 599–609.
Rosen, S., and A. Tesser. “On the Reluctance to Communicate Undesirable Information: The MUM Effect.” Sociometry 33 (1970): 253–63.
Rosenfeld, P., J. G. Kennedy, and R. A. Giacalone. “Decision Making:
A Demonstration of the Postdecision Dissonance Effect.” Journal of
Social Psychology 126 (1986): 663–65.
Rosenfield, D., and W. G. Stephan. “Intergroup Relations Among
Children.” In Developmental Social Psychology, edited by S. Brehm, S.
Kassin, and F. Gibbons. New York: Oxford University Press, 1981.
Rosenthal, A. M. Thirty-eight Witnesses. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1964.
Rosenthal, M. J., E. Ni, and R. E. Robertson. “A Study of Mother-Child
Relationships in the Emotional Disorders of Children.” Genetic Psychology Monographs 60 (1959): 65–116.
Ross, A. S. “Effect of Increased Responsibility on Bystander Intervention:
The Presence of Children.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
19 (1971): 306–10.
Russell, D. “Leave It to the Merry Prankster, the Artful Dodger, and
the Body Puncher,” TV Guide, Dec. 16, 1978.
Sabin, R. The International Cyclopedia of Music and Musicians. New York:
Dodd, Mead, 1964.
Schein, E. “The Chinese Indoctrination Program for Prisoners of War:
A Study of Attempted ‘Brainwashing.’” Psychiatry 19 (1956): 149–72.
Schmidtke, A., and H. Hafner. “The Werther Effect After Television
Films: New Evidence for an Old Hypothesis.” Psychological Medicine
18 (1988): 665–76.
Schwarz, N. “Experimentelle Untersuchungen zur Reduktion durch
Freiheitswerderstellung.” Doctoral dissertation, Universität Mannheim, 1980.
Schwarzwald, J., M. Raz, and M. Zvibel. “The Efficacy of the Door-inthe-Face Technique When Established Behavioral Customs Exist.”
Journal of Applied Social Psychology 9 (1979): 576–86.
Segal, H. A. “Initial Psychiatric Findings of Recently Repatriated Prisoners of War.” American Journal of Psychiatry 61 (1954): 358–63.
Settle, R. B., and L. L. Gorden. “Attribution Theory and Advertiser
Credibility.” Journal of Marketing Research 11 (1974): 181–85.
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 237
Sherif, M. et al. Intergroup Conflict and Cooperation: The Robbers’ Cave
Experiment. Norman, Okla.: University of Oklahoma Institute of Intergroup Relations, 1961.
Sherman, S. J. “On the Self-Erasing Nature of Errors of Prediction.”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39 (1980): 211–21.
Smith, G. H., and R. Engel. “Influence of a Female Model on Perceived
Characteristics of an Automobile.” Proceedings of the 76th Annual
Convention of the American Psychological Association 3 (1968): 681–82.
Smith, M. M., and R.G.C. Fuller. “Effects of Group Laughter on Responses to Humorous Materials.” Psychological Reports 30 (1972):
132–34.
Smith, R. E., and S. D. Hunt. “Attributional Processes in Promotional
Situations.” Journal of Consumer Research 5 (1978): 149–58.
Stephan, W. G. “School Desegregation: An Evaluation of Predictions
Made in Brown v. Board of Education.” Psychological Bulletin 85 (1978):
217–38.
Stewart, J. E., II. “Defendant’s Attractiveness as a Factor in the Outcome
of Trials.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology 10 (1980): 348–61.
Styron, W. “A Farewell to Arms.” New York Review of Books 24 (1977):
3–4.
Suedfeld, P., S. Bochner, and C. Matas. “Petitioner’s Attire and Petition
Signing by Peace Demonstrators: A Field Experiment.” Journal of
Applied Social Psychology 1 (1971): 278–8.
Swap, W. C. “Interpersonal Attraction and Repeated Exposure to Rewards and Punishers.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 3
(1977): 248–51.
Tiger, L., and R. Fox. The Imperial Animal. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston, 1971.
Toffler, A. Future Shock. New York: Random House, 1970.
Tversky, A., and D. Kahnemann. “Judgment Under Uncertainty:
Heuristics and Biases.” Science 185 (1974): 1124–31.
Walker, M. G. “Organizational Type, Rites of Incorporation, and Group
Solidarity: A Study of Fraternity Hell Week.” Doctoral dissertation,
University of Washington, 1967.
West, C. K. The Social and Psychological Distortion of Information. Chicago:
Nelson-Hall, 1981.
Whiting, J.W.M., R. Klukhohn, and A. Anthony. “The Function of Male
Initiation Cermonies at Puberty.” In Readings in Social Psychology,
238 / Influence
edited by E. E. Maccoby, T. M. Newcomb, and E. L. Hartley. New
York: Holt, 1958.
Whitney, R. A., T. Hubin, and J. D. Murphy. The New Psychology of Persuasion and Motivation in Selling. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall,
1965.
Wicklund, R. A., and J. C. Brehm. Cited in Wicklund, R. A., Freedom and
Reactance. Potomac, Md.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1974.
