What Really Causes Alzheimers by Harold Foster
What Really Causes Alzheimers by Harold Foster
What Really Causes Alzheimers by Harold Foster
HAROLD D. FOSTER
A free copy of this book is available at www.hdfoster.com. What really causes AIDS and What really causes schizophrenia also can be downloaded at this website.
i
ii
iii
AUTHORS NOTE
This book is written and published to provide information on Alzheimers disease. It is sold with the understanding that the publisher and author are not engaged in rendering legal, medical, or other professional services. In addition, this book is not to be used in the diagnosis of any medical condition. If expert assistance is desired or required, the services of a competent professional, especially one who is an expert in nutrition, should be sought. Every effort has been made to make this book as complete and accurate as possible. However, there may be mistakes both typographical and in content. Therefore, this text should be used as a general guide and not as the ultimate source of information. Factual matters can be checked by reading the cited literature. This book seeks to stimulate, educate, and entertain. The publisher and the author shall have neither liability nor responsibility to any entity or person with respect to any loss or damage caused, or alleged to be caused, directly or indirectly by the concepts or information contained in this book. Anyone not wishing to be bound by the above may return this volume for a refund of its purchase price.
iv
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The unusual cover of this book is based on the painting When what who why? by Keith Loreth. My thanks go to him for giving his permission to use this work of art for this purpose. I would also like to thank Dr. Abram Hoffer and Dr. Joseph Campbell with whom I have spent many pleasant lunches at the University Club discussing orthomolecular medicine in general and nutrition in particular. My gratitude is also expressed to several other people who assisted me in the preparation of this volume. Jill Jahansoozi typed the manuscript. Diane Braithwaite undertook the demanding task of typesetting, while cover design was in the expert hands of Ken Josephson. My wife, Sarah, helped proofread several drafts. Their dedication and hard work is acknowledged with thanks. Debt is also acknowledged to the professional staff at Trafford Publishing for their assistance with on-demand manufacturing and Internet marketing of this book.
There is a principle which is a bar against all information, which is proof against all argument, and which cannot fail to keep man in everlasting ignorance. That principle is condemnation without investigation.
Herbert Spencer (1820-1903)
vi
phosphohydrolase.3 As a result, it can cause rapid thinning of the myelin sheath4 and increase its susceptibility to oxidative stress.5 It seems very likely that these destructive processes are linked to demyelinization and so to associated retrogenesis. The APO E4 allele plays a key role in promoting Alzheimers disease because of the inefficiency with which those possessing this genetic aberration can remove brain beta-amyloid and tau.6 Genetically, however, there is more to Alzheimers disease than the APO E4 gene. To date, four genes have been identified as playing a role in either early- or late-onset Alzheimers disease: beta-amyloid precursor protein, presenilin-1, presenilin-2, and apolipoprotein E genes.7 Workers have linked most of these variants to familial early-onset Alzheimers, but the apolipoprotein E4 allele is a relatively common risk factor for developing late-onset Alzheimers disease.8 Considerable progress has been made in interpreting the significance of such genetic variants. To illustrate, mutations in the presenilin-1 gene seem associated with increased superoxide production and greater vulnerability to amyloid beta peptide toxicity.9 Interestingly, mutations in the presenilin genes, which are linked to more than 40 percent of all familial Alzheimers cases, cause enhanced production of an abnormal form of beta-amyloid precursor protein.10 This protein is longer than normal, aggregates more rapidly, kills neurons in culture more effectively, and precipitates preferentially to form amyloid plaques. The same elongated protein also is produced as a result of mutations in the gene encoding beta-amyloid precursor protein. The literature suggests, therefore, that the gene variants that predispose to both early- and late-onset Alzheimers disease do so because they either increase susceptibility to, or mimic, aluminum-related degenerative processes. That is, the genetic
viii
mutations involved in promoting the development of Alzheimers disease duplicate some of aluminums deleterious impacts on the brain and, in so doing, encourage at least one of the following: the growth of neuritic plaques or neurofibrillary tangles, excessive free radical formation, and/or higher neural oxidative stress. Consequently, unfortunate individuals carrying any one of the genetic variants are much more likely to develop Alzheimers disease, even if they are not exposed to the aluminum excess, or to the vitamin and mineral deficiencies, that are normally associated with its etiology. Alzheimers disease incidence appears to be rising faster than the population is aging. Obviously, such an increase cannot be due to any genetic cause. One does not have epidemics of genetic diseases, simply because the human genome does not change rapidly enough to trigger them. If, as the evidence strongly hints, Alzheimers disease is becoming generally more common, it must be because the harmful environments that trigger it are now more widespread. There is no doubt that, globally, soils and water are becoming more acidic and, consequently, aluminum more soluble. Throughout the 20th and early 21st centuries, as a result of expanding fossil fuel consumption, ever increasing quantities of sulphur and nitrogen were emitted into the atmosphere. Here they were converted into sulphuric and nitric acids, elevating the acidity of subsequent precipitation.11 Such acid rain has caused extensive damage to the environment at local, regional, and even global scales. It has been particularly problematic in northern and central Europe, eastern North America, and eastern China where it has been associated with many health costs.12 Simultaneously, commercial fertilizers have been used with increasing frequency. These consist predominantly of nitrogen, phosphorus, and potassium. As a consequence of heavy crop yields, agricultural soils have been depleted of several minerals that are important for human health, including calcium and magnesium.
ix
To illustrate, Marier and coworkers,13 in Water Hardness, Human Health and the Importance of Magnesium, have pointed out how this mineral is becoming less and less common in the food we eat because of the fertilizers used in the Green Revolution. Food processing and cooking also remove minerals from food, and packaging and canning often add aluminum to it. As a result, most of the populations of the Western World appear to be very magnesium and often calcium deficient.14-15 Simply put, drinking water is becoming more acidic, and so aluminum is more soluble, foodstuffs contain fewer minerals as the result of commercial fertilizers, and many of the remaining minerals are removed by processing and cooking. We are creating, therefore, harmful environments that allow aluminum to reach the human brain more easily, where it then inhibits numerous crucial enzymes. It is not surprising that the highest known Alzheimers disease mortality rates in the world occur in southern Norway.16 This is because the regions drinking water is being made highly acidic by polluted rainfall, lacks calcium and magnesium because of the local geology, and contains high levels of aluminum.17 From a scientific point of view, all these risk factors, with the exception of genetic inheritance, are relatively simple to mitigate. Alzheimers disease, in theory, therefore, is easy to avoid. There is no need for a pandemic, or the $100 billion annual loss that it causes in the USA alone. Theoretically, it should be a relatively simple matter to pass legislation reducing levels of aluminum in, and promoting the addition of calcium, magnesium, and perhaps silicic acid to, drinking water. It would seem to be in the best interest of every government to save the billions of dollars spent in caring for Alzheimers disease victims. Unfortunately, politics is rarely so logical. Not only do governments show little interest in increasing the magnesium content of drinking water, they routinely allow the use of aluminum sulfate as a flocculant by water treatment plants. This additive reduces
x
the amount of sediment in the water supply, but simultaneously greatly increases levels of dissolved aluminum, especially if the water is acidic.18 The western diet promotes Alzheimers disease in three distinct ways. Firstly, it tends to be deficient in calcium and magnesium,19-20 making those who eat it very susceptible to aluminum toxicity. Secondly, many foods are canned, wrapped, and/or cooked in aluminum. The more acid the food, the more easily it dissolves this metal. Thirdly, maltol is added to many processed foods in an attempt to improve flavour. This additive facilitates the passage of aluminum through the blood-brain barrier. There can be little doubt also that the typical western diet is too low in many minerals. Consider, for example, magnesium. This occurs at relatively high levels in unrefined whole grain cereals and in green leafy vegetables, nuts, seeds, lentils, beans, and peas.21 However, farmers do not routinely add magnesium to soils, so its levels are often relatively depleted in their crops. Since it is fairly soluble, food processing and cooking also often can greatly reduce magnesium levels in foods. To illustrate, the milling of whole grain lowers the magnesium content to only 20 percent of that initially present. Processing further reduces it, so that while one slice of whole wheat bread provides 24 milligrams of magnesium, a slice of white bread contains only 6 milligrams.22 For such reasons, dietary intakes of magnesium have been declining for at least 100 years in the USA, falling from about 500 mg to 175-225 mg per day. Fortunately, there is a great deal that individuals can do to reduce their chances of getting Alzheimers disease. For most of those reading this book, the average day will begin with a shower. If the water used is acidic and deficient in calcium and magnesium, it is possible that it will be a source of aluminum that enters the body through the pores and nose. This exposure to aluminum is more likely if the water supplier uses
xi
aluminum sulfate as a flocculant to remove sediment. Once dried off, most readers will smear their bodies with a layer of aluminum provided by antiperspirants and deodorants.23 How much of this aluminum passes through the skin into the body is unclear, but McGrath24 has argued that underarm shaving and frequent use of antiperspirants and deodorants appear to be linked to an early age of breast cancer diagnosis. British researchers25-26 have provided evidence to support the feasibility of McGraths hypothesis, reporting traces of parabens in every sample of tissue taken from 20 different breast tumours. Parabens are chemicals used in deodorants and other cosmetics that can mimic estrogen. The hormone estrogen is known to encourage breast tumour growth. Clearly, parabens can enter the body from deodorants and it is likely that aluminum can do the same. Deodorants with a herbal base do not usually contain these toxins. Then comes breakfast. Tea, coffee, hot chocolate are usually made with water from the tap. It is important not to use soft, acidic water which is likely to contain monomeric aluminum. Most water supply companies will provide chemical analyses, allowing the assessment of the aluminum, calcium, and magnesium content of their product. If not, private companies can conduct such analyses relatively cheaply. If colas or fruit juices are drunk, they are likely to have come from cans. These are typically made of aluminum. The longer the drink has been in the can, the higher the aluminum levels in it are likely to be.27 In addition to any aluminum it contains, hot chocolate is often enhanced with maltol, so increasing the likelihood that this metal will reach the brain. Similarly, tea brewed in acidic water or flavoured with lemon juice contains significantly higher levels of bioavailable aluminum than normal.28 After breakfast comes lunch, dinner, and a variety of snacks. Junk food, because it is so heavily processed, is usually a very
xii
poor source of minerals, such as calcium and magnesium. As previously pointed out, the average British and North American diet contains less than half the calcium and magnesium required to avoid the associated deficiency illnesses, including Alzheimers disease. The most effective way to address this problem is to eat many of the mineral enriched foods. These include salmon, sardines, broccoli, spinach, and bok choy, for example, which are all high in calcium.29-30 Pumpkin seeds, almonds, Brazil nuts, and whole grain brown rice are good sources of magnesium.31 Certain supplements, especially mineral ascorbates, also are excellent sources of both calcium and magnesium. Alacer Corporation, Foothill Ranch, California, a company with which I have no financial associations, provides excellent mineral ascorbate products. One tablet of Super-Gram II, for example, contains 4 percent of calcium and 8 percent of magnesium recommended daily allowance. Emergen-C is a fizzing drink mix that is pleasant to take when added to water. It provides 1,000 mg of vitamin C and 32 mineral complexes, including calcium and magnesium. Alacers products were used in the joint Committee on World Health and Russian research projects that produced a marked reversal of memory loss in the elderly.32-34
REFERENCES
1. Genetics and the Alzheimers Diseases. Chromosome 19 (AD2). http:// www.macalester.edu/~psych/whathap/UBNRP/alzheimer.chrom 19.html Foster, H.D. (2000). How aluminum causes Alzheimers disease: The implications for prevention and treatment of Fosters multiple antagonist hypothesis. Journal of Orthomolecular Medicine, 15(1), 21-51. Sarin, S., Gupta, V., and Gill, K.D. (1997). Alteration in lipid composition and neuronal injury in primates following chronic aluminum exposure. Biological Trace Element Research, 59(1-3), 133-143.
2.
3.
xiii
4.
Golub, M.S., and Tarara, R.P. (1999). Morphometric studies of myelination in the spinal cord of mice exposed developmentally to aluminum. Neurotoxicology, 20(6), 953-959. Verstraeten, S.V., Keen, C.L., Golub, M.S., and Oteiza, P.I. (1998). Membrane composition can influence the rate of Al3+-mediated lipid oxidation: Effect of galactolipids. Biochemical Journal, 333(Pt 3), 833-838. Genetics and the Alzheimers diseases chromosome, 19(AD2). http:/ /www. macalester.ed~psych/whathap/UBNRP/alzheimer/chrom19.html Velez-Pardo, C., Jimenez Del Rio, M., and Lopera, E. (1998). Familial Alzheimers disease: Oxidative stress, beta-amyloid, presenilins, and cell death. General Pharmacology, 31(5), 675-681. Khachaturian, Z.S. (1997). Plundered memories. The Sciences, 37(4), 20-25. Guo, Q., Sebastian, L., Sopher, B.L., Miller, M.W., Ware, C.B., Martin, G.M., and Mattson, M.P. (1999). Increased vulnerability of hippocampal neurons from presenilin-1 mutant knock-in mice to amyloid beta-peptide toxicity: Central roles of superoxide production and caspase activation. Journal of Neurochemistry, 72(3), 10191029.
5.
6. 7.
8. 9.
10. Haass, C., and Baumeister, R. (1998). What do we learn from a few familial Alzheimers disease cases? Journal of Neural Transmission. Supplementum, 54, 137-145. 11. Foster, H.D. (1994). Health and the physical environment: The challenge of global change. In M.V. Hayes, L.T. Foster, and H.D. Foster (Eds.), The determinants of population health: A critical assessment (pp. 73-120). Victoria, BC: University of Victoria. Department of Geography, Western Geographical Series Vol. 39. 12. Elsom, D.M. (1992). Atmospheric pollution: A global problem. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. 13. Marier, J.R., with Neri, L.C., and Anderson, T.W. (1979). Water hardness, human health, and the importance of magnesium. Ottawa: National Research Council of Canada. NRC Associate Committee on Scientific Criteria for Environmental Quality. NRCC No. 17581. 14. Peak Performance. Magnesium: Why magnesium matters to athletes. http://www.pponline.co.uk/encyc/magnesium.html 15. Marier et al., op.cit. 16. Vogt, T. (1986). Water quality and health. Study of a possible relationship between aluminum in drinking water and dementia (in Norwegian, English summary). Sosiale Og Okonomiske Studier, 61, Statistisk Oslo-Kongsvinger, 60-63.
xiv
17. Flaten, T.P. (1990). Geographical association between aluminum in drinking water and death rates with dementia (including Alzheimers disease), Parkinsons disease and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, in Norway. Environmental Geochemistry and Health, 12(1 & 2), 152-167. 18. Barnett, P.R., Skougstad, M.W., and Miller, K.J. (1969). Chemical characteristics of a public water supply. Journal, American Water Works Association, 61, 61-67. 19. Marier et al., op.cit. 20. Garland, C., and Garland, G., with Thro, E. (1989). The calcium connection. New York: Simon and Schuster Inc. 21. Peak Performance. Magnesium: Why magnesium matters to athletes. http//:www.pponline.co.uk/encyc/magnesium.html 22. Health and Nutrition Letter. For warding off diabetes mellitus. http:// www.healthletter.tufts.edu/issues/2004-06/magnesium.html 23. Mercola, J. (2003). Five common hygiene mistakes and how to avoid them. http:/ /www.mercola.com/2003/sep27/hygiene_mistakes.htm 24. McGrath, K.G. (2003). An earlier age of breast cancer diagnosis related to more frequent use of antiperspirants/deodorants and underarm shaving. European Journal of Cancer Prevention, 12(6), 479-485. 25. Darbre, P.D. (2003). Underarm cosmetics and breast cancer. Journal of Applied Toxicology, 23(2), 89-95. 26. Darbre, P.D., Alijarrah, A., Miller, W.R., Coldham, N.G., Saver, M.J., and Pope, G.S. (2004). Concentrations of parabens in human breast tumours. Journal of Applied Toxicology, 24(1), 5-13. 27. Abercrombie, D.E., and Fowler, R.C. (1977). Possible aluminum content of canned drinks. Toxicology and Industrial Health, 13(5), 649-654. 28. Flaten, T., and Odegard, M. (1988). Tea, aluminum and Alzheimers disease. Chemical Toxicology, 26, 959-960. 29. In-Depth Food - Calcium Facts. http://www.vegsource.com/nutrition/ explainers.calcium_facts.html 30. Garland et al., op.cit. 31. Peak Performance. Magnesium op.cit. 32. Bobkova, N.V., paper on The Impact of Mineral Ascorbates on Memory Loss presented at the III World Congress on Vitamin C, Committee for World Health, Victoria, BC, Canada, June 2001. 33. Galeev, A., Kazakova, A., Zherebker, E., Dana, E., and Dana, R. (nd). Mineral ascorbates improve memory and cognitive functions in older individuals with pre-Alzheimers symptoms. Copy of paper given
xv
to this author by R. Dana and E. Dana, Committee for World Health, 20331 Lake Forest Drive, Suite C-15, Lake Forest, California 92630, USA. 34. Bobkova, N.V., Nesterova, I.V., Dana, E., Nesterov, V.I., Aleksandrova, I.Iu., Medvinskaia, N.I., and Samokhia, A.N. (2003). Morpho-functional changes of neurons in temporal cortex in comparison with spatial memory in bulbectomized mice after treatment with minerals and ascorbates. Morfologiia, 123(3), 27-31 [in Russian].
xvi
TABLE OF CONTENTS
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Who Was I? ...................................................................... 1 In the Beginning: A History ............................................ 11 The Brain Drain: Pathology and Genetics ....................... 17 Putting the Disease in its Place ...................................... 25 Biochemical Abnormalities ............................................. 39 Other Risks, Further Hazards ........................................ 63 Pulling the Trigger: Location, Location, Location ............. 77 Checking Out Alternatives .............................................. 87 Conventional Medical Wisdom ........................................ 99 Back From the Abyss ................................................... 109 Understanding the System: Dementia .......................... 121 It Takes Two (or more) to Tango ................................... 137 A Barrier to Antagonism ............................................... 147 Other Enzymes - Further Problems .............................. 161 The Acid Test: Do the Pieces Fit? .................................. 209 Goodbye to the Long Goodbye ...................................... 231
xvii
Physicians, chemists, and theorists had by the early eighteenth century succeeded in making scurvy so complicated and obtuse that it would have been nearly impossible to get a useful or consistent diagnosis from a physician. The world of medicine was extraordinarily confused, with a great variety of physicians each offering his own personal variation on the humoral theory. To make universal sense of it is nearly impossible: indeed, the theories of many physicians were contradictory. With the layers of speculation building upon each other like the skins of an onion, and physicians tweaking their predecessors theories to accommodate glaring inconsistencies, scurvy and its causes and cures became ever more fanciful and bewilderingly disconnected from reality.
S.R. Brown, Scurvy, 2003
xviii
WHO WAS I?
LEAR:
Does any here know me? This is not Lear. Does Lear walk thus, speak thus? Where are his eyes? Either his notion weakens, his discernings Are lethargied Ha! Waking? Tis not so. Who is it that can tell me who I am? Lears shadow. William Shakespeare, King Lear
FOOL:
Life expectancies have risen dramatically over the past century. As a consequence, in both the Developing and Developed World, the number of elderly has undergone an unprecedented increase, with the proportion of the very old in the population doubling in one generation. Globally, in 1950 there were 214 million people aged 60 or over; by 2025 there probably will be one billion, a more than four-fold increase.1 Although, of course, there are major advantages associated with this trend, there are also serious costs. Not only are more people surviving into old age and, therefore, increasing their chances of developing dementia, but those who do so are living longer after its onset. In the USA, for example, Alzheimers disease incidence, currently some 4.5 million, is expected to increase by 350 percent by the mid-21st century, clustering in those states with the highest numbers of retired boomers. It is predicted, for example, that by 2025, 820,000 elderly Californians, 712,000 Florida residents, and 552,000 Texas will be suffering from Alzheimers disease. Other states with high projected patient numbers include New York, Pennsylvania, and Ohio, that
1
together are expected to have a further 1,088,000 Alzheimers cases.2 The high costs of caring for these millions of demented elderly may wreak havoc on the health care system. Gruenberg3 termed this paradox the failure of success because it was a major problem that was largely attributable to progress in medical care. As he and his colleagues4 pointed out, the old mans friend, pneumonia, is deada victim of medical progress. While this is an oversimplification, pneumonia is certainly less common than it used to be, as are many other diseases that were previously fatal to the elderly. As a consequence, 5 to 6 percent of the USA population now has Alzheimers disease or related dementia, some 4.5 million Americans. This figure is expected to rise to 14 million by 2050.5 Of course dementia is not limited to the USA. It has been estimated, for example, that as of the year 2000, approximately eight million people in the European Union Member States had Alzheimers disease. Since this disorder accounts for some 50 percent of all dementia in people over 65, total estimates for dementia in Europe are closer to 16 million. As in the USA, the European population is aging rapidly and the number of senile dementia cases increasing dramatically.6 Clearly, in the Western World, dementia is not a rare problem. Indeed, Katzman and colleagues7 have argued that, in those aged over 75, new cases of dementia occur as frequently as myocardial infarction and twice as often as stroke.
RETROGENESIS
Alzheimers disease is the most common form of dementia in the Developed World, where it seems to affect some 5 percent of those aged over 65. It is not, however, limited to the elderly, but is found also in a much smaller percentage of the younger
2
population.8-9 David Shenk,10 in his interesting book The Forgetting, describes Alzheimers disease as death by a thousand subtractions. The scientific evidence appears to support this characterization. In 1980, for example, Barry Reisberg,11-12 a neurologist from New York University, established the presence of an inverse relationship between the progressive stages of Alzheimers disease and those of infant and childhood development. He demonstrated that as the symptoms of this form of senile dementia worsen the patient begins to lose abilities in cognition, coordination, behaviour, language, and feeding, in the reverse order that they were acquired in the early years of life. In the final stage of the disorder, the patient becomes infant-like, and can no longer walk, sit up without assistance, smile, or hold up their head. Reisberg13 called this process of a thousand subtractions retrogenesis, meaning back to birth. Although retrogenesis is not a perfect reversal, neurological tests do show that, as Alzheimers disease progresses, there is an almost precise inverse relationship in neurologic reflexes, brain glucose metabolism, and EEG activity. As the disorder worsens, all these abilities decline. Such evidence led Reisberg to present a picture of the brain as a giant ball of string wound up in infancy and childhood but unwound by Alzheimers disease. From birth and throughout childhood and beyond the ball grows rapidly, but in Alzheimers it is unraveled in reverse, slowly but surely reducing the ability of the brain to function.14-15 Such a string analogy may not be far from the truth since researchers have found that Alzheimers degeneration is linked to brain demyelinization, that is, to the destruction of the white myelin sheath that insulates nerve axons and boosts their signal strength.16 This destruction does not occur randomly but in the reverse order in which myelin was laid down early in life. To illustrate, in 1939 J.L. Conel, a Boston neuropathologist, began to dissect the brains of deceased children of a variety of
3
ages. In this way, he was able to trace the normal process of myelinization, establishing that it begins first in the primary motor area which permits the infant gross movements of its hands, arms, upper trunk, and legs. Slightly later, myelinization of the primary sensory area neurons of the parietal lobe occurs, so allowing gross touch sensations. This process of myelinization continues throughout childhood and insulating myelin is laid down, for example, in the occipital lobe for visual acuity, and the temporal lobe for auditory processes. One of the very last brain structures to be covered with a layer of protective myelin is the hippocampus. This part of the brain is essential for consolidating immediate thoughts and impressions into longer-lasting memories that are then stored elsewhere. This late myelinization of the hippocampus explains why children younger than three can rarely recall permanent memories and why adults cannot remember their earliest life experiences. In Alzheimers disease, brain damage appears to begin in the most recently and least-myelinated area of the brain, specifically in the hippocampus. As a consequence, the first symptoms of developing Alzheimers are losses of recent memories. From the hippocampus, demyelinization begins to impact on the frontal cortex, adversely affecting concentration, abstract thought and planning ability. This reverse myelinization relentlessly continues, unwinding the ball of string in a very predictable manner until the primary motor area is finally affected and the late stage Alzheimers patient is again infantile, unable to speak, sit up unassisted, or hold up their own head.17
CONSEQUENCES
The only way to grasp the full meaning of Alzheimers disease is to listen to someone who is suffering from it. At 57, Thomas DeBaggio was diagnosed with Alzheimers disease.18 At first he
4
saw this as a death sentence. Tears welled up in my eyes uncontrollably; spasms of depression grabbed me by the throat. I was nearer to death than I anticipated. After a few days of reflection, he decided that some good might come out of the diagnosis. After 40 years of pussyfooting with words, I finally had a story of hell to tell. The end result of this courageous decision is his book Losing My Mind: An Intimate Look at Life with Alzheimers, which is filled with journal quotations that chronicle his descent into hell as he passes further into the Alzheimers abyss. In his own words: This is an unfinished story of a man dying in slow motion. It is filled with graffiti, sorrow, frustration, and short bursts of anger. While the narrator suffers his internal spears, he tries to surround himself with memories in a wan attempt to make sense of his life and give meaning to its shallow substance before he expires. Although incomplete, the story is full of sadness and missed opportunity, a lonely tale of the human condition. Behind it is hope, the tortured luck of a last chance.19 If you want to really learn what it is like to sit at the edge of failure and hope, read DeBaggios book.20 The process of reverse myelinization that is so characteristic of Alzheimers disease often destroys the life, not only of the affected patient, but also of those who love and care for them. This relationship has been described graphically by Shenk.21 The unique curse of Alzheimers is that it ravages several victims for every brain it infects. Since it shuts down the brain very slowly, beginning with higher functions, close friends and loved ones are forced not only to witness an excruciating fade but also increasingly to step in and compensate for lost abilities. We all rely on the assistance of other people in order to live full, rich lives. A person with dementia relies increasinglyand, in the fullness of time, completelyon the care of others.
5
This is no simple matter. The effects can be enormous and the term caregivers dementia is widely applied to describe the unfortunate symptoms, such as depression, fatigue, and forgetfulness, that accompany this thankless task. In the late 1990s, some 10 to 15 million Americans did their duty in such an unpaid role. Many were women who had just relinquished more conventional parenting when their children left the nest.22 The statistics suggest that this problem will only worsen.23 Currently, slightly more than half of Alzheimers patients receive home care.24 Until a cure is found, the total number of Alzheimers cases is likely to double by 2030 and triple by 2050. Using Census Bureau population estimates and the rates of Alzheimers incidence by age bracket, determined in the East Boston Studies (i.e., 3.0% for ages 65-74, 18.7% for ages 75-84, and 47.2% for those 85 and older), the U.S. may expect a tremendous increase in the burden of caring for those stricken by this mindrobbing ailment. The ratio of Alzheimers cases for every 100 persons of working age may nearly double, from 2.8 cases per 100 working age persons to 5.3 cases. Not only does Alzheimers disease frequently devastate the lives of the family and friends of patients, but it also has major societal economic repercussions. The cost each year of caring for one Alzheimers patient suffering from the early, mild symptoms of the disease is estimated at $18,408. This figure rises to $30,096 when the symptoms become moderate and climbs to $36,132 in the severest stage.25 As a consequence, the total direct and indirect annual costs of Alzheimers disease in the USA alone is roughly $100 billion. What does a figure like that really mean? I teach and conduct my research at a moderatesized university in Canada with a student body of about 15,000. Its annual budget is roughly 0.25 percent of that spent in the USA each year to watch the health of 4.5 million Alzheimers
6
patients deteriorate. That is, the USA spends much more on Alzheimers disease than Canada does on its entire system of higher education. Canada, of course, has problems of its own, with roughly 300,000 Alzheimers patients who cost $3.9 billion to care for in 1991. This figure is expected to rise to $12 billion by 2030.26 It would appear from this brief description that one of the keys to solving the Alzheimers jigsaw is an understanding of the demyelinization that accompanies retrogenesis. What is it that is driving the process and so progressively reversing the gains made by the brain in infancy and childhood? It is clear that the disease cannot simply be a form of accelerated aging because many Alzheimers patients with deteriorated cognitive ability still have excellent physical health.27 Similarly, it is not due to any hardening of the arteries, that is to cerebral arteriosclerosis, although this condition is more likely to be associated with stroke or multi-infarct dementia. For this reason, the use of hyperbaric oxygen chambers to treat Alzheimers disease has proven unsuccessful.28 While slow viruses and prions have been identified as the causes of some brain disorders, such as Kuru and variant CreutzfeldtJacob disease,29 none have been unequivocally isolated from the brains of former Alzheimers patients. In addition, there is no evidence that Alzheimers disease is infectious and so can be transmitted among humans. Similarly, with a few exceptions, there is little evidence to suggest that non-human primates can be infected with it.30 The distribution of Alzheimers disease is also very unlike that caused by an infectious agent, being both geographically extremely variable but relatively spatially constant over time.30 Similarities in the nature of the degeneration of the central nervous system occur in Alzheimers disease, Guamanian
7
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, and Parkinsonism with dementia. This suggests that all three illnesses may have a common pathogenic mechanism.32 Clues about which aspects of the environment may be acting as triggers have been provided by biopsies that reveal abnormally high levels of calcium, silicon, and aluminum in the central nervous systems in patients dying from all three disorders.33-35
SUMMARY
Although there is no evidence that autosomal dominant inherited mutated genes cause late-onset Alzheimers disease, it has been shown36 that this illness is more common in individuals who have inherited one or two copies of the apolipoprotein (APO E4) allele on chromosome 19. This gene directs the manufacture of the APO E protein that carries blood cholesterol throughout the body. In contrast, the APO E3 allele is the most common version seen in the general population, where it seems to play a neutral role in the development of Alzheimers disease.37 The fundamental problem with the slow and tedious search for the cause(s) of Alzheimers disease is that it is completely dominated by experts.38 That means that, as Shenk39 pointed out, the research is so intensely specialized that few individual scientists appear to even be working on the problem of Alzheimers disease per se. It was more like each was unearthing a single two-inch tile in a giant mosaic. By themselves, these individual experiments were so narrowly focused that they were far removed from a comprehensive understanding of the disease. What is really needed is not more detailed scientific research but a holistic attempt to put together the existing pieces of the Alzheimers mosaic, so that it becomes possible to forestall the impending wave of future Alzheimers patients and the enormous personal and social costs associated with it. This volume is an attempt to achieve that goal.
8
REFERENCES
1. 2. 3. 4. Henderson, A.S. (1986). The epidemiology of Alzheimers disease. British Medical Bulletin, 42(i), 3-10. PersonalMD. Alzheimers epidemic could bankrupt Medicare. http:// www.personalmd.com/news/n0322070411.shtml Gruenberg, E.M. (1977). The failures of success. Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly Health and Society, 55(1), 3-24. Gruenberg, E.M., Hagnell, O., Ojesjo, L., and Mittelman, M. (1976). The rising prevalence of chronic brain syndrome in the elderly. Paper presented at the Symposium on Society, Stress and Disease: Aging and Old Age, Stockholm, cited by Henderson, A.S., op. cit., p. 3. Neuroscience for kidsAlzheimers disease. http://faculty.washington. edu/chudler/alz.html European Institute of Womens Health. Dementia care. http://www. eurohealth.ie/remind/intro.htm Katzman, R., Aronson, M., Fuld, P., Kawas, C., Brown, T., Morgenstern, H., Frishman, W., Gidez, L., Elder, H., and Ooi, W.L. (1989). Development of dementing illnesses in an 80-year-old volunteer cohort. Annals of Neurology, 25(4), 317-324. Cohen, G.D. (1987). Alzheimers disease. In G.L. Maddox (ed.), The encyclopedia of aging (pp. 27-30). New York: Springer Verlag. Stone, J.H. (1987). Alzheimers disease and related disorders association. In G.L. Maddox (ed.), The encyclopedia of aging (p. 30). New York: Springer Verlag.
5. 6. 7.
8. 9.
10. Shenk, D. (2001). The Forgetting. Alzheimers: Portrait of an epidemic. New York: Doubleday. 11. Reisberg, B. (ed.) (1983). Alzheimers disease: The standard reference book. New York: Free Press. 12. Reisberg, B., Franssen, E.H., Hasan, S.M., Monteiro, I., Boksay, I., Souren, L.E., Kenowsky, S., Auer, S.R., Elahi, S., and Kluger, A. (1999). Retrogenesis: Clinical, physiologic and pathologic mechanisms in brain aging, Alzheimers and other dementing processes. European Archive of Psychiatry in Clinical Neurosciences, 249(3), 28-36. 13. Ibid. 14. Ibid. 15. Shenk, op. cit.
16. Ibid. 17. Ibid. 18. DeBaggio, T. (2003). Losing My Mind. An intimate look at life with Alzheimers. New York: The Free Press. 19. Ibid. 20. Ibid. 21. Ibid. 22. Ibid. 23. Kucewicz, W.P. (2000). Baby boomer retirement. GeoInvestor.com. http://www.geoinvestor.com/archives/fpgarchives/march2300.htm 24. ADEAR. Alzheimers disease Education and Referral Center. http:// www.alzheimers.org/pubs/prog00.htm 25. Leon, J., Cheng, C.K., and Neumann, P.J. (1998). Alzheimers disease care: Costs and potential savings. Health Affairs (Milwood), 17(6), 206-216. 26. Molloy, W., and Caldwell, P. (2003). Alzheimers disease. Toronto: Key Porter Books. 27. Cohen, G.D., op. cit. 28. Ibid 29. OBrien, C. (1996). Mad Cow Disease: Scant data cause widespread concern. Science, 271(5257), 1798. 30. Roos, R.P. (1981). Alzheimers disease and the lessons of transmissible virus dementia. In J.A. Mortimer and L.J. Schuman (eds.), The epidemiology of dementia (pp. 73-86). New York: Oxford University. 31. Foster, H.D. (2002). Why the preeminent risk factor in sporadic Alzheimers disease cannot be genetic. Medical Hypotheses, 59(1), 57-61. 32. Gajdusek, D.C. (1985). Hypothesis: Interference with axonal transport of neurofilament as a common pathogenetic mechanism in certain diseases of the central nervous system. The New England Journal of Medicine, 312(ii), 714-719. 33. Candy, J.M., Oakley, A.E., Klinowski, J., Carpenter, T.A., Perry, R.H., Atack, J.R., Perry, E.K., Blessed, G., Fairbairn, A., and Edwardson, J.A. (1986). Aluminosilicates and senile plaque formation in Alzheimers disease. Lancet, 1(8477), 354-357. 34. Fujita, T. (1987). Calcium and your health. Tokyo: Japan Publications.
10
35. Garruto, R.M., Swyt, C., Yanagihara, R., Fiori, C.E., and Gajdusek, D.C. (1986). Intraneuronal co-localization of silicon with calcium and aluminum in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and Parkinsonism with dementia in Guam. The New England Journal of Medicine, 315(ii), 711-712. 36. Strittmatter, W.J., Saunders, A.M., Schmechel, D., Pericak-Vance, M., Enghild, J., Salvesen, G.S., and Roses, A.D. (1993). Apolipoprotein E: High-avidity binding to beta-amyloid and increased frequency of type 4 allele in late-onset familial Alzheimers disease. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 90(5), 1977-1981. 37. ADEAR, op. cit., 10-11. 38. Horrobin, D.F. (2002). Evidence-based medicine and the need for noncommercial clinical research directed towards therapeutic innovation. Experimental Biology and Medicine, 227(7), 435-437. 39. Shenk, op. cit., 64-65.
11
Words ought to be a little wild for they are the assault of thoughts on the unthinking.
John Maynard Keynes (1883-1946)
12
I am going to tell the story of my life in an alphabet of ashes. Blas De Otero, Twenty Poems
In 1998, Neurogenetics1 reported that Manuel Graeber and his colleagues from the Max Planck Institute for Neurobiology in Martinsried, near Munich, had discovered over 250 slides made from samples of the brain of Auguste D. These slides had been housed for almost a century in a basement of the University of Munich. This find was big news. Researchers had been looking for Auguste D.s brain samples since 1996, when her original hospital file had turned up at an institute of the University of Frankfurt.2 Why was the discovery of tissue slides from a patient who died in 1906 so significant to Science?
follow the progression of Auguste D.s illness from a distance. As a consequence, when she died in April 1906, the director of the Frankfurt hospital sent her brain to Alzheimer, who used it to produce the recently rediscovered 250 sample slides. 4 Alzheimer studied these slides in detail, presenting his initial results at a psychiatry meeting in Tbingen 7 months later. An assessment of Auguste D.s case was subsequently published by him in 1907. These events ensured a place in medical history for both Auguste D. and Dr. Alois Alzheimer. Auguste D. is remembered as the first patient with documented Alzheimers disease, while her physicians name will be eternally linked to this terrible form of senile dementia.5 What was it about Auguste D.s brain samples that Alzheimer found so striking and unusual? He noticed something in the slides that was extremely raregum-like clumps outside some cells and abnormal collections of proteins inside others, that is plaques and tangles respectively. A fresh look at the recently rediscovered Auguste D. slides confirms Alzheimers claims. Her cortex displayed what are now accepted as the classic pathological signs of the disease named after him: amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. Indeed, neurofibrillary tangles were described for the first time ever in this brain.6
HISTORY OF DEMENTIA
It is clearly established, then, that Alzheimers disease existed in 1901. However, there are several significant questions about the history of this illness that should be asked, even though it may not be possible to answer them. For example, Was Auguste D. one of the first in a wave of patients suffering from a new disease, or had millions of previous cases (of what would from now on be called Alzheimers disease) been dismissed as simply age-related, that is senile dementia?
14
In 1901 there was certainly nothing new about dementia in the elderly. This condition had been known by the Greeks as morosis, and as oblivio and dementia by the Romans. In Middle English it was known as dotage, in French it was called dmence, and in 18th century English it was categorized as fatuity.7 The term senile dementia itself was first coined by the French psychiatrist Jean tienne Esquirol in 1838, who wrote: Senile dementia is established slowly. It commences with enfeeblement of memory, particularly the memory of recent impressions.8 Classical literature is also full of references to the elderly demented. The Roman poet Juvenal, for example, wrote in the 1st century AD: worse than any loss in body is the failing mind which forgets the names of slaves, and cannot recognize the face of the old friend who dined with him last night, nor those of the children whom he has begotten and brought up.9 Even earlier, in the 4 th century BC, the Greek historian Xenophon wrote in his Memorabiblia: haply [by chance] I may be forced to pay the old mans forfeitto become sand-blind and deaf and dull of wit, slower to learn, quicker to forget, outstripped now by those who were behind me.10 Even the Bible, in Ecclesiasticus chapter 3, verses 12-13 encourages the young to understand the mental decline often seen in the elderly with this advice: O son, help your father in his old age, and do not grieve him as long as he lives / even if he is lacking in understanding, show forbearance. There is no doubt, therefore, that dementia has been a curse of many of the elderly for millennia, providing fodder for authors as diverse as Euripides, Chaucer, Shakespeare, Chekhov, Trollope, Darwin, and Sir Walter Scott.11 What is not so obvious, however, is whether the senile dementia characterized by these and other scribes was actually Alzheimers disease. There are, for example, more than 70 known types of dementia.12
15
Even today, in some ethnic groups or at specific locations, Alzheimers disease might not be the most common form of this illness amongst the elderly. To illustrate, in Japan, consistently high incidence rates have been recorded for multiinfarct dementia, caused by many small strokes, while those for senile dementia of Alzheimers type appear considerably lower than in Europeans.13 Similarly, in China, there are marked regional variations in specific dementia prevalence. Vascular dementia, for example, predominates in Beijing, yet Alzheimers disease is the major form of dementia experienced in Shanghai.14
falciparum, which causes malaria, and Echinococcus granulosus, often linked to liver and lung disease. Trichinella, a parasite that causes trichinosis and which is usually contracted through eating pork or other infected meats, was also found to have been common in Ancient Egypt. Schistosoma haematobium, the parasite responsible for the current schistosomiasis pandemic, has been identified in two mummies.16-17 It must be admitted, however, that Egyptian dead are likely to be a very poor source of information about Alzheimers disease, since their brains were removed through their noses during the mummification process.18 Since the Egyptians thought intelligence arose from the heart, ancient embalmers typically threw away the brain. The Egyptians, of course, were not the first to practise mummification. The oldest mummies known are Chilean, created by the Chinchorro culture, predating their Egyptian counterparts by thousands of years. The Chiribaya, a pre-Columbian coastal people who lived in what is now the desert of southern Peru from 950 to 1350 AD, also mummified their dead. Beyond this, many mummies have been discovered in China and elsewhere.18 A PubMed search using two key words, disease and mummies, revealed 61 matching publications. These described everything from evidence of prehistoric tuberculosis in America19 and China20 to louse infection in the Chiribaya.21 A similar search based on Alzheimers and mummies found no matches. Similarly, in the indexes of two books on the topic, specifically Mummies, Disease and Ancient Cultures, edited by Thomas Cockburn and colleagues,22 and Disease , authored by Joyce Filer,23 no mention of Alzheimers disease was found. While none of this negative evidence establishes that the senile dementia recorded in the classical literature for millennia was not Alzheimers disease, the crucial physical evidence that it was appears to be missing. At best, the case remains unproven.
17
techniques, medical researchers must still rely on such problematic diagnosis by exclusion until the characteristic lesions of Alzheimers disease can be documented on pathological examination of the brain.26 Since so few autopsies are conducted on the elderly, it is very difficult to obtain accurate data capable of establishing variations in the incidence of Alzheimers disease over time. As a result, it is hard to determine how much, if any, the risk of developing the disease has changed in specific age groups since it was first recognized in the early 20th century by Alzheimer.27 This problem of diagnosis without autopsy does not mean that there have been few surveys of the demented. Indeed, in 1998, Ineichen28 identified over 100 epidemiological surveys of dementia published from a wide variety of countries. What can all this information tell us about the age-adjusted incidence of Alzheimers disease? Is it increasing? This is still a very difficult question to answer with any certainty. There can be no doubt, for example, that the age-adjusted mortality rates for Alzheimers disease have risen over the past decade in both Arizona29 and Missouri.30 However, how much this is the result of a true increase in incidence of the disease in particular age groups, and how much it simply reflects improved diagnosis, changes in public and physician attitudes towards Alzheimers disease, alterations in the coding and classification of dementia, and decreases in other leading causes of death is very unclear.31 Studies of temporal change in dementia incidence really require repeated surveys of the health of large populations. As a consequence, they tend to be rare because they are complex, costly, and involve extended fieldwork over long periods of time. The most robust such study comes from the Swedish Island of Lundby32 whose entire population of approximately 2,500 was examined several times between 1947 and 1972.
19
The established incidence rates for dementia on this island were 0.7 percent for men and 0.5 percent for women in their 70s, and 1.9 percent and 2.5 percent for males and females, respectively, aged 80 and over. However, much more interesting than the actual dementia incidence rates was the fact that these surveys established that such rates were lower in the second time period (1957-1972) than they had been in the first (19471957) for both genders and at all levels of dementia severity. Hagnell and colleagues32 attributed this decline to improved economic security which had brought with it more social activity and better diets. Nevertheless, more recent studies tend to hint that Alzheimers disease may be becoming a more common cause of death amongst specific age groups, that is that its age-adjusted annual mortality rate may be rising. In the USA,34 for example, Alzheimers disease was recorded as the underlying cause of death for 21,397 persons in 1996, contributing to the mortality of an additional 21,703. The age-adjusted death rate for this disease in the USA rose rapidly from 1979 to 1988, changed little from 1988 to 1992, and increased again from 1992 to 1995. It then appeared to level off once more. In 1999, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention35 attributed 44,509 deaths to Alzheimers disease in the USA, but much of this increase stemmed from abolition of the term presenile dementia as a possible cause of death. Nevertheless, the most recent information from the US Department of Health and Human Services36 claims a 5 percent increase in the age-adjusted rate of Alzheimers disease death for the time period 2000 to 2001, suggesting that this disease may be increasing in specific age groups. While it is undoubtedly true that a heightened awareness may have encouraged USA physicians to diagnose cognitive impairment as Alzheimers disease more often in 1996 than in 1979,
20
USA age-adjusted mortality rates for senile and presenile dementias also rose rapidly at the same time. The associated decline in deaths recorded as due to senility could not totally account for these trends. In conclusion, it is unclear how much of the recent increase in USA age-adjusted Alzheimers disease death rates have been the result of methodological changes and how much they have been caused by real growth in the prevalence of this illness. However, the apparent increase in mortality from Alzheimers disease at specific ages recorded in the USA37 is consistent with very similar trends seen in Australia,38 Canada,39 England,40 and Norway41 where Alzheimers disease death rates also appear to have risen. The best present answer that can be given truthfully to the question, Is an 80 year old man or woman in the USA or Europe more likely to develop Alzheimers disease now than they would have been in 1907 when Alois Alzheimer first identified the disease? is Probably.
SUMMARY
Studies of the history of Alzheimers disease are bedevilled by problems of identification. Although a wide range of high and low technology42-43 has been developed to aid in its diagnosis, the most definitive way of establishing the presence of this disorder is still that which was first pointed out by Alois Alzheimer extensive brain deposits of amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. As a consequence of this difficulty with diagnosis, and since there are some 70 other types of dementia, it is very hard to know whether historical references to senile dementia really referred to Alzheimers disease. There is no evidence from mummies, known to this author, that suggests that they did. It is almost as difficult to establish whether age-adjusted incidence and mortality have been rising since 1907, when
21
Alzheimer first identified this form of dementia. This is due to the impact of improved diagnosis, changes in public and physician education about and attitudes towards Alzheimers disease, alterations in the coding and classification of dementia, and decreases in other significant causes of death. As a result, while the prevalence of Alzheimers disease is clearly rising rapidly as the population ages, it is not certain whether the probability of a specific individual developing this form of dementia, at any given age, is also increasing.
REFERENCES
1. Graeber, M.B., Kosel, S., Grasbon-Frodl, E., Moller, H.J., and Mehraein, P. (1998). Histopathology and APOE genotype of the first Alzheimer disease patient, Auguste D. Neurogenetics, 1(3), 223-228. Maurer, K., Volk, S., and Gerbaldo, H. (1997). Auguste D. and Alzheimers disease. Lancet, 349(9064), 1546-1549. Enserink, M. (1998). First Alzheimers diagnosis confirmed. Science, 279(5359), 2037. Graeber et al., op. cit. Enserink, op. cit. Graeber et al., op. cit. Shenk, D. (2001). The Forgetting. Alzheimers: Portrait of an epidemic. New York: Doubleday. Ibid. Ibid.
2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9.
10. Ibid. 11. Ibid. 12. Office of Technology Assessment, Congress of the United States (1987). Losing a million minds: Confronting the tragedy of Alzheimers disease and other dementias. OTA-BA-323. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office. 13. WHO Scientific Group on Senile Dementia (1986). Dementia in later life: Research and action. Technical Report Series 730. Geneva: World Health Organization.
