Aprendizaje Ingles en Niños
Aprendizaje Ingles en Niños
Aprendizaje Ingles en Niños
com
WCETR 2011
b
Department of English Language, Toyserkan Branch, Islamic Azad University, Toyserkan, IRAN
Abstract
This paper attempts to clarify Importance of learning a foreign language in childhood. Among philosophers, empiricism and the
psychologists, behaviourists believe that language is a social creature and like other social behaviours are acquired. This is
perhaps the most complex and arguable question that have risen among linguists, psychologists and philosophers about language:
how a child learns foreign language?Language learning is natural. Babies are born with the ability to learn it and that learning
begins at birth. Many experts believe that learning the language before the age of ten years allow children to speak correct and
fluent as an indigenous person. Therefore, whatever the earlier children become familiar with foreign language, he have better
chance to speak proficiency. Research suggests that from birth through age 10 is the best time to introduce new languages to a
young child. In this paper the child will learn the language faster, retain it better and most often speak it with near-native
pronunciation. Finally, this paper highlights advantages and disadvantages foreign language learning in childhood.
1. Introduction
Many experts believe that learning the language before the age of ten years allow children to speak correct and
fluent as an indigenous person. Therefore, whatever the earlier children become familiar with foreign language, he
have better chance to speak proficiency. On the other hand language learning, except native language, can provide
develop a lifelong ability to more communicate with others. One of the important advantages of mastering a foreign
language is access to better job opportunitiesand the person will find deeper understanding to their own culture and
other nations. Including the benefits of knowing a foreign language in today's society, enhancing economic
competitiveness in the external surface, improving global communications and maintains and manage political and
security interests of a country. Research has shown that, if languages learning in children before puberty, children
are found more chances to speak a foreign language with a completely native pronunciation. In addition, familiarity
child with the culture of other nations spread his views and attitudes and provides the opportunity for him to
communicate with other people. We know now that studying a foreign language offers surprising benefits to
children. Research has demonstrated improved ability to communicate, better cognitive development, richer cultural
awareness and, ultimately, better job opportunities for those who know a foreign language Ferreira, F., & Morrison,
1877-0428 © 2011 Published by Elsevier Ltd. Open access under CC BY-NC-ND license.
doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.11.160
Babak Ghasemi and Masoud Hashemi / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 28 (2011) 872 – 876 873
F. J. (1994). What’s more, today’s children will all be required to have command of two languages by the time they
reach college Research suggests that from birth through age 10 is the best time to introduce new languages to a
young child. The child will learn the language faster, retain it better and most often speak it with near-native
pronunciation. Recent research indicates a young child up through age 5 can learn and process up to five languages!
Many parents deliberate over how to bring a new language into their little one’s life. Many experts agree the
bilingual approach for the very young child is best. Today’s parents know the importance of being bilingual. Now
they just need to know where to turn for assistance in finding fun and affordable bilingual products that will bring
the target language into their child’s life. The internet has made their search much easier than five years ago. Look
for bilingual programs that allow you to sample their visual or audio products on line so that you get a good feel for
the content and style of the language learning within that particular program.
Children begin learning languages at birth (infants pay attention to their parents' voices, as opposed to random
noises or even other languages), and haven't really mastered it subtleties before the age of ten years. Indeed, we
never really stop learning our language. (David Singleton.) This isn't exactly the sort of behavior (like foals walking
an hour after birth) that we call 'instinct' in animals.
But at least it's effortless, isn't it? Well, no, as we can see when children have a choice of languages to learn.
What's found is that, to be frank, children don't learn a language if they can get away with not learning it.
874 Babak Ghasemi and Masoud Hashemi / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 28 (2011) 872 – 876
When babies are born, they can make and hear all the sounds in all the languages in the world. That’s about 150
sounds in about 6500 languages! However, no language uses all 150 sounds. The sounds a language uses are called
phonemes and English has about 44. Some languages use more and some use fewer.
In this stage, babies learn which phonemes belong to the language they are learning and which don’t. The ability to
recognize and produce those sounds is called “phonemic awareness,” which is important for children learning to
read.
