Nigel C. Hunt Memory, War and Trauma PDF
Nigel C. Hunt Memory, War and Trauma PDF
Nigel C. Hunt Memory, War and Trauma PDF
Nigel C. Hunt
CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS
Cambridge, New York, Melbourne, Madrid, Cape Town, Singapore,
So Paulo, Delhi, Dubai, Tokyo
Published in the United States of America by Cambridge University Press, New York
www.cambridge.org
Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9780521887847
Nigel C. Hunt
Preface page ix
References 206
Index 222
vii
Preface
I first became interested in the different ways people interpret war and
the memory of war when I was a child, when I saw how my parents
looked back on the Second World War with very different views. My
father, who, in the RAF, was a participant throughout, tended to be
neutral, not indifferent, but saw those 6 years with a mixture of positive,
negative and inevitably fading memories; my mother, who was a child
and adolescent, looked back with anger, seeing the war as a period which
disrupted and destroyed her childhood; so I knew from an early age that
people thought about the same events very differently.
This book is a culmination of my thinking over the last few years: from
a recognition that much of what I was taught about memory as an
undergraduate student was simplistic, focusing largely on a narrow
individualistic view of memory as some kind of inputstorageoutput
device which lacked explanatory value in terms of the way memory really
works; to a growing recognition that in order to understand people and
their thought processes psychologists such as myself should draw on the
knowledge and understanding from other disciplines, such as history
and sociology, in order to develop a fuller understanding of the ways
in which people think. Furthermore, psychologists can learn as much,
or more, from reading a good novel as from reading the scientific
literature. A narrow disciplinary focus does not lead to good science,
just restricted understanding. In the same way, modularisation has had
many negative effects in higher education by artificially separating our
subjects into discrete areas that are perceived as having little overlap; the
modularisation of disciplines restricts the development of knowledge
and understanding about whatever aspect of the world one is interested in.
The title of the book gives away the foci. If we are to understand
memory we must explore the influence of culture and society, and how
memories are affected by the stories we tell ourselves and others the
narratives. At this level there is also a complex association between
memory and history, and the ways in which memory becomes history
over generations. War and trauma are unfortunately closely interlinked.
ix
x Preface
Many people who experience war are traumatised by its effects (though
many more are not). Most previous research into war trauma has
focused on the individual effects. In this book I attempt to integrate
material across disciplines (e.g. history) and across methods (not only
standard quantitative and qualitative methods, but also literature and a
battlefield tour) to develop a more rounded understanding of what war
does to people.
I would like to thank the people who, in one way or another, have
contributed to the making of this book. In the first place the veterans of
war. I have talked to many of these over the years veterans of wars
going back to the Second World War and sometimes back even further to
the First World War and coming forward to the wars that are still being
fought. In one sense all these people share common experiences, from
the boredom, excitement, fun and comradeship on one side, to the
terror, horror and grief on the other. In another sense they differ greatly.
People who fight in wars in different times and places are not treated the
same, and the psychological outcomes are not the same. Prior to the
twentieth century, few people took any interest in what happened to
soldiers. Those who fought in the British armed forces in the Second
World War were treated as heroes; those who fought in the German
armed forces at the same time were forgotten, as were the experiences of
many British troops during the final colonial period in the 1950s, 1960s
and 1970s. In the USA, troops returning from Vietnam were often seen
as murderers. In terms of treatment for war trauma, that has changed
dramatically throughout the twentieth century, as indicated by the front
cover of this book, which represents the men who were shot at dawn by
the British authorities in the First World War. In modern times many
would have been treated for post-traumatic stress disorder.
I would also like to thank those people who have personally helped me
through the long process of writing this book. Many of them were
probably unaware that they were doing so, because the writing of a book
is a lonely business, and getting on with real life with family and friends is
very important to me, even if it does delay the completion of the book.
I would like to thank my parents for inspiring me in the first place, and
for being there when I have needed them; the rest of my family and the
children (both now adults) Jack and Conor; my friends, including every-
one in the Yew Tree, South Wingfield, and the footballers at Lea
Green on a Monday night. I would also like to thank those who have
helped me along with my thoughts over the years, particularly Ian
Robbins and Peter Coleman. My employer, the University of Nottingham,
by enabling me to take slightly longer than usual holidays in the summer
has inadvertently granted me short sabbaticals during which I write.
Preface xi
war. Casualties are not just those who are killed or wounded, or civilians
who are caught up in the fighting or just happened to get in the way of
marching troops; they are ordinary people who cannot bear their
memories of what has happened the traumatised. We cannot accurately
estimate the number of victims of war who are psychologically damaged
by their experiences.
This book is an account of the psychosocial impact of war in its
broadest sense that of understanding memory not just as individual
memory, but also as the ways in which other people, society and culture,
and history, all affect how we remember. It considers the relationship
between memory, war and traumatic stress. Many people have psycho-
logical problems as a direct consequence of war; many have terrible
memories of these experiences that they find difficult to deal with; and
many never do learn to deal with these memories. How can you come to
terms with killing people, the loss of a child, or being raped multiple
times, or remembering that you have killed civilians, or that you have
had to permanently leave your home and your family?
On the other hand, we also know that the majority of people who go
through these experiences do not have serious long-term problems, and
that they are able to handle their memories and emotions and get on
with their lives, more or less successfully. Many may still experience
intense emotion when they think of what they have been through, but
that does not mean they are traumatised. There is ample evidence to
show that many of the psychosocial responses that we observe within a
culture are not universal, that in some historical periods more people are
likely to have problems, and in different cultures and historical periods
they have different kinds of problems. Why is this so? What is it about
memory, war and traumatic stress that make it so difficult to fully
comprehend? Psychologists have studied memory for well over a cen-
tury. We have studied the impact of war for just about as long. We have
developed good theories and effective ways of treating people trauma-
tised by war, yet still our understanding has serious limitations. It is
argued here that some of these limitations are due to focusing too much
on the individual, and not enough on the social and cultural world in
which we live.
While there is a lot of good research fascinating, detailed and useful
theories about traumatic stress, and, perhaps most importantly, thera-
pies that help people to cope with the overwhelming response our
understanding of memory and trauma still has something missing.
Memory is not objective; it is not some kind of computer-like registra-
tion, storage and retrieval system. Memory is flexible, permeable,
changeable, and critically affected by the social and cultural world
Background and purpose 3
in which people live. We live in the world as social beings; we do not and
cannot live in isolation. No matter what the Zeitgeist says that we live
in an increasingly individualistic society in the end we depend on
culture and we depend on each other. These are essential to psycho-
logical health. This is why social support consistently comes out as being
the most important factor concerning how people deal with stress and
difficulties in their lives.
The other key concept that is used throughout the book is that of
narrative. We constantly narrate our lives, creating and telling stories
about who and what we are, and why we exist. We are natural storytellers
and natural audiences (you can see the link to social support). Narrative
is an essential function. We use and manipulate our memories, con-
sciously and unconsciously, in order to present ourselves to the world
in a particular way. Our life stories are constantly changing according to
our circumstances. We do not have any choice in the matter. We are
compelled to narrate.
Low perceived social support is seen as a predictor of traumatic stress.
If a person experiences a traumatic event and they do not perceive that
they have good social support, then they are more likely to be trauma-
tised than if they perceive that they have good social support. Our
fundamental need for narrative is met by interacting with others, by
being able to narrate their problems, work them through, with someone
who will listen appropriately. Social support is used to help people
resolve their issues through discussion.
While narration is about storytelling and the construction of narratives
that may relate closely to how events actually happened, or they may
be largely fabricated, the argument is not that we fabricate our lives,
but that psychological reality is more fluid, social and malleable than
we usually think. In the context of the response to war then, this must
be taken into account when we are building our theories, when we are
trying to treat people with war-related psychological problems, and
when we are just listening to war stories.
We must include the social and narrated worlds in our psychological
theories. In order to do this effectively, psychological research is not
enough. If we are to understand the nature of war, and the impact it has
on people, then we must examine other approaches to understanding,
through, for example, literature, history and the media. This book
weaves together the story of memory, war and trauma by drawing on
these different elements to increase our understanding of the lived
experience and impact of war. Any psychologist who tells you that they
can only learn about human nature from reading a psychology journal
article or textbook, without considering the contribution of a good
4 Background and purpose
novel, play or poem, is nave. We are studying behaviour, in all its shapes
and forms, and good literature is part of that tapestry of understanding,
along with historical accounts, sociology and politics.
1
Interestingly, mainstream psychologists only started to talk about autobiographical
memory about 20 years ago. It was then thought of as a novel concept, rather than
something that people in all societies have understood the need for and purpose of for
millennia. It was the same with the concept of consciousness around the same time.
Narrative, social discourse and collective memory 5
periods in history that have been just as bad as the Holocaust for
example, the Stalinist era of the USSR, the Maoist massacres in China,
Pol Pots regime in Cambodia or the ethnic cleansing in Bosnia. This is
not about scale (though some were on a greater scale than the Holocaust);
it is about the depth of human tragedy.
Another example where the term holocaust may apply is the destruc-
tion of the native people of North America. Many of the terms
that generally applied to Nazi Germany are at least as applicable in the
nineteenth-century USA context. For instance, Lebensraum represents
the idea that the white man wanted the whole of North America for
himself, and that there was no room for the untermensch (the native
people). This was genocide (the natives of North America were destroyed
in the same way Hitler intended for the Jews) and, to use a more modern
term, ethnic cleansing. The term holocaust may be even more appro-
priate to nineteenth-century USA than to twentieth-century Germany
because the state the USA deliberately set out to cleanse a continent of
its indigenous people and replace them with Europeans. They succeeded
almost completely, while Hitler tried to remove one tribe from Europe,
and only partially succeeded. We now see that tribe thriving in Palestine
(the Holy Land, Israel take your pick of social constructs), while the
few survivors of the North American tribes live mainly in reservations,
perhaps better described as concentration camps.
It is not that the destruction of native North Americans was
conducted in the same way as the destruction of the Jews in Hitlers
Germany, nor that the intentions of the perpetrators were necessarily
the same; it is that the nature of the social discourse the ways in which
the events are interpreted plays a crucial part in peoples individual and
collective memories of those events.
If we are to understand our narratives of war or of anything else we
must understand the power of the social constructions we use when
describing our behaviours and our thoughts and feelings. The examples
above are not wrong; it is just that society at least Western society
accepts a particular social construct. If the allies, including the USA, had
lost the war against Germany in 1945, then the situation the social
construct would be very different. The history books, which contain
the social constructs of a society, would contain very different stories.
There is a relationship between individual narrative and social dis-
course, with one impacting on the other, but there are other key variables
that must be included in the equation: the first is collective memory.
Collective memory is information about society that is accumulated over
the years and develops into a kind of social fund, and is drawn upon
in the development of social discourses and individual narratives.
6 Background and purpose
War trauma
An agreed definition of the central concept of war trauma is difficult to
obtain, as there is disagreement over the terms that should be used when
discussing the psychological effects of traumatic situations such as war.
War trauma 7
The distinction between stress and trauma is critical; while most authors
would agree there is a distinction, this is not always made clear. The
term stress was used by Cannon, an early pioneer in the area, to
describe a stimulus physical or emotional that disturbs a persons
internal homeostasis or balance, and that may be pathological if it
reaches a critical level (Cannon, 1929). Selye (1956) defined stress as
changes within a biological system that occur as a response to stressors,
environmental stimuli that evoke such internal changes. Mason (1975),
building on the work of Selye, argued that whether or not a stress
response occurs depends on a range of individual variables such as
appraisal, coping style and critically for our discussion the social
world. Levine and Ursin (1991) define stress as a situation where the
body anticipates or determines that there is some threat to the organism,
and organises the bodys defences against that threat in order to restore
homeostasis. The stress response is a normal and predictable response to
environmental threats. It only becomes a problem when the threat is
sufficiently prolonged or intense that it overwhelms the bodys resources.
Traumatic stress is fundamentally different to ordinary stress, in the
sense that there is a fundamental rift or breakdown of psychological
functioning (memory, behaviour, emotion) which occurs as a result of
an unbearably intense experience that is life threatening to the self or
others. It is usually a time-limited experience (even within the context of
war, traumatic experiences usually occur relatively rarely) of such inten-
sity that the resources of the person are overwhelmed. There are a set of
symptoms associated with these changes, including intrusive recollec-
tions, avoidance and emotional numbing, and hyperarousal. The over-
whelming nature of the event is such that it leads to important and often
permanent changes in the physiology and mental state of the individual.
A traumatic memory is formed, a memory that is at once cognitive,
emotional and possibly behavioural. The traumatic memory does not
exist in normal stress. The traumatic memory relates to the persons
initial unconscious response to the traumatic event. As the person sur-
vived the event, the memory is indelibly fixed within the mind. This
is adaptive. The person experienced a life-threatening situation and
survived, and so if the same traumatic situation arose in the future, they
should behave in the same manner again, hence increasing their chances
of survival. So in this way, the traumatic response can be an evolutionary
useful process. Unfortunately, owing to the mechanisms involved (which
will be explained later), that response contains memorised bodily and
psychological responses that are potentially damaging to the psyche.
This traumatic response can recur in different ways. For some people,
the memories of the event are overwhelming and continuous, and they
8 Background and purpose
Civilians
The concern in this book is not just with traumatised soldiers, but also
with all those, including civilians, who are caught up in the trauma of
war. Throughout the twentieth century, an ever-increasing proportion
of the victims of war were civilians. The proportion of civilians affected
by war in the twentieth century rose from around 10% of casualties
in World War One, to 50% of casualties in World War Two, to 90% in
Bosnia. Of course, this is not a linear increase, and it is not that civilians
12 Background and purpose
did not in the past suffer greatly because of war. In the English Civil War,
from 1642 to1648, it has been estimated that around 3% of the popula-
tion died, from soldiers in battle to civilians from famine and disease.
Civilians experience war differently to soldiers. With significant
exceptions, they do not have guns; they are not trained to kill. They
are often women, children or older people, and there are many psycho-
logical issues specific to these groups. Children who experience war
trauma have their beliefs shattered before those beliefs have even been
fully formed. That can affect the rest of their lives. The same goes for
women who are raped by soldiers. Civilians are generally powerless to
protect themselves. They have not been trained in, and prepared for,
the brutalities of war.
Finally
This opening chapter has looked at the background to the book, and has
begun to define what we mean by war trauma. We will return to this
theme in later chapters. After one further introductory chapter exploring
the historical background to our understanding of trauma (Chapter 3),
the book is split into three main sections.
The first section explores our current perspectives on trauma and
memory. We cannot function as scientists without clear methods, so
Chapter 3 focuses on methods and ethics. Trauma psychologists use
methods that are similar to other psychologists experiments, inter-
views, surveys and so on but they are adapted to the practice of applied
trauma research. That is, the methods may not always be applied in a
pure manner; compromises are made according to the sometimes diffi-
cult circumstances in which researchers find themselves. The ethical
issues in trauma research are potentially serious, as there is the potential
to cause distress to participants, for example by making them relive their
experiences, and harm to researchers, particularly if they collect data in a
war zone, but also just through interviewing traumatised people, some of
whom can be angry and violent. Chapter 4 focuses on PTSD, describing
the construct, and its validity and limitations with regard to war trauma.
For a full description of the symptoms associated with war trauma, we
need to look at comorbid disorders such as depression and substance
abuse. Chapter 5 focuses on our broader understanding of war
trauma, drawing on research from a number of fields to describe current
theory. It also looks at ideas about how we manage to cope with these
memories. Chapter 6 takes a different angle, looking at research on post-
traumatic growth, a relatively recent area within psychology which
focuses on how many people go through traumatic experiences and
Finally 13
1
Assuming he existed as a single person, which appears unlikely.
14
Historical perspective 15
Light Brigade, and how a round shot had taken off the arm of one of his
comrades, leaving his own arm splattered with blood. The descriptions
are quite detached, and Reid describes how this indicates a numbing of
responsiveness, linking it to the PTSD criteria of diminished interest,
restricted range of affect and feelings of estrangement. Bombardment
often plays a key role. Major Henry Clifford described how shelling
made a great impression on soldiers nerves, and that it was a trying
thing to sit idly by while being bombarded. Again, note the use of
language. Major Clifford also noticed how bombardments could affect
sleep patterns, how his tent-mate developed restlessness under night
bombardment and shooting. The Russian view of the Crimean War
was well described by Tolstoy in his recollections of the time he fought
against the British. As a novelist, Tolstoy spent time describing the
impact of a shell landing close to an officer, and how the soldiers spirit
went from life through death to utter oblivion (Reid, 2000). Another
example provided by Reid is that of Colin Campbell, who developed
symptoms of PTSD during the Crimean War. His letters showed that he
became destroyed emotionally as he was experiencing traumatic events
on a daily basis. He became desensitised to the death around him, and
he failed to experience a great deal of feeling even when he heard about
his sisters death. This is a classic case of emotional numbing. Campbell
described the different reactions of the soldiers, highlighting that during
one of the attacks, a good many of the men did not behave well; this
looks euphemistic to modern eyes. Interestingly, Campbell explored
substance abuse, distinguishing between those who drank steadily all
night and performed their duty during the day and those who got very
drunk whenever they could and spent a lot of time in the guardhouse.
The latter tended to survive longer than the former; though perhaps that
was because they spent a lot of time in the guardhouse.
During the US Civil War in the 1860s, soldiers experienced psycho-
logical symptoms in and after battle. Oliver Wendell Holmes described
suffering the most intense anxiety during the early days. The psycho-
logical problems experienced by the common soldier were described
some years later by Da Costa (1871), who coined the term soldiers
heart, and explained the symptoms as resulting from soldiers having a
weak heart, that is they had a physiological problem underlying the
psychological symptoms. The idea that there must be a physiological
explanation for traumatic symptoms was usual in the nineteenth cen-
tury. Disorders were thought to be physiological, with physical causes
(and physical cures). It was not until the 1880s that people such as
Charcot, Freud, Breuer and Janet would open up the debate regarding
the psychological genesis of mental disorders. These authors produced
18 Historical perspective
seminal works based on their clinical experiences. During the last years
of the nineteenth century and into the early years of the twentieth, they
fundamentally changed our understanding of traumatic stress, moving
from the physiological origin to the psychological one, recognising that
psychological and physical symptoms could arise from psychological
causes.
It was not until World War One that the psychological genesis of
trauma was at least partially recognised by the military authorities in
the West, particularly through the work of people such as Rivers, who
treated Wilfred Owen and Siegfried Sassoon. It was recognised much
later than this in countries such as Germany and Russia, both of which
regularly shot soldiers who displayed symptoms of war trauma up until
the end of World War Two. In the UK we stopped shooting them after
World War One.
There was, of course, a long period where it was not clear whether
there were physiological or psychological causes of war trauma. During
the Russo-Japanese war of 19045, psychiatrists served near the front.
The Russians had learned from their experiences in the Crimean War,
where they had lost significant numbers of men to psychiatric problems.
The Russians had a psychiatric hospital at Harbin where they treated
2,000 psychiatric casualties. Unfortunately the Russians wrongly attrib-
uted psychological symptoms to physiological injury a problem to be
repeated in the First World War. Nevertheless, this was the first war in
which psychological reactions to the heat of battle were recognised
(Baker, 1975). The symptoms formed the basis for the classification
systems used in the First World War.
Hesnard (1914) reported psychological effects resulting from the
sinking of the French ships Liberte and lena at Toulon in 1911 and
1907. These conformed to PTSD-type symptomatology: intrusive
images, nightmares, anxiety, avoidance, somnambulism, hypervigilance,
lack of concentration, and amnesia. It was becoming clear by this stage
that there were a number of symptoms that tended to co-occur after a
traumatic event.
Throughout the historical periods discussed there was never a wide
recognition that men could experience psychological problems in battle;
apart from the letters sent home (and these were only written by the
educated and the literate) it was rare to recognise the reality of psycho-
logical trauma. There would have been many examples of psychological
breakdown; it is just that few people wrote about it. Generally, if some-
one experienced symptoms in battle they were usually seen as cowards,
and would often be executed. This applies across cultures and across
historical time periods. Breaking down in battle has always been seen as
Historical perspective 19
Dr Albert Wilson stated that I do not think psychologists will get many
cases (quoted in Jones, 2004, p. 92). But it quickly became clear that
soldiers who were repeatedly exposed to bombardment could experience
an emotional breakdown, and it was debated whether this was caused by
microscopic damage to the central nervous system (literally, shellshock),
or whether it was a psychological problem. The term shellshock was
popular during the First World War (Salmon, 1921). Charles Myers, a
doctor in the Royal Army Medical Corps, described the condition of
emotionally disturbed troops arriving back from France in 1914 as shell-
shock. It was thought at the time that soldiers brains had been damaged
by exploding shells, that the forces of compression and decompression led
to microscopic brain haemorrhage. It was also thought that carbon mon-
oxide from the blast might contribute to cerebral poisoning (Jones, 2004).
Articles began to be published on the subject. These were summarised by
Salmon (1921) in his post-war report on shellshock, by which time it was
widely recognised that shellshock was a psychological disorder, not a
physiological one.
In order to best understand the traumatic response of the soldiers of
World War One, it may be most appropriate to turn to the poetry and
literature of the time. Both Wilfred Owen and Siegfried Sassoon experi-
enced shellshock during World War One and were sent to Craiglockhart
hospital outside Edinburgh for treatment, though Sassoon was actually
sent there for criticising the Governments war policy perhaps demon-
strating his sanity rather than his insanity. It was that or be tried for
treason, and the Government did not want that. The decision to
send Sassoon to Craiglockhart was, unfortunately, political rather than
medical. The best-known doctor at Craiglockhart was undoubtedly
W.H.R. Rivers (1922), who treated both Owen and Sassoon, and who
told his story in his autobiography after the war. The Regeneration
trilogy of Pat Barker, published in the 1990s, provided a vivid account
of the experiences of the traumatised soldiers in Craiglockhart. Rivers
was a strong advocate of the psychological cause of shellshock, believing
that part of the problem was that civilian soldiers, who had been trained
quickly, had been unable to build up effective defences against the
intense emotions they were to experience as a result of trench warfare.
Rivers believed, like Janet (1925) in Paris, in a cathartic approach, and
in attempting to reintegrate the traumatic experience into consciousness.
Wilfred Owen illustrated both cognitive processing and avoidance
through his poetry. In this instance, he shows how soldiers commonly
used avoidance in relation to the deaths of their fellows:
Hibberd (1992) claims that only soldiers would see the point of this
question, because only soldiers have the memories of their experiences.
Owen states that soldiers have problems with the emotions associated
with memories of dead comrades and so they avoid thinking about them.
Through literature we can gain an understanding of war experience,
again in the words of Owen regarding battle:
I can find no word to qualify my experiences except the word SHEER . . .
it passed the limits of my abhorrence. I lost all my earthly faculties, and
fought like an angel.
Owen recognises the traumatic nature of the war, that to experience war
is to experience something entirely alien to normal behaviour, and
something that is not possible to describe.
Not all medical practitioners were as enlightened as Rivers. Lewis Yeal-
land became renowned for his physical treatments. He used Faradism,
basically electric shock treatment, to cure paralysis. Yealland, not unlike
many senior military officers, believed that soldiers with functional symp-
toms had weak wills and that they needed strict discipline and harsh
methods to be cured. While he claimed a high success rate, this was likely
to be due to fear on the part of his patients. Patients experienced what
we might now view as torture. One man was strapped in a chair for
20 minutes at a time while strong electricity was applied to his neck and
throat; lit cigarettes had been applied to the tip of his tongue and hot
plates placed at the back of his mouth (Jones, 2004). The patient had
been told he could not leave the room until he was cured 4 hours of
electric shocks did the trick, according to Yealland (Binneveld, 1997).