22
14. Chiu, H.F.K., and Zhang, M. (2000). Dementia research in China. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15, 947-953. 15. David, A.R. (1997). Disease in Egyptian mummies: The contribution of new technologies. Lancet, 349(9067), 1760-1763. 16. Ibid. 17. Doctors Guide. Global Edition (1995). Unlocking mummies secrets to study todays diseases. http://www.pslgroup.com/dg/2c6ae.htm 18. Mummies: Unwrapped: The mysterious world of mummies. http:// www.tvdailyonline.com/tvguide/announce/coming10.htm 19. Gomez i Prat, J., and de Souza, S.M. (2003). Preehistoric tuberculosis in America: Adding comments to a literature review. Memorias do Instituto Oswaldo Cruz, 98 Suppl 1, 151-159. 20. Fusegawa, H., Wang, B.H., Sakurai, K., Nagasawa, K., Okauchi, M., and Nagakura, K. (2002). Outbreak of tuberculosis in a 2000-yearold Chinese population. Kansenshogaku Zasshi, 77(3), 146-149. 21. Reinhard, K.J., and Buikstra, J. (2003). Louse infection of the Chiribaya culture, Southern Peru: Variation in prevalence by age and sex. Memorias do Instituto Oswaldo Cruz, 98 Suppl 1, 173-179. 22. Cockburn, T.A., Cockburn, E., and Reyman, T.A. (eds.) (1998). Mummies, disease and ancient cultures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 23. Filer, J. (1995). Disease. London: Egyptian Bookshelf, British Museum Press. 24. Foster, H.D. (2002). Why the preeminent risk factor in sporadic Alzheimers disease cannot be genetic. Medical Hypotheses, 59(1), 57-61. 25. Office of Technology Assessment, op. cit. 26. Schoenberg, B.S. (1981). Methodologic approaches to the epidemiologic study of dementia. In J.A. Mortimer and L.M. Schuman (eds.), The epidemiology of dementia: Monographs in epidemiology and biostatistics (pp. 117-131). New York: Oxford University Press. 27. Fotuhi, M. (2003). The memory cure. New York: McGraw-Hill. 28. Ineichen, B. (1998). The geography of dementia: An approach through epidemiology. Health & Place, 4(4), 383-394. 29. Arizona Department of Health Services. Public Health Services. Mortality from Alzheimers disease Among Arizona Residents, 1990-2000. http://www.hs.state.az.us/plan/report/mfad/mfad00/figures00. htm
23
30. Missouri Department of Health Center for Health Information Management & Epidemiology (1998). Missouri Monthly Vital Statistics, Provisional Statistics, 32(8). Jefferson City: Missouri. http://www. health.state.mo.us/MonthlyVitalStatistics/oct98vol32no8.pdf 31. Arizona Department of Health Services, op. cit. 32. Hagnell, O., Lanke, J., Rorsman, B., and Ojesjo, L. (1981). Does the incidence of age psychoses increase? Neuropsychobiology, 7, 201-211. 33. Ibid. 34. Centers for Disease Control. Mortality from Alzheimers disease - United States, 1979-1987 (1991). Journal of the American Medical Association, 265(3), 313-317. 35. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2001). Mortality declines for several leading causes of death in 1999. Media Relations Press Release (404). http://www.organicconsumers.org/madcow/ mortality62601.cfm 36. National Center for Health Statistics (2003). HHS News. HHS study finds life expectancy in the US rose to 77.1 years in 2001. http:// www.cdc.gov/nchs/releases/03news/lifeex.htm 37. Centers for Disease Control (1991), op. cit. 38. Jorm, A.F., Henderson, A.S., and Jacomb, P.A. (1989). Regional differences in mortality from dementia in Australian analysis of death certificate data. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 79, 179-185. 39. Neuman, S.C., and Bland, R.C. (1987). Canadian trends in mortality from mental disorders, 1965-1983. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 76, 1-7. 40. Martyn, C.N., and Pippard, E.C. (1988). Usefulness of mortality data in determining the geography and time trends in dementia. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 42, 134-137. 41. Flaten, T.P. (1989). Mortality from dementia in Norway, 1969-1983. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 43, 285-289. 42. Shenk, op. cit. 43. Fotuhi, op. cit.
24
Oh God, that men should put an enemy in their mouths to steal away their brains! William Shakespeare, Othello
Alzheimers disease, when amyloid precursor protein breaks down it creates a disproportionate amount of beta-amyloid but less alpha-amyloid and gamma-amyloid protein than usual. This excess of beta-amyloid protein overwhelms the brains capacity to remove it and so it accumulates as insoluble gumlike plaques,5 which may also contain other molecules, neurons, and non-nerve cells.6 Such plaques gum up the works by damaging the connection points, that is the synapses, between neurons and, as a consequence, interfere with such cells ability to communicate. In Alzheimers disease, early plaques develop in the hippocampus, a brain structure involved in encoding memories, and also in other parts of the cerebral cortex that are necessary for thought and decision making. As the disease progresses, additional plaques form in the frontal lobes of the brain. The more severe the symptoms of Alzheimers disease, the more plaques will typically be found in the patients brain during autopsy.7 Such beta-amyloid plaques also trigger an inflammatory response. Part of this process involves the creation of oxygen free radicalshighly reactive molecules which can damage or kill other cells by creating holes in their membranes or binding to their DNA and interfering with survival. This plaque-related inflammatory process appears to destroy large numbers of brain cells in Alzheimers patients and its effects are obvious in stained brain sections.8 This could be why taking anti-inflammatory drugs, for other health problems, may accidentally reduce the probability of developing Alzheimers disease. Healthy neurons have a support structure that is made up, in part, of microtubules that act like tracks, guiding nutrients and other molecules from the cell body to the ends of the axon and back again.10 The stability of these microtubules is maintained by a type of protein known as tau. In patients with Alzheimers disease, tau becomes chemically abnormal and begins to pair
26
with other threads of tau and become tangled. As this happens, neuron microtubules disintegrate. These tangles prevent the movement of nutrients and other molecules to the nerve endings of the neurons, and as a result, communication malfunctions can occur, often followed by cell death. Tangles initially interfere with the functions of the brains temporal lobe, causing memory loss and difficulties in reading and writing. As plaques and tangles begin to appear in the frontal lobes, personality disorders and other symptoms appear. While tangles can be seen also in the brains of healthy older people, they are relatively rare. In Alzheimers patients, the worse the symptoms, the more common tangles are usually found to be on brain autopsy.11
of neurons and their associated neurites, short protrusions that are used to communicate with neighbouring nerve cells. The grey matter then can be viewed as the brains central processor. Neurons, however, also send information through the brain to the central nervous system by transmitting electrical signals that travel over long appendages termed axons. These axons are covered by myelin, an insulating sheath of fat that speeds signal transmission. The brains white matter is largely composed of such myelin.13 Interestingly, because memory loss is so typical of Alzheimers disease, most research has focussed on the grey matter of the brain, especially of plaques and tangles. However, it is becoming evident that in Alzheimers disease myelin proteins, lipids, and cholesterol are significantly reduced.14-16 This loss is termed demyelinization and appears to play a significant role in retrogenesis, the decline of the patients abilities in the reverse order to which they were developed during childhood and infancy.17
GENE DREAMING
It is obvious that Alzheimers disease involves a complex biochemical collapse of the brain that appears to proceed with a strange, perverted logic. Abilities are lost in the reverse order to which they were acquired, as plaques and tangles damage the grey matter, myelin degrades in the white matter, and acetylcholine-producing neurons are killed at the base of the brain. This destruction is accompanied by tissue shrinkage, known technically as progressive cerebral atrophy, a process that can be monitored using computer-enhanced volumetric magnetic resonance imaging scans.18 Undoubtedly, the key question to be asked is What is the root cause of this brain disintegration?
28
The most extreme of the geneticists believe that the misery of Alzheimers disease is all preordained; that is, it has a simple Mendelian causality. If you have the wrong genetic aberration, you will develop the disease, unless you die earlier in life from some other cause.19 This is the position taken by Rudolph E. Tanzi, director of the Genetics and Aging Research Unit at the Massachusetts General Hospital.20 Based on recent findings of 12 new potential sites for Alzheimers genes, he claims that within 50 years, patients will be routinely screened for these genes, and be given prescription drugs tailored to reduce their identified genetic risk. Speaking at the 2003 annual meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science (AAAS), held in Denver, Colorado, Tanzi said: The genetic underpinnings for up to 70 percent of Alzheimers cases remain unsolved. This research lays the ground work for identifying genes that will allow us to reliably predict the disease before it strikes, giving us new clues about biological causes of disease so that we can help prevent it. The ultimate goal is to custom-make drugs to address our own genetic properties. Our laboratorys goal of early prediction-early prevention of this insidious neurological disorder will likely emerge in the future as the preferred means for treating cancers, diabetes, heart disease, and other common yet complex genetic disorders that challenge the health of our elderly population. Like Alzheimers, all of these disorders involve, on one hand, rare gene mutations that cause early onset forms of the disease and, on the other hand, common gene variants that increase susceptibility to these diseases as we age.21 So far, however, the evidence that tangles, plaques, myelin degradation, and the other pathologic symptoms of Alzheimers disease have a simple Mendelian causality is relatively poor.
29
In total, four genes have been identified that appear to play some role in Alzheimers disease. Three of these have been linked to the relatively rare early-onset type, while the fourth seems to increase sporadic Alzheimers disease risk in people as they age.22 More specifically, only a small minority of patients, between 1 and 7 percent, suffer from the early-onset form of Alzheimers disease.23 Those who do can begin to show symptoms as early as 30 years of age. Early-onset Alzheimers disease appears to have links to specific rare genetic aberrations that occur on chromosome 1, 14, and 21. However, the great majority of Alzheimers disease patients do not begin to display symptoms until they reach the age of 65 and suffer from the much more common, that is the sporadic, type of the disorder.24-25 This form of Alzheimers disease seems linked, to some degree, to a gene in chromosome 19 called the APO E gene, which codes for apolipoprotein E. This protein is involved in the cellular movement of cholesterol throughout the body. There are three slightly different types (known as alleles) of the APO E gene, namely APO E2, APO E3, and APO E4. Everybody has inherited two copies of this gene, one from each parent. The E3 variant is the most common and occurs in between 40 and 90 percent of the populations of particular regions; E2 and E4 are less common, being present in 2 percent and 6 to 37 percent of people respectively.26 It has been demonstrated that the probability of developing sporadic late-onset Alzheimers disease is much higher in those possessing the EPO E4 allele. Indeed, anyone who has inherited copies of the APO E4 alelle from both parents has a 15 times greater risk of developing sporadic Alzheimers disease than someone without this form of the APO E gene.27 Consequently, in Alzheimers patients, carriers of the APO E4 are common, with this allele being present in approximately 40 percent.28
30
SUMMARY
Major pathologic changes occur slowly but surely, often over a decade or more, in the brains of Alzheimers patients. Plaques and tangles form in the grey matter, myelin degenerates in the brains white matter, and acetylcholine-producing neurons are killed at its base. How much of this destruction is genetically controlled is as yet unclear, but as far as the mitigation of the Alzheimers disease pandemic is concerned, genetics has so far promised a great deal more than it has delivered.
REFERENCES
1. 2. Enserink, M. (1998). First Alzheimers diagnosis confirmed. Science, 279(5359), 2037. ADEAR. Alzheimers disease Education and Referral Center. Alzheimers disease Unravelling the Mystery. Plaques and Tangles: The Hallmarks of Alzheimers disease. http://www.alzheimers.org/ unraveling/06.htm Ibid. Fotuhi, M. (2003). The memory cure. New York: McGraw-Hill. Ibid. ADEAR. op. cit. Fotuhi, op.cit. Ibid. McGeer, P.L., and McGeer, E.G. (2002). Innate immunity, local inflammation, and degenerative disease. Science of Aging Knowledge Environment [electronic source]: SAGE KE, 2002(29), re 3.
3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9.
10. ADEAR, op. cit. 11. Fotuhi, op. cit. 12. Iraizoz, I., Guijarro, J.L., Gonzalo, L.M., and de Lacalle, S. (1999). Neuropathological changes in the nucleus basalis correlate with clinical measures of dementia. Acta Neuropathologica (Berl) 98(2), 186-196.
31
13. American Chemical Society. Press release, 18 September 2002. Alzheimers disease may originate in the brains white matter. http:// www.eurekalert.org/pub_releases/2002-09/acs-adm091802.php 14. Ibid. 15. Wallin, A., Gottfies, C.G., Karlsson, I., and Svennerholm, L. (1989). Decreased myelin lipids in Alzheimers disease and vascular dementia. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 80(4), 319-323. 16. Roher, A.E., Weiss, N., Kokjohn, T.A., Kuo, Y.M., Kalback, W., Anthony, J., Watson, D., Luehrs, D.C., Sue, L., Walker, D., Emmerling, M., Goux, W., and Beach, T. (2002). Increased A beta peptides and reduced cholesterol and myelin proteins characterize white matter degeneration in Alzheimers disease. Biochemistry, 41(37), 1108011090. 17. Shenk, D. (2001). The Forgetting: Alzheimers disease: Portrait of an epidemic. New York: Doubleday. 18. Scahill, R.I., Schott, J.M., Stevens, J.M., Rossor, M.N., and Fox, N.C. (2002). Mapping the evolution of regional atrophy in Alzheimers disease: Unbiased analysis of fluid-registered serial MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 99(7), 41354137. 19. Dr. George Perry, professor of Pathology and Neurosciences, Case Western Reserve University, Institute of Pathology, Cleveland, Ohio. Personal communication, 14 November, 2002. Dr. Perry does not believe that Alzheimers disease has a simple Mendelian causality. 20. American Association for the Advancement of Science. Public Press Release 14 February 2003. New potential sites for Alzheimers genes suggest a future of custom-designed treatment. http://www. eurekalert.org/pub_releases/2003-02/aaft-nps020303.php 21. Ibid. 22. Ibid. 23. Ingram, V. (2003). Alzheimers disease. American Scientist, 91(4), 312321. 24. Cutler, N.R., and Sramek, J.J. (1996). Understanding Alzheimers disease. Jackson, Mississippi: University Press of Mississippi. 25. St. George-Hyslop, P.H. (2000) Piecing together Alzheimers. Scientific American, 283(6), 76-83. 26. Ibid. 27. Cutler et al., op. cit. 28. Ibid.
32
An invasion of armies can be resisted, but not an idea whose time has come. Victor Hugo, Histoire dun crime (1852)
For millennia, it has been recognized that patterns of human and indeed animal disease generally reflect the nature of both the environment and of behaviour. In his book On Airs, Waters and Places, the ancient Greek physician Hippocrates,1 for example, wrote: Whoever wishes to investigate medicine properly, should proceed thus: in the first place to consider the seasons of the year, and what effects each of them produces... Then the winds, the hot and the cold, especially such as are common to all countries, and then such as are peculiar to each locality. We must also consider the qualities. In the same manner, when one comes into a city to which he is a stranger, he ought to consider its situation, how it lies as to the winds and the rising of the sun: for its influence is not the same whether it lies to the north or to the south, to the rising or to the setting sun. These things one ought to consider most attentively, and concerning the water which the inhabitants use, whether they be marshy and soft, or hard, and running from elevated and rocky situations, and then if saltish and unfit for cooking, and the ground, whether it be naked and deficient in water, or wooded and well watered, and whether it lies in a
33
hollow, confined situation, or is elevated and cold: and the mode in which the inhabitants live, and what are their pursuits, whether they are fond of drinking and eating to excess, and given to indolence, or are fond of exercise and labour, and not given to excess in eating and drinking.
smoking, over-consumption of alcohol and food, lack of hygiene and failure to exercise.8 Given the multiple threats to health from certain aspects of human biology, environment, and lifestyle, it is hardly surprising that every society develops some provisions for health care. Obviously, choice of the adopted medical system(s) has a major impact on which diseases are considered curable and which are not. As a result, medical care is also reflected in geographical patterns of death and disease. As a geographer I am very biased. It is my belief that until we can convincingly establish why a particular disease is common in some regions and very rare or absent from others, we have achieved very little of intellectual significance. This chapter, then, examines variations in the global and regional patterns of Alzheimers disease and attempts to identify which aspects of human biology, environment, lifestyle, and medical care are responsible for them. That is, it is a review of what medical geographers and epidemiologists can tell us about this form of dementia. If the development of sporadic (late-onset) Alzheimers disease was largely a consequence of genetic inheritance, then two geographic corollaries follow.9 Firstly, since the APO E4 form of the gene (which is thought to predispose to sporadic Alzheimers disease) is apparently already very widely distributed throughout the entire human population, this form of dementia ought to display relatively similar, random patterns of ageadjusted mortality. Incidence and prevalence of Alzheimers disease, in contrast, would be expected to vary with global differences in life expectancy. That is, if a widely distributed genetic aberration were the major cause of Alzheimers disease, this form of dementia would occur with roughly the same frequency everywhere. However, cases would peak in those areas with disproportionately high populations of elderly.
35
Secondly, if a disease is the result of a globally dispersed genetic characteristic, migration should not cause a significant alteration to its incidence or mortality. This is because the dominant risk factor(s) would be internal. What this means, for example, is that amongst the same age groups, Alzheimers disease ought to be as common in Japan as it is in AmericanJapanese living in the USA. These two corollaries allow the current belief that the major risk factor(s) in Alzheimers disease are genetic to be tested using geographical evidence. This goal can be achieved by comparing global and regional spatial patterns of Alzheimers disease incidence and mortality with those that one would expect to see if the disease were largely genetic in origin. It must follow that if the geography of Alzheimers disease is very like that implied by the genetic hypothesis, then there is a strong probability that the genetic model is correct. Naturally, the reverse is also true. If implied and real geographies greatly differ, it is impossible for the genetic hypothesis for sporadic Alzheimers disease to be valid.10
reported more commonly from Russia, Japan and China, and in atypical surveys in North America. While Ineichen is not sure whether these geographical differences really exist, it seems likely that they do, but may be altering with changing diets and pollutant levels. The reality of such global geographical variations in the incidence and prevalence of both Alzheimers disease and multi-infarct dementia (vascular dementia) have been established, for example, by Chinese dementia researchers who have shown that multi-infarct is the dominant form of dementia in Beijing, while Alzheimers disease is the more common dementia in Shanghai.14-15 Such regional variations in the incidence of Alzheimers disease are not limited to China. In a rare whole country study, Sulkara and coworkers,16 for example, established that Alzheimers disease was significantly more prevalent in the north and east of Finland than elsewhere in the country. The reasons for this pattern were unknown. Clearly, there are great variations in the incidence and prevalence of Alzheimers disease at the local level. This form of dementia, for example, appears particularly rare in Maracaibo, Venezuela. Molina and colleagues,17 for example, have described the results of autopsies conducted in four large hospitals in that city on brains that included those of all known dementia patients. In the first of these studies covering a 10 year period, 3,657 successive autopsies revealed that 86.7 percent of such patients had suffered from vascular dementia and the rest from other forms of dementia which did not include Alzheimers disease. Six years later, a further 611 adult brains had been autopsied, including all of the 39 dementia patients who had died in any of the four hospitals during the period 1992 to 1998. Once again, vascular dementia (84.6 percent) had dominated; 5 (12.8 percent) showed evidence of some other form of dementia, but not Alzheimers disease. Only one brain (2.6 percent) displayed plaques and tangles, that is the diagnostic form of damage seen in Alzheimers disease. Taken together,
37
these two hospital-based studies cover over 15 years of autopsied dementia deaths in Maracaibo, a city that has a population of some 650,000. Indeed, these hospitals are used by many Venezuelans living outside the urban limits and service roughly 1,200,000 people. High quality autopsy evidence, therefore, suggests that the annual age-adjusted Alzheimers disease mortality rate in this part of Venezuela is probably much less than 1 per 1 million population.18 From a geographical viewpoint, Maracaibo is clearly the key to halting the worldwide Alzheimers disease pandemic since it is a city where this form of dementia is virtually nonexistent. In contrast to the dearth of Alzheimers disease patients found in Maracaibo, the highest rates of this type of dementia appear to occur in parts of Norway. Excellent death certificate data is recorded in this country, with up to three contributory causes of death being coded, in addition to the major underlying cause. This data bank allowed Flaten19-20 to calculate mean annual Norwegian age-adjusted dementia death rates per 100,000, for both males and females, for the 10 year period 1974 to 1983. That is, he worked out how many demented Norwegian people died in both gender groups over a decade. This calculation was completed for 193 Norwegian municipality aggregates, designed so that each statistical unit had a population of at least 10,000 inhabitants. From many autopsies it was known that approximately 60 to 75 percent of such Norwegians dying of dementia suffered from Alzheimers disease. It is clear, then, that Flatens illustrations of dementia21 in Norway were dominated by the spatial distribution of Alzheimers disease. Interestingly, although the maps illustrating male and female death rates with dementia were similar, both showed very marked regional variations. In both genders, dementia was a much more frequent cause of mortality along the south and southeastern coasts, while northern Norway and the inland areas of southern Norway generally experienced relatively low dementia death rates.
38
These differences in the spatial distribution of mortality were illustrated by the male annual age-adjusted dementia death rate per 100,000. This had a mean value of 25 for Norway as a whole, but varied at the municipal scale from approximately 5 to 74; in females the median rate was higher at 39, and ranged from 8 to 145. Given that autopsies had revealed that 60 to 75 percent of these Norwegian dementia cases had died with Alzheimers disease, it seems that in the worst affected Norwegian municipalities, during the 10 year period 1974 to 1983, the median age-adjusted Alzheimers mortality rates were between 44 to 55 per 100,000 for males and 87 to 109 per 100,000 for females. Clearly, within Norway, Alzheimers disease-related mortality had been higher, by a factor of 15, in some regions than in others. If the higher rate municipalities in Norway are also compared with Maracaibo, it is apparent that death from Alzheimers disease during roughly the same time period had been one thousand times more common in parts of Northern Europe than it had been in at least one region of Venezuela. If the main causal variable(s) in Alzheimers disease is genetic, that is an aberration that has been very widely dispersed in the human population, then age-adjusted mortality from this type of dementia would be relatively uniform. International, national, and regional patterns of death from Alzheimers disease would show little variation, even though the individuals involved would appear to have been selected randomly. However, this is not the case. Everywhere that Alzheimers disease mortality and incidence has been studied in detail geographically, including England and Wales,22 Scotland,23 Norway,24 Canada,25 the USA,26 and China,37 strong regional differences in incidence and/or mortality rates have been identified. Alzheimers disease does not occur uniformly. It is clear that the available evidence, from international to postal code scale,
39
strongly suggests that some areas unfortunately experience much higher Alzheimers incidence and mortality rates than others. The rates of the two distribution extremes (Maracaibo and southeast coastal Norway) appear to differ by a factor of at least 1,000. That is, at every scale, Alzheimers disease shows non-random geographical variation. This spatial distribution is the reverse of what would be expected if the preeminent risk factor(s) for this type of dementia were one or more widely dispersed, common, genetic aberrations. Simply put, while genetic differences probably play a role in sporadic Alzheimers disease, they cannot be the major actors in this drama.
years and older, the dementia prevalences were 30, 50, and 74 percent respectively. For the same three groups, Alzheimers disease prevalence rates reached 14, 36, and 58 percent. It was apparent from the data collected in this King County, Washington State study that Japanese Americans suffered much higher rates of dementia in general and Alzheimers disease in particular than are found in similar aged populations living in Japan. Beyond this, the distribution of subtypes of dementia much more closely resembled that found in European and North American Caucasians than it did amongst elderly Japanese living in their homeland. Consequently, the occurrence of Alzheimers disease was higher and the prevalence of vascular dementia lower in King Countys Japanese Americans than might have been anticipated from available Japanese dementia data. Similar results were reported by Hendrie and colleagues,29-30 who have shown that African Americans develop Alzheimers disease more frequently than West Africans. This research team conducted the Indianapolis-Ibadan Dementia Project, a longitudinal prospective population-based study designed to see if there were international differences in the prevalence of Alzheimers disease and other forms of dementias in those of African descent. This project consisted of baseline surveys, conducted in both Indianapolis and Ibadan, during the period 1992 and 1993 and two subsequent waves of interviews and diagnoses, taking place 2 (1994-1995) and 5 (1997-1998) years later. The participants in this project were 2,459 Yoruba residents in Ibadan, Nigeria and 2,147 African American residents of Indianapolis, Indiana, all aged 65 or older. At the start of the project, none of those studied had dementia and all lived freely in their respective communities. To avoid any question of diagnostic differences or errors in diagnosis, identical methods were used in both countries by the same team of research workers. Nevertheless, they were able to show that African
41
Americans developed Alzheimers disease at more than double the rate seen in Nigerian Yoruba of the same age. That is, the age-standardized annual incidence rates of Alzheimers disease for African Americans were more than twice those of Nigerian Yoruba.31 Both the King County, Japanese survey and the IndianapolisIbadan Dementia Project clearly show that the types of dementia and, indeed, their frequency, appear strongly influenced by location. When ethnic groups move, migrant disease patterns soon seem to mirror those of new neighbouring ethnic groups rather than the current inhabitants of the region of origin. Such changes strongly suggest that the preeminent risk factor(s) in Alzheimers disease and indeed vascular dementia cannot be genetic and are much more likely to be of either lifestyle and/ or environmental origin.
SUMMARY
The evidence presented in this chapter strongly suggests that there is no overriding, key genetic aberration in Alzheimers disease. If there were, the global and regional incidence, prevalence, and mortality patterns for this type of dementia would be much more uniform than they are. Beyond this, migration would have little impact on the incidence and prevalence of Alzheimers disease in identifiable ethnic groups, but clearly it has a significant influence. The only valid conclusion to be drawn from this epidemiological and geographical evidence appears to be that, although certain genetic aberrations may predispose to sporadic Alzheimers disease, they must do so through their ability to promote the negative impact of specific lifestyle and/or environmental variables.
42
REFERENCES
1. 2. 3. Hippocrates On Airs, Waters, and Places, quoted in Adams, F. (1849). The Genuine Works of Hippocrates, Vol 1, 190. Foster, H.D. (1986). Reducing cancer mortality: A geographical perceptive. Victoria, BC: Western Geographical Series, 23. Foster, H.D. (1987). Disease family trees: The possible roles of iodine in goitre, cretinism, multiple sclerosis, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Alzheimers and Parkinsons diseases and cancers of the thyroid, nervous system and skin. Medical Hypotheses, 24, 249-263. Foster, H.D. (1993). Sudden infant death syndrome: The Bradford Hill criteria and the evaluation of the thyroxine deficiency hypothesis. Journal of Orthomolecular Medicine, 8(4), 201-225. Foster, H.D. (1995). The iodine-selenium connection in respiratory distress and sudden infant death syndromes. The Townsend Letter for Doctors and Patients, 149, 30-35. Foster, H.D., and Zang, L. (1995). Longevity and selenium deficiency: Evidence from the Peoples Republic of China. The Science of the Total Environment, 170, 130-139. Lalonde, D. (1974). A New Perspective on the Health of Canadians: A Working Document. Ottawa: Canadian Department of Health and Welfare. Foster, H.D. (1992). Health, Disease and the Environment. London: Belhaven Press. Foster, H.D. (2002). Why the preeminent risk factor in sporadic Alzheimers disease cannot be genetic. Medical Hypotheses, 59(1), 57-61.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8. 9.
10. Ibid. 11. Henderson, A.S. (1994). Dementia, Geneva: WHO 12. Ineichen, B. (1998). The geography of dementia: An approach through epidemiology. Health & Place, 4(4), 383-394. 13. Ibid. 14. Chiu, H.F.K., and Zhang, M. (2000). Dementia research in China. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15, 947-953. 15. Zhang, M., Katzman, R., Salmon, D., Jin, H., Cai, G., Wang, Z., Qu, G., Grunt, I., Yu, E., Levy, P., Klauber, M.R., and Liu, W.T. (1990). The prevalence of dementia and Alzheimers disease in Shanghai, China: Impact of age, gender, and education. Annals of Neurology, 27(4), 428-437.
43
16. Sulkava, R., Heliovaara, M., and Palo, J. (1988). Regional differences in the prevalence of Alzheimers disease. In Proceedings of the International Symposium on Alzheimers disease. Department of Neurology, University of Kuopio, Finland. 17. Molina, O., Cardozo, D., and Cardozo, J. (2000). Causes of dementia in Maracaibo, Venezuela: A re-evaluation. Revista de neurologia, 30(2), 115-117 (Spanish). 18. Ibid. 19. Flaten, T.S. (1989). Mortality from dementia in Norway, 1969-1983. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 43, 285-289. 20. Flaten, T.P. (1990). Geographical associations between aluminum in drinking water and death rates with dementia (including Alzheimers disease), Parkinsons disease and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis in Norway. Environmental Geochemistry and Health, 12(), 152167. 21. Ibid. 22. Martyn, C.N., Barker, D. J. O., Osmond, C. Edwardson, J.A., and Lacey, R.F. (1989). Geographical relation between Alzheimers disease and aluminum in drinking water. Lancet, 1(8629), 59-62. 23. Whalley, L.J., Thomas, B.M., McGongial, G., McQuade, C.A., Swingler, R., and Black, R. (1995). Epidemiology of presenile Alzheimers disease in Scotland (1974-1988). Non-random geographical variation. British Journal of Psychiatry, 167(6), 728-731. 24. Flaten (1990), op. cit. 25. Neuman, S.C., and Bland, R.C. (1987). Canadian trends in mortality from mental disorders, 1965-1983. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 76, 1-7. 26. Centers for Disease Control (1991). Mortality from Alzheimers disease United States, 1979-1987. JAMA: Journal of the America Medical Association, 265(3), 313-317. 27. Chiu et. al., op. cit. 28. Graves, A.B., Larson, E.B., Edland, S.D., Bowen, J.D., McCormick, W.C., McCurry, S.M., Rice, M.M., Wenzlow, A., and Uomoto, J.M. (1996). Prevalence of dementia and its subtypes in the Japanese American population of King County, Washington State. The Kame Project. American Journal of Epidemiology, 144(8), 760-771. 29. Hendrie, H.C., Ogunniyi, A., Hall, K.S., Baiyeuw, O., Unverzagt, F.W., Gyreje, O., Gao, S., Evans, R.M., Ogunseyine, A.O., Adeneyinka, A.O., Musick, B., and Hui, S.L. (2001). Incidence of dementia and
44
Alzheimers disease in two communities: Yoruba residing in Ibadan, Nigeria and African Americans residing in Indianapolis, Indiana. JAMA: the Journal of the American Medical Association, 285, 739747. 30. Ogunniyi, A., Baiyewu, O., Guregi, O., Hall, K.S., Unverzagt, F.W., Siu, S.H., Gao, S., Farlow, M., Oluwole, O.S., Komolafe, O., and Hendrie, H.C. (2000). Epidemiology of dementia in Nigeria: Results from the Indianapolis-Ibadan study. European Journal of Neurology, 7(5), 485-490. 31. Hendrie et. al., op. cit.
45
Some circumstantial evidence is very strong, as when you find a trout in the milk.
Henry David Thoreau
46
BIOCHEMICAL ABNORMALITIES
What we call the future is the shadow which our past throws in front of us. Marcel Proust (1871-1922)
Every disease or disorder involves biochemical abnormalities. All pathogens cause such irregularities, and so too do toxins. Genetic aberrations inevitably result in biochemical quirks, and so does trauma. Deficiencies or excesses of vitamins, minerals, fats, proteins, and other nutrients interfere with, or correct, biochemical activity. Drugs, herbs, and supplements are intended to rectify biochemical imbalances, while even stress, meditation, and exercise directly impact on the bodys biochemistry. Clearly, if we do not identify a diseases abnormal biochemistry we will never adequately explain its causes.
GLUCOSE
Every bodily function from breathing, feeling, and thinking to walking, swimming, and running requires energy. One major fuel source for such energy is glucose, which is available from circulating blood and stored fuels, such as glycogen, in the liver. Glucose is the bodys primary fuel source and is derived from the breakdown of sugars and starches contained in foods.1 However, it is unable to pass directly through cell walls and must be escorted by the hormone insulin. This relationship is often called the lock and key model, with insulin permitting
47
glucose access to cells by attaching itself to receptor sites on their walls. Once glucose gains access to such cells with the assistance of insulin, it is used as an energy source. It is not surprising then that type 1 diabetics, who typically lack adequate insulin, suffer from hyperglycemia, a condition characterized by abnormally high blood glucose levels.2 Diabetics are not the only ones who display glucose abnormalities. Several studies have shown that patients with mild and severe Alzheimers disease have low levels of brain glucose metabolism. Alexander and coworkers, for example, used fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) position emission tomography (PET) to study the brains of a group of Alzheimers patients over a period of 1 year. Their results demonstrated that not only do those with this disorder have significantly lower than normal brain glucose metabolism, but that this declines as Alzheimers disease progresses. Such glucose abnormalities may occur many years before any signs of the disorder become apparent. Reiman and colleagues,4 for example, identified abnormally low glucose metabolism in the brains of patients aged between 20 and 39, that is decades before the usual onset of any Alzheimers symptoms. In their study, brain scans were taken of 12 such youthful patients who carried the APO E4 gene and 15 who did not. Results showed that those with this genetic characteristic already experienced abnormally low brain glucose metabolism.5-6 Nevertheless, the available evidence does not seem to show that a lack of glucose kills brain cells directly.7 Under normal circumstances, brain cells are able to protect themselves against toxins, such as calcium homeostasis and aspartate. However, if they are glucose deficient this ability declines and they become much more prone to the destruction caused by such excitotoxins. Excitatory amino acids, including glutamate and
48
aspartate, normally exist in low concentrations in the brain without causing significant damage. However, if the brain becomes hypoglycemic, that is short of glucose, such toxins start to destroy brain cells, even at their normal levels. This may be what occurs in Alzheimers disease.8-9
be created without choline. It is to be expected, therefore, that acetylcholine is deficient in the brains of Alzheimers patients and that this abnormality is parallelled by memory decline.16
types of glutamate receptors in the human brain and it appears that glutamate is as important a synaptic transmitter as acetylcholine. There is a profound reduction in glutamatergic neurotransmission in Alzheimers disease that results from the loss of pyramidal neurons and cholinergic innervation.23 This deficit is associated with significantly depressed plasma glutamate levels and abnormally low glutamine cerebrospinal fluid/ plasma ratios.24 Furthermore, there appears to be a strong correlation between behaviour and coping ability in Alzheimers patients and cerebrospinal fluid glutamate levels, which provides clear evidence of a role for the disruption of amino acid metabolism in the disease.
suggested that the adenylyl cyclase catalytic mechanism involves two magnesium ions. While adenylate cyclase activity declines in the non-demented elderly, no such reduction is seen in Alzheimers patients, who typically show abnormally high brain adenylate cyclase activity.31 This may be a reflection of their calcium deficiency.
There is also growing evidence that aluminum ions act synergistically with iron ions to increase free-radical damage.39 The ingestion of aluminum by rats, for example, has been demonstrated to increase lipid peroxide formation by 142 percent in their brains and to stimulate iron-dependent peroxidation of liposomes, micelles, and red blood cells. The iron-binding and transporting protein transferrin is, in addition, the chief aluminum-binding protein of the plasma.40 Once it has bound to transferrin, aluminum is able to enter cells, including those of the central nervous system, via transferrin receptors. In this way, aluminum and iron can both reach the brain.41 This may be very significant since, as Sohler and coworkers42 showed in a sample of 400 psychiatric outpatients in New Jersey, memory loss increased as blood aluminum levels rose.
Phosphorylation consists of the addition of a phosphate (PO4) group to a protein or to a small molecule.45 Phosphorylation provides a very fast way of regulating proteins. If a protein, such as tau, is regulated by phosphorylation it is always present in standby mode. When an activating signal arrives, the protein is phosphorylated and then performs in the way intended. With the arrival of a deactivating signal, the protein again becomes dephosphorylated and ceases to work. As can be seen, phosphorylation and dephosphorylation act rather like a protein light switch that consists, in part, of a phosphate group.46 Tau is always present in cerebrospinal fluid. Sjgren and coworkers,47 for example, have measured levels found in healthy adults, aged from 21 to 93 years, finding that tau typically increases with age. It is known, however, that the tau in the brains of Alzheimers patients is abnormal in that it is hyperphosphorlylated, that is phosphorlylated to excess. It has been corrupted by several extra molecules of phosphorus.49 As a result, the tau malfunctions and becomes unable to support tubulins role in the production of microtubules, which, therefore, lack integrity and begin to twist. Communication and cell nourishment is compromised and eventually declines to zero. The neuron cannot be sustained and begins to wither. The cell membranes collapse and every part of the neuron disintegrates and with it synapses, each representing a memory fragment. How much of this process of the formation of neurofibrillary tangles and neuron destruction requires the presence of the iron and aluminum, previously described, is unclear.
of Alzheimers disease and so researchers tried for many years to understand their biochemistry. In 1984, George G. Glenner of the University of California at San Diego found that their chief component was a peptide, that is a very short protein fragment, that consisted of 42 amino acids. This substance, the chief component of the plaques seen in the Alzheimers disease brain, is now called beta-amyloid peptide.51 It quickly was discovered to be formed from a more complex protein, the canning. This bigger protein is broken down in several different ways in the body. To illustrate, alpha- and gamma-secretase enzymes can cut it, giving rise to the three fragments which are not toxic. Alternatively, it can be cut by beta- and gammasecretase. When this occurs, the process yields a harmless 40-amino-acid-long beta-amyloid peptide or a toxic 42-aminoacid peptide. It is the latter, of course, that builds up outside nerve cells to form the plaque seen in Alzheimers disease brains.52 In some cases, this accumulation occurs because the E4 form of apoliprotein is selectively removed from extracellular space instead of beta-amyloid peptide. As a consequence, the latter builds up to create plaques.53 Unlike tau, the bodys production of the 42-amino acid form of beta-amyloid, measured in cerebrospinal fluid, does not increase with age.54 Indeed, levels of this peptide decrease in the cerebrospinal fluid of patients with Alzheimers disease.55 Interestingly, studies examining the role that cholesterol may play in plaque formation have shown that the 42-amino acid form of betaamyloid builds up more easily in the brains of rabbits given tap water than it does if these animals drink distilled water.56 Obviously, this suggests a water quality role in Alzheimers disease. The 42-amino acid form of beta-amyloid seems to damage the brain in various ways.57 It appears, for example, to interfere with calcium regulation, to promote destructive free radicals, and to cause immune cells such as microglia to aggregate, a
55
process which leads to inflammation and to the exacerbation of previous injury. Recent research has shown that the beta-amyloid of Alzheimers neurofibrillary tangles consists of more than just protein. It is a metalloprotein, housing atoms of the metals copper, zinc, and iron within its tangles.58-59 Each of these metals can react with oxygen but copper promotes free radical damage while zinc is generally considered a protective antioxidant. Interestingly, copper chelators that can attach themselves to this element and eliminate it for the body, have been shown to greatly reduce amyloid plaques in the brains of living mice.60 Clinical trials are in progress to determine whether the same is true of humans. A study at the Sanders-Brown Center on Aging in Louisville, Kentucky also has found that at low levels zinc provides protection against amyloid plaques, while at elevated levels it promotes their formation.61
OTHER PROTEINS
Recent research has shown that other proteins also appear to play roles in Alzheimers disease. To illustrate, endoplasmicreticulum associated binding protein (ERAB) can combine with beta amyloid which, as a result, attracts more beta amyloid protein into the cell.62 Elevated endoplasmic-reticulum associated binding protein also increases beta amyloids nervedestructive power. In addition, 100 kD AMY protein is capable of forming plaques that are very similar to those composed of beta amyloid. Beyond this, the protein prostate apoptosis response-4 can encourage nerve cells to self-destruct.63
56
SUMMARY
It is apparent from this short review that Alzheimers patients display a wide variety of biochemical abnormalities. These include low brain glucose metabolism and the malfunctioning of neurotransmitters such as acetylcholine, dopamine, norepinephrine, serotonin and glutamate. Also present in this form of dementia are deficiencies of both brain calcium and magnesium and excess of iron and aluminum. Beyond this, disruption is caused by two abnormal proteins, hyperphosphorylated tau and the 42-amino acid form of beta-amyloid peptide. It is possible, but unlikely, that each one, or most, of the biochemical abnormalities just described in Alzheimers disease is linked to its own distinct genetic aberration or environmental trigger. It is possible also that these may be part of a chain of cause and effect. If this is the case, a single genetic aberration or environmental toxin may result in one initial biochemical abnormality. This in turn may trigger a second and so on, eventually pushing over dominoes known as dopamine, serotonin, calcium, and hyperphosphorylated tau to name but a few. If Alzheimers disease is caused by such a reaction, its key is the identification of where each of these biochemical dominoes occur in the causal chain. If we can prevent the first domino from falling, we can stop the collapse of all and so prevent the disorder.
57
REFERENCES
1. 2. 3. Warren Clinic. The role of glucose and insulin. http://www.warrenclinic. com/services/diabetes/glucose.asp Ibid. Alexander, G.E., Chen, K., Pietrini, P., Rapoport, S.I., and Reiman, E.M. (2002). Longitudinal PET evaluation of cerebral metabolic decline in dementia: A potential outcome measure in Alzheimers disease studies. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(5), 738-745. Reiman, E.M., Chen, K., Alexander, G.E., Caselli, R.J., Bandy, D., Osborne, D., Saunders, A.M., and Hardy, J. (2004). Functional brain abnormalities in young adults at genetic risk for late-onset Alzheimers dementia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(1), 284-289. Ibid. CNN.com. Health. Alzheimers may start decades before signs appear. http://www.cnn.com/2003/HEALTH/conditions/12/16/early. alzeimers.ap/ Blaylock, R.L. (1996). Energy-producing enzymes and Alzheimers reprinted from Health and Natural Journal, 3(2), by the San Francisco Medical Research Foundation. http://www.lightparty.com/ Health/Additives.html Mattson, M.P. (2003). Excitotoxic and excitoprotective mechanisms: Abundant targets for the prevention and treatment of neurodegenerative disorders. Neuromolecular Medicine, 3(2), 65-94. Blaylock, op. cit.
4.
5. 6.
7.
8.
9.
10. Davies, P., and Maloney, A.J.F. (1976). Selective loss of central cholinergic neurones in Alzheimers disease. Lancet, 2(8000), 1403. 11. Toghi, H., Abe, T., Hashiguchi, K., Saheki, M., and Takahashi, S. (1994). Remarkable reduction in acetylcholine concentration in the cerebrospinal fluid from patients with Alzheimers type dementia. Neuroscience Letters, 177, 139-142. 12. Rehman, H.U., and Masson, E.A. (2001). Neuroendocrinology of ageing. Age and Ageing, 30, 279-287. 13. Klinger, M., Apelt, J., Kumar, A., Sorger, D., Sahri, O., Steinbach, J., Scheunemann, M., and Schliebs, R. (2003). Alterations in cholinergic and non-cholinergic neurotransmitter receptor densities in transgenic Tg2576 mouse brain with beta-amyuloid plaque pathology. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 21(7), 357-369.
58
14. Kirschmann, J.D. with Dunn, L.J. (1984). Nutrition Almanac. New York: McGraw-Hill. 15. Iraizoz, I., Guijarro, J.L., Gonzalo, L.M., and de Lacalle, S. (1999). Neuropathologic changes in the nucleus basalis correlate with clinical measures of dementia. Acta Neuropathologica, 98(2), 186-196. 16. Ibid. 17. Kay, A.D., Milstein, S., Kaufman, S., Creasey, H., Haxby, J.V., Cutler, N.R., and Rapoport, S.I. (1986). Cerebrospinal fluid bioptein is decreased in Alzheimers disease. Archives of Neurology, 43(10), 996-999. 18. Reinikainen, K.J., Soininen, H., and Riekkinen, P.J. (1990). Neurotransmitter changes in Alzheimers disease: Implications for diagnostics and therapy. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 27(4), 576-86. 19. Engelburghs, S., and DeDeyn, P.P. (1997). The neurochemistry of Alzheimers disease. Acta Neurologic Belgica, 97(2), 67-84. 20. Joyce, J.N., Myers, A.J., and Gurevich, J.E. (1998). Dopamine D2 receptor bands in normal human temporal cortex are absent in Alzheimers disease. Brain Research, 784(1-2), 7-17. 21. Palmer, A.M. (1996). Neurochemical studies of Alzheimers disease. Neurodegeneration, 5(4), 381-391. 22. Glutamate as a neurotransmitter. http:www.soton.ac.uk/~gk/scifi/ glut.htm 23. Enz, A., and Francis, P.T. (1998). The rationale for development of cholinergic therapies in Alzheimers disease. In A. Fisher, I. Hanin, and M. Yoshida (Eds.), Progress in Alzheimers and Parkinsons diseases (pp. 445-450). New York: Plenum Press. 24. Bosun, H., Forssell, L.G., Almkvist, O., Cowburn, R.F., Eklf, R., Winblad, B., and Wetterberg, L. (1990). Amino acid concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid and plasma in Alzheimers disease and healthy control subjects. Journal of Neural Transmission: Parkinsons disease and dementia section, 2(4), 295-304. 25. Ferrier, I.N., Leake, A., Taylor, G.A., McKeith, I.G., Fairbairn, A.F., Robinson, C.J., Francis, R.M., and Edwardson, J.A. (1990). Reduced gastrointestinal absorption of calcium in dementia. Age and Ageing, 19(6), 368-375. 26. Sato, Y., Asoh, T., and Oizumi, K. (1998). High prevalence of vitamin D deficiency and reduced bone mass in elderly women with Alzheimers disease, Bone 23(6), 555-557. 27. Danielsson, E., Eckerns, S.A., Westlilnd-Danielsson, A., Nordstrm, O., Bartfai, T., Gottfries, C.G., and Wallin, A. (1988). VIP-sensitive
59
adenylate cyclase, guanylate cyclase, muscarinic receptors, choline acetyltransferase in brain tissue affected by Alzheimers disease/ senile dementia of the Alzheimers type. Neurobiology of Aging, 9(2), 153-162. 28. Kurokawa, T., Kitamura, Y., Moriuchi, M., and Ishibashi, S. (1981). Differences between magnesium and manganese ions in modification of effect of catecholamine on adenylate cyclase system in Ehrlich ascites tumour cells. Journal of Pharmacobio-dynamics, 4(10), 794-797. 29. Mahaffe, D.D., Cooper, C.W., Ramp, W.K., and Ontjes, D.A. (1982). Magnesium promotes both parathyroid hormone secretion and adenosine 3'4'-monophosphate production in rat parathyroid tissues and reverses the inhibitory effects of calciumon adenylate cyclase. Endocrinology, 110(2), 487-95. 30. Zimmermann, G., Zhou, D., and Taussig, R. (1998). Mutation uncovers a role for two magnesium ions in the catalytic mechanism of adenylyl cyclase. Journal of Biological Chemistry, 273(31), 19650-5. 31. Danielsson et. al., op. cit. 32. Perl, D.P., and Broady, A.R. (1980). Alzheimers disease: X ray spectrometric evidence of aluminum accumulation in neurofibirllary tangle-bearing neurons. Science, 208, 297-299. 33. Perl, D.P., and Droady, A.R. (1980). Detection of aluminum by SEM-X ray spectrometry within neurofibrillary tangle-bearing neurons of Alzheimers disease. Neurotoxicology, 1(special issue 4), 133-137. 34. Glick, J.L. (1990). Dementias: The role of magnesium deficiency and a hypothesis concerning the pathogenesis of Alzheimers disease. Medical Hypotheses, 31, 211-225. 35. Korf, J., Gramsbergen, J-B.P., Prenen, G.H.M., and Go, K.G. (1986). Cation shifts and excitotoxins in Alzheimers and Huntington disease and experimental brain damage. Proceedings of Brain Research, 70, 213-226. 36. Durlach, J. (1990). Magnesium depletion and pathogenesis of Alzheimers disease. Magnesium Research, 3(3), 217-218. 37. Mesco, E.R., Kachen, C., and Timiras, P.S. (1991). Effects of aluminum on tau proteins in human neuroblastoma cells. Molecular and Chemical Neuropathology, 14(3), 199-212. 38. Yamamoto, A., Shin, R.W., Hasegawa, K., Naiki, H., Sato, H., Yoshimasu, F., and Kitamoto, T. (2002). Iron (III) induces aggregation of hyperphosphorylated tau and its reduction to iron (II) reverses the aggregation: Implications in the formation of neurofibrillary tangles of Alzheimers disease. Journal of Neurochemistry, 82(5), 1137-1147.
