Evolutionary Theory, Culture and Psychiatric Diagnosis: Horacio Fabrega JR
Evolutionary Theory, Culture and Psychiatric Diagnosis: Horacio Fabrega JR
Evolutionary Theory, Culture and Psychiatric Diagnosis: Horacio Fabrega JR
Edited by Mario Maj, Wolfgang Gaebel, Juan Jose LoÂpez-Ibor and Norman Sartorius
Copyright # 2002, John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. ISBNs: 0±471±49681±2 (Hardback); 0±470±84647±X (Electronic)
CHAPTER
5
Evolutionary Theory, Culture and
Psychiatric Diagnosis
Horacio Fabrega Jr.
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
INTRODUCTION
Psychiatric Diagnosis and Classification. Edited by Mario Maj, Wolfgang Gaebel, Juan Jose LoÂpez-Ibor and
Norman Sartorius. # 2002 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
108 PSYCHIATRIC DIAGNOSIS AND CLASSIFICATION
care and treatment of mental illness. In some societies, the central govern-
ment played an influential role in spearheading treatment, education, and
research; in others, universities and faculties of medicine; and in still others,
local municipalities.
The evolution of actual knowledge of clinical psychiatry is largely a
product of developments during the nineteenth century. It involved two
empirically interconnected trends that can be separated only analytically.
One development culminated in the refinement of a system of concepts
about and terms referring to disturbances of human psychology and behav-
ior along with the criteria and principles pertaining to how this descriptive
system was to be used. The other one involved the creation of a science
about the many psychiatric disorders that came to be named and described,
disorders that were delineated by means of the descriptive system and
which came to be studied by means of the new science of medicine preva-
lent in the nineteenth century, involving diagnosis and explanation pertain-
ing to causes, lesions, and natural history. The former development
involved the evolution of a science of descriptive psychopathology and the
latter the scientific knowledge linked to the historiography of clinical psych-
iatry [4, 5].
In summary, it was in groups of physicians involved in the study and care
of mentally ill patients, in both private and public institutions, that the
modern approach to mental illness evolved. Different national conceptual-
izations about psychopathology evolved reflecting linguistic, cultural, and
societal experiences and traditions. However, to improve communication
and promote research, there arose a need for the discernment of commonal-
ities. A conviction grew that the various syndromes, disorders, and illnesses
that had emerged in national classification systems exhibited common fea-
tures and conformed to a smaller set of conditions that transcended national
boundaries and cultural experience. The members of this class of disorders
are assumed to be amenable to careful scientific definition and description
in a general language of psychopathology. The traditional view holds that
human populations show different vulnerabilities to disease linked to dif-
ferences in geography, social ecology, and culture, but it does not under-
mine the official position about universals in the pathology and clinical
presentation that underpin the international approach to psychiatric diag-
nosis and classification.
An emphasis on the development of psychiatric knowledge and mental
health services in European and Anglo-American societies is important to
emphasize, because it gained international eminence and now claims alle-
giance across the world. The imperialism and colonialism of the nineteenth
and twentieth centuries had many obvious political and economic reper-
cussions. One of them was the exportation of biomedical knowledge that
initially came to be applied to improve general public health. As the modern
110 PSYCHIATRIC DIAGNOSIS AND CLASSIFICATION
General Remarks
Evolutionary theory has been used in the study of disease and the general
medical care of patients [7]. A disorder has been defined as a harmful
dysfunction (HD); namely, a failure or breakdown of an internal mechanism
to perform its natural function [8]. Harmfulness is a condition that is painful
and/or detrimental to an individual's well-being and functioning. Harmful
conditions have many causes, being based on environmental happenings
that conflict with biological imperative; however, only dysfunctions of
natural mechanisms are applicable to the evolutionary argument of disorder.
The HD slant on a disorder is compelling. On the one hand, it has general
resonance: a ``natural function'' and a ``failure'' of it are, from a conceptual
standpoint, what persons ordinarily intuitively mean when they think of
disease or disorder involving something that has gone wrong or is not
working properly. On the other hand, it also has a seeming rigor. It rests
on the classical theory of categories (see below) and invokes a scientific
epistemology (i.e. a failure of a naturally designed function). While the HD
approach has general medical implications (e.g. diabetes, hypertension,
kidney failure), it has been systematically applied to psychopathology.
The psychological adaptations singled out by evolutionary psychologists
were naturally designed to solve recurring biological problems during
evolution and hence are examples of ``natural functions''. The HD analysis
holds that true or ``scientifically valid'' psychiatric disorders are based
on harmful dysfunctions of such adaptations. The HD analysis of a psychi-
atric disorder reinforces the link between psychiatry and general medicine
[9, 10].