Wilson, P. R., “The Perceptual Distortion of Height as a Function of
Ascribed Academic Status.” Journal of Social Psychology 74 (1968):
97–102.
Wilson, T. D. et al. “Introspection, Attitude Change, and Behavior
Consistency,” Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 22, ed.
L. Berkowitz. San Diego: Academic Press, 1989.
Wilson, W. R. “Feeling More Than We Can Know: Exposure Effects
Without Learning.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37
(1979): 811–21.
Woodside, A. G., and J. W. Davenport. “Effects of Salesman Similarity
and Expertise on Consumer Purchasing Behavior.” Journal of Marketing
Research 11 (1974): 198–202.
Worchel, S. “Beyond a Commodity Theory Analysis of Censorship:
When Abundance and Personalism Enhance Scarcity Effects.” Basic
and Applied Social Psychology 13 (1992): 79–90.
——, and S. E. Arnold. “The Effects of Censorship and the Attractiveness
of the Censor on Attitude Change.” Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology 9 (1973): 365–77.
——, and M. Baker. “The Effect of Censorship on Attitude Change: The
Influence of Censor and Communicator Characteristics.” Journal of
Applied Social Psychology 5 (1975): 222–39.
Worchel, S., J. Lee, and A. Adewole. “Effects of Supply and Demand
on Ratings of Object Value.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
32 (1975): 906–14.
Young, F. W. Initiation Ceremonies. New York: Bobbs-Merrill, 1965.
Zajonc, R. B. “The Attitudinal Effects of Mere Exposure.” Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology Monographs 9:2 (part 2) (1968).
——. “Feeling and Thinking: Preferences Need No Inferences.” American
Psychologist 35 (1980): 151–75.
——, H. Markus, and W. R. Wilson. “Exposure Effects and Associative
Learning.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 10 (1974): 248–63.
Zellinger, D. A. et al. “A Commodity Theory Analysis of the Effects of
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 239
Age Restrictions on Pornographic Materials.” Institute for Research
in the Behavioral, Economic and Management Sciences, Purdue
University, Paper No. 440, 1974.
Zweigenhaft, R. L. “Signature Size: A Key to Status Awareness.”
Journal of Social Psychology 81 (1970): 49–54.
INDEX
Note: Entries in this index, carried over verbatim from the print edition of this title,
are unlikely to correspond to the pagination of any given e-book reader. However,
entries in this index, and other terms, may be easily located by using the search
feature of your e-book reader.
Abrams, Robert, 243
advertising:
age restrictions in, 252–253
association and, 191
average-person testimonials in, 140, 160–161
of claqueurs, 159
health authority in, 220, 221, 230–231
social proof and, 117, 140, 159, 160–161
toy, 65, 66
Advertising Age, 289n
aggression:
physical attractiveness and, 172
similarity and, 151
aid, see favors, gifts, and aid
airline crashes, suicide and, 144–147, 149–151
Allen, Irwin, 264
Ambrose, Mike, 190
American Broadcasting Company (ABC), 264–265
American Cancer Society, 68
American Salesman, 72
Amway Corporation, 27–29, 79
Anabaptists, 286n
animal behavior:
authority and, 223, 290n
competition and, 262–263
trigger features and, 2–4, 8–9, 273, 281n
Anthony, A., 85–86
anthropology, rule of reciprocation in, 18
antiphosphate ordinance, psychological reactance and, 250–251
antismoking study, 142, 287n
anxiety:
dental, 142, 287n
242 / Influence
swimming, 142–143
Ardry, R., 290n
Arizona State University, 117, 200, 201
Armstrong, Thomas, 121–125, 127–128
Aronson, Elliot, 89–90, 178–179, 182–184
Asimov, Isaac, 197–198
association, see conditioning and association
Assurance des Succès Dramatiques, L’, 158
auctions, movie, 264–265
Australia, size-status study in, 222–223
authority, 208–236
clothes and, 226–229
connotation and, 220–229
health and, 219–220, 221, 224–226, 290n
Milgram study and, 208–215, 217, 218, 289n–290n
obedience to, 213–218, 224–226
reader’s report on, 235–236
reciprocity and, 234
saying no and, 230–235
titles and, 222–226
trappings of, 228–229
automobile accidents:
Chicago jury project and, 254–255
social proof and, 139–140, 144–147, 149–151, 162–163
suicide and, 144–147, 149–151
automobiles:
authority and, 229, 235–236
unfair exchanges and, 34
automobile sales:
advertising and, 191
authority and, 235–236
contrast principle and, 14
liking and, 170, 173, 174–175, 185, 205–206, 289n
lowball tactic and, 98–99, 284n
scarcity and, 268–270
similarity and, 173
autonomy, development of, 247
Bandura, Albert, 118
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 243
bank-examiner scheme, 227–228
bargaining, UCLA study of, 49–50