60
39. Symons, M.C.R., and Gutteridge, J.M.C. (1998). Free radicals and iron: Chemistry, biology and medicine. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 40. Ibid. 41. Ibid. 42. Sohler, A., Pfeiffer, C.C., and Papaionnov, R. (1981). Blood aluminum levels in a psychiatric outpatient population. High aluminum levels related to memory loss. Journal of Orthomolecular Psychiatry, 19(1), 54-60. 43. St. George-Hyslop, P.H. (2000). Piecing together Alzheimers. Scientific American, 76-83. 44. Ibid. 45. Wikipedia. The Free Encyclopedia. Phosphorylation. http://en.wikipedia. org/wiki/phosphorylation 46. Ibid. 47. Sjgren, M., Vanderstichele, H., Agren, H., Zachrisson, O., Edsbagge, M., Wikkels, C., Skoog, I., Wallin, A., Wahlun, L-O., Marcusson, J., Ngga, K., Andreasen, N., Davidsson, P., Vanmechelen, E., and Blennov, K. (2001). tau and A42 in cerebrospinal fluid from healthy adults 21-93 years of age: Establishment of reference values. Clinical Chemistry, 47, 1776-1781. 48. Sunderland, T., Linker, G., Mirza, N., Putman, K.T., Friedman, D.L., Kimmel, L.H., Bergeson, J., Manetti, G.J., Zimmermann, M., Tang, B., Bartko, J.J., and Cohen, R.M. (2003). Decreased -Amyloid42 and increased tau levels in cerebrospinal fluid of patients with Alzheimers disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 289, 2094-2103. 49. Shenk, D. (2001). The Forgetting: Alzheimers: Portrait of an epidemic. New York: Doubleday. 50. Mesco et. al., op. cit. 51. St. George-Hyslop, op. cit. 52. Ibid. 53. Ibid. 54. Sjgren et. al., op. cit. 55. Sunderland et. al., op. cit. 56. Sparks, D.L., Lochhead, J., Horstman, D., Wagoner, T., and Martin, T. (2002). Water quality has a pronounced effect on cholesterolinduced accumulation of Alzheimer amyloid beta (Abeta) in rabbit brain. Journal of Alzheimers disease, 4(6), 523-529.
61
57. St. George-Hyslop, op. cit. 58. Lovell, M.A., Robertson, J.D., Teesdale, W.J., Campbell, J.L., and Markesberg, W.R. (1998). Copper, iron and zinc in Alzheimers disease plaques. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 158(1), 47-52. 59. Curtain, C.C., Ali, F., Volitakis, I., Cherny, R.A., Norton, R.S., Beyreuther, K., Barrow, C.J., Masters, C.L., Bush, A.I., and Barnham, K.J. (2001). Alzheimers disease amyloid-beta binds copper and zinc to generate an allosterically ordered membrane-penetrating structure containing superoxide dismutase-like subunits. Journal of Biological Chemistry, 276(23), 20466-20473. 60. American Federation for Aging Research. Alzheimers Information Center. The latest research on metals and Alzheimers disease. http://www. infoaging.org/d-alz-9-r-metals.html 61. Ibid. 62. What causes Alzheimers disease? http://www.ucdmc.ucdavis.edu/ ucdhs/health/a-z/02Alzheimer/doc02causes.html 63. Ibid.
62
I cant understand why people are frightened of new ideas. Im frightened of the old ones. John Cage (1912 - 1992)
LONGITUDINAL STUDIES
One lengthy, painstaking, and costly way of attempting to identify other risk factors for Alzheimers disease is the longitudinal study.1 These studies often involve enrolling thousands of people in projects that may last for decades. Volunteers are given medical examinations at regular intervals, perhaps once or twice a year, and are repeatedly questioned about their lifestyles, occupations, hobbies, and other topics that might be related to disease development. They may be monitored like this anywhere from a couple of years to half a century, or until their deaths. In some longitudinal studies, the bodies of volunteers are eventually subjected to autopsy. Numerous longitudinal studies of memory decline have been in progress for two or three decades and are now beginning to identify risk factors that may be related to Alzheimers disease. Some of these variables, such as high blood pressure, elevated cholesterol and homocysteine levels, obesity, poor diet, smoking, depression, head trauma, chronic stress, osteoporosis, and diabetes mellitus, can be controlled. In contrast, other such
63
variables, including having the APO E4 gene, or family members with Alzheimers disease, or lack of childhood education or aging, can no longer be influenced.2
Hypertension
The Honolulu Heart Program/Honolulu-Asia Aging Study was one such longitudinal project. As described in two articles published in the Neurobiology of Aging,3-4 this involved measuring the blood pressure, in 1965, 1968, and 1971, of 4,678 Japanese-American males who had been born between 1900 and 1919 and who resided in Hawaii. Some 25 years later, 3,734 of these volunteers, who could still be traced, had their blood pressures remeasured and in addition were given cognitive tests. It was discovered that those subjects who, 25 years earlier, had suffered from untreated hypertension (systolic blood pressure greater than 160 mm Hg) were 4.8 times more likely to be demented than those whose pressure readings had been normal.5 By the year 2000, the brains of 243 of these volunteers had been autopsied. A direct relationship was identified between elevated diastolic blood pressure (95 mm Hg or greater) and both atrophy (shrinkage) of the brain and the presence in it of neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. That is, high blood pressure in mid-life was found to help predict the development of Alzheimers disease in old age. A similar longitudinal project, the Systolic Hypertension in Europe (Syst-Eur) Study has demonstrated further that blood pressure-lowering therapy, using long-acting dihydropyridine, protected against dementia in those with this form of hypertension.6 The available evidence, recently summarized by Fotuhi,7 therefore, strongly suggests that hypertension, especially if it goes untreated, increases the probability of subsequently developing Alzheimers disease.
64
Cholesterol
Whether or not high cholesterol is a significant risk factor in Alzheimers disease is still being debated. Experimental evidence, however, suggests that cholesterol promotes the formation of amyloid plaques.8 Sparks, for example, was able to increase or decrease the amyloid brain load in rabbits by providing them with cholesterol-enhanced, or depleted, diets. This relationship was confirmed in experiments using genetically engineered mice.9 Similarly, Simons and coworkers10 showed that, in the test tube, cholesterol depletion with drugs inhibits the formation of beta-amyloid in the hippocampal neurons of fetal rats. Does a similar cholesterol-amyloid plaque relationship occur in humans? There is evidence from several longitudinal studies that suggests that it does. In Scandinavia,11 the blood pressures and cholesterol levels of 1,449 volunteers were followed for more than 20 years. Those with high cholesterol levels (more than 250 mg/decilitre) in mid-life were found to be 2.2 times as likely to develop Alzheimers disease as they aged than were those with normal mid-life cholesterol levels. This probability figure rose to 3.5 in individuals with both high cholesterol and elevated blood pressure. Similarly, for 4 years, Yaffe12 and coworkers monitored total cholesterol, LDL (low density lipoproteins), and HDL (high density lipoproteins) in 1,037 post menopausal women who suffered from heart disease and were part of the Heart and Estrogen/Progestin Replacement Study. Those with the highest total cholesterol levels were found to be 1.76 times more likely to develop mild cognitive impairment than those with the lowest cholesterol levels. Interestingly, it appeared to be LDL, not HDL, that were associated with this impairment. Similarly, Kawas and Corrada13 found a link between a high ratio of LDL to HDL and dementia in the data collected during a Baltimore longitudinal study of aging.
65
It is not surprising, therefore, that evidence is growing that cholesterol-lowering medications, such as statins, may reduce the risk of developing Alzheimers disease. Using data from the British General Practice Research Database, Drachman and coworkers14 compared 25,000 people with normal lipid values, 25,000 with elevated levels who were on medication to lower these, and 25,000 with high values who were not on medication. Over 6 years, 284 of the patients were diagnosed with dementia. It was found that those taking statins had a 70 percent lower risk of becoming demented. Similarly, Wolozin and colleagues15 studied the records of 56,000 patients treated in military hospitals in the USA and discovered that those on statins had a 60 to 73 percent lower risk of developing Alzheimers disease than patients who took other medications to reduce their blood pressures. Beyond this, Green and coworkers16 studied 2,581 patients at 15 medical centres in Canada, the USA, and Germany where they found that, regardless of genetic risk or ethnicity, those who took statins for at least 6 months reduced the risk of developing Alzheimers disease by 79 percent. There is, however, at least one major study that denies this cholesterol-amyloid plaque-Alzheimers disease association. In 2003, Tan and colleagues17 examined this possible link using data from the Framingham Study, one of the largest and most comprehensive longitudinal projects ever undertaken. Their research involved a subgroup of Framingham Study volunteers who had been evaluated biannually for cardiovascular risk factors since 1950. Members of this subgroup were alive and free of both stroke and dementia when re-examined during 1988 and 1989 and had also undergone apolipoprotein E (APO E) genotyping. Alzheimers disease subsequently developed in 77 of these 1,027 volunteers during the time period 1992 to 2000. However, no link was found between average cholesterol level (measured 15 times) or total cholesterol at examination number 20 and the future risk of developing Alzheimers disease.
66
Homocysteine
The essential amino acid methionine is the indirect source of homocysteine. As methionine is metabolized it produces homocysteine before either recycling it to methionine, or creating a final breakdown product, cystathionine. The former step requires vitamin B12 and folate and the latter needs vitamin B6.18 Inadequacies of these three key vitamins can slow homocysteine metabolism and allow abnormal levels of it to build up, creating a condition known as homocysteinemia. This has been associated with strokes, atherosclerosis, and cardiovascular disease. It is well known also that the oxidation product of homocysteine, homocysteic acid, exerts potent excitatory effects.19 Beyond the elevation of homocysteine caused by deficiencies of vitamins B6, B12, and folate, it is known also that certain genetic aberrations can promote excesses of this amino acid. To illustrate, methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase (MTHRF) is involved in the remethylation of homocysteine to methionine. The gene for MTHRF is located on chromosome 1. Rare allelic variants can cause severe methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase deficiencies resulting in homocystinuria.20 It would seem, therefore, that some people have above normal blood levels of homocysteine because of one or more of vitamin B6, vitamin B12, or folate deficiencies, while others have inherited a genetic aberration that predisposes them to this condition. In 2002, Seshadri and coworkers21 described the results of a longitudinal homocysteine and dementia study, conducted with the aid of 1,092 elderly people living in Framingham, near Boston. The subjects in this study were non-demented men and women with an average age of 76. Over the 8 years for which their health was followed, 111 developed dementia. It was found that those with high plasma homocysteine levels, when first measured, were twice as likely to have developed
67
Alzheimers disease than those with normal levels, that is with less than 14 micromoles per litre. For each five-point increase in the levels of plasma homocysteine, a 40 percent increase in the risk of Alzheimers disease occurred. A study of community-dwelling elderly Latinos in the Sacramento area has since confirmed that plasma homocysteine is an independent predictor of cognitive function.22
Diet
Animal studies have shown that diets rich in antioxidants can improve memory. The evidence seems best for spinach23 and blueberries,24 both of which have been shown to alter rat behaviour and make measurable differences in their memory performances. Curcumin, a component of curry spice, is also a strong antioxidant which, in both the test tube and in animal models of Alzheimers disease appears capable of reducing amyloid protein toxicity.25-26 Interestingly, curry is much more widely used in India than in the USA and Alzheimers disease is far less common in the elderly of the former country than of the latter. Another herb that seems to improve memory and that may be useful in reducing the risk of Alzheimers disease is Spanish Sage (Salvia lavandulaefolia). As early as 1597, the herbalist John Gerard wrote that sage is singularly good for the heart and brain and quickeneth the nerves and memory. Similarly, Nicholas Culpeper writing in 1652 claimed that sage also heals the memory, warming and quickening the senses.28 Support for these claims has been provided by Tildesley and colleagues29 of the Division of Psychology, Northumbria University, who tested the effects of sage and a placebo on the memories of 44 healthy young adults. Volunteers in this study were given tests at intervals to establish immediate word recall. Those who took sage oil consistently outperformed those who received
68
placebos. As has been described earlier, Alzheimers disease patients are typically deficient in the chemical messenger acetylcholine. Sage has been found to inhibit the enzyme acetylcholinesterase which breaks down acetylcholine and so presumably increases levels of this neurotransmitter in the brain, improving memory.30 Two recent longitudinal studies have shown also that vitamin E reduces the risk of developing Alzheimers disease. This vitamin, of course, is also a strong antioxidant. In one such project, Breteler and colleagues31 interviewed 5,395 Dutch volunteers, aged 55 or more, and gathered details of their diets, smoking habits, and antioxidant supplement use. From available food charts, these researchers then estimated the participants normal daily vitamin intake. After 6 years, 146 of these volunteers had developed Alzheimers disease. It was found that this form of dementia occurred most often in those who consumed the lowest levels of vitamin E. In contrast, volunteers who ate a diet that contained the highest amounts of this antioxidant were 43 percent less likely to have developed this form of dementia. Similarly, Morris and coworkers32 conducted a further longitudinal study in Chicago, and monitored 815 people, aged 65 or older, for 4 years. Those who ate a diet that contained the highest levels of vitamin E had 70 percent less chance of developing Alzheimers disease than who consumed the lowest levels. The Dutch longitudinal study also suggested that those with high intakes of vitamin C also were less likely to develop Alzheimers disease.33 Fish consumption also seems to decrease the possibility of developing this form of dementia.34-36 This could be because of the protection that is provided by the high levels of omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acids that are present in fish. This association appears to have been first pointed out by Grant,37 but it has been confirmed numerous times since then, most
69
notably by Barberger-Gateau and colleagues,38 who studied diet and the development of dementia amongst 1,674 elderly people, residing at home, in southwestern France. Three follow-up visits, made over a 7 year period, determined which of the volunteers were beginning to suffer memory loss. This longitudinal study established that eating fish at least once a week appeared to provide such elderly volunteers with a onethird reduction in the risk of developing dementia, during the 7 years of the study. A similar project, involving USA elderly, also confirmed that those who ate fish at least once a week were 60 percent less likely to develop Alzheimers disease, during a 2 to 3 year follow-up period, than those who rarely or never ate it.39 In an interesting study,40 1,600 Italians over the age of 70 were asked what they ate regularly. Once this information had been collected, they then were subjected to neurological tests of language skills, memory, and attention. Their reported diets were then classified on a scale of 1 to 7, with the latter considered the healthiest, being low in fat and cholesterol but elevated in fibre, polyunsaturated omega-3 oils, nuts, fruits, and vegetables. Results suggested a close link between diet and cognitive impairment since those who ate the worst diets proved to be three times as likely to have scored poorly on their cognitive tests than those who consumed the best. Beyond this, Wang and coworkers41 studied the serum vitamin B12 and folate levels in 370 non-demented Swedish elderly. After 3 years it became apparent that those with the lowest initial serum levels of either, or both, of these vitamins were twice as likely to develop Alzheimers disease. Clearly, there appear to be links between vitamin B12, folate, and antioxidant use, and other indicators of dietary quality, and the development of Alzheimers disease. This association
70
may be stronger in women. Gustafson and colleagues,42 for example, followed the changes in health of 392 non-demented Swedish adults from age 70 to 88, and discovered that for every 1.0 increase in body mass index (BMI) at age 70, women were 36 percent more likely to develop Alzheimers disease by the time they reached 88. This association between obesity and Alzheimers disease did not occur in males.
Smoking
There is no doubt that smoking is a very dangerous habit that is thought to have killed some 200 million people during the 20th century. It has been linked to approximately 50 diseases, but whether Alzheimers disease is one of them is still the subject of heated debate. On one side of the issue are those such as Lee,43 and supporters in the Smokers Rights Action Group.44 The latter, for example, claim that in the USA non-smokers are more prone to develop Alzheimers disease and as a result suffer 73,000 excess cases causing $17.5 billion in unnecessary costs. Set against this position, for example, are Doll, Peto, and colleagues45 from the Clinical Trial Services Unit and Epidemiological Studies Unit, Radcliffe Infirmary, Oxford and Wang and coworkers46 of the Stockholm Gerontology Research Centre, who believe that smoking is not protective against Alzheimers disease and, indeed, may promote it. Naturally, this position has the support of Action on Smoking and Health (ASH),47 a 31-year-old legal-action antismoking and nonsmokers rights organization. In 1993, Lee48 conducted a meta-analysis based on all of the data available at that time contained in English language casecontrol studies in which smoking had been investigated as a possible risk factor in Alzheimers disease. His study combined data from 19 research publications which included 1,691 cases, most of whom had been patients in Veterans hospitals.
71
Ten of these original research projects had been conducted in the USA, six in Europe, and one each in Japan, Colombia, and Australia. Lees meta-analysis suggested that there was a highly statistically significant (p<0.001) negative association between ever having smoked and Alzheimers disease. Smokers had only a 0.64 relative risk (RR) of developing this form of dementia. Lee concluded that his analysis supported the view that nicotine was protective against Alzheimers disease and smokers, therefore, were less likely to develop it. In 2002, Almeida and coworkers49 from the University of Western Australia published the results of a further meta-analysis of the smoking-Alzheimers relationship. This was based on 21 case-control studies that had been reported on between 1955 and 2000. They concluded that: ... the results reported in these meta-analyses show that the direction of the association between smoking and AD [Alzheimers disease) remains unclear. Previous claims that smoking reduces the risk of AD can no longer be supported. In fact, the results of recent cohort studies suggest that a history of smoking is associated with an increased incidence of AD these findings are particularly robust for subjects who were current smokers at the time of enrolment. One obvious limitation, however, is that the number of cohort studies (n=8) and incident cases available for analysis (n= 1076) is still too small to be used reliably as evidence that smoking increases the risk of AD. It is our view that this issue can only be addressed properly with a large cohort study designed to investigate the effects of smoking on cognitive decline and risk of AD if its results confirm the trend observed by recent studies ... then smoking prevention and cessation should become public health priorities in the fight against AD. At least two longitudinal studies have suggested that smoking does indeed promote, rather than protect against, Alzheimers
72
disease. The first of these followed 6,870 men and women aged 55 and over, living in a suburb of Rotterdam, The Netherlands. None of them had Alzheimers disease before the study, conducted by faculty at the Erasmus University Medical School, began.50 Over a 2-year period, any of these 6,870 individuals who appeared to show early signs of dementia were assessed and, if possible, given brain scans. As a consequence, 105 were diagnosed with Alzheimers disease. It was established that those volunteers who smoked were 2.3 times more likely to develop this form of dementia than those who had never done so. Interestingly, however, it was found that smoking did not increase the risk of developing Alzheimers disease in those who carried the APO E4 gene, which has been linked to this form of dementia in the general population. Indeed, one of the researchers involved, Dr. Monique Breteler,51 is quoted as saying It seems that if you have the gene, youre better off if you smoke. In 2000, the results of a second longitudinal study, conducted by Doll and coworkers52 of Radcliffe Infirmary Oxford, was released. This was based on observations of more than 34,000 British doctors whose smoking habits had been recorded every 6 to 12 years since 1951. By the end of 1998, over 24,000 of these doctors were dead and dementia had been reported on the death certificates of 483 of them. From this available data these researchers argued that: Among 473 whose smoking habits were recorded at least 10 years before their death, when they would not have been influenced by the start of the disease, the prevalence of both Alzheimers disease (the predominant cause) and of other dementias was similar in both smokers and non-smokers. If anything, persistent smoking may increase rather than decrease the age specific onset rate of dementia, conclude the authors.
73
The previous suggestions that smoking might be protective, say the authors, came from studies that were flawed because they were too small, or had relied on information about smoking habits from people other than the sufferers themselves.
Depression
As part of the ongoing Religious Orders Study,53 in which researchers have been evaluating memory skills and the onset of Alzheimers disease, more than 650 older nuns, priests, and brothers were given annual neurologic and memory examinations for 7 years. The results indicated that those participants who showed the greatest number of symptoms of depression at the beginning of the study were more likely to develop Alzheimers disease as it progressed. If they did, their cognitive decline was faster than usual. Given the prognosis for the disorder, it is hardly surprising that clinically significant depression occurs in some 20 to 40 percent of those diagnosed with Alzheimers disease.54 However, the Religious Orders Study55 results suggest that the link between depression and this form of dementia goes deeper than that. In 2003, researchers from the Boston University School of Medicine56 described results from the MIRAGE (Multi-Institutional Research in Alzheimers Genetic Epidemiology) Study obtained by examining nearly 2,000 Alzheimers patients and a matching number of their unaffected relatives. It was discovered that Alzheimers disease was more likely to develop in both men and women who had displayed symptoms of depression within 1 year of onset of their dementia. However, those exhibiting depression several years earlier were still at increased risk of developing Alzheimers disease. Even those suffering from depression 25 years previously were at slightly increased risk for this form of dementia.
74
Head Trauma
Controversy continues over whether traumatic brain injury increases the probability of developing Alzheimers disease.57 Launer and coworkers58 sought to answer this question by performing a pooled analysis of four European population-based prospective studies of individuals aged 65 years and older. These data included 528 incident dementia patients and 28,768 person-years of follow-up. Their analysis established that a history of head trauma with unconsciousness did not significantly increase the risk of subsequent Alzheimers disease. Similarly, Nemetz and colleagues59 followed up the medical histories of 1,283 traumatic brain injury cases that had occurred in Olmsted County, Minnesota, from 1935 to 1984. Thirty-one of these trauma patients subsequently developed Alzheimers disease, a number similar to that normally expected in individuals without head injuries. However, the data clearly indicates that such head trauma had reduced the time-ofonset of Alzheimers disease by about 8 years amongst persons at risk for developing it. That is, head trauma does not appear to increase the probability of developing Alzheimers disease in the general population; however, those prone to it tend to suffer from it earlier than normally expected. Why this happens is a question that appears to have been answered by Nicoll and coworkers.60 These researchers have shown that the deposition of beta-amyloid in the brain had been promoted by head trauma, in approximately one third of individuals who died shortly afterwards from severe injury. The probability of deposition of such beta-amyloid, following trauma, is greater than would be anticipated statistically in individuals with the apolipoprotein E-epsilon 4 allelle, that is the allele that has been linked to late-onset Alzheimers disease. In short, in individuals with this genotype, severe head trauma often appears to initiate beta-amyloid deposition. Not
75
surprisingly, if they survive the trauma, such deposition reduces the time-of-onset of sporadic Alzheimers disease in those genetically prone to it since, of course, beta-amyloid is the major constituent of neuritic plaques. This sounds like a clever and simple explanation of why there may be a head traumaAlzheimers disease link but it has been questioned by Mehta and coworkers61 from Erasmus University Medical School in Rotterdam. They studied 6,645 people, aged 55 years or older, who had suffered mild head trauma with loss of consciousness and reported that it was not a risk factor for dementia or Alzheimers disease in the elderly, nor did it promote the latter disorder in those with the APO E genotype. Clearly, the issue is still debatable although it may be that the head trauma must be severe before it promotes early onset of Alzheimers disease.
Chronic Stress
Investigators from Rush University Medical Center in Chicago have been taking part in the previously described Religious Orders Study evaluating aging in Catholic nuns, priests, and brothers.62-63 As part of this research project nearly 800 participants about 75 years of age were evaluated for some 5 years. To assess their propensity to stress, they were questioned about worrying and feelings of being tense or jittery. During this study, 140 of these volunteers developed Alzheimers disease. The collected data established that those in the 90th stress percentile were twice as likely to develop this form of dementia as those in the 10th percentile. Simply put, worriers were twice as likely to develop Alzheimers disease as were more laid back individuals. The study also established that stress level was related to the rate of decline in episodic memory. Worriers, for example, were least capable of remembering word lists. This association between chronic stress and the risk of developing Alzheimers disease remained even after controlling for factors such as depression and cognitive activity level.
76
Osteoporosis
A recent longitudinal study followed 987 men and women with an average age of 76 years for a further 13 years.64 Its aim was to examine possible links between bone mass and dementia. Interestingly, those women with the lowest bone mass measurements at the beginning of this study proved twice as likely to subsequently develop dementia as those with the densest, strongest bones. This relationship did not occur in men, suggesting that it may be linked to declining estrogen levels.65
Diabetes Mellitus
Evidence from the previously described Religious Order Study also suggests that diabetes is associated with both a higher likelihood of developing Alzheimers disease and a greater rate of decline in perceptual speed.66 This risk is independent of a history of stroke and whether or not an individual has the APO E4 genotype. These conclusions were based on evidence from 869 older volunteers, none of whom had dementia when the study began. Those individuals were evaluated annually for up to 8 years in order to determine whether they were developing Alzheimers disease or other forms of dementia. Cognitive function testing was also carried out. Amongst these volunteers were 125 (14.4%) who had diabetes. They were examined regularly for an average of 5.1 years. It was determined that such Catholic volunteers with diabetes had a 73 percent higher chance of developing Alzheimers disease than controls, even after the impacts of age, gender, and education had been accounted for. Similarly, Luchsinger and coworkers67 investigated the impact of diabetes mellitus on the incidence of Alzheimers disease in American Blacks and Hispanics. In a sample of 1,262 elderly subjects, followed for an average of 4.3 years, Alzheimers
77
disease had an adjusted relative risk of 1.3 for those with diabetes as compared with those without it. These data were consistent with the belief that there is a modest relationship between diabetes and Alzheimers disease.
Childhood Education
There is considerable evidence from a variety of sources that those with relatively poor childhood education are more likely to suffer from Alzheimers disease as they age. De Ronchi and coworkers68 at the University of Bologna studied 495 elderly people of high to middle socioeconomic class. Those without education were 4.7 times more likely (after adjustment for age, gender, and occupation) to show signs of dementia than those with some formal education when young. This relationship was especially obvious amongst the youngest members of the study, aged 61 to 69 years, where the relative risk of dementia associated with no education was an amazing 139.5. Interestingly, this figure decreased with age. De Ronchi and colleagues concluded that early childhood education was critical in reducing the probability of developing Alzheimers disease later in life. Support for this view came from Hall and associates69 of the Indiana University School of Medicine who studied links between Alzheimers disease and the level of education and location of childhood residence in 2,212 African Americans, 65 years or older, living in Indianapolis. They concluded that rural residence combined with 6 or less years of education was linked to a higher risk of developing Alzheimers disease amongst members of their sample. Moceri and colleagues,70 who conducted a case-control study in Seattle with 393 Alzheimers patients and 379 controls for comparison also confirmed an early-life childhood and adolescent environment link to Alzheimers disease risk. Similar results were reported from Finland by Kaplan and coworkers.71
78
Aging
If, as has been previously argued, the preeminent risk factor in sporadic Alzheimers disease is not genetic, then what is it?72 Clearly, there must be other crucial causal variables(s) driving the dementia pandemic. The most obvious of these is simply the process of growing older. Unfortunately, short of deliberately increasing the mortality rate, an alternative that is promoted by right to die organizations such as The Hemlock Society,73 there is little that can be done to halt the impact of an aging population on the incidence of Alzheimers disease, until society is willing to accept the true cause of this form of dementia. The evidence that the incidence and prevalence of Alzheimers disease increases with aging is overwhelming. In the USA, the prevalence of severe dementia (much of it Alzheimers disease), occurring amongst those aged 65 to 74 is roughly 1 percent, compared to 25 percent for those over 84.74 There is a disputed suggestion that the risk of developing dementia may decline after age 84 is reached,75 but this hypothesis appears to be in conflict with the results of detailed surveys of the elderly.76 Evans and coworkers,77 for example, found that in San Marino, an urban working class community of some 32,000 inhabitants, an estimated 10.3 percent of the population aged over 65 had probable Alzheimers disease. The prevalence of this disorder increased steadily with aging, from 3.0 percent at age 65 to 74 years to 18.7 percent for those aged 75 to 84. This trend continued so that 47.2 percent of those 85 years or older were diagnosed as suffering from probable Alzheimers disease. This age-related increase in dementia was identified again in San Marino.78 At age 67 only 1.8 percent of the population suffered from it, a figure that rose to 25.0 percent in those 87 years of age. The general situation was summarized by Jorm, Korten, and Henderson79 who, after a survey of the
79
international literature, concluded that dementia prevalence rates reflected the age of the sample population, doubling every 5.1 years.
SUMMARY
The evidence presented in this chapter, drawn mainly from longitudinal and case-control studies, clearly shows that certain health conditions increase the future likelihood of developing Alzheimers disease. These include high blood pressure, elevated cholesterol and homocysteine, chronic stress, head trauma, depression, osteoporoosis, and diabetes mellitus. What is less obvious is whether the genetic aberration(s) that predispose to Alzheimers disease and/or the environmental factors that trigger these genotypes also cause such health states, or whether they themselves promote dementia directly. Does high blood pressure, for example, increase the risk of Alzheimers disease directly or do the variable(s) that cause hypertension also promote this form of dementia? Other factors that appear implicated in a greater risk of developing Alzheimers disease include poor diet and obesity, and a lack of childhood education. Aging, of course, is also a very key variable, while the jury is still out on the significance of smoking in the etiology of Alzheimers disease.
80
REFERENCES
1. 2. 3. Fotuhi, M. (2003). The memory cure. New York: McGraw-Hill. Ibid. Launer, L.J., Ross, G.W., Petrovitch, H., Masaki, K., Foley, D., White, L.R., and Havlik, R.J. (2000). Midlife blood pressure and dementia: The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Neurobiology of Aging, 21(1), 49-55. Petrovitch, H., White, L.R., Izmirilian, G., Ross, G.W., Havlik, R.J., Markesbery, W., Nelson, J., Davis, D.G., Hardman, J., Foley, D.J., and Launer, L.J. (2000). Midlife blood pressure and neuritic plaques, neurofibrillary tangles, and brain weight at death: The HAAS, Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Neurobiology of Aging, 21(1), 57-62. Fotuhi, op. cit. Forette, F., Seux, M.L., Staessen, J.L., Thijs, L., Babarskiene, M.R., Babeanu, S., Bossini, A., Fagard, R., Gil-Extemera, B., Laks, T., Kobalava, Z., Sarti, C., Tuomilehto, J., Vanhanen, H., Webster, J., Yodfat, Y., Birkenhager, W.H., and Systolic Hypertension in Europe Investigators (2002). The prevention of dementia with antihypertensive treatment: New evidence from the Systolic Hypertension in Europe (Syst-Eur) study. Archives of Internal Medicine, 162(18), 2046-2052. Fotuhi, op. cit. Ibid. Sparks, L. cited in Ibid.
4.
5. 6.
7. 8. 9.
10. Simons, M., Keller, P., De Stroops, B., Beyreuther, K., Dotti, C.G., and Simons, K. (1998). Cholesterol depletion inhibits the generation of -amyloid in hippocampal neurons. Neurobiology, 95(ll), 6460-6464. 11. Kivipelto, M., Helkala, E.L., Laakso, M.P., Hanninen, T., Hallikainen, M., Alhainen, K., Soininen, H., Tuomilehto, J., and Nissinen, A. (2001). Midlife vascular risk factors and Alzheimers disease in later life: Longitudinal, population-based study. British Medical Journal, 322(7300), 1447-1451. 12. Yaffe, K., Barrett-Connor, E., Lin, F., and Grady, D. (2002). Serum lipoprotein levels, statin use, and cognitive function in older women. Archives of Neurology, 59(3), 378-384. 13. Kawas, C., and Corrada, M. (2002). Paper presented at the American Academy of Neurology, April 2002, Denver, Colorado, cited by Fotuhi op. cit., 89.
81
14. Jick, H., Zornberg, G.L., Jick, S.S., Seshadri, S. and Drachman, D.A. (2000) Statins and the risk of dementia. Lancet 356(9242), 16271631. 15. Goodman A. (2002). Statins are promising therapeutic tools for Alzheimers disease. Neurology Today 2(3), 22-25. 16. Green, R.C., Jayakumar, P., Benke, K., Farrer, L., McNagny, S.C., and Cupples, L.A. (2002). Statin use is associated with reduced risk of Alzheimers disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 23, S273-274. Abstract 1025. 17. Tan, Z.S., Seshadri, S., Beiser, A., Wilson, P.W., Kiel, D.P., Tocco, M., DAgostino, R.B., and Wolf, P.A. (2003). Plasma total cholesterol level as a risk factor for Alzheimers disease: The Framingham Study. Archives of Internal Medicine, 163(9), 1053-1057. 18. Challem, J., and Dolby, V. (1996). Homocysteine: The new cholesterol. New Canaan, CT: Keats Publishing. 19. Boushey, C.J., Beresford, S.A.A., Omenn, G.S., and Motulsky, A.G. (1995). A quantitative assessment of plasma homocysteine as a risk factor for vascular disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 274, 1049-1057. 20. Arinami, T., Yamada, N., Yamakawa-Kobayashi, K., Hamaguchi, H., and Toru, N. (1997). Methyletetrahydrofolate reductase variant and schizophrenia/depression. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 74(5), 526-528. 21. Seshadri, S., Beiser, A., Selhub, J., Jacques, P.F., Rosenberg, I.H., DAgostino, R.B., Wilson, P.W., and Wolf, P.A. (2002). Plasma homocysteine as a risk factor for dementia and Alzheimers disease. New England Journal of Medicine, 346(7), 476-483. 22. Miller, J.W., Green, R., Ramas, M.I., Allen, L.H., Mungas, D.M., Jagust, W.J., and Haan, M.N. (2003). Homocysteine and cognitive function in the Sacramento area Latino study on aging. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 78(3), 441-447. 23. Cartford, M.C., Gemma, C., and Bickford, B.C. (2002). Eighteen-monthold Fischer 344 rats fed a spinach-enriched diet show improved delay classical eyeblink conditioning and reduced expression of Tumor Necrosis Factor Alpha (TNFalpha) and TNFbeta in the cerebellum. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(14), 5813-5816. 24. Galli, R.L., Shukitt-Hale, B., Youdim, K.A., and Joseph, J.A. (2002). Fruit polyphenolics and brain aging: Nutritional inventions targeting age-related neuronal and behavioral deficits. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 959, 128-132.
82
25. Lim, G.P., Chu, T., Yang, F., Beech, W., Frautschy, S.A., and Cole, G.M. (2001). The curry spice curcumin reduces oxidative damage and amyloid pathology in an Alzheimers transgenic mouse. Journal of Neuroscience 21, (21), 8370-8377. 26. Fotuhi, op.cit. 27. Ibid. 28. Pro Health Network (2003). Alzheimers News: Sage improves memory, study shows. http://www.prohealthnetwork.com/library/show article.cfm/ID/2045/T 29. Tidesley, N.T., Kennedy, D.O., Perry, E.K., Ballard, C.G., Savelev, S., Wesnes, K.A., and Scholey, A.B. (2003). Salvia lavandulaefolia (Spanish Sage) enhances memory in healthy young volunteers. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behaviour, 75, 669-674. 30. Pro Health Network, op. cit. 31. Engelhart, M.J., Geerlings, M.I., Ruitenberg, A., van Swieten, J.C., Hofman, A., and Witteman, J.C. (2002). Dietary intake of antioxidants and risk of Alzheimers disease: Food for thought. Journal of the American Medical Association, 287(24), 3223-3229. 32. Morris, M.C., Evans, D.A., Bienias, J.L., Tangney, C.C., Bennett, D.A., Aggarwal, N., Wilson, R.S., and Scherr, P.A. (2002). Dietary intake of antioxidant nutrients and the risk of incident Alzheimers disease in a biracial community study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 287(24), 3230-3237. 33. Engelhart et. al., op. cit. 34. Grant, W.B. (1999). Dietary links to Alzheimers disease: 1999 update. Journal of Alzheimers disease, 1(4,5), 197-201. 35. Barberger-Gateau, P., Letenneur, L., Deschamps, V., Prs, K., JeanFranois Dartigues, J.F., and Renaud, S. (2002). Fish, meat, and risk of dementia: Cohort study. British Medical Journal, 325, 932933. 36. Arbor Clinical Nutrition Update #177 (December 18, 2003). Fish and Alzheimers disease. http://arborcom.com 37. Grant, op. cit. 38. Barberger-Gateau et. al., op. cit. 39. Arbor Clinical Nutrition Update #177, op. cit. 40. Correa Leite, M.L., Nicolosi, A., Cristina, S., Hauser, W.A., and Nappi, G. (2001). Nutrition and cognitive deficit in the elderly: A population study. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 55(12), 1053-1058.
83
41. Wang, H.X., Wahlin, A., Basun, H., Fastbom, J., Winblad, B., and Fratiglioni, L. (2001). Vitamin B(12) and folate in relation to the development of Alzheimers disease. Neurology, 56(9), 1188-1194. 42. Gustafson, D., Rothenberg, E., Blennow, K., Steen, B., and Skoog, I. (2003). An 18-year follow-up of overweight and risk of Alzheimers disease. Archives of Internal Medicine, 163(13), 1524-1528. 43. Lee, P.N. (1993). Smoking and Alzheimers disease: A review of epidemiological evidence. http://www.library.ucsf.edu/tobacco/batco/ html/15400/15445/ 44. Thompson, C.(n.d.). Smokers Rights Action Group. Alzheimers disease is associated with non-smoking. http://www.forces.org/evidence/ carol/carol15.htm 45. Doll, R., Peto, R., Boreham, J., and Sutherland, I. (2000). Smoking and dementia in male British doctors: A prospective study. British Medical Journal, 320(7242), 1097-1102. 46. Wang, H-X., Fratiglioni, L., Frisoni, G.B., Viitanen, M., and Winblad, B. (1999). Smoking and the occurrence of Alzheimers disease: Crosssectional and longitudinal data in a population-based study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 149(7), 640-644. 47. Action on Smoking and Health (ASH) (1999). Debunked: Smoking and Alzheimers effects minimization. http://www.no-smoking.org/ april99/04-06-99-1.htm 48. Lee, op. cit. 49. Almeida, O.P., Hulse, G.K., Lawrence, D., and Flicker, L. (2002). Smoking as a risk factor for Alzheimers disease: Contrasting evidence from a systematic review of case-control and cohort studies. Addiction, 97(1), 15-28. 50. Ott, A., Slooter, A.J., Hofman, A., van Harskamp, F., Witteman, J.C., van Broeckhoven, C., van Duijn, C.M., and Breteler, M.M. (1998). Smoking and risk of dementia and Alzheimers disease in a population-based cohort study: The Rotterdam Study. Lancet, 351(9119), 1840-1843. 51. BBC News (1998). Health, Smoking may double the risk of Alzheimers. http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/health/115829.stm 52. British Medical Journal Press Release, 20 April 2000. Smoking does not protect against dementia or Alzheimers disease. http://www. eurekalert.org/pub_releases/2000-04/BMJ-Sdnp-2004100.php 53. alzinfo.org. The Alzheimers information site (2003). Symptoms of depression linked to onset of Alzheimers. http://www.aalzinfo.org/ news/5_22.aspx
84
54. Ibid. 55. Wilson, R.S., Barnes, L.L., Mendes de Leon, C.F., Aggarwal, N.T., Schneider, J.S., Bach, J., Pilat, J., Beckett, L.A., Arnold, S.E., Evans, D.A., and Bennett, D.A. (2002). Depressive symptoms, cognitive decline, and the risk of Alzheimers disease in older persons. Neurology, 59(3), 364-370. 56. Green, R.C., Cupples, L.A., Kurz, A., Auerbach, S., Go, R., Sadovnick, D., Duara, R., Kukull, W.A., Chui, H., Edeki, T., Griffith, P.A., Friedland, R.P., Bachman, D., and Farrer, L. (2003). depression is a risk factor for Alzheimers disease: The MIRAGE Study. Archives of Neurology, 60(5), 753-759. 57. Salib, E., and Hillier, V. (1997). Head injury and the risk of Alzheimers disease: A case control study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12(3): 363-368. 58. Launer, L.J., Anderson, K., Dewey, M.E., Letenneur, L., Ott, A., Amaducci, L.A., Brayne, C., Copeland, J.R., Dartigues, J.F., KraghSorensen, P., Lobo, A., Martinez-Lage, J.M., Stijnen, T., and Hofman, A. (1999). Rates and risk factors for dementia and Alzheimers disease: Results from EURODEM pooled analyses. EURODEM Incidence Research Group and Work Groups European Studies of Dementia. Neurology, 52(1), 78-84. 59. Nemetz, P.N., Leibson, C., Naessens, J.M., Beard, M., Kokmen, E., Annegers, J.F., and Kurland, L.T. (1999). Traumatic brain injury and time of onset of Alzheimers disease: A population-based study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 149(1), 32-40. 60. Nicoll, J.A., Roberts, G.W., and Graham, D.L. (1996). Amyloid betaprotein, APOE genotype and head injury. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 777, 271-275. 61. Mehta, K.M., Ott, A., Kalmijn, S., Slooter, A.J., van Duijn, C.M., Hofman, A., and Breteler, M.M. (1999). Head trauma and risk of dementia and Alzheimers disease: The Rotterdam Study. Neurology, 53(9), 1959-1962. 62. Earthlink. Stress can lead to Alzheimers disease. http://www.health scout.com/news/43/8007548/main.htm 63. Wilson, R.S., Evans, D.A., Bienlas, J.L., Mendes De Leon, C.F., Schneider, J.A., and Bennett, D.A. (2003). Proneness to psychological distress is associated with risk of Alzheimers disease. Neurology, 61(11), 1479-1485. 64. Dr. Merolas Newsletter. 5/31/03. Osteoporosis linked to Alzheimers. http://www.mercola.com/2003/may/31/alzheimers_osteoporosis. htm
85
65. Ibid. 66. Defeat Diabetes Foundation Inc. (2003). Diabetes linked to development of Alzheimers disease. http://www.defeatdiabetes.org/ Articles/alzheimers030416.htm 67. Luchsinger, J.A., Tang, M.X., Stern, Y., Shea, S., and Mayeux, R. (2001). Diabetes mellitus and risk of Alzheimers disease and dementia with stroke in a multiethnic cohort. American Journal of Epidemiology, 154(7), 635-641. 68. De Ronchi, D., Fratiglioni, L., Rucci, P., Paternico, A., Graziani, S., and Dalmonte, E. (1998). The effect of education on dementia occurrence in an Italian population with middle to high socioeconomic status. Neurology, 50(5), 1231-1238. 69. Hall, K.S., Gao, S., Unverzagt, F.W., and Hendrie, H.C. (2000). Low education and childhood rural residence: Risk for Alzheimers disease in African Americans. Neurology, 54(1), 95-99. 70. Moceri, V.M., Kukull, W.A., Emanuel, I., van Belle, G., and Larson, E.B. (2000). Early-life risk factors and the development of Alzheimers disease. Neurology, 54(2), 415-420. 71. Kaplan, G.A., Turrell, G., Lynch, J.W., Everson, S.A., Helkala, E.L., and Salonen, J.T. (2001). Childhood socioeconomic position and cognitive function in adulthood. International Journal of Epidemiology, 30(2), 256-263. 72. Foster, H.D. (2002). Why the preeminent risk factor in sporadic Alzheimers disease cannot be genetic. Medical Hypotheses, 59(1), 5761. 73. The Hemlock Society USA. End of Life Choices. www.endoflifechoices.org 74. Cross, P.D., Gurland, B.J. (1986). The Epidemiology of Dementing Disorders. Contract report prepared for the Office of Technology Assessment, US Congress. 75. Mortimer, J.A., and Hutton, J.T. (1995). Epidemiology and etiology of Alzheimers disease. In J.T. Hutton and A.D. Kenny (Eds.), Senile dementia of the Alzheimers type. New York: AR Liss. 76. Sayetta, R.B. (1986). Rates of senile dementia in Alzheimers type in the Baltimore Longitudinal study. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 39, 271-286. 77. Evans, D.A., Funkenstein, H.B., Albert, M.S., Scherr, P.A., and Cook, N.R. (1989). Prevalence of Alzheimers disease in a community population of older persons higher than previously reported. Journal of the American Medical Association, 262(18), 2551-2556.
86
78. DAlessandro, R., Gallassi, R., Benassi, G., Morreale, A., and Lugaresi, E. (1988). Dementia in subjects over 65 years of age in the Republic of San Marino. British Journal of Psychology, 153, 182-186. 79. Jorm, A.F., Korten, A.E., and Henderson, A.S. (1987). The prevalence of dementia: A quantitative integration of the literature. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 75(50), 465-479.
87
When the vet had finished his visit we had the opportunity to chat. I asked him what was, in his opinion, the cause of these many cases of grass tetany on his clients farms. He replied that he did not know. I then put to him the question: Do you know to what extent your client employs potash on his grassland? The reply has remained fixed in my memory: This question concerns the farmer. My role is to care for sick animals and cure them. It think that it is this idea which should not dominate veterinary and medical science in the future. It is not merely a question of healing the animal or Man stricken by disease, it is necessary to heal the soil so as not to have to heal the animal or Man. We concentrate our efforts on the results and neglect the causes.