An evolutionary conception of psychiatric disorder is an essentialist or
classical approach to the definition of a concept. The definition of HD
stipulates two individually necessary and jointly sufficient defining features
of ``psychiatric disorder''. The HD analysis has been the target of critical
112 PSYCHIATRIC DIAGNOSIS AND CLASSIFICATION
General Remarks
Culture theory argues for the influence of social symbols and their meanings
on a person's general psychology, outlook and tenor of life, including
psychopathology. There are two ways in which the ideas of culture and
cultural differences have been used in the study of psychiatric disorders.
First, as a marker for a group, like age, gender, social class, religion, or
ethnicity. This demographic view is then used to compare group differences in
psychopathology. A contrast is the psychological view that stresses deter-
minative and constitutive aspects of a person's sense of reality, personal
identity, and behavior.
The cultural theory position stipulates that culture cannot easily be separ-
ated out of the material basis of psychiatric disorders [13]. While disorders
are universal, conditioned if not produced by human biological evolution
(i.e. having a phylogeny), they also are cultural and hence variable. This
position holds human psychology is an essential locus of psychopathology.
The traditional history of psychiatry informs that conditions of interest
involve the ``psyche''. The latter includes cognition, emotion, and motiv-
ation, on the one hand, and social, symbolic behavior, on the other. Together
they constitute essential characteristics of psychopathology (as well as
psyche), however it may be formulated. Culture theory emphasizes and
complements the social mandate that gave rise to the discipline and profes-
sion of psychiatry and that underlies the efforts of all societies to cope with
mental illness. The second conception of culture reviewed above implies
that cultural psychology is a proper locus of psychopathology. It stresses
EVOLUTIONARY THEORY, CULTURE AND PSYCHIATRIC DIAGNOSIS 113
General Remarks
Disorders associated with the mood of depression are firmly placed in the
history of psychiatric nosology and systems of classification [17]. They have
a complex etiology, pathogenesis, set of manifestations, and natural history.
Many contemporary conditions (e.g. chronic anxiety, somatoform disorders,
fibromyalgia, irritable bowel) resemble or overlap with depression. The
medical authenticity of depression is beyond reproach: it enjoys a universal
prevalence in human societies and presence in medical traditions of the
world [1].
General Remarks
Few human conditions embody as much face validity for a form of social
maladaptation as do those marked by worry, fearfulness, psychic pain,
somatic experiences of autonomic hyperactivity, and associated social
EVOLUTIONARY THEORY, CULTURE AND PSYCHIATRIC DIAGNOSIS 117
avoidance. The distress, misery, and social disruption that anxiety can cause
extend beyond psychiatry to encompass religion and philosophy. Because
of its wide prevalence, anxiety has received attention from evolutionary
and cultural psychiatrists. The anxiety that seems concentrated in social
relations and interactions has evolutionary importance because of the hom-
inid trait of sociality and it has cultural relevance because in personal
experience and human activity one finds concentrated the meanings of any
culture.
Anxiety, like fear, pain, and fever, is a natural defensive response, one of the
body's protective mechanisms [7, 29, 30]. The process of natural selection in
the environment of evolutionary adaptedness (EEA) designed the regula-
tory mechanisms that underlie anxiety so as to enable individuals to avoid
threats and promote survival and reproduction. Anxiety, in other words, is
a ``good thing''. Whenever a threat or the likelihood of harm occurs, anxiety
can be expected to result and its degree will bear a relationship to the
magnitude of harm/threat. However, even if the cost is low, the defense
will be expressed in anxious behavior when the mechanism is operating
normally, much like a smoke detector may be triggered even in the absence
of a real fire. It is assumed that hominid ancestors existed in environments
that had a wide range of levels of danger that were recurrent. Genes that
shaped the anxiety response continued to be adaptive for a very long time
and have left a residue of low threshold for the generation of protective
responses to situations of potential harm and danger.
Many varieties of phobias have been the object of evolutionary analysis
and each one has been explained as the outcome of ``the smoke detector
principle'' in response to an evoking situation that had fitness implications
in EEA. In the case of social phobia, threats to reputation and status have
been singled out as important. Drawing on principles from ethology and
evolutionary biology, Stevens and Price [20] emphasize the importance of
contests and tournaments as a way of establishing social rank, something
individuals persist in striving to maintain or improve upon. Success in such
tournaments earns individuals a measure of value and power, termed
resource holding power (RHP). During evolution, hedonic as compared to
strictly agonistic modes of social interaction became increasingly important.
