Client Counselling Procedure
Client Counselling Procedure
Client Counselling Procedure
LAWYERS,
what they do and their competence, have become
subjects of great interest both among lawyers and the community
at large, in this country and in the United States.’
The general interest has been fired by consumerism and
dissatisfaction with lawyers’ work and lawyers’ charges in the
United Kingdom and by the “unethical and illegal practices and
actions” of lawyers in government service during the “Watergate”
episode in the United States’ coupled with reaction there to Chief
Justice Warren E. Burger’s 1973 Sonnett Memorial L e ~ t u r e . ~
Sample
Some 20 firms of solicitors were approached, and nine of those
firms permitted articled clerks to participate in this study. The
sample generated for this study was thus based on the co-operation
of a few firms and clerks newly appointed to those firms. The fact
that the research involved a training scheme for these new law ers
may well have acted as a major incentive to their involvement. 1 7
The question of sample” is probably the most difficult one to be
faced by any researchers into the ways lawyers operate. As Danet,
l1 Danet, Hoffman and Kermish, “Obstacles to the Study of Lawyer-Client Interaction:
the Biography of a Failure” (1979) 14 Law and SOC.Rev. 923 (herinafter referred to as
“Danet”).
l2 See note 4 above. It seems that Cain was allowed to observe some interviews but
not to tape them.
l 3 See, e.g. Janet Gilboy, The Perspectives and Practices of Defense Lawyers in
Criminal Cases, Ph.D. dissertation, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois, 1976 and
(1979), 70 Jo. of Crim. Law and Criminology, p.1. There has also been participant
observation in Holland by Miek Berrends in Gronigen and in California and New York
by Austin Sarat and William Felstiner. Where lawyers have been studied closely it has
often been the public sector of “legal-services-for-the-poor.’’ See, e.g. Carl J. Hosticka,
“We don’t care about what happened, we only care about what is going to happen:
Lawyer-Client Negotiations of Reality” (1979) Social Problems Vol. 26, No. 5, p.599.
l4 “Evaluating the Competence of Lawyers” (1976) 11 Law and Soc. Rev. 257.
lS Ibid. at p.285.
l6 In the second part of the experiment they were divided into three groups (a) one
group receiving training in interviewing by traditional teaching methods and audio-visual
replay of their interviews, (b) one receiving traditional teaching methods only and (c) a
“control” group receiving no interviewing training at all. They were then all filmed
interviewing other clients. The results were thus intended to show how untrained lawyers
interview and the relative benefits of training, different forms of training and of learning
by experience without training. This part of the experiment is reported elsewhere.
Rosenthal, in 14 Law and Soc. Rev. 923 hypothesises that the failure of researchers
to obtain access to the legal profession might be connected with the absence of a “fixed
social objective” that the researchers declare they are trying to attain. In this study the
participating firms have been able to accept training as such an objective.
For the second part of the experiment the clerks were randomly assigned to one of
the three experimental treatment groups, thereby ensuring that if any systematic bias
existed such as the particular selection biases of the firms the clerks came from, it would
be equally present in all three groups.
326 THE MODERN LAW REVIEW [Vol. 49
Hoffman and Kermish” found, it is not easy to gain the co-
operation of lawyers for any such project and a system of random
selection of a sample will almost certainly have to be sacrificed in
favour of obtaining any sample at all.n’
Procedure
An attempt was made to recreate reality within certain confines in
order to sharpen the focus of the experiment onto a minimised set
of factors. The clients were drawn from a pool of previous and
existing clients of local Legal Advice and Law Centres. They were
paid f4-00each to attend and relate their problem in the same way
as they would have done when meeting their lawyer for the first
time. This enabled control over the subject matter of interviews
which in turn enabled some control over lack of legal knowledge as
a factor in lawyer’s performance (see text between notes 22 and 23
below). It also circumvented many of the problems of confidentiality.
Table I
System of Assessment
Data Assessment
Information Tasks Performance Skills Performance
Video- Information emerg- (a) By practising (a) By practising law-
tapes ing during the inter- lawyers. yers
view (b) By legal inter- (b) By legal interview
view trainer trainer
File Information noted
Notes down by lawyer
Information
The prime objectives for the first meeting between lawyer and
client are a subject of some discussion in the academic literature.
Some seem to suggest that it is most important to set up a rapport
between the two, to engender mutual confidence.= Others point
out the need for the lawyer to ascertain with some urgency what
the client has consulted the lawyer about.26 There is agreement
that a basic outline of the problem should be obtained and this
Although they would have all studied basically the same subjects for the Solicitors’
Finals. some might have studied more of certain subjects at University, than others. In
order to balance this, some two weeks before the experiment, all clerks were given a
“top-up” course of reading in the law relating to any subject matter the clients would
raise which was not covered by Solicitors’ exams.
24 Cases involving only welfare benefit calculations were excluded since interviews in
these cases typically might involve long periods of reading and checking calculations
against scales and would therefore not be good material for our exercise.
25 See, e.g. A. S . Watson, The Lawyer in the Interviewing and Counselling Process
(1976) pp.11-16 and Benson, note 8 above, Vol. 1, 22-29; M. K. and B. P. Schoenfield,
“The Art of Interviewing and Counselling” in The Practical Lawyer’s Manual on Lawyer-
Client Relations ALI-ABA 1983, pp.3, 7-8.
26 See, e.g. D. A. Binder and S. C. Price, Legal Interviewing and Counselling, A
Client-Centered Approach, (1977) p.59.
328 THE MODERN LAW REVIEW [Vol. 49
would seem to conform to the practitioner’s view of the process.”
The interviews in this study were therefore assessed first in terms
of the information the lawyers elicited.