A. Voisin, Soil, Grass and Cancer
88
The relentless American focus on health sustains popular magazines and fills the airwaves with health claims for cereals, vitamins, pills, and exercise equipment. We are told how to eat the right foods and avoid the wrong ones, practise safe sex, work out properly, get regular check-ups, think healthy thoughts, and accept responsibility for our own destinies. But no matter how diligently we take care of ourselves, no matter how often we say no to unsafe sex or dangerous drugs or choose the salad instead of the hamburger, we cannot control the influences of the world around us. Where you live and work, what you eat and drink and breathe, what happened to you just before birth all those things play critical roles in determining your prospects for health. But when illness is not a matter of personal prevention, scientists and media alike become strangely reticent. Devra Davis (2002)1
In the early 1980s, I was dedicated to searching for the causes of cancer.2 This illness does not strike at random. Patterns in Cancer Mortality in the United States: 1950-19673 clearly revealed very significant differences in the geographical distribution of death from various types of malignant neoplasms. This volume, Monograph 33 of the United States National Cancer Institute, contains age adjusted death rates per 100,000 population, for 65 specific or subgroups of cancers, and for the disease as a whole. In total, it provides data on the type of cancer which caused the deaths of more than 4,600,000 people during the
89
period 1950 through 1967. These data had been taken from death certificates and mapped at the state scale. Correlation of these 66 cancer groups with each other allowed the identification of those malignant neoplasms that have the same environmental triggers and those that do not. Breast cancer, for example, tends to be elevated where cancers of the ovary and digestive tract are also common, but shows a marked negative relationship with both skin cancer and melanoma.4 I developed this technique further in a later book, Health, Disease and the Environment.5 In this volume, and in subsequent papers, I correlated (that is compared statistically) maps of the incidence, prevalence, or death rates from 74 different health problems, ranging from SIDS, diabetes mellitus, and multiple sclerosis to cancer, Parkinsons disease, and schizophrenia. Again, it was obvious that some of these illnesses tended to occur at high levels together in the same populations, while others never did. This suggested to me that a variety of environmental triggers were involved, some of which probably either helped cause, or prevent, more than one illness. To try to identify what these triggers were, I developed a computer database containing information on the spatial distributions, in the USA, of 219 natural or man-made environmental substances6 ranging from sunlight and precipitation through road salt use, dieldrin, lindane, and arsenic in surface waters to hay, potatoes, tobacco, and cotton production. Extensive data on soil geochemistry also were included in this database. These 219 potential triggers were then correlated with the spatial patterns of all of the 66 cancers or groups of cancers to identify which had either very similar, or very different, distributions. The thousands of resulting correlations were suggestive of links between many cancers and soils containing lower than normal levels of calcium and selenium. Death rates from such malignant neoplasms in the USA were also very high where
90
road salt was used extensively and soil mercury levels were elevated. Although I have read and published widely on cancer since 1986, when this original work was published, nothing has made me alter my opinion on this topic. Rather, the evidence is now much stronger that these triggers are involved in the etiology of cancer. I have since used this environmental database to explore for triggers in numerous other diseases and disorders, identifying, for example, what I believe to be a lack of selenium in myocardial infarction7 and iodine deficiencies in Sudden Infant Death Syndrome (SIDS),8 amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS), multiple sclerosis (MS), and Parkinsons disease.9 From this research it became obvious that it is unrealistic to study the impacts of single environmental variables on human health.6 Many such factors interact either antagonistically or synergistically, so that there is an increase or decrease in health impacts when individuals are exposed to them in combination. Most, if not all, essential trace and bulk elements are subject to these types of interactions which can mitigate or aggravate deficiencies or excesses. Mercury, for example, combines with selenium in the soil to form mercury selenide which is very insoluble and, therefore, reduces selenium uptake by crops. As a result, people living in areas with high soil mercury levels tend to suffer unusually high rates of cancer because they are largely denied the beneficial protective effects of selenium.10 While calcium may, in itself, be protective against many cancers, especially of the digestive tract,11 it also increases soil alkalinity, encouraging the movement of selenium into crops and animal feed. This, of course, means that people who live in calcareous regions are less likely to be selenium deficient. While road salt may be carcinogenic, it also mobilizes mercury, the selenium antagonist.12 It is clear, therefore, that in cancer, various substances react together to increase or decrease risk.
91
Clearly, the impact of environmental variables on health must be studied in combination. What follows is an effort to identify those environmental factors that either individually, or in combinations, trigger development of Alzheimers disease, especially in those people who are genetically susceptible. Attention is also paid to substances that may be able to block this process.
ALUMINUM
The best available review of the possible role of aluminum as a trigger for Alzheimers disease is published by Erik Jansson,13 in the Journal of Alzheimers disease, in 2001. There is no doubt that aluminum accumulates in the body. It has been estimated, for example, that it is deposited at a rate of some 6 mcg per year in the human brain.14 As a consequence, autopsies have shown that the brains of normal elderly people, aged between 75 to 101, contain far more aluminum than those of younger people, who died at ages of 32 to 46.15 Specifically, in this study, each member of the older group had an average of 28 times as much aluminum in their hippocampus and 19 times as much of this metal in their frontal cortex as did those dying younger. Interestingly, although not showing signs of dementia at death, senile plaques and neurofibrillary tangles were common in the elderly and their densities seemed to increase with aluminum levels. Evidence from other autopsies, however, suggest that Alzheimers disease patients generally tend to have roughly twice the aluminum brain content found in the non-demented of similar ages.16-17 Where does all this metal come from? Aluminum forms about 8 percent of the Earths crust and so is ubiquitous. It has been known to be a human neurotoxin since 1886, when it was discovered to have caused brain damage in Prussian army amputees who had been treated with alum (a double sulphate
92
of aluminum and potassium) to staunch the bleeding of their wounds.18 The first animal study to link aluminum to brain damage was published in 1937.19 There is an extensive literature, reviewed by Jansson,20 which has established aluminums role in kidney dialysis, encephalopathy, cognitive impairment, childhood learning disabilities, and brain damage to workers in the welding, coal mining, and smelting industries. All this evidence establishes beyond doubt that aluminum can cause serious brain malfunctions. Most of the research trying to establish where the aluminum deposits in the aging human brain come from has focussed on dissolved aluminum in drinking water. According to Jansson,21 by the year 2000, there had been 18 drinking water studies that had linked aluminum levels to elevated Alzheimers disease and to cognitive impairment of the elderly. In contrast, five epidemiological studies could identify no effect. Jansson,22 however, points out that most studies claiming no relationship between drinking water levels of aluminum and Alzheimers disease incidence, prevalence, or mortality involve very low levels of exposure, small sample size, and a lack of inclusion of modifying factors, making a statistical resolution over background variability unlikely. Simply put, the great bulk of the evidence from epidemiology and geography supports a link between high levels of aluminum in drinking water and elevated incidence of Alzheimers disease. Those studies that dont are either very small in scale or poorly designed. Drinking water usually contains between 0.01 and 0.15 mg per litre of aluminum, but some potable water may have as much as 0.40 mg per litre or more.23 While this represents only a small percentage of total dietary aluminum, it is possible that because of repetitious exposure and increased species solubility, aluminum from drinking water may provide a large component of the total aluminum absorbed. Priest24 has shown
93
that the fraction of dietary aluminum entering the tissues may vary from as much as 0.01 for aluminum citrate to 0.0001 for insoluble species, such as aluminum silicates and oxides. Most of this absorbed aluminum tends to be excreted if kidney action is normal, but about 5 percent is deposited in the body, mainly in the skeleton.25 It was established by Sohler and coworkers26 as early as 1981, however, that in 400 psychiatric outpatients in New Jersey, memory loss increased as blood aluminum levels rose. Four geographical studies that clearly support a role for aluminum in Alzheimers disease will now be discussed in more detail. Martyn and colleagues27 conducted a survey of 88 English and Welsh county districts to find out the rates of Alzheimers disease in people under the age of 70. These were estimated from the records of computerized tomographic scanning units. It was found that the risk of developing Alzheimers disease appeared to be 1.5 times higher in districts where the mean water aluminum concentration exceeded 0.11 mg per litre. There were no such associations between aluminum and other forms of dementia. Vogt28 studied links between aluminum in Norwegian drinking water and mortality from both senile and presenile dementia. He estimated that some 50 to 70 percent of these deaths were, in fact, due to Alzheimers disease and related conditions. Vogt29 divided Norway into five zones on the basis of the levels of aluminum in lakes, the main source of potable water, and was able to show that as the aluminum content of drinking water rose so too did the death rate from age-related dementia. This cause of death was particularly common in the zone that experienced both elevated aluminum and the highest levels of acid rain. The latter, of course, would raise the solubility of aluminum and increase levels in drinking water. A national survey of the Norwegian Institute of Gerontology also demonstrated
94
that, within psychiatric nursing homes, counties receiving the worst acid rain accounted for the largest number of dementia patients per 100,000 inhabitants.30 Four years later, Flaten31 used more sophisticated techniques, such as correlation and regression analysis, to confirm that in Norway, mortality involving dementia seemed linked to the aluminum content of drinking water. Clear evidence was provided in 1988 that excess aluminum in potable water can affect memory. An accident at an English water supply plant resulted in drinking water that contained enormously elevated levels of aluminum sulphate being drunk by the local population. Memory loss was an extremely common complaint amongst those unfortunate enough to use such contaminated water.32 Significant evidence of the link between dementia and aluminum comes from McLachlans Ontario study involving 668 autopsy-verified Alzheimers brains.33 These demonstrated that the risk of developing Alzheimers disease had been about 2.5 times greater in individuals from communities that drank water that contained more than 100 mcg per litre of aluminum than it had been in those from areas where the potable water had contained less than this level of aluminum. McLachlans results were even more spectacular for those who had drunk water that contained 175 mcg per litre of aluminum. Depending on how those patients were grouped, the odds ratio of developing Alzheimers disease varied from 6.7 to 8.14. That is, their brains were some 7 to 8 times more likely to show the characteristic signs of Alzheimers disease if such patients had normally consumed water that was very high in aluminum. Several authors have attempted to quantify the strength of the association between Alzheimers disease and aluminum. Forbes and McLachlan,34 for example, studied this link in the very elderly, those aged 85 years or more. They discovered that,
95
after controlling for six other factors, such as fluoride, silicic acid, iron, pH, and turbidity, those living in districts that supplied drinking water that contained more than 250 mcg of aluminum per litre were almost 10 times more likely to develop Alzheimers disease. This confirmed an earlier Ontario longitudinal study35 which established that men 75 years and older, who were drinking water containing at least 0.0847 mg per litre of aluminum, were 1.72 times as likely to show impaired mental functioning. Similarly, after statistical control for five other variables, Alzheimers mortality displays an odds ratio of 3.54 for those who had drunk water that contained at least 0.336 mg per litre of aluminum.36 A more recent 8 year longitudinal study involved 3,777 people aged 65 years and older who lived in southwest France in 1988 to 1989. This confirmed that double the risk of developing Alzheimers disease occurred in those who drank water with an aluminum concentration greater than 0.1 mg per litre.37 Looking for a link between Alzheimers disease and total drinking water aluminum may be too simplistic. In 2000, Gauthier and coworkers38 described a case control study in which the chemical characteristics of the water historically drunk by 58 elderly Alzheimers patients were compared with those of potable water used by age and gender matched non-demented controls. This was conducted in the Saguenay-Lac-Saint-Jean region of Quebec. Aluminum specification was assessed using standard analytical protocols. Long-term drinking water exposure (from 1945 to the onset of Alzheimers disease) was estimated for total, total dissolved Al, monomeric organic Al, monomeric inorganic Al, polymeric Al, Al(3+), AlOH, AlF, AlH(3)SiO(2+)(4), and AlSO(4). While there was no obvious relationship between total aluminum in potable water and Alzheimers disease, after adjustment for educational level, family cases of the disorder and the APO E4 allele, exposure to monomeric aluminum clearly was associated with this form of dementia (odds ratio 2.67).
96
The importance of monomeric (single molecule) aluminum has been confirmed again by a more recent study conducted by Prolo and colleagues,39 in northwest Italy, where the drinking water contained between 5 and 1,220 mcg per litre of total aluminum. Levels of monomeric aluminum, (the type of this element most easily able to enter human cells), ranged from 5 to 300 mcg per litre. These researchers from the University of California at Los Angeles established that Alzheimers disease was most common where drinking water levels of monomeric aluminum were highest. They also discovered that monomeric aluminum interfered with cell function in cultures, accelerating cell death especially in the presence of beta-amyloid protein. Not all geographical and epidemiological studies have focused on the role of drinking water aluminum in the etiology of Alzheimers disease. Aluminum is always present in ambient air, but many industrial workers inhale far more than the 4.4 mcg average daily intake from this source.40 Such inhaled aluminum appears to be neurotoxic under certain special conditions. To illustrate, silica has a well known ability to protect against aluminum and vice versa. For this reason, between 1944 and 1979, many miners were given aluminum powder as a prophylaxis against silicotic lung disease. An aluminum bomb was let off after each shift and the miners inhaled its dust in an effort to protect themselves against silica. During a study conducted during 1988 and 1989, miners who had been exposed to aluminum dust in this way were found to show abnormal cognitive deficits, their neurological problems increasing with the duration of their exposure.41 The use of water that contained high levels of aluminum in the treatment of kidney patients was also linked to significant increases in dialysis encephalopathy, a degenerative disorder of the brain that had some similarities to Alzheimers disease. Parkinson and colleagues42 demonstrated that in Newcastle,
97
for example, of the 10 patients in their twenties on home dialysis, none survived for 2 years; all died of dialysis encephalopathy, osteomalacia, or unexplained cardiorespiratory failure. Beyond this, Rogers and Simon43 identified an 8.6 fold increased risk of developing Alzheimers disease amongst residents of the Loretto Geriatric Center of Syracuse, New York who had eaten foods with aluminum additives or that had been cooked or stored in aluminum containers. It is probable, however, that much of the aluminum reaching the brain is absorbed through the skin. Certainly, aluminum applied to the skin of laboratory animals can reach the brain,44 as can that injected into the nose.45 It must also be remembered that a wide range of drugs, including antacids and buffered aspirin, and vaccines and deodorants contain aluminum. Most cans are now also made of aluminum and their contents are increasingly polluted with this metal.46 Nevertheless, Templer, Chicora, and Russell47 have shown recently that there is a statistically significant association between the US Alzheimers disease age-adjusted death rate for the period 1979 to 1991, and surficial sediment aluminum concentration, established at the state level by the US Geological Survey. That is, the higher the aluminum levels in soils and sediments, the more elevated the states Alzheimers disease death rate. In all races, in people aged 65 and over, the calculated product-moment correlation coefficient was 0.27 (p=0.033). This statistical measure was 0.23 (p=0.064) in the white population alone.
SILICA
There is evidence that amorphous aluminosilicates may promote the formation of beta amyloid protein and the senile plaques that are so characteristic of Alzheimers disease.48 If this is the case, one might expect that environmental silica
98
would also promote this form of dementia. Geographical and epidemiologic evidence, however, seems to suggest the reverse. In 1995, Taylor and coworkers49 published a paper that examined the relationship between silicon and aluminum in the water supplies of northern England and Early Onset Alzheimers disease. They concluded that as soluble silicon rose in such potable water, soluble aluminum fell, so that at about 3.5 mg per litre of silicon, aluminum levels were less than 25 mcg in all districts. In addition, those below 65 years of age who drank water that contained more than 3 mg per litre of watersoluble silicon had an odds ratio for developing Early Onset Alzheimers disease of 0.8, suggestive of a reduction of risk by silicon of 20 percent. The sample size, however, was too small to be considered statistically significant.50 Two studies involving silicon and Alzheimers disease also have been conducted in France. Jacqmin-Gadda and colleagues,51 for example, examined the relationships between silica and aluminum and early cognitive impairment in 3,777 French elderly, aged 65 years or more. They concluded that: The association between cognitive impairment and aluminum depended on the pH and the concentration of silica: high levels of aluminum appeared to have a deleterious effect when the silica concentration was low, but there was a protective effect when the pH and the silica levels were high. The threshold for an aluminum effect, however, was very low (3.5 mcg per litre) and did not support the hypothesis of a deleterious effect for only high levels of aluminum. As mentioned previously, Rondeau and coworkers52 have conducted an 8 year longitudinal follow-up study on the impact of drinking water quality on the mental health of the elderly in southwestern France. Their data showed that silica in excess of 11.25 mg per litre in potable water was associated with
99
about a 27 percent decline in the risk of developing Alzheimers disease. All three available studies, therefore, suggest that silica is protective against this form of dementia. Interestingly, aquaculturalists have long been aware that the acute toxicity of aluminum to fish is prevented by silicic acid.53 This acid is naturally present at high levels in certain waters as a result of the weathering of the aluminosilicates of rocks and minerals and has a strong and unique affinity for aluminum.54
FLUORIDE
There is growing evidence that suggests that fluoride is, to some degree, protective against Alzheimers disease. Still and Kelley,55 for example, reviewed first admissions of dementia patients, over the age of 55, in three South Carolina hospitals during the period July 1971 to June 1979. Horry County was studied because its water had the highest fluoride content (4.2 mg/ litre) of any in the state. In contrast, the water of the other two counties, Anderson and York, had the states lowest fluoride contents, 0.5 and 0.6 mg/litre, respectively. Only patients with 10 or more years of continuous residence in one of these three counties, prior to hospital admission, were included in the survey. Still and Kelley56 established that although the annual hospital admission rates for vascular dementia were similar for all three counties, that for Alzheimers disease in Horry, the high fluoride county, was only one-fifth the average rate of the other two low fluoride counties. This difference was significant at the 0.01 level. Similarly, in 1986, Liss and Thornton57 reported on a doubleblind randomized trial in which a group of Alzheimers patients received either a placebo or 40-60 mg of sodium fluoride daily. Twelve patients who had been study participants were decoded after 30 to 36 months. In 11 of 12 cases the researchers were
100
correctly able to identify whether the patient had received fluoride or the placebo. This was possible because, it was claimed, there had been an observable difference in the rate of progression of the symptoms between the two groups, with those receiving fluoride deteriorating more slowly. In 1991, Forbes and coworkers58 interviewed surviving members of the Ontario Longitudinal study of Aging and identified 285 who displayed evidence of mental impairment. A further 280 individuals were selected from the same cohort who were matched for both age and gender but showed no sign of mental impairment. Water quality information was then collected about the places of residence of all participants in the study. Forbes and colleagues59 identified high and low aluminum and fluoride drinking water, divided according to concentrations below or above the 50th percentile. It was found, for example, that in men the relative risk of mental impairment varied with the aluminum and fluoride content of the drinking water traditionally drunk by the study participant. Mental impairment was greatest in high aluminum-low fluoride areas (odds ratio 2.7) and lowest in high fluoride areas (odds ratio 0.7). Indeed, the level of fluoride in drinking water seemed more important than the level of aluminum as an indication of the risk of developing mental impairment. In a later paper about the same study, Forbes and coworkers60 provided new data for impaired mental functioning. These had been derived from a logistic regression model, in which the odds ratios for high aluminum and high fluoride were 1.86 and 0.58 respectively. That is, aluminum appeared to promote, and fluoride protect against, Alzheimers disease. Two later studies in which Forbes61-62 was involved also both confirmed that the probability of developing Alzheimers disease decreased as drinking water fluoride levels rose. The first of these was a longitudinal study of 75-year-old Canadians
101
and provided data that indicated that fluoride equal to, or in excess of, 0.88 milligrams per litre was associated with a 36 percent reduction in the risk of developing Alzheimers disease. Results of a second research project published by Forbes and McLachlan,64 of 85-year-old Canadians, indicated that drinking water that contained fluoride levels of 0.5 to 0.98 mg per litre reduced Alzheimers risk by some 30 percent.
and teeth. As a consequence, under steady-state conditions, 99 percent of the fluoride in the body is sequestered in calcified tissues. Most of the rest occurs in the plasma and is available for excretion.69 Given this obvious antagonism between fluoride and calcium one might expect that because the former seems to be protective against Alzheimers disease, the latter element would promote it. However, the available evidence seems to suggest otherwise. Yase and coworkers70 have studied the high incidence of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (which can be associated with Parkinsonian features and dementia), in the Kii Peninsula of Japan, since the early 1960s. The brains of such patients can display neurofibrillary tangles identical to those seen in Alzheimers disease, both in the cerebral cortex and in the brainstem nuclei.71 Interestingly, the water of the two disease foci in the Kii Peninsula is exceptionally low in both calcium and magnesium. This also appears to be true of similar foci of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and Parkinsonism with dementia found in Guam and Western New Guinea.73 The waters of the Kii Peninsula are noted for their crystalline transparency and Fujita74 claims that, because of a lack of mineralization, the Koza River is unable to support fish. Garruto and colleagues75 have analysed numerous drinking water samples from areas with high incidences of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis in the Kii Peninsula and have found them to be exceptionally deficient in both calcium and magnesium. Potable water from these regions generally contained less than 1 to 2 ppm calcium and below 1 ppm magnesium. The significance of the deficiencies has been recently demonstrated by Kihira and coworkers,76 who have established that chronic low calcium and magnesium, high aluminum diet induces neuronal loss in mice. This is not really such a new discovery since in 1992, Armstrong and colleagues77 used such a diet to increase the negative effects of aluminum on mouse brain biopterins.
103
Further evidence that calcium does not promote and may reduce the risk of developing Alzheimers disease was provided by Jacqmin and coworkers78 in their study of 3,777 French men and women, 65 years or older. They discovered a significant protective effect against cognitive impairment in those elderly who drank water containing 75 milligrams per litre or more of calcium.
DRINKING WATER PH
Whatever is triggering Alzheimers disease appears to be very sensitive to water pH. Three studies, conducted by Forbes and his colleagues in Ontario, established that drinking water with a pH in excess of 7.85 is associated with a reduction in the risk of developing this type of dementia. To illustrate, in 1994, data was published from the Ontario Longitudinal study of Aging that showed that potable water with a pH value of between 7.85 and 8.05 was associated with a 60 percent reduced risk of the development of impaired mental functioning, in 75-yearold men.79 This water pH-Alzheimers disease link was supported further by Forbes80 later publication based on an analysis of death certificates of former volunteers in this longitudinal study. These documents were used to establish that, in Canada, water pH in the range of 7.85 to 7.95 had been linked to a 30 percent reduction in Alzheimers disease mortality rates, when compared with consumption of both more acid and alkaline potable waters. The relationship between water pH and this form of dementia appeared to form a U-shaped curve. Further support for the value of drinking water that had a pH in excess of 7.85 was provided by Forbes and McLachlan81 who showed it to be associated with approximately a 50 percent reduction in Alzheimers disease risk in the very elderly. Jacqmin-Gadda
104
and coworkers82 also established that the impact of high aluminum on elderly cognitive impairment in the French was linked to both the pH and silica content of their drinking water. The odds ratio for elderly mental impairment, for example, was 1.30 in water districts that supplied a product that was high in aluminum but low in both pH and silica. In contrast, those drinking high aluminum water that also was elevated in both pH (above 7.35) and silica had only a 0.75 odds ratio of developing elderly cognitive impairment.
SUMMARY
The great bulk of evidence from numerous geographical and epidemiological studies supports a strong link between aluminum consumption, especially monomeric aluminum from drinking water, and an elevated incidence of Alzheimers disease. The negative impact of aluminum, however, appears mitigated by silicic acid, calcium, and magnesium, especially in potable water with a pH of between 7.85 and 8.05. Acidic drinking water that is high in aluminum and lacking in silicic acid, calcium, and magnesium seems to be particularly dangerous. Fluoride also may protect against Alzheimers disease when the pH is high.
105
REFERENCES
1. 2. 3. Davis, D. (2002). When smoke ran like water: Tales of environmental deception and the battle against pollution. New York: Basic Books. Foster, H.D. (1986). Reducing cancer mortality: A geographical perspective. Western Geographical Series, 23, 169 pp. Burbank, F. (1971). Patterns in cancer mortality in the United States: 1950-1967. National Cancer Institute Monograph 33, United States Department of Health, Education and Welfare, 594 pp. Foster, op. cit. Foster, H.D. (1992). Health, Disease and the Environment. London: Belhaven Press. Foster (1986), op. cit. Foster, H.D. (2002). Coxsackie B virus and myocardial infarction. The Lancet, 359, 804. Foster, H.D. (1993). Sudden infant death syndrome: The Bradford Hill Criteria and the evaluation of the thyroxine deficiency hypothesis. Journal of Orthomolecular Medicine, 8(4), 201-225. Foster, H.D. (1999). Parkinsons disease, multiple sclerosis and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: The iodine-dopachrome-glutamate hypothesis. Journal of Orthomolecular Medicine. 14(3), 128-136.
4. 5. 6. 7. 8.
9.
10. Foster (1986), op. cit. 11. Norie, I., and Foster, H.D. (1989). Water quality and cancer of the digestive tract: The Canadian experience. Journal of Orthomolecular Medicine, 4(2), 59-69. 12. Foster, H.D. (1993). Road de-icing salt and cancer: The need for further study. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 85(19), 1603-1605. 13. Jansson, E.T. (2001). Aluminum and Alzheimers disease. Journal of Alzheimers disease, 3, 541-549. 14. Nicolini, M., Zatta, P.F., and Corain, B. (Eds.) (1991). Aluminum in chemistry, biology and medicine. New York: Raven Press. 15. Shimizu, H., Mori, T., Koyama, M., Sekiya, M., and Ooami, H. (1994). A correlative study of aluminum content and aging changes of the brain in non-demented elderly subjects. Nippon Ronem Igakkai Zasshi, 31, 950-960. 16. Bouras, C., Giannakopoulos, P., Good, P.F., Hsu, A., Hof, P.R., and Perl, D.P. (1997). A laser microprobe mass analysis of brain aluminum and iron in Dementia Pubilistica: Comparison with Alzheimers disease. European Neurology 38, 53-58.
106
17. Crapper, D.R., Krishnan, S.S., and Quittkat, S. (1976). Aluminum, neurofibrillary degeneration and Alzheimers disease. Brain, 99(1), 67-69. 18. Siem, quoted in Doellken (1887). Ueber die Wirkung des aluminum besonderer Berucksichtigung der durch das aluminum verursachten Lasionen im Centralnervensystem. Naunyn-Schmiedenbergs, Archiev fur Experimentalle Pathologie Und Pharmakologie, 40, 58120. This reference was sited in Jansson, op. cit. 19. McLachlan, D.R.C. (1994). Personal communication with Jansson, op. cit. 20. Jansson, op. cit. 21. Ibid. 22. Ibid. 23. Doll, R. (1993). Review: Alzheimers disease and environmental aluminum. Age and Ageing, 22, 138-153. 24. Priest, N.D. (1992). The bioavailability and metabolism of aluminum compounds in man. Proceedings of the Nutritional Society - cited by Doll, R. op. cit., 138. 25. Priest, N.D., Newton, D., and Talbot, R.J. (1991). Metabolism of aluminum-26 and gallium-67 in a volunteer following their injection as citrates. UKAEA Report AEA-EE-0206. Harwell Biomedical Research, Harwell. 26. Sohler, A., Pfeiffer, C.C., and Papaionnov, R. (1981). Blood aluminum levels in a psychiatric outpatient population. High aluminum levels related to memory loss. Journal of Orthomolecular Psychiatry, 19(1), 54-60. 27. Martyn, C.N., Barker, D.J.O., Osmond, C., Harris, E.C., Edwardson, J.A., and Lacey, R.F. (1989). Geographical relation between Alzheimers disease and aluminum in drinking water. Lancet, 1(8629), 59-62. 28. Vogt, T. (1986). Water quality and health. Study of a possible relationship between aluminum in drinking water and dementia (in Norwegian, English summary). Sosiale Og Okonomiske Studier, 61, Statistisk Sentralbyra Oslo-Kongsvinger, 60-63. 29. Ibid. 30. Ibid. 31. Flaten, T.P. (1990). Geographical association between aluminum in drinking water and death rates with dementia (including Alzheimers disease), Parkinsons disease and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, in Norway. Environmental Geochemistry and Health, 12(1 and 2), 152-167.
107
32. Gidney, N. (1991). Causes of dementing illness not pinned down. Horizons, February 3, 1991, 5. 33. McLachlan, D.R. (1995). Aluminum and the risk of Alzheimers disease. Environmetrics, 6, 233-238. 34. Forbes, W.F., and McLachlan, D.R.C. (1996). Further thoughts on the aluminum-Alzheimers disease link. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 50, 401-403. 35. Forbes, W.F., McAiney, C.A., Hayward, L.M., and Agwani, N. (1994). Geochemical risk factors for mental functioning, based on the Ontario Longitudinal study of Aging (LSA) II, the role of pH. Canadian Journal on Aging, 14, 830-841, cited by Jansson, op. cit. 36. Forbes, W.R., McAiney, C.A., Hayward, L.M., and Agwani, N. (1995). Geochemical risk factors for mental functioning, based on the Ontario Longitudinal study of Aging (LSA) V, comparisons of the results, relevant to aluminum water concentrations obtained from the LSA and from death certificates mentioning dementia. Canadian Journal on Aging, 14, cited by Jansson, op. cit. 37. Rondeau, V., Commenges, D., Jacqmin-Gadda, H., and Dartigues, J.F. (2000). Relationship between aluminum concentrations in drinking water and Alzheimers disease: An 8-year follow-up study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 154(3), 288-290. 38. Gauthier, E., Fortier, I., Courchesne, F., Pepin, P., Mortimer, J., and Gauvreau, D. (2000). Aluminum forms in drinking water and risk of Alzheimers disease. Environmental Research, 84(3), 234-246. 39. Stenson, J., Rense.com. Aluminum in drinking water tied to Alzheimers. http://www.rense.com/general37/SSTER.HTM 40. Priest, N.D. (1992), cited by Doll, op. cit., 138. 41. Rifat, S.L., Eastwod, M.R., McLachlan, D.R.C., and Corey, P.N. (1990). Effect of exposure of miners to aluminum powder. Lancet, 336, 1162-1165. 42. Parkinson, I.S., Ward, M.K., Feest, T.G., Fawcett, T.G., Fawcett, R.W.P., and Kerr, D.N.S. (1979). Fracturing dialysis osteodystrophy and dialysis encephalopathy: Epidemiological survey. Lancet, 1, 406409. 43. Rogers, M.A.M., and Simon, D.G. (1999). A preliminary study of dietary intake and risk of Alzheimers disease. Age and Ageing, 28, 205209. 44. Anane, R., Bonini, M., Grafeille, J.M., and Creppy, E.E. (1995). Bioaccumulation of water soluble aluminum chloride in the hippocampus after transdermal uptake in mice. Archives of Toxicology, 69(8), 568-571.
108
45. Perl, D.P., and Good, P.F. (1987). Uptake of aluminum into central nervous system along nasal-olfactory pathways. Lancet, 1(8540), 1028. 46. Jansson, op. cit. 47. Templer, D.I., Chicota, C.L., and Russell, M.C. (2001). Alzheimers disease and aluminum soil concentration. Journal of Orthomolecular Medicine, 16(2), 115-116. 48. Candy, J.M., Oakley, A.E., and Edwardson, J.A. (1993). Amorphous aluminosilicates promote nucleation of amyloid beta protein and tachykinins. Biochemical Society Transactions, 21(1), 53S. 49. Taylor, G.A., Newens, A.J., Edwardson, J.A., Kay, D.W., and Forster, D.P. (1995). Alzheimers disease and the relationship between silicon and aluminum in water supplies in northern England. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 49(3), 323-324. 50. Jansson, op.cit. 51. Jacqmin-Gadda, H., Commenges, D., Letenneur, L., and Dartigues, J.F. (1996). Silica and aluminum in drinking water and cognitive impairment in the elderly. Epidemiology, 7(3), 281-285. 52. Rondeau et al., op. cit. 53. Birchall, J.D., Exley, C., Chappell, J.S., and Phillips, M.J. (1989). Acute toxicity of aluminium to fish eliminated in silicon-rich acid waters. Nature, 338, 146-148. 54. Iler, R.K. (1979). The chemistry of silica. New York: Wiley. 55. Still, C.N., and Kelley, P. (1980). On the incidence of primary degenerative dementia vs. water fluoride content in South Carolina. Neurotoxicity Research, 1, 125-132. 56. Ibid. 57. Liss, L., and Thornton, D.J. (1986). The rationale for aluminum absorption control is early stages of Alzheimers disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 7, 552-554. 58. Forbes, W.F., Hayward, L.M., and Agwani, N. (1991a). aluminum and fluoride. Lancet, 338, 1592-1593. Dementia,
59. Forbes, W.F., and McAiney, C.A. (1991b). Aluminum and dementia. Lancet, 340, 668-669. 60. Ibid. 61. Forbes et al. (1994), op. cit. 62. Forbes and McLachlan, op. cit. 63. Forbes et al. (1994), op. cit.
109
64. Forbes and McLachlan, op. cit. 65. Disterhoft, J.F., Gispen, W.H., Traber, J., and Khachaturian, Z.S. (1994). Preface. Calcium Hypothesis of Aging and Dementia. New York: Annals of The New York Academy of Sciences, 747, IX-X. 66. Ibid. 67. Khachaturian, Z.S., Cotman, C.W., and Pettegrew, J.W. (1989). Calcium, membranes, aging and Alzheimers disease. New York: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 568. 68. Editorial Board (1989). The atlas of endemic diseases and their environments in the Peoples Republic of China. Beijing: Science Press. 69. Safe Drinking Water Committee, National Research Council (1977). Drinking water and health. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences. 70. Kimura, K., Yase, Y., Higashi, Y., Uno, S., Yamamoto, K., Iswaski, M., Tsumoto, I., and Sugiura, M. (1963). Epidemiological and geomedical studies on amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Diseases of the Nervous System, 24, 155-159. 71. Yase, Y., Yoshida, S., Kihira, T., Wakayama, I., and Komoto, J. (2001). Kii ALS dementia. Neuropathology, 21(2), 105. 72. Garruto, R.M., Fukaton,R., Hanagihara, R., Gajdusek, D.C., Hook, G., and Fiori, C. (1984). Imaging of calcium and aluminum in neurofibrillary tangle-bearing neurons in Parkinsonism dementia of Guam. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 81, 1875-1879. 73. Gajdusek, D.C. (1984). Calcium deficiency induced secondary hyperparathyroidism and the resultant CNS deposition of calcium and other metallic cations as the cause of ALS and PD in high incidence among the Auyu and Jakai people in West New Guinea. In K-M. Chen and Y. Yase (Eds.), Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (pp. 145-155). Taipei: National Taiwan University. 74. Fujita, T. (1987). Calcium and your health. Tokyo: Japan Publications. 75. Garruto et al., op. cit. 76. Kihira, T., Yoshida, S., Yase, Y., Ono, S., and Kondo, T. (2002). Chronic low-Ca/Mg high-Al diet induces neuronal loss. Neuropathology, 22(3), 171-179. 77. Armstrong, B.A., Andersson, J., Cowburn, J.D., Cox, J., and Blair, J.A. (1992). Aluminum administered in drinking water but not in the diet influences biopterin metabolism in the rodent. Biological Chemistry Hoppe - Seyler. 373(10), 1075-1078.
110
78. Jacqmin, H., Commenges, D., Letenneur, L., Barberger-Gateau, P., and Dartigues, J.F. (1994). Components of drinking water and risk of cognitive impairment in the elderly. American Journal of Epidemiology, 139(1), 48-57. 79. Forbes et al. (1994), op. cit. 80. Forbes et al. (1995), op. cit. 81. Forbes and McLachlan (1996), op. cit. 82. Jacqmin-Gadda et al. (1996), op. cit.
111
The value of what one knows is doubled if one confesses to not knowing what one does not know. What one knows is then raised beyond the suspicion to which it is exposed when one claims to know what one does not know.
Schopenhauer
112
8
Goethe
Knowing is not enough; we must apply. Willing is not enough; we must do.
In his book The 100: A Ranking of the Most Influential Persons in History, Michael Hart1 argues that Thomas Edison should occupy 38th place. That puts him one rank below the economic theorist Adam Smith and one above Antony van Leeuwenhoek, discoverer of microbes. Edison had more than 1,000 patents. He invented, among other things, the stock ticker, the phonograph, a practical incandescent light bulb, and its associated electric power distribution network. How could one inventor be so creative? Edison progressed by trial and error, always searching for something that worked. That is, he believed more in what later came to be known as Lateral Thinking2 than he did in the Scientific Method. Alternative medicine has evolved in a similar manner. For millennia, illnesses have been treated with a wide diversity of herbs, stone drugs, animal parts, exercises, magnetic fields, and other techniques. Eventually, those approaches that worked well are retained. Those that fail to benefit the patient are abandoned. At any one time, alternative medicine, therefore, includes numerous potential approaches to specific disorders or diseases. Some may be extremely effective, others useless or even very harmful. What is described in this chapter are current alternative treatments for Alzheimers disease that may, or may not, be beneficial. Supporting evidence for their use is provided where this is available.
113
HERBS
Sage
Sage (Salvia lavandulaefolia) has been promoted by herbalists, such as John Gerard and Nicholas Culpeper, as a memory stimulant for hundreds of years. It is currently added to foods and used to produce herbal teas. Scientific research is beginning to support its benefits for those suffering memory loss, probably because it inhibits the enzyme acetylcholinesterase, which breaks down the neurotransmitter acetylcholine.3 It may be recalled that a shortage of acetylcholine is characteristic of Alzheimers patients.
Ginkgo biloba
Ginkgo biloba is used in alternative medicine for a variety of reasons, including prevention of altitude sickness, improvement of blood circulation, and enhancement of memory. While nearly every arboretum or botanical gardens has a Ginkgo tree, supreme specimens are usually restricted to temple grounds
114
in Japan, China, and Korea. Some of these ancient trees are thought to be over 2,000 years old and have been venerated for their medicinal properties for millennia.8 Animal studies using transgenic mice recently have confirmed the positive impact of Ginkgo biloba on memory,9 perhaps due to its ability to reduce the oxidative damage caused by free radicals.10 As a result, medical trials have been conducted to investigate whether, or not, ginkgo is useful in the treatment of Alzheimers disease. These too have generally proven positive, as for example, a large multi-centre clinical German trial conducted by Kanowski and Hoerr.11 This growing body of evidence of the value of Ginkgo in memory loss has encouraged its use in the treatment of early-stage Alzheimers disease by conventional physicians.12
Vinpocetine
Vinpocetine is a synthetic ethyl ester of apovincamine, an alkaloid obtained from the leaves of the Lesser Periwinkle (Vinca minor).13 It is thought to be an excellent vasodilator and cerebral metabolic enhancer. Data from cells treated with beta amyloid suggests that it can protect against amyloid beta-peptide toxicity and prevent excessive oxidative stress.14 Despite these promising characteristics, 15 Alzheimers patients treated with increasing doses of vinpocetine, ranging from 30 to 60 mg per day, for one year, developed cognitive deficits that were similar to an untreated control group.15 As a consequence, Thal and coworkers16 concluded that vinpocetine is ineffective in improving cognitive deficits and does not slow the rate of decline in individuals with Alzheimers disease.
Huperzia serrata
Huperzine A is an alkaloid isolated from the Chinese moss Huperzia serrata. It has been found to cross the blood-brain barrier easily and act as a potent, yet reversible cholinesterase
115
inhibitor.17 As a result, it, like sage, appears able to increase acetylcholine availability. Huperzia serrata has been collected in the colder regions of China for centuries and used to treat fever and inflammation.18 Animal studies have shown that an extract from it, Huperzine A, can help protect against both brain injury and memory loss.19 Beyond this, a double-blind study of 50 Alzheimers patients receiving either a placebo or 0.4 mg of Huperzine A daily, for 8 weeks, showed statistically significant memory, cognitive, and behavioural improvements in those receiving the extract.20 The researchers suggested that Huperzine A was a promising, safe new treatment for Alzheimers disease. It is noted in the literature that high doses might prove more toxic. Huperzine A has been approved as the drug of choice for the treatment of Alzheimers disease in China, while in North America it is still marketed as a dietary supplement.21
Galantamine
Galantamine is a natural supplement derived from the snowdrop.22 The common snowdrop from which this substance is produced is closely related to the daffodil, which also has been shown to be an effective inhibitor of acetylcholinesterase. In a double-blind, placebo controlled trial conducted in both Europe and Canada, 653 patients with mild to moderate Alzheimers disease were provided with either Galantamine or a placebo. After 6 months, it was clear that the snowdrop extract was slowing declines in cognition and functional ability, with little sign of adverse effects.23 Similarly, in a second study involving patients diagnosed with Alzheimers disease and cerebral vascular disease, 285 were given either 24 mg per day of Galantamine or a placebo. After 1 year, those receiving the Galantamine had showed clinically significant improvements in cognitive functions which they maintained throughout the year. In the placebo group, cognitive functions had deteriorated.24 The results of a series of other trials25-27 have been published recently.
116
All appear favourable and Galantamine is now approved for the treatment of Alzheimers disease in Canada28.
MINERALS
Calcium and Magnesium Ascorbate
Research suggesting a notable breakthrough in the prevention of Alzheimers disease was described by Dr. Natalia Bobkova,32 in June 2001, at the III World Congress on Vitamin C. Bobkova
117
is a member of the Russian Academy of Sciences research group that had been screening the elderly for signs of early memory loss. Those older Russians found to be experiencing the greatest declines were given the B vitamins, together with elevated levels of several mineral ascorbates, specifically potassium, magnesium, calcium, zinc, manganese, and chromium ascorbates. These, of course, provided such elderly patients with high levels of vitamin C and very easily absorbed minerals. According to Bobkova, in every case memory returned to normal in a few weeks. Galeev and coworkers33described a similar study in which 52 to 78 year old patients who had complained of failing memory and deteriorating cognitive functions were provided with 1 to 2 grams of Alacer Corps Emergen-C and Super Gram II mineral ascorbates. Fifteen such patients were tested after receiving these supplements for 1 month and 13 after 2, 3, 5, 7, 9, 12, and 18 months. Initially, a control group was used to compare the effects of such mineral ascorbate supplementation. However, after psychophysiological testing showed marked improvements in those receiving Emergen-C and Super Gram II but not in controls, the latter group also were provided with these minerals and subsequently began to display significant memory and cognitive function benefits. Bobkova and colleagues34 also described a related animal experiment in which mineral ascorbates were given to mice after olfactory bulbectomies had been carried out. Olfactory bulbectomy involves an operation to destroy an animals ability to smell. In untreated animals, subjected to this operation, a deficit in memory occurs, accompanied by various neurodegenerative processes in brain structures that appear to mimic those of Alzheimers disease. Bobkova and coworkers, however, found the mineral ascorbates prevented this memory loss, and protected the neurons of the temporal cortex from degenerative
118
changes. These observations may explain why mineral ascorbates can reverse early memory loss in the elderly, as described at the III World Congress on Vitamin C, in 2001.35
Lithium
Low dose lithium is used as an alternative treatment for the early stage of Alzheimers disease.36 There are several good reasons this mineral may be beneficial. Lithium is known to slow the formation of beta amyloid37-38 and reduce the damage caused by this protein, even after it has been secreted.39 Beyond this, it also inhibits the formation of tau and neurofibrillary tangles.40 This may be because lithium is an effective electrolyte for aluminum detachment and helps to chelate this metal so it can be more readily removed from the body. Lithium also inhibits the formation of abnormal crosslinks caused by aluminum.41
AMINO ACIDS
Acetly-l-carnitine
Acetyl-l-carnitine is derived from carnitine, which consists of two amino acids, lysine and methionine. It is known to be active at cholinergic neurons, help with membrane stabilization, and improve mitochondrial function.42 Although some studies suggest it slows the progression of Alzheimers disease, evidence from others disputes this. Pettegrew and associates,43 for example, gave 3 grams of acetyl-l-carnitine to seven Alzheimers patients daily for a year. Five similar controls were given a placebo. While all 12 probable Alzheimers patients had virtually identical cognitive scores at the beginning of the year, by the end of it, the group of patients receiving acetyl-l-carnitine had significantly higher test performances. This was because the control group had deteriorated, while the acetyl-l-carnitine
119
group maintained their earlier cognitive abilities. In contrast, a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial involving 229 probable Alzheimers patients found no significant differences, after 1 year, between those receiving acetyl-l-carnitine and those who did not.44 A recent Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, by Hudson and Tabat,45 concluded that acetyl-l-carnitine is unlikely to be an important therapeutic agent in Alzheimers disease but still called for more research into the subject.
LIPIDS
Phosphatidylserine
Another memory aid, phosphatidylserine is a lipid a fat, or fat-like, molecule found in all functioning cells but concentrated in the brain. Recent clinical research appears to support its use by those experiencing cognitive difficulties. To illustrate, in 1991, in a study coordinated by the Memory Assessment Clinic of Bethesda, Maryland,46 149 elderly volunteers were given either phosphatidylserine or a placebo for 12 weeks. Those receiving the nutrient showed significant improvements in both memory and the ability to perform tests. In a follow-up study, Crook and coworkers47 provided 51 Alzheimers patients with either phosphatidylserine or a placebo for 3 months. Those receiving the lipid improved on several cognitive measures, suggesting that phosphatidylserine may be a useful treatment in the early stages of Alzheimers disease. Cenacchi and colleagues48 took this research further by providing 300 mg of phosphatidylserine daily, or a placebo, to 494 elderly geriatric patients who were suffering from cognitive impairment. These were drawn from 23 health care facilities located in northeastern Italy. The study continued for 6 months, by the end of which time those patients being given phosphatidylserine showed statistically significant improvements in both memory
120
and behaviour. The reason for this occurrence seems to have been established by Heiss and colleagues,49 who used positron emission tomography (PET) to show that phosphatidylserine stimulates the cerebral metabolic rate for glucose. It may be recalled that a deficiency in glucose metabolism is characteristic of those suffering from Alzheimers disease.
SUMMARY
It seems likely that several of the herbs traditionally used to treat memory loss have some benefits for Alzheimers disease patients. Sage, for example, inhibits the enzyme acetylcholinesterase, which breaks down acetylcholine. Bacopa reduces nitric oxide induced oxidative stress and mitigates depression. Ginkgo biloba also appears able to reduce free radical damage, as may vinpocetine. The Chinese medical establishment feels that Huperzine A, a moss extract, has shown enough promise in the treatment of Alzheimers disease to recommend it to physicians for this purpose. The research being carried out jointly by members of the Russian Academy of Sciences and the Committee for World Health, using high dose ascorbate minerals, is also very impressive. Ascorbate minerals appear capable of reversing early-stage memory loss, while lithium may be able to inhibit the formation of senile plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. The B vitamins, especially folic acid, niacin,50 and B12 also may be of use in protecting against memory loss. The value of acetyl-l-carnitine in the treatment of Alzheimers disease is less obvious, since clinical trials have been contradictory. In contrast, phosphatidylserine appears to have the potential to stimulate glucose metabolism, known to be deficient in Alzheimers patients.
