Abstract
Stressful experiences, both physical and psychological, that are overwhelming (i.e., inescapable and unpredictable), can measurably affect subsequent neuronal properties and cognitive functioning of the hippocampus. At the cellular level, stress has been shown to alter hippocampal synaptic plasticity, spike and local field potential activity, dendritic morphology, neurogenesis, and neurodegeneration. At the behavioral level, stress has been found to impair learning and memory for declarative (or explicit) tasks that are based on cognition, such as verbal recall memory in humans and spatial memory in rodents, while facilitating those that are based on emotion, such as differential fear conditioning in humans and contextual fear conditioning in rodents. These vertically related alterations in the hippocampus, procedurally observed after subjects have undergone stress, are generally believed to be mediated by recurrently elevated circulating hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis effector hormones, glucocorticoids, directly acting on hippocampal neurons densely populated with corticosteroid receptors. The main purposes of this review are to (i) provide a synopsis of the neurocognitive effects of stress in a historical context that led to the contemporary HPA axis dogma of basic and translational stress research, (ii) critically reappraise the necessity and sufficiency of the glucocorticoid hypothesis of stress, and (iii) suggest an alternative metaparadigm approach to monitor and manipulate the progression of stress effects at the neural coding level. Real-time analyses can reveal neural activity markers of stress in the hippocampus that can be used to extrapolate neurocognitive effects across a range of stress paradigms (i.e., resolve scaling and dichotomous memory effects issues) and understand individual differences, thereby providing a novel neurophysiological scaffold for advancing future stress research.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Stress is an all-encompassing term used across diverse disciplines to describe the strain on a given structure or entity. In physical sciences such as physics and engineering, stress is precisely (quantitatively) defined as the force applied per area of an inanimate object [1]. In life sciences such as biology and psychology, stress is generally (qualitatively) defined as any perturbing life situations/events—comprising both real physiological and perceived psychological stressors—that instigate adaptive bodily responses to preserve the well-being (homeostasis) of organisms [2]. While stress is an integral part of daily life, overwhelming adverse experiences can take their toll on physical health (e.g., cardiovascular, digestive, metabolic diseases) and mental health (e.g., anxiety, depression, post-traumatic stress disorder, schizophrenia, drug use relapse) in humans [3,4,5,6]. A common denominator of various psychopathologies linked to stress appears to be alterations in learning and memory processes, in particular the medial temporal lobe-based declarative or explicit memory that can shape and guide cognitive outlook negatively [7,8,9,10]. For instance, Holocaust survivors with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) showed explicit, but not implicit, memory dysfunctions [11]. Similarly, PTSD is the primary risk factor for a higher incidence of mild cognitive impairment among the 9/11/2001 responders at the World Trade Center [12]. More recently, the stress of uncertainty and disruption to our daily lives caused by the COVID-19 pandemic has been linked to lower working and prospective (declarative) memory performances as well as higher anxiety and depression levels, which are expected to have enduring societal consequences [13, 14].
At the outset, it should be acknowledged that the majority of preclinical and clinical stress research is rooted in the use of diverse stress paradigms—for example, a single footshock stressor [15] to 210 footshock stressor [16]—characterized by adjectives such as mild, traumatic, acute, chronic, etc. Since there are no standard metrics of stress to evaluate stressors across laboratories, the resulting complex and sometimes contradictory data is difficult to make sense of. Hans Selye [17] succinctly highlighted this issue in 1973 by stating, “Everyone knows what stress is and nobody knows what it is.” Even decades later, some researchers continue to question whether stress is a useful scientific term [18]. With this caveat in mind, this review, which is admittedly narrow in scope, serves to highlight the negative effects of inescapable and unpredictable stress (cf. [19]; henceforth, stress) on subsequent neuronal and mnemonic functions of the hippocampus and to critically evaluate the putative role of corticosteroids synthesized and secreted by the HPA axis, which is widely acknowledged as the central stress response system [20, 21]. Whenever possible, stress techniques will be described so that the reader can assess the range of experimental outcomes. The findings and points of view discussed here could lead to new theoretical and empirical research on the neurocognitive effects of stress and offer a novel understanding of the various stress-related mental disorders that severely limit the quality of human life in today’s hectic, globalized society. Because the neurocognitive effects of stress likely vary depending on the severity of the stress and the brain region of interest, are undoubtedly mediated by multiple molecular-cellular mechanisms, and are not limited to neurons, no single review can satisfactorily integrate various findings on stress. Thus, we refer the interested reader to the following related topics and references for detailed discussion: time-dependent synaptic plasticity- and glucocorticoid-based stress models of flashbulb/traumatic memories and amnesia [6, 22], cognitive enhancing effects of mild stress [23, 24], various candidate neurochemical mediators of stress [6, 25,26,27,28], developmental stress effects [29, 30], transcriptomic-translatomic-proteomic changes to stress [31], divergent effects of stress on different brain structures [32, 33], and non-neuronal contributions in stress effects [34].
A brief history of stress research
The detrimental effects of stress can be found anecdotally throughout human history; encountering wild animals, enemies, natural catastrophes, etc. [17, 35]. For instance, it has been suggested that King Saul in the Bible displayed “well-known signs of depression” characteristic of job insecurity stress [36], presumably resulting from the self-generated pressure of losing his kingship to a more popular David. According to the United States Library of Medicine (PubMed), the term stress first appeared in an article entitled “The Stress and Strain of Medicine” published in 1891 [37], describing the tragic suicide of Dr. Charles Edward Sheppard of the University of London, who was distressed over the death of a young patient he was administering chloroform to during an operation.
The modern scientific study of stress and how it affects health began decades later, in 1936, with Hans Selye’s [38] seminal paper “A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents.” This paper showed that rats exposed to different stressors, such as cold, surgical injury, and excessive/forced exercise, had similar physiological symptoms, such as enlarged adrenal glands, decreased thymus and spleen lymph nodes, and gastric ulceration. Based on these empirical findings, Selye proposed a biological model of stress called “general adaptation syndrome” comprising of sequential alarm reaction, adaptation and exhaustion stages and hypothesized that normal stress responses protect the organism against stressors during the alarm and adaptation stages, but depleted stress responses to severe and/or persistent stressors lead to vulnerability to stress-related illnesses (Fig. 1). However, the evidence for exhausted stress protective elements, namely the HPA axis effector corticosteroid hormones [39], to overwhelming/persistent stress was unsupported (e.g., [40]).
The general adaptation syndrome theory was then succeeded by stress models based on glucocorticoids [41,42,43,44], which still largely guide basic and clinical neuroscience research today. The glucocorticoid hypothesis of stress, while staying focused on the HPA axis effector hormones, basically reformulated Selye’s [38, 39] exhausted/depleted corticosteroid activity→stress disorder model to a heightened/protracted corticosteroid activity→stress disorder model, similar to autoantibodies attacking the body in autoimmune diseases [45]. The glucocorticoid hypothesis of stress is especially espoused in the hippocampus and its well-documented learning and memory functions (e.g., [46]). This is because hippocampal neurons are densely packed with corticosteroid-binding mineralocorticoid receptors (MRs) and glucocorticoid receptors (GRs), take part in glucocorticoid-mediated negative feedback of the HPA axis, and are sensitive to heightened corticosteroid (cortisol in human, corticosterone in rodent) actions [47]. To put it succinctly, stress is not only implicitly (neurochemically) defined as cortisol/corticosterone stress hormones, but their elevated levels are considered both necessary and sufficient to cause stress effects on the hippocampus. However, as will be discussed later, it is becoming increasingly clear that the multifaceted nature of stress cannot be simplified or emulated merely by elevated glucocorticoid levels.
Stress effects on behavior
There is suggestive literature, predating the formal scientific studies of stress, that ‘demanding’ situations can influence behavior performances [7]. For instance, Yerkes and Dodson demonstrated in 1908 that mice given relatively challenging brightness discrimination tasks displayed a nonlinear (U-curve) learning rate as a function of shock intensity (i.e., learning was ‘favorable’ at intermediate range compared to low and high shock strengths), whereas mice given a relatively simpler brightness discrimination task displayed a linear relationship between learning rate and shock intensity [48]. Despite the fact that these early findings were inexactly represented as the Yerkes-Dodson’s inverted-U law (see [22]), they had a significant impact on the current understanding of the nonlinear relationship between stress and hippocampus function (e.g., [49, 50]). In a similar way, Pavlov [51] described experimental neurosis in dogs that were given increasingly difficult ‘a circle stimulus-food’ versus ‘an ellipse stimulus-no food’ discrimination conditioning. This could be seen as the first scientific example of how stress can affect learning and memory, which can lead to mental disturbances.
In the 1960s, Seligman, Overmier and Maier (see [19]) used a triadic experimental design with groups of dogs that underwent escapable shock, yoked inescapable shock, and control conditions. They found that the yoked inescapable shock animals (after 7-day rest) failed to show escape-avoidance learning in a novel shuttle-box. On the other hand, the escapable shock animals that received the same amount of shock but could terminate the shock showed normal escape-avoidance learning. This behavioral deficit phenomenon termed learned helplessness has been verified in numerous species, including cats, rats, fish, and humans, utilizing a variety of stressors (such as loud noise) [19]. They postulated that when a subject internalizes that its behavioral response (R) has no influence (or control) over the outcome of the aversive stimulus (S), this dissonant S-R learning results in cognitive, emotional, and motivational changes that obstruct subsequent learning of other tasks. Learned helplessness has been likened, in a contrawise manner, to Maudsley’s meta-learning, described as “the process by which learners become aware of and increasingly in control of habits of perception, inquiry, learning, and growth that they have internalized” [7].
