Early Trauma PDF
Early Trauma PDF
Early Trauma PDF
Edited by: Over half a century of converging clinical and animal research indicates that early life
Nikolaos P. Daskalakis, Icahn School
experiences induce enduring neuroplasticity of the HPA-axis and the developing brain.
of Medicine at Mount Sinai, USA
This experience-induced neuroplasticity is due to alterations in the frequency and inten-
Reviewed by:
Aniko Korosi, University of sity of stimulation of pups’ sensory systems (i.e., olfactory, somatosensory, gustatory)
Amsterdam, Netherlands embedded in mother–infant interactions. This stimulation provides “hidden regulators”
James A. Carr, Texas Tech University, of pups’ behavioral, physiological, and neural responses that have both immediate and
USA
enduring consequences, including those involving the stress response. While variation in
*Correspondence:
stimulation can produce individual differences and adaptive behaviors, pathological early
Millie Rincón-Cortés, Sullivan
Laboratory, New York University Child life experiences can induce maladaptive behaviors, initiate a pathway to pathology, and
Study Center, Department of Child increase risk for later-life psychopathologies, such as mood and affective disorders, suggest-
and Adolescent Psychiatry, New York ing that infant-attachment relationships program later-life neurobehavioral function. Recent
University School of Medicine, 1 Park
evidence suggests that the effects of maternal presence or absence during this sensory
Avenue, New York, NY 10016, USA
e-mail: millie.rinconcortes@ stimulation provide a major modulatory role in neural and endocrine system responses,
med.nyu.edu which have minimal impact on pups’ immediate neurobehavior but a robust impact on
neurobehavioral development. This concept is reviewed here using two complementary
rodent models of infant trauma within attachment: infant paired-odor-shock conditioning
(mimicking maternal odor attachment learning) and rearing with an abusive mother that
converge in producing a similar behavioral phenotype in later-life including depressive-like
behavior as well as disrupted HPA-axis and amygdala function. The importance of maternal
social presence on pups’ immediate and enduring brain and behavior suggests unique pro-
cessing of sensory stimuli in early life that could provide insight into the development of
novel strategies for prevention and therapeutic interventions for trauma experienced with
the abusive caregiver.
Keywords: infant-attachment, maternal programming, development, amygdala, social behavior, rodent models,
stress
during infancy (i.e., artificial or natural maternal odor), which models have provided some insight into the mechanisms by
have the ability to normalize adult neurobehavioral dysregulation which disruptions in parental care alter the development of stress
stemming from early life trauma. response systems (92, 93), which may contribute to our under-
standing of resilience following infant trauma (62, 94–98). For
ATTACHMENT example, research using animal models of maternal deprivation in
Attachment is a psychosocial process referring to the deep and rodents and non-human primates parallel human imaging stud-
enduring emotional bond that connects two individuals across ies suggesting that disruptions in infant-attachment also produce
space and time, with an individual deriving security from physi- long-term alterations in the limbic system and the stress axis that
cal and psychological contact with the attachment figure (38–40). may compromise the development of emotion- and attention-
Attachment requires experience-dependent learning of the sen- regulatory systems, which has been used to explain the heightened
sory stimuli associated with infant–caregiver interactions, and a risk of behavioral and affective disorders in human children expe-
strong attachment to the caregiver is crucial for survival in altri- riencing adverse parental care (13, 31, 32, 75, 93, 99–108). Overall,
cial species, including humans (41–48). In children, attachment these studies demonstrate that parental care affects the matura-
is characterized by specific behaviors such as seeking proximity tion of these brain areas and offers potential sites to understand the
to the caregiver, whom provides a sense of safety and security damaging effects of early life abuse on subsequent neurobehavioral
for the infant (49–51). Like humans, infants from altricial species development (31, 70, 71, 74, 84, 94, 109–115). For these reasons,
also exhibit attachment related behaviors to their caregiver shortly we employ rodent models of abusive attachment and study the
after birth that elicit nurturing and attachment from the care- infant’s immediate response to trauma as well as the neurobiolog-
giver, which entails responding appropriately to the infant’s needs ical sequelae leading to later-life neurobehavioral dysregulation to
by providing nourishment, protection, and warmth necessary for better understand the infant mechanisms that initiate the pathway
survival (51–53). Thus, infant-attachment is an adaptive and rec- to later-life psychopathologies.
iprocal process consisting of a dynamic and complex exchange of
mother–infant behavioral interactions that enhance the infant’s THE STRESS-HYPORESPONSIVE PERIOD AND MATERNAL REGULATION
chance of survival by maintaining contact with the caregiver. OF THE HPA-AXIS
The mother–infant attachment bond is among the strongest In rats, infant-attachment occurs within a unique developmen-
social attachments formed by most mammals (54). As such, tal context – the stress-hyporesponsive period (SHRP) – during
human infants seek proximity to and maintain contact with the which neonates show low basal plasma concentrations of CORT
caregiver despite the quality of care they receive (55), including and reduced stress-reactivity, as indexed by limited adrenocor-
attachment to an abusive caregiver. This paradoxical phenomenon ticotropic hormone (ACTH) and CORT responses to stressful
also occurs in dogs, chicks, and non-human primates, suggesting stimuli compared to older animals, as well as low levels of corti-
a phylogenetically preserved system (32, 41, 43, 56–63). From an costeroid binding globulin (CBG), which regulates glucocorticoid
evolutionary perspective, attachment to an abusive caregiver is (GC) access into the brain (92, 116–123). Thus, the neuroen-
thought to be adaptive because it provides immediate benefits, docrine stress response of the neonatal rat is characterized by
as the infant still has access to some care (48, 64). Albeit infant attenuated hormonal responses and altered gene regulation in
organisms are biologically predisposed to attach to their caregiver response to stress compared to adults due to hyporesponsiveness
and possess behavioral systems that allow them to rely on these at all levels of the HPA-axis, namely: (1) a blunted pituitary ACTH
bonds for survival (38), clinical and preclinical studies suggest that secretion, resulting from a combination of immaturity of neural
adverse parental care compromises brain development and has inputs to the corticotropin releasing hormone (CRH) neurons,
longstanding effects in stress-responsive neurobiological systems, (2) decreased pituitary peptide content or decreased sensitivity to
including the HPA-axis, neurotransmitter systems, as well as cor- CRH stimulus; and (3) an adrenal gland hyporesponsive to cir-
tical and limbic structures such as the prefrontal cortex, amygdala, culating ACTH levels (18, 119, 121, 124–130). Accumulating evi-
and hippocampus (65–75). Moreover, traumatic early life expe- dence suggests that human infants exhibit a period of dampened
riences involving the caregiver increase the risk for a wide-range cortisol reactivity analogous to the rodent SHRP, which develops
of deleterious mental health and behavioral outcomes, including gradually over the course of the first year of life (~6–12 months),
developmental psychopathology, affective, and mood disorders although it remains unclear how long it extends (131–135). In
(37, 72, 76–86). Therefore, perturbations in infant-attachment both humans and rodents, the SHRP is thought to protect the
appear to induce immediate neurobiological changes that shape developing brain from the detrimental effects of elevated HPA-axis
subsequent development and lead to neurobehavioral dysregu- activity and excess GC exposure, and the sensitivity and respon-
lation associated with compromised emotionality and increased siveness from the caregiver appears critical in maintaining low
vulnerability to psychopathology during later-life, suggesting that cortisol activity and controlling the offspring’s physiological and
the quality of an infant’s first social relationships programs the behavioral responses to stressors during this period (3, 30, 32, 68,
infant’s emotional and cognitive capabilities to adapt to later-life 100, 122, 127, 129, 136–140).
environments. However, the SHRP during development appears to be stres-
Despite the fact that childhood abuse remains a major public sor specific, since the HPA-axis is fully capable of responding to
health concern (87–91), the mechanisms by which infant trauma stimuli that may be considered stressful to a neonatal rat such
initiates the pathway to psychopathology are poorly understood, as cold or saline injection (141–145). Indeed, the HPA-axis and
although the stress axis is evidently implicated. However, animal CORT receptors are functional at birth, but are modulated by
the sensory stimulation provided by the mother (100, 119, 126, to learn and develop a preference for the mother’s odor, which is
146–152). Moreover, the mother is able to directly regulate the diet dependent and can change postnatally (47, 170–174). Since
pups’ CORT levels through hidden regulators embedded in typical rat pups are born deaf and blind, they must rapidly learn their
mother–infant interactions, such as the sensory, motor, nutrient, mother’s odor, which conveys distal and proximal information
and thermal events associated with caregiving, which exert regu- about the mother’s location, and helps the pups orient to the
latory influence over the infant’s immediate and long-term behav- mother, approach her and elicit care (169, 175). The maternal
ioral and physiological responses by affecting sleep-wake states, odor is critical in guiding infant-attachment; without it, pups show
cardiac rates, and HPA-axis function (6, 10–12, 17, 129, 153, 154). reduced contact with the mother, are unable to nipple attach and
Removal of maternal sensory stimulation during the SHRP, such exhibit low survival rates (25, 176). Moreover, any neutral odor
as that occurring when the pups are separated from the mother for can acquire properties of the natural maternal odor and act as a
a prolonged period of time (i.e., maternal deprivation paradigm), new maternal odor by simply being placed on the mother, in a
increases CORT secretion (16), elevates CORT levels in pups (11, cage during mother-infant interactions (177–182) or learned in
12, 129), and enables higher CORT/ACTH responses to acute stress classical conditioning paradigms (i.e., odor-stroke, odor-shock)
(15, 19, 100, 145, 155). Importantly, these changes are similar to performed outside the nest in the absence of the mother (111, 165,
those induced by normal variations in maternal care (i.e., maternal 171, 183–188).
high/low licking paradigm) (7, 27, 29) as well as atypical or abu- Our lab uses infant olfactory classical conditioning in which an
sive maternal care (20, 144, 156), suggesting that the hypothalamic artificial odor (i.e., peppermint) is paired with a 0.5 mA shock
mechanisms controlling physiological stress responses in the pup as a rodent model of abusive attachment. While the adult rat
are regulated by elements of maternal care. Taken together, these responds to shock with a robust CORT response, the neonatal
findings suggest that maternal deprivation, variations in maternal rat does not (9, 100, 189). Unlike older animals, which readily
care, and abusive maternal care influence the development and learn odor aversions to painful stimuli paired with an odor, rat
function of the HPA-axis (8, 9, 30, 112, 114, 157–160). In summary, pups actually exhibit an odor preference and approach the odor
maternal stimulation modulates the infant’s HPA-axis and main- (111, 165, 190–193). This odor preference, however, is not due to
tains the SHRP, although potent stressors involving disruptions the inability of pups to feel pain, since the pain threshold varies lit-
in maternal stimulation (i.e., cold, maternal deprivation, atypical tle during the neonatal period and pups emit vocalizations to the
maternal care) can activate the HPA-axis and override maternal shock, suggesting that they are experiencing distress (165, 190,
control of the SHRP. 194–197). Instead, infant paired-odor-shock conditioning pro-
duces a new artificial maternal odor that acquires the ability to
ATTACHMENT LEARNING DURING A SENSITIVE-PERIOD IN regulate pup behaviors typically controlled by the maternal odor;
RAT PUPS it induces proximity-seeking/approach responses in pups (distal
Infants possess a predisposition to approach the mother as well cues), guides mother–infant interactions by facilitating contact
as specific sensory cues associated with her care, such as her odor with the mother and nipple attachment (proximal cues), and acti-
and vocalizations (161, 162). Within an evolutionary context, the vates the same neural circuitry as the natural maternal odor (25,
infant-attachment system serves to establish a preference for the 111, 165, 169), suggesting that this odor has comparable quali-
mother regardless of whether or not she is associated with pain ties to the natural maternal odor. Importantly, infant odor-shock
or pleasure (48, 64). This type of survival-dependent learning is conditioning is a useful experimental paradigm for understanding
known as imprinting, has wide phylogenetic representation, and is how early life trauma (i.e., pain-shock) can support and maintain
temporally confined to a sensitive-period in development (50, 161, attachment and provide insights into the particular ways the infant
163) typically involving a hypofunctioning HPA-axis – the prin- brain processes painful stimuli and its relationship to the endur-
cipal pathway of the mammalian stress response that regulates ing effects of this experience due to the well documented neural
the production of GCs (cortisol in humans, CORT in rodents) circuitry underlying this type of learning (198–202). Three brain
(40, 164). In rats, we refer to this period of enhanced attach- structures have been shown to play a role in the neonatal rat’s
ment/preference learning as the “sensitive-period,” or postnatal sensitive-period for enhanced odor learning: the olfactory bulb
(PN) days 1–9 (see Figure 1). As we will discuss below, sensitive- (OB), the noradrenergic locus coeruleus (LC), and the amygdala
period learning is due to the pup’s unique learning circuit, pre- (50, 203).
sumably one sculpted through evolution to provide infants with
the neural circuitry required to survive and maximize attachment NEUROBIOLOGY OF INFANT-ATTACHMENT AND THE ROLE OF
to a caregiver (48). THE HPA-AXIS IN TERMINATING ATTACHMENT LEARNING
Intriguingly, the sensitive-period for attachment learning in Neonatal odor learning produces changes in the OB, which can
rat pups overlaps with the SHRP, suggesting that low levels of be induced both naturally in the nest and experimentally in con-
CORT and reduced HPA-axis responsiveness may contribute to the trolled learning experiments outside the nest (182, 186, 204–210).
