Lynn PDF
Lynn PDF
Lynn PDF
Intelligence
a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t
Article history: Regional differences in IQ are presented for 12 regions of Italy showing that IQs are highest in
Received 13 January 2009 the north and lowest in the south. Regional IQs obtained in 2006 are highly correlated with
Received in revised form 27 July 2009 average incomes at r = 0.937, and with stature, infant mortality, literacy and education. The
Accepted 27 July 2009
lower IQ in southern Italy may be attributable to genetic admixture with populations from the
Available online 19 August 2009
Near East and North Africa.
© 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords:
IQ
Income
Infant mortality
Stature
Education
Italy
0160-2896/$ – see front matter © 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.intell.2009.07.004
94 R. Lynn / Intelligence 38 (2010) 93–100
Latitude
determinant of regional differences in economic develop-
46.0
46.0
45.5
43.5
45.0
45.0
44.5
44.5
43.0
41.5
41.0
40.5
40.0
40.0
39.0
37.0
ment in Italy and western Europe.
Years
2001
educ
1.1. IQ and income: individuals
9.0
8.9
8.8
8.6
8.6
9.0
8.7
8.7
9.4
8.5
8.2
8.0
8.2
8.0
8.0
–
In this paper it is proposed that regional differences in
Years
1971
educ
5.7
5.7
5.3
5.2
5.5
5.9
5.2
4.9
5.8
4.6
4.7
4.5
4.6
4.5
4.5
intelligence are the major factor responsible for the regional
–
differences in Italy in per capita income and in the related
Years
1951
educ
45.9
45.9
38.1
63.0
67.8
55.5
52.2
28.6
41.8
18.1
24.6
20.0
19.1
14.6
19.1
duals, across regions within nations, and across nations. At
–
2.50
3.47
3.17
3.24
3.61
3.86
4.05
3.73
3.76
4.56
5.21
5.88
4.10
5.54
6.62
Inequality. In this he estimated a correlation of 0.31 (corrected
–
38.3
44.9
36.7
35.2
45.4
40.8
36.2
39.8
68.1
62.2
70.4
53.6
57.0
117.5
20,750
23,079
20,338
19,666
22,639
20,519
20,000
22,439
17,070
20,207
15,480
11,862
12,030
13,722
11,595
12,488
9223
9517
7815
6814
6481
6313
8054
6128
6525
10,930
10,022
11,693
10,964
10,058
10,317
1970
178.0
177.1
177.0
175.8
175.2
175.3
175.1
175.4
175.8
175.5
174.0
173.1
173.3
171.6
172.4
172.7
1980
171.7
169.2
169.0
169.8
168.1
168.9
169.7
169.2
167.2
167.6
164.2
164.9
164.3
162.1
163.3
164.7
1927
167.4
167.1
166.2
166.2
167.0
167.7
166.6
164.6
164.6
162.8
162.9
163.1
160.6
162.2
163.3
1910
165.3
164.8
163.7
162.5
162.1
163.2
163.4
161.9
161.8
159.6
160.2
159.7
158.5
158.3
160.2
1855
92
90
91
90
89
103
101
101
100
100
100
IQ
–
–
492
502
481
500
447
439
441
438
427
–
–
–
that have shown that siblings with higher IQs have higher
earnings than their lower IQ brothers and sisters (Bound,
Science
499
508
488
510
451
442
447
449
433
–
–
–
513
508
510
487
492
473
494
443
436
435
429
423
–
–
491
506
483
496
446
438
440
435
424
goods and services with greater value than those with lower
–
–
–
Puglia Arulia
Lombardy
Campania
Piedmont
Trentino
Sardinia
Tuscany
Calabria
Umbria
Liguria
Table 1
Sicily
Lazio
reporting this have been published for the British Isles, duals, and populations with higher IQs can supply goods and
France, and the United States. The first of these studies was services with greater value than those with lower IQs, and
concerned with IQ differences in 13 regions of the British Isles hence command higher incomes.
