Raine 2002 PDF
Raine 2002 PDF
Raine 2002 PDF
in Crime~
Public
Policies
J.QWilson
and J.Pet~rsilia
ICS Press:
Oakland,
California
The
Basis
for
Crime
Control
(Editors)
2002.
(pp.
4374)
Biological
of Crime
ADRIAN
RAINE
43
44
Adrian
Raine
Studies
The twin method for ascertaining whether a given trait is to any extent
heritable makes useof the fact that monozygotic (MZ) or "identical" twins
are genetically identical, having lOO percent of their genes in common
with one another. Conversely, dizygotic (DZ) or "fraternal" twins are less
genetically alike than MZ twins, and are in fact no more alike genetically
than non-twin siblings.
When the trait being measured is a dichotomy (for example, criminal/noncriminal), "concordance" rates are calculated for MZ and DZ
twins separately.A 70 percent concordance for crime in a set of MZ twins,
for example, would mean that if one of the MZ pair is criminal, then the
chance of the co-twin being criminal is 70 percent. Similar concordance
ratescan be calculated for DZ twins. IfMZ twins have higher concordance
rates for crime than DZ twins, then this constitutes some evidence for the
notion that crime has a heritable component. The difference between these
correlation coefficients, when doubled, gives an estimate of heritability, or
the proportion of variance in criminality that can be attributed to genetic
influences (Falconer 1965).
Are identical twins more concordant for criminality than fraternal
twins? The answer from many reviews conducted on this expanding field
is undoubtedly yes. As one example, a review of all the twin studies of
crime conducted up to 1993 showed that although twin studies vary
widely in terms of the age, sex, country of origin, sample size, determination of zygosity, and definition of crime, neverthelessall thirteen studies of
The Biological
Basis
of
Crime
45
46
Adoption
Adrian
Raine
Studies
Adoption studies also overcome the problem with tWin studies because
they more cleanly separateout genetic and environmental influences. We
can examine offspring who have been separatedfrom their criminal, biological parents early in life and sent out to other families. If these offspring
grow up to become criminal at greater rates than foster children whose biological parents were not criminal, this would indicate a genetic influence
with its origin in the subject's biological parents.
A variation of this type of study is the "cross-fostering" technique that
hasbeen usedextensivelyin experimental genetic studiesof animals. Applied
to humans, the offspring whose biological parents are criminal or noncriminal are raisedby parentswho themselvesare either criminal or noncriminal.
This 2 x 2 design capitalizeson what is effectively a natural experiment, and
allows for a more systematicexploration of genetic and environmental influences.As will be seenlater, this method also allows an assessmentof possible
interactions between genetic and environmental influences.
A good example of a cross-fosteringadoption study is a classic study
conducted by Mednick et al. (1984), illustrated in Table 3.1. These
researchersbased their analyseson 14,427 adoptions that took place in
Denmark between 1927 and 1947. Infants were adopted out immediately in
25.3 percent of cases,50.6 percent within one year, 12.8 percent in the
secondyear,and 11.3 percent after agetwo. Court recordswere obtained on
65,516 biological parents, adoptive parents, and adopteesin order to assess
which subjectshad convictions. When both adoptive and biological parents
were noncriminal (neither genetic nor environmental predispositions present), 13.5 percent of the adopteeshad a criminal record. This increasedto
14.7 percent When adoptive parents only were criminal, meaning that an
environmental but not genetic effect was operating. When only the biological parents were criminal, the conviction rate in the adoptees increasedto
20.0 percent. When both adoptive and biological parents were criminal
(both genetic and environmental predispositions present), the conviction
rate increasedto 24.5 percent.The effect of an adopted child having a criminal biological parent was associatedwith a statistically significant increasein
the likelihood of the adoptee becoming criminal.
While this is but one example, a review of fifteen other adoption studies conducted in Denmark, Sweden,and the United Statesshows that all but
one find a genetic basisto criminal behavior (Raine 1993). Importandy, evidence for this genetic predisposition has been found by severalindependent
researchgroups in severaldifferent counuies. These data, therefore, provide
evidencethat the basic finding is robust. Interestingly, the three studies that
The Biological
Basis
of
47
Crime
Table 3.1
ResuJtsof Cross-Fostering Analyses (Percentagesrefer to
the proportion of adoptees who had court convictions.)