neonate’s unique neural circuitry for attachment learning. How- For example, both natural and learned odors produce a simi-
ever, in order for infant-attachment to occur, the rat pup must lar enhancement of OB responding during the sensitive-period,
first learn to identify the caregiver and exhibit the social behaviors which has been assessed through a variety of techniques includ-
necessary for survival such as orienting to and approaching the ing 2-deoxy-glucose (2-DG) uptake, c-Fos immunoreactivity (ir),
caregiver, grasping the nipple and nursing (50, 168, 169). Infant- CREB phosphorylation, electrophysiology, and optical imaging
attachment learning in rodents revolves around the pup’s ability (205–208, 211–214). Thus, olfactory-based attachment learning
FIGURE 1 | The neural circuitry underlying pup attachment learning present at this age, pups will revert back to preference learning and the neural
changes over development. During the earliest days of life, pups have a circuitry of the sensitive-period. Thus, the mother’s presence socially buffers
sensitive-period in which odor-shock conditioning produces an odor pups (i.e., attenuates pups shock-induced CORT release) and pups learn a
preference. At 10 days of age, pups begin the transitional sensitive-period, preference. As pups mature and enter the post-sensitive-period, odor-shock
when pups endogenous CORT levels have increased sufficiently to enable conditioning induces amygdala-dependent fear and odor avoidance learning
amygdala-dependent fear/avoidance learning. However, with the mother (25, 165–167).
in neonatal rats is associated with the acquisition of odor-specific infant amygdala plasticity and avoidance learning (Figure 1) (201,
neural changes in the OB, which can only be acquired during the 218, 241).
sensitive-period, and are retained throughout development (111, Indeed, the natural increase of stress-induced CORT release
201, 215–218). marks the end of sensitive-period learning (165, 201, 203, 238),
Infant rats (PN1–9) readily learn an odor preference to neu- which has been demonstrated experimentally by increasing CORT
tral odors paired with pleasant (i.e., milk, stroking) (47, 171, 179, systemically (3 mg/kg, i.p.) or through intra-amygdala CORT
183–185, 219) or painful stimuli, such as 0.5 mA shock or tail infusions (50–100 ng) prior to odor-shock conditioning, which
pinch (111, 165, 190, 191, 201), which is partly due to a uniquely enables sensitive-period pups to learn an odor aversion and exhibit
large noradrenergic input to the OB from the LC, the sole source learning-evoked neural activity (i.e., enhanced 2-DG uptake)
of norepinephrine (NE) for the OB (220, 221), which prompts in the amygdala, while preventing the acquisition of learning-
abundant release of NE into the OB (203, 222). Furthermore, induced changes in the OB (201, 238, 241, 242). In contrast, CORT
the neonatal LC shows prolonged stimulus-evoked excitation and depletion (via adrenalectomy or social buffering, discussed below)
greater NE release to odors during the sensitive-period compared in PN12 pups results in shock-induced odor preference learning
to later-life due to the immaturity of the LC alpha-2 inhibitory and acquisition of OB neural changes. Thus, within the context of
autoreceptors, which functionally emerge around PN10 and cause paired-odor-shock conditioning, CORT appears to play a modu-
a shift from prolonged excitatory alpha-1 mediated responses to latory role on infant learning by switching whether the amygdala
inhibitory alpha-2 mediated responses, resulting in brief excitation learns attraction or avoidance: if CORT is low, pups learn a pref-
due to inhibited LC firing and decreased NE output (203, 222– erence to an odor paired with shock due to a lack of amygdala
227). Importantly, NE release from the LC is both necessary and involvement; if CORT is high, the amygdala is activated by odor-
sufficient for odor preference learning during the sensitive-period shock conditioning and pups learn an avoidance. Recently, we have
(228–232). identified a role for amygdala dopamine (DA) in mediating these
Experimental evidence indicates a lack of amygdala participa- infant learning transitions, as conditions that block aversion/fear
tion in the neural circuitry underlying infant paired-odor-shock learning are associated with downregulated DA function (243).
conditioning during the sensitive-period, as suggested by amyg- Altogether, these findings suggest that neonatal rat pups have
dala lesions, 2-DG, and c-Fos-ir (111, 201, 203, 216, 232), although unique learning capabilities that aid olfactory-based attachment
the amygdala is strongly implicated in adult classical conditioning to the mother, which are dependent on low levels of CORT.
(198–200, 202, 233). These data suggest that the infant amygdala In summary, the infant learning circuit is characterized by an
is not part of the sensitive-period learning circuit during which enhanced ability to learn odor preferences to aversive stimuli, due
aversions are difficult to learn because of its failure to exhibit the to a hyper-functioning LC, as well as a decreased ability to learn
plasticity required for this type of learning (234–236), although the odor aversions that may interfere with proximity-seeking during
amygdala is responsive to odors and other environmental stimuli the sensitive-period due to a hypo-functional amygdala, suggest-
by PN10 (165, 201, 237). Like the infant amygdala, the infant HPA- ing that the infant brain is specialized for maximizing attachment
axis is limited in function, resulting in reduced shock-induced to a caregiver (Figure 1) (41, 165, 186, 221, 225, 229, 234, 235, 244–
CORT release during the neonatal sensitive-period (189), which 247). As the sensitive-period ends, owing to the natural emergence
limits pups’ ability to acquire learned odor aversions (201, 238). of CORT, odor aversions can be learned because of changes in the
Endogenous CORT levels increase gradually and reach a critical infant learning circuit, including maturation of LC autoinhibition,
level by PN10 (92, 136, 239, 240), at which time stressful or painful which reduces NE release and greatly attenuates rapid odor pref-
stimuli are able to elicit a sufficient CORT response that permits erence learning, but also due to the functional emergence of the
amygdala, all of which enable the plasticity required for aversion and the sensitive-period for attachment learning. In addition, this
learning (50, 165, 223, 225, 229, 232, 241). procedure results in striking changes in the expression and activ-
ity patterns of key regulatory elements of the neuroendocrine
MATERNAL MODULATION OF HPA-AXIS FUNCTION AND stress response, which result in persistent alterations of HPA-axis
SENSITIVE-PERIOD DURATION function such as elevated basal GC concentrations, impaired GC
Empirical evidence suggests that social support is a powerful mod- feedback, and modifications in CRF-receptor regulation (20, 25,
ulator of individual differences in response to potentially stressful 114, 156, 174). Since the mother serves as a primary link between
events in both humans and animals (248–253). In rodents, mater- the environment and the infant, environmentally driven alter-
nal presence is known to blunt CORT release to stressful and ations in maternal care could transduce an environmental signal to
painful stimuli in older pups (>PN12) through olfactory and the pups, alter the development of central CRF systems activating
somatosensory cues (9, 148, 152, 166, 167, 254, 255). The process behavioral, endocrine and autonomic responses to stress, as well
by which the presence of a social companion and/or social sen- as systems regulating CRF and HPA-axis activity, which may serve
sory cues can dampen HPA responses to stressors (i.e., decrease to increase or decrease stress-reactivity in the offspring, so that it
CORT levels) is termed “social buffering” and has been reported in mirrors that of the mother.
humans and other species (139, 249, 250, 253, 256–259). Our lab
has identified a transitional sensitive-period in pups from PN10– IMMEDIATE AND ENDURING EFFECTS OF EARLY LIFE STRESS
15, during which odor-shock conditioning produces either olfac- Responses to stressors, or conditions that threaten or are per-
tory preference or aversion in infant rats depending on social con- ceived to threaten physiological equilibrium, are mediated by the
text (166, 260). In the absence of the mother, paired-odor-shock activation of stress-responsive neurobiological systems that help
conditioning yields a learned odor avoidance that is accompanied preserve allostasis, or stability through change, thereby making
by amygdala activation. However, maternal presence is able to sup- the stress response an essential endocrine mechanism for survival
press amygdala activity and block aversion learning induced by (268–270). Stressors, which can include psychological and physi-
odor-shock conditioning, indicating that maternal presence reen- cal challenges, increase the amount of hypothalamic CRF that is
gages the sensitive-period attachment circuitry to reinstate odor released into the anterior pituitary gland, stimulating ACTH secre-
preference learning through modulation of CORT (see Figure 1), tion in the anterior pituitary and resulting in GC production in
and therefore CORT regulation of amygdala activity. Importantly, the adrenal gland (268, 271, 272). GCs facilitate the mobilization
these animal data are consistent with the principles of attach- of substrates for energy sources, potentiate the release of cate-
ment theory (38), in which access to a secure base provided by cholamines, and enhance cardiovascular tone while suppressing
the attachment figure reduces the probability of HPA/CRF stress “non-essential systems” for immediate survival, such as immu-
reactions that could have unfavorable long-term consequences on nity, growth, and reproduction (273–276). Stress-induced HPA-
brain development (9, 137, 261). axis activation is associated with acute release of stress-related
Yet, human parental care is disturbed under conditions of neuropeptides, hormones, and neurotransmitters, including NE,
chronic stress (262), which can be modeled in rodents by creating serotonin (5-HT), and DA, in cortical and limbic structures (21, 27,
an abnormal rearing environment that alters maternal behavior 277–289). Although acutely elevated GCs help orchestrate physi-
(20, 23, 111) and mimics the effects of a stressful environment as a ological and behavioral responses that promote allostasis, chronic
risk factor for potentiating infant abuse, including humans (62, 77, activation of the HPA-axis, and prolonged elevations of GCs and
263, 264). Because bedding type and volume are important com- CRF increase the risk of stress-related disorders and psychological
ponents of the dam’s nesting environment, limiting the amount illnesses during later-life (269, 290–292).
of bedding available constitutes a continuous stressor for the dam The effects of HPA-axis activation depend on multiple factors,
and her pups, disrupts mother–pup interactions, and alters the including the developmental stage in which the insult occurs, num-
development of the pup’s HPA-axis by reducing the frequency of ber of exposures, and type of adversity (71, 293–297). Numerous
positive maternal behaviors (i.e., licking, grooming, nursing) and behavioral, endocrine, and clinical studies have shown that vari-
increasing the frequency of negative maternal behaviors that are ous early life stressors cause a premature increase in CORT levels
painful to the pup and elicit vocalizations, such as stepping, drag- (129) that produces profound alterations in growth and develop-
ging, and rough handling of the pups (20, 25, 111, 156, 188, 265). ment and negatively affects mental health (40, 72, 135, 298, 299).
Thus, one could conceptualize a stressed dam as a poor regulator, Moreover, repeated exposure to early life stressors, both physical
which is supported by findings showing that ICV infusion of cor- and psychological, induce changes in endocrine (HPA-axis), neu-
ticotropin releasing factor (CRF) reduces maternal responsivity rotransmitter (DA, 5-HT), and brain memory systems, including
(266). the hippocampus, amygdala, and PFC that persist throughout the
Furthermore, because maternal stimulation of pups modulates life-span (8, 67, 101, 300, 301). Furthermore, the HPA-axis is mod-
pups’ endogenous CORT, maternal care quality alters sensitive- ulated by limbic and cortical regions such as the amygdala, hip-
period duration. Pups reared with a stressed mother (i.e., poor pocampus, and the PFC (269, 302), which enable the activation of
regulator) exhibit a precocious emergence of CORT, which is stress responses by psychosocial stressors (303–307). Importantly,
delivered through the mother’s milk (267), that facilitates aver- the timing of early life stress may affect brain regions undergoing
sion learning and engages the amygdala, as indexed by increased specific growth spurts during that time (308, 309), so that brain
odor-shock-induced amygdala neural activity (188), suggesting regions rich in GC receptors and characterized by extended PN
that experience with a stressed mother prematurely ends the SHRP development, such as the amygdala, hippocampus, and PFC, are
particularly susceptible to the long-term effects of stress (71, 92), temporary inactivation (i.e., muscimol) of amygdala function
which affects later-life memory, cognitive, executive, and affective during the FST, which normalized these behaviors to a level com-
function as well as stress-reactivity in humans (296, 297). Alter- parable to controls (26). Collectively, these findings suggest that
ations in stress-sensitive neurobiological systems, including regu- the expression of depressive-like behavior in the FST following
lation of GCs and CRF, have been posited as mechanisms through early life abuse is characterized by a hyper-functioning amygdala.
which early life stress, including inadequate/disorganized parental Thus, abusive attachment appears to disrupt the developmental
care, increases the likelihood of psychopathology by influencing trajectory of the amygdala and modify the way that it responds
HPA hyperreactivity to stressors and promoting the development to future stressors, which is supported by our work using rodent
of stress-induced illnesses throughout life (31, 40, 290, 310–312). models of early life abuse.
