Finkelhor1986 Risk Factors Child Sexual Abuse-Dikonversi
Finkelhor1986 Risk Factors Child Sexual Abuse-Dikonversi
Finkelhor1986 Risk Factors Child Sexual Abuse-Dikonversi
persons from various back grounds to experience sexual abuse during childhood.
Interest ingl'f, they are Haiti y uni Joim in failing to find differences in rates according
Io social class or race. How cues, s‹nieral other factors haue emerged from communit y
studies as being consistently associated uiitfi higher risk/or a6use. (a) when a child
lives u›ithout one oJ the biological parents; (b) when the mother is unauailab le to the
child either as a result of employment outside the home or disabilit y and illness; (c)
u'hen the chtld reports that the parents’ marriage is un happy or conJliclual; (d) when
the chiId reports hauing a poor relationshi p ui ith the parents or being sub)ect to
extremely punitive discipline or child abuse; (e) ui hen the child reports having a
stepfather. The article drams some implications Jiom these 'findings and makes
recotnmetidations for uiays to improve subsequent studies of risk factors.
Authors’ Note: This research has been made possible by a grant from the
National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect (90CA 0936/01). We would also like
to thank Linda Gott and Ruth Miller for help in preparing the manuscript. This is
one of a series of papers on sexual abuse and family violence available from the
Family Violence Research Program at the University of New Hampshire.
43
THE SURVEYS
Sex
Virtually all studies that have included men as well as women have
found higher abuse rates for women. The differences range from more
than 4 women for each man in a Minnesota phone survey (Murphy,
1985) to 1.5 women for each man in one of the more obsc:ure student
48 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / March 1986
Age af Onse t
Do
wn Swdies 1 2 3 4 5 6 Z 8 9 10 11 12 I3 l4 I5 l6 l7 IB
lo
ad
ed Finkelhor (1979) (17 (271 (19)
fro
m (N — 530) .06 1.06 1.06 1.69 1.69 1.69 3.52 3.52 3.52 .89 .89 .89 .89
jiv.
sa
Finkelhor (1984)
ge (1 ) (6) (5) (9) (2) (4) (12) (9) (6) (4) (5) (2)
pu (N = 334)
b. .29 1.79 1.49 2.69 .59 1.19 3.59 2.69 1.79 1.19 1.49 .59
co
m Fromuth (198 3)
at
(1 ) (15) (2 7) 72 (24)
(N = 482)
G .06 .06 .06 1.03 1.03 1.03 *1.86 1.86 1.86 4.97 4.97 4.97 1.24 1.24 1.24 1.24
E
O Keckley (1983)
R (31 ) (9) (2)
G (N — 320)
E .43 .43 .43 .43 .43 1.93 1 93 1 9 1 3 1.93 .70 .70 .70 .70 .1B .15 .15 US
M Lewis (1985)
A 53 (34) (41 ) (26) (56) (30) (53) (39) (15) (8) (2 3)
S (N = 1374)
O .64 .64 .64 .64 .64 ,64 2.47 2.98 1.89 4.07 2.18 3.85 2.18 1.09 .58 .55 .55 .55
N
Russell (1983)
U (2) (3) (1 4) (15) (26) (22 T (23) (23) (42) (31 ) (7 6) (105) (42) (65) (81 ) (70)
NI (N — 930)
V .2 1 .32 1.50 1.61 2.79 2.36 2.47 2.47 4.51 3.33 8.17 11.29 4.5 1 6.98 8,70 7.52
on
De Summated
ce
average
m
be
.37 .33 .36 .82 1.09 1.49 2.16 1.92 1.83 3.76 2.89 3.83 2.91 1.68 1.88 2.02 2.74 .35
50 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / March 1986
Social Class
Child abuse in general has been thought to be more prevalent in
lower social classes. But child welfare advocates have generally found
this a politically unappealing stereotype and have tried to emphasize
the fact that child abuse occurs in all social strata. For some writers,
this emphasis has turned into a claim that child abuse is unrelated to
social class. Empirical studies have repeatedly undercut these claims,
however, finding strong associations between class and physical
abuse (Pelton, 1981; Straus, Gelles, & Steinmetz, 1980). Although
abuse is certainly not limited to the lower class as the stereotype might
suggest, to most researchers it makes sense that the frustrations of
poverty, joblessness, lack of education, and inadequate housing
would contribute to the conditions that increase violence toward
children.
