Citation 1
Citation 1
Citation 1
OUTDOOR SPACES ARE A FEATURE of most Australian early learning settings and have
potential for many learning opportunities. This article reports on a study that investigated threeand four-year-oldchildren's perspectives of the outdoor environment in their early childhood
education setting. The research was conducted using multi-method approaches including childled tours and photography, photographic elicitation and conversations. Findings emphasise the
importance for children of being able to pretend, move, observe and be social. These findings
have implications for designers of bothcurriculum and outdoor spaces for young children.
Full Text:
When discussing the 'space' of educational settings there is a temptation to think in terms of
indoor spaces, of the buildings and their contents. However, outdoor spaces are also part of
educational environments, and research indicates that experiences outside not only improve
academic performance, but also physical activity levels, social interactions and emotional
wellbeing (Malone, 2008; Nicol, Higgins, Ross, & Mannion, 2007; Rickinson et al., 2004; Waite,
2011). But as Malone (2008) notes, much research focuses on school-aged children and there
have been few investigations of outdoor spaces for younger children in settings such as
kindergartens and child care, particularly in Australian contexts.
Despite a lack of research, the Australian Government has made a clear statement of the role of
outdoor spaces in its nationally mandated document, Belonging, being and becoming: The Early
Years Learning Framework for Australia (DEEWR, 2009) for all young children (birth to five) who
attend early childhood educational settings. It states: 'Outdoor learning spaces are a feature of
Australian learning environments. They offer a vast array of possibilities not available indoors'
(DEEWR, 2009, pp. 15-16). Given this statement, researching outdoor spaces in educational
settings for young children is a pressing need in Australia, and as children are the main users of
such spaces, their perspectives are a logical starting point for the research effort. However,
research including young children's perspectives is uncommon, not just with respect to the
outdoors, but in general (Clark, 2005; Lansdown, 2005; Smith, 2011). Therefore, the goals of
this study were not only to investigate children's perspectives of the outdoor spaces in their
early learning setting, but also to investigate methods for doing this. Findings relating to the
children's perspectives are the focus of this article; research methods findings are described
elsewhere (Merewether & Fleet, 2014).
Valuing children's perspectives
Smith (2011) points out that the world abounds with research on children, but the body of
research with children that has found ways to make children's voices visible is limited, although
this has been changing over the last two decades. A significant catalyst is the United Nations
Convention on the Rights of the Child (UN, 1989), which recognises children as active citizens
with rights to participate in matters affecting them, including research. A second contributor to
changing perspectives is the field of Childhood Studies (James & James, 2008; Kehily, 2009;
Qvortrup, Corsaro & Honig, 2009), which focuses on children's rights, voices and participation.
By conceptualising children as social actors and capable holders of opinions and ideas,
Childhood Studies presents the possibility for researchers to work together with children as coconstructors of research data. A third major contributor to strengths-based views of young
children is the educational project of the city of Reggio Emilia in Italy which, through its use of
multiple forms of listening and documentation, has graphically made visible the capabilities of
young children (Edwards, Gandini & Forman, 2012; Giudici, Rinaldi & Krechevsky, 2001;
Rinaldi, 2006).
These catalysts influenced the work of Clark and Moss (2001) who coined the term 'Mosaic
approach' as a metaphor for their multi-method, participatory research approach which draws
together pieces from different sources to create a 'picture' of children's perspectives. The
approach uses the traditional research tools of observation and interviews, as well as a variety
of participatory tools that can be used by children. The participatory tools include child-led
photography, book making, tours and drawing. The Mosaic approach has informed researchers
who have found ways to engage and empower young children in research (Cremin & Slatter,
2004; Fleet & Britt, 2011; Smith, Duncan & Marshall, 2005; Stephenson, 2009). This work has
revealed young children to be capable of providing valuable perspectives in matters of
importance to them.
The present study took inspiration from the Mosaic approach to seek young children's
perspectives about their early childhood setting's outdoor environment with a view to joint
decision making about its future design and use. The inquiry was guided by the question: What
does it mean to be in this outdoor space? The research used case study methodology (Merriam,
1998; Stake, 1995), as the primary intention was to gain in-depth perspectives from children in
one particular context. While single-case studies do not lend themselves to generalising (Stake,
1995), insights gained from this study may be relevant for others.
