2009 Viewing CC As Dilemmas
2009 Viewing CC As Dilemmas
2009 Viewing CC As Dilemmas
GUILLEM FEIXAS
University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
LUIS ANGEL SAUL
Universidad Nacional de Educacion a Distancia (UNED), Madrid, Spain
ALEJANDRO AVILA-ESPADA
Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
The idea that internal conflicts play a significant role in mental health has
been extensively addressed in various psychological traditions, including per-
sonal construct theory. In the context of the latter, several measures of conflict
have been operationalized using the Repertory Grid Technique (RGT). All of
them capture the notion that change, although desirable from the viewpoint of
a given set of constructs, becomes undesirable from the perspective of other con-
structs. The goal of this study is to explore the presence of cognitive conflicts in
a clinical sample (n = 284) and compare it to a control sample (n = 322).
It is also meant to clarify which among the different types of conflict studied
provides a greater clinical value and to investigate its relationship to symptom
severity (SCL-90-R). Of the types of cognitive conflict studied, implicative dilem-
mas were the only ones to discriminate between clinical and nonclinical samples.
These dilemmas were found in 34% of the nonclinical sample and in 53% of
the clinical sample. Participants with implicative dilemmas showed higher symp-
tom severity, and those from the clinical sample displayed a higher frequency of
dilemmas than those from the nonclinical sample.
141
142 G. Feixas et al.
Such conflicts can be overt or latent, and in certain cases the latter
can reveal themselves in a different but related manner within the
overt conflict and translate into the formation of symptoms, be-
havioral disorders, personality disruptions, and so on (Laplanche
& Pontalis, 1967).
On the other hand, cognitive-social theorists such as Heider
(1946) and Festinger (1957), with their respective theories on
balance and cognitive dissonance, address the issue of there
being little internal consistency. These theories postulate that
such dissonances generate a motivational tendency to elude or
resolve these contradictory cognitions regarding social reality: If
no balanced state exists, then forces towards this state will arise.
. . . If a change is not possible, the state of imbalance will produce
tension (Heider, 1946, p. 108).
Interestingly, the relevance of conflict has also been ad-
dressed by constructivist pioneers. Jean Piaget (e.g., 1974), pro-
posed the term cognitive conflict for describing the contradic-
tions encountered by a child when attempting to explain certain
events. In this case, such conflicts cause a cognitive imbalance and
the child is forced to reorganize his or her intellectual processes
in order to rid him- or herself of the problem; hence the subse-
quent intellectual change.
However, Kellys (1955) personal construct theory (PCT)
probably represents the most elaborate framework for an under-
standing of cognitive conflict and its relevance for personality and
clinical psychology. Its emphasis on human freedom and choice
allow for a view of human beings facing personal dilemmas. Cer-
tainly, if human beings are not seen with a certain degree of free-
dom and agency, they can hardly have dilemmas. The central no-
tion in Kellys PCT is that of personal constructs. These, for him,
are avenues of meaning with two poles. Because of their bipolar
nature, they capture a difference. They also allow for similarity,
when a third element (or more) is added to one of the two poles.
Because the notion of personal construct is based on difference it
allows the possibility of conflict.
A constructivist position is necessary to consider conflicts oc-
curring in the context of the persons own constructions.
Over and over again, it appeared that our clients were making their
choices, not in terms of the alternatives we saw open to them, but in terms
Viewing Cognitive Conflicts as Dilemmas 143
of the alternatives they saw open to them. It was their network of construc-
tions that made up the daily mazes that they ran, not the pure realities
that appeared to us to surround them. To try to explain a temper tantrum
or an acute schizophrenic episode in terms of motives only was to miss
the whole point of the clients system of personal dilemmas. (Kelly, 1969,
p. 84)
A given event can be said to be ambiguous when it becomes the focus of ex-
pectations which are inconsistent with one another in terms of the specific
relationships between constructs which articulate the logical structure of
an individuals system. (Adams-Webber, 1981, p. 55)
the GRIDCOR v. 4.0 (Feixas & Cornejo, 2002) grid analysis pro-
gram, is based on the distinction between these two types of
constructs: discrepant constructs (e.g., timidsocial) reflect a lack
of satisfaction for the self as located at the pole representing
the symptom or problem and viewing the ideal self represented
by the opposite pole. On the contrary, congruent constructs (e.g.,
modestarrogant) represent a construct where self and ideal self
concur (e.g., both on modest). Thus, the subject doesnt view
any need for change in this construct.
