Multigenerational Giftedness: Perceptions of Giftedness Across Three Generations
Multigenerational Giftedness: Perceptions of Giftedness Across Three Generations
Multigenerational Giftedness: Perceptions of Giftedness Across Three Generations
606
Multigenerational Giftedness
607
608
& Oden, 1947). However, the Terman samples children still had a significantly above-average mean IQ score, which again suggests that IQ is
heritable. Terman and Oden (1947) discussed the discrepancy between
the IQ scores of the original gifted sample and their offspring as being
due to Galtons law of filial regression, which they stated explained that
only half of ones heredity comes from the two parents while the other
half is from more remote ancestry. The regression is about the same as
that found for height of offspring from exceptionally tall parents (p.
236). In follow-up studies in 19391940 and 19511952, the IQ scores
of the Terman gifted samples offspring were again measured using the
Stanford-Binet (Terman & Oden, 1959). At that time, the mean IQ
score for offspring was 132.7 (Terman & Oden, 1959). This finding
lent additional support to the notion of a hereditary component of
intelligence.
Landau and Weissler (1993) conducted a study in Israel that compared the parents of identified gifted children who attended a special
school for the gifted to parents of children who had not been identified as gifted. They found that parents of identified gifted children
had a high level of academic achievement, had more cognitive interactions with their children, and had more positive attitudes toward
their childrens intelligence and school achievements. Similarly, Gross
(2004) found that most of the parents of Australian students with IQ
scores greater than or equal to 160 had high education levels and highprestige, professional occupations. In Gross sample, 86% of gifted students fathers and 50% of gifted students mothers had at least a 4-year
college degree. Many of these parents were recognized as eminent
contributors to their fields of employment, such as science, medicine,
and literature. In a study of mathematically and intellectually gifted
students, Albert (1980) found that both parents and grandparents of
mathematically gifted students had higher education levels than the
national norms, which suggests that a love of learning and high educational aspirations may be multigenerational family characteristics.
In a more recent study of high-achieving third-grade students who
were former Head Start participants, researchers found that parents
of high-achieving students also had higher education levels than parents of students of average achievement (Robinson et al., 2002). In
another study, Weissler and Landau (1993) compared three types of
Israeli parents: those with no gifted children, those with one gifted
Multigenerational Giftedness
609
child, and those with two or more gifted children. They found that
parents with a gifted child had more discussions with their children,
discussed family issues and problems with the educational system
more often with their children, and engaged in more verbal problemsolving activities with their children. Parents of a gifted child also differed in their verbal behaviors: they used more correct grammar, used
more analogies and examples in their speech, and used more images
and metaphors when communicating with their children. Again, the
characteristics of the parents of gifted children mirrored theoretical
literature on adult giftedness. Jacobsen (1999) also noted that gifted
adults enjoy problem solving, tend to be idealistic and have high standards, enjoy having frequent discussions and debates, and are often
highly creative.
In Gross (2004) longitudinal study of exceptionally gifted children (those with an IQ score greater than or equal to 145) in Australia,
the majority of participants who had siblings had one or more siblings
who also met criteria for giftedness, with IQ scores ranging from 127
to 175+. The tendency for families to have multiple gifted children
makes sense in light of Weissler and Landaus (1993) finding that parents of gifted children tend to provide their children with environments that encourage the development of intellectual abilities. The
homes of identified gifted children in Weissler and Landaus study,
relative to the homes of nongifted children, contained more concrete environmental stimuli such as books, toys, and works of art
(p. 145). In addition, parents of gifted children took their children
on more family trips. Such early exposure to a variety of stimulating
experiences and objects, especially in conjunction with frequent intellectually stimulating verbal interactions with parents, may promote
the development of giftedness in all children in a family. However,
research into the lives of hundreds of eminent people has found that
not all gifted individuals home environments are havens full of intellectual stimulation (Goertzel, Goertzel, Goertzel, & Hansen, 2004).
Goertzel et al. (2004) found that some gifted individuals came from
family backgrounds characterized by problems such as emotional
strife, alcoholism, family conflicts, and poverty. They concluded
that while not all families of gifted people were able to provide their
children with concrete environmental stimuli or great emotional sta-
610
Multigenerational Giftedness
611
612
understanding of giftedness across generations for counselors, educators, or others who work with gifted individuals. The research questions were as follows:
1. Will participants perceive one or both of their parents as
gifted, and if so, in what areas?
2. Will participants perceive any of their children to be gifted,
and if so, in what areas?