Bar-Ilan University, 40
Baron, Robert, 286n
bartenders, tipping of, 117
beatings, initiations and, 85, 86–87
beautification petitions, 73–74
beef, scarcity of, 255–256, 292n
beetles, rove, 282n
“bereavement” account of suicide-crash connection, 144–145
Berry, Dave, 161
“Bertha, the,” 123
Best Doctors in the U.S., The (Pekkanen), 203–204
betting, 57–58, 164–166, 283n
Bible, authority and, 217–218
Bickman, Leonard, 226–227
billboards, public-service, 72–74
blennies, saber-toothed, exploitative behavior of, 8–9, 282n
blood donation, rejection-then-retreat strategy and, 48, 283n
Bloomington, Ind., volunteers in, 68
bluethroats, trigger features and, 3, 281n
Bollen, Kenneth, 148
Bonner, Tom, 190
boyfriends:
commitment and consistency and, 58–59
Romeo and Juliet effect and, 247–250, 271, 291n–292n
Boy Scouts, reciprocal concessions and, 36–39
breast-examination study, 239, 291n
Brehm, Jack, 245, 291n
Brock, Timothy, 255
Bronner, Frederick, 87
Brown, Jerry, 27
buffalo hunting, 163–164
BUG device, 28–29
business suits, authority and, 227
bystander aid, bystanders, 129–140
apathy theory of, 131–132
conditions that cause decrease in, 136
devictimizing yourself and, 136–140
244 / Influence
in emergencies, 133–136, 138–139, 286n–287n
Genovese case and, 129–132
Latané-Darley view of, 132–133
pluralistic ignorance and, 129, 132–135, 139, 162, 286n–287n
single, 133–135
California, University of, at Los Angeles (UCLA), bargaining study at,
49–50, 283n
Calley, William, 290n
camp, contact and cooperation at, 179–182, 288n
Canada:
election study in, 171, 287n
rejection-then-retreat tactic in, 48
Carlos study, 183–184, 288n
Carter, Jimmy, 26
Castro, Fidel, 77
CBS Television, 266
celebrity endorsements, 192, 220
censorship:
scarcity and, 252–256
TV, 40–41
charity:
commitment and, 67–68
liking and, 169–170
self-image and, 77
social proof and, 117–118
uninvited gifts from, 33
Charlie’s Angels (TV show), 12
Chesterton, G. K., 237
Chevrolet sales, liking and, 170
Chicago, University of, Law School, 255–256, 292n
child rearing:
authority and, 216
personal responsibility and, 94–97
scarcity and, 261
children:
aggressive, 172
coin sizes and, 223, 290n
with dog phobia, 118
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 245
psychological reactance in, 246–247, 291n
social proof and, 118–119, 285n–286n
withdrawn, 119
Chinese prisoner-of-war (POW) camps, 70–71, 74–82
altered self-images and, 75–80
collaboration in, 70–71, 284n
political essay contests of, 78–79, 92–93
written statements in, 76–82
Christmas, toy purchases and, 64–66
Christmas cards, reciprocation and, 17
church collections, 117
Cialdini, Richard, 268–270
circumcision, 85
claquing, 158–159
clothing, clothing stores:
authority and, 226–229
contrast principle and, 13–14
“expensive = good” stereotype and, 10–11
similarity and, 173
trappings and, 228–229
Cohen, Michael, 219–220
coins, size vs. value of, 223, 290n
collectibles, scarcity and, 239, 240
college students:
authority influence and, 222–223, 229, 290n
censorship and, 252–253
contrast principle used by, 15–16
in Deutsch-Gerard commitment experiment, 82–83
in perceptions-of-size experiments, 222–223, 290n
rejection-then-retreat technique and, 39–40, 48
similarity and, 173
Colorado, study of couples in, 248, 292n
Columbia University, wallet study of, 140–142
Columbus, Ohio, voter turnout in, 68
commitment, 57–58, 67–103
altered self-images and, 73–75
effective, 69–71
foot-in-the-door technique and, 71–74
initiations and, 85–92
246 / Influence
inner choice and, 92–103
lowball tactic and, 98–103
public, 81–85
strategies for, 67–69
Tupperware party and, 167
written, 76–81, 85
Committee to Re-elect the President (CRP), 43
commodity theory, 255–256, 292n
competition, scarcity and, 262–266, 268–270
compliments, liking and, 174–176, 288n
computers, 276
con artists, 221, 226
concessions, reciprocal, 36–51
see also rejection-then-retreat technique
Concord (Calif.) Naval Weapons Station protest, 215–216, 217
conditioning and association:
advertising and, 191
food and, 193–194
good news vs. bad news and, 194–195
liking and, 188–204, 289n
mother’s role and, 190
politics and, 191–193
sports and, 195–203
weathermen and, 188–190
Congress, U.S., 25–26
consistency, 57–113, 283n–285n
automatic, 60–67, 103, 105–111
being right vs., 60
exploitation of, 64–67, 98–100
“Knowing what I now know” question and, 110–111
psychological view of, 59–60
reader’s report on, 111–113
saying no and, 103–111
stomach signal and, 105–109
thinking vs., 61–64
as valued and adaptive, 60, 82
see also commitment
Constitution, U.S., 256
Consumer Reports, 47, 283n
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 247
