Uas Ica
Uas Ica
Uas Ica
Introduction
There have also been debates over whether teachers’ moral professionalism
should be analyzed and developed with a focus on moral principles and their
application or whether a better approach would be to focus on practices of
‘good moral reasoning’ and pay attention to the intuitive processes of moral
judgment (Coombs, 1998Coombs, J.R. (1998). Educational ethics: Are we on
the right track?Educational Theory, 48, 555–569.10.1111/edth.1998.48.issue-
4[Crossref], [Google Scholar]). It is known that quick and intuitive processing
of social information in practical settings—such as the classroom—influences
the process of making moral judgments (Greene & Haidt, 2002Greene, J.,
& Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 6, 517–523.10.1016/S1364-6613(02)02011-
9[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]). These
intuitions are often culturally bound (Vauclair, Wilson, &
Fischer, 2014Vauclair, C.-M., Wilson, M., & Fischer, R. (2014). Cultural
conceptions of morality: Examining laypeople’s associations of moral
character. Jounal of Moral Education,43, 54–74.[Taylor & Francis
Online], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]) and have roots in personal life
histories (Van Manen, 1994Van Manen, M. (1994). Pedagogy, virtue, and
narrative identity in teaching. Curriculum Inquiry, 24, 135–
170.10.1080/03626784.1994.11076157[Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of
Science ®], [Google Scholar]); therefore, they also reflect the individual’s
belief systems. We agree with the notions of Sanger and Osguthorpe
(2005Sanger, M., & Osguthorpe, R.(2005). Making sense of moral
education. Journal of Moral Education, 34, 57–
71.10.1080/03057240500049323[Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science
®], [Google Scholar], 2011Sanger, M.N., & Osguthorpe, R.D.(2011). Teacher
education, preservice teacher beliefs, and the moral work of
teaching. Teaching and Teacher Education, 27, 569–
578.10.1016/j.tate.2010.10.011[Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google
Scholar]) on the centrality of teacher beliefs in the development of teachers’
moral professionalism—a view based on constructivist understanding of
teacher learning. Sanger and Osguthorpe (2011Sanger, M.N.,
& Osguthorpe, R.D.(2011). Teacher education, preservice teacher beliefs, and
the moral work of teaching. Teaching and Teacher Education, 27, 569–
578.10.1016/j.tate.2010.10.011[Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google
Scholar]) call for empirical research to identify teacher beliefs that are
particularly relevant for the moral matters of teaching. In this article we present
empirical evidence for the implications of a particular set of core beliefs which
have proved to be highly influential on learning, motivation, and well-being, but
have previously been discussed very little in the field of moral education,
namely, implicit beliefs concerning the malleability of human qualities.
Implicit theories, however, do not act alone; they are connected to a network
of other specifiable beliefs, which form structured meaning systems that direct
the way in which people understand themselves and others and give meaning
to their social experiences (Molden & Dweck, 2006Molden, D.C.,
& Dweck, C.S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in psychology. A lay theories
approach to self-regulation, social perception, and social
development. American Psychologist, 61, 192–203.10.1037/0003-
066X.61.3.192[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar];
Plaks, Levy, & Dweck, 2009Plaks, J.E., Levy, S.R., & Dweck, C.S.(2009). Lay
theories of personality: Cornerstones of meaning in social cognition. Social
and Personal Psychology Compass, 3, 1069–
1081.10.1111/spco.2009.3.issue-6[Crossref], [Google Scholar]). For example,
incremental theorists are found to interpret human behavior in terms of
context-sensitive psychological processes and situational factors, whereas
entity theorists have a tendency toward dispositional attribution and
emphasize deep-seated, cross-situational traits as the key causes of behavior
(Chiu, Hong, & Dweck, 1997Chiu, C., Hong, Y., & Dweck, C.S.(1997). Lay
dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 73, 19–30.10.1037/0022-
3514.73.1.19[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar];
Dweck, Chiu, & Hong 1995Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y.(1995). Implicit
theories and their role in judgements and reactions: A world from two
perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267–285.[Taylor & Francis
Online], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]; Molden &
Dweck, 2006Molden, D.C., & Dweck, C.S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in
psychology. A lay theories approach to self-regulation, social perception, and
social development. American Psychologist, 61, 192–203.10.1037/0003-
066X.61.3.192[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]).
