The Dark Side of Luxury Consumption: Psychological and Social Consequences of Using Luxury Goods

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 156

THE DARK SIDE OF LUXURY CONSUMPTION:

PSYCHOLOGICAL AND SOCIAL CONSEQUENCES OF USING LUXURY


GOODS

A DISSERTATION
SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF
UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA
BY

YAJIN WANG

IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS


FOR THE DEGREE OF
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

DEBORAH ROEDDER JOHN, ADVISER

JUNE 2015

i
© Copyright by Yajin Wang 2015

ii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The completion of this dissertation would not have been possible without the help of

several important people. First, I thank my parents and my grandma. I learned so much

about life from them, and their support was unwavering.

I also thank the faculty members who have shaped my development as a scholar. Vladas

Griskevicius has been a terrific mentor, co-author, source of great advice, and dose of

humor throughout my time in the PhD program. He changed the way I think about and

approach research. His contribution to my professional development cannot be

understated.

I would also like to thank Kathleen D. Vohs and Carlos Torelli who have been great

mentors and co-authors supporting me through every step I take in the PhD program. I

would also like to express my gratitude to Mark Snyder who encouraged me to pursue the

line of inquiry in this dissertation. Although there are simply too many other contributing

faculty members to name here individually, I can think of no better way to thank them

than to promise to invest in my future students the same way they all invested in me.

I also want to express my deep appreciation to Rohini Ahluwalia and Ronald Faber.

Without them, I would have never had the chance to enter into this wonderful profession.

They both have inspired me and guided me to become who I am today.

During my PhD program, I also benefited from a spectacular group of PhD colleagues.

Special thanks are dedicated to Jennifer Stoner for being a valued co-author and

cherished friend. I am also greatly indebted to Xiaolin Li and Nick Olson for numerous

inspiring afternoon coffee breaks. I have also received valuable intellectual input and

i
personal support from many other current and former students, including Ji Kyung Park,

Madhu Viswanathan, Paola Mallucci, Jannine Lasaleta, Nelson Amaral, Ryan Rahinel,

Jong An Choi, Jayoung Koo, Chiraag Mittal, Michael Covey, Zuhui Xiao, Marco Qin,

Maria Rodas, Veena Kondaveeti, Ruitong Wang, and Yilong Liang.

More broadly, I have been fortunate to have the social support of many close friends

during the course of my PhD studies. To Yanzhu Ji, Jinxi You, Zhiqiang Xing, Xue Bai

and Zhan Wang: Thank you for always being there for me. Li Tang and Yang Geng,

thank you for taking care of Lala during the busiest times of my dissertation process.

I would be remiss to not also extend my sincerest gratitude to those who make behavioral

research possible at the Carlson School of Management. Skye Vanderwoude, Paige

Thorburn and Amanda Casselton, thank you for helping in many of the studies in this

dissertation. Dori Higgin-Houser and Bonita Bartholomew were instrumental in this work,

as were the thousands of research participants that provided their valuable input. This

dissertation would not have been possible without them.

ii
DEDICATION

This dissertation is dedicated to my adviser – Deborah Roedder John. So much of

my development as a scholar can be traced back to Debbie’s countless hours of

investment in me. Her intellect, compassion, generosity, and integrity have given me

unflagging support and inspiration at every step of this journey. Moreover, she has gone

above and beyond her official role as an adviser to help me through life’s unexpected

hurdles, and for that I am truly grateful. I aspire to one day affect a student’s life in the

same way she has affected mine.

iii
TABLE OF CONTENTS

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ................................................................................................ ii
DEDICATION ................................................................................................................... iii
TABLE OF CONTENTS ................................................................................................... iv
LIST OF FIGURES .............................................................................................................v
CHAPTER I: Overview of Dissertation ..............................................................................1
CHAPTER II: Essay 1: The Devil Wears Prada: How Luxury Consumption Influences
Social Behaviors ..................................................................................................................8
CHAPTER III: Essay 2: Louis Vuitton and Your Waistline: How Luxury Consumption
Influences Self-Control ......................................................................................................68
CHAPTER IV: Summary and Future Research Directions .............................................126
BIBLIOGRAPHY ............................................................................................................133

iv
LIST OF FIGURES

CHAPTER II: Essay 1: The Devil Wears Prada? How Luxury Consumption Influences
Social Behaviors
Figure 1: Effects of Luxury Consumption on Selfish Behavior (Study 1) ........................60
Figure 2: Effects of Luxury Consumption on Selfish Behavior (Study 2) ........................61
Figure 3: Effects of Luxury Consumption on Selfish Behavior (Study 3) ........................62
Figure 4: Effects of Luxury Consumption on Selfish Behavior Mediated By Perceived
Social Status (Study 3) .......................................................................................................63
Figure 5: Effects of Luxury Consumption on Charity Donations Depending on Whether
Donation is Public or Private (Study 4) .............................................................................64
Figure 6: Effects of Luxury Consumption on Charity Donations Depending on Whether
Donation is Public or Private (Study 5) .............................................................................65
Figure 7: Effects of Luxury Consumption on Charity Donations Depending on Whether
Donation is Public or Private (Study 6) .............................................................................66
Figure 8: Effects of Luxury Consumption on Public Donation Mediated by Perceived
Social Status (Study 6) .......................................................................................................66

CHAPTER III: Essay 2: Louis Vuitton and Your Waistline: How Luxury Consumption
Influences Self-Control
Figure 1: Effects of Luxury Use on Spotlight Mindset and M&M consumption
(Study 1)...........................................................................................................................119
Figure 2: Mediation Analysis...........................................................................................120
Figure 3: Effects of Luxury Use on Food Consumption (Study 2)..................................121
Figure 4: Mediation Analysis (Study 2)...........................................................................122
Figure 5: Effects of Consumption Context and Spotlight Mindset Prime on M&M
Consumption (Study 3) ....................................................................................................123
Figure 6: Effects of Luxury Use on M&M Consumption (Study 4) ................................124
Figure 7: Effects of Luxury Use and Trait Self-Control on M&M Consumption
(Study 4)...........................................................................................................................125

v
CHAPTER I:

OVERVIEW OF DISSERTATION

In the U.S. and around the world, luxury consumption is at an all-time high.

Among over 100 categories of consumer spending, luxury goods show the strongest

growth (Sparshott 2014). The number of luxury consumers has more than tripled over the

past 20 years to a total of 330 million consumers at the end of 2013, and in the next five

years, global luxury sales are expected to grow up to 50% faster than global GDP

(D’Arpizio 2013). Not surprisingly, many of the most valuable brands in the world are

luxury brands, such as Burberry, Louis Vuitton, and Prada (Forbes 2013).

Ever since Veblen’s Theory of the Leisure Class, scholars have been interested in

why consumers have such a voracious appetite for luxury goods. In marketing,

researchers have found a number of motivations for owning and using luxury goods,

including expressing one’s identity, signaling prestige and status, and compensating for

feelings of powerlessness (e.g., Berger and Ward 2010; Griskevicius et al. 2007; Han,

Nunes, and Dreze 2010; Rucker, Galinsky, and Dubios 2012).

In contrast to prior research examining motivations for attaining luxury goods, my

dissertation examines the psychological and behavioral consequences of using luxury

products. Specifically, I seek to answer the following question: When consumers use

luxury products, how does it affect the way they feel and behave? Whereas prior research

has looked at motivations for desiring luxury goods, I look at the actual consumption

experience of using luxury goods. In my research, I provide individuals with an

opportunity to use luxury goods, and then assess the consequences of their luxury

consumption.

1
My dissertation consists of two essays. The first essay focuses on the

interpersonal aspect of behavioral consequences of using a luxury product. In particular,

I look at how using a luxury product affects whether individuals are selfish or generous

toward others. The second essay focuses on the intrapersonal aspect of behavioral

consequences of using a luxury product. In particular, I look at how the experience of

using a luxury product affects an individual’s self-control. Essay 1 examines what

happens DURING the consumption of a luxury product, while essay 2 examines what

happens AFTER the consumption of a luxury product. Taken together, these two essays

explore the psychological and behavioral consequences of using luxury products.

Essay 1: The Devil Wears Prada:

How Luxury Consumption Influences Social Behavior

In this essay, I examine whether using a luxury product makes people behave in a

selfish or generous way. Previous work suggests that consumers want luxury products as

a way to signal one’s status and power (e.g., Han et al. 2010; Rucker and Galinsky 2008,

2009). Further, recent research in social psychology has established a link between higher

status/power and behaviors that are self-interested, including selfishness and

uncooperative behaviors (Piff et al. 2010; 2014). Based on these lines of research, I

propose that using a luxury product can induce higher feelings of status, and these

perceived higher status feelings result in self-interested behaviors.

Given that luxury consumption should promote self-interested behavior, what

form might these self-interested behaviors take? I further propose that self-interested

behavior is often equated with selfish behavior, where people take more resources for the

self and give less to others. Accordingly, I predict that using a luxury product will

2
generally lead people to engage in selfish behavior. However, self-interested behavior

does not always mean being less generous. I propose that luxury consumption will lead to

generous behaviors only if engaging in such behaviors provides an explicit opportunity to

boost reputation for the luxury users.

I completed six studies to test these hypotheses. In these studies, female

participants were provided with either a luxury handbag (Louis Vuitton, Prada, or

Burberry) or a non-luxury handbag to use while walking around a campus building. After

using the product, the participants were engaged in tasks to measure self-interested

behavior. In study 1 and 2, for example, participants came back to the lab after using the

luxury product, and were asked to divide prize money between themselves and another

person. Awarding oneself more prize money was considered evidence of selfish behavior.

Across six studies, I found that:

Study 1 & 2: Women who used a luxury handbag indicated they would give less

money to others.

Study 3: Women who used a luxury handbag indicated they would give less

money to others. Moreover, this selfish behavior was mediated by perceptions of higher

status triggered by using a luxury product.

These three experiments provide behavioral evidence that luxury product

consumption makes people engage in more selfish behaviors. The next three studies

tested the prediction that using a luxury product can also result in more generous

behavior. Participants were assigned to use a luxury or non-luxury product, and after

using the product, were given $5 for participating in the study. Then, they were provided

with an opportunity to donate cash from their payment to a charity of their choice. Their

3
donation was made and recorded either in public in front of other people or in a private

cubicle in the lab. The results show that:

Study 4 & 5: Women who used the luxury (vs. non-luxury) product donated

significantly more money in public. However, women who used the luxury product

donated significantly less money when the donation was made in private.

Study 6: Women who used the luxury (vs. non-luxury) product donated

significantly more money in public. However, this effect did not emerge when women

using the luxury product were told that many other women also owned the product. Thus,

relative status (not absolute status) drives the luxury effects on generous behaviors.

Essay 2: Louis Vuitton and Your Waistline:

How Luxury Consumption Influences Self-Control

In the four experiments of essay 2, I examine the negative psychological costs of

luxury consumption after using a luxury product, particularly how luxury consumption

reduces self-control. I propose that using a luxury product in public elicits a spotlight

mindset, which triggers feelings of being paid attention to and more mindful of needing

to monitor their behavior in front of other people. Past research suggests that such a

process requires self-regulation (Vohs et al. 2005). Moreover, an extensive body of

research shows that our ability to engage in self-regulation is limited, and our ability to

self-regulate is depleted as we engage in more and more attempts to self-regulate

(Baumeister, Vohs, and Tice 2007; Vohs and Heatherton 2000). Thus, I propose that

using a luxury product in public depletes one’s self-regulatory resources, which reduces

one’s ability to exert self-control in a subsequent task, such as resisting tasty but

unhealthy food (such as candy).

4
In order to test my prediction, I have completed four experiments. As before,

women used a luxury or non-luxury product. After using the product, the participants

then completed tasks designed to assess their ability to exert self-control when a snack of

candy was made available to them. Looking at the amount of candy consumed, I found

results supportive of my prediction. For example:

Study 1 & 2: Women who carried a luxury handbag were less able to resist candy

and ate more of it than women who carried a non-luxury handbag, and this difference was

due to feelings of being in a spotlight. Also, the effects of carrying a luxury handbag on

food consumption were stronger for women who generally struggle with self-control.

Studies 3 & 4: These studies ruled out several alternative explanations for the

observed effects. Increases in food consumption for luxury users were not driven by

changes in feelings of power, status, positive mood, negative mood, or how much the

participants liked the handbag.

The rest of my dissertation is organized as follows. Chapter II covers the

introduction, theoretical framework, empirical findings of six studies, and general

discussion for Essay 1. Chapter III covers the introduction, theoretical framework,

empirical findings of four studies, and general discussion for Essay 2. Chapter IV then

summarizes the empirical evidence across essays, identifies my research contributions,

and discusses limitations and directions for future research.

5
References

Baumeister Roy F., Kathleen D. Vohs, and Dianne M. Tice (2007), “The Strength Model

of Self-Control,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16 (6), 351-55.

Berger, Jonah and Morgan Ward (2010), “Subtle Signals of Inconspicuous

Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (December), 555-69.

