This document provides a summary and review of the book "Schizophrenia and the Need-Fear Dilemma" by D. L. Burnham, A. I. Gladstone and R. W. Gibson. The summary highlights that the book presents a thesis that schizophrenia results from deficient psychological differentiation and integration in early life, leading to a vulnerable personality structure with an inordinate need for and fear of others' influence. The book is based on a research study of relationships between chronic schizophrenic male patients and staff at a psychiatric hospital, using psychoanalytic theories of schizophrenia.
This document provides a summary and review of the book "Schizophrenia and the Need-Fear Dilemma" by D. L. Burnham, A. I. Gladstone and R. W. Gibson. The summary highlights that the book presents a thesis that schizophrenia results from deficient psychological differentiation and integration in early life, leading to a vulnerable personality structure with an inordinate need for and fear of others' influence. The book is based on a research study of relationships between chronic schizophrenic male patients and staff at a psychiatric hospital, using psychoanalytic theories of schizophrenia.
This document provides a summary and review of the book "Schizophrenia and the Need-Fear Dilemma" by D. L. Burnham, A. I. Gladstone and R. W. Gibson. The summary highlights that the book presents a thesis that schizophrenia results from deficient psychological differentiation and integration in early life, leading to a vulnerable personality structure with an inordinate need for and fear of others' influence. The book is based on a research study of relationships between chronic schizophrenic male patients and staff at a psychiatric hospital, using psychoanalytic theories of schizophrenia.
This document provides a summary and review of the book "Schizophrenia and the Need-Fear Dilemma" by D. L. Burnham, A. I. Gladstone and R. W. Gibson. The summary highlights that the book presents a thesis that schizophrenia results from deficient psychological differentiation and integration in early life, leading to a vulnerable personality structure with an inordinate need for and fear of others' influence. The book is based on a research study of relationships between chronic schizophrenic male patients and staff at a psychiatric hospital, using psychoanalytic theories of schizophrenia.
categories, e.g., self/nonseIf. It is apparent that Lidz and his co-
workers have made a giant effort and have persisted in following u p their observations despite changing clinical and research fads. Their contributions continue to bear fruit in rational treatment ap- proaches and increasingly sophisticated research methodologies, and the author has been successful in demonstrating, even to many “organically” oriented psychiatrists, the value of a psychodynamic approach to schizophrenia.
Johns. Kafka, M.D.
Washington, D.C.
SCHIZOPHRENIA AND THE NEED-FEAR DILEMMA. By D. L . Burnham,
A . I Gladstone and R . W.Gibson. New York: International Universities Press, 1969, xv 474 pp., $15.00. + Respectable as it has become again in recent years, the notion of schizophrenia is acceptable to most of us, clinicians and researchers in the field of mental illness, as something of a compromise. As Burn- ham, author of the theoretical chapters of this book points out, schizophrenia is but a conventional label, a diagnostic umbrella for “diverse and far-reaching” phenomena, difficult if at all possible to account for by means of unitary theory. Burnham and his collabora- tors actually prefer to speak of “the schizophrenic person” rather than of “schizophrenia,” making use of the more operational, Meyerian notion of a “schizophrenic reaction” or “disorganization,” which official psychiatry decided to do away with not too long ago.’ Burnham’s thesis is that a deficient psychological differentiation and integration in early life results in a vulnerable personality struc- ture, one that predisposes to a schizophrenic reaction. According to this thesis, the person who is predisposed to such a reaction suffers from an “inordinate need and fear of the influence of other persons’’ -a “dilemma” that not only creates a morbid predisposition, but also accounts for the specificity of the psychotic reaction. All this is in line with the traditional Freudian view of mental illness, the 1 I understand that thecommitteecurrentlyat work to revise APA’sDiagnosfic & SfafrjficalAfanunf ofAZenfal Disorders (DShl-11) is in favor of changing again the oyer-all label. this time from “Schizophrenia”to “Schizophrenic Disorders.”