Early life adversity may lead to a maladaptive outcome to a Our findings are in accordance with clinical and animal liter-
given later environmental context. Depression is a common out- ature indicating that early life adversity constitutes a prime risk
come of childhood abuse, and children with a comorbid history factor for the development of psychopathologies characterized by
of depression and abuse have elevated CRF levels in the cere- dysregulated HPA-axis function (1, 5, 24, 32, 133, 319, 331–334),
brospinal fluid (313) as well as an increased ACTH response to such as mood and affective disorders (37, 93, 335, 336), which
a CRF challenge compared to children with depression without also exhibit a developmental delay (309, 337, 338) Thus, these
abuse, suggesting excessive CRF release (3, 314, 315). Additional rodent models of early life abuse allow us to explore the ontogeny
clinical evidence indicates that severe early life stressors in child- of depressive-like behavior and amygdala dysregulation, which is
hood are associated with the long-term HPA-axis disturbances in of clinical relevance because abnormal amygdala function and
depressed patients (316–319), which is supported by preclinical social behavior deficits as well as their relationship to later-life
studies of non-human primates showing that poor rearing con- depressive-like behaviors have been documented in individuals
ditions and conditions that disrupt responsive maternal care have with a history of early life abuse (71, 310, 331, 336).
a long-term impact on the neurobiology of stress and negative
emotionality (21, 31, 32, 109, 158). For example, variable foraging MODULATION OF ADULT NEUROBEHAVIORAL FUNCTION BY
paradigms that result in neglectful maternal care produce adult INFANT-ATTACHMENT RELATED CUES
offspring that are more fearful, low in dominance, have elevated An ample body of evidence suggests that the quality of infant-
levels of CRF in the CSF and high in brain levels of CRH, exhibit attachment relationships results in long-term adaptations that
persistent alterations in metabolites of 5-HT, DA, and NE, as well have the ability to program subsequent behavioral, endocrine, and
as changes in noradrenergic and serotonergic responses to stress neural function (28, 109, 261, 310, 336). Results from our lab-
(99, 320–324). Given the importance of noradrenergic and sero- oratory have shown that infant paired-odor-shock conditioning
tonergic systems in mood disorders, these findings postulate a results in reduced fear learning and attenuated related amygdala
mechanism by which early life stress may predispose an individual function, dysregulation in neural networks underlying olfactory
to later-life depression (32, 300, 325–327). learning, and depressive-like behavior during adulthood (339–
342). Importantly, attachment related sensory cues learned during
CONVERGENCE OF BOTH ABUSIVE ATTACHMENT MODELS infancy can play a critical role in modulating neurobehavioral
IN PRODUCING A DEPRESSIVE-LIKE BEHAVIORAL responses during later-life. In humans, for example, cues associ-
PHENOTYPE DURING LATER-LIFE ated with early life abuse elicit strong attraction and feelings of
Recently, our lab has demonstrated that both rodent models of comfort (343). In rodents, presentation of an artificial maternal
abusive attachment (paired-odor-shock, abusive mother) during odor, resulting from infant paired-odor-shock conditioning, is able
infancy result in later-life depressive-like behavior in the Forced to reverse the behavioral effects of abusive attachment in rodent
Swim Test (FST), a measure of behavioral despair in rodents (328, measures of depressive-like behavior, such as the sucrose con-
329), that is accompanied by changes in amygdala function and sumption test and the FST (342). Specifically, the odor increased
preceded by disruptions in social behavior (26). When employed the latency to immobility and reduced the time spent immobile
from PN8–12, these two complementary rodent models of early in the FST, but also increased the percentage of sucrose consumed
life abuse produced a reduction in sociability, as indexed by spend- during a sucrose preference test to levels comparable to controls.
ing significantly less time in a social chamber compared to control Furthermore, these restorative effects of a learned infant maternal
animals reared with a normal mother – a behavioral pattern that odor on adult function were also observable at electrophysiological
was observable prior to weaning (PN23) and maintained in adoles- level, as odor presentation also normalized paired-pulse inhibition
cence (PN45). However, animals experiencing early life abuse only deficits in the amygdala. Collectively, these data suggest that early
showed depressive-like behavior in the FST during adolescence life experiences are able to shape adult neural circuits underlying
(PN45), as indicated by immobility – the passive state in which behavior and that adult behaviors can be modified under envi-
the animal makes only those movements necessary to keep its ronmental conditions in which learned infant cues are present.
head above water (328, 330). In addition, depressive-like behavior The discovery that infant cues can retain their value throughout
in the FST in animals experiencing early life abuse was associated the life-span and regulate later-life behaviors controlled by cir-
with increased c-Fos-ir in the basal, lateral, and central amygdala cuits implicated in emotion, learning, and social behavior is of
nuclei, suggesting that increased neural activity in these structures great interest because it provides an opportunity for interven-
may contribute to the expression of depressive-like behavior in tion and possibly correction of maladaptive outcomes related to
the FST (26). A causal relationship between amygdala function psychopathology induced by adverse early life experiences within
and depressive-like behavior in the FST was suggested through attachment. Thus, it appears that the enduring neurobehavioral
dysregulation stemming from early life abuse can be positively 11. Hofer MA. Hidden regulatory processes in early social relationships. In: Klopfer
modulated by learned sensory cues related to infant-attachment. PH, editor. Perspectives in Ethology: Social Behavior. New York, NY: Plenum
Press (1978). p. 135–66.
12. Hofer MA. Hidden regulators in attachment, separation, and loss. Monogr Soc
CONCLUSION Res Child Dev (1994) 59:192–207. doi:10.2307/1166146
In species requiring parental care, evolution has ensured that 13. Dettling AC, Feldon J, Pryce CR. Repeated parental deprivation in the infant
infants quickly learn and express robust preferences to the care- common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus, primates) and analysis of its effects
giver, regardless of the quality of care (48, 50). However, trauma on early development. Biol Psychiatry (2002) 52:1037–46. doi:10.1016/S0006-
3223(02)01460-9
within attachment leaves the infant particularly vulnerable to adult
14. Levine S. Developmental determinants of sensitivity and resistance to
psychiatric disorders, behavioral changes in fear and anxiety, and stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2005) 30:939–46. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.
alterations in neural circuits, particularly those regulating stress 2005.03.013
and emotion (71, 133, 334, 344, 345). In addition, early life stress 15. Stanton ME, Gutierrez YR, Levine S. Maternal deprivation potentiates
can have negative effects on the neurobiology of the develop- pituitary-adrenal stress responses in infant rats. Behav Neurosci (1988)
102:692–700. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.102.5.692
ing brain that are comparable to those induced by disruptions in
16. Kuhn CM, Pauk J, Schanberg SM. Endocrine responses to mother-infant sepa-
infant-caregiver interactions (25, 346) Thus, early life experiences ration in developing rats. Dev Psychobiol (1990) 23:395–410. doi:10.1002/dev.
have enduring effects on the neuroplasticity of the HPA-axis, sug- 420230503
gesting the HPA-axis is programmable via multiple environmental 17. Levine S, Huchton DM, Wiener SG, Rosenfeld P. Time course of the effect of
sources across development. In early development, stressors and maternal deprivation on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in the infant
rat. Dev Psychobiol (1991) 24:547–58. doi:10.1002/dev.420240803
maternal care jointly program HPA-axis responses and later-life 18. Rosenfeld P, Suchecki D, Levine S. Multifactorial regulation of the
function. The HPA-axis, however, remains modifiable during later hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis during development. Neurosci Biobehav
stages of development during which infant-attachment related Rev (1992) 16:553–68. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(05)80196-4
cues can exert a positive modulatory effect on later-life HPA-axis 19. Avishai-Eliner S, Yi SJ, Newth CJ, Baram TZ. Effects of maternal and sibling
function as well as behavioral and endocrine responses to stress. deprivation on basal and stress induced hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal com-
ponents in the infant rat. Neurosci Lett (1995) 192:49–52. doi:10.1016/0304-
3940(95)11606-W
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 20. Gilles EE, Schultz L, Baram TZ. Abnormal corticosterone regulation in an
This work was supported by the National Science Founda- immature rat model of continuous chronic stress. Pediatr Neurol (1996)
tion Graduate Research Fellowship (DGE-1137475) to Millie 15:114–9. doi:10.1016/0887-8994(96)00153-1
21. Ladd CO, Owens MJ, Nemeroff CB. Persistent changes in corticotropin-
Rincón-Cortés and NIH-MH091451, NIH-DC009910 to Regina
releasing factor neuronal systems induced by maternal deprivation. Endocrinol-
M. Sullivan. ogy (1996) 137:1212–8. doi:10.1210/endo.137.4.8625891
22. Cirulli F, Berry A, Alleva E. Early disruption of the mother-infant relation-
REFERENCES ship: effects on brain plasticity and implications for psychopathology. Neurosci
1. Anisman H, Zaharia MD, Meaney MJ, Merali Z. Do early-life events perma- Biobehav Rev (2003) 27:73–82. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(03)00010-1
nently alter behavioral and hormonal responses to stressors? Int J Dev Neurosci 23. Ivy AS, Brunson KL, Sandman C, Baram TZ. Dysfunctional nurturing behav-
(1998) 16:149–64. doi:10.1016/S0736-5748(98)00025-2 ior in rat dams with limited access to nesting material: a clinically relevant
2. Meaney MJ, Szyf M. Environmental programming of stress responses through model for early-life stress. Neuroscience (2008) 154:1132–42. doi:10.1016/j.
DNA methylation: life at the interface between a dynamic environment and a neuroscience.2008.04.019
fixed genome. Dialogues Clin Neurosci (2005) 7:103–23. 24. McEwen BS. Understanding the potency of stressful early life experiences on
3. Romeo RD, Tang AC, Sullivan RM. Early life experiences: enduring behavioral, brain and body function. Metabolism (2008) 57(Suppl 2):S11–5. doi:10.1016/
neurological and endocrinological consequences. In: Anne DWP, Etgen M, edi- j.metabol.2008.07.006
tors. Molecular Mechanisms of Hormone Actions on Behavior. San Diego, CA: 25. Raineki C, Moriceau S, Sullivan RM. Developing a neurobehavioral animal
Academic Press (2009). p. 543–72. model of infant attachment to an abusive caregiver. Biol Psychiatry (2010)
4. Bale TL, Baram TZ, Brown AS, Goldstein JM, Insel TR, McCarthy MM, et al. 67:1137–45. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.019
Early life programming and neurodevelopmental disorders. Biol Psychiatry 26. Raineki C, Cortes MR, Belnoue L, Sullivan RM. Effects of early-life abuse dif-
(2010) 68:314–9. doi:10.1016/jbiopsych.2010.05.028 fer across development: infant social behavior deficits are followed by adoles-
5. Grace CE, Kim SJ, Rogers JM. Maternal influences on epigenetic programming cent depressive-like behaviors mediated by the amygdala. J Neurosci (2012)
of the developing hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Birth Defects Res A Clin 32:7758–65. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5843-11.2012
Mol Teratol (2011) 91:797–805. doi:10.1002/bdra.20824 27. Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. Early, postnatal experience alters hypothalamic
6. Levine S. Maternal and environmental influences on the adrenocortical corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content
response to stress in weanling rats. Science (1967) 156:258–60. doi:10.1126/ and stress-induced release in adult rats. Brain Res Mol Brain Res (1993)
science.156.3772.258 18:195–200. doi:10.1016/0169-328X(93)90189-V
7. Liu D, Diorio J, Tannenbaum B, Caldji C, Francis D, Freedman A, et al. Mater- 28. Canetti L, Bachar E, Galili-Weisstub E, De-Nour AK, Shalev AY. Parental bond-
nal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary- ing and mental health in adolescence. Adolescence (1997) 32:381–94.
adrenal responses to stress. Science (1997) 277:1659–62. doi:10.1126/science. 29. Caldji C, Tannenbaum B, Sharma S, Francis D, Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. Mater-
277.5332.1659 nal care during infancy regulates the development of neural systems mediat-
8. Francis DD, Meaney MJ. Maternal care and the development of stress responses. ing the expression of fearfulness in the rat. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1998)
Curr Opin Neurobiol (1999) 9:128–34. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(99)80016-6 95:5335–40. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.9.5335
9. Levine S. Primary social relationships influence the development of the hypo- 30. Caldji C, Diorio J, Meaney MJ. Variations in maternal care in infancy regu-
thalamic – pituitary – adrenal axis in the rat. Physiol Behav (2001) 73:255–60. late the development of stress reactivity. Biol Psychiatry (2000) 48:1164–74.
doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00496-6 doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01084-2
10. Walker CD. Maternal touch and feed as critical regulators of behavioral 31. Heim C, Nemeroff CB. The role of childhood trauma in the neurobiology of
and stress responses in the offspring. Dev Psychobiol (2010) 52:638–50. mood and anxiety disorders: preclinical and clinical studies. Biol Psychiatry
doi:10.1002/dev.20492 (2001) 49:1023–39. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01157-X
32. Sanchez MM, Ladd CO, Plotsky PM. Early adverse experience as a developmen- 59. Salzen EA. Imprinting and environmental learning. In: Aronson LR, Lehrman
tal risk factor for later psychopathology: evidence from rodent and primate DS, Rosenblatt J, editors. Development and Evolution of Behavior. San Francisco,
models. Dev Psychopathol (2001) 13:419–49. doi:10.1017/S0954579401003029 CA: W.H. Freeman (1970). p. 158–78.
33. De Bellis MD. The psychobiology of neglect. Child Maltreat (2005) 10:150–72. 60. Helfer ME, Kempe RS, Krugman RD. The Battered Child. Chicago, IL: Univer-
doi:10.1177/1077559505275116 sity Press (1997).
34. Hane AA, Fox NA. Ordinary variations in maternal caregiving influence human 61. Carroll KA, Maestripieri D. Infant abuse and neglect in monkeys – a discussion
infants’ stress reactivity. Psychol Sci (2006) 17:550–6. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280. of definitions, epidemiology, etiology, and implications for child maltreatment:
2006.01742.x reply to Cicchetti (1998) and Mason (1998). Psychol Bull (1998) 123:234–7.