D’Arpizio, Claudia (2013), “Luxury Goods Worldwide Market Study, Fall 2013,” Bain

& Company, accessed April 27, 2014, http://www.bain.com/publications/articles/

luxury-goods-worldwide-market-study-fall-2013.aspx

D’Arpizio, Claudia (2014), “Luxury Goods Worldwide Market Study, Winter 2014,”

Bain & Company, accessed April 27, 2014, http://www.bain.com/publications/

articles/luxury-goods-worldwide-markey-study-winter-2014.aspx

Forbes (2013), “The World’s Most Valuable Brands,” Thomson Reuters Fundamentals

via FactSet Research System, accessed April 27, 2014, http://www.forbes.com/

powerful-brands/list/#page:1_sort:6_direction:asc_search:

Griskevicius, Vladas, Joshua M. Tybur, Jill M. Sundie, Robert B. Cialdini, Geoffrey F.

Miller, and Douglas T. Kenrick (2007), “Blatant Benevolence and Conspicuous

Consumption: When Romantic Motives Elicit Strategic Costly Signals,” Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 93 (1), 85-102.

Han, Young Jee, Joseph C. Nunes, and Xavier Drèze (2010), “Signaling Status with

Luxury Goods: The Role of Brand Prominence,” Journal of Marketing, 74 (July),

15-30.

Piff, Paul K. (2014), “Wealth and the Inflated Self Class, Entitlement, and Narcissism,”

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(1), 34-43.

6
Piff, Paul K., Michael W. Kraus, Stéphane Côté, Bonnie Hayden Cheng, and Dacher

Keltner (2010) “Having Less, Giving More: The Influence of Social Class on

Prosocial Behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 771-84.

Rucker, Derek D. and Adam D. Galinsky (2009), “Conspicuous Consumption Versus

Utilitarian Ideals: How Different Levels of Power Shape Consumer Behavior,”

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(3), 549-55.

Rucker, Derek D. and Adam D. Galinsky (2008), “Desire to Acquire: Powerlessness and

Compensatory Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35(2), 257-67.

Rucker, Derek D., Adam D. Galinsky, and David Dubois (2012), “Power and Consumer

Behavior: How Power Shapes Who and What Consumers Value,” Journal of

Consumer Psychology, 22, 352-68.

Sparshott, Jeffrey (2014), “What Products Drove Consumer Spending? Luxury Items,

Mostly,” The Wall Street Journal, accessed February 11, 2014,

http://blogs.wsj.com/economics/2014/01/22/what-products-drove-consumer-

spending-luxury-items-mostly/

Vohs, Kathleen D., Roy F. Baumeister, and Natalie J. Ciarocco (2005), “Self-Regulation

and Self-Presentation: Regulatory Resource Depletion Impairs Impression

Management and Effortful Self-Presentation Depletes Regulatory Resources,”

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88 (4), 632-57.

Vohs, Kathleen D. and Todd F. Heatherton (2000), “Self-Regulatory Failure: A

Resource-Depletion Approach,” Psychological Science, 11 (3), 249-54.

7
CHAPTER II:

Essay 1: The Devil Wears Prada?

How Luxury Consumption Influences Social Behavior

One of the biggest trends in consumer behavior over the last two decades is the

growth of luxury consumption. During this time, sales of luxury goods have skyrocketed

from $80 billion to over $500 billion per year (D’Arpizio 2014) and the number of luxury

consumers has more than tripled from 90 million to 330 million (Statista 2014). Across

100 categories of consumer spending, luxury goods have the strongest growth trajectory

of any category (Sparshott 2014), and in the next five years, global luxury sales are

expected to grow up to 50% faster than the global Gross Domestic Product (D’Arpizio

2014).

Once the purview of only the rich, luxury goods are in the hands of more

consumers than ever before. Despite this, we have very little understanding of how the

introduction of luxury goods into our lives affects our thoughts, feelings, and behaviors.

Luxury goods are sought after as a means to satisfy a variety of psychological needs, such

as signaling status and prestige, as well as compensating for feelings of powerlessness

(Rucker, Galinsky, and Dubois 2012; Griskevicius et al. 2007; Han, Nunes, and Dreze

2010; Wilcox, Kim, and Sen 2009). If luxury goods can have these transformative

qualities, there is every reason to believe that using luxury products can have a significant

influence on how we feel and how we interact with others.

We explore this possibility by examining the effects of luxury consumption on

social behavior. We ask the question: Does luxury consumption make consumers more

8
selfish or more generous? To answer this question, we provide women with a luxury

product to use, such as a Prada or Louis Vuitton handbag. After using the luxury product,

we provide a number of opportunities for people to exhibit selfish or generous behaviors.

We find that using a luxury product generally results in selfish behaviors, such as taking

more money from others. However, we also find that using a luxury product can result in

more generous behavior, such as donating more money to charity.

How can luxury consumption make people more selfish and more generous? We

reason that using a luxury product increases a consumer’s own sense of social status,

which encourages one to behave in ways to benefit the self. Most of the time, this

tendency leads one to engage in selfish behaviors that directly benefit the self, such as

acquiring more money and giving less to others. However, money and resources are not

the only valuable currencies. One’s social reputation and social standing are also of value.

We find that when there is an explicit opportunity to enhance one’s reputation by giving

to others, such as by donating money to a charity in a public setting, luxury users will

exhibit more generous behavior. In other words, luxury users behave in ways to benefit

the self, which can result in either more selfish or more generous behavior depending on

the situation.

Our findings make several contributions to luxury research. First, to our

knowledge, we are the first to examine the psychological and behavioral consequences

that are triggered by luxury use. Past research has examined people’s attitudes,

preferences, and motivations for acquiring luxury goods (Berger and Ward 2010; Han et

al. 2010; Lee and Shrum 2012; Ordabayeva and Chandon 2011; Rucker, Galinsky, and

Dubois 2012; Wang and Griskevicius 2014; Wilcox et al. 2009). However, what happens

9
after individuals acquire and use luxury goods has not been explored. Doing so requires a

different experimental approach than prior research, which has typically assessed

attitudes and preferences for luxury goods, but has not observed what happens when

individuals actually use luxury goods. Our novel experimental approach includes actual

use of luxury products and observations of real behavior.

Second, we show that luxury consumption can influence social behavior in

seemingly opposite ways. Luxury users feel a boost in social status, and these feelings

give rise to behaviors that benefit self, which can result in more selfish or more generous

behavior. The idea that individuals with higher social status can act more selfishly and

more generously is novel, and qualifies prior findings that individuals with higher

socioeconomic status are willing to behave more antisocially (Piff et al. 2010, 2012) and

are less compassionate toward other people (Stellar et al. 2012). Although individuals

with higher status do behave in ways to benefit the self, this behavior is not restricted to

selfish or antisocial acts. Even seemingly generous behavior, such as donating money to

others, can also be exhibited by these individuals if there is something to be gained from

one’s generosity, such as the reputational benefits that come from giving.

Finally, we show that luxury goods have an impact that goes beyond the luxury

user alone. Whereas prior work on luxury consumption has examined well-being from

the standpoint of the consumer, we show that luxury consumption has negative

consequences for the well-being of others because the selfish behaviors triggered by

luxury usage in our studies impose costs on others. Such behaviors, which can be viewed

as the societal costs of luxury consumption, are not only unexpected but are also

unwelcome consequences of luxury usage.

10
CONCEPTUAL BACKGROUND

The Consumption of Luxury

Luxury goods are products or brands offered at a premium price and quality level

(Patrick and Hagtvedt 2008). Luxury brands, in particular, have been defined as being

relatively rare and exclusive (Fuchs et al. 2013, Han et al. 2010, Phau and Prendergast

2000; Vigneron and Johnson 2004). Our research focuses on premier luxury brands—

such as Prada, Louis Vuitton, and Burberry—that are consistent with these definitions

and whose products are widely viewed by consumers as luxury goods (Fuchs et al.

2013).

Luxury consumption has grown exponentially, and key to this growth has been an

increasing appeal to a broader audience. Although historically associated with older,

ultra-affluent individuals, luxury consumption today is much more common among other

groups such as young professionals and college students (Phillips 2012). These younger

luxury consumers not only outnumber older, ultra-affluent consumers by a 10:1 margin,

but they also drive the sales of luxury accessories, such as wallets and sunglasses, which

constitute the largest segment of the luxury market (D’Arpizio 2014). Luxury goods can

also be consumed without actually purchasing them. Modern consumers can rent a

variety of luxury products, such as designer handbags, clothing, and jewelry, as well as

expensive luxury cars. For example, instead of paying $4,500 for a highly-coveted

Chanel 2.55 flap handbag, women can rent the same handbag for $300 a month from

companies such as Bag Borrow or Steal and Rent Me a Handbag.

Past research examining luxury consumption has primarily considered why

people seek luxury products. Studies find that people seek luxury goods for a variety of

11
reasons such as boosting their self-esteem, and attaining happiness and fulfillment,

especially when they feel deprived or powerless (Belk 1985; Charles, Hurst, and

Roussanoy 2009; Richins 1987; Rucker et al. 2012). People also seek luxury goods to

signal important information to others (Belk, Bahn, and Mayer 1982; Richins 1994;

Wang and Griskevicius 2014). For instance, luxury products can be used to signal their

owners’ wealth and prestige, identity, personal taste, and can even serve to attract and

protect romantic partners (Bagwell and Bernheim 1996; Berger and Ward 2010;

Griskevicius et al. 2007; Han et al. 2010; Mazzocco et al. 2012; Sundie et al. 2011;

Wernerfelt 1990; Veblen 1899).

Although past research on luxury has examined people’s attitudes, preferences,

and motivations for acquiring luxury goods, little is known about the psychological and

behavioral consequences of using luxury goods. Prior research has stopped short of

examining what happens when consumers actually use luxury goods—do they see

themselves differently, or act differently? This is the question we address in our research.

Specifically, we examine whether the mere act of using a luxury product, such as

carrying a Prada handbag versus a non-luxury handbag, alters a person’s perceptions of

themselves as well as their behavior.

Luxury Consumption, Social Status, and Self-Interested Behavior

Luxury products have often been described as status symbols (Han et al. 2010;

Veblen 1899). The connection between luxury and status is evident in research

examining consumer motivations for desiring luxury goods. For example, consumers

with a higher need for status show a stronger desire for luxury goods (Han et al. 2010),

and powerless people exhibit greater interest in luxury goods in order to compensate for

12
their lack of status (Rucker and Galinsky 2008, 2009). Indeed, people who wear luxury

brands are perceived as possessing higher status by others (Nelissen and Meijers 2011),

which suggests that the people wearing or using a luxury product may perceive

themselves as possessing elevated status.

We propose that using luxury goods elevates the user’s sense of social status. For

example, we predict that a woman will perceive herself as having higher social status

when she is carrying a Prada handbag as opposed to a non-luxury handbag. Social status

refers to an individual’s rank in a social hierarchy or position in society (Dreze and

Nunes 2009; French and Raven 1959; Magee and Galinsky 2008). Higher social status is

often associated with having power (Rucker and Galinsky 2008, 2009; Wong and Shavitt

2010) and being admired by others (Magee and Galinsky 2008; Ridgeway and Walker

1995). Although prior research has made a connection between luxury goods and status,

there is no empirical evidence that people actually perceive themselves as having higher

social status when they use luxury goods.

If using luxury goods increase perceptions of consumers’ own social status,

behavioral consequences are likely to follow. Possessing higher status is a desirable state,

and individuals are naturally motivated to promote status once it is acquired (Griskevicius

and Kenrick 2013; Kenrick et al. 2010). Status is often promoted through self-interested

behaviors. For example, past correlational work shows that people with higher

socioeconomic status are less generous, less charitable, and less helpful (Piff et al. 2010,

2012) and are less compassionate toward another person’s suffering (Stellar et al. 2012).

Likewise, giving people power leads them to prioritize self-interest over group goals

(Maner and Mead 2010), spend more on themselves rather than on others (Rucker,

13
Dubois, and Galinsky 2011), and exhibit less perspective-taking (Galinsky et al. 2006).

Thus, we propose that using luxury goods increases self-perceptions of social status,

which then promotes self-interested behavior.

Self-Interested Behaviors: Selfishness and Generosity

Given that luxury consumption should promote self-interested behavior, what

form might these self-interested behaviors take? As described earlier, self-interested

behavior is often equated with selfish behavior, where people take more resources for the

self and give less to others. Accordingly, we predict that using a luxury product will

generally lead people to engage in selfish behavior. For example, we expect that using a

luxury handbag (vs. non-luxury handbag) will cause women to engage in more selfish

behaviors, such as taking more scarce resources, splitting a pool of money in a way that

favors themselves, and donating less money to a charity. In these examples, the luxury

user places their own self-interest in obtaining money and other resources ahead of

concern for others. Further, we expect this effect of luxury consumption on selfish

behavior to be statistically mediated by self-perceptions of social status.

But while the consumption of luxury products should lead to self-interested

behavior, this doesn’t mean that that luxury consumers should always behave less

generously. Self-interested behavior does not always mean being less generous. At times,

it is on one’s self-interest to behave more generously and give more to others.