These tendencies also have implications for moral ethos and conduct. Since
entity theorists tend to believe in a fixed reality with a rigid moral order, they
regard fulfilling duties and striving for justice as fundamental to morality. By
contrast, incremental theorists, who tend to believe in a malleable social moral
reality tend to focus on improving the wrongdoer rather than carrying out
punishments in order to achieve justice (Chiu, Dweck, Tong, &
Fu, 1997Chiu, C., Dweck, C., Tong, Y., & Fu, J.(1997). Implicit theories and
conceptions of morality. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 73, 923–940.10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.923[Crossref], [Web of
Science ®], [Google Scholar]; Plaks et al., 2009Plaks, J.E., Levy, S.R.,
& Dweck, C.S.(2009). Lay theories of personality: Cornerstones of meaning in
social cognition. Social and Personal Psychology Compass, 3, 1069–
1081.10.1111/spco.2009.3.issue-6[Crossref], [Google Scholar]). Thus, the
assumption can be made that, if a teacher is inclined to entity theory, his/her
endeavors to teach morally are channeled by these tendencies, which can
also be described as features of duty-based morality (see
Dworkin, 1978Dworkin, R. (1978). Taking rights seriously. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]) and by a professional moral
ethos primarily concerned with the concern for professional responsibilities
(see Gholami & Husu, 2010Gholami, K., & Husu, J. (2010). How do teachers
reason about their practice? Representing the epistemic nature of teachers’
practical knowledge. Teaching and Teacher Education, 26, 1520–
1529.10.1016/j.tate.2010.06.001[Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google
Scholar]). Likewise, the ethos of an incremental theorist teacher is presumably
inclined towards taking responsibility for students’ personal development,
meeting individual student needs and preserving individual rights—tendencies
related to a rights-based morality (Dworkin, 1978Dworkin, R. (1978). Taking
rights seriously. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]).
Furthermore, since we also know that implicit theories influence how teachers
interpret social information in the classroom as well as the pedagogical
choices they make and that they continuously become communicated to the
students (Rattan, Good, and Dweck, 2012Rattan, A., Good, C.,
& Dweck, C.S.(2012). “It’s ok—Not everyone can be good at math”:
Instructors with entity theory comfort (and demotivate) students. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 48, 731–737. [Google Scholar]; Rissanen,
Kuusisto, Hanhimäki, & Tirri, 2016Rissanen, I., Kuusisto, E., Hanhimäki, E.,
& Tirri, K. (2016). Teachers’ implicit meaning systems and their implications
for pedagogical thinking and practice: A case study from
Finland. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research.
doi:10.1080/00313831.2016.1258667[Taylor & Francis Online], [Google
Scholar]), we also assume that teachers’ implicit theories influence their
endeavors to teach morality. Subtle cues communicated to students can affect
the development of students’ implicit theories, which then affects the
development of their ethical capabilities. For example, different attribution
styles connected to implicit theories determine whether anger is directed
toward a person (entity theorists) or a situation (incremental theory); thus,
incremental theory fosters adaptive conflict resolution strategies, such as
forgiveness and voicing concerns and diminishes such things as desire for
revenge (Chiu et al., 1997Chiu, C., Dweck, C., Tong, Y.,
& Fu, J.(1997). Implicit theories and conceptions of morality. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 923–940.10.1037/0022-
3514.73.5.923[Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]; Kammrath &
Dweck, 2006Kammrath, L.K., & Dweck, C. (2006). Voicing conflict: Preferred
conflict strategies among incremental and entity theorists. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1497–
1508.10.1177/0146167206291476[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science
®], [Google Scholar]; Loeb & Dweck, 1994Loeb, L.,
& Dweck, C. (1994). Beliefs about human nature as predictors of reactions to
victimization. Sixth Annual Convention of the American Psychological
Society, Washington, DC. [Google Scholar]). Implicit theories also affect goal
choice in morally challenging situations; entity theory increases the tendency
for ego-defensive action choices, such as lying, and incremental theory
promotes honesty (Mueller & Dweck, 1998Mueller, C.M.,
& Dweck, C.S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children’s
motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 75, 33–52.10.1037/0022-
3514.75.1.33[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]).
Furthermore, since incremental theories are known to prevent helplessness
responses and promote persistence after academic failures, there are good
reasons to suspect that the same applies to ethical failures and that it is easier
for children with incremental beliefs about personality to remain engaged with
the moral domain even after ethical setbacks
(Yeager, 2008Yeager, D. (2008). Adolescent mindsets and the education of
ethical sensitivity. In K. Tirri (Ed.), Educating moral sensibilities in urban
schools (pp. 63–78). Rotterdam: Sense Publishers. [Google Scholar]). In
short, it is of utmost importance for teaching morality, whether or not the
teacher and the students all actually believe that ethical capabilities can be
changed.
Finnish context
In Finland, the ethical role of teachers has changed from being religious and
moral examples to being principled professionals with academic university
education, beginning from the 1970s. Decentralization of curricula and the
principles of ethics for teachers published in 1998 by the Trade Union of
Education have further strengthened the autonomous professional role of
teachers in Finland. Finnish teachers typically view themselves as responsible
professionals whose task is to teach the basic knowledge of their subject, but
they also consider themselves responsible for the holistic education of their
students, including the social and affective domains
(Tirri, 2014Tirri, K. (2014). The last 40 years in Finnish teacher
education. Journal of Education for Teaching, 23, 1–
10.10.1080/02607476.2014.956545[Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science
®], [Google Scholar]). Previous research on the role of the professional
teacher indicates that teachers need more education in the moral domain
(Hanhimäki & Tirri, 2009Hanhimäki, E., & Tirri, K. (2009). Education for
ethically sensitive teaching in critical incidents at school. Journal of Education
for Teaching, 35, 107–121.10.1080/02607470902770880[Taylor & Francis
Online], [Google Scholar]).
