Mental Abacus PDF
Mental Abacus PDF
Mental Abacus PDF
Please note: this manuscript is for noncommercial use only. It is the pre-publication version, and as such,
may contain errors not present in the final publication.
instruction
David Barner1, George Alvarez2, Jessica Sullivan3, Neon Brooks2, Mahesh Srinivasan4, and
Michael C. Frank5
1
University of California, San Diego
2
Harvard University
3
Skidmore College
4
University of California, Berkeley
5
Stanford University
Acknowledgements: We would like to thank the children, families, and staff of Zenith School in
Vadodara, India for their patience and generosity over the past several years. In particular, we
would like to thank Abbasi Barodawala and Mary Joseph of Zenith School, and Snehal Karia and
Anand Karia of UCMAS India, for their invaluable contributions. Thank you also to Sean Barner,
Eleanor Chestnut, Jonathan Gill, Ali Horowitz, Talia Konkle, Ally Kraus, Molly Lewis, Bria
Long, Ann Nordmeyer, Viola Strmer, Jordan Suchow, and Katharine Tillman for help with data
collection. This work was funded by a grant to D.B. and G.A. from NSF REESE grant #0910206
Contact Information
Abstract: Mental abacus (MA) is a technique for performing fast and accurate arithmetic using a
mental image of a physical abacus. Expert users exhibit astonishing calculation abilities. In a 3-
year randomized, controlled trial of 204 elementary-school students, we investigated the nature
whether expertise which requires sustained practice of mental imagery is driven by changes
to basic cognitive capacities like working memory. MA students improved on arithmetic tasks
relative to controls, but training was not associated with changes to basic cognitive abilities.
Instead, differences in spatial working memory at the beginning of the study mediated MA
Introduction
and a set of arithmetic routines that operate on these numerals. For many children around the
world, early math lessons are supplemented by the use of an abacus, a physical manipulative
designed for representing exact quantities via the positions of counters, whose historical origins
date to 1200 AD or earlier.1 Extending the use of the physical abacus, children in some countries
also learn a technique known as mental abacus (MA). Using MA, users create and manipulate a
mental image of the physical device to perform arithmetic operations (see Figure 1 for details of
how MA represents number). Expert users of MA exhibit abilities that are nothing short of
enables rapid calculation even when users are speaking concurrently (Hatano, Miyake, & Binks,
1977), and it allows its users to dominate international calculation competitions like the Mental
Calculation World Cup. In the present study, we explored the nature of MA expertise.
Specifically, we asked whether the extraordinary levels of achievement witnessed in experts can
be attained by students in large K-12 classroom settings. In doing so, we asked a more general
question regarding the nature of expertise, and whether attaining unusual levels of performance
requires changes to basic cognitive capacities, or can instead arise via the exploitation of existing
visuospatial working memory, as well as motor procedures that are learned during initial
physical abacus training. Although arithmetic computations of untrained college students are
1
Systems with similar physical structure perhaps direct predecessors of the abacus emerged
much earlier, and included the Roman counting board and the Babylonian counting tablet
(Menninger, 1969).
Mental Abacus 4
Figure 1. The Japanese soroban-style abacus used by participants in this study, shown here
representing the value 123,456,789. A physical abacus represents number via the arrangement
of beads into columns, each of which represents a place value (e.g., ones, tens, hundreds,
thousands, etc...), with values increasing from right to left. To become proficient at MA, users
of the physical abacus learn to create a mental image of the device and to manipulate this
image to perform computations.