This involved competition not by intimidation but by attraction, with com-
petitors disarming rivals and attracting mates and also achieving status and
rank in the group. This gave rise to a new capacity for self-assessment,
termed social attention holding power (SAHP). According to Stevens and
Price [20], anxiety generally and social anxiety in particular is commonly
118 PSYCHIATRIC DIAGNOSIS AND CLASSIFICATION
General Remarks
that in the modern world science, secularism and rationalism have become
so integral to the idiom of contemporary societies and of medicine more
generally that these developments undermine monolithic cultural differ-
ences, homogenize world-views, and create internationalist cultures and
human psychologies that a science of descriptive psychopathology has
evolved to cope with [4, 5]. In this view of the matter, holding on to the
reality and importance of cultural variability becomes an impediment and
distraction. All of this would appear to demand a common language of
psychopathology and undermine the cultural conception.
Theoretical Perspective
Psychopathology is one of the social problems that societies cope with. Insti-
tutions for this are diverse and include social welfare, religion, medicine, and
the systems of social control that embrace ethics, morality and criminal
adjudication. Depending on context, any particular variety of psychopath-
ology can be interpreted as a condition of disadvantage requiring support
and assistance, a condition of wickedness and impiety requiring spiritual and
religious counseling, a type of sickness requiring medical treatment, a special
category of sickness as per psychiatry, or a moral transgression and offense
that needs control, correction and/or incarceration. Provided it takes
into consideration culture and language, a science of diagnosis seeks to
address universal characteristics. It allows determining exactly where in the
social spaces and institutions of any society conditions of psychopathology
are situated, keying in on essential characteristics. A culturally sensitive
science of diagnosis allows claims that some members of devotional sects
of ancient India or medieval Islam may have been victims of psychopath-
ology whereas many dissidents labeled as schizophrenic in the former Soviet
Union decades ago were not. Such claims are possible because the system
would handle specific disorders as tokens of types defined on the basis of a
theory or nosology that incorporates biology, neurobiology, language, and
culture.
Given the apparent trends in migration and immigration and the possible
future weakening of totalitarian/autocratic governmental controls as a
function of the spread of modern ideas of individualism and liberalism,
one would argue that human populations are likely to manifest greater
genetic mixing and assimilation in the long run. Since evolutionary biology
points to the innate bases for human psychology, it can safely be assumed
that a view about the universality or essentialism of psychopathology will
continue to be relevant. Furthermore, given modern developments in trans-
portation and communication, one may assume the continued spread of an
internationalist political economy and associated values of capitalism. In the
long run, this should lessen cultural boundaries and distinctions, contrib-
uting however slowly to the homogenization of human beliefs, values,
traditions, and outlooks. Barring major collisions among large and small
national powers, with consequent time-limited reactions of insularity and
isolationism, the pace of social and cultural change in the direction of a
common global culture can be expected to continue. Events in recent history
both support and challenge these generalizations [55, 56].
Prospects of future social change may be anticipated. The role of cul-
tural factors in critically influencing political economic developments in
130 PSYCHIATRIC DIAGNOSIS AND CLASSIFICATION
Western and non-Western societies has been emphasized [57]. Social crises
undermine traditional institutions of social control and legitimate structures
of authority, with consequent loosening of psychological controls and the
hold of traditional systems of morality and conscience [58±60]. Modern
societies show waxing and waning of the hold of traditional values, grud-
ging tolerance of social deviance seen in juxtaposition to racial hatreds
and divisive competition, openness to differences in lifestyle and religion
yet increased distrust, and suspicion; they resort to adversative modes of
conflict resolution, and a heightening of narcissism. Modernity tends to
increase interpersonal self-disclosure along with an awareness and open-
ness to cultural differences, sometimes including sexual experience and
behavior.
One can assume that such features of culture will not only continue to
influence the character of personal experience and social behavior, but will
also sharply influence interpersonal conflicts in circumscribed communities.
Migration and cultural pluralism will likely increase and this implies not
only a clash between ``old'' traditions and the ``new'' narcissism and inter-
nationalism of the culture of capitalism, differences between host and
parent country, but also clashes between competing traditions, values, and
sects in large urban ``melting pots''. In other words, individuals come to be
influenced by global, secular trends and migrate to foreign soils where they
then interact with other immigrant, minority populations.