The change in the idea from trauma being viewed as a psychological
disorder with environmental causes rather than a physiological one was
the fundamental change in thinking that enabled future psychologists to
make progress in understanding and treating war trauma. Prior to World
War One, the physiological argument reigned supreme. After the war,
many accepted that an environmental event could be the cause. This was
a Kuhnian paradigm shift that had been building up since Charcots
work in Paris in the 1880s, and was forced through by the unbearable
horror of the trenches. Of course, even by the end of the war there was
still strong disagreement about the existence of shellshock, with many
claiming it did not exist as a disorder at all.
During the war, psychiatric battle casualties had been initially
returned to the UK along with the wounded, but by the end of 1915 it
was clear that the chances of a soldier being cured decreased the further
they were sent from the front line. It was Myers who first suggested that
the troops should be treated relatively near the front line, but far enough
22 Historical perspective
away so that they were safe. Sir Arthur Sloggett, who had the rather long
title of the Director-General of Medical Services of the British Armies in
the Field, agreed that Myers could open specialist psychiatric units in
casualty clearing stations about 10 miles from the front. Sloggett himself
was against psychological treatment, believing that shellshock could be
treated with military discipline. Nevertheless, four treatment centres,
termed NYDNs (not yet diagnosed nervous) were established (Jones,
2004). The soldiers were rested and given encouragement to recover. This
strategy was later described as PIE proximity, immediacy, expectancy
the basic principles of treatment for battle stress:
Proximity The soldier must be treated as close to the front line as
practicable.
Immediacy He must be treated as soon as the symptoms appear.
This relies on his officers being aware of the symptoms, and
recognising them as traumatic stress rather than as signs of
cowardice.
Expectancy At all times the soldier must expect to return to his
frontline unit. If the soldier was allowed to think that he was a
casualty just like any physically wounded comrade, then the
symptoms would be less likely to dissipate.
This few hours or days bed rest meant the difference for many men
between breaking down completely and being able to cope. The dividing
line between clinical PTSD and sub-clinical PTSD is not that clear. PIE
probably helped many with sub-clinical disorders. The same issue arises
now: we make little effort to deal with those who have sub-clinical problems,
apparently not realising that by treating people before they have full-blown
PTSD we might save a lot of heartache and pain.
These principles, though sound, were not widely applied during the
First World War. After the war, when there were thousands of veterans
suffering from chronic psychological trauma, there was very little in the
way of effective treatment whether psychological or physiological
available. Most of the sufferers were left in mental institutions, and many
remained there for decades, never regaining their sanity, despite the good
work of many psychiatrists and psychologists2. The conditions persisted,
though many psychiatrists and others had believed that once a neurotic
soldier had been discharged from the armed forces he would stop his
subconscious malingering and recover.
2
These people were the unsung heroes of the interwar years, caring for many thousands of
forgotten heroes, often with little success, but with much perseverance and care.
Historical perspective 23
the hand which pulled the trigger. A sodium amytal injection resolved
the problem.
Sedation was used to keep patients asleep for days or weeks. At the
end of this period many soldiers would be much improved. (This was
also a technique which had been applied in the Spanish Civil War.)
Modified insulin treatment was also widely used, by both the British
and American forces (the latter used it with over 15,000 patients). This
treatment, which was effective with emotionally disturbed patients, but
not depressives, consisted of giving an insulin injection which made
the patient very drowsy, and just before they slipped into a (potentially
life-threatening) coma they were given plates of potatoes, which helped
restore glycogen levels to normality.
It has been estimated that around 1015% of the casualties sustained
by the British Expeditionary Force (BEF) during 1940 were psychiatric
cases (Keegan, 1976). Keegan also estimates that throughout the war
the psychiatric casualty rate varied from 2% to 30%, depending on the
conditions of the fighting, demonstrating the importance of the particu-
lar event, as well as the personality and resilience of the individual.
When the USA entered the war and became involved with major
fighting in North Africa, their authorities noticed that frontline units
were being rapidly depleted. Around 30% of all casualties were psychi-
atric ones. Psychiatric cases were being sent down the line and back to
the USA with the physically wounded. They quickly realised that they
needed fast and effective forms of treatment. They stopped sending
most psychiatric cases down the line and reintroduced the PIE principles
from the First World War. They also used the sodium amytal treatment
being used by the British. These treatments ensured that around 60%
of the psychiatric casualties could be returned to their units within a
few days. The USA failed to acknowledge the help the British
had given them regarding these treatments, claiming that their new
treatment narcosynthesis relied not only on sodium amytal or
Sodium Pentothal, but also on Freudian principles of psychoanalysis.
In 1943, General George Marshall, the US Chief of Staff, was still
complaining that more soldiers were being discharged on psychiatric
grounds than were being inducted into the army. During the whole
war 409,887 members of the US armed forces were admitted as psychi-
atric patients to overseas hospitals. Of these, 127,000 were returned
to the USA (Babington, 1983).
It was medical personnel from the USA who introduced the diagnostic
terms battle fatigue and combat exhaustion to try and remove the
stigma associated with war neurosis. There was a recognition that most
soldiers did not get used to combat; they put up with it for as long as they
26 Historical perspective
could. Soldiers would become ineffective after around 140 or 180 days,
with the peak of effectiveness around 90 days.
The RAF used the term lack of moral fibre to describe war neurosis
in aircrews. Many airmen were labelled LMFs, due to the very high
casualty rates in Bomber Command. There was little support for aircrew
who experienced psychological problems. Some were grounded, around
one-third managed to fly again, and a very few were invalided out. The
social pressure is important here. Bomber Command experienced very
high casualties around 50% during the course of the war and the men
knew there was a limited chance of them surviving the war, so one way
out could be traumatic stress. Because of the need for large numbers of
trained aircrew, and a shortage of highly trained men, it would be
difficult to allow people to become traumatised in this group. It was
almost a throwback to World War One, with any signs of fear being seen
as cowardice. The stiff upper lip was at its stiffest under the moustaches
of the men of Bomber Command.
Later in the war, the large numbers of patients arriving after the
Normandy invasion ensured that Sargant and his team, now based at
Graylingwell Hospital, Chichester, would have to use emergency treat-
ments at a level that would be difficult in peacetime. Typically there
would be around thirty patients all being given the combination of deep
sleep and modified insulin therapy simultaneously. It was around this
time that Pavlovs work on classical conditioning became known in
Britain and new forms of treatment were tried. Sargant started encour-
aging abreaction by forcing patients not to relive their own traumatic
experiences, but an invented related experience, e.g. asking a tank-
man to imagine being in a burning tank trying to save a comrade.
In theory this led to an emotional release, after which the patient would
improve. This is similar to the imaginal processing now widely applied
as part of cognitive behaviour therapy.
Anderson et al. (1944) also described the condition of the men who
had broken down after the Normandy invasion. The soldiers were
labelled as having combat exhaustion, a controversial term which many
psychiatrists would not accept as the soldiers were not exhausted
on admission to hospital. Unfortunately some soldiers, learning the label
of their condition, convinced themselves that they were too exhausted to
leave their stretchers and that this would be a permanent condition.
Anderson et al. state that 12% were unable to speak, 8% were temporar-
ily blind, 7% had lost their hearing, and 15% developed a stammer.
None was affected by paralysis. Around 70% had nightmares about
their experiences. One difference between these men and those of the
BEF 4 years before was that these had not lost their morale no doubt
Historical perspective 27
a function of the defeat of the earlier BEF and the expected victory of
the Normandy invaders.
Sargants wartime experiences led him, along with his colleague
Slater, to produce the classic book on psychiatric treatment, An
Introduction to Physical Methods of Treatment in Psychiatry. The Second
World War had seen an improvement in the methods used for treating
traumatised individuals. The treatment was designed to remove the
unpleasant memories and emotions of the traumatic event (Wilson,
1994). Dollard and Miller (1950) used combat as an example of learned
repression. They proposed that there are several stages of learning to
repress memories: (1) during combat there are many external and
internal cues; (2) the traumatic conditions attach strong fears to all these
cues; (3) afterwards when the soldier thinks about combat these
thoughts are the cues that evoke fear; (4) when he stops thinking, the
fear diminishes; (5) the decrease in fear reinforces stopping thinking, so
the veteran does not think about the war. These methods are again not
dissimilar to those used in modern treatments.
This chapter has provided an outline of some of the historical
moments in the understanding of war trauma up to the Second World
War. It is clear that the language of trauma has changed only recently is
it intelligible in modern terms and it is with difficulty that we interpret
the feelings of people who wrote letters home from battle before the mid-
nineteenth century. This has been a flavour of war trauma, a background
and context for developing our understanding throughout the twentieth
and into the twenty-first century. The next chapter takes up where this
one left off, with the research conducted during the Vietnam War which
led directly to the introduction of PTSD into the modern vocabulary
of mental illness.
3 Methods and ethics
Trying to understand and treat war trauma requires a good practical and
theoretical understanding of the subject. In order to obtain such an
understanding, effective methods must be used. Within psychology we
have developed a whole range of methods, from experimental through to
qualitative approaches; all can provide us with good theory, but in order
to do so they must be used appropriately. As in all science, there is good
and less good practice. It is not just that some methods are applied
inappropriately, with our ever-advancing technological and methodo-
logical understanding; researchers are constantly at the forefront of
new techniques and approaches, so it is inevitable that there will be
problems along the way. Another difficulty faced by trauma researchers
is that the work is often carried out in difficult circumstances, so com-
promises with regard to method are often made. It is difficult to design
the ideal experiment in a battlefield situation.
Another area of importance is that of ethics. While psychologists have
broadly conformed to ever more sophisticated ethical codes and proce-
dures over the last few decades, particularly in the West, there is an
ongoing debate about ethics, both in the protection of participants and
in ensuring the safety of researchers themselves. Ethical issues play an
important part in war trauma research. Inevitably, this research involves
some danger to both participants and researchers, and there are no clear
boundaries between what is acceptable and what is not acceptable.
Traumatised participants are often personally vulnerable, and there is a
possibility of further psychological damage occurring as a result of taking
part in a psychological study. We also must take care as researchers,
particularly when conducting research in war zones, though sometimes
participants can themselves present a danger to researchers trauma-
tised people can be aggressive and dangerous. These issues are conten-
tious, and it is important that we discuss them and act appropriately
when designing studies. It is equally important that we do not become
so rigid that a lot of good research cannot be conducted for ethical
reasons.
28
A range of methods 29
A range of methods
Researchers have used virtually every methodological technique avail-
able to the discipline of psychology, which is one thing that makes the
area exciting it is an area where we can genuinely triangulate research
from different methodological sources, and thus attempt to strengthen
our theories.
There are studies involving the so-called gold standard technique
of medical science the randomised controlled trial where the
researcher wishes to determine the effectiveness of a particular treatment,
e.g. cognitive behaviour therapy, and compare it with either another
form of treatment, or with a no-treatment control group. Traumatised
participants are randomly allocated to one of the groups, and after the
treatment is completed the groups are compared to find out whether
the treatment group has improved to a significantly greater extent than
the control group. If so, it is argued that, assuming all key variables have
been controlled for, the treatment is effective with this group.
Researchers have used brain scanning technology such as magnetic
resonance imaging (MRI) to explore those areas of the brain that are
thought to be important for war trauma, such as the amygdala (which
is involved with emotion and conditioning), the hippocampus (which is
important for memory input) and prefrontal cortex (involved with atten-
tion, decision making and working memory, all important in trauma
theory). By comparing traumatised and non-traumatised people, and
looking at the differences in brain structure or function, we can draw
conclusions about which areas of the brain are affected by trauma.
Other researchers use self-report questionnaires, which measure a
range of psychological variables, so we can see which variables play an
important part in how and why one person becomes traumatised and
another does not. For instance, we can measure extent of trauma, along
with age, sex, coping styles, social support and personality, and see which
of these predict a poor outcome after war experience.
Clinicians carry out detailed assessments of their clients throughout
a treatment programme. These can be used as the basis for case studies,
a method which allows the researcher or clinician to take a very detailed
look at the factors associated with trauma. These are illuminating to the
theoretician.
Interviews have been widely used as a method for the study of trau-
matised individuals because they enable the participant to discuss their
memories and feelings in detail, in the way they want to discuss it,
providing the information the participant wants to provide, i.e. focusing
on the issues that are important to them as individuals rather than the
30 Methods and ethics
issues the researcher thinks are important they may not be the same
thing. It is possible, through interviews, to develop a detailed under-
standing of the problems and issues faced by the person with war
trauma.
The narrative interview is a specific method linked to theory that is
important in this book. Narrative refers to a broad approach to a general
understanding of war trauma, and relates not only to the trauma itself
but also to the participants life in general, so providing a fuller and more
coherent picture of the role of the traumatic event in the persons
remembered experience. Narratives can be derived from a number of
sources, clinical interviews, narrative therapy, research interviews,
unpublished or published writings, etc., but if we are going to deal with
these we have to know how to carry out narrative analyses; we need to
apply a narrative method.
As in all science, understanding method is a critical pre-requisite for
understanding theory. Only by knowing how we studied an area can we
understand the potential strengths and weaknesses of a particular
theory. What follows is not a detailed consideration of each method
this is not a book on methods but an outline of some of the key issues.
for dealing with startle reactions, and Treatment D for faulty beliefs.
It is complicated. Cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) is recommended
by the NICE guidelines in the UK as the most effective treatment for
PTSD (NICE, 2005), along with eye movement desensitisation and
reprocessing. But CBT is only useful and effective for certain kinds of
people. Generally, they have to be people with some insight into their
condition, they have to be relatively intelligent in order to understand
how the treatment is meant to work, they have to be motivated to help
it work, and they have to be able to cope with the severe emotional
distress that will often result from the treatment. CBT will not work
for everyone.
Another problem with RCTs in this area is that they assume that a
patient can be randomly allocated to either a treatment or a control
condition. This is often just not possible. A lot of treatment for war-
traumatised people such as refugees takes place in the field, in the
country where the war is taking place, or in a refugee camp nearby. This
may not be the situation where an RCT can be used. People need
treating, and a war zone or a refugee camp is not always the best place
for exact science. Therapists have to be practical. One of the problems
psychologists working in this area face is criticism from the medical
community regarding the inadequacy of our methods. The situation
when treating traumatised people, especially in the field, is very different
from a hospital in a safe country where participants can be easily
accessed for the study.
This is also an ethical issue. If the clinician believes that a particular
treatment alleviates war trauma, and there is already some evidence that
this treatment works, then they may consider it their duty to use that
treatment. Refusing such treatment to psychologically damaged people
goes against the ethical principles of the professional psychologist. This
can sometimes be resolved if a waiting list control is used, i.e. where one
group receives immediate treatment, and the other group receives treat-
ment after a specific period of time. The key measurement is carried out
before the second group starts treatment. Again, while this is a good
experimental design, it may not be practical if the clinician is working
in the war zone.
Health service ethical procedures are at least in the UK very strict,
and there can be more emphasis on design matters than on ethics. That
may be appropriate in the hospital setting, but the problems faced by
psychologists should be recognised. As we can see from the above,
sometimes the most ethical solution for a study is to design it badly,
in terms of medical ethics. If this makes sense, the well-designed
study may be unethical while the poorly designed study may be ethical.
32 Methods and ethics
Again, there are ethical difficulties when carrying out certain types of
experimentation in this area. Obviously, it is not ethical to traumatise
people and randomly allocate them to groups. That means that our
experiments on humans are generally quasi-experiments, where we
compare already traumatised people with those who are not traumatised,
attempting to match them according to traumatic experience and other
variables we consider to be relevant. We then test to see whether they
perform differently on, for example, a cognitive task (e.g. memory,
attention or perception). If there is a difference, we may claim that this
is because of the presence of PTSD. The problem is that the participants
effectively self-allocated themselves to the groups (due to their traumatic
stress). It may have been that the difference was already there before the
traumatic experience. We have no way of telling when we are working
retrospectively in this way.
There are further problems with experimentation in psychology
that are general ones, not associated specifically with war trauma.
Experimentation, by its nature, manipulates only one or two variables.
Humans vary on many thousands of factors, and so it can be difficult to
tease out an effect that is specific to the experimental design. When this
effect is small, as is the case with many cognitive tasks, then this can
become even more difficult.
Problems notwithstanding, experimental procedures have produced
some of the critical findings for our understanding of traumatic stress,
and these should not be overlooked. These will be examined in more
detail in the next two chapters.
Brain-imaging techniques
Brain imaging has become very popular in the last 20 years or so.
A plethora of technology is now available to look at what is happening
inside the brain, where it is happening and how activation in a specific
area of the brain is related to specific behaviours. This is a major advance
in psychology which is fruitful for theory and understanding. There is a
wide range of technology available, including magnetic resonance
imaging or functional magnetic resonance imaging, positron emission
tomography and computed axial tomography scans, and the newest
approach transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). All are invasive
techniques, particularly TMS, which involves deliberate stimulation of
specific brain areas and the analysis of the responses to that stimulation.
These techniques are used to help identify which regions of the brain are
active while people are displaying certain behaviours, or how the brains
of different groups of people (e.g. traumatised vs non-traumatised)
34 Methods and ethics
differ. They have transformed our understanding of the brain in the last
couple of decades. They have changed the whole nature of neuroscience.
This is a Kuhnian revolution in our understanding of the brain.
One of the problems with many imaging studies is that they only
show that activity is occurring in a certain area in the brain. This in
itself is not demonstrating a great deal. At one level these studies have
little more explanatory value than Galls phrenology, the measurement
of bumps on the head. The correlation between brain activity or size
and behaviour may be closer than Galls approach, but it does not go
much further than correlations. Fortunately, as we shall see in later
chapters, brain-imaging techniques, though they are still in their
relative infancy, have helped our theoretical understanding of trauma
enormously. We now have a far more detailed understanding of the
neuropsychological mechanisms underlying trauma than we did even
1015 years ago.
A good example of the kind of work that has been carried out in the
field of traumatic stress is that of Doug Bremner et al. (1995, 2005).
Since the 1990s this team has been trying to establish the important sites
relating to traumatic stress. Using PET, they have assessed a number of
different traumatised populations and consistently found that trauma-
tised people compared with non-traumatised people have a smaller
volume in the right hippocampus, an area of the limbic system that is
critical to the storage of explicit, verbal, conscious memories. This
finding is contentious, and other research has shown the relationship
to be more complex. Yehuda et al. (2006) found that veterans with
PTSD did not differ in hippocampal volume from matched veterans
without PTSD (though they did show lower urinary cortisol and poorer
memory performance on the Wechsler logical memory test and digit
span). Smaller left hippocampal volume was found in veterans who
developed PTSD in response to their first reported traumatic exposure,
compared with veterans who experienced PTSD after a second or
subsequent exposure. These researchers concluded that smaller hippo-
campal volumes may be associated with specific risk and resilience factors.
This is a demonstration of the difficulty of carrying out good experi-
mental research in this area. It is not clear from this quasi-experimental
study whether or not the veterans with a smaller hippocampal volume
had this before they were exposed to the trauma or whether it was caused
by the traumatic event. Teasing this apart is essential to establishing
causality. This demonstrates the need for longitudinal studies (see Jelicic
and Bonke, 2001) to establish hippocampal volume before exposure
to traumatic events, e.g. using samples of members of the armed forces
or the emergency services people who can be assessed at an early stage
Self-report measures 35
Self-report measures
The self-report questionnaire is probably the most common method
used in war trauma research, and the one that has the most problems
in practice if not in theory. There are a number of reasons why it is the
most ubiquitous method, and they are not always good ones. The
questionnaire, used well, can provide very useful information about a
person. Questionnaires are used to measure the type and extent of the
trauma, coping styles, social support, dissociation, anxiety, depression,
substance abuse, general demographic details (age, sex, socio-economic
status) and a host of other variables. Many of the measures used are
based on good psychometric principles. They have been standardised
using appropriate populations, and are reliable across time (assuming
the construct is stable across time) and measure (if there are two mea-
sures of the same construct). It is usual for a good standardised psycho-
metric instrument to have norms established so that a person can be
assessed against those norms. For instance, if we have a measure of
coping, we may be interested in knowing whether a person uses a particu-
lar coping strategy more, less or about the same as the average person.
Similarly, we need to establish cut-offs for many measures. If we want
to find out what proportion of people have PTSD after a particular
incident, then we need to establish an accurate cut-off so that we can
conclude with some confidence that people who score X or above are
likely to have PTSD, while those who score below X are unlikely to
have PTSD. We do not draw a firm conclusion from any self-report
measure. An assessment of PTSD or any other mental disorder requires
an in-depth assessment by a qualified clinician.
36 Methods and ethics
Second, war trauma is more complex than simple PTSD. Not only do
war-traumatised people experience PTSD, they also have problems with
depression, anxiety, substance abuse, coping and social support a
whole range of problems so a measure of PTSD alone will not suffice.
Added to this there are specific issues relating to particular wars. For
instance, in the civil war in Sierra Leone, many males had their arms
chopped off, a traumatic incident not usual in war. In Bosnia, women
were systematically, politically even, mass raped. These kinds of inci-
dents must be accounted for in any self-report questionnaire that is
being used to assess war trauma, because one key part of war trauma
is the type of incidents that occur. This does not obviate the need for the
basic PTSD measure, but it does mean that there will often be adapta-
tions to existing measures in order to obtain full coverage of the material
required.
Interviews
The interview is a very useful technique used widely by researchers.
The interview avoids many of the disadvantages of the questionnaire,
though it is, in its own ways, a potentially very imperfect instrument.
Interviewing comprises a number of techniques, from highly structured
(where there is a questionnaire to which the respondent provides
specific answers), to semi-structured (where there are open-ended ques-
tions, but the questions are still specific and there is a set order), to
unstructured rather a misnomer, but where there are a number of
topics the researcher wishes to address, and where the interview can flow
in different directions, depending on the way the respondent talks.
The researchers role is to ensure that the topics are adequately covered.
There are many different ways to analyse interviews. Analytic tech-
nique is the subject of many books, and it is not necessary to go into
detail here. There are many disagreements among psychologists not only
about how to conduct interviews, but also about how to transcribe them
and analyse the data. Some interviewers use content analysis a quanti-
tative approach where the analysis involves adding up the number
of times something is said and analysing these frequencies. More com-
monly, interviews are analysed using qualitative techniques, of which
there are many, for instance grounded theory (Strauss and Corbin, 1998),
42 Methods and ethics
explore particular ideas, to look at how they have coped (or failed to
cope), their memories and their experiences. They want to discuss the
story of their war. A questionnaire is often seen as trivial, asking the
wrong questions. By devising an interview that has a number of themes
(e.g. the war experience, memories, coping styles), the researcher can
enable the participant to discuss what is important about these themes.
The researcher must be a good interviewer. They must be able to draw
out relevant information, even if the participant is reluctant to discuss an
issue. They must also be prepared to deal with the often raw emotions
that such interviews bring out in participants not just upset, crying and
bewilderment, but anger and sometimes aggression. The full gamut of
human emotion can be present in an interview with a victim of war trauma.
