Big Five History, Goldberg
Big Five History, Goldberg
Big Five History, Goldberg
cditcd by
Patrick E. Shrout
New York University
Susan T. Fiske
University of Massaeh usetts at Anlherst
CHAPTE(\
3
WHAT THE HELL TOOK So LONG?
DONALD W. FISKE AND THE l3IG-FIVE
FAGOR
STRUGURE
Lewis rt Goldberg
Universiry of Oregon ond Oregon (\eseorch Insrirure
If a 11l0dern-day Rip van Winkle had been asleep during the 40-year interval
between 1949 and 1989, he would have been slartled upon a\vakening by the
alnount of scientific progress that had occurred in genclics and nledicinc, space
science, nlaterials technology, conlpute." science, and even in lhe cognitive,
behavioral, and neurosciences. On lhe olher hanu. if he had becn pat1icularly
inlerested in the n10st basic and fundamental problenl in personality psychologylhe slruclure of hunlan pcrsonality lraits-,he would \vonder why it had laken 40
years to begin to develop a scientific consensus about an appropriate taxononlic
nlodel. In this chapter, I briefly describe sonle of the events leading to loday's
en1erging consensus on a structuraJ represenlation for phenolypic personality truits.
highlighting lhe historieal significance of Donald Fiske's (1949) senlinal analyses.
1 thcn point out sonle of the obstncles to scicntific agreenlent that occurrcd betwccn
lhe publication of Fiske's report and the .present-day enlerging consensus. nlC
overall ainl of this chaptcr is to provok~,lconjcctures about those praeticcs and
procedures that could help ensure that fulure research on lhe "cutting edge' in
pcrsonaJity psychology will nol always take so torturously long lO be acecpted.
30
GOLDDERG
31
lAs pointcd out by Peabody and Goldberg (1989), the interpretalion of Factor V as Cullure arose
from a historieal acddent: Although Callell had initially constructed a subset of variables reJaling to
me/lc:I, in lhe seminal sludies of Canell (1947) he omittcd all .hose variables in favor of an
intelligence test. In tumo this test was omiucd from his I~tcr studies. leaving no direct rcpresentation
of most mcllccl variables. In lheir abscnce. Factor Vwas cancd Culturc by Tupes and Chrislal
( 1% 1) and so me laler investigators. However. when variables related 10 II/ellcel were reintroouced
(e.g . Goldbcrg. 19(0). il beca me clear lhat lIlcllcel was lhe more appropriale label for lhe fiflh
broad factor.
02
GOLDDERG
factors with the much larger number rcportcd by CattelI, and expressed his
feelings about the correspondence bctwcen lli lwO solutions: "A thorough study
of these two sets of factors, one from Catten and one fron10ur ratings by
teamnlates, leaves one with feelings of both optinlism and discouragement. Even
in the face of no cpmplcte congruence, the similarities support a belief in the
possibility of eventual agreement upon the basic variables in personality" (p.
341). Note the similarity between that last sentence and the one quoted earHer
fron1 1l1urstone (1934). who also obtained five personality factors.
04
GOLDBERG
Dorgatta used other variables in separatc analyses of maJe and female samples
and found five replicated factors, which he Jabcled Asserriveness (1), Likability
(II), Responsibility (IlI), Enlotionality (IV), and Inrelligellce (V). Using composite
scores on each of the five factors, he compared seIf-ratings, self-rankings, and
pccr rankings (the laUer two in samples conlposed of cIose fricnds, as wcll as
in samplcs of acquaintanccs) in a multitrait-multimethod (MTMM) dcsign
(Campbcll & Fiskc, 1959). Using the variables of CatteIl (1947), Smilh (1967)
conlpared lhe structures derivcd from three large samples (N = 583 t 521, and
324) and found five robust factors, which he labeled EXlraversioll (I),
Agreeableness (Il), Srrengrh ofCharacter (In), Enlotionality (IV), and Refinemellt
(V). Moreover, Smith found that scores derived fronl the Character (ni) factor
corrclatcd .43 with coUegc grades. In a latcr pccr-nomination study t Snlith (1969)
rccovercd the four factors other than Refinen,ellt in nlale and female sanlples at
bOth the junior high school and high school levels. Moreover, he found high
correlations with snl0king status, smoking being negatively rclated to Factors n
(Agreeableness) and III (Strenglh of Character), and positively related to Factor
I (Exlraversion).