Stress effects on cognitive-centric tasks
Because the HPA axis responds to stressors by increasing the synthesis and release of corticosteroids that bind to corticosteroid receptor-enriched hippocampal neurons [47], substantial stress research in recent decades has focused on hippocampus-based learning and memory (Table 1). In general, it has been found that environmental stressors and exogenous cortisol/corticosterone administration can impair declarative-explicit memory in humans and spatial-relational memory in rodents [7, 52, 53]. For example, inescapable and unpredictable tone exposure (using a triadic design) impaired a human analogue escape-avoidance learning and anagram (word) solution testing in college students [54], and individuals diagnosed with PTSD showed deficits in verbal recall tasks compared to control subjects (e.g., [55, 56]). An acute hypoxic gas breathing-induced stress in healthy subjects has also been shown to impair the subsequent learning of composite memory, verbal memory, visual memory, and other declarative tasks [57]. Similarly, acute administration of cortisol in healthy subjects has been reported to selectively impair verbal (but not nonverbal) memory [58, 59], suggesting that increasing the circulating cortisol level is sufficient to mimic behavioral stress effects on declarative memory. In further accordance with the glucocorticoid hypothesis of stress, hypercortisolism associated with certain depressions and Cushing’s disease have been implicated in declarative memory deficits [60, 61].
Correspondingly to human studies, rats that underwent uncontrollable stress experiences exhibited memory deficits in various hippocampal-dependent tasks (for reviews, see [7, 8]). For instance, Diamond and colleagues found that 4-hr exposure to a novel environment and 30-min exposure to a cat predator selectively impaired hippocampal-dependent spatial working but not hippocampal-independent reference memory in radial arm maze tasks [62, 63]. However, a brief 2-min cat predator exposure promptly followed by radial arm watermaze training was found to enhance spatial memory, indicating a time-dependent stress effects on hippocampal functions (see [22] for a temporal dynamics model of stress and [64] for time-dependent biphasic stress effects on synaptic plasticity). Subsequent studies employing 60-min restraint+60 intermittent tailshocks and 2-hr bright light+loud noise (1-s bursts of 100 dB white noise, 30–90 s apart) also found impairments in spatial memory in a Morris water maze task [65,66,67] as well as object recognition memory in a visual paired comparison task ([68, 69]; see also [70]). As in the human cortisol studies mentioned above [58,59,60,61], spatial memory deficits were observed in rats subjected to acute (3.0 mg/kg, s.c.) [71] and chronic (e.g., 100 mg s.c. pellets for 1–3 months) [72] corticosteroid treatments and in trangenic mice with elevated corticosterone levels caused by the central overexpression of corticotropin-releasing factor [73]. Moreover, the stress-induced memory impairments have been found to correlate with the rise of corticosterone in the dorsal hippocampus [74]. Similar to stress effects on spatial memory in rodents, a recent study on humans found that civilians, law enforcement officers, and veterans with previous trauma exposure showed impaired spatial navigation in a virtual environment task [75].
Interestingly, the same stressors that impair hippocampal-dependent spatial memory were found to enhance the caudate-dependent response memory in rats [67, 76], mice [77], and humans [78, 79]. Since brain-memory systems are dynamic, interactive units rather than independent modules that work on their own (for a review, see [80]), it remains unclear whether the increased performance on caudate memory tasks is caused by stress directly (actively) influencing non-hippocampal-memory systems or by reducing the hippocampal memory system’s ability to compete, which indirectly (passively) enhances other brain-memory systems [67, 81].
Stress effects on emotion-centric tasks
In contrast to its impairing effects on cognitive tasks, stress generally enhances aversive conditioning tasks in both humans and animals. In 1951, Spence and Taylor [82] reported that college students in a high-anxiety group (i.e., upper 20% anxiety scores) displayed significantly enhanced delay eyeblink conditioning (to both low- and high-intensity airpuff unconditioned stimuli) compared to those in a non-anxious group (lower 20% anxiety scores). Later, Shors and colleagues directly applied 90-min restraint+90 intermittent tailshocks to rats and found enhancements in both hippocampal-independent delay eyeblink conditioning [83] as well as hippocampal-dependent trace eyeblink conditioning [84]. It should be noted that eyeblink conditioning is thought to engage two learning processes, where rapid amygdala-based nonspecific fear response influences gradual cerebellar-based specific eyelid response acquisition (for a review, see [85]). Consistent with eyeblink conditioning, stress has been shown to enhance fear conditioning [24, 86]. For example, various stress procedures, such as 30-min and 1-hr restraint [87, 88], 15 unsignaled footshocks [89], and four days of alternating restraint+food/water restriction+predator odor+constant light+flooded cage+forced swim [90] have all been reported to increase contextual fear conditioning (cued fear conditioning was not examined in these studies). Other studies that examined contextual and tone fear conditioning in the same animals found that 1-hr restraint stress selectively enhanced contextual (but not tone) fear [91] whereas chronic restraint stress (CRS; 6-hr/day, 21 days) enhanced both contextual and tone fear [92].
Because pre-training [93] and post-training [94] lesions of the hippocampus have been shown to impair acquisition and retention, respectively, of contextual fear memory while sparing auditory fear memory, the hippocampus is thought to play an important role in processing and integrating complex context cues but not simple cues like a tone. Then, the results that stress enhances contextual fear memory vs. stress impairs spatial-relational memory seem to be at odds with each other. However, hippocampal lesioned rats can acquire contextual fear memory (especially foreground contextual fear conditioning where there is no discrete cue that competes for association with the footshock) presumably using elemental contextual cues (for a review, see [95]). Furthermore, stress-enhanced contextual fear occurs in juvenile rats (before the declarative memory system matures) and appears to be due to a nondeclarative sensitization process in adult rats (e.g., [96, 97]).
Whether stress-induced enhancement of contextual fear memory reflects hippocampal-dependent compound encoding or hippocampal-independent elemental encoding processes is yet unknown. This critical issue could be studied by subjecting hippocampal-lesioned or -inactivated animals to stress; the absence of contextual fear (or absence of enhanced contextual fear) would suggest that stress-enhanced contextual fear is likely caused by stress effects on the hippocampus, whereas the presence of enhanced contextual fear would suggest that stress-enhanced contextual fear is independent of the hippocampus. Given the evidence that the hippocampus can be functionally segmented into dorsal, intermediate, and ventral regions (for a recent review, see [98]), the effects of stress on subsequent hippocampal memory tasks can be further examined with subregion-specific hippocampal lesions or inactivation. Key stress-molecular pathways identified in hippocampal neurons (e.g., [6, 26]) can also be exploited to assess the dichotomous effects of stress on emotional vs. cognitive hippocampal memory tasks. For instance, intrahippocampal infusions of drugs that decrease/increase mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK) activity and subsequently decrease/increase contextual fear (similar to those shown after low and high stress) [99] would be expected to attenuate/exacerbate stress impairments of spatial-relational memory. These lines of research may advance our understanding of stress effects on fear generalization and extinction (for a recent review, see [32]), as well as fragmented trauma narratives sometimes reported in PTSD (e.g., [100]).
Stress effects on hippocampal neurons
Acute intense and chronic stressors have been shown to cause many neurophysiological changes in the hippocampus that are postulated to subserve learning and memory mechanisms (Fig. 2).
Synaptic plasticity
In 1987, Thompson and colleagues [101] reported that hippocampal slices prepared from rats exposed to 30-min restraint and 30 inescapable tailshocks exhibited markedly reduced long-term potentiation (LTP) in the Schaffer collateral-CA1 synapses. LTP, which refers to a sustained increase in synaptic transmission triggered by a brief high-frequency stimulation of afferent fibers, has long been considered a leading candidate synaptic model of memory in the mammalian brain [102, 103]. A triadic design study [16] then showed that rats able to escape the shock in a shuttle box (30 footshocks/day for seven days) showed reliable CA1 LTP, whereas yoked inescapable animals that received the same amount of shock showed impaired LTP, despite both groups showing comparable corticosterone elevations to shock exposure. Hence, the LTP deficit in yoked rats was essentially due to stress-associated with a lack of control over the shock, rather than nociception, fear emotion, or corticosteroid-mediated physiological effects associated with the shock [104]. Stress-induced LTP impairments have also been confirmed using in vivo CA1 recordings in awake rats placed in a novel (but not acclimatized) chamber [63, 105] and with theta-burst stimulation in anesthetized rats following 60-min restraint+60 intermittent tailshocks [106]. LTP deficits have been demonstrated in other regions of the hippocampal formation, such as the mossy fiber-CA3 synapses (in vitro) to CRS [107] and 90-min restraint+90 tailshocks [108], the perforant pathway-dentate gyrus synapses (in vivo) to CRS [109], and the dorsal CA1-subiculum synapses (in vivo) to acute 30-min restraint stress [110]. The fact that stress interferes with LTP in the hippocampus is significant because it offered a post-hoc neurophysiological marker to compare how different types of stress affect memory (i.e., those that impair LTP vs. those that do not).
The functional relationship between stress and LTP likely exists in nature because rats exposed to an ethologically-relevant cat predator (75-min sans physical contact) were impaired in CA1 primed burst potentiation, a low threshold form of LTP [111]. N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptors and the amygdala play crucial roles because NMDA receptor antagonists and amygdala lesions/inactivation block stress impairment of LTP [65, 67, 112]. Other studies showed that stress effects on CA1 LTP last ~48/24-hr in rats/mice [113, 114] and are time-dependent and biphasic, with an initial effect that increases LTP followed by a longer-lasting effect that decreases LTP [64]; the latter parallels the 2-min cat predator-spatial memory finding mentioned above [22]. Interestingly, the same stressors (i.e., 60-min restraint+60 intermittent tailshocks [112] and brightly lit novel environment [105]) that decrease CA1 LTP have been shown to enhance CA1 long-term depression (LTD). LTD is defined by a decrease in synaptic efficacy after low-frequency stimulation of afferent fibers and is thought to work in tandem with LTP (e.g., reset LTP saturation of synapses) to process and store information (e.g., [115]). The use of different stress paradigms and afferent stimulation protocols, as well as time-dependent, biphasic effects of stress may have led to this more complicated picture of how stress affects hippocampal plasticity. Hypothetically, if LTP develops and saturates synapses in the hippocampus as a subject learns that its action and stressful situation are unrelated (i.e., learned helplessness), then subsequent LTP may be occluded while LTD is enhanced [112]. Alternatively, a metaplasticity hypothesis (i.e., the plasticity of synaptic plasticity) [116], analogous to previously mentioned meta-learning [117], has been proposed to account for the dynamic relationship between LTP and LTD with respect to stress [118, 119].