Narrative approaches
As narrative is central to this book, it is important that we examine
narrative methods. Narrative methods are used with a number of sources
of data, mainly the one-to-one interview, but written accounts (e.g.
diaries, journals, books) can also be analysed using narrative methods.
Narrative methods arose in part from the style of answer that many
participants made to interviewers questions. They typically tended to
be long-winded, and represented a fuller story than the interviewer had
actually asked for. There is a tendency for people to tell stories when
they are being interviewed, and it is every interviewers painful experi-
ence to hear such stories when they wanted just a short answer to a
simple question. Narrative methods had to be developed to take into
account peoples normal behaviour, which was to refuse to allow the
interviewer to fragment the coherency of their lived experience.
Of course, it was not that simple, but it is good to think that the ordinary
non-specialist has some power over the academic researcher.
According to Bamberg and McCabe (1998), human agency is critical
to narrative: people strive to configure space and time, deploy cohesive
devices, reveal identity of actors and relatedness of actions across scenes.
They create themes, plots and dramas. In so doing, narrators make sense
of themselves, social situations, and history (p. iii).
Mishler (1986) was one of the earlier proponents of a narrative
approach, though much work had been carried out prior to this. The
so-called narrative turn has affected many disciplines in one way or
another, for instance sociology, history, anthropology, law, social work
and, of course, psychology. Academics in each discipline have used
different narrative methods in their research, but this is not a full review
of narrative methods, so we will focus on methods used in psychology.
44 Methods and ethics
Ethics
The researcher who is interested in war trauma will always come up
against ethical issues. All professional psychologists are bound by their
code of ethics, which means that they must ensure that the work they do
is within the bounds of their professional competence, that they do not
cause harm to others, and that they do not place themselves under
significant risk. The British Psychological Societys (2006) Code of Conduct
and Ethical Guidelines provides full details.
Any code of ethics presents problems to the researcher interested in
war trauma. By the very nature of the research we are asking people to
46 Methods and ethics
discuss those things that have caused them the most distress. By asking
them about their war experience we are deliberately causing them fur-
ther distress without even the promise of effective therapy. At the same
time the researcher is exposing him or herself to stories which might be
personally damaging. Furthermore, researchers who travel to war zones
can run significant personal risks. How do we deal with these issues?
Much of what follows is about interviewing traumatised individuals,
though the material is relevant for other methods.
able to stop at any point. After the data are collected, the researcher
should still ask whether the participant is happy for their information to
be included. The participant should then be provided with a list of
possible help points in case they have issues they wish to discuss further
(e.g. with a doctor or counsellor). The researcher should follow up the
meeting a few days later, just with a telephone call to make sure the
person is all right, to guide them to help if necessary, and importantly
for interviews to ask if there is anything they wish to add to what they
said. It is very common for people, after being interviewed, to think
about the issues over the next few days and either regret something they
said or want to add further details.
In the end, data collection with traumatised people is difficult for
the participants. They are likely to get upset; they are likely to cry.
While they should be free to stop the interview at any point, it is likely
to be more damaging to stop when the person is distressed than to
continue through the distress until they emerge at the other side.
There are added complications if the data collection is taking place in
a war zone or a refugee camp, perhaps with people who are not literate.
The rules stay the same; it is just that the method of dealing with the
rules changes. The researcher still has to obtain full informed consent,
and there should be no pressure on the person to take part in the study.
The participant must always be a volunteer. It is ethically questionable
to try to force people to take part in any research; it is particularly
unreasonable to ask traumatised people to discuss issues that many of
them do not wish to discuss. This does mean that samples are not
representative; it also means that any figures you obtain cannot be gener-
alised to the traumatised population. Owing to the nature of trauma,
there are some types of people who want to talk about their experiences
in details, and others who do not want to talk in any circumstances. These
differences must be respected, even though it means there will be a
methodologically and theoretically significant proportion of the popula-
tion who are not in your study, hence your findings may be biased.
These factors are a function of this type of research design compromise
before ethical compromise. As long as the issues are addressed fully,
including in the written report, this is still good science.
Self-exposure
The researcher who interviews traumatised people may experience
some symptoms themselves. When one constantly hears terrible stories
about death and cruelty, and about unbearable emotions, the stories
themselves can affect you. There is a whole literature on this. It is known
48 Methods and ethics
risk. Obviously, if a researcher visits a war zone, then they are putting
themselves at risk, so who is responsible for this? Any risk assessment
carried out in such a place would show very high risk on all categories,
and that would normally mean the member of staff should not visit the
war zone. If these findings were actually applied, then a lot of good
research would not get carried out. It perhaps illustrates one way in
which the health and safety culture has gone too far. While we want to
be reasonably healthy and reasonably safe, we also need some autonomy
in deciding what we consider appropriate. A university should never
force someone to work in a dangerous situation but, in the end, the
responsibility has to lie with the researcher. If they need to visit a war
zone for their research, then so be it. The employer should take reason-
able steps to protect the safety of the employee, but not to the extent of
banning travel for bona fide research.
Introduction
This chapter focuses on our current understanding of war trauma. This
is a rapidly changing area, and space does not permit a thorough cover-
age of all theories. Instead, the focus will be on the key constructs and
the relationship between them. The area of war trauma is fascinating
because it is an area within psychology that allows us to draw together
the often disparate and contradictory perspectives, particularly psycho-
neurology, cognition, psychodynamics, behaviourism and social theory.
The purpose of the chapter, then, is not to provide a full theoretical
account of our understanding of war trauma, but to provide the scaf-
folding from which we can build that understanding. Chapter 7 focuses
on memory, particularly traumatic memory. This chapter discusses the
construct of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and associated
comorbid disorders. PTSD is the main classification that is used in the
diagnosis of war trauma, and it has been an effective heuristic, helping
bring about many thousands of publications on the subject. While it is a
popular diagnosis, and very useful for clinicians, there are some prob-
lems with it, and there is debate about the structure of the syndrome
(its symptoms have been revised with each revision of the diagnostic
classification system) and some question as to whether it should even be
retained.
PTSD
As described in Chapter 2, the construct now known as PTSD has
existed for many years under a variety of formulations and names,
mainly relating to battle experience (battle fatigue, combat neurosis,
etc.). These terms all refer to a specific set of symptoms that are the
result of a traumatic experience. In the twentieth century, interest in
the subject understandably tended to increase around wartime and
decrease during peacetime, though since 1980, when PTSD was itself
50
PTSD 51
are individual factors that help predict PTSD (at least in groups).
Women are usually more likely to get PTSD than men. Kessler et al.
(1995), in a US study, found twice as many women with PTSD than
men. Frans et al. (2005) found similar results in a Swedish cohort. It is
not clear why women are more likely to be diagnosed with PTSD,
though there are a number of possible reasons. First, women are exposed
to different types of trauma, e.g. rape and sexual abuse, which may be
more personally violating, and hence more traumatic. Second, there is
the depression effect, whereby women are more likely to be open and
honest about their symptoms; they are more likely to go to the doctor
and report that they are experiencing problems than men. However, the
results are not clear, as some studies seem to show that PTSD rates can
be similar for men and women when they are exposed to similar events.
For instance, Kang et al. (2005) found that male and female US veterans
of the Iraq and Afghanistan wars have similar rates of PTSD. As Nemeroff
et al. (2006) discuss, female gender is usually a strong risk factor for the
development of PTSD, though the mechanisms for the disparity are
uncertain. There are other risk and resilience factors that help to predict
PTSD. The presence of effective coping strategies is a good predictor,
as is the presence of perceived social support, and previous mental
health conditions including PTSD (e.g. McCauley et al., 1997).
PTSD is recognised in British law, so people who have been trauma-
tised should expect to be treated justly according to the law because they
can get a diagnosis of PTSD. The Belmarsh 12 were a group of
individuals who were locked up without trial in 2002 because the British
government perceived that they were a terrorist threat. Usually recent
British governments have respected habeas corpus, and have not tended
to lock people up without just cause. A person is charged, and then they
are sent to trial, where it will be determined whether they committed a
crime and, if so, what the punishment should be. The Belmarsh 12 were
not charged, and were imprisoned without any knowledge of whether
they would be charged or when they might be released. A group of
psychologists and psychiatrists assessed them, and concluded that they
were experiencing PTSD as a result of the trauma of being locked up
without charge. In the end the courts recognised this and controversially
freed the men (Robbins, 2007).
PTSD has proved to be a useful diagnostic category for people who
are in need of treatment for traumatic stress. While the response to
traumatic events can be complicated, there are therapeutic procedures
that work with the symptoms of PTSD, at least for some traumatised
people. Providing therapy can benefit peoples lives by reducing their
symptoms. There are a range of therapies available. The NICE (2005)
Critique of PTSD 55
Critique of PTSD
While PTSD has some very positive features, there are problems with
the construct. There is the complexity of the psychosocial response to
traumatic events. While people do consistently display the symptoms
described in DSM, the construct of PTSD is not entirely coherent or
consistent. Many, if not most, people with PTSD also experience other
diagnostic constructs, or symptoms from other such constructs. This
means that if we were to apply a medical diagnosis strictly, we would
have problems in recognising PTSD as a construct.
Many researchers and clinicians are critical of PTSD. At some levels
this does not matter. If treatments arising from a diagnosis of PTSD
work, then why should we worry too much? The problem is that PTSD
is, as stated earlier, recognised in law.
If a person can prove that they contracted PTSD as a result of their
working conditions they can sue their employer. If a diagnostic category
is going to be used in law, it must be as good at assessing disease as
possible. Rosen and Lilienfeld (2008), in a recent review, carried out an
empirical evaluation of the core assumptions of PTSD, and the results
were very critical of the construct. There were a number of key issues that
they addressed, starting from the position that PTSD rests on a number of
core assumptions, particularly that a class of traumatic events is linked to
a distinct clinical syndrome. This should, theoretically, distinguish PTSD
from other mental disorders. Rosen and Lilienfeld argue that, though
research has attempted to distinguish markers for PTSD, for example
biological or cognitive, it has largely failed to do so. In the end, the authors
conclude that virtually all of the core assumptions and mechanisms
relating to PTSD lack compelling or consistent empirical evidence.
56 Current theory: PTSD
Criterion A (p. 52) has always been difficult for PTSD. There is,
unlike most diagnostic categories, an assumption that there is a specific
environmental cause for the resultant disorder. There must be a link
between the traumatic event and subsequent symptoms. The key prob-
lems are that: (a) there is no clear definition of what a traumatic event is;
(b) it is not always possible to determine whether a traumatic event has
taken place; (c) it is not always possible to determine whether the event
actually produced the symptoms; and (d) the causal event is not neces-
sarily sufficient to lead to PTSD. Many usually most people, after a
traumatic event, are not traumatised. Criterion A has developed and
changed throughout the various editions of DSM in order to try to
clarify the first of these problems. Initially, in DSM-III, the event was
considered as something outside the range of normal human experi-
ence (American Psychiatric Association, 1980). This would immedi-
ately exclude war experience, the death of loved ones, and many other
traumatic stressors, and was clearly designed with the safe western
person in mind. In DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994)
there is confusion between the event, which is something that leads to, or
threatens to lead to, death or injury, and the persons responses to the
event, feelings of fear, helplessness or horror. This confusion between
the event and the psychological response to the event is no improvement
on earlier versions of DSM. By confusing what happened with how
someone feels about it, the authors open the possibility of excluding
those people who experience the event, but who do not experience any
symptoms at the time or in the immediate aftermath, and who develop
symptoms at some future point. Perhaps this is why some people argue
that PTSD should not be linked specifically to a traumatic stressor.
Of course, there is the further problem with the stressor criterion
that some people develop a more positive and understanding outlook on
life after going through a traumatic event. This will be covered in more
depth in Chapter 6, but it is another reason why there is a problem with
the stressor criterion; such an event does not predictably lead to the
particular pattern of symptoms we call PTSD.
PTSD is defined within the traditional medical model of a number
of symptoms regularly clustering together and forming a coherent
syndrome. Unfortunately, there is no clearly definable group of symp-
toms that leads to a definite syndrome of PTSD. This may be a criticism
of the use of the medical model in these circumstances. The model may
be relevant to most physical disorders, and many mental disorders, but it
becomes problematic when considering complex psychological dis-
orders. Most research into PTSD has shown that there is nearly always
a comorbid disorder often depression, sometimes generalised anxiety,
Critique of PTSD 57
Concluding comments
While there is much to say in criticism of the construct of PTSD, it has
proved to be very useful in guiding clinicians regarding the treatment of
traumatised individuals. It has also been an effective heuristic, generat-
ing a great deal of research around the world. Without PTSD, it is
difficult to imagine that our understanding of the response to traumatic
incidents would be anything like as good as it is now. Nevertheless, while
some people have argued the case for introducing a construct of complex
PTSD, the case of war trauma does show that there is a need to have a
broader explanation of peoples responses to such complex events as
war. The next chapter focuses on the role of traumatic memory in
enabling a deeper understanding of war trauma.
5 Approaches to understanding trauma
probe paradigm (Bryant and Harvey, 1997), i.e. a bias towards noticing
trauma-related words. Less positive results were found in an auditory
recognition test using traumatised Vietnam veterans (Trandel and
McNally, 1987). One of the problems with this type of research is that
traumatised people are not particularly conditioned to respond to words
it is particular sights, smells and sounds that they respond to so it is not
surprising that a task using words may generate conflicting results.
People experiencing a trauma experience a range of emotions,
depending on their own characteristics and on the type of traumatic
experience. As the first diagnostic criterion for PTSD includes fear,
horror or helplessness as concomitant to the event itself, it is not sur-
prising that there is a strong relationship between these reactions and the
risk of PTSD some time later (Brewin et al., 2000). Other people report
shame or anger at the time of the trauma. It depends on the person and
the situation. Another useful emotional concept is that of mental
defeat, which goes beyond the normal reactions and is concerned with
a breakdown of identity. This was demonstrated in former political
prisoners in East Germany (Ehlers et al., 2000). Those who experienced
mental defeat were still more likely to have PTSD many years later.
Mental defeat is a useful concept when discussing trauma and narrative,
as it indicates a total breakdown in the narrative that is difficult to
rebuild and restore.
The emotional response is also associated with peoples beliefs.
Emotions have to be at the core of any understanding of why we think
as we do. Janoff-Bulman (1992) argued that traumatic events shatter
peoples basic beliefs and assumptions about the world. Her view is that
we have fundamental beliefs that the world is meaningful, that it is
benevolent and that the self is worthy. Trauma disrupts those beliefs,
and recovery is about rebuilding them, or developing a narrative.
A similar notion was put forward by Bolton and Hill (1996), who
proposed a slightly different set of fundamental beliefs that the self is
sufficiently competent to act, that the world is sufficiently predictable,
and that the world provides a sufficient satisfaction of our needs. Again,
the traumatised person has these beliefs shattered.
Dissociation
Dissociation was discussed widely in the nineteenth and early twentieth
centuries, and has been revived in trauma theory. It is not clearly
defined, but relates to a breakdown of what is normally a relatively
continuous set of interrelated processes that help us to deal with the
world, whether that of the present, or the links between the past and the
Cognition and emotion 63
found similar results (e.g. Shalev et al., 1998). Michaels et al. (1998)
found that dissociative states were more likely to persist in PTSD
patients.
Cognitive processing
A cognitive processing model of trauma proposes that a person enters a
traumatic situation with pre-existing schemata about the nature of the
world, their belief systems, and expectations regarding the future. The
experience of trauma confronts individuals with information that is
inconsistent with these schemata, which contain information about
safety and invulnerability. Horowitz (1986) argued that, for recovery to
occur, the individual must process the traumatic experience such that
the new inconsistent information is resolved and incorporated into the
persons schemata via a process of adaptation. In order to do this,
the pre-existing schemata must be adjusted to incorporate the new
information. The individuals attempts to assimilate the trauma-related
information will inevitably lead to increased arousal and hence a desire
to escape from, or avoid, thoughts and reminders of the event. Horowitz
argued that until the trauma-related information is assimilated, it is
stored in active memory, and will continue to produce intrusive recol-
lections. Psychological numbing of responsiveness is a psychological
defence mechanism against such intrusive thoughts.
Creamer et al. (1992, 1995) argued that there is little empirical evi-
dence to support Horowitzs work, and so presented a general model of
cognitive processing in a way that would allow for longitudinal empirical
testing (see also McFarlane, 1992). Their model is an attempt at a
synthesis of previous models, but it has the limitation that it fails to
include many elements that affect post-trauma adjustment, such as per-
sonality, social support and biological factors. It consists of five stages:
Objective exposure
The severity of exposure to the event has been found to be a critical
factor in the development of subsequent pathology (e.g. Foy et al., 1987;
Speed et al., 1989). But the severity of the stressor is not the only
determinant. The model proposes that the effects of the stressor will
be mediated by processing variables. These processing variables are
intrusion and avoidance.
Network formation
This can only happen if the person subjectively appraises the event as
being traumatic. The formation of the traumatic memory network in
Cognition and emotion 65
Intrusion
The memory network must be activated for processing to take place
(Foa et al., 1989). This occurs when the person is presented with infor-
mation that matches stimulus, response or meaning information in the
memory network (i.e. a reminder). Activation of the network results not
only in intrusion but also the accompanying aversive responses. These
intrusive memories, while causing distress, may also relate to cognitive
processing. Exposure allows stimulusresponse connections to be
weakened and encourages modification of the meaning associated
with the incident. Intrusion can be functional in this way, or it can be
dysfunctional, perhaps because it can result in very high arousal and can
prompt attempts to avoid the traumatic memories. Creamer et al. (1992)
conceptualise avoidance as a coping mechanism that is activated when
intrusive thoughts become too difficult for the person to deal with.
Intrusion leads to the automatic fear response, which is too strong for
the individual, who reverts to a strategy of avoidance; thus no processing
of the traumatic information takes place. Creamer et al. propose that
resolution occurs not only through intrusion, but also through more
adaptive processing, e.g. discussion of the trauma with family and friends.
Avoidance
Avoidance is a coping strategy, a response to the discomfort caused by
the intrusion stage. Network activation produces a state of high physio-
logical arousal. People may attempt to escape this by avoiding reminders
of the past. Our work (Hunt and Robbins, 2001a) suggests that avoid-
ance can be a very successful long-term coping strategy for some indi-
viduals. Creamer et al. (1992) argue that it can be maladaptive if relied
on to excess. They suggest that high levels of avoidance will be associ-
ated with the continued presence of psychological symptoms. While this
is true, it is also the case that avoidance may be a very effective strategy
for people for life. It is difficult to determine whether someone is
using avoidance effectively or whether they really do have no potential
symptoms. This presents another methodological difficulty.
Avoidance levels will also be determined by prior coping strategies.
While avoidance may successfully alleviate symptoms, traumatic infor-
mation cannot be processed. Creamer et al. predict that high levels of
avoidance will be associated with continuing symptoms. This stage is
critical for combat veterans. During combat, individuals use avoidance
66 Approaches to understanding trauma
as an important coping strategy. After World War Two they were told to
forget about what they had done, and wives were discouraged from
talking about it. But evidence, even from the 1940s, suggests that those
who avoid discussing a trauma may have more severe longer term
consequences (Lindemann, 1944).
Outcome
Recovery is achieved through activation and modification of the fear
network. This is evidenced by high levels of intrusion, which lead to
high levels of symptomatology at the time, but reduced symptomatology
later.
Creamers theory has had some influence on the development of
trauma theory, but there are difficulties. One of the biggest problems is
that it is difficult to test because it requires detailed longitudinal work,
and peoples complex reactions mean that they may be in several of the
above stages at any one time. Those who have apparently achieved
outcome may revert to earlier stages when they are reminded of their
traumatic event. It may not make sense to think of people being in
particular stages of recovery at all.
Another difficulty concerning processing is that it is assumed that,
when the memory network is activated, it automatically begins process-
ing. There is no reason to suppose that this is the case. If an individual
simply goes over and over the memory, it will not necessarily become less
horrific or traumatic. This is simply depressive rumination. As Weine
et al.s (1995) victim of ethnic cleansing stated, the memories may be like
films played over and over again. Another problem is that recovery is
oversimplified. In learning terms, the model suggests that a kind of
behavioural flooding takes place, and that if the person has high symp-
tom levels, then these will reduce over time through a process of extinc-
tion. There is no evidence that this is the case. In order for recovery to
take place, the individual has to act on the memories. This is where the
link with the work on explicit and implicit memories and developing a
narrative is important. Creamers notion of processing is equivalent to
the development of the narrative (van der Kolk and Fisler, 1995): if
traumatic recollections are avoided, then they remain in implicit
memory, and are in a position to be activated should the right stimulus
conditions be experienced, and as they are implicit memories, they are
not being acted on or narrated.
A full model of the response to trauma has to take into account how
the individuals schemata interact with traumatic recollections when
developing the narrative, though even over the long term, the indivi-
duals response can be broadly defined within the two mechanisms of
Learning theory 67
Learning theory
Learning theory has been productive for theory, for empirical work and
for treatment of trauma. The basic assumption is that a person develops
fear through classical conditioning, usually taking the form of one-trial
learning known as the Garcia effect,1 which is a basic survival mechan-
ism. If a person behaves in a particular way in a life-threatening situation
and survives, then they automatically remember this for a similar situa-
tion in the future. While this may be adaptive in the traumatic situation,
it may be less so in other situations. The problem, as we shall see, is that
the memory is not just of the survival behaviour in the appropriate
context, but also the emotional, cognitive and behavioural elements,
which constitute traumatic stress. Any reminder of the traumatic event
can automatically activate the fear memory. This works for the condi-
tioning process as well. Orr et al. (2000) showed that people with PTSD
develop conditioned responses to aversive stimuli much more readily
than people without PTSD.
Lang (1979) suggested that emotional images are composed of three
main classes of propositional unit; these are concerned with stimulus
information (e.g. location and physical characteristics of the situation),
response information (verbal, physiological and behavioural responses)
and meaning information (interpretation of the stimulus and response
elements and their significance for the individual; this is tied to narrative
development, as will be discussed in Chapter 8). Lang proposed a
prototype fear image stored in memory. New events are tested against
this prototype. If there is a sufficiently close correspondence, the stored
memory, complete with response elements, is activated and the individ-
ual experiences fear. This programmatic construction explains how the
fear information is only sometimes available to consciousness, in effect
when the individual is provided with a reminder.
The emotional processing of fear and the construction of memories of
traumatic events have been widely studied. Rachman (1980) suggested
1
After Garcia and Koelling (1966), who gave rats food with sufficient poison in it to make
them ill, then when they gave them the same food some months later the rats would not
touch it.
68 Approaches to understanding trauma
the context of the schemata. This has implications for the changes that
may occur in memories over time, depending on individual circum-
stances. Memories can be confabulated, so presumably they can range
along a continuum from being highly accurate to completely inaccurate.
Davis and Lehman (1995) describe what they call counterfactuals
memories of a traumatic event that are not true. This was explored in
detail in the 1990s in the so-called false memory debate, where it was
thought by some that patients were recovering memories of child abuse,
which led to the prosecution of some people, particularly fathers. In the
end, the debate fizzled out, with the general consensus being that these
memories were fictitious and created during the therapeutic process (e.g.
British Psychological Society, 1995). People coping with traumatic
events appeared uninhibited in their ability to generate counterfactuals,
i.e. false memories. The memories of traumatised individuals should be
treated with caution, but not the individuals responses to those memories.