35
ehapters (Goldberg, 1981 t 1982), both stimulated by the lexical hypothesis. TI1C
first provided sorne eonjectures about the possible universality of the Big-Fivc
factor strueture, and the sccond described a program of research focusing on
English trait-deseriptive adjeetives and nouns.
TIle 1980s wit~essed the emergenee of the two dominant variants of lhe
five-factor nlodel",one developed by McCrae and Costa (1985a, 1987) ano
operationalizco in the Neuroticism-Extraversion .. Opcnness Personality Inventory
(NEO-PI; Costa & MeCrae, 1985), and the other associated with studies baseo
on the lexieal hYPolhesis and operationaHzcd in the sets of factor nlarkers provided
by Norman (1963), Pcabody and Goldberg (1989), Goldbcrg (1990, 1992), John
(1989), Trapnel1 and Wiggins (1990), and Digman and his assoeiatcs (e.g.,
Dignlan, 1989; Dignlan & Inouye, 1986). Much is the sanle in both variants: (a)
lhe nunlber of dinlensions is identical, namely five; (b) the eontenl of Factor IV
is essentially lhe sanle, although it is orientcd in the opposite direclion in the
two Jnodels, and is thus so labcled (Emorional Srability vs. Neuroticisln); and
(e) therc is considerable sinli1arity, although not identity in the eontent of Factor
III (Collsciclltiousllcss). On the olher hand, al leasl two of the differences bctween
lhe vuianls are quite striking: (a) lhe localions of Faelors 1 and 11 are
systenlatically rotateo, such thal Warmlll is a faeel 01' Exlraversioll in lhe NEO-PI,
whereas it is a facet of Agrecableness in lhe lexical nlodcJ;2 and (b) Factor V is
eoneeived as Openncss lo E~7}erienee in the NEO-PI and as Inlel/cet, or
Imaginario" in lhe lexical nlodeI.
These differenees stenl fronl the history of the NEO..PI, which starled out as
a questionnaire nleasurc of a thrce-faetor nlodel. including only Ncurolicisl1J.
EXlraversioll, and Openness 10 Expericncc. Whcreas olher three-faetor lheorists
sueh as Eysenek (1991) have stood firm as proponents of their original
representations t Costa and MeCrae reacted to the events of the early 19805 \Vilh
sueh relllarkable Openness lo E~7}eriellce that,by the cnd of the deeade, lhese
investigalors had beeome the world's nlost prolific and influential proponents of
lhe five-faelor nlodel. TI1C prodigious oulpouring of rcports by McCrae and Costa
probably did Jnore lo form the modem eonsensus aboul pcrsonalily structure lhan
anything eIse that oeeurred during the 1980s. Specifieally, they used the NEO-PI
seales as a franlework for integrating a wide varietyof other questionnaire scales,
incIuding those developcd by Eysenek (McCrae & Costa, 1985b), Jackson (Costa
& McCrae, 1988a), Spielbcrger (Costa & MeCrac, 1987), and Wiggins (McCrac
& Costa, 1989b), as wcll as the scales included in lhe Minnesota Multipha~ic
Personal ity Inventory (MMPI; Costa, Buseh, Zondcnnan, & MeCrae, 1986) ano
the Myers-Briggs Typc Indicator (MeCrae & Costa, I989a).
t
2Actually. (he trait descriptor Warm has been classified in racel 11+/1+ in sorne AB5C (Hofslee.
de Raad. & Goldberg. 1992) analyses in (he lexical model. whereas Warmlh is considered a 1+/11+
facel in lhe McCrnc and Cosaa model. suggesting more agreemenl when both primary and secondary
loadings are considercd (han when one cOllsiders lhe primary loadings alonc.
06
GOLDBERG
Certainly one of the forces that worked against ear)y rcplications of lhe five-factor
nl0del was the sheer c.lifficulty of the calcuJations prior to the widespread
availabiJity of reasonably efficient conlputers and standardizcd COtllputcr
progranls. From Thurstone (1934) through Fiske (1949) to Tupes and Christal
(1961), factor analyses were carried out by hand, which virtuaIly guaranteed the
possibility of sorne clerical errors, as Digman and Takemoto-Chock (1981)
discovered when they reanalyzed sonle of the datasets provided by Cattell. Tupes
anc.l Christal included lhe findings fronl one computer analysis in their report,
07
and were able to show its conlparabiJity with tlleir painstaking hand ..ca1culations.