The divergence between self and ideal self in the discrepant
construct calls for a change (e.g., becoming social) that appears
to be meaningful for the system because it is based on the subjects
own appreciation. However, the dilemma occurs when change in
this construct is linked to an undesired change in a congruent
construct (e.g., becoming arrogant). By virtue of this associa-
tion, change is at the same time meaningful for the discrepant
construct and meaningless for the congruent one. This contradic-
tion reflects that the system has generated diverging, incompati-
ble goals (e.g., being social and modest), a phenomenon that
could be associated to fragmentation. To put it in other terms,
a movement of the self to the opposite pole of the discrepant
construct (symptom cessation) would result in validation. But, be-
cause this construct is connected by an implication line with a con-
gruent construct, that change would result in invalidation. There-
fore, we consider the situation as dilemmatic because remaining
in the undesired pole (e.g., being timid) is at the same time
validating and invalidating. Equally, the perspective of change is
viewed as providing both validation and invalidation. In this situ-
ation, following the choice corollary, we can infer that the system
chooses the present (problematic) position for the self as a way
to prevent the congruent construct from invalidation. This no-
tion of implicative dilemma (see Feixas & Saul [2005] for a case
illustration), and its operative definition in the grid, reflects the
tradition of PCT in explaining the clinical phenomena called
neurotic paradox and resistance.
Method
Participants
This study includes two different samples with a total of 606 sub-
jects. The first group consists of 322 participants (53% of the
total), from now on the nonclinical sample (volunteers evalu-
ated by trained psychology students). The second, with 284 par-
ticipants (47% of the total), is the clinical sample (persons who
attend a clinic in order to receive psychotherapy).
Gender is not equally distributed in both samples. In
the nonclinical sample, no significant difference exists in the
male/female ratio (44% and 56%, respectively). However, there is
a noticeable difference in the clinical sample (27% male and 73%
female). Indeed, this uneven proportion in the clinical sample
reflects what is usually found in clinical psychotherapy services,
wherein the majority of attendants are women (Caro, 2001).5
Concerning the age of the participants who make up the two
samples, the average for the nonclinical sample is 27.66 years
(SD = 8.92), and the average for the clinical sample is 30.89
(SD = 9.38). As these two samples are not comparable in terms of
either sex or age, these variables are controlled in the data analysis
process.
With respect to the type of diagnosis in the clinical sample,
the most common are anxiety (29%), eating (29%), and mood
disorders (19%). These three diagnoses represent over 76% of the
Viewing Cognitive Conflicts as Dilemmas 153
sample and 15,17 for the clinical one). Elements are rated on
these constructs on a 7-point Likert-type scale for which 1 refers
to the left or emerging pole, 7 the right pole, and 4 the middle
point.
Of all the indices and measures that can be obtained through
the RGT, only those relating to cognitive conflicts have been used
in this study.
Triadic conflicts are determined by the imbalance in a
triad of constructs. The proponents of this measure (Slade &
Sheehan, 1979) identify a TC whenever three constructs corre-
late negatively, or when two correlate positively and the third
negatively. As a criterion of association between two constructs,
however, Feixas and Cornejo (1996; see also Feixas, Bach & Laso,
2004) established a cut-off point with a minimum magnitude of
0.20 in the Pearsons product momentum correlations among
them as a more astringent criterion.6 Two measures can then be
derived from this type of conflict. The first, presence/absence of
TC, is categorical with two values: presence of at least one triadic
conflict and complete absence of triadic conflicts.
We call the second measure the index of triadic conflicts (ITC).
This index was first used by Feixas and Cornejo (1996) and in-
dicates the scale of triadic conflict that exists in a grid as re-
garding the number of possible triads based on the number of
constructs. It is calculated by dividing the number of conflicts
found in the grid by the total number of possible triads of con-
flict. It is a quantitative variable that ranges from 0 to 1, although
its values are usually very low, as the number of possible tri-
ads of constructs (the possible combinations of constructs taken
three at a time) is very large. For that reason it is multiplied by
100.
ID
PID = 100
(n!/2[(n 2)!])
ID: number of implicative dilemmas identified in a grid
n: number of constructs in a grid
no. of DCs
PDC =
no. of constructs
Procedure
The data from the clinical sample were gathered from various
psychotherapy services, both public and private, cooperating in
Viewing Cognitive Conflicts as Dilemmas 157
Results
In this section, incidence levels are shown for each conflict type
by samples, with the results from the comparative statistical anal-
yses performed. Next, and only for participants with each type of
conflict, comparative results for the quantitative conflict indices
(ITC, PID, PDC) in the two samples are shown. Such indices are
even more reliable as frequency indicators than the raw frequency
of cognitive conflict, as they do not depend on the size of the grid
applied.
After having established which type of conflict displays the
strongest differences between clinical and nonclinical samples,
the relationship between that type of cognitive conflict and the
level of symptom severity (GSI) will be explored.
158 G. Feixas et al.
Triadic Conflicts
Implicative Dilemmas
Dilemmatic Constructs
IDs and in women. Besides, but only among women, this proba-
bility decreases with age.
Looking at the correlations among the quantitative measures
(ITC, PID, PDC) of each type of conflict might provide some in-
formation about their relationships. To avoid skewed distributions
(with more than half of the subjects with a 0 value), the sample
used to compute these correlations was composed only by the sub-
jects who had at least one of each of the three types of dilemmas
(n = 88 from the total sample of the study). The only signifi-
cant correlation was between ITC and PID (r = 0.43, p = 0.01),
whereas the other two were close to zero.