35. Zhang TY, Bagot R, Parent C, Nesbitt C, Bredy TW, Caldji C, et al. Maternal pro- doi:10.1037/0033-2909.123.3.234
gramming of defensive responses through sustained effects on gene expression. 62. Maestripieri D, Carroll KA. Child abuse and neglect: usefulness of the animal
Biol Psychol (2006) 73:72–89. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.009 data. Psychol Bull (1998) 123:211–23. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.123.3.211
36. Neigh GN, Gillespie CF, Nemeroff CB. The neurobiological toll of child 63. Hofer MA, Sullivan RM. Toward a neurobiology of attachment. In: Nelson
abuse and neglect. Trauma Violence Abuse (2009) 10:389–410. doi:10.1177/ CA, Luciana M, editors. Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.
1524838009339758 Cambridge: MIT Press (2008). p. 787–806.
37. Gershon A, Sudheimer K, Tirouvanziam R, Williams LM, O’Hara R. The long- 64. Sapolsky R. Any kind of mother in a storm. Nat Neurosci (2009) 12:1355–6.
term impact of early adversity on late-life psychiatric disorders. Curr Psychiatry doi:10.1038/nn1109-1355
Rep (2013) 15:352. doi:10.1007/s11920-013-0352-9 65. Joseph R. Environmental influences on neural plasticity, the limbic system,
38. Bowlby J. Attachment. Attachment and Loss: Vol. I. Loss. New York, NY: Basic emotional development and attachment: a review. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev
Books (1969). (1999) 29:189–208. doi:10.1023/A:1022660923605
39. Ainsworth MDS. The development of infant-mother attachment. In: Ricciuti 66. Glaser D. Child abuse and neglect and the brain – a review. J Child Psychol
BCH, editor. Review of Child Developmental Research. Chicago: University of Psychiatry (2000) 41:97–116. doi:10.1017/S0021963099004990
Chicago Press (1973). p. 1–94. 67. Bremner JD, Vermetten E. Stress and development: behavioral and bio-
40. Gunnar M, Quevedo K. The neurobiology of stress and development. Annu logical consequences. Dev Psychopathol (2001) 13:473–89. doi:10.1017/
Rev Psychol (2007) 58:145–73. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605 S0954579401003042
41. Hess E. Ethology: an approach to the complete analysis of behavior. In: Brown 68. Dent GW, Smith MA, Levine S. Stress-induced alterations in locus coeruleus
EGR, Hess E, Mendler G, editors. New Directions in Psychology. New York, NY: gene expression during ontogeny. Brain Res Dev Brain Res (2001) 127:23–30.
Holt, Rineheart and Winston (1962). p. 159–99. doi:10.1016/S0165-3806(01)00108-0
42. Ainsworth MDS. Object relations, dependency, and attachment: a theoreti- 69. Vythilingam M, Heim C, Newport J, Miller AH, Anderson E, Bronen R, et al.
cal review of the infant-mother relationship. Child Dev (1969) 40:969–1025. Childhood trauma associated with smaller hippocampal volume in women
doi:10.2307/1127008 with major depression. Am J Psychiatry (2002) 159:2072–80. doi:10.1176/appi.
43. Rajecki DWL, Michael E, Obmascher P. Toward a general theory of infantile ajp.159.12.2072
attachment: a comparative review of aspects of the social bond. Behav Brain 70. Bremner JD. Long-term effects of childhood abuse on brain and neurobiol-
Sci (1978) 1:417–64. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00075816 ogy. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am (2003) 12:271–92. doi:10.1016/S1056-
44. DeCasper AJ, Fifer WP. Of human bonding: newborns prefer their mothers’ 4993(02)00098-6
voices. Science (1980) 208:1174–6. doi:10.1126/science.7375928 71. Teicher MH, Andersen SL, Polcari A, Anderson CM, Navalta CP, Kim DM.
45. Hennessy MB, Li J, Levine S. Infant responsiveness to maternal cues in mice of The neurobiological consequences of early stress and childhood maltreat-
2 inbred lines. Dev Psychobiol (1980) 13:77–84. doi:10.1002/dev.420130111 ment. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2003) 27:33–44. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(03)
46. Gewirtz SBP. The attachment learning process and its relation to cultural and 00007-1
biological evolution: proximate and ultimate considerations. In: Field MRAT, 72. Cirulli F, Francia N, Berry A, Aloe L, Alleva E, Suomi SJ. Early life stress as a risk
editor. The Psychobiology of Attachment and Separation. Orlando, FL: Academic factor for mental health: role of neurotrophins from rodents to non-human
Press Inc (1985). p. 259–86. primates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2009) 33:573–85. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.
47. Polan HJ, Hofer MA. Olfactory preference for mother over home nest shav- 2008.09.001
ings by newborn rats. Dev Psychobiol (1998) 33:5–20. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098- 73. Braun K, Bock J. The experience-dependent maturation of prefronto-
2302(199807)33:1<5::AID-DEV2>3.0.CO;2-P limbic circuits and the origin of developmental psychopathology: impli-
48. Hofer MA, Sullivan RM. Towards a neurobiology of attachment. In: Nelson CA, cations for the pathogenesis and therapy of behavioural disorders. Dev
editor. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. Cambridge: MIT Press (2001). Med Child Neurol (2011) 53(Suppl 4):14–8. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.
p. 599–616. 04056.x
49. Bowlby J. The nature of the child’s tie to his mother. Int J Psychoanal (1958) 74. Blaze J, Scheuing L, Roth TL. Differential methylation of genes in the medial
39:350–73. prefrontal cortex of developing and adult rats following exposure to mal-
50. Sullivan RM. Developing a sense of safety: the neurobiology of neonatal attach- treatment or nurturing care during infancy. Dev Neurosci (2013) 35:306–16.
ment. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2003) 1008:122–31. doi:10.1196/annals.1301.013 doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2013.10.001
51. Sullivan R, Perry R, Sloan A, Kleinhaus K, Burtchen N. Infant bonding and 75. Gee DG, Gabard-Durnam LJ, Flannery J, Goff B, Humphreys KL, Telzer EH,
attachment to the caregiver: insights from basic and clinical science. Clin Peri- et al. Early developmental emergence of human amygdala-prefrontal connec-
natol (2011) 38:643–55. doi:10.1016/j.clp.2011.08.011 tivity after maternal deprivation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2013) 110:15638–43.
52. Ainsworth MS. Infant – mother attachment. Am Psychol (1979) 34:932–7. doi:10.1073/pnas.1307893110
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.932 76. Rutter M. The long-term effects of early experience. Dev Med Child Neurol
53. Ainsworth MDS. Patterns of infant-mother attachments: antecedents and (1980) 22:800–15. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.1980.tb03751.x
effects on development. Bull N Y Acad Med (1985) 61:771–91. 77. Cicchetti D. How research on child maltreatment has informed the study of
54. Bowlby J. Attachment: Attachment and Loss. New York, NY: Basic Books (1982). child development: perspectives from development psychopathology. In: Cic-
55. Bowlby J. Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books (1965). chetti D, Carlson V, editors. Child Maltreatment: Theory and Research on the
56. Fisher AE. The Effects of Differential Early Treatment on the Social and Causes and Consequences of Child Abuse and Neglect. New York, NY: Cambridge
Exploratory Behavior in Puppies. Ph.D. thesis. University Park (PA): Pennsylva- University Press (1990). p. 377–431.
nia State University (1955). 78. Shields AM, Cicchetti D, Ryan RM. The development of emotional and
57. Stanley W. Differential human handling as reinforcing events and as treat- behavioral self-regulation and social competence among maltreated
ments influencing later social behavior in basenji puppies. Psychol Rep (1962) school-age children. Dev Psychopathol (1994) 6:57–75. doi:10.1017/
10:775–88. doi:10.2466/pr0.1962.10.3.775 S0954579400005885
58. Harlow HF, Harlow MK. The affectional systems. In: Schrier HF, Stollnitz F, edi- 79. Cicchetti D, Toth SL. A developmental psychopathology perspective on child
tors. Behavior of Nonhuman Primates. New York, NY: Academic Press (1965). abuse and neglect. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (1995) 34:541–65.
p. 287–344. doi:10.1097/00004583-199505000-00008
80. Rutter M. Developmental catch-up, and deficit, following adoption after severe 103. Brake WG, Zhang TY, Diorio J, Meaney MJ, Gratton A. Influence of early
global early privation. English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) Study Team. postnatal rearing conditions on mesocorticolimbic dopamine and behavioural
J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1998) 39:465–76. doi:10.1017/S0021963098002236 responses to psychostimulants and stressors in adult rats. Eur J Neurosci (2004)
81. O’Connor TG, Rutter M. Attachment disorder behavior following early severe 19:1863–74. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03286.x
deprivation: extension and longitudinal follow-up. English and Romanian 104. Pryce CR, Dettling AC, Spengler M, Schnell CR, Feldon J. Deprivation of par-
Adoptees Study Team. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2000) 39:703–12. enting disrupts development of homeostatic and reward systems in marmoset
doi:10.1097/00004583-200006000-00008 monkey offspring. Biol Psychiatry (2004) 56:72–9. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.
82. De Bellis MD. Developmental traumatology: the psychobiological develop- 2004.05.002
ment of maltreated children and its implications for research, treatment, and 105. Tottenham N, Hare TA, Quinn BT, McCarry TW, Nurse M, Gilhooly T,
policy. Dev Psychopathol (2001) 13:539–64. doi:10.1017/S0954579401003078 et al. Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amyg-
83. Schore AN. Dysregulation of the right brain: a fundamental mechanism of trau- dala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Dev Sci (2010) 13:46–61.
matic attachment and the psychopathogenesis of posttraumatic stress disorder. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00852.x
Aust N Z J Psychiatry (2002) 36:9–30. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1614.2002.00996.x 106. Hostinar CE, Stellern SA, Schaefer C, Carlson SM, Gunnar MR. Associa-
84. Nemeroff CB. Neurobiological consequences of childhood trauma. J Clin Psy- tions between early life adversity and executive function in children adopted
chiatry (2004) 65(Suppl 1):18–28. internationally from orphanages. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2012) 109(Suppl
85. Rogosch FA, Cicchetti D. Child maltreatment, attention networks, and poten- 2):17208–12. doi:10.1073/pnas.1121246109
tial precursors to borderline personality disorder. Dev Psychopathol (2005) 107. Tottenham N. Human amygdala development in the absence of species-
17:1071–89. doi:10.1017/S0954579405050509 expected caregiving. Dev Psychobiol (2012) 54:598–611. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.
86. Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Bremner JD, Walker JD, Whitfield C, Perry BD, et al. The 068.2009
enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. A con- 108. Malter Cohen M, Jing D, Yang RR, Tottenham N, Lee FS, Casey BJ. Early-life
vergence of evidence from neurobiology and epidemiology. Eur Arch Psychiatry stress has persistent effects on amygdala function and development in mice
Clin Neurosci (2006) 256:174–86. doi:10.1007/s00406-005-0624-4 and humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2013) 110:18274–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.
87. Barber-Madden R, Cohn AH, Schloesser P. Prevention of child abuse: a public 1310163110
health agenda. J Public Health Policy (1988) 9:167–76. doi:10.2307/3343003 109. Ladd CO, Huot RL, Thrivikraman KV, Nemeroff CB, Meaney MJ, Plotsky PM.
88. Barnett D, Manly JT, Cicchetti D. Defining child maltreatment: the interface Long-term behavioral and neuroendocrine adaptations to adverse early experi-
between policy and research. In: Toth DCSL, editor. Child Abuse, Child Devel- ence. Prog Brain Res (2000) 122:81–103. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)62132-9
opment, and Social Policy. Norwood, NJ: Ablex (1993). p. 7–72. 110. Liu D, Caldji C, Sharma S, Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. Influence of neonatal
89. Merrick J, Browne KD. Child abuse and neglect – a public health concern. rearing conditions on stress-induced adrenocorticotropin responses and nor-
Public Health Rev (1999) 27:279–93. epinephrine release in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus. J Neuroen-
90. Dawson G, Ashman SB, Carver LJ. The role of early experience in shaping docrinol (2000) 12:5–12. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2826.2000.00422.x
behavioral and brain development and its implications for social policy. Dev 111. Roth TL, Sullivan RM. Memory of early maltreatment: neonatal behavioral
Psychopathol (2000) 12:695–712. doi:10.1017/S0954579400004089 and neural correlates of maternal maltreatment within the context of classical
91. Kendall-Tackett K. The health effects of childhood abuse: four pathways conditioning. Biol Psychiatry (2005) 57:823–31. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.
by which abuse can influence health. Child Abuse Negl (2002) 26:715–29. 01.032
doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00343-5 112. Fenoglio KA, Chen Y, Baram TZ. Neuroplasticity of the hypothalamic-
92. De Kloet ER, Rosenfeld P, Van Eekelen JA, Sutanto W, Levine S. Stress, glu- pituitary-adrenal axis early in life requires recurrent recruitment of
cocorticoids and development. Prog Brain Res (1988) 73:101–20. doi:10.1016/ stress-regulating brain regions. J Neurosci (2006) 26:2434–42. doi:10.1523/
S0079-6123(08)60500-2 JNEUROSCI.4080-05.2006
93. Heim C, Plotsky PM, Nemeroff CB. Importance of studying the contribu- 113. Branchi I, D’Andrea I, Cirulli F, Lipp HP, Alleva E. Shaping brain develop-
tions of early adverse experience to neurobiological findings in depression. ment: mouse communal nesting blunts adult neuroendocrine and behavioral
Neuropsychopharmacology (2004) 29:641–8. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300397 response to social stress and modifies chronic antidepressant treatment out-
94. Kaufman J, Plotsky PM, Nemeroff CB, Charney DS. Effects of early adverse come. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2010) 35:743–51. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.