Anthropological research shows that having a reputation as a cooperative and

helpful member of a group elevates a person’s status. Across cultures, engaging in

behaviors that benefit others can help build a good reputation (Semmann et al. 2005;

Wedekind and Braithwaite 2002), which elevates one’s status in a group (Smith and Bird

14
2000). For example, past research shows that people promote their status by sharing more

food with others (Gurven et al. 2000), contributing more to public goods (Hardy and Van

Vugt 2006; Van Vugt and Hardy 2009), and giving more resources to people in need

(Cole and Chaikin 1990; Murdock 1970).

Although behaving in a generous way can be beneficial in terms of promoting

status, this type of behavior also increases the well-being of others at a cost to the

individual. Donating money to charity, for example, may be advantageous as a way to

promote one’s status, but the individual donating money is also giving up resources,

which is disadvantageous in terms of maintaining status. Given this central trade-off, we

consider when higher status should lead to selfish behavior versus leading to generous

behavior.

We propose that higher status individuals will give more to others only when

there is an explicit opportunity for such behavior to elevate one’s reputation. Typically,

these opportunities take place in public, where the giver’s generous behavior can be seen

and readily acknowledged by other people. For instance, consider donating money at a

charity event where givers are acknowledged in public. Publically visible prosocial

behaviors allow an individual to benefit in terms of reputation, whereas private prosocial

behaviors do not (Hardy and Van Vugt 2006; Van Vugt and Hardy 2009). Accordingly,

previous research finds that consumers with a status-seeking motive are more willing to

sacrifice benefits to themselves and make choices that benefit the group when making

such choices in public but not in private (Griskevicius et al. 2010).

We predict that luxury (vs. non-luxury) consumption will lead to generous

behaviors only when engaging in such behaviors provides an explicit opportunity to boost

15
reputation for the luxury user. For example, using a luxury (vs. non-luxury) product

should make people more willing to donate money to charity if the donation occurs in

public in front of others. However, if the donation context is private and there is no

opportunity to boost reputation by displaying one’s generosity, luxury (vs. non-luxury)

users will revert to being selfish, and will donate less money to charity. Although the

public setting elicits generous behavior, while the private setting encourages selfish

behavior, both types of behaviors are self-interested in nature. In the public setting, the

luxury user can gain status by giving money to others; in the private setting, the luxury

user can gain status by keeping more money for themselves. Thus, our central prediction

is that luxury consumption triggers self-interested behaviors, but the type of self-

interested behavior we observe (selfish or generous) varies by context.

Overview of Empirical Studies

We test our predictions in six studies. The first three studies show that using a

luxury product encourages more selfish behavior. In addition, Study 3 provides evidence

that using a luxury product boosts perceptions of one’s own social status, which

statistically mediate the effect of product use (luxury vs. non-luxury) on selfish behavior.

Study 4 and 5 demonstrates that using a luxury (vs. non-luxury) product can also

result in more generous behavior. Per our prediction, luxury users donated more money

to a charity than non-luxury users when the donation was made in public in front of

others. However, luxury users donated less money to a charity than non-luxury users

when the donation was made in private.

Finally, Study 6 demonstrates a theoretically-derived boundary condition for our

effects. We show that when the luxury item is perceived to be less exclusive, luxury users

16
do not feel a boost in their social status, and thereby do not behave differently. Taken

together, this set of studies demonstrates that using luxury products encourages self-

interested behaviors, which present as selfish behavior in private contexts and as

generous behavior in public contexts that afford the opportunity to boost reputation

through giving. Furthermore, we show that both selfish and generous behaviors are

caused by the higher sense of status that luxury users experience.

STUDY 1: LOUIS VUITTON AND SELFISH BEHAVIOR

Study 1 tested how the consumption of a luxury product influences women’s

behavior. Participants first walked around a busy area either wearing or not wearing a

luxury handbag. Afterwards, they were presented with an opportunity to behave in a

selfish way. We predicted that the women in the luxury consumption condition would

behave more selfishly.

Method

Participants. One-hundred and eleven female students (Mage = 20.46, SD = 1.57)

in an introductory business course at a large North American university participated in

the study in exchange for partial course credit. Twelve participants did not follow the

study instructions (e.g., clicked through the survey without reading, didn’t understand the

questions) and were excluded from the data analysis.

Luxury Product Pre-test. To ensure participants had familiarity with luxury

brands, a pre-test was conducted with a separate sample of 48 female participants (Mage =

20.08, SD = 0.74) from the same sample population as the main study. All participants

answered several yes/no questions about ten luxury brands: (1) Have you ever purchased

any of the following brands: Gucci, Prada, Louis Vuitton, Burberry, Coach, Tory Burch,

17
Boss, Michael by Michael Kors, Kate Spade, Marc by Marc Jacobs? And (2) Have you

ever seen your friends or classmates with products from these brands?

Findings showed that 93.8% of the women indicated that they had purchased at

least one of these brands, and 97.9% had seen their friends or classmates with products

from these brands. Because some of these brands are considered more exclusive than

others, we also asked the same two questions again by focusing only on the four high-end

luxury brands: Gucci, Prada, Louis Vuitton, and Burberry. Findings showed that 52.1%

of women indicated that they had purchased at least one of these four brands, and 87.5%

of the women had seen their friends or classmates with products from these brands. Thus,

consistent with trends in the luxury goods industry showing that luxury consumption is

common among younger consumers, women in our sample population were both highly

familiar with and common consumers of luxury brands.

Procedure. All participants arrived individually for a study on consumer products

and were randomly assigned to one of the three between-subjects conditions: control,

luxury logo, and luxury consumption. In the luxury consumption condition, participants

were given a Louis Vuitton bag and were explicitly told that this is a brand new Louis

Vuitton bag (see illustration A for pictures). The Louis Vuitton bag retails for $750 but

no price information was provided to participants. Louis Vuitton was chosen as the

luxury brand because it is not only well-known, but is also highly desirable. Pretesting

indicated that our participant population rated Louis Vuitton an average of 5.67 in

desirability on a 1-7 scale.

To get a sense of what it’s like to own and use the handbag, each participant was

first instructed to imagine that the handbag belonged to her. She was then instructed to

18
put her personal items into the handbag and walk around campus. To ensure that

participants would follow a similar walking path, each participant was given instructions

to follow a specific path. This included taking a specific elevator to a specified floor,

walking through a busy area to a specific coffee shop, and then following the same path

to get back. After coming back to the lab about 15 minutes later, each participant was

directed to a private room with a computer to fill out a survey about the handbag. This

portion of the study contained the dependent measures.

Participants in the control condition were given a campus map and a tour guide

brochure and walked around the same path as the participants in the luxury consumption

condition did. The cover story for them was to evaluate the campus map and tour guide

brochure. They also walked for about 15 minutes and came back to the lab for the

computer survey.

Participants in the luxury logo condition were given the campus map and tour

guide brochure and were asked to read them while sitting in an individual room. In the

room were papers and pencils with the Louis Vuitton brand name and logo. The goal of

this procedure was to expose (prime) participants with the luxury brand, but not allow

them to consume the product. After about 15 minutes in the room that contained the

brand logo, these participants were also asked to evaluate the campus maps and tour

guide brochure before they completed the final survey on computer.

Dependent Measures. After participants answered questions about the handbag or

the maps (to maintain consistency with the cover story), they completed the dependent

measure. It was taken from a previous study that measured selfish behavior (Zitek et al.

2010) and examined how people choose to split communal prize money when they win a

19
contest. Participants were told that the researcher needed feedback for a future study. In

this future study, two people would compete with each other for a prize of $10 that would

be divided between the winner and the loser. Participants in the current study were asked

to imagine that they were in that study and that they won the competition by answering 7

out of 10 questions correctly. Participants were asked to indicate how the researcher

should split the $10 of prize money between the winner (the participant) and the loser

(the other person). The dependent measure consisted of how much money the participant

gave to themselves. Participants provided their response by choosing one of 11 options

that ranged from “$0 to me, $10 to my opponent” to “$10 to me, $0 to my opponent.”

The options differed from each other in $1 increments.

Results and Discussion

We examined how much of the $10 prize money people gave to themselves

across three conditions. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed a main effect

(F(2,96) = 3.98, p = .022). As seen in figure 1, participants in the luxury consumption

condition gave significantly more money to themselves (M = $8.65, SD = 1.76) than

participants in both control condition (M = $7.67, SD = 1.98; t(96) = -2.66, p = .009) and

in the luxury logo condition (M = $7.36, SD = 2.11; t(96) = -2.09, p = .039). There was

no difference between participants in the control condition and luxury logo condition (p

= .53).

--------------------------
figure 1 about here
--------------------------

In summary, Study 1 showed that the consumption of a luxury product led people

to behave in a more self-interested manner. We found that wearing a Louis Vuitton

20
handbag, but not merely seeing a Louis Vuitton logo, led women to award themselves

more prize money if they imagined winning a contest.

STUDY 2: PRADA AND SELFISH BEHAVIOR

Study 2 sought to conceptually replicate the findings from Study 1. In Study 2, all

participants walked around a busy area wearing either a luxury (Prada) or a non-luxury

handbag. In addition to the dependent measure used in Study 1, Study 2 also assessed

selfishness in a second way. The second measure was based on the notion of selfishly

taking the last desirable item from a group’s common resources, such as when a person

takes the last piece of food from a communal plate. Study 2 examined the percentage of

women who took the last desirable pen from a communal pen box.

Method

Participants. Seventy-two female students (Mage = 19.90, SD = 1.01) in an

introductory business course at a large North American university participated in the

study in exchange for partial course credit. Five participants did not follow the study

instructions (e.g., taking their own pen out of a bag) and were excluded from the data

analysis.

Procedure. The procedure was similar to Study 1. All participants arrived

individually for a study about handbags. Each participant was first given a handbag to try

out by carrying it around campus. Afterwards, they provided feedback about the handbag.

The study had two between-subjects conditions: Women were randomly assigned to

either carry around a luxury or a control handbag.

Luxury Consumption Experience. Participants were given one of two mid-size

handbags. In the luxury condition, participants were given a Prada bag and were

21
explicitly told that this is a brand new Prada bag. In the control condition, participants

were given a non-luxury bag similar in size and style to the Prada bag and were told it

was a brand new bag from a department store (see illustration A for pictures). Whereas

the Prada bag retails for $1890, the control bag retails for $75, although no price

information was provided to participants. Prada was chosen as the luxury brand, like

Louis Vuitton, it is not only well-known, but is also highly desirable. Pretesting indicated

that our participant population rated Louis Vuitton an average of 5.7 in desirability on a

1-7 scale. As the luxury condition in Study 1, participants were asked to imagine the bag

belonged to them, put their personal items into the bag, and walked around campus with

the bag following the same path.

Dependent Measures. The study had two dependent measures. The first measure

examined whether women would take the last desirable item from a communal tray. As

participants were about to begin the computer survey, the experimenter informed them

that part of the survey would be done on paper and that they would need a pen. The

experimenter directed the participant to a research lab pen tray, which contained three

pens specifically placed there for the study. One pen looked brand new and two other

pens were clearly less desirable; they had been slightly chewed on and had dark smudges

from ink leaks. The experimenter casually noted that the participant should take only one

pen because other research participants would need pens for other studies going on in the

lab. The dependent measure was whether the participant took the last desirable pen or

whether she left it on the tray for other participants.

After participants answered questions about the handbag (to maintain consistency

with the cover story), they completed the second dependent measure. The second

22
measure was identical to the one used in Study 1. We measured how much of a $10 prize

participants would give to themselves rather than another person.

Results and Discussion

Pen. We first examined the percentage of people who took the last desirable pen

in the luxury and the control condition. As seen in figure 2, the result indicated that

people were far more likely to take the last desirable pen in the luxury condition

compared to the control condition (61.8% vs. 30.3%; χ2 (1) = 6.67, p = .01). Thus,

women who wore the Prada handbag were more likely to take the last desirable pen.

Money Allocation. We next examined how much of the $10 prize money people

gave to themselves in the luxury and the control conditions. As seen in figure 2,

participants in the luxury condition gave significantly more money to themselves (M =

$7.44, SD = 1.52) than participants in the control condition (M = $6.69, SD = .95; t(66) =

2.39, p = .02). Thus, women who wore the Prada handbag gave themselves more money

when dividing a communal prize between themselves and another person.

--------------------------
figure 2 about here
--------------------------

In summary, Study 2 showed that the consumption of a luxury product led people

to behave in a more selfish manner. Using the luxury handbag led women to be more

than twice as likely to take the last desirable pen from a communal pen tray. Wearing the

luxury handbag also led women to award themselves more prize money if they imagined

winning a contest, thereby leaving less prize money for the other person.

23
STUDY 3: PSYCHOLOGICAL MECHANISM

Study 3 sought to conceptually replicate and extend the findings from the first two

studies. Women once again walked around a busy area with either a luxury or a non-

luxury handbag. Afterwards, they had the opportunity to behave in a more selfish manner.

Consistent with findings from Study 1 and 2, we predicted that wearing luxury products

would lead women to once again behave in a more selfish way.

Study 3 also tested the psychological mechanism responsible for how luxury

consumption influences behavior. As described earlier, we hypothesize that consuming a

luxury good increases a person’s current sense of social status, which should then alter a

person’s behavior. We therefore measured people’s perceptions of their current social

status during their luxury consumption experience. We then tested whether perceptions of

current social status statistically mediated the effect of luxury consumption on behavior.