121
REFERENCES
1. 2. 3. Hart, M.H. (1982). The 100: A ranking of the most influential persons in history. New York: Galahad Books. De Bono (1986). Lateral thinking. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books. Pro Health Network (1993). Alzheimers news: Sage improves memory, study shows. http://www.prohealthnetwork.com/library/show article.cfm/ID/1804/e/1/T/Health/ Kidd, P.M. (1999). A review of nutrients and botanicals in the interactive management of cognitive dysfunction. Alternative Medicine Review, 4(3), 144-161. Russo, A., Borrelli, F., Campisi, A., Acquaviva, R., Raciti, G., and Vanella, A. (2003). Nitric oxide-related toxicity in cultured astrocytes: Effect of Bacopa monniera. Life Sciences, 73(12), 1517-1526. Ibid. Sairam, K., Dorababu, M., Goel, R.K., and Bhattachayaa, S.K. (2002). Antidepressant activity of standardized extract of Bacopa monniera in experimental models of depression in rats. Phytomedicine, 9(3), 207-211. The Ginkgo Pages. http://www.xs4all.nl/~kwanten/thetree/htm Stackman, R.W., Eckenstein, F., Frei, B., Kulhanek, D., Nowlin, J., and Quinn, J.F. (2003). Prevention of age-related spatial memory deficits in transgenic mouse model of Alzheimers disease by chronic Ginkgo biloba treatment. Experimental Neurology, 184(1), 510-520.
4.
5.
6. 7.
8. 9.
10. Smith, J.V., and Luo, Y. (2003). Elevation of oxidative free radicals in Alzheimers disease models can be attenuated by Ginkgo biloba extract EGb 761. Journal of Alzheimers disease, 5(4), 287-300. 11. Kanowski, S., and Hoerr, R. (2003). Ginkgo biloba extract EGb 761 in dementia: Intent-to-treat analysis of a 24-week, multi-center, double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized trial. Pharmacopsychiatry, 36(6), 297-303. 12. Sloane, P.D. (1998). Advances in the treatment of Alzheimers disease. American Family Physician. http://www/aafp.org/afp/981101ap/ sloane.html 13. Szatmari, S.Z, and Whitehouse, P.J. (2003). Vinpocetine for cognitive impairment and dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, (1), CD003119. 14. Pereira, C., Agostinho, P., and Oliveira, C.R. (2000). Vinpocetine attenuates the metabolic dysfunction induced by amyloid beta-peptides in PC12 cells. Free Radical Research, 33(5), 497-506.
122
15. Thal, L.J., Salmon, D.P., Lasker, B., Bower, D., and Klauber, M.R. (1989). The safety and lack of efficacy of vinpocetine in Alzheimers disease. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 37(6), 515-520. 16. Ibid. 17. Jiang, H., Luo, X., and Bai, D. (2003). Progress in clinical, pharmacological, chemical and structural biological studies of Huperzine A: A drug of traditional Chinese medicine origin for the treatment of Alzheimers disease. Current Medical Chemistry, 10(21), 2231-2252. 18. Wholehealthmd.com. Huperzine A. http://www.wholehealthmd.com/ refshelf/substances_view/1,1525,10038,00.html 19. Wang, L.S., Zhou, J., Shao, X.M., and Tang, X.C. (2003). Huperzine A attenuates cognitive deficits of brain injury after hypoxia-ischemic brain damage in neonatal rats. Zhongguo Er Ke Za Zhi, 41(1), 42-45. 20. Xu, S.S., Gao, Z.X., Weng, Z., Du, Z.M., Xu, W.A., Yang, J.S., Zhang, M.L., Tong, Z.H., Fang, Y.S., and Chai, X.S (1995). Efficacy of tablet huperzine-A on memory, cognition, and behaviour in Alzheimers disease. Zhongguo Yao Li Xue Bao, 16(5), 391-395. 21. Jiang et al., op. cit. 22. Schilhab, G. (2004). Galantamine: A dual-action cholinesterase inhibitor to treat Alzheimers disease. Nutrition and Mental Health, Spring, 3. 23. Ibid. 24. Ibid. 25. Kurz, A.F., Erkinjuntt, T., Small, G.W., Lilienfeld, S., and Damaraju, C.R. (2003). Long-term safety and cognitive effects of Galantamine in the treatment of probable vascular dementia or Alzheimers disease with cerebrovascular disease. European Journal of Neurology, 10(6), 633-640. 26. Cummings, J.L., Schneider, L., Tariot, P.N., Kershaw, P.R., and Yuan, W. (2004). Reduction of behavioral disturbance and caregiver distress by Galantamine in patients with Alzheimers disease. American Journal of Psychiatry,161(3), 532-538. 27. Raskind, M.A., Peskind, E.R., Truyen, L., Kershaw, P., and Damaraju, C.V. (2004). The cognitive benefits of Galantamine are sustained for at least 36 months: A long-term extension trial. Archives of Neurology, 61(2), 252-256. 28. Caro, J., Getsios, D., Migliaccio-Walle, K., Ishak, J., and El-Hadi, W. (2003). Rational choice of cholinesterase inhibitor for the treatment of Alzheimers disease in Canada: A comparative economic analysis. BMC Geriatrics, 3(1), 6.
123
29. Kawagishi, H., Zhuang, C., and Shnidman, E. (2004). The anti-dementia effect of Lions Mane mushroom (Hericium erinaceum) and its clinical application. Townsend Letter for Doctors and Patients, 249, 54-56. 30. Ibid. 31. Ibid. 32. Bobkova, N.V. (2001). The impact of mineral ascorbates on memory loss. Paper presented at the III World Congress on Vitamin C, Committee for World Health, Victoria, BC, Canada, June 2001. 33. Galeev, A., Kazakova, A., Zherebker, E., Dana, E., and Dana, R. (nd). Mineral ascorbates improve memory and cognitive functions in older individuals with pre-Alzheimers symptoms. Copy of paper given to this author by R. Dana and E. Dana, Committee for World Health, 20331 Lake Forest Drive, Suite C-15, Lake Forest, California 92630, USA. 34. Bobkova, N.V., Nesterova, I.V., Dana, E., Nesterov, V.I., Aleksandrova, IIu, Medvinskaia, N.I., and Samokhin, A.N. (2003). Morpho-functional changes of neurons in temporal cortex in comparison with spatial memory in bulbectomized mice after treatment with minerals and ascorbates. Morfologiia, 123(3), 27-31 [in Russian]. 35. Bobkova, op.cit. 36. Wright, J.V. (2004). Lithium, Part I: Protect and renew your brain. Townsend Letter for Doctors and Patients, 247/248, 78-81. 37. Sun, X., Sato, S., Murayama, O., Murayama, M., Park, J.M., Yamaguchi, H., and Takashima, A. (2002). Lithium inhibits amyloid secretion in COS7 cells transfected with amyloid precursor protein C100. Neuroscience Letters, 321(1-2), 61-64. 38. Phiel, C.J., Wilson, C.A., Lee, V.M., and Klein, P.S. (2003). GSK-3alpha regulates production of Alzheimers disease amyloid-beta peptides. Nature, 432(6938), 435-439. 39. Alvarez, G., Munoz-Montano, J.R., Satrustegui, J., Avila, J., Bogonez, E., and Diaz-Nido, J. (1999). Lithium protects cultured neurons against beta-amyloid-induced neurodegeneration. FEBS Letters, 453(3), 260-264. 40. Munoz-Montano, J.R., Moreno, F.J., Avila, J., and Diaz-Nido, J. (1997). Lithium inhibits Alzheimers disease-like tau protein phosphorylation in neurons. FEBS Letters, 411(2-3), 183-188. 41. Bjorksten, J. (1977). Pathways to the decisive extension of the human lifespan. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 25, 396-399. 42. Hudson, S., and Tabet, N. (2003). Acetyl-l-carnitine for dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, (2), CD003158.
124
43. Pettegrew, J.W., Klunk, W.E., Panchalingam, K., Kanfer, J.N., and McClure, R.J. (1995). Clinical and neurochemical effect of acetyl-lcarnitine in Alzheimers disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 16(1), 1-4. 44. Thal, L.J., Calvani, M., Amato, A., and Carta, A. (2000). A 1-year controlled trial of acetyl-l-carnitine in early-onset AD. Neurology, 55(6), 805-810. 45. Hudson et al., op.cit. 46. Crook, T.H., Tinklenberg, J., Yesavage, J., Petrie, W., Nunzi, M.G., and Massari, D.C. (1991). Effects of phosphatidylserine in age-associated memory impairment. Neurology, 41(5), 644-649. 47. Crook, T., Petrie, W., Wells, C., and Massari, D.C. (1992). Effect of phosphatidylserine in Alzheimers disease. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 28(1), 61-62. 48. Cenacchi, T., Bertoldin, T., Farina, C., Fiori, M.G., and Crepaldi, G. (1993). Cognitive decline in the elderly: A double-blind, placebo-controlled multicenter study of efficacy of phosphatidylserine administration. Aging (Milano), 5(2), 123-133. 49. Heiss, W.D., Kessler, J., Slansky, I., Mielke, R., Szelies, B., and Herholz, K. (1993). Activation PET as an instrument to determine therapeutic efficacy in Alzheimers disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 695, 327-331. 50. Morris, M.C., Evans, D.A., Bienias, J.L., Scherr, P.A., Tangney, C.C., Hebert, L.E., Bennett, D.A., Wilson, R.S., and Aggarwal, N. (2004). Dietary niacin and the risk of Alzheimers disease and cognitive decline. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 75(8), 1093-1097.
125
Ideology, like theater, is dependent on the willing suspension of disbelief. At the core of every ideology lies the worship of a bright new future, with only failure in the immediate past. But once the suspension goes, willingness converts into suspicion the suspicion of the betrayed. Our brilliant leaders abruptly appear naive, even ridiculous.
J.R. Saulo, The Collapse of Globalism, 2004 Harpers Magazine
126
Were pushing our bodies past their own innate limits. Due to extraordinary medical interventions in cancer, heart disease, and other conditions, humankind is now living longer than our genes would ordinarily allow. We are outliving our own mortality signature, living on what epidemiologists call manufactured time. It is the cushion of extra life that we are creating for ourselves with our ingenuity and our tools. The real challenge, of course, is to ensure that this new time is something we are happy to have. D. Shenk1
The truth is, being a care provider for a progressively declining Alzheimers patient is a thankless, mentally and physically draining task which conventional medicine currently only can make worse. This is because it offers no effective treatment, merely drugs which, to the detriment of both caregivers and society, prolong this process of mental and physical decline. Indeed, most people, myself included, if asked (when in full possession of their mental faculties) whether they wanted such an extended death would almost certainly opt for a more rapid demise. In short, the current medical treatment of Alzheimers disease benefits no one, or nothing, except the medical profession and the pharmaceutical industry. Until we prevent or halt and effectively reverse Alzheimers disease, this will continue to be the case.
expected, a deficiency of this neurotransmitter in the brains of Alzheimers patients. Clearly, you would expect such patients to benefit if acetylcholine levels rose. However, previous chapters of this book also have shown that, in dementia, biological abnormalities are not limited simply to acetylcholine, but also include numerous other neurotransmitters, for example, glutamate, dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin. Several other hormones and mineral levels in the Alzheimers disease brain are also atypical. It is not surprising, therefore, that drugs, such as donepezil and tacrine, that aim to impose acetylcholine balance are only of marginal value, since many other biochemical abnormalities remain unaddressed. As a consequence, effects are modest and results are considered successful if either drug returns the patients ability to function to the level possessed 6 to 12 months earlier. The use of tacrine, for example, has been shown to delay the need for nursing home admittance.6 Other cholinesterase inhibitors that are also designed to enhance neuronal transmission by increasing acetylcholine at receptors include Galantamine, rivastigmine, and metrifonate.7 However, since all such drugs address only one abnormal aspect of Alzheimers disease biochemistry, it is not surprising that their benefits tend to be relatively limited.8 Memantine, a low- to moderate-affinity, noncompetitive N-methyl-D-asparate receptor antagonist has been used in German clinics for over 10 years to treat patients with dementia.9 This drug often is used in combination with acetylcholinesterase inhibitors such as donepezil.10 The major value of memantine is that it is designed to protect against the excitotoxicity of low glutamate concentrations. As would be expected, addressing biochemical imbalances in the cholinergic and glutamatergic systems simultaneously appears to have some added value in the treatment of Alzheimers disease.11
129
In addition to cholinesterase inhibitors and memantine, several nonspecific drugs also are being used to try to delay progression in Alzheimers disease. To illustrate, at least three longitudinal studies have suggested that estrogen replacement therapy may have a protective effect against dementia and, as a result, it is being used by physicians to treat Alzheimers disease.12 Some research supports the use of this medication. Lambert and colleagues,13 for example, have studied in vitro binding between estrogen receptors and fragments of the human APO E gene. It may be recalled this gene appears involved in determining much of the genetic risk of developing Alzheimers disease. Their results suggested that the significance of the APO E4 allele may be influenced by estrogen. Beyond this, Granholm and coworkers14 have shown that in the mouse model of Downs syndrome, estrogen alters the amyloid precursor protein. It will be recalled that this protein appears to play the key role in the formation of the senile plaques that are so characteristic of Alzheimers disease brains. Nevertheless, the largest clinical trial completed to date, conducted by Mulnard and colleagues15 from the University of Californias Institute for Brain Aging and Dementia, does not support the use of estrogen in the treatment of Alzheimers disease. Between October 1995 and January 1999, 120 women suffering from mild to moderate Alzheimers disease were given randomly either estrogen (0.625 mg per day or 1.25 mg per day) or a placebo for one year. Their memory, attention spans, language skills, motor functions and activities of daily living were monitored and compared on several subsequent occasions. All these women previously had undergone a hysterectomy. The results caused these researchers to conclude that estrogen did not improve global, cognitive or functional outcomes in women with mild or moderate Alzheimers disease. With such treatment there was no slowing of disease progression which may, if anything, have been worsened by estrogen therapy.
130
There may be a case for the use of the hormone testosterone in the treatment of Alzheimers disease, although as yet it is not widely used. This is because the impact of testosterone on the risk of developing Alzheimers disease is still unclear. In 2003, Almeida and Flicker16 published a paper entitled Testosterone and dementia: Too much ado about too little data in The Journal of the British Menopause Society. In it they pointed out that test tube and animal studies have suggested that testosterone protects neurons and can reduce the levels of beta-amyloid and the phosphorylation of tau. That is, testosterone can reduce neural damage and slow down the formation of both senile plaques and neurofibrillary tangles, all of which are characteristics of Alzheimers disease. Unfortunately, the results of a small number of cross-sectional studies and five randomized trials of testosterone treatment on healthy older men have been inconsistent and, at best, demonstrated that this hormone has a weak association with memory scores.17 Beyond this, the serum levels of testosterone in male Alzheimers patients are very like those of similarly aged men without the disorder, and so are brain levels of the hormone. Almeida and Flicker,18 therefore, concluded that currently available evidence does not support the existence of a strong association between testosterone and cognitive function/AD [Alzheimers disease]. Two more recent studies add further confusion to the picture. Hoskin and coworkers19 measured levels of sex-hormone binding globulin in the serum of 576 women aged over 65. Elevated levels were found in those who were Alzheimers patients, suggesting that they had higher than normal blood testosterone and estrogen. In contrast, in 574 men followed for about 19 years as part of the Baltimore longitudinal study of aging, elevated free blood testosterone was associated with a reduced risk of developing Alzheimers disease.20 The relationship between testosterone and Alzheimers disease risk is still uncertain and controversial.21 It also should be pointed out that
131
there is a possibility that testosterone supplements may promote stroke and prostate cancer.22 Nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs), including aspirin and ibuprofen, also have been linked to a lower incidence of dementia.23 It is as yet unclear whether this is because they reduce the brain inflammation seen in Alzheimers disease or protect against stroke. Beyond this, Alenog and colleagues24 have shown that in mixed rat cell cultures, the common nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs aspirin and indomethacin induced significant increases in extracellular apolipoprotein E levels. As will be recalled this is involved in senile plaque formation in Alzheimers disease. Etminan, Gill, and Samii25 recently have conducted a metaanalysis of all the studies that have examined the possible effects of non-steroidal, anti-inflammatory drugs on the risk of Alzheimers disease. There have been nine in all. Six were cohort studies with a total of 13,211 participants and three case-control studies with 1,443 volunteers involved. The evidence suggests that taking such drugs does reduce the risk of Alzheimers disease. To illustrate, the risk of developing this type of dementia declines with the time for which they have been taken, the relative risk dropping to a very impressive 0.27 among long term (mostly over 2 years) users. The pooled relative risk in eight studies of subjects taking aspirin was 0.87. Nevertheless, to quote Sloane,26 Because of the risks of NSAID use, which include gastrointestinal and renal toxicity, these agents cannot be routinely recommended for use as a preventative measure against Alzheimers disease at the present time. Gastrointestinal bleeding is one significant risk associated with prolonged use of non-steroid anti-inflammatory drugs. Selegiline (deptenyl), a monoamine oxidase-B inhibitor that may reduce oxidative stress,27 also has been used in the treatment
132
of Alzheimers patients.28 Birks and Flicker29 have recently conducted a meta-analysis of selegilines impact on Alzheimers disease as demonstrated by all available clinical trial evidence. They concluded that there is no evidence of a clinically meaningful benefit for Alzheimers disease sufferers from the use of selegiline. They also suggest that its use as a treatment for this form of dementia be halted. Vitamin E, the benefits of which have been described previously, is also prescribed by some physicians since it has far fewer adverse effects than selegiline. Dosages of up to 2,000 IU daily are used.30
SUMMARY
A variety of drugs are used routinely to treat patients suffering from Alzheimers disease. The evidence supporting their value is mixed. Some, such as the cholinesterase inhibitors donepezil and tacrine, appear to slow disease progression by a few months. Others, including selegiline, may have little, if any, beneficial effect on this form of dementia. It is interesting that the best case of real value appears to be for non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs, such as aspirin and ibuprofen, which physicians hesitate to use to reduce the risk of developing Alzheimers disease as they can cause serious gastrointestinal and renal toxicity. It can be argued that since the drugs in common use merely prolong disease progression, they are of no real benefit to either patients or caregivers and simply increase the cost of the Alzheimers disease pandemic to society. Obviously, what is needed are new approaches that either completely prevent, or quickly reverse, this form of dementia.
133
REFERENCES
1. 2. 3. 4. Shenk, D. (2002). The Forgetting Alzheimers: Portrait of an epidemic. New York: Anchor Books. Thompson, D. (2003). Older couples at risk of murder/suicide. Health Scout. http://www.healthscout.com/news/1/516719/main.html Ibid. Sloane, P.D. (1998). Advances in the treatment of Alzheimers disease. American Family Physician. http://www.aafp.org/afp/981101ap/ sloane.html Ibid. Small, G.W., Rabins, P.V., Barry, P.P., Buckholz, N.S., DeKosky, S.T., Ferris, S.H., Finkel, S.I., Gwythe, L.P., Khachaturian, Z.S., Lebowitz, B.D., McRae, D., and Morris, J.C. (1997). Diagnosis and treatment of Alzheimers disease and related disorders. Consensus statement of the American Association of Geriatric Psychiatry, the Alzheimers Association, and the American Geriatrics Society. Journal of the American Medical Association, 278, 1363-1371. Sloane, op.cit. Ibid. Koch, H.J., Szecsey, A., and Haen, E. (2004). NMDA-antagonism (memantine): An alternative pharmacological therapeutic principle in Alzheimers and vascular dementia. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 10(3), 253-259.
5. 6.
7. 8. 9.
10. Tariot, P.N., Farlow, M.R., Grossberg, G.T., Graham, S.M., McDonald, S., and Gergel, I., Memantine Group (2004). Memantine treatment in patients with moderate to severe Alzheimers disease already receiving donepezil: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 291(3), 317-324. 11. Ibid. 12. Sloane, op.cit. 13. Lambert, J.C., Coyl, N., and Lendon, C. (2004). The allelic modulation of apolipoprotein E expression by oestrogen: Potential relevance for Alzheimers disease. Journal of Medical Genetics, 41(2), 104-112. 14. Granholm, A.C., Sanders, L., Seo, H., Lin, L., Ford, K., and Isacson, O. (2003). Estrogen alters amyloid precursor protein as well as dendritic and cholinergic markers in a mouse model of Down syndrome. Hippocampus, 13(8), 905-914. 15. Mulnard, R.A., Cotman, C.W., Kawas, C., van Dyck, C.H., Sano, M., Doody, R., Koss, E., Pfeiffer, E., Jin, S., Gamst, A., Grundman,
134
M., Thomas, R., and Thal, L.J. (2000). Estrogen replacement therapy for treatment of mild to moderate Alzheimers disease: A randomized controlled trial. Alzheimers disease Cooperative Study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 283(8), 1007-1015. 16. Almeida, O.P., and Flicker, L. (2003). Testosterone and dementia: Too much ado about too little data. Journal of the British Menopause Society, 9(3), 107-110. 17. Ibid. 18. Ibid. 19. Hoskin, E.K., Tang, M.X., Manly, J.J., and Mayeux, R. (2004). Elevated sex-hormone binding globulin in elderly women with Alzheimers disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 25(2), 141-147. 20. Moffat, S.D., Zonderman, A.B., Metter, E.J., Kawas, C., Blackman, M.R., Harman, S.M., and Resnick, S.M. (2004). Free testosterone and risk for Alzheimers disease in older men. American Academy of Neurology, 62(2), 188-193. 21. Pennanen, C., Laakso, M.P., Kivipelto, M., Ramberg, J., and Soininen, H. (2004). Serum testosterone levels in males with Alzheimers disease. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 16(2), 95-98. 22. Gupta, S. (2004). Brawn and brain. Can testosterone supplements ward off Alzheimers? Read on. Time Europe, February 9, p.118. 23. Sloane, op. cit. 24. Aleong, R., Aumont, N., Dea, D., and Poirier, J. (2003). Non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs mediate increases in vitro glial expression of apolipoprotein E protein. European Journal of Neuroscience, 18(6), 1428-1438. 25. Etminan, M., Gill, S., and Samii, A. (2003). Effect of non-steroidal antiinflammatory drugs on risk of Alzheimers disease: Systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. British Medical Journal, 327(7407), 128. 26. Sloane, op. cit. 27. Halliwell, B. (2001). Role of free radicals in the neurodegenerative diseases: Therapeutic implications for antioxidant treatment. Drug Aging, 18(9), 685-716. 28. Yamada, M., and Yasuhara, H. (2004). Clinical pharmacology of MAO inhibitors: Safety and future. Neurotoxicology, 25(1-2), 215-221. 29. Birks, J., and Flicker, L. (2003). Selegiline for Alzheimers disease. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. CD000442. 30. Sloane, op. cit.
135
All living organisms require a regular supply of nutrients. This is true for microbes as it is for man. If there is a deficiency, or an excess, in fact if there is an imbalance of nutrients, then normal development will cease, and abnormal development will result. If the imbalance is severe death will eventually result.
K.H. Schutte, The biology of the trace elements, 1979
136
10
Abide with me; fast falls the eventide; The darkness deepens; Lord with me abide; When other helpers fail, and comforts flee, Help of the helpless, O, abide with me. Swift to its close ebbs out lifes little day; Earths joy grows dim, its glories pass away; Change and decay in all around I see; O thou who changest not, abide with me. Henry F. Lyte (1847) Abide With Me
As Siegel1 points out, we should be paying more attention to the exceptional patients, those who get well unexpectedly, instead of staring bleakly at all those who die in the usual pattern. I first used this quotation from Love, Medicine and Miracles in 1988, in a paper entitled Lifestyle changes and the spontaneous regression of cancer: An initial computer analysis.2 The idea behind this article was very simple. I used a questionnaire to collect information from 200 published accounts of spontaneous regression that included patients treatment(s), dietary and lifestyle changes, mineral, vitamin, and herbal supplements, and detoxification procedures. This approach generated a great deal of useful new information, which was analysed using simple statistics. It served to stimulate numerous hypotheses that may eventually help to account for spontaneous cancer regression. I used a similar approach in What really causes schizophrenia3 to collect information from the testimonies of 50 recovered schizophrenics.
137
Navely, I considered using it again to examine what recovered Alzheimers patients had done that might account for their reversal of this type of dementia. I soon found it to be an impossible approach since so few people ever come back from this particular abyss. Indeed, I could find only two convincing cases of the spontaneous regression of Alzheimers disease. Fortunately, however, both of those concerned had written books about their experiences.4-5 These cases are now reviewed but in a very short chapter. The following material is abstracted from the prologue of Louis Blanks6 autobiographical book, Alzheimers Challenged and Conquered?. It shows how serious his memory loss had become and how deep he must have been into the Early Stage of Alzheimers disease when the events described took place. They occurred when he was taking an elevator to his apartment. The young woman interrupted my thoughts as she said something to me. I pretended to ignore her, but she spoke louder so I looked up so as not to appear impolite When I was young, a lady would not speak to a strange man until someone introduced him to herThe couple got out at the ninth floor, and I was alone with the young woman. She was now looking at me openly, with a quizzical expression on her face.Keeping my eyes averted, I fled through the doors as soon as they opened and I sensed, even without seeing her, that she had got out as well. She might be visiting someone on my floor, I thought. Strange, most of the people on this floor are still at work. I put my key in the lock and opened the door. Suddenly, I was uncomfortably aware that the strange woman was right behind me. I tried to close the door but she put out her hand and held it open. Wwhat do you want? I stammered. I want to come in, of course. Her brazen cheek took me aback. What do you mean, you want to come in? Ive come to visit you, Dad. Whats the matter? You look as if you dont know me.7
138
Louis Blank could no longer recognize his own daughter. It was later confirmed, after detailed hospital testing,8 that he had Alzheimers disease. As his disorder progressed, he completely lost the ability to recognize his own family or to speak, and to dress, wash, or eat without assistance. For 6 months, at the peak of his illness in 1993, he sat virtually motionless. His recovery appeared to begin after his family replaced all its aluminum cooking utensils and started to avoid aluminum cans. In addition, he was fed a high magnesium diet, designed to chelate aluminum. By May of 1994, Louis Blank was once again able to carry on conversations and venture outside, and by January of 1996, he had written and published his book, Alzheimers Challenged and Conquered?,9 despite the fact that one of his specialists continued to argue that since there is no cure for Alzheimers disease, he must still have it. An interesting aspect of Blanks description of his experiences is that, while most of his long-term memory remains, he still has no recollections of his worst 6 months. This confirms that, as Alzheimers disease progresses, the patient loses the ability to form short-term memories. Older-term memories remain intact for much longer. In his book Beating Alzheimers: A Step Towards Unlocking the Mysteries of Brain Diseases, Tom Warren10 also describes his experiences with dementia. In June 1983, a computer-assisted tomography scan confirmed that Warren had Alzheimers disease. His physicians gave him a maximum of 7 years to live. Yet nearly 4 years later, a new scan indicated that the disease process had reversed. Warrens self-treatment had included rectifying low hydrochloric stomach acid, the removal of all his teeth and mercury amalgam fragments from his gums, ethylene diamine tetracetic acid (EDTA) chelation therapy and high doses of vitamins and minerals. The latter included calcium, magnesium, and vitamins B3, B6, B12, and folic acid. According to Warren,11 as a result of this regimen:
139
My health, after several years, has so improved that I can say, in all honesty, that being able to feel good again has been worth all the pain and struggle. The road to recovery is not an easy path or a straight line, nor is it fast. Rather it is a cobblestone road of many tiny lifestyle changes, each another step toward renewed vitality and clarity of thought.
SUMMARY
At the core of both Blank and Warrens treatments was a reduction in exposure to metals, especially aluminum, and, in Warrens case, mercury. Chelation therapy also reduced body burdens of those toxins. Beyond this, diets were supplemented with extra minerals, particularly magnesium.
REFERENCES
1. 2. Siegel, B.S. (1986). Love, medicine and miracles. New York: Harper and Row. Foster, H.D. (1988). Lifestyle changes and the spontaneous regression of cancer: An initial computer analysis. International Journal of Biosocial Research, 10(1), 17-33. Foster, H.D. (2003). What really causes schizophrenia. Victoria, BC: Trafford Publishing. Warren, T. (1991). Beating Alzheimers: A step towards unlocking the mysteries of brain disease. Garden City Park, NY: Avery. Blank, L. (1995). Alzheimers challenged and conquered? Foulsham. Ibid. Ibid. Ibid. Ibid. London:
3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9.
140
11
The acceptance of theories depends as much on the psychology of human beings as on the content of the theories. It is human beings who decide, individually and as a community, whether a theory indeed has explanatory power or provides understanding. This is why seemingly extrascientific factors such as productivity, portability, storytelling power and aesthetics matter. Sometimes it takes a long time (witness continental drift), but often the acceptance is immediate and intuitive it fits. Like a nice sweater. Roald Hoffmann1
I know from sad experience that is it not easy to accurately predict the outcome of a horse race. If you consider only how fast each horse has run previously and bet on the one with the best times, you will probably identify the favourite, which wins only one race in three. Concentrating on class, the quality of the horses competed against in earlier races, will probably provide you with a fairly similar winning percentage. Running styles, track condition, jockey ability and nerve, legal and illegal drug use, equipment differences and failures, trainer skills, owner instructions (both good and bad), weather, the distance run, and horse health and mood all combine to affect the outcome of every race. That is why handicapping is so difficult, yet so much fun. Horse racing is a system that is influenced by a very large group of variables. As a consequence, even the most sophisticated computer programmes still have great difficulty in predicting winners with any reliable frequency. Perhaps
141
Alzheimers disease is like that. Maybe many of the variables already discussed play small, but significant, roles in determining who will, and who will not, get the disorder. In contrast, some systems have outcomes that are easy to predict from a few, or even one, key inputs. An atomic bomb is dropped and explodes. If you are at ground zero, or nearby, you will die. This is true for people within many miles of this location, whose injuries at greater distances may be affected by the type of building they are in at the time and the way the wind is blowing. There is no doubt, however, that the key input is the explosion of the weapon. Maybe Alzheimers disease is similar. There may be one key, overriding variable that determines who does and doesnt get this form of dementia.
gene that is responsible.3 There is no doubt that Huntingtons disease is an autosomal dominant, inherited form of dementia linked to a single gene that codes for a mutated form of the protein, huntingtin. Although genetic testing can now predict who will develop the disease, there is no effective treatment. Unfortunately, every child of a parent with Huntingtons disease has a 50 percent chance of subsequently developing it.4 Is it possible, then, that Alzheimers disease also is simply genetic, the consequence of the miscoding of some protein? The major target for researchers into the genetics of late-onset Alzheimers disease has been the APO E gene. This encodes for apolipoprotein which plays an important role in the movement and distribution of cholesterol for repairing nerve cells during early development and after any injury.5 There are three major types of the APO E gene and considerable evidence suggests that the deposition of beta amyloid is greatest in those with APO E4 allele(s). This may be because one function of the APO E protein is thought to be the removal of beta amyloid. If this is the case, it is possible that the APO E4 variant does so less effectively than the other two APO E genetic types. Certainly, fewer beta amyloid deposits seem to occur in individuals with the APO E3 variant and even less of this protein is found in those with APO E2, which may even be protective. Supporters of a major role for genetics in the genesis of Alzheimers disease claim that, by age 85, people without the APO E4 allele have an estimated risk for developing this form of dementia of between 9 and 20 percent. In individuals with one copy of the gene the risk is thought to be between 25 and 60 percent, while in those carrying two copies of the APO E4 gene, the risk of developing Alzheimers disease ranges from 50 to 90 percent.6 However, those figures are clearly in error since in Maracaibo, Venezuela Alzheimers disease is virtually unknown.7 This must mean that globally, for people carrying two copies of
143
the APO E4 gene, the risk of developing Alzheimers disease by age 85 ranges from 0 to 90 percent. In other words, the significance of the APO E4 gene is controlled by where you are, that is by location. Other lines of evidence support this position. Alzheimers disease is extremely common in Norway.8 However, even there, mortality from it varies dramatically from region to region. It is, for example, 15 times more common amongst males and 18 times more frequent in females in certain areas of Norway than it is in others. This countrys population, however, is very homogeneous and these significant variations in death rates for Alzheimers disease are extremely unlikely to be a reflection of an uneven distribution of the APO E4 allele in local inhabitants.9 Another example of evidence for the role of location is found in northern Israel,10 where all 821 elderly Arab residents of Wadi Ara were screened genetically because of the towns very high Alzheimers disease prevalence rate. Surprisingly, the APO E4 gene frequency in this population was the lowest level ever recorded.11 Taken as a whole, this evidence suggests that it is quite possible to develop Alzheimers disease without even one copy of APO E4 allele and that some elderly people are never affected by this form of dementia, even if they have two copies of the APO E4 allele. This gene, therefore, may be important in Alzheimers disease, but it is not a dominant causal variable.
Pathogens
Dementia also can be caused by a variety of pathogens. For example, it occurs in the late stages of syphilis when the bacterium, Treponema pallidum, seriously damages the brain.12 Similarly, dementia is seen in AIDS as HIV infection results in an extreme deficiency of tryptophan, leading to many of the symptoms of pellagra.13 Dementia may also be the result of infection by abnormal prions, as observed in Kuru, and variant Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease (mad cow disease).14 Prions are
144
misfolded proteins that have the unusual ability to induce normal proteins to also misfold, setting in motion a very slow chain reaction. Such misfolded prions are biologically inert, unable to participate in normal biological reactions and, as a consequence, tend to clump together forming plaques in the brains of patients with prion diseases, such as Kuru. In their recent book, Dying for a Hamburger: Modern Meat Processing and the Epidemic of Alzheimers disease, Waldman and Lamb15 argue that Alzheimers disease is probably caused by an as yet unidentified prion that has been widely spread by eating ground beef. While they provide some supporting evidence for this hypothesis, it seems very unlikely for several reasons. There is no feasible explanation, for example, why prions, viruses, bacteria, or other pathogens would be so much more likely to infect those who carry two copies of the APO E4 allele,16 nor do Waldman and Lamb mention aluminum anywhere in their book so, of course, they make no attempt to explain why high fluoride and silicic acid might protect against prion infection, or why elevated monomeric aluminum might promote it.17 In short, the idea that Alzheimers disease is infectious is not backed up by any animal studies, nor does it account for the most convincing evidence summarized in the preceding chapters.
Toxins
It has been known for centuries that mercury toxicity can cause dementia. In the 1850s, the descriptive phrase mad as a hatter was coined because of the many felters who became demented as a result of chronic mercury exposure during hat making.18 Several other metals, including lead, bismuth, aluminum, and arsenic, are capable of badly damaging the brain.19 As has been shown in the preceding literature survey, aluminum appears the most likely of these to be implicated in
145
Alzheimers disease. Evidence was presented earlier in this book from Canada, Italy, England, Wales, and Norway that strongly suggests a role for aluminum in Alzheimers disease. However, the relationship must be complex and exposure does not inevitably result in Alzheimers disease. As previously pointed out, Maracaibo, Venezuela has a population of some 1,262,000 yet is virtually free of Alzheimers disease.20 Nevertheless, Maracaibos drinking water has a mean aluminum content of 533 micrograms per litre, ranging from a minimum of 100 micrograms to a maximum of 2,060 micrograms per litre.21 In short, Maracaibos potable water aluminum levels are considerably higher than those acceptable by most international water quality standards and, indeed, frequently violate the maximum permitted by Venezuelan law.22 Indeed, aluminum levels in Maracaibos drinking water are higher than those found in most parts of Norway which suffer some of the highest Alzheimers mortality rates on earth.23
Trauma
The most obvious link between trauma and dementia occurs in many former boxers, who, during their fighting days, were hit repeatedly on the head. Such old sportsmen are often referred to as punch drunks because of dementia related to the brain damage caused by repeated blows.24 The official name for the condition is pugilistic dementia. Primary and metatstatic brain tumours also can damage the brain causing dementia. It has been discussed earlier that, in people with the APO E4 allele, severe head trauma often seems to initiate beta-amyloid deposition.25 As a consequence, not surprisingly, if victims survive their injuries, such deposition reduces the time-ofonset of sporadic Alzheimers disease in those genetically prone to it. This takes place, of course, because beta-amyloid is the major constituent of neuritic plaques. There seems to be no evidence that head trauma promotes this type of dementia in
146
those who do not carry the APO E4 allele. There must be more to Alzheimers disease than trauma, therefore, whether brain injury is the result of physical force, tumour growth, or stroke.
major settlement in Groote Eylandt is located very close to a large manganese mine. As a result, lumps of black manganese oxide can be picked up off the streets in Angurugu and a thin film of black dust can be wiped from furniture, cars, and other exposed objects. The local river, from which the water supply is derived, runs over a bed of manganese ore. It is not surprising then that manganic madness is common on the island.30 The evidence seen in the earlier literature review seems to suggest that calcium and magnesium deficiencies may be involved in Alzheimers disease.31-32 Nevertheless, the fact that there is a strong genetic component and a well established aluminum link to the dementia clearly shows there is more to Alzheimers disease than simple deficiencies of these two bulk elements.
SUMMARY
Almost anything that can seriously damage the brain can cause dementia, from the genetic abnormalities of Huntingtons chorea and Wilsons disease to the repeated head blows of pugilistic dementia. In many cases the key causal variable is obvious. There is little doubt about the driving force of manganic madness or Kuru. Such dementias have a dominant trigger, such as a genetic abnormality or toxin. Other dementias, including Alzheimers disease, do not. They are more like a horse race than a bomb explosion. Clearly, neither being born with two copies of the APO E4 gene, nor drinking water containing high levels of aluminum or low concentrations of calcium and magnesium can, in and by itself, adequately explain global variations in the risk of developing Alzheimers disease. There must be several important causal variables involved in this form of dementia. The remainder of this book is devoted to identifying these variables and how they interact to raise, or lower, the probabilities of developing Alzheimers disease.
148
REFERENCES
1. 2. Hoffman, R. (2003). Why buy that theory? In O. Sacks (Ed.), The best science writing 2003 (pp. 222-227). New York: Harper Collins. Bonelli, R.M., and Hofmann, P. (2004). A review of the treatment of options for Huntingtons disease. Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy, 5(4), 767-776. Ask the Doctor. http://www.hdsa.org/edu/AskADoctor.pl?show=10 Fast factors About HD (Tri-fold brochure). http://hdsa.org/edu/fast_ facts_brochure.html What causes Alzheimers disease? http://www.ucdmc.ucdavis.edu/ ucdhs/health/a-z/02Alzheimer/doc02causes.html Ibid. Molina, O., Cardozo, D., and Cardozo, J. (2000). Causes of dementia in Maracaibo, Venezuela: A reappraisal. Revista de Neurologia. Barcelona, 30(2), 115-117. Vogt, T. (1986). Water quality and health. Study of a possible relationship between aluminum in drinking water and dementia (in Norwegian, English summary). Sosiale Og Okonomiske Studier, 61, Statistisk Sentralbyra Oslo-Kongsvinger, 60-63. Ibid.
3. 4. 5. 6. 7.
8.
9.
10. Bowirrat, A., Friedland, R.P., Chapman, J., and Kurczyn, A.D. (2000). The very high prevalence of AD in an Arab population is not explained by APOE epsilion4 allele frequency. Neurology, 55(5), 731. 11. What causes Alzheimers disease? op.cit. 12. Wikipedia. The Free Encyclopedia (2004). Syphilis. http://en.wikipedia. org.wiki/Syphilis 13. Foster, H.D. (2002). What really causes AIDS. Victoria, BC: Trafford Publishing. 14. Waldman, M., and Lamb, M. (2004). Dying for a hamburger: Modern meat processing and the epidemic of Alzheimers disease. Toronto: McClelland and Stewart. 15. Ibid. 16. Cutler, N.R., and Sramek, J.J. (1996). Understanding Alzheimers disease. Jackson, Mississippi: University Press of Mississippi. 17. Gauthier, E., Fortier, I., Courchesne, F., Pepin, P., Mortimer, J., and Gauvreau, D. (2000). Aluminum forms in drinking water and risk of Alzheimers disease. Environmental Research, 84(3), 234-246.
149
18. Diner, B. (n.d.). eMedicine - Toxicity, Mercury. http://www.emedicine. com/EMERG/topic813.htm 19. Summers, W.K. (2003). Reversible dementia. http://www.alzheimers corp.com/article13.htm 20. Molina et al., op. cit. 21. Tahn, J.E., Sanchez, J.M., Cubilln, H.S., and Romero, R.A. (1994). Metal content of drinking water supplied to the city of Maracaibo, Venezuela. The Science of the Total Environment, 144, 59-71. 22. Ibid. 23. Vogt, op.cit. 24. Retired Boxers Foundation. The Undisputed Champions of Dignity. http://www.retiredboxers.com/ 25. Nicoll, J.A., Roberts, G.W., and Graham, D.L. (1996). Amyloid betaprotein, APOE genetype and head injury. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 777, 271-275. 26. Summers, op.cit. 27. Hoffer, A. (1989). Orthomolecular medicine for physicians. New Canaan, CT: Kents Publishing. 28. Wilsons Disease. http://www.crohns-disease-probiotics.com/wilsonsdisease.html 29. Cawte, J., Hams, G., and Kilburn, C. (1987). Manganism in a neurological ethnic complex in Northern Australia. Lancet, 2, 1257. 30. Kiloh, L.G., and Cawte, J.E. (1984). A neurological ethnic-geographic isolate on Groote Eylandt. In K-M. Chen and Y. Yase (Eds.), Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (pp. 141-144). Taipei: National Taiwan University. 31. Fujita, T. (1987). Calcium and your health. Tokyo: Japan Publications. 32. Kihira, T., Yoshida, S., Yase, Y., Ono, S., and Kondo, T. (2002). Chronic low-Ca/Mg high-Al diet induces neuronal loss. Neuropathology, 22(3), 171-179.
150
12
Cause and effect, means and ends, seed and fruit, cannot be severed; for the effect already blooms in the cause, the end preexists in the means, the fruit in the seed. Ralph Waldo Emerson: Essay on Composition
Clearly, there seems to be no single dominant causal variable in Alzheimers disease. There is nothing unusual about this. Most chronic degenerative diseases occur as a result of interaction between a genetic aberration and a physical or social environment that magnifies its significance. This concept was described succinctly by Bishop and Waldholz1 in their book Genome. These authors point out that aberrant genes do not, in and of themselves, cause disease. By and large their impact on an individuals health is minimal until the person is plunged into a harmful environment. In short, the genetic aberration is only a weakness under certain circumstances. The key to the prevention and treatment of Alzheimers disease, therefore, is not just the identification of the genetic variable(s) involved, but also an appreciation of which environments magnify and which diminish their significance.
even one copy of this allele seems to increase the risk of developing Alzheimers disease by a factor of three when compared with the general population.2 It has been shown that the: effect of the ApoE4 gene seems to be dose-related, and not an autosomal dominant trait, which was previously assumed and observed from certain analyses. The presence of an ApoE4 gene lowers the age of onset for AD symptoms, while having two copies of the gene increases the chances for developing symptoms below the age of 70 by eight times, compared with someone who has copies of the ApoE3 gene. For those people with no copies of the ApoE4 gene, the average age of onset of AD is 85 years of age. The AD risk is increased because the age of onset is decreased. The possessing of an ApoE4 gene may speed up the AD process.3 There can be little doubt, therefore, that the dominant aberrant gene involved in late-onset (sporadic) Alzheimers disease is the APO E4 allele.4 To grasp the genetic base of this type of dementia, it is essential to understand how this gene differs from other APO E isoforms. These biochemical differences have been described by Weisgraber.5 ApoE is a 299-amino acid, single-chain protein with two structural domains that also define functional domains. The three common human isoforms, ApoE2, ApoE3, and ApoE4, differ at two positions in the molecule and have very different metabolic properties and effects on disease. ApoE3 (Cys-112, Arg-158) binds normally to low density lipoprotein (LDL) receptors and is associated with normal lipid metabolism, whereas ApoE2 (Cys-112, Cys-158) binds defectively to LDL receptors and, under certain circumstances, is associated with the genetic disorder type III hyperlipo-proteinemia. ApoE4 (Arg-112, Arg-158) binds normally to LDL receptors but is associated with elevated cholesterol levels
152
and increased risk for cardiovascular disease. In addition ApoE4 is a major risk factor for Alzheimers disease and predictor for poor outcome from head injury. It is clear from this description that the three common human forms of the APO E gene have distinct molecular structures and, as a consequence, different metabolic properties. The key question to be answered is, of course, What is unique about the way the APO E4 isoform functions that encourages the growth of the plaques and neurofibrillary tangles that are so characteristic of Alzheimers disease?. The answers to this query appear to be available in the literature. It would seem that beta-amyloid is normally soluble, but when the APO E4 protein latches on, it becomes insoluble and more likely to be deposited in plaques.6 If this is correct, then people carrying the APO E4 allele have greater than usual difficulty in removing beta-amyloid from the brain and are, for genetic reasons, more likely to form plaques. It also has been established that there are marked differences in the rates at which ApoE3 and ApoE4 bind to tau protein and also to a similar protein in dendrites. It is speculated that the ApoE4 product allows the microtubule structure to come undone, leading to the observed neurofibrillary tangles.7 It appears, therefore, that individuals who inherit the APO E4 allele(s) have greater difficulty than usual in removing brain beta-amyloid and tau. As a result, these proteins are accumulated more easily and those with the allele are more likely to develop Alzheimers disease at an early age. The APO E4 allele also may be linked to even more neurological problems than this. To illustrate, animal experiments suggest that the APO E3 gene is much more effective at promoting regrowth of nerve cell extensions after injury8 than is the APO E4 allele. APO E3 also may be more protective in preventing the loss of connections between neurons.9 Beyond this, in cell cultures
153
APO E4 inhibits neurite outgrowth in rabbit dorsal root ganglion neurons.10 Simply put, individuals with the APO E4 allele(s) are more likely to suffer from brain plaques and tangles and also probably are less capable of protecting against associated neuronal damage, or of recovering from it. That is, they are prime candidates for the development of Alzheimers disease.
of drinking water and this form of dementia. Specifically, there is obviously a strong positive link between aluminum consumption, especially in its monomeric form, and the probability of locally elevated Alzheimers disease incidence and mortality.12 The negative health impact of aluminum, however, seems to be mitigated by silicic acid, calcium, and magnesium,13-14 especially in potable water with a pH of between 7.85 and 8.05.15 The impact of fluoride on Alzheimers disease risk, however, seems to vary with acidity. The harmful environment for those who carry the APO E4 allele(s) would appear to be one in which drinking water is very acidic, and contains high levels of monomeric aluminum. Such threatening potable water also contains little or no silicic acid, calcium, or magnesium. The absence of the two latter bulk elements, of course, largely accounts for its softness and low alkalinity.
