Kolb Wishaw (2015)
Kolb Wishaw (2015)
Neuropsychological
Assessment
Lingering Effects of Brain Trauma
Driving home from work one afternoon, in using environmental feedback to regu-
R.L., a 32-year-old nurse and mother of late or change their behavior. One mani-
four, stopped at a red light. The vehi- festation is response inhibition: patients
cle traveling behind rear-ended her car. with frontal-lobe lesions consistently per-
R.L.’s head snapped back and struck severate on responses in various test sit-
the headrest, then the side window uations, particularly those in which the
as she bounced forward. She blacked solution demands change. The Wisconsin
out for a few minutes, but by the time Card-Sorting Test exemplifies the predic-
the emergency vehicles arrived, she ament of a patient with frontal damage.
was conscious—albeit disoriented and A subject is given a deck of cards con-
dysphasic—and experiencing severe
taining multiple copies of those repre-
pain in her back and neck from the whip- sented here. Presented with a row of
lash. Several vertebrae were damaged. four cards selected from among them,
R.L. spent about a week in the hospi- the subject’s task is to place each card
tal. Neither a CT nor an MRI scan iden- from the deck in front of the appropriate
tified any cerebral injury. Nonetheless, card in the row, sorting by one of three
evidence of closed-head trauma (dia- possible categories: color, number of el-
grammed in Figure 26.4) was abun- ements, or shape. Subjects are not told
dant. An accomplished musician, R.L. could still play the correct sorting category but only whether their re-
the piano well from memory, but she could no lon- sponses are correct or incorrect. When a subject se-
ger read music. Her oral language skills remained im- lects the correct category ten consecutive times, the
paired, and she was completely unable to read. correct solution changes unexpectedly.
R.L.’s difficulties did not abate, and she had spells Shifting response strategies is particularly difficult
of apraxia. For example, she often found herself un- for people with frontal damage. R.L. eventually per-
able to figure out how to put on her makeup; she would formed the card-sorting task, but with great difficulty.
stare at her lipstick with no idea how to use it. When Our neuropsychological evaluation revealed a woman
she came to us a year after the accident, R.L. was de- of above-average intelligence with a significant loss
pressed because the neurologists could find no reason of verbal fluency and verbal memory as well as severe
for her continuing impairments. dyslexia even a year after the accident. Nearly 20 years
As described in Section 16.3, perhaps the most com- later, R.L. is still unable to read music and reads text
monly observed trait of frontal-lobe patients is difficulty only with great difficulty.
People with closed-head traumatic brain injuries often show little or no vis-
ible sign of cerebral injury on neuroimaging but still have significant cognitive
deficits, often so severe that they cannot resume their preinjury lifestyles. For
many, the extent of neurological disorder becomes clear only from the results
of neuropsychological tests.
This fundamentally diagnostic role of neuropsychological assessment
has changed radically since its heyday in the 1980s, when clinically trained
793
794 Part V Plasticit y and Disorders
Cognitive Neuroscience
The growth of clinical neuropsychology promoted a diversification of methods
used by individual practitioners. The choice of tests varies with the disorder
being investigated and the question being asked. From the early 1950s through
the early 1990s, batteries of tests were developed, each with a different focus
(Table 28.1). Many, such as the Halstead–Reitan Battery, retain the concept
of cutoff scores, although performance below a particular level cannot always
be taken as indicative of brain damage.
Among the difficulties with cutoff scores is cerebral organization, which var-
ies with such factors as sex, handedness, age, education, culture, and life experi-
ence. Another is problem-solving strategy: test problems can be solved by using
different strategies and can thus entail different cortical regions. Symptoms of
cortical injury can be highly specific (see the case of J.I., the color-blind painter,
described in Section 13.4). Finally, because many tests require various kinds of
problem solving, we might expect task performance to vary with intelligence.
All these factors make the use of cutoff scores difficult to justify.
A serious handicap in developing test batteries was the absence of neuro
logical theory in test construction or use. Knowledge of brain function was
based largely on clinical observation, and few clinicians other than Alexander
Luria had tried to formulate a general theory of how the brain functions to pro-
duce cognition (shown in Figure 10.17).