However. by the time of Norman (1963), computers were hcre to stay, and today
this problem has all bUl disappeared.
lock of Follow..Up ,
Had Thurstone (1934) more fully enlbraccd lhe lexical hypothesis and realized the
significance of his nitial discovery, lhe history of the Big-Five model might have
been quite different. But Thurstone devoted hinlself to other scicntific pursuits, as
did Fiske, Borgatta, and Smith. Seenlingly. tllesc investigators did not reaBre lhe
inlportance of their discoveries. Why? Here are sorne spcculative possibiHties.
Doing Our Own Things
111C
08
GOLDBERG
3.
09
In many ways it sccms rcmarkable that such stability shouJd be found in an area
which to date has granted anything but consistent results. Undoubtedly lhe
consistency has always beCn thcre, bUl it has becn hiddcn by inconsistcncy of
factorial lcchniques and philosophics, the lack of replication using identical
variables, amI disagrccment among analysts as lo factor titles. None of lhe factors
idcntificd in this s,tudy are new. They have becn idcntificd many times in previous
analyscs, although they havc nol always becn callcd by thc sanlC names. (Tupes
& Christal, 1961, p. 12)
Littlc did those investigators realize that it would lake yet another 25 years
for lhe ficld to separate lhe Big-Five signal from the noise of alternative factor
representations. Inconsistency of factorial techniques and philosophies have
renlained, and have increased over tinle. On the other hand, lhe widcsprcad
availability of cotnputer progrmns forconducting a standard analysis (such as
principal cornponents rotated by varimax) has servcd to decrcase lhe use of
idiosyncratic proccdurcs. In any case, wc now know that nlost reservations about
the fragility of factor solutions are unfounded, provided that the datasets nelude
substantial nunlbcrs of variables and sanlples of largc sizc. Por example, Goldbcrg
(1990) dcnlonslratcd lhe virtual idenlity of Big-Five factor loadings across 10
factorial procedurcs, incJuding bOlh factor anuconlponent analyses, and both
orthogonal and obl que rotational algorithrns.
A sccond problenl identificd by Tupes and Christal (1961) was a )ack of
replications using identical variables. Indecd, it hasproved 10 be extrcrnely
difricult lo cunlulatc concJusions fronl single-shot studics with uniquc scts of
variables. In lhe history of lhe Big-Five factor SlruClure, the studies thal have
provcd to be most persuasive to critics of the nlodel were those that conlpared
the factor slructurcs from similar sets of variables across divcrse samplcs or
procedurcs (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1988b; Dignlan & Takemoto-Chock, 1981;
Fiskc, 1949; Goldbcrg, 1990; McCrae & Cosla, 1987; Norman, 1963; Tupes &
Christal, 1961).
lllC final problelll identified by Tupes and Christal waS that of disagrecnlcnt
anlong analysts as to factor tilles. Everyone scems to have a favorite labcJ.
espccially for the Big-Pive factors, which are c1early loo broad to be captured
wcll by any single trait-descriptive teml. Critics of lhe rnodel are quick lo nole
the discord an10ng investigators in lheir factor Jabcls, and to attribute such
diffcrences in labcls to fundamental differences in the undcrlying factor structures.
However, n10st of the diffcrences in labcls rcsult from individual styles or tastes
(e.g., COllscienliollslless vs. WiU for Pactor 111). One difference inlabcls docs
renect a sonlewhat different conception of the underlying factor: Factor V is
labcJcd as Openness lo Experience in the McCrae and Costa variant of the
five-factor nlodel, and as Illtellect in the lexical variant. Actually, as suggestcd
by Saucicr (1992), neither Openness nor Illlellect captures weJl the central cluster
of traits thai define Factor V. Perhaps a more apt tabe) is Imaginarion for a
dornain incJuding such traits as Crealivily and Curiosity.
40
GOLDDERG
A Malignant Zeitgeist
In 1961, Tupes and Christal had no way of knowing that the 1970s would witness
a virtual moratorium on the investigation of many fundamental problems in
personality psychology, inc1uding the search for a compclIing taxononly of
personality traits. However, the 1970s turoed out to be a decade of controversy
about the scientific status of traits in general and broad traits in particular,
stimulated by such critics as D' Andrade (1965), Mischel (1968), and Shweder
(1975). Many of the issues nvolved in this controversy may have now been
resolved (e.g., Borkenau, 1992; Funder, 1991), with optinlistk implications for the
eventual transformation of personality psychology into a nlore cumulative seientifk enterprise.
CODA
Fiske was nluch nvolved in the controversies of the 1970s, and he was profoundly
infiuenced by the behavioristic Zeitgeist that was swccping the field at thallinle.