Finally, in contrast to aforementioned studies [16, 104] that found an orthogonal relationship between stress effects on LTP and corticosterone, other studies found a causal relation because stress impairment of LTP is blocked by GR antagonists [120, 121], reproduced by systemically administered corticosterone [122, 123] and observed in bath-applied corticosterone on hippocampal slices [124, 125]. Specifically, the magnitudes of LTP and corticosterone levels have been shown to have a biphasic relationship [122]. In support, the preferential activation of high-affinity MRs (reflecting mild corticosteroid levels and stress) increases LTP, while the activation of lower-affinity GRs (reflecting high corticosteroid levels and stress) decreases LTP and increases LTD [126]. The fact that NMDA receptor antagonists reduce the effects of corticosteroids [127] and prevent the effects of stress on CA1 LTP and LTD [112] suggest that Ca2+ influx through NMDA receptors leads to molecular-genetic cascades that change synaptic plasticity and memory functions in the hippocampus (e.g., [119]). It should be noted that corticosterone has also been shown to alter the intrinsic properties of hippocampal neurons that are not related to synaptic plasticity, i.e., corticosterone prolongs the afterhyperpolarization of CA1 pyramidal neurons via increasing the intracellular Ca2+ level and thereby activating the Ca2+-gated K + channels [128].
Neural activity
In the rodent hippocampus are place cells that support spatial navigation by encoding memories of familiar spatial locations. Each place cell fires selectively when the animal visits a preferred location, called a place field, in a familiar environment [129, 130]. As an animal explores a new environment, it seems that hippocampal synaptic plasticity is involved in the formation of stable place fields because genetic and pharmacological manipulations that block LTP and spatial memory also disrupt the stability of place cells [131, 132]. Conversely, reactivation of place cell ensembles labeled with activity-dependent opsins can alter spatial navigation and memory [133]. Hence, if stress impairs LTP and spatial memory in the hippocampus, it should similarly affect the activity of place cells. Consistent with this view, rats exposed to audiogenic stress (pseudorandom 1-s bursts of 100 dB white noise) for 2 hours displayed reduced CA1 LTP, spatial memory, and place cell stability [66]. The stability of place cell activity was also found to be impacted by CRS in mice [77, 134]. Interestingly, CRS increased the local cue-dependency of place fields [77], which is in line with behavioral findings that stress enhances caudate-dependent response memory [67, 76, 77]. Lastly, a 1971 study [135] found that intraperitoneal injection of 1 mg/kg corticosterone decreases single-unit activity in the dorsal hippocampus in freely moving rats. Since then, it has been found that corticosterone increases the excitability of CA1 pyramidal cells by decreasing the amplitude of afterhyperpolarization [136, 137]. However, corticosterone (3 mg/kg) failed to change the activity of CA1 place cells in freely moving animals [138].
Stress has also been found to affect local field potentials (LFPs) in the hippocampus. LFPs are integrative excitatory and inhibitory synaptic processes [139] that span a wide range of frequencies, including theta (4–10 Hz), beta (10–30 Hz), and gamma (30–120 Hz) oscillations, as well as sharp-wave ripples (SWRs, 110–200 Hz) [140]. Rodent studies found that restraint+tailshock and CRS enhanced hippocampal theta rhythms [114] and SWRs [141] and decreased CA1 slow/fast gamma power [142]. Chronic unpredictable stress (restraint+noise+shaking+cold air stream) also heightened theta power in ventral hippocampus (vHPC) and basolateral amygdala (BLA) and increased vHPC-BLA coherence [143], whereas chronically restrained rats (2-hr/day for 10 days) exhibited decreased beta and gamma synchrony between CA1-CA3 areas but increased lateral amygdala (LA)-hippocampus synchrony with strong LA→CA1 directional activity [144]. In line with studies on animals, a recent human magnetoencephalography study found that the acute Trier Social Stress Test (public speaking stress) increased theta oscillations in the hippocampus [145]. Dynamic interactions between LFPs and spikes are thought to improve the accuracy of place coding and synchronize activities of neurons to/from multiple hippocampal efferent/afferent structures [146, 147]. However, the effect of stress on phase coupling between place cells and LFPs needs to be further investigated [148].
Morphology and neurogenesis
In the 1990s, McEwen and colleagues [149, 150] found selective apical dendritic atrophy of CA3 pyramidal neurons (but not CA1 pyramidal neurons and dentate gyrus granule cells) in rats that either underwent CRS (6-hr/day, 21 days) or 21 days of daily corticosterone injections. A similar CA3 atrophy was observed in subordinate treeshrews subjected to chronic psychosocial stress (exposures to dominant males for 1-hr/day for 28 days) [151]. Surprisingly, the atrophied apical and basal dendrites of CA3 pyramidal neurons were found to have an increased number of dendritic spines and excresences [152], possibly reflecting compensatory mechanisms [153].
Chronic stress has also been implicated in exacerbating apoptosis and suppressing adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus [154,155,156,157]. Subordinate male (but not female) vervet monkeys were found to have fewer neurons selectively in CA3/CA2/CA1 subfields [158], gonadectomized rats that underwent restraint+water immersion for 15-min/day for 30 days showed decreased CA4/CA3 neurons [159], and rats exposed to a cat predator for 60-min/day for 2 or more weeks showed reduced CA3/CA1 neurons [160]. Studies have found that stress suppresses neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus in rats that underwent CRS [161], in mice that underwent 10 social defeat episodes (in a resident-intruder test) [162], and in tree shrews and marmosets that were exposed to dominant conspecifics for 1 h [163, 164].
In accordance with animal results, magnetic brain imaging (MRI) studies have reported reduced hippocampus volume in individuals diagnosed with PTSD due to combat, childhood abuse, and workplace (i.e., police work) traumas (e.g., [165, 166] but see [167]). Moreover, the verbal declarative memory impairments were more evident in PTSD patients with smaller hippocampal volume (e.g., [165]) and decreased hippocampal activity [168]. However, because these (including poststress longitudinal) studies all measured hippocampal volume after the trauma and/or clinical diagnosis, it is not possible to dissociate traumatic events causing hippocampus reduction from having smaller hippocampus enhancing PTSD vulnerability. In fact, a monozygotic twin study [169] has suggested that reduced hippocampal volume is a predisposing factor for the development of PTSD instead of an outcome of trauma. However, Helmstetter and colleagues [170] performed a prestress-poststress longitudinal, within-subjects MRI in rats, employing the aforementioned CRS (6-hr restraint/day, 21 days) paradigm that causes dendritic atrophy, exacerbates apoptosis, and suppresses neurogenesis, and where the animals’ heads were precisely aligned and scanned under anesthesia, and found a reliable (~3%) reduction in hippocampal volume. A comparable within-subjects investigation, such as imaging the brains of enlistees before and after experiencing combat-related PTSD symptoms, may help determine whether PTSD-inducing events reduce the hippocampus in humans.
Glucocorticoid hypothesis of stress: revisited
With Selye’s general adaptation syndrome concept empirically unsupported, the stress field underwent a renaissance with glucocorticoid-based models that continue to dominate basic and clinical research today (Fig. 1). The glucocorticoid hypothesis of stress, which is based on a single factor or class of neurochemicals, has obvious advantages in terms of parsimony, elegance, and conceptual coherence, especially when applied to hippocampal neurons densely packed with corticosteroid receptors [47, 171, 172]. As mentioned earlier, various studies have reported that glucocorticoid levels can be raised in vivo or in vitro to mimic the effects of environmental stress on the hippocampus. This includes LTP deficits [120,121,122,123, 173], dendritic atrophy [149, 150, 174], apoptosis exacerbation [156, 157], neurogenesis suppression [154, 155], and learning and memory impairments [53, 59, 62, 71, 175].
But other behavioral and neurophysiological studies disagree with this interchangeable concept of equating behavioral stress with corticosteroids (Table 2). For example, the findings of stress impairing LTP in adrenalectomized rats with dampened corticosterone levels [176] and amygdalar lesions/inactivation unaffecting stress-induced elevation of corticosterone but preventing stress impairments of LTP and spatial memory [65, 67] suggest that increased corticosterone activity is neither necessary nor sufficient to cause stress effects. The LTP was also impaired in yoked inescapable shock (but not escapable shock) animals, although the corticosterone levels were comparably elevated in the two shocked groups [16], whereas the antidepressant tianeptine blocked stress effects on LTP without affecting stress elevation of corticosterone [177]. Contrary to the glucocorticoid hypothesis, then, whether corticosteroids affect the hippocampus seems to depend on psychological conditions in which levels rise. For example, in the studies performed by McEwen and colleagues [123, 149,150,151, 174, 178], the animals were given corticosterone injections every day for 21 days. The animals were probably agitated because they were taken out of their homecages and had to be handled and held down for injections, both of which were out of their control and painful. Consistent with this view, male rats exposed to either sexually receptive females (appetitive condition) or a cat predator (aversive condition) showed an equivalent increase in corticosterone levels, but spatial working memory deficit was observed only under the latter condition [179]. Also, recent studies found that stress and environmental enrichment both caused corticosterone levels to rise, but they had opposite effects on the growth of new neurons in the hippocampus (see [180] for more information).
Perhaps “natural” human experiments are needed to shed more light on the relationship between stress effects and corticosteroids in the real world. For example, the glucocorticoid hypothesis would predict that endurance sports events, such as the Tour de France and Vuelta a España bicycle races, involving up to six hours a day of physically and psychologically grueling cycling for 21 days, which would increase the levels of glucocorticoids to meet the metabolic demands of the body [181,182,183], should lead to detrimental hippocampal functioning and physiology. However, both acute and chronic exercise are usually associated with beneficial effects on the brain, cognition, and mental health [184,185,186]. Interestingly, after the 3-week race period, the cyclists showed decreased basal cortisol levels [187]; this finding suggests that the clinical significance of low basal cortisol condition in PTSD patients [188] may need to be reconsidered as it also occurs under non-PTSD conditions.