Children's preference for the outdoors
Studies that have investigated children's environments consistently find children prefer the
outdoors. For example, research with school-aged children in England (Burke, 2005; Titman,
1994) and younger children in Canada and New Zealand (Blanchet-Cohen & Elliot, 2011;
Stephenson, 2002) note children's preferences for outdoor over indoor spaces in educational
settings. Stephenson (2002) postulates that children preferred the outdoor environment in the
case she studied as it was more 'open' in terms of space, educator attitudes, routines and
equipment, and Titman's (1994, p. 27) study of school-aged children led her to conclude the
outdoors 'signified opportunities for a range of the things children wanted and needed to do
which were not possible indoors'. Rogers and Evans (2007, p. 164), in a study of young
children's role-play, note outdoor contexts enable children to 'exercise greater choice over
materials, location and playmates'. Australian studies involving school children have
investigated children's preferences for certain outdoor equipment and materials (Bundy et al.,
2008; Lucas & Dyment, 2010) and similar constructs have been investigated for younger
children in the United States(Cosco, Moore & Islam, 2010; Holmes & Procaccino, 2009) and
Norway (Storli & Hagen, 2010). These studies suggest that if outdoor environments are to
appeal to children, equipment and materials must be open-ended, and facilitate challenge and
social engagement.
It is important to acknowledge that while research suggests the outdoors is popular with most
children, a few prefer the indoors (Einarsdottir, 2011). Perhaps outdoor time is not as relevant
for some children due to increased opportunities indoors (Waller, Sandseter, Wyver, ArlemalmHagser & Maynard, 2010). Agency, choice and opportunities to socialise appear to be central to
children's preference for outside spaces, but these elements are not necessarily unique to the
outdoors. When some Australian school children were asked to recall a special place in their
kindergarten, the outdoors featured in their memories; however, their choices were not so much
grounded in 'outdoorness', but in the opportunity to have 'creative agency and relationships'
(Fleet & Britt, 2011, p. 159). The mere fact of being outside, or the amount of physical space
available, does not necessarily lead to positive outcomes. Some outdoor play environments with
large amounts of space but a lack of equipment and materials lead to an increase in schoolaged children's boredom and aggression, along with lower levels of social, physical and
cognitive development (Evans, 2001; Moore & Wong, 1997). Little (2010) suggests that in some
Australian early childhood settings, in an attempt to remove risk, 'outdoor play experiences have
become so sterile that the thrill and exhilaration that provide a perception of risk have also been
removed' (p. 16). These 'risk-free' outdoor environments are unlikely to be favoured by children.
Methods
The following is an overview of the methods used in this study. Methods are more fully
described in Merewether and Fleet (2014). The research took place over two months in a Perth
early childhood learning setting. Within this centre, two classes of three- and four-year-old
children, each welcoming 25 children, and each with a university-qualified early childhood lead
educator, participated in the study. These two classes shared one outdoor space. In consultation
with the educators and children, eight children--four boys and four girls--were chosen as key
informants. This subset consisted of five three-year-olds and three four-year-olds; three were
from homes where a language other than English was spoken. For the research to proceed, it
was also important to select children who demonstrated willingness to articulate or draw their
points of view. Before commencing the study, ethical approval was granted by the Macquarie
University Human Research Ethics Committee. In accordance with this approval, pseudonyms
are used in this article and photos do not reveal children's identity.
Data generation involved observations, along with child-led photography, conversations and
drawing. The research activities in this study were designed to be part of the everyday
pedagogical activities in the classroom and were grounded in a desire to: a) avoid undue
Despite repeated reminders that I wanted to know what children thought were the most
interesting or special places at the centre, they were keen to include people in their
photographs, conversations and drawings. None did this more overtly than Layla who managed
to resist my prompting while we were on tour:
Jane: [emphasising the words 'place' and 'where'] Places. Where's that? Remember, you are
showing me the important places. Okay, so where's the next important place?