Participants
The participants in this case study were four Finnish subject teachers whose
students came from diverse socio-economic, cultural and academic
backgrounds. An important criterion for the selection of these particular
teachers was the opportunity for the researchers to learn from them
(Stake, 2000Stake, R.E. (2000). Case studies.
In N.K. Denzin & Y.S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative
research(pp. 134–164). Thousand Oaks, California: Sage
Publications. [Google Scholar]): the teachers were all experienced and eager
to share their ideas. They were also motivated in their work and had between
nine and 19 years of teaching experience.
These four teachers were chosen from a survey measuring Finnish teachers’
(N = 463) implicit theories using Carol Dweck’s mindset inventory
(Dweck, 2000Dweck, C.S. (2000). Self-theories: Their role in motivation,
personality and development. New York, NY: Psychology Press. [Google
Scholar], 2006Dweck, C.S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of
success. New York, NY: Random House Publishing Group. [Google Scholar];
Laine et al., 2016Laine, S., Kuusisto, E., & Tirri, K.(2016). Finnish teachers’
conceptions of giftedness. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 39, 151–
167.10.1177/0162353216640936[Crossref], [Web of Science ®], [Google
Scholar]). The sample item for the scale was ‘Your giftedness is something
very basic about you that you can’t change very much.’ The scale was
dichotomous; after reversing two scales, means close to 0 indicated an entity
view of giftedness, while 1 indicated an incremental view. The means of the
teachers were: Sally (M = 1), Kate (M = .75), Jack (M = .25) and Patsy
(M = .25).
The data include preliminary interviews with the teachers in which they were
asked questions related to their backgrounds, current job and professional
aims. We observed each teacher during approximately 15 lessons, of which
we videotaped 10 with Sally, 10 with Kate, six with Jack and nine with Patsy.
We endeavored to get as much from each teacher as was possible during the
research period, but had to respect individual teachers’ schedules and wishes:
this is why the number of videotapes and interviews conducted among the
teachers varies. There were also stimulated recall interviews (STR) following
the days on which we videotaped the lessons (five STR interviews with Sally
and three with each of the other teachers). The observations were also
verbally recorded by means of intensive field notes. STRs are directed to the
past: when interviewees view past actions with the video recordings used to
stimulate their memory, they are able to remember their past thoughts with
greater validity (Tochon, 2009Tochon, F.V. (2009). From video cases to video
pedagogy: A framework for video feedback and reflection in pedagogical
research praxis. In R. Goldman, R. Pea, B.Barron, & S.J. Derry (Eds.), Video
research in the learning sciences (pp. 53–56). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis
Group. [Google Scholar]). After every videotaped lesson, we watched the tape
and, with the help of written notes, identified critical incidents. According to
Flanagan (1953Flanagan, J. (1953). The critical incident
method. Pittsburgh, PA: American Institute for Research and University of
Pittsburgh. [Google Scholar]), this technique is essential when the aim is to
collect certain important facts related to well-defined situations: it has already
proved to be a valuable tool in studies analyzing ethical tensions in education
(Shapira-Lishchinsky, 2011Shapira-Lishchinsky, O. (2011). Teachers’ critical
incidents: Ethical dilemmas in teaching practice. Teaching and Teacher
Education, 27, 648–656.10.1016/j.tate.2010.11.003[Crossref], [Web of
Science ®], [Google Scholar]). In this study, the critical incidents were
moments in which teachers appeared to be interpreting their students’
behavior, learning, or achievement, and acting according to these
interpretations. In the STR interview, we watched the critical incidents with the
teachers (28 critical incidents with Sally, 52 with Kate, 22 with Jack and 21
with Patsy) and asked them about their thoughts and feelings during these
moments and the reasons for their actions. In addition, the teachers were
given an opportunity to reflect freely on the observed lessons. We identified a
greater number of critical incidents in the lessons of incremental theorist
teachers; this indicates they interpreted student behavior and reacted to it in a
more explicit manner.
Research design
This study was conducted at two Finnish comprehensive schools and one
upper secondary school during the spring of 2014. The study is part of a
mixed-methods project investigating teachers’ implicit beliefs concerning the
malleability of academic abilities. In the qualitative phase of the project, the
implications of teacher’s implicit theories for moral education became
apparent, and a sub-study focusing on them was planned. In the present sub-
study, our research questions were: What are the implications of teachers’
implicit theories for moral education? In particular, what are the implications
for teaching morally and teaching morality?