Method

Participants and Procedure. Forty-one female students (Mage = 20.02, SD = 0.96)

in an introductory business course at a large North American university participated in

the study in exchange for partial course credit. The procedure was highly similar to the

one described in the previous studies. Participants again came to the lab individually,

were provided with the same cover story, were randomly assigned to carry either a luxury

or a control handbag, walked around campus with the handbag for about 15 minutes, and

then came back to the lab to complete the dependent measures in a private room.

Luxury Consumption Experience. In the luxury condition, participants were given

a Louis Vuitton bag and were explicitly told that this is a brand new Louis Vuitton bag.

In the control condition, participants were given a non-luxury bag similar in size and

24
style to the Louis Vuitton bag and were told that this is a new bag is from a department

store (same as in Study 1). As in the previous studies, participants imagined owning the

bag, put their belongings in the bag, and walked around campus and a coffee shop with

the bag following the same specified path.

Perceived Social Status. To assess whether wearing the handbag influenced

people’s perceptions of social status, participants filled out a short survey during their

consumption experience. Before leaving the lab to try the handbag, participants were

instructed that when they arrived at the coffee shop, they should find a place to sit and

open the handbag. Inside the handbag, they would find a pencil and a survey to fill out at

the coffee shop.

Embedded in this survey were five items that measured participants’ perceptions

of their current level of social status. As described earlier, social status refers to a

person’s standing in a hierarchy, whereby higher social status is associated with having

more power and higher position in society. To assess people’s perceptions of their current

social status, participants responded to five items regarding how they were currently

feeling. Specifically, “Do you feel… (1) you have higher status, (2) you are superior to

others, (3) you are powerful, (4) like you are at the top, and (5) you have the power to

influence others?” Participants provided responses to each item on a 1-7 scale with

endpoint labels “Not at all” and “Very much.” The five items were averaged to form a

perceived social status index (α = .88).

Dependent Measure. Immediately after participants returned to the lab, they were

directed to a private room with a computer to fill out a survey about the handbag. This

portion of the study contained the dependent measure, which was identical to the one

25
used in previous studies. Once again, participants indicated how much of a $10 prize they

would give to themselves rather than another person if they beat that other person in a

competition.

Results and Discussion

Money Allocation. We first examined how much of the $10 prize women gave to

themselves in the luxury and the control conditions. As seen in figure 3, participants in

the luxury condition gave more money to themselves (M = $8.00, SD = 2.00) than did

participants in the control condition (M = $6.75, SD = 1.94, t(40) = 2.03, p = .049).

Conceptually replicating the finding from Study 1 and 2, women who wore a luxury

handbag once again gave themselves more money when dividing a communal prize

between themselves and another person.

--------------------------
figure 3 about here
--------------------------
Perceived Social Status. We next tested whether people differed in their sense of

social status in the luxury and the control conditions. Findings showed that participants in

the luxury condition perceived having significantly higher social status (M = 4.13, SD =

1.45) than participants in the control condition (M = 2.90, SD = 1.09; t(40) = 3.08, p

= .004). This means that women carrying the Louis Vuitton handbag felt that they having

higher social status compared to women carrying a non-luxury handbag.

Mediation Analysis. We next tested whether people’s sense of social status

statistically mediated the effect of luxury consumption on the monetary allocation. A

visual depiction of the mediation model is presented in figure 4.

--------------------------
figure 4 about here
--------------------------

26
First, the results showed that type of product (luxury vs. control) predicted

perceptions of social status (path a: β = .44, p = .004) and money allocation (path c: β

= .31, p = .049). Furthermore, sense of social status also predicted money allocation (path

b: β = .42, p = .007). Finally, the effect of product type on money allocations became

non-significant once sense of social status was entered in the model (path c’: β = .16, p

= .35). Following Preacher and Hayes (2008), a 10,000 resample bootstrap (Hayes (2012),

Model 4) revealed a significant indirect effect of type of product on money allocation via

sense of social status, b = .62 (SE = .32), 95% CI [.128, 1.404]. Because the confidence

interval does not include 0, this means that the effect of product type on money allocation

was statistically mediated by sense of social status.

In summary, Study 3 conceptually replicated and extended the findings from the

first two studies. Women who walked around with a luxury handbag once again behaved

more selfishly compared to women who walked around with a non-luxury handbag

similar in size and style. In addition, Study 3 tested the psychological mechanism

responsible for how luxury consumption influences behavior. Findings showed that

consuming a luxury good increased a person’s sense of current social status, and that this

increased perception of status statistically mediated the effect of luxury consumption on

behavior. In other words, the consumption of luxury led people to behave in a more

selfish manner because it led them to feel that they have higher social status.

STUDY 4: LUXURY CONSUMPTION AND DONATIONS

Thus far, we have shown that luxury consumption increases women’s sense of

social status, which then leads them to behave in a more selfish manner. In the next study,

we examine the conditions under which luxury consumption might lead to more generous

27
behavior. Per our earlier discussion, we hypothesize that luxury consumption should have

a different effect on behavior depending on whether the situation explicitly allows a

person to enhance their reputation by behaving generously. If the situation does not

present an explicit opportunity to enhance reputation by being more generous, as in Study

1, 2 and 3, luxury consumption should produce more selfish behavior. However, if the

situation explicitly allows a person to enhance their reputation by engaging in giving

behavior, then consuming luxury products should lead to people to become more

generous. For example, if a person is presented with an opportunity to give to charity in a

public place where other people will see their behavior, then luxury consumption should

lead people to behave more generously.

To test whether donating money in public could enhance a person’s reputation,

we surveyed one hundred and one participants (Mage = 30.50, SD = 8.87) on Amazon’s

Mechanical Turk (MTurk). Participants responded to two yes/no questions: “Do you

think donating money to a charity in public would: (1) increase someone’s reputation,

and (2) lead someone to have a good reputation?” Results showed that 83.2% of people

believed that donating money to a charity in public would increase reputation and 82.2%

believed that it would lead that person to have a good reputation. Thus, donating in public

can have a positive impact on one’s reputation.

In Study 4 women once again walked around a busy area with either a luxury or a

non-luxury product. Afterwards, all participants were asked a hypothetical question about

their willingness to donate to charity. Importantly, the donation location was varied to be

either private or public. We predicted that when the donation was made in private,

wearing a luxury product would lead people to donate less money, consistent with the

28
selfish tendencies found in Studies 1-3. By contrast, when the donation was made in

public, we predicted that wearing the luxury product would lead people to behave more

generously and donate more money.

Method

Participants and Procedure. Eighty-three female students (Mage = 19.92, SD = .95)

in an introductory business course at a large North American university participated in

the study in exchange for partial course credit. The experiment had the following

between-subjects design: 2 (product type: luxury vs. control) X 2 (donation context:

public vs. private).

Similar to previous studies, participants arrived individually for a study on

consumer products. Women were told that the study was about handbag and that each

participant would be given a Prada handbag or a control handbag to wear it around

campus. After about 15 minutes, all participants were directed back the lab.

Dependent Measure. Participants were asked to imagine that they had an

opportunity to donate money to charity. In the private condition, participants were

presented with an opportunity to donate as they were finishing a purchase at the checkout

stage on the website of an online retailer. In the public condition, participants were

presented with an opportunity to donate as they were standing in a checkout line at an

actual store. Participants indicated how much money they would like to donate on a scale

that ranged from $0 to $8.

Results and Discussion

An ANOVA with product type and donation context did not reveal any main

effects (ps > .27), but it revealed an interaction (F(1,82) = 34.03, p = .001). As seen in

29
figure 5, luxury consumption had a different effect on donation behavior depending on

whether the donation was made in public versus in private. To test our specific

hypotheses, we performed a series of planned contrasts in the private and public donation

conditions.

--------------------------
figure 5 about here
--------------------------
In the private context, people indicated that they would donate less money to

charity in the luxury condition (M = $2.00, SD=1.85) versus the control condition

(M=$3.08, SD=1.95; t(79) = 2.01, p = .04). Thus, as in Studies 1-3, luxury consumption

led people to be more selfish when the situation did not explicitly provide an opportunity

to enhance reputation through generosity.

In the public context, people indicated they would donate more money to charity

in the luxury condition (M = $3.72, SD=1.66) compared to the control condition (M =

$2.22, SD=1.47; t(79) = 2.69, p = .009). Thus, when the donation was made in public and

therefore presented an opportunity to enhance reputation, women who carried a Prada

handbag became more generous.

To look at the findings another way, the study found that participants who wore

the control handbag generally did not differ in their donations when the donation context

was public or private (p = .12; see figure 5). However, participants who wore the luxury

handbag exhibited a large difference in donations depending on whether the donation

context was public or private. Women wearing the luxury handbag gave much more

money to charity when the donation context was public rather than private (M = $3.72 vs.

$2.00; t(79) = 3.13, p = .002).

30
In summary, Study 4 found that when women were asked to make a donation to

charity in private, those wearing a Prada handbag indicated that they would donate less

money than those wearing a non-luxury handbag. Consistent with the findings from

Studies 1-3, luxury products made people more selfish when the situation did not allow

people to explicitly enhance their reputation through giving. However, when women

were asked to make a donation to charity in public, those wearing a Prada handbag

indicated that they would donate more money than those wearing a non-luxury handbag.

That is, when the situation explicitly allowed a person to enhance their reputation by

engaging in helping behavior, luxury products led people to behave in a more generous

manner.

Taken together, Study 4 is consistent with the notion that luxury consumption

spurs self-interested behavior. Self-interested behavior is manifested as more selfish

behavior in private, but is manifested as more generous behavior in public where it can

boost a person’s reputation.

STUDY 5: LUXURY CONSUMPTION AND REAL DONATION BEHAVIOR

Study 5 aimed to replicate the findings in Study 4 with actual money donation as

the dependent measure and a different brand luxury product. Women once again walked

around a busy area with either a luxury or a non-luxury product—a Burberry scarf or a

non-luxury scarf. All people received money for participating in the study and were later

presented with an opportunity to donate some or all of the money to charity. Importantly,

the donation was made either privately or publically. As in Study 4, we predicted that

when people made the donation in private, wearing a luxury product would lead people to

31
donate less money, while wearing the luxury product would lead them to behave more

generously and donate more money in public.

Method

Participants. One-hundred and two female students (Mage = 20.10, SD = 1.07) in

an introductory business course at a large North American university participated in the

study in exchange for partial course credit. Two participants did not follow instructions

(e.g., they spent an inordinate amount of time walking around campus and came back to

the lab after the session had already ended) and were excluded from the analyses. The

study had the following between-subjects design: 2 (product type: luxury vs. control) X 2

(donation context: public vs. private).

Procedure. Participants arrived individually for a study on consumer products.

Women were told that the study was about scarves and that each participant would be

given a scarf to try out by wearing it around campus. To make the experience as realistic

as possible, participants were asked to imagine the scarf belonged to them and were

allowed to wear it in any style they preferred. Participants were instructed to follow the

same walking path as in previous studies.

Luxury Consumption Experience. Participants were randomly assigned to receive

one of two scarves. In the luxury condition, participants were given a Burberry scarf and

were explicitly told that this is a brand new Burberry scarf. In the control condition,

participants were given a non-luxury scarf similar in size and style to the Burberry scarf

and were told this is a brand new scarf from a department store (see illustration A for

pictures). The Burberry scarf retails for $385, whereas the control scarf retails for $45,

although no price information was provided to participants. Burberry was chosen as the

32
luxury brand because, like Prada and Louis Vuitton, it is not only well-known, but is also

highly-desired. Pretesting indicated that our participant population rated Burberry an

average of 5.50 in desirability on a 1-7 scale.

Dependent Measure. The dependent measure consisted of the amount of money

participants donated to charity. To ensure that all participants had some money they could

donate, participants were informed that they would receive $5 in cash for being in the

study (in addition to receiving the expected partial course credit). Participants were also

informed that they would have an opportunity to donate some or all of the money to a

charity of their choice. Later in the study, all participants received a request to donate to

charity. The donation request was made either in private or in public.

In the private condition, the donation request was made via a letter while the

person was alone in a lab room (same location used for administering the main survey in

the previous studies). After participants had walked around campus with the scarf and

had come back to the lab, they were seated in a private room to fill out a survey on the

computer. The computer instructions directed them to open an envelope that was placed

on the desk that contained a note, a request card, and a pencil. The note reminded

participants about getting $5 for their participation in the study and explained that, “In an

effort to increase social awareness, we usually ask participants in our lab if you would

like to make a donation to a charity of your choice.” The card provided names of three

charities that participants could select (American Red Cross, Children’s Hospital,

Livestrong Foundation). Participants could also write in a different charity if they

preferred.

33
The card provided six options for the donation amount: $0, $1, $2, $3, $4, or $5.

After making a choice, participants were directed (via the computer) to place the card

back in the sealed envelope and put it back on the table. The dependent measure

consisted of how much money participants donated to charity.

In the public condition, the donation was made publically. After their walk around

campus with the scarf, participants were instructed to stop by a donation desk that was set

up at the entrance of a busy administrative office. When participants arrived at the desk,

they were greeted by a person working at the desk, who was actually a female research

assistant blind to the study hypotheses. The person at the desk handed out the same

donation request letter as the one used in the private condition to the participants.