The emergence of cognitive neuroscience in the 1990s brought a dramatic
change in the theoretical understanding of brain and cognition. Case studies
once again became popular, each directed by sophisticated cognitive theory and
assisted by structural- and functional-imaging technologies (see Shallice, 1988).
These more-cognitive approaches also use multivariate statistical methods, such
as structural equation modeling, to attempt to understand how the neural net-
works and the connectome are disrupted, both in individual cases and in groups.
Test design now incorporates this knowledge, and the cognitive approach will
continue to change neuropsychological assessment in the future.
Perhaps the greatest influence of cognitive neuroscience is in clarifying
the functions of the right frontal lobe (see a review by Stuss and Levine,
2002). Historically, the right frontal lobe proved remarkably unresponsive
to neuropsychological assessment. The combination of functional imaging
and neuropsychological test development has now led to an understanding of
the right frontal lobe’s role in formerly inaccessible functions such as social
cognition (see Section 20.6).
Managed Care
Economics is perhaps the greatest challenge faced by practicing psychologists in
recent decades (see Zillmer, 2004). With the high cost of medical care, clinicians
often face pressure to reduce the time and money spent on neuropsychological
services, which are time consuming and expensive. In particular, the perception
that medical imaging can provide faster and more accurate assessments of cere-
bral dysfunction sometimes fosters unreasonable pressure to reduce the number
of neuropsychological tests given to individual patients.
Clearly, imaging has changed the way in which neuropsychological assessment
will be used, but in head-trauma cases, as R.L. and the many examples of TBI
throughout this book demonstrate, neuropsychological assessment is often the
only way to document cognitive disturbances. Gary Groth-Marnat (1999) sug-
gests that psychologists must develop and promote assessment procedures that:
• Focus on diagnostic matters most clearly linked to treatment choice and
outcomes
• Identify conditions likely to result in cost savings
• Are time efficient
• Integrate treatment planning, progress monitoring, and outcome evaluation
Clearly, clinical assessment has to adapt if it is to survive the challenge of
health care costs. But we emphasize once more that many diagnoses remain
invisible to neuroimaging and can be reached only by careful observation. Im-
proving patient outcomes thus requires teamwork—a combination of medical
Chapter 28 Neuropsychological Assessment §28.2 797
28.2 Rationale Behind
Neuropsychological Assessment
By the 1990s, neuropsychologists had an impressive choice among tests, as sum-
marized in Table 28.1. At one end of the spectrum are standardized test bat-
teries with fixed criteria for organicity, atypical behavior assumed to have a
biological (organic) basis. Thus, organicity criteria are used to distinguish be-
havioral symptoms from those not directly related to brain pathology. These
tests have in common the advantage of straightforward administration, scoring,
and interpretation. There is little need to understand the theoretical bases
of the tests or the nuances of cerebral organization to administer them. Such
Table 28.2 Representative
understanding is necessary for interpretation, however. Examples include
tests used in the Boston
the Halstead–Reitan Battery and the Luria–Nebraska Battery. Process Approach battery
More recently, Trevor Robbins and his colleagues at the University of
Cambridge devised a computerized version of a standardized battery, the Intellectual and Conceptual Functions
Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB), that Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale IV
has the advantage of being administered in a highly structured manner (e.g., Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices
Robbins et al., 1994; 1998). CANTAB has been used extensively for research Shipley Institute of Living Scale
and increasingly in clinical practice. At last count, over 600 publications were Wisconsin Card-Sorting Test
using the CANTAB, although to date it has not been directly compared to Proverbs test
older comprehensive test batteries (see Smith et al., 2013, for a discussion). Memory Functions
At the other end of the testing spectrum are individualized test batteries Wechsler Memory Scale IV
that require particular theoretical knowledge to administer and interpret. Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
These assessments are more qualitative than quantitative. The testing of each Rey Complex-Figure Test
patient is tailored both to that person’s etiology and to the qualitative nature Benton Visual-Recognition Test
of his or her performance on each test. An example is Luria’s neurological Consonant trigrams test
approach, which is not really so much a test battery as a strategy for examin- Cowboy Story-Reading Memory Test
ing patients. (The Luria–Nebraska Battery was an attempt to make Luria’s
Language Functions
procedure more structured and quantitative, but the process made the battery
Narrative writing sample
into a completely different analysis.)