ARE
PARENTS
BIOLOGICAL
CRIMINAL?
48
Adrian
Raine
Further analysesindicated that the occupational status of both biological and adoptive parents were the main postnatal variables involved in this
nonadditive interaction. Cloninger and Gottesman (1987) later analyzed
data for femaleswith larger sample sizes.As would be expected,these crime
rates in female adopteesare much lower than for males, but the sameinteractive pattern is present: Crime rates in adoptees are greatest when both
heritable and environmental influences are present, with this interaction
accounting for twice as much crime as is produced by genetic and environmental influences taken alone.
Evidence for gene x environment interaction is also provided by
Cadoret et al. (1983), who presented data from three adoption studies.
When both genetic and environmental factors are present, they account for
a greater number of antisocial behaviors than either of these two factors
acting alone. Crowe (1974) also found some evidencefor a gene x environment interaction in his analysis of adopted-away offspring of female
prisoners, though this trend was only marginally significant (p < .10).
Cadoret et al. (1995) in an adoption study of 95 male and 102 female
adoptees whose parents had either antisocial personality and/or alcohol
abuse showed that parental antisocial personality predicted increased
aggressionand conduct disorders in the offspring-illustrating evidence for
genetic processes.But in addition, adverseadoptive home environment was
found to interact with adult antisocial personality in predicting increased
aggressionin the offspring, that is, a gene x environment interaction effect.
A related but different concept is that of gene-environment correlations. An interesting example of this is a study by Ge et al. (1996), who
showed that the adopted-awayoffspring of biological parents who had antisocial personality I substanceabusewere more likely to show antisocial and
hostile behaviors in childhood. This helps establish genetic transmission of
childhood antisocial behavior. In addition, an association was found
between antisocial behavior in the biological parent and the parenting
behaviors of the adoptive parents. This can be explained by a transmission
pathway in which the biological parent contributes a genetic predisposition
toward antisocial behavior in the offspring. The antisocial offspring then in
turn elicit negative parenting behaviors in the adoptive parents. This study
provides direct evidence of an "evocative" gene-environment correlation,
and suggeststhat the association between negative parenting in the adoptive parent and antisocial behavior in the child is mediated by genetic
processes.One of the goals of future behavior genetic studies should be to
further examine the interplay between genesand environment in this fashion. More generally,there are likely to be future exciting developmentswith
The
Bi%gica/
Basis
of
Crime
49
Genetic x Environmental
Interaction
in Male Adoptees
(Cloninger et al. 1982)
CRIME
IN
ADOPTEES
Postnatal
None
Both
Congenital
Level of Predisposition
in Female
Genetic x Environmental
Interaction
Adoptees
(Cloninger
and Gottesman,
1982)
CRIME
IN
ADOPTEES
Postnatal
None
Congenital
Both
Level of Predisposition
Figure 3.1
Results of a Cross-Fostering
Analysis
Indicating
50
Adrian
Raine
respect to identifying the specific geneswhich give rise to the risk factors
that shapecriminal behavior (seesection on neurogenetics).
PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY
Since the 1940s an extensive body of researchhas been built up on the
psychophysiological basis of antisocial, delinquent, criminal, and psychopathic behavior. For example, there have been at least 150 studies on
electrodermal (sweat.rate) and cardiovascular (heart rate) activity in such
populations, and in electroencephalographic (EEG) research alone there
have been hundreds of studies on delinquency and crime (Gale 1975).
This body of researchhas received little attention in the broader field of
criminology, and is rarely referred to in textbooks on crime. One purpose
of this chapter is to bring this body of knowledge to the attention of this
more general audience.
Definitions of psychophysiology vary, but one useful perspective outlined by Dawson (1990) is that it is "concerned with understanding the
relationships berween psychological states and processeson the one hand
and physiological measureson the other hand" (p. 243). Psychophysiology
is uniquely placed to provide important insights into criminal behavior
becauseit rests at the interface berween clinical science, cognitive science,
and neuroscience (Dawson 1990). Thus, it is sometimes easier to see the
relevance of this research for crime relative to biochemistry research
becauseconcepts in psychophysiology are more easily linked to broader
concepts such as learning, emotion, arousal, and cognition.
