When death thoughts lead to death fears: Mortality
salience increases death anxiety for individuals who lack meaning in life Clay Routledge and Jacob Juhl North Dakota State University, Fargo, North Dakota, USA Research derived from terror management theory demonstrates that subtle reminders of mortality increase strivings for meaning. It is argued that such strivings reflect efforts to prevent the anxiety that death reminders may otherwise cause. However, no research has directly tested the assertions that subtle mortality primes increase death anxiety and perceptions of meaning in life moderate this effect. The current study examined these predictions. Meaning in life was measured, death cognition primed, and death anxiety assessed. A mortality prime increased death anxiety, but only for individuals who lack perceptions of meaning in life. Theoretical and applied implications are discussed. Keywords: Mortality salience; Death anxiety; Meaning; Well-being. Humans are self-aware, abstract thinking, and temporally conscious organisms and thus possess the requisite cognitive capacities to fully contem- plate their mortality. Since humans, like other animals, strive for self-preservation, the realisation that death is certain has the potential to cause great anxiety (Becker, 1973). Further, this aware- ness of eventual demise cannot simply be avoided because life is full of situations that render salient human fragility and transience (e.g., doctor ap- pointments, terrorist threat warnings, news broadcasts). This poses the question: Do situa- tional cues that heighten death awareness increase death anxiety? Further, if so, why do people often seem relatively unaffected by reminders of mor- tality? Though life is full of experiences that make death salient, many people navigate these situa- tions relatively free of distress. Therefore, if reminders of mortality can increase death anxiety, who is most susceptible to this effect, and who appears to be insulated from it? According to terror management theory (TMT; Solomon, Greenberg, & Pyszczynski, 1991) con- ditions that remind people of mortality have the potential to increase death anxiety. However, the theory asserts that people can circumvent this Correspondence should be addressed to Clay Routledge, Minard Hall, North Dakota State University, Fargo ND, 58108, USA. E-mail: Clay.Routledge@ndsu.edu COGNITION AND EMOTION 2010, 24 (5), 848854 848 # 2009 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business http://www.psypress.com/cogemotion DOI:10.1080/02699930902847144 anxiety by feeling like their lives are imbued with meaning and purpose. In other words, the inevit- ability of death is not as threatening if people believe that their existence, though transient, is meaningful and serves a greater purpose. In support of this assertion, a large body of experi- mental research indicates that when the aware- ness of death is heightened (mortality salience), people respond with efforts to reinforce a sense that human life is meaningful (see Pyszczynski, Solomon, & Greenberg, 2003). For example, different mortality-salience inductions (e.g., death writing task, presence of funeral home in visual field, subliminal death primes) have been shown to increase prejudice against those who subscribe to different meaning-providing cultural belief systems (Greenberg et al., 1990), belief in afterlife and divine intervention (Norenzayan & Hansen, 2006), denial of similarity to other animals (Goldenberg et al., 2001), and dislike of modern art that undermines perceptions of meaning (Landau, Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski, & Martens, 2006). Though studies such as these indicate that mortality salience promotes efforts to perceive human life as meaningful, they do not provide evidence that these meaning-related de- fences serve to mitigate mortality salience induced death anxiety, or that reminders of mortality even increase death anxiety. Research by Greenberg and colleagues (2003) provided clearer evidence for these assertions by demonstrating that giving participants a placebo purported to prevent the experience of anxiety eliminates the typical effect of mortality salience leading to strivings for meaning. That is, when participants believed that they were inoculated from future anxiety they did not feel compelled to defend the cultural beliefs that provide a broader sense of meaning after death thoughts were primed. However, to date, no research has directly assessed whether or not mortality salience actually increases death anxiety or whether or not feelings of meaning mitigate this potential effect. The current study explored these questions. Specifically, the current research tested the hypothesis that priming thoughts of death would heighten death anxiety for people who lacked feelings of meaning in life but not for people who possessed such feelings. To test this hypothesis, in the current study, meaning in life was measured and mortality salience manipulated. Subsequently, death anxiety was assessed. METHOD Participants and procedure Sixty introductory psychology students (33 fe- males, 24 males, 3 unidentified) participated in a study called Personality and Attitudes. They completed all materials in partitioned cubicles in the order described below. Upon completion of the study materials participants were fully debriefed. Materials Meaning in Life (MIL). The MIL measure (Ryff, 1989) consisted of the four items: My personal existence is purposeful and meaningful; In life, I have clear goals and aims; I have clear goals and a satisfying purpose in life; and I regard my ability to find a meaning, purpose, and mission in life as being great rated on a 5-point scale (1disagree strongly; 5agree strongly; a.70). These items were taken from the purpose in life subscale of