in the mid-twentieth century (Lynn, 1979). It was found that In this paper we examine the possibility that the north–
the highest IQ (102.1) was in London, and the lowest IQs in south difference in per capita income in Italy may be due to
Scotland (97.3), Northern Ireland (96.7), and the Republic of differences in intelligence. There is some existing evidence
Ireland (96.0). These regional IQs were positively correlated suggesting this may be the case. In northern Italy, Prunetti
with per capita income at 0.73. They were also positively (1985) has reported a standardization of the Colored
correlated with intellectual achievement indexed by fellow- Progressive Matrices on 500 6–11 year olds in Pisa and the
ship of the Royal Society (r = 0.94), and negatively with surrounding countryside, and Tesi and Young (1962) have
infant mortality (r = −0.78) (Lynn, 1979). It has been shown reported a standardization of the Standard Progressive
subsequently that these regional differences in IQ are strongly Matrices on 2462 11–16 year olds in Florence and the sur-
associated negatively with differences in stature (Boldsen & rounding countryside. Both studies found that the mean IQ in
Mascie-Taylor, 1985). northern Italy is approximately the same as in Britain and
Similar results have been found in France, where regional other countries of northern and central Europe. It has not
differences in intelligence were reported for the mid-1950s proved possible to find normative data for IQs in the south of
by Montmollin (1958). IQs were obtained from 257,000 Italy. However, Peluffo (1962, 1964, 1967) has reported that
18 year old male conscripts into the armed forces, and mean the cognitive development of children in southern Italy and
IQs were given for the 90 French departments. The highest Sardinia (one of the poorest regions and part of the south)
IQs were obtained by conscripts from the Paris region and the lags behind that of children in Genoa in northern Italy and in
lowest by conscripts from Corsica. As in the British Isles, it Switzerland in the performance of Piagetian tasks of the
was shown that these departmental IQs were moderately understanding of conservation and causality. For example,
well positively correlated with average earnings (r = 0.61), 65% of 9 year olds in Genoa succeeded in the conservation of
with intellectual achievement indexed by membership of volume task, compared with only 35% of 9 year olds in the
the Institut de France (r = 0.26), and negatively with infant south. Piagetian tasks can be regarded as tests of intelligence.
mortality (r = 0.30) (Lynn, 1980). A correlation between the two of 0.49 is reported by Jensen
An association between regional IQ and per capita income (1980, p. 674) as the average of 14 studies.
has also been reported in the United States. It has long been The present paper examines three hypotheses. First, that
known that in the United States the populations of the northern IQs in Italy are higher in the north than in the south. Second,
states have higher average IQs than those of the south east that these IQ differences explain most of the per capita
(Kaufman, McClean, & Reynolds 1988). This has been con- income differences. Third, that regional IQ differences in Italy
firmed by McDaniel (2006a) who has calculated the IQs of the are also manifest in variables that can be regarded as
populations of the American states and found that these are correlates or effects of IQs, including stature, infant mortality,
highest in the north eastern states of Massachusetts (104.3), literacy, and years of education.
New Hampshire (104.2) and Vermont (103.8), and lowest in
the southern states of Mississippi (94.2) and Alabama (95.7), 2. Method
and in California (95.5). The McDaniel (2006a) average state
IQs are positively correlated with gross state product per capita Data have been assembled for 12 Italian regions for mean IQ,
(a measure of per capita income) at Pearson's r = 0.28. These average per capita income in euros for 1970 and 2003 given by
state differences in average IQ are partly determined by the the Italian Statistical Office (2008), percentages of the popula-
proportions of blacks and Hispanics, who have lower average tions that were literate in 1880, taken from Tabellini (2007,
IQs than Europeans at approximately 85, 89, and 100, 2009), statures of military conscripts born in 1855, 1910, 1927
respectively (Lynn, 2006). McDaniel (2006a) calculated that and 1980, taken from A'Hearn, Peracchi, and Vecchi (2009) and
state IQs are correlated at −0.51 with the percentage of blacks Arcaleni (2006), infant mortality 1955–57 and 1999–2002,
and −0.34 with the percentage of Hispanics. Similar state taken from Felici (2007), years of education in 1951, 1971 and
differences in IQ using a different methodology have been 2001, taken from Felici (2007), and latitude taken as the
reported by Kanazawa (2006). The different methodologies are approximate geographical mid-point of the regions.