Andrew Watson in “The Lawyer in the Interviewing and
Counselling Process” discusses the “‘diagnostic’ mind-set” of
lawyers.28He says they too often do not try to understand why the
client has come to see the lawyer or what motivates clients and
their actions. It is not easy to gain a full understanding of this area,
not least because the client may not “‘know’ exactly what he
wants.” Hosticka, in his empirical study of the work of some
publicly funded lawyers in Boston, shows results which echo
Watson’s criticism.29Watson also cogently argues3’ that “the client’s
goals will shape the outcome of the interview regardless of what
the lawyer does or does not do.” It is therefore more sensible to
take these into account and work with them, rather than ignore
them. It was thus interesting to see how successful the lawyers
were in gathering such information as well as the information
which seemed more relevant to the legal position.
Method
Information was broken down into 12 major ~ a t e g o r i e s as
, ~ ~set out
in Table I1 below. These categories were adapted from the
categories of information used in D. R. Rutter and G. P.
McGuire’s, work on medical students’ history taking techniques
( R ~ t t e r )The
. ~ ~ Rutter categories were adapted to this form after
testing them out on a small number of interviews. Added to these
headings was a category for assessment of how understandable the
file notes would be to another lawyer picking up the case without
having met the client.
The work of assessment of information was carried out by two
objective observers with graduate legal knowledge who were
randomly assigned to mark. The observers manually recorded the
information on to prepared record sheets enumerating the 12
major categories and some 85 sub-categories. The observers were
instructed how to code the information systematically into
the categories and sub-categories resulting in a comprehensive
breakdown of all information for subsequent analysis.
See, e.g. Laurence Sherman, The Practical Skills of the Solicitor (1981) pp.4-12.
pp.1&16.
29 See above, note 13.
y, pp.32-33.
31 There were also numerous sub-categories such as the following relating to heading
1. “1. Client Personal Information: (a) full name, (b) full address, (c) telephone number,
(d) work, (e) marital status, ( f ) children, (g) form of residence, etc.” The analysis of
these details is not reported here, but the full breakdown of information headings are on
file with the author.
32 “History Taking for Medical Students. I Deficiencies in Performance, I1 Evaluation
of a Training Programme” (1976) Lancet 11, 556-560.
MAY 19861 LAWYERS AND CLIENTS: THE FIRST MEETING 329
Ratings of the quality of the information for each lawyer for
each category were assessed on a seven point scale ranging from 1
“bad” through 4 “neither good nor bad” to 7 “good.” Assessment
in this manner was carried out on the basis of information
observable emerging during the interviews by reference to the
videotapes as well as information which was noted down by the
lawyers in their case notes for the “file.”
Results
Table I1 compares the ratings of information emerging in the
interview (by reference to the video-tapes), with information noted
by the lawyer in file notes. The scores shown here are the
percentage whose performances were bad at a “failure” level (i.e.
1-3 score on the 7 point scale).
Table I1
Information Emerging and Information Noted By Lawyer
% Scoring % Scoring
Category of Information 1-3 on notes 1-3 on tapes
1. Personal Information about client 56 30
2. Other parties 63 41
3. Witnesses 85 71
4. Problem (legal) subject categorisation 56 71
5. Events 67 41
6. What the client wants 70 41
7. Previous advice and assistance 96 78
8. Legal Proceedings 93 82
9. Next client/lawyer contact 85 78
10. Work to be done by lawyer 48 63
11. Work to be done by client 83 78
12. Advice given 82 59
13. Clarity of notes 56 -
The information noted by the lawyers was poor. 56 per cent. of
the lawyers failed adequately to record the clients’ personal
information including full names, addresses, telephone numbers,
e t ~ 85. ~per~ cent. failed to obtain adequate information about
witnesses and 70 per cent. failed to identify what the client wanted
as a result of coming to the lawyer. 56 per cent. of the lawyers
could not place the problem in legal subject category and the
incidents and events involved in the cases were inadequately
elicited in 67 per cent. of the cases.
These problems led to inadequate performance in the rest of the
interview. 96 per cent. of the interviewers failed to inquire about
any previous legal assistance on the matter (this would cause
enormous problems if another lawyer were already handling the
33 See note 31 above.
330 THE MODERN LAW REVIEW [Vol. 49
case) and even more importantly 93 per cent. failed to inquire
about any legal proceedings past or pending. 85 per cent. did not
suggest or plan the next lawyerklient contact which was especially
important since this was a first interview with the client.”
The best score on information noted down by the lawyer, on
which a relatively low 48 per cent. “failed”, concerned the work to
be done by the lawyer. However, the disparity between what the
lawyers noted they had to do and what they told to the clients is
interesting. The analysis of the tapes of what was said in the
interview to the client shows a much higher rate of 63 per cent.
“failure” on this point. This seems to suggest that the lawyers were
aware of the work they had to do and recorded it in their notes,
but did not communicate or feel it necessary to communicate this
to the client, a point noticed in the Which? report on lawyers and
also by the Benson Cornmissi~n.~~ Similarly, there is less failure,
and the lawyers were therefore better, at recording problem subject
categories, in their notes, than informing the client of such
categorisaqion.
82 per cent. of the lawyers did not inform the client of any work
needed to be done by the client such as gathering papers, making
calls, etc. The same number failed in terms of their recording of
the legal advice they gave, although here the situation was reversed
and there is a lower failure rate of 59 per cent. on the analysis of
the tapes of the advice they actually gave the client at the time.
From both the lawyer’s and the client’s point of view it may be just
as important to note down advice given, as to give correct advice.
Even the clarity of the notes, which would be extremely important
for future reference to a file, was failure standard in 56 per cent.
of the cases.