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MICHIGAN on June 17, 2015
BOOK REVIEWS 715
“conflict model,” according to which schizophrenia is a defensive
reaction to anxiety, on the same continuum of pathology as the neuroses. The far-reaching assumption of such a view is that some schizo- phrenics may be born normal. As Burnham puts it, “We believe that among the various types of schizophrenia there is at least one group of persons whose inborn psychic apparatus is normal but whose dis- ordered early object relations interfered with normal development” (p. 15). In support of this contention, the authors point to the fact that psychological treatment can help schizophrenics become more or less normal. There is a “deficit,” but it is acquired and reversible. Burnham elaborates this thesis in the opening chapters of the book by using findings and arguments from a wide variety of sources: Sherrington, Coghill, Werner, Allport, Rapaport, Piaget, Wynne, Searles,. and many others. The emphasis, however, is on psycho- analytic-ego-psychologyand object-relations theory as developed by Erikson, Jacobson, and Mahler. Faulty differentiation between the self and the environment (mothering object in particular) leads to a blurring or loss of self/ nonself boundaries, body-image uncertainty, as well as general in- ner/outer uncertainty or confusion and hallucinations, (deriving from disintegration of the superego), difficulty in distinguishing one’s own thoughts from those of others, and selective interpretation of reality to fit one’s own emotions of the moment. Faulty integration, on the other hand, leads to schizophrenic ambivalence, splitting, dissociation-drive impulses come to dominate the entire psychic apparatus: affects become all engulfing, with uncontrollable floods of feeling; action loses its purposiveness, control, coordination, and continuity. Poor differentiation and integration make for an unreliable in- ternal structure and an excessive need for external guidance and control. Excessive dependence on objects make for a special vulner- ability to rejection or abandonment. The schizophrenic person per- ceives love objects as dangerous because he is so susceptible to their influence, which can literally dominate or appropriate his self as well as his ego and superego systems. The book itself is rather loosely organized around a research project that set out to study the kind of relationships schizophrenic persons are capable of developing, dyadic ones in particular, under the controlled conditions of a psychiatric hospital. For, as the authors put it, “[su~hrelationships]may become either the tenuous bridge to
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MICHIGAN on June 17, 2015
716 BOOK SECTION
hope, reorganization, and reintegration into shared reality or the
final disillusioning seal on a doom to irreversible despair, disorgan- ization and alienation.” What the authors describe in terms of interpersonal relations, however, is but a function of the treatment program and the theory that underlines it in the social system of a unique institution-not the relationships that a schizophrenic person can develop in his natural habitat. The treatment program was there, and the theory behind it was more or less there, before the subjects of the study came under scrutiny; and both the treatment and its philosophy were largely constructed in terms of a long experience with hospitalized schizo- phrenics-the way subjects diagnosed as schizophrenic are expected to behave toward those who try to help them in a hospital-profes- sional people who take over when close relatives and friends back home concede failure. The theory of the hospital in which the study took place was basically psychoanalytic, Harry Stack Sullivan’s inter- actional, field-dependent version; the treatment method, Dexter Bullard’s version of the psychoanalytic hospital that Ernst Simmel developed in Tegel-Berlin during the late 1920’s and the Menninger brothers adapted in Topeka in the mid-1930’s. The decision to undertake the study was inspired by Stanton and Schwartz’szclassic study of the social structure of the same hospital, which scrutinized staff interpersonal relations and showed how im- portant, often adverse, their influence is on patients. The present study concentrated on the relations of patients with staff, as these un- folded in a treatment program prescribed without concern for the research as such, within the confines of a small locked unit. The researchers managed to exclude borderline patients and,, in contrast to other major studies of treatment with schizophrenics, made sure that their population was likely to remain hospitalized for a long period of time. In fact, they chose to study only chronic schizophren- ic patients, whose illness is not likely to respond to the usual ap- proaches of pharmacological treatment, outpatient psychotherapy, or brief hospitalization. Such patients usually reach hospitals like Chestnut Lodge following gradual deterioration or a crisis in.another hospital. An important characteristic of such patients is their family structure, not only as a major factor in the development of their illness, but in contributing to the breakdown of all treatment pro- grams. * Stanton, A. H. & Schwartz, M.S. (1954), The MentalHospita1.-A Study of Institutional Partic2’pation in Psychiatric Illness and Treatment. New York: Basic Books.
