Hypnosis, Memory, and Frontal Executive Functioning
Hypnosis, Memory, and Frontal Executive Functioning
00
and Experimental Hypnosis # The International Journal of Clinical
2004, Vol. 52, No. 1, pp. 3–26 and Experimental Hypnosis
ERIK Z. WOODY
University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ont., Canada
Manuscript submitted August 30, 2001; final revision received July 15, 2002.
1
This article is based on a doctoral dissertation by the first author, supervised by
Kenneth S. Bowers and the second author. The research was supported by grant from the
Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council and a fellowship from the National
Science and Engineering Research Council.
2
Address correspondence to Peter Farvolden, Ph.D., Clinical Research Division,
Section on Personality and Psychopathology, Center for Addiction and Mental Health,
250 College Street, Toronto, Ont., Canada M5T 1R8. E-mail: peter_farvolden@camh.net
3
4 PETER FARVOLDEN AND ERIK Z. WOODY
memory (Furneaux, 1946; Hammer, Evans, & Bartlett, 1963) can be best
accounted for by demand, context, and expectation effects (Kihlstrom,
1992; Kirsch, 1997; Spanos, 1986).
In contrast, according to the theory of dissociated control (Woody &
Bowers, 1994), hypnosis alters the actual underlying control of behavior
and not just attributions about control. That is, hypnosis results in a
relative weakening of the executive level of cognitive control respon-
sible for the initiation and monitoring of behavior. According to Woody
and Bowers, the unsuggested effects of hypnosis on memory bear a
strong resemblance to what Shallice (1988) and others (Shimamura,
1995) have termed frontal amnesia.
The present study was designed to be a strong test of the dissociated
control explanation of hypnotic amnesia. The notion that the unsuggest-
ed effects of hypnosis on memory resemble the symptoms of frontal
amnesia suggests that hypnotized participants should have difficulty
with the sorts of memory tasks that are used to distinguish frontal
amnesia from other amnesic syndromes (i.e., temporal-lobe problems).
Shimamura (1995) has described a variety of such memory tests. What
follows is a brief review of Shimamura’s battery of frontal memory
tasks and a brief summary of relevant research in hypnosis and
memory to date.
Free Recall
Patients with frontal lobe lesions show impaired free recall for lists of
unrelated words (Gershberg & Shimamura, 1991). Shimamura (1995)
has proposed that the impairment in free recall observed in frontal lobe
patients is due to the demands that such a task places on internally
generated memory strategies and effortful search and retrieval pro-
cesses (Baddeley, 1986). Interestingly enough, Kihlstrom (1980) re-
ported that hypnotized participants take significantly more trials to
learn a list of unrelated words than do control participants.
Proactive Interference
Patients with frontal lobe lesions have difficulty ignoring irrelevant
information (Milner, 1982; Perret, 1974). There are a number of memory
tasks designed to assess the impact of prior learning in the learning of new
information, and the term proactive interference is used to describe the
negative effects that prior learning can have on new learning. Shimamura
(1995) has reported that patients with frontal lobe damage show impair-
ment on tests of memory that require participants to inhibit previously
learned responses in an AB-AC paired-associate learning task.
There has only been one reported study of proactive-interference
effects following a hypnotic induction. Dillon and Spanos (1983)
administered a Brown-Peterson memory task designed to induce
proactive interference. They reported that an amnesia suggestion
HYPNOSIS, MEMORY, AND FRONTAL FUNCTION 5
Word Fluency
The word-fluency test is well established as a measure of left frontal
lobe functioning, in contrast to which the design-fluency task is a less
well-established measure of right frontal lobe functioning (Shimamura,
1995). Patients with left frontal lobe damage demonstrate marked
impairment on the word-fluency test (Janowsky, Shimamura,
Kritchevsky, & Squire, 1989). Gruzelier and Warren (1993) have re-
ported that hypnotized participants show a reduction in word genera-
tion to letter categories, results that are broadly consistent with the
hypothesis that hypnotized participants have a difficulty in word
finding that is similar to the anomia observed in patients with left
frontal damage.
Temporal Organization
Milner (1971) reported that frontal lobe patients exhibit deficits in the
temporal organization of memory. Using a word-sequencing task,
Shimamura, Janowsky, and Squire (1990) have also demonstrated
impaired memory for temporal order in patients with frontal lobe
lesions. Participants were presented with a list of 15 words, one at a
time, and then were asked to reconstruct the correct list order from a
random display of the stimulus words. Patients with frontal lobe
lesions exhibited significant impairment on this task as compared to
control participants (Shimamura et al., 1990).