Tests of verbal fluency
Composite test batteries occupy a middle ground between the standard-
Visual-perceptual functions
ized and individualized batteries. Each test is administered in a formalized way
Cow-and-circle experimental test
and may have comparison norms, but qualitative performance and the pattern
Automobile puzzle
of test results are considered. An example is the Boston Process Approach
Parietal-lobe battery
(Table 28.2). Arthur Benton and his colleagues (Benton et al., 1983; Benton,
Hooper Visual Organization Test
1994) have described other composite batteries that can be tailored to indi-
viduals, as have Muriel Lezak et al. (2012), Pat McKenna and Elizabeth War- Academic Skills
rington (1986), William Milberg and his colleagues (1986), Freda Newcombe Wide Range Achievement Test
(1969), Aaron Smith (1981), Laughlin Taylor (1979), and Kevin Walsh (1991). Self-Control and Motor Functions
Across this spectrum, each battery is constantly changing in response to Proteus Maze Test
test revisions and developments as well as to the clinical population being Stroop Color-Word Interference Test
evaluated. One constraint on the choice of any test, however, is the clinical Luria Three-Step Motor Program
neuropsychologists’ training. The use of tests based on theory requires an Finger tapping
understanding of the theory of cerebral organization.
798 Part V Plasticit y and Disorders
Working Memory Index (WMI), and Processing Speed Index (PSI). The FSIQ
has a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15, and the manual states that for
clinical decision making, the VCI and PRI indexes now substitute for the Verbal
and Performance IQ scores (Hartman, 2009).
An advantage of the WAIS-IV is that it can be given more quickly than the
WAIS-III (about 70 versus 80 or more minutes, respectively), an important im-
provement for testing patients who might fatigue easily. A briefer test can be
given by using only the GAI, which yields a composite score based on three VCI
subtests and three PRI subtests. The GAI correlates at 0.97 with the FSIQ and
may provide a “purer” estimate of intelligence in older or disabled adults with
compromised memory and/or motor function (Hartman, 2009).
Sufficient studies have not as yet been conducted to determine the usefulness
of the five WAIS-IV indexes in neuropsychological assessment, although there
are hints that they might prove useful. For example, adults with ADHD show sig-
nificant decrements in WMI and PRI relative to matched controls (Theilling &
Petermann, 2014). Similarly, TBI patients show a specific decrement in process-
ing speed relative to neurologically healthy controls (Donders & Strong, 2014).
Although the verbal and performance scales of earlier WAIS versions were
not designed to measure left- and right-hemisphere functions, respectively, the
FSIQ core subtests have proved useful as a rough measure. FSIQ scores ob-
tained on both the VCI and PRI have a mean of 100 and a standard deviation
of 15. A difference of more than 10 points between the verbal comprehension
and the perceptual reasoning index scores is usually taken as clinically signifi-
cant, although statistically this interpretation is liberal.
The results of numerous studies on earlier editions of the Weschler Scales
demonstrate that well-defined left-hemisphere lesions produce a relatively low
verbal IQ score compared with performance score, whereas well-defined right-
hemisphere lesions produce a relatively low performance score. Diffuse damage,
on the other hand, tends to produce a low performance score, leading to the er-
roneous belief that the verbal–performance IQ difference was not diagnostically
useful. Although a reduced performance score was not definitive, study results
reveal that obtaining a relatively low verbal IQ was rare and that its appearance
should not be ignored.
Elizabeth Warrington and her colleagues (1986) evaluated the WAIS-R sub-
scales and IQ values in a retrospective study of 656 unselected patients with
unilateral brain damage. Overall, their results showed that left-hemisphere le-
sions depress verbal IQ scores, whereas right-hemisphere lesions depress per-
formance IQs. The exception in both cases is occipital lesions.
However, the verbal–performance discrepancy score was fewer than 10 points
in 53 percent of left-hemisphere cases and in 43 percent of right-hemisphere
cases. A small number of patients had discrepancy scores greater than 10 points
in the opposite direction: 6 percent of those with left-hemisphere lesions and
3 percent with right-hemisphere lesions. (It is curious that the patients with left
parietal or temporoparietal lesions did not show a large drop in IQ score, con-
sidering, presumably, that they would be dysphasic. Because language skills were
not mentioned in the Warrington study, her analysis could have excluded apha-
sic subjects. In our experience, dysphasic patients have very depressed verbal IQ
scores, as would be expected.)