Layla: They have to be persons, I think.
When I later asked Layla to select three photos of the most important places, even though she
had taken some photos that did not have people in them, she chose one I had taken of her and
two of other children. It could be tempting, perhaps, to believe that a three-year-old may not
have a clear notion of 'place', however Layla demonstrated she indeed understood the notion in
this encounter:
Child: Take a picture of me!
Layla: No, I'm not taking a picture of you. I'm taking a picture of the places. Jane said.
Children did not directly make statements like, 'I like this place because I can play with others in
it'. Rather, this was inferred from their choices of place, other conversations, and observations.
For example, during the photo-elicitation, I noticed Thierry looking closely at a photo he had
taken of the swings. I asked him if he played on the swings and he replied:
Thierry: Yes I do. Look, one, two. So two people.
It may be that Thierry was demonstrating his counting prowess, or indeed, something
completely different. Nonetheless, I interpreted this as demonstrating his desire to be with
others, in the light of the other pieces of the mosaic, most particularly my observations on every
visit to the site of Thierry actively initiating and maintaining social interactions with both children
and adults.
On the tour, Travis frequently took several photos from a range of angles of his nominated
important places, but when it came to selecting one from his series of 'lighthouse' (the children's
name for two one-metre-high platforms) photos for inclusion in the documentation book, he was
definite about his choice:
Jane: The lighthouse--okay. Is that the best photo of the lighthouse that you took? I think there
are some over there as well.
Travis: This one with Ryan [Travis's friend].
Jane: That one with Ryan in it? What's special about that one?
Travis: Ryan is there. (Figure 1)
By choosing this photo, I inferred that this place was important for Travis for the potential it
afforded to be with his friends.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Often, as if to affirm their relationship, each child in the pair took photos of the same thing,
celebrating the act with an excited gesture or statement, frequently using the plural pronoun:
Jane: Sometimes when you are trying to take a photo you sometimes need to stand back from
it.
Travis: Maybe.
However, as Travis's demurral helped me realise, it was the children's intention to get in close--it
was the detail they were interested in.
[FIGURE 5 OMITTED]
On the other hand, the importance of having a vista was repeatedly reiterated and, of the four
most frequently photographed places (the bridge, the swings and the lighthouses), three
provided an opportunity to watch the goings-on of the outside area (Figure 6). Many photos
were taken of the view from these vantage points, often through railings and past other
obstacles. Tara said that the lighthouses were the best places to play at the centre:
outdoors, in this setting at least, offers places for socialising, pretending, observing and moving,
and these are much valued by the children. Other outdoor settings may not offer these
possibilities. Nonetheless, the study has implications for those who are responsible for
designing outdoor spaces and curriculum for young children and these will now be discussed.
Places for socialising
In UK and Scandinavian studies that have examined young children's perspectives about the
educational spaces they inhabit, places to be social have been identified as being important
(Clark, 2005, 2007a; Clark & Moss, 2001; Einarsdottir, 2005). In these studies, many of the
social spaces were outside. The importance of social spaces was also one of the major findings
in research undertaken in Australia by Fleet and Britt (2011) in their investigation of children's
memories of their first year of primary school, and, like the other studies, many of these social
spaces were also outdoors. The study reported here only investigated outdoor spaces;
therefore, it makes a further contribution through its finding that within an outdoor space, places
to be social are highly valued by the children.
For educational institutions, this is an important finding. Vygotsky (1978, p. 88) states that
'human learning presupposes a specific social nature and a process by which children grow into
the intellectual life of those around them'. Bruner (1986) too has proposed that learning is not
merely a solo pursuit, but is also a communal activity: 'It is not just that the child must make his
knowledge his own, but that he must make it his own in a community of those who share his
sense of belonging to a culture' (p. 127).