It has been demonstrated, more often than not, that highly hypnotiz-
able participants are likely to recall in a more disorganized fashion
following suggestions for amnesia (Bertrand & Spanos, 1986;
Geiselman, Bjork, & Fishman, 1983; Kihlstrom & Wilson, 1984; Radtke
& Spanos, 1981; Radtke, Spanos, Malva, & Stam, 1986; Schwartz, 1980;
Spanos & Bodorik, 1977; Spanos & D’Eon, 1980; Spanos, Radtke-
Bodorik, & Stam, 1980). The evidence for temporal disorganization
in hypnosis in the absence of specific suggestions for amnesia is also
mixed. However, in contrast to Kihlstrom and Evans (1979), Radtke
et al. (1986) and Schwartz (1980) have reported that even prior to
receiving suggestions for amnesia, hypnotized participants are signifi-
cantly less sequential in their recall of their hypnotic experiences than
are nonhypnotized control participants. Indeed, Radtke et al. pointed
out that the effects of hypnosis on temporal organization might not be
due to suggested amnesia.
6 PETER FARVOLDEN AND ERIK Z. WOODY
Source Amnesia
Source amnesia occurs when one can remember factual information
but not when or where the information was either originally or last
encountered. Source-error effects are usually associated with impair-
ment in frontal lobe functioning (Moscovitch, 1994; Shimamura, 1995).
For example, Janowsky, Shimamura, and Squire (1989b) asked patients
with frontal lobe lesions and control participants to learn a set of 20
obscure trivia facts (e.g., the name of the dog on the Cracker Jacks box is
‘‘Bingo’’). After a 6- to 8-day retention interval, participants were tested
for recall of both the facts learned during the previous session (e.g.,
what is the name of the dog on the Cracker Jacks box?) and for 20 new
facts, as well as for their knowledge of the source of the information.
Janowsky et al. (1989b) reported that patients with frontal lobe lesions
were able to recall as many of the previously learned facts as normal
controls. However, Janowsky et al. (1989b) also reported that patients
with frontal lesions tended to make two kinds of source errors: (a)
errors in which they falsely reported that an ‘‘old’’ fact learned in the
first session was most recently encountered at some time prior to the
first session; and (b) errors in which they incorrectly reported that a
recently ‘‘new’’ fact was encountered during the first learning session.
According to these criteria, source memory was impaired in patients
with frontal lobe lesions, even though their recall of the ‘‘old’’ facts was
as good as that of control participants.
There are considerable data regarding source amnesia following
specific suggestions for amnesia (Cooper, 1966; Evans & Thorn, 1966;
Gheorgui, 1967). In these experiments, during hypnosis participants
are generally asked several questions, the answers to which they do not
usually know (e.g., an amethyst is a blue or purple gemstone. What
color does it turn when exposed to heat?). Participants are told the
correct answer to the questions, and then posthypnotic amnesia is
usually suggested in the standard way. After participants are tested for
their memory of the content of the hypnosis session (standard recall
amnesia), the same questions are asked again. Like frontal lobe pa-
tients, hypnotized participants often respond with the correct answer to
the question, even though they are unable to specify how they know the
answer (Evans, 1979; Evans & Thorn, 1966). Indeed, Evans (1979, 1988)
concluded that some spontaneous source amnesia is a genuine effect of
hypnosis and not simply an artifact of the demand characteristics of the
situation.
Metamemory and Cognitive Estimation
When people are asked to remember, they can be either more or less
sure that the information is available and/or accurate. At one extreme,
people with anomic deficits will often report that the information is ‘‘on
the tip of the tongue’’ even though they cannot access it (Shimamura,
HYPNOSIS, MEMORY, AND FRONTAL FUNCTION 7
Kihlstrom (1980) demonstrated that despite their amnesia for the target
words, amnesic participants were able to recall the words in the context
of the CIT.
In a similar study, participants were asked to rate their confidence
that each of the items (old and new) had appeared on their lists. Waking
control participants made a perfect discrimination between old and
new items. In contrast, hypnotized participants found it more difficult
to distinguish between old and new material, being less confident that
old material had been learned previously and more confident that they
had previously encountered the new material (Kihlstrom, 1985).