experiences on brain structure and function: clinical implications. Biol Psychi- 2009
atry (2000) 48:778–90. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00998-7 114. Ivy AS, Rex CS, Chen Y, Dube C, Maras PM, Grigoriadis DE, et al. Hippocampal
95. Stevens HE, Leckman JF, Coplan JD, Suomi SJ. Risk and resilience: early manip- dysfunction and cognitive impairments provoked by chronic early-life stress
ulation of macaque social experience and persistent behavioral and neuro- involve excessive activation of CRH receptors. J Neurosci (2010) 30:13005–15.
physiological outcomes. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2009) 48:114–27. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1784-10.2010
doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e318193064c 115. Daskalakis NP, Diamantopoulou A, Claessens SE, Remmers E, Tjalve M, Oitzl
96. Korosi A, Baram TZ. Plasticity of the stress response early in life: mechanisms MS, et al. Early experience of a novel-environment in isolation primes a fear-
and significance. Dev Psychobiol (2010) 52:661–70. doi:10.1002/dev.20490 ful phenotype characterized by persistent amygdala activation. Psychoneuroen-
97. Lyons DM, Parker KJ, Schatzberg AF. Animal models of early life stress: docrinology (2014) 39:39–57. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.021
implications for understanding resilience. Dev Psychobiol (2010) 52:402–10. 116. Butte JC, Kakihana R, Farnham ML, Noble EP. The relationship between brain
doi:10.1002/dev.20429 and plasma corticosterone stress response in developing rats. Endocrinology
98. Franklin TB, Saab BJ, Mansuy IM. Neural mechanisms of stress resilience and (1973) 92:1775–9. doi:10.1210/endo-92-6-1775
vulnerability. Neuron (2012) 75:747–61. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.016 117. Henning SJ. Plasma concentrations of total and free corticosterone during
99. Rosenblum LA, Coplan JD, Friedman S, Bassoff T, Gorman JM, Andrews MW. development in the rat. Am J Physiol (1978) 235:E451–6.
Adverse early experiences affect noradrenergic and serotonergic functioning in 118. Guillet R, Saffran M, Michaelson SM. Pituitary-adrenal response in neonatal
adult primates. Biol Psychiatry (1994) 35:221–7. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(94) rats. Endocrinology (1980) 106:991–4. doi:10.1210/endo-106-3-991
91252-1 119. Walker SJ, Vrana KE. Pituitary corticotroph function during the stress hypore-
100. van Oers HJ, De Kloet ER, Whelan T, Levine S. Maternal deprivation effect on sponsive period in neonatal rats. Neuroendocrinology (1993) 57:1003–10.
the infant’s neural stress markers is reversed by tactile stimulation and feeding doi:10.1159/000126464
but not by suppressing corticosterone. J Neurosci (1998) 18:10171–9. 120. Viau V, Sharma S, Meaney MJ. Changes in plasma adrenocorticotropin, cor-
101. Andersen SL, Lyss PJ, Dumont NL, Teicher MH. Enduring neurochemical ticosterone, corticosteroid-binding globulin, and hippocampal glucocorticoid
effects of early maternal separation on limbic structures. Ann N Y Acad Sci receptor occupancy/translocation in rat pups in response to stress. J Neuroen-
(1999) 877:756–9. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09317.x docrinol (1996) 8:1–8. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2826.1996.tb00680.x
102. Huot RL, Plotsky PM, Lenox RH, McNamara RK. Neonatal maternal separa- 121. Vazquez DM. Stress and the developing limbic-hypothalamic-pituitary-
tion reduces hippocampal mossy fiber density in adult Long Evans rats. Brain adrenal axis. Psychoneuroendocrinology (1998) 23:663–700. doi:10.1016/
Res (2002) 950:52–63. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02985-2 S0306-4530(98)00029-8
122. Dallman MF. Moments in time – the neonatal rat hypothalamo-pituitary- 144. Hatalski CG, Guirguis C, Baram TZ. Corticotropin releasing factor
adrenal axis. Endocrinology (2000) 141:1590–2. doi:10.1210/endo.141.5.7527 mRNA expression in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus and the central
123. Petersen HH, Andreassen TK, Breiderhoff T, Brasen JH, Schulz H, Gross nucleus of the amygdala is modulated by repeated acute stress in the imma-
V, et al. Hyporesponsiveness to glucocorticoids in mice genetically deficient ture rat. J Neuroendocrinol (1998) 10:663–9. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2826.1998.
for the corticosteroid binding globulin. Mol Cell Biol (2006) 26:7236–45. 00246.x
doi:10.1128/MCB.00400-06 145. Dent GW, Okimoto DK, Smith MA, Levine S. Stress-induced alterations in
124. Levine S. The pituitary-adrenal system and the developing brain. Prog Brain corticotropin-releasing hormone and vasopressin gene expression in the par-
Res (1970) 32:79–85. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)61521-6 aventricular nucleus during ontogeny. Neuroendocrinology (2000) 71:333–42.
125. Cote TE, Yasumura S. Effect of ACTH and histamine stress on serum corticos- doi:10.1159/000054554
terone and adrenal cyclic AMP levels in immature rats. Endocrinology (1975) 146. Meaney MJ, Sapolsky RM, McEwen BS. The development of the glucocorticoid
96:1044–7. doi:10.1210/endo-96-4-1044 receptor system in the rat limbic brain. II. An autoradiographic study. Brain
126. Martin CE, Cake MH, Hartmann PE, Cook IF. Relationship between foetal cor- Res (1985) 350:165–8. doi:10.1016/0165-3806(85)90260-3
ticosteroids, maternal progesterone and parturition in the rat. Acta Endocrinol 147. Alexis MN, Kitraki E, Spanou K, Stylianopoulou F, Sekeris CE. Ontogeny of the
(Copenh) (1977) 84:167–76. glucocorticoid receptor in the rat brain. Adv Exp Med Biol (1990) 265:269–76.
127. Walker CD, Perrin M, Vale W, Rivier C. Ontogeny of the stress response in doi:10.1007/978-1-4757-5876-4_25
the rat: role of the pituitary and the hypothalamus. Endocrinology (1986) 148. Stanton ME, Levine S. Inhibition of infant glucocorticoid stress response: spe-
118:1445–51. doi:10.1210/endo-118-4-1445 cific role of maternal cues. Dev Psychobiol (1990) 23:411–26. doi:10.1002/dev.
128. Walker CD, Sapolsky RM, Meaney MJ, Vale WW, Rivier CL. Increased pituitary 420230504
sensitivity to glucocorticoid feedback during the stress nonresponsive period 149. Widmaier EP. Glucose homeostasis and hypothalamic-pituitary-
in the neonatal rat. Endocrinology (1986) 119:1816–21. doi:10.1210/endo-119- adrenocortical axis during development in rats. Am J Physiol (1990)
4-1816 259:E601–13.
129. Levine S. The ontogeny of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. The influ- 150. Arai M, Widmaier EP. Activation of the pituitary-adrenocortical axis in day-
ence of maternal factors. Ann N Y Acad Sci (1994) 746:275–88. doi:10.1111/j. old rats by insulin-induced hypoglycemia. Endocrinology (1991) 129:1505–12.
1749-6632.1994.tb39245.x doi:10.1210/endo-129-3-1505
130. Grino M, Paulmyer-Lacroix O, Anglade G, Oliver C. Molecular aspects of the 151. Rosenfeld P, Van Eekelen JA, Levine S, De Kloet ER. Ontogeny of cor-
regulation of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis during development in ticosteroid receptors in the brain. Cell Mol Neurobiol (1993) 13:295–319.
the rat. Ann N Y Acad Sci (1995) 771:339–51. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995. doi:10.1007/BF00711575
tb44693.x 152. Suchecki D, Rosenfeld P, Levine S. Maternal regulation of the hypothalamic-
131. Ramsay DS, Lewis M. Developmental change in infant cortisol and behavioral pituitary-adrenal axis in the infant rat: the roles of feeding and stroking. Brain
response to inoculation. Child Dev (1994) 65:1491–502. doi:10.2307/1131513 Res Dev Brain Res (1993) 75:185–92. doi:10.1016/0165-3806(93)90022-3
132. Gunnar MR, Brodersen L, Krueger K, Rigatuso J. Dampening of adrenocortical 153. Eghbal-Ahmadi M, Avishai-Eliner S, Hatalski CG, Baram TZ. Differential regu-
responses during infancy: normative changes and individual differences. Child lation of the expression of corticotropin-releasing factor receptor type 2 (CRF2)
Dev (1996) 67:877–89. doi:10.2307/1131867 in hypothalamus and amygdala of the immature rat by sensory input and food
133. Gunnar MR. Integrating neuroscience and psychological approaches in the intake. J Neurosci (1999) 19:3982–91.
study of early experiences. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2003) 1008:238–47. doi:10.1196/ 154. Hofer MA. Psychobiological roots of early attachment. Curr Dir Psychol Sci
annals.1301.024 (2006) 15:84–8. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00412.x
134. Gunnar MR, Fisher PA. Bringing basic research on early experience and 155. Baram TZ, Yi S, Avishai-Eliner S, Schultz L. Development neurobiology of
stress neurobiology to bear on preventive interventions for neglected the stress response: multilevel regulation of corticotropin-releasing hormone
and maltreated children. Dev Psychopathol (2006) 18:651–77. doi:10.1017/ function. Ann N Y Acad Sci (1997) 814:252–65. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.
S0954579406060330 tb46161.x
135. Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout 156. Avishai-Eliner S, Gilles EE, Eghbal-Ahmadi M, Bar-El Y, Baram TZ. Altered
the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci (2009) regulation of gene and protein expression of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
10:434–45. doi:10.1038/nrn2639 axis components in an immature rat model of chronic stress. J Neuroendocrinol
136. Sapolsky RM, Meaney MJ. Maturation of the adrenocortical stress response: (2001) 13:799–807. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2826.2001.00698.x
neuroendocrine control mechanisms and the stress hyporesponsive period. 157. Avishai-Eliner S, Eghbal-Ahmadi M, Tabachnik E, Brunson KL, Baram TZ.
Brain Res (1986) 396:64–76. doi:10.1016/0165-0173(86)90010-X Down-regulation of hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing hormone messen-
137. Nachmias M, Gunnar M, Mangelsdorf S, Parritz RH, Buss K. Behavioral inhi- ger ribonucleic acid (mRNA) precedes early-life experience-induced changes in
bition and stress reactivity: the moderating role of attachment security. Child hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor mRNA. Endocrinology (2001) 142:89–97.
Dev (1996) 67:508–22. doi:10.2307/1131829 doi:10.1210/en.142.1.89
138. Dettling AC, Parker SW, Lane S, Sebanc A, Gunnar MR. Quality of care 158. Sanchez MM. The impact of early adverse care on HPA axis development: non-
and temperament determine changes in cortisol concentrations over the day human primate models. Horm Behav (2006) 50:623–31. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.
for young children in childcare. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2000) 25:819–36. 2006.06.012
doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(00)00028-7 159. Kaffman A, Meaney MJ. Neurodevelopmental sequelae of postnatal maternal
139. Gunnar MR, Donzella B. Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early care in rodents: clinical and research implications of molecular insights. J Child
human development. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2002) 27:199–220. doi:10. Psychol Psychiatry (2007) 48:224–44. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01730.x
1016/S0306-4530(01)00045-2 160. Howell BR, Sanchez MM. Understanding behavioral effects of early life stress
140. Levine S. Regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in the neona- using the reactive scope and allostatic load models. Dev Psychopathol (2011)
tal rat: the role of maternal behavior. Neurotox Res (2002) 4:557–64. doi:10. 23:1001–16. doi:10.1017/S0954579411000460
1080/10298420290030569 161. Bolhuis JJ, Honey RC. Imprinting, learning and development: from behav-
141. Schoenfeld NM, Leathem JH, Rabii J. Maturation of adrenal stress responsive- iour to brain and back. Trends Neurosci (1998) 21:306–11. doi:10.1016/S0166-
ness in the rat. Neuroendocrinology (1980) 31:101–5. doi:10.1159/000123058 2236(98)01258-2
142. Walker CD, Scribner KA, Cascio CS, Dallman MF. The pituitary-adrenocortical 162. Ziabreva I, Poeggel G, Schnabel R, Braun K. Separation-induced receptor
system of neonatal rats is responsive to stress throughout development changes in the hippocampus and amygdala of Octodon degus: influence of
in a time-dependent and stressor-specific fashion. Endocrinology (1991) maternal vocalizations. J Neurosci (2003) 23:5329–36.
128:1385–95. doi:10.1210/endo-128-3-1385 163. Insel TR, Young LJ. The neurobiology of attachment. Nat Rev Neurosci (2001)
143. Yi SJ, Baram TZ. Corticotropin-releasing hormone mediates the response to 2:129–36. doi:10.1038/35053579
cold stress in the neonatal rat without compensatory enhancement of the pep- 164. Herman JP, Cullinan WE. Neurocircuitry of stress: central control of the
tide’s gene expression. Endocrinology (1994) 135:2364–8. doi:10.1210/endo. hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Trends Neurosci (1997) 20:78–84.