Therefore, participants in the public condition did not receive more or less

encouragement from the donation request. After reading the donation request letter,

participants were then given a pencil and the same donation card with the same charities

as in the other condition. Participants were verbally informed that after they made a

decision they would need to hand the card to the person at the desk so that she could

write down the person’s name and record the donation. After handing in the donation

card, participants were directed back to the lab to complete the remainder of the study.

Results and Discussion

The average donation amount in the study was $3.57 (SD = $1.87). To test

whether wearing the luxury scarf had a different effect in public and private donation

contexts, we conducted an ANOVA with product type and donation context. The analysis

did not reveal any main effects (ps > .27), but it did show an interaction with product type

and donation context (F(1,98) = 0.87, p = .004). As seen in figure 6, luxury consumption

34
had a different effect on donations depending on whether the donation was made in

public versus in private.

--------------------------
figure 6 about here
--------------------------
In the private donation condition, women donated less money to charity in the

luxury condition (M = $2.84, SD=1.87) versus the control condition (M=$3.88, SD=1.92;

t(98) = 2.06, p = .041). Thus, wearing a luxury product once again led women to behave

in a more selfish manner when the situation explicitly did not allow people to enhance

their reputation by helping.

In the public donation condition, by contrast, women donated more money to

charity in the luxury condition (M = $4.31, SD=1.40) compared to the control condition

(M=$3.20, SD=1.99; t(98) = 2.14, p = .034). Thus, when the donation was made in public,

women who wore a luxury products once again became more generous.

To look at the findings another way, the study found that participants who wore

the control scarf did not differ in their donations when the donation context was public or

private (p =. 19; see figure 6). However, participants who wore the luxury scarf exhibited

a large difference in donations depending on whether they were public or private. Women

wearing the luxury scarf gave substantially more real money to charity when the donation

context was public rather than private (M = $4.31 vs. $2.84; t(98) = 2.91, p = .001).

In summary, Study 5 replicated the specific pattern of findings from Study 4 using

actual monetary donations. When asked to make a donation to charity in private, women

wearing a Burberry scarf donated over 25% less money than those wearing a non-luxury

scarf. But when asked to make a donation in public, those wearing a Burberry scarf

donated over 40% more money than those wearing a non-luxury scarf. Taken together,

35
Study 5 once again showed that luxury products can make people less generous or more

generous depending on whether the situation explicitly allows a person to enhance their

reputation via helping behavior.

STUDY 6: ELIMINATING THE EFFECT OF LUXURY CONSUMPTION

The last study sought to “turn off” the effect of luxury consumption by

considering a situation in which luxury products should not influence behavior. The

psychological reason why luxury products alter behavior is because consuming luxury

goods increases a person’s sense of social status, as demonstrated via mediation in Study

3. The current study derived and tested a condition under which luxury should lose its

luster by experimentally manipulating whether a luxury product does or does not increase

a person’s sense of social status.

Luxury products are associated with social status, in part, because such products

are positional goods that derive their value from being rare, exclusive, and difficult to

obtain (Nelissen and Meijers 2011; Plourde 2008). For example, a person driving a

Ferrari is perceived as having higher social status in part because most other people do

not have this car. If many other people were to drive the same car, however, a Ferrari

would be much less effective at enhancing social status. Indeed, luxury products do not

effectively increase social status if other people have similar products (Kuksov and Xie

2012).

In Study 6, women once again walked around a busy area with either a luxury

(Burberry) or a non-luxury scarf. As before, the Burberry scarf featured the iconic check

pattern (see illustration A), which is easily identified as the brand’s signature by

consumers familiar with luxury brands. In Study 6, we experimentally manipulated

36
whether the Burberry iconic check pattern was perceived as rare and exclusive or as

commonplace. We predicted that when the luxury scarf is perceived as commonplace, it

would no longer boost a person’s sense of status.

Study 6 assessed how much money people donated to charity in a public context

using the same behavioral measure as in Study 5. As in Study 5, we predicted that

wearing the Burberry scarf would once again make women more generous. However, we

predicted that depicting the luxury product as commonplace would eliminate this effect.

When a luxury product does not increase a person’s sense of status, wearing the luxury

product should have no effect on donation behavior.

Method

Participants and Design. One-hundred and seven female students (Mage = 20.09,

SD = 1.03) in an introductory business course at a North American university participated

in the study in exchange for partial course credit. Seven participants failed to follow

instructions (e.g., they held the scarf in their hand instead of wearing it) and were not

included in the data analysis. The study had three between-subjects conditions: control,

luxury, and luxury without status boost.

Procedure and Dependent Measures. The procedure was identical to the public

donation context condition in Study 5. Participants came to the lab individually, were

given either a brand new luxury (Burberry) scarf or a brand new non-luxury scarf, walked

around campus with the scarf, and learned that they would receive $5 for participating in

the study (in addition to receiving the expected course credit). All participants were

instructed that they would stop by a coffee shop, and should find a place to sit and open

the handbag. Inside the handbag, they would find a pencil and a survey to complete.

37
Embedded in this survey were five items measuring perceptions of their current level of

social status. The five items measuring perception of social status were identical to the

mediation items used in Study 3 and were averaged to form a perceived social status

index (α = .83). Finally, all participants were provided with a public opportunity to

donate some or all of the money they received for participating in the study to charity,

using the same procedure described in Study 5.

Luxury Consumption Experience. Participants were randomly assigned to receive

either a luxury scarf or a non-luxury scarf, using the same scarves described in Study 5.

Unlike in Study 5, the current study had two different luxury product conditions. One of

the luxury conditions (“luxury”) was essentially identical to the luxury product conditions

in Studies 1-5.

In the other luxury condition (“luxury without status boost”), participants read a

short news article about Burberry before trying out the scarf. The article briefly described

the history of the brand, including the iconic check pattern. It then described that the

check pattern was becoming common, with over 50% of women owning a Burberry

accessory with the iconic check pattern. This material was, in fact, taken from an actual

news article (Jones 2008). To bolster the notion that the check pattern was commonplace,

several Burberry products sporting the iconic check pattern were placed along the

walking path these participants followed: (1) a female confederate with a large Burberry

check pattern tote bag was stationed on a sofa next to the elevator used by participants; (2)

in the office where charity donations were solicited, a check-patterned Burberry garment

bag with a dry cleaning tag and a pair of Burberry rain boots with the check pattern were

38
placed by the chair; and (3) a Burberry umbrella with the check pattern was placed in a

lost and found box next to the donation desk.

To ensure consistency in the procedure in both luxury conditions, in the basic

“luxury” condition, participants also read a news article before receiving and wearing the

Burberry scarf. The article briefly described the history of the brand, noting how the

iconic check pattern had become a symbol of luxury.

Results and Discussion

Donation. An omnibus ANOVA revealed a significant effect of condition on

donations (F(2,97) = 3.97, p = .051). As seen in figure 7, women in the luxury condition

donated more money (M = $4.35, SD=1.54) to charity than women in the control

condition (M=$3.47, SD=1.79; t(97) = -2.02, p = .047). Thus, replicating the finding from

Studies 4 and 5, women wearing a Burberry scarf became more generous when they had

an opportunity to donate in public.

--------------------------
figure 7 about here
--------------------------

By contrast, wearing the same luxury scarf had no effect on donations when the

luxury product was commonplace. There was no difference in donations in the luxury

without status boost condition and the control condition (M = $3.38 vs. $3.35; t(97) = .19,

p = .84). Further, even though women wore a Burberry scarf in both conditions, women

in the luxury without status boost condition donated significantly less money compared to

women in the regular luxury product condition (M = $3.38 vs. $4.35; t(97) = -2.24, p

= .027).

39
Perceived Social Status. An omnibus ANOVA revealed a significant main effect

of condition on the social status index (F(2, 97) = 3.55, p = .033). Planned contrasts

confirmed that participants in the luxury condition perceived themselves as having higher

social status (M = 4.00, SD = 1.04) than participants in the luxury without status boost

condition (M = 3.47, SD = 1.01, t(97) = -2.08, p = .04), indicating that our manipulation

was successful. Further, there was no difference in perceived social status between the

luxury without status boost condition and the control condition (M = 3.36, SD = 1.09,

t(97) = .42, p = .68). Finally, replicating the previous finding from Study 3, participants

in the luxury condition experienced higher perceived social status than participants in the

control condition (t(97) = 2.47, p = .015).

Mediation Analysis. Next, we tested whether women’s perceived social status

statistically mediated the effect of luxury consumption on donation behavior. As reported

earlier, there was no difference between the control and luxury without status boost

conditions in terms of the donation amounts and perceived social status. We, therefore,

combined these two conditions for the mediation analysis.

The first regression revealed that condition (luxury vs. other conditions) predicted

perceptions of social status (path a: β = .26, p = .01) and donation amount (path c: β = .24,

p = .008). Additionally, perceived social status also predicted donation amount (path b: β

= .29, p = .003). Following Preacher and Hayes (2008), a 10,000 resample bootstrap

(Hayes (2012), Model 4) revealed a significant indirect effect of condition on donation

amount via perceived social status, b = .24 (SE = .13), 95% CI [.04, .61]. Because the

confidence interval does not include 0, this means that the effect of condition on donation

40
amount was statistically mediated by perceived social status (see figure 8 for a visual

depiction of the mediation model).

--------------------------
Figure 8 about here
--------------------------

We repeated the mediation analysis for the luxury and the luxury without status

boost conditions, since these were the conditions of focal interest for the study. The first

regression revealed that condition (luxury vs. luxury without status) predicted perceptions

of social status (path a: β = .25, p = .037) and donation amount (path c: β = .27, p = .027).

Further, perceived social status also predicted donation amount (path b: β = .30, p = .014).

A 10,000 resample bootstrap (Hayes (2012), Model 4) revealed a significant indirect

effect of condition on donation amount via perceived social status, b = .22 (SE = .16),

95% CI [.01, .66]. Once again, this analysis confirmed that effect of the luxury condition

(luxury vs. luxury without status) on donation amount was statistically mediated by

perceived social status.

In summary, Study 6 replicated the public giving findings from Studies 4 and 5.

Women wearing a luxury product once again behaved more generously in public contexts.

In addition, Study 6 showed that the effects of luxury consumption can be “turned off”

when the luxury product is perceived as more common and, therefore, does not increase a

person’s sense of social status. Furthermore, Study 6 tested the psychological mechanism

underlying how luxury consumption affects generous behavior. The same psychological

mechanism responsible for triggering selfish behavior—perceived social status—also

proved to be the underlying process that triggers generous behavior when there was an

opportunity to enhance reputation through generous behavior.

41
GENERAL DISCUSSION

Does the Devil wear Prada? This phrase implies that “bad” people wear luxury

brands, but our empirical findings suggest the opposite possibility: wearing Prada can

lead ordinary people to behave badly. We found that the experience of using a luxury

product boosted women’s self-perceptions of social status. This state then triggered self-

interested behavior, with women making choices to benefit themselves.

We found that most of the time self-interested behavior manifested as selfish

behavior. We repeatedly found that women wearing luxury products behaved more

selfishly, including by taking more money for themselves and donating less money to

charity when no one was around to see it. However, we found that self-interested

behavior manifested as generous behavior when the situation explicitly afforded people

an opportunity to enhance their reputation by acting generously. When donations were

made in public in front of other people, wearing luxury products led women to donate

more money to charity. We contend that the types of selfish and generous behavior

assessed in our studies all constitute self-interested behavior. Both selfish and generous

behavior benefitted the person by either enabling them to acquire more resources or by

enabling them to gain a boost in reputation. Indeed, both selfish and generous behaviors

were driven by the same psychological mechanism, whereby both types of behavior were

triggered by an increased sense of social status that resulted from consuming luxury

goods.

Overall, we show that luxury consumption leads consumers to act in ways to

benefit the self, which can result in either more selfish or more generous behavior

depending on the situation. We obtained these robust effects across six studies that varied

42
the type of luxury product (handbag, scarf) and luxury brand (Prada, Louis Vuitton,

Burberry). The pattern also persisted regardless of whether the dependent measure

consisted of hypothetical situations, actual behavior, or incentive-compatible decisions

concerning real money. Taken together, these findings provide novel and compelling

evidence that using luxury goods affects how consumers feel and behave. Below, we

discuss the contributions of our findings to several research streams, including luxury

consumption and branding research, and suggest avenues for future research.

Luxury Consumption Research

Ever since Veblen’s classic work The Theory of Leisure Class (1899), researchers

in domains such as sociology, psychology, economics, and marketing have been

interested in the phenomenon of luxury consumption. For more than a century,

researchers have focused on the motivational aspects of luxury consumption. Findings

show that consumers desire luxury products for a variety of reasons, including increasing

one’s sense of well-being (Belk 1985; Richins 1987), increasing self-esteem

(Sivananthan and Pettit 2010; Solomon 1983), compensating for feelings of powerless

(Rucker et al. 2012), and affiliating with favored social groups (Berger and Ward 2010;

Han et al. 2010). While this rich set of findings has bettered our understanding of why

people desire luxury goods, there is a dearth of research examining the actual experience

and consequences of consuming luxury goods.