Finally, Dywan and Bowers (1983), as well as others (e.g., Orne, Soskis,
Dinges, & Orne, 1984; Sheehan, 1988), have demonstrated that using
hypnosis to ‘‘refresh’’ memory for previously learned material leads to
performance changes that suggest deficits in metamemory. These include
a more lax report criterion for reporting mental content as a memory
(Dywan, 1995) and high confidence about erroneous memories.
Participants
Participants at the University of Waterloo were selected on the basis
of being initially tested in a large group session, using the Harvard
Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A (Shor & Orne, 1962),
followed by a second assessment conducted in smaller groups of 2 to 10
people, using a group adaptation of the Waterloo-Stanford Group Scale
of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form C (WSGC; Bowers, 1998). Thirty
participants with high hypnotic ability were recruited from those
who scored nine or above and passed the amnesia item on both scales.
Thirty participants with low hypnotic ability were recruited from
HYPNOSIS, MEMORY, AND FRONTAL FUNCTION 9
Procedure
Participants were telephoned and either asked to participate in a
study of hypnosis and memory (hypnosis condition) or a study of
memory and individual differences (nonhypnotic-context condition).
Across two sessions, participants completed the battery in one of four
possible counterbalanced orders. However, all participants began and
ended the first session with the source amnesia task and began and
ended the second session with the metamemory task. In addition, the
order of tasks was arranged such that participants were not asked to
perform two memorization tasks in a row. Participants in the non-
hypnotic condition were not informed about the relevance of their
hypnotic ability until the end of the second session. Participants in
the hypnosis condition received a standard hypnotic induction at the
outset of each session, whereas participants in the nonhypnotic con-
dition were simply introduced to the source amnesia task as the first
test.
Tests
Free recall. The task was closely based on the Rey Auditory Verbal
Learning Test (Lezak, 1995). Participants were aurally presented with a
list of 15 unrelated words and asked to recall as many words as possible
following the presentation. Five successive study-test trials were pre-
sented, with the same 15 words presented in a different order for each
study trial. The list of words was read to each participant at the rate of
one word per second, followed by an oral test of free recall that
continued for thirty seconds. Participants’ responses were recorded
on audiotape. The measure of interest was the number of words
recalled on the fifth learning trial.
Proactive interference (AC1). The stimuli consisted of two lists of 12
paired associates (e.g., RIVER-POND, LION-HUNTER) (Shimamura,
Janowsky, & Squire, 1991). Across the two lists, the cue words were
the same (i.e., the first word in each pair), whereas the response words
(i.e., the second word in each pair) were different. Three study-test
trials of each paired-associate (AB) list were administered. Participants
were shown the word pairs one at a time and instructed to read the
word pairs out loud and to try to remember them as pairs so that they
could later report the second word when presented with the first.
After each study trial, participants were visually presented with the
cue words and asked to report the word associated with each cue
word.
10 PETER FARVOLDEN AND ERIK Z. WOODY
Following the three study-test trials for the first (AB) pairs of words,
a second set of three study-test trials (AC) was administered. Partici-
pants were explicitly informed that the second list involved the same
cue words but different test words. Otherwise, the instructions and
procedures for the second list were the same. Finally, in order to
determine if interference effects were due to problems in list discrimi-
nation, participants were given a final cued-recall test in which each cue
word was presented and both of the response words were requested, a
‘‘modified-modified free recall’’ (MMFR) test (Barnes & Underwood,
1959). Participants’ responses were audio taped throughout this task.
The measure of interest was the number of errors on the first test trial
for the second (AC) list. It is important to note that the AB1 and MMFR
trials do not discriminate frontal lobe patients from normal controls.
Patients with frontal lobe damage are characterized by normal perfor-
mance on the AB1 and MMFR trials and poor performance, as com-
pared to normals, only on the AC1 trials (Shimamura, 1995).
Word fluency. Participants were given 60 seconds per category to say
as many words as possible beginning with the letters F, A, and S (Lezak,
1995). Participants’ responses were audio taped, and performance was
measured using three indices: (1) total number of words produced for
the three letters; (2) number of errors, i.e., the number of times the
participant failed to follow instructions; and (3) number of persevera-
tive responses.