Given that the outdoor spaces are integral to early childhood educational settings, it would
therefore seem to be paramount that these environments enable the communal making of
knowledge. Outdoor spaces in such settings are not just places to let off steam or relax, they
are places for learning, so the need for these environments to facilitate learning is vital. In this
study, places for being social were intimate and partially enclosed. Enclosure may not be the
only way spaces can facilitate being social, but those responsible for designing outdoor spaces
need to consider ways for children to be together in outdoor spaces. This is not just a matter of
physical design but is also a consideration of curriculum, the provision of equipment and
materials, and pedagogical approaches.
vast number of other possibilities the outdoors offer (Maynard & Waters, 2007; Robertson,
2009). Nonetheless, the children in this study were constantly on the move and greatly valued
the opportunities to be physically active that this particular space afforded them. This may not
be the finding in other settings. Indeed, studies of US and European early learning settings have
found levels of physical activity are typically very low, while levels of sedentary behaviour are
typically high (Reilly, 2010). Simply having an outdoor space does not mean it facilitates
children's desire to move. Rather, it is the policies, practices, attitudes and culture of the setting
that determine the amount of physical activity that children undertake (Emilsen & Koch, 2010;
Moser & Martinsen, 2010; Pate, Pfeiffer, Trost, Ziegler & Dowda, 2004). In addition, the physical
features of the outdoor environment, such as the amount of space per child and the presence of
vegetation, appear to be important influences on physical activity (Trost, Ward & Senso, 2010).
Furthermore, the addition of loose unstructured materials increases playfulness (Bundy et al.,
2008), variety of activity (Moore & Wong, 1997), and activity levels (Bundy et al., 2009) among
children in the early school years.
Interdependence of themes
It is important to note that although the four themes of places to be social, pretend, observe and
move have been isolated here for the purposes of discussion, in reality they nearly always
coexisted. For example, moving involved pretending, pretending involved being social--and so
on. This example from my notes while 'on tour' illustrates this interdependence:
Travis: [said while running around the grassed area--me in pursuit] When I was playing with
Ryan and Layla we gotta some sprinkly things [plastic stepping 'stones'] and the monsters
would die. We're getting sprinkly things [demonstrates]. Scuse me, Lucy [educator], we're
getting all the sprinkly things so we can kill all the monsters.
Travis is being social, pretending, observing and moving all at the same time. Each is
dependent on the other, and speaking about each theme separately masks the complexity of
the way that the children were experiencing the outdoor space.
It is also important to note that this study was a single case in which eight children's
perspectives were sought. The themes that emerged are not finite and may have been
observed and interpreted differently by another researcher. Nonetheless, readers may be able
to draw parallels with other early childhood contexts and thus the findings from this study may
open a dialogue for possibilities in the outdoor spaces in other settings.
Conclusion
The Early Years Learning Framework (DEEWR, 2009) suggests outdoor environments are
integral to young children's learning environments; however, space can enhance or inhibit
children's competence by the way it stimulates their curiosity, skills, actions and communication
(Rinaldi, 2006). As children are the actual users of outdoor space in educational settings,
understanding what children think is important in these spaces and is vital if children's
competencies are to be enhanced rather than limited. This study, by seeking to give voice to
children's knowledge, insights and emotions regarding the outdoor space they encountered,
revealed that in this particular setting, children value the opportunity to move, pretend, observe,
and do these things in a social context. Such insights will be crucial to future decision making
regarding outdoor provision and use at this particular site, but they also offer a provocation for
those responsible for the design of curriculum and outdoor spaces at other learning settings for
young children.
[FIGURE 9 OMITTED]
References
Blanchet-Cohen, N., & Elliot, E. (2011). Young children and educators engagement and learning
outdoors: A basis for rights-based programming. Early Education & Development, 22(5), 757777. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2011.596460
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bundy, A. C., Luckett, T, Naughton, G. A., Tranter, P J., Wyver, S. R., Ragen, J., & Spies, G.
(2008). Playful interaction: Occupational therapy for all children on the school playground.
American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 62, 522-527.
Bundy, A. C., Luckett, T, Naughton, G. A., Tranter, P J., Wyver, S. R., Ragen, J., & Spies, G.