Consumers From Mars, 161
contact and cooperation, 176–187, 288n–289n
automobile sales and, 185, 289n
at camp, 179–182, 288n
crime and, 185–187
politics and, 176–177, 288n
race relations and, 177–185
contests:
political essay, 78–79, 92–93
testimonial, 80
contrast principle, 11–16
physical attractiveness and, 12, 282n
reciprocity rule and, 42–45
contributions, political, 26
cookie study, scarcity and, 256–257, 261–262, 267
cooperation:
between fish, 8–9
see also contact and cooperation
Cornell University, reciprocity experiment at, 20–21
correct behavior, social proof and, 116–119, 140, 155–156
Coué, Emile, 272
courtship behavior, 3, 8
Craig, Jim, 196
crime:
consistency and, 59–60
contact and cooperation and, 185–187
physical attractiveness and, 171–172, 287n–288n
see also murder
cults:
mass suicide and, 29–30, 152–156
social proof and, 119–128, 152–156, 286n
Dade County antiphosphate ordinance, 250–251
Darley, John, 132–133, 286n–287n
Darrow, Clarence, 167
Davies, James C., 257–259
Davis, Neil, 219–220
deadline tactic, 242–244
Dealin’ Dan, 205–206
248 / Influence
Dean, John, 44
death threats, initiations and, 86, 88
Democratic National Committee, break-in at, 42–45
dental anxiety study, 142, 287n
Deutsch, Morton, 82–83
Diller, Barry, 264–266
dime-request experiment, 173, 288n
Disabled American Veterans, 30
discotheques, social proof and, 118
discount coupons, 7–8
dog-phobia treatment, 118, 285n
door-to-door sales:
consistency and, 105–109
rejection-then-retreat technique and, 41–42
scarcity and, 244
written commitments and, 79
driver safety, public-service billboards and, 72–74
Drubeck, Harry, 10
Drubeck, Sid, 10–11
earache, “rectal,” 219–220
education, contact and cooperation and, 177–179, 182–185
Edwards, John, 91
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1., 29
Einstein, Albert, 1
elections:
candidate size and, 290n
familiarity and, 176–177, 288n
physical attractiveness and, 171, 287n
electric shock:
initiation and, 90, 284n
in Milgram study, 209–215
Eliot, Sonny, 190
emergencies, bystander aid in, 133–136, 138–139, 286n–287n
Emerson, Ralph Waldo, 17, 103, 105, 107, 285n
endorsements, celebrity, 192, 220
energy conservation, 100–104, 284n
Ethiopia, Mexican relief aid from, 19
ethology, 2–3, 8–9
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 249
evangelical preachers, 117
“expensive = good” stereotype, 5–6, 10–11
exploitation:
of consistency, 64–67, 98–100
lowball tactic and, 98–100
of reciprocation, 31–36
of scarcity, 242–244
of weapons of influence, 8–11, 282n
exposure to cold, initiations and, 85–86, 87
Faintich, Barry, 240
Faraday, Michael, 60
fathers, association and, 203
favors, gifts, and aid:
asking for, 137–138, 139
physical attractiveness and, 172, 288n
reciprocation and, 17–30, 52–53, 282n
refusal of, 29–30, 52
unfair exchanges and, 33–36
unwanted, 30–33, 52
feelings, intellect vs., 110, 285n
Festinger, Leon, 59, 121–128, 286n
films:
auctions and, 264–265
social proof and, 118–119, 285n–286n
First Amendment, 252
fish:
competition and, 262–264
cooperating, 8–9
fixed-action patterns, 3–4
flawed items, scarcity and, 239, 240
Florida, study of bystander aid in, 135, 287n
food:
association and, 193–194
authority and, 290n
initiations and, 86, 87
foot-in-the-door technique, 71–74
Fox, M. W., 2
Fox, Robin, 18
250 / Influence
Fraser, Scott, 72–74
fraternal societies, initiation ceremonies of, 86–90, 92, 93
freedom:
autonomy and, 247
scarcity and, 245–251, 291n
Freeman, Jonathan, 72–74, 94–96, 284n
free samples, 27–29
Frenzer and Davis, 168
Fromkin, Howard, 255
fund-raising, food and, 193
Future Shock (Toffler), 275
gas prices, consistency and, 110–111
General Foods, 80
Genovese, Catherine, 129–132
Georgia, University of, 194–195
Gerard, Harold, 82–83
gifts, see favors, gifts, and aid
Girard, Joe, 170, 174–175
Goethe, Johann von, 145
Good Cop/Bad Cop, 186–187
Gorbachev, Mikhail, 259–260
Gouldner, Alvin, 18, 282n
Graham, Billy, 117, 285n
Green, Donna, 250
Greenwald, Anthony, 68
greeting cards:
liking and, 174–175
from strangers, 17, 282n
Gregory, Bob, 189
Griffin, Merv, 204
Grush, Joseph, 288n
Guardians, 121, 123
Gulban, Daniel, 242–243
gun law, psychological reactance and, 249, 250
Guyana, mass suicide in, 29–30, 152–156
halo effects, 171–172
Happy Days (TV show), 41
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 251
Hare Krishna Society, 22–24, 31–33
Harris, James, 77
Harvard University, 219
hazing, initiation ceremonies and, 85–93
health:
authority and, 219–220, 221, 224–226, 290n
scarcity and, 239
Health Care Financing Administration, U.S., 219, 225
Hell Week, 86–90, 92