To our knowledge, our research is the first to systematically examine how the

consumption of luxury goods affects consumers’ psychological and behavioral responses.

In doing so, we show that using luxury goods causes a set of unintended consequences,

many of them quite negative in nature. In contrast, the prior research cited above focuses

43
on expected positive consequences of luxury consumption such as increasing self-esteem

or providing some positive benefit. Although luxury users in our studies do feel in a boost

in social status, this often leads them to behave in selfish and mean ways. Thus, we

demonstrate that there is a “dark side” of luxury consumption, beyond one’s pocketbook

or materialistic tendencies.

Notably, the “dark side” of luxury consumption also impacts others who interact

with luxury users. The self-interested behaviors exhibited by luxury users in our studies

impose costs on other people. Prior work in areas related to luxury consumption, such as

conspicuous consumption and materialism, have shown that this type of consumption can

negatively affect the well-being of individual consumers (de Graaf, Wann, and Naylor

2001; Kasser 2002; Schor 2004). In contrast, we show that luxury consumption can have

negative consequences for the well-being of other people. In a sense, this also affects

societal well-being in that prosocial behavior (giving, sharing, and helping) is important

to the fabric that holds communities and societies together.

There is a silver lining to our research, however. Luxury can lead people to be

more generous, but only if the behavioral context affords an explicit opportunity to

enhance reputation by behaving more generously. Although giving behavior under these

circumstances is beneficial to the giver, it does indicate that it is not inevitable that luxury

use will lead to selfish behavior.

Branding Research

The current research also relates to findings showing how brands can influence

our behavior. Just being exposed to a brand name or logo can be consequential. For

example, incidental exposure to a brand logo (e.g., Apple logo) can activate behaviors

44
related to the brand’s image, such as being creative (Chartrand et al. 2008; Fitzsimons,

Chartrand, and Fitzsimons 2008). Similarly, using a branded product that is associated

with certain outcomes (e.g., Gatorade and athletic performance) can increase the user’s

exercise performance, despite the fact that the actual beverage consumed has no

properties that would be beneficial to this type of performance (Park and John 2014;

Irmak, Block, and Fitzsimons 2005).

We add to this emerging body of research by showing that using luxury brands

also impacts behavior, albeit in a very different way than prior demonstrations. Prior

research has found that brands affect behavior through direct associations with the

brand’s image, such as Apple (creative) or Gatorade (athletic performance). Our studies

are different in the sense that the behaviors we observe (e.g., splitting money, donations)

are not directly related to the brand’s image, but are downstream consequences of using a

luxury brand.

Further, we find that use of luxury brands not only affects the user, but also other

people who interact with the luxury user. Prior research has focused on a brand’s effect

on individual behavior, such as being more creative or exercising longer. We extend this

research into new territory by examining whether brands use can trigger certain types of

behavior that have consequences for others as well as oneself.

Future Research

Our research provides a starting point for further examination of luxury

consumption. First, our finding that luxury consumption produces feelings of higher

social status suggests that luxury product use might have a variety of behavioral

consequences. For example, past research shows that individuals with higher status or

45
power are more independent, exhibit greater action-orientation and greater abstraction,

and engage in more risk taking behaviors (Dubois and Ordabayeva forthcoming; Maner

et al. 2007). This suggests that using a luxury product might result in more risky behavior.

Second, the behavioral effects of luxury consumption we observed may be

moderated by a number of individual differences. An individual’s need for status would

appear to be a strong candidate in this regard. It might be expected that consumers with a

higher need for status (Eastman, Goldsmith, and Flynn 1999; Han et al. 2010), defined as

a tendency to purchase products for their status and prestige value, would experience

stronger feelings of status when using a luxury product, leading to stronger behavioral

effects. A second potential moderator is culture. In the current study, the overwhelming

majority of participants were U.S. citizens. Women from other countries and cultures

may respond differently. For example, women from Eastern cultures, where the social

hierarchy tends to be more salient than in Western cultures, might experience stronger

feelings of increased social status and exhibit stronger behavioral effects from using

luxury products.

Third, we focused our studies on women because the products we examined (e.g.,

handbag, scarf) are more relevant to women consumers. However, we expect that luxury

consumption should also have the similar effects on men. In fact, based on past research

has shown that males exhibit a stronger preference for signaling power and status (Hays

2013; Melnyk and Osselaer 2012), it is reasonable to argue that the effects demonstrated

in the current paper should even be stronger among male consumers. Future research can

expand the product type to be more male relevant and explore how luxury consumption

can influence male consumers’ social behaviors.

46
Our findings also link to related research on hormones. For example, compared to

driving a non-luxury car, the experience of driving a luxury has been shown to boost

men’s testosterone levels (Saad and Vongas 2009). Future research is needed to examine

how luxury consumption might influence women’s hormonal responses. Finally, the

behavioral effects of luxury consumption may also differ depending on the audience,

given that the nature of the audience is an important factor that affects luxury

consumption (e.g., Berger and Ward 2010; Han et al. 2010; Wang and Griskevicius 2014).

Addressing these questions, and others, holds the promise of understanding the

consequences of luxury consumption in a more compelling way. Our findings show that

luxury consumption can lead to both selfish and generous behaviors, and we invite

further research to examine a wider range of psychological and behavioral consequences.

47
ILLUSTRATION A

PRODUCTS USED ACROSS STUDIEs


Study 1 and Study 3

Luxury Handbag (Louis Vuitton) Control Handbag

Study 2 and Study 4

Luxury Handbag (Prada) Control Handbag

Study 5 and Study 6

Luxury Scarf (Burberry) Control Scarf

48
REFERENCES

Archer, John (2006), “Testosterone and Human Aggression: An Evaluation of the

Challenge Hypothesis,” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 319-45.

Bagwell, Laurie S. and B. Douglas Bernheim (1996), “Veblen Effects in a Theory of

Conspicuous Consumption,” The American Economic Review, 86, 3 (Jun), 349-73.

Belk, Russell W. (1985), “Trait Aspects of Living in the Material World,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 12 (December), 265-80.

Belk, Russell W., Kenneth D. Bahn, and Robert N. Mayer (1982), "Developmental

Recognition of Consumption Symbolism,” Journal of Consumer Research, 9

(June), 4-16.

Berger, Jonah and Morgan Ward (2010), “Subtle Signals of Inconspicuous

Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (December), 555-69.

Boksem, Maarten A. S., Pranjal H. Mehta, Bram Van den Bergh, Veerie van Son, Stefan

T. Trautmann, Karin Roelofs, Ale Smidts, and Alan G. Sanfey (2013),

“Testosterone Inhibits Trust but Promotes Reciprocity,” Psychological Science,

24(11), 2306-14.

Charles, Kerwin Kofi, Erik Hurst, and Nikolai Roussanov (2009), “Conspicuous

Consumption and Race,” The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 124 (2), 425-67.

Chartrand, Tanya L., Joel Huber, Baba Shiv, and Robin J. Tanner (2008), “Nonconscious

Goals and Consumer Choice,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (August), 189-

201.

Cole, Douglas and Ira Chaikin (1990). An Iron Band upon the People – The Law Against

the Potlatch on the Northwest Coast, Seattle, WA: University of Washington

Press.

49
Cottrell, Catherine A., Steven L. Neuberg, and Norman P. Li (2007), “What do People

Desire in Others? A Sociofunctional Perspective on the Importance of Different

Valued Characteristics,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(2),

208-31.

D’Arpizio, Claudia (2014), “Luxury Goods Worldwide Market Study,” Bain & Company,

accessed May 01, 2014, http://www.bain.com/about/press/press-

releases/worldwide-luxury-goods-continues-double-digit-annual-growth.aspx

Dabbs, James M. and Marian F. Hargrove (1997), “Age, Testosterone, and Behavior

Among Female Prison Inmates,” Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 477-80.

Dabbs, James M. and Robin Morris (1990), “Testosterone, Social Class, and Antisocial

Behavior in a Sample of 4,462 Men,” Psychological Science, 209-11.

de Graaf, John, David Wann, and Thomas H. Naylor (2001). Affluenza: The All-

Consuming Epidemic. San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler.

Dreze, Xavier and Joseph C. Nunes (2009), “Feeling Superior: The Impact of Loyalty

Program Structure on Consumers’ Perceptions of Status,” Journal of Consumer

Research, 35 (April), 890- 905.

Dubois, David and Nailya Ordabayeva (in press), “Social Hierarchy, Social Status, and

Status Consumption,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Consumer Psychology, eds.

Michael Norton, Derek Rucker, and Cait Lamberton.

Eastman, Jacqueline K., Ronald E. Goldsmith, and Leisa Reinecke Flynn (1999), “Status

Consumption in Consumer Behavior: Scale Development and Validation,”

Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 7(Summer), 41-52.

50
French, John R. P. Jr. and Bertram Raven (1959), The Bases of Social Power, Ann Arbor,

MI: University of Michigan Press.

Fitzsimons, Gráinne M, Tanya L. Chartrand, and Gavan Fitzsimons (2008), “Automatic

Effects of Brand Exposure on Motivated Behavior: How Apple Makes You

“Think Different”,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (June), 21-35.

Fuchs, Christoph, Emanuela Prandelli, Martin Shreier, and Darren W. Dahl (2013), “All

That Is Users Might Not Be Gold: How Labeling Products as User Designed

Backfires in the Context of Luxury Fashion Brands,” Journal of Marketing, 77

(September), 75-91.

Galinksy, Adam D., Joe C. Magee, M. Ena Inesi, and Deborah H. Gruenfeld (2006),

“Power and Perspective Not Taken,” Psychological Science, 17 (2), 1068-74.

Griskevicius, Vladas and Douglas T. Kenrick (2013), “Fundamental Motives for Why

We Buy: How Evolutionary Needs Influence Consumer Behavior,” Journal of

Consumer Psychology, 23(3), 372-386.

Griskevicius, Vladas, Joshua M. Tybur, Jill M. Sundie, Robert B. Cialdini, Geoffrey F.

Miller, and Douglas T. Kenrick (2007), “Blatant Benevolence and Conspicuous

Consumption: When Romantic Motives Elicit Strategic Costly Signals,” Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 93 (1), 85-102.

Griskevicius, Vladas, Joshua M. Tybur, and Bram Van den Bergh (2010) “Going Green

to Be Seen: Status, Reputation, and Conspicuous Conservation,” Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 392-404.

51
Gurven, Michael, Wesley Allen-Arave, Kim Hill, and Magdalena Hurtado (2000), ““It's a

Wonderful Life”: Signaling Generosity among the Ache of Paraguay." Evolution

and Human Behavior 21(4), 263-82.

Han, Young Jee, Joseph C. Nunes, and Xavier Drèze (2010), “Signaling Status with

Luxury Goods: The Role of Brand Prominence,” Journal of Marketing, 74 (July),

15-30.

Hardy, Charlie L. and Mark Van Vugt (2006), “Nice Guys Finish First: The Competitive

Altruism Hypothesis,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1402-13.

Hayes, Andrew F. (2012), “PROCESS: A Versatile Computational Tool for Observed

Variable Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Modeling [White

paper],” Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/

Hays, Nicholas A. (2013), “Fear and Loving in Social Hierarchy: Sex Differences in

Preferences for Power versus Status,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,

49(6), 1130-1136.

Iredale, Wendy, Mark Van Vugt, and Robin Dunbar (2008), “Showing Off in Humans:

Male Generosity as a Mating Signal,” Evolutionary Psychology, 6(3), 386-92.

Irmak, Caglar, Lauren G. Block, and Gavan J. Fitzsimons (2005), “The Placebo Effect in

Marketing: Sometimes You Just Have to Want It to Work,” Journal of Marketing

Research, 42 (November), 406-09.

Jones, Liz (2008), “The Luxury Brand with a Chequered Past, Burberry’s Shaken off Its

Chav Image to Become the Fashionistas’ Favourite Once More,” Mail Online,

accessed February 10, 2014, http://www.dailymail.co.uk/femail/article-

1023460/Burberrys-shaken-chav-image-fashionistas-favourite-more.html.

52
Jones, Lyle V. (1952), “Tests of Hypotheses: One-Sided vs. Two-Sided Alternatives,”

Psychological Bulletin, 49 (1), 43-6.

Kasser, Tim (2002). The High Price of Materialism. Mass.: MIT Press.

Kenrick, Douglas T., Vladas Griskevicius, Steven L. Neuberg, and Mark Schaller (2010),

“Renovating the Pyramid of Needs: Contemporary Extensions Built Upon

Ancient Foundations,” Perspective on Psychological Science, 5(3),292-314.

Kuksov, Dmitri and Ying Xie (2012), “Competition in a Status Goods Market,” Journal

of Marketing Research, 49(October), 609-23.

Lee, Jaehoon and L. J. Shrum (2012), “Conspicuous Consumption versus Charitable

Behavior in Response to Social Exclusion: A Differential Needs Explanation,”

Journal of Consumer Research 39 (October), 530-44.

Lindberg, Nina, Pekka Tani, Bjorn Appelberg, Hannu Naukkarinen, Ranan Rimon, Tarja

Porkka-Heiskanen, and Matti Vrikkunen (2003), “Human Impulsive Aggression:

A Sleep Research Perspective,” Journal of Psychiatric Research, 37, 313-24.