a larger measure of different dimensions of psychological well-being. Ryff (1989) described the purpose in life subscale as reflecting the extent to which one sees ones life, past and present, as meaningful. Experimental condition. Participants received either the mortality salience (MS) or control manipulation (Rosenblatt, Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski, & Lyon, 1989). Specifically, they responded to two open-ended statements: Briefly describe the emotions that the thought of your own death arouses in you and Jot down, as specifically as you can, what you think will happen to you physically as you die and once you are physically dead. The control condition consisted of parallel statements regarding the experience of failing an important exam, so as to control for general elicitation of self-threatening negative MORTALITY SALIENCE INCREASES DEATH ANXIETY COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2010, 24 (5) 849 thoughts. As in previous research, the experi- mental induction was followed with a puzzle distraction task (Greenberg, Arndt, Simon, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 2000). Affect. The 20-item Positive and Negative Af- fect Scheme (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Telle- gen, 1988) consisted of 10 positive affect items (PA; a.85) and 10 negative affect items (NA; a.88). Death Anxiety (DA). The DA measure (Lester, 1990) consisted of the 8-item death of self subscale from the Revised CollettLester Fear of Death Scale in which participants indicated how anxious they were about different aspects of death (e.g., the shortness of life, the total isolation of death) on a 5-point scale (1not anxious; 5very anxious; a.87). The full scale contains subscales relating to the process of dying and the death and process of others dying. In the current study, only the fear of death of self subscale was administered so as to measure specifically anxiety about personal mortality. The scale has been shown to be highly correlated with other measures of death anxiety and sensitive to state changes in death anxiety (Mooney & OGorman, 2001). RESULTS There were no significant effects involving gender (ps.25). To test the predictions that mortality salience would increase death anxiety but only for those lacking meaning in life, regression equations were computed in which MS (dummy coded) and MIL (centred) were entered in the first step and the interaction in the second step. There were no significant main effects for MS (B0.15, SE0.28, t1.07, p.30) or MIL (B0.28, SE0.29, t1.53, p.13) in the first step. There was, however, a signifi- cant interaction in the second step, B0.48, SE0.57, t1.99, p.05 (see Figure 1). Predicted means tests were conducted at low and high levels of MIL (91 SD) to further probe the nature of this interaction. At low levels of MIL, MS, relative to the control condition, was associated with increased DA, B0.41, SE0.39, t2.18, p.03. There was no Figure 1. The effects of meaning in life and mortality salience on death anxiety. Note: Higher scores reect higher levels of death anxiety and meaning in life. ROUTLEDGE AND JUHL 850 COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2010, 24 (5) significant effect of MS at high levels of MIL, B0.13, SE0.40, t0.67, p.51. Furthermore, simple slope tests revealed that within the MS condition, higher MIL scores were associated with decreased DA, B0.48, SE0.41, t2.53, p.02. No such relation- ship was found within the control condition, B 0.04, SE0.39, t0.24, p.81. Potential effects of MS and MIL on positive and negative affect were also considered. As in previous research, MS had no significant effect on PA or NA (ps.80). There was a main effect of MIL on PA such that higher MIL was associated with increased PA, B0.27, SE0.20, t2.09, p.04. However, there was no significant inter- action between MS and MIL (p.85) on PA. There was also no significant main or interaction effects on NA (ps.22) and controlling for NA and PA did not impact the previously reported significant interaction between MS and MIL, B0.46, SE0.58, t2.04, p.05. DISCUSSION The current study provides the first direct evi- dence that subtle mortality primes lead to death anxiety for individuals not inoculated by existen- tial meaning. People who indicated that their lives were full of meaning did not respond to mortality salience with increased death anxiety. Individuals lacking this existential resource did evidence increased death anxiety after thoughts of death were primed, and this effect was specific to death anxiety as generalised negative affect could not account for it. In addition, when death was salient, meaning was inversely related to death anxiety. These findings have important theoretical and applied implications. The current study contributes critical empirical evidence to underlying assumptions of TMT. A number of hypotheses derived from TMT have received a great amount of empirical support. For example, research has shown that when meaning- providing structures (e.g., cultural worldviews) are undermined, the accessibility of death thoughts increases (e.g., Schimel, Hayes, Williams, & Jahrig, 2007). When death thoughts are made accessible, people cling to and defend these meaning-providing structures (e.g., Rosenblatt et al., 1989). When feelings of broader purpose and meaning are bolstered prior to a mortality- salience induction, people do not respond to thoughts of death with further psychological defences (e.g., Dechesne et al., 2003). When mortality is salient, people dislike stimuli found to be meaningless (Landau et al., 2006) and will not use cultural icons to complete a problem- solving task if doing so denigrates the meaning of that icon (e.g., using a crucifix as a hammer; Greenberg, Simon, Porteus, Pyszczynski, & So- lomon, 