There are many psychophysiological correlates of antisocial, criminal,
and psychopathic behavior.2 The focus here will lie with one particular
psychophysiological construct, low arousal, because-as will become
clear-it is the strongest psychophysiological finding in the field of antisocial and criminal behavior.
EEG Underarousal
One influential psychophysiological theory of antisocial behavior is that
antisocial individuals are chronically underaroused.Traditional psychophysiological measuresof arousal include heart rate, skin conductance activity,
and electroencephalogram(EEG) measured during a "resting" state. Low
heart rate and skin conductance activity, and more excessiveslow-waveEEG
(delta activity with a frequency of 1-4 cyclesper second [cps], 4-7 cps theta
activity, and 8-10 cps slow alpha) indicate underarousal, that is, less than
The Biological
Basis
of
Crime
51
Cardiovascular
Underarousal
52
Adrian
Raine
The
Biological
Basis
of
Crime
53
Studies
of
Underarousal
One of the major difficulties in trying to draw conclusions on the psychophysiological basisof criminal behavior is that most studies conducted
to date have been nonprospective and have utilized institutionalized populations. In addition, most studies report results from only one of the three
most commonly measured psychophysiological responsesystems (electrodermal, cardiovascular,and cortical). Prospectivelongitudinal research-that
is; researchthat follows people forward through their lives-a1lows for much
more powerful statements to be made about predispositions for criminal
behaviorand to elucidate causeand effect relationships;but becauseprospective researchis more difficult to execute,there are few such studies.
Regarding heart rate levels, five prospective studies of heart rate alone
haveconfirmed that low heart rate is predictive of later antisocial behavior,
while five additional prospective studies also show significant effects for
c)ectrodermal and electrocortical arousal. Wadsworth (1976) found that
tower resting heart rate in unselectedeleven-year-old schoolboys predicted
de)inquency measured from ages eight to twenty-one. The very lowest
54
Adrian
Raine
Interpretations
Fearlessness
of
and
Low Arousal:
Stimulation-Seeking
Theories
Why should low arousal and low heart rate predispose to antisocial and
criminal behavior? There are two main theoretical interpretations.
Fearlessnesstheory indicates that low levels of arousal are markers of low
levels of fear (Raine 1993; Raine 1997). For example, particularly fearless
individuals such as bomb disposal experts who have been decorated for
their bravery have particularly low HRU and reactivity (Cox et al. 1983;
O'Connor, Hallam, and Rachman 1985), as do British paratroopers decorated in the Falklands War (McMillan and Rachman 1987). A fearlessness
The
Bi o logi
cat
B asis
of
Grim
55
Crime
Protective
Development
Until recently, there had been no researchon biological factors that protect
against crime development, but that is changing. We are discovering that
higher autonomic activity during adolescencemay act as a protective factor
against crime development. Raine, Venables, and Williams (1995, 1996)
rcport on a fourteen-year prospective study in which measuresof arousal,
orienting (sweat rate and heart rate responsesto tone stimuli), and classical
conditioning (learning through association) were taken in 101 unselected
fifteen-year-old males. Of these, seventeen adolescent antisocials who
desistedfrom adult crime (Desistors)were matched on adolescentantisocial
Adrian
56
Raine
IMAGING
Advances in brain imaging techniques in the past fifteen years have provided the opportunity to gain dramatic new insights into the brain
mechanisms that may be dysfunctional in violent, psychopathic offenders.
In the past, the idea of peering into the mind of a murderer to gain insights
into his or her acts was the province of pulp fiction or space-agemovies.
Yet now we can literally look at, and into, the brains of murderers using
functional and structural imaging techniques that are currently revolutionizing our understanding of the causesof clinical disorders.