The importance of file notes was stressed to the lawyers before
the interviews but it was also thought that the interview tapes
should be analysed so that it could be determined whether any
problems were simply a matter of remedying note-taking skills
rather than interviewing t e ~ h n i q u eAlthough
.~~ there was generally
a lower percentage of failures on the analysis of the tapes, the
failure rates were still very high, including 71 per cent. of the
lawyers failing to categorise the problem for the client and an
equal number failing to inquire properly about witnesses. The
figures regarding “client personal information” and “other parties”
fared rather better in the actual interview (only 30 per cent. and 41
per cent. failed respectively). However, the lawyers did not feel
this information sufficiently important to put into their files and it
This fact might have been a direct result of the experimental nature of the study and
the lawyer’s knowledge that in reality there would be no follow-up, though they were
instructed to treat this in every way as a real first interview.
35 See above notes 7 and 8 respectively.
Indeed, some previous research in the medical arena has concentrated more on the
notes than on what actually occurred in the interview. See Rutter and McGuire note 32
above.
MAY19861 LAWYERS AND CLIENTS: THE FIRSTMEETING 331
might have therefore been forgotten and not passed on to another
lawyer taking over the case. Inquiries about previous advice and
assistance and any relevant legal proceedings were still particularly
low even on the tapes (78 per cent. and 82 per cent. failing
respectively).
As the information the lawyers obtained was seriously incomplete,
the analysis of the interviews was carried further. A detailed look
at the skills and techniques employed by the lawyers could perhaps
shed more light on this poor information output.
I. Tasks
The legal interview can be analysed by division into three main
stages according to the functions involved and the level of active
involvement of each parti~ipant.~’ The first main stage involves
helping and enabling the client to tell the story naturally and is
characterised by little speech on the part of the lawyer, other than
encouragement. In the middle section of the interview the lawyer
begins to take a more obviously active role, questioning the client
on ambiguities or gaps in the factual details or gaining more depth
of information on particular points. This is rounded off by the
lawyer’s summing up of the major facts and the client’s wishes, to
check with the client that they have been properly understood.
Both lawyer and client generally participate about equally at this
stage.
Finally, the lawyer gives the advice or outlines the plan of action
by “counselling” with the client all available options. The lawyer
then makes sure that the client is in agreement, and begins to carry
out the plan of action involved, setting the next contact before
terminating the interview. Here the activity is mainly from the
lawyer.
Method
(a) The Construction of the Tasks Model. In this research the
stages of an interview were broken down further into an organising
plan of some 13 major tasks it was considered important for a
37 These three stages roughly correlate with the first three stages mentioned in
Goodpaster, “The Human Arts of Lawyering: Interviewing and Counselling” (1975) 27
Jo.Leg.Ed. 5, 33; Redmount, “Humanistic Law Through Legal Counselling” (1%9) 2
Conn.L.Rev. 98, 11C111; Binder and Price, op. cit. note 26, pp.53-59 et seq. They also
relate to the six phases of doctor-patient consultations discovered by P. S. Byme, and B.
E. L. Long in their D.H.S.S. study, Doctors Talking to Patients, pp.19-29.
332 THE MODERN LAW REVIEW [Vol. 49
lawyer to cover in the first interview.% The interview was divided
sequentially into these tasks, and the relationship between them
and the three stages enumerated above is set out in Table I11
below.
Table III
The First InterviewThe 13 Tasks by Stages39
Stage One 1. Greeting, seating and introducing 6.
2. Eliciting story with opening question or helpful N
silence
3. Obtaining basic outline of personalities and case 0
from client’s own unhindered words T
Stage Two 4. Questioning on facts for gaps, depth, background E
and ambiguities and relevance
5 . Summing up and recounting lawyer’s view of
facts, A N D checking for client’s agreement or T
amending A
Stage Three 7. Stating advice and/or plan of action
8. Repeating advicelplan of action A N D checking
for client’s agreement or amending
9. Recounting follow-up work to be done by client K
10. Recounting follow-up work to be done by lawyer I
11. Stating next contact between lawyer and client
12. Asking if Any Other Business and dealing with it N
13. Terminating, helping out and goodbyes. G
Table V
Tasksstage Two
Rating
Bad or Neither Good or
very bad good nor very good
Task (1-3) bad (4) (5-7)
4. Factual questioning, gaps, 9 (33%) 9 (33%) 9 (33%)
depth
5. Summing up, recounting, 14 (52%) 7 (26%) 6 (22%)
checking client agreement
6. Note taking 9 (33%) 11 (41%) 7 (26%)
The lawyers in our study performed across the board with equal
numbers in each collapsed score category for this heading, Task
number 4. This means that although 33 per cent. scored in the
highest category (all at the “5” score level), another 33 per cent.
actually failed, which failure figure may provide explanation for
some of the poor results already noted in relation to the Information
analysis above.
The lawyer’s tendency heavily to structure an interview into
narrow lines of inquiry may be fatal to a proper understanding of
the client’s problem if there is also no attempt to check the
lawyer’s comprehension of the story with the client. It is implicit in
the concept of the lawyer “collaborating”” with the client that the
lawyer should check back with the client that the client’s intended
story and needs have been perceived by the lawyer. Task 5 was
therefore a vital element in conducting a first interview, and a
crucial first step for the lawyer before proceeding to advise or work
out a plan of action for the client. Byrne and Long in their study
of “Doctors Talking to patient^"^' note how summarising can be
used either to “close off” or “open up” a part of an interview.
Task 5 was intended specifically to measure whether the lawyer
allowed the client to comment, and therefore if necessary disagree
with and modify, the summary. It therefore represents only the
concept of “summarising to open up” in Byrne and Long’s
categorisation.
The 52 per cent. failure (73 per cent. as measured by the expert)
for this task is one of the highest failure rates in this section. It
bears out the frequent complaints about lawyers’ remoteness and
inability to communicate to clients what is going on in their
% Shaffer, op. cit., pp.234 et seq.
57 (D.H.S.S. 1976), at ppS(k53.