highly disrupted by verbal interference (e.g., concurrent speaking), MA users are less affected by
concurrent linguistic tasks, and much more affected by motor interference (Frank & Barner,
2011; Hatano, Miyake, & Binks, 1977). Consistent with this, while standard arithmetic routines
recruit brain regions related to verbal processing and verbal working memory, MA computations
selectively activate regions associated with vision and spatial working memory (Chen et al.,
2006; Hu et al., 2011; Tanaka, Michimata, Kaminaga, Honda, & Sadato, 2002). Finally, MA
users computational limits and their patterns of computation errors are consistent with known
limits to visual working memory, and suggest that each abacus column is represented as a
not shed light on whether MA training can produce benefits for a broad range of students in a
standard classroom setting.2 The present study tested this, and asked whether MA expertise
2
Most previous studies of MA have investigated experts who have elected to receive extensive
training outside of school, raising the possibility that expertise is possible only in individuals
who have particular mathematical talents or interests. Similarly, studies that have tested MA
Mental Abacus 5
results from changes in the users ability to create and manipulate structures in visual working
existing abilities such that expertise arises only in individuals with relatively strong spatial
working memory abilities (a hypothesis we call cognitive moderation, because these cognitive
abilities would serve as moderators of the techniques efficacy; Baron & Kenny, 1986).
for large groups of children in a K-12 setting, and these benefits may be due to gains in basic
cognitive abilities like imagery, working memory, and attention that appear to be important
for MA computation. Examples of this kind of transfer come from executive function and spatial
cognition interventions, which are both argued to provide a cognitive route for improving
academic performance (Diamond & Lee, 2011; Uttal et al., 2013). In the case of MA, mastery
requires hours of intensive practice with mental imagery, attentional allocation, and cross-
domain integration of information (e.g., from numerical symbols to beads and back again), hence
subset of students, such that expertise is best predicted by performance on cognitive batteries at
the beginning of MA training (Frank & Barner, 2011). Rather than stemming from changes to an
individuals basic cognitive capacities, MA expertise may result when MA is learned and
practiced by individuals who are particularly able to perform complex computations in working
To explore the nature of MA and its utility in large classroom settings, we randomly
assigned children from the same school to receive training in either MA or a standard
training in non-experts have not randomly assigned participants to training conditions,
complicating their interpretation (e.g., Stigler, Chalip, & Miller, 1986).
Mental Abacus 6
mathematics curriculum. We conducted our study at a school located in Vadodara, India, where a
short (1 hour) weekly MA training had previously supplemented the standard curriculum for
students in the second grade and above, and thus instructors and appropriate training
infrastructure were already in place. Starting at the beginning of the second grade, half of the
children in our study were assigned to study MA for three hours per week (MA group). The
remaining half of students were assigned to receive no abacus training, and instead perform three
their existing mathematics coursework (Control group). Thus, the total amount of mathematics
training received by each group was identical. We followed children over the course of three
years, and assessed outcomes using a battery of mathematical and cognitive assessments,
including measures of mental rotation, approximate number, and spatial and verbal working
memory. These tasks were administered both prior to intervention and at the end of each school
year so as to probe the extent of any possible cognitive mediation or transfer effects.
Methods
Participants
children in Vadodara, India. At the initiation of the study, over 80% of children attending the
school came from families who earned less than $2,000 US per annum (~$5.50/day). In our
sample, 59% of children came from Hindu families and 41% from Muslim families. The total
population of the school was approximately 2100 students, ranging from pre-K to high school.
At the time of enrollment (which we refer to as Year 0), the participants were 204
children aged 5 7 years old who were beginning their 2nd grade year. We randomly assigned
these students to two groups, MA and Control. We then further randomly assigned children into
Mental Abacus 7
assignments in the previous year), with one classroom comprised of MA students, one of Control
Of the 100 students in the MA group and the 104 students in the Control group, 88 (88%)
and 99 (95%) provided some data in every year of testing, respectively. Missing data were
sometimes due to sickness or absenteeism, but primarily to changes of school. We analyzed data
Intervention Procedure
Children in both the MA and Control groups studied the schools standard (non-abacus)
mathematics curriculum over the duration of the study. Additionally, both groups received three
hours per week of additional mathematics instruction as follows. In the MA group, children were
given three hours per week of instruction in the use of the physical and mental abacus by an
experienced MA teacher outside the childrens home classroom (such that control group children
were not exposed to MA technique). Abacus instruction was broken into two 90-minute sessions
per week and followed a common international curriculum that begins with use of the physical
abacus for addition and subtraction, and then moves to mental abacus computations. The first
year of training focused primarily on the physical abacus, with greater emphasis placed on MA
in subsequent years. Common activities in the MA training program included worksheet practice
of addition and subtraction, practice translating abacus configurations into Arabic numerals, and
Control students were provided with three hours per week of supplemental mathematics
training using international curriculum materials. Activities from the supplemental curriculum
Mental Abacus 8
included extra practice on topics from the standard math curriculum, clock reading, and the use
Assessment Procedure
The study spanned three years of the participants elementary education, and began with
a baseline test before training began. In each of four annual assessments, children received a
large battery of computerized and paper-based tasks. Year 0 assessments were given at the
beginning of 2nd grade; Year 1 3 assessments were given at the end of 2nd, 3rd, and 4th grade,
respectively. All assessments included both measures of mathematics and more general cognitive
measures. A small number of other tasks were included but are not discussed in the current
manuscript (see Supplemental Online Material for detailed description of all measures).