The Biological
Basis
of
Crime
57
i
~
!':
~
f!
r:,
}
f
I;
!\:
""
C;t
Dysfunction
in Murderers
In the first published brain imaging study of murderers (Raine, Buchsbaum, Stanleyet al. 1994), we scannedthe brains of twenty-two murderers
pleading not guilty by reason of insanity (or otherwise found incompetent
t~ ,stand trial) and compared them to the brains of twenty-two normal
controls who were matched with the murderers on sex and age.The technique we used was positron emission tomography (PET), which allowed
usto measurethe metabolic activity of many different regions of the brain
'ihcluding the prefrontal cortex, the frontalmost part of the brain. We had
subjectsperform a task that required them to maintain focusedattention and
bevigilant for a continuous period of time, and it is the prefrontal region of
the brain that in part subservesthis vigilance function.
,The key finding was that the murderers showed significantly poorer
,functioning of the prefrontal conex, that part of the brain lying above the
eyesand behind the forehead. It is thought that poorer functioning of the
,prefrontal cortex predisposespeople to violence for a number of reasons.
,Reducedprefrontal functioning can result in a loss of the ability of this
part of the brain to control deeper and more primitive subconical structures, such as the amygdala, which are thought to give rise to aggressive
feelings. Prefrontal damage also encourages risk-taking, irresponsibility,
rule breaking, emotional and aggressive outbursts, and argumentative
Adrian
58
Raine
behavior that can also predispose to violent criminal acts. Loss of selfcontrol, immaturity, lack of tact, inability to modify and inhibit behavior
appropriately, and poor social judgment could predispose to violence as
well. This loss of intellectual flexibility and problem-solving skills, and
reduced ability to use information provided by verbal cues can impair
social skills essential for formulating nonaggressivesolutions to fractious
encounters. Poor reasoning ability and divergent thinking that results from
prefrontal damage can lead to school failure, unemployment, and economic deprivation, thereby predisposing to a criminal and violent way of
life. A further study showed that it was especially the impulsive, emotionally undercontrolled murdererswho were especiallylikely to show prefrontal
deficits (Raine, Meloy et al. 1998). Nevertheless, it should be recognized
that, while there is an association betWeenpoor prefrontal function and
impulsive violence, this brain dysfunction may be essentially a predisposition only, requiring other environmental, psychological, and social factors
to enhance or diminish this tendency.
Corpus
Callosum,
and the Subcortex
Left
Angular
Gyrus,
j
~
i~
J
The
Biological
Basis
of
Crime
60
Adrian
Raine
Matter
Disorder
Ranging from single casestudies (Damasio et al. 1994) to seriesof neurological patients (Damasio, Tranel, and Damasio 1990; Stuss and Benson
1986), those Who have suffered demonstrable damage to both gray and
white matter within the prefrontal region of the brain proceed to acquire an
antisocial, psychopathic-Iike personality. These patients alSo ShoW auronomic arousaland attention deficits to socially meaningful events (Damasio
1994, Damasio, Tranel, and Damasio 1990), a finding consistent with the
role played by the prefrontal cortex in modulating emotion, arousal, and
attention (Srussand Benson 1986; Davidson 1993; Raine, Reynolds, and
Sheard 1991).
We recently conducted a structural magnetic resonance imaging
(MRI) study on volunteers from the community with Antisocial Personality Disorder and made volumetric assessments
of prefrontal gray and white
matter (Raine et al. 2000). Skin conductance and heart rate activity during
a social stressor was also assessedin addition to psyChOSOCial
and demographic risk factors for violence. Subjects were drawn from temporary
employment agencies in Los Angeles and consisted of rwenty-one males
with Antisocial Personality Disorder, a normal control group of thirry-four
males, and a psychiatric control group of rwenty-seven males with substance dependence. Antisocials had significantly lower prefr9ntal gray
volumes than both Controls and SubstanceDependents. In contrast, groups
did not differ on white prefrontal volume, indicating specificity of the deficit
to gray matter (neurons). Furthermore, Antisocials also showed reduced
autonomic reactivity during the social stressorcompared to both Controls
and SubstanceDependents.