help, see favors, gifts, and aid
Help Week, 88, 89
herd, cult as, 156
Hidden Persuaders, The (Packard), 27
Hieder, Fritz, 59
hiring, physical attractiveness and, 171
Hobbes, Thomas, 216
home-fire-safety inspections, 53–55
home-insulation study, 238, 291n
homicide, see murder
Howard, Daniel, 68–69
“How are you feeling” technique, 68, 69
hunting, of buffalo, 163–164
image, see self-image
inconsistency, as undesirable personality trait, 60, 82
indebtedness, feeling of:
uninvited favors and, 30–33
unpleasant character of, 34–35
influence, weapons of, 1–16, 281n–282n
exploitability of, 8–11, 282n
force lent to user by, 11–16
reader’s report and, 15–16
see also authority; commitment; consistency; liking; reciprocation;
scarcity; social proof
information, 273–277
psychological reactance and, 251–256
technology and, 275–276
initiations, 85–93
Aronson-Mills study of, 89–90
252 / Influence
of fraternal societies, 86–90, 92, 93
military, 90–92
tribal, 85–86, 90
insurance sales, similarity and, 174, 288n
intellect, feelings vs., 110, 285n
interpersonal relations:
foolish consistency and, 109–110
lowball tactic and, 100–104, 284n
reciprocity and, 29–30
see also boyfriends
Iowa energy research, 100–104, 284n
Israel, study of rejection-then-retreat technique in, 40, 283n
jewelry, weapons of influence in sale of, 1–2, 5–6, 10
Johnson, Lyndon B., 25–26
Jolls, Tom, 190
Jones, Edward, 77
Jones, Jim, 29–30, 152–156
Jonestown mass suicide, 29–30, 152–156
juries, jury trials:
censorship in, 253–255, 292n
hung, 83
Kalogris, Michael, 86–87
Keating, Charles H., Jr., 26
Keech, Marian, 121, 123–127
Kellerman, Sally, 27
Kelley, G. Warren, 47
Kennedy, John F., 287n
Kennesaw gun law, 249, 250
Kluckhohn, R., 85–86
Korean War, see Chinese prisoner-of-war (POW) camps
labor negotiations, rejection-then-retreat technique in, 40
Langer, Ellen, 4–5, 281n
Langford, David L., 189–190
Lansbury, Angela, 160–161
LaRue, Frederick, 43–45
Latané, Bibb, 132–133, 286n–287n
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 253
laughter, canned, 114–117, 158, 159, 285n
Laverne and Shirley (TV show), 41
Layden, Frank, 198
Leakey, Richard, 18
legal system:
physical attractiveness and, 171–172, 287n–288n
see also juries, jury trials
Leiden des jungen Werthers, Die (The Sorrows of Young Werther) (Goethe),
145
Liddy, G. Gordon, 43–45
Liebert, Robert, 286n
liking, 167–207, 287n–289n
automobile sales and, 170, 173, 174–175, 185, 205–206, 289n
compliments and, 174–176, 288n
conditioning, association and, 188–204, 289n
contact, cooperation and, 176–187, 288n–289n
Joe Girard formula and, 170
physical attractiveness and, 171–172, 287n–288n
reader’s report on, 206–207
saying no and, 204–206
similarity and, 173–174, 288n
Tupperware party and, 167–169
limited-number tactic, 239, 241–242
Lippmann, Walter, 114
loss, potential, as motivation, 238–239
lottery games, 284n
Louie, Diane, 29–30
love, interference and, 248–249, 291n–292n
lowball tactic, 98–104, 284n
energy conservation and, 100–104, 284n
luncheon technique, 193–194
Lussen, Frederick M., 130
lying, discouragement of, 96–97
McGovern, George, 43
MacKenzie, Bob, 265
Macrae, Norman, 276
Magruder, Jeb Stuart, 42–45
Marshall, Garry, 40–41
254 / Influence
Mauss, Marcel, 31
MCI Calling Circle, 207
Medication Errors (Cohen and Davis), 219–220
meditation, transcendental (TM), 61–64
merchandising, reciprocity and, 27–29
Mesa temple, 237–238
Mexico, Ethiopia’s aid to, 19
Mihaly, Orestes J., 243
Milgram, Stanley, 208–215, 217, 218, 282n–283n, 289n–290n
military, initiations and, 90–92
Mill, John Stuart, 274–275
Mills, Judson, 89–90
mimics, 8–9
minorities, see race relations
“mirroring and matching” evidence, similarity and, 174, 288n
mirror study, liking and, 176, 288n
Mitchell, John, 43–45
models, association and, 191
monkeys, authority and, 290n
Moriarty, Thomas, 59
Mormons, 237–238
Morrow, Lance, 260
mothers:
negative associations and, 190
stage, 203
Multiple Sclerosis Association, 77, 284n
murder:
bystander inaction and, 129–132, 286n–287n
similarity and, 151
Muskie, Edmund, 43
My Lai massacre, 290n
Nazi Germany, 289n
negligence-award study, 172, 288n
Newcomb, Theodore, 59
New Orleans Saints, 199
news, good vs. bad, 194–195
New York, studies of bystander aid in, 133–135, 287n
New York Times, 129–131
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 255
Nixon, Richard, 287n
Nixon administration, Watergate and, 42–45
no, saying:
authority and, 230–235
consistency and, 103–111
liking and, 204–206
reciprocation and, 51–55
scarcity and, 267–271
social proof and, 157–164
no-cost information offer, 53–55, 283n
North Carolina, experiment on compliments in, 176