Johnson Tests of Achievement (WJ-III) and the Math Fluency subtest of the Wechsler Individual
skill that, unlike the standardized tests, were designed to specifically target arithmetic skills
acquired between 2nd and 4th grade (see SOM). The first measured childrens arithmetic abilities
problems from Ravens Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1998), as well as two paper-based tests of
speeded mental rotation (one using letters and one using shapes). They also completed three
computerized tasks: (1) an adaptive test of spatial working memory (SWM); (2) an adaptive test
of verbal working memory (VWM); (3) a number comparison task, in which children were asked
Mental Abacus 9
to indicate the larger of two dot arrays (see SOM Section 1 for detailed task descriptions and
related citations). For the working memory tasks, we report estimates of span i.e., the average
number of items on which a child was successful. For the number comparison task, we report
Weber fractions (a measure of approximate number acuity, estimated from our task via the
Grades. For each year, we obtained childrens grades in English, Math, Science, and
Abacus Only Measures. For each year after the intervention began, students in the MA
group completed a set of three paper and pencil tasks to assess their ability to use an abacus. All
three were administered after the end of all other testing. The first two, Abacus Sums (addition)
and Abacus Arithmetic (addition and subtraction), tested the ability to do computations using a
physical abacus, while the third, Abacus Decoding, tested the ability to decode abacus images
into standard Arabic numerals. These tasks allowed us to verify that any differences between the
Attitude measures. In Year 3, we administered two measures that explored whether the
intervention had changed childrens attitudes towards mathematics, and thus whether training
effects might be mediated by differences in motivational level and engagement with mathematics.
probed childrens attitudes regarding the malleability of their own intelligence. Second, we
administered a math-anxiety questionnaire (adapted from Ramirez et al., 2013), which measured
the anxiety that children experienced when solving different kinds of math problems and
Data Analysis3
We used a longitudinal mixed models approach to quantify the statistical reliability of the
effects of randomization to training group on our outcome measures. For each outcome measure,
we fit a baseline model that included a growth term for each student over time and an overall
main effect of intervention-group (to control for differences between groups at study initiation).
We then tested whether the fit of this model was improved significantly by an interaction term
Because we did not have any a priori hypotheses about the shape of the dose-response
function between the intervention and particular measures of interest, we fit models using three
types of growth terms: (1) simple linear growth over time, (2) quadratic growth over time, and
(3) independent growth for each year after baseline. This last type of model allows for the
assignment and growth in each of these models. All p values are reported from likelihood ratio
Results
mathematical abilities relative to the control group. Consistent with this, in Year 3, we observed
numerical differences between the two groups on three out of the four mathematics tasks, with
effect sizes of Cohens d = .60 (95% CI: .30 - .89) for arithmetic, .24 (-.05 - .52) for WJ-III,
3
All data and code for the analyses reported here are available at
http://github.com/langcog/mentalabacus
Mental Abacus 11
Figure 2. Mathematics outcome measures for the two intervention conditions, plotted by study
year (with 0 being pre-intervention). Error bars show 95% confidence intervals computed by
non-parametric bootstrap.
and .28 (.00 - .57) for place value. 4 We observed only a small numerical difference for WIAT-III,
group were in fact driven by additional MA training (Figure 2). All three of these models (i.e.,
4
The confidence intervals for these effect sizes represent confidence intervals on pairwise tests
for Year 3 alone; the statistical significance of the broader patterns we observed are best captured
by the longitudinal models described below.