135.6.7988418 doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(96)10069-2
165. Sullivan RM, Landers M, Yeaman B, Wilson DA. Good memories of bad events 190. Haroutunian V, Campbell BA. Emergence of interoceptive and exteroceptive
in infancy. Nature (2000) 407:38–9. doi:10.1038/35024156 control of behavior in rats. Science (1979) 205:927–9. doi:10.1126/science.
166. Moriceau S, Sullivan RM. Maternal presence serves as a switch between learn- 472715
ing fear and attraction in infancy. Nat Neurosci (2006) 9:1004–6. doi:10.1038/ 191. Camp LL, Rudy JW. Changes in the categorization of appetitive and aver-
nn1733 sive events during postnatal development of the rat. Dev Psychobiol (1988)
167. Shionoya K, Moriceau S, Bradstock P, Sullivan RM. Maternal attenuation 21:25–42. doi:10.1002/dev.420210103
of hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus norepinephrine switches avoidance 192. Otto T, Cousens G, Rajewski K. Odor-guided fear conditioning in rats: 1. Acqui-
learning to preference learning in preweanling rat pups. Horm Behav (2007) sition, retention, and latent inhibition. Behav Neurosci (1997) 111:1257–64.
52:391–400. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.06.004 doi:10.1037/0735-7044.111.6.1257
168. Miller SS, Spear NE. Olfactory learning in the rat neonate soon after birth. Dev 193. Paschall GY, Davis M. Olfactory-mediated fear-potentiated startle. Behav Neu-
Psychobiol (2008) 50:554–65. doi:10.1002/dev.20318 rosci (2002) 116:4–12. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.116.1.4
169. Sullivan RM, Holman PJ. Transitions in sensitive period attachment learning 194. Collier AC, Bolles RC. The ontogenesis of defensive reactions to shock
in infancy: the role of corticosterone. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2010) 34:835–44. in preweanling rats. Dev Psychobiol (1980) 13:141–50. doi:10.1002/dev.
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.010 420130206
170. Leon M. Dietary control of maternal pheromone in the lactating rat. Physiol 195. Emerich DF, Scalzo FM, Enters EK, Spear NE, Spear LP. Effects of 6-
Behav (1975) 14:311–9. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(75)90039-6 hydroxydopamine-induced catecholamine depletion on shock-precipitated
171. Pedersen PE, Williams CL, Blass EM. Activation and odor conditioning of suck- wall climbing of infant rat pups. Dev Psychobiol (1985) 18:215–27. doi:10.
ling behavior in 3-day-old albino rats. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process (1982) 1002/dev.420180303
8:329–41. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.8.4.329 196. Barr GA. Ontogeny of nociception and antinociception. NIDA Res Monogr
172. Leon M. Neuroethology of olfactory preference development. J Neurobiol (1995) 158:172–201.
(1992) 23:1557–73. doi:10.1002/neu.480231012 197. Fitzgerald M. The development of nociceptive circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci (2005)
173. Kojima S, Alberts JR. Maternal care can rapidly induce an odor-guided hud- 6:507–20. doi:10.1038/nrn1701
dling preference in rat pups. Dev Psychobiol (2009) 51:95–105. doi:10.1002/ 198. Fanselow MS, LeDoux JE. Why we think plasticity underlying Pavlovian fear
dev.20349 conditioning occurs in the basolateral amygdala. Neuron (1999) 23:229–32.
174. Moriceau S, Roth TL, Sullivan RM. Rodent model of infant attachment learning doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80775-8
and stress. Dev Psychobiol (2010) 52:651–60. doi:10.1002/dev.20482 199. Fendt M, Fanselow MS. The neuroanatomical and neurochemical basis of con-
175. Landers MS, Sullivan RM. The development and neurobiology of infant attach- ditioned fear. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (1999) 23:743–60. doi:10.1016/S0149-
ment and fear. Dev Neurosci (2012) 34:101–14. doi:10.1159/000336732 7634(99)00016-0
176. Singh PJ, Tobach E. Olfactory bulbectomy and nursing behavior in rat pups 200. Maren S. The amygdala, synaptic plasticity, and fear memory. Ann N Y Acad
(Wistar DAB). Dev Psychobiol (1975) 8:151–64. doi:10.1002/dev.420080207 Sci (2003) 985:106–13. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07075.x
177. Galef BG Jr, Kaner HC. Establishment and maintenance of preference for nat- 201. Moriceau S, Wilson DA, Levine S, Sullivan RM. Dual circuitry for odor-shock
ural and artificial olfactory stimuli in juvenile rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol conditioning during infancy: corticosterone switches between fear and attrac-
(1980) 94:588–95. doi:10.1037/h0077693 tion via amygdala. J Neurosci (2006) 26:6737–48. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.
178. Galef BG Jr. Acquisition and waning of exposure-induced attraction to a non- 0499-06.2006
natural odor in rat pups. Dev Psychobiol (1982) 15:479–90. doi:10.1002/dev. 202. Johansen JP, Cain CK, Ostroff LE, Ledoux JE. Molecular mechanisms of fear
420150510 learning and memory. Cell (2011) 147:509–24. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2011.10.009
179. Alberts JR, May B. Nonnutritive, thermotactile induction of filial huddling in 203. Sullivan RM. Unique characteristics of neonatal classical conditioning: the
rat pups. Dev Psychobiol (1984) 17:161–81. doi:10.1002/dev.420170207 role of the amygdala and locus coeruleus. Integr Physiol Behav Sci (2001)
180. Caza PA, Spear NE. Short-term exposure to an odor increases its subsequent 36:293–307. doi:10.1007/BF02688797
preference in preweanling rats: a descriptive profile of the phenomenon. Dev 204. Coopersmith R, Lee S, Leon M. Olfactory bulb responses after odor aver-
Psychobiol (1984) 17:407–22. doi:10.1002/dev.420170407 sion learning by young rats. Brain Res (1986) 389:271–7. doi:10.1016/0165-
181. Duveau A, Godinot F. Influence of the odorization of the rearing environment 3806(86)90195-1
on the development of odor-guided behavior in rat pups. Physiol Behav (1988) 205. Wilson DA, Sullivan RM, Leon M. Single-unit analysis of postnatal olfactory
42:265–70. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(88)90080-7 learning: modified olfactory bulb output response patterns to learned attractive
182. Sullivan RM, Wilson DA, Wong R, Correa A, Leon M. Modified behavioral and odors. J Neurosci (1987) 7:3154–62.
olfactory bulb responses to maternal odors in preweanling rats. Brain Res Dev 206. McLean JH, Harley CW, Darby-King A, Yuan Q. pCREB in the neonate rat
Brain Res (1990) 53:243–7. doi:10.1016/0165-3806(90)90013-O olfactory bulb is selectively and transiently increased by odor preference-
183. Johanson IB, Hall WG. Appetitive learning in 1-day-old rat pups. Science (1979) conditioned training. Learn Mem (1999) 6:608–18. doi:10.1101/lm.6.6.608
205:419–21. doi:10.1126/science.451612 207. Yuan Q, Harley CW, Darby-King A, Neve RL, McLean JH. Early odor preference
184. Johanson IB, Teicher MH. Classical conditioning of an odor preference in 3- learning in the rat: bidirectional effects of cAMP response element-binding
day-old rats. Behav Neural Biol (1980) 29:132–6. doi:10.1016/S0163-1047(80) protein (CREB) and mutant CREB support a causal role for phosphorylated
92596-0 CREB. J Neurosci (2003) 23:4760–5.
185. Brake SC. Suckling infant rats learn a preference for a novel olfactory stimu- 208. Zhang JJ, Okutani F, Inoue S, Kaba H. Activation of the cyclic AMP response
lus paired with milk delivery. Science (1981) 211:506–8. doi:10.1126/science. element-binding protein signaling pathway in the olfactory bulb is required
7192882 for the acquisition of olfactory aversive learning in young rats. Neuroscience
186. Sullivan RM, Leon M. Early olfactory learning induces an enhanced olfactory (2003) 117:707–13. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00962-4
bulb response in young rats. Brain Res (1986) 392:278–82. doi:10.1016/0165- 209. Yuan Q, Mutoh H, Debarbieux F, Knopfel T. Calcium signaling in mitral cell
3806(86)90256-7 dendrites of olfactory bulbs of neonatal rats and mice during olfactory nerve
187. Sullivan RM, Wilson DA. Neural correlates of conditioned odor avoidance in stimulation and beta-adrenoceptor activation. Learn Mem (2004) 11:406–11.
infant rats. Behav Neurosci (1991) 105:307–12. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.105.2. doi:10.1101/lm.75204
307 210. Yuan Q. Theta bursts in the olfactory nerve paired with beta-adrenoceptor
188. Moriceau S, Shionoya K, Jakubs K, Sullivan RM. Early-life stress disrupts activation induce calcium elevation in mitral cells: a mechanism for odor pref-
attachment learning: the role of amygdala corticosterone, locus coeruleus cor- erence learning in the neonate rat. Learn Mem (2009) 16:676–81. doi:10.1101/
ticotropin releasing hormone, and olfactory bulb norepinephrine. J Neurosci lm.1569309
(2009) 29:15745–55. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4106-09.2009 211. Woo CC, Coopersmith R, Leon M. Localized changes in olfactory bulb
189. Levine S. Plasma-free corticosteroid response to electric shock in rats stimu- morphology associated with early olfactory learning. J Comp Neurol (1987)
lated in infancy. Science (1962) 135:795–6. doi:10.1126/science.135.3506.795-a 263:113–25. doi:10.1002/cne.902630110
212. Johnson BA, Woo CC, Duong H, Nguyen V, Leon M. A learned odor evokes 236. Stanton ME. Multiple memory systems, development and conditioning. Behav
an enhanced Fos-like glomerular response in the olfactory bulb of young rats. Brain Res (2000) 110:25–37. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00182-5
Brain Res (1995) 699:192–200. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(95)00896-X 237. Sullivan RM. Developmental changes in olfactory behavior and limbic cir-
213. Yuan Q, Harley CW, McLean JH, Knopfel T. Optical imaging of odor preference cuitry. Chem Senses (2005) 30(Suppl 1):i152–3. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjh159
memory in the rat olfactory bulb. J Neurophysiol (2002) 87:3156–9. 238. Moriceau S, Sullivan RM. Neurobiology of infant attachment. Dev Psychobiol
214. Shakhawat AM, Harley CW, Yuan Q. Olfactory bulb alpha 2-adrenoceptor acti- (2005) 47:230–42. doi:10.1002/dev.20093
vation promotes rat pup odor-preference learning via a cAMP-independent 239. Takahashi LK, Rubin WW. Corticosteroid induction of threat-induced behav-
mechanism. Learn Mem (2012) 19:499–502. doi:10.1101/lm.027359.112 ioral inhibition in preweanling rats. Behav Neurosci (1993) 107:860–6. doi:10.
215. Wilson DA, Leon M. Spatial patterns of olfactory bulb single-unit responses to 1037/0735-7044.107.5.860
learned olfactory cues in young rats. J Neurophysiol (1988) 59:1770–82. 240. Takahashi LK. Organizing action of corticosterone on the development of
216. Sullivan RM, Wilson DA. Role of the amygdala complex in early olfactory asso- behavioral inhibition in the preweanling rat. Brain Res Dev Brain Res (1994)
ciative learning. Behav Neurosci (1993) 107:254–63. doi:10.1037/0735-7044. 81:121–7. doi:10.1016/0165-3806(94)90074-4
107.2.254 241. Moriceau S, Sullivan RM. Corticosterone influences on mammalian neona-
217. Wiedenmayer CP, Barr GA. Developmental changes in c-fos expression to an tal sensitive-period learning. Behav Neurosci (2004) 118:274–81. doi:10.1037/
age-specific social stressor in infant rats. Behav Brain Res (2001) 126:147–57. 0735-7044.118.2.274
doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00260-1 242. Moriceau S, Roth TL, Okotoghaide T, Sullivan RM. Corticosterone controls the
218. Thompson JV, Sullivan RM, Wilson DA. Developmental emergence of fear developmental emergence of fear and amygdala function to predator odors in
learning corresponds with changes in amygdala synaptic plasticity. Brain Res infant rat pups. Int J Dev Neurosci (2004) 22:415–22. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.
(2008) 1200:58–65. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.01.057 2004.05.011
219. Sullivan RM, Hofer MA, Brake SC. Olfactory-guided orientation in neonatal 243. Barr GA, Moriceau S, Shionoya K, Muzny K, Gao P, Wang S, et al. Transitions
rats is enhanced by a conditioned change in behavioral state. Dev Psychobiol in infant learning are modulated by dopamine in the amygdala. Nat Neurosci
(1986) 19:615–23. doi:10.1002/dev.420190612 (2009) 12:1367–9. doi:10.1038/nn.2403
220. Shipley MT, Halloran FJ, De La Torre J. Surprisingly rich projection from 244. Collier AC, Mast J, Meyer DR, Jacobs CE. Approach-avoidance conflict in
locus coeruleus to the olfactory bulb in the rat. Brain Res (1985) 329:294–9. preweanling rats: a developmental study. Anim Learn Behav (1979) 7:514–20.
doi:10.1016/0006-8993(85)90537-2 doi:10.3758/BF03209712
221. McLean JH, Shipley MT. Postnatal development of the noradrenergic projec- 245. Sullivan RM, Wilson DA, Leon M. Norepinephrine and learning-induced plas-
tion from locus coeruleus to the olfactory bulb in the rat. J Comp Neurol (1991) ticity in infant rat olfactory system. J Neurosci (1989) 9:3998–4006.