Metaparadigm view of stress
The HPA axis hormones have broad functions in cell metabolism and immune responses [189, 190] and do not respond uniquely to stressful situations [8, 191]. Hence, defining stress and stress effects in terms of corticosteroids is a misnomer and overlooks the psychological complexity of stress [7, 8, 17, 18, 191]. Then, how can stress research go beyond the current HPA axis dogma?
Unlike in physical sciences where stress can be precisely defined, i.e., units of force per area (N/m2) [1], and applied across different laboratories, in life sciences there is a problem of scaling stress across laboratories because a variety of behavioral paradigms are used under the umbrella term “stress” [2]. Before neurobiology can build on a solid psychological foundation, stress needs to be clearly defined from a theoretical point of view so that it is equally applicable to human and animal studies and analyzed across behavioral paradigms, like in physical sciences. This can perhaps be achieved by advancing the learned helpless hypothesis [19], such as a three-component definition of stress that includes arousal (heightened excitability), affective (perceived aversiveness), and cognitive (lack of controllability) factors, where each is necessary but not sufficient to cause stress effects (Fig. 3; [8]). It has been further suggested that elevated levels of corticosteroids, increased activity in the amygdala, and dampened activity in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) might represent the biological substrates of arousal, affective, and cognitive psychological constructs of stress, respectively [192]. Corticosteroids are linked to energy expenditure actions [43, 193], the amygdala is thought to process aversive information [65, 67, 144, 194], and the mPFC is linked to behavioral control over aversive events [195, 196], including the neuroendocrine stress response [197, 198]. Moreover, the amygdala and mPFC are ideally positioned to integrate and execute stress operations as they receive diverse sensory inputs from a number of brain regions, such as the thalamus and sensory cortex, and project to various motor output structures, such as the lateral hypothalamus for sympathetic activation, the bed nucleus of stria terminalis for stress hormones, and the periaqueductal gray for defensive behavior [199]. All of these findings support the tri-factor model of stress [7, 8]. It appears then that the multifaceted arousal-affective-cognitive notion of stress is incompatible with a single neurochemical model of stress and requires a systems-level analysis.
To date, no study has looked at how neural activities in the hippocampus change during stress in real-time in freely behaving animals and how they are reflected in subsequent learning and memory processes. Most, if not all, stress-induced physiological changes in the hippocampus we know of, such as synaptic plasticity, dendritic atrophy, apoptosis, and neurogenesis, have been observed after animals underwent stress (post-hoc), mainly in in vitro or postmortem tissue preparations. Recently, an unprecedented study by McHugh and colleagues [141] examined the dynamic effects of stress on CA1 neural activity during the first 30-min of daily immobilization stress (2-hr/day, 10 consecutive days) in mice. They found that stress decreased the overall activity of pyramidal cells but increased the synchrony between CA1 spikes and SWRs, which could be one way that synaptic saturation blocks subsequent LTP-dependent learning in the stressed hippocampus [141]. This intriguing possibility could be tested by using closed-loop, real-time interruption and prolongation of hippocampal ripples [200, 201] to reduce and increase, respectively, stress-induced alterations in hippocampal synchrony, LTP, and memory. It also remains to be determined if the onset and magnitude of this “neural signature” of stress is related to the severity of subsequent cognitive dysfunctions. Regardless, real-time monitoring of spike and LFP activities in the hippocampus, ideally simultaneously with the amygdala and mPFC, in behaving animals experiencing stress (e.g., audiogenic stress, known to alter LTP, place fields, and spatial memory; [66]), will yield new results at the neural circuit level of computations. Indeed, decoding early neural signatures of stress that reliably predict later effects on learning and memory can, in theory, serve as a new common currency across different stress paradigms and explain why stress impairs hippocampal memory in some animals but not others (individual differences) and why the same stressor impairs spatial memory but enhances contextual fear memory, when both are thought to depend on the hippocampus. Real-time analysis of how stress affects hippocampal neurons (across functionally segmented dorsal, intermediate, and ventral regions [98]) will fill crucial gaps between synaptic, cellular, and cognitive levels of analysis that are just not possible with the current post-hoc approach.
A metaparadigm approach also provides a new scaffold to experimentally manipulate the progression of stress effects, which can ultimately lead to a better understanding of and developing treatment for psychopathologies of stress. For instance, optogenetics can be applied to suppress/excite mPFC neurons and excite/suppress amygdalar neurons in conjunction with corticosteroid administration to mimic/attenuate acute stress effects on hippocampal neural activity, synaptic plasticity, and memory (Fig. 3c). Using chemogenetics [202], similar manipulations of mPFC and amygdalar neurons can be applied, except on a longer timescale, e.g., several hours daily for numerous days. This can effectively be used to induce and prevent chronic stress effects on hippocampal morphology, neurogenesis/apoptosis, and memory.
Concluding remarks
The sine qua non of stress research has long been rooted in the HPA axis effector hormones, with the harmful effects of stress attributed initially to exhausted corticosteroid activity making the body more susceptible to illnesses [38] and then presently to persistent corticosteroid activity causing illnesses [41,42,43,44, 203]. While glucocorticoids and other supposed neurochemical [25,26,27] agents of stress offer a model systems approach to study stress at multiple levels of analysis, whether they accurately reflect the psychophysiological complexity of stress is questionable. For example, corticosteroids serve multifaceted functions, being involved in both aversive and appetitive situations, and thus do not respond uniquely to stress. There is also growing evidence that corticosteroids are neither necessary nor sufficient to produce stress effects in the hippocampus. Accordingly, the continued reliance on a single factor model of stress to know all about the glucocorticoids’ actions on brain and cognition will have little explanatory power; it remains unclear how biologically significant stress affects future neural and psychological processes. Perhaps Selye [17] foreseen the psychological poverty of the glucocorticoid hypothesis of stress when he concluded that “stress is not that which causes a secretion by the adrenal cortex of its hormones. ACTH, the adrenal-stimulating pituitary hormones, can discharge these hormones without producing any evidence of stress.”
References
Kocks UF, Mecking H. Physics and phenomenology of strain hardening: the FCC case. Prog Mater Sci. 2003;48:171–273.
Selye H. Confusion and controversy in the stress field. J Hum Stress. 1975;1:37–44.
Widiger TA, Clark LA. Toward DSM-V and the classification of psychopathology. Psychol Bull. 2000;126:946–63.
Sandi C, Richter-Levin G. From high anxiety trait to depression: a neurocognitive hypothesis. Trends Neurosci. 2009;32:312–20.
Bale TL. Stress sensitivity and the development of affective disorders. Horm Behav. 2006;50:529–33.
Finsterwald C, Alberini CM. Stress and glucocorticoid receptor-dependent mechanisms in long-term memory: from adaptive responses to psychopathologies. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;112:17–29.
Kim EJ, Pellman B, Kim JJ. Stress effects on the hippocampus: a critical review. Learn Mem. 2015;22:411–6.
Kim JJ, Diamond DM. The stressed hippocampus, synaptic plasticity and lost memories. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:453–62.
McEwen BS, Akil H. Revisiting the stress concept: implications for affective disorders. J Neurosci. 2020;40:12–21.
Goosens KA, Sapolsky RM. Stress and glucocorticoid contributions to normal and pathological aging. In: Riddle DR (ed). Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms: Boca Raton (FL), 2007.
Golier JA, Yehuda R, Lupien SJ, Harvey PD, Grossman R, Elkin A. Memory performance in Holocaust survivors with posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1682–8.
Clouston SAP, Deri Y, Diminich E, Kew R, Kotov R, Stewart C, et al. Posttraumatic stress disorder and total amyloid burden and amyloid-beta 42/40 ratios in plasma: results from a pilot study of World Trade Center responders. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2019;11:216–20.
Pisano F, Torromino G, Brachi D, Quadrini A, Incoccia C, Marangolo P. A standardized prospective memory evaluation of the effects of COVID-19 confinement on young students. J Clin Med. 2021;10:3919.
Daly M, Robinson E. Depression and anxiety during COVID-19. Lancet. 2022;399:518.
Finsterwald C, Steinmetz AB, Travaglia A, Alberini CM. From memory impairment to posttraumatic stress disorder-like phenotypes: the critical role of an unpredictable second traumatic experience. J Neurosci. 2015;35:15903–15.
Shors TJ, Seib TB, Levine S, Thompson RF. Inescapable versus escapable shock modulates long-term potentiation in the rat hippocampus. Science. 1989;244:224–6.
Selye H. The evolution of the stress concept. Am Sci. 1973;61:692–9.
Kemeny ME. The Psychobiology of Stress. Curr Direct Psychol Sci. 2003;12:124–9.
Maier SF, Seligman ME. Learned helplessness at fifty: insights from neuroscience. Psychol Rev. 2016;123:349–67.
McDermott WV, Fry EG, Brobeck JR, Long CN. Release of adrenocorticotrophic hormone by direct application of epinephrine to pituitary grafts. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med. 1950;73:609–10.
Sayers G, Sayers MA. Regulation of pituitary adrenocorticotrophic activity during the response of the rat to acute stress. Endocrinology. 1947;40:265–73.
Diamond DM, Campbell AM, Park CR, Halonen J, Zoladz PR. The temporal dynamics model of emotional memory processing: a synthesis on the neurobiological basis of stress-induced amnesia, flashbulb and traumatic memories, and the Yerkes-Dodson law. Neural Plast. 2007;2007:60803.
McGaugh JL. Emotional arousal regulation of memory consolidation. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2018;19:55–60.
Sandi C. Stress and cognition. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2013;4:245–61.