At this stage children essentially learn how the sounds in a language go together to make meaning. For example,
they learn that the sounds m, ah, m, and ee refer to that “being” that cuddles and feeds them – mommy. That’s a
significant step because everything we say is really just a stream of sounds. To make sense of those sounds, a child
must be able to recognize where one word ends and another one begins. These are called “word boundaries.”
It’s not exactly words, though, that children are learning. What children are actually learning are morphemes, which
may or may not be words. That’s really not as confusing as it sounds. A morpheme is just a sound or sounds that
have a meaning, like the word mommy. The word mommy, however, has two morphemes: mommy and ±s. Children
at this stage can recognize that the ±s means "more than one" and will know that when that sound is added to other
words, it means the same thing – "more than one."
During this stage, children learn how to create sentences. That means they can put words in the correct order. For
example, they learn that in English we say "I want a cookie" and "I want a chocolate cookie," not "Want I a cookie"
or "I want cookie chocolate."
Children also learn the difference between grammatical correctness and meaning. Noam Chomsky created an
example of this difference in the sentence “Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.” Children will know that although
the sentence is grammatically correct, it doesn’t make sense. They know that green is a color and can't, therefore, be
colorless.(Harrison, B., & Papa, R. (2005)
Window of Opportunity
Early childhood is the best time for language acquisition. Ease of learning a foreign language diminishes with
age. Between birth and adolescence the brain is hard-wired to acquire language naturally. As child approaches
puberty, the nature of language learning and storage changes, becoming less flexible.
Why do children learn languages well, when even adults who want to learn them have trouble with them? Innate
abilities aside, children have a number of powerful advantages:
x They can devote almost their full time to it. Adults consider half an hour's study a day to be onerous.
x Their motivation is intense. Adults rarely have to spend much of their time in the company of people they
need to talk to but can't; children can get very little of what they want without learning language(s).
If adults could be placed in a similar situation, they might well learn languages as readily (I don't say 'easily'!) as
children. The closest such situation I can think of is cross- cultural marriage. And indeed, this works quite well. My
wife, for instance, a native Spanish speaker who came here in her late 20s, has learned exceptional English, since we
speak it at home. By contrast, some of her Spanish-speaking friends of the same age, married to other Spanish
speakers, speak English haltingly and with a strong accent
876 Babak Ghasemi and Masoud Hashemi / Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 28 (2011) 872 – 876
2. Conclusion
English language should be more than one course and it can be used as a scientific language and other training
courses.For example, science courses, history, social or biology should be taught in English. In this way, power and
speed of learning the language significantly increases. Primary school and children is the best course to learn the
language. Because learning the language with native language in the first decade of life provides for disabled
students to independently and directs language without translation, interpretation and changes its meaning from
Persian to learn English in your mind. This means that the person will be able to speak like native language without
including Persian to English in your mind . Early childhood is the best time for language acquisition. Ease of
learning a foreign language diminishes with age. Between birth and adolescence the brain is hard-wired to acquire
language naturally. As child approaches puberty, the nature of language learning and storage changes, becoming less
flexible. Many experts believe that learning the language before the age of ten years allow children to speak correct
and fluent as an indigenous person. Therefore, whatever the earlier children become familiar with foreign language,
he have better chance to speak proficiency. On the other hand language learning, except native language, can
provide develop a lifelong ability to more communicate with others.
References
Bloch, C., & Edwards, V. (1999). Young children’s literacy in multilingual classrooms: Comparing developments in South Africa and the UK. In
L. Limage (Ed.), Comparative perspectives on language and literacy: Selected papers from the work of the Language and Literacy
Commission on the 10th World Congress of Comparative Education Societies. Dakar: UNESCO/BREDA.
Ferreira, F., & Morrison, F. J. (1994). Children’s metalinguistic knowledge of syntactical constituents: Effects of age and schooling.
Developmental Psychology, 30, 663-678.
Hart, B., & Risley, T.R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H.
Brookes.
Harrison, B., & Papa, R. (2005). The development of an indigenous knowledge programme in a New Zealand Maori-language immersion school.
Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 36(1), 57-72.
Honig, A. S. (n.d.). Raising a bilingual child. Retrieved January 21, 2005, from
www.scholastic.com/earlylearner/experts/language/0_2_bilingualch.htm
Pusack, J. P., & Otto, S. K. (1990). Applying Instructional technologies. Foreign Language Annals, 23(5), 409-417.