Here it is important to keep in mind the distinction between the two
conceptions of culture mentioned earlier. While the demographic (demar-
cating) view of culture may diminish in importance because of the assimila-
tion of modernity, culture as lived reality shaped by diverse and even
competing tenets and feelings (and different in emphasis from that of
other citizens) will continue to be important. Even if one agrees that a
brain-based model of rationality and belief formation is an innate property
of Homo sapiens, ascertaining its workings necessarily enmeshes the diag-
nostician in a complex exegesis that requires knowledge of his/her and
client's language and culture. How items of information are labeled, con-
firmed, disconfirmed, and incorporated into meaningful social discourse
constitutes the essence of culture and language and of higher cortical func-
tions. Consequently, while cultural differences across societies may lessen in
importance, intra-societal differences between an individual in work and
institutional settings, including psychiatrist/patient dialogues, are likely to
increase in societies of the future. It is thus to be expected that symbols,
meanings, and world-views will continue to be influential in shaping per-
sonal experience and behavior, constituting aspects of social reality that
systems of psychiatric diagnosis should contend with in the future, if such
systems are to realistically incorporate important characteristics of the indi-
vidual.
EVOLUTIONARY THEORY, CULTURE AND PSYCHIATRIC DIAGNOSIS 131
REFERENCES
13. Fabrega H. Jr. (2000) Culture, spirituality and psychiatry. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry,
13: 525±530.
14. Donald M. (1991) Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of
Culture and Cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge.
15. Deacon D.W. (1997) The Symbolic Species. W.W. Norton, New York.
16. Hayden B. (1993) The cultural capacities of Neanderthals: a review and re-
evaluation. J. Human Evolution, 24: 113±146.
17. Jackson S.W. (1986) Melancholia and Depression: From Hippocratic Times to Modern
Times. Yale University Press, New Haven.
18. McGuire M., Troisi A., Raleigh M. (1997) Depression in evolutionary context.
In The Maladapted Mind: Classic Readings in Evolutionary Psychopathology (Ed.
S. Baron-Cohen) pp. 255±282. Psychology Press, East Sussex.
19. McGuire M., Troisi A. (1998) Darwinian Psychiatry. Oxford University Press,
New York.
20. Stevens A., Price J. (1996) Evolutionary Psychiatry: A New Beginning. Routledge,
New York.
21. Price J., Sloman L., Gardner R., Jr., Gilbert P., Rohde P. (1994). The social
competition hypothesis of depression. Br. J. Psychiatry, 164: 309±315.
22. Nesse R.M. (2000) Is depression an adaptation? Arch. Gen. Psychiatry, 57: 14±20.
23. Kirmayer L.J. (1984) Culture, affect and somatization. Transcult. Psychiatr. Res.
Rev., 23: 159±188.
24. Kleinman A. (1986) Social Origins of Distress and Disease: Depression, Neurasthe-
nia, and Pain in Modern China. Yale University Press, New Haven.
25. Lee S. (1999) Diagnosis postponed: Shenjing Shuairuo and the transformation of
psychiatry in post-Mao China. Cult. Med. Psychiatry, 23: 349±380.
26. Manson M., Shore J.H., Bloom J.D. (1985) The depressive experience in Ameri-
can Indian communities: a challenge for psychiatric theory and diagnosis. In
Culture and Depression: Studies in the Anthropology and Cross-Cultural Psychiatry of
Affect and Disorder (Eds A. Kleinman, B. Good), pp. 331±368. University of
California Press, Berkeley.
27. Kinzie J.D., Manson S.M., Vinh D.T., Tolan N.T., Anh B., Pho Y.N. (1982)
Validation of the Vietnamese Depression Scale. Am. J. Psychiatry, 139:
1276±1281.
28. Kleinman A., Good B. (1985) Culture and Depression: Studies in the Anthropology
and Cross-cultural Psychiatry of Affect and Disorder. University of California Press,
Berkeley.
29. Marks I.M. (1987) Fears, Phobias, and Rituals: Panic, Anxiety, and Their Disorders.
Oxford University Press, New York.
30. Nesse R. (1998) Emotional disorders in evolutionary perspective. Br. J. Med.
Psychol., 71: 397±415.
31. Kirmayer L.J. (1991) The place of culture in psychiatric nosology: Taijin Kyo-
fusho and DSM-III-R. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 179: 19±28.
32. White G.M. (1980) Conceptual universals in interpersonal language. Am.
Anthropol., 82: 759±781.
33. D'Andrade R.G. (1984) Cultural meaning systems. In Culture Theory: Essays on
Mind, Self and Emotion (Eds R.A. Shweder, R.A. Levine), pp. 89±108. Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge.