Discussion
The data obtained from this study do not appear to confer any
clear role for TCs or DCs in relation to mental health. However,
they do reveal differences between the clinical and nonclinical
samples in regard to IDs. The clinical sample displays a greater
Viewing Cognitive Conflicts as Dilemmas 161
[T]he failure of the construct system to embrace urgent events may ac-
company ones use of incompatible subsystems of constructs. Most of us
can tolerate some amount of incompatibility. Our Fragmentation Corol-
lary assumes that one may successively employ a variety of construction
subsystems which are inferentially incompatible with each other. The Mod-
ulation Corollary . . . assumes that the variation in a persons construc-
tion system is limited by the permeability of the constructs within whose
range of convenience the variants lie. Taken together, these two corollar-
ies assume that one can tolerate some incompatibility but not too much.
(p. 496)
Conclusions
Notes
7. The only rationale provided by the authors for the use of this criterion was
that it excludes low correlations as indicators for an ID. Again, we used this
criterion because the program used was GRIDCOR, and because there are
other studies using the same criterion (e.g., Feixas & Saul, 2004; Feixas,
Cipriano, & Varlotta, 2007).
8. This program, available in Spanish with the name RECORD, was developed by
Feixas and Cornejo (2002) in order to conduct a statistical analysis of the data
matrix obtained by applying the RGT, and to provide the implicit structure of
the data both quickly and clearly. A full RGT manual and a limited-use version
of this program can be found at www.terapiacognitiva.net/record/gridcor-
3.htm.
9. Checking the GSI scores obtained in both groups with respect to the Spanish
norms, we found that, whereas the nonclinical sample bears quite comparable
levels to those of the norms, the clinical group scores below the psychiatric
samples. This suggests the participants in the clinical group of this study were
not showing high levels of symptoms. Therefore, further studies with more
severe or acute samples are required.
References
Warren (Eds.), Personal construct theory: A psychology for the future (pp. 173197).
Melbourne: APS Imprint Books.
Derogatis, L. R. (1977). SCL-90-R: Administration, scoring and procedures manual I
for the revised version of the SCL-90. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University
Press.
Derogatis, L. R. (1983). SCL-90-R: Administration, scoring and procedures manual II
for the revised version of the SCL-90. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University
Press.
Drysdale, B. (1989). The construing of pain: A comparison of acute and chronic
low back pain patients using the repertory grid technique. International Jour-
nal of Personal Construct Psychology, 2, 271286.
Feixas, G., Bach, L., & Laso, E. (2004). Factors affecting interpersonal construct
differentiation when measured using the repertory grid. Journal of Construc-
tivist Psychology, 17, 297311.
Feixas, G., Cipriano, D., & Varlotta, N. (2007). Somatizacion y conflictos cogni-
tivos: estudio exploratorio con una muestra clnica. Revista Argentina de Clnica
Psicologica, 16, 194203.
Feixas, G., & Cornejo, J. M. (1992). Manual de la tecnica de rejilla mediante el pro-
grama RECORD ver. 1.0. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
Feixas, G., & Cornejo, J. M. (1996). Manual de la tecnica de rejilla mediante el pro-
grama RECORD v. 2.0. Barcelona: Paidos.
Feixas, G., & Cornejo, J. M. (2002). GRIDCOR v. 4.0: Correspondence analysis
of personal constructs. Barcelona: Psimedia. Available online at http://www.
terapiacongitiva.net/record
Feixas, G., Geldschlager, H., & Neimeyer, R. A. (2002). Content analysis of per-
sonal constructs. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 15, 119.
Feixas, G., & Saul, L. A. (2004). The multi-center dilemma project: An inves-
tigation on the role of cognitive conflicts in health. The Spanish Journal of
Psychology, 7(1), 6978.
Feixas, G., & Saul, L. A. (2005). Resolution of dilemmas by personal construct
psychotherapy. In D. Winter & L. Viney (Eds.), Personal construct psychotherapy:
Advances in theory, practice and research (pp. 13647). London: Whurr.
Feixas, G., Saul, L. A., & Sanchez Rodrguez, V. (2000). Detection and anal-
ysis of implicative dilemmas: implications for the therapeutic process. In
J. W. Scheer (Ed.), The person in society: Challenges to a constructivist theory
(pp. 391399). Gieen, Germany: Psychosozial-Verlag.
Feixas, G., & Villegas, M. (2000). Constructivismo y psicoterapia (3rd ed.). Bilbao:
Desclee de Brower.
Fernandes, E. (2007). When what I wish makes me worse . . . To make the co-
herence flexible. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, & Practice, 80,
165180.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford
University Press.
Fransella, F. (Ed.). (2003). International handbook of personal construct psychology.
Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Fransella, F., Bell, R., & Bannister, D. (2003). A manual for repertory grid technique
(2nd ed.). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
168 G. Feixas et al.