While ordinary memories are not fixed, traumatic recollections often
are. Weine et al. (1995), in a study of Bosnian refugees, quote one
survivor who says that he has films of traumas that constantly play in
his head; although he may look away from them, they continue to inhabit
him (p. 540). This is not an isolated case. Much of the work involving
trauma survivors has indicated that individuals appear to relive the
traumatic event, not just recall it. This reliving is one of the characteris-
tics of PTSD. Koss et al. (1996), in a review of work on traumatic
memories, conclude that there is a consensus of evidence which suggests
that emotion facilitates accurate recall of central details of the event,
though there is no similar increment for the peripheral details. They also
concluded that emotion slows forgetting, and time may actually enhance
such memories, because shortly after the trauma emotions may disrupt
retrieval, but over time such disruption diminishes. Brewer (1992) sug-
gests that the explanation for the durability of emotional memories is not
rehearsal, but occurs at the encoding stage, which would suggest that it
is a form of one-trial learning that is adaptive, in support of the work
outlined above.
Perhaps the most prominent current theory regarding traumatic
memory is Brewins dual-representation theory (Brewin et al., 1996;
Brewin and Holmes, 2003), which proposes that there are two memory
systems known as verbally accessible memory (VAM) and situationally
accessible memory (SAM). The two systems operate in parallel, and
one may take precedence over the other at particular times. VAM
memories of trauma contain information that the person has attended
to before, during and after the traumatic event, and contains sufficient
information to be encoded and stored for later deliberate retrieval. These
Neuropsychological understanding 71
memories only contain what was consciously attended to. They may also
contain the elaborated memories of the narrative, the elements which
did not actually exist during the traumatic situation but which the person
has added to the information, consciously or non-consciously.
In contrast with the VAM system, flashbacks and other trauma mem-
ories are contained within the SAM system, demonstrating that flash-
backs are triggered involuntarily by external or internal reminders. The
information is usually lower level, with more perceptual information
from the scene of the trauma, i.e. information that has received no, or
minimal, processing. The SAM system has no verbal code, and so the
memories are difficult to access and communicate to other people. The
memories do not interact with other memories and so are not processed.
This is an important point, which explains why trauma memories
are fragmented and uncontrolled, and why, in order to develop the
narrative, a person must find ways of accessing the VAM.
The implications of the dual-representation approach are that PTSD
is a hybrid disorder that has potentially two pathological processes. The
first involves dealing with negative and shattered beliefs and assumptions
and their accompanying emotions, and the second involves managing
classically conditioned flashbacks or other intrusive thoughts (Brewin
and Holmes, 2003). Full recovery depends on both being resolved, and
perhaps shows why cognitive behaviour therapy is effective (at least for a
proportion of traumatised people), as it deals with both elements. To
resolve the VAM problems, the person needs to reappraise the infor-
mation, and reintegrate it into their schemata. To resolve SAM-related
problems will require exposure therapy.
In the end, the VAM/SAM model provides integration between cog-
nitive understanding and learning theory. Both are critical to developing
a full account of what happens when someone is traumatised. Now we
have an effective psychological understanding, it is important to under-
pin this with a neuropsychological understanding of trauma processes.
Neuropsychological understanding
Until fairly recently, comparatively little research had been carried
out on the biological underpinnings of PTSD and war trauma. This
is interesting given that the early theories of war trauma implicated
physiological systems, either the heart (soldiers heart during the US
Civil War) or microscopic brain damage (shellshock during the First
World War). The problem with developing neuropsychological under-
standing was that the appropriate methodological tools had not been
developed. It is only with the introduction of imaging techniques that
72 Approaches to understanding trauma
Psychodynamic approaches
Psychodynamic theory has been used throughout the twentieth century
to explain post-traumatic stress, and while such an approach is rarely
accepted in mainstream psychology, at least in Anglo-Saxon psychology,
it is important not to dismiss the contribution made by this perspective,
particularly on the Continent. By exploring psychodynamic theory, we
may find that it enhances our understanding. Emery and Emery (1989)
related psychodynamic theory to the diagnostic criteria of PTSD and
concluded that the distinction between psychoneuroses and traumatic
neuroses should be retained, and that the aetiology of PTSD lies in the
stressor itself. This argument is directly relevant to the debate regarding
the role of the stressor. The critical distinction between explicit and
Existentialism and humanism 77
Vietnam veterans keep alive their memories of the war because they are
significant and meaningful. Many continue to suffer from PTSD
because of this (Bradshaw et al., 1991).
Greening (1990) discusses PTSD in terms of a fundamental assault
on ones right to live, on our sense of worth and on our sense that the
world (including people) basically supports human life. This is similar to
Horowitzs cognitive theory (Horowitz, 1986), whereby our world sche-
mata are shattered by traumatic experiences, and in order to be cured
we have to reconcile these schemata with the evidence provided by the
trauma that the world isnt as pleasant as we believed it to be.
Coping
Coping is critical to understanding how people respond to traumatic
events. There has already been a discussion of some of the different
coping strategies people use, but we can get bogged down when discuss-
ing coping. There are numerous theories, and 40 years of research has
failed to generate any sense of coherence in the literature. Some theories
have just a few coping strategies, others list ten or twenty. This is not the
place to summarise this research, but to outline a simple model that
will inform our understanding of war trauma. It links closely with the
narrative approach taken in this book.
While there are complications, and many theories suggest a range of
coping mechanisms, it is argued here for the sake of simplicity that there
are two fundamental mechanisms for coping with traumatic recolle-
ctions: avoidance and processing. These are the main strategies we use
in everyday life, and the ones we use after a traumatic event. Most people
do most of their coping through avoidance; it is often easier not to think
about a problem and hope it will go away, and in the real world, it often
does. With psychological problems, including PTSD, effective avoid-
ance means that symptoms are rarely or never experienced, but there is
the potential for future problems arising as any trauma-related infor-
mation (e.g. cognitions, emotions) remains in the memory and is not
dealt with. Processing used by most of us for a minority of the time
concerns the active working through of problems, in our case traumatic
recollections. Traumatic memories that are worked through are turned
into narrative-explicit memories. Through the narrative, the individual
deals with the cognitions, emotions and behaviours associated with the
memory.
Both strategies work, but if a person usually employs an avoidant
strategy, traumatic memories may re-emerge many years, even decades,
later. As discussed above, researchers, including Creamer, Horowitz and
Integrating theory 79
Integrating theory
It should be clear from the above brief discussion that the different
perspectives in psychology can all contribute to our understanding of
traumatic memory and PTSD. While the core of the book is concerned
with narrative and we will move on to that area in detail in the next
chapter it is critical to acknowledge that an understanding of trauma
from any perspective including narrative must involve integrating
findings from other areas if we are to develop a coherent understanding
of traumatic stress.
It is unfortunate that, in many areas of psychology, researchers focus
on a particular theoretical perspective. The ease with which trauma can
be explained in terms of several perspectives says a great deal about
how trauma theory taps into the fundamental attributes of human
functioning, memory, coping, emotions, beliefs, implicit and explicit
functioning, and our response to stress, and how these are grounded in
80 Approaches to understanding trauma
Our greatest glory is not in never falling, but in rising every time we fall
Confucius
Terminology
In the relatively few years since research began in this area, a number
of terminological problems have developed. Researchers have used a
number of terms to represent the positive outcomes that occur after
traumatic experiences. Linley and Joseph (2004) list a number of these
81
82 Positive outcomes of traumatic experiences
and that needs at one level need to be satisfied before moving up to the
next level. At the bottom of this hierarchy there are needs for shelter and
food, and at the top there are special moments in life called peak experi-
ences, where, in words not used by Maslow, we experience positive
growth. There are many cases from the horrors of the twentieth century
that demonstrate growth, particularly from the Holocaust. Viktor Frankl
(1963/1984), a Holocaust survivor who wrote extensively about his
experiences and, in his terms, became a better person and a better
psychologist because of his experiences, discussed growth and the way
he managed to interpret his experiences over the following years and
decades and use these interpretations to help others.
What do we mean by growth? Is it simply a change after a traumatic
event that, in some cases, leads to a perceived better understanding
of the world in some way, or do people actually become happier or
more satisfied with life? Learning something about the world does not
necessarily make someone happier, so growth is not about happiness.
Fundamentally, the concern is with a changed perception regarding the
self a better understanding of self, others or the world. Growth implies
change towards a different (and assumed better) structure of the person-
ality and identity. The person who experiences growth after a traumatic
event will look at the world differently, perhaps in a less nave manner,
recognising for instance that the world is more complex than they
thought or less good, or that by seeing the vulnerability of people there
is a recognition of the importance of living ones life to the full, of not
wasting time on trivial activities, or gaining a positive outlook even when
carrying out the most trivial tasks. Many people look on others in a
different way, recognising them as people with rights and responsibilities.
There is no one area where people experience growth, but these are
some of the key aspects. The traumatic event makes a person re-evaluate
his or her life; that person tends to reflect on the traumatic event how
they and others behaved, thought and felt, and the implications of these.
Through this, fundamental change relating to the self can occur. This
may entail a changed philosophy of life, with the realisation perhaps
that the acquisition of ever-more wealth is unimportant compared with
having a good relationship with ones family and friends. The change of
perspective is fundamental. The person may realise that the sound of
birds in the morning, the rustle of the wind in the leaves, or the varying
nature of the earth around us, is much more important than social status
and wealth. This change occurs specifically because of the threat to life
that has been experienced.
Much of this may seem to be stating the obvious, but in reality many
of us living ordinary lives have a philosophy of life that is not thought
84 Positive outcomes of traumatic experiences
Assessing growth
There are a number of ways to assess post-traumatic growth, both
qualitative and quantitative. Most research has focused on questionnaire
measures, designed to assess particular aspects of growth, though quali-
tative approaches are also used.
There are a number of quantitative measures available, and while this
is not the place to review them, it is worth mentioning two that have been
used widely and to good effect. The Post-traumatic Growth Inventory
(PTGI) was devised by Tedeschi and Calhoun (1996) as a 21-item self-
report inventory that measures the persons perception of positive
change resulting from a traumatic life experience. People are asked to
use a 06 rating scale to measure the extent to which their views have
Evidence for growth 85
Spirituality
One area that is often neglected by psychotherapists and psychologists in
general is the role of spirituality. The mainstream monotheistic religions,
Christianity, Islam and Judaism, are rarely about personal growth
and more about appropriate behaviours for a better future in heaven.
Spirituality, on the other hand, while it is closely linked with belief in
god, is also concerned with growth and the development of meaning in
ones life. Spirituality is important for many people. It is notoriously
difficult to measure, as it is a complex construct. Spiritual issues often
come to the surface when people face serious crises. The link with post-
traumatic growth can be seen in the work of Tedeschi et al. (1998), who
include a new sense of spirituality or religion in their conception of post-
traumatic growth. This is quite rare; notions of spirituality (and religion)
Narrative growth 89
Narrative growth
Narrative is a useful concept that can help us to explain growth. As we
have seen in earlier chapters, if a person is going to resolve traumatic
problems that have arisen because of the fundamental rift that occurs as
a result of a traumatic event, then they need, if they are not going to
use an effective avoidant strategy, to develop a narrative of the event.
The strong argument here is that the development of an effective narra-
tive more or less guarantees that the person will experience growth. The
focus is on the word effective. Many narratives are not effective, and do
90 Positive outcomes of traumatic experiences
not enable someone to move on. A narrative may lack coherence, and
in so doing may be ineffective at helping the person come to terms with
their experiences.
As noted, one of the problems with the literature on psychological
growth is that it takes a relatively narrow focus, and attempts to quantify
growth through questionnaires, when growth is, by nature, qualitative
and personally meaningful. To measure growth is to ignore the key
meaningful elements of growth.
Neimeyer and his colleagues (Neimeyer, 2005, 2006a, b; Neimeyer
and Levitt, 2001) have discussed the notion of post-traumatic growth
through narration. Post-traumatic growth is seen as a form of meaning
reconstruction in the wake of crisis and loss (Neimeyer and Levitt, 2001).
Neimeyer does believe that a narrative approach to understanding growth
is little utilised, but is capable of being among the richest literatures in
the field of growth. This is, in part, because of our natural penchant
for both telling stories and for being an audience, as already discussed.
This kind of evidence should link the literature on social support and
on narrative as all stories need a listener but there is little evidence for
this link. Few authors have explicitly discussed how it is that social
support is usually the best predictor of recovery from a traumatic event
and the role of narrative in such recovery. This highlights a problem
with the social support literature. Much of it is based on crude measures
of support, whereas its effects may be best identified through the study of
narrative. If this link was made explicit, then we are likely to provide a
much better explanation for the role of social support. This notion is
hinted at by Neimeyer (2006b) when he discusses how considering
narrative at social levels can establish the context for post-traumatic
stress or for growth not only at the level of the individual but also at
the level of society as a whole.
Neimeyer (2006a) discusses several forms of narrative disruption,
including disorganised narratives, where the individual is immersed in
the perceptual elements of the traumatic experience and is unable to
draw them together (this is the area most closely associated with work on
post-traumatic growth), dissociated narratives, which are silent stories,
those in which the person is unable to relate in the social situation, such
as the suicide of a spouse, and finally dominant narratives, which are, in a
sense, too cohesive in providing an explanation for the traumatic event.
A healthy profile of a person post trauma is one in which such narrative
disruptions either do not take place, or they take place temporarily, only
to be displaced by a new, more effective and growthful narrative.
According to Neimeyer (2006b), healthy profiles of post-loss adaptation
concur with the view that resilient survivors are able to assimilate their
Narrative growth 91
loss into their existing narratives in a way that does not radically alter or
undermine the way they look at life, the key themes of their life story.
Neimeyer (2005), in a discussion on bereavement, argued that accom-
modation is also important, with bereaved individuals struggling to
accommodate their self-narratives to integrate their loss, and also
accommodating their lives to the changes necessary to adapt to the loss.
According to Neimeyer, this interpretation of assimilation and adapta-
tion may help as a heuristic by which we can understand the multiple
pathways people negotiate bereavement.
There are a number of methods by which people have studied narra-
tive and growth, e.g. repertory grid (Fransella et al., 2004), where
biographical grids are constructed to help the person articulate their
key life themes, systematically comparing critical life events. These
critical life events are not just related to the trauma; they are other key
aspects of life. For instance, if a person has a theme of helplessness/
control, then by applying this to different events, e.g. getting married or
being at work, and to the traumatic event, the person can work out the
relationships between the key themes and these events, and see which
events are linked, and how they are linked across themes. This may be of
benefit in helping a traumatised person to make sense of how that
persons traumatic events fit within his/her larger life story. Another
approach, widely used in different forms, both in research and clinically,
is to encourage the person to reflect on the key events within his/her life,
and then to build a narrative picture of that persons life from this. Each
key event is a narrative or a chapter of the life story in itself, and the
individual is encouraged to link these together to establish important life
themes and to enable the key events surrounding the trauma to be
incorporated. A further approach, which has varying levels of support,
is that of Pennebaker (1997), which involves the individual writing about
their experiences for 20 minutes a day for 3 days. According to Pennebaker,
this procedure is effective in reducing symptoms, and contributes towards
the development of a meaningful narrative. The evidence for this is
mixed, and different forms of the paradigm have been employed.
Clinicians and researchers regularly encourage traumatised or
bereaved people to develop narratives about their experiences, to help
them make sense of, and hopefully learn from, their experiences.
Guidano (1995) has proposed a method whereby emotionally discrepant
episodes in a persons life are replayed in a slow motion fashion, focusing
on the difficult, emotional and painful details, and then panning out to
consider the impact of these episodes on the persons broader life. The
idea is that this method will help the client to make sense of his/her
experiences, to close the gap between experience and explanation.
92 Positive outcomes of traumatic experiences
Resilience
According to Meichenbaum (2006), self- and group-narratives play a
critical role in determining whether individuals and groups display
persistent distress and PTSD or whether they will evidence resilience
and post-traumatic growth. Meichenbaum takes a constructive narrative
perspective of the concepts of resilience and post-traumatic growth. This
perspective has five key points:
(1) People are storytellers.
(2) The type of story used by individuals and groups following a
traumatic event determines the level of distress versus resilience.
(3) Research indicates the specific features of negative behaviour and
thinking that lead to PTSD.
(4) Healing activities work because they enable individuals and groups
to engage in non-negative thinking.
(5) To move from resilience to post-traumatic growth, individuals
and groups must find benefits, establish a future orientation and
construct meaning.
This kind of model is helpful in demonstrating the processes that occur
in people after a traumatic event. The concept of resilience is helpful
because it shows how people can show positive adaptation and not break
when exposed to adversity. Meichenbaum proposes that resilience is not
an inbuilt personality trait, but a skill that can be developed. He draws
on an American Psychological Association Help Centre that suggests
ten ways to build resilience:
(1) Make connections.
(2) Avoid seeing a crisis as an insurmountable problem.
(3) Accept that change is part of living.
(4) Move towards goals, but stay flexible.
(5) Take decisive action.
(6) Look for opportunities for self-discovery.
(7) Nurture a positive view of oneself.
(8) Keep things in perspective and learn from the past.
(9) Maintain a hopeful outlook.
(10) Take care of yourself.
Meichenbaums constructive narrative approach builds on our under-
standing of narrative that people live by storytelling and listening
to others stories and incorporates the notion of resilience, building to
a demonstration of what is meant by growth, which includes benefit
seeking, finding and reminding for self and others, engaging in downward
Issues surrounding post-traumatic growth concepts 93
Conclusions
The notion of post-traumatic growth has become very popular over the
last few years, and research in the area shows no sign of abating. There is
evidence that, after a traumatic event, people learn and experience
growth. This is demonstrated in the novels that emerge out of war, or
the non-fictional accounts written by people who have been through
Conclusions 95
terrible events and have somehow learned to live with their memories.
After a burgeoning of psychological work in the 1960s and beyond that
looked at humanistic and existential approaches to psychotherapy there
is, at least within mainstream psychology, a mechanical and reductionist
approach to much of the work on growth. This is seen particularly in the
various questionnaires that are meant to measure growth. They take a
complex concept and break it down into parts that are less than the sum
of the whole. The narrative perspective does enable us to address issues
of meaning-making and story development within the context of growth,
and enables a deeper understanding of exactly what we mean by growth.
Finally, we should note that many people do not experience growth at
all, nor do they experience PTSD. Many, perhaps most, people just carry
on with their lives. The factors that predict how and whether someone
will experience growth are not well understood. Perhaps as psycholo-
gists, as clinicians, as academics generally, we should stand back and
think about what happens to those people who neither experience deep
distress nor narrate their experiences. They carry on with their lives, but
perhaps with an enhanced wisdom that they have learned from their
experiences. I spoke with a war veteran who had been captured at Calais
in May 1940, served down the mines as a prisoner in Silesia, and been on
the forced marches in 1945 away from the advancing Russian lines.
He said that whenever he becomes emotional about the war, he will go
out into his vegetable garden, tend the plants and realise that what is
important about the world is the everyday things growing onions, the
changing seasons and life itself. Did he learn this at least partly through
his traumatic experiences, or did he just know it because he was brought
up and lived his whole life in a village?
7 Memory and history
This chapter and the next form the heart of the thesis in the book. This
chapter focuses on the role history plays in understanding the psycho-
social response to war trauma, while the next chapter focuses on the role
social factors play in psychological understanding, building together to
develop the concept of the personal narrative. The arguments can be
expressed from a variety of cultural perspectives, but due to my own
upbringing and background, the examples that are used to illustrate
what is meant by memory, history, social discourse and narrative are
drawn largely from my own experiences of being brought up English and
European.
The focus here is on the psychological perspective. We are trying to
understand at the level of the individual, but we are all socio-cultural
creatures, so in order to understand the individual effectively we need to
draw on broader social and cultural forces so as to understand how a
person interprets the past and the present, and how that person looks to
the future. If we want to understand psychological processes fully, we
need to be able to separate the universal from the cultural aspects. What
are the key universal psychological processes about memory, coping and
the response to stressful events, and what are the cultural aspects of
these? This is intensely relevant to trauma studies, but is also relevant to
the ways in which all of us live our lives as social beings. There are two
fundamental truths about humans that are relevant here. First, that
without memory we are nothing. If we do not remember the past we
have no structured life. Second, that we are social animals. If we lived in
an isolated world we would not learn; we would not have any sense of
structure in our lives. We would not have life.
Definitions
Definitions are necessary here because there is dispute about the mean-
ing of many of the terms used. This largely arises because they are used
in different ways across disciplines.
96
Definitions 97
fixing the past in aspic. From the example of the Paradores, Spain uses
the past to look to the future, illustrating how memory (living, breathing,
personal and social) has the ability to remember what is important about
the past, and to make use of this in a way that is beneficial to the present
and the future. History in the shape of heritage - fails to do this. This
does, to some extent, support the views of Nora (1989), who is very anti-
history, arguing against the terrorism of historicised history (p. 14),
yet highly supportive of memory. Heritage should be contrasted with
academic historians, who debate the meaning of the past in ways pertin-
ent to the present, attempting to provide a historical context that is
meaningful in the terms of both past and current society.
into history, or how these are all transformed into heritage are really
beyond the scope of this book, though they have relevance for our under-
standing of the past.
An illustration of the transition from individual to collective memory
can be seen in the ways that novels emerge. Immediately after major
wars, many accounts, including fictional accounts, were written by the
participants (not just soldiers). For about 1520 years, very few novels
emerge; then, once most of the participants are dead, there is another
flurry of novel writing. This can be seen in relation to the First World
War through novels written after the war by Erich Maria Remarque,
Henri Barbusse, Rebecca West, Siegfried Sassoon and others. There was
then a period when fewer novels were written, and it was only after most
participants were dead that WWI novels again became popular, through
work by Pat Barker, Sebastian Faulkes and others.
Social continuity
Collective memory ensures continuity in a community. It is the way in
which we preserve our collective knowledge and pass it on from one
generation to the next. This enables future generations to construct their
own personal and social identities, constructing the present by building
on the past. Having social or collective memories ensures that members
of a community share a sense of unity. This can work at all levels, from
the village in which all men once worked in the same pit, and so have
shared experiences, to the country that has a shared memory of the role
it played in the Second World War. Individuals are linked to social
memories. Most people in England will have knowledge of individual
members of their families who fought in the First and Second World
Wars. Most communities have war memorials which show how individ-
uals from that community shared in the common social goals repre-
sented by those wars.
This sense of social identity is important; it is more important than
having accurate objective memories of the past. In terms of the Second
World War, it is more important to have had a member of the family who
fought in that war and so contributed towards the greater good than to
know that perhaps that person was a coward or a thief who stole from his
comrades and ran away from the enemy. During World War Two, the
British displayed the stiff upper lip, the ability to stand firm against all
adversity. Of course, at one level this was nonsense people broke down,
they failed to cope but at another it was a necessary idea, a necessary
collective ideal that helped the nation through the difficult years of the
war; and after the war it became a collective memory. People looked
106 Memory and history
back and thought that they pulled through those years at least partly
because they had a stiff upper lip. This was portrayed in many of the
war films that emerged during and after the war, such as Dambusters,
Reach for the Sky or The Longest Day (USA).
We reconstruct the past in ways we wish to reconstruct it, with little
consideration for any objective truth. Collective memory does not
involve testimony about the truth. This is even the case in a courtroom,
which is an entirely artificial situation with regard to memory, where
witnesses are expected to recall the past with a degree of accuracy and
details that as psychologists we know is hardly possible. Witnesses
statements will also be influenced by the groups they are in, by the police
officers and lawyers who question them, and by the narratives they have
constructed about the events in question.