34. Murphy J.M. (1976) Psychiatric labeling in cross-cultural perspective. Science,
191: 1019±1028.
35. Werlinder H. (1978) Psychopathy: A History of Concepts. Almqvist and Wiksell
International, Uppsala.
134 PSYCHIATRIC DIAGNOSIS AND CLASSIFICATION
36. Kittrie N.N. (1971) The Right to Be Different. Johns Hopkins Press, Baltimore.
37. Fabrega H., Jr. (1989) The self and schizophrenia: a cultural perspective. Schi-
zophr. Bull., 15: 277±290.
38. Fabrega H., Jr. (1994) Personality disorders as medical entities: a cultural inter-
pretation. J. Personal. Disord., 8: 149±167.
39. Mealey L. (1995) The sociobiology of sociopathy: an integrated evolutionary
model. Behav. Brain Sci., 18: 523±599.
40. Chisholm J.S. (1999) Death, Hope and Sex: Steps toward an Evolutionary Ecology of
Mind and Morality. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
41. Cooke D.J. (1998) Cross cultural aspects of psychopathy. In Psychopathy: Theory,
Research and Implications for Society (Eds D.J. Cooke, A.E. Forth, R.D. Hare), pp.
13±45. Kluwer, Dordrecht.
42. Cooke D.J. (1996) Psychopathic personality in different cultures. J. Personal.
Disord., 10: 23±40.
43. Cooke D.J., Michie C. (1999) Psychopathy across culture: North America and
Scotland compared. J. Abnorm. Psychol., 108: 58±68.
44. Hare R.D. (1991) The Hare Psychopathy Check List, Revised. Multi Health Systems,
Toronto.
45. Cosmides L., Tooby J. (1999) Toward an evolutionary taxonomy of treatable
conditions. J. Abnorm. Psychol., 108: 453±464.
46. Kirmayer L.J., Young A. (1999) Culture and context in the evolutionary concept
of mental disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol., 108: 446±452.
47. Sadler J.Z. (1999) Horsefeathers: a commentary on ``Evolutionary versus
prototype analyses of the concept of disorder''. J. Abnorm. Psychol., 108:
433±437.
48. Gould S.J. (1991) Exaptation: a crucial tool for an evolutionary psychology. J. Soc.
Issues, 47: 43±65.
49. Lakoff G. (1987) Wonder, Fire and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about
the Mind. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
50. Lakoff G., Johnson M. (1980) Metaphors We Live By. University of Chicago Press,
Chicago.
51. Holland D., Quinn N. (1987) Cultural Models in Language and Thought. Cam-
bridge University Press, Cambridge.
52. Lucy J.A. (1992) Language Diversity and Thought. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
53. Ingold T. (Ed.) (1996) Key Debates in Anthropology. Routledge, London.
54. Fabrega H., Jr. (1994) International systems of diagnosis in psychiatry. J. Nerv.
Ment. Dis., 182: 256±263.
55. Mittelman J.H. (2000) The Globalization Syndrome. Princeton University Press,
Princeton.
56. Micklethwait J., Wooldridge A. (2000) A Future Perfect: The Challenge and Hidden
Promise of Globalization. Crown Publishers, Random House, New York.
57. Lal D. (1998) Unintended Consequences: The Impact of Factor Endowments, Culture,
and Politics on Long-Run Economic Performance. MIT Press, Cambridge.
58. Sanchez L. (1986) Social crises and psychopathy. In Antisocial Personality and
Related Syndromes (Eds W.H. Reid, D. Door, J.I. Walker, J.W. Bonner III),
pp. 78±97. Norton, New York.
59. Fromm E. (1941) Escape from Freedom. Avon Books, New York.
60. Lasch C. (1991) The Culture of Narcissism. Norton, New York.
61. Jablensky A. (1988) Methodological issues in psychiatric classification. Br.
J. Psychiatry, 152 (Suppl. 1): 15±20.
EVOLUTIONARY THEORY, CULTURE AND PSYCHIATRIC DIAGNOSIS 135
62. Kendell R.E. (1989) Clinical validity. Psychol. Med., 19: 45±55.
63. Millon T. (1991) Classification in psychopathology: rationale, alternatives and
standards. J. Abnorm. Psychol., 100: 245±261.
64. Fabrega H., Jr., Mezzich J., Ulrich R.F. (1989) Interpreting the structure of
diagnosis in intake settings. J. Psychiatr. Res., 23: 169±186.