Mental Abacus 12
linear, quadratic, and non-monotonic) showed strong time by condition interactions for both the
arithmetic and WJ-III measures, suggesting that performance on each of these tasks did improve
with additional MA training (likelihood-ratio tests for adding the time by condition interaction
term to the growth model were 2linear(1) > 6.33, 2quadratic(2) > 11.56, and 2independent(3) > 12.51,
with ps < .01 in all cases). Consistent with the small numerical difference observed between
groups on the WIAT-III, this measure did not approach significance in any of the three growth
models. The smaller effects observed on the standardized measures are perhaps not surprising,
given the smaller number of arithmetic-focused items on these measures and hence the
likelihood of them having lower sensitivity to individual differences between children (see SOM
Section 4.7 for further analysis). More surprising, however, was that the place-value measure did
not approach significance in the growth models, especially given that we observed substantial
numerical differences between the groups (e.g., performance in year 3 differed significantly
between groups in a univariate analysis, t(185) = 1.96, p = .05). We speculate that we did not
observe consistent growth with this measure due to its low reliability from Year 0 to Year 1 (r
= .22). Together, these analyses suggest that the MA intervention was more effective in building
mathematics techniques. Effects of MA training on arithmetic ability were observed not only in
our in-house measure, which included many arithmetic problems tailored to the level of
elementary school students, but also on the WJ-III, a widely-used standardized measure that
includes a range of problem types and formats. While the evidence for differential gains in
conceptual understanding of place value was more limited, MA students did not fall behind
students in the Control group, despite the fact that the MA curriculum primarily stresses rote
Figure 3. Cognitive outcome measures for the two intervention conditions, plotted by study
year. Top axes show mean items correct in working memory span tasks, while bottom axes
show proportion correct across trials in number comparison and mental rotation tasks. Error
bars show 95% confidence intervals computed by non-parametric bootstrap.
Cognitive outcomes. Whereas MA training produced consistent gains in arithmetic ability, it did
not produce consistent gains in the cognitive abilities we measured (Figure 3). Higher math
performance in the MA group was therefore not the result of improved cognitive capacity due to
MA training. For example, in Year 3, we observed between-group effect sizes of -0.16 (95% CI:
-0.45 - 0.12) on our number comparison measure (note that smaller Weber fractions indicate
more accurate estimations). Also, we found an effect size of -0.14 (-0.43 0.15) for Ravens
Mental Abacus 14
progressive matrices, of -.06 for mental rotation (-0.34 0.23), and of .05 (-.24 - .34) for spatial
working memory. Only one cognitive measure verbal working memory showed an advantage
For these cognitive measures, as with the arithmetic tasks, we used longitudinal growth
models to assess whether advantages for the MA group were driven by training in MA. Because
we used different sets of Ravens problems for each year, we could not fit growth models, but t-
tests showed no reliable effects of MA training for any year (all ts < .96, ps > .34). Similarly,
longitudinal models (linear, quadratic, and independent) confirmed that none of the cognitive
tasks (numerical comparison, mental rotation, verbal working memory, or spatial working
memory), showed significant time by condition interactions, with one exception. For verbal
working memory, the non-independent growth model showed a significant time by condition
interaction (p < .01), though both linear and quadratic growth models showed no significant time
by condition interaction (SOM Section 4.4). Thus, this result appears to have been driven by the
fast growth in verbal working memory span in Year 1 exhibited by the MA group, relative to the
similar trend observed in spatial working memory in Years 1 and 2 (significant or close to
significant in individual t-tests, t(185) = 2.36, p = .02 and t(184) = 1.84, p = .07, but not in any
longitudinal model). In both cases, the overall shape of the developmental curve is asymptotic,
with working memory spans approaching approximately four items by Year 2 in the MA group.