Perhaps as a reaction to lhe "nattering nabobs of ncgativism" who were
punlnleling personality psyehology he lost nlerest in broad pcrsonalily traits
(e.g., Fiske, 1973, 1974), and began to argue in favor of more bchavioral unils
of anaIysis. so as to ensure high levcls of interjudge agreement on lhe bask
elements of pcrsonality. Moreover, he began to feel quite pessimistic about lhe
likelihood of ever reaching agreetnent on a taxonomy of personality traits, with
the result that he did not further pursue his seminal study of trait factor struetures.
Today t Fiske is renowned for his methodological and philosophkal contributions
to the ficld of personality. Wouldn't it be ronie if historians of lhe future also
judged his 1949 empirical study as bcing of such signa1 importance that it served
as a verification of his earlier "belief in the possibility of eventual agreement
upon the basic variables of personal ity" (Fiske, 1949 p. 341)1 If so, he has lhe
right lo ask, What lhe hell look so long?
t
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Preparation of this chapter was supporled by Grant MH-49227 from the Nalional
Institute of Mental Health. 1 am indebted to A. Timothy Church, Jacob Cohen,
Paul T. Costa, Jr., Lec J. Cronbach, Robyn M. Dawes, Qlarlcs F. Dkken, John
M. Digman, Donald W. Fiske, Susan T. Fiske, Sarah E. Hampson, Willem K. B.
Hofslee, Oliver P. John, Daniel Lcvitin, Maurice Lorr, Clarence McCormkk,
Ivan Mervielde, Warren T. Norman, Delroy L. Paulhus, Lawrence A. Pervin,
James A. Russell, Gerard Saueier, Mkhael F. Scheier, Patrick E. Shrout, and
Auke Tellegen for lheir thoughtful suggestions. Portions of this chaptcr have
41
bccn adaptcd from Goldbcrg (1993), which can be consultcd for furthcr dctails
about thc Big-Fivc factor structurc and its historical dcvclopn1cnt.
REFERENCES
Allport. G. W. & Odbcrt. H. S. (1936). Trail-names: A psycho-Iexical sludy. Ps)'dlOlogical
MonogrofJhs, 47 (1, Wholc No. 211).
Barrick. M. R & Mount. M. K. (1991). The Big Fivc pcrsonality dimensions and job performance:
A meta-analysis. PersO/Ille! PsydlOlogy, 44. 1...26.
Borgatla. E. F. (1964). Thc structure of pcrsonaJity characteristics. Behavioral Science. 9. 8-17.
Borkenau. P. (1992). Implicil personality theory and lhe five-faclor model. JourtuJl of Personality.
60. 295-327.
Campbell. O. T . & Fiske. O. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validalion by (he
multitrait-multimethod matrix. PsydlOlogic:al Bullel;n, 56. 81-105.
Cantor. N. & Mischel. W. (1979). Prototypes in person perception.ln L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Ad\ance.f
ill experimelltal sodall'syt:llOlogy (Vol. 12. pp. 2-52). San Diego. CA: Aeademic Prcss.
Callell. R. B. (1943). The deseription of personality: Baste traits resolved into cluslers. Jourllal of
Abllormal ond Sodal PsydlOlo:y. 38. 476-506.
Canell, R. U. (1947). Confirmation and clnrificalion of primary personality faclors. PsydlOmelril.:a.
/2, 197-220.
Christal. R. E. (1992). Aulhor's note On "Recurrent persona lit y ractors based en tral ratings." Joumal
of Personality. 60. 221-224.
Costa. P. T . Jr., Busch, C. M., Zonderman. A. B., & McCrae. R. R. (1986). Correlations of MMPI
faclor scales with measures of the five factor model of personality. JuurtuJl of Ptrsonality
Aues.fmelll. 50. 640-650.
Costa, P. T . Jr., & McCrae, (~. R. (1985). Tire NEO Persollality Imtlllory malllllll. Odessa. FL:
Psychological Assessmenl Resources.
Costa. P. T . Jr. & McCrae. R. R. (1987). Personality assessment in psychosornatie medicine: Value
of a lral taxonomy. In G. A. Fava & T. N. Wise (Eds.). Advanc'es illl'syc:Jwsomalk medicine:
Vol. /7. Researd, I,aradi:"'s illl,sydlOsomal;c medic;lte (pp. 71-..82). Basel, Swilzerland: Karger.
Costa, P. T . Jr & McCrae. R. R. (1988a). From eatalog lO classification: Murray's needs and lhe
five-factor model. )oufl,al of Persolla/ity and Social Psyehology. 55. 258-265.