Chapter 28 Neuropsychological Assessment §28.2 801
scores but not other test performance. The neuropsychological scores are linked
to an MRI developmental database.
Sports medicine is a growing area for neuropsychological assessment. Of
particular interest is tracking athletes with concussions. Alison Cernich and
colleagues (2007) describe a test battery (the Automated Neuropsychological
Assessment Metrics Sports Medicine Battery, ASMB) specifically designed for
use in concussion surveillance and management. The ASMB is currently being
refined with the development of appropriate norms and with the goal of pre-
testing athletes in sports with high incidence of concussion (for example, U.S.
football and ice hockey). This type of battery has a clear utility, given the num-
ber of athletes in professional football and university hockey who experience
lingering effects from head injuries (see Chapter 25 Portrait and Section 26.3).
28.5 Case Histories
Having surveyed the basic principles of neuropsychological theory and assess-
ment, we now apply the tests and the theory in considering the case histories
and test results of three patients. This sampling of clinical problems illustrates
the use of neuropsychological tests in neuropsychological assessment.
Because of our affiliation with the Montreal Neurological Institute, our com-
posite assessment battery is based on tests derived from the study of neuro
surgical patients by Brenda Milner, Laughlin Taylor, and their colleagues. Most
of the tests have been discussed elsewhere in the text, especially in Chapters 14
through 16 in relation to neuropsychological assessment of parietal-, temporal-,
and frontal-lobe function.
If this patient was like other patients with similar lesions, on follow-up a year
after surgery, his intelligence ratings and memory scores would likely have re-
turned to the preoperative level. His card sorting, however, would be unlikely
to show any improvement.
Case 3: Rehabilitation
This 37-year-old man had been in a traffic accident some 15 years earlier. He
was in a coma for 6 weeks and had secondary injury from brain infection. At
the time of his accident, he was a student in a graduate program in journal-
ism, having previously obtained a bachelor’s degree with honors in English
literature.
When we first met him, he had severe motor problems, used canes to walk,
and was both apraxic and ataxic. He had great difficulty in pronouncing words,
especially when hurried or stressed, but careful language testing on the Token
Test revealed no aphasic symptoms; his language problems were entirely due to
a difficulty in coordinating the muscles of the mouth (anarthria).
Since the time of his accident, this man had lived at home with his parents
and had not learned the social skills necessary to cope with his handicap. In
short, he was being treated as though he were intellectually disabled and was
806 Part V Plasticit y and Disorders
being completely looked after by his family. Indeed, the patient believed himself
to be intellectually disabled and was very reluctant to attempt rehabilitation.
At his family’s urging, we administered a thorough assessment to evaluate his
intellectual potential. The results were surprising, even to us. His intellect was
superior (WAIS verbal IQ score of 127) and although he had deficits on some
tests, especially those requiring motor skills, his performance on most tests was
average or above average. Despite his obvious motor handicaps, this man clearly
was not intellectually disabled.
One significant cognitive loss, however, was his nonverbal memory, which
was very poor. Armed with our test results, we were able to show him—and his
family—that he could look after himself and should seek occupational therapy.
He is now a chartered accountant in Canada, equivalent to a certified public
accountant (CPA) in the United States.
SUMMARY
28.1 The Changing Face of of assessment varying with the particular clinical question
Neuropsychological Assessment being asked. Analysis of test results must consider a range
Developments in functional and structural brain imaging of variables including age, sex, handedness, cultural back-
have had a significant impact on the fields of neurology and ground, IQ score, and life experience.
clinical neuropsychology. Whereas neuropsychological as- 28.3 Neuropsychological Tests and Brain Activity
sessment had promised a way to localize focal cerebral injury,
One way to validate neuropsychological tests is to measure
medical imaging techniques now have largely replaced this
brain activity as subjects perform them. Although activity in
function. But imaging cannot detect all neurological dysfunc-
the expected regions is usually enhanced, activity elsewhere
tion. Rather, the most sensitive measure of cerebral integrity
in the brain increases as well, corresponding to the wide-
is behavior, and behavioral analysis using neuropsychological
spread neural networks within the connectome that underlie
assessment can identify dysfunction invisible to MRI or CT,
cognition. Such results remind us that test performance does
especially in cases of TBI, epilepsy, and mild stroke.
not necessarily equate to focal neural anatomy.