MAY19861 LAWYERS AND CLIENTS: THE FIRST MEETING 337
minds.58 It is also interesting to compare the even higher failure
rate for Task 8 below, which similarly involves obtaining the
client’s agreement.
The task of note-taking, which does not naturally fall into any
one categorys9 and should probably take place before and after, as
well as throughout, the interview, receives much discussion in the
literature. The content of the notes has already been analysed in
the Information section above, and here the assessment was of the
manner in which notes were taken. Freeman and Weihoffenm
mention the distortion effect resulting from the client’s perception
of those parts of the interview being noted down by the lawyer as
being more “important.” They suggest that no notes be taken and
a recording device used for better recall. Similar views are
expressed by Meltsner and Schrag.61 Some clients however object
to recording devices and lawyers may object to the extra time
needed to listen to recordings. Kinsey managed his research whilst
making short notes in the presence of his interviewees without
affecting their relationship,62 and note-taking during interviews
seems to be the model used by solicitors in England and Wales.63
It is also necessary for the lawyer to take notes during an interview
to remind himherself of questions to be asked later in the interview
itself.
Assessment of Task 6 was therefore on the basis that the lawyers
would take notesb0 when necessary but that it should be done
unobtrusively or in some other way so as to have a minimal effect
on the interview. 33 per cent. of the lawyer subjects failed on their
note-taking largely because it was performed in such a way that
eye contact was lost for long periods of time and the client’s
natural flow was thereby discouraged.
Table VI
Tasks-Stage Three
Rating
Bad or Neither Good or
Very Bad Good nor Very Good
Task (1-3) Bad (4) (5-7)
7. Stating advicelplan of 12 (45%) 8 (30%) 7 (26%)
action
8. Repeating advice and 16 (59%) 6 (22%) 5 (19%)
checking client’s
agreement
9. Recounting client’s follow 17 (63%) 7 (26%) 3 (11%)
UP
10. Recounting lawyer’s 15 (56%) 7 (26%) 5 (19%)
follow up
11. Setting next contact 18 (67%) 5 (19%) 4 (15%)
12. Asking for Any Other 25 (93%) 0 (0%) 2 (7%)
Business and dealing with
it
13. Terminating and 19 (70%) 4 (15%) 4 (15%)
goodbyes
lS Ibid. at p.25.
79 See above notes 7 and 8.
Benson, para. 22.29.
MAY19861 LAWYERS AND CLIENTS: THE FIRST MEETING 341
section. In the present exercise the way these tasks were carried
out, including both the quality of the information involved, as well
as the way it was communicated, was rated by the practising
lawyers.
A frequent complaint about lawyers which does not seem
primarily to relate to communication is that of delay.61Even if they
do spend time when meeting the client explaining the law in detail
it still seems to the client a long time after the meeting before
anything occurs. This may of course be due to the lawyer’s pressure
of work, the presence of more remunerative cases, the court’s
schedules or a multitude of other possible factors.62 However,
clients’ dissatisfaction is almost certainly due to one main factor,
the difference between their expectations and their lawyer’s actual
performance. If the client is told precisely what to expect from the
outset, the chances of dissatisfaction must be lessened. There is
also an important reassurance effect for the client in being told
specifically what steps to take and not to take, what papers to
gather and calls to make. It gives clients the feeling that they are
positively involved in the progress of their case.= This detailed
exposition approach gives the “concrete-specific’’ advice suggested
in relation to the doctor-patient relationship above.&l It is also
proper that the lawyer should explain what procedural steps are
going to be taken and how long each of these is expected to last.65
There is no special mystery which needs to be kept from the client
and the greater the client’s awareness of the procedure and
practical steps necessary, the greater the chances of a client’s
expectations being closer to reality. This is most likely to increase
client satisfaction and enable the client to provide real help to the
lawyer, rather than pester the lawyer with telephone calls to find
out what is happening.%
There is a very high proportion of failures on these headings, 63
per cent., 56 per cent. and 67 per cent. respectively, and these are
among the larger rates of failures on all exercises. One possible
explanation is that these “continuation” plans may have been
perceived by lawyer and client alike as the more unreal of the tasks
involved in the “experimental” interview situation which both knew
would go no further. These very poor scores might therefore be
something of an artefact of the experiment. On the other hand the
findings of Benson8’ and the Lay ObserverBB so clearly substantiate
Summary
The overall impression on the basis of analysis of the lawyers’
interviews by tasks and stages was that their performance was
worst in the last stage and particularly bad on Tasks 11, 12 and 13.
The lawyers were generally not good at giving their clients the
opportunity to tell their own story and to agree or disagree with
the lawyers’ view of the facts and law. They were also poor at
informing the clients of the details of what was going to happen or
what might happen on their case. The general mode of operation
for the lawyers was to narrow down the factual area of inquiry as
soon as possible with series of questions and to force the issue into
a semblance of legal category with which they were acquainted.
II. Skills
In this section the lawyer’s conduct of the interview overall was
rated in accordance with the level of ability shown in skilful
application of good interviewing techniques. The lawyers would be
using these skills and techniques in order to carry out the tasks
listed above. Unlike the tasks, the usage of these skills would not
necessarily be confined to a particular stage of the interview and
might carry on throughout. By analysing the interview in this way
it was hoped to find out not only what particular tasks presented
difficulties for the lawyers but also, in what particular skills their
strengths and deficiencies lay, thus causing the successes and
failures noted above.
Method
The skills to be used in the first interview were broken down
into categories partly adapted from some of the studies in doctor-
patient communication and supplemented by categories which,
after pilot studies, seemed helpful in analysing the lawyer-client
relationship.