North Carolina, University of, 253
nurses, authority and, 219–220, 224–226, 290n
obligation, reciprocation and, 17–19, 21, 31, 33–37, 53
O’Brien, Lawrence, 43, 45
O’Connor, Robert, 119
Ohio State University, 87
Olympics, association and, 191, 196, 201, 289n
opera, claquing and, 158–159
Packard, Vance, 27
Pallak, Michael, 100–103, 284n
participant observation method, social proof and, 119–128
parties, Tupperware, 167–169, 194
Pavlov, Ivan, 193–194
Pekkanen, John, 203–204
Pennsylvania, study of physical attractiveness of criminals in, 171–172,
288n
People’s Temple, Jonestown mass suicide and, 29–30, 152–156
perceptions of size, authority and, 222–224, 290n
perceptual contrast principle, 11–16, 282n
reciprocity rule and, 42–45
persuasion study, liking and, 172, 288n
pest-exterminator companies, 283n
petitions, signing of:
altered self-image and, 73–74
similarity and, 173, 288n
Philadelphia Phillies, 202
256 / Influence
Phillips, David, 145–149, 151–152
phobias, social proof and, 118
Photuris, 8, 282n
physical attractiveness:
association and, 191
contrast principle and, 12, 282n
liking and, 171–172, 191, 287n–288n
Pittsburgh, University of, 35, 283n
plastic surgery, rehabilitation vs., 287n–288n
pluralistic ignorance phenomenon:
bystander behavior and, 129, 132–135, 139, 162, 286n–287n
Jonestown mass suicide and, 155–156
politics:
association principle and, 191–193
censorship and, 252
familiarity and, 176–177, 288n
physical attractiveness and, 171, 287n
POW essay contests and, 78–79, 92–93
reciprocity and, 24–27
scarcity and, 257–261
Porcher (opera-house habitué), 158–159
Poseidon Adventure, The (movie), 264–266
praise, liking and, 174–176, 288n
prisoner-of-war camps, see Chinese prisoner-of-war (POW) camps
prize fights, homicide rates and, 151
Procter & Gamble, 80
psychological reactance:
censorship and, 251–255
in children, 246–247, 291n
Dade County antiphosphate ordinance and, 251–252
free choice and, 245–252
information restrictions and, 252–257
Kennesaw gun law and, 249, 251
Romeo and Juliet effect and, 247–250, 272, 291n–292n
scarcity and, 244–257
public-service billboards, 72–74
punishment:
child rearing and, 94–96
initiations and, 86, 87–88
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 257
in Milgram study, 208–211
Purdue University study, 253–254, 292n
Pyne, Joe, 273–274
race relations:
contact-cooperation approach to, 177–185
scarcity and, 259
racetracks, betting behavior at, 57–58, 164–166, 283n
raffle tickets, reciprocation and, 20–21, 31, 34
Razran, Gregory, 193–194
real estate, contrast principle and, 14
reasons, providing:
automatic compliance and, 4–5
child rearing and, 96–97
reciprocation, 17–56, 282n–283n
authority and, 234
concessions and, 36–51; see also rejection-then-retreat technique
exploitation of, 33–36
fund-raising dinners and, 193
obligation and, 17–19, 21, 31, 33–36, 53
power of, 21–30
prevention of repayment and, 35, 283n
reader’s report and, 55–56
saying no and, 51–56
Tupperware parties and, 167
unfair exchanges and, 33–36
uninvited debts and, 30–33
violation of, 19–20, 35
“rectal earache,” 219–220
Regan, Dennis, 20–21, 23, 31, 34
rehabilitation, plastic surgery vs., 287n–288n
rejection-then-retreat technique, 36–51, 283n
Israeli study of, 40, 283n
perceptual contrast rule and, 42–45
responsibility and, 50
satisfaction and, 50–51
victim reactions and, 47–48
religion:
authority and, 217
258 / Influence
social proof and, 119–128, 286n
responsibility:
bystander aid and, 132, 136, 138
commitment and, 92–97
rejection-then-retreat tactic and, 50
restaurants, tipping in, 232–235
revolution, scarcity and, 257, 259
Reynolds, Sir Joshua, 61
Riecken, Henry, 121–128, 286n
Robert, Cavett, 118
robins, trigger features and, 3, 281n
robot study, 94–96, 284n
Romeo and Juliet effect, 247–250, 271, 291n–292n
Rosenthal, A. M., 129–132
Rosten, Leo, 10
Russell, Dick, 40–41
Ryan, Leo J., 152
Sabin, Robert, 158
Sabini, John, 282n–283n
sales agreements, customer’s filling out of, 79–80
Sales Management, 47
sales operations and strategies:
commitment and, 71–72
commitment to goals as, 79
liking and, 167–169, 172–175, 185, 205–207, 289n
lowball tactic and, 98–99
rejection-then-retreat technique and, 41–42
scarcity and, 239–244, 255–256, 262, 263
social proof and, 118
see also automobile sales; door-to-door sales
Sananda, 121, 123
Sanka coffee ad, 220, 221
satisfaction, rejection-then-retreat tactic and, 50–51
Sauton (opera-house habitué), 158–159
scarcity, 237–271, 291n–292n
collectibles and, 239, 240
competition and, 262–266, 268–270
deadline tactic and, 242–244
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 259