Results
In some respects, each of the teachers in our study exemplified the holistic
orientation typical of Finnish teachers (Tirri, 2014Tirri, K. (2014). The last
40 years in Finnish teacher education. Journal of Education for
Teaching, 23, 1–10.10.1080/02607476.2014.956545[Taylor & Francis
Online], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]) in that they believed that,
besides teaching the subject matter, it is also the teacher’s job to nurture the
students and help them face the future. However, their different implicit
theories seemed to be reflected in their professional moral ethos. Sally and
Kate, the teachers who exhibited a dominant incremental theory, took a great
deal of responsibility for their students’ personal development. In general, their
incremental interpretation of social-moral reality as dynamic and malleable
allowed for an orientation toward supporting changes (see Chiu, Dweck, Tong,
& Fu, 1997Chiu, C., Dweck, C., Tong, Y., & Fu, J.(1997). Implicit theories and
conceptions of morality. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 73, 923–940.10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.923[Crossref], [Web of
Science ®], [Google Scholar]). Their strong beliefs in their own ability to
promote the moral progress of their students and even the whole of society
seemed to be the most important motivators for their work:
While the actions and reflections of Sally, Kate and Jack in many ways proved
the relevance of the assumptions concerning the implications of implicit
theories for teachers’ moral ethos (Table1), Patsy seemed to have more a
unstable and ambiguous moral ethos. She had an entity view of giftedness,
but more belief in the malleability of personality and ethical capabilities. Patsy
was very much involved with her students and sometimes referred to herself
as a ‘social worker,’ yet her conscience constantly bothered her because she
believed she was not fulfilling her professional responsibilities, for instance,
not following the curriculum strictly or giving individual students more freedom
than allowed according to the school rules. Her own understanding of the
grounds for teaching morally seemed to be in conflict with the school ethos
wherein morality was apparently understood in terms of following norms and
regulations. The school culture repressed Patsy’s emerging incremental ideals
on morality and made her constantly feel unsure about her choices. The
research process and deep reflections during the STR interviews seemed to
make her conscious of the conflicts she experienced and actually led her to
seek a job at another school.
The teachers’ implicit theories, or in other words their beliefs in the possibility
of moral growth in their students, were communicated to the students in many
ways. When their students faced ethical failures—for example, when they
misbehaved or came to the lessons late, Kate and Sally often made them find
the reasons for their failures outside their personal qualities, thereby
illustrating an incremental view about morality. Furthermore, the most obvious
feature that Kate and Sally shared was persistence. In their view,
implementing punishments and striving for justice were not the only parts of
their job as moral educators: they tirelessly demanded change in their
students’ behavior and, by communicating their high expectations for the
development of ethical capabilities, they left no room for helpless behavior
patterns. In the classroom, Kate could be somewhat harsher than Sally, and
was also quite outspoken about her dissatisfaction with her students’ actions.
However, she thought the students’ actions could be strictly controlled and
judged, without actually judging the students themselves:
Kate talks to a student who has come in late to the lesson for the second time
in a row:
So is there a problem with your alarm clock? This is the second time you show
up late. If the reason is, I’m sorry but I assume the reason is that you’re on a
computer too late in the evening, so cut it out. I mean come on, it is the middle
of the day already. I woke up at six today and so have many others here, so I
will send a message to your parents about this; they have to know about this.
(Kate, critical incident 41)
In the STR interview Kate reflected on the situation:
Well, that was a bit mean of me, but I think it’s not that bad that I say these
things, because it’s not the result of your appearance, but what you do or don’t
do. That’s what I criticize. I don’t even know the families of these students,
like, your brother was terrible and I’ve taught your mother and she was terrible
too, always late. … I know people have heard these things, that you will never
achieve anything because your dad didn’t either. … I would never fall for that
kind of thinking. (Kate, STR interview)
Furthermore, the teachers’ incremental beliefs were communicated and
actualized in their rather determined ways of trusting the students. Trust and
forgiveness were central conflict resolution strategies, particularly for Sally.
According to Yeager (2008Yeager, D. (2008). Adolescent mindsets and the
education of ethical sensitivity. In K. Tirri (Ed.), Educating moral sensibilities in
urban schools (pp. 63–78). Rotterdam: Sense Publishers. [Google Scholar], p.
74), the incremental theory allows for an attributional style that construes the
causes of bullying as an interaction between personal qualities and the
situation. As soon as one can attribute causes to the situation, new worlds of
conflict resolution strategies become opened. Changing one’s own actions,
changing social systems, empathizing with the bully’s construal of the situation
and even forgiveness become plausible ways to fix the problem.
Sally talks about a girl who has been bullied in the school for years. The girl
started in Sally’s class together with other students who had been members of
the group that had bullied her. Sally talks about how she dealt with this
situation during a field trip.