Child sexual abuse, however, may be an entirely different story.
The most representative surveys of child sexual abuse in the com-
Ethnicity
Across the board, studies have consistently failed to find any black-
white differences in rates of sexual abuse. Even among reported cases,
where it is thought that blacks suffer from a labeling bias (O’Toole,
Turbott, & Nalepka, 1983), the percentage o1 black cases is no more
than the percentage of blacks in the population as a whole (NCCAN,
1981). In four community surveys (Keckley Market Research, 1983;
Kercher, & McShane, 1984; Russell, 1983; Wyatt, 1984), rates among
whites and blacks have been very similar. In the only survey showing
any black preponderance (Miller, 1976)—l8Po for blacks and 139c for
whites—the difference disappeared when controlling for city size and
other demographic factors.
Among reported cases, sexual abuse has consistently been the
type of abuse for which blacks have the lowest representation. In
the American Humane Association national records (Trainor,
1984), for example, black families accounted for 15.39» of sexual
abuse cases, almost exactly their proportion in the American
population. In the National Incidence Study (NCCAN, 1981), the
percentage of blacks was ll%, somewhat below their representation
in the population. This is especially significant considering that
discrimination, pover- ty, and stereotyping usually influence
professionals to more readily label black families as abusive.
Wyatt’s (1984) study, based on a community survey, and the
related follow-up by Peters (1984) were specifically designed to
look at possi- ble black-white differences, and were remarkable in
the similarities they found. Wyatt (1984) showed no overall
difference in rates or types of abuse, and Peters (1984) found few
differences either in risk factors or effects. The studies did show
that because of different family patterns in the black community,
certain factors associated with vulnerability in the white
community—such as separation from father, changes in caretakers,
and mother’s labor force participation— did not operate in the
black community (Peters, 1984). However, overall the studies were
remarkable in the similarities that they showed between sexual
abuse in the black and white communities.
Although studies have found no black-white differences, there
are hints of possible differences for other ethnic group. Kercher
and
54 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / March 1986
Social Isolation
Social isolation is a risk factor of interest to sexual abuse
researchers for two reasons. First, there are anecdotal reports of
concentrations of sexually abusive families in rural areas (Summit &
Kryso, 1978). Second, social isolation has proven to be correlated in
some research with other forms of child abuse and neglect (Garbarino
& Stocking, 1980).
However, rural residence has been associated with more sexual
abuse in only one study, and in two it has actually been associated
with less. Finkelhor (1984) found a significantly high rate (449o)
among the college student women who had grown up on a farm.
Russell (1983) also found rates of incestuous abuse somewhat higher
among women from both rural (l7$o) and town (209») backgrounds,
compared to city-raised women (149c), but the relationship did not
reach statistical significance. On the other hand, Miller’s (1976)
results were completely opposite, with adolescents from farm com-
munities reporting the lowest rates (109c) compared to urban (l59o)
and suburban (l99o) areas. Wyatt (1985a) also found higher rates
among women raised in urban areas. So the idea that sexual abuse is
more common in rural areas seems doubtful.
However, another aspect of social isolation has been empirically
confirmed by three studies: sexual abuse victims appear to be isolated
among their peers. Finkelhor (1984) found that women with two or
fewer friends at age 12 had more experiences of sexual abuse.
Fromuth
Finkelhor, Baron / CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE 65
(1983) confirmed this in her student sample with the exact same
question. Peters (1984) found higher rates associated with women
who reported either lack of closeness with peers or lack of
closeness with siblings. (The peers were a better predictor with
white women and the siblings with black women). In fact, lack of
closeness with siblings was one of three variables that best
predicted sexual abuse in Peters’s multivariate analysis of all risk
factors.