Word-sequencing task. Fifteen common one- or two-syllable words
were selected according to the same criteria as for the free-recall task
and printed individually on 4 6 inch index cards. The words were
presented visually at the rate of 3 seconds per word, and participants
were instructed to read each word aloud and try to remember the order
in which the words appeared. Immediately following the study phase,
participants were instructed to place the words in the same sequence in
which they had just been presented. The measure of interest was the
Spearman rank correlation between the actual study order and the
order in which the participant placed the cards during the organization
task (perfect score ¼ þ1.0).
Source amnesia. Thirty difficult general-information questions were
employed that were designed to tap participants’ knowledge about
obscure facts across a variety of topics including literature, movies and
music, geography, sports, and history (e.g., what is the name of the
town through which Lady Godiva supposedly made her famous ride?)
(Janowsky et al., 1989b). For each participant, one set of 15 facts was
presented in the study phase and the remaining 15 facts were used in
the test phase as foils. Ten easy factual questions were also included in
the test phase (e.g., what is the name of a dried grape?) to ensure that
HYPNOSIS, MEMORY, AND FRONTAL FUNCTION 11
RESULTS
Table 1
Results of Hypnotic Ability (High, Low) Context (Induction, No Induction)
ANOVAs of the Frontal Memory Tasks
Task Hypnotic
Ability F Error
High Low HA HA C df MS
Free recall
Trial 5 errorsy
M 13.18 13.79 5.36 1.84 55 0.95
SD 1.04 0.94
Perseverationsy
M 5.96 3.14 6.81 0.94 55 15.89
SD 4.60 3.17
Proactive interference
AB1 errors
M 1.12 0.98 1.15 1.13 56 0.38
SD 1.02 1.13
AC1 errorsy
M 2.35 1.17 12.57 1.84 56 1.60
SD 1.42 0.94
MMFR errors
M 0.72 0.41 2.72 1.13 55 0.49
SD 0.70 0.69
Word fluency
Perseverationsy
M 0.43 0.21 2.03 3.23 55 0.28
SD 0.63 0.42
Word sequencing
Spearman rank correlationy
M 0.81 0.68 9.52 0.49 56 0.03
SD 0.13 0.18
Source amnesia
Retrieval failures
M 2.33 1.46 0.82 1.25 54 1.26
SD 1.73 1.32
Source omission errorsy
M 2.33 1.46 4.23 0.03 54 2.40
SD 1.73 1.32
Total source errorsy
M 2.78 1.50 6.28 0.43 55 3.73
SD 2.25 1.55
(continued)
HYPNOSIS, MEMORY, AND FRONTAL FUNCTION 15
Table 1
(continued)
Task Hypnotic
Ability F Error
High Low HA HA C df MS
Metamemory task
Recall
M 3.38 3.63 0.17 0.56 56 0.15
SD 1.32 1.54
Recognition
M 1.00 1.11 0.14 0.42 56 0.18
SD 1.17 1.12
Total metamemory errorsy
M 3.48 2.28 4.18 1.46 56 5.01
SD 2.53 1.86
Note. HA ¼ hypnotic ability; C ¼ Context; p < .05, p < .01; MMFR ¼ modified-modi-
fied free recall; AC1 ¼ proactive interference trial; AB1 ¼ first study-test AB trial;
y
indicates a frontal task (all others are nonfrontal).
study order and the order in which the participant placed the stimulus
cards (Spearman rank correlation). However, somewhat surprisingly,
participants with high hypnotic ability performed better on this task
than did participants with low hypnotic ability.
On the source-amnesia task, participants with high hypnotic ability
made more total source errors, as compared to participants with low
hypnotic ability. This difference was mainly due to errors of omission.
Although there were no significant group differences on the number of
errors of commission, participants with high hypnotic ability tended to
make more errors of omission, as compared to participants with low
hypnotic ability. It is important to note that an analysis of the number of
simple retrieval failures, i.e., the number of the fifteen trivia facts
categorized at the outset of the session that the participant failed to
recall at the end of the session, revealed no significant effects.
The measure of interest for the cognitive-estimation task was the
total number of extreme responses (Shallice & Evans, 1978). A Hypnotic
Ability (high, low) Context (induction, no induction) ANOVA of
number of extreme responses failed to yield any significant main effects
or interaction (and hence this task is omitted from Table 1).