Sally:
At the beginning of the fall we had a field trip, which made me very nervous,
because I did not know these students very well at the time. This girl stayed
close to me all the time, and I encouraged her to join the others, but she did
not have the courage. She started to cry and said that she was afraid they
would reject her. So, I asked what she would think if I went over and explained
this to the other students; she gave me her permission, and I told everyone
that she had been bullied harshly. Now she’s afraid to be around you, that you
will say something mean to her. They talked for a while about who else had
been bullied and what it feels like, and then they decided to go and ask the girl
to come with them, and told her they did not intend to bully her. The girls
asked her to play football with them, and since then it has been okay; the
group made her feel like she fits in.
Researcher:
It’s quite amazing that you have enough trust in these students that you
weren’t afraid they’d refuse to collaborate with you on this.
Sally:
Yes, I can trust them. … But I needed to be sure. I could not know that
beforehand, but when I saw their reaction I was so relieved; I realized that this
is the place where this girl’s situation will get better. … I know in this group
there are students who have been involved in bullying her, but since they
began in this new class and the atmosphere that we have, it [the bullying]
ended instantly. There have not even been mean looks or anything like that. I
would have seen them if there were—not a single one. (Sally, STR interview,
critical incident 11)
Instead of labeling people as bullies or punishing them, Sally resolved the
situation based on closely monitoring the students, creating a safe
atmosphere for everyone, trusting the students and believing in their ability to
change—an approach that proved to be effective. And her situational
attribution was also communicated to the students. Showing trust, avoiding
judging the students and avoiding labeling were also central ideals for Kate:
I need to try to understand and to be flexible. My principle is that I don’t always
expect the worst: if they tell me something, I try to believe it if there is no
evidence pointing in another direction. I know some colleagues always
assume that they [the students] are lying, but somehow I would like to see the
good in the students, because it easily becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy. But
at the same time I try to stay alert if there is something that seems suspicious.
(Kate, preliminary interview)
Thus, Kate’s and Sally’s practices were likely to support students’ situational
attributions, belief in the possibility of moral growth and adaptive courses of
action (see Table1). However, Jack’s low belief in students being able to
develop ethical capabilities was communicated to them in his low expectations
and his use of labeling language, e.g., referring to some students in the
classroom as “late arrivers.” His tendency of giving up on the students also
induced helpless responses:
So they are like … as long as they sometimes come to the lessons and do
something… it’s typical for them to arrive twenty minutes late, but I have only
given them their tasks then. And it’s like. … I can’t do anything about it; it’s
their own choice. (Jack, STR interview, critical incident 13)
Since entity theorists attribute moral violations to unchanging character traits,
they tend to consider judgment and punishment necessary to control behavior
(Chiu et al., 1997Chiu, C., Dweck, C., Tong, Y., & Fu, J.(1997). Implicit
theories and conceptions of morality. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 73, 923–940.10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.923[Crossref], [Web of
Science ®], [Google Scholar]). Implementing consistent consequences for
moral violations was important for Jack; however, Jack had labeled some
students ‘lost causes,’ and had almost completely given up trying to control
them. Even though Jack cared about his students and their personal
development, he considered it very unlikely that he as a teacher could
influence students’ moral behavior:
Let’s say … it is very difficult to aim at the personal development of the
students. Mostly, we try to keep them under some kind of control. At this
phase, in upper comprehensive school, it is not possible to teach manners
that much anymore; I cannot regard that as my responsibility anymore,
whether they wear hats in the classroom, spit on the streets, wait on traffic
lights, stay quiet in the classroom … even though in my lessons, of course, I
take care of that. But it’s like … their behavior, they have already learned it,
and you cannot change it anymore. (Jack, preliminary interview)
Jack’s understanding of students’ moral development here mostly relates to
the students’ ability to follow the rules and norms.
Patsy, on the other hand, did not give her students negative labels, but rather
tried to encourage them by praising their good traits and communicating
positive judgments of their personal qualities. She seemed to have decided to
see only the good in her students:
Patsy reflects on her discussion with a student, an immigrant boy who Patsy
has encouraged by saying that she could see him as a film director one day:
Patsy:
He is such an artistically talented boy, he shapes all these amazing things and
has such good spatial perception; he has made movies with his friend here …
Researcher:
So why do you think you wanted to tell the boy about this movie?
Patsy:
[starts crying] I’m sorry … oh, this is embarrassing … but he is such a smart
and talented boy.
Researcher:
And you wanted to encourage him by telling about that director? [whose name
resembles the boy’s name]
Patsy:
Researcher:
So what do you think, what should you do with this boy in school? What can
you do as a teacher?
Patsy:
The boy occasionally misbehaved at school, and Patsy was afraid that he
would lose his opportunities as a result; however, her way of dealing with the
situation by trying to encourage the student by praising his personal qualities
rather than demanding an improvement in his behavior reflects her entity
beliefs and could have unintended negative consequences for the boy’s
motivation for moral growth.