DANGEROUS WATERS
Thus the epidemiological and geographical evidence allows us to identify the harmful environments for those carrying the APO E4 allele(s). What is needed now is an explanation of why such regions are so dangerous. Consider first the apparently protective effects of silicic acid. Silica and aluminum are well known antagonists.16 As a consequence, water rich in silicic acid is unlikely to carry much free aluminum. It seems likely also that aluminums high affinity for silicon influences its absorption by the intestinal tract. This was demonstrated by a British clinical trial, conducted by Edwardson and coworkers,17 in which volunteers were given an aluminum tracer (26A1) dissolved in orange juice. Elevated levels of this element were detected later in their blood. Six weeks later the same volunteers drank orange juice, again containing
155
aluminum but to which sodium silicate also had been added. After this second challenge, blood aluminum levels rose to only 15 percent of that previously reached in the absence of silica. Edwardson and colleagues18 argued that the geographical association between Alzheimers disease and the levels of aluminum in water supplies reflects this inverse relationship between aluminum and silicates. It is believed that silica promotes the formation of aluminosilicate species, limiting the gastrointestinal absorption of aluminum. Aluminum also has a strong affinity for fluoride,19 although the results are chemically complex because they vary with pH. In acidic water, fluoride and aluminum produce species such as AlF2+ but at a higher pH, the predominant form of aluminum created is Al(OH4). The significance of interactions between these two substances has been studied in some detail. Tennakone and Wickramanayake,20 for example, have shown that the presence of only 1 ppm of fluoride in water adjusted with sodium bicarbonate or citric acid to ph3, and boiled in an aluminum vessel, releases nearly 200 ppm aluminum in 10 minutes. Prolonged water boiling can elevate dissolved aluminum to 600 ppm. In contrast, if such water contains no fluoride, only 0.2 ppm aluminum levels are reached. In addition, in 10 minutes, 50 grams of acidic crushed tomatoes cooked in 200 ml of water containing 1 ppm fluoride produced a paste containing 150 ppm aluminum. In summary, acidic food or water, especially if fluoride is present, can leach excessive aluminum from cooking vessels. Furthermore, tea brewed in soft (acidic) water or flavoured with lemon juice contains significantly higher levels of bioavailable aluminum21-22 than normal. These links between fluorine and aluminum may be of great significance in Alzheimers disease since, in a recent study, Varner and coworkers23 have demonstrated that the chronic administration of the fluoraluminum complex (AlF3) to rats, in drinking water, resulted in elevated aluminum levels in brains and kidneys.
156
These high levels of aluminum were associated with damage to rat neuronal integrity, not seen in controls drinking double distilled deionized water. Interestingly, the epidemiologic and geographical evidence strongly suggests that in alkaline drinking water, fluoride is protective against Alzheimers disease.24 This implies that only those species created by aluminum and fluoride in acidic waters are dangerous to carriers of the APO E4 allele(s). The solubility of aluminum, and probably the ease with which it is absorbed, vary markedly with pH, being lowest at about pH 6.5.25 It is, therefore, quite soluble in both acidic and alkaline waters. However, the environmental evidence is very clear. Alzheimers disease is common in regions where the water is acidic, yet relatively uncommon where the potable water is alkaline.26 Taken as a whole, then, the epidemiological and geographical data shows that Alzheimers disease incidence and mortality is highest in acidic environments which lack the protective effects of silicic acid. In contrast, they are depressed in populations drinking alkaline water that is elevated in silicic acid, calcium, magnesium, and fluoride. The most logical interpretation of this data seems to be that the harmful effects of aluminum, especially in its monomeric form, are greatest in individuals carrying the APO E4 gene when they are calcium and magnesium deficient. The only missing link in the argument that Alzheimers disease occurs most often in those carrying the APO E4 gene(s) who are exposed to monomeric aluminum is evidence showing that this toxin stimulates the overproduction of beta-amyloid and tau. If it does then the generation of these two proteins will cause the greatest difficulty to carriers of the APO E4 gene(s). In such individuals, beta-amyloid and tau will tend to accumulate most quickly, forming the plaques and neurofibrillary tangles that are characteristic of Alzheimers disease. Evidence
157
is now presented to show that aluminum does, indeed, stimulate beta-amyloid and tau overproduction in individuals who are deficient in calcium and magnesium.
acid and elevated aluminum.40 Under these circumstances, the risk of developing this form of dementia is highest in those members of the population who carry the APO E4 gene(s) because they are the least capable of removing beta-amyloid, a sticky, abnormal protein that is deposited in the brain as a result of aluminums inhibition of the enzyme choline acetyltransferase, which is needed for the synthesis of acetylcholine. This beta-amyloid accumulates to form neuritic plaques.
Abnormally low phosphatase concentrations also have been reported from the brains of Alzheimers patients.53 Aluminuminduced abnormalities in levels of phosphatases, enzymes required to remove phosphate groups from protein, also appear to be involved in the formation of neurofibrillary tangles. It appears that such an aluminum-induced lack of phosphatase,54 in the brains of Alzheimers patients, occurs because of an excess of phosphates in tau that prevent this protein from performing its normal role of securing vital parts of the neuronal cytoskeleton. Thus, the cell is harmed and hyperphosphorylated tau is precipitated to form tangles. According to Roushi,55 animal studies suggest that such extra phosphates may cause neural damage even before tangles form, by interfering with one of taus normal functions, assembling and stabilizing the microtubules that carry cell organelles, glycoproteins, and other vital substances through neurons. It has been demonstrated that the more phosphate groups that are attached to synthetic neurofilament fragments, the easier it is for aluminum ions to bind and cross-link neurofilaments.56 The presence of aluminum, therefore, appears to change the paired helical filaments that make up neurofibrillary tangles so they accumulate and are not removed, in the normal way, by protein-digesting enzymes. Interestingly, a laser microprobe study of the elemental content of neurofibrillary tangles in Alzheimers disease, undertaken by Good and coworkers,57 established that the only metallic elements found to be consistently present were aluminum and iron. Evidence that aluminum can stimulate the accumulation of both beta-amyloid and tau proteins has been provided by Kawahara and coworkers.58 The researchers investigated the neurotoxicity of aluminum by exposing cultured rat cerebral cortex neurons to aluminum chloride for in excess of 3 weeks. As a result, a degeneration of neuritic processes occurred,
160
accompanied by a build-up of both beta-amyloid and tau proteins. Aluminum, therefore, can stimulate the production, in the rat cerebral cortex, of the key components of both neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles, diagnostic of Alzheimers disease. Again the evidence appears to be conclusive. Alzheimers disease is most common in populations that use acidic drinking water that contains little or no silicic acid, calcium, or magnesium. Such potable water, however, often carries elevated aluminum.59 Under these conditions, the risk of developing Alzheimers disease is greatest amongst those members of the population who carry the APO E4 allele(s).60 In part, this is due to the fact that they are the least capable of removing tau from the brain. Unfortunately, in such acid environments this abnormal protein is deposited more often because of the inhibition of the enzymes calcium/calmodulin kinase II and alkaline phosphatase by aluminum. This deposition gives rise to the neurofibrillary tangles that are a hallmark of Alzheimers disease.
SUMMARY
Individuals who inherit APO E4 gene(s) are less capable than the general population of removing the beta-amyloid and tau proteins that form the bulk of neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. As a result, such people are at higher risk of developing Alzheimers disease in regions that promote the deposition of beta-amyloid and tau. Such harmful environments are those in which the potable water is acidic, high in monomeric aluminum, and lacks silicic acid, calcium, and magnesium. Under such circumstances, aluminum can enter the brain and impair the enzyme choline acetyltransferase, creating an acetylcholine deficiency. A shortage of acetylcholine encourages the
161
growth of senile plaques. Similarly, aluminum interferes with the enzymes calcium/calmodulin kinase II and alkaline phosphatase, promoting the formation of neurofibrillary tangles. Plaques and tangles created in this manner are the hallmarks of Alzheimers disease. Such relationships, therefore, explain why this form of dementia is most common in regions of high water acidity, in those members of the population that carry the APO E4 isoform.
REFERENCES
1. Bishop, J.E., and Waldholz, M. (1990). Genome, cited by Bland, J.S. in J.R. Williams (Ed.), Biochemical individuality: The basis for the genetotrophic concept. New Caanan, CT: Keats Publishing, 1998. VIII. Strittmatter, W.J., Saunders, A.M., Schmechel, D., Pericak-Vance, M., Enghild, J., Salvesen,G.S., and Roses, A.D. (1993). Apolipoprotein E: High-avidity binding to beta-amyloid and increased frequency of type 4 allele in late-onset familial Alzheimers disease. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 90(5), 1977-1981. Genetics and the Alzheimers diseases. Chromosome 19 (AD2). http:// www.macalester.edu/~psych/whathap/UBNRP/alzheimer.chrom 19.html Strittmatter et al., op. cit. Weisgraber, K.H. (2002). Structure and function of Apolipoprotein E. Reports from the Research Units. 2002 Annual Report. Gladstone Institute of Cardiovascular Disease. San Francisco: The J. David Gladstone Institutes, University of California, San Francisco General Hospital Medical Center, 18-20. Genetics and the Alzheimers diseases op.cit. Ibid. Press Release. School of Medicine, Washington University in St. Louis. New Clue to APOE Function. http://medicine.wustl.edu/~wumpa/ news/apoe.html Ibid.
2.
3.
4. 5.
6. 7. 8.
9.
10. Huang, Y., and Mahley, R.W. (2000). Differential effects of cytosolic APOE3 and APOE4 on neurite outgrowth and the cytoskeleton. http://www.worldeventsforum.com/2000Abstracts/huang.htm
162
11. Bishop and Waldholz, op. cit. 12. Jansson, E.T. (2001). Aluminum and Alzheimers disease. Journal of Alzheimers disease, 3, 541-549. 13. Jacqmin, H., Commenges, D., Letenneur, L., Barberger-Grateau, P., and Dartiques, J.F. (1994). Components of drinking water and risk of cognitive impairment in the elderly. American Journal of Epidemiology, 139(1), 48-57. 14. Taylor, G.A., Newens, A.J., Edwardson, J.A., Key, D.W., and Forster, D.P. (1995). Alzheimers disease and the relationship between silicon and aluminum in water supplies in northern England. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 49(3), 323-324. 15. Forbes, W.F., McAiney, C.A., Hayward, L.M., and Agwani, N. (1996). Geochemical risk factors for mental functioning, based on the Ontario Longitudinal study of Aging (LSA) ii. The role of pH. Canadian Journal on Aging, 13(2), 249-267. 16. Edwardson, J.A., Moore, P.B., Ferrier, I.N., Lilley, J.S., Newton, G.W.A., Barker, J., Templar, J., and Day, J.P. (1993). Effect of silicon on gastrointestinal absorption of aluminum. Lancet, 342, 211-212. 17. Ibid. 18. Ibid. 19. Forbes et al., op. cit. 20. Tennakone, K., and Wickramanayake, S. (1987). Aluminum leaching from cooking utensils. Nature, 325(6105), 202. 21. Flaten, T.P., and Odegard, M. (1988). Tea, aluminum and Alzheimers disease. Food and Chemical Toxicology, 26, 959-960. 22. Slanina, P., French, W., Ekstrom, L.G., Loof, L., Slorach, S., and Cedergren, A. (1986). Dietary citric acid enhances absorption of aluminum in antacids. Clinical Chemistry, 32, 539. 23. Varner, J.A., Jenseen, K.F., Horvath, W., and Isaacson, R.L. (1998). Chronic administration of aluminum-fluoride or sodium-fluoride to rats in drinking water: Alterations in neuronal and cerebrovascular integrity. Brain Research, 784, 284-298. 24. Jansson, op. cit. 25. Driscoll, C.T., and Schecher, W.D. (1990). The chemistry of aluminum in the environment. Environmental Geochemistry and Health, 12(1/ 2), 28-49. 26. Jansson, op. cit.
163
27. Cherroret, G., Desor, D., Hutin, M.F., Burnel, D., Capolaghi, B., and Lehr, P.R. (1996). Effects of aluminum chloride on normal and uremic adult male rats. Tissue distribution, brain choline acetyltransferase activity, and some biological variables. Biological Trace Element Research, 54(1), 43-53. 28. Wurtman, R.J. (1992). Choline metabolism as a basis for the selective vulnerability of cholinergic neurons. Trends in Neurosciences, 15(4), 117-122. 29. Khachaturian, Z.S. (1997). Plundered memories. The Sciences, 37(4), 20-25. 30. Scinto, F.M., Daffner, K.R., Dressler, D., Ransil, B.I., Rentz, D., Weintraub, S., Mesulam, M., and Potter, H. (1994). A potential noninvasive neurobiological test for Alzheimers disease. Science, 255(5187), 1051-1054. 31. Cherroret et al., op. cit. 32. Inestrosa, N.C., and Alarcon, R. (1998). Molecular interactions of acetylcholinesterase withsenile plaques. Journal of Physiology, Paris, 92(5-6), 341-344. 33. Gottfries, C.G. (1985). Alzheimers disease and senile dementia: Biochemical characteristics and aspects of treatment. Psychopharmacology, 86(3), 245-254. 34. Perry, E.K. (1980). The cholinergic system in old age and Alzheimers disease. Age and Ageing, 9(1), 1-8. 35. Gottfries, op. cit. 36. Quirion, R., Martel, J.C., Robitaille, Y., Etienne, P., Wood, P., Nair, N.P., and Gauthier, S. (1986). Neurotransmitter and receptor deficits in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Canadian Journal of Sciences, 13(4 suppl), 503-510. 37. Yasuhara, O., Nakamura, S., Akiguchi, I., Kimura, J., and Kameyama, M. (1991). The distribution of senile plaques and acetylcholinesterase staining in the thalamus in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Rinsho Shinkeigaku, 31(4), 377-382. 38. Perry, op. cit. 39. Bonnefont, A.B., Muoz, F.J., and Inestrosa, N.C. (1998). Estrogen protects neuronal cells from the cytotoxicity induced by acetylcholinesterase-amyloid complexes. FEBS Letters, 441(2), 220-224. 40. Jansson, op. cit. 41. Levi, R., Wolf, T., Fleminger, G., and Solomon, B. (1998). Immunodetection of aluminum and aluminum induced conformational
164
changes in calmodulin-implications in Alzheimers disease. Molecular and Cellular Biochemistry, 189(1-2), 41-46. 42. Xiao, J., Perry, G., Troncoso, J., and Monteiro, M.J. (1996). Alphacalcium-calmodulin-dependent kinase II is associated with paired helical filaments of Alzheimers disease. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, 55(9), 954-963. 43. Baudier, J., and Cole, R.D. (1987). Phosphorylation of tau protein to a state like that in Alzheimers brain is catalyzed by a calcium/ calmodulin-dependent kinase and modulated by phospholipids. Journal of Biological Chemistry, 262(36), 17577-17583. 44. Levi et al., op. cit. 45. Xiao et al., op. cit. 46. Baudier et al., op. cit. 47. Savazzi, G.M., Allergi, L., Bocchi, B., Celendo, M.T., Garini, G., Raimondi, C., Vinci, S., and Borghetti, A. (1989). The physiopathologic bases of the neurotoxicity of phosphorus chelating agents containing soluble aluminum salts in patients with renal insufficiency. Progressi in Medicina, 80(4), 227-232. 48. Levi et al., op. cit. 49. Baudier et al., op. cit. 50. Corrigan, F.M., Horrobin, D.F., Skinner, E.R., Besson, J.A., and Cooper, M.B. (1998). Abnormal content of n-6 and n-3 long-chain unsaturated fatty acids in the phosphoglycerides and cholesterol esters of parahippocampal cortex from Alzheimers disease patients and its relationship to acetyl CoA content. International Journal of Biochemistry and Cell Biology, 30(2), 197-207. 51. Yamamoto, H., Saitoh, Y., Yasugawa, S., and Miyamoto, E. (1990). Dephosphorylation of tau factor by protein phosphatase 2A in synaptosomal cytosol fraction, and inhibition by aluminum. Journal of Neurochemistry, 55(2), 683-690. 52. Chan, M.K., Varghese, Z., Li, M.K., Wong, W.S., and Li, C.S. (1990). Newcastle bone disease in Hong Kong: A study of aluminum associated osteomalacia. International Journal of Artificial Organs, 13(3), 162-168. 53. Pei, J.J., Gong, C.X., Iqbal, K., Grundke-Iqbal, I., Wu, Q.L., Winblad, B., and Cowburn, R.F. (1998). Subcellular distribution of protein phosphates and abnormally phosphorylated tau in the temporal cortex from Alzheimers disease and control brains. Journal of Neural Transmission, 105(1), 69-83. 54. Yamamoto et al., op. cit.
165
55. Roushi, W. (1995). Protein studies try to puzzle out Alzheimers tangles. Science, 267(5199), 793-794. 56. Yamamoto et al., op. cit. 57. Good, P.F., Perl, D.P., Bierer, L.M., and Schmeidler, J. (1992). Selective accumulation of aluminum and iron in the neurofibrillary tangles of Alzheimers disease: A laser microprobe (LAMMA) study. Annals of Neurology, 31(3), 286-292. 58. Kawahara, M., Kato, M., and Kuroda, Y. (2001). Effects of aluminum on the neurotoxicity of primary cultured neurons and on the aggregation of beta-amyloid protein. Brain Research Bulletin, 55(2), 211-217. 59. Jansson, op. cit. 60. Genetics and the Alzheimers diseases, op. cit.
166
A BARRIER TO ANTAGONISM
13
It all looks beautifully obvious in the rear mirror. But there are situations where it needs great imaginative power, combined with disrespect for the traditional current of thought, to discover the obvious. Arthur Koestler1
Aluminums ability to cross the blood-brain barrier is influenced by the form in which it is absorbed and the levels of other compounds in the blood. To illustrate, aluminum maltolate is absorbed easily by the intestinal tract. It seems also to be able to quickly cross the blood-brain barrier.5 Furthermore, as it does, it increases the permeability of this barrier, a process with subsequent serious toxicity implications. That is, aluminum maltolate may affect the blood-brain barrier adversely, making it permeable to other damaging toxins. This may account for the variety of symptoms seen in subtypes of Alzheimers disease and even in some other forms of dementia. Interestingly, Rao and coworkers6 have suggested that maltolate-treated, elderly rabbits can be used as a good animal model of Alzheimers disease, because of their neurofibrillary pathology. In 2003, Kawahara and colleagues,7 researchers from Tokyos Metropolitan Institute for Neuroscience, described the application of aluminum maltolate to primary cultured neurons of the rat cerebral cortex, rat hippocampus, and to an immortalized hypothalamic neuronal cell line. They discovered that this form of aluminum was extremely toxic to neurons, causing rapid death. Aluminum acetylacetonate also was shown to have a similar ability to kill neurons. Aluminum maltolate is not the only substance that can enhance aluminums ability to cross the blood-brain barrier. To illustrate, Deloncle and coworkers8 have shown that when there is an increase in sodium L-glutamate in whole blood, plasma aluminum penetrates red blood cells. This would suggest that aluminum crosses the erythrocyte membrane as a glutamate complex. Experiments with rats have shown that, similarly, aluminum can pass the blood-brain barrier as a glutamate complex and then be deposited in the cortex. This aluminumL-glutamate complex is neurotoxic in vivo.9 Nevertheless,
168
aluminum enters neurons and alone induces possible conformational changes in tau which are detected by the Alz-50 antibody. Aluminum combined with glutamate does not, and neither does glutamate alone.10
of the cofactors are ions of metallic elements such as zinc, copper, manganese, potassium, calcium, and magnesium. Others are small organic molecules called coenzymes. The B vitamins, thiamine (B1), riboflavin (B2), and nicotinamide (B3), for example, are precursors of such coenzymes.14 Enzymes are usually specific to the particular reactions that they catalyse and to the substrates that are involved in these reactions. That normally means that one enzyme cannot replace another, nor are cofactors usually interchangeable. It is clear, consequently, that bulk and trace elements are essential for the correct functioning of enzymes and for life. In a discussion of the roles played by metal ions in living cells, Kench15 points out that they form a cationic climate in which all interactions occur. He further argues that in enzymatic processes there is a complex competition for sites on enzymes and substrates at the sub-cellular level. As a result, the preferential absorption of the ions of certain bulk or trace elements, or their antagonists, can either block or promote enzyme activity, altering the entire biochemistry of the cell. According to Kench:16 Around all the macro-molecular structures in the living cell hovers a cloud of metal ions, jostling for position on the surface of the large molecules and according to their numbers and characters, in the case of enzymes, helping or hindering the movement of molecules substrates or products to and fro. The wealth of living forms is reflected at the cellular and subcellular level by a vast number of possible molecular interrelationships, among which the relatively indestructible metal ions appear to have been exploited fully in a directive capacity, quickening or slowing the rate of structural change of the more evanescent carbon compounds, helping to provide these metabolic bridges and feeds-back.
170
It is at this level that the genetic and, hence, biochemical susceptibilities to harmful environments operates. It is hardly surprising that depressed calcium and magnesium intake, combined with abnormally high aluminum absorption, may inhibit some enzymatic processes. This is most likely to be true for those involving enzymes that have aluminum antagonists, such as calcium, magnesium, and iron, as cofactors.
ANTAGONISM
Antagonism among elements is widely established. To illustrate, many diseases in livestock occur because fodder, enriched in particular minerals, results in shortages in others. Cobalt deficiencies, which cause wasting in sheep and cattle, for example, can usually be linked to a high iron and manganese soil content.17 Selenium is antagonistic with arsenic, cadmium, and mercury, relationships with major human health implications. Regions that experience elevated soil mercury levels are typically those that experience high cancer mortality rates. This is because, while selenium is very protective against many cancers, it combines easily with mercury to form mercury selenide. This compound is extremely insoluble and does not pass into the food chain. Crops and livestock produced in such high soil mercury areas, therefore, tend to be selenium deficient and so are associated with elevated cancer mortality.18 Such antagonistic relationships between minerals are commonplace and appear to occur because ions with similar valence of electronic shell structures, or similar electronic configurations,19-22 are antagonistic towards each other. Aluminum shows this type of antagonism towards divalent metals including zinc, phosphorus, calcium, and magnesium.23-26 Aluminums biological activity is influenced further by at least two other elements, silicon
171
and fluorine, some compounds of which are able to chelate it while others promote its solubility. Dietary levels of minerals formed from these six elements greatly affect aluminums absorption by the digestive tract and its ability to cross the bloodbrain barrier. If it reaches the brain, the negative impact of aluminum again is due largely to its antagonism with calcium, magnesium, phosphorus, and zinc, since it has a strong tendency to replace them in important enzymes. The resulting novel compounds then create cascades of biochemical dysfunctions which eventually cause neuronal degeneration, ultimately culminating in Alzheimers disease. Evidence for such antagonism already has been provided in the previous discussion of the inhibition of the enzymes choline acetyltransferase and calmodulin by aluminum and the role of these deactivations on the formation of plaques and neurofibrillary tangles.
SUMMARY
The ability of aluminum to cross the blood-brain barrier is strongly influenced by the form in which it is absorbed and the levels of other compounds in the blood. Aluminum maltolate is particularly dangerous. On reaching the brain, aluminum interferes with those enzyme cofactors with which it is most antagonistic. These include calcium, magnesium, and probably zinc and phosphorous. Some of the enzymes involved, and the roles they play in the development of Alzheimers disease, are discussed in detail in the following chapter.
172
REFERENCES
1. 2. Koestler, A. (1959). The sleepwalkers:A history of mans changing vision of the universe. London: Penguin Wisniewski, H.M. (1986). Advances in Behavioral Biology, 29, 25. Cited by Yumoto, S., Kakimi, S., Ogawa, Y., Nagai, H., Imamura, M., and Kobayashi, K. Aluminum neurotoxicity and Alzheimers disease. In I. Hanin, M. Yoshida, and A. Fisher (Eds.), Alzheimers and Parkinsons diseases: Recent developments (pp. 223-229). New York: Plenum Press. Alfrey, A.C. (1986). Neurobiology of Aging, 7, 543. Cited by Yumoto, S. et al., op. cit. Yumoto, S., Ogawa, Y., Nagai, H., Imamura, M., and Kobayashi, K. (1995). Aluminum neurotoxicity and Alzheimers disease. In I. Hanin, M. Yoshida, and A. Fisher (Eds.), Alzheimers and Parkinsons diseases: Recent developments (pp. 223-229). New York: Plenum Press. Favarato, M., Zatta, P., Perazzolo, M., Fontana, L., and Nicolini, M. (1992). Aluminum (III) influences the permeability of the blood-brain barrier to [14C] sucrose in rats. Brain Research, 569(2), 330-335. Rao, J.K., Katsetos, C.D., Herman, M.M., and Savory, J. (1998). Experimental aluminum encephalomyelopathy. Relationship to human neurodegenerative disease. Clinics in Laboratory Medicine, 18(4), 687-698. Kawahara, M., Hosoda, R., Kato-Negishi, M. and Kuroda, Y. (2003). Neurotoxicity of aluminum on primary cultured neurons. Paper presented at the Fifth Keele Meeting on Aluminum, February 2225, 2003. http://www.keele.ac.uk/depts/ch/groups/aluminium/ Kawahara.pdf Deloncle, R., Guillard, O., Clanet, F., Courtois, P., and Piriou, A. (1990). Aluminum transfer as glutamate complex through blood-brain barrier. Possible implication in dialysis encephalopathy. Biological Trace Element Research, 25(1), 39-45. Deloncle, R., Huguet, F., Babin, P., Fernandez, B., Quellard, N., and Guillard, O. (1999). Chronic administration of aluminium L-glutamate in young mature rats: Effects on iron levels and lipid peroxidation in selected brain areas. Toxicology Letters, 104(1-2), 65-73.
3. 4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10. Jones, K.R., and Oorschot, D.E. (1998). Do aluminum and/or glutamate induce Alz-50 reactivity? A light microscopic immunohistochemical study. Journal of Neurocytology, 27(1), 45-57. 11. Enzymes. http://users.rcn.com/jkimball.ma.ultranet/BiologyPages/E/ Enzymes.html
173
12. Ibid. 13. Ibid. 14. Ibid. 15. Kench, J.E. (1961). Interactions of ions in living cells. In K.H. Schtte (Ed.), Some aspects of trace elements in nature. Cape Town: Botany Department, University of Cape Town. Cited by Schtte, K.H. and Myers, J.A. (1979). Metabolic aspects of health, nutritional elements in health and disease. Kenfield, CA: Discovery Press. 16. Ibid. 17. Bowie, S.H.U., and Thornton, I. (Eds.) (1985). Environmental geochemistry and health report to the Royal Societys British National Committee for Problems of the Environment. Dordrecht: D. Reidel Publishing Co. 18. Foster, H.D. (1986). Reducing cancer mortality: A geographical perspective. Victoria, BC: Western Geographical Series, Vol. 32. 19. Hill, C.H., and Matrone, G., (1970). Chemical parameters in the study of in vivo and in vitro interactions of transition elements. Federation Proceedings, 29, 1474-1481. 20. Hartman, R.H., Matrone, G., and Wise, H.G., (1955). Effect of dietary manganese on haemoglobin formation. Journal of Nutrition, 55, 429-439. 21. Thompson, A.B.R., Olatumbosun, D., and Valberg, L.S. (1971). Interaction in intestinal transport system for manganese and iron. Journal of Laboratory and Clinical Medicine, 78, 642-655. 22. Chetty, K.N., (1972). Interaction of cobalt and iron in chicks. PhD thesis. North Carolina State University. 23. Tamari, G.M. (1994). Aluminum-toxicity and prophylaxis. Data Medica, 2(1), 48-52. 24. Yasui, M., and Ota, K., (1998). Aluminum decreases the magnesium concentrations of spinal cord and trabecular bone in rats fed a low calcium, high aluminum diet. Journal of Neurological Sciences, 157(1), 37-41. 25. Neathery, M.W., Crowe, N.A., Miller, W.J., Crowe, C.T., Varnado, J.L., and Blackmon, D.M. (1990). Effect of dietary aluminum and phosphorus on magnesium metabolism in dairy calves. Journal of Animal Science, 68(4), 1133-1138. 26. Hussein, A.S., Cantor, A.H., Johnson, T.H., and Yokel, R.A., (1990). Relationship of dietary aluminum, phosphorus, and calcium to phosphorus and calcium metabolism and growth performance of broiler chicks. Poultry Science, 69(6), 966-971.
174
14
The principle Pluralitas non est ponenda sine neccesitate or plurality should not be posited without necessity is often accredited to the medieval Franciscan monk William of Occam (ca. 1285-1349).2 The concept is known as Occams razor, or the principle of parsimony, and is now interpreted to mean dont multiply hypotheses unnecessarily or the simpler the explanation the better. More crudely, it has become keep it simple, stupid. Here, I interpret Occams razor to mean that the aluminum antagonist hypothesis is more likely to be correct if it can account, not just for the vulnerability caused by the APO E4 allele(s) and associated plaques and tangles, but also other pathological and biochemical abnormalities seen in Alzheimers disease. An attempt now is made to show that this is possible.
synthesis of acetylcholine, it is expected that neurons dedicated to acetylcholine production and distribution are affected very adversely by aluminum. Interestingly, as expected, this decrease in the metabolic activity in the nucleus basalis neurons in Alzheimers disease is more common in patients with either one or two APO E4 allele(s).6 The eventual death of nucleus basalis neurons in this type of dementia means that less than normal acetylcholine is available to the brain, reducing both memory and learning capacity.7
mechanisms for triggering and directing myelination are not known, it is well established that myelination does not occur in the absence of axons or axon/neuron-derived factors. This appears to be true. Despite our incomplete knowledge of myelination, it is clear that aluminum disrupts the process. Deloncle and colleagues,10 for example, did an ultrastructural study of the rat hippocampus in five distinct groups of rats. Three of these consisted of young animals, one group of which received daily subcutaneous injections of aluminum L-glutamate. The other two groups of young rats were injected with either sodium L-glutamate or saline solution. The two remaining rat groups consisted of older animals, one of which received subcutaneous aluminum L-glutamate on a daily basis. The other was untreated. The evidence collected showed that aluminum L-glutamate caused a thinning of the myelin sheath even in young rats. This breakdown in myelin did not occur in the young control group of sodium L-glutamate rats. It was seen, however, in the aluminum injected older rats where the myelin sheath was thinned to breaking point. Myelin loss also occurred, but less dramatically, in untreated elderly rats. It is clear from this experiment that aluminum accelerates myelin loss in rats, a process which occurs naturally but more slowly in elderly animals. A similar experiment was conducted by Golub and Tarara,11 who fed Swiss-Webster mice diets containing aluminum lactate from conception to maturity, a period of 45 days. Mean myelin sheath widths on the spinal cord were found to be 16 percent less in the aluminum treated group than in the control group. Golub and Tarara12 concluded that aluminum exposure can interfere with myelination of the spinal cord in mice. Verstraeten and coworkers13 have shown that because myelin has a high relative ratio of lipids to proteins compared to other membranes, it appears particularly susceptible to oxidative
177
damage promoted by aluminum. This is due to the high content of galactolipids found in myelin from aluminum intoxicated mice. These lipid abnormalities cause changes in membrane physical properties that seem to accelerate oxidation rates.14 These animal experiments clearly demonstrate that aluminum can alter the nature of myelin, accelerate its oxidation rates, and promote its rapid loss from the hippocampus and spinal cord. Exactly how these procedures occur is uncertain. In a study of brains from monkeys chronically administered aluminum, Sarin and colleagues15 were able to show, however, that this metal had inhibited three membrane-bound enzymes: specifically Na+K+ ATPase, acetylcholinesterase, and, most interestingly, the myelin-specific enzyme 23-cyclic nucleotide phosphohydrolase. Aluminum, therefore, interferes with myelin in numerous ways. It has the ability to inhibit three membrane-bound enzymes in the brain membranes of primates.16 It can cause a rapid thinning of the myelin sheath in both rats17 and mice,18 and it can alter its composition by increasing galactolipids and so make myelin more prone to oxidation.19-20 It does not seem much of a step to suggest that these destructive processes probably lie behind the demyelinization and associated retrogenesis seen in Alzheimers patients.21
of the phospholipids that concentrate in such brain cell membranes. Corrigan and coworkers,23 for example, have shown that in Alzheimers disease, phospholipids from the parahippocampal cortex, including phosphatidylcholine, phosphatidylserine, and phosphatidylinositol, contain below normal levels of alpha-linolenic acid. In addition to this depression of the level of n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acid, abnormalities also occur in levels of n-6 essential fatty acids. It has been demonstrated further that not only are the biochemical compositions of phospholipids from Alzheimers patients abnormal but that total concentrations of such membrane phospholipids are low24 and that their regional distribution in the brain is irregular.25 It has been suggested that the biochemical abnormalities seen in phospholipids in Alzheimers disease are the result of elevated oxidative stress. However, aluminum also seems to be more directly involved. To illustrate, aluminum chloride has been shown to inhibit the incorporation of inositol into phospholipids.26 Deleers and coworkers27 also have demonstrated aluminum-induced lipid phase separation and fusion of phospholipid membranes. However, it seems more likely that disruption of phospholipase A2 by aluminum28 is the primary cause of the biochemical abnormalities seen in phospholipids in Alzheimers disease, and the chief cause of related brain membrane dysfunctions. Certainly, phospholipase A2 plays a key role in the metabolism of membrane phospholipids,29 is decreased in Alzheimers disease, 30 and is inhibited by aluminum chloride.31
Much of the destruction of neurons that characterizes Alzheimers disease has been linked to the lipid peroxidation of cell membranes caused by free radicals. This process seems to occur because of disturbed defense mechanisms in Alzheimers disease which are associated with a self-propagating cascade of neurodegeneration.32-33 It has been established, for example, that Alzheimers patients display depressed plasma antioxidant status associated with significantly low vitamin E levels.34 There is considerable evidence that aluminum itself reduces the bodys defence against free radical damage. In dialysis patients, for example, serum glutathione-peroxidase levels are significantly depressed.35 Similarly, animal studies have demonstrated that the oral administration of aluminum sulphate, especially in the presence of citric acid, inhibits brain superoxide dismutase and catalase activities.36 Interestingly, vitamin E, which is depressed in Alzheimers patients, can protect rats against associated aluminum-induced free radical damage.37 Other evidence of the significance of oxidative stress includes a significant increase in erythrocyte Cu/Zn superoxide dismutase and catalase activity in the blood of Alzheimers patients38 and a pronounced increase in superoxide dismutase immunoreactivity in olfactory epithelium.39 Exactly how aluminum is involved in the catastrophic loss of neurons from free radical damage is being established by van Rensburg and coworkers40-41 and Fu and colleagues.42 The former have shown, for example, that both beta-amyloid and aluminum dose-dependently increase lipid peroxidation in platelet membranes. Their research has established that betaamyloid is toxic to biological membranes and that aluminum is even more so.43 Beyond this, van Rensburg and colleagues have demonstrated that iron encourages lipid peroxidation both by aluminum and by beta-amyloid protein. This is of considerable interest since only the metallic elements found in the
180
neurofibrillary tangles of Alzheimers disease are aluminum and iron.44 Van Rensburg and coworkers in vitro model also demonstrated that melatonin prevented lipid peroxidation by aluminum and beta-amyloid protein in the absence of hydrogen peroxide. If the latter were present, melatonin could only slow the process.45 Fu and coworkers46 have begun to explain how beta-amyloid specifically damages neurons. Their cell culture research has shown that beta-amyloid interferes with calcium homeostasis and induces apoptosis in neurons by oxidative stress. This latter process involves the catecholamines (norepinephrine, epinephrine, and dopamine), which increase the toxicity of beta-amyloid to cultured hippocampal neurons. These findings are consistent with the much earlier research of Hoffer, Osmond, and Smythies,47-48 who argued that the oxidation of adrenalin to adrenochrome was responsible for the hallucinogenic symptoms seen in schizophrenia. More recently, Hoffer49 has suggested that the oxidation of dopamine to dopachrome lies behind the psychotic symptoms seen in Parkinsonism, in many long-term levodopa users. Fu and coworkers50 also have been able to show that the antioxidants vitamin E, glutathione, and propyl gallate can protect neurons against damage caused by amyloid beta-peptide and the catecholamines. Aluminum also may increase free radical damage in Alzheimers disease by inhibiting the protective copper/zinc metalloenzyme, superoxide dismutase. Normally, this is one of the major enzymes that provides protection against free radicals. However, Shainkin-Kesterbaum and coworkers51 showed that in vitro, at the levels of the enzyme found in dialysis patients, aluminum severely inhibited its protective effects. This inhibition of superoxide dismutases antioxidant activity was directly proportional to the level of aluminum. Silicon was found to have a similar inhibitory effect on the enzyme. The disruptive
181
influence of aluminum on superoxide dismutase may account for the fact that, while zinc supplementation generally improves mental alertness in the elderly, in Alzheimers patients it accelerates deterioration of cognition, encouraging amyloid plaque formation.52 This may be due to the fact that, in the latter stages of Alzheimers disease, it cannot be used in disrupted superoxide dismutase production and so merely stimulates free radical formation.
SUMMARY
Aluminum does more than just cause the accumulation of the beta-amyloid and tau that disrupts the brains of Alzheimers patients with neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. Its inhibition of choline acetyltransferase eventually kills the acetylcholine neurons in the Nucleus Basalis of Meynert. Beyond this, its negative impact on Na+K+ ATPase, acetylcholinesterase, and on 23-cyclic nucleotide phosphohydrolase appears responsible for the destruction of myelin and the associated retrogenesis that accompanies Alzheimers disease. Aluminum also inhibits the enzyme phospholipase A2, probably causing brain membrane dysfunctions, and seems to cause depression of antioxidant status by reducing levels of brain glutathione peroxidase, superoxide dismutase, and catalase. As a result, the lipid peroxidation of cell membranes by free radicals is accelerated.
182
REFERENCES
1. 2. 3. Heinlein, R. cited by Northcutt, W. (2003). The Darwin Awards III. Dutton: New York, p. 73. Carroll, R.T. The Skeptics Dictionary. http:skepdic.com/occam.html Iraizoz, I., Guijarro, J.L., Gonzalo, L.M., and de Lacalle, S. (1999). Neuropathological changes in the nucleus basalis correlate with clinical measures of dementia. Acta Neuropathologica (Berl), 98(2), 186-196. Cherroret, G., Desor, D., Hutin, M.F., Burnel, D., Capolaghi, B., and Lehr, P.R. (1996). Effects of aluminum chloride on normal and uremic adult male rats. Tissue distribution, brain choline acetyltransferase activity, and some biological variables. Biological Trace Element Research, 54(1), 43-53. Inestrosa, N.C., and Alarcon, R. (1998). Molecular interactions of acetylcholinesterase with senile plaques. Journal of Physiology, Paris, 92(5-6), 341-344. Salehi, A., Dubelaar, E.J.G., Mulder, M., and Swaab, D.F. (1998). Aggravated decrease in the activity of nucleus basalis neurons in Alzheimers disease is apolipoprotein E-type dependent. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 95(19), 11445-11449. http:www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=21662 Iraizoz et al., op. cit. Shenk, D. (2001). The Forgetting: Alzheimers disease: Portrait of an epidemic. New York: Doubleday. Raval-Fernandes, S., and Rome, L.H. (1998). Role of axonal components during myelination. Microscopy Research and Technique, 41(5), 379-392.
4.
5.
6.
7. 8. 9.
10. Deloncle, R., Huguet, F., Fernandez, B., Quellard, N., Babin, P., and Guillard, O. (2001). Ultrastructural study of rat hippocampus after chronic administration of aluminum L-glutamate: An acceleration of the aging process. Experimental Gerontology, 36(2), 231-244. 11. Golub, M.S., and Tarara, R.P. (1999). Morphometric studies of myelination in the spinal cord of mice exposed developmentally to aluminum. Neurotoxicology, 20(6), 953-959. 12. Ibid. 13. Verstraeten, S.V., Golub, M.S., Keen, C.L., and Oteiza, P.I. (1997). Myelin is a preferential target of aluminum-mediated oxidative damage. Archives of Biochemistry and Biophysics, 344(2), 289-294. 14. Verstraeten, S.V., Keen, C.L., Golub, M.S., and Oteiza, P.I. (1998). Membrane composition can influence the rate of Al3+-mediated
183
lipid oxidation: Effect of galactolipids. Biochemical Journal, 333 (Pt.3), 833-838. 15. Sarin, S., Gupta, V., and Gill, K.D. (1997). Alteration in lipid composition and neuronal injury in primates following chronic aluminum exposure. Biological Trace Element Research, 59(1-3), 133-143. 16. Ibid. 17. Deloncle et al., op. cit. 18. Golub et al., op. cit. 19. Verstraeten et al. (1997), op. cit. 20. Verstraeten et al. (1998), op. cit. 21. Shenk, op. cit. 22. The SMID Group (1987). Phosphatidylserine in the treatment of clinically diagnosed Alzheimers disease. Journal of Neural Transmission Supplementum, 24, 287-292. 23. Corrigan, F.M., Horrobin, D.F., Skinner, E.R., Besson, J.A., and Cooper, M.B. (1998). Abnormal content of n-6 and n-3 long-chain unsaturated fatty acids in the phosphoglycerides and cholesterol esters of parahippocampal cortex from Alzheimers disease patients and its relationship to acetyl CoA content. International Journal of Biochemistry and Cell Biology, 30(2), 197-207. 24. Gottfries, C.G., Karlsson, I., and Svennerholm, L. (1996). Membrane components separate early-onset Alzheimers disease from senile dementia of the Alzheimers type. International Psychogeriatrics/ IPA, 8(3), 365-372. 25. Prasad, M.R., Lovell, M.A., Yatin, M., Dhillon, H., and Markesberg, W.R. (1998). Regional membrane phospholipid alterations in Alzheimers disease. Neurochemical Research, 23(1), 81-88. 26. Johnson, G.V., and Jope, R.S. (1986). Aluminum impairs glucose utilization and cholinergic activity in rat brain in vitro. Toxicology, 40(1), 93-102. 27. Deleers, M., Servais, J.P., and Wlfert, E. (1987). Aluminum-induced lipid phase separation and membrane fusion does not require the presence of negatively charged phospholipids. Biochemistry International, 14(6), 1033-1034. 28. Jones, D.L., and Kochian, L.V. (1997). Aluminum interaction with plasma membrane lipids and enzyme metal binding sites and its potential role in Al cytotoxicity. FEBS Letters, 400(1), 51-57. 29. Gattaz, W.F., Cairns, N.J., Levy, R., Frstl, H., Braus, D.F., and Maras, A. (1996). Decreased phospholipase A2 activity in the brain and plate-
184
lets of patients with Alzheimers disease. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 246(3), 129-131. 30. Gottfries, C.G., Karlsson, I., and Svennerholm, L. (1996). Membrane components separate early-onset Alzheimers disease from senile dementia of the Alzheimers type. International Psychogeriatrics/ IPA, 8(3), 365-372. 31. Jones et al., op. cit. 32. Retz, W., Gsell, W., Mnch, G., Rsler, N., and Riederer, P. (1998). Free radicals in Alzheimers disease. Journal of Neural Transmission. Supplementum, 54, 221-236. 33. Van Rensburg, S.J., Daniels, W.M., Potocnik, F.C., Van Zyl, J.M., Talijaard, J.J., and Emsley, R.A. (1997). A new model for the pathophysiology of Alzheimers disease. Aluminum toxicity is exacerbated by hydrogen peroxide and attenuated by an amyloid protein fragment and melatonin. South African Medical Journal, 87(9), 1111-1115. 34. Sinclair A.J., Bayer, A.J., Johnson, J., Warner, C., and Maxwell, S.R. (1998). Alteredplasma antioxidant status in subjects with Alzheimers disease and vascular dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13(12), 840-845. 35. Turan, B., Delilba, Si, E., Dalay, N., Sert, S., Afrasyap, L., and Sayal, A. (1992). Serum selenium and glutathione-peroxidase activities and their interaction with toxic metals in dialysis and renal transplantation patients. Biological Trace Element Research, 33, 95-102. 36. Swain, C., and Chainy, G.B. (1998). Effects of aluminum sulphate and citric acid ingestion on lipid peroxidation and on activities of superoxide dismutase and catalase in cerebral hemisphere and liver of developing chicks. Molecular and Cell Biochemistry, 187(1-2), 163172. 37. Abd el-Fattah, A.A., al-Yousef, H.M., al-Bekairi, A.M., and al-Sawaf, H.A. (1998). Vitamin E protects the brain against oxidative injury stimulated by excessive aluminum. Biochemistry and Molecular Biology International, 46(6), 1175-1180. 38. Perrin, R., Brian, C.S., Jeandeal, C., Artur, Y., Minn, A., Penin, F., and Siest, G. (1990). Blood activity of Cu/Zn superoxide dismutase, glutathione peroxidase and catalase in Alzheimers disease: A casecontrol study. Gerontology, 36(5-6), 306-313. 39. Kulkarni-Narla, A., Getchell, T.V., Schmitt, F.A., and Getchell, M.L. (1996). Manganese and copper-zinc superoxide dismutase in the human olfactory mucosa: Increased immunoreactivity in Alzheimers disease. Experimental Neurology, 140(2), 115-125. 40. Van Rensburg et al. (1997), op. cit.
185
41. Daniels, W.M., van Rensburg, S.J., van Zyl, J.M., and Talijaard, J.J. (1998). Melatonin prevents beta-amyloid-induced lipid peroxidation. Journal of Pineal Research, 24(2), 78-82. 42. Fu, W., Luo, H., Parthasarathy, S., and Mattson, M.P. (1998). Catecholamines potentiate amyloid beta-peptide neurotoxicity: Involvement of oxidative stress, mitochondrial dysfunction, and perturbed calcium homeostatis. Neurobiology of Disease, 5(4), 229243. 43. Daniels et al., op. cit. 44. Good, P.F., Perl, D.P., Bierer, L.M., and Schmeidler, J. (1992). Selective accumulation of aluminum and iron in the neurofibrillary tangles of Alzheimers disease: A laser microprobe (LAMMA) study. Annals of Neurology, 31(3), 286-292. 45. Daniels et al., op. cit. 46. Fu et al., op. cit. 47. Hoffer, A., and Osmond, H. (1963). Malvaria: A new psychiatric disease. Acta Psychiatrica Scandanavica, 39, 335-366. 48. Hoffer, A., Osmond, H., and Smythies, J. (1954). Schizophrenia: A new approach. Results of a years research. Journal of Mental Science, 100, 29. 49. Hoffer, A. (1998). Vitamin B-3 schizophrenia: Discovery recovery controversy. Kingston, ON: Quarry Press. 50. Fu et al., op. cit. 51. Shainkin-Kesterbaum, R., Adler, A.J., Berlyne, G.M., and Caruso, C. (1989). Effect of aluminum on superoxide dismutase. Clinical Science, 77(5), 463-466. 52. Kaiser, K. (1994). Alzheimers: Could there be a zinc link? Science, 265(5177), 1365.