This pattern could be interpreted as evidence that differences in working memory between the
MA and control groups do exist, but are expressed only in the rate of growth to asymptote, rather
than in the absolute level of the asymptote itself. Against this hypothesis, however, additional
Mental Abacus 15
analyses (SOM Section 4.5) find that (1) our spatial working memory task did not exhibit ceiling
effects and (2) data from 20 American college undergraduates and 67 high socio-economic status
(SES) Indian children from the same region of India show that children in our study had overall
lower spatial working memory than higher-SES children, and were far from being at adult levels
of performance. Most important, these Year 1 effects surfaced before children began to receive
training on the mental component of MA and were still learning the physical technique. We
therefore do not believe that this result is likely to be related to the ultimate gains we see in MA
Academic outcomes. MA did not produce large, consistent changes in students grades
across academic subjects, although we saw some small trends towards better math, science, and
computer grades in the MA group in some models. These differences are subject to teacher bias,
however, since teachers were of course knowledgeable about the intervention. Thus, we do not
believe they should be weighted heavily in evaluating performance, especially since our own
standardized measures of mathematical competence were available for analysis (for additional
Attitude Measures. There were no differences between groups on either childrens self-
reported mathematics anxiety (t(184) = 1.05, p = .29) or their endorsement of a growth mindset
measures were due to differential effects of our intervention on childrens anxiety about
Figure 4. Performance on the arithmetic task, split by both intervention condition and median
spatial working memory performance in Year 0. Error bars show 95% confidence intervals;
lines show best fitting quadratic curves.
Mediators of intervention effects. Given that MA training produced gains in math
outcomes, we next asked which factors mediated these gains, and thus whether individual
already noted, MA training did not augment cognitive abilities, so the math advantages in the
MA group could not have been driven by enhanced working memory, mental imagery, or
approximate number acuity that resulted from MA training. However, it is possible that
individual cognitive differences between children in the MA group (prior to their entry into the
study) were responsible for how well they learned and benefitted from MA. To explore this
Our analytic approach relied on the same longitudinal modeling approach described
above. For each math outcome variable, we fit models that included participants Year 0
performance on each cognitive predictor (for simplicity and to avoid over-parameterizing our
models, we used linear and quadratic models only). The coefficient of interest was a three-way
Mental Abacus 17
interaction of time, condition, and initial performance on the cognitive predictor of interest. This
three-way interaction term captures the intuition that growth in performance on a task for MA
used likelihood ratio tests to assess whether these interaction terms improved model fit.5
SWM group with an average threshold of 1.9 items, and a high SWM group with an average
threshold of 3.7 items. Previous studies of SWM thresholds for middle to high SES 5- to 7-year-
olds find thresholds of approximately 3.5 4 items (Pickering, 2010; Logie & Pearson, 1997).
Thus, a subset of children in our study exhibited especially low SWM capacity. Related to this,
spatial working memory was a reliable moderator in both linear and quadratic models of
arithmetic (Figure 4). Those children who began the study with relatively strong spatial working
memory skills and who were randomly assigned to MA training showed significantly stronger
growth in our arithmetic assessment (2linear(1) = 4.63, p < .05; 2quadratic(2) = 5.93, p = .05).
Relative to receiving equal amounts of standard math training, children with weaker spatial
working memory did not appear to benefit differentially from the MA intervention, and instead
performed at an equivalent level to the students in the control group. Since the MA technique
relies on visuo-spatial resources for storage of the abacus image during computation (Frank &
Barner, 2011), it seems likely that those children with relatively lower spatial working memory
5
Although with greater numbers of longitudinal measurements we could potentially have
detected interactive growth patterns (e.g. gains in working memory driving later gains in
mathematics), our current study did not have the temporal resolution for these analyses. We thus
restrict our analysis to testing for mediation in mathematics outcomes on the basis of each of the
cognitive variables measured at Year 0.