experiencing vs. possessing and, 267–268
free choice and, 245–251, 291n
limited-number tactic and, 239, 241–242
optimal conditions and, 256–266
psychological reactance and, 244–256; see also psychological reactance
reader’s report on, 250, 271
sales and, 239–244, 255–256, 262, 263
saying no and, 266–270
shortcuts and, 244–245, 291n
violence and political turmoil and, 257–261
Schachter, Stanley, 121–128, 286n
Schein, Edgar, 70–71, 76, 284n
school desegregation:
liking and, 177–179, 182–185
scarcity and, 258
Segal, Henry, 75
Self, William, 264
self-image:
altering of, 73–75
association and, 201, 203
behavior and, 75–76
thoughts of others as factor in, 77
“Self-Reliance” (Emerson), 103, 105, 285n
sex, censorship and, 252–253
sexual obligation, 35–36, 283n
Shakespeare, William, 188, 253, 289n
Shaklee Corporation, 169
Sherif, Muzafer, 180–182, 288n
Sherman, Steven J., 67–68
similarity:
homicides and, 151
liking and, 173–174, 288n
social proof and, 140–156
suicide and, 144–156
wallet study and, 140–142, 149, 287n
size, perceptions of, authority and, 222–224
smoking:
psychological reactance and, 249
quitting, 84–85
260 / Influence
“social conditions” interpretation of suicide-crash connection, 144, 145
social proof, 114–166, 278–279, 285n–287n
advertising and, 117, 140, 159, 160–161
as automatic-pilot device, 157, 159–160, 163
automobile accidents and, 139–140, 144–147, 149–151, 162–163
bystander behavior and, see bystander aid, bystanders
canned laughter and, 114–117, 158, 159, 285n
claquing and, 158–159
correct behavior and, 116–119, 140, 155–156
cults and, 119–128, 152–156, 286n
falsified social evidence and, 158–162
influence of number of others and, 118, 285n
pluralistic ignorance and, 129, 132–135, 139, 155–156, 162, 286n–287n
reader’s report on, 164–166
religious movements and, 119–128
saying no and, 157–164
similarity and, 140–156; see also similarity
Tupperware parties and, 167–168
uncertainty and, 128–140, 153–154, 156
Sorrows of Young Werther, The (Die Leiden des jungen Werthers) (Goethe),
145
Southern California, University of (USC), 87, 88
Soviet Union, scarcity and, 259–260
sports, association and, 195–203
stage mothers, 203
standard solicitation approach, 69
status:
clothing and, 229
size and, 222–224, 290n
see also authority
stereotypes, 7, 9
“expensive = good,” 5–6, 10–11
Stevenson, McLean, 174
Storke, Bill, 264
Styron, William, 91
subway experiment, 283n
success, association and, 198–204
suicide:
auto and plane accidents and, 144–147, 149–151
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 261
Jonestown mass, 29–30, 152–156
similarity and, 144–156
Werther effect and, 145–147
Supreme Court, U.S., 258
surgery, plastic, 287n–288n
surprise, compliance and, 32, 282n–283n
Swanson, Richard, 87, 88
swimming anxiety, 142–143
technology, information and, 275–276
teenagers:
psychological reactance in, 247–250, 291n–292n
suicides of, 148
telephone solicitations:
charitable, 68–69
liking and, 206–207
television:
canned laughter on, 114–117, 285n
Nixon-Kennedy debates on, 287n
rejection-then-retreat technique and, 40–41
suicide and, 148
violence on, 285n–286n
“terrible twos,” 246–247, 291n
territorial defense, 3
thinking, consistency vs., 61–64
thirst, initiation and, 86, 87
Thonga, initiation ceremony of, 85–86, 90
Thorne, Avril, 200
threat, commitment and, 94–96
Tiananmen Square massacre, 93
Tiger, Lionel, 18
Time, 276
Tinker, Grant, 40–41
tips, increasing the size of, 117, 232–235
titles, 222–226
Toffler, Alvin, 275
Toronto, study of bystander aid in, 135, 287n
toy manufacturers, consistency and, 64–67
transcendental meditation (TM), consistency and, 61–64
trappings, of authority, 228–229
262 / Influence
tribal behavior:
buffalo hunting and, 163–164
initiations and, 85–86, 90
trigger features, 2–5, 7, 273, 281n
mimicking of, 8–9
Tupperware Home Parties Corporation, 168
Tupperware parties, 167–169, 194
turkey experiment, 2–3, 4, 116–117, 273, 281n
TV Guide, 40–41, 283n
uncertainty, social proof and, 128–140, 153–154, 156
uniforms, authority and, 226–227
urban environments, bystander aid discouraged by, 136
van Kampen, Jakob, 286n
“Vartan Bhanji,” 282n
Vinci, Leonardo da, 57
violence:
security and, 257–261
televised, 285n–286n
Virgil, 208
volunteer work:
commitment and, 67–68
rejection-then-retreat tactic and, 39–40, 48
voter turnout, 68
wallet study, 140–142, 149, 287n
Watergate break-in, 42–45
water temperature, contrast principle and, 12
weathermen, association and, 188–190
weight reduction, commitment to, 83–84
Werther effect, 145–147
West, Louis Jolyon, 153
Whitaker, Chuck, 190
Whitehead, Alfred North, 7
Whiting, J.W.M., 85–86
Willson, S. Brian, 215–216, 217