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MICHIGAN on June 17, 2015
BOOK REVIEWS 717
At least one of theresearchers (Gibson) both participated in and
observed the patients’ treatment. About a dozen patients at any particular time were studied over a period of five years and by methods that, because of the evolvingnature of the study, varied over the course of these years. They came from socially affluent families, ranged from late adolescence to middle age, and were overtly disturbed long before admission. They were all men, presumably because the subjects of Stanton and Schwartz’s study were women. A wide array of patient relationships, representing various aspects of the treatment and living circumstances on the unit, were studied-with psychotherapist and unit administrator (a psychi- atrist), social worker, chief nurse and several student nurses (all women), nursing aides, activities therapists, housekeeping staff, and fellow patients. For practical reasons, however, only certain relation- ships were observed systematically and in detail, and these did not include relations with other patients. All in all, each patient was studied on the basis of fifteen to eighteen relationships with different staff members at various periods of his career as a patient on the unit. Besides the psychotherapist (who often saw his patient in his own office out of the unit, usually four hours a week) and the unit administrator, the most available staff members were the psychi- atric aides (all men). A major methodological problem was to define the “experi- mental” variable “relationship” in the context of the schizophrenic illness. A related issue was the need for a standard way to collect in- formation concerning one patient’s relationship with those of an- other, or with his own at another time. The wish to obtain such private information about as large a number of diverse relationships as possible and the scarcity of research assistants made the investiga- tors decide to rely mainly on reports from participants in these relationships-regular hospital personnel and patients-rather than upon direct observations. The data indicate some consistent aspects of patient-staff inter- action, specific for the conditions prevailing on the “experimental” ward. T h e most typical cluster of patient-staff behaviors reflected a tendency in the patients to approach nursing personnel in a sociable way and a reciprocal tendency on the part of the nursing staff. An- other consistent pattern of interaction was €or the patients to engage in objectionable behavior- “problem behavior”-and for the staff to get annoyed or angered and try to control or change it. “Problem behavior”brought to the foreground the issue of dominance and sub- mission. Both kinds of behavior resulted in increasing patient-staff
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MICHIGAN on June 17, 2015
718 BOOK SECTION
contact. The kind of interaction that developed between patient and
therapist -a relationship that, in contrast to patient-nursing staff ‘interaction, was literally imposed upon the patient-was similar to that described between patients and other staff members: “conven- tional sociability” and “problem behavior” were the most important determinants, patient relationships with therapists being more varied and differentiated. These findings are in no way surprising and, indeed, add little to what we know about patient-staff relationships or to what we might expect under the circumstances. A series of extensive case reports illustrate various aspects of patient-staff relationships in terms of the “need-fear dilemma,” describing the patients’ attempts to restructure reality in search of constancy and security ties with the object world by means of clinging, avoidance, object redefinition, splitting, displacement, etc. These case reports afford an opportunity to revisit pathogenic factors in the family and the environment in general -contradictions and in- consistencies in the early child-parent relationship that prevent a satisfactory self-object differentiation, sex and role identification- as well as typical circumstances that lead to schizophrenic disorgan- ization. The treatment task was to break the cycle of desperate search for objects and retreat to autistic relationships by attempting to contain and integrate the patient’s need and fear of objects within actual relationships in shared reality. The goal was to achieve genuine object constancy. Progress was assessed in terms of the patient’s ability to acknowl- edge awareness of the inconsistency of his feelings, especially the awareness that what he experiences as badness is within himself rather than in the outside world. Such awareness is presumably helpful in containing the patient’s “need-fear dilemma,” in particu- lar, the need to avoid or destroy the “bad” outside world. As he is able to engage in self-criticism, he tends to make less use of projec- tion. But improvement in terms of increased self-awareness and objectivity is obviously hard to take and does not lead to a quick recovery. A patient may know that he is not well, yet to know does not make him well. In the case of most patients discussed in the book, help went so far as to enable them to tolerate self-criticism and admit their craziness: but, possibly because of a failure to internalize the objects that helped them become less anxious and defensive, they remained vulnerable to psychotic disorganization: once they lost the supportive objects, their psychotic defenses returned and they be- came as sick as ever.
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MICHIGAN on June 17, 2015
BOOK REVIEWS 719
Treatment responsibility was shared by the psychotherapist and
a clinical administrator. The administrator’s function was the man- agement of the patient’s daily schedule, physical health, and freedom of movement; in this, he functioned also as an agent of social control, protecting society from the patient’s potential destructiveness. He exercised his functions mostly indirectly, through a team of nurses and aides. But he delegated little in terms of decisions to anyone, including the nursing personnel and activities therapist. The social worker’s contribution to treatment was presumably to negotiate visits with the family. Apparently, there was little work for a psychologist in the team, either: in fact there is no mention of a psychologist at all. The administrator’s efforts aimed at lessening the disruptive effect of anxiety so as to facilitate relationships and maintain com- munication, and at controlling behavior when it reached dangerous proportions. It also sought to provide opportunities and chal- lenges for ego growth, eventually fostering initiative, responsi- bility, and independence. The long-range goal of treatment was to modify the patient’s ego deficit, yet the definitive work in this direc- tion was to be done by the psychotherapist “through the transference relationship, genetic interpretations, and working through.’’ Indeed, the clinical administrator’s primary goal was to provide a propitious setting, congenial to the development of constructive relationships, above all the relationship with the psychotherapist. Gibson, the clinical administrator in the study, provides a clear and detailed description of the techniques he used in trying to maintain an optimal setting for the psychoanalytic treatment of his schizophrenic patients. Some of these techniques had to do with the patient’s defensive-regressivepatterns of behavior in response to the reactivation or intensification of the “need-fear dilemma” brought about by the therapist. The clinical administrator tried to mobilize the patient’s human environment, mainly nurses and aides, and to counteract the patient’s defensive responses, identified over and again as object avoidance, object clinging, and object redefinition. At thesame time, the clinical administrator tried to educate the staff to the meaning of the patient’s behavior-an essential task if the staff were not to behave in a rigid, thoughtless manner like guarding the rules and keeping order with disorganized, assaultive patients, or as servants of infantile needs, grotesque mother substitutes. Individual supervision, group discussion, even group therapy, were used in order to help the staff deal with the intense anxiety that a schizophrenic patient’s needs tend to arouse. An even more difficult problem was to help a patient give up a relationship after a certain point as patient-staff relations became
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MICHIGAN on June 17, 2015
720 BOOK SECTION
symbiotic, reconstructions of infantile, mother-child relationships -
a necessary development in the patient’s course of treatment, but dif- ficult for both patient and staff to give up. Excerpts from therapists’ notes, however, afford a suggestive glimpse of the method, which, psychoanalytic as it was in principle, varied considerably from psychotherapist to psychotherapist. Ap- parently determined by the therapist’s temperament rather than the patient’s actual condition, it ranged from free association on the couch even when the patient had to be forcibly held to it, to a face-to- face confrontation that allowed for bodily contact and an occasional exchange of blows. Working in close cooperation with the clinical administrator, psychotherapists had the benefit of the supervision with someone outside of the treatment team. Schizophrenia and the Need-Fear Dilemma offers a panoramic view of the hospital career of a schizophrenic person. But this is no ordinaryhospital career. At the time this particular study was taking place, Chestnut Lodgestill counted among its staff members some of Sullivan’s most creative students, notably Otto Will and Harold Searles. Frieda Fromm-Reichmann was still alive and, according to the authors, contributed actively to their work. This was a hospital where members of the staff took pride in developing an intense personal involvement with their patients, and where such involve- ment was, from the very start, expected to last for a long time. All of which may make one wonder. This volume is already seven years old, and the events it describes took place at least as many years earlier. Are itscontents dated? The stated problem-object relations in schizophrenia-is certainly not. But what about the theory and the treatment that its authors advocatei The psychoanalytic hospital was created with the understanding that the patient would need to remain in treatment and specifically within the hospital setting for a prolonged time. Such a condition was easier to defend-if not to afford-in the days when antipsychotic drugs were not yet available and custodial hospitalization was the rule in most other places. It was easier to point out that in psychiatry, in contrast with most other medical specialties, one “thinks in long-time terms; treatment extends over a long period, prognosis considers a long interval; the patient’s whole life is under consideration rather than an acute episode or relatively brief illne~s.’’~ Things did not change so quickly in those days, there were no third-party payers, no
3 hlenninger, K. A. (1936), Psychiatry and Medicine. Bull. Menninger Clinic,
1:l-9.
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MICHIGAN on June 17, 2015
BOOK REVIEWS 721
patients’ lib movement, few if any government regulations. Times
were different for psychiatry, for psychoanalysis and psychoanalytic hospitals-life, in general, was different then, not better or easier, but simpler it seems. Indeed, evidence from recent controlled studies4 suggests that the hospital career of schizophrenic patients is more successful if based on psychotropic drugs rather than hospital-based psycho- therapy. Yet, in going over such studies, a thoughtful reader may ask: What hospital and what psychotherapy? For that matter, what success and what patients are we talking about? The question is not to defend the psychoanalytic hospital and psychoanalysisas efficient forms of treatment for schizophrenia, for it is obvious that neither is more efficient than drugs in returning chronic schizophrenic patients to the community. The present book makes no claim to efficiency. The point is that there may be something very worthwhile and hopeful in efforts such as the one this book describes, beyond the mere pursuit of social expediency. In fact, when it comes to results of hospital treatment based on psychoana- lytic theory and method, this volume may suggest a more pessimistic picture than is really warranted, paying, as it does, exclusive atten- tion to very sick patients for the ostensible purpose of studying their object relations, which are then consistently interpreted in terms of a theory that seems to owe more to an encyclopedic grasp of the literature than to the research data at hand. As for the validity of the theory itself, I will let one of the authors have the last word: “. . . there is clearly a distance to go before we arrive at a n adequate and generally accepted theory of schizophren- ia, or more accurately, of the schizophrenias,” said Burnham5 in summarizing the proceedings of a panel on the subject during a recent meeting of the American Psychoanalytic Association. “As theory-builders, we are like the would-be builders of the Tower of Babel; we suffer a confusion of tongues” (p. 198).
Peter Hartocollis, M.D., Ph.D.
C. F. Menninger Memorial Hospital
4 May, P. R. A. (1968), Treatment of Schizophrenia. New York: Science
House. Also: Grinspoon, L., Ewalt, J. R. & Shader, R. I. (1972). Schizophrenia: Pharmacology and Psychotherapy. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins. The Influence of Theoretical Model of Schizophrenia on Treatment Practice, J. G. Gunderson, reporter. Th&Joumal(1974), 22:182-199.
Downloaded from apa.sagepub.com at UNIV OF MICHIGAN on June 17, 2015