(2009). The risk is that there is 'no risk': A simple, innovative intervention to increase children's
activity levels. International Journal of Early Years Education, 17(1), 33-45. doi:
10.1080/09669760802699878
Burke, C. (2005). Play in focus: Children researching their own spaces and places for play.
Children,Youth, Environments, 15(1), 27-53.
Clark, A. (2005). Ways of seeing: Using the Mosaic approach to listen to young children. In A.
Clark, A. Kjorholt & P. Moss (Eds.), Beyond listening: Children's perspectives on early childhood
services (pp. 29-50). Bristol, UK: The Policy Press.
Clark, A. (2007a). Early childhood spaces: Involving young children and practitioners in the
design process. Working paper 45. The Hague: The Netherlands: Bernard van Leer Foundation.
Clark, A. (2007b). Views from inside the shed: Young children's perspectives of the
outdoorenvironment. Education 3-13, 35(4), 349-363.
Clark, A. (2010). Transforming children's spaces: Children's and adults' participation in
designing learning environments. London: Routledge.
Clark, A., & Moss, P (2001). Listening to young children: The Mosaic approach. London:
National Children's Bureau.
Clark, A., & Moss, P (2005). Spaces to play: More listening to young children using the Mosaic
approach. London: National Children's Bureau.
Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2008). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures for
developing grounded theory (3rd edn). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cosco, N. G., Moore, R. C., & Islam, M. Z. (2010). Behavior mapping: A method for linking
preschool physical activity and outdoor design. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise,
42(3), 513-519.
Cremin, H., & Slatter, B. (2004). Is it possible to access the 'voice' of pre-school children?
Results of a research project in a pre-school setting. Educational Studies, 30(4), 457-470. doi:
10.1080/0305569042000310363
Department of Education, Employment and Workplace Relations (DEEWR). (2009). Belonging,
being and becoming: The Early Years Learning Framework for Australia. Retrieved 12 January,
2015, from http://docs.education.gov.au/node/2632.
Edwards, C., Gandini, L., & Forman, G. (Eds.). (2012). The hundred languages of children: The
Reggio Emilia experience in transformation (3rd edn). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.
Einarsdottir, J. (2005). Playschool in pictures: Children's photographs as a research method.
Early Child Development and Care, 17(6), 523-541. doi: 10.1080/03004430500131320
Holmes, R. M., & Procaccino, J. K. (2009). Preschool children's outdoor play area preferences.
Early Child Development and Care, 179(8), 1103-1112.
James, A., & James, A. L. (2008). Key concepts in childhood studies. London: Sage
Publications.
Kehily, M. J. (Ed.). (2009). An introduction to childhood studies (2nd edn). Maidenhead,
England: Open University Press.
Lansdown, G. (2005). Can you hear me? The right of young children to participate in decisions
affecting them. The Hague: Bernard van Leer Foundation.
Little, H. (2010). Risk, challenge and safety in outdoor play: Pedagogical and regulatory tension.
Asia-Pacific Journal of Research in Early Childhood Education, 4(1), 3-24.
Lucas, A. J., & Dyment, J. E. (2010). Where do children choose to play on the school ground?
The influence of green design. Education 3-13, 38(2), 177-189.
Malaguzzi, L. (1993). For an education based on relationships. Young Children, 11 (1), 9-13.
Malone, K. (2008). Every experience matters: An evidence based research report on the role of
learning outside of the classroom for children's whole development from birth to eighteen years.
Wollongong: University of Wollongong.
Maynard, T., & Waters, J. (2007). Learning in the outdoor environment: A missed opportunity?
Early Years, 27(3), 255-265. doi: 10.1080/09575140701594400
Merewether, J., & Fleet, A. (2014). Seeking children's perspectives: a respectful layered
research approach. Early Child Development and Care, 184(6), 897-914. doi:
10.1080/03004430.2013.829821
Merriam, S. (1998). Qualitative research and case study applications in education. San
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Moore, R., & Wong, H. (1997). Natural learning: Creating environments to rediscover nature's
way of teaching. Berkeley, CA: MIG Communications.
Morrow, V., & Richards, M. (1996). The ethics of social research with children: An overview.