Table VII
Skills Headings
1. Handling personal and confidential topics.
2. Not accepting client’s jargon.
3. Not overusing legal terminology.
4. Precision in obtaining information.
5. Efficiency in obtaining information.
6. Picking up client’s verbal cues.
7. Not over-repeating of same topics.
344 THE MODERN LAW REVIEW [Vol. 49
8. Clarifymg gaps or confusions.
9. Controlling the client and “irrelevant” information.
10. Facilitating the client to talk.
11. Not using “leading” or “closed” questions.
12. Not using complex questions.
13. Ease with client.
14. Empathy with client.
15. Reassurance of client.
16. Time control throughout.
17. Openinglclosing ease and control.
18. Giving advice and counselling.
Table IX
Skills Results 4-8
Rating
Bad or Neither Good or
Very Bad Good nor Veiy Good
(1-3) Bad (4) (5-7)
4. Precision in obtaining infor- 10 (37%) 11 (41%) 6 (29%)
mation
5. Efficiency in obtaining infor- 11 (41%) 13 (48%) 3 (11%)
mation
6 . Picking up client’s verbal cues 6 (22%) 17 (63%) 4 (15%)
7. Not over repeating same topics 10 (37%) 12 (44%) 5 (19%)
8. Clarifying gaps or confusions 8 (30%) 14 (52%) 5 (18%)
Obtaining Information. The quality of the information itself was
addressed in the first assessment section above. In this section
headings 4 to 8 cover the skills involved in eliciting those facts
from the client. Few university level or professional courses
introduce lawyers to what often becomes the major part of their
work, finding the facts.3 The tenor of legal education is to train
students how to “think like a lawyer” but not how to act like
Legal education concentrates on sifting and presenting legal
arguments and findings relating to a given and finite set of facts.
Young lawyers who have spent some four years being trained only
to answer examination questions with neatly laid out facts, are
likely to feel unease when faced with the uncertainty and complexity
of real life problems whose facts have not been sifted for legal
relevance. Since legal practitioners usually spend far more time on
fact finding than on legal research the ability to obtain information
with precision is an essential item in lawyers’ work.S Yet, on
heading 4, “Precision in obtaining information,” 37 per cent. of our
lawyers scored at a failure rate with 45 per cent. reaching the mid-
point.
Even our expert assessor marked the lawyers as highly on heading 3.
See, e.g. W. L. Twining, “Taking Facts Seriously” (1984) 34 J. Legal Educ. 22 and
more generally J. W. Frank, Courts on Trial (1949), Law and rhe Modern Mind (1930);
“Why not a Clinical Lawyer-School?” (1933) 81 U.Pa.L.Rev. 907.
‘See, e.g. F. K. Zemans & V. G. Rosenblum, The making of a Public Profession
(1981).
See the short summary of research to this effect in Shaffer. op. cit., pp.1-2.
MAY19861 LAWYERS AND CLIENTS: THE HRST MEETING 347
The lawyers performed similarly on Skill 5 “Efficiency in
obtaining information.” Whilst “precision” aimed at a careful
identification of what facts might be necessary, “efficiency”
attempted to measure the general approach of the lawyers and how
effectively they kept to a structure for obtaining the facts.
If lawyers are imprecise and inefficient in obtaining information,
how does the imprecision and inefficiency occur? This was the
subject of the next three headings aiming to isolate or emphasise
particular deficiencies in this area.
Much of the literature concerns the hidden meanings behind
clients’ presentation of verbal information. The form of expression
as well as the content, sudden lapses into silence after implicit
statements,6 and shifts in conversation’ are all taken to be important
sources of information on unconscious feelings, as are “parapraxes”
or “Freudian slips.”8 Just as important, although understudied in
the literature, perhaps because it is so obvious, is the necessity to
listen carefully also to the direct and straightforward meanings of
what is being said by the client. Observation of both experienced
and inexperienced lawyers interviewing has shown numerous
examples of relevant and sometimes crucial facts stated and
repeated by a client which are not picked up by the lawyer. It
seems that the lawyer’s concentration on facts thought by the
lawyer to be strictly legally relevant to the (lawyer’s perception of
the) case may deafen the lawyer to anything else the client may
say. The majority of the lawyers performed at the average point on
“picking up verbal cues” with some 22 per cent. failing.’
The problems mentioned above in Stage Two of the Tasks
analysis regarding questioning of the client arise again here. It is
necessary for the lawyer to maintain the client’s confidence that the
lawyer understands and is supportive of the client. However,
inconsistencies, gaps and confusions will undermine the lawyer’s
belief in the veracity and honesty of the client. lo Questioning must
Watson op. cir., pp.48-53, Binder and Price op. cir., pp.25-28.
’ Shafter op. cit., p.125.
Watson op. cit., p.50.
A good example of problems arising when lawyers fail to pick up verbal cues
happened in one interview where a client who came in for another problem was in fact a
battered wife who alluded to this many times. The cues were not picked up at all by the
lawyer even though this problem was directly relevant to the problem she was presenting
and was far more urgent. Fortunately, the remote control camera crew pointed the
problem out after the interview and the client was then given help by one of the solicitors
involved in a local Law Centre.
lo Watson, op. cir., p.23 describes a method of “cross-corroboration” of the lawyer’s
impressions by introducing questions “at random times and out of context” in order to
give the lawyer a clearer picture, without giving the client too much of a hint about
where the lawyer thinks the inadequacies of the story lie. Such inadequacies, when part
of a truthful story, are likely to result, he says, from a particular psychological motivation
such as the need to block out an incident or person because that memory may be painful.
Direct questioning (although it may be necessary later) will at this point elicit a similar
response from the client, hence the need for a more indirect method. Such a method of
questioning seems more applicable to a later interview in the relationship than the first
time that the lawyer and client meet.