Arnsten AF, Goldman-Rakic PS. Noise stress impairs prefrontal cortical cognitive function in monkeys: evidence for a hyperdopaminergic mechanism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:362–8.
Joels M, Baram TZ. The neuro-symphony of stress. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:459–66.
Maier SF, Watkins LR. Stressor controllability and learned helplessness: the roles of the dorsal raphe nucleus, serotonin, and corticotropin-releasing factor. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:829–41.
Grinevich V, Neumann ID. Brain oxytocin: how puzzle stones from animal studies translate into psychiatry. Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26:265–79.
Avishai-Eliner S, Brunson KL, Sandman CA, Baram TZ. Stressed-out, or in (utero)? Trends Neurosci. 2002;25:518–24.
Kosten TA, Kim JJ, Lee HJ. Early life manipulations alter learning and memory in rats. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:1985–2006.
von Ziegler LM, Floriou-Servou A, Waag R, Das Gupta RR, Sturman O, Gapp K, et al. Multiomic profiling of the acute stress response in the mouse hippocampus. Nat Commun. 2022;13:1824.
Fenster RJ, Lebois LAM, Ressler KJ, Suh J. Brain circuit dysfunction in post-traumatic stress disorder: from mouse to man. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19:535–51.
Vyas A, Mitra R, Shankaranarayana Rao BS, Chattarji S. Chronic stress induces contrasting patterns of dendritic remodeling in hippocampal and amygdaloid neurons. J Neurosci. 2002;22:6810–8.
Cathomas F, Holt LM, Parise EM, Liu J, Murrough JW, Casaccia P, et al. Beyond the neuron: Role of non-neuronal cells in stress disorders. Neuron. 2022;110:1116–38.
Hutmacher F. Putting stress in historical context: why it is important that being stressed out was not a way to be a person 2,000 years ago. Front Psychol. 2021;12:539799.
Huisman M. King Saul, work-related stress and depression. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2007;61:890.
The Stress and Strain of Medicine. The Hospital (Lond 1886) 1891;10:169–70.
Seyle H. A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature. 1936;138:32.
Selye H. Forty years of stress research: principal remaining problems and misconceptions. Can Med Assoc J. 1976;115:53–56.
Miller WL. The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis: a brief History. Horm Res Paediatr. 2018;89:212–23.
Munck A, Guyre PM, Holbrook NJ. Physiological functions of glucocorticoids in stress and their relation to pharmacological actions. Endocr Rev. 1984;5:25–44.
Rivier C, Vale W. Modulation of stress-induced ACTH release by corticotropin-releasing factor, catecholamines and vasopressin. Nature. 1983;305:325–7.
Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, Munck AU. How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocr Rev. 2000;21:55–89.
Vale W, Spiess J, Rivier C, Rivier J. Characterization of a 41-residue ovine hypothalamic peptide that stimulates secretion of corticotropin and beta-endorphin. Science. 1981;213:1394–7.
Marrack P, Kappler J, Kotzin BL. Autoimmune disease: why and where it occurs. Nat Med. 2001;7:899–905.
Burgess N, Maguire EA, O’Keefe J. The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron. 2002;35:625–41.
Sapolsky RM, Krey LC, McEwen BS. Glucocorticoid-sensitive hippocampal neurons are involved in terminating the adrenocortical stress response. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1984;81:6174–7.
Yerkes RM, Dodson JD. The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J Comp Neurol Psychol. 1908;18:459–82.
Salehi B, Cordero MI, Sandi C. Learning under stress: the inverted-U-shape function revisited. Learn Mem. 2010;17:522–30.
Sapolsky RM. Stress and the brain: individual variability and the inverted-U. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:1344–6.
Pavlov IP. Conditioned reflexes: an investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. Oxford University Press, 1927.
Gagnon SA, Wagner AD. Acute stress and episodic memory retrieval: neurobiological mechanisms and behavioral consequences. Ann N. Y Acad Sci. 2016;1369:55–75.
McEwen BS, Sapolsky RM. Stress and cognitive function. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1995;5:205–16.
Hiroto DS, Seligman ME. Generality of learned helplessness in man. J Per Soc Psychol. 1975;31:311–27.
Bremner JD, Scott TM, Delaney RC, Southwick SM, Mason JW, Johnson DR, et al. Deficits in short-term memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150:1015–9.
Nijdam MJ, Martens IJM, Reitsma JB, Gersons BPR, Olff M. Neurocognitive functioning over the course of trauma-focused psychotherapy for PTSD: changes in verbal memory and executive functioning. Br J Clin Psychol. 2018;57:436–52.
Turner CE, Barker-Collo SL, Connell CJ, Gant N. Acute hypoxic gas breathing severely impairs cognition and task learning in humans. Physiol Behav. 2015;142:104–10.
Newcomer JW, Craft S, Hershey T, Askins K, Bardgett ME. Glucocorticoid-induced impairment in declarative memory performance in adult humans. J Neurosci. 1994;14:2047–53.
de Quervain DJ, Roozendaal B, Nitsch RM, McGaugh JL, Hock C. Acute cortisone administration impairs retrieval of long-term declarative memory in humans. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:313–4.
Keller J, Gomez R, Williams G, Lembke A, Lazzeroni L, Murphy GM Jr., et al. HPA axis in major depression: cortisol, clinical symptomatology and genetic variation predict cognition. Mol Psychiatry. 2017;22:527–36.
Starkman MN, Gebarski SS, Berent S, Schteingart DE. Hippocampal formation volume, memory dysfunction, and cortisol levels in patients with Cushing’s syndrome. Biol Psychiatry. 1992;32:756–65.
Diamond DM, Park CR, Heman KL, Rose GM. Exposing rats to a predator impairs spatial working memory in the radial arm water maze. Hippocampus. 1999;9:542–52.
Diamond DM, Rose GM. Stress impairs LTP and hippocampal-dependent memory. Ann N. Y Acad Sci. 1994;746:411–4.
Akirav I, Richter-Levin G. Biphasic modulation of hippocampal plasticity by behavioral stress and basolateral amygdala stimulation in the rat. J Neurosci. 1999;19:10530–5.
Kim JJ, Koo JW, Lee HJ, Han JS. Amygdalar inactivation blocks stress-induced impairments in hippocampal long-term potentiation and spatial memory. J Neurosci. 2005;25:1532–9.
Kim JJ, Lee HJ, Welday AC, Song E, Cho J, Sharp PE, et al. Stress-induced alterations in hippocampal plasticity, place cells, and spatial memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:18297–302.
Kim JJ, Lee HJ, Han JS, Packard MG. Amygdala is critical for stress-induced modulation of hippocampal long-term potentiation and learning. J Neurosci. 2001;21:5222–8.
Baker KB, Kim JJ. Effects of stress and hippocampal NMDA receptor antagonism on recognition memory in rats. Learn Mem. 2002;9:58–65.
Kim EJ, Kim JJ. Amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex and glucocorticoid interactions produce stress-like effects on memory. Front Behav Neurosci. 2019;13:210.
Beck KD, Luine VN. Food deprivation modulates chronic stress effects on object recognition in male rats: role of monoamines and amino acids. Brain Res. 1999;830:56–71.
de Quervain DJ, Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL. Stress and glucocorticoids impair retrieval of long-term spatial memory. Nature. 1998;394:787–90.
Bodnoff SR, Humphreys AG, Lehman JC, Diamond DM, Rose GM, Meaney MJ. Enduring effects of chronic corticosterone treatment on spatial learning, synaptic plasticity, and hippocampal neuropathology in young and mid-aged rats. J Neurosci. 1995;15:61–9.
Heinrichs SC, Stenzel-Poore MP, Gold LH, Battenberg E, Bloom FE, Koob GF, et al. Learning impairment in transgenic mice with central overexpression of corticotropin-releasing factor. Neuroscience. 1996;74:303–11.
Tronche C, Pierard C, Coutan M, Chauveau F, Liscia P, Beracochea D. Increased stress-induced intra-hippocampus corticosterone rise associated with memory impairments in middle-aged mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2010;93:343–51.
Miller JK, McDougall S, Thomas S, Wiener JM. Impairment in active navigation from trauma and post-traumatic stress disorder. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2017;140:114–23.
Quirarte GL, de la Teja IS, Casillas M, Serafin N, Prado-Alcala RA, Roozendaal B. Corticosterone infused into the dorsal striatum selectively enhances memory consolidation of cued water-maze training. Learn Mem. 2009;16:586–9.
Park M, Kim CH, Jo S, Kim EJ, Rhim H, Lee CJ, et al. Chronic stress alters spatial representation and bursting patterns of place cells in behaving mice. Sci Rep. 2015;5:16235.
Pruessner JC, Dedovic K, Khalili-Mahani N, Engert V, Pruessner M, Buss C, et al. Deactivation of the limbic system during acute psychosocial stress: evidence from positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:234–40.
Schwabe L, Oitzl MS, Philippsen C, Richter S, Bohringer A, Wippich W, et al. Stress modulates the use of spatial versus stimulus-response learning strategies in humans. Learn Mem. 2007;14:109–16.
Kim JJ, Baxter MG. Multiple brain-memory systems: the whole does not equal the sum of its parts. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24:324–30.
Schwabe L. Stress and the engagement of multiple memory systems: integration of animal and human studies. Hippocampus. 2013;23:1035–43.
Spence KW, Taylor J. Anxiety and strength of the UCS as determiners of the amount of eyelid conditioning. J Exp Psychol. 1951;42:183–8.
Shors TJ, Weiss C, Thompson RF. Stress-induced facilitation of classical conditioning. Science. 1992;257:537–9.
Beylin AV, Shors TJ. Stress enhances excitatory trace eyeblink conditioning and opposes acquisition of inhibitory conditioning. Behav Neurosci. 1998;112:1327–38.
Lavond DG, Kim JJ, Thompson RF. Mammalian brain substrates of aversive classical conditioning. Annu Rev Psychol. 1993;44:317–42.
Joels M, Fernandez G, Roozendaal B. Stress and emotional memory: a matter of timing. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:280–8.