Finally, turning to the metamemory task, there were no group
differences for the number of words recalled during the recall phase
nor for the number of recognition failures. For the FOK task, partici-
pants were considered to have committed an error of omission if they
rated the confidence of their ability to recognize a word as ‘‘low’’ or a
‘‘pure guess’’ when they had, in fact, been presented with the sentence
16 PETER FARVOLDEN AND ERIK Z. WOODY
Table 2
Intercorrelations of Selected Frontal Memory Tasks (N ¼ 60)
Task 1 2 3 4 5 6
1 Proactive interference – .42 .30 .07 .56 .48
2 Free recall – .02 .27 .27 .30
3 Perseverations (on free recall) – .09 .14 .23
4 Word sequencing – .07 .24
5 Total source errors – .21
6 Metamemory errors –
p < .05, p < .01.
that best discriminated between people with high versus low hypnotic
ability, according to the results from the ANOVAs reported above. The
tasks appeared to be modestly intercorrelated in the expected direc-
tions, with performance on the proactive-interference task (AC1) most
clearly related to most of the other measures. The pattern of results was
essentially the same when performed on the data set trimmed at two
SDs for each measure.
Table 3
Classification Results of Stepwise Discriminant Analysis Used to Predict Hypnotic
Ability from Performance on Frontal Memory Tasks (N ¼ 60)
Highs Lows
Highs 30 20 10
(66.7%) (33.3%)
Lows 30 7 23
(23.3%) (76.77%)
Note. Overall percent of cases correctly classified: 71.67%.
18 PETER FARVOLDEN AND ERIK Z. WOODY
However, data from the word-sequencing task were not entered into
this analysis because the results obtained from that measure were so
contrary to what was predicted. Thus, the measures entered into the
discriminant function analysis included number of AC1 errors on the
proactive-interference task, number of words recalled on the fifth free-
recall trial, number of perseverations on the free-recall task, total
source-amnesia errors, and total metamemory errors. A subset of
two predictors, number of AC1 errors and number of target words
recalled on the fifth free-recall trial, correctly classifying 71.6% of all
participants. Of those high in hypnotic ability, 66.7% were correctly
classified, and the remaining 33.3% were incorrectly classified as lows.
Of those low in hypnotizability, 76.7% were correctly classified, and the
remaining 23.3% were incorrectly classified as highs. A corresponding
analysis of the data set trimmed at two SDs on each measure produced
similar results.
DISCUSSION
Kallio et al. than Aikins and Ray; however, neither of these other,
relatively small-sample studies involved the administration of a battery
of memory tasks.
The results of the present study are both broadly consistent with
what one would expect from a dissociated control perspective and also
somewhat inconsistent with it. It would appear that participants with
high hypnotic ability have difficulty with many of the same kinds of
tasks that frontal lobe patients are reported to have difficulty with,
indicating relatively weak executive control of memory. Such a finding
dovetails quite nicely with the dissociated control model of hypnotic
responding first proposed by Bowers (1992).
However, it was anticipated that the hypnotic context, including
provision of a hypnotic induction, would enhance any differences
between those of high versus low hypnotic ability, as was indeed
reported by Kallio et al. (2001); yet there was almost no evidence at
all in the present study to support this idea. The lack of context by
hypnotic ability interactions is surprising, given arguments and evi-
dence presented by Woody, Bowers, and Oakman (1992) about the
strong effect of a hypnotic context in many other studies. It also appears
to clash with a strong state position about the nature of hypnosis (cf.
Kirsch & Lynn, 1995). Experimental data reported by Ray (1997) and
broader, conceptual arguments by Kirsch (1997) likewise indicate that a
hypnotic induction may not be necessary for revealing differences due
to hypnotic susceptibility.
However, alternatively it seems possible that asking hypnotized
participants to complete a fairly extensive battery of demanding cog-
nitive tasks, such as the memory tasks used in this study, is simply
incompatible with maintaining a ‘‘state’’ of hypnosis. In order to
perform this battery of complex cognitive tasks, participants may have
‘‘alerted’’ themselves from hypnosis. In this view, participants with
high hypnotic ability who received an induction might be considered to
have been completing the tasks under essentially the same conditions
as participants who participated outside of the context of hypnosis. In
contrast, it may be possible that participants in previous studies who
completed only one memory task over a shorter duration were not
similarly alerted over time and may have been completing the tasks
while hypnotized. Such an explanation for state-related differences
across studies may fit with the concept of changes in attentional and
disattentional processes that occur during hypnosis, as proposed by
Crawford (1994).