Mental Abacus 18
Figure 5. Performance on the Physical Abacus Sums, Decoding, and Arithmetic tasks
(administered in years 1 3), plotted by a median split on spatial working memory. Error bars
show 95% confidence intervals computed by non-parametric bootstrap.
There was no comparable mediation effect with verbal working memory (additional
analysis in SOM Section 4.4), but a small number of additional moderation effects did approach
significance in one of the two models. There was a trend towards an effect of spatial working
memory on WJ-III (the other mathematics measure that showed strong MA effects; 2quadratic(2) =
5.59, p = .06, Figure S3). In addition, there were trends towards effects of Year 0 mental rotation
performance on arithmetic (2linear(1) = 2.71, p = .10) and place value (2linear(1) = 3.27, p = .07),
and an effect of number comparison acuity on WJ-III (2quadratic(2) = 8.29, p = .02, Figure S4).
These effects, though more tentatively supported, are nevertheless consistent with the hypothesis
that the MA intervention was most effective for children with greater visuo-spatial abilities at the
beginning of instruction.
Abacus only measures. Confirming that children in the MA group learned to use an
abacus, we found consistently high performance in Abacus Decoding for the MA group (> 80%
correct for all years). Also, performance on the abacus arithmetic and sums tasks rose
substantially from year to year, suggesting that childrens abacus computation abilities improved
Mental Abacus 19
over the course of their training (Figure 5). Performance on these tests of physical abacus
arithmetic were significantly correlated with performance on our other math measures (In-house
arithmetic: r = .69, .74, and .81 for Years 1 3 respectively, all ps < .0001; WIAT: r = .57, .54, .73, all ps
Critically, spatial working memory span in Year 0 was also related to intervention uptake,
as measured by the Abacus Only tasks, which were accomplished using a physical abacus.
Because we did not have Abacus Only data for Year 0, we could not directly test whether spatial
working memory moderated growth, but we did find a main effect of spatial working memory on
all three measures of abacus uptake for both linear and quadratic growth models (all 2(1) > 3.97,
Discussion
Our study investigated the nature of mental abacus (MA) expertise, and whether MA is
an effective tool for improving math outcomes in a standard classroom setting. To do this, we
conducted a three-year longitudinal study of mental abacus training. We found that MA training
led to substantial gains in students ability to perform accurate arithmetic computations. These
gains began to emerge after a single year of training suggesting that simply learning to use the
physical abacus had some effects on students mathematics aptitude, even prior to learning the
MA technique and became more pronounced with time. Consistent with this explanation and
with a role for abacus expertise in explaining the intervention effect we found that physical
abacus expertise at the end of the study was significantly correlated with arithmetic performance
across all math measures within our experimental group. Although there were signs of early
gains in cognitive capacities like spatial working memory in the MA group, such effects did not
persist to the end of the study, and could not explain gains in mathematics achievement. Also,
Mental Abacus 20
Our findings are consistent with previous suggestions that cognitive transfer is rare.
Although performance on basic measures of attention and memory can be improved via direct
training on those measures (Diamond & Lee, 2011; Gathercole, Dunning, Holmes, & Wass,
under review; Melby-Lervg, & Hulme, 2012; Noack, Lvdn, Schmiedek, & Lindenberger,
2009), it may be difficult to achieve far transfer from training on unrelated tasks, even with
hours of focused practice (Dunning, Holmes, & Gathercole, 2013; Owen et al., 2010; Redick et
al., 2013). However, our findings suggest that although cognitive capacities are not importantly
altered by MA, they may predict which children will benefit most from MA training. MA
students who began our study with low SWM abilities did not differ in their math performance
from Control students, while those above the median made large gains on our arithmetic measure
Because MA relies on visuo-spatial resources for the storage and maintenance of abacus
images during computation (Frank & Barner, 2011), children with especially weak SWM may
have attained only basic MA abilities enough to reap benefits equal to additional hours of
standard math, but not to acquire unusual expertise. This finding suggests that the development
of MA expertise is mediated by childrens pre-existing cognitive abilities, and thus that MA may
not be suitable for all K-12 classroom environments, especially in groups of children who have
low spatial working memory or attentional capacities (which may have been the case in our
study). Critically, this does not mean that MA expertise depends on unusually strong cognitive
abilities. Perhaps because we studied children from relatively disadvantaged backgrounds, few
Mental Abacus 21
children in our sample had SWM capacities comparable to those seen among typical children in
Our study leaves open several questions about MA as an educational intervention. First, it
remains uncertain how much training is necessary to benefit from MA. In our study, children
received over 100 hours of MA instruction over three years; however, we found evidence that
children began to show improvements after a smaller amount of training (within one year of
exposure to the technique). Future studies should investigate the efficacy of MA training in
smaller, more focused sessions, and also whether the required number of training hours is
smaller in different populations (e.g., in middle or high-SES groups). Second, our results speak
to the efficacy of concrete manipulative systems in the classroom, and suggest that relatively
extensive training may be required to yield benefits. Previous studies have found mixed results
regarding the effectiveness of manipulatives for teaching mathematics (Ball, 1992; Uttal,
Scudder, & Deloache, 1997). However, MA may be unlike other manipulative systems.