Rodriguez Manzanares PA, Isoardi NA, Carrer HF, Molina VA. Previous stress facilitates fear memory, attenuates GABAergic inhibition, and increases synaptic plasticity in the rat basolateral amygdala. J Neurosci. 2005;25:8725–34.
Sananbenesi F, Fischer A, Schrick C, Spiess J, Radulovic J. Mitogen-activated protein kinase signaling in the hippocampus and its modulation by corticotropin-releasing factor receptor 2: a possible link between stress and fear memory. J Neurosci. 2003;23:11436–43.
Rau V, Fanselow MS. Exposure to a stressor produces a long lasting enhancement of fear learning in rats. Stress. 2009;12:125–33.
Camp RM, Johnson JD. Repeated stressor exposure enhances contextual fear memory in a beta-adrenergic receptor-dependent process and increases impulsivity in a non-beta receptor-dependent fashion. Physiol Behav. 2015;150:64–8.
Tronson NC, Guzman YF, Guedea AL, Huh KH, Gao C, Schwarz MK, et al. Metabotropic glutamate receptor 5/Homer interactions underlie stress effects on fear. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:1007–15.
Conrad CD, LeDoux JE, Magarinos AM, McEwen BS. Repeated restraint stress facilitates fear conditioning independently of causing hippocampal CA3 dendritic atrophy. Behav Neurosci. 1999;113:902–13.
Phillips RG, LeDoux JE. Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci. 1992;106:274–85.
Kim JJ, Fanselow MS. Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science. 1992;256:675–7.
Anagnostaras SG, Gale GD, Fanselow MS. Hippocampus and contextual fear conditioning: recent controversies and advances. Hippocampus. 2001;11:8–17.
Nishimura KJ, Poulos AM, Drew MR, Rajbhandari AK. Know thy SEFL: Fear sensitization and its relevance to stressor-related disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2022;142:104884.
Poulos AM, Reger M, Mehta N, Zhuravka I, Sterlace SS, Gannam C, et al. Amnesia for early life stress does not preclude the adult development of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in rats. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;76:306–14.
Fanselow MS, Dong HW. Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron. 2010;65:7–19.
Revest JM, Di Blasi F, Kitchener P, Rouge-Pont F, Desmedt A, Turiault M, et al. The MAPK pathway and Egr-1 mediate stress-related behavioral effects of glucocorticoids. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:664–72.
O’Kearney R, Perrott K. Trauma narratives in posttraumatic stress disorder: a review. J Trauma Stress. 2006;19:81–93.
Foy MR, Stanton ME, Levine S, Thompson RF. Behavioral stress impairs long-term potentiation in rodent hippocampus. Behav Neural Biol. 1987;48:138–49.
Bliss TV, Lomo T. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol. 1973;232:331–56.
Nicoll RA. A Brief History of Long-Term Potentiation. Neuron. 2017;93:281–90.
Shors TJ, Foy MR, Levine S, Thompson RF. Unpredictable and uncontrollable stress impairs neuronal plasticity in the rat hippocampus. Brain Res Bull. 1990;24:663–7.
Xu L, Anwyl R, Rowan MJ. Behavioural stress facilitates the induction of long-term depression in the hippocampus. Nature. 1997;387:497–500.
Shors TJ, Dryver E. Effect of stress and long-term potentiation (LTP) on subsequent LTP and the theta burst response in the dentate gyrus. Brain Res. 1994;666:232–8.
Pavlides C, Nivon LG, McEwen BS. Effects of chronic stress on hippocampal long-term potentiation. Hippocampus. 2002;12:245–57.
Chen CC, Yang CH, Huang CC, Hsu KS. Acute stress impairs hippocampal mossy fiber-CA3 long-term potentiation by enhancing cAMP-specific phosphodiesterase 4 activity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:1605–17.
Radahmadi M, Hosseini N, Nasimi A. Effect of chronic stress on short and long-term plasticity in dentate gyrus; study of recovery and adaptation. Neuroscience. 2014;280:121–9.
MacDougall MJ, Howland JG. Acute stress, but not corticosterone, disrupts short- and long-term synaptic plasticity in rat dorsal subiculum via glucocorticoid receptor activation. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23:2611–9.
Mesches MH, Fleshner M, Heman KL, Rose GM, Diamond DM. Exposing rats to a predator blocks primed burst potentiation in the hippocampus in vitro. J Neurosci. 1999;19:RC18.
Kim JJ, Foy MR, Thompson RF. Behavioral stress modifies hippocampal plasticity through N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor activation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93:4750–3.
Garcia R, Musleh W, Tocco G, Thompson RF, Baudry M. Time-dependent blockade of STP and LTP in hippocampal slices following acute stress in mice. Neurosci Lett. 1997;233:41–4.
Shors TJ, Gallegos RA, Breindl A. Transient and persistent consequences of acute stress on long-term potentiation (LTP), synaptic efficacy, theta rhythms and bursts in area CA1 of the hippocampus. Synapse. 1997;26:209–17.
Dudek SM, Bear MF. Homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of hippocampus and effects of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor blockade. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1992;89:4363–7.
Bienenstock EL, Cooper LN, Munro PW. Theory for the development of neuron selectivity: orientation specificity and binocular interaction in visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1982;2:32–48.
Maudsley DB. A theory of meta-learning and principles of facilitation: An organismic perspective. University of Toronto, Canada, 1979.
Abraham WC, Tate WP. Metaplasticity: a new vista across the field of synaptic plasticity. Prog Neurobiol. 1997;52:303–23.
Kim JJ, Yoon KS. Stress: metaplastic effects in the hippocampus. Trends Neurosci. 1998;21:505–9.
Kamal A, Ramakers GM, Altinbilek B, Kas MJ. Social isolation stress reduces hippocampal long-term potentiation: effect of animal strain and involvement of glucocorticoid receptors. Neuroscience. 2014;256:262–70.
Xu L, Holscher C, Anwyl R, Rowan MJ. Glucocorticoid receptor and protein/RNA synthesis-dependent mechanisms underlie the control of synaptic plasticity by stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:3204–8.
Diamond DM, Bennett MC, Fleshner M, Rose GM. Inverted-U relationship between the level of peripheral corticosterone and the magnitude of hippocampal primed burst potentiation. Hippocampus. 1992;2:421–30.
Pavlides C, Watanabe Y, McEwen BS. Effects of glucocorticoids on hippocampal long-term potentiation. Hippocampus. 1993;3:183–92.
Alfarez DN, Wiegert O, Joels M, Krugers HJ. Corticosterone and stress reduce synaptic potentiation in mouse hippocampal slices with mild stimulation. Neuroscience. 2002;115:1119–26.
Pavlides C, Ogawa S, Kimura A, McEwen BS. Role of adrenal steroid mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptors in long-term potentiation in the CA1 field of hippocampal slices. Brain Res. 1996;738:229–35.
Pavlides C, Watanabe Y, Magarinos AM, McEwen BS. Opposing roles of type I and type II adrenal steroid receptors in hippocampal long-term potentiation. Neuroscience. 1995;68:387–94.
Armanini MP, Hutchins C, Stein BA, Sapolsky RM. Glucocorticoid endangerment of hippocampal neurons is NMDA-receptor dependent. Brain Res. 1990;532:7–12.
Kerr DS, Campbell LW, Hao SY, Landfield PW. Corticosteroid modulation of hippocampal potentials: increased effect with aging. Science. 1989;245:1505–9.
O’Keefe J, Dostrovsky J. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 1971;34:171–5.
O’keefe J, Nadel L. The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford University Press, 1978.
Kentros C, Hargreaves E, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER, Shapiro M, Muller RV. Abolition of long-term stability of new hippocampal place cell maps by NMDA receptor blockade. Science. 1998;280:2121–6.
McHugh TJ, Blum KI, Tsien JZ, Tonegawa S, Wilson MA. Impaired hippocampal representation of space in CA1-specific NMDAR1 knockout mice. Cell. 1996;87:1339–49.
Robinson NTM, Descamps LAL, Russell LE, Buchholz MO, Bicknell BA, Antonov GK, et al. Targeted activation of Hippocampal place cells drives memory-guided spatial behavior. Cell. 2020;183:2041–2.
Tomar A, Polygalov D, Chattarji S, McHugh TJ. The dynamic impact of repeated stress on the hippocampal spatial map. Hippocampus. 2015;25:38–50.
Pfaff DW, Silva MT, Weiss JM. Telemetered recording of hormone effects on hippocampal neurons. Science. 1971;172:394–5.
Beck SG, List TJ, Choi KC. Long- and short-term administration of corticosterone alters CA1 hippocampal neuronal properties. Neuroendocrinology. 1994;60:261–72.
Joels M, de Kloet ER. Mineralocorticoid receptor-mediated changes in membrane properties of rat CA1 pyramidal neurons in vitro. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1990;87:4495–8.
Kim EJ, Kim ES, Park M, Cho J, Kim JJ. Amygdalar stimulation produces alterations on firing properties of hippocampal place cells. J Neurosci. 2012;32:11424–34.
Logothetis NK. The underpinnings of the BOLD functional magnetic resonance imaging signal. J Neurosci. 2003;23:3963–71.
Buzsáki G. Rhythms of the brain. Oxford University Press, 2006.
Tomar A, Polygalov D, Chattarji S, McHugh TJ. Stress enhances hippocampal neuronal synchrony and alters ripple-spike interaction. Neurobiol Stress. 2021;14:100327.
Tomar A, Polygalov D, McHugh TJ. Differential impact of acute and chronic stress on CA1 spatial coding and gamma oscillations. Front Behav Neurosci. 2021;15:710725.
Jacinto LR, Reis JS, Dias NS, Cerqueira JJ, Correia JH, Sousa N. Stress affects theta activity in limbic networks and impairs novelty-induced exploration and familiarization. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013;7:127.
Ghosh S, Laxmi TR, Chattarji S. Functional connectivity from the amygdala to the hippocampus grows stronger after stress. J Neurosci. 2013;33:7234–44.