A plausible hypothesis is that social isolation is related to abuse. If
children have few friends, this may create a need for contact and
friendship that sexual abusers can capitalize on. Friendless children
may be easy marks. However, the friendlessness found in three studies
may, unfortunately, be the result of having been victimized rather
than a risk factor. Children who are being abused by family members
are often prohibited from having friends. Children who are feeling
shame and stigma as a result of having suffered victimization often
isolate themselves from others. At age 12, the year asked about in both
the Finkelhor and Fromuth studies, more than half of the respondents
had already suffered abuse. This casts doubt on whether friendlessness
was truly a risk factor. Social isolation may indeed be a risk factor for
sexual victimization, but we need to find other, more sensitive vari-
ables with which to measure it.
Finkelhor, Fromuth, and Peters all found that girls who had ever
lived without their natural father were at higher risk, although the
Peters finding applied only to white women. Russell (198$) also
found higher rates among girls living with their biological mother
and no father, but the finding applied only to extrafamily abuse.
Similarly, Miller (1976) found elevated risk for adolescents who
were currently living without their father or without both natural
parents. Bagley and Ramsay (in press) found that any separation
from either parent for a period of six months or more before the
age of 16 was correlated with the likelihood of being a victim. This
is an impressive number of studies with positive findings on the
question of parental absence.
Another variable that in some of its forms might be related to
parental absence is the mother’s employment outside the home. At
least four studies have found this related to sexual abuse (Fromuth,
1983; Landis, 1956; Peters, 1984; Russell, 1983). Peters found that
the relationship held only for white women, and Russell, whose
findings were short of statistical significance for the whole sample,
found that mother’s employment increased risk significantly in
households with stepfathers. Finkelhor (1984) did not find any risk
for mother’s employment.
Parental unavailability may also be indicated by a disabled or ill
parent. Finkelhor (1984) found higher rates of sexual victimization
among girls who reported that their mothers were often ill. Peters
(1984) reported somewhat higher rates (only at p = .10) for girls
with one parent who was disabled by substance addiction,
emotional problems, or physical ailments. Looking specifically at
father- daughter incest families, Herman and Hirschman (1981)
found signif- icantly more mothers who were seriously ill (most
commonly due to alcoholism, depression, or psychosis) and also
mothers who were burdened with many children and pregnancies.
However, Fromuth (1983), using the same variables as Finkelhor,
did not confirm any association between sexual abuse and mother’s
illness.
Stepfather Families
Besides parental absence, conflict, and lack of support, another
parental factor—the presence of a nonbiologically related father—has
been widely discussed as a risk factor for sexual abuse. Out of six
studies examining this factor, four have confirmed its significance.
Of these studies, three—Miller (1976), Finkelhor (1980), and Gruber
and Jones (1983)—found that having a stepfather increased a girl’s
risk for all types of sexual abuse. Russell (1983) found an increased
risk for father-daughter abuse only. Fromuth (1983) and Peters (1984)
did not find any increased risk for any type of abuse. Although most of
these studies, as mentioned earlier, indicate that simple father absence
is a risk factor, both Finkelhor (1984) and Gruber and Jones ( 1983)
Finkelhor, Baron / CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE 61
to confirm and even specify the risk. One caution about some of
the findings reported here is that they may be historically outdated.
They are based mostly on studies of women who grew up before
the 1970s, when fewer children had stepfathers. It may be very
different to live in a stepfather family in an era when there are few
such families, com- pared to the current era when such families are
much more common- place. Assuming that this vulnerability does
show up even in studies of current cohorts of children, it would
support an important new direction for social policy. A case might
be made that families con- taining stepfathers should be the target
of sexual abuse prevention efforts.