Another possibility is that, rather than capturing what is essential to
describe a hypnotic state, altered frontal lobe functioning may be one of
the baseline individual characteristics of people with high hypnotic
ability that is necessary to attain this state. Finally, the absence of effects
for hypnotic induction and for interactions of hypnotic ability with
20 PETER FARVOLDEN AND ERIK Z. WOODY
hypnotic induction could possibly have been the result of omitting the
middle range of hypnotic ability. Specifically, although people with
high hypnotic ability may be comparably different from people with
low hypnotic ability in and outside of the hypnotic state, the perfor-
mance of moderately susceptible subjects may possibly be more af-
fected by an induction.
Also contrary to expectations was the finding that on the word-
sequencing task participants with high hypnotic ability performed
better than those with low hypnotic ability. Specifically, the average
correlation between the actual study order and the order in which the
participant placed the cards during the organization task was higher
for participants with high hypnotic ability as compared to participants
with low hypnotic ability. This result seems quite inconsistent with
what one would expect from the dissociated control perspective.
However, it seems at least possible that in an undergraduate popula-
tion, such a task is not difficult enough and/or measures something
quite different from what it measures in a population of patients with
frontal lobe damage.
In retrospect, when compared to the temporal recency judgment task
of Milner, Corsi, and Leonard (1991), in which participants are shown
a long series of stimuli, either words or pictures, and occasionally
asked to make a judgment about which of two stimuli was presented
more recently, the word-sequencing task used in this study may
simply not have been a difficult enough interference task to find the
subtle differences we were looking for. Using a task very similar to
that of Milner, Corsi, and Leonard, in which participants are shown
a long series of stimulus words and occasionally asked to make a
judgment about which of two stimuli was presented more recently,
Vongphrachanh (1998) has demonstrated that participants with high
hypnotic ability tend to perform more poorly than participants with
low hypnotic ability, especially when making judgments about items
at moderate temporal distance from each other, i.e., when asked to
judge the relative recency of two items which are neither extremely
close nor extremely far apart in presentation.
Alternatively, there may be something about the word-sequencing
task that makes it different from the other tasks, on which subjects with
low hypnotic ability consistently did slightly better. Performance on the
word-sequencing task involves the intentional encoding and recall of
the order of the material presented. In contrast, the temporal disorga-
nization of recall is usually observed in tasks involving free recall of the
material encoded. These two types of tasks are quite different, and the
differential effect observed in this study may reflect a distinction
between the ability of highs and lows to recall temporal information
(here higher in highs) versus their spontaneous use of temporal ordering
as a strategy to facilitate free recall (lower in highs).
HYPNOSIS, MEMORY, AND FRONTAL FUNCTION 21
REFERENCES
Aikens, D., & Ray, W.J. (2001). Frontal lobe contributions to hypnotic susceptibility: A
neuropsychological screening of executive functioning. International Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis, 49, 320–329.
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Barber, T.X. (1999). A comprehensive three-dimensional theory of hypnosis. In I. Kirsch,
A. Cardeña, & S. Amigo (Eds.), Clinical hypnosis and self-regulation: Cognitive-behavioral
perspectives (pp. 21–48). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Barnes, J.M., & Underwood, B.J. (1959). ‘‘Fate’’ of first-list associations in transfer theory.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 97–105.
Bertrand, L.D., & Spanos, N.P. (1986). Effects of instructing subjects to pretend forgetting
on disorganized recall. Psychological Reports, 58, 483–490.
Bowers, K.S. (1992). Imagination and dissociation in hypnotic responding. International
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 40, 253–275.
Bowers, K.S. (1998). Waterloo-Stanford Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form C:
Manual and response booklet. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Hypnosis, 46, 250–268.
Cooper, L.M. (1966). Spontaneous and suggested posthypnotic source amnesia.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 14, 180–193.
Crawford, H.J. (1994). Brain dynamics and hypnosis: Attentional and disattentional
processes. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 204–232.
Davidson, T.M., & Bowers, K.S. (1991). Selective hypnotic amnesia: Is it a successful
attempt to forget or an unsuccessful attempt to remember? Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 100, 133–143.
Dillon, R.F., & Spanos, N.P. (1983). Proactive interference and the functional ablation
hypothesis: More disconfirmatory data. International Journal of Clinical and Experi-
mental Hypnosis, 31, 47–56.
Dywan, J. (1995). The illusion of familiarity: An alternative to the report-criterion
account of hypnotic recall. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,
43, 194–211.
Dywan, J., & Bowers, K.S. (1983). The use of hypnosis to enhance recall. Science, 222,
184–185.