Costa. P. T., Jr. & McCrac. R. R. (1988b). Personality in adullhood: A six-year longiludinal study
of self-reports and spouse ralings on lhe NEO Personalily Inventory. Journal of Persolfalily and
Scx..'ial Psyelroloxy. 54. 853-863.
O' Andrade, R. G. (1965). Trail psychology and eomponential analyss. AmericlllJ AltlhrOI)()lo:isl.
67.215-228.
Oigman. J. M. (1989). Five robusl trail dimcnsions: OeveJopmenl, stabilily. and utility. Jourltal uf
/)er.wnalily. 57. 195-214.
Digman. J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of 'he five-faclor model. In M. R. Rosenzweig
& L. W. Porter (Eds.). A""ual review of psyclw/u:y (Vol. 41. pp. 417... 440). Palo Alto. CA:
Annual I~eviews.
Oigman. J. M. & Inouye. J. (1986). Further spccification of lhe five robust faclors of personality.
Juumal of Personality all(1 Scx..ial PsydlOlo:y. 50, 116-123.
Digman. J. M & Takemolo-Chock, N. K. (1981). Factors in lhe natural languageof personality:
Re-analysis. comparison. and interpretation of sx major studies. Mulli,'ariale Belra";oral
Researe". /6. 149-170.
Eysenck. H. J. (1991). Dimensions of personality: 16. S. or 3?-Criteria for a taxonomic paradigm.
Per.wnalily.alUl lI,di"idllal Dij)erences. 12. 773-790.
42
GOLDBERG
Eysenck. H. J. (1992). rour ways five faclors are 1101 basic. Personality and J"d;~'id'lQl DW'crenc:cs,
IJ.667-673.
Fiskc. D. W. (1949). Consislcncy of lhe factorial struclures of personalily ralings from differcnl
sources. 10urllal of Ab"ormal a"d Sodal PsycJlOlogy. 44, 329-344.
Fiske. D. W. (1973). Can a personality construct be validated empirically? PsycllOlogic:al Bulle/i",
80.89-92.
Fiske. D. W. (1974). The IimUs for lhe convcnlional scicnce of personaJity. Joumal 01 Pcr.'iOIlality,
42, 1-11.
Funder. D. C. (1991). Globaltrals: A neo-A 11 portian apprCh1ch lo pcrsonality. Psyc:110108ical SdCIICC,
2.31-39.
Galton. F. (1884). Measuremcnt of charaeter. Forllf;8hlly Review, J6. 179-185.
Goldberg. L. R. (1981). Language and individual differences: The search for universals in personality
Icxicons. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), RevietV cifl'ersollalily a"d sodal psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 141-165).
Beverly Bilis. CA: Sage.
Goldberg. L. R. (1982). From ace lo zombie: Sorne exploralions in lhe language of personality. In
C. D. Spiclbergcr & J. N. Butcher (Eds.), Allvalu:c./f in I'cr.wllalily aS.U.umCIII (Vol. 1, (lp.
203-234). HiIlsdale. NJ: L1wrenee Erlbaum Associates.
Goldhcrg, L. R. (1990). An altemalive uDescription of pcrsonalily": ll1e Dig-Five factor stnacture.
Joumal of I'crsollalily all Social Psychol08Y. 59. 1216-1229.
Goldberg. L. R. (1992). 111e devcJopmcnt of markers for lhe Big-Five fclor slructure. l'sYc:/10108ic:al
A.uc.umclfI, 4, 2()-42.
Goldhcrg, L. Ro (1993). ll1e slruclure of phcnolYpic pcrsonality tmls. Amcricm, I'sydw/08i:t1. 4t't.
2()-34.
Ilmnpsoll. S. E. o John, O. P. & Goldbcrg, L. It (1986). Calcgory brcadlh and hierarchieal Slnaclure
in personalily: Studies of asymmelries in judgmenls of lral implications. JOllrl.al o/ Persollalily
alld SIH.:ial Psyellology. 5/, 37-54.
Ilofslec, W. K. B. de Raad. B., & Goldberg. L. R. (1992). Illlegralion of lhe Big-Five and cireumplcx
apprChlches lo (mil struelure. Joumal of Pcrsollalily and Sodal Psyd10108Y. 6J. 146-163.
Jolm, O. P. (1989).' Towards a laxonomy of personalily descriptors. In D. M. Buss & N. Cantor
(Eds.). /'ersollalily l,sycI10108Y: Rece," Irelldf a"d cmcr8i"8 dircl'liolls (pp. 261-271). Ncw York:
Springer-Verlag.