Our identities are tied up with memory and history. Without memory
we have no identity. In order to create our identities we draw on cultural
memories and historical understanding of our cultures. Remembrance
of the past is important in terms of our socialisation into our culture.
Family traditions are also important in this.
Oral history
Oral history hovers between academic history and collective memory in
our attempts to understand the past. Modern oral history approaches are
a shift back towards the tradition of collective memory rather than the
objectification of the past in traditional academic history. It is an accept-
ance that what people actually say about their pasts is meaningful and
relevant. The journal Oral History has done a great deal in highlighting
the history of the ordinary person, as opposed to the faceless but
Politics, memory and history 109
well as the dead, to remember the veterans who fought in wars and
survived, whether wounded or not, and to remember the families who
survived bereavement and loss. This notion of remembrance is perhaps
easier when we are looking back into the more distant past. After the
First World War, and less so after the Second World War, the shock of
loss focused society on the dead. In recent years we do now recognise the
need to remember the living.
Conclusion
The central constructs discussed in this chapter history and collective
memory are interpreted in different ways by academics of a number
of disciplines. The purpose of the chapter was to provide an overview of
the constructs as they are used in this book, as they are used for the
purpose of broadening our psychological understanding of the individ-
ual response to traumatic events. This chapter should not be read as a
criticism of the discipline of history; no such criticism is intended. The
intention was to highlight how history detracts from our full understand-
ing of how memory works in the social world and across generations.
Memory is crucial. People need to remember the past in order to have a
successful present and future, in order to be able to interpret what has
happened to them in the past. This is not just an individual process; it is,
as Halbwachs would argue, a social process. We turn to this interaction
between the individual and the social in the next chapter.
8 Personal narrative and social discourse
The previous chapter demonstrated the role and importance of the past
in enabling individual understanding. This chapter moves to the pre-
sent to show how the social world plays a crucial role in determining
how we think about ourselves and the world. In order to develop a
complete theory of the response to traumatic events, it is critical to
consider the response, not only from the psychological perspective, but
also the broader social world. War trauma is not an individual disorder.
It arises out of a complex interaction of personal, social, cultural,
historical and political forces the relationship between the personal
narrative and social discourse building on the discussion in the
previous chapter.
The personal narrative has already been touched on, but here it
is considered in more detail, particularly with regard to the role of
narratives and how they are developed. William James (1890, 2007)
was one of the first to establish psychology as the science of the mind,
which since has mainly been conceived as the science of the individual
mind. More recently, researchers have recognised that the attribution
of psychological traits is not the exclusive domain of psychology and
the cognitive sciences (Wilson, 2005), and that other disciplines con-
tribute to our understanding of the mind simply because the mind is
socially and culturally situated, and cannot be seen outside of that
social context. Attempting to understand the individual mind without
recourse to the social aspect is always going to limit the efficacy of any
theory.
The key point is that there is a constant interaction between individual
(narrative) and social forces; the individual draws on the social context in
order to make sense of the world, and the social context itself is derived
from a range of sources, including the media, academic argument (e.g.
in history or sociology), government policy and other sources where
individuals interact. The flexibility and fluidity of narrative and individ-
ual narrative change is crucial. This relates strongly to the development
and treatment of war trauma.
114
Narrative 115
Narrative
As discussed in the last chapter, humans are prolific storytellers; it is
what we do best. In low-technology societies, the epic poet would keep
the past alive through reciting the deeds of great people in the society,
providing the context for current discourses and showing how people
within a society should live. Even now, we use stories in a vast number of
contexts: from entertainment whether telling a child a bedtime story or
reading a novel to developing understanding of the world by listening
to news stories or reading magazine and journal articles, to gossiping to
each other about the latest events and about friends and acquaintances,
to the more detailed stories we tell ourselves and others about our lives,
the stories that are our autobiographies life stories should be pluralised
because we have alternative versions for different people and situations.
I have one narrative of my past that I would share with my students, but
a very different one I might share with my close friends.
Narrative is necessarily complex; a good story has a number of
elements. It must have a sense of time, with one event following another
in a logical sequence; the events themselves play a key role. They are
included because they have some importance. There is a context, with
each event being set in a particular situation for a good reason. There is
the format of the narrative whether it is oral or written, and in which
style it is presented. There is cohesion, so that the story hangs together in
a meaningful way. And finally, there is the important notion of agency.
The person who is telling the story owns it; it is that persons story to
present as he/she wishes to whoever he/she wishes. This then links to the
plurality of narrative. We all have numerous narratives, many intercon-
nected, that we use with different audiences at different times.
The narration of experience is not only very widespread, it does
appear to be essential for health, and that health gains can be made as
a result of such narration (Pennebaker and Graybeal, 2001). Storytelling
is not optional it is something we have to do; we are compelled by our
nature to create narratives. According to Gergen and Gergen (1988), we
select and organise our personal memories to build a coherent sense of
the self and establish and maintain our identities (see also MacIntyre,
1984). Wrye (1994) noted that personal stories are not just about telling
stories; they are the means by which identities can be fashioned and
developed. People need to make meaningful sense of their experiences
through the use of language and stories. This relationship between
narrative, self and identity is central to our understanding of the
response to trauma, and links with the social constructions that help to
build notions of self and identity. According to Squire (2000), narrative
116 Personal narrative and social discourse
research helps us to explore how the self is developed out of our cultural
resources (or social constructs, see below), and how we believe that our
lives need to have a certain shape or structure (narrative) which has
personal identity at the core. In this sense, narrative is the performance
of the self in the sense of self-identity.
Kihlstrom (2002) argues that memories are reconstructed in accord-
ance with our theories of self our personal views regarding who we are
and how we got to be that way. Autobiographical memory (how we
remember our own past) is a part of the personal narrative, reflecting
our views about ourselves. These views do not (as Freud claimed)
necessarily derive from our childhood experiences; this is a nave per-
spective given the importance of the social world. A more interesting
perspective is that of Adler (1958), who proposed that memories of
childhood can change if the current personal narrative changes. This is
because childhood memories remind us of who we are now. In other
words, as Ross (1994) argued, people, through narrative, reconstruct
their personal histories around their tacit theories of the self, revising
their histories as their self-concepts (narratives) change.
As we have seen, narratives are systems of personal knowledge
(Crossley, 2000). The turn to narrative in psychology (e.g. Bruner,
1990), provided an important opportunity to develop our understanding
of memory processes. Narrative involves the attempt to develop a coher-
ent past, which involves over-emphasising the role of some events (mem-
ories) and under-emphasising others (perhaps memories about which we
are embarrassed, or which do not fit our current conceptions of our-
selves). Narrative is about making sense of our lives, integrating the past
with the present with a view to how we intend to live our lives in the
future. Because our lives can change, either dramatically, or in a slow
evolutionary manner, we are constantly updating our personal narra-
tives. This may involve confabulation (Bartlett, 1932), or selective
remembering and forgetting. Other factors may also impinge, such as
personality (Pals, 2006), coping styles, social support, our friends and
family (Burnell et al., 2006), age (Coleman, 2005), personal experience
and the social world.
Research into narrative can help us to develop our understanding of
memory. Narrative researchers have explored how memory works in a
number of fields. Psychologists have focused on the impact of traumatic
stress, and how individuals learn to cope with their memories by cogni-
tively processing emotional content through narrative, attempting to
understand their trauma by making it meaningful (Linley and Joseph,
2006; Ness and Macaskill, 2003). Twentieth-century examples include
Simon Westons charitable work after being heavily scarred in the
Narrative 117
Falklands War, Oliver Stones dealing with his own Vietnam experience
through the production of a series of Vietnam War films or Pablo
Picassos representation of the bombing of Guernica during the Spanish
Civil War. Creating a coherent story about a traumatic event is essential
to trauma recovery (Herman, 1992; Brewin et al., 1996). Such narratives
may provide positive benefits for the individual, particularly in terms of
helping to resolve traumatic issues through narrative development.
Narratives may or may not correspond to reality. There was a debate
in the 1990s regarding recovered memories, where it appeared that
traumatised patients were, with the help of therapists, retrieving sup-
pressed stories of child sex abuse which, it was claimed, was the cause of
the person being traumatised. Many cases came to court, and parents
and other carers were convicted of sex abuse largely on the basis of
stories that emerged in therapeutic sessions. The therapeutic environ-
ment is one in which vulnerable patients can be induced, through
discussion with therapists, to develop a rationale for their problems,
and this rationale can be based on entirely fabricated memories. This
is not to say therapists deliberately attempt to manipulate memories,
but that, through conversation, patients find ways to explain their
feelings, and these explanations can be non-consciously fabricated.
The recovered memory debate is largely over, but is an effective demon-
stration of not only how trauma narratives may not be objectively true,
but also how within the profession social constructions (in this case
recovered memories) can dominate at a particular time.
Knapp (1988), discussing the context of biblical stories, asked why it
should ever matter whether a narrative corresponds to reality. This is
relevant to the current discussion because, except for certain contexts
(e.g. law) where we do want a narrative to be accurate, in other contexts
objective truth is irrelevant. It does not matter whether biblical stories
are an accurate representation of the past; what matters is that there is,
for Christians, a foundation for their faith. In the context of war trauma
narratives the situation is complex. On the one hand, when treating
victims of war trauma, it is more important to reduce symptomatology,
and if this means some fabrication within the narrative, then that can be
of little importance though the case above of recovered memories
shows how this can go wrong; on the other hand, if victims narratives
are potentially going to be used as witness testimony (e.g. narrative
exposure therapy, Shauer et al., 2005), then the narratives should be as
close to the truth as possible.
In everyday life these errors and distortions are common. In a sense
we all go around hiding the truth (though it may be portrayed as
projecting the right image). This is the case in the narratives we call
118 Personal narrative and social discourse
British people were broadly similar. Now our social discourse of the
Empire has led to changes in the representations of the past through
both history and memory. Historians write of the tragedies which befell
other civilisations when the white man came, and the social memories of
younger people if they include the Empire at all look back negatively,
and see a time of oppression. In this way we can see how memory and
history develop together, how individual social memories of the past are
built on current social representations, and how historians write using
those same social representations.
We can go back 100 years in relation to the British Empire by looking
at the Empire of the USA today. Social representations in the USA now
are broadly similar to those in Britain 100 years ago. Then we thought
we could export our culture to the world; now the USA does the same.
Then we thought we were more advanced than other people; now the
USA thinks the same. Then we used guns to support our position; now
the USA does the same.
Narrative development
As we have seen (Spiro et al., 1994), memories consist of individual
recollections of the past, which may or may not be accurate, which may
or may not be drawn together to form an individual narrative about the
past that enhances, enlightens and supports an individuals personal self-
image. Narratives consist of recollections of individual experience that
has been affected by social and cultural influences such as the media,
family and friends, and also by self-education and development. We
deliberately choose to emphasise certain memories and try to forget
others. This is in order that we have coherence in our lives (e.g. Singer
and Rexhaj, 2006). We try to forget those things in the past which we
perceive as bad and which do not fit our current concepts of the self
(Kihlstrom et al., 2002). Just as remembering is important in under-
standing our narratives, so is forgetting. It is as important to forget as it is
to remember. Just as a novel does not display all the characteristics of the
main protagonists, our sense of self does not incorporate everything we
know about ourself, particularly when it involves projecting that sense of
self outward to family, friends and work colleagues.
This life story, or autobiography, is critical to understanding memory,
and it is conceptualised into a narrative (or narratives). We all have many
thousands of memories, and many ways of interpreting our memories.
We cannot incorporate them all into our life story. We deliberately select
particular elements of our past that fit with the way we want to perceive
and be perceived in society. There are many factors that will impact on
126 Personal narrative and social discourse
Narrative coherence
What makes a story coherent? Many people have highlighted the signifi-
cance of coherence as a vital characteristic of a good story, both
theoretically and empirically. Examples range from students of literature
to psychologists. In all fields, a good story is a good story. Linde (1993)
defines narrative coherence as a property of the text, which represents
the relationship between parts of the text and, consequently, the overall
text. Coleman (1999) takes this further by suggesting that coherence
comprises subjective truths, interpretations, emotions, unity/integration,
purpose and meaning as characteristics. Coherence is a social obligation
that must be fulfilled in order for the participants to appear as competent
members of their culture. The necessity for an audience to understand
the story has implications for identity, problem solving and sharing.
In the case of war trauma, veterans must have an audience to provide
support for the processing of traumatic memories, which leads to
reconciliation.
Index Description
Orientation The narrative introduces the main characters and locates the story in
a specific temporal, social and personal context. The narrative describes
the habitual circumstances that serve as the parameters for the action
of the story.
Structure The narrative displays the structural elements of an episode system. Thus,
the narrative has at least one of the following: an initiating event; an
internal response to this event (e.g. a goal, a plan, thought, feeling); an
attempt (e.g. to reach a goal, carry out a plan, remedy a crisis, resolve
a state of emotional disequilibrium); and a consequence. These elements
are presented in a causally and temporally logical way (e.g. the initiating
event precedes the response, which in turn precedes the attempt).
Affect The narrative reveals something about the narrator, or about what the
events described therein mean to the narrator; the narrative makes an
evaluative point. The narrative uses emotion in order to make this
evaluative point, employing explicit statements of feeling in order to
create an affective tone or signify emotional meaning. Thus, the narrative
uses tension, drama, humour or pathos to communicate and emphasise
the evaluative point.
Integration The narrative communicates information in an integrated manner,
expressing the meaning of the experiences described within the context
of the larger story. Discrepancies, contradictions and inconsistencies are
eventually resolved, and the various narrative elements are synthesised
into a unified life story. Although complexity, ambiguity and
differentiation may be used to indicate suspense, conflict or growth, the
narrative ultimately reconciles these disparate story elements with one
another.
Note: This is an adapted version of the original table presented by Baerger and McAdams
(1999).
Participants
The model was tested using samples of British war veterans. It was first
applied to narratives of ten Second World War veterans. After this initial
test, in which it did not need to be modified, it was applied to the
narratives of twelve post-war veterans of wars and conflicts, including
Suez, Korea, Aden, Cyprus, Northern Ireland, Falklands (Malvinas),
the Gulf and Iraq. The use of different war veteran groups demonstrates
the applicability of the model to different groups.
Transcripts
The interviews were transcribed in full, including non-verbal utterances
(such as ah, oh, er). Brief intervals between utterances are marked
with . . . and longer intervals marked in seconds, e.g. [3]. All quota-
tions are anonymous, but each participant is identified by his/her initials,
and quotations note the line number(s) of the transcript.
For further details of the analysis and coding, please refer to Burnell
et al. (2007).
Orientation and structure 133
Coding
Table 2 describes the proposed coding criteria.
Affect
Affect and integration are vital aspects of narrative coherence because
they highlight complexities of the narrative. If stories are perceived as
being too simple, they cannot realistically reflect lived experience. Emo-
tional evaluation also reflects the ability to express congruent emotion of
traumatic events, which is especially indicative of reconciliation.
The affect criterion (A4) was influenced by Baerger and McAdams
(1999), which captures the emotional evaluation contained within
the narrative. A4 is only applied to explicit statements of emotion.
A5 represents Androutsopoulou et al.s concern with unreconciled
emotion displayed either verbally or non-verbally. From initial applica-
tion of these criteria to the interviews, a number of emotional
Integration 135
Integration
Integration represents the last element of narrative coherence, and is
split into three levels: the presence of a uniting theme (Criterion I6),
explanation or absence of contradictions (Criterion I7), and absence of
fragmentation and disorganisation (Criterion I8).
Criterion I6 is a necessary criterion as it relates to the presence of a
theme within the narrative, which brings elements of the narrative
together in a meaningful way, and advances lived experience in a way
that could provide a meaningful message for future generations. From
the perspective of reconciled war trauma, a theme is necessary because it
gives meaning to events that challenge the coherence of the life story.
Criterion I6 represents global coherence, and provides a means of deter-
mining the extent to which war experiences have been integrated into the
life story.
This criterion was present across age groups and cohorts as demon-
strated by one Falklands (Malvinas) War veteran. Aged 40 years at
the time of the study, this veteran explained how his experience in
the Falklands had a positive impact on the way he felt about everyday
life, and in this sense the experience is integrated as a beneficial
experience. There is emotional evaluation in this excerpt. However, it
136 Illustrating narrative as a scientific technique: the role of social support
falls into the I6 category rather than the A4 category because he indi-
cates how his Falklands War experience has affected subsequent percep-
tions of life:
I often say [2] I say to people that know . . . and who would have some kind of
understanding having been in the . . . in similar circumstances [2] that it was the
best and the worst time of my life [5] erm and I say that because [3] the worst
time because of [3] people getting killed erm [2] erm . . . things that you see [1]
but the best of times because erm . . . life is so much simpler [5] it really doesnt
matter whether you [1] whether youre all overdrawn thousands of pounds . . .
what kind of car you drive [2] erm [2] whether youre married . . . thats different . . .
no [1] but material things and the [1] minutiae of sort of daily life . . . it
doesnt . . . it doesnt even come into it [1] its life or death . . . and that
makes life very simple . . . its very pure [3] and each day is [1] fantastic . . .
you know . . . you can wake up and it can be absolutely chucking it down [1]
and er [3] you know you feel it may be cold [1] but . . . you think . . . well . . .
you know . . . its another day . . . everyone is around me . . . and [3] and
you have that comradeship [3] but ah [1] it made [3] life that much
sweeter . . . [DW]
feelings towards, serving in the RAF are incongruous, but this is not
explained or recognised by the veteran in the narrative, resulting in
contradictory personal beliefs:
. . . yeah . . . I enjoyed flying . . . it never bothered me . . . its a job . . . just a job . . .
work which I was being paid for . . . ah . . . it wasnt my main love . . . my main
love was football . . . ah [2] but the RAF hadnt paid me to play football [1]
[laughs] [1] [WS]
. . . I joined up to fly for here . . . from Britain . . . to defend my Mum . . . and
Dad . . . thats . . . the reason behind my joining up [1] I thought if peoples going
to drop bombs on Mum . . . Im going to . . . do my damnedest for them . . . and
thats my simple thinking in 1940 . . . [WS]
Finally, Criterion I8, which identifies fragmentation and disorganisation
within the narrative, was included in order to combine findings from
clinical trauma narrative studies within the narrative analysis. This cri-
terion represents symptoms of unreconciled trauma. At the local level,
veterans were perceived as having a fragmented narrative if there was
broken speech, unfinished sentences and long pauses, or if it contained
incongruent, but not contradictory, material. At the global level, I8 was
also defined as incongruent (but not contradictory) information within
the context of the larger narrative.
In order to be considered a coherent narrative, all criteria must be
present within the narrative; no one criterion was perceived as being more
important than another. While this may seem contentious, all elements
of the narrative must be present in order for it to be classified as a
narrative. Future research can identify which elements are relatively more
important for a coherent narrative. Here, if a criterion was absent, the
narrative would be considered incoherent. Without the narrative orienta-
tion and structure, the veteran cannot capture a supportive audience
to aid reconciliation, and without this audience, consistent emotional
evaluation and integration are more difficult to achieve. As noted earlier,
after applying the model to narratives of Second World War veterans,
all criteria within the model were successfully applied to the post-war
veterans narratives without needing to be adapted or supplemented
with additional criteria. This demonstrates the models transferability
and effectiveness in assessing narrative coherence across age and cohort.
The model was able to differentiate between coherent and incoherent
narratives; both coherent and incoherent narratives were present in the
sample. The differentiation of coherence linked to the thematic analysis
of narrative content concerning the presence or absence of traumatic
war memories. The overall pattern in the data indicated that narrative
content of coherent narratives explicitly stated that there was no trauma,
whereas the content of incoherent narratives contained themes of
138 Illustrating narrative as a scientific technique: the role of social support
Case studies
In the broader context of the data analysis, narrative coherence was
presented within qualitative case studies for each participant. These case
studies included thematic analysis of perceptions and nature of war
experience, and analysis of experiences of social support (as well as the
presence of traumatic memories). The combination of these two levels of
analysis allows for the exploration of potential relationships between the
types of social support that are associated with narrative coherence. This
potential causality has interesting therapeutic implications.
By interviewing veterans of different wars and age groups, knowledge
about the ways in which traumatic war memories are reconciled at
different stages in life, and by different war cohorts, can be developed.
Also, the generic model can be applied to the reconciliation of traumatic
memories resulting from other traumatic experiences, not only war.
authors (e.g. van der Kolk and Fisler, 1995; Brewin et al., 1996), with
narrative development transferring sensory-activated memories to
verbal-activated memories, thus increasing voluntary control over the
memories (similar to Janets description of trauma resolution consisting
of turning trauma memories into narrative memories). This approach
brings together Bruners distinct narrative and paradigmatic cognitive
ways of knowing within one research approach narrative affects cogni-
tion, and vice versa (Bruner, 1986).
There are several benefits to this approach. The main one is that it is a
systematic procedure applied to interview transcripts that appear to
predict the effectiveness of narrative development in the light of social
support. Much more work needs to be carried out in this area, but it is a
substantive framework for further research, both with war veterans and
other traumatised groups, and with both the role of social support and
other key variables. In the end, this should help to provide more effective
ways to facilitate reconciliation than can be carried out using the natural
process of meaning-making. Equally, it can contribute to an initial
assessment of veterans in therapy sessions, which can provide knowledge
of the veterans social support networks, and aspects of the narrative
coherence that are absent.
10 Ageing, trauma and memory
I used to think what landmines have I tripped over now . . . Theres nothing
to be seen but if you tread on the bugger it goes up.
Normandy veteran, 1994
This chapter examines the very long-term effects of war. Central to this
is a study I carried out a few years ago with WWII veterans (Hunt, 1997;
Hunt and Robbins, 2001a, b). It addresses the extent and nature of war-
related distress experienced by the veteran population, and the factors
that predict such distress. The effects of traumatic stress on the older
population are exacerbated by developmental changes, which in them-
selves are stressors. These include diminished sensory capacity, reduced
mobility, frailty, reduced income and social status due to retirement, loss
of friends and subsequent isolation, ill health and reduced self-care
(Cook, 2001), though many people do not experience significant decline
in their cognitive abilities, and many develop special expertises. Major
physical and mental decline often does not occur until very advanced age
(Coleman, 1999).
War has very long-term or permanent effects (Hunt, 1997; Spiro et al.,
1994; Bramsen and van der Ploeg, 1999), though some of the symptoms
may be less marked than in younger people (Fontana and Rosenheck,
1998). For instance, dissociation may be less persistent over time
(Yehuda et al., 1995). There may also be complications involving
coexistent syndromes, or different patterns of symptoms; for instance,
Goenjian et al. (1994), in a group of earthquake survivors with PTSD,
found higher arousal levels and lower levels of intrusion, though the
overall PTSD severity was similar. There is also some evidence of older
adults having protective mediating variables, for example appraisals of
the desirable and undesirable effects of trauma respectively decreasing
and increasing the relationship between combat exposure and PTSD
(Aldwin et al., 1994).
People who experience war often claim that the memories they have
when they get old are as strong as they ever were, or even that they
140
Ageing, trauma and memory 141
become stronger. This links with Butlers (1963) notion of the life
review. As we age we look back on our lives, particularly the key areas,
and attempt to make sense of it, i.e. we actively work on the narrative of
our lives. Inevitably, we focus on the more difficult areas, which, for
many old people in the UK now, are the war years. This links to
Eriksons (1982) need for personal integration as part of normal ageing.