Method
Participants
Participants were 89 gifted adults (34 male, 55 female) who have
participated in an ongoing longitudinal study of academically talented students who graduated high school in 1988. At the time of
data collection, participants ages ranged from 35 to 38 (M = 36.73,
SD = .62). Most participants were employed full-time outside the
home (85%), whereas 11% were full-time homemakers, 1% were fulltime students, 1% were looking for work, and 2% did not provide
information regarding employment status. In terms of family roles,
83% of participants were married, 9% of participants were single, 5%
responded that they were divorced, and 3% of participants indicated
that they were living with a partner. Most participants had children
(78%). There were 22% of respondents with no children, 12% had
one child, 43% had 2 children, 21% had 3 children, 1% had 4 children, and 1% had 5 children. In terms of educational level 20 years
post-high school, 33% of participants had completed a bachelors level
degree, 5% had completed some graduate school, 37% had completed
a masters level degree, and 25% had completed a doctoral level degree.
Procedures
Participants were initially recruited by asking all school counselors at
private and public high schools in a Midwestern state to identify the
top two graduates in schools graduating less than 250 students, and
the top five graduates in schools graduating more than 250 students.
Multigenerational Giftedness
613
614
discuss the purpose of the analysis, which in this case was to identify major themes among responses to questions about giftedness in
participants parents and children. The next step is for at least two
researchers to independently generate categories and meet to determine consensus. In the present study, the six researchers generated
categories of responses independently and then convened to determine specific categories based on consensus among members. The last
step is assignment of data to categories. The researchers each independently assigned responses to categories and then met to determine
consensus of assignment to categories.
Results
Participants were asked if either or both of their parents were identified as gifted or if they had noticed signs of giftedness in either parent. Thirty-seven percent of participants stated that neither of their
parents were gifted, 34% stated that both parents were gifted, 14%
reported that only their mother was gifted, 14% reported that only
their father was gifted, and 1% said they were unsure if either parent
was gifted. This is interesting because the number of participants who
thought neither of their parents were gifted was roughly equal to the
number of participants who thought both their parents were gifted;
and the number of participants who thought only their mother was
gifted was exactly equal to those who thought only their father was
gifted. However, participants perceived their mothers and fathers to
be gifted in slightly different areas.
Those participants who saw their parents as gifted were asked to
describe the areas in which their parents were gifted. The research
team, using the method described above, arrived at 10 categories of
giftedness among participants responses. These areas were: math and
science; exceptional memory; mechanical or spatial ability; interpersonal skills; language and writing; problem solving and critical
thinking; athletic ability; leadership, business, or organizational
skills; creative or artistic abilities; and general academic abilities. Of
the participants who reported their mothers were gifted, the three
most frequently cited areas of giftedness for mothers were creative
or artistic abilities (22%), language and writing (20%), and general
Multigenerational Giftedness
615
616
Table 1
Participants Perceptions of Areas of Giftedness for Their
Mothers and Fathers
Area/Examples
Mothers
Fathers
11%
25%
Exceptional memory
My father has always had an exceptional memory, which I
inherited.
My father has a sharp memory and can memorize almost every
phone number he comes across.
2%
3%
0%
17%
Interpersonal skills
Both my parents are gifted socially. They are very kind and caring.
My mom is very in tune to others thoughts, feelings, and
perspectives.
11%
10%
20%
8%
3%
7%
Athletic ability
My mother is gifted athletically.
My dad is gifted at sports.
2%
2%
14%
8%
22%
10%
15%
10%
Multigenerational Giftedness
617
Table 2
Participants Perceptions of Areas of Giftedness
for Their Children
Area/Examples
Math and science
My first grader is doing fifth-grade math.
He is taking advanced classes in math and science.
Percent of
responses
21%
Exceptional memory
My daughter remembers words to stories and books after hearing them one
time and memorizes movies.
He has an ability to recall statisticsespecially football or sports related
information.
3%
2%
Interpersonal skills
She has a remarkable ability to connect with people, particularly children with
special needs. She intuitively knows what they need and how to interact with
them.
Our oldest daughter appears to be very socially intelligent and empathic.
3%
28%
8%
Athletic ability
He has been throwing balls the correct way since age 2 and loves just about
every sport.
My son is a very gifted athlete.
3%
5%
11%
16%
618
Table 3
Age That Parents First Noticed Giftedness in Their Children
and Area of Giftedness for Each Age
Age That Signs of Giftedness Were First Noticed in Area
Area
<1
Exceptional memory
Mechanical or spatial
ability
Interpersonal skills
10
Athletic ability
Early achievement of
developmental milestones
Quick learner/general
intelligence
Multigenerational Giftedness
619
category for problem solving and critical thinking for both children
and parents. Marlands area of leadership ability was echoed in our
category of leadership, business, or organizational skills giftedness
in participants parents. Marlands fifth area of giftedness, visual and
performing arts, corresponded to our category of creative or artistic abilities that participants described their parents and children
possessing. Finally, Marlands sixth area, psychomotor ability, was
echoed in our category of athletic ability in participants parents and
children. The similarity of our categories of giftedness derived from
participants responses and the categories of giftedness described in
the Marland Report suggests that gifted adults in the general public
(a) may believe that giftedness can occur in a variety of areas and (b)
believe they can detect the expression of above-average abilities in several domains in people with whom they have frequent contact.