Magee, Joe C. and Adam D. Galinsky (2008), “Social Hierarchy: The Self-Reinforcing

Nature of Power and Status,” The Academy of Management Annals, 2(1), 351-98.

Maner, Jon K., Matthew T. Gailliot, David A. Butz, and B. Michelle Peruche (2007),

“Power, Risk, and the Status Quo: Does Power Promote Riskier or More

Conservative Decision Making?” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,

33(4), 451-62.

Maner, Jon K. and Nicole L. Mead (2010), “The Essential Tension Between Leadership

and Power: When Leaders Sacrifice Group Goals for the Sake of Self-Interest,”

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99 (3), 482-97.

53
Mazzocco, Philip. J., Derek D. Rucker, Adam D. Galinsky, and Eric T. Anderson (2012),

“Direct and Vicarious Conspicuous Consumption: Identification with Low-Status

Groups Increase the Desire for High-Status Goods,” Journal of Consumer

Psychology, 22, 520-28.

Melnyk, Valentyna, Stijn MJ van Osselaer and Tammo HA Bijmolt (2009), “Are Women

More Loyal Customers Than Men? Gender Differences in Loyalty to Firms and

Individual Service Providers,” Journal of Marketing, 73(4), 82-96.

Miller, Geoffrey F. (2007), “Sexual Selection for Moral Virtues,” Quarterly Review of

Biology, 82, 97-125.

Murdock, George Peter (1970), “Rand and Potlatch among the Haida,” Yale University

Publications in Anthropology, 13, New Haven, CT: Human Relations Area Files

Press.

Nelissen, Rob M.A. and Marijn H.C. Meijers (2012), “Social Benefits of Luxury Brands

as Costly Signals of Wealth and Status,” Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(5),

343-55.

Ordabayeva, Nailya and Pierre Chandon (2011), “Getting ahead of Joneses: When

Equality Increases Conspicuous Consumption Among Bottom-Tier Consumers,”

Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (June), 27-41.

Park, Ji Kyung and Deborah Roedder John (2014), “I Think I Can, I Think I Can: Brand

Use, Self-Efficacy, and Performance,” Journal of Marketing Research, 51(April),

233-47.

Patrick Vanessa M. and Henrik Hagtvedt (2009), “Luxury Branding,” The Handbook of

Brand Relationships. NY: Society for Consumer Psychology, 267-80.

54
Phau, Ian and Gerard Prendergast (2000), “Consuming Luxury Brands: The Relevance of

the ‘Rarity Principle’,” Journal of Brand Management, 7 (5), 366-75.

Phillips, Jordan (2012), “The Lure of Luxe: Climbing The Luxury Consumption

Pyramid,” North Charleston, SC: CreateSpace Independent Publishing Platform.

Piff, Paul K, Michael W. Kraus, Stephane Cote, Bonnie Hayden Cheng, and Dacher

Keltner (2010), “Having Less, Giving More: The Influence of Social Class on

Prosocial Behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99 (5), 771-

84.

Piff, Paul K, Daniel M. Stancato, Stephane Cote, Rodolfo Mendoze-Denton, and Dacher

Keltner (2012), “Higher Status Class Predicts Increased Unethical Behavior,”

Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 109(11), 4086-91.

Plourde, Aimee M. (2008), “The Origins of Prestige Goods as Honest Signals of Skill

and Knowledge,” Human Nature, 19 (4), 374-88.

Preacher, Kristopher J. and Andrew Hayes (2008), “Asymptotic and Resampling

Strategies for Assessing and Comparing Indirect Effects in Multiple Mediation

Model,” Behavior Research Method, 40 (3), 879-91.

Richins, Marsha L. (1987), “Media, Materialism, and Human Happiness,” Advances in

Consumer Research, 14 (1), 352-56.

———--- (1994), “Special Possessions and the Expression of Material Values,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 21 (December), 522-33.

Ridgeway, Cecilia L. and Henry A. Walker (1995), “Status Structure,” in Karen S. Cook,

Gary Alan Fine, and James S. House, eds. Sociological Perspectives on Social

Psychology. Needham Heights: Allyn and Bacon.

55
Rucker, Derek D., David Dubois, and Adam D. Galinsky (2011), “Generous Paupers and

Stingy Princes: Power Drives Consumer Spending on Self versus Others,”

Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (April), 1015-29.

Rucker, Derek D. and Adam D. Galinsky (2008), “Desire to Acquire: Powerlessness and

Compensatory Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35(August), 257-

67.

———--- (2009), “Conspicuous Consumption versus Utilitarian Ideals: How Different

Level of Power Shape Consumer Behavior,” Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 45(3), 549-55.

Rucker, Derek D., Adam D. Galinsky, and David Dubois (2012), “Power and Consumer

Behavior: How Power Shapes Who and What Consumer Value,” Journal of

Consumer Psychology, 22, 352-68.

Saad, Gad and John G. Vongas (2009), “The Effect of Conspicuous Consumption on

Men’s Testosterone Levels,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 110, 80-92.

Schor, Juliet B. (2004), Born to Buy, New York: Scribner.

Semmann, Dirk, Hans-Jürgen Krambeck, and Manfred Milinski (2005), “Reputation is

Valuable Within and Outside One’s Own Social Group,” Behavioral Ecology and

Sociobiology, 57(6), 611-16.

Sivanathan, Niro and Nathan C. Pettit (2010), “Protecting the Self through Consumption:

Status Goods as Affirmational Commodities,” Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 46, 564-70.

56
Smith, Eric Alden and Rebecca Bird (2000), “Turtle Hunting and Tombstone Opening:

Public Generosity as Costly Signaling,” Evolution and Human Behavior, 21(4),

245-61.

Solomon, Michael R. (1983), “The Role of Products as Social Stimuli: A Symbolic

Interactionism Perspective,” Journal of Consumer Research, 10 (December), 319-

29.

Sparshott, Jeffrey (2014), “What Products Drove Consumer Spending? Luxury Items,

Mostly,” The Wall Street Journal, accessed February 11, 2014,

http://blogs.wsj.com/economics/2014/01/22/what-products-drove-consumer-

spending-luxury-items-mostly/

Statista (2014), “Value of the Personal Luxury Goods Market Worldwide From 1995 to

2013,” The Statistics Portal, accessed September 2014, http://www.statista.com/

statistics/266503/value-of-the-personal-luxury-goods-market-worldwide/

Stellar, Jennifer E., Vida M. Manzo, Michael W. Kraus, and Dacher Keltner (2012),

“Class and Compassion: Socioeconomic Factors Predict Responses to Suffering,”

Emotion,12 (3), 449-59.

Stiff, Chris and Mark Van Vugt (2008), “The Power of Reputations: The Role of Third

Party Information in the Admission of New Group Members,” Group Dynamics:

Theory, Research, and Practice 12, 155-66.

Sundie Jill M., Douglas T. Kenrick, Vladas Grikevicius, Joshua M. Tybur, Kathleen D.

Vohs, and Daniel J. Beal (2011), “Peacocks, Porsches, and Thorstein Veblen:

Conspicuous Consumption as a Sexual Signaling System,” Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 100 (4), 664-80.

57
Van Vugt, Mark and Charlotte L. Hardy (2009), “Cooperation for Reputation: Wasteful

Contributions as Costly Signals in Public Goods,” Group Processes Intergroup

Relations, 13(1), 101-11.

Veblen, Thorstein (1899), The Theory of the Leisure Class, New York: Penguin.

Vigneron, Franck and Lester W. Johnson (2004), “Measuring Perceptions of Brand

Luxury,” Brand Management, 11 (July), 484-506.

Wang, Yajin and Vladas Griskevicius (2014), “Conspicuous Consumption, Relationships,

and Rivals: Women’s Luxury Products as Signals to Other Women,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 40 (February), 834-54.

Wedekind, Claus and Victoria A. Braithwaite (2002), “The Long-Term Benefits of

Human Generosity in Indirect Reciprocity,” Current Biology, 12(12), 1012-15.

Wernerfelt, Birger (1990), “Advertising Content When Brand Choice is a Signal,” The

Journal of Business, 63 (1), 91-8.

Wilcox, Keith, Hyeong Min Kim, and Sankar Sen (2009), “Why Do Consumers Buy

Counterfeit Luxury Brands?” Journal of Marketing Research, 46 (April), 247-59.

Wong, Jimmy and Sharon Shavitt (2010), “Be Rude to Me and I Will Buy a Rolex:

Effects of Culture Orientation on Responses to Power Threat in a Service

Setting,” Advances in Consumer Psychology, 2, 95.

Zak, Paul J., Robert Kruzban, Sheila Ahmadi, Ronald S. Swerdloff, Jang Park, Levan

Efremidze, Karen Redwine, Karla Morgan, and William Matzner (2009),

“Testosterone Administration Decreases Generosity in the Ultimatum Game,”

PLoS One, 4 (12), e8330.

58
Zitek, Emily M., Alexander H. Jordan, Benoit Monin, and Frederick R. Leach (2010),

“Victim Entitlement to Behave Selfish,” Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 98(2), 245-55.

59
FIGURE 1

EFFECT OF LUXURY CONSUMPTION ON SELFISH BEHAVIOR (STUDY 1)

60
FIGURE 2

EFFECT OF LUXURY CONSUMPTION ON SELFISH BEHAVIOR (STUDY 2)

61
FIGURE 3

EFFECT OF LUXURY CONSUMPTION ON SELFISH BEHAVIOR (STUDY 3)

62
FIGURE 4

EFFECT OF LUXURY CONSUMPTION ON SELFISH BEHAVIOR MEDIATED

BY PERCEIVED SOCIAL STATUS (STUDY 3)

Perceived
Social Status
.44** -.42**

Product Type:
Luxury vs. Control Selfish Money
.31** Allocation
(.16)

Note: Coefficients are standardized


p<.001, ***; p<.01, **; p<.05, *

63
FIGURE 5

EFFECT OF LUXURY CONSUMPTION ON CHARITY DONATIONS

DEPENDING ON WHETHER DONATION IS PUBLIC OR PRIVATE (STUDY 4)

64
FIGURE 6

EFFECT OF LUXURY CONSUMPTION ON CHARITY DONATIONS

DEPENDING ON WHETHER DONATION IS PUBLIC OR PRIVATE (STUDY 5)

65
FIGURE 7

EFFECT OF LUXURY CONSUMPTION ON PUBLIC CHARITY DONATIONS

DEPENDING ON IF THE LUXURY PRODUCT BOOSTS SOCIAL STATUS

(STUDY 6)

66
FIGURE 8

EFFECT OF LUXURY CONSUMPTION ON PUBLIC DONATIONS MEDIATED

BY PERCEIVED SOCIAL STATUS (STUDY 6)

Perceived
Social Status
.26* .29**

Condition: Amount of Money


Luxury vs. Other Conditions Donated in Public
.24*
(.18)

Note: Coefficients are standardized


p<.001, ***; p<.01, **; p<.05, *

67
CHAPTER III:

Essay 2: Louis Vuitton and Your Waistline:

How Luxury Consumption Influences Self-Control

Luxury consumption is skyrocketing. Among over 100 categories of consumer

spending, luxury goods show the strongest growth (Sparshott 2014). In the next five

years, global luxury sales are expected to grow up to 50% faster than global GDP

(D’Arpizio 2013), with the highest growth in countries such as India (86%) and China

(72%) (Euromonitor 2013). Many top luxury brands racked up double-digit sales

increases last year, including Prada (29% increase; Paton 2013) and Louis Vuitton (19%

increase; Roberts 2013). No wonder luxury brands such as Louis Vuitton, Prada, and

Gucci are among the most valuable brands in the world (Interbrand 2013).

Not surprisingly, these trends in luxury consumption have drawn the attention of

consumer researchers, who have identified a variety of motivations for owning luxury

goods. Consumers desire luxury goods as a way to satisfy psychological needs, such as

elevating self-esteem and creating stable selves (Belk 1985; Richins 1987) and

compensating for feelings of powerlessness (Rucker, Galinsky, and Dubois 2012).

Research also shows that consumers use luxury products to express their identity, signal

prestige and status, and even attract and protect romantic partners (Berger and Ward 2010;

Griskevicius et al. 2007; Han et al. 2010; Wilcox et al. 2009; Wang and Griskevicius

2014).

Interestingly, though, there has been little attention to the actual experience of

using luxury goods. How does it feel to carry a luxury handbag or wear a luxury watch?

Does using a luxury handbag or watch change our psychological state or affect our

68
behavior? We might expect the luxury consumption experience to be a largely positive

one, given the positive outcomes consumers seek when acquiring luxury goods. However,

is it possible that using luxury goods impacts consumers in negative ways that have not

been anticipated? Answering these questions requires us to observe consumers actually

using luxury products, which has not been a feature of prior work in the luxury area. As a

result, we understand a good deal about consumer motivations for wanting luxury goods,

but have little understanding of what happens once consumers acquire luxury goods and

use them in their daily lives.