Many people still have problems many decades after their war experi-
ences, it being estimated that in the 1990s up to 10% of the older
population may still have been suffering from earlier (mainly war-
related) traumatic experience (Hunt et al., 1997). What we dont know
is whether treatment at the time of the war would have been of any long-
term benefit, or whether these problems would have continued to
emerge as the veterans got older.
Various treatment models have been developed or adapted for
older veterans. For instance, Robbins (1997) developed a treatment
model based on his experiences treating Second World War veterans.
Robbins recognised the emotional power of the traumatic memories,
and their potential to overwhelm the therapist. The framework for
treatment was developed to enable the therapist to cope with these
emotions and his/her own responses to them. The treatment consists
of four phases. The first phase, disclosure of events, is carried out in
two stages getting an overall picture of what happened, followed by a
detailed review of events to clarify any confusion and identify dysfunc-
tional cognitions and the emotions associated with them. The second
phase is an exploration of the cognitions and emotions associated with
the events. The therapist and the individual work together to identify
specific key issues or themes, and the link between past events and
current thoughts and feelings is established. The third stage is behav-
ioural change, working with the individual to establish ways to enable
change and improved coping. This might involve discussing problems
with relatives or learning anxiety management techniques. The fourth
and final stage critical to the process is termination. This is partly
about the end of treatment, but is also about the individual taking
responsibility for future planning. Issues around loss, future contact
and follow-up are discussed and agreed. This treatment model is
not dissimilar to other models, but takes into account the special
circumstances of traumatised people, and also the impact of ageing.
As Robbins (1997) notes, the impact of ageing and loss of work
status (see also Ehrlich, 1988) can lead to feelings of helplessness,
which is reinforced by infirmity. Such helplessness, for those who
were ex-POWs, can reawaken emotions associated with captivity (see
also Elder and Clipp, 1989).
142 Ageing, trauma and memory
Questionnaire
The questionnaire consisted of several sections: biographical informa-
tion, which asked questions regarding history of illness and details of war
service; combat experience, which was developed to provide a valid and
easily completed measure of the amount and severity of combat an
individual has experienced; the Impact of Event Scale (IES) (Horowitz
et al., 1979); the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ) (Goldberg,
1978); and several open-ended questions.
Participants
There were 731 veterans from the Second World War (n 657, 90%)
and the Korean War (n 22, 3%), with n 52 (7%) taking part in both
wars, with a mean age of 72.4 years (range 5989, sd 4.2) when the
study was conducted. Nearly all (97%) were retired. They represented
the range of armed forces: 53% had served in the British Army, 11% in
the RAF, 27% in the Royal Navy, and 6% in the Royal Marines. Veterans
were asked to provide their highest rank. This was recoded in three
categories: officers (n 125, 17%), NCOs (n 346, 47%) and Privates
(n 234, 32%). Many of the sample were ex-POWs (n 105, 14%).
Findings
Nineteen per cent (139 individuals) scored above the cut-off points on
both the GHQ and the IES, indicating possible war-related psychiatric
caseness. No claim is made regarding the PTSD status of these individ-
uals, as the measures are self-reported ones. While there are problems
with the interpretation of self-report measures, these findings are inter-
esting simply because they show that people many decades after their
war experiences still have psychological symptoms relating to those
experiences.
Open-ended questions 143
Open-ended questions
The veterans were asked general questions about what they found inter-
esting and disturbing about the war. The most disturbing aspects of
their experience were: battle/combat experience, physical conditions,
the effects on others and the government.
It is perhaps inevitable that many of the strongest memories veterans
have is of battle experience. For many, the horror of such experience was
still strong after 50 years, and it is the type of memory that is most likely
to lead to psychological difficulties on the part of the veteran, though it
should be borne in mind that the strength of the memory may not
correlate closely with the traumatic nature of the original experienced
event. The next most common response involved the physical conditions
under which the men lived, both during battle and at other times during
the war. The cramped conditions of ships, the weather and the poor
food all were mentioned on numerous occasions. British Second World
War veterans suffered these conditions for up to 6 years. Apart from
worrying about themselves, veterans would consider the worries they
had about their loved ones at home parents, wives and children whom
they knew to be suffering through German air attack or food shortages.
Family separation has received little attention in the post-war years,
144 Ageing, trauma and memory
along with the consequences such separations might have on the longer
term functioning of such families. This was perhaps particularly the case for
those married with young children. It is also only recently that research has
considered the effects of war experience on those too young to have been
involved in the fighting, or the women left at home (Waugh, 1997).
Another common response was the veterans attitudes towards the
government. There was bitterness towards the present-day government
that stems from a sense of unfairness at the governments refusal to assist
ageing veterans. People who had spent their early years defending the
country were now getting a poor deal regarding the health service and
pensions. Their present problems may be exacerbated by this bitterness.
These issues are similar to those arising for current veterans of Iraq and
Afghanistan, who have similar problems relating to obtaining help for
their problems. This illustrates the importance of accounting for the
relevant socio-cultural factors.
Many veterans thought that the novelty of their experiences was the
most interesting memory they had of the war, particularly meeting new
people both from the same culture and from cultures around the world,
and seeing new places. The majority of this generation would not have
had the opportunity to travel as many of these veterans did. Most would
never have been abroad before, and would not have done so had the war
not occurred. With regard to meeting people of different social classes in
their own culture, this had a major impact on the changing social
attitudes of the wartime generation, an impact that would last a lifetime
and change the nature of the post-war country.
The next major response to the question concerned comradeship.
While none chose to define what they meant by comradeship, it was
clearly important to very many veterans. It was concerned with the unity
of people (whether battle unit or nation), and peoples reliance on one
another. Veterans felt that comradeship has gone from modern society.
This may also have had an impact on their present psychological state.
The interviews
Twenty-five respondents were interviewed. The first ten were Normandy
veterans. The rest included veterans who were ex-POWs and others
who had experienced high levels of combat or other war experiences,
and who either scored very high or very low on the self-reported symp-
tom scales.
The veterans were asked in detail about their memories (the ways they
have changed, what was most important, intrusive recollections, etc.),
and also about the ways in which they coped with their experiences,
Intrusive recollections and other memories 145
including social support, both at the time and later, and about any
effects they felt they still had.
Findings
A large number of themes emerged, only a sample of which is included
here. The veterans experienced three main types of memory, of which the
first two are particularly important for the general argument of the book:
(1) Intrusive recollections. These are implicit, uncontrolled and associated
with strong emotion and/or perceptual elements, the memories
usually associated with traumatic stress.
(2) Consummate memories. These are detailed, likely to be confabulated,
and may in the past have been intrusive recollections. The veteran
has learned to deal with the emotions attached to the memory and
developed a narrative. He may still experience anger or sadness
when recalling the event, but these emotions are under control.
These are important memories when dealing with distant retro-
spective trauma, as they indicate which memories were problematic
to the person.
(3) Ordinary decayed memories. These are memories that were never trau-
matic, which the individual remembers in the ordinary kind of way,
and which are subject to the normal processes of decay. The individual
is likely to remember aspects of an event, but not with emotion.
As these memories are retrospective of a time many decades prior to
their recall, no attempt was made to verify them through other means
and no comment is made regarding their accuracy. By definition, some
of the memories will have been adapted and changed; consummate
memories are likely to consist of elements of what really happened
and elements that the veteran has incorporated in order to make
sense of the memory. There is also the issue of the audience. Veterans
are likely to tell different stories to a researcher (me) than the stories they
tell to each other or to their families. The audience for the narrative
affects the narrative that is told, as all narratives depend on there being
an audience.
Nightmares
I dream a lot Im shooting them, I tweak things off the bed table, I dream about it . . .
its like all silly dreams. Its like I say, Im shooting Germans but they dont go down
and they chase me and I cant get away from them. Im running and running but the
beggars are still behind me. I cant get away. Most peculiar, silly dreams.
Guilt
Many veterans describe the experience of battle:
[driving in a tank, the driver said] Theres a wounded German officer, [and the
commander said] well I wont tell you what he said and we drove on. The only . . .
point about that was we werent squeamish I had a quick look, but of course the first
What kinds of memory are potentially traumatic? 147
thing in harbour at night was you had to clean your tank down. Fetching pieces of
German officer out of your tracks which wouldnt have been bad if hed been dead
but you realise you got him [speaking quietly] . . . That stuck in my mind, though it is
50 years ago next February when it happened.
This recollection has been through some cognitive processing, but
remains traumatic. The veteran was experiencing difficult emotions as
he was talking. It may be the guilt that is causing the problem that the
memory itself is manageable, but the guilt cannot be reconciled. This
demonstrates the complexity of the problems experienced by people
with these kinds of memories.
Others discussed the loss of friends, both at the time and seeing the
graves. Again there were feelings of guilt:
Why am I standing here looking at him [in the grave] and not the other way round?
Atrocities
Other veterans describe atrocities that they witnessed:
I was with a sergeant one day and a German came out of a trench and this sergeant
killed him because he was crying because [his friend had been killed]. Of course he
was so upset and in fact he ran forward and killed two more Germans who were giving
themselves up.
Atrocities were undoubtedly committed on both sides, often in the heat
of battle, but most veterans were unwilling to discuss them. Atrocities
were either only committed by the enemy, or they had heard stories of
atrocities but had rarely witnessed or taken part or at least they rarely
admitted to witnessing or taking part.
148 Ageing, trauma and memory
These quotations show the effects of age. People focus on what has been
important to them through their lives, and for many veterans this was the
war. It is important to distinguish between traumatic recollections and
the simple remembering of things past. Many of the war memories these
veterans focus on are not necessarily traumatic, but they are among the
most important events in their lives.
Avoidance
Avoidance was used as a coping strategy during the war, partly because of
training. Soldiers were trained to respond automatically to particular situ-
ations, to avoid thinking about the potential personal consequences of their
or the enemys actions. The individual who responds in such a way in
battle is more likely to perform effectively than the individual who becomes
distressed at the sudden death of comrades. Even after battle there was
usually little time or desire to think about, to grieve, these deaths. This use of
avoidant thinking may have led veterans to continue the use of the strategy
after the war via keeping busy with work and family commitments.
As we saw in Chapter 5, behaviour that is adaptive in a traumatic
situation, that helps someone survive, may be maladaptive outside of
that situation, but the person is conditioned to respond in a particular
way they have no choice in the matter.
Fatalism
Associated with the concept of avoidance is fatalism, where the individ-
ual accepts what is going to happen as inevitable, so there is little point in
worrying about it. There is a sense of acceptance with ones lot, that
there was no alternative to what would happen and what had happened.
This indicates a sense of helplessness with the situation, that the soldier
cannot have an effect on the situation. This is a passive coping strategy
that may have affected post-war coping responses, making it more diffi-
cult to actively process the traumatic information, leaving avoidance as
the best strategy available.
No choice
This is different from fatalism. Veterans felt there was no way of
escaping from battle. During the war people experienced many
150 Ageing, trauma and memory
different difficult situations that they were forced to either cope with or
die. POWs had to cope with particular difficulties. One ex-FEPOW
(Far East prisoner of war) kept himself together physically by eating the
rice polishings chewing slowly, then regurgitating at the point of
swallowing to chew again. This would both ensure maximum nutrition
and stave off the pangs of hunger. Others cut cigarettes in half to make
them last longer and to make them less hungry. For others the sense of
comradeship in the camps was important. Veterans also mentioned the
lack of choice when one is being shelled, that there is nothing one can
do about it.
Problems can arise when individuals do not have a means of actively
dealing with stressors. As Foa et al. (1989) showed, those who cannot
actively deal with stress often develop more psychological difficulties
than those who do actively deal with it. For instance, shooting at the
enemy is perceived to be less stressful than being shelled. In the former
situation the soldier is in control of the situation; in the latter he has no
control.
Comradeship
As one of the most effective strategies for coping with the traumatic
events of war, comradeship is a sense of belonging to a group of people
who share similar experiences. Comradeship is difficult to define. Some
suggest it only existed between small groups of men fighting in the same
unit, others that the whole country was in comradeship. Comradeship is
not the same as friendship; it is deeper in the sense that comrades share
experiences and lifestyles friends normally wouldnt. Because it involves
sharing hardship and danger and that training prepares soldiers to
depend on one another for their lives, there is a very strong bond that
ties people together. On the other hand, comradeship is weaker than
friendship because of the nature of how the bond is created; one does not
choose ones comrades. It is not appropriate to consider comradeship as
weaker than friendship because of the ability to virtually ignore the death
of a comrade in battle.
Coping in the immediate post-war years 151
I was about 17 when I fell in love with my wife and then she was 1314 . . . Ive never
been with anyone else . . . I had her photograph with me all the time and I brought it
back, the same photograph that Ive had. It was a photograph Id had enlarged in
India, only an ordinary photograph, it was with me all the time . . . [Down in the] coal
mine, 12-hour shifts, but it was the thought that the wife would be here, knew that she
would be here when I came back . . .
NH: Did you ever doubt that she would wait?
No no.
NH: Never in the camps?
No.
NH: Even at your worst moments?
Never, never, never entered my mind that she wouldnt be here.
NH: Was she the main reason you survived?
Yes.
While I was interviewing him, she was cooking his dinner in the
kitchen.
Unfortunately, we do not have the stories of those who failed to
survive, and the reasons they lost their lives, and of course there are
many stories where girlfriends or wives did not wait.
There was no psychological support for most people. This itself may
have created or exacerbated many of the problems that came later.
Perhaps if they had been able to process the memories through counsel-
ling at the time, then there would have been fewer traumatic recollec-
tions now.
Just after the war I got blackouts you see, I had blackouts for a long time, I got dizzy . . .
They couldnt get me around . . . I had it very very bad . . . When I think of the daft
things, any noise of a car, something like that, Id be under the table, covering my eyes,
shivering with fright.
This lasted for 78 years. Fortunately, his wife helped him come to terms
with it. His wife helped him through years of psychological suffering. She
was his main source of support. Unfortunately, she died not long before
I interviewed him and this has led to a re-emergence of war-related
problems. If veterans have relied on their wives for many years as social
support, then the loss, whether through death or infirmity, may affect
their ability to cope at a time when they have fewer physical resources
themselves.
The Camp
The Corps centre line ran alongside
Beech Wood. The leading division
Paused. Urgent calls crackled
Demanding medics, food, ambulances pronto.
Behind barbed wire skeletons with skins
Stared out from shaven skulls.
Around us neat piles of
Dead and dying, like logs
Layered criss-cross with dangling heads.
Bursts of fire from pale soldiers
Ended the slouching arrogance
Of guards who failed
To leap to instant orders
Officers turned a blind eye.
The first ten years after coming out of the army. I never told anybody, the wife and
daughter knew nothing about being a prisoner of war and all that.
This veteran never discussed his problems. When his memories emerge,
he still avoids them:
I try to do some little jobs you know like in the basement, I do a bit of joinery work and
things like that. When I get something Im interested in I can take me mind off that and
put it to that, thats how I cope and if in a morning Im having my breakfast about
what Ive been thinking in bed I go walking for two or three hours, thats how I cope . . .
people keep lending me military books like D-Day and all that. Id take it off them but
I never read it, Id say it were a good book but dont ask me what was insider I wont
read them now.
and trauma (e.g. Hassan, 1997). The Spanish Civil War provided evi-
dence that a situation of common danger reinforces morale (Hargreaves,
1939). Evidence for the use of social support here arises in three separate
examples: wives, comrades and veterans associations, with veterans using
such support in different ways according to whether they are with family
or other veterans.
Wives/family
The support received by veterans from wives has rarely been officially
acknowledged. Many took and take an important role in the care of
their husbands for many years, providing both practical assistance and
emotional support. The FEPOW described above, who took the photo
to India, says:
Shes been marvellous, marvellous like anything. I worship the ground she walks on.
Wives have not only provided the emotional support that veterans have
needed in the post-war years; many have had to provide their husbands
with practical help throughout marriage:
Shes more or less a nurse you know, I told the pension people she ought to get assistance.
If I had to go to hospital theyd pay for that but because shes here we dont get anything.
Millions of people in this country look after a mother or father for 50 years and get
nothing. Not fair really is it?
If the veteran loses his partner the person he has depended on then it
may be very difficult to cope if he has depended heavily on her over the
years. This is another reason why war memories may emerge. This
becomes more likely to happen as the veteran gets older. If he has
depended on his wife for emotional and/or practical support since the
war, then the joint effects of losing wife and losing support can be
extremely traumatic. This veteran got over his problems through the
support of his family, but not all veterans have such a family.
Comradeship
Comradeship helped veterans to cope with war, and it still exists for
some, often in the form of veterans associations. While comradeship
may be a source of social support, it may also function as a reminder of
traumatic recollections, which conflicts with its social support role.
Whatever function it provides, it is a bond that is difficult to break.
Comradeship is still important today for many veterans. While there
are differences in the ways that comradeship is perceived by different
Concluding remarks 157
Veterans associations
For many veterans, these associations formed an important part of
their lives. They became more popular as time went by, coinciding
with veterans reaching retirement age. Veterans associations served
as a means of retaining or regaining comradeship, and providing
practical help.
Associations provided a means of remembering the war and the
comrades who were killed:
Next Sunday we go to Leicester for a commemorative service there. I suppose Ive got to
be honest we like to march and we love a good band to march to. A commemorative
service to try again to remember the pals youve lost, it doesnt do them any good, it
doesnt hurt you to remember them.
Meetings are generally regular, perhaps once a month, and veterans get
together to have a drink and a chat often about the war:
[We] get together and chat on wartime activities more than anything. We do go to
different places. Its a social evening really.
Veterans associations provide more than this; they have a supportive
role for veterans who are retired and in physical decline:
Its a question of welfare first of all.
Concluding remarks
Some veterans still experienced problems with their memories
many years after the war. There was evidence of both traumatic and
consummate memories, the former still causing emotional problems,
and the latter appearing to be resolved traumatic memories, or narratives.
Veterans have several ways of coping with these memories, via processing,
avoidance and social support. The results show the complexity of narra-
tives, the integration with social constructs common within society,
and how it is difficult to simply say that someone has resolved their
traumatic memories through the development of a narrative. It is more
158 Ageing, trauma and memory
complex than that. People may have developed their narratives, yet
still experience problems when they are faced with reminders. Others
perhaps most use avoidance as a strategy, and one that works in many
circumstances, though sometimes less so once the veteran retires.
The most frequently used coping strategy is avoidance. Many veterans
learned to use this strategy through their war experiences, where it was
an adaptive strategy, enabling the individual to carry on. After the war it
ceased to be adaptive, but veterans could, because they were very busy
working and building families, successfully avoid their traumatic recol-
lections. For many, this strategy has been successful almost to the
present day. It is retirement that has led to an increase in difficulties.
The veteran has more time to think, more time to dwell on his past life,
and if the war was the most important time of life, as many claim, then
they are more likely to dwell on that era. Perhaps it is retirement when
more people start to need narrative development, and when it is more
effective, than immediately after the event.
As shown in the previous chapter, social support is a complex
phenomenon. Wives and families provide both practical and emotional
support, but not as individuals with whom the war could be discussed.
On several occasions during the interviews, veterans would explain that
they were telling me things they had never told their wives. Why?
Perhaps wives are not appropriate audiences; they provide a safe and
protective environment, one which the veteran does not want to endan-
ger by discussing the emotional issues surrounding the war. Veterans
believed comrades understand their problems. Veterans who wished to
discuss the war find it easiest to discuss it with people who have had
similar experiences. This highlights the role of the researcher or clin-
ician. If the veteran is going to discuss his traumatic memories with
somone unknown, then it helps that he is not emotionally attached, as
the sharing of the memories does not endanger anyone, and it also helps
if the researcher or clinician has some understanding and awareness
of what the person has been through. Again, social support demonstrates
the important role of the audience in the expression of narratives.
A veteran has one narrative for his family, and another to share with
other people.
The permanent effects of war experience can be mitigated by appro-
priate coping strategies, but for many older veterans these strategies are
only partially effective. Wartime memories, even when processed into
narrative form, remain powerful, and can still generate intense emotions,
even after 50 years. Successful coping still means having to cope
throughout life. For many, this is becoming difficult due to the extra
problems created by ageing.
Concluding remarks 159
The use of literature (novels, poetry) can both support the psychological
evidence we obtain regarding the impact of war, and, in some circum-
stances, help to develop our understanding. A common way for many
people to deal with their traumatic memories is to write them down as a
story; this, for some, is an effective way of dealing with memories. It is
not just literature, but also films and plays. Perhaps the best-known
example of a film director dealing with his own nightmares is Oliver
Stone, with his series on the Vietnam War, in which he fought. Several
books could be written about the different ways in which traumatised
people have dealt with their traumatic experiences by getting their work
published. Many other books could explore all the manuscripts that have
not been published.
Psychologists have traditionally ignored literature as a potential source
of data. Analysis of literary sources can potentially provide psychologists
with rich data from which to develop and test psychological theories.
The traditional scientific approach of psychology has occasionally been
unscientific in focusing too much on methods and too little on examining
the theories they are interested in testing. After all, the nature of psycho-
logical data can include all behaviours of people, including writing.
Our understanding of the psychological effects of war benefits by a
detailed consideration of literature published in the area. Apart from
a means of validating theory, it provides an opportunity to explore
responses to war trauma in other times and cultures. For instance,
Hanley (1991) reviewed the literature regarding women and war, inter-
spersing this with her own short stories fictionalising accounts of real
experiences (see Cobley, 1994) and provided the reader with an
understanding of the trauma of war from a womans perspective.
Literature has provided countless examples of how soldiers cope with
war experience. Wilfred Owen was shellshocked during World War One
and was sent to Craiglockhart hospital outside Edinburgh for treatment.
Through his work he describes instances of many concepts current
to psychotraumatological theory. Re-experiencing, a key component of
161
162 Literature and trauma
Tragedy
The concept of the tragedy has been used in drama throughout history.
Drama is often about traumatic events and the ways in which people deal
with them. Krook (1969) looked at tragedy in literature and identified
four stages: (1) Precipitant: the act of shame or horror tragic circum-
stances always arise from the fundamental nature of humans;
(2) suffering: this is only tragic if it generates knowledge or insight or
understanding of the fundamental nature of humans; (3) Knowledge;
and (4) affirmation; of the worthwhile nature of human life and the
dignity of the human spirit. Tedeschi et al. (1995) used this to consider
how traumatised individuals can benefit from the insights gained after a
traumatic event. War trauma can be considered in the light of these
stages. Effective processing of emotional traumatic memories is directly
analogous to the model of tragedy. There is a traumatic event which
involves suffering, and the individual who successfully processes the
information gains knowledge and understanding of themself and also
of the true nature of what it is to be human.
The construct of post-traumatic growth was discussed in Chapter 6.
Tragedy provides good examples demonstrated through tragic litera-
ture of how people, after a traumatic event, learn from the event in
terms of increasing wisdom (processing and growth).
If trauma victims believe that their suffering has no meaning, if it
cannot be interpreted in terms of saving civilisation or doing a worth-
while job of work, then it becomes despair (Frankl, 1963/1984). In order
to successfully process the traumatic recollections, trauma victims must
recognise the meaning of their suffering. This is not a matter of reverting
to a former state, but an acceptance that things are permanently
changed, and thus a learning experience. Many veterans acknowledge
the beauty of the simplicity of life, the value of others and of relation-
ships, in a way that they did not prior to combat experience. It could be
argued that they would have learned this anyway with increasing age and
wisdom, but it is very likely that their experiences enabled them to alter
the ways in which they look at the world.