304:467–77. doi:10.1002/cne.903040310 246. Sullivan RM, Wilson DA. The role of norepinephrine in the expression
222. Nakamura S, Kimura F, Sakaguchi T. Postnatal development of electrical activ- of learned olfactory neurobehavioral responses in infant rats. Psychobiology
ity in the locus coeruleus. J Neurophysiol (1987) 58:510–24. (Austin, Tex) (1991) 19:308–12.
223. Nakamura S, Sakaguchi T. Development and plasticity of the locus coeruleus: 247. Sullivan RM, Moriceau S, Raineki C, Roth T. Ontogeny of infant fear learning
a review of recent physiological and pharmacological experimentation. Prog and amygdala. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. Cognitive Neuroscience. Cambridge:
Neurobiol (1990) 34:505–26. doi:10.1016/0301-0082(90)90018-C MIT Press (2009). p. 889–904.
224. Marshall KC, Christie MJ, Finlayson PG, Williams JT. Developmental aspects 248. Vogt JL, Coe CL, Levine S. Behavioral and adrenocorticoid responsiveness of
of the locus coeruleus-noradrenaline system. Prog Brain Res (1991) 88:173–85. squirrel monkeys to a live snake: is flight necessarily stressful? Behav Neural
doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)63807-8 Biol (1981) 32:391–405. doi:10.1016/S0163-1047(81)90826-8
225. Rangel S, Leon M. Early odor preference training increases olfactory bulb nor- 249. Hennessy MB. Presence of companion moderates arousal of monkeys with
epinephrine. Brain Res Dev Brain Res (1995) 85:187–91. doi:10.1016/0165- restricted social experience. Physiol Behav (1984) 33:693–8. doi:10.1016/0031-
3806(94)00211-H 9384(84)90033-7
226. Winzer-Serhan UH, Leslie FM. Expression of alpha 2A adrenoceptors during 250. Kirschbaum C, Klauer T, Filipp SH, Hellhammer DH. Sex-specific effects of
rat neocortical development. J Neurobiol (1999) 38:259–69. doi:10.1002/(SICI) social support on cortisol and subjective responses to acute psychological stress.
1097-4695(19990205)38:2<259::AID-NEU8>3.0.CO;2-U Psychosom Med (1995) 57:23–31.
227. Moriceau S, Sullivan RM. Unique neural circuitry for neonatal olfactory learn- 251. Heinrichs M, Baumgartner T, Kirschbaum C, Ehlert U. Social support and
ing. J Neurosci (2004) 24:1182–9. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4578-03.2004 oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychoso-
228. Sullivan RM, Zyzak DR, Skierkowski P, Wilson DA. The role of olfactory bulb cial stress. Biol Psychiatry (2003) 54:1389–98. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(03)
norepinephrine in early olfactory learning. Brain Res Dev Brain Res (1992) 00465-7
70:279–82. doi:10.1016/0165-3806(92)90207-D 252. Taylor SE, Burklund LJ, Eisenberger NI, Lehman BJ, Hilmert CJ, Lieberman
229. Sullivan RM, Wilson DA. The locus coeruleus, norepinephrine, and mem- MD. Neural bases of moderation of cortisol stress responses by psychoso-
ory in newborns. Brain Res Bull (1994) 35:467–72. doi:10.1016/0361-9230(94) cial resources. J Pers Soc Psychol (2008) 95:197–211. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.
90160-0 95.1.197
230. Sullivan RM, Wilson DA, Lemon C, Gerhardt GA. Bilateral 6-OHDA lesions 253. Hostinar CE, Sullivan RM, Gunnar MR. Psychobiological mechanisms under-
of the locus coeruleus impair associative olfactory learning in newborn rats. lying the social buffering of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis: a
Brain Res (1994) 643:306–9. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(94)90038-8 review of animal models and human studies across development. Psychol Bull
231. Langdon PE, Harley CW, McLean JH. Increased beta adrenoceptor activation (2013) 140(1):256–82. doi:10.1037/a0032671
overcomes conditioned olfactory learning deficits induced by serotonin deple- 254. Stanton ME, Wallstrom J, Levine S. Maternal contact inhibits pituitary-
tion. Brain Res Dev Brain Res (1997) 102:291–3. doi:10.1016/S0165-3806(97) adrenal stress responses in preweanling rats. Dev Psychobiol (1987) 20:131–45.
00090-4 doi:10.1002/dev.420200204
232. Sullivan RM, Stackenwalt G, Nasr F, Lemon C, Wilson DA. Association 255. Wiedenmayer CP, Magarinos AM, McEwen BS, Barr G. Mother lowers gluco-
of an odor with activation of olfactory bulb noradrenergic beta-receptors corticoid levels of preweaning rats after acute threat. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2003)
or locus coeruleus stimulation is sufficient to produce learned approach 1008:304–7. doi:10.1196/annals.1301.038
responses to that odor in neonatal rats. Behav Neurosci (2000) 114:957–62. 256. DeVries AC, Glasper ER, Detillion CE. Social modulation of stress responses.
doi:10.1037/0735-7044.114.5.957 Physiol Behav (2003) 79:399–407. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00152-5
233. Fanselow MS, Gale GD. The amygdala, fear, and memory. Ann N Y Acad Sci 257. Kikusui T, Winslow JT, Mori Y. Social buffering: relief from stress and anxiety.
(2003) 985:125–34. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07077.x Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (2006) 361:2215–28. doi:10.1098/rstb.2006.
234. Blozovski D, Cudennec A. Passive avoidance learning in the young rat. Dev 1941
Psychobiol (1980) 13:513–8. doi:10.1002/dev.420130510 258. Hennessy MB, Kaiser S, Sachser N. Social buffering of the stress response:
235. Myslivecek J. Inhibitory learning and memory in newborn rats. Prog Neurobiol diversity, mechanisms, and functions. Front Neuroendocrinol (2009) 30:470–82.
(1997) 53:399–430. doi:10.1016/S0301-0082(97)00036-1 doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.06.001
259. Takahashi Y, Kiyokawa Y, Kodama Y, Arata S, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y. Olfactory sig- acute stress. J Neurochem (1988) 50:1678–81. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.1988.
nals mediate social buffering of conditioned fear responses in male rats. Behav tb02462.x
Brain Res (2013) 240:46–51. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2012.11.017 281. Abercrombie ED, Keefe KA, Difrischia DS, Zigmond MJ. Differential effect of
260. Upton KJ, Sullivan RM. Defining age limits of the sensitive period for attach- stress on in vivo dopamine release in striatum, nucleus accumbens, and medial
ment learning in rat pups. Dev Psychobiol (2010) 52:453–64. doi:10.1002/dev. frontal cortex. J Neurochem (1989) 52:1655–8. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.1989.
20448 tb09224.x
261. Gunnar MR, Brodersen L, Nachmias M, Buss K, Rigatuso J. Stress reactiv- 282. Deutch AY, Roth RH. The determinants of stress-induced activation of the
ity and attachment security. Dev Psychobiol (1996) 29:191–204. doi:10.1002/ prefrontal cortical dopamine system. Prog Brain Res (1990) 85:367–402.
(SICI)1098-2302(199604)29:3<191::AID-DEV1>3.3.CO;2-T doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)62691-6
262. Eisenberg L. The biosocial context of parenting in human families. In: Kras- 283. Pei Q, Zetterstrom T, Fillenz M. Tail pinch-induced changes in the turnover
negor NAB Sr, editor. Mammalian Parenting Biochemical, Neurobiological, and release of dopamine and 5-hydroxytryptamine in different brain regions
and Behavioral Determinants. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (1990). of the rat. Neuroscience (1990) 35:133–8. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(90)90127-P
p. 9–24. 284. Nisenbaum LK, Zigmond MJ, Sved AF, Abercrombie ED. Prior exposure to
263. Field T. Child abuse in monkeys and humans: a comparative perspective. In: chronic stress results in enhanced synthesis and release of hippocampal nor-
Reite M, Caine NG, editors. Child Abuse: The Nonhuman Primate Data. New epinephrine in response to a novel stressor. J Neurosci (1991) 11:1478–84.
York, NY: Alan R Liss, Inc. (1983). p. 151–74. 285. Inoue T, Tsuchiya K, Koyama T. Regional changes in dopamine and sero-
264. Schapiro S, Mitchel G. Infant-directed abuse in a seminatural environment: tonin activation with various intensity of physical and psychological stress in
precipitating factors. In: Reite M, Caine NG, editors. Child Abuse: The Nonhu- the rat brain. Pharmacol Biochem Behav (1994) 49:911–20. doi:10.1016/0091-
man Primate Data. New York, NY: Alan R Liss, Inc. (1983). p. 29–48. 3057(94)90243-7
265. Rice CJ, Sandman CA, Lenjavi MR, Baram TZ. A novel mouse model for 286. Finlay JM, Zigmond MJ, Abercrombie ED. Increased dopamine and norep-
acute and long-lasting consequences of early life stress. Endocrinology (2008) inephrine release in medial prefrontal cortex induced by acute and chronic
149:4892–900. doi:10.1210/en.2008-0633 stress: effects of diazepam. Neuroscience (1995) 64:619–28. doi:10.1016/0306-
266. Pedersen CA, Caldwell JD, McGuire M, Evans DL. Corticotropin-releasing 4522(94)00331-X
hormone inhibits maternal behavior and induces pup-killing. Life Sci (1991) 287. Makino S, Schulkin J, Smith MA, Pacak K, Palkovits M, Gold PW. Regulation
48:1537–46. doi:10.1016/0024-3205(91)90278-J of corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor messenger ribonucleic acid in the
267. Yeh KY. Corticosterone concentrations in the serum and milk of lactating rat brain and pituitary by glucocorticoids and stress. Endocrinology (1995)
rats: parallel changes after induced stress. Endocrinology (1984) 115:1364–70. 136:4517–25. doi:10.1210/en.136.10.4517
doi:10.1210/endo-115-4-1364 288. Inglis FM, Moghaddam B. Dopaminergic innervation of the amygdala is highly
268. McEwen BS, Seeman T. Protective and damaging effects of mediators of stress. responsive to stress. J Neurochem (1999) 72:1088–94. doi:10.1046/j.1471-4159.
Elaborating and testing the concepts of allostasis and allostatic load. Ann N Y 1999.0721088.x
Acad Sci (1999) 896:30–47. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08103.x 289. Kaehler ST, Singewald N, Sinner C, Thurnher C, Philippu A. Conditioned fear
269. McEwen BS. The neurobiology of stress: from serendipity to clinical relevance. and inescapable shock modify the release of serotonin in the locus coeruleus.
Brain Res (2000) 886:172–89. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02950-4 Brain Res (2000) 859:249–54. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(00)01967-3
270. Weinstock M. The potential influence of maternal stress hormones on devel- 290. McEwen BS. Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. N Engl J Med
opment and mental health of the offspring. Brain Behav Immun (2005) (1998) 338:171–9. doi:10.1056/NEJM199801153380307
19:296–308. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2004.09.006 291. Herbert J, Goodyer IM, Grossman AB, Hastings MH, De Kloet ER, Light-
271. Cone RD, Low MJ, Elmquist JK, Cameron JL. Neuroendocrinology. 12th ed. In: man SL, et al. Do corticosteroids damage the brain? J Neuroendocrinol (2006)
Larsen PR, Melmed S, Polonsky KS, editors. Williams Textbook of Endocrinology. 18:393–411. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2826.2006.01429.x
Philadelphia, PA: Saunders (2003). p. 81–176. 292. Chrousos GP. Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nat Rev Endocrinol
272. Herman JP, Figueiredo H, Mueller NK, Ulrich-Lai Y, Ostrander MM, Choi (2009) 5:374–81. doi:10.1038/nrendo.2009.106
DC, et al. Central mechanisms of stress integration: hierarchical circuitry con- 293. Nelson CA, Carver LJ. The effects of stress and trauma on brain and memory:
trolling hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical responsiveness. Front Neuroen- a view from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Dev Psychopathol (1998)
docrinol (2003) 24:151–80. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2003.07.001 10:793–809. doi:10.1017/S0954579498001874
273. Munck A, Guyre PM, Holbrook NJ. Physiological functions of glucocorticoids 294. Toth SL, Cicchetti D. Remembering, forgetting, and the effects of trauma
in stress and their relation to pharmacological actions. Endocr Rev (1984) on memory: a developmental psychopathology perspective. Dev Psychopathol
5:25–44. doi:10.1210/edrv-5-1-25 (1998) 10:589–605. doi:10.1017/S0954579498001771
274. Dallman MF, Akana SF, Strack AM, Hanson ES, Sebastian RJ. The 295. Matthews SG. Early programming of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal
neural network that regulates energy balance is responsive to glucocorticoids axis. Trends Endocrinol Metab (2002) 13:373–80. doi:10.1016/S1043-2760(02)
and insulin and also regulates HPA axis responsivity at a site proximal to CRF 00690-2
neurons. Ann N Y Acad Sci (1995) 771:730–42. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995. 296. Tottenham N, Sheridan MA. A review of adversity, the amygdala and the
tb44724.x hippocampus: a consideration of developmental timing. Front Hum Neurosci
275. Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, Munck AU. How do glucocorticoids influence stress (2009) 3:68. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.068.2009
responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative 297. Pechtel P, Pizzagalli DA. Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective
actions. Endocr Rev (2000) 21:55–89. doi:10.1210/er.21.1.55 function: an integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology (Berl)
276. Seckl JR. Glucocorticoids, ageing and nerve cell damage. J Neuroendocrinol (2011) 214:55–70. doi:10.1007/s00213-010-2009-2
(2000) 12:709–10. 298. Suomi SJ. Early stress and adult emotional reactivity in rhesus monkeys. Ciba
277. Thierry AM, Tassin JP, Blanc G, Glowinski J. Selective activation of mesocortical Found Symp (1991) 156:171–83.
DA system by stress. Nature (1976) 263:242–4. doi:10.1038/263242a0 299. Gunnar M, Vazquez D. Stress neurobiology and developmental psychopathol-
278. Herman JP, Guillonneau D, Dantzer R, Scatton B, Semerdjian-Rouquier L, Le ogy. In: Cicchetti DCD, editor. Developmental Psychopathology: Developmental
Moal M. Differential effects of inescapable footshocks and of stimuli previously Neuroscience. New York, NY: Wiley (2006). p. 533–77.