TABLE 2
Risk Factors for Child Sexual Abuse: Summary of
Empirical Findings from Retrospective Surveys
Considering that the study of child sexual abuse is still in its early
stages, it is impressive that there are so many convergent findings
Finkelhor, Baron / CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE 63
Multivariate Analysis
One implication of the more complex questions that we have
proposed here is that researchers need to use more complex data
analysis techniques. Much of the research to date has limited itself
to simple bivariate relationships. As a result, it is still unknown to
what extent some of these observed risk factors are simply
expressions of other antecedent risk factors, and would disappear in
multivariate analysis. For example, it is possible that the increased
risk in step- parent families may be explained by parental
unavailability or low marital satisfaction. It is imperative that in
any future analysis of risk factors researchers take advantage of
multivariate techniques.
Sample Size
Many of the foregoing suggestions for improving research also
dictate research designs using large samples. Large samples are
needed for studies that (1) look at the risk in small subpopulations;
(2) use multivariate techniques; (3) break up the dependent variable
into subcategories of extra-familial and intra-familial abuse; and (4)
look at abuse in the various stages of childhood. The majority of
sexual abuse studies to date have had under 1,000 respondents.
New studies are needed that use samples of 2,000 or more
respondents. Such studies, of course, will be expensive, but there
are techniques for reducing survey costs and these need to be
reviewed for their applica- bility to sexual abuse research. It may be
possible, for example, to do surveys using the telephone, at least for
the identification of subjects (Wyatt, 1984). It may be possible to
combine sexual abuse research with other kinds of studies, such as
health status and crime victimi- zation surveys.
66 JOURNAL OF INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE / March 1986
abuse had occurred, even if it had never been reported. The data
gathered early in the study would give fairly unbiased information on
possible predictors of subsequent abuse.
A modified version of this design, which has advantages
because it does not take as much time, would be for investigators
to find groups of children and families of a previous generation
who had already been the subjects of study early in the family life
cycle in some other unrelated research. It might then be possible to
follow up those children (now adults) and find out whether they
had been abused during their childhood. The data gathered in the
earlier study on their families and childhoods, if it were still
available, could be used to test risk factors. It would provide a
particularly good opportunity to see if it would have been possible
to predict which children would be subsequently abused.
preconditions must exist for sexual abuse to occur: (1) There must
be an offender with the motivation to sexually abuse, (2) the
offender must overcome internal inhibitions against abusing, (3) the
offender must overcome external obstacles against abusing; and (4)
the offender must overcome resistance by the child. To “explain”
sexual abuse fully, one must account for the presence of all four of
these preconditions.
Most of the risk factors that we have been discussing in this
article refer to considerations of preconditions 3 and 4. They are
situations that may reduce the supervision of the child and thus
make it easier to overcome external obstacles to abuse or they are
conditions that under- mine a child’s emotional health and thus
their ability to resist abuse. But one of the values of the model is
that it makes clear that preconditions 3 and 4 only make a
difference after preconditions l and 2 have already been met. That
is, they are only relevant once there is an offender who is
predisposed to molest and who has overcome internal inhibitions
against doing so. Thus many children who may suffer from risk
factors at preconditions 3 and 4 will never experi-
ence abuse.
It is important that we know about risk factors at preconditions 3
and 4. Through knowledge of these factors we may be able to
design programs to help protect children and families at higher risk.
For exmaple, if children with few friends are more vulnerable,
these might be children who school personnel want to target for
special attention. But the search for these risk factors should not
obscure the fact that these are not the ones that cause abuse. Our
ult•rriate goal should be not simply to protect high risk children
from abuse but to eliminate abusive behavior itself.
CONCLUSION
analysis are also well developed. Such studies become even more
important as we develop techniques of prevention education. They
may mean that we can better target our education at the child popula-
tion at greater risk. It is an area where our research should soon bear
results in terms of achieving concrete reductions in the number of
sexually abused children.
NOTES
REFERENCES
American Humane Association. (1981). National stud y on child neglect arid abuse
reporting. Denver: American Humane Association.