Discussion
However, while one of our entity theory teachers was oriented toward
controlling student behavior through punishment and strict assessment, and
he also commonly labeled the students, there was much more situational
variation in the practices of the other teacher, who held entity beliefs.
Moreover, her case allowed us to conclude that, while implicit theories and the
moral ethos related to them are often analyzed and discussed as individually-
held orientations, they can also be implemented in the wider community
culture. Furthermore, when an individual teacher’s implicit beliefs and
understanding of morality contradict what is cultivated in the school culture, a
teacher can become exhausted, frustrated and confused in her job.
In teacher education, this can be done, for example, by teaching about implicit
theories and familiarizing the students with the research literature
demonstrating the counter-intuitive effects of such things as praising personal
qualities and giving comforting feedback (Mueller &
Dweck, 1998Mueller, C.M., & Dweck, C.S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can
undermine children’s motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 75, 33–52.10.1037/0022-
3514.75.1.33[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar];
Rattan et al., 2012Rattan, A., Good, C., & Dweck, C.S.(2012). “It’s ok—Not
everyone can be good at math”: Instructors with entity theory comfort (and
demotivate) students. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 731–
737. [Google Scholar]). Making teacher beliefs and their implications explicit
entails giving teachers more control over what they are doing
(Sanger, 2008Sanger, M.N. (2008). What we need to prepare teachers for the
moral nature of their work. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 40, 169–
185.10.1080/00220270701670856[Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science
®], [Google Scholar] in Tirri et al., 2013Tirri, K., Toom, A.,
& Husu, J. (2013). The moral matters of teaching: A Finnish perspective.
In C.J. Graig, P.C. Meijer, & J. Broeckmans (Eds.), From teacher thinking to
teachers and teaching: The evolution of a research community (pp. 223–
239). Bingley: Emerald Publishing.10.1108/art[Crossref], [Google Scholar]).
However, we should aim further than just raising awareness. It is known that
entity beliefs are detrimental, and, according to current knowledge about the
malleability of the brain, such beliefs are not true; this is why they should be
changed. Interventions have successfully been used to promote incremental
beliefs in students. The main feature of such interventions has been to teach
students about the potential of the brain to change and reorganize when
people learn and practice new ways of thinking (Blackwell et
al., 2007Blackwell, L.S., Trzesniewski, K.H., & Dweck, C.S. (2007). Implicit
theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A
longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78, 246–
263.10.1111/cdev.2007.78.issue-1[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science
®], [Google Scholar]; Dweck, 2012Dweck, C.S. (2012). Mindsets and human
nature: Promoting change in the Middle East, the schoolyard, the racial divide,
and willpower. American Psychologist, 67, 614–
622.10.1037/a0029783[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®], [Google
Scholar]; Yeager & Dweck, 2012Yeager, D.S.,
& Dweck, C.S. (2012). Mindsets that promote resilience: When students
believe that personal characteristics can be developed. Educational
Psychologist,47, 302–314.10.1080/00461520.2012.722805[Taylor & Francis
Online], [Web of Science ®], [Google Scholar]; Yeager et
al., 2011Yeager, D., Trzesniewski, K.H., Tirri, K., Nokelainen, P.,
& Dweck, C.S.(2011). Adolescents’ implicit theories predict desire for
vengeance after remembered and hypothetical peer conflicts. Developmental
Psychology, 47, 1090–1107.10.1037/a0023769[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of
Science ®], [Google Scholar]). There are good reasons to consider developing
interventions with focuses other than academic learning and to teach about
the malleability of the personality and moral behavior. The effects of brief
interventions in students’ academic achievement have proven to be powerful
and relatively long lasting (Paunesku, 2013Paunesku, D. (2013). Scaled-up
social psychology: Intervening wisely and broadly in education (Doctoral
Dissertation). Stanford University. Retrieved
fromhttps://web.stanford.edu/~paunesku/paunesku_2013.pdf [Google
Scholar]). The next step is to develop interventions for teachers.
Disclosure statement
Notes on contributors
Inkeri Rissanen is a University Lecturer at the Faculty of Education in the
University of Tampere, with a specialization on multicultural education. Her
academic work is focused around religious education and Islamic religious
education, questions of Muslim inclusion in education, teacher’s intercultural
competences and sensitivities and teacher’s implicit meaning systems. She
has published on these topics in international educational journals and books.