Method
The data source is AQOTWF, the Wheen translation (Remarque,
1929). The book was analysed using a grounded approach (Strauss
and Corbin, 1998). Appropriate quotations were coded, typed into
Word and coded. Concepts arose out of the initial coding. These codes
were then grouped under conceptual headings which could then form
the basis for developing a theoretical model. This type of model is not
developed here but it is used simply to illustrate the use of literature.
The resultant conceptual structure is presented in the Results section
below, with the main category headings presented as subheadings. For
a more detailed explanation of this method, see Hunt (1999).
Results
The following is not a comprehensive analysis of AQOTWF, but focuses
on a restricted number of themes: battle experience and understanding,
memory, the past and the future, and coping.
Memory
In order to understand psychological trauma we have to understand
traumatic memory, though relatively little of the novel is concerned with
memories. AQOTWF is set during the war. Paul and the others are not
looking back, they are living through the traumatic experience of war.
Remarque demonstrates that the soldiers avoided thoughts of the past,
of civilian life, because this led to problems. In extreme cases this could
lead to desertion, such as in the case of Detering, who was finally broken
by the sight of blossom on a tree that reminded him of his home on the
farm. He disappeared and was not heard of again, with the implication
that the military police had caught and shot him.
166 Literature and trauma
These memories create difficulties for Paul. Perhaps the worst occasion
is after coming back from leave, when he finds it difficult to come to
terms with being in the front line again:
What is leave? a pause that only makes everything after it so much worse. (p. 119)
These examples illustrate how the soldiers have problems with memor-
ies, that memories of other times can intrude and make life difficult or
impossible to bear. In order to succeed at the front the soldiers have to
relinquish much of what makes them human, the ability to look into the
past and the ability, as we shall see, to explore the future. They have to
live for the present, because when they realise that the past and the
future exist, they may break down.
younger ones, those who were adolescents when they joined the Army,
had laid down few roots and so there is nothing for them:
They have a background which is so strong that the war cannot obliterate it. We young
men of twenty however have only our parents, and some, perhaps, a girl that is not
much, for at our age the influence of parents is at its weakest and girls have not yet got a
hold over us. (p. 19)
Memories of the past give an indication of how the future might be if
only one could survive the war. But once one begins thinking in this way
then one cannot perform effectively as a soldier, for instance becoming
unwilling to go over the top because of the fear of death becoming
predominant.
This way lies the abyss. It is not now the time but I will not lose these thoughts, I will
keep them, shut them away until the war is ended. My heart beats fast: this is the aim,
the great, the sole aim, that I have thought of in the trenches; that I have looked for as
the only possibility of existence after this annihilation of all human feeling; this is a task
that will make life afterward worthy of these hideous years. (p. 128)
The war has fully taken over for the young men there is no future other
than the war because there are no memories of adulthood from before
the war:
He is right. We are not youth any longer. We dont want to take the world by storm. We
are fleeing. We fly from ourselves. From our life. We were 18 and had begun to love life
and the world; and we had to shoot it to pieces. The first bomb, the first explosion, burst
in our hearts. We are cut off from activity, from striving, from progress. We believe in
such things no longer, we believe in the war. (p. 61)
The loss of purpose appears to be known to Paul and the others while
they are still in the trenches. It is confusing; on the one hand we are led
to believe that the soldiers do not think about the past or the future, on
the other that they do and have done in great detail, thinking through
the consequences of the war years. Do we have here an example of
dissociation?
Remarque is concerned with how the men will be unable to do
anything in the future because:
Through the years our business has been killing - it was our first calling in life. Our
knowledge of life is limited to death. What will happen afterwards? And what shall come
out of us? (p. 173)
This destruction of a generation is a very important theme throughout
the book. Erikson (1982) proposed that there are seven stages of life,
and that the transition from adolescent to adulthood is critical. This is
when we develop our identities independent of parents. Remarques
argument is that this stage transition was shattered, and that as Erikson
168 Literature and trauma
proposed this will have profound effects on the abilities of the soldiers
to adapt to post-war life.
Had we returned home in 1916, out of the suffering and the strength of our experiences
we might have unleashed a storm. Now if we go back we will be weary, broken, burnt
out, rootless, and without hope. We will not be able to find our way any more.
And men will not understand us for the generation that grew up before us, though it
has passed these years with us already had a home and a calling; now it will return to its
old occupations, and the war will be forgotten and the generation that has grown up
after us will be strange to us and push us aside. We will be superfluous even to ourselves,
we will grow older, a few will adapt themselves, some others will merely submit, and most
will be bewildered;- the years will pass by and in the end we shall fall into ruin. (p. 190)
This has implications for recent work on long-term effects of trauma
(Hunt, 1997). Remarque is showing that this shattering of lives shall be
permanent for many of the soldiers, that what is being destroyed on the
battlefield shall never be rebuilt.
Coping
Through much of the book Remarque shows how Paul and his comrades
use various coping strategies, including avoidance, comradeship and
dehumanisation. These strategies concur with psychological research
(e.g. Elder and Clipp, 1989; Hunt and Robbins, 1998).
Avoidance is perhaps the most important means of coping used by the
soldiers on the battlefield. Soldiers are trained to fight battles. Through
training, they are prepared for their traumatic experiences. Training
develops both unit cohesiveness and the ability to respond automatically
in given situations (Watson, 1978). During wartime these skills are
adaptive. The individual who is part of a unit where he can trust his
comrades and who responds automatically to life-threatening situations
is more likely to come out of battle alive than the one who does not have
these advantages. Avoidance also stops men from thinking too much.
There are many examples of avoidance in AQOTWF, relating specif-
ically to avoiding particular subjects such as thinking about a recent
battle:
Often we lay aside the cards and look about us. One of us will say; Well boys . . . Or It
was a near thing that time . . . And for a moment we fall silent. There is in each of us a
feeling of constraint. We are all sensible of it; it needs no words to communicate it. (p. 12)
It can occur during the battle itself. In a bombardment the soldiers are
close to breaking down because there is nothing to do except wait to see
if a shell falls on them. Having an active task enables the soldiers to avoid
thinking about the danger to their lives. At one point:
Coping 169
We are buried and must dig ourselves out. After an hour the entrance is clear again, and
we are calmer because we have had something to do. (p. 74)
Avoidance has to be employed:
Habit is the explanation of why we seem to forget things so quickly . . . They are too
grievous for us to be able to reflect on them at once. If we did that, we should have been
destroyed long ago. I soon found out this much:- terror can be endured so long as a man
simply ducks;- but it kills if a man thinks about it. (p. 93)
The same attitude is used in regard to friends and comrades who have
been killed. The soldier is not allowed to grieve, and uses active strat-
egies to avoid thinking too much about such people:
The terror of the front sinks deep down when we turn our backs upon it; we make grim,
coarse jests about it, when a man dies, then we say he has nipped off his turd, and so we
speak of everything; that keeps us from going mad; as long as we take it that way we
maintain our own resistance. (p. 94)
Avoidance is not about forgetting, and it can only be an effective strategy
for a limited length of time:
But we do not forget. Its all rot that they put in the war-news about the good humour of the
troops . . . We are in a good humour because otherwise we should go to pieces. Even so we
cannot hold out much longer; our humour becomes more bitter every month. (pp. 945)
His name, it is a nail that will be hammered into me and never come out again. (p. 147)
This man is bound up with my life, therefore I must do everything, promise
everything in order to save myself; I swear blindly that I mean to live only for his
sake and his family. (p. 148)
But this feeling does not last. Paul, in order to survive, must again
become an unthinking soldier:
I think no more of the dead man, he is of no consequence to me now. With one bound the
lust to live flares up again and everything that has filled my thoughts goes down before
it. (p. 149)
Conclusion
This analysis illustrates how literature can be used as data to enhance
our understanding of the response to war trauma. As Shay used the Iliad,
we can use AQOTWF. Remarque (1929) demonstrates a depth of
understanding about the consequences of battle experience, the memor-
ies, the problems associated with the family, or of coping through avoid-
ance. Two areas that are not dealt with well in the psychological
literature are: (a) dealing with war through dehumanisation, and
(b) descriptions of battle experience and the immediate impact on
participants. AQOTWF is an eloquent expression of the use of narrative
in coping with memories. It deals with notions of past and future, and
the ways in which men experienced dissociation with regard to these
concepts, at the same time rejecting both and yearning for both.
AQOTWF describes in great detail the ways in which men dealt with
the suffering on the Western Front.
The concepts of processing (narrative development) and avoidance
are well described in the literature (Creamer et al., 1992; van der
Kolk and Fisler, 1995), but descriptions of the experience of battle
and dehumanising the enemy are rarely discussed. Neither is there a
clear understanding of how and why verbal descriptions are inadequate
to describe traumatic experiences, though this has implications for all
psychological work involving linguistic data.
Remarques personal experiences can be interpreted using Krooks
(1969) four stages of tragedy. For Remarque the precipitant was the
Conclusion 171
war and the suffering the trenches. Through this came knowledge
about the nature of people and how they deal with difficult situations.
Affirmation is concerned with Remarques post-war life, how his books,
including AQOTWF, illuminated facets of peoples lives, far beyond
simple notions of trauma. Could Remarque have been a novelist had
he not experienced the trenches of the First World War?
While literature should not be considered as a substitute for the
scientific analysis of the consequences of trauma, it is useful to use
such writings to triangulate with our psychological theories. A note of
caution, though: novels are not designed to be data they are stories
about aspects of the world from the perspective of the writer. In the case
of writers with battle experience, they may tell us something about what
it is to be traumatised, but there are dangers of over-interpretation.
Literary theory is full of interpretations of what writers are trying to
say, but as Umberto Eco points out, there are limits to what we can make
a text mean (Eco, 1992). There is the danger that we may be claiming
more from a novel than the author actually intended. Nevertheless, our
understanding of war trauma can be enriched by the study of works of
literature, novels and poems written by war veterans and others.
12 Memorialisation and commemoration
The CWGC keeps records of all the dead, and there is a searchable
database where relatives of those who have died, and anyone else with an
interest, can find details, including place of burial (if any).
The Cenotaph
Much of this and the following section is based on the book Remembrance
by Mark Quinlan (2005a), which provides a detailed account of many of
the key memorials for the CWGC, along with some fascinating back-
ground about how they came to be erected. There is also a series of short
biographies of some of the key figures who worked on the memorials.
He has also written another volume, British War Memorials, focusing on
the British memorials. Remembrance relating to the First World War is a
fascinating case of bringing together the need to remember the dead
through memory and commemoration. At the end of the war the British
Government wanted a day of celebration to mark the signing of the
Treaty of Versailles in June 1919. At this point, there was some nervous-
ness about the attitudes of serving and demobilised soldiers, and also
unrest within the labour force. As so often after a war, there were many
discontented and often unemployed men. The Victory Parade was, in
part, an attempt to draw the nation together to both celebrate the
successful (or so it was thought) end of the war, and to commemorate
the dead. The French were arranging to have a march past the Arc de
Triomphe, where a temporary catafalque was to be erected and
which the soldiers would salute. The British Government thought this
was a good idea and commissioned Sir Edwin Lutyens to design the
Cenotaph. This was to be non-denominational, and he had two weeks to
design it and get it built. Lutyens met with the Principal Architect of
Works, Sir Frank Baines, and sketched out his design. Lutyens gave it
the name Cenotaph, which was simply an empty tomb on a high
pedestal. The word derives from the Greek words kenos (empty) and
taphos (tomb). The origin is from the importance the Greeks attached
to burying their dead, even if there was no body. This links neatly with
the memorials to the dead who have no known grave at Thiepval on the
Somme, and the Menin Gate and the remembrance wall at Tyne Cot
Cemetery.
The Cenotaph was built and used for the victory celebrations, and it
caught the publics imagination. It was temporary, and meant to be
removed after the parades, but the public wanted a permanent memor-
ial. After discussion in Parliament, it was agreed. Westminster Council
initially objected to its site, but there was public acclaim for it to be
on the site in Whitehall that was occupied by the temporary structure.
The Unknown Warrior 177
British Soldier. The bodies were selected from the early years of the war,
so decomposition had taken place. Proof of Britishness was the wearing
of a British Army uniform. The remains were transported to a chapel at
Saint Pol near Arras. The bearers arrived at different times and were
immediately sent away so they would not know if their body had been
selected. Two British officers, Brigadier Wyatt and Lieutenant-Colonel
Gell, entered the chapel alone at midnight and chose one of the bodies,
each of which was covered by the Union Flag. The other bodies were
taken for re-burial, and the chosen body was transported, with due
ceremony, across the Channel on the British Destroyer HMS Verdun.
The funeral in Westminster Abbey took place on 11 November 1920,
with full ceremonial. The grave was filled with earth brought from
France. The French Unknown Warrior was interred the same day under
the Arc de Triomphe.
The burial brought back memories. A blinded veteran, Herbert
Thompson, described how the atmosphere was impregnated with mean-
ing, and the occasion brought back the most poignant memories. I felt
with my comrades almost ashamed that I had given so little, while he
who was sleeping by us had given all . . . I came to the Abbey glad that
I had been chosen from among so many, I went away sorrowing, but
with the message of hope in my heart (quoted in Quinlan, 2005a, p. 22).
This moving quotation draws out the best in memorialisation how it
stirs memories of the war and comrades who fought and died, yet also
looks forward to a better future. It is also interesting that a blind man felt
that he had given so little compared with the soldier who had died. This
shows the inherent contradictions in any war memorialisation; the men
who were maimed are not the ones who are remembered. In some way it
is thought that they have not contributed as much as those who died,
they are not as important as those who died; because they did not die,
they should not be remembered.
Commemoration in Leningrad
The siege of Leningrad lasted nearly 900 days. Around 670,000 people
died, a third of the population, with up to 8,000 in a single day mostly
of starvation. In the first winter of the siege, 19412, with temperatures
dropping as low as 40 C, the daily bread ration went as low as 125 grams.
Commemoration of the Great Patriotic War, particularly the siege of
Leningrad, started very soon after the Germans invaded in June 1941.
Lisa Kirschenbaum (2004) has described how, days after the Germans
invaded the Soviet Union, it was reported by the Leningrad Komsomol
(youth organisation) that efforts to chronicle the war had already begun.
Commemoration in Leningrad 179
Conclusions
Memorialisation and commemoration are experiencing a growth in the
West. It is difficult to determine whether this is because we are reaching
the endpoint of commemoration for the dead of the World Wars, with
most of the participants dead and the rest very old, whether it relates to
the breakdown of the nation state and an attempt by the nation state to
unify the nation again, and to draw on unknown reserves of patriotism,
whether it is a result of the increasing publicity of current wars and the
relatively large-scale casualties, or whether there is a psychological
need that has not been fulfilled in previous decades. It is likely that there
is a combination of factors. The state could not force individuals to
commemorate, and individuals, though they do carry out small-scale
memorialisation and commemoration, need the state to take part in the
large-scale events.
Memorialisation and commemorative events are yet another way of
demonstrating and creating narratives that are linked to the social dis-
courses of the time. Both memorials and commemorative events tend to
be public, and they tend to be aspects of a discourse that is accepted by
most people within the country. In reality, many people from families
who have immigrated to the UK in the last few decades may not feel
anything about such rituals they may not feel part of it but remem-
bering the dead of the World Wars and conflicts since then has become
an important part of remembering the history of Britain. It is almost
unheard of for groups to protest at a commemorative event. Anti-war
Conclusions 185
This chapter is a little unusual in a book about the psychology of war. One
purpose of the book is to try and understand something about how
humans behave in response to war, with a special focus on exploring the
role of socio-cultural factors and memory. Peoples understanding of war
if they are lucky enough not to have genuinely experienced it is largely
derived from books, television and films. The aim of this chapter is to take
a more active approach than just reading, but one where the danger has
passed to walk around the battlefields themselves, in the footsteps of the
soldiers. In this way, one can improve ones understanding of war, and try
to understand something more of what people go through when they
fight. This is addressing the van der Kolk statement: the body keeps
the score. War trauma is about bodily experience as much as emotional
or cognitive factors, so walking the battlefields adds something to ones
understanding. The chapter does not provide the detail for specific tours,
but instead gives some guidance on how to organise a tour, and on how
what you put in to such a tour can enhance what you get out of it.
The chapter uses a single example: to Ypres, for Passchendaele. I make
no excuses for choosing this site. It has been described many times before
and is familiar to many people interested in war. Its woods and fields
have been trodden over by many thousands of people since the battles
took place. Many other battlefields could have been chosen, but I chose
Passchendaele for personal reasons: because my fathers cousin, Charles
Hunt, died there on 1 August 1917, and is remembered on the Menin
Gate. I know very little about him, as those who did know him are now
dead. Those I could have asked when I was a child I did not ask because
I was a child. My father, who was born the year after Charles died, was
named after him. My middle name is also Charles, so there is a clear link.
Passchendaele
What Jerusalem is to the Jewish race, and what Mecca is to the
Mohammedan, Ypres must always be to the millions who have lost a
husband, son or brother, slain in its defence, and now sleeping their
eternal sleep within sight of its silent belfry.
These words were written by Lieutenant-Colonel Beckles Wilson,
the Canadian Town Major until October 1919, who was fanatical in
his desire to preserve the town of Ypres as a ruined memorial (see www.
cwgc.org/ypres/content.asp?id=38&menu=subsub). Fortunately for the
inhabitants, once he had gone, the town was slowly rebuilt as a copy
of its former self the houses, shops, churches and offices being built
from old plans, photographs and the memories of people. The centre of
the town is now an almost exact copy of how it was before 1914.
Virtually the only thing that had survived more or less intact were the
fortified walls of the town, built to Vaubans plans for defence against
very different artillery, several hundred years before.
The Battle of Passchendaele or, more formally, the Third Battle of
Ypres, took place between July and November 1917. The word
Passchendaele itself became a very emotive word in the English lan-
guage for several generations, through to the grandchildren of those who
fought there, providing evidence of the nature of cultural memory, i.e.
that it has a relatively short emotional life. Now, the word does not
convey the emotions to younger generations that it once had.
The site of the battlefield, extending several miles to the south, east
and north of the town of Ypres in southwest Belgium, was fought
over throughout the First World War. In 1914 it was the scene of
bitter fighting as the Allies and the Germans were involved in their
race to the sea. It helped to establish where the front lines were going
to stay for most of the war. By 1917, the soldiers who fought at the
Third Battle were fighting over ground that was already scarred from
shellfire and contained the bodies of many thousands of troops of
both sides.
The British, defending the salient around Ypres where there had been
little movement for nearly 3 years, planned to strike through the German
lines, cut off the occupied Belgian ports, and take Antwerp in the north
of Belgium. It was an ambitious plan and, as with so many ambitious
plans in this war, it was doomed to failure. In the end, the British
managed to take a few square miles of territory, the biggest advantage
188 Battlefield tours
Tyne Cot Cemetery, the largest on the battlefield, situated on the slopes
at the top of which is Passchendaele village, the main aim of the battle of
1917 finally reached after many months of fighting, and of which
nothing remained. The village now is completely rebuilt.
Visiting the Ypres area for the first time, you experience emotional
numbness at the sight of so many cemeteries containing the graves of so
many young men. The British graves are all of white Portland stone,
and the graves are all well tended, situated on land that has been donated
to the UK forever. It may help to imagine each stone as a man, returning
to the parade ground, to get a feeling for the numbers involved.
A tour of a battlefield when you are exploring the experiences of a single
soldier is, in many ways, more satisfying than visiting without such a focus.
While an understanding of the grand plan is developed, you learn little of
the experiences of war. Focusing on a specific individual and walking
the battlefield as though seeing it through his eyes provides a personal
perspective that gives a greater psychological understanding. Before going
to the site of the battle, read about what happened to the person, or to the
persons battalion if, as I did, you have little personal information about
the individual. Look at maps, try and trace the route the person took so
that when you are there you can follow that route and not the routes
specified by any tourist agency. Get the personal perspective.
Once at the battlefield it is important to visit the key sites and the
museums so that you can see the personal items carried by the soldiers,
the weapons used, but also the very personal things the letters, the tin
openers, the copies of newspapers, and the pictures of girlfriends. Again,
it personalises the experience and allows you to get inside the head of the
soldier. Personally, I do not like the modernisation of museums in the
Ypres area (and elsewhere). They distract from the experience. You can
look at a computer screen and see fancy animations and films anywhere,
but you can rarely see the individual items, the rusting guns, the uni-
forms, the personal equipment. The museums in the Ypres area vary
from a few piles of rusting junk to ultra-modern high-tech installations.
The preservation of memory for the First World War is undergoing
massive change; as the generations who knew veterans die, then the
events become history, not memories handed down through genera-
tions, something which ceases usually at the second generation.
The tour
Ypres itself is a good place to base the visit, though there are villages all
around where you can get good accommodation. The shape of the
battlefield makes Ypres the central focus. Take a good map of the area,
190 Battlefield tours
and mark it with the key sites of interest. This two-day tour uses both
walking and driving. There is no attempt to explore the whole battle-
field, as it is a personal tour, though I would recommend you drive
around the whole area just to get a sense of the size of the battlefield,
to see the perspectives of other soldiers, and to explore the monuments
and museums in other areas. The first day is spent around the area in
which Charles Hunt fought, and the second covers some of the key sites
of the battlefield.
Take the road to the right indicating Hill 60. You pass another, large,
cemetery on the right. You are then at the edge of Sanctuary Wood.
A little way further along is Hill 60 Museum, which is well worth a visit.
Inside is an array of stereoscopes, machines that are set up with hun-
dreds of three-dimensional photographs of the war. Many are quite
gruesome, men and horses dead. This gives a good impression of the
battlefield of the time. The museum also has many artefacts from the
war many displayed well, and others piled up rusting. Outside there is
an area of original trenches, some of which have been reconstructed,
including tunnelled areas. There are even some tree trunks remaining
which had been destroyed by shellfire. The trenches were British.
On leaving the museum, Hill 60 is on the right. Again, there are
memorials to see. Charles Hunt was positioned around here before the
battle started on 31 July. He would have seen the first troops advancing
up through the ruins of Sanctuary Wood towards the chateau at the
top of the hill occupied by the Germans. No doubt he, like most
others, was terrified of what was to come, but the soldiers supported
each other.
The next day Charless battalion advanced across the line of that taken
by the troops the previous day. Standing on Hill 60, they advanced
through Sanctuary Wood towards what is now Crater Cemetery. Now
walk up the path to the south of Sanctuary Wood. This opens out onto a
track, and then a road. Turn left. Look around and get an impression.
The slope you have just come up saw the deaths of many men. When you
are at the top you have reached the German lines, though very little now
remains. After a couple of hundred yards there is a track which heads
into Sanctuary Wood. Go down this and into the wood. When I first
walked in this wood alone I started to imagine the ghosts of the thou-
sands of men who had died here. Then I realised that these men were
fighting for my country and would be protective. The path leads straight
down through the wood to a track, where you turn right and head up to
the Menin Road. But while in the wood, it is worth exploring. It is in
places like this that you often get the best feel for a battleground, a place
that is relatively untouched and which still has traces of the lines of
trenches if you look carefully. But be careful, not every ditch is a trench!