When prefrontal gray matter was expressedas a function of whole
brain volume, groups were again found to differ significantly, so the results
are not due to general difference in brain size but a specific difference in
The
Biological
Basis
of
Crime
61
Deficits
a History
in Violent
Offenders
of Childhood
Abuse
While the relationship between physical child abuse and violence is well
established (Lewis et al. 1988; Tarter et al. 1984; Widom 1997), there
appearsto be little or no research,biological or social, on factors that differentiate abused victims who go on to perpetrate violence from those who
refrain from adult violence. We recently conducted a study (Raine, Park, et
al. In press) in which we used functional magnetic resonance imaging
(tMRI) to addresstwo important gapsin our knowledge of brain functioning and violence: (1) What are the brain correlates of adults in the
community who have suffered severephysical abuseearly in life and who go
on to perpetrate seriousviolence in adulthood? (2) What characterizesthose
who experienceseverephysical abusebut who refrain from seriousviolence?
We recruited four groups of participants from the community: (1)
nonviolent controls who had not suffered abuse; (2) participants with
severephysical child abuse only (that is, had suffered severephysical or
sexual abuse in the first eleven years, but were not violent); (3) ones with
serious violence only (violence that causedeither bodily injury or trauma,
or that were life-threatening acts); and (4) severelyabused, seriously violent offenders. All underwent tMRI while performing a visual/verbal
working memory task. Functional magnetic resonance imaging measures
blood flow within brain tissue, and thus allows one to assessthe functional
properties of the brain. In this sense,it is like PET, but unlike PET there
is no exposure to radioactivity. Furthermore, it detects activity in brain
regions that are as small as 1 millimeter. The task involved subjects hold-
62
Adrian
Raine
BIOLOGICAL
BIRTH
COMPLICATIONS,
PHYSICAL
AND
Birth
ANOMALIES,
PROCESSES:
MINOR
NUTRITION,
NEUROCHEMISTRY
Complications
Severalstudies have shown that babies who suffer birth complications are
more likely to develop conduct disorder, delinquency, and impulsive crime
and violence in adulthood (seeRaine 1993 for a detailed review). Birth complications such as anoxia (getting too little oxygen), forceps delivery, and
preeclampsia(hypenension leading to anoxia) are thought to contribute to
brain damage, and this damage in turn may predispose to antisocial and
criminal behavior. On the other hand, birth complications may not by
themselvespredispose to crime, but may require the presenceof negative
environmental circumstance to trigger later adult crime and violence.
One example of this "biosocial interaction" is a study of birth compli"
cations and maternal rejection in all 4,269 live male births that took place
in one hospital in Copenhagen, Denmark (Raine, Brennan, and Mednick
1994). Birth complications were assessedby obstetricians and midwives.
When the baby was one year old, the mother was interviewed by a social
worker and the degree of maternal rejection was assessedby three measures: mother did not want the pregnancy, mother made an attempt to
abort the fetus, and the baby was institutionalized for at least four months
in the first year of life. Babies were then followed for eighteen years, when
The
Bi / ogica/
B as is
of
Grim
63
Minor
Physical
Anomalies
64
Adrian
Raine
Halverson and Victor (1976) also found higher levels ofMPAs in elementary male schoolchildren with problem school behavior. MPAs have even
been linked to peer aggressionas earlyas age three (Waldrop et al. 1978).
Although MPAs have generally characterized behavior disorders in children drawn from the normal population (see Pomeroy, Sprafkin, and
Gadow 1988 for a review), at least one study failed to observe a link
betWeenMPAs and conduct disorder within a mixed group of emotionally
disturbed children (Pomeroy, Sprafkin, and Gadow 1988).
Mednick and Kandel (1988) assessed
MPAs in a sample of 129 tWelveyear-old boys examined by an experiencedpediatrician. MPAs were found
to be related to violent offending as assessednine years later when the
subjects were age tWenty-one, though not to property offenses without
violence. However, when subjects were divided into those from unstable,
nonintact homes versusthose from stablehomes, a biosocial interaction was
observed. MPAs only predicted violence in those individuals raised in
unstable home environments. A similar interactive relationship was also
observedfor birth weight and family stability (Kandel and Mednick, 1991) .
These findings are quite similar to those on birth complications reported
above; in both studies the presence of a negative psychosocial factor is
required to "trigger" the biological risk factor, and in both casesthe effects
are specific to violent offending. In a stUdy confirming specificity to violence, Arseneault et al. (2000) found that MPAs predicted to violent
delinquency during adolescencein 170 teenagers,but not to nonviolent
delinquency. Furthermore, effects were independent of family adversity.