References
1. Bebeau, M., Rest, J., & Narvaez, D. (1999). Beyond the promise: A perspective on
research in moral education. Educational Researcher, 28, 18–
26.10.3102/0013189X028004018
[Crossref]
[Google Scholar]
2. Blackwell, L.S., Trzesniewski, K.H., & Dweck, C.S. (2007). Implicit theories of
intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study
and an intervention. Child Development, 78, 246–263.10.1111/cdev.2007.78.issue-1
,
[Google Scholar]
3. Chiu, C., Dweck, C., Tong, Y., & Fu, J. (1997). Implicit theories and conceptions of
morality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 923–940.10.1037/0022-
3514.73.5.923
[Google Scholar]
4. Chiu, C., Hong, Y., & Dweck, C.S. (1997). Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of
personality.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 19–30.10.1037/0022-
3514.73.1.19
[Google Scholar]
5. Coombs, J.R. (1998). Educational ethics: Are we on the right track? Educational
Theory, 48, 555–569.10.1111/edth.1998.48.issue-4
[Crossref]
[Google Scholar]
[Google Scholar]
[Google Scholar]
8. Dweck, C.S. (2012). Mindsets and human nature: Promoting change in the Middle
East, the schoolyard, the racial divide, and willpower. American
Psychologist, 67, 614–622.10.1037/a0029783
[Google Scholar]
9. Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995). Implicit theories and their role in
judgements and reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psychological
Inquiry, 6, 267–285.
[Google Scholar]
10. Dweck, C.S., & Leggett, E.L. (1988). A social cognitive approach to motivation and
personality.Psychological Review, 95, 265–273.
[Google Scholar]
11. Dworkin, R. (1978). Taking rights seriously. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
[Google Scholar]
12. Fenstermacher, G.D., Osguthorpe, R.D., & Sanger, M.N. (2009). Teaching morally
and teaching morality. Teacher Education Quarterly, 36, 7–19.
[Google Scholar]
13. Flanagan, J. (1953). The critical incident method. Pittsburgh, PA: American Institute
for Research and University of Pittsburgh.
[Google Scholar]
14. Gholami, K., & Husu, J. (2010). How do teachers reason about their practice?
Representing the epistemic nature of teachers’ practical knowledge. Teaching and
Teacher Education, 26, 1520–1529.10.1016/j.tate.2010.06.001
[Google Scholar]
15. Greene, J., & Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 517–523.10.1016/S1364-6613(02)02011-9
[Google Scholar]
[Google Scholar]
17. Hanhimäki, E., & Tirri, K. (2009). Education for ethically sensitive teaching in critical
incidents at school. Journal of Education for Teaching, 35, 107–
121.10.1080/02607470902770880
[Google Scholar]
18. Jonsson, A.-C., Beach, D., Korp, H., & Erlandson, P. (2012). Teachers’ implicit
theories of intelligence: Influences from different disciplines and scientific
theories. European Journal of Teacher Education, 35, 387–400.
[Google Scholar]
19. Kammrath, L.K., & Dweck, C. (2006). Voicing conflict: Preferred conflict strategies
among incremental and entity theorists. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 32, 1497–1508.10.1177/0146167206291476
[Google Scholar]
20. Kärkkäinen, R. (2011). Doing better? Children’s and their parents’ and teachers’
perceptions of malleability of the child’s academic competences. (Doctoral
dissertation). University of Eastern Finland. Retrieved
from https://epublications.uef.fi/pub/urn_isbn_978-952-61-0420-1/urn_isbn_978-952-
61-0420-1.pdf
[Google Scholar]
21. Kärkkäinen, R., & Räty, H. (2010). Parents’ and teachers’ views of the child’s
academic potential.Educational Studies, 36, 229–232.10.1080/03055690903162424
[Google Scholar]
22. Laine, S., Kuusisto, E., & Tirri, K. (2016). Finnish teachers’ conceptions of
giftedness. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 39, 151–
167.10.1177/0162353216640936
[Google Scholar]
23. Loeb, L., & Dweck, C. (1994). Beliefs about human nature as predictors of reactions
to victimization.Sixth Annual Convention of the American Psychological
Society, Washington, DC.
[Google Scholar]
24. Molden, D.C., & Dweck, C.S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in psychology. A lay theories
approach to self-regulation, social perception, and social development. American
Psychologist, 61, 192–203.10.1037/0003-066X.61.3.192
[Google Scholar]
25. Molden, D.C., Plaks, J.E., & Dweck, C.S. (2006). “Meaningful” social inferences:
Effects of implicit theories on inferential processes. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 42, 738–752.10.1016/j.jesp.2005.11.005
[Crossref], [Web of Science ®]
[Google Scholar]
26. Mueller, C.M., & Dweck, C.S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine
children’s motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 75, 33–52.10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.33
[Google Scholar]
27. Narvaez, D. (2007). How cognitive and neurobiological sciences inform values
education for creatures like us. In D.N. Aspin & J.D. Chapman (Eds.), Values
education and lifelong learning (pp.127–146). Netherlands: Springer.10.1007/978-1-
4020-6184-4
[Crossref]
[Google Scholar]
[Google Scholar]
29. Paunesku, D. (2013). Scaled-up social psychology: Intervening wisely and broadly in
education(Doctoral Dissertation). Stanford University. Retrieved
fromhttps://web.stanford.edu/~paunesku/paunesku_2013.pdf
[Google Scholar]
30. Plaks, J.E., Levy, S.R., & Dweck, C.S. (2009). Lay theories of personality:
Cornerstones of meaning in social cognition. Social and Personal Psychology
Compass, 3, 1069–1081.10.1111/spco.2009.3.issue-6
[Crossref]
,
[Google Scholar]
31. Rattan, A., Good, C., & Dweck, C.S. (2012). “It’s ok—Not everyone can be good at
math”: Instructors with entity theory comfort (and demotivate) students. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 48, 731–737.