It is also in places like this that you find relics of the war: bullets,
shellcases and, sometimes, intact shells complete with detonator and
explosive! If you do find a shell and they are quite common dont
touch, but report it to the police for disposal. I have found intact
shells up to 3 feet long, and they still look frightening. There was so
much ordnance fired in these battles, and so much of it was ineffective,
192 Battlefield tours
i.e. it didnt explode. There are still many tons to be found. And no
doubt more deaths. I found a large piece of shellcase just outside the
wood, splintered and twisted by its explosion. While it almost certainly
didnt kill Charles Hunt, there is some comfort in thinking that it did,
instantly.
Across the road there is a hotel, in the grounds of which are a number
of mine craters, now filled with water and forming a decorative feature in
the garden. For a small sum you can enter the area and explore. There
are other artefacts and the remains of a German trench, one that was
blown up by the mines that made these craters.
Turn right onto the Menin Road and walk down to Crater Cemetery,
so called because it was built on the site of a large crater, the shape of
which can still be seen at the top end of the cemetery. This is a large
cemetery, containing around 6,000 men. From here you can see why
they died. You are standing near the German front line, from where they
shot down onto the advancing British troops. It is here that I like to think
Charles Hunt is buried, one of the 2,000 or so unknown graves. I have
no idea whether he is here, but this view illustrates our need for a site of
memory to use Noras phrase, a place to mourn.
After visiting the cemetery, go to the museum across the road.
It contains a large number of artefacts, well displayed, and even partially
labelled. There is a good collection of shells of many sizes. There is also
a cafe.
After the museum, head back down the Menin Road towards the
British lines and Ypres. When you start to go uphill there is a crossroads.
Turn left and follow the road to the next village, Gommecourt. At the
end of the road there is another cemetery, on the site of an old tile
factory. Turn left (south) down the road and after a few hundred yards
turn right onto a small road which leads to the lake. The nearest part of
the lake is called Hellblast Corner, and was the area where Charles
Hunts battalion formed up before heading into the front line. Walk by
the lake (either side) and head back to Ypres. By the roundabout with a
large tap you can either go straight on, then turn left and follow the walls
back to the Menin Gate, though you can enter the town through an
earlier path, or you can turn left and enter by the Lille Gate, but we will
be returning this way tomorrow.
Before 8 p.m. you should be standing in the Menin Gate, probably
with hundreds of other people. Just before the hour the road is closed to
traffic, then several firemen of the town march up and play the Last
Post. It is a very moving ceremony, and has been carried out every
evening since the Menin Gate was finished in 1926 apart from the
years of occupation, 19404.
The tour 193
Ypres has a number of interesting restaurants and bars, and the local
beers are varied and flavourful. By the end of the day you may be in need
of several beers and a good talk about what you have seen.
old concrete sentry boxes. The museum itself has a range of good
displays, but the best part is the dugout. This is a reconstruction
of a WWI dugout, complete with various rooms fulfilling a number of
functions, from hospitals to officers quarters.
Return to Ypres. There are many walks and places to visit in Ypres. Just
wandering aimlessly around the town you will get a feel for the architec-
ture. The centre of the town does feel a little frozen in time due to it being
rebuilt in the pre-war style. The Cloth Hall is a magnificent building,
ruined by war and rebuilt over 50 years. The main museum is in here, but
take time to look at the building itself. Parts of the original walls remain,
but only the very lower parts. The museum itself is modern, and this is
where I have reservations. When I visited, there was a group of school-
children going around, but few of them were looking at the exhibits, the
artefacts of war. Most of them spent their time pressing buttons for
the computer demonstrations, something which they might as well have
stayed at home to do via the Internet. The modernisation of museums,
while there is an attempt to create a particular narrative of war, distracts
from the contemplation of the museum pieces themselves. This does seem
to reflect a trend towards accepting shorter attention spans and not
enabling visitors to learn about the war.
Walk down to the Lille Gate. Here there is a peaceful little cemetery
built as part of the town walls, where you can sit and contemplate, and
look out across the moat towards the south towards Hill 62 and the
Messines Ridge. Just by the Lille Gate is a bar and museum. Both are
excellent. They are run by a Belgian enthusiast, who is very happy to
discuss his museum project with visitors.1 The museum is at the back of
the bar. It contains many artefacts from the war, and the benefit is that
the owner is always thinking of new ways to display them. One memor-
able display has the visitor at the foot of the stairs of a dugout, and a
soldier who has been shot and has fallen halfway down the stairs.
After you have been in the museum, have a drink in the bar. They have
a special brew to commemorate the war and you can buy a beer jug to
remember your visit.
1
I recently visited Ypres (December 2009) and unfortunately the owner had died, aged
only forty-six. Currently, his wife is still running the bar and the museum is still open, but
not being developed.
Creating your own battlefield tour 195
This brief chapter draws together the material that has been examined
through the book, identifies some key themes relating to memory, war
and trauma, and proposes a framework for future research. In the end
the purpose of research in this field has to be helping those who have
suffered psychologically due to war or other traumatic events. This book
has not looked at psychological therapies in any detail, because that has
not been the purpose of the book, but it is hoped that clinicians and
therapists will find valuable lessons for therapeutic practice within these
pages. We do now understand a great deal about war and the psycho-
logical impact war can have on people. The introduction of PTSD in
1980 provided a useful impetus for research, and there are now well over
20,000 academic papers published on this topic. While they do, of
course, vary in quality, overall it has meant that we have a much better
grasp of the key issues surrounding war trauma.
Interdisciplinarity
Throughout the book the discussions have been largely about the
importance of interdisciplinarity. This is currently a trendy word in
academia, and trendy is not necessarily either interesting or pertinent,
but within the area of war trauma interdisciplinarity is invaluable. As we
have seen, we cannot fully understand the nature and effects of war
trauma without drawing on the expertise of researchers in a number of
disciplines, including not only psychology but also sociology, history,
196
Narrative 197
political science and literature. And it is not just academics who are
making the contribution it is novelists, poets and biographers, people
who think about and write about war. Without their contribution, most
of us would know little about war.
In order to understand an individuals memories of war and the
impact they have on behaviour, we have to look beyond the individual
to wider society, social constructs, politics and history. What I hope has
been made clear in this book is that we gain a more coherent and
complete understanding of war trauma through a thorough study of
these different areas. The chapter on literature, with the focus on
Remarques All Quiet on the Western Front, was an attempt to show
how literature can contribute to a psychological understanding when
the literature itself is seen as data. Remarque (1929) experienced the
trenches of the Western Front first hand, and so was in a good position to
write about what they were like, drawing on the freedom of a novelist
to arrange and present complex ideas about the impact of war experience
on people. The book is not only a psychological account; it is also
presenting an account of the culture in WWI Germany.
Literature is a rich source of narrative psychological understanding,
and psychologists should be encouraged to read more. Good novelists
are also good psychologists, as characters have to be strong and well
rounded. The interactions between the characters in a good novel are
necessarily psychologically insightful; they are telling the story of the
human condition. Psychologists who sneer at the psychological insights
from literature are missing an important point about science: science is
not about the methods used; it is about increasing systematic knowledge
and understanding about ones subject. The experimental method might
be very helpful at helping us to gain insight, but so is reading Dostoevsky.
Narrative
The key argument within the book has been the centrality of narrative
to the human condition. Without narrative we are nothing. Without
narrative we cannot understand either the personal or the social world.
It has been argued here and elsewhere that humans are essentially
storytellers, and we have a whole range of stories that we use, stories that
we tell our families, stories for our friends, stories for our professional
lives and stories we tell ourselves. One of the wonderful things about
being a person is that we can manipulate our memories more or less
at will though often such manipulations are implicit or unconscious.
Depending on the audience, we can remember certain events about
the past and conveniently forget or omit others, depending on the
198 Conclusions and future directions
Social discourse
Narratives are not only developed through an internal focus on memories
and the ways these are structured; they are developed, as we have seen,
through interpersonal interaction and through internalising and interacting
with social discourses. Narratives do not develop in isolation. If we are to
understand why people think as they do, we have to look around them at the
world in which they live. No man is an island is very true for us all. The
way we think, what we believe, how we express emotions, all depend on our
culture. This has important implications for memory, war and trauma, as
memories are constructions, and such constructions depend as Bartlett
(1932) showed with The War of the Ghosts on the society in which
we live. A person traumatised by war is traumatised via the culture in which
he lives, and any treatment or therapy for trauma must take account of that.
History and memory 199
period die, memories become the past and then the collective memories
are reinterpreted into history (then some events and objects of the past
are translated into heritage and become enshrined and unchanging, but
that is not central to the current discussion). In this way, we see how
memories fade, not just through the individuals who experienced signifi-
cant events, but also through their children and grandchildren who
have their own versions of the memories of those times, partly because
they grew up with hearing about the events, partly because parently
behaviour may have been altered by living through the events, and partly
through the surrounding culture, which remains affected by significant
events for many years.
This can be linked to memorialisation and commemoration. We
memorialise to remember, but memorials are stone. Commemorations
make memorials human, and enable us to recall the past. With regard to
the World Wars, until now such commemorations have been attended by
participants and their families; now most participants are dead or very
old. If such commemorations are going to be helpful, they need to also
look forward. One thing humans have been very bad at is learning from
the past. Each generation repeats the mistakes of the previous one. If we
listen to our elders it is to eventually ignore them. For instance, in the
Balkans there have been major wars for most generations for many
hundreds of years. (Ivo Ilich, 1916, 1994 The Bridge Over the Drina
illustrates this very well.)
We are perhaps at a turning point in our understanding of the role of
commemoration, and may be moving from a position of looking
back to looking forward with the past at the forefront. The National
Memorial Arboretum at Alrewas, near Lichfield, is not only a site
containing many memorials commemorating past battles and armed
forces units; it has an explicitly educational focus, and it is also a
meeting place for veterans groups. We may well be, in the Western
world, undergoing a fundamental revision of the ways in which we
interpret history and memory. We are now approaching the point when
everyone who experienced the World Wars is dead. Will we continue to
have a need to commemorate these wars in the ways we have done in
the past? What sense is there in such commemorations for people who
did not live through the wars? If commemoration of war is going to
continue, then it is likely that it will start to take different forms. The
educative function mentioned above is likely to be critical here, and will
necessarily bring together people from a range of disciplines who are in
a position to teach younger people about war, memory and history.
It will certainly not be the sole responsibility of historians; it will
become the preserve of others such as psychologists, cultural theorists
Beyond memory, war and trauma 201
Treatment
While treatment has not been a key focus of this book, it is hoped that
the contents will be useful for clinicians of various perspectives when
dealing with war trauma. It is clear that something more than clinical
skills is needed for a war veteran. The clinician must have an under-
standing of the historical period of the war the battles and politics
involved which all add towards an empathic understanding of the
persons experiences. Beyond that, the clinician should have an under-
standing of the role of narrative and social discourse, both as part of
normal psychological life and as a key part of the way in which a person
thinks and acts as a response to a traumatic event. Explicitly or impli-
citly, narrative development must play a key role in the resolution of
traumatic memories.
Future directions
This is an exciting time to be interested in war trauma research. The area
has matured over the last couple of decades, drawing on the theoretical
work of psychologists who have displayed an interest in the area for more
than a century. There is a sense that an integrative understanding of the
processes biological, neurological, behavioural, cognitive, psycho-
dynamic, humanist, social and cultural is now possible. It is possible
because war trauma presents a practical problem that has to be dealt
with, and once professionals get their hands on a practical problem, they
will find the means of dealing with it irrespective of personal perspective;
so the cognitivist clinician makes use of behavioural understanding, the
humanist uses cognitive behaviour therapy, and the psychodynamic
practitioner recognises that there are occasions when a traumatised
person needs drug therapy. The more open-minded therapists take this
eclectic approach, drawing on the expertise developed over many years
by practitioners in other perspectives.
This then leaves the scientist with a job to do, integrating the work of
these multifaceted perspectives and learning to understand how they fit
together. And in order to do this the scientist must draw on a range of
methods. The experimentalist must now apply qualitative techniques,
and the qualitative practitioner must learn the benefits of quantification.
But this is all within psychology. We are now at the start of something
bigger. Within academia there is now a push for interdisciplinary work,
for academics within one discipline to draw on the skills and expertise of
academics in other disciplines who have very different theories, methods
and even language. The work on war trauma is open to this interdisci-
plinarity. As already discussed, if we want to fully understand the
Future directions 203
We may also be at the point where we are going to change the nature of
commemoration. Throughout the twentieth century, commemoration
related to the World Wars. Once the participants are dead, and the
memories of these wars fades into the past and into the history books,
we will have to question why and how we use commemoration. What is
the purpose of 11 November? Why do we continue to parade in the
streets? Is it appropriate to make these events militaristic? I do not have
the answers to these questions, but it is certain that there will be changes
and, as discussed above, the changes will have to take account of what it
is that we want to suggest to the younger generations about war and
remembering war. Up to now, commemoration of the World Wars has
been a way of letting veterans know that we remember what they did,
and that the difficult and traumatic memories they might have are not
forgotten. This, in itself, has been of personal benefit to thousands of
veterans. Once these people are dead, can commemoration take on a
new role and be educative?
Of course, we still have people dying in what, to us outsiders, are
smaller wars, but to the participants are just as important as any war of
the past. People are endangered, wounded, maimed and killed. Friends
are lost and hardships endured, and still in the cause of the nation, or
the greater good. The difference is that these wars are irrelevant to
most people in the UK. They are minor sideshows that make for pub
arguments and good TV, but they do not affect most of us. Why should
we commemorate the dead of these smaller wars? We will not commem-
orate them in the same way as those of the World Wars simply because
they do not affect us all. There is no total war. Nevertheless, these
participants should not be forgotten, and their actions on behalf of the
nation should not be forgotten, but if we are to remember them, it has to
be in a different context to the current ceremonies.
Finally, this book has been concerned with memory, war and trauma,
specifically with the effects of war on veterans and on others who take
part. Instead of just focusing on the psychological problems that veterans
face the PTSD, anxiety and depression, the alcohol and drug abuse,
and the problems relating to families and work I have tried to take a
much broader perspective and show how veterans live in a social context
and have narratives about their experiences that derive, at least in part,
from that social context. I have tried to show, without providing any
specific techniques, how clinicians may benefit from drawing on this
broader perspective in their treatment of war veterans. Wars can deeply
affect societies, and it is important, if we are to understand how we
remember wars, to take into account the broader perspective, to show
how memory works in the social and collective senses, and how
Future directions 205
206
References 207
Baddeley, A. and Wilson, B. 2002. Prose recall and amnesia: implications for
the structure of working memory, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 41723
Baerger, D. R. and McAdams, D. P. 1999. Life story coherence and its relation
to psychological well-being, Narrative Inquiry 9: 6996
Baker, S. L. 1975. Military psychiatry, in Freedman, A. M., Kaplan, H. I. and
B. J. Sadock (eds.) Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry, vol. II, p. 2355.
Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins
Bamberg, M. and McCabe, A. 1998. Editorial, Narrative Inquiry 8(1): iiiv
Barclay, C. R. and Smith, T. S. 1992. Autobiographical remembering: creating
personal culture, in Conway, M. A., Rubin, D. C., Spinnler, H. and
Wagenaar, W. A. (eds.) Theoretical Perspectives on Autobiographical Memory.
Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers
Barker, C. R. and Last, R. W. 1979. Erich Maria Remarque. London: Oswald
Wolff
Barrett, T. W. and Mizes, J. S. 1988. Combat level and social support in the
development of posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam veterans,
Behavior Modification 12: 100115
Bartlett, F. C. 1932. Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology.
Cambridge University Press
Benowitz, L. I., Moya, K. L. and Levine, D. N. 1990. Impaired verbal reasoning
and constructional apraxia in subjects with right hemisphere damage,
Neuropsychologia 28: 2341
Bertaux, D. 1995. A response to Thierry Kochuyts Biographic and empiricist
illusions: a reply to recent criticism. Biography and Society Newsletter, 26.
Research Committee 38: International Sociological Association
Binneveld, J. M. W. 1997. From Shell Shock to Combat Stress: A Comparative
History of Military Psychiatry. Amsterdam University Press
Bolton, D. and Hill, J. 1996. Mind, Meaning and Mental Disorder. Oxford
University Press
Bonne, O., Brandes, D., Gilboa, A. et al. 2001. Longitudinal MRI study
of hippocampal volume in trauma survivors with PTSD, The American
Journal of Psychiatry 158: 124851
Bornat, J., Dimmock, B., Jones, D. and Peace, S. 2000. Researching the
implications of family change for older people: the contribution of a
life-history approach, in Bornat, J., Chamberlayne, P. and Wengraf, T.
(eds.) The Turn to Biographical Methods in Social Science. London: Taylor &
Francis/Routledge
Bradshaw, S. L., Ohlde, C. D. and Horne, J. B. 1991. The love of war: Vietnam
and the traumatised veteran, Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic 55: 96103
Bramsen, L. and van der Ploeg, H. M. 1999. Fifty years later: the long term
psychological adjustment of aging World War II survivors, Acta Psychiatrica
Scandinavica 100: 3508
Braun, A. R., Guillemin, A., Hosey, L. and Varga, M. 2001. The neural
organisation of discourse: an H2150-PET study of narrative production
in English and American sign language, Brain 124: 202844
Bremner, J. D. 1998. Neuroimaging of posttraumatic stress disorder, Psychiatric
Annals 28: 44550
208 References
Fletcher, P. C., Happe, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C., Dolan, R. J. and Frackowiak,
R. S. J. et al. 1995. Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of
theory of mind in story comprehension, Cognition 57: 10928
Foa, E. B. and Riggs, D. S. 1993. Post-traumatic stress disorder in rape victims,
in Oldham, J., Riba, M. B. and Tasman, A. (eds.) American Psychiatric Press
Review of Psychiatry, vol. 12, pp. 273303. Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Press
Foa, E. B. and Rothbaum, B. O. 1998. Treating the Trauma of Rape: Cognitive
Behavioural Therapy for PTSD. New York: Guilford Press
Foa, E. B., Steketee, G. and Rothbaum, B. O. 1989. Behavioural/cognitive
conceptualisations of post-traumatic stress disorder, Behavior Therapy
20: 15576
Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S. and Gershuny, B. S. 1995. Arousal, numbing, and
intrusion: symptom structure of PTSD following assault, The American
Journal of Psychiatry 152: 11620
Fontana, A. and Rosenheck, R. 1998. Psychological benefits and liabilities of
traumatic exposure in the war zone, Journal of Traumatic Stress 11: 485505
Foy, M. R., Stanton, M. E., Levine, S. and Thompson, R. F. 1987. Behavioural
stress impairs long-term potentiation in rodent hippocampus, Behavioral
and Neural Biology 48: 13849
Frankl, V. E. 1963/1984. Mans Search for Meaning. New York: Pocket Books
Frankl, V. E. 1969. The Will to Meaning: Foundations and Applications of
Logotherapy. New York: World Publishing Company
Frans, O., Rimmo, P. A., Aberg, L. and Fredrikson, M. 2005. Trauma exposure
and post-traumatic stress disorder in the general population, Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica 111: 2919
Fransella, F., Bell, R. and Bannister, D. 2004. A Manual for Repertory Grid
Technique, 2nd edn. West Sussex: John Wiley
Freeman, M. 1993. Rewriting the Self: History, Memory, Narrative. London:
Routledge
Freud, S. 1921. Introduction to psychoanalysis and the war neuroses,
in Strachey, J. (ed.) Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of
Sigmund Freud, vol. 17. London: Hogarth Press
Fromholt, P., Larsen, P. and Larsen, S. F. 1995. Effects of late-onset depression
and recovery on autobiographical memory, Journals of Gerontology Series
B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 50(2): 7481
Fuster, J. M., Bodner, M. and Kroger, J. K. 2000. Cross-modal and cross-
temporal association in neurons of the frontal cortex, Nature 405: 34751
Garcia, J. and Koelling, R. A. 1966. The relation of cue to consequence in
avoidance learning, Psychonomic Science 4: 1234
Gardner, R. A. 1997. The embedment-in-the-brain circuitry phenomenon:
implications for psychoanalytic treatment, The Journal of the American
Academy of Psychoanalysis 25: 16176
Geary, P. 1994. Phantoms of Remembrance: Memory and Oblivion at the End of the
First Millennium. Princeton University Press
Gergen, K. and Gergen, M. M. 1988 Narrative and the self as relationship,
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 21: 1756
212 References
Kang, H., Dalager, N., Mahan, C. and Ishii, E. 2005. The role of sexual
assault on the risk of PTSD among Gulf War veterans, Annals of
Epidemiology 15: 1915
Kenyon, G. M. and Randall, W. L. 1999. Introduction: Narrative Gerontology,
Journal of Aging Studies 13: 115
Kessler, R. C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E. et al. 1995. Posttraumatic stress
disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey, Archives of General Psychiatry
52: 104860
Kihlstrom, J. F. 2002. No need for repression, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6: 12
Kihlstrom, J. F., Beer, J. S. and Klein, S. B. 2002. Self and identity as memory,
in Leary, M. R. and Tangney, J. (eds.) Handbook of Self and Identity,
pp. 6890. New York: Guilford Press
Kilshaw, S. 2004. Friendly fire: the construction of Gulf War Syndrome
narratives, Anthropology & Medicine 11(2): 14960
Kirschenbaum, L. A. 2004. Commemorations of the siege of Leningrad:
a catastrophe in memory and myth, in Gray, P. and Oliver, K. (eds.)
The Memory of Catastrophe. Manchester University Press
Knapp, A. B. 1988. The History and Culture of Ancient Western Asia and Egypt.
Thomson Learning
Knox, D. 2006. The sacralised landscapes of Glencoe: from massacre to
mass tourism, and back again, International Journal of Tourism Research
8: 18597
Koss, M. P., Figuerado, A. J., Bell, I., Tharan, M. and Tromp, S. 1996.
Traumatic memory characteristics: a cross-validated mediational model of
response to rape among employed women, Journal of Abnormal Psychology
105: 42131
Krook, D. 1969. The Elements of Tragedy. Yale University Press
Kubany, E. S. 1994. A cognitive model of guilt typology in combat-related
PTSD, Journal of Traumatic Stress 7: 319
Lang, P. J. 1979. A bio-information theory of emotional imagery, Journal of
Psychophysiology 16: 495512
LeDoux, J. 1995. Emotion: clues from the brain, Annual Review of Psychiatry
46: 20935
Lennon, J. J. and Foley, M. 2000. Dark Tourism: The Attraction of Death and
Disaster. Thomson Learning
Levine, S. and Ursin, H. 1991. What is stress?, in Brown, M. R., Koob, G. F.
and Rivier, C. (eds.) Neurobiology and Neuroendocrinology of Stress, pp. 321.
New York: Marcel Dekker
Lev-Wiesel, R. and Amir, M. 2001. Secondary traumatic stress, psychological
distress, sharing of traumatic reminiscences and marital quality among
spouses of Holocaust child survivors, Journal of Marital and Family Therapy
27(4): 297308
Lev-Wiesel, R. and Amir, M. 2006. Growing out of the ashes: posttraumatic
growth among Holocaust child survivors is it possible?, in Calhoun, L. G.
and Tedeschi, R. G. (eds.) Handbook of Posttraumatic Growth: Research and
Practice, pp. 24863. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum
Linde, C. 1993. Life Stories: The Creation of Coherence. Oxford University Press
References 215
222
Index 223