discussed by McDaniel (2006b). The regional IQs have been calculated from the 2006 PISA
At a third level of generality, positive correlations between (Program for International Student Assessment) study of
IQ and per capita income have been reported across nations at reading comprehension, mathematical ability, and science
a magnitude of approximately 0.7 (Lynn & Vanhanen, 2002, understanding administered to 15 year olds in 52 countries
2006). This finding has been confirmed in studies that have (OECD, 2007). Scores on these tests are used as a proxy for
re-examined the data using alternative measures of per capita IQs, adopting the procedure that Rindermann (2007, 2008)
income (Barber, 2005; Dickerson, 2006; Whetzell & McDa- has used for nations. The PISA reading test is a measure of
niel, 2006; Templer & Arikawa, 2006; Hunt & Wittmann, verbal comprehension and the mathematics test is a measure
2008; Gelade, 2008), and by studies that have used national of “quantitative reasoning”, and both of these are major
scores in math, science, and literacy as proxies for intelligence components of general intelligence (e.g. Carroll, 1993, p. 597;
(Rindermann, 2007, 2008; Hunt & Wittmann, 2008). The McGrew & Flanagan, 1998, p. 14–15), while science under-
positive correlation between IQ and per capita income across standing is highly correlated with general intelligence (e.g. at
populations is to be expected from the correlation among 0.68 in the study by Deary, Strand, Smith, & Fernandes, 2007).
individuals, because populations are aggregates of indivi- More generally, numerous studies have shown that tests of
96 R. Lynn / Intelligence 38 (2010) 93–100
Infant mortality Infant mortality Literacy Years educ Years educ Years educ
gence at around 0.5 to 0.7, reviewed in Lynn and Mikk (2007),
0.726
2001
and sometimes more highly, e.g. at 0.81 in a recent study of
70,000+ English children whose IQs were measured at the
age of 11 years and educational achievement was measured
at the age of 16 years (Deary et al., 2007). It has been shown
0.908
0.802
1971
that there is a strong genetic correlation between cognitive
ability measured by tests of intelligence and educational, i.e.
the same genes determine ability measured in both kinds of
0.965
0.862
0.888
test (Bartels, Rietveld, van Baal, & Boomsma, 2002; Petrill &
1951
Wilkerson, 2000). These are designated “generalist genes” by
Kovas, Harlaar, Petrill and Plomin (2005) because they
0.924
0.863
0.689
0.842
1880
determine many expressions of cognitive ability including
IQs, math, reading, science, etc. The terms “intelligence” and
“IQ” are used in this paper in the sense of the sum of all
cognitive abilities or global IQ, as measured by intelligence
1999–02
tests such as the Wechslers and the Binets. The PISA tests
− 0.642
− 0.765
− 0.750
− 0.868
− 0.897
measure some mix of g (Spearman's g, the general factor
present in all cognitive abilities), gf (fluid intelligence or
reasoning ability) and gc (comprehension/knowledge) but it
is not considered possible to quantify the contributions of
1955–57
these three factors to the PISA scores.