186
15
By the eighteenth century, the theorists had succeeded in obfuscating with a bewildering fog of shifting hypotheses a problem that should have been, and indeed was at one time, solved by basic observation and common sense. Physicians would have been better off throwing out everything they had learned and starting over with basic, observable truths. It would take a remarkably bright and original thinker to even begin to deconstruct the intricate latticework of preposterous ideas to uncover the small kernel of truth that lay underneath it all. S.R. Brown, Scurvy1
The evidence presented in this book so far strongly suggests that people who inherit APO E4 allele(s) are less capable than the general population of removing brain beta-amyloid and tau proteins. Consequently, such individuals are at higher risk of developing Alzheimers disease, as these abnormal proteins build up and form neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. Naturally, this process occurs more often and most rapidly in regions that promote the deposition of beta-amyloid and tau. Such harmful environments are those where drinking water is acidic, high in monomeric aluminum, and lacking magnesium, calcium, and silicic acid. Under such circumstances, aluminum enters the brain and impairs various enzymes, including choline acetyltransferase, calcium/calmodulin kinase II, alkaline phosphatase, and phospholipase A2. The end result of this process is the abnormal brain pathology seen in Alzheimers disease patients and the disrupted biochemistry
187
with which it is associated. In an earlier publication,2 I called this explanation of the downward spiral, known as Alzheimers disease, Fosters Multiple Antagonist Hypothesis. The most effective way to test the validity of this hypothesis is to attempt to use it to explain the evidence that has been collected about Alzheimers disease by disciplines as diverse as genetics and history. That is, to try to see whether the scattered pieces of the Alzheimers disease jigsaw puzzle can be put together into a coherent picture, using the multiple antagonist hypothesis as a dominant theme. To assist in this process, Table 1 lists the clues identified in the preceding literature review chapters. For ease of identification, each clue is numbered according to the chapter in which it is first discussed and it is lettered to identify its position within that chapter. To illustrate, clue 1C (Demyelinization) is the third clue discussed in Chapter 1 - What Was I? What follows is my attempt to explain each of the clues in this table using the multiple antagonist hypothesis as a starting point. I cannot explain some of them. This may be because the multiple antagonist hypothesis is incorrect or incomplete, or the data the clue is based on was in error, or I am too ignorant to be aware of the real link between the tested hypothesis and the clue. My goal, therefore, is to explain the majority of the clues in Table 1 and to do so in a manner that is more convincing than explanations that have been put forward using all other competing hypotheses.
188
WHO WAS I?
This books first chapter describes the current Alzheimers disease pandemic and emphasizes its growing potential for future chaos. Three of the five major clues to the etiology of this dementia, retrogenesis, demyelinization, and the APO E4 gene, have been accounted for already. Retrogenesis, for example,
191
occurs because aluminum inhibits at least three membranebound enzymes, Na+K+ ATPase, acetylcholinesterase, and, most interestingly, the myelin-specific enzyme 23-cyclic nucleotide phosphohydrolase.3 As a result, it can cause rapid thinning of the myelin sheath4 and increase its susceptibility to oxidative stress.5 It seems very likely that these destructive processes cause demyelinization and associated retrogenesis. The APO E4 allele(s) frequently occur in Alzheimers patients. This is because individuals inheriting this gene are inefficient in removing the beta-amyloid and tau proteins that form the bulk of the brain neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles.6 This gene is, therefore, a major handicap for those who are living in milieus and eating diets that promote the formation of beta-amyloid and tau. Such harmful environments are those in which drinking water is acidic, high in monomeric aluminum, and lacks silicic acid, calcium, and magnesium. Under these environmental conditions, especially if diet is dominated by processed foods that lack calcium and magnesium, aluminum enters the brain and impairs the enzymes choline acetyltransferase, calcium/calmodulin kinase II, and alkaline phosphatase, promoting the formation of plaques and tangles.7-8 These are poorly removed by those who inherited the APO E4 allele(s). There are similarities in the central nervous system decline in Alzheimers disease, Guamanian amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, and Parkinsonism with dementia. The two latter disorders occur most frequently in Guam, the Kii Peninsula of Japan, and Western New Guinea. The river waters in such loci are exceptionally deficient in calcium and magnesium, suggesting that the intake of these minerals must be abnormally low in the local inhabitants.9-10 In the Kii Peninsula, for example, potable water contains less than 2 ppm calcium and below 1 ppm magnesium.11 Obviously, under such conditions, aluminums ability to inhibit several enzymes is exacerbated.
192
Alzheimers patients are very glucose deficient and seem to have been that way long before symptoms of this dementia become apparent. This glucose abnormality occurs, in part, due to aluminum binding with the phosphate enzyme, glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase and its interference with hexokinase.12 To illustrate, Cho and Toshi13 purified two isozymes from pig and human brains and showed that they contained an enzymealuminum complex. They were then able to show that glucose6-phosphate could be completely inactivated by aluminum, but that this enzymes potency could be restored by the three aluminum chelators: citrate, sodium fluoride, and apotransferrin. However, aluminums negative impact on glucose metabolism is not limited to inhibiting the glucose-6-phosphate enzyme. Lai and Blass14 have shown that this metal also inhibits hexokinase activity in the rat brain, but that high levels of magnesium can reverse this process. Aluminum also seems to inactivate hepatic phosphofructokinase, an important control site in the glycolytic pathway.15 It is hardly surprising, therefore, that Alzheimers disease patients display glucose abnormalities.
Scottish law permits a unique verdict. When the prosecutor is unable to provide conclusive evidence but the jury feels that the accused, nevertheless, is guilty as charged, it is able to bring down a verdict of not proven. My Scottish grandmother explained to me that this verdict meant not guilty, but dont do it again. This is how I feel about the history of Alzheimers disease. All the evidence points towards a steady increase in age-adjusted incidence and mortality, but such trends are very difficult to prove beyond a shadow of a doubt. In Dying for a Hamburger, Waldman and Lamb16 examined the number of articles in the medical literature in the years since the initial identification of Alzheimers disease. From an ever rising interest in this form of dementia they concluded: Alzheimers disease has become more and more common to the point that today the dementias of aging are considered the norm rather than the exception. But, as we will see, it was even in fairly recent times so rare that the greatest medical observers of all time did not even mention it. If one accepts that Waldman and Lamb17 are correct about a steady increase in the incidence of Alzheimers disease, can the multiple antagonist hypothesis explain it? Obviously, such an increase cannot be due to any genetic cause. We do not have epidemics of genetic diseases, simply because the human genome does not change rapidly enough to trigger them. If, as the evidence strongly suggests, Alzheimers disease is becoming generally more common, it must be because the harmful environments that trigger it are now more widespread. As has been established, these are areas where potable water is acidic, high in monomeric aluminum, and lacking magnesium, calcium, and silicic acid. These environments are particularly dangerous if the local population is eating mineral depleted diets.
194
There is no doubt that, globally, soils and water are becoming more acidic and, as a consequence, aluminum more soluble. Throughout the 20th and early 21st centuries, as a result of expanding fossil fuel consumption, ever increasing quantities of sulphur and nitrogen were emitted into the atmosphere. Here they were converted into sulphuric and nitric acids, raising the acidity of subsequent precipitation.18 Such acid rain has caused extensive damage to the environment at local, regional, and even global scales. It has been particularly problematic in northern and central Europe, eastern North America, and eastern China, where it has been associated with many health costs.19 Simultaneously, commercial fertilizers have been used with increasing frequency. These consist mainly of nitrogen, phosphorus, and potassium. Due to heavy crop yields, agricultural soils have been depleted of several minerals that are important for human health, including calcium and magnesium. To illustrate, Marier and colleagues,20 in Water Hardness, Human Health, and the Importance of Magnesium, have pointed out how this mineral is becoming less and less common in the food we eat because of the fertilizers used in the Green Revolution. As early as 1974, Elmstrom and Fiskele21 claimed: Magnesium deficiency is a frequently-occurring nutrient deficiency in the South-Eastern USA [and] is a frequently-developing problem on well-fertilized soils, [involving] an imbalance between Mg and Ca, or between Mg and K. Similar magnesium soil deficiencies have been reported for many other countries, including France, Germany, Denmark, and Canada.22 As previously discussed, processing and cooking also remove minerals from food, and packaging and canning often add aluminum to it. Consequently, most of the populations of the
195
Western World appear to be very magnesium and often calcium deficient.23-24 Simply put, drinking water is becoming more acidic, and so aluminum is more soluble, foodstuffs contain fewer minerals as the result of commercial fertilizers, and many of the remaining minerals are removed by processing and cooking. We are creating more harmful environments that allow aluminum to reach the human brain where it inhibits crucial enzymes. If there has been no increase in the incidence of Alzheimers disease as a result of these environmental and social trends, then the multiple antagonist hypothesis must be incorrect. However, it should be pointed out that, in Norway, there is no doubt that Alzheimers disease is now common in those counties receiving the most acidic rainfall.25 This is not surprising since the ancient Shield rocks of Scandinavia are naturally highly deficient in both calcium and magnesium.26
genetic aberration can remove beta-amyloid and tau, has been reviewed in detail.31 Genetically, however, there is more to Alzheimers disease than the APO E4 gene. In fact, there seem to be several such links. To date, four genes have been identified as playing a role in either early- or late-onset Alzheimers: canning, presenilin-1, presenilin-2, and apolipoprotein E genes.32 Workers have linked most of these variants to familial earlyonset Alzheimers, but the apolipoprotein E4 allele is a relatively common risk factor for developing late-onset Alzheimers disease.33 Considerable progress has been made in the interpretation of the significance of such genetic variants. To illustrate, mutations in the presenilin-1 gene seem associated with increased superoxide production and greater vulnerability to amyloid beta peptide toxicity.34 Interestingly, mutations in the presenilin genes, which are linked to more than 40 percent of all familial cases of Alzheimers disease, cause enhanced production of an abnormal form of canning.35 This protein is longer than normal, aggregates more rapidly, kills neurons in culture more effectively, and precipitates preferentially to form amyloid plaques. The same elongated protein also is produced as a result of mutations in the gene encoding canning. The literature suggests, therefore, that the gene variants that predispose to both early- and late-onset Alzheimers disease do so because they either increase susceptibility to, or mimic, the aluminum-related degenerative processes previously described. That is, the genetic mutations involved in promoting the development of Alzheimers disease duplicate some of aluminums deleterious impacts on the brain and in so doing, encourage at least one of the following: the growth of neuritic plaques or neurofibrillary tangles, excessive free radical formation, and higher neural oxidative stress. As a result, unfortunate individuals carrying any one of the genetic variants are much more
197
likely to develop Alzheimers disease, even if they are not exposed to the aluminum excess or vitamin and mineral deficiencies, that are normally associated with its etiology.
also that diabetics would benefit from magnesium supplements. In early 2004, researchers from the Harvard Medical School came to almost the same conclusion after studying the dietary habits of 85,060 women and 42,872 men, for 18 and 12 years respectively, and determining who developed diabetes. Such a study, supported by the National Institutes of Health and National Institutes of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases probably cost tens of millions of dollars and was completed almost 20 years after my own research was published. Geographical evidence that can identify the triggers for genetic aberrations is readily available, generally free, and simple to analyse. Using it, one can locate the spatial extremes of any diseases incidence and mortality. These I term hot spots, where the disease is particularly common and lethal, and cold spots, where it is extremely rare or even unknown. These environmental extremes and their characteristics have been determined for numerous common illnesses. Consider, for example, the cold spot for osteoporosis.40 The ranchers of Texas are well aware that migration can alter bone strength and density. Cattle raised on the high plains of Deaf Smith County are larger and heavier than those from elsewhere in Texas.41 Indeed, fully grown 6-year old cattle moved onto pasture in the high plains will gain a minimum of 250 pounds as their bones increase in size and weight. The skeletons of residents of Deaf Smith County are similarly dense and highly mineralized. The elderly there rarely show signs of demineralization and osteomalacia which are so common in other mature Texans. Cortices of their long bones are about one-half greater in thickness than those seen in Dallas County. In Deaf Smith County, the bones of residents 80 years or older only break as a result of severe trauma and then heal rapidly without pins or supports. In contrast, bones of the elderly of Dallas County often break as a result of demineralization and
199
then heal only with great difficulty. It is obvious from these observations that, in cattle, migration can greatly affect bone formation and that the environmental factors that promote it also affect humans in the same manner. The key to reducing the impact of osteoporosis, therefore, lies in the soils and water supply of Deaf Smith County, Texas, a cold spot for this disease.42 Each genetic aberration linked with a chronic disease has its own specific triggering (hot spots) and protective (cold spots) environments. Two hospital-based studies43 involving brain autopsies of every patient dying with dementia in Maracaibo, Venezuela, a city with a population of 650,000, discovered only one Alzheimers case in over a decade. In contrast, in the worst affected Norwegian municipalities, during the period 19741983, the median annual age-adjusted Alzheimers disease mortality rates were between 44-55 per 100,000 for males and 87-109 per 100,000 for females.44 These figures suggest that Alzheimers disease is at least 1,000 times more common in the municipalities along the south and southeastern coasts of Norway than in Maracaibo, Venezuela. Even within Norway itself, Alzheimers mortality was higher by a factor of 15 in some municipalities than in others, during this period. The hottest spots for Alzheimers disease, therefore, appear to occur in the acidic, aluminum-enriched, calcium and magnesium deficient municipalities of southern Norway. This is exactly what the multiple antagonist hypothesis would predict. In contrast, the key to reversing the global Alzheimers disease pandemic lies in Maracaibo, Venezuela. Here even those with the genetic aberrations that are normally invariably associated with early-onset Alzheimers disease are not developing this form of dementia. Whether this is because of an aluminum antagonist in the water supply or herb, spice, or some other food or additive commonly eaten in Maracaibo is unknown.
200
We do not need a giant, time wasting population-based study of the genetic-environmental links in Alzheimers disease. What is required is a detailed examination of the drinking water and diets of this Maracaibo cold spot. Naturally, the migration of any ethnic group from a cold spot to a hot spot or from either of these to a more neutral environment is likely to change its Alzheimers disease incidence and mortality rates. This is especially true if the migration is associated with significant dietary change. This probably accounts for the rise in Alzheimers disease seen in immigrants to the USA from Japan and Nigeria.45-46
BIOCHEMICAL ABNORMALITIES
It appears as if, in individuals who have a depressed calcium and magnesium intake combined with abnormally high aluminum absorption, some enzymatic processes are inhibited. This inhibition is most likely to occur in enzymes that have aluminum antagonists, such as calcium, magnesium, and iron, as cofactors. It is not surprising, then, that since the end result of such enzyme inhibition is Alzheimers disease, patients with this form of dementia experience a wide variety of biochemical abnormalities, some of which appear in Table 2. The longstanding glucose deficiencies seen in Alzheimers patients, for example, are probably caused by aluminums inhibition of glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase47 and its interference with the hexokinase.48 Abnormalities in the cholinergic system seem to be linked to aluminums ability to inhibit choline acetyltransferase.49-50 A depression of tetrahydrobioterin, which is required for the synthesis of the neurotransmitters dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin, also occurs in Alzheimers disease patients. This deficiency and its associated repercussions appear linked to aluminums inhibition of the enzyme
201
dihydropteridine reductase,51 which is essential for the maintenance of tetrahydrobiopterine. The depressed glutamate levels seen in Alzheimers disease may reflect the inhibition of brain glutamate decarboxylase activity52 and the impairment of the glutamate-nitric oxide-cyclic GMP pathway in neurons.53 Adenylate cyclase is a catecholamine sensitive enzyme that plays a major role in parathyroid hormone secretion. Its levels are elevated in Alzheimers patients.54 This seems to be because aluminum can cause an irreversible activation of adenylate cyclase, that Ebstein and colleagues have suggested may account for some of the neurotoxicity of that metal.55 Similarly, the multiple antagonist hypothesis can account for the hyperphosphorylation of tau which appears linked to aluminums ability to inhibit the dephosphorylation of this protein.56 Indeed, in the presence of elevated aluminum, both the phosphorylation and dephosphorylation of tau are disrupted, largely by the replacement of calcium by aluminum in calmodulin57 (Table 2).
202
Consequence
Potential Treatment Calcium, magnesium, citrate, sodium fluoride, apotransferrin Calcium, magnesium Calcium, magnesium Vitamin B12m[zinc], estrogen, folic acid, calcium, magnesium phosphatidylcholine, lecithin, acetyl-Lcarnitine Magnesium Desferrioxamine, magnesium, copper, zinc, iron, calcium Calcium, magnesium Desferrioxamine, calcium, magnesium Calcium, magnesium n-3 and n-6 essential fatty acids, calcium, magnesium, phosphatidylserine, phosphatidylcholine, phosphatidyl ethanolamine, phosphatidylinoditol
Phosphofructokinase Glucose metabolism impaired Choline acetyltransferase Acetylcholine deficiency Malfunction of cholinergic neurons Formation of senile plaques Elevated parathyroid activity Depressed dopamine, norepinephrine and serotonin Reduction in glutamatergic neurotransmission Loss of calmodulin flexibility; formation of neurofibrillary tangles Abnormal brain cell membranes
No claim is made that this enzyme list is complete. It almost certainly is not.
203
There are many enzymes that have calcium, magnesium, and other minerals or cofactors. It is quite possible that under certain deficiency conditions, aluminum can inhibit almost any of these. Two things are clear. So many inhibited enzymes have been identified, aluminum must be causing a very wide range of biochemical abnormalities in Alzheimers disease patients. It is also clear that no pharmaceutical aimed at correcting the malfunction of a specific enzyme is ever going to be effective in the prevention and reversal of Alzheimers disease. The only logical approach to preventing the disease is to block aluminums access to the body, while simultaneously preventing mineral deficiencies.
(1) Hypertension
It comes as no surprise that hypertension is a risk factor for Alzheimers disease. After all, both of these disorders are promoted by calcium and magnesium deficiency.58-60 Naturally, therefore, they tend to occur together in the same people. Many individuals who have hypertension have developed it because they are deficient in calcium and magnesium, a condition that also increases their vulnerability to Alzheimers disease.
(2) Cholesterol
Cholesterol promotes the production and accumulation of beta amyloid protein.61 In animal studies, it has been shown that
204
this process occurs more slowly if rabbits are given distilled, rather than tap water.62 Such studies also have demonstrated that aluminum chloride increases plasma cholesterol levels in the rat, while simultaneously inhibiting numerous enzymes and decreasing plasma total lipids.63 Exactly how aluminum elevates plasma cholesterol is, as yet, unclear to this author, but it does.
(3) Homocysteine
High plasma homocysteine levels are also known to be a risk factor in Alzheimers disease.64 The Framingham study, for example, established that the higher the homocysteine level when first measured in elderly people, the more likely they were to become demented later in life.65 Interestingly, Gottfries and coworkers66 have shown that in the early stages of Alzheimers disease, elevated serum homocysteine is a sensitive marker for cognitive impairment. Alzheimers patients are choline deficient, probably because of aluminums ability to inhibit the activities of the enzyme acetyltransferase.67-68 Choline deficiency raises homocysteine levels by altering the metabolism of methionine69-70 and would, as a result, account for both homocysteines presence at high levels in the serum of those most at risk for Alzheimers disease and the associated cognitive impairment that follows.
(4) Diet
As previously described, a diet rich in fish, nuts, fruits, and vegetables is linked to a lower risk of developing Alzheimers disease.71 This is hardly surprising since nuts, fruits, and vegetables are usually good sources of calcium and magnesium,72 thus mitigating the enzyme-inhibiting impacts of aluminum. Furthermore, Durlach and colleagues73 have suggested that, in humans, vitamin E, selenium, magnesium, and other anti205
oxidants can protect against the deleterious metabolic consequences of apolipoprotein E4-4. In addition, cold water fish are typically elevated in the Omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acids that are involved in maintaining brain structure.74-75
(5) Smoking
Smoking is a lethal habit, but whether or not Alzheimers disease is promoted by it is uncertain. It may well be one more disease laid at tobaccos door, since smoking decreases calcium absorption in the intestines and so promotes bone loss.76 For reasons that are unclear, smokers exacerbate the problems this causes by choosing to eat diets that are lower in both calcium and vitamin D than those eaten by non-smokers.77 These deficiencies tend to be corrected in ex-smokers, 5 years or more after stopping the habit.
(6) Depression
There are many reasons depression may be an early indicator of increased risk of developing Alzheimers disease.78 It has been shown, for example, that aluminum inhibits glucose-6phosphate dehydrogenase79 and hexokinase.80 The inhibition of these two enzymes seems to be associated with a longstanding glucose deficiency in Alzheimers patients. There also appears to be a link between low glucose levels and depression. Patients with hypoglycemia, or low blood sugar, exhibit higher depression scores than those with normal blood sugar levels.81 Since potential Alzheimers patients have lower blood sugar levels, they are more likely to test positively for depression. Dialysis patients, exposed to highly elevated aluminum, suffered severe depression of their serum glutathione peroxidase levels.82 This seleno-enzyme plays a major role in protecting against oxidative stress. Changes in selenium concentration in blood and brain have also been noted in Alzheimers disease
206
patients.83 It has been established further by five studies that selenium deficiency is a significant cause of depression in many people who do not have Alzheimers disease and will probably never develop it.84 It appears, therefore, that aluminum lowers brain selenium levels in individuals who will ultimately have Alzheimers disease, and when it does they exhibit signs of depression.
(8) Stress
The Religious Orders Study that is evaluating aging in Catholic nuns, priests, and brothers has identified stress as a risk factor for Alzheimers disease.86 Although medical interest in stress can be traced back to Hippocrates,87 it was not until the 1920s that physiologist Walter Cannon88 showed that emotionarousing incidents trigger an outpouring of epinephrine (adrenaline), norepinephrine (noradrenaline), and cortisol. During stress, the sympathetic nervous system increases respiration and heart rate, diverts blood to skeletal muscles, and releases fat from storage. All such changes prepare the body for what Cannon called Fight or Flight, and are obviously part of a response system that has evolved in an effort to deal with perceived threats.
207
In the modern world, fighting or running away from problems are often impossible solutions. The result of chronic stress is overexposure to adrenaline, noradrenaline, and cortisol. These can lead to nervous disorders ranging from high blood pressure to depression,89 both of which have already been linked to Alzheimers disease. Beyond this, stress promotes free radical damage, a characteristic of Alzheimers disease.
(9) Osteoporosis
The probable link between Alzheimers disease and osteoporosis90 is hardly surprising. It is clear from the published literature that aluminum can promote extensive bone loss. In addition, both Alzheimers disease and osteoporosis are more common in individuals who are deficient in calcium and magnesium.91 Naturally, therefore, these disorders tend to occur together in the same person.92 There is no doubt that aluminum can cause bone loss. During the 1970s, a number of patients undergoing dialysis treatment, with tap water containing aluminum from treatment plants, developed osteomalacia, a disorder involving a softening of the bones resulting from impaired mineralization. This problem was recorded in Europe, North America, and Australia. In Great Britain, a survey of 1,293 patients in 18 centres established a very strong statistical relationship between bone softening and the aluminum content of the water supply used in dialysis.93 This disorder was particularly common in Glasgow, Leeds, Newcastle, Plymouth, and Oxford, where drinking water aluminum levels were elevated. To explore the relationship between bone strength and aluminum content further, Mjberg94 carried out 20 bone biopsies on patients who had not experienced dialysis, but who had suffered hip fractures. The aluminum and calcium content of
208
sampled materials established that there was no significant relationship between bone aluminum and gender or fracture type, but there was a statistically significant tendency towards higher aluminum levels in the bones of younger hip fracture patients. In general, therefore, while more research is necessary, Mjbergs95 results tend to support the hypothesis that aluminum impairs bone mineralization and increases fracture rates in the general population. The use of water containing high levels of aluminum in the treatment of renal patients was also associated with significant increases in dialysis encephalopathy, a degenerative disorder of the brain that showed some similarities with Alzheimers disease. Parkinson and colleagues96 showed in Newcastle, for example, that none of the 10 patients in their twenties on home dialysis survived for 2 years. All died of dialysis encephalopathy, osteomalacia, or unexplained cardiorespiratory failure. Beyond the dialysis evidence, aluminum has been linked to bone loss in Chinese villagers eating corn contaminated with mud and coal that were elevated in aluminum and fluoride.97 Interestingly, such villagers experienced an extremely high urinary calcium loss, associated with both osteoporosis and osteosclerosis. The literature also contains a description of an 8-month-old boy suffering metabolic bone disease due to chronic aluminum-containing antacid use.98 Both osteoporosis and Alzheimers disease occur frequently in individuals who are exposed to high levels of aluminum while simultaneously being calcium and magnesium deficient. It is not surprising, therefore, that the former is a predictor of the latter. Exactly how aluminum causes bone loss is not completely clear, but it appears to be due to its ability to cause an irreversible activation of adenylate cyclase, so stimulating the production of parathyroid hormone.99 Aluminum absorption
209
is enhanced in the presence of abnormally high circulating parathyroid hormone and is preferentially deposited in both the bone and brain.100
(10)Diabetes mellitus
Diabetes mellitus is a disorder that, like Alzheimers disease, is common in those who live in magnesium deficient environments,101-102 especially if they eat diets that are deficient in this mineral.103-104 Like Alzheimers disease, diabetes mellitus also involves glucose abnormalities. Aluminum interferes with the normal function of at least three enzymes, glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase,105 hexokinase,106 and phosphofructokinase.107 These impacts cause glucose abnormalities in future Alzheimers disease patients long before any cognitive deficits are seen. It seems likely, therefore, that similar enzyme inhibition plays a role in diabetes mellitus type II, accounting for its identification as a risk factor for Alzheimers disease. Naturally, the impact of aluminum will be worse in those who are calcium and magnesium deficient.108
(11)Restricted Education
Beyond the possibility that less educated individuals may be more likely to eat inappropriate diets, there appear to be three hypotheses that may explain why lack of education might increase the risk of developing Alzheimers disease.109 Firstly, it is possible that the APO E4 allele(s) that predisposes a significant section of the population to late-onset Alzheimers disease might somehow also adversely affect an individuals ability to cope with the demands of an education. There seems, however, to be no available evidence to support this hypothesis. Secondly, education might stimulate the brains development, and thus increase its ability to withstand more degenerative
210
damage before Alzheimers symptoms become apparent. There is certainly growing evidence that stimulation affects brain development. To illustrate, Rosenzweig110 demonstrated that the number of neurons in the brains of rats was influenced by the stimuli in their environment. Rats that grew up in an enriched milieu were found to have more neurons in the cerebral cortex than those that did not. In addition, a rat from an enriched environment had a heavier cortex with thicker cortical coverings. Brain enzymes were also elevated. Globus111 further discovered that such enriched environments increased the number of dendritic spines in the rat brain. Perhaps, as Restak112 muses, learning, memory, and other brain functions in humans may depend to a large degree on the quality of environmental stimulation. A third hypothesis that may account for the apparent link between lack of education and the risk of developing Alzheimers disease would focus on exposure to toxic metals and inadequate dietary mineral intake. If a child was exposed to elevated aluminum while their calcium, magnesium, zinc, and phosphorous intakes were depressed, they might be unable to handle the rigours of higher education. Ultimately, these imbalances might also result in the development of Alzheimers disease. There is clearly this type of negative relationship between lead exposure and depressed childhood intelligence.113 Furthermore, Varner and coworkers114 have shown recently that the chronic administration of drinking water containing aluminum-fluoride or sodium-fluoride to rats causes significant deficits in neuronal integrity that show regional brain differences. It has been established also that elevated hair aluminum levels seem to be associated with classroom withdrawal by young children.115 Much of this aluminum may come from cans, but it should be noted that aluminum concentrations in most formulas derived from cows milk are 10- to 20-fold greater than in human breast milk, and they are 100-fold greater in soy-based formulas.116
211
Deficiencies in trace and bulk elements also seem to adversely affect school performance. To illustrate, Marlowe and Palmer117 compared 26 hair trace elements in two sets of young Appalachian children: an economically disadvantaged group of 106 from Head Start programs and 56 control group children from more prosperous backgrounds. Developmental disabilities, including communication and behavioural disorders, were noted in 13 members of the Head Start group, but were absent from the control group. Hair analysis also established that the mean levels of calcium, magnesium, and zinc were significantly depressed in children from the economically disadvantaged group. Conversely, Benton118 reviewed five studies that suggested that vitamin/mineral supplements improved many childrens performances during intelligence tests. To summarize, the evidence suggests that many children are exposed to excess aluminum, while being simultaneously mineral deficient. Such individuals appear to experience schooling difficulties early in life and may possibly develop Alzheimers disease when older. This may be particularly true if they eat a high fat diet.119
Not only is the aluminum burden of the brain likely to increase with aging in this way, but its ability to protect itself also characteristically declines. Hypovitaminosis, for example, is common in the elderly,121 who are all too frequently deficient in antioxidants and are, therefore, more prone to oxidative stress. Beyond this, two hormones that decline with age, melatonin and estrogen, play roles in protecting the brain from aluminum. As their levels fall, damage from this element inevitably increases. The aluminum-estrogen association probably explains why, as Cohen122 pointed out, Alzheimers disease is more common in women than in men, a gender bias that cannot be explained entirely by the greater longevity of females.
phosphofructokinase. All the necessary references to support these claims are given in chapter eight.
oxidative stress seen in Alzheimers disease. It was argued earlier, of course, that this results, in part, from aluminums increase of lipid peroxidation.132
SUMMARY
In his book, Science is God, Horrobin135 claims that: A good hypothesis has three major characteristics. It accounts for those facts in a precise, direct way. It makes predictions which are amenable to experimental testing and which suggests the direction in which further progress may be made. I feel sure that the evidence provided to this point in What Really Causes Alzheimers disease has shown that Fosters Multiple Antagonist Hypothesis can account for the known facts about this type of dementia in a precise, direct way. Indeed, I would like to challenge those with a vested interest in promoting the use of aluminum to explain the observations in Tables 1 and 2 in a more convincing manner. What remains, then, is to demonstrate how progress in the prevention and treatment of Alzheimers disease can be made by the application of the multiple antagonist hypothesis.
216
REFERENCES
1. 2. Brown, S.R. (2003). Scurvy. Toronto: Thomas Allen. Foster, H.D. (2000). How aluminum causes Alzheimers disease: The implications for prevention and treatment of Fosters Multiple Antagonist Hypothesis. Journal of Orthomolecular Medicine, 15(1), 21-51. Sarin, S., Gupta, V., and Gill, K.D. (1997). Alteration in lipid composition and neuronal injury in primates following chronic aluminum exposure. Biological Trace Element Research, 59(1-3), 133-143. Golub, M.S., and Tarara, R.P. (1999). Morphometric studies of myelination in the spinal cord of mice exposed developmentally to aluminum. Neurotoxicology, 20(6), 953-959. Verstraeten, S.V., Keen, C.L., Golub, M.S., and Oteiza, P.I. (1998). Membrane composition can influence the rate of Al3+-mediated lipid oxidation: Effect of galactolipids. Biochemical Journal, 333(Pt 3), 833-838. Genetics and the Alzheimers diseases. Chromosome 19(AD2). http://www.macalester.edu/~psych/whathap/UBNRP/ alzheimer/chrom19.html Cherroret, G., Desor, D., Hutin, M.F., Burnel, D., Capolaghi, B., and Lehr, P.R. (1996). Effects of aluminum chloride on normal and uremic adult male rats. Tissue distribution, brain choline acetyltransferase activity, and some biological variables. Biological Trace Element Research, 54(1), 43-53. Yamamoto, H., Saitoh, Y., Yasugawa, S., and Miyamoto, E. (1990). Dephosphorylation of tau factor by protein phosphatase 2A in synaptosomal cytosol fraction, and inhibition by aluminum. Journal of Neurochemistry, 55(2), 683-690. Fujita, T. (1987). Calcium and your health. Tokyo: Japan Publications. Garruto, R.M., Fukaton, R., Hanagihara, R., Gajdusek, D.C., Hook, G., and Fiori, C. (1984). Imaging of calcium and aluminum in neurofibrillary tangle-bearing neurons in Parkinsonism Dementia of Guan. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 81: 1875-1879. Fujita, op.cit. Joshi, J.G. (1991). Neurochemical hypothesis: Participation by aluminum in approaching critical mass of colocalized errors in brain leads to neurological disease. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, 100(1-2), 103-105.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9. 10.
11. 12.
217
13.
Cho, S.W., and Joshi, J.G.(1989). Inactivation of glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase isozymes from human and pig brain by aluminum. Journal of Neurochemistry, 53(2), 616-621. Lai, J.C., and Blass, J.P. (1984). Inhibition of brain glycolysis by aluminum. Journal of Neurochemistry, 42(2), 438-446. Xu, Z.X., Fox, L., Melethil, S., Winberg, L., and Badr, M. (1990). Mechanisms of aluminum-induced inhibition of hepatic glycolysis: Inactivation of phosphofructokinase. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 254(1), 301-305.
14. 15.
16. Waldman, M., and Lamb, M. (2004). Dying for a hamburger. Toronto: McClelland and Stewart. 17. 18. Ibid. Foster, H.D. (1994). Health and the physical environment: The challenge of global change. In M.V. Hayes, L.T. Foster, and H.D. Foster (Eds.), The determinants of population health: A critical assessment. Victoria, BC: University of Victoria, Department of Geography, Western Geographical Series, 39, 73-120. Elsom, D.M. (1992). Atmospheric pollution: A global problem. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
19.
20. Marier, J.R. with Neri, L.C., and Anderson, T.W. (1979). Water hardness, human health, and the importance of magnesium. Ottawa: National Research Council of Canada. NRC Associate Committee on Scientific Criteria for Environmental Quality. NRCC No. 17581. 21. Elmstrom, G.W., and Fiskell, J.G.A. (1974). Watermelon yield response to supplemental magnesium. Soil Crop Science Society Florida Proceedings, 33, 222-223 cited in Ibid. Marier et al., op.cit.
22.
23. Peek Performance. Magnesium: Why magnesium matters to athletes. http://www.pponline.co.uk/encyc/magnesium.html 24. Marier et al., op.cit.
25. Vogt, T. (1986). Water quality and health. Study of a possible relationship between aluminum in drinking water and dementia (in Norwegian, English Summary). Sosiale Og Okonomiske Studier, 61, Statistisk Oslo-Kongsvinger, 60-63. 26. Flaten, T.P. (1990). Geographical association between aluminum in drinking water and death rates with dementia (including Alzheimers disease), Parkinsons disease and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, in Norway. Environmental Geochemistry and Health, 12 (1 and 2), 152-167.
218
27.
Kawahara, M., Kato, M., and Kuroda, Y. (2001). Effects of aluminum on the neurotoxicity of primary cultured neurons and on the aggregation of beta-amyloid protein. Brain Research Bulletin, 55(2), 211-217. Cherroret, op.cit. Inestrosa, N.C., and Alarcon, R. (1998). Molecular interactions of acetylcholinesterase with senile plaques. Journal of Physiology, Paris, 92(5-6), 341-344. Sarin, S., Gupta, V., and Gill, K.D. (1997). Alteration in lipid composition and neuronal injury in primates following chronic aluminum exposure. Biological Trace Element Research, 59(1-3), 133-143. Genetics and the Alzheimers diseases Chromosome, 19(AD2). http://www.macalester.ed~psych/whathap/UBNRP/alzheimer/ chrom19.html Velez-Pardo, C., Jimenez Del Rio, M., and Lopera, E. (1998). Familial Alzheimers disease: Oxidative stress, beta-amyloid, presenilins, and cell death. General Pharmacology, 31(5), 675-681. Khachaturian, Z.S. (1997). Plundered memories. The Sciences, 37(4), 20-25. Guo, Q., Sebastian, L., Sopher, B.L., Miller, M.W., Ware, C.B., Martin, G.M., and Mattson, M.P. (1999). Increased vulnerability of hippocampal neurons from presenilin-1 mutant knock-in mice to amyloid beta-peptide toxicity: Central roles of superoxide production and caspase activation. Journal of Neurochemistry, 72(3), 10191029. Haass, C., and Baumeister, R. (1998). What do we learn from a few familial Alzheimers disease cases? Journal of Neural Transmission. Supplementum, 54, 137-145. Anderson, M.W. (2004). A 500,000 person study? Gene-environment interactions would be focus of NIH-led effort. The Scientist. http://www.biomedcentral.com/news/20040526/04 Ibid. Ibid. Foster, H.D. (1987). Diabetes mellitus and low environmental magnesium levels. Lancet, 8559, II, 633. Foster, H.D. (2004). Capes, bays and the double helix: Why geography has more to offer in the prevention of chronic degenerative diseases than genetics. Journal of Orthomolecular Medicine, 19(1), 39-47. Myers, J.A. (1979). The role of some nutritional elements in the health of the teeth and their supporting structures. In Schutte, K.H. and
28. 29.
30.
31.
32.
33. 34.
35.
36.
41.
219
Myers, J.A. (eds). Metabolic aspects of health: Nutritional elements in health and disease. Kenfield, CA: Discovery Press, pp. 229-243. 42. 43. Ibid. Molina, O., Cardozo, D., and Cardozo, J. (2000). Causes of dementia in Maracaibo, Venezuela: A re-evaluation. Revista de Neurologia, 30 (2), 115-117. Flaten, T.P .(1990). Geographical associations between aluminum in drinking water and death rates with dementia (including Alzheimers disease), Parkinsons disease and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis in Norway. Environmental Geochemistry and Health, 12(1/2), 152-167 .
44.
45. Graves, A.B., Larson, E.B., Edland, S.D., Bowen, J.D., and McCormick, W.C. (1996). Prevalence of dementia and its subtypes in the Japanese American population of King County, Washington State. The Kame Project. American Journal of Epidemiology, 144(8), 760-771. 46. Hendrie, H.C., Ogunniyi, A., Hall, K.S., Baiyeuw, O., Unverzagt, F.W., and Gyreje, O. (2001). Incidence of dementia and Alzheimers disease in two communities: Yoruba residing in Ibadan, Nigeria and African Americans residing in Indianapolis, Indiana. Journal of the American Medical Association, 285, 739-747.
47. Cho, S.W., and Joshi, J.G. (1989). Inactivation of glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase isozymes from human and pig brain by aluminum. Journal of Neurochemistry, 53(2), 616-621. 48. Lai, J.C., and Blass, J.P. (1984). Inhibition of brain glycolysis by aluminum. Journal of Neurochemistry, 42(2), 438-446. 49. Cherroret et al., op.cit. 50. Inestrosa et al., op cit. 51. Altman, P., Al-Salihi, F., Butter, K., Cutler, P., Blair, J., Leeming, R., Cunningham, J., and March, F. (1987). Serum aluminum levels and erythrocyte dihydropteridine reductase activity in patients on hemodialysis. New England Journal of Medicine, 317(2), 80-84. Hofstetter, J.R., Vincent, I., Bugiani, O., Ghetti, B., and Richter, J.A. (1987). Aluminum-induced decrease in choline acetyltransferase, tyrosine hydroxylase, and glutamate decarb-xylase in selected regions of the rabbit brain. Neurochemical Pathology, 6(3), 177-193.
52.
53. Cucarella, C., Montoliu, C., Hermenegildo, C., Sez, R., Manzo, L., Miana, M.D., and Felipo, V. (1998). Chronic exposure to aluminum impairs neuronal glutamate-citric oxide-cyclic GMP pathway. Journal of Neurochemistry, 70(4), 1609-1614. 54. Danielsson, E., Eckerns, S.A., Westlind-Danielsson, A., Nordstrm, O., Bartfai, T., Gottfries, C.G., and Wallin, A. (1988). VIP-sensitive
220
adenylate cyclase, guanylate cyclase, muscarinic receptors, choline acetyltransferase and acetylcholinesterase in brain tissue affected by Alzheimers disease/senile dementia of the Alzheimers type. Neurobiology of Aging, 9(2), 153-162. 55. Ebstein, R.P., Oppenheim, G., Ebstein, B.S., Amiri, Z., and Stessman, J. (1986). The cyclic AMP second messenger system in man: The effects of heredity, hormones, drugs, aluminum, age and disease on signal amplification. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 10(3-5), 323-353. Yamamoto et al., op.cit. Levi, R., Wolf, T., Fleminger, G., and Solomon, B. (1998). Immunodetection of aluminum and aluminum induced conformational changes in calmodulin-implications in Alzheimers disease. Molecular and Cellular Biochemistry, 189(1-2), 41-46. Whitaker, J. (2000). Reversing hypertension. New York: Warner Books. Lancaster, K.J., Smiciklas-Wright, H., Weitzel, L.B., Mitchell, D.C., Friedmann, J.M., and Jensen, G.L. (2004). Hypertension related dietary patterns of rural older adults. Preventive Medicine, 38(6), 812-818. Touyz, R.M., Campbell, N., Logan, A., Gledhill, N., Petrella, R., Padwal, R., and the Canadian Hypertension Education Program (2004). The 2004 Canadian recommendations for the management of hypertension: Part III - Lifestyle modifications to prevent and control hypertension. Canadian Journal of Cardiology, 20(1), 55-59. Wolozin, B., Brown, J. 3rd, Theisler, C. 3rd, and Silberman, S. 3rd (2004). The cellular biochemistry of cholesterol and statins: Insights into the pathophysiology and therapy of Alzheimers disease. CNS Drug Reviews, 10(2), 127-146. Sparks, D.L., Lochhead, J., Horstman, D., Wagoner, T., and Martin, T. (2002). Water quality has a pronounced effect on cholesterolinduced accumulation of Alzheimers amyloid beta (Abeta) in rabbit brain. Journal of Alzheimers disease, 4(6), 523-529. Sarin et al., op.cit. Seshadr, S., Beiser, A., Selhub, J., Jacques, P.F., Rosenberg, I.H., DAgostino, R.B., Wilson, P.W., and Wolf, P.A. (2002). Plasma homocysteine as a risk factor for dementia and Alzheimers disease. New England Journal of Medicine, 346(7), 476-483. Ibid. Gottfries, C.G., Lehmann, W., and Regland, B. (1998). Early diagnosis of cognitive impairment in the elderly with a focus on Alzheimers disease. Journal of Neural Transmission, 105(8-9), 773-786.
56. 57.
58. 59.
60.
61.
62.
63. 64.
65. 66.
221
Cherroret et al., op.cit. Inestrosa et al., op.cit. Riggs, K.M., Spiro, A. 3rd, Tucker, K., and Rush, D. (1996). Relations of vitamin B-12, vitamin B-6, folate, and homocysteine to cognitive performance in the Normative Aging Study. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 63(3): 306-314. Challen, J., and Dolby, V. (1996). Homocysteine: The new cholesterol. New Caanan: Keats Publishing. Correa, M.L., Nicolosi, A., Cristina, S., Hauser, W.A., and Nappi, G. (2001). Nutrition and cognitive deficit in the elderly: A population study. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 55(12), 1053-1058. Whitaker, op. cit. Durlach, J., Durlach, A., and Durlach, V. (1996). Antioxidant dietary status and genetic cardiovascular risk, or how an adequate intake of a-topopherol, selenium, taurine, magnesium, and various other natural antioxidants may overcome the deleterious metabolic consequences related to the E4-4 type of apolipoprotein E (editorial). Magnesium Research, 9(2), 139-141. Mercola, J. (2001). Omega-3 is essential to the human body. http:// www.mercola.com/2001/sep/8/omega_3.htm
70. 71.
72. 73.
74.
75. Salem, N. Jr., Moriguchi, T., Greiner, R.S., McBride, K., Ahmad, A., Catalan, J.N., and Slotnick, B. (2001). Alteration in brain function after loss of docosahexaenoate due to dietary restrictions of n-3 fatty acids. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience, 16(2-3), 299- 307. 76. Krall, E.A., and Dawson-Hughes, B. (1999). Smoking increases bone loss and decreases intestinal calcium absorption. Journal of Bone and Mineral Research, 14(2), 215-220. Morabia, A., Bernstein, M.S., and Antonini, S. (2000). Smoking, dietary calcium and vitamin D deficiency in women: A populationbased study. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 54(9), 684-689.
77.
78. Green, R.C., Cupples, L.A., Kurz, A., Auerbach, S., Go, R., Sadovnick, D., Duara, R., Kukull, W.A., Chui, H., Edeki, T., Griffith, P.A., Friedland, R.P., Bachman, D., and Farrer, L. (2003). depression is a risk factor for Alzheimers disease: The MIRAGE Study. Archives of Neurology, 60(5), 753-759. 79. 80. Cho et al., op. cit. Lai et al., op. cit.
81. Berlin, I., Grimaldi, A., Landault, C., Cesselin, F., and Puech, A.J. (1994). Suspected postprandial hypoglycemia is associated with beta-
222
adrenergic hypersensitivity and emotional distress. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 79(5), 1428-1433. 82. Turan, B., Delilba, Si E., Dalay, N., Sert, S., Afrasyap, L., and Sayal, A. (1992). Serum selenium and glutathione-peroxidase activities and their interaction with toxic metals in dialysis and renal transplantation patients. Biological Trace Element Research, 33, 95-102. Chen, J., and Berry, M.J. (2003). Selenium and selenoproteins in the brain and brain diseases. Journal of Neurochemistry, 86(1), 1-22. Benton, D. (2002). Selenium intake, mood and other aspects of psychological functioning. Nutritional Neuroscience, 5(6), 363-374. Nicoll, J.A., Roberts, G.W., and Graham, D.L. (1996). Amyloid betaprotein, APO E4 genotype and head injury. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 777, 271-275. Earthlink. Stress can lead to Alzheimers disease. http://www/health scout.com/news/43/8007548/main.htm Myers, D.G. (1992). Psychology. New York: Worth Publishers. Ibid. Mind/Body Education Center. The Fight or Flight Response. http:// www.mindbodymed.com/EducationCenter/fight.html Dr. Merolas Newsletter 5/31/03. Osteoporosis linked to development of Alzheimers disease. http://www.defeatdiabetes.org/Articles/ alzheimers030416.htm Prentice, A. (2004). Diet, nutrition and the prevention of osteoporosis. Public Health Nutrition, 7(1A), 227-243. Dr. Mercolas Newsletter, op.cit. Parkinson, I.S., Ward, M.K., Feest, T.G., Fawcett, R.W.P., and Kerr, D.N.S. (1979). Fracturing dialysis osteodystrophy and dialysis encephalopathy: Epidemiological survey. Lancet, 1, 406-409. Mjberg, B. (1988). Aluminum kam ge benskrhet. [Aluminum can cause bone fragility]. Lkartid-ningen, 85(51), 4511. Ibid. Parkinson, op.cit. Chen, X.G., Zhou, S., Jiao, J., Ding, Y.H., and Zhoa, Z.Q. (1997). Skeletal changes with toxicity from fluoride and aluminum. Fluoride, 30(2), 85-88. http://www.fluoride-journal.com/97-30-2/30285.htm
83.