[Google Scholar]
32. Räty, H., Kasanen, K., Kiiskinen, J., Nykky, M., & Atjonen, P. (2004). Children’s’
notions of the malleability of their academic ability in the mother tongue and
Mathematics. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 48, 413–
426.10.1080/0031383042000245807
[Google Scholar]
[Google Scholar]
34. Rissanen, I., Kuusisto, E., Hanhimäki, E., & Tirri, K. (2016). Teachers’ implicit
meaning systems and their implications for pedagogical thinking and practice: A
case study from Finland.Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research.
doi:10.1080/00313831.2016.1258667
[Google Scholar]
35. Sahlberg, P. (2011). Finnish lessons: What can the world learn from educational
change in Finland?.New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
[Google Scholar]
36. Sanger, M.N. (2008). What we need to prepare teachers for the moral nature of their
work.Journal of Curriculum Studies, 40, 169–185.10.1080/00220270701670856
[Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®]
[Google Scholar]
37. Sanger, M., & Osguthorpe, R. (2005). Making sense of moral education. Journal of
Moral Education, 34, 57–71.10.1080/03057240500049323
[Google Scholar]
38. Sanger, M.N., & Osguthorpe, R.D. (2011). Teacher education, preservice teacher
beliefs, and the moral work of teaching. Teaching and Teacher Education, 27, 569–
578.10.1016/j.tate.2010.10.011
[Google Scholar]
[Google Scholar]
40. Sockett, H. (1993). The moral base for teacher professionalism. New
York, NY: Teachers College Press.
[Google Scholar]
41. Stake, R.E. (2000). Case studies. In N.K. Denzin & Y.S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of
qualitative research (pp. 134–164). Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications.
[Google Scholar]
42. Tirri, K. (2011). Holistic school pedagogy and values: Finnish teachers’ and students’
perspectives. International Journal of Educational Research, 50, 159–165.
[Google Scholar]
43. Tirri, K. (2014). The last 40 years in Finnish teacher education. Journal of Education
for Teaching,23, 1–10.10.1080/02607476.2014.956545
[Google Scholar]
44. Tirri, K., & Husu, J. (2006). Pedagogical values behind teachers’ reflection of school
ethos. In M.Klein (Ed.), New teaching and teacher issues (pp. 163–182). New
York, NY: Nova Science publishers.
[Google Scholar]
45. Tirri, K., Husu, J., & Kansanen, P. (1999). The epistemological stance between the
knower and the known. Teaching and Teacher Education, 15, 911–
922.10.1016/S0742-051X(99)00034-7
[Google Scholar]
46. Tirri, K., & Kuusisto, E. (2013). How Finland serves gifted and talented
pupils. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 36, 84–
96.10.1177/0162353212468066
[Crossref]
[Google Scholar]
47. Tirri, K., Toom, A., & Husu, J. (2013). The moral matters of teaching: A Finnish
perspective. In C.J.Graig, P.C. Meijer, & J. Broeckmans (Eds.), From teacher
thinking to teachers and teaching: The evolution of a research community (pp. 223–
239). Bingley: Emerald Publishing.10.1108/art
[Crossref]
,
[Google Scholar]
48. Tochon, F.V. (2009). From video cases to video pedagogy: A framework for video
feedback and reflection in pedagogical research praxis.
In R. Goldman, R. Pea, B. Barron, & S.J. Derry (Eds.),Video research in the learning
sciences (pp. 53–56). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis Group.
[Google Scholar]
49. Uljens, M., & Nyman, C. (2013). Educational leadership in Finland or building a
nation withBildung. In L. Moos (Ed.), Transnational influence on values and practices
in Nordic Educational Leadership: Is there a Nordic Model? (pp. 31–48). Studies in
Educational Leadership 19. Dordrecht:Springer. Doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-6226-8_3
[Crossref]
[Google Scholar]
[Google Scholar]
51. Vauclair, C.-M., Wilson, M., & Fischer, R. (2014). Cultural conceptions of morality:
Examining laypeople’s associations of moral character. Jounal of Moral
Education, 43, 54–74.
[Google Scholar]
52. Yeager, D. (2008). Adolescent mindsets and the education of ethical sensitivity.
In K. Tirri (Ed.),Educating moral sensibilities in urban schools (pp. 63–
78). Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.
[Google Scholar]