Nutrition
Although deficiency in nutrition itself has been rarely stUdied in relation
to childhood aggression,several stUdieshave demonstrated.the effects of
related processesincluding food additives, hypoglycemia, and more recently
cholesterol on human behavior (Rutter, Giller, and Hagell 1998; Raine
1993; Fishbein and Pease1994). In addition, somestudies have shown associations between overaggressive
behavior and vitamin and mineral deficiency
(Breakey 1997; Werbach 1995). Furthermore, one stUdy (Rosen 1996)
claimed that nearly a third of a population of juvenile delinquents (mostly
males) showed evidence of iron deficiency. Nevertheless, these findings
remain both conflicting and controversial (Rutter, Giller, and Hagell1998).
One intriguing study illustrates the potentially causal role of malnutrition as early as pregnancy in predisposing to antisocial behavior. Toward
the end ofWorld War II when Germany was withdrawing from Holland,
The
Biological
Basis
of
Crime
65
they placed a food blockade on the country that led to major food shortagesand near starvation in the cities and towns for severalmonths. Women
who were pregnant at this time were exposedto severemalnutrition at different stages of pregnancy. The male offspring of these women were
followed up into adulthood to ascertain rates of Antisocial Personality
Disorder and were compared to controls who were not exposedto malnutrition. Pregnant women starved during the blockade had 2.5 times the
rates of Antisocial Personality Disorder in their adult offspring compared
to controls (Neugebauer,Hoek, and Susser1999).
Initial evidencealso shows relationships between both protein and zinc
deficiency and aggressionin animals (Tikal, Benesova,and Frankova 1976;
Halas, Reynolds, and Sanstead,1977). Recent studies of humans support
theseanimal findings. Protein and zinc deficiency may lead to aggressionby
negatively impacting brain functioning. There is extensiveexperimental evidence in animals that the offspring of rats fed a diet containing marginal
levels of either zinc or protein throughout pregnancy and lactation showed
impaired brain development (Oteiza et al. 1990; Bennis-Taleb et al. 1999).
In humans, zinc deficiency in pregnancyhas been linked to impaired DNA,
RNA, and protein synthesisduring brain development, and congenital brain
abnormalities (Pfeiffer and Braverman 1982). Similarly, protein provides
essentialamino acids for the rapid growth of fetal tissue.PET studiesof violent offenders have revealed deficits to the prefrontal cortex and corpus
callosum (Raine, Buchsbaum, and La Casse1997; Volkow et al. 1995), and
it is of interest that the offspring of rats fed a low-protein diet during pregnancy show a specific impairment to the corpus callosum (Wainwright and
Stefanescu 1983) and reduction in DNA concentration in the forebrain
(Bennis-Taleb et al. 1999). The amygdala,which also shows abnormal functioning in PET imaging of violent offenders (Raine, Buchsbaum, and La
Casse 1997; Raine, Meloy, et al. 1998; Davidson, Pumam, and Larson
2000), is densely innervated by zinc-containing neurons (Christensen and
Frederickson 1998), and males with a history of assaultivebehavior were
found to have lower zinc relative to copper ratios in their blood compared to
nonassaultivecontrols (Walsh et al. 1997). Consequently, protein and zinc
deficiency may contribute to the brain impairments shown in violent
offenders which in turn are thought to predisposeto violence.
Environmental
Pollutants
and
Neurotoxicity
66
Adrian
Raine
impairment which in turn predisposesto antisocial and aggressivebehavior. One of the best studies to date is that of Needleman et al. ( 1996) who
assessedlead levels in the bones of 301 eleven-year-old schoolboys. Boys
with higher lead levels were found to have significantly higher teacher
ratings of delinquent and aggressivebehavior, higher parent ratings of
delinquent and aggressivebehavior, and higher self-report delinquency
scores.These findings do not occur in isolation: Similar links between lead
levels and antisocial, delinquent behavior and aggressionhave been found
in at least six other studies in severaldifferent countries (seeNeedleman et
al. 1996 for a review). Furthermore, experimental exposure to lead during
development increases aggressivebehavior in hamsters (Delville 1999),
thus suggestinga causal link.