Although the abacus is a concrete representation of numerosity that can be used to reinforce
abstract concepts, MA is unique in requiring the use of highly routinized procedures for
arithmetic calculation. Thus, additional research is needed to understand how MA differs from
other manipulatives. Finally, and most importantly, our study did not investigate the long-term
technology for arithmetic and accounting affords some children a significant advantage in
arithmetic calculation compared with additional hours of standard math training. Our evidence
also suggests that MA provides this benefit by building on childrens pre-existing cognitive
Mental Abacus 22
capacities. Future studies should explore the long-term benefits of enhanced arithmetic abilities
using MA and the generalizability of this technique to other groups and cultural contexts.
Mental Abacus 23
Author Contributions
All authors contributed to the design of the study and to data collection. Data analyses were
prepared chiefly by MF. All authors contributed to writing the manuscript. All authors approved
References
Ball, D. L. (1992). Magical hopes: Manipulatives and the reform of math education. American
Baron, R., & Kenny, D. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social
Chen, F., Hu, Z., Zhao, X., Wang, R., Yang, Z., Wang, X., et al. (2006). Neural correlates
Diamond, A. & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to Aid Executive Function Development in
Dunning, D.L., Holmes, J., & Gathercole, S. (2013) Does working memory training lead to
Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (Eds.). (1991). Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and
Frank, M. C & Barner, D. (2011). Representing exact number visually using mental abacus.
Halberda J., Mazzocco, MM. & Feigenson, L. (2008). Individual differences in non-verbal
Hatano, G., Miyake, Y. & Binks, M. G. (1977). Performance of expert abacus operators.
Mental Abacus 25
Cognition, 5, 57-71.
Hu, Y., Geng, F., Tao, L., Hu, N., Du, F., Fu, K., & Chen, F. (2011). Enhanced white matter tract
integrity in children with abacus training. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 1021.
Kojima, T. (1954). The Japanese abacus: Its use and theory. Tokyo, Japan: Tuttle.
Logie, R., & Pearson, D. (1997). The inner eye and the inner scribe of visuo-spatial working
Psychology, 9, 241-257.
Menninger, K. (1969). Number words and number symbols: A cultural history of numbers.
Noack, H., Lvdn, M., Schmiedek, F., Lindenberger, U. (2013). Age-related differences in
temporal and spatial dimensions of episodic memory performance before and after
Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A. S., & Ballard, C.G.
Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E. A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2013). Math anxiety, working
memory, and math achievement in early elementary school. Journal of Cognition and
Raven, J. (1998). Manual for Ravens progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Oxford
Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L. Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D. Z., Kane, M.
Stigler, J. W., Chalip, L., & Miller, K. (1986). Consequences of skill: The case of abacus training
Tanaka, S., Michimata, C., Kaminaga, T., Honda, M., & Sadato, N. (2002). Superior digit
13(17), 2187.
Uttal, D. H., Scudder, K. V., & DeLoache, J. S. (1997). Manipulatives as symbols: A new
Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., and Newcombe,