paired with inescapable footshocks on dopamine turnover in cortical and lim- 300. Andrews MH, Matthews SG. Programming of the hypothalamo-pituitary-
bic areas of the rat. Life Sci (1982) 30:2207–14. doi:10.1016/0024-3205(82) adrenal axis: serotonergic involvement. Stress (2004) 7:15–27. doi:10.1080/
90295-8 10253890310001650277
279. Abercrombie ED, Jacobs BL. Single-unit response of noradrenergic neurons 301. Rodrigues AJ, Leao P, Carvalho M, Almeida OF, Sousa N. Potential program-
in the locus coeruleus of freely moving cats. I. Acutely presented stressful and ming of dopaminergic circuits by early life stress. Psychopharmacology (Berl)
nonstressful stimuli. J Neurosci (1987) 7:2837–43. (2011) 214:107–20. doi:10.1007/s00213-010-2085-3
280. Adell A, Garcia-Marquez C, Armario A, Gelpi E. Chronic stress increases sero- 302. De Kloet ER, Rots NY, Cools AR. Brain-corticosteroid hormone dialogue: slow
tonin and noradrenaline in rat brain and sensitizes their responses to a further and persistent. Cell Mol Neurobiol (1996) 16:345–56. doi:10.1007/BF02088100
303. Diorio D, Viau V, Meaney MJ. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex (cin- 323. Coplan JD, Trost RC, Owens MJ, Cooper TB, Gorman JM, Nemeroff CB, et al.
gulate gyrus) in the regulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to Cerebrospinal fluid concentrations of somatostatin and biogenic amines in
stress. J Neurosci (1993) 13:3839–47. grown primates reared by mothers exposed to manipulated foraging condi-
304. Sullivan RM, Gratton A. Prefrontal cortical regulation of hypothalamic- tions. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1998) 55:473–7. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.55.5.473
pituitary-adrenal function in the rat and implications for psychopathology: 324. Maestripieri D, Higley JD, Lindell SG, Newman TK, McCormack KM, Sanchez
side matters. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2002) 27:99–114. doi:10.1016/S0306- MM. Early maternal rejection affects the development of monoaminergic sys-
4530(01)00038-5 tems and adult abusive parenting in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Behav
305. Herman JP, Ostrander MM, Mueller NK, Figueiredo H. Limbic system Neurosci (2006) 120:1017–24. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.120.5.1017
mechanisms of stress regulation: hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. 325. Bremner JD, Krystal JH, Southwick SM, Charney DS. Noradrenergic mech-
Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2005) 29:1201–13. doi:10.1016/j. anisms in stress and anxiety. I. Preclinical studies. Synapse (1996) 23:28–38.
pnpbp.2005.08.006 doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(199605)23:1<28::AID-SYN4>3.3.CO;2-4
306. Jankord R, Herman JP. Limbic regulation of hypothalamo-pituitary- 326. Bremner JD, Krystal JH, Southwick SM, Charney DS. Noradrenergic mech-
adrenocortical function during acute and chronic stress. Ann N Y Acad Sci anisms in stress and anxiety. II. Clinical studies. Synapse (1996) 23:39–51.
(2008) 1148:64–73. doi:10.1196/annals.1410.012 doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(199605)23:1<28::AID-SYN4>3.3.CO;2-4
307. Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP. Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic 327. Heim C, Owens MJ, Plotsky PM, Nemeroff CB. The role of early adverse
stress responses. Nat Rev Neurosci (2009) 10:397–409. doi:10.1038/nrn2647 life events in the etiology of depression and posttraumatic stress disorder.
308. Cunningham MG, Bhattacharyya S, Benes FM. Amygdalo-cortical sprouting Focus on corticotropin-releasing factor. Ann N Y Acad Sci (1997) 821:194–207.
continues into early adulthood: implications for the development of normal doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb48279.x
and abnormal function during adolescence. J Comp Neurol (2002) 453:116–30. 328. Porsolt RD, Brossard G, Hautbois C, Roux S. Rodent models of depression:
309. Andersen SL, Teicher MH. Stress, sensitive periods and maturational events in forced swimming and tail suspension behavioral despair tests in rats and mice.
adolescent depression. Trends Neurosci (2008) 31:183–91. doi:10.1016/j.tins. Curr Protoc Neurosci (2001) Chapter 8:Unit810A. doi:10.1002/0471142301.
2008.01.004 ns0810as14
310. Teicher MH, Andersen SL, Polcari A, Anderson CM, Navalta CP. Developmental 329. Pollak DD, Rey CE, Monje FJ. Rodent models in depression research: clas-
neurobiology of childhood stress and trauma. Psychiatr Clin North Am (2002) sical strategies and new directions. Ann Med (2010) 42:252–64. doi:10.3109/
25:397–426, vii–viii. doi:10.1016/S0193-953X(01)00003-X 07853891003769957
311. Meaney MJ. Plasticity and health: social influences on gene expression and 330. Porsolt RD, Le Pichon M, Jalfre M. Depression: a new animal model sensitive
neural development. In: Kessel F, Rosenfeld PL, Anderson NB, editors. Expand- to antidepressant treatments. Nature (1977) 266:730–2. doi:10.1038/266730a0
ing the Boundaries of Health and Social Science: Case Studies in Interdisciplinary 331. McEwen BS. Early life influences on life-long patterns of behavior and health.
Innovation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2008). 147 p. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev (2003) 9:149–54. doi:10.1002/mrdd.10074
312. Loman MM, Gunnar MR. Early experience and the development of stress 332. Cicchetti D, Toth SL. Child maltreatment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol (2005)
reactivity and regulation in children. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2010) 34:867–76. 1:409–38. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.144029
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.05.007 333. Pryce CR, Ruedi-Bettschen D, Dettling AC, Weston A, Russig H, Ferger B, et al.
313. Nemeroff CB, Widerlov E, Bissette G, Walleus H, Karlsson I, Eklund K, et al. Long-term effects of early-life environmental manipulations in rodents and
Elevated concentrations of CSF corticotropin-releasing factor-like immunore- primates: potential animal models in depression research. Neurosci Biobehav
activity in depressed patients. Science (1984) 226:1342–4. doi:10.1126/science. Rev (2005) 29:649–74. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.011
6334362 334. Heim C, Shugart M, Craighead WE, Nemeroff CB. Neurobiological and
314. Gold PW, Calabrese JR, Kling MA, Avgerinos P, Khan I, Gallucci WT, et al. psychiatric consequences of child abuse and neglect. Dev Psychobiol (2010)
Abnormal ACTH and cortisol responses to ovine corticotropin releasing factor 52:671–90. doi:10.1002/dev.20494
in patients with primary affective disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol 335. Hart J, Gunnar M, Cicchetti D. Altered neuroendocrine activity in maltreated
Psychiatry (1986) 10:57–65. doi:10.1016/0278-5846(86)90044-8 children related to symptoms of depression. Dev Psychopathol (1996) 8:201–14.
315. Young EA, Haskett RF, Murphy-Weinberg V, Watson SJ, Akil H. Loss of glu- doi:10.1017/S0954579400007045
cocorticoid fast feedback in depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1991) 48:693–9. 336. Heim C, Nemeroff CB. The impact of early adverse experiences on brain sys-
doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810320017003 tems involved in the pathophysiology of anxiety and affective disorders. Biol
316. Arborelius L, Owens MJ, Plotsky PM, Nemeroff CB. The role of corticotropin- Psychiatry (1999) 46:1509–22. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00224-3
releasing factor in depression and anxiety disorders. J Endocrinol (1999) 337. Costello EJ, Pine DS, Hammen C, March JS, Plotsky PM, Weissman MM, et al.
160:1–12. doi:10.1677/joe.0.1600001 Development and natural history of mood disorders. Biol Psychiatry (2002)
317. Heim C, Newport DJ, Heit S, Graham YP, Wilcox M, Bonsall R, et al. Pituitary- 52:529–42. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01372-0
adrenal and autonomic responses to stress in women after sexual and physical 338. Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge dur-
abuse in childhood. JAMA (2000) 284:592–7. doi:10.1001/jama.284.5.592 ing adolescence? Nat Rev Neurosci (2008) 9:947–57. doi:10.1038/nrn2513
318. Heim C, Nemeroff CB. Neurobiology of early life stress: clinical studies. Semin 339. Sevelinges Y, Moriceau S, Holman P, Miner C, Muzny K, Gervais R, et al.
Clin Neuropsychiatry (2002) 7:147–59. doi:10.1053/scnp.2002.33127 Enduring effects of infant memories: infant odor-shock conditioning atten-
319. Heim C, Newport DJ, Mletzko T, Miller AH, Nemeroff CB. The link between uates amygdala activity and adult fear conditioning. Biol Psychiatry (2007)
childhood trauma and depression: insights from HPA axis studies in humans. 62:1070–9. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.025
Psychoneuroendocrinology (2008) 33:693–710. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008. 340. Sevelinges Y, Sullivan RM, Messaoudi B, Mouly AM. Neonatal odor-shock con-
03.008 ditioning alters the neural network involved in odor fear learning at adulthood.
320. Kraemer GW, Ebert MH, Schmidt DE, McKinney WT. A longitudinal study Learn Mem (2008) 15:649–56. doi:10.1101/lm.998508
of the effect of different social rearing conditions on cerebrospinal fluid nor- 341. Moriceau S, Raineki C, Holman JD, Holman JG, Sullivan RM. Enduring neu-
epinephrine and biogenic amine metabolites in rhesus monkeys. Neuropsy- robehavioral effects of early life trauma mediated through learning and corti-
chopharmacology (1989) 2:175–89. doi:10.1016/0893-133X(89)90021-3 costerone suppression. Front Behav Neurosci (2009) 3:22. doi:10.3389/neuro.
321. Rosenblum LA, Andrews MW. Influences of environmental demand on mater- 08.022.2009
nal behavior and infant development. Acta Paediatr Suppl (1994) 397:57–63. 342. Sevelinges Y, Mouly AM, Raineki C, Moriceau S, Forest C, Sullivan RM. Adult
doi:10.1111/j.1651-2227.1994.tb13266.x depression-like behavior, amygdala and olfactory cortex functions are restored
322. Coplan JD, Andrews MW, Rosenblum LA, Owens MJ, Friedman S, by odor previously paired with shock during infant’s sensitive period attach-
Gorman JM, et al. Persistent elevations of cerebrospinal fluid concentrations ment learning. Dev Cogn Neurosci (2011) 1:77–87. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2010.07.
of corticotropin-releasing factor in adult nonhuman primates exposed to 005
early-life stressors: implications for the pathophysiology of mood and anxi- 343. Haynes-Seman C. Developmental origins of moral masochism: a failure-to-
ety disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1996) 93:1619–23. doi:10.1073/pnas. thrive toddler’s interactions with mother. Child Abuse Negl (1987) 11:319–30.
93.4.1619 doi:10.1016/0145-2134(87)90005-6
344. Zeanah CH, Keyes A, Settles L. Attachment relationship experiences and child- Received: 15 December 2013; paper pending published: 08 February 2014; accepted: 05
hood psychopathology. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2003) 1008:22–30. doi:10.1196/ March 2014; published online: 21 March 2014.
annals.1301.003 Citation: Rincón-Cortés M and Sullivan RM (2014) Early life trauma and attachment:
345. Bos KJ, Fox N, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA III. Effects of early psychosocial depri- immediate and enduring effects on neurobehavioral and stress axis development. Front.
vation on the development of memory and executive function. Front Behav Endocrinol. 5:33. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00033
Neurosci (2009) 3:16. doi:10.3389/neuro.08.016.2009 This article was submitted to Neuroendocrine Science, a section of the journal Frontiers
346. Anand KJ, Coskun V, Thrivikraman KV, Nemeroff CB, Plotsky PM. Long-term in Endocrinology.
behavioral effects of repetitive pain in neonatal rat pups. Physiol Behav (1999) Copyright © 2014 Rincón-Cortés and Sullivan. This is an open-access article distributed
66:627–37. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00338-2 under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, dis-
tribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s)
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was conducted or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in
in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is
as a potential conflict of interest. permitted which does not comply with these terms.