Less strong to date, but neverthelessprovocative, are findings with
respect to manganese.At high levels, manganesehas toxic effects on the
brain and can damage the brain so much that it can even lead to
Parkinson-like symptoms. Furthermore, it reduceslevels of serotonin and
dopamine, neurotransmitters that playa key role in brain communication
(see later "Neurotransmitters" section). One study by Gottschalk et al.
(1991) found that three different samplesof violent criminals compared to
controls had higher levels of manganesein their hair. On the other hand,
not all studies have found this association (Schauss1981).It may be that
low levels of calcium intake interact with high manganesein predisposing
to violent behavior becauseanimal researchindicates that the neurotoxic
effect of manganeseis particularly strong when the animal is deficient in
calcium.
Hormones
Testosterone. Excellent reviews and discussions of the potential role
played by testosterone in both animals and man can be found in Olweus
(1987), Brain (1990), Archer (1991), and Susman and Ponirakis (1997).
Animal researchsuggeststhat the steroid hormone testosterone plays an
important role in the genesisand maintenance of some forms of aggressive
behavior in rodents, and early exposure to testosterone has been found to
increaseaggressionin a wide range of animal species(Brain 1990).
A key question generated by this literature is whether testosterone is
involved in aggressionand violent crime in man. Studies correlating questionnaire measures of aggression in normals to testosterone levels have
generally produced weak or nonsignificant findings (Rubin 1987). Studies
of violent incarcerated inmates on the other hand have been more consis-
The
Biological
Basis
of
Crime
67
68
Adrian
Raine
The
Biological
Basis
of
Crime
69
Neurotra
nsmitters
70
Adrian
Raine
particularly lower serotonin. Humans low in social class have also been
found to have reduced serotonin, and it is possible that reductions in serotonin produced by a fall in dominance triggers impulsive aggressionas a
way of raising the individual in the dominance hierarchy. In evolutionary
terms this would be adaptive as increaseddominance givesgreater accessto
food and sex, thus increasing the individual's ability to reproduce and pass
on their genes.Yet again, poor diet may playa role. Diets low in, or otherwise blocking, the uptake of tryptophan or tyrosine (the precursors of
serotonin and norepinephrine respectively) have been found to lower the
levels of these transmitters in the brain (seeWeisman 1986) .In addition,
even when returned to a normal diet, brain serotonin levels are never fully
compensated (Timiras, Hudson, and SegalI1984). Poor nutrition possibly
occurring in individuals of lower socioeconomic status (including poor
dietary care during pregnancy) could conceivably influence neurotransmitter levels throughout life.
Neurogenetics
As outlined earlier, genetic studies are now beginning to progress from
simply demonstrating that there is indeed a genetic basis to crime. A new
generation of neurogenetic studies is now beginning to identify specific
genesthat give rise to abnormally aggressivebehavior, often via abnormal
functioning of specific neurotransmitters.
Some of the first clues have come from animal researchwhere "knockout" mice have been genetically engineered to lack individual genes that
normally give rise to specific neurotransmitters. One of the neurotransmitter systemsimplicated to date is nitric oxide. Mice that lack the gene
essential for nitric oxide in the brain have been found to become highly
aggressive,and furthermore agents which inhibit nitric oxide also lead to
very aggressivebehavior in normal mice (Demas et al. 1997).
Particularly interesting are findings, on the knockout of the gene that
codes for monoamine oxidase-A (MAOA), an enzyme that metabolizes
brain neurotransmitters including serotonin. Adult mice who are deficient
in MAOA are abnormally aggressiveto other males and are much rougher
and aggressivein their mating behavior with females (Caseset al. 1995;
Nelson et al. 1995). These mice also showed very high levels of serotonin.
What makesanimal MAOA findings such as these more provocative is that
they have also been found in humans. Brunner et al. (1993) have shown
that a family in the Netherlands with a history of highly aggressivebehav-'
ior have a mutation in the MAOA gene.They also showed impaired IQs,
and like the MAOA mice, had very high levels of serotonin.
The
Biological
Basis
of
Crime
71
Animal findings such as these which are also found in humans are
particularly interesting. Nevertheless, such genetic mutations affecting
the MAOA gene are rare in humans and therefore it will be difficult to
test whether Brunner's findings will be replicated in other samples.