1995). All of these findings are consistent with the assertions that certain psychological and social structures imbue life with existential mean- ing and this sense of meaning shields people from the potential psychological consequences of mor- tality awareness. However, prior to the current research, a direct test of the effect of mortality salience on death anxiety had not been conducted. That is, though research has demonstrated that highly arousing conditions with death imagery lead to generalised anxiety (Greenberg et al., 1992), no studies had tested the assertion that subtle reminders of death, the types of primes that lead to increased meaning seeking, can lead to increased death anxiety, and particularly among those lacking the psychological protec- tion afforded by perceptions of broader meaning. As previously noted, Greenberg and colleagues (2003) demonstrated that people will not engage the psychological defences that affirm a sense of meaning in response to mortality salience primes if they are led to believe that they are immune from the experience of anxiety. This finding strongly suggests that reminders of mortality can lead to death anxiety but does not directly test this assertion. The current research built upon Green- berg and colleagues finding in two distinct ways in order to directly test basic tenets of TMT. First, the current study directly examined whether or not reminders of mortality increase death anxiety. Second, the current study directly as- sessed perceptions of meaning in life. Whereas previous research has relied heavily on the MORTALITY SALIENCE INCREASES DEATH ANXIETY COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2010, 24 (5) 851 examination of peoples efforts to bolster and defend the psychological and social structures (e.g., cultural worldviews) that ultimately imbue life with meaning, the current research directly assessed to what extent individuals perceive life as meaningful. Thus, the findings of the current study complement the findings of Greenberg et al. (2003) and advance the broader research on terror management processes to indicate that percep- tions of meaning in life protect people from the anxiety that contemplations of mortality may otherwise foster. The current study also contributes to a wide range of research that indicates that meaning in life plays an important role in adaptive human func- tioning. Perceiving life as meaningful is a critical component of psychological health and well-being (King & Napa, 1998; Zika & Chamberlain, 1992). For example, a lack of meaning in life is a predictor of depression (Wong, 1998) and maladaptive health behaviour (Harlowe, Newcomb, & Bentler, 1986). Furthermore, feelings of purpose and mean- ing in life predict psychological well-being at almost every developmental stage fromadolescence to late adulthood (Reker, Peacock, & Wong, 1987). Just as a sense of meaning in life has been shown to aid in coping with life stress (e.g., Mascaro & Rosen, 2006), the current research suggests that perceptions of meaning assist indivi- duals in coping with the realisation that life is finite. Meaning in life helps ensure that death thoughts do not turn into death fears because to have a meaningful life is to be more than the sumof ones biological parts (Becker, 1973). That is, as many existential thinkers have noted (see Solomon et al., 1991), being part of something larger, more enduring, and of greater significance than ones physical existence provides feelings of death transcendence. The current research has a number of limita- tions. For example, for the sake of brevity, the measure of meaning consisted of only four items. Though this measure has been demonstrated to be a reliable and valid assessment of meaning in life (see Ryff, 1989), future research should utilise additional measures of meaning. Also, in the current study, no baseline measure of death anxiety was administered so as not to interfere with the experimental manipulation. Future re- search could, however, assess pre-existing levels of death anxiety some time prior to the experiment. However, since random assignment was used in the current research we are somewhat confident that baseline levels of death anxiety do not account for the observed significant differences between conditions. The current findings also suggest a number of exciting future research directions. In the current study, meaning in life was measured, not manipu- lated. Just as life is full of situations that render death salient, life is full of experiences that both challenge and reinforce perceptions of meaning. Future studies should investigate the extent to which undermining and bolstering perceptions of meaning impact death anxiety when death is salient. In addition, if death primes increase death anxiety for those who lack sufficient meaning in life, situational cues that prime death may com- promise psychological and physical health for those who do not perceive life as meaningful. Future research should consider the extent to which meaning in life also contributes to positive physical and psychological health outcomes when people are in situations in which they must confront their mortality (e.g., cancer screenings). For example, recently Goldenberg, Routledge, and Arndt (in press) found that conditions that reminded people of their similarity to other animals led women to perceive a cancer screening (mammogram) as more physically and psycholo- gically uncomfortable. Did this threat to the belief that humans are more significant than other animals undermine perceptions of meaning, thus making the cancer-screening experience more threatening? Would bolstering perceptions of meaning increase the likelihood that individuals would adhere to recommended health screenings, screenings that are potentially anxiety-provoking because they remind people of their mortality? Future research should explore these questions as there could be many applied implications based on the basic assertion supported in the current research that when facing death-priming life ROUTLEDGE AND JUHL 852 COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2010, 24 (5) events meaning in life buffers individuals from death anxiety. Manuscript received 13 November 2008 Revised manuscript received 27 January 2009 Manuscript accepted 20 February 2009 First published online 18 May 2009 REFERENCES Becker, E. (1973). The denial of death. New York: Free Press. Dechesne, M., Pyszczynski, T., Arndt, J., Ransom, S., Sheldon, K., van Knippenberg, A., et al. (2003). Literal and symbolic immortality: The effect of evidence of literal immortality on self-esteem striv- ing in response to mortality salience. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 84, 722737. Goldenberg, J. L., Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Kluck, B., & Cornwell, R. (2001). I am not an animal: Mortality salience, disgust, and the denial of human creatureliness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 427435. Goldenberg, J. L., Routledge, C., & Arndt, J. (in press). Mammograms and the management of emotional discomfort: Threats associated with the physicality of the body and neuroticism. Psychology and Health. Greenberg, J., Arndt, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (2000). Proximal and distal defenses in response to reminders of ones mortality: Evidence of a temporal sequence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 9199. Greenberg, J., Martens, A., Jonas, E., Eisenstadt, D., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (2003). Psycholo- gical defense in anticipation of anxiety: Eliminating the potential for anxiety eliminates the effects of mortality salience on worldview defense. Psychologi- cal Science, 14, 516519. Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosen- blatt, A., Veeder, M., Kirkland, S., et al. (1990). Evidence for terror management II: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 58, 308318. Greenberg, J., Simon, L., Porteus, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1995). Evidence of a terror manage- ment function of cultural icons: The effects of mortality salience on the inappropriate use of cherished cultural symbols. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 12211228. Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., Rosen- blatt, A., Burling, J., Lyon, D., et al. (1992). Why do people need self-esteem? Converging evidence of an anxiety-buffering function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 913922. Harlowe, L., Newcomb, M., & Bentler, P. (1986). Depression, self-derogation, substance abuse, and suicide ideation: Lack of purpose in life as a mediational factor. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 521. King, L. A., & Napa, C. K. (1998). What makes a good life? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 156165. Landau, M. J., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Martens, A. (2006). Windows into nothing- ness: Terror management, meaninglessness, and negative reactions to modern art. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 90, 879892. Lester, D. (1990). The CollettLester Fear of Death scale: The original version and a revision. Death Studies, 14, 451468. Mascaro, N., & Rosen, D. H. (2006). The role of existential meaning as a buffer against stress. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 46, 168190. Mooney, D. C., & OGorman, J. G. (2001). Construct validity of the revised CollettLester Fear of Death and Dying scale. Omega, 43, 157173. Norenzayan, A., & Hansen, I. G. (2006). Belief in supernatural agents in the face of death. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 174187. Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Greenberg, J. (2003). In the wake of 9/11: The psychology of terror. Washington, DC: American Psychological Associa- tion. Reker, G. T., Peacock, E. J., & Wong, P. T. P. (1987). Meaning and purpose in life and well-being: A life span perspective. Journal of Gerontology, 42, 4449. Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszc- zynski, T., & Lyon, D. (1989). Evidence for terror management theory I: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 681690. Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 57, 10691081. Schimel, J., Hayes, J., Williams, T. J., & Jahrig, J. (2007). Is death really the worm at the core? MORTALITY SALIENCE INCREASES DEATH ANXIETY COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2010, 24 (5) 853 Converging evidence that worldview threat increases death-thought accessibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 789803. Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (1991). Terror management theory of self-esteem. In C. R. Snyder & D. Forsyth (Eds.), Handbook of social and clinical psychology: The health perspective (pp. 2140). New York: Pergamon Press. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063 1070. Wong, P. T. P. (1998). Implicit theories of meaningful life and the development of the personal meaning prole. In P. T. P. Wong & P. S. Fry (Eds.), The human quest for meaning. A handbook of psychological research and clinical applications (pp. 111140). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Zika, S., & Chamberlain, K. (1992). On the relation between meaning in life and psychological well- being. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 133145. ROUTLEDGE AND JUHL 854 COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2010, 24 (5) Copyright of Cognition & Emotion is the property of Psychology Press (UK) and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.