348 THE MODERN LAW REVIEW [Vol. 49
therefore take place, but in a careful, understanding way. Over-
repetition of the same topics was thought to be a problem likely to
be suffered from by inexperienced interviewers. It wastes time and
may show a lack of cohesive plan. It also telegraphs to the client
the lawyer’s lack of understanding of the case and can lead to
annoyance on the part of the client and lack of confidence in the
lawyer’s ability. The larger proportion of the lawyers were in the
average category for Skills headings 7 and 8 but there were still 37
per cent. and 30 per cent. failure rates respectively for these.
Table X
Skills Results 9-12
Rating
Bad or Neither Good or
Very Bad Good nor Very Good
0-3) Bad (4) (5-7)
9. Controlling the client and 8 (30%) 13 (48%) 6 (22%)
“irrelevant” information
10. Facilitating the client to talk 5 (19%) 11 (41%) 11 (40%)
11. Not using “leading” or 10 (37%) 8 (30%) 9 (33%)
“closed” questions
12. Not using complex questions 1 (4%) 13 (48%) 13 (48%)
ESTABLISH.
ELlClTlNQ
INFORMATION TRUST
r
Y
C- +>
T I M E
Table XI
Skills Results 13-1 7
Rating
Bad or Neither Good or
Very Bad Good nor Very Good
(1-3) Bad (4) (5-7)
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
Ease with client
Empathy with client
Reassurance of client
Time control throughout
Openinglclosing ease and con-
16 jJp*j pz/ !b;z/
6 22%
11 41%
12 45%)
11 (41%)
8 30%
10 37%
7 25%
trol 17 (63%) 5 (19%) 5 (18%)
Reassurance and time. Headings 13 to 17 attempted to inquire
Is It may be that the lawyer assessors expected more “control” than, from the
Rosenthal viewpoint, would be necessary and marked accordingly. Our expert thought
the lawyers fared rather better at their control, marking them at 33 per cent. failure, 25
per cent. average and 41 per cent. in the Good-Very Good Category, but his marks
generally tended more to the extreme than the lawyer assessors.
I6 See notes 12, 13 above.
I7 See the Summary at the end of the Tasks section.
MAY19861 LAWYERS AND CLIENTS: THE FIRST MEETING 35 1
into how the lawyers balanced their own “professional” needs of
objectivity, formality and time constraints against the necessity to
reassure their clients, gain their confidence and empathise with
their clients’ difficulties. The time tension can again be viewed
clearly under these headings.
Much of the client’s ease toward the lawyer will result from
attention to the physical surroundings of a lawyer’s reception room
and the office or interview room,l* which were not items within the
lawyer’s control in this experiment. But the lawyer must also adopt
what Watson calls “a non-judgmental and nonmoralising p o ~ t u r e ” ’ ~
so that the client will be able to tell the whole story as the client
sees it and so that the lawyer will be able “to listen more closely to
what his client tells him”20without feeling the need to react other
than showing interest. This may come in the form of what Binder
and Price call “non-committal acknowledgements’’ such as “mmm,”
“really” and “interesting” (or just “head-nodding”),21 or “active
listening” by reflecting back to the client feelings they have
expressed in the interview.22 The lawyers seemed overall to fare
quite well on this skill with 41 per cent. at the mid-point and 37
per cent. above.
Showing empathy is a rather more active skill and may require
more active listening, reflection of feelings back to a client and
“motivational statements” such as explanations of why particular
information is necessary.23 Clear statements of understanding will
often be necessary24 and a consciousness shown throughout the
interview of the needs of the client. The lawyers scored less well in
this category with 41 per cent. below the mid-point.
Benjamin refers to “reassurance” of the client as “directed
toward helping the client achieve a sense of comfort about the
interview relation~hip.”~~ This stands opposed to a more intuitive
understanding of “reassurance.” Such an understanding would be
more directed towards the lawyer removing the anxiety of the case
from the client by taking the responsibility for the case off the
client’s shoulders and on to the lawyer’s. If a collaborative
relationship between lawyer and client is to occur then this latter
form of reassurance is not This is therefore a somewhat
problematic heading for assessment. Our lawyers proved very poor
See, e.g. L. Shurman, The Practical Skills of the Solicitor, and T. L. Shaffer, op. cit.
pp.94105.
l9 Op. cit., p.7.
2o Watson, op. cit., p.7.
Op. cit., p.24.
Op. cit., pp.25-37. For a strong reaction against this “community of two” see W.
Simon, “Homo Psychologicus: Notes On A New Legal Formalism”, 32 Stanford Law
Rev. 487, (1980).
Op. cit., pp.lOS108.
2‘ Watson, op. cit., pp.8-10.
2s See Watson, op. cit., p.92 and A. Benjamin, The Helping Interview (1969) pp.108-
155.
26 Watson, op. cit., p.86.
352 THE MODERN LAW REVIEW [Vol. 49
at this skill, 59 per cent. falling in the 1-3 category and a further
22 per cent. functioning at the mid-point.
The skill of time control in an interview is most difficult to
master in any helping situation where the interviewee is placing
some reliance on the interviewer. The time itself that the
professional “gives” is part of the service offered to the client.”
Rarely is there enough time in an initial interview to address all
the lawyer’s and all the client’s concerns. The lawyer should
therefore concentrate on obtaining an overview of the facts and
needs of the clientB whilst checking that there is nothing urgent
which needs to be carried out before they can meet or communicate
again.