The tests used in neuropsychological assessment and the
use of test results have changed, owing in part to the con- 28.4 The Problem of Effort
tinuing development of cognitive neuroscience and enhanced Lack of effort poses a serious problem for assessing people
neuroimaging. Tests remain useful for diagnosis and have ex- who might benefit from doing poorly on neuropsychological
panded to become integral to rehabilitation. This changing tests. If subjects are seeking compensation of some sort, for
role has economic implications as managed health care chal- instance, lack of effort invalidates the entire assessment.
lenges the cost of extensive neuropsychological evaluations, Several tests that are simple to administer can detect a lack
especially when adequate imaging data are available, regard- of effort.
less of its effectiveness.
28.5 Case Histories
28.2 Rationale Behind Despite technological advances, case histories demonstrate
Neuropsychological Assessment that neuropsychological assessment remains an important tool
A wide range of clinical neuropsychological assessment tools for demonstrating functional localization after discrete func-
is now available, the factors affecting test choice and the goals tional injury and for assisting in planning for rehabilitation.
References
Alvarez, J. A., and E. Emory. Executive function and the Benton, A. L. Neuropsychological assessment. Annual Review of
frontal lobes: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology Review Psychology 45:1–23, 1994.
16:17–42, 2006. Benton, A. L., D. de S. Hamsher, N. R. Varney, and
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical O. Spreen. Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment:
Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. Washington, D.C.: A Clinical Manual. New York: Oxford University Press,
American Psychiatric Association, 2013. 1983.
Chapter 28 Neuropsychological Assessment 807
Bigler, E. D. Response to Russell’s (2007) and Hom’s (2008) McGlone, J. Personal communication, August 2007.
commentary on “A motion to exclude and the ‘fixed’ versus McKenna, P., and E. K. Warrington. The analytical approach
‘flexible’ battery in ‘forensic’ neuropsychology.” Archives of to neuropsychological assessment. In I. Grant and K. M.
Clinical Neuropsychology 23:755–761, 2008. Adams, Eds. Assessment of Neuropsychiatric Disorders. New
Cernich, A., D. Reeves, W. Sun, and J. Bleiberg. Automated York: Oxford University Press, 1986.
neuropsychological assessment metrics sports medicine Milberg, W. P., N. Hebben, and E. Kaplan. The Boston
battery. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22:S101–S114, Process Approach to neuropsychological assessment.
2007. In I. Grant and K. M. Adams, Eds. Assessment of Neuro
Christensen, A.-L. Luria’s Neuropsychological Investigation. New psychiatric Disorders. New York: Oxford University Press,
York: Spectrum, 1975. 1986.
Christensen, A.-L., and B. P. Uzzell. International Handbook Newcombe, F. Missile Wounds of the Brain. London: Oxford
of Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. New York: Plenum, University Press, 1969.
2000. Reitan, R. M., and L. A. Davison. Clinical Neuropsychology: Cur
Donders, J., and C. A. Strong. Clinical utility of the Wechsler rent Status and Application. New York: Wiley, 1974.
Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition after traumatic Robbins, T. W., M. James, A. M. Owen, B. J. Sahakian, A. D.
brain injury. Assessment, in press, 2014. Lawrence, L. McInnes, and P. M. Rabbitt. A study of per-
Flaro, L., P. Green, and E. Robertson. Word memory test formance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to
failure 23 times higher in mild brain injury than in parents frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volun-
seeking custody: The power of external incentives. Brain teers: Implications for theories of executive functioning and
Injury 21:373–383, 2007. cognitive aging. Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Auto-
Golden, C. J. A standardized version of Luria’s neuropsycho- mated Battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychology
logical tests. In S. Filskov and T. J. Boll, Eds. Handbook of Society 4:474–490, 1998.
Clinical Neuropsychology. New York: Wiley-Interscience, Robbins, T. W., M. James, A. M. Owen, B. J. Sahakian,
1981. L. McInnes, and P. M. Rabbit. Cambridge Neuro
Green, P. The pervasive effect of effort on neuro psychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB): A factor
psychological tests. Archives of Clincial Neuropsychology analytic study of a large sample of normal elderly volun-
18:43–68, 2007. teers. Dementia 5:266–281, 1994.