84. 85.
223
98.
Robinson, R.F. Casavant, M.J., Nahata, M.C., and Mahan, J.D. (2004). Metabolic bone disease after chronic antacid administration in an infant. Annals of Pharmacotherapy, 38(2), 265-268. Ebstein et al., op. cit.
99.
100. Burnatowska-Hledin, M.A., Kaiser, L., and Mayor, G.H. (1983). Aluminum, parathyroid hormone, and osteomalacia. Special Topics in Endocrinology and Metabolism, 5, 201-226. 101. Foster (1987), op.cit. 102. Foster, H.D. (1992). Health, Disease and the Environment. London: Belhaven. 103. Anon (2004). Food choices may effect diabetes risk. Coffee and magnesium-rich foods may deflect diabetes, while red meat may promote it. Health News, 10(3), 3. 104. Lopez-Ridaura, R., Willett, W.C., Rimm, E.B., Liu, S., Stampfer, M.J., Manson, J.E., and Hu, F.B. (2004). Magnesium input and risk of type 2 diabetes in men and women. Diabetes Care, 27(1), 134-140. 105. Cho et al., op. cit. 106. Lai et al., op. cit. 107. Xu et al., op. cit. 108. Foster (1987), op. cit. 109. De Ronchi, D., Fratiglioni, L., Rucci, P., Paternico, A., Graziani, S., and Dalmonte, E. (1998). The effect of education on dementia occurrence in an Italian population with middle to high socioeconomic status. Neurology, 50(5), 1231-1238. 110. Rosenzweig, M.R., Bennet, E.L., and Diamond, M.C. (1972). Brain changes in response to experience. Scientific American, 226(2), 22-29. 111. Globus cited by Restak, R.M. (1979). The brain: The last frontier. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. 112. Restak, R.M. (1979). The brain: The last frontier. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. 113. Tuthill, R.W. (1996). Hair lead levels related to childrens classroom attention-deficit behaviour. Archives of Environmental Health, 51 (3), 214-220. 114. Varner, J.A., Jenseen, K.F., Horvath, W., and Isaacson, R.L. (1998). Chronic administration of aluminum-fluoride or sodium-fluoride to rats in drinking water: Alterations in neuronal and cerebrovascular integrity. Brain Research, 784, 284-289.
224
115. Marlowe, M., and Bliss, L.B. (1993). Hair element concentrations and young childrens classroom and home behaviour. Journal of Orthomolecular Medicine, 8(2), 79-88. 116. Bishop, N., McGraw, M., and Ward, N. (1989). Aluminum in infant formulas. Lancet, 8636, 490. 117. Marlowe, M., and Palmer, L. (1996). Hair trace element status of Appalachian Head Start children. Journal of Orthomolecular Medicine, 11(1), 15-22. 118. Benton, D. (1992). Vitamin/mineral supplementation and the intelligence of children - A review. Journal of Orthomolecular Medicine, 7(1), 31-38. 119. Grant, W. (1997). Dietary links to Alzheimers disease. Alzheimers disease Review, 2, 42-55. 120. Fujita, op. cit. 121. Baker, H. (1983). Hypo-vitaminosis in the elderly. Geriatric Medicine Today, 2(10), 61-66. 122. Cohen, G.D. (1987). Alzheimers disease. In Maddox, G.L. (ed), The encyclopaedia of aging (pp. 27-30). New York: Springer Verlag. 123. Vogt, op. cit. 124. Flaten, op. cit. 125. Genetics and the Alzheimers diseases, op. cit. 126. Bobkova, N.V., Nesterova, I.V., Dana, E., Nesterov, V.I., Aleksandrova, Iiu, Medvinskaia, N.I., and Samokhin, A.N. (2003). Morpho-functional changes of neurons in temporal cortex with spatial memory in bulbectomized mice after treatment with minerals and ascorbates. Morfologiia, 123(3), 27-31 [in Russian]. 127. Iraizoz, I., Guijarro, J.L., Gonzalo, L.M., and de Lacalle, S. (1999). Neuropathological changes in the nucleus basalis correlate with clinical measures of dementia. Acta Neuropathologica (Berl), 98(2), 186-196. 128. Hofstetter et al., op. cit. 129. Cucarella et al., op. cit. 130. Alenog, R., Aumont, N., Dea, D., and Poirier, J. (2003). Non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs mediate increases in vitro glial expression of apolipoprotein E protein. European Journal of Neuroscience, 18(6), 1428-1438. 131. Kawahara et al., op. cit.
225
132. Van Rensburg, S.J., Daniels, W.M., Potocnik, F.C., Van Zyl, J.M., Taljaard, J.J., and Emsley, R.A. (1997). A new model for the pathophysiology of Alzheimers disease. Aluminum toxicity is exacerbated by hydrogen peroxide and attenuated by an amyloid protein fragment and melatonin. South African Medical Journal, 87(9), 1111-1115. 133. Blank, L. (1995). Alzheimers challenged and conquered? London: Foulsham. 134. Warren, T. (1991). Beating Alzheimers: A step towards unlocking the mysteries of brain disease. Garden City Park, NY: Avery. 135. Horrobin, D.F. (1969). Science is God. Aylesbury: Medical and Technical Publishing Company.
226
16
Before I draw nearer to that stone to which you point, said Scrooge, answer me one question. Are these the shadows of the things that Will be, or are they the shadows of the things that May be only? Still the Ghost pointed downward to the grave by which it stood. Mens courses will foreshadow certain ends, to which, if perservered in, they must lead, said Scrooge. But if the courses be departed from, the ends will change. Say it is thus with what you show me! Charles Dickens, A Christmas Carol
While I was writing this, the final chapter of What Really Causes Alzheimers disease, the news media reported the death of former US President Ronald Reagan. This event took place on June 5th, 2004, when Americas only movie star president died of pneumonia complicated by Alzheimers disease.1 Ronald Reagan had suffered from dementia for 10 years and had been the worlds most famous Alzheimers disease patient. Aluminum inhibits a wide variety of enzymes. Those people who carry the APO E4 allele(s) have the greatest susceptibility to these disruptions and so develop Alzheimers disease more easily. Aluminum, especially in its monomeric form, is highly toxic in acidic environments that are deficient in calcium, magnesium, and silicic acid. From a scientific point of view, all of these risk factors, with the exception of genetic inheritance, are relatively simple to mitigate. Therefore, Alzheimers disease, in theory, is easily avoidable. There is no need for a
227
pandemic, or the $100 billion annual loss that it degenerates in the USA alone. Unfortunately, several major lobbies are likely to consider that a global Alzheimers disease pandemic is an acceptable cost of doing business. If you doubt this, I suggest that you read When Smoke Ran Like Water by Devra David2 and The Fluoride Deception by Christopher Bryson.3 Remember them when you next read a statement that claims the idea that aluminum has anything to do with Alzheimers disease is an old, disproven theory. I am dedicating this chapter on strategies for reducing Alzheimers disease incidence to former US president Ronald Reagan in the hope that those who honoured him may have the political power and drive to ensure that he did not suffer in vain. I am, however, a realist. That is why June 5th, 2004 was also the day on which I bet on Birdstone (34/1) to beat Smarty Jones (1/5) in the Belmont Stakes. Therfore, I am including in this chapter a series of steps that any individual can take to help protect themselves against Alzheimers disease. These can be used in the likely event that pressure groups continue to place profits ahead of health.
It would seem to be in the best interest of every government to save the billions of dollars spent in caring for Alzheimers disease victims. Unfortunately, little in politics is so logical. In August 1997, Paul W. Mason,4 owner, operator, and proprietor of Adobe Springs, a mineral spring (which contains 110 mg/litre of magnesium), near San Jose, California filed suit against Donna Shalala, US Secretary of Health and Human Services, and Dr. Michael Friedman, Acting Commissioner of the US Food and Drug Administration. This case, No. C-9720686 was heard in the United States District Court, Northern District of California. The plaintiff, Mason,5 charged that for 77 years the US Food and Drug Administration had forbidden any health claims to be made for mineral water and that this, and other activities, had deliberately driven thousands of mineral water companies out of business. As a result of US Food and Drug Administration policies, therefore, the magnesium content of the average bottle of American water was, at the time of the case, 2.7 mg per litre. That is, bottled water produced in the USA contained very little magnesium. In contrast, similar products bottled outside the country carried, on average, 28 mg per litre of magnesium. That is, they contained over 10 times as much . Mason6 provided numerous scientific citations demonstrating that, for a wide variety of illnesses, incidence and mortality decrease as magnesium intake rises. From such evidence, he estimated that the US Food and Drug Administrations actions in promoting the use of magnesium-deficient water had resulted in the deaths of about 215,000 Americans each year, roughly one every 2.5 minutes. In total, this is more than the combined death toll of Americans killed in every war the country has fought, including the Civil War. Mason hoped that by filing such a suit he had provided the opportunity to argue the merits of increasing the magnesium content of the drinking
229
water of the USA. The case was dismissed quickly by Federal Judge Jeremy Fogel.7 He essentially ruled that, although the magnesium-deficiency deaths caused by Food and Drug Administration policies may have exceeded those of the Holocaust, the matter was political rather than judicial. Indeed, Mason8 was chastised for comparing the number of magnesium-deficiency caused deaths with those of the Holocaust, even though the totals appear similar. As far as I am aware, there have been no improvements in US Food and Drug Administration regulations. If this is so, Americans are still dying at the rate of roughly one every 2.5 minutes from government-promoted drinking water magnesium deficiency.9 Unfortunately, former president Ronald Reagan may have been one of these victims. Mason and his wife have not given up. They continue their battle for better quality drinking water through locations that include The Magnesium Web Site10 and that of the Arab Healthy Water Association.11 Not only do governments show little interest in increasing the magnesium content of drinking water, they routinely allow the use of aluminum sulfate as a flocculant by water treatment plants. This reduces the amount of sediment in the water supply, but greatly increases levels of dissolved aluminum, especially if the water is acidic.12 Clearly, aluminum sulfate must be replaced by alternatives. Its use probably explains why Alzheimers disease is so common in western Wales, amongst the elderly who were exposed for many years to this practice.13-14
Foods
The adoption of the western diet has been associated with major changes in the incidences of many illnesses.15 Mortality from infection tends to decline significantly, only to be replaced by chronic, degenerative diseases, including heart disease,
230
cancer, and diabetes mellitus.16 Such western illnesses appear to have as their root cause the consumption of highly refined foods that are low in minerals and fibre but contain elevated sugar, salt, animal protein, and saturated fats.17-18 Support for this idea can be drawn from the global geographical distribution of such chronic degenerative diseases, their emergence in native populations that migrate to industrialized societies, and the lower mortalities for such diseases experienced by vegetarians, including the Seventh-Day Adventists, even though they live in the Developed World.19 The western diet promotes Alzheimers disease in three distinct ways. Firstly, it tends to be deficient in calcium and magnesium,20 making those who eat it very susceptible to aluminum toxicity. Secondly, many foods are canned, wrapped, and/or cooked in aluminum. The more acid the food, the more easily it appears to dissolve this metal. Thirdly, maltol is added to many processed foods in an attempt to improve flavour.21 There can be little doubt that the typical western diet is too low in many minerals.22 Consider, for example, magnesium. This occurs at relatively high levels in unrefined whole grain cereals and in green leafy vegetables, nuts, seeds, lentils, beans, and peas.23 However, farmers do not routinely add magnesium to soils, so its levels are often relatively depleted in their crops. Since it is fairly soluble, processing and cooking also often can greatly reduce magnesium levels in foods. To illustrate, the milling of whole grain lowers the magnesium content to only 20 percent of that initially present. Processing further reduces it, so that while one slice of whole wheat bread provides 24 mg of magnesium, a slice of white bread contains only 6 mg.24 For these reasons, dietary intake of magnesium has been declining for at least 100 years in the USA, falling from about 500 mg to 175-225 mg per day. The average daily intake is now about 228 mg in both American and British women.25 These
231
levels are far too low only two-thirds of the US Recommended Daily Amount, which is, in itself, not adequate.26 Simply put, the western diet is deficient in magnesium, increasing susceptibility to a wide range of illnesses, including Alzheimers disease. If the government is truly interested in promoting better health, it must pass legislation that encourages the addition of both calcium and magnesium to widely consumed foods and to drinking water in regions where this is acidic. Silicic acid might also be a very beneficial additive to drinking water. It would be valuable to increase the use of calcium and magnesium-enriched fertilizers in regions where soil pH is low. Wrapping, canning, and cooking food in aluminum also needs to be addressed. At the very least, standards could be set to minimize the leaching of aluminum into the beverages that are drunk on a massive scale by children. Beyond this, it is imperative that the use of maltol to enhance food flavour be prohibited. It may be remembered that this additive greatly increases the ability of aluminum to cross the blood-brain barrier.27 Indeed, when researchers want to study a rabbit whose brain has been badly damaged by Alzheimers-like plaques and tangles they feed maltol to it.28 There is no logical reason why maltol should be allowed to be routinely added to hot chocolate, beer, and some commercially baked goods and many other products.
in the Developed World, by government responsibility for safety and public health. Much of the power for the detection of threats and the maintenance of public health now rests with the operators of sophisticated technology, such as satellites, radar, medical equipment, and computer systems, which are beyond the control and often the understanding of the general public. Power now rests with experts whose prestige and wealth largely depends on paradigms that support the Establishment. In short, the scientific elite, to which we have given the responsibility for maintaining health and avoiding disasters, is not going to bite the hand that feeds it. There is much more to be gained from the search than from the discovery, and out of treatment than cure. This is especially true when prevention threatens the interests of powerful industrial lobbies. For these reasons, I do not expect to see government restrictions on monomeric aluminum levels in drinking water. Nor is it likely that elevated pH levels, or the widespread addition of calcium, magnesium, or silicic acid to potable water will be encouraged. Despite the obvious threat it poses to the human brain, the food industry lobby will probably prove capable of preventing a maltol ban. Products enriched with this flavour-enhancer are likely to continue to fill supermarket shelves. Nevertheless, there are numerous steps that individuals can take to reduce their own chances of developing Alzheimers disease, if they choose to take the Paul Revere approach to this illness.
Avoidance of Aluminum
For most of those reading this book, the average day will begin with a shower. If the water used is acidic and deficient in calcium and magnesium, it is possible that it will be a source of aluminum that enters the body through the pores and nose. This exposure to aluminum is most likely if the water supplier uses aluminum sulfate as a flocculant to remove sediment.
233
Once dried off, most readers will smear their bodies with a layer of aluminum provided by antiperspirants and deodorants.30 How much of this aluminum passes through the skin into the body is unclear, but McGrath31 has argued that underarm shaving and frequent use of antiperspirants and deodorants seem linked to an early age of breast cancer diagnosis. British researchers32-33 provided evidence to support the feasibility of McGraths hypothesis, reporting traces of parabens in every tissue sample taken from 20 different breast tumors. Parabens are chemicals used in deodorants and other cosmetics that can mimic estrogen. The hormone estrogen is known to encourage breast tumor growth. Clearly, parabens can enter the body from deodorants and it is possible that aluminum can do the same. Deodorants with a herbal base do not usually contain these toxins. Then comes breakfast. Tea, coffee, hot chocolate are usually made with water from the tap. It is important not to use soft, acidic water which is likely to contain monomeric aluminum. Most water supply companies will provide chemical analyses, allowing the assessment of the aluminum, calcium and magnesium content of their product. If not, private companies can conduct such analyses relatively cheaply. If colas or fruit juices are drunk, they are likely to have come from cans. These are typically made of aluminum. The longer the drink has been in the can, the higher the aluminum levels in it are likely to be.34 In addition to any aluminum it contains, hot chocolate is likely to be enhanced with maltol, so increasing the likelihood that this metal will reach the brain. Similarly, tea brewed in acidic water or flavoured with lemon juice contains significantly higher levels of bioavailable aluminum than normal.35
poor source of minerals, including calcium and magnesium. As previously pointed out, the average British and North American diet contains less than half the calcium and magnesium required to avoid the associated deficiency illnesses, including Alzheimers disease. The best way to address this problem is to eat many of the mineral enriched foods listed in Table 3. These include salmon, sardines, broccoli, spinach, and bok choy, for example, which are all high in calcium.36-37 Pumpkin seeds, almonds, Brazil nuts, and whole grain brown rice are good sources of magnesium.38 Certain supplements, especially mineral ascorbates, provide high levels of both calcium and magnesium. Alacer Corporation, Foothill Ranch, California, a company with which I have no financial associations, provides excellent mineral ascorbate products. One tablet of Super-Gram II, for example, contains 4 percent of calcium and 8 percent of magnesium recommended daily allowance. Emergen-C is a fizzing drink mix that is pleasant to take when added to water. It provides 1,000 mg of vitamin C and 32 mineral complexes, including calcium and magnesium. Alacers products were used in the joint RussianCommittee on World Health research projects that produced a marked reversal of memory loss in the elderly.39-41
235
Calcium
Almonds Brazil Nuts Hazelnuts Sunflower Seeds Black beans Bok Choy Broccoli Collards Kale Pinto beans Spinach Turnip Greens Flanksteak (Beef) Rabbit Chili con carne (with beans)
VEGETABLES
MEATS
SEAFOOD
Agar-agar Carp Crab Hijiki Kombu Oysters Salmon Sardines Shrimp Wakame Bran muffin Oat flakes (fortified) Peanut butter cookies Custard (baked) Molasses (blackstrap) Rice Pudding Cheddar cheese Edam Gruyre Parmesan Ricotta Sour Cream Whole milk (dry) Yogurt Fig Pear (dry) Papaya
OTHER FOODSTUFFS
236
SUMMARY
There is currently a global Alzheimers pandemic involving tens of millions of victims. In the USA alone the number of sufferers is expected to reach 14 million by 2050.42 As the evidence presented in this volume illustrates, this suffering and the associated financial costs are totally unnecessary. Alzheimers disease is caused by aluminum and is particularly common in those carrying the APO E4 allele(s), who are, in consequence, the most susceptible to this toxic metal. In my book Disaster Planning: The Preservation of Life and Property,43 published in 1980, I wrote Communities, like individuals, may often work towards their own destruction through neglect, ignorance, or a deliberate emphasis on fulfilling superficially advantageous short-term goals. Incrementally, in doing so, they magnify risk and eventually suffer the disasters they deserve. It has been known for over a century that aluminum is a neurotoxin. The unfortunate truth that its widespread use, by a calcium and magnesium deficient population, is the major cause of Alzheimers disease is now unavoidable. Time will tell whether we are intelligent enough to avoid the coming Alzheimers catastrophe that is now routinely predicted.
REFERENCES
1. Clayton, I. (2004). Indepth: Ronald Reagan. Americas movie star president. CBC News. http:www.cbc.ca/news/background/reagan_ ronald/index/html Davis, D. (2002). When smoke ran like water: Tales of environmental deception and the battle against pollution. New York: Basic Books. Bryson, C. (2004). The fluoride deception. New York: Seven Stories Press. United States District Court Northern District of California. Case No. C97-20686. The Magnesium Web Site. http://www.mgwater.com/ fdamotn1.shtml
2. 3. 4.
237
5. 6. 7.
Ibid. Ibid. United States District Court Northern District of California Case No. C97-20686. The Magnesium Web Site. http://www.mgwater.com/ judgment.shtml Paul W. Mason personal communication. The Magnesium Web Site, op.cit.
8. 9.
10. Ibid. 11. Arab Healthy Water Association. http://www.mgwater.com/joinahwa. shtml 12. Barnett, P.R., Skougstad, M.W., and Miller, K.J. (1969). Chemical characteristics of a public water supply. Journal, American Water Works Association, 61, 61-67. 13. Funge, R., and Perkins, W. (1991). Aluminum and heavy metals in the potable water of the north Ceredigion area, mid-Wales. Environmental Geochemistry and Health, 13(2), 56-65. 14. Foster, H.D. (1992). Health, Disease and the Environment. London: Belhaven Press. 15. Trowell, H. (1981). Hypertension, obesity, diabetes mellitus and coronary heart disease. In H. Trowell and D.P. Burkitt (Eds). Western diseases: Their emergence and prevention (pp. 3-32). Cambridge, MS: Harvard University Press. 16. Pyle, G. (1979). Applied medical geography. New York: Wiley. 17. Trowell, op.cit. 18. Kellock, B. (1985). The fibre man: The life story of Dr. Denis Burkitt. Belleville, MI: Lion Publishing. 19. Dwyer, J.J. (1988). Health aspects of vegetarian diets. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 48, 712-738. 20. Garland, C., and Garland, G., with Thro, E. (1989). The calcium connection. New York: Simon and Schuster Inc. 21. Yiming Fine Chemicals Co. Ltd. http://www.asian-tg.com/en/maltol.html 22. Editors of Prevention Magazine (1988). The complete book of vitamins and minerals for health. Emmaus, PA: Rodale Press. 23. Peak Performance. Magnesium: Why magnesium matters to athletes. http://www.pponline.co.uk/encyc/magnesium.html 24. Health and Nutrition Letter. For warding off diabetes mellitus. http:// healthletter.tufts.edu/issues/2004-06/magnesium.html
238
25. Peak Performance, op.cit. 26. Littlefield, N.A., and Hass, B.S. (1996). Is the RDA for magnesium too low? 1996 FDA Science Forum Abstract cited by The Magnesium Web Site. http://www.mgwater.com/mgrda.shtml 27. McLachlan and Kruck cited in Ross, A. (1991). The silent scourge. Equinox, 60, 86-100. 28. Rao, J.K., Katseto, C.D., Herman, M.M., and Savory, J. (1998). Experimental aluminum encephalomyelopathy. Relationship to human neurodegenerative disease. Clinics in Laboratory Medicine, 18(4), 687-698. 29. McLuckie, B.F. (1970). The warning system in disaster situations: A selective analysis. Ohio State University, Disaster Research Center Report Series No. 9, prepared for the Office of Civil Defense, Office of Secretary of the Army, Washington, DC. 30. Mercola, J. (2003). Five common hygiene mistakes and how to avoid them. http:/ /www.mercola.com/2003/sep27/hygiene_mistakes. htm 31. McGrath, K.G. (2003). An earlier age of breast cancer diagnosis related to more frequent use of antiperspirants/deodorants and underarm shaving. European Journal of Cancer Prevention, 12(6), 479-485. 32. Darbre, P.D. (2003). Underarm cosmetics and breast cancer. Journal of Applied Toxicology, 23(2), 89-95. 33. Darbre, P.D., Aljarrah, A., Miller, W.R., Coldham, N.G., Sauer, M.J., and Pope, G.S. (2004). Concentrations of parabens in human breast tumours. Journal of Applied Toxicology, 24(1), 5-13. 34. Abercrombie, D.E., and Fowler, R.C. (1977). Possible aluminum content of canned drinks. Toxicology and Industrial Health, 13(5), 649-654. 35. Flaten, T.P., and Odegard, M. (1988). Tea, aluminum and Alzheimers disease. Chemical Toxicology, 26, 959-960. 36. In-Depth Food - Calcium Facts. http://www.vegsource.com/nutrition/ explainers/calcium_facts.html 37. Garland et al., op.cit. 38. Peak Performance. Magnesium op.cit. 39. Bobkova, N.V. (2001). The impact of mineral ascorbates on memory loss. Paper presented at the III World Congress on Vitamin C, Committee for World Health, Victoria, BC, Canada. 40. Galeev, A., Kazakova, A., Zherebker, E., Dana, E., and Dana, R. (nd). Mineral ascorbates improve memory and cognitive functions in older individuals with pre-Alzheimers symptoms. Copy of paper given to this author by R. Dana and E. Dana, Committee for World Health,
239
20331 Lake Forest Drive, Suite C-15, Lake Forest, California 92630, USA. 41. Bobkova, N.V., Nesterova, I.V., Dana, E., Nesterov, V.I., Aleksandrova, IIu, Medvinskaia, N.I., and Samokhia, A.N. (2003). Morpho-functional changes of neurons in temporal cortex in comparison with spatial memory in bulbectomized mice after treatment with minerals and ascorbates. Morfologiia, 123(3), 27-31. [In Russian] 42. Neuroscience for kids - Alzheimers disease. http://faculty.washington. edu/chudler/alz.html 43. Foster, H.D. (1980). Disaster planning: The preservation of life and property. New York: Springer-Verlag.
240
Index
A
A1-calmodulin complex 159 acetyl-l-carnitine 119-121, 125, 191 acetylcholine vii, 27-28, 31, 49-51, 57-58, 69, 114, 116, 121, 128-129, 158-165, 169, 176, 178, 182-183, 189, 192, 197, 203, 215, 219, 221 acid rain acidic food ix, 94-95, 195 156 71, 84
adenylate cyclase 51, 52, 60, 190, 202-203, 210, 221 age 1, 6, 9, 14-16, 18-24, 29-30, 34-43, 54-61, 64, 67, 70-73, 76-80, 82, 87, 89, 94, 96, 98-101, 107-108, 122, 125, 143-144, 152-153, 164, 189, 194, 200, 213, 221, 234, 239 age-standardized prevalence Alacer Corporation alcohol 35 alkaline phosphatase Alois Alzheimer alpha-amyloid alum 92 10, 98, 100, 109 aluminosilicates 25-26 xiii, 236 vii, 160-162, 188, 192, 203 18
aluminum vii-xiii, 8, 11, 44, 52-54, 57, 60-61, 92-110, 119, 139-140, 145-150, 155-221, 224-225, 227-235, 237-240 aluminum additives aluminum bomb aluminum cans aluminum citrate 139 94 98, 191 97
aluminum cooking utensils 139, 191 aluminum L-glutamate 177, 183 aluminum lactate aluminum silicates aluminum sulfate aluminum tracer 177 94 xii, 230-231, 234 156 1, 36, 93, 155, 157, 201, 228 98, 109 241
14, 21, 25-26, 56, 65, 180, 189, 198 25-26, 55, 124, 130, 135, 158, 189, 197-198 26, 54, 56, 68, 97-98, 158, 167, 181, 185, 189, 205, 7, 11, 43-44, 91, 103, 106-107, 110, 150,
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis 189, 193, 219-220 antagonism anti-cholinesterase inhibitors anti-inflammatory drugs anticholinergic drugs antiperspirants APO E APO E2 APO E3 30 8, 143 30, 143, 154 8, 30, 143, 154 APO E protein 49
102-103, 134, 167, 171-172, 176 159 26, 132-133, 135, 191, 216, 226
APO E4 vii, 8, 30, 35, 48, 64, 73, 77, 96, 130, 143-148, 151-162, 175-176, 187, 189-190, 192, 197, 207, 211, 213-214, 223, 228, 237 APO E4 allele vii-viii, 96, 130, 143-147, 152-157, 161, 175-176, 187, 189-190, 192, 197, 207, 211, 213-214, 228, 237 apolipoprotein E genes apoliprotein apovincamine Arizona aspartate Auguste D. Australia axon 55 115 viii, 197
19, 23-24 48-49 13-14, 22, 194 21, 24, 72, 148, 150, 208 8, 143, 152
B
bacopa beans Beijing 114, 121-122, 191, 214 65, 131 xi, 231, 236 16, 37, 110 Baltimore longitudinal study of aging
beta-amyloid vii-viii, 11, 25, 26, 54-57, 65, 75-76, 97, 124, 131, 146-147, 153-154, 158-159, 161-162, 167, 176, 181-182, 186-189, 192, 197-198, 207, 216, 219 Bible 15 242
blood pressure blueberries bone char bone mass brain autopsy breast cancer 68 102
blood-brain barrier
C
calcium vii-xiii, 8, 10-11, 51-52, 55, 57, 59-60, 90-91, 102-105, 110, 117-118, 139, 148, 150, 155-162, 165, 170-175, 181, 186, 188, 190-193, 195-196, 199, 201-206, 208-214, 218, 223, 228-240 calcium homeostasis calcium/calmodulin California Californians Canada 1 48, 51 vii, 159, 161-162, 165, 188, 192, 203
55, 97, 130, 229, 236 6-7, 21, 39, 66, 104, 116-117, 124, 146, 189, 196, 199, 218, 240
cancer 16, 29, 34, 43, 88-91, 106, 127, 132, 137, 140, 171, 174, 231, 234, 239 canning cans x-xi, 55, 196-198, 232 6 98, 139, 191, 212, 235 180, 183, 185-186 50-51, 60, 181, 186, 202 6 20, 24 7
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention cerebral arteriosclerosis cerebrospinal fluid Chaucer Chekhov chelators China Chinchorro Chinese moss Chiribaya 15 15 56, 193 49-51, 54-55, 58-61
ix, 16-17, 23, 37, 39, 43, 102, 110, 115-116, 195 17 115 17, 23
cholesterol 8, 28, 30, 32, 55, 61, 63, 65-66, 70, 80-82, 143, 153, 165, 176, 184, 189-190, 205, 222 cholinergic deficiencies 49 243
chromium ascorbates
cognitive function testing 29, 58, 63, 77-78, 80, 86, 90, 190, 199, 210, 220, 224, 231, 238-239 cognitive impairment 163, 205, 222 cold spots 199-200 195-196 94 commercial fertilizers cooking 20, 65, 70, 93, 99, 104-105, 109, 111, 120, 122,
copper 56, 62, 147, 170, 182, 186, 190, 203 cost vii, 1-2, 6-8, 10, 71, 133, 195, 199, 228, 237 Creutzfeldt-Jacob disease Cu/Zn superoxide dismutase curcumin curry spice 68, 83, 190 68, 83 7 180, 186
D
Darwin 15, 183 200 Deaf Smith County
dementia 1-11, 14-24, 31-32, 35-45, 49-51, 57-60, 64-86, 89, 92, 94-95, 97, 99-110, 117, 122-125, 129-150, 153, 155, 159, 162, 164-165, 168, 176, 183, 186-187, 191-196, 202-204, 215, 217-223, 227, 229-230 demyelinization viii, 3-4, 7, 28, 176, 178, 188-189, 192, 204 deodorants xii, 98, 234, 239 dephosphorylation 54, 159, 160, 165, 203, 218 depression 5-6, 63, 74, 76, 80, 82, 84-85, 121-122, 127, 179, 183, 190, 202, 206-208, 223 developmental disabilities diabetes diabetic donepexil dopamine drugs 212 29, 58, 63, 77-80, 86, 90, 190, 199, 210, 220, 224, 231, 238-239 48, 199 202, 221 128, 215 50, 57, 59, 129, 181, 190, 202-203 50
dihydropteridine reductase
dopamine receptors
26, 29, 47, 49, 65, 89, 98, 113, 128-135, 191, 215-216, 221, 226 244
E
Early Onset Alzheimers disease Earths crust 92, 102 East Boston, Massachusetts East Boston Studies Edison 113 xiii, 118, 236 Emergen-C 6 79 viii, 99, 189
encephalopathy 93, 97-98, 108, 173, 209, 224 endoplasmic-reticulum associated binding protein 56 England 10-11, 21, 39, 82, 99, 109, 146, 163, 189, 221-222 environment vii-x, 8, 18, 31, 33-35, 42-44, 57, 78-82, 90-92, 98-99, 106-110, 146, 151-164, 171, 174, 188, 192, 195-196, 198-201, 210-211, 213, 218-220, 224-225, 228, 238 enzyme vii, x, 50-51, 55, 58, 69, 114, 121, 128, 158-162, 169-179, 182-183, 185, 188, 192-193, 196-197, 202-207, 210-211, 213-215, 228 epidemiological surveys epidemiologists epinephrine estrogen receptors 181, 208 130 139 19, 36 35, 127, 155
Europe ix, 2, 9, 16, 21, 39, 41, 45, 64, 72, 75, 81, 83, 85, 106, 116, 123, 135, 185, 195, 208, 222-223, 226, 239 excitotoxins exercise 48, 60 34-35, 47, 89, 113
F
failure of success fatigue fibre Finland 6, 114 70, 231, 239 37, 44, 78 2
fish 69-70, 83, 100, 103, 109, 190, 206, 236 fluoraluminum complex 157 fluorapatite 102 fluoride 96-109, 145, 155--163, 191, 193, 209, 212, 224-225, 228, 238 fluorite 102 folate 67, 70, 82, 84, 147, 222 food 34-35, 47, 69, 83, 89, 98, 102, 114, 147, 156, 163, 171, 191-192, 196, 201, 224, 229-233, 235-237, 240 245
forgetfulness 6 Fosters Multiple Antagonist Hypothesis vii, 188, 217 free radicals ix, 26, 55, 61, 115, 122, 135, 180, 182-183, 185 French men and women 104 frontal cortex 4, 92 frontal lobes 26-27 fruits 70, 190, 206
G
galantamine 116-117, 123, 129, 191, 215 gamma-amyloid 25-26 gamma-secretase enzymes 55 gender 20, 34, 38, 43, 77-78, 96, 101, 209, 213 genetic testing 143 genetic underpinnings 29 genetics 25-31, 34, 40, 82, 134, 143, 162, 167, 188-189, 197, 217-220, 225 geographers 34-35, 155 ginkgo biloba 114-115, 121-122, 191, 214 globally ix, 1, 36, 144, 195 glucose 3, 47-49, 57-58, 121, 184, 189-190, 193, 202-203, 206, 210, 215, 218, 220 glucose metabolism 3, 48, 57, 121, 189, 193, 203 glucose-6-phosphate 193, 202-203, 206, 210, 215, 218, 220 glutamate 48, 50-51, 57, 59, 106, 129, 169, 173-174, 177, 183, 190, 202-203, 216, 221 glutamatergic systems 129 glutamic acid 52 glycogen 47 glycoproteins 160 green leafy vegetables xi, 231 Groote Eylandt 147-150 Gruenberg 2, 9 Guam 11, 103, 110, 193 Guamanian amyotrophic lateral sclerosis 7, 189, 193
H
hair trace elements hamburger 212 89, 145, 149, 194, 218 246
hardening of the arteries 7 harmful environments 155, 162, 171, 192, 195-196, 199 HDL (high density lipoproteins) 65, 190 head trauma 63, 75-76, 80, 85, 146-147, 190, 207 Health, Disease and the Environment 43, 90, 106, 224, 238 herbs 47, 113-114, 121, 191, 214-215 hexokinase 193, 202-203, 206, 210, 215 hippocampal neurons 65, 81, 181, 219 hippocampus 4, 26-27, 49, 52, 92, 108, 135, 168, 177-178, 183, 190 homocysteic acid 67 homocysteine 63, 67-68, 80, 82, 190, 205, 222 Honolulu Heart Program/Honolulu-Asia Aging Study 64 hot spots 199-200 human biology 34-35 Huntingtons chorea 142, 148 Huperzine A 115-116, 121, 123, 191, 214-215 hygiene 35, 239 hyperphosphorylated tau 52, 57, 60, 160
I
Ibadan 41, 45, 220 III World Congress on Vitamin C 118-119, 124, 240 Indianapolis 41, 45, 78, 220 Indianapolis-Ibadan Dementia Project 41-42 inflammatory response 26 insulin 47-48, 58 intelligence tests 212 iron 52-62, 96, 106, 147, 160, 166, 171, 174, 181, 186, 190, 202-203 Island of Lundby 19
J
Japan 10, 16, 36-37, 40-42, 44, 64, 72, 103, 110, 115, 117, 150, 190, 193, 201, 218, 220 Juvenal 15
K
kidney dialysis 93 Kii Peninsula 103, 193 kinase II 159, 161-162, 165, 188, 192, 203 247
King County 40-42, 44, 220 Korea 115 Kuru 7, 145, 148
L
Latinos 68 LDL (low density lipoproteins) 65, 152-153, 190 lentils xi, 231 Lesser Periwinkle 115, 191 lifestyle 34-35, 42, 63, 137, 140, 199, 221 Lions Mane mushroom 117, 124, 191, 215 lipid peroxide 53 lithium 119, 121, 124, 191, 214 lock and key model 47 longitudinal study 63-65, 69-70, 73, 77, 86, 96, 101-104, 108, 131, 163 Loretto Geriatric Center of Syracuse 98 Losing My Mind 5, 10 low hydrochloric stomach acid 139
M
magnesium vii-xiii, 51-52, 57, 60, 102-105, 117-118, 139-140, 148, 155-162, 170-175, 188-224, 228-240 magnesium deficiency 60, 196, 204, 230 magnesium-deficient water 230 malaria 16-17 maltol xi-xii, 231-233, 235, 239 manganese 60, 118, 147-148, 170-171, 174, 186 manganese ore 148 manganic madness 148 Maracaibo 37-40, 44, 144, 146, 149-150, 190, 200-201, 220 medical care 2, 34, 35 Memorabiblia 15 mercury 91, 139-140, 145, 150, 171, 191, 216 mercury amalgam fragments 139 metalloprotein 56 methionine 67, 119, 205 metrifonate 129, 191, 215 mineral ascorbates xiii, 118-119, 124, 236, 240 mineral spring 229 248
miners 97, 108 MIRAGE 74, 85, 223 Missouri 19, 24 monomeric (single molecule) aluminum vii, xii, 97 MTHRF 67 multi-infarct dementia 7, 16, 36-37, 51 multiple sclerosis 43, 90-91, 106 mummies 16-17, 21, 23 myelin vii, 3-5, 7, 27-32, 176-178, 182-184, 188-189, 192, 197, 204, 217 myelin-specific enzyme 23-cyclic nucleotide phosphodydrolase vii, 178, 192
N
Na+K+ ATPase vii, 178, 182, 192, 197, 204 Nerve Growth Factor 117 neurites 28 neurofibrillary tangles vii, ix, 14, 21, 25, 53-56, 60, 64, 81, 92, 103, 119-121, 131, 153-154, 158-162, 166, 172, 181-182, 186, 189-192, 197-198, 203 neurons 4, 25-28, 31, 49-54, 60, 65, 81, 102, 110, 117-119, 124, 128-129, 131, 154, 160-164, 167-169, 173, 176, 180-183, 189-219, 226, 240 New York 1, 3, 98, 102 Newcastle 97, 165, 209 Nigeria 41-42, 45, 201, 220 nitric acid ix, 195 non-steroidal 132-133, 135, 226 norepinephrine 50, 57, 129, 181, 190, 202-203, 208 Norway x, 21, 24, 38-40, 44, 94-95, 107, 144, 146, 189-190, 196, 201, 213, 219-220 Norwegian Institute of Gerontology 94 Nucleus Basalis of Meynert 27, 49, 129, 175-176, 182, 197, 215 nuts xi, 70, 190, 206, 231, 235-236
O
Occams razor 175 Ohio 1 omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acids 69, 206 orange juice 156 organelles 160 osteoporosis 63, 77, 85, 190, 200, 208-210, 223-224 oxides 94 249
P
packaging x, 196 parabens xii, 234, 240 parathyroid hormone 51, 60, 202, 210, 224 Parkinsonism with dementia 8, 11, 103, 189, 193 Parkinsons disease 43-44, 59, 90-91, 106-107, 173, 219, 220 peas xi, 232, 236 pellagra 145, 147 Pennsylvania 1 peptide viii, 32, 54-55, 57, 115, 123-124, 181, 186, 197, 219 pH 96, 99, 104-105, 108, 155-157, 159, 163, 191, 232-233 pH-Alzheimers disease link 104 phosphatidylserine 120-121, 125, 159, 179, 184, 191, 203, 215 phosphofructokinase 193, 203, 210, 215, 218 phospholipase vii, 179, 183, 185, 188, 203 phospholipids 159, 165, 179-180, 185 phosphorylation 53-54, 61, 124, 131, 159-160, 165, 169, 203, 218 plaques vii-ix, 14-16, 21, 25-31, 37, 49, 54-56, 62-65, 76, 81, 92, 98, 121, 130-131, 145, 147, 153-154, 158-164, 172, 175-176, 180, 182-183, 187-219, 232 pneumonia 2, 142, 228 poor childhood education 78 positron emission tomography (PET) 121 potassium ix, 52, 93, 118, 170, 190, 195 presenilin-1 viii, 197, 219 presenilin-2 viii, 197 prion 7, 145 processing x-xi, 145, 149, 196, 232 propyl gallate 181 proteins vii, 14, 28, 32, 47, 54, 56-57, 60, 65, 117, 145, 153-154, 158-161, 170, 176, 178, 187, 190, 192, 223 Prussian army amputees 92
Q
Quebec 96
R
rats 53, 65, 82, 114, 122-123, 157, 164, 167, 169, 173-174, 177, 180, 183, 211-212, 217, 225 Religious Orders Study 74, 76, 207 250
retired 1 retrogenesis vii-viii, 2-3, 7-9, 28, 176, 178, 183, 189, 192, 204 rivastigmine 129, 191, 215 road salt 90-91 Ronald Reagan 227-228, 230, 238 rural residence 78, 86 Russia 37, 92, 118, 121, 124, 226, 236, 240 Russian-Committee on World Health xiii, 236
S
Sacramento 68, 82 sage 68-69, 83, 114, 116, 121-122, 190-191, 214-215 Saguenay-Lac-Saint-Jean 96 San Marino 79, 87 schistosomiasis 17 schizophrenia 82, 90, 138, 140, 181, 186-187 Seattle 78 Selegiline (deptenyl) 133, 216 selenium 43, 90-91, 171, 185, 206-207, 222-223 serotonin 50, 57, 129, 190, 202-203 Shakespeare 1, 15, 25 Shanghai 16, 37, 43 SIDS 90-91 silicic acid vii, x, 96, 100, 105, 145, 155, 157, 159, 161-162, 188, 191-192, 195, 213, 228-229, 232-233 silicon 8, 11, 99, 109, 156, 163, 172, 182 silicotic lung disease 97 Sir Walter Scott 15 slow viruses 7 Smokers Rights Action Group 71 smoking 35, 63, 69, 71-74, 80, 84, 190, 206, 223 socioeconomic 78, 86, 199, 225 Spanish Sage 68, 83 spinach 68, 82, 235-236 spontaneous regression of cancer 137 statins 66, 82, 222 stress 9, 43, 47, 63, 76, 80, 85, 114-115, 121, 123, 133, 179-181, 186, 190, 192, 198, 207-208, 213-214, 216, 219, 223 stroke 2, 7, 16, 66-67, 77, 86, 132, 147 251
sulphuric acid
ix, 195
Super-Gram II xiii, 236 supplements 47, 118, 132, 135, 137, 199, 212, 236 syphilis 16, 144, 149 Systolic Hypertension in Europe 64, 81
T
tacrine 128-129, 133, 191, 215 tangles vii, 14, 16, 21, 25, 27-29, 31, 37, 49, 52-54, 56, 60, 64, 81, 92, 103, 119, 121, 131, 153-154, 158-162, 166, 172, 175-176, 181-182, 186-187, 189-190, 192, 197-198, 203, 232 tau vii-viii, 26-27, 52-55, 57, 60-61, 119, 124, 131, 153-154, 158-162, 165-166, 169, 182, 187-189, 192, 197, 203, 218 tea xii, 156, 163, 234-235, 240 temporal change 19 testosterone 131-132, 135, 191 Texas 1, 200 The Forgetting 3, 9, 22, 32, 61, 134, 183 The Hemlock Society 79, 86 tomatoes 156 toxins xii, 47-49, 140, 145, 168, 216, 234 trichinosis 17 Trollope 15 tuberculosis 16-17, 23 turbidity 96 tyrosine 50, 221
U
United States National Cancer Institute 89 USA 1-2, 6-7, 11, 20-21, 32, 36, 39, 66, 68, 70-72, 79, 86, 90, 110, 124, 127, 143, 147, 152, 162, 183, 189, 196, 198-199, 201, 218, 228-230, 232, 237, 240
V
variant Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease (mad cow disease) 145 vegetables 70, 190, 206, 231, 236 Venezuela 37-39, 44, 144, 146, 149-150, 190, 200-201, 220 Veterans hospitals 71 vinpocetine 115, 122-123, 191, 214 vitamin B 67, 70, 84, 147, 187, 203, 222 252
67, 70, 147, 203 67 xiii, 69, 118-119, 124, 236, 240 51, 59, 206, 213, 223 69, 133, 180-181, 186, 190-191, 206, 216 28
W
Washington State 40-41, 44, 220 94 Welsh county districts West Africans 41 western diet Western New Guinea whole wheat bread
X
Xenophon 15
Z
zinc 56, 62, 118, 147, 170, 172-173, 182, 186-187, 190, 203, 211-212
253
The Author The author lives with his wife Sarah and cat McNuff in Victoria, British Columbia. A Canadian by choice, he was born in Tunstall, Yorkshire, England where he was educated at the Hull Grammar School and University College London. While at university, he specialized in geology and geography, earning a B.Sc. in 1964 and Ph.D. in 1968 from London University. He has been a faculty member in the Department of Geography, University of Victoria since 1967. A tenured professor, he has authored or edited some 230 publications, the majority of which focus on reducing disaster losses or identifying the causes of chronic disease or longevity. He has published hypotheses on the origins of numerous diseases including myocardial infarction, SIDS, cancer, diabetes, schizophrenia, multiple sclerosis, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, Alzheimers and Parkinsons diseases, stroke, and AIDS. In May 2004, he was presented with the Orthomolecular Doctor of the Year Award by the International Society for Orthomolecular Medicine. His many books include Disaster Planning: The Preservation of Life and Property, Springer Verlag, New York; Reducing Cancer Mortality: A Geographical Perspective, Western Geographical Press, Victoria; and The Ozymandias Principles, Southdowne Press, Victoria. Further books by the author include Health, Disease, and the Environment, Bellhaven Press (now John Wiley), London, and What Really Causes AIDS, Trafford Publishing, Victoria. He is a member of the Explorers Club and several academic organizations including The New York Academy of Sciences, The Royal Geographical Society, and The Royal Society of Literature. In addition, he is the editor of both the International and Canadian Western Geographical Series and is a member
254
of the boards of the Journal of Orthomolecular Medicine and the International Schizophrenia Foundation. He has been a consultant to numerous organizations, including the United Nations, NATO, and the governments of Canada, Ontario, and British Columbia. Every day he takes at least the recommended daily allowance of the known essential nutrients, in the belief that this will slow the aging process. As a consequence, most of his salary is spent in health food stores. His other bad habits include providing treats to all the neighbourhood dogs; losing at chess to his computer; being regularly beaten by his stepson Dan at video games; and, with the assistance of @Derby and various computer models, failing to correctly predict the outcomes of horse races. For a more complete curriculum vitae visit http:/ /www.hdfoster.com. A free copy of this book and What Really Causes AIDS and What Really Causes Schizophrenia can be downloaded at this website.
255
The man who discovers a new scientific truth has previously had to smash to atoms almost everything he had learned, and arrives at the new truth with hands blood-stained from the slaughter of a thousand platitudes.
Jos Ortega y Gasset, The Revolt of the Masses, 1930
256