Heinbockel H, Quaedflieg C, Schneider TR, Engel AK, Schwabe L. Stress enhances emotional memory-related theta oscillations in the medial temporal lobe. Neurobiol Stress. 2021;15:100383.
Colgin LL, Moser EI. Gamma oscillations in the hippocampus. Physiol (Bethesda). 2010;25:319–29.
O’Keefe J, Recce ML. Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm. Hippocampus. 1993;3:317–30.
Tomar A, McHugh TJ. The impact of stress on the hippocampal spatial code. Trends Neurosci. 2022;45:120–32.
Watanabe Y, Gould E, McEwen BS. Stress induces atrophy of apical dendrites of hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons. Brain Res. 1992;588:341–5.
Woolley CS, Gould E, McEwen BS. Exposure to excess glucocorticoids alters dendritic morphology of adult hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Brain Res. 1990;531:225–31.
Magarinos AM, McEwen BS, Flugge G, Fuchs E. Chronic psychosocial stress causes apical dendritic atrophy of hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons in subordinate tree shrews. J Neurosci. 1996;16:3534–40.
Sunanda RaoMS, Raju TR. Effect of chronic restraint stress on dendritic spines and excrescences of hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons-a quantitative study. Brain Res. 1995;694:312–7.
Coleman PD, Flood DG. Neuron numbers and dendritic extent in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 1987;8:521–45.
Gould E, Tanapat P. Stress and hippocampal neurogenesis. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:1472–9.
Joels M, Karst H, Krugers HJ, Lucassen PJ. Chronic stress: implications for neuronal morphology, function and neurogenesis. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2007;28:72–96.
Lucassen PJ, Heine VM, Muller MB, van der Beek EM, Wiegant VM, De Kloet ER, et al. Stress, depression and hippocampal apoptosis. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets. 2006;5:531–46.
Sapolsky RM. Stress, the Aging Brain, and the Mechanisms of Neuron Death. The MIT Press, 1992.
Uno H, Tarara R, Else JG, Suleman MA, Sapolsky RM. Hippocampal damage associated with prolonged and fatal stress in primates. J Neurosci. 1989;9:1705–11.
Mizoguchi K, Kunishita T, Chui DH, Tabira T. Stress induces neuronal death in the hippocampus of castrated rats. Neurosci Lett. 1992;138:157–60.
Zhao H, Xu H, Xu X, Young D. Predatory stress induces hippocampal cell death by apoptosis in rats. Neurosci Lett. 2007;421:115–20.
Pham K, Nacher J, Hof PR, McEwen BS. Repeated restraint stress suppresses neurogenesis and induces biphasic PSA-NCAM expression in the adult rat dentate gyrus. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:879–86.
Yap JJ, Takase LF, Kochman LJ, Fornal CA, Miczek KA, Jacobs BL. Repeated brief social defeat episodes in mice: effects on cell proliferation in the dentate gyrus. Behav Brain Res. 2006;172:344–50.
Gould E, McEwen BS, Tanapat P, Galea LA, Fuchs E. Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the adult tree shrew is regulated by psychosocial stress and NMDA receptor activation. J Neurosci. 1997;17:2492–8.
Gould E, Tanapat P, McEwen BS, Flugge G, Fuchs E. Proliferation of granule cell precursors in the dentate gyrus of adult monkeys is diminished by stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:3168–71.
Bremner JD. Does stress damage the brain? Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45:797–805.
Lindauer RJ, Vlieger EJ, Jalink M, Olff M, Carlier IV, Majoie CB, et al. Smaller hippocampal volume in Dutch police officers with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56:356–63.
Bonne O, Brandes D, Gilboa A, Gomori JM, Shenton ME, Pitman RK, et al. Longitudinal MRI study of hippocampal volume in trauma survivors with PTSD. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:1248–51.
Bremner JD, Vythilingam M, Vermetten E, Southwick SM, McGlashan T, Nazeer A, et al. MRI and PET study of deficits in hippocampal structure and function in women with childhood sexual abuse and posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:924–32.
Gilbertson MW, Shenton ME, Ciszewski A, Kasai K, Lasko NB, Orr SP, et al. Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:1242–7.
Lee T, Jarome T, Li SJ, Kim JJ, Helmstetter FJ. Chronic stress selectively reduces hippocampal volume in rats: a longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroreport. 2009;20:1554–8.
de Kloet ER, Oitzl MS, Joels M. Functional implications of brain corticosteroid receptor diversity. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 1993;13:433–55.
McEwen BS, Weiss JM, Schwartz LS. Selective retention of corticosterone by limbic structures in rat brain. Nature. 1968;220:911–2.
Kaminska M, Harris J, Gijsbers K, Dubrovsky B. Dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEAS) counteracts decremental effects of corticosterone on dentate gyrus LTP. Implications for depression. Brain Res Bull. 2000;52:229–34.
McEwen BS, Albeck D, Cameron H, Chao HM, Gould E, Hastings N, et al. Stress and the brain: a paradoxical role for adrenal steroids. Vitam Horm. 1995;51:371–402.
de Quervain DJ, Henke K, Aerni A, Treyer V, McGaugh JL, Berthold T, et al. Glucocorticoid-induced impairment of declarative memory retrieval is associated with reduced blood flow in the medial temporal lobe. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:1296–302.
Shors TJ, Levine S, Thompson RF. Effect of adrenalectomy and demedullation on the stress-induced impairment of long-term potentiation. Neuroendocrinology. 1990;51:70–5.
Shakesby AC, Anwyl R, Rowan MJ. Overcoming the effects of stress on synaptic plasticity in the intact hippocampus: rapid actions of serotonergic and antidepressant agents. J Neurosci. 2002;22:3638–44.
Luine VN, Spencer RL, McEwen BS. Effects of chronic corticosterone ingestion on spatial memory performance and hippocampal serotonergic function. Brain Res. 1993;616:65–70.
Woodson JC, Macintosh D, Fleshner M, Diamond DM. Emotion-induced amnesia in rats: working memory-specific impairment, corticosterone-memory correlation, and fear versus arousal effects on memory. Learn Mem. 2003;10:326–36.
Schoenfeld TJ, Gould E. Stress, stress hormones, and adult neurogenesis. Exp Neurol. 2012;233:12–21.
Chen C, Nakagawa S, An Y, Ito K, Kitaichi Y, Kusumi I. The exercise-glucocorticoid paradox: How exercise is beneficial to cognition, mood, and the brain while increasing glucocorticoid levels. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2017;44:83–102.
Hill EE, Zack E, Battaglini C, Viru M, Viru A, Hackney AC. Exercise and circulating cortisol levels: the intensity threshold effect. J Endocrinol Invest. 2008;31:587–91.
Li TL, Cheng PY. Alterations of immunoendocrine responses during the recovery period after acute prolonged cycling. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2007;101:539–46.
Chang YK, Labban JD, Gapin JI, Etnier JL. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2012;1453:87–101.
Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:58–65.
Novak CM, Burghardt PR, Levine JA. The use of a running wheel to measure activity in rodents: relationship to energy balance, general activity, and reward. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:1001–14.
Lucia A, Hoyos J, Santalla A, Earnest C, Chicharro JL. Tour de France versus Vuelta a Espana: which is harder? Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2003;35:872–8.
Yehuda R, Southwick SM, Nussbaum G, Wahby V, Giller EL Jr, Mason JW. Low urinary cortisol excretion in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1990;178:366–9.
Thompson EB, Lippman ME. Mechanism of action of glucocorticoids. Metabolism. 1974;23:159–202.
Vegiopoulos A, Herzig S. Glucocorticoids, metabolism and metabolic diseases. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2007;275:43–61.
Koolhaas JM, Bartolomucci A, Buwalda B, de Boer SF, Flugge G, Korte SM, et al. Stress revisited: a critical evaluation of the stress concept. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:1291–301.
Kim JJ, Haller J. Glucocorticoid hyper- and hypofunction: stress effects on cognition and aggression. Ann N. Y Acad Sci. 2007;1113:291–303.
Dallman MF, Strack AM, Akana SF, Bradbury MJ, Hanson ES, Scribner KA, et al. Feast and famine: critical role of glucocorticoids with insulin in daily energy flow. Front Neuroendocrinol. 1993;14:303–47.
LeDoux JE. Anxious: Using the Brain to Understand and Treat Fear and Anxiety. Penguin Books, 2015.
Maier SF. Behavioral control blunts reactions to contemporaneous and future adverse events: medial prefrontal cortex plasticity and a corticostriatal network. Neurobiol Stress. 2015;1:12–22.
Maier SF, Watkins LR. Role of the medial prefrontal cortex in coping and resilience. Brain Res. 2010;1355:52–60.
Diorio D, Viau V, Meaney MJ. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex (cingulate gyrus) in the regulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. J Neurosci. 1993;13:3839–47.
Radley JJ, Gosselink KL, Sawchenko PE. A discrete GABAergic relay mediates medial prefrontal cortical inhibition of the neuroendocrine stress response. J Neurosci. 2009;29:7330–40.
LeDoux JE. The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. Simon & Schuster, 1996.
Fernandez-Ruiz A, Oliva A, Fermino de Oliveira E, Rocha-Almeida F, Tingley D, Buzsaki G. Long-duration hippocampal sharp wave ripples improve memory. Science. 2019;364:1082–6.
Lewis S. Novel inhibition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19:581.
Roth BL. DREADDs for neuroscientists. Neuron. 2016;89:683–94.
Smith SM, Vale WW. The role of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in neuroendocrine responses to stress. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2006;8:383–95.
Diamond DM, Fleshner M, Ingersoll N, Rose GM. Psychological stress impairs spatial working memory: relevance to electrophysiological studies of hippocampal function. Behav Neurosci. 1996;110:661–72.
Ferragud A, Haro A, Sylvain A, Velazquez-Sanchez C, Hernandez-Rabaza V, Canales JJ. Enhanced habit-based learning and decreased neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus in a murine model of chronic social stress. Behav Brain Res. 2010;210:134–9.