Green, P., M. L. Rohling, P. R. Lees-Haley, and L. M. Allen. Ropper, A. H., and B. D. Burrell. Reaching Down the Rabbit
Effort has a greater effect on test scores than severe brain Hole. New York: St. Martin’s Press, 2014.
injury in compensation claimants. Brain Injury 15:1045– Shallice, T. From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure. Cam-
1060, 2001. bridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press, 1988.
Groth-Marnat, G. Financial efficacy of clinical assessment: Smith, A. Principles underlying human brain functions in
Rationale guidelines and issues for future research. Journal neuropsychological sequelae of different neuropathological
of Clinical Psychology 55:813–824, 1999. processes. In S. B. Filskov and T. J. Boll, Eds. Handbook
Guilmette, T. J., W. Whelihan, F. R. Sparadeo, and G. of Clinical Neuropsychology. New York: Wiley-Interscience,
Buongiorno. Validity of neuropsychological test results in 1981.
disability evaluations. Perceptual Motor Skills 78:1179–1186, Smith, P. J., A. C. Need, E. T. Cirulli, O. Chiba-Falek, and
1994. D. K. Attix. A comparison of the Cambridge Automated
Hartman, D. E. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale IV Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB) with “tra-
(WAIS‑IV): Return of the gold standard. Applied Neuro ditional” neuropsychological testing instruments. Journal
psychology 16:85–87, 2009. of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 35:319–328,
2013.
Kaplan, E. A process approach to neuropsychological
assessment. In T. Boll and B. K. Bryant, Eds. Clinical Spreen, O., and E. Strauss. A Compendium of Neuropsychological
Neuropsychology and Brain Function: Research, Development, Tests. New York: Oxford University Press, 1991.
and Practice, pp. 129–167. Washington, D.C.: American Stuss, D. T., and B. Levine. Adult clinical neuropsychology:
Psychological Association, 1988. Lessons from studies of the frontal lobes. Annual Review of
Kimura, D., and J. McGlone. Neuropsychology Test Procedures. Psychology 53:401–433, 2002.
Manual used at the University Hospital, London, Ontario, Taylor, L. B. Psychological assessment of neurosurgical
Canada, 1983. patients. In T. Rasmussen and R. Marino, Eds. Functional
Lepach, A. C., M. Daseking, F. Petermann, and H. C. Wald- Neurosurgery. New York: Raven Press, 1979.
mann. The relationships of intelligence and memory Theiling, J., and F. Petermann. Neuropsychological profiles on
assessed using the WAIS-IV and the WMS-IV (article in the WAIS-IV of ADHD adults. Journal of Attention Disor
German). Gesundheitswesen 75:775–781, 2013. ders in press, 2014.
Lezak, M. D., D. B. Howieson, B. Diane, E. D. Bigler, and Waber, D. P., C. De Moor, P. W. Forbes, R. Almli,
D. Tranel. Neuropsychological Assessment, 5th ed. New York: K. N. Botteron, G. Leonard, D. Milovan, T. Paus,
Oxford University Press, 2012. and J. Rumsey. The NIH MRI study of normal brain
808 Part V Plasticit y and Disorders
development: Performance of a population based sample Zillmer, E. A. National Academy of Neuropsychology: Presi-
of healthy children aged 6 to 18 years. Journal of the Inter dent’s address. The future of neuropsychology. Archives of
national Neuropsychological Society 13:1–18, 2007. Clinical Neuropsychology 19:713–724, 2004.
Walsh, K. W. Understanding Brain Damage, 2nd ed. London: Zillmer, E. A., and M. V. Spiers. Principles of Neuropsychology.
Churchill Livingstone, 1991. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, 2001.
Warrington, E. K., M. James, and C. Maciejewski. The WAIS Zillmer, E. A., M. V. Spiers, and W. C. Culbertson. Prin
as a lateralizing and localizing diagnostic instrument: A ciples of Neuropsychology, 2nd ed. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth,
study of 656 patients with unilateral cerebral excisions. Neu 2008.
ropsychologia 24:223–239, 1986.
Wilson, R. S., G. Rosenbaum, and G. Brown. The problem of
premorbid intelligence in neuropsychological assessment.
Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology 1:49–56, 1979.