It is helpful to give a client a clear idea of the amount of time
the lawyer has available for the interview so that the client does
not have unreal expectations of what will occur.?9As measured by
the American Law Professor only 15 per cent. of the lawyers
thought of doing this in an open way verbally. The remainder
either chose a non-verbal method of communication such as looking
at their watches, or simply ignored the problem altogether until it
became too late. It is also the job of the lawyer to control the
interview so that it is possible to carry out the necessary tasks
within the time allotted. It was hypothesised that such control
would be more difficult and more crucial at the beginning and end
of the interview. The lawyer would be setting the scene and
creating expectations of time available at the beginning, and would
be even more conscious of time running out at the end.30 The
interview is likely to be a cathartic experience for the client who is
anxious when beginning and and should be more reassured at the
end. If there is difficulty in ending, it could well be because of a
feeling of lack of reassurance, or something more to say, or
absence of agreement with the advice or plan of action offered. It
was interesting to see that many of our new lawyers experienced
difficulty in controlling their allocation of time available, 45 per
cent. failing to reach the mid-point. This rose to 63 per cent. when
the opening and closing critical phases were assessed, which seems
to suggest that the difficulties involved are manifested more clearly
in those parts of the interview.
To summarise this section, there was a tension for our lawyer
subjects between the needs for time control and the needs to show
ease, empathy and reassurance to their clients. They did not
perform highly in either group of skills but they did seem to be
somewhat better at showing the more natural social skills of ease
Watson, op. cit., p.132.
Binder and Price, op. cit., pp.53-86.
r, See how this affects doctors in P. Ley, “Psychological Studies of Doctor-Patient
Communication” in S. J. Rachman, Contributions to Medical Psychology (1977) Vol. 1,
p.9. See also Watson, op. cit., p.31.
a The counselling part of the interview which occurs towards the end, may be
especially time consuming. See Binder and Price, op. cir., pp.1%191.
MAY19861 LAWYERS AND CLIENTS: THE FIRST MEETING 353
and empathy. Time.contro1 appears to be a skill which needs some
practice and experience or may need specific training to develop in
a beneficial manner.
The advising and counselling of the client are the main “products”
of the legal interview and the “service” for which the client has
approached the lawyer. In the Information section we looked at
the quality of the advice as noted down by the lawyer and as
occurring during the interview in terms of its informative role. In
the Tasks section assessment by the practising lawyers covered how
well the advice or plan of action were set out by the experimental
subjects. Here in the Skills section more attention was paid to the
interactive skills involved in both advising and counselling clients.
A distinction may be drawn from the American writings on this
subject between “advising” and “counseling” (sic)clients. “Advising”
seems to mean the lawyer suggesting to the client one particular
course of action which the lawyer considers best for the client.
“Counseling” seems to mean the presentation of a number of
options to the client and helping the client go thoroughly through
those options, whilst allowing the client to make the final choice.”
Explained in this way it does not necessarily need to borrow too
strongly from ideas based in the psychotherapeutic relationship.”
“Counselling” is not a word which seems to be easily accepted
by English ears and does not appear in any of the English short
practitioner although almost all of the American texts
refer to both “interviewing” and “counseling” (sic) in their titles.34
Counselling as explained above, the presentation of more than one
option and allowing the client to choose, is essential to a more
collaborative relationship along the lines of the Rosenthal model.35
The American literature does not however in general differentiate
31 See, e.g. Binder and Price op. cir. pp.135-191. It also conforms to Styles 4-7 of the
Byrne and Long diagnostic styles, op. cir., pp.103-112.
32 Watson, op. cir., pp.141-156.
33 See, e.g. Shurman, op. cir. note 23. A welcome harbinger of change is the note by
M. A. Haley, “Legal Services and Counselling: Changing Attitudes for Changing Times,”
(1983) L.S.Gaz. 2293-2294.
Binder and Price op. cir., Freeman and Weihoffen op. cir., Shaffer op. cir. and
Watson op. cir.
35 See Watson op. cir., p.142.
354 THE MODERN LAW REVIEW [Vol. 49
between the work to be carried out in first and subsequent
interviews. The major work of counselling the client seems more
likely to occur in a subsequent interview when all the legal options
have been researched by the lawyer and all the necessary facts
obtained. If any counselling as such is to take place in a first
interview it may concentrate more on the immediate plan of action
rather than the long term goals of the client regarding the case.
There are numerous pitfalls in both advising and counselling
clients. The skills involved cover the more traditional skills of
“thinking like a lawyer,” applying law to facts, as well as the skills
of developing practical alternatives and explaining all of these to a
lay client. Apart from the legal analysis (which was not the primary
focus of this study) errors can also be made for example in the use
of difficult language, withholding background information or
procedure, failing to perceive that advice is often the telling of
“bad news,” and not being aware of client reaction to the advice.36
The high level of 67 per cent. of the lawyers below the mid range
for this heading is the highest “failure” rate noted by the English
practitioner assessors on any of the skills in the list. They clearly
felt the lawyers were not at all skilful in advising and counselling.
The American assessor was not quite so pessimistic, however, with
only 41 per cent. falling in the 1-3 range and an equal number
rising into the 5-7 range. It could therefore be that the English
assessors were looking more for the “advising” model than the
“counselling” one.
Summary
From a general view of the Skills section it seems that the
lawyers were more adept at language skills such as dealing with
client’s jargon, legal terminology, client’s verbal cues and not using
complex questions. They also showed some skill in facilitating the
client to talk. They were poor at investigative skills and even less
able to handle personal topics, show empathy or give reassurance,
although they did show social ease with the client. Finally, control
of time in the interview and advising and counselling were their
areas of least skilfulness. Not surprisingly, the skills items which
were particularly related to the lawyer-client interview seem to
have been more poorly handled than those which the inexperienced
lawyers would have already encountered in normal social discourse.
DISCUSSION
Table XI11
Comparison of Average Numbers of Lawyers Falling in 1-3 Range
by Section
Information Information
(notes) (tapes) Tasks Skills
Average % falling in
1-3 range. 72.2 61 50.2 34.7
Table XI11 looks at the proportions of bad performers in each
36 See below under the Discussion.
MAY19861 LAWYERS A N D CLIENTS: THE FIRST MEETING 355
section of this study. The numbers were computed by averaging
out the percentages falling in the 1-3 category for all the categories
in each section. The highest “failure” rates occurred in the
Information section with 72.2 per cent. average on the notes and
61 per cent. average on the tapes. The Tasks section shows an
average of 50.2 per cent. of the lawyers falling into the 1-3
category and the Skills section comes out best with only 34.7 per
cent. falling below the mid-point.