0.670
0.876 − 0.661
0.936 − 0.631
0.877 − 0.642
0.850 − 0.716
0.899 − 0.676
To calculate IQs for the Italian regions the scores on
reading comprehension, mathematic ability, and science
Income
2003
been expressed in standard deviation unit deviations from
the British PISA mean (502, SD = 99). This gives scores for the
0.934
0.902
0.889
0.864
0.774
0.798
Income
Italian regions expressed in standard deviation units in 1970
0.604
0.821
0.666
0.830
0.788
0.796
0.875
Stature
0.919
0.782
0.842
0.807
0.894
0.858
0.761
0.870
Stature
1927
3. Results
0.965
0.868
0.843
0.841
0.875
0.901
0.831
0.685
0.874
Stature
1910
0.748
0.820
0.782
0.721
0.863
Stature
0.861
0.929
0.871
0.886
15 year olds on reading comprehension, mathematics and 0.963
science understanding for the Italian regions obtained in the
Mean educ IQ
0.820
0.893
0.860
0.878
0.956
Science
0.829
0.899
0.855
0.880
0.961
the first year of life) per 1000 births for 1955–1957 and 1999–
2002 (Felici, 2007). Column 15 gives the percentage of the
0.993
0.993
0.997
0.993
0.918
0.906
0.926
0.936
0.729
0.914
− 0.844
Inf Mrt 1999–02 − 0.867
0.863
0.922
0.883
0.886
0.970
Latitude
Science
Math
Laycock, F., & Caylor, J. S. (1965). Physiques of gifted children and their less Peracchi, F. (2008). Stature and economic development in Italy. American
gifted siblings. Child Development, 35, 63–74. Economic Review, 98, 475–481.
Lutz, V. (1962). Italy: A study in economic development Oxford: Oxford Petrill, S. A., & Wilkerson, B. (2000). Intelligence and achievement: A behav-
University Press. ioural genetics perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 12, 185–199.
Lynn, R. (1979). The social ecology of intelligence in the British Isles. British Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & McClearn, G. E. (1990). Behavioral genetics New York:
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 18, 1–12. Freeman.
Lynn, R. (1980). The social ecology of intelligence in the France. British Prunetti, C. (1985). Dati normative del test PM47 colored sur un calpioni di
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19, 325–331. bambini italiani. Bollettino di Psicologia Applicata, 176, 27–35.
Lynn, R. (1990). The role of nutrition in secular increases of intelligence. Putnam, R. D. (1993). Making democracy work Princeton, NJ: Princeton
Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 273–285. University Press.
Lynn, R. (2006). Race differences in intelligence: An evolutionary analysis Rindermann, H. (2007). The g-factor of international cognitive ability com-
Athens, GA: Washington Summit Publishers. parisons: The homogeneity of results in PISA, TIMMS, PIRLS and IQ tests
Lynn, R., & Mikk, J. (2007). National differences in intelligence and across nations. European Journal of Personality, 21, 667–706.
educational attainment. Intelligence, 35, 115–121. Rindermann, H. (2008). Relevance of education and intelligence at the national
Lynn, R., & Vanhanen, T. (2002). IQ and the wealth of nations Westport, CT: level for the economic welfare of people. Intelligence, 36, 127–142.
Praeger. Rowe, D., Vesterdal, W., & Rodgers, J. (1999). Herrnstein's syllogism: Genetic
Lynn, R., & Vanhanen, T. (2006). IQ and global inequality Athens, GA: and shared environmental influences on IQ, education and income.
Washington Summit Publishers. Intelligence, 26, 405–423.
Marston, E. (2002). The Phoenicians New York: Benchmark Books. Rushton, J. P., & Čvorović, J. (2009). Data on the Raven's Standard Progressive
M.M.C. (1960). Sicily. Encyclopedia Britannica Chicago: William Benton. Matrices from four Serbian samples. Personality and Individual Differences,
McDaniel, M. A. (2006). State preferences for the ACT versus SAT complicates 46, 483–486.
inferences about SAT-derived state IQ estimates: A comment on Kanazawa Savage, S. W. (1946). Intelligence and infant mortality in problem families.
(2006). Intelligence, 34, 601–606. British Medical Journal, 19, 86–87 Jan.