Evans, F.J. (1979). Hot amethysts, eleven fingers and the Orient Express. In G.D.
Burrows, D.R. Collison, & L. Dennerstein (Eds.), Hypnosis (pp. 47–53). Amsterdam:
Elsevier/North Holland.
Evans, F.J. (1988). Posthypnotic amnesia: Dissociation of content and context. In H.M.
Pettinati (Ed.), Hypnosis and memory (pp. 157–192). New York: Guilford.
Evans, F.J., & Thorn, W.A.F. (1966). Two types of posthypnotic amnesia: Recall amnesia and
source amnesia. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 14, 162–179.
Furneaux, W.D. (1946). The prediction of susceptibility to hypnosis. Journal of Personality,
14, 281–294.
Geiselman, R.E., Bjork, R.A., & Fishman, D.L. (1983). Disrupted retrieval in directed
forgetting: A link with posthypnotic amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 112, 58–72.
Gershberg, F.B., & Shimamura, A.P. (1991). The role of the frontal lobes in free recall:
Interference, organization, and serial position effects. Society for Neuroscience
Abstracts, 17, 136.
Gheorgui, V. (1967). Some peculiarities of posthypnotic source amnesia of information.
In L. Chertok (Ed.), Psychophysiological mechanisms of hypnosis (pp. 112–122).
New York: Springer.
Gruzelier, J., & Warren, K. (1993). Neuropsychological evidence of reductions on left
frontal tests with hypnosis. Psychological Medicine, 23, 93–101.
HYPNOSIS, MEMORY, AND FRONTAL FUNCTION 23
Hammer, A.J., Evans, F.J., & Bartlett, M. (1963). Factors in hypnosis and suggestibility.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 15–23.
Janowsky, J.S., Shimamura, A.P., Kritchevsky, M., & Squire, L.R. (1989). Cognitive
impairment following frontal lobe damage and its relevance to human amnesia.
Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 548–560.
Janowsky, J.S., Shimamura, A.P., & Squire, L.R. (1989a). Memory and metamemory:
Comparisons between patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic patients.
Psychobiology, 17, 3–11.
Janowsky, J.S., Shimamura, A.P., & Squire, L.R. (1989b). Source memory impairment in
patients with frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 27, 1043–1056.
Kallio, S., Revonsuo, A., Hämäläinen, H., Markela, J., & Gruzelier, J. (2001). Anterior
brain functions and hypnosis: A test of the frontal hypothesis. International Journal of
Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 49, 95–108.
Kihlstrom, J.F. (1980). Posthypnotic amnesia for recently learned material: Interactions
with ‘‘episodic’’ and ‘‘semantic’’ memory. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 227–251.
Kihlstrom, J.F. (1985). Posthypnotic amnesia and the dissociation of memory. In R.E.
Mayer (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 131–178). New York:
Academic Press.
Kihlstrom, J.F. (1992). Hypnosis: A sesquicentenial essay. International Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis, 40, 301–314.
Kihlstrom, J.F. (1997). Convergence in understanding hypnosis? Perhaps, but perhaps
not quite so fast. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 45, 324–332.
Kihlstrom, J.F., & Evans, F.J. (1979). Memory retrieval processes during posthypnotic
amnesia. In J.F. Kihlstrom & F.J. Evans (Eds.), Functional disorders of memory
(pp. 179–218). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Kihlstrom, J.F., & Schacter, D.L. (1995). Functional disorders of autobiographical
memory. In A.D. Baddeley, B.A. Wilson, & F.N. Watts (Eds.), Handbook of memory
disorders (pp. 337–364). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Kihlstrom, J.F., & Wilson, L. (1984). Temporal organization of recall during posthypnotic
amnesia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 200–208.
Kirsch, I. (1997). Suggestibility or hypnosis: What do our scales really measure?
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 45, 212–225.
Kirsch, I., & Council, J.R. (1992). Situational and personality correlates of hypnotic
responsiveness. In E. Fromm & M.R. Nash (Eds.), Contemporary hypnosis research
(pp. 267–290). New York: Guilford.
Kirsch, I., & Lynn, S.J. (1995). The altered state of hypnosis: Changes in the theoretical
landscape. American Psychologist, 50, 846–858.
Lezak, M.D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.
Metcalfe, J., & Shimamura, A.P. (Eds.). (1994). Metacognition: Knowing about knowing.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological
processes in man. British Medical Bulletin, 127, 272–277.