Furthermore, deletion of this produces high, not low, serotonin levels, a
finding not consistent with the finding of lower serotonin in the cerebrospinal fluid of impulsive violent offenders. Nevertheless, it is not
unlikely that the field of neurogenetics will produce multiple breakthroughs in the next few years on the molecular genetic basis of crime and
violence.
POLICY
IMPLICATIONS
One of the biggest and widely held myths in criminology researchis that
biology is destiny. Instead, the reality is that the biological basesof crime
and violence are amenableto change through benign interventions. In the
past fifty years, intervention programs have not been assuccessfulin reducing crime and violence as had been hoped, and it is possible that part of
their failure has been due to the fact that they have systematically ignored
the biological component of the biosocial equation.
Brain damage and poor brain functioning have been shown to predispose to violence, and one possible source of this brain damage could be
birth complications (Raine, Brennan, and Mednick 1997). The implication is that providing better pre- and postbirth health care to poor mothers
may help reduce birth complications and thus reduce violence.
Alternatively, rather than attempting to reduce birth complications, interventions could focus on the psychosocialhalf of the biosocial equation and
attempt to reduce early maternal rejection which intensifies the effect of
birth complications (Raine, Brennan, and Mednick 1994; Raine, Brennan,
and Mednick 1997). Consideration might be given to multiple efforts
acrosstime to reduce maternal rejection by, for example, making parenting skills classescompulsory in high school to the next generation of
mothers with unwanted pregnancies;providing more pre-birth visits from
nurses to monitor both the pregnancy and the parent's attitude to the
unborn baby; and providing home visits from pediatricians specifically to
mothers who suffered birth complications to monitor the mother-infant
bonding process, assessthe physical and cognitive development of the
infant, and provide appropriate remediation of cognitive and physical
deficits which are known to follow from perinatal complications (Liu and
Raine 1999; Raine and Liu 1998).
72
Adrian
Raine
The
Biological
Basis
of
Crime
73
ual, then the question becomeswhether or not that person should be held
fully responsible for the crimes. Of coursewe have to protect society, and
unless we can treat this brain dysfunction, we may need to keep violent
offenders in secureconditions for the rest of their lives; but do they deserve
to be executed given the early constraints on their free will? It could be
argued that if an individual possessesrisk factors that make him disproportionately more likely to commit violence, then he has to take responsibility for these predispositions. Just like an alcoholic who knows he suffers
from the diseaseof alcoholism, the person at risk for violence needs to recognize his risk factors and take preventive steps to ensure that he does not
harm others. These persons have risk factors, but they still have responsibility and they have &ee will.
This makes good, practical sense,but responsibility and self-reflection
are not disembodied, ethereal processesbut are rooted firmly in the brain.
Patients who have damage to the ventromedial (lower) prefrontal cortex
are known to become irresponsible, lack self-discipline, and fail to reflect
on the consequencesof their actions. The ability to take responsibility for
one'sactions is damagedin violent offenders. It can be argued that they are
no longer able to reflect on their behavior as others do and take responsibility for their predispositions. It is not just that the brain mechanisms
subserving responsibility and internal soul-searching are damaged in the
violent offender and prevent him from taking action to rectify the causes
of his violence.
Brain scan information is increasingly being used in capital cases,most
frequently in the punishment stageof a trial as a mitigating circumstance
against the death penalty, but it has also been used successfullyin the guilt
phase of a trial (D'Agincourt 1993). Nevertheless, use of brain imaging
data in law courts is hotly debated, with some arguing against their use. Of
course, brain scansare not diagnostic in that they do not perfectly predict
who is violent and who is not. On the other hand, brain imaging data such
as PET and fMRI constitute more direct indexes of brain function than
EEG and neuropsychological test data, which have frequently been used in
courts. Still, others worry that brain imaging data will be inappropriately
usedto "excuse"violent crimes.
Yet perhaps brain imaging researchon violence is most troubling to
somebecauseit challengesthe way we conceptualizecrime. It questions our
treatment of murderers in just the same way that we now look back 200
yearsand question the way in which the mentally ill were kept in shackles
and chains, treated little better than animals. The history of civilization has
shown that as time progresses,society becomeswiser and more humane.
74
Adrian
Raine