McDaniel, M. A. (2006). Estimating state IQ: Measurement challenges and Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection
preliminary correlates. Intelligence, 34, 607–619. methods in psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of
McGrew, K. S., & Flanagan, D. (1998). The intelligence test desk reference 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 262–274.
Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Semino, O., Passarino, G., Brega, A., Fellous, M., & Santachiara-Benerecetti, A. S.
Mitchell, R. J., Earl, L., & Fricke, B. (1997). Y-chromosome specific alleles and (1996). A view of the Neolithic demic diffusion in Europe through two Y
haplotypes in European and Asian populations: Linkage disequilibrium chromosome-specific markers. American Journal of Human Genetics, 59,
and geographic diversity. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 104, 964–968.
167–176. Stoddard, G. D. (1943). The meaning of intelligence New York: Macmillan.
Montmollin, M. (1958). Le niveau intellectuel des recrues du contingent. Strenze, T. (2007). Intelligence and socioeconomic success: A meta-analytic
Population, 13, 259–268. review of longitudinal research. Intelligence, 35, 401–426.
Murray, C. (1997). IQ and economic success. Public Interest, 128, 21–35. Tabellini, G. (2007). Personal communication.
Murray, C. (1998). Income Inequality and IQ. Washington, D.C.: AEI Press. Tabellini, G. (2009). Culture and institutions: Economic development in the
Murray, C. (2002). IQ and income inequality in a sample of sibling pairs from regions of Europe. Journal of the European Economic Association, Papers
advantaged family backgrounds. American Economics Association: Papers and Proceedings (to appear).
and Proceedings, 92(2), 339–343. Templer, D. I., & Arikawa, H. (2006). Temperature, skin color, per capita
Murray, C. (2003). Human accomplishment New York: Harper Collins. income, and IQ: An international perspective. Intelligence, 34, 121–139.
Neal, D. A., & Johnson, W. R. (1996). The role of premarket factors in black- Tesi, G., & Young, B. H. (1962). A standardisation of Raven's Progressive Matrices.
white wage differences. Journal of Political Economy, 104, 98–123. Archive de Psicologia Neurologia e Psichiatrica, 5, 455–464.
Ng, T. W. H., Eby, L. T., Sorensen, K. L., & Feldman, D. C. (2005). Predictors of Toniolo, G. (1990). An economic history of liberal Italy, 1850–1918 New
objective and subjective career success: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, York: Routledge.
58, 367–408. Whetzell, D., & McDaniel, M. A. (2006). Prediction of national wealth. Intelligence,
OECD (Organization for Economic Co-operation & Development). (2007). 34, 449–458.
PISA 2006 competencies for tomorrow's world Paris: OECD. Zagorosky, J. L. (2007). Do you have to be smart to be rich? The impact of IQ
Paterson, D. G. (1930). Physique and intellect New York: Century. on wealth, income and financial distress. Intelligence, 35, 489–501.
Peluffo, N. (1962). Les notions de conservation et de causalité chez les Zalloua, P. A., Platt, D. E., El Sibai, M., & Khalife, J. (2008). Identifying genetic
enfants provenant de différents milieux physiques et socioculturels. traces of historical expansions: Phoenician footprints in the Mediterra-
Archives de Psychologie, 38, 275–291. nean. American Journal of Human Genetics, 83, 633–642.
Peluffo, N. (1964). La nozione di conservazione del volume e le operazioni di Zax, J. S., & Rees, D. I. (2002). IQ, academic performance, environment and
combinazione come indice di sviluppo del pensiero operatorio in soggetti earnings. Review of Economics and Statistics, 84, 600–614.
appartenenti ad ambienti fisici e socioculturali diversi. Rivista di Psicologia Zetterberg, J., 2004. The impact of cognitive and non-cognitive ability on
Sociale, 11(99-1), 32. earnings – Swedish evidence. Unpublished.
Peluffo, N. (1967). Culture and cognitive problems. International Journal of
Psychology, 2, 187–198.