Badgley, R., Allard, H., McCormick, N., Proudfoot, P., For tin, D., Ogilvie, D., Rae-
C rant, Q., Gelinas, P., Pepin, L., & Sutherland, S. [Committee on Sexual Offences
Against Children and Youth l ( 1984). Sexttof offenses against children (Vol. 1).
Ottawa: Canadian Government Publishing Centre.
Bagley, C., & Ramsay, R. (in press). Disrupted childhood and vulnerability to sexual
assault: Long-term sequels with implications for counselling. Social lYor/t and
Human Sexuality.
Bart, P. (1981). A study of women who both were raped and avoided rape. Journal oJ
Sociaf Issues, 37 (4), 123-137.
Burnam, A. (1985). Personal communication concerning the Los Angeles
Epidemio- logical Catchment Area Study.
De Francis, V. (1969). Protecting the child victim o/ sex crimes committed by adults.
Denver: American Humane Association.
Finkelhor, D. (1979). Sexual 1 y -victimized childien. New York: Free Press.
Finkelhor, Baron / CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE 71
Pelton, L. H. (Ed.) (1981). The social context oJ child abuse and neglect. New York:
Human Sciences Press.
Peters, S. D. (1984). The relationshi p between c/it/d/iood sexual victimization and
adult depression among APro-American and inhile uiomen. Unpublished Ph.D.
dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.
Peters, S. D., Wyatt, G., & Finkelhor, D. ( 1986). Prevalence: A review of the research. In
D. Finkelhor, A sourcebook on child sexual abuse. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Plummer, C. (August, 1984). Prwenting sexual abuse: What in-school programs teach
children. Paper presented at the Second National Conference for Family Violence
Researchers, Durham, NH.
Russell, D. (1983). IntiaJamil iaI child sexual abuse: A San Francisco suruey. Final
report to the National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect.
Seidner, A. L., & Calhoun, K. S. (1984, August). Childhood sexual abuse: Factors
related to differential adult ad justment. Presented at the 2nd National Conference
on Family Violence Research, Durham, NH.
Straus, M. A., Gelles, R., & Steinmetz, S. {1980). Behind closed doors: P'iolence in the
American Family. New York: Doubleday.
Summit, R., & Kryso, J. (1978). Sexual abuse of children: A clinical spectrum. Amexi-
can Journal oJ Oithopsychiatry, 48, 287-251.
Timnick, L. (1985a, August 25). 22$ in survey were child abuse victims. Los Angeles
Times, p. 1.
Timnick, L. ( 1985b, August 26). Children’s abuse reports reliable, most believe. Los
Angeles Times, p. I.
Tobias, J. L., & Gordon, R. (1977). Operation Lore. Michigan State Police. (mimeo)
Trainor, C. (1984). Sexual maltieatment in the United Slates: A fire-year perspective.
Paper presented at the International Congress on Child Abuse and Neglect,
Montreal.
Wood, S. C., & Dean, K. S. (1984). Final report: !S exual abuse of males research project.
Washington, DC: National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect (90 CA/812).
Wyatt, G. (1984). Sociocttfturaf issues in the prevention of child sexual abuse. Unpub-
lished manuscript.
Wyatt, G. (1985a). Personal communication.
Wyatt, G. E. (1985b). The sexual abuse of Afro-American and American White women
in childhood. Child Abuse and Neglect, 9, 507-519.
David Finkelhor is Assotiate Chair of the Family Research Laboratory and Associate
Director of the Pamily Violence Research Program. He has published three books on
the subject of thild sexual abuse. the most recent of which, Sourcebook on Child Sexual
Abuse, reviews current knowledge concerning prevalence, offenders, risk yactors, and
long-term effects.
Larry Baron received his Ph. D. in sociolog y from Rutgers Universit y and is a
Lecturer irt the Department of Sociolog at Yale Univeisit J. He is cuiientl y completing
a hook on tape artd its relalionsh ip to pornography, sexual inequalit , and cultural
support Jot violence in the United S tates.