Rei D, Mason X, Seo J, Graff J, Rudenko A, Wang J, et al. Basolateral amygdala bidirectionally modulates stress-induced hippocampal learning and memory deficits through a p25/Cdk5-dependent pathway. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112:7291–6.
Uddo M, Vasterling JJ, Brailey K, Sutker PB. Memory and attention in combat-related post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 1993;15:43–52.
Kirschbaum C, Wolf OT, May M, Wippich W, Hellhammer DH. Stress- and treatment-induced elevations of cortisol levels associated with impaired declarative memory in healthy adults. Life Sci. 1996;58:1475–83.
Schwabe L, Wolf OT. Stress modulates the engagement of multiple memory systems in classification learning. J Neurosci. 2012;32:11042–9.
Brown TI, Gagnon SA, Wagner AD. Stress disrupts human hippocampal-prefrontal function during prospective spatial navigation and hinders flexible behavior. Curr Biol. 2020;30:1821–33 e1828.
Gagnon SA, Waskom ML, Brown TI, Wagner AD. Stress impairs episodic retrieval by disrupting hippocampal and cortical mechanisms of remembering. Cereb Cortex. 2019;29:2947–64.
Wilson LM, Wilson JR, Dicara LV. Facilitation of Pavlovian conditioned cardiodecelerations following preshock in immobilized rats. Physiol Behav. 1975;15:653–8.
Dias-Ferreira E, Sousa JC, Melo I, Morgado P, Mesquita AR, Cerqueira JJ, et al. Chronic stress causes frontostriatal reorganization and affects decision-making. Science. 2009;325:621–5.
Yuen EY, Liu W, Karatsoreos IN, Ren Y, Feng J, McEwen BS, et al. Mechanisms for acute stress-induced enhancement of glutamatergic transmission and working memory. Mol Psychiatry. 2011;16:156–70.
Leong KC, Packard MG. Exposure to predator odor influences the relative use of multiple memory systems: role of basolateral amygdala. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;109:56–61.
Taylor SB, Anglin JM, Paode PR, Riggert AG, Olive MF, Conrad CD. Chronic stress may facilitate the recruitment of habit- and addiction-related neurocircuitries through neuronal restructuring of the striatum. Neuroscience. 2014;280:231–42.
Schwabe L, Dalm S, Schachinger H, Oitzl MS. Chronic stress modulates the use of spatial and stimulus-response learning strategies in mice and man. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008;90:495–503.
Maddox WT, Koslov S, Yi HG, Chandrasekaran B. Performance Pressure Enhances Speech Learning. Appl Psycholinguist. 2016;37:1369–96.
Hordacre B, Immink MA, Ridding MC, Hillier S. Perceptual-motor learning benefits from increased stress and anxiety. Hum Mov Sci. 2016;49:36–46.
Wegner M, Koedijker JM, Budde H. The effect of acute exercise and psychosocial stress on fine motor skills and testosterone concentration in the saliva of high school students. PLoS One. 2014;9:e92953.
Cahill L, Gorski L, Le K. Enhanced human memory consolidation with post-learning stress: interaction with the degree of arousal at encoding. Learn Mem. 2003;10:270–4.
Bremner JD, Randall P, Scott TM, Bronen RA, Seibyl JP, Southwick SM, et al. MRI-based measurement of hippocampal volume in patients with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152:973–81.
Luine V, Villegas M, Martinez C, McEwen BS. Repeated stress causes reversible impairments of spatial memory performance. Brain Res. 1994;639:167–70.
Wu YP, Gao HY, Ouyang SH, Kurihara H, He RR, Li YF. Predator stress-induced depression is associated with inhibition of hippocampal neurogenesis in adult male mice. Neural Regen Res. 2019;14:298–305.
Eadie BD, Redila VA, Christie BR. Voluntary exercise alters the cytoarchitecture of the adult dentate gyrus by increasing cellular proliferation, dendritic complexity, and spine density. J Comp Neurol. 2005;486:39–47.
Gomes da Silva S, Unsain N, Masco DH, Toscano-Silva M, de Amorim HA, Silva Araujo BH, et al. Early exercise promotes positive hippocampal plasticity and improves spatial memory in the adult life of rats. Hippocampus. 2012;22:347–58.
Griffin EW, Bechara RG, Birch AM, Kelly AM. Exercise enhances hippocampal-dependent learning in the rat: evidence for a BDNF-related mechanism. Hippocampus. 2009;19:973–80.
Kanaley JA, Weltman JY, Pieper KS, Weltman A, Hartman ML. Cortisol and growth hormone responses to exercise at different times of day. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2001;86:2881–9.
Radahmadi M, Hosseini N, Alaei H. Effect of exercise, exercise withdrawal, and continued regular exercise on excitability and long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus of hippocampus. Brain Res. 2016;1653:8–13.
Serra FT, Carvalho AD, Araujo BHS, Torres LB, Cardoso FDS, Henrique JS, et al. Early exercise induces long-lasting morphological changes in cortical and hippocampal neurons throughout of a sedentary period of rats. Sci Rep. 2019;9:13684.
van Praag H, Shubert T, Zhao C, Gage FH. Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. J Neurosci. 2005;25:8680–5.
Castro AE, Young LJ, Camacho FJ, Paredes RG, Diaz NF, Portillo W. Effects of mating and social exposure on cell proliferation in the adult male prairie vole (microtus ochrogaster). Neural Plast. 2020;2020:8869669.
Bai HY, Cao J, Liu N, Xu L, Luo JH. Sexual behavior modulates contextual fear memory through dopamine D1/D5 receptors. Hippocampus. 2009;19:289–98.
Glasper ER, Gould E. Sexual experience restores age-related decline in adult neurogenesis and hippocampal function. Hippocampus. 2013;23:303–12.
Kim JI, Lee JW, Lee YA, Lee DH, Han NS, Choi YK, et al. Sexual activity counteracts the suppressive effects of chronic stress on adult hippocampal neurogenesis and recognition memory. Brain Res. 2013;1538:26–40.
Leuner B, Glasper ER, Gould E. Sexual experience promotes adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus despite an initial elevation in stress hormones. PLoS One. 2010;5:e11597.
Phoenix CH, Dixson AF, Resko JA. Effects of ejaculation on levels of testosterone, cortisol, and luteinizing hormone in peripheral plasma of rhesus monkeys. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1977;91:120–7.
Wright H, Jenks RA. Sex on the brain! Associations between sexual activity and cognitive function in older age. Age Ageing. 2016;45:313–7.
Bronzino JD, Austin La France RJ, Morgane PJ, Galler JR. Diet-induced alterations in the ontogeny of long-term potentiation. Hippocampus. 1996;6:109–17.
Oomura Y, Sasaki K, Li AJ. Memory facilitation educed by food intake. Physiol Behav. 1993;54:493–8.
Lee J, Seroogy KB, Mattson MP. Dietary restriction enhances neurotrophin expression and neurogenesis in the hippocampus of adult mice. J Neurochem. 2002;80:539–47.
Moberg GP, Bellinger LL, Mendel VE. Effect of meal feeding on daily rhythms of plasma corticosterone and growth hormone in the rat. Neuroendocrinology. 1975;19:160–9.
Rosmond R, Holm G, Bjorntorp P. Food-induced cortisol secretion in relation to anthropometric, metabolic and haemodynamic variables in men. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2000;24:416–22.
Viggiano A, Viggiano A, Monda M, Turco I, Incarnato L, Vinno V, et al. Annurca apple-rich diet restores long-term potentiation and induces behavioral modifications in aged rats. Exp Neurol. 2006;199:354–61.
Zhang Y, Li N, Yang Z. Perinatal food restriction impaired spatial learning and memory behavior and decreased the density of nitric oxide synthase neurons in the hippocampus of adult male rat offspring. Toxicol Lett. 2010;193:167–72.
Stewart J, Mitchell J, Kalant N. The effects of life-long food restriction on spatial memory in young and aged Fischer 344 rats measured in the eight-arm radial and the Morris water mazes. Neurobiol Aging. 1989;10:669–75.
Frick KM, Fernandez SM. Enrichment enhances spatial memory and increases synaptophysin levels in aged female mice. Neurobiol Aging. 2003;24:615–26.
Leggio MG, Mandolesi L, Federico F, Spirito F, Ricci B, Gelfo F, et al. Environmental enrichment promotes improved spatial abilities and enhanced dendritic growth in the rat. Behav Brain Res. 2005;163:78–90.
Moncek F, Duncko R, Johansson BB, Jezova D. Effect of environmental enrichment on stress related systems in rats. J Neuroendocrinol. 2004;16:423–31.
Monteiro BM, Moreira FA, Massensini AR, Moraes MF, Pereira GS. Enriched environment increases neurogenesis and improves social memory persistence in socially isolated adult mice. Hippocampus. 2014;24:239–48.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by NIH grants AG067008 (EJK) and MH099073 (JJK), the Brain Convergence Research Program (2021M3E5D2A01023887) funded by the Korean Government (MSIT) (EJK), and the University of Washington Royalty Research Fund FA205582 (JJK). We thank Bryan Schuessler for helpful comments on the manuscript. Figures 1–3 were created with BioRender.com
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
EJK and JJK conceptualized and wrote the initial draft, revised it, and approved the final manuscript. The figures and tables were prepared by EJK and JJK.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Kim, E.J., Kim, J.J. Neurocognitive effects of stress: a metaparadigm perspective. Mol Psychiatry 28, 2750–2763 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-023-01986-4
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-023-01986-4
This article is cited by
-
Effects of chronic empathic stress on synaptic efficacy, as well as short-term and long-term plasticity at the Schaffer collateral/commissural– CA1 synapses in the dorsal hippocampus of rats
Metabolic Brain Disease (2024)
-
From Perspective of Hippocampal Plasticity: Function of Antidepressant Chinese Medicine Xiaoyaosan
Chinese Journal of Integrative Medicine (2024)