Jolm. O. r. (1990). Thc UBig Five" factor taxonomy: Dimcnsions of pcrsonality in lhe naturallanguage
and in questionnaircs. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), /Ialldhook (Jll,crsollalily: r'eory alUl rescare" (pp.
66-100). New York: Guilrord.
John. 0.1' .. Hampson. S. E . & Goldbcrg, L. R. (1991). ll1e basie levcl in personality-trail hierarchies:
Studies of trail use and aceessibility in differenl conlexlS. JOllmal of Pcrsollalily allll Sodal
Psydlolo8Y. 60, 348-361.
Kelly. E. L., & Fiske. D. W. (1951). rile predic:lioll o/I'er/ormmu:e in dillkal I's)'('/10108Y. Ann
Arbor. MI: University of Michigan I'ress.
McCrac. R. R. (1992). Editor' s introduClion lo Tupes and Chrislal. JOllma/ (if Per.wllalilJ'. 60.
217-219.
McCrae. R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1985a). Updating Norman's "adcquate laxonomy": Intelligcnce
and personality dimensions in natural languagc and in questio.nnaires. Joumal (Jf Pcr.wnalily and
Sodal P./fyC:/10108Y, 49, 710-721.
McCrae. R. R, & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1985b). Comparison or EPI and psycholicism scalcs wilh measures
of lhe (ve-factor model of personalily; Persollalily a"d Indh'idlllJl Dij)'erenc:cs. 6. 587-597.
McCrac~ R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1987). Validation of lhe five-ractor modcl of personality across
instruments and observcrs. Jounwl 01 Persollalily alld Social Psychol08Y, 52. 81-90.
McCrae. R. R. & Costa. P. T Jr. (1989a). Reinterpreling lhe Myers-Origgs Type Indicator from
lhe pcrspectivc of lhe five-faclor model or personaJity. Jourllal o/ Per.'iOIlality, 57, 17-40.
McCrae. R. R. & Costa. P. T . Jr. (1989b). The struclure of interpersonallrails: Wiggins' circumplex
lhe fivefaetor model. Joumal o/ PersOIUllity a"d Social Psyc'oI08Y, 56, 586-595.
:too
43
McCrae. R. R., & Jolm, O. P. (1992). An introduction lo lhe nve-factor model and its applications.
574-583.
Norman, W. T. (1967). 2800 pcrsollality Irail dcscriptors: Normalive 0l)(!raling characlcristics lor
a university IXlIJUlalioll. Ann Arbor, MI: Dcpartmcnlof Psychology, University of Michigan.
Peabody. D., & Goldbcrg. L R. (1989). Sorne delcrminants of factor structures from pcrsonality-lrait
descriptors. Journal 01 Persotrality alld Social Psyclwlogy, 57.552-567.
Saucicr, G. (1992). Openness versus ntelleca: Much ado about nothing? European Journal 01
PersonalilY, 6, 381-386.
Schmidl, F. L.. & Ones, D. S. (1992). Personnel selection. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Portcr
(Eds.), A""ual rcview OllJSyclwlogy (Vol. 43, pp. 627-670). Palo Alto. CA: Annual Reviews.
Shweder. R. A. (1975). How relevanl is nn individual dirrcrcncc theory of personality? Journal 01
781-790.
Tupes. E. C . & Christal, R. E. (1958). Stability of personality tral raling factors obtaincd undcr
diverse conditions (USAF WADC Tech. Note No. 58-61). L.ackland Air Force Base. TX: U.S.
Air Force.
Tupes. E. C., & Chrislal. R. E. (1961). Recurrent personality factors bascd on trait ratings (USAF
ASD Tech. Rep. No. 61-97). L.ackland Air Force Base. TX: U.S. Air Force.
Tupes, E. C . & Christal. R. E. (1992). Recurrent personalily factors based on trail ratings. Journal
01 Persollalty. 60. 225-25 l.
Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, A. L. (1992). Personality: Slruclure and assessmcnt. In M. R. Rosenzwcig
& L. W. Porter (Eds.), Amulal rcview 01 psychology (Vol. 43. pp. 473-504). Palo Alto, CA:
Annual Rcviews.
Wiggins, J. S & Trapncll, P. D. (in press). Personality structure: The retum oC the Big Ave. In R.
Bogan, J. A. Johnson, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), lIalldhook 01 persona/ity psychology. Orlando. FL:
Acadcmic Prcss.