Wilson, Lee Alexis, 286n–287n
withdrawn children, social proof and, 119
women, reciprocation and, 35–36
Robert B. Cialdini Ph.D / 263
Wood, Robert, 265, 266
Worchel, Stephen, 256
World War I, 29
World War II, 70
Wriston, Walter, 275
written testaments:
publicizing of, 81–82
self-image and, 76–80
Xerox study, 4–5, 281n
Yale University, 211
Young, Robert, 220, 221, 230–231
Zappa, Frank, 272–273
Zweigenhaft, Richard, 290n
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
An array of people deserve and have my appreciation for their aid in
making this book possible. Several of my academic colleagues read and
provided perceptive comments on the entire manuscript in its initial
draft form, greatly strengthening the subsequent version. They are Gus
Levine, Doug Kenrick, Art Beaman, and Mark Zanna. In addition, the
first draft was read by a few family members and friends—Richard and
Gloria Cialdini, Bobette Gorden, and Ted Hall—who offered not only
much-needed emotional support but insightful substantive commentary
as well.
A second, larger group provided helpful suggestions for selected
chapters or groups of chapters: Todd Anderson, Sandy Braver, Catherine
Chambers, Judy Cialdini, Nancy Eisenberg, Larry Ettkin, Joanne Gersten,
Jeff Goldstein, Betsy Hans, Valerie Hans, Joe Hepworth, Holly Hunt,
Ann Inskeep, Barry Leshowitz, Darwyn Linder, Debbie Littler, John
Mowen, Igor Pavlov, Janis Posner, Trish Puryear, Marilyn Rall, John
Reich, Peter Reingen, Diane Ruble, Phyllis Sensenig, Roman Sherman,
and Henry Wellman.
Certain people were instrumental at the beginning stages. John Staley
was the first publishing professional to recognize the project’s potential.
Jim Sherman, Al Goethals, John Keating, and Dan Wegner provided
early, positive reviews that encouraged author and editors alike. William
Morrow and Company’s then president, Larry Hughes, sent a small
but enthusiastic note that fortified me importantly for the task that lay
ahead. Last and hardly least, Maria Guarnaschelli believed with me
from the start in the book I wanted to write. It is to her editorial credit
that the finished product remains that book, much improved. For her
insightful direction and her potent efforts in the book’s behalf, I am
most grateful.
In addition, I would be remiss if I did not recognize the skill and efficiency of Sally Carney at manuscript preparation and the sound counsel
of my attorney, Robert Brandes.
Finally, throughout the project, no one was more on my side than
Bobette Gorden, who lived every word with me.
About the Author
Robert B. Cialdini, Ph.D. holds dual appointments at Arizona State
Univercity. He is a W. P. Carey Distinguished Professor of Marketing
and Regents’ Professor of Psychology, and has been named Distinguished Graduate Research Professor. Dr. Cialdini is also president
of INFLUENCE AT WORK (www.influenceatwork.com), an international
training, speaking and consulting company based on his groundbreaking body of research on the ethical business applications of the
science of influence. 480-967-6070.
Visit www.AuthorTracker.com for exclusive information on your
favorite HarperCollins author.
Copyright
Some images not available for electronic edition.
INFLUENCE.
Copyright © 1984, 1994, 2007 by Robert Cialdini. All
rights reserved under International and Pan-American Copyright
Conventions. By payment of the required fees, you have been granted
the non-exclusive, non-transferable right to access and read the text
of this e-book on-screen. No part of this text may be reproduced,
transmitted, down-loaded, decompiled, reverse engineered, or stored
in or introduced into any information storage and retrieval system,
in any form or by any means, whether electronic or mechanical, now
known or hereinafter invented, without the express written
permission of HarperCollins e-books.
Adobe Acrobat eBook Reader March 2009
ISBN 978-0-06-189990-4
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
About the Publisher
Australia
HarperCollins Publishers (Australia) Pty. Ltd.
25 Ryde Road (PO Box 321)
Pymble, NSW 2073, Australia
http://www.harpercollinsebooks.com.au
Canada
HarperCollins Publishers Ltd.
55 Avenue Road, Suite 2900
Toronto, ON, M5R, 3L2, Canada
http://www.harpercollinsebooks.ca
New Zealand
HarperCollinsPublishers (New Zealand) Limited
P.O. Box 1
Auckland, New Zealand
http://www.harpercollins.co.nz
United Kingdom
HarperCollins Publishers Ltd.
77-85 Fulham Palace Road
London, W6 8JB, UK
http://www.harpercollinsebooks.co.uk
United States
HarperCollins Publishers Inc.
10 East 53rd Street
New York, NY 10022
http://www.harpercollinsebooks.com