Table XIV
Comparison of Average Numbers of Lawyers in 5-7 Range by
Section
TaSkS Skills
Average % falling in 5-7 range. 20.03 254
When one looks at the other side of the coin, the “good”
performing category within the 5-7 range, there is not a
correspondingly higher percentage in the Skills than the Tasks
section (see Table XIV above). Although the Skills are still about
5 per cent. higher than Tasks, the previous 15 per cent. difference
has largely been swallowed up at the mid-point (4 grading). Whilst
the lawyers are clearly better at the Skills than the Tasks, there is
not a large percentage falling into the “good” category for either.
Judging from this assessment the lawyer subjects appeared to
possess at least some measure of behavioural skills necessary for
the work of interviewing clients and dealing generally with people
and their problems. They appeared to have much less of a
conception of when and how to use those skills to further the tasks
necessary to be carried out in a first interview. They showed little
obvious framework for the use of their skills. This perhaps caused
them to lose sight of some of the tasks or to carry out the tasks
improperly or in a confusing order. All of this had the effect of
poor information gathering within the interview.
It seems therefore that social experience and legal education do
provide new lawyers with some of the basic skills necessary for
being a lawyer. What is more lacking is an understanding of the
contexts in which these skills are to be used, insight into the roles
and activities of lawyers and proper frameworks for putting such
skills into practice. The subject lawyers appeared to treat their
clients more as walking examination questions to be opened up to
rigorous scrutiny; than as people with problems. The lawyers
showed many of the “controlling” characteristics discussed in the
literature, deciding both what the client’s problem was and how it
should be solved, through the narrow confines of legal categorisa-
tions. The lawyer subjects pounced on words with legal connotation
and concentrated their questioning on areas which sounded legally
356 THE MODERN LAW REVIEW [Vol. 49
relevant long before their clients had finished describing the
elements of their problems.
The lawyers did not give their clients time to tell their stories
properly. They did not check that they had understood the clients’
facts and feelings correctly or that their clients agreed with their
advice or plan of action. Because of this, collaboration between
lawyer and client, or even an agreeable working relationship were
unlikely sequels to these first interviews. The working model was
more clearly that of the “High Priest” of the law handing down
mystical pronouncements to grateful recipients. If the most
important objective of a first interview is to achieve a good working
relationship between lawyers and clients based on mutual
understanding and c~nfidence,~’ our lawyers were not successful.
As we have already seen they were even less successful if the most
important objective is to gather all the information at the first
interview.
Learning the law in isolation of the social context in which it
operates can be seen as a significant gap in legal education which
leaves those emerging into at least the solicitor’s branch of the
profession inadequately equipped to perform competently as
practising lawyers. It may be argued that the appropriate uses of
skills learned at the academic and vocational stages will “come” in
time and with experience during the practical stage of training in
articles. It is however doubted whether experience, untutored and
uncorrected, is by itself a sufficient teacher to guard against the
entrenchment of poor techniques and poor attitudes towards clients.
The clients who have to suffer during this learning process should
also be taken into account. It is also submitted that there are some
aspects of interviewing which are not intuitive and that no amount
of experience can teach. They need to be explained and discussed
and worked with, in the context of a teaching environment.
The second part of this study, which is to be published
e l s e ~ h e r e , addresses
~~ some of the questions raised by this
Conclusion. It asks whether interviewing and counselling can be
taught, whether certain teaching methods are better than others
and whether experience alone is an effective teacher.
This study shows major deficiencies in the abilities of new
entrants to the profession to carry out one of their most important
functions, that of interviewer and counsellor. Neither the new
system of Law Society Finals, nor the new system of supervision in
articles of clerkship39take sufficient account of these needs. The
introduction of a seminar on interviewing and counselling in the
new mandatory post-qualification course is therefore a most
See note 25 above..
uI To be published, 1987. An extract appears in Chapter 7 of A. H. Sherr, Client
Interviewing for Lawyers: An Analysis and Guide (1986).
)9 See A. H. Sherr, “Lip Service Under Articles or Chances Missed (1982) 132 N.L.J.
395, and Evidence of the Law Society to the Ormrod Committee on Legal Education,
Appendix F, p.212 of the Ormrod Report, Cmnd. 4595, pp.56-70.
MAY19861 LAWYERS AND CLIENTS: THE FIRST MEETING 357
welcome innovation, and an important first step.40 However, one
two-hour seminar in eight years of legal education is insufficient for
such an important element of solicitors’ work, and such courses
need to be expanded and included earlier in the lawyer’s training
programme.
AVROM SHERR*
40 See the Law Society’s scheme as set out in I19831 L.S.Gaz. 2987-2988. Training in
the “office efficiency” section of the mandatory course (in which interviewing is to be
taught) started in January 1986 and is carried out by the British Institute of Management.
One wonders what image of legal practice they will be portraying.
* Visiting Professor of Law, U.C.L.A. 1984-85; Lecturer in Law, Director of Legal
Practice; Wanvick University. The initial research for this paper was funded by the
Nuffield Foundation Small Grants Scheme for innovations in teaching methods. Thanks
are due to Derek Rutter for initial advice; Peter Smith for some of the assessment,
William Twining, Alan Paterson and Sol Picciotto for reading drafts of the article, and
most of all to Lorraine Sherr for advice; assistance and encouragement from idea through
implementation to final draft. If, faults there be, our combined legal skills will probably
enable us to apportion blame elsewhere.