Through our analysis, we found that around one third of participants indicated that either both of their parents were gifted or both
parents were not gifted. Furthermore, an equal number of participants (14%) reported that only their mother or only their father was
gifted. Underestimation of abilities could be one potential explanation of why some participants did not view their parents as gifted. Just
as teachers sometimes underestimate and fail to identify gifted children (Hodge & Kemp, 2006), participants may have not effectively
identified either of their parents area(s) of giftedness. Additionally,
gifted adults are less frequently identified as gifted than are children
(Perrone, Perrone, Ksiazak, Wright, & Jackson, 2007). An early study
could provide clarification for the reason some participants viewed
both of their parents as gifted. As previously mentioned, Weissler and
Landau (1993) found that parents with gifted children tend to discuss more problems with their children and help children solve problems in a step-by-step manner; parents asked more questions and gave
extensive answers to their questions; and provided more examples and
analogies. It is possible that our participants parents demonstrated a
greater openness in conversations and used a more complex language
style that provided participants with information about their parents
being gifted. We did not ask about the amount or quality of interaction that participants had with their mothers or fathers, but based
on traditional gender roles it is likely that participants spent more
time with their mothers in the home. Thus, more interaction with
620
Multigenerational Giftedness
621
622
infant giftedness first identified giftedness in 8-, 10-, and 11-montholds who subsequently achieved IQ scores above 130 when the children were tested at the age of 4 (Morelock, Brown, & Morrissey,
2003). Similarly, in the Fullerton longitudinal study of giftedness, a
cohort of 130 one-year-old infants and their parents were studied for
24 years (Gottfried, Gottfried, & Guerin, 2006). Mothers reported
significantly higher early advancement in intelligence, verbal, and
social functioning between the ages of 1 and 3.5 years for the 19% of
children in the Fullerton study who were identified at age 8 as having
IQ scores greater than or equal to 130 on the Wechsler Intelligence
Scale for Children-Revised than for the control group of children who
were not identified as gifted at age 8 (Gottfried, Gottfried, Bathurst,
& Guerin, 1994). Additionally, nearly all of the children who were
identified as gifted at age 8 had developmental index scores of 130
or higher at the ages of 1.5 or 2 years on the Bayley Scales of Infant
Development (Gottfried et al., 2006). Children in the control group,
those who were not identified as gifted at age 8, were not reported
by parents to have early advancement, and they had significantly
lower developmental index scores. This suggests that early advancement is predictive of later identifiable giftedness. Findings from the
Fullerton study also support the notion that evidence of giftedness
can be observed in infancy and early childhood.
In actuality, there may have been more children in our study who
displayed signs of giftedness prior to the age of one without the participants recognizing their childrens gifted abilities. As the recognition
of giftedness can be difficult, participants may have underestimated
their childrens abilities, particularly in the early years. Research has
confirmed the difficulty of identifying gifted children, as one study
found less than a 60% effectiveness rate of teachers identification of
giftedness in their students (Hodge & Kemp, 2006). Accurate identification of gifted traits may be particularly difficult for parents who
are not familiar with characteristics of giftedness.
Giftedness seemed to be more easily recognized by our participants as their children aged. By the age of 2, participants identified
numerous areas of giftedness in their children. The area of reading,
language, and writing was the most frequently noticed area of giftedness in participants children. Our finding is consistent with another
qualitative study that assessed parental identification of multiple areas
Multigenerational Giftedness
623
624
Multigenerational Giftedness
625
626
Multigenerational Giftedness
627
Simonton, K. (2005). Giftedness and genetics: The emergenic-epigenetic model and its implications. Journal for the Education of
the Gifted, 28, 270286.
Teeter-Ellison, P. A. (2005). School neuropsychology of Attention
Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. In R. C. DAmato, E. FletcherJanzen, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of school neuropsychology (pp. 460486). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons.
Terman, L. M. (1925). Mental and physical traits of a thousand gifted
children: Genetic studies of genius, Vol. I. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
Terman, L. M., & Oden, M. H. (1947). The gifted child grows up:
Twenty-five years follow-up of a superior group: Genetic studies of
genius, Vol. IV. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Terman, L. M., & Oden, M. H. (1959). The gifted group at mid-life:
Thirty-five years follow-up of a superior group: Genetic studies of
genius, Vol. V. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Tokar, D. M., Thompson, M. N., Plaufcan, M. R., & Williams, C.
M. (2007). Precursors of learning experiences in social cognitive
career theory. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 71, 319339.
Webb, R. M., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2007). Spatial ability:
A neglected dimension in talent searchers for intellectually precocious youth. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 397420.
Weissler, K., & Landau, E. (1993). Characteristics of families with
no, one, or more than one gifted child. The Journal of Psychology,
127, 143152.
628
629
630
631
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.