Milner, B. (1982). Some cognitive effects of frontal lobe lesions in man. In D.E. Broadbent
& L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), The neuropsychology of cognitive function (pp. 211–226).
London: The Royal Society.
Milner, B., Corsi, P., & Leonard, G. (1991). Frontal-lobe contribution to recency
judgments. Neuropsychology, 29, 601–618.
Moscovitch, M. (1994). Memory and working with memory: Evaluation of a component
process model and comparisons with other models. In D.L. Shacter & E. Tulving
(Eds.), Memory systems (pp. 269–310). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Orne, M.T., Soskis, D.A., Dinges, D.F., & Orne, E.C. (1984). Hypnotically induced
testimony. In G.L. Wells & E.F. Loftus (Eds.), Eyewitness testimony: Psychological
perspectives (pp. 438–452). Boston: Harvard University Press.
24 PETER FARVOLDEN AND ERIK Z. WOODY
Perret, E. (1974). The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses
in verbal categorical behaviour. Neuropsychologia, 12, 323–330.
Radtke, H.L., & Spanos, N.P. (1981). Temporal sequencing during posthypnotic amnesia:
A methodological critique. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 476–485.
Radtke, H.L., Spanos, N.P., Malva, C.L., & Stam, H.J. (1986). Temporal organization
and hypnotic amnesia using a modification of the Harvard Group Scale of
Hypnotic Susceptibility. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,
34, 44–54.
Ray, W.J. (1997). EEG concomitants of hypnotic susceptibility. International Journal of
Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 45, 301–313.
Schwartz, W. (1980). Hypnosis and episodic memory. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 28, 375–385.
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Shallice, T., & Evans, M.E. (1978). The involvement of the frontal lobes in cognitive
estimation. Cortex, 14, 294–303.
Sheehan, P.W. (1988). Memory distortion in hypnosis. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 36, 296–311.
Shimamura, A.P. (1995). Memory and frontal lobe function. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.),
The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 803–813). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Shimamura, A.P., Janowsky, J.S., & Squire, L.R. (1990). Memory for temporal order of
events in patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 28,
803–813.
Shimamura, A.P., Janowsky, J.S., & Squire, L.R. (1991). What is the role of frontal lobe
damage in amnesic disorders? In H.S. Levin, H.M. Eisenberg, & A.L. Benton (Eds.),
Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (pp. 173–195). New York: Oxford University
Press.
Shimamura, A.P., & Squire, L.R. (1986). Memory and metamemory: A study of the
feeling-of-knowing phenomenon in amnesic patients. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 12, 452–460.
Shor, R.E., & Orne, M.T. (1962). Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A.
Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Smith, M.L., & Milner, B. (1984). Differential effects of frontal lobe lesions on cognitive
estimation and spatial memory. Neuropsychologia, 22, 697–705.
Spanos, N.P. (1986). Hypnotic behavior: A social psychological interpretation of
amnesia, analgesia and ‘‘trance logic.’’ Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 449–467.
Spanos, N.P., & Bodorik, H.L. (1977). Suggested amnesia and disorganized recall
in hypnotic and task-motivated subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86,
295–305.
Spanos, N.P., & D’Eon, J.L. (1980). Hypnotic amnesia, disorganized recall and
inattention. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 744–750.
Spanos, N.P., Radtke-Bodorik, H.L., & Stam, H.L. (1980). Disorganized recall during
suggested amnesia: Fact not artifact. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 1–19.
Vongphrachanh, C. (1998). The relationship of hypnotizability, absorption, and schizotypy to
recency and recognition memory. Unpublished honors thesis, University of Waterloo,
Ontario, Canada.
Woody, E., & Sadler, P. (1998). On reintegrating dissociated theories: Comment on
Kirsch and Lynn. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 192–197.
Woody, E.Z., & Bowers, K.S. (1994). A frontal assault on dissociated control. In S.J. Lynn
& J.W. Rhue (Eds.), Dissociation: Clinical and theoretical perspectives (pp. 52–79).
New York: Guilford.
Woody, E.Z., Bowers, K.S., & Oakman, J.M. (1992). A conceptual analysis of hypnotic
responsiveness: Experience, individual differences, and context. In E. Fromm & M.R.
Nash (Eds.), Contemporary hypnosis research (pp. 3–33). New York: Guilford.
HYPNOSIS, MEMORY, AND FRONTAL FUNCTION 25