Children and Society, 10(2), 90-105.
Moser, T., & Martinsen, M. (2010). The outdoor environment in Norwegian kindergartens as
pedagogical space for toddlers' play, learning and development. European Early Childhood
Education Research Journal, 18(4), 457.
Nicol, R., Higgins, P., Ross, H., & Mannion, G. (2007). Outdoor education in Scotland: A
summary of recent research. Perth, Scotland: Scottish Natural Heritage & Learning and
Teaching Scotland.
Paley, V. G. (1990). The boy who would be a helicopter: The uses of storytelling in the
classroom. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
Paley, V. G. (2004a). A child's work: The importance of fantasy play. Chicago & London: The
University of Chicago Press.
Paley, V. G. (2004b). The classroom as narrative. Schools: Studies in Education, 1 (2), 63-74.
Pate, R. R., Pfeiffer, K. A., Trost, S. G., Ziegler, P., & Dowda, M. (2004). Physical activity among
children attending preschools.
Pediatrics, 114(5), 1258-1263. doi: 10.1542/peds.2003-1088-L Qvortrup, J., Corsaro, W. A., &
Honig, M. S. (Eds.). (2009). The Palgrave handbook of childhood studies. Basingstoke, UK:
Palgrave Macmillan.
Reilly, J. J. (2010). Low levels of objectively measured physical activity in preschoolers in child
care. Medicine and Science in Sports & Exercise, 42(3), 502-507. doi:
10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181cea100
Rickinson, M., Dillon, J., Teamey, K., Morris, M., Choi, M., Sanders, D., & Benefield, P (2004). A
review of research on outdoor learning. Shrewsbury, UK: National Foundation for Educational
Research and King's College London.
Rinaldi, C. (2006). In dialogue with Reggio Emilia: Listening, researching and learning. London:
Routledge.
Robertson, J. (2009). The gardenista: A place for pedagogy, a space of pedagogy. Paper
presented at the Unpacking change: People, places and pedagogy conference, Macquarie
University, Sydney.
Rogers, S., & Evans, J. (2007). Rethinking role play in the reception class. Educational
Research, 49(2), 153-167.
Smith, A. (2011). Respecting children's rights and agency: Theoretical insights into ethical
research procedures. In D. Harcourt, B. Perry & T. Waller (Eds.), Researching young children's
perspectives: Debating the ethics and dilemmas of educational research with children (pp. 1125). London & New York: Routledge.
Smith, A., Duncan, J., & Marshall, K. (2005). Children's perspectives on their learning: Exploring
methods. Early Child Development and Care, 175(6), 473-487.
Stake, R. E. (1995). The art of case study research. London: Sage.
Stephenson, A. (2002). Opening up the outdoors: Exploring the relationship between the indoor
and outdoor environments of a centre. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal,
10(1), 29-38. doi: 10.1080/13502930285208821
Stephenson, A. (2009). Horses in the sandpit: Photography, prolonged involvement and
'stepping back' as strategies for listening to children's voices. Early Child Development and
Care, 179(2), 131-141.
Storli, R., & Hagen, T. L. (2010). Affordances in outdoor environments and children's physically
active play in pre-school. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 18(4), 445456.
Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative research: Grounded theory procedures
and techniques. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Titman, W. (1994). Special people, special places: The hidden curriculum of school grounds.
Godalming, Surrey: World Wide Fund for Nature I Learning Through Landscapes Trust.
Trost, S. G., Ward, D. S., & Senso, M. (2010). Effects of child care policy and environment on
physical activity. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 42(3), 520-525.
United Nations (UN). (1989). The United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child.
Geneva: UNICEF.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Waite, S. (Ed.). (2011). Children learning outside the classroom: From birth to eleven. London:
Sage.
Waller, T, Sandseter, E. B. H., Wyver, S., Arlemalm-Hagser, E., & Maynard, T. (2010). The
dynamics of early childhood spaces: Opportunities for outdoor play? European Early Childhood
Education Research Journal, 18(4), 437-443.
Jane Merewether
Curtin University, Macquarie University
Merewether, Jane
Source Citation