In this article, we examine the effects of luxury consumption by providing women

with a luxury handbag to carry, such as a Louis Vuitton, Prada, or Burberry handbag. We

then examine the psychological and behavioral effects of this luxury consumption

experience. In particular, we focus on the negative effects that using a luxury good can

trigger. We find that using a luxury item in public makes women feel they are getting

attention from others, but it also makes them feel more self-conscious, and more mindful

of needing to watch their behavior in front of others. This need to self-regulate one’s

behavior depletes self-regulatory resources, and impairs women’s ability to exert self-

control in subsequent situations. For example, we find that women who carry a luxury

handbag are less able to resist tempting but unhealthy food (candy), and eat more of this

unhealthy food than women who carry a non-luxury handbag.

Our research makes a number of novel and important contributions. First, to our

knowledge, we are the first to examine the psychological and behavior consequences that

accompany luxury consumption. Although consumers are motivated to acquire luxury

goods for a variety of reasons, this does not guarantee that they will experience these

69
imagined benefits, nor does it guarantee that they will experience only positive

psychological states when actually using luxury goods. Second, our research documents a

novel effect not anticipated by prior research—that luxury use can result in negative

psychological and behavioral effects. In terms of psychological states, using a luxury

good makes individuals feel self-conscious and watchful of their behavior in front of

others. This results in a depletion in self-regulatory resources, which negatively affects

the luxury user because these resources are needed for many aspects of daily life where

self-control is required—such as resisting tasty but unhealthy food, exercising daily, and

curbing one’s temper with a child or fellow worker. Overall, our findings suggest that

luxury consumption has psychic and behavioral costs that are in stark contrast to the

prevailing wisdom regarding the positive benefits that consumers can derive from luxury

goods.

Finally, these findings extend our knowledge of self-regulatory resource depletion

theory. We demonstrate that enacting behaviors in a social context (using a luxury

product) can deplete resources. Most prior research examines depletion using

intrapersonal self-regulatory tasks, with very few studies showing that interpersonal tasks

can also be depleting (for an exception, see Vohs, Baumeister, and Ciarocco 2005).

Further, we show that tasks generally considered to be interesting and enjoyable (carrying

a luxury handbag) can also deplete one’s self-regulatory resources. Prior depletion

research incorporates tasks that are usually challenging, tedious, or uncomfortable. Thus,

we provide a novel demonstration of self-regulatory resource depletion with a more

positively valenced task.

70
CONCEPTUAL OVERVIEW

In this section, we first describe the nature of luxury goods and luxury

consumption. Next, we propose that using luxury goods in public elicits a particular state

of mind, which we refer to as a spotlight mindset, which triggers impression management

concerns. Then, we propose that dealing with these concerns draws on self-regulation

resources, which depletes the pool of resources available for subsequent self-control tasks,

resulting in worse performance in a subsequent self-control task (e.g., resisting candy).

Nature of Luxury Consumption

We define luxury goods as products or brands that have premium prices and

quality, and are relatively rare and unique. Occupying a premium position in the

marketplace, with perceptions of high quality offered at premium prices, is a key

characteristic of luxury brands (Patrick and Hagtvedt 2008). Rarity is a defining feature is

found in early writings on luxury consumption (Veblen 1899), and today, luxury brands

continue to be defined in terms of uniqueness and exclusivity (Phau and Prendergast

2000; Vigneron and Johnson 2004).

Most luxury goods are consumed in public settings. Although a consumer might

drink a bottle of Domaine Laflaive (French wine priced at over $4,000/bottle) at home

alone, it is more likely the bottle of wine will be consumed with others. A millionaire

might drive his Astin Martin One-77 (price of $1.8 million) around his estate, but most of

the time, the car will be driven in public in view of others. In fact, very few luxury goods

are intended for private consumption. More than 70% of global luxury sales are for goods

people typically consume in public—cars, jewelry, leather goods (handbags, shoes), and

apparel (D’Arpizio 2013).

71
Consistent with public usage, researchers have focused on the role of luxury

goods as signals of status and power to others, as opposed to the intrinsic enjoyment of

using a luxury good. Over 100 years ago, Thorstein Veblen coined the term “conspicuous

consumption” to capture people’s desire to seek out and flaunt luxury goods in his classic

book, Theory of The Leisure Class. Today, the terms “conspicuous consumption” and

“luxury consumption” are often used interchangeably. Consumer researchers have

documented that luxury products can signal one’s status and prestige (Bagwell and

Douglas 1996; Han et al. 2010; Mazzocco et al. 2012; Wernerfelt 1990; Wilcox et al.

2009), power (Rucker et al. 2012), group affiliation (Han et al. 2010; Berger and Ward

2010), and tastes (Berger and Ward 2010).

In accordance with these themes, our research focuses on premier luxury goods

that meet our definition (such as Louis Vuitton, Prada, and Burberry) and the

consequences of using these luxury goods in public settings.

Luxury Consumption and the Spotlight Mindset

Although prior research has identified consumer motivations for desiring luxury

goods, there has been virtually no attention given to the actual experience of using luxury

goods. What are consumers thinking and feeling when they use a luxury good?

We propose that using a luxury good in public places the user in a spotlight,

where they receive attention just like an actor stepping into a spotlight. Many consumers

invite this attention in the hope that using a luxury good will signal identity, status, power,

and group affiliation. However, even absent this desire, luxury goods naturally draw

attention from others because they are relatively rare, novel, and stand out from other

consumer goods. For example, drivers of luxury cars often receive glances from other

72
drivers at a stoplight, and women with expensive handbags often catch other women

staring at them. Thus, regardless of one’s intention, using a luxury good in public garners

attention from others, placing the luxury user in a spotlight.

The spotlight metaphor offers several clues about the psychological state of

consumers using luxury goods in public, which we refer to as a “spotlight mindset.” First,

we expect luxury users to feel that other people are paying attention and watching them.

Luxury goods are relatively rare and distinctive, and research shows that individuals

using a distinctive item not only believe people are paying attention to them, but also

overestimate the amount of attention directed their way (Gilovich, Medvec, and Savitsky

2000; Gilovich and Savitsky 1999). In addition, because luxury goods are so often used

as signaling devices, anyone using a luxury item in public can reasonably expect that

other people will be paying attention to them.

Second, we expect luxury users will feel self-conscious, defined as being

concerned with one’s social appearance and the impression one is making on others

(public self-consciousness: Fenigstein, Scheier, and Buss 1975). Feelings of being

watched and in the public eye are associated with feelings of self-consciousness

(Fenigstein 1984). Regardless of whether signaling motives are present, luxury users feel

they are the focus of other people’s attention, and concerns about how others are judging

them naturally follow. These concerns may be quite valid, as research shows that people

are more likely to draw inferences about others when they have higher-priced items and

status goods (Charles, Hurst and Roussanov 2007; Richins 1994).

Third, and relatedly, we expect luxury users to be more careful about the way they

behave in public. The need to be more careful about one’s behavior is related to feelings

73
of self-consciousness. Individuals high in (public) self-consciousness are more willing to

alter their behavior to avoid being negatively evaluated or rejected (Fenigstein 1979;

Raichle et al., 2001) and are more susceptible to pressures to conform (Froming and

Carver 1981; Scheier 1980). These findings suggest that (public) self-consciousness is

related to being careful about how one behaves in public and altering one’s behavior to

be socially acceptable.

Spotlight Mindset and Self-Regulatory Resource Depletion

Consuming a luxury product in public elicits a spotlight mindset, with thoughts of

others watching you, being concerned about the impression you are making, and needing

to be careful about one’s behavior in front of others. Together, these aspects of a spotlight

mindset suggest the need to manage one’s thoughts and behaviors to create a more

positive impression when using a luxury product in public, which as described below, is

linked to self-regulation.

When individuals seek to achieve important social goals, maintain or meet social

standards, or conform to group norms, the process of presenting a desirable image

involves increased attentional and behavioral control. That is, individuals need to initiate,

adjust, stop, or otherwise change or promote certain thoughts, feelings, or actions

(Baumeister, Heatherton, and Tice 1994; Baumeister and Heatherton 1996; Carver and

Scheier 2001). These activities are linked to self-regulation, which is essentially the

ability to alter one’s natural tendencies to bring them in line with standards (Baumeister

and Heatherton 1996; Baumeister, Vohs, and Tice 2007). Especially relevant to our

context is the finding that self-presentation requires self-regulation (Vohs et al. 2005).

Furthermore, recent neuroscience research finds that regions of the brain (MPFC: medial

74
prefrontal cortex) activated when individuals process information about the self and

others (e.g., making inferences about others’ evaluations of the self: for a review, see

Heatherton 2011) overlap with parts of the brain responsible for self-regulation (for

reviews, see Bandfield et al. 2004; Krendl and Heatherton 2009).

Research also documents that self-regulation draws on a general resource, and

these resources are subject to depletion. Baumeister and Heatherton’s (1996) strength

model of self-regulation makes these two important points. First, self-regulatory

resources are domain general, with the same source used for seemingly separate self-

regulatory tasks, such as regulating one’s emotions, thoughts, and behaviors (Baumeister

et al. 1998; Muraven, Tice, and Baumeister 1998). Therefore, self-regulation tasks related

to impression management, regardless of whether they are cognitive, emotional, or

behavioral, should all draw resources from the same general pool. Second, this domain-

general pool is exhausted by repeated attempts at self-regulation (Baumeister et al., 2007;

Vohs and Heatherton 2000). Acts of self-regulation deplete the resource pool, and

therefore, impair subsequent self-regulation performance.

We draw upon these themes to develop the following predictions about the

consequences of luxury consumption. As proposed earlier, using a luxury product in

public elicits a spotlight mindset, which triggers impression management concerns.

Dealing with these concerns draws on self-regulation resources, which depletes the pool

of resources available. When these resources are need to exert self-control in a

subsequent situation, such as resisting tempting but unhealthy food (candy), luxury users

have fewer self-regulation resources available to meet this challenge. As a consequence,

75
luxury users are less successful at exerting self-control, and will consume more candy

than will consumers using non-luxury products.

THE CURRENT RESEARCH

Across four experiments, we provide evidence that consuming a luxury product in

public elicits a spotlight mindset, which draws on and depletes self-regulatory resources,

and thus impairs self-control in subsequent situations. Our basic experimental approach is

as follows. We ask research participants to use a luxury or non-luxury handbag, and we

measure feelings of being in a spotlight for both conditions. Next, after using the luxury

product, we present participants with a situation that requires self-regulatory resources—

resisting a tasty but unhealthy snack (candy). We measure how much candy participants

consume, with higher consumption indicating that self-regulatory resources have been

depleted. This experimental paradigm is common to self-regulatory resource depletion

research, where participants perform a first task that requires either significant self-

regulation or little self-regulation. In our studies, this first task is using a luxury good

(requires self-regulation) or using a non-luxury good (requires little or no self-regulation).

Next, all participants perform a second unrelated self-regulation task (in our studies,

resisting candy). Evidence for self-regulation resource depletion is obtained if

participants perform worse in this second task (in our studies, eating more candy) when

they performed a first task also requiring self-regulation resources.

Study 1 demonstrates the luxury depletion effect—using a luxury (vs. non-luxury)

handbag in public depletes self-regulatory resources, which impairs self-control in a

subsequent situation (resisting candy). As expected, we find that this effect is mediated

by feelings of being in a spotlight (spotlight mindset). In Study 2, we provide further

76
evidence for the luxury depletion effect, showing that using a luxury handbag increases

consumption of unhealthy food (which requires self-regulatory resources to resist) but not

consumption of healthy food (which requires little or no self-regulatory resources to

resist). We also rule out power and mood effects as alternative explanations for the

luxury depletion effect. In study 3, we provide further process evidence by showing that

the luxury depletion effect occurs only when luxury products are used in public, not in

private. Further, we directly manipulate a spotlight mindset, and find that eliciting this

mindset among individuals using a luxury product in private produces the same self-

regulatory depletion effect experienced by public luxury users. In our final study, we

examine two important moderators of the luxury depletion effect, price-quality luxury tier

(premier vs. affordable) and trait self-control. Consistent with our theorizing, we find that

the luxury depletion effect is stronger for premier luxury products and individuals with

chronically lower levels of self-control.

STUDY 1: LOUIS VUITTON

The objectives of this study are twofold. First, we demonstrate the basic luxury

depletion effect by showing that consumers who use a luxury (vs. non-luxury) product

perform worse in a subsequent self-control task, resisting candy offered to them. Second,

we provide evidence that the luxury depletion effect is due to feelings that one is in a

spotlight when using luxury products. Overall, we show that consumers who use a luxury

(vs. non-luxury) product in public exhibit stronger feelings of being in a spotlight, and

these feelings mediate the relationship between the type of product used (luxury vs. non-

luxury) and the amount of candy consumed.

77
Participants and Design

Ninety-one female students (Mage = 20.51, SD = 2.32) from a public university

participated in exchange for partial course credit. Participants carried a luxury or non-

luxury handbag while walking around a busy campus building. Participants in the luxury

condition were either given the luxury handbag or were allowed to choose the luxury

handbag from two options (a luxury and non-luxury handbag). This variation was

included to examine whether feelings of being in a spotlight are different if one chooses

versus is given a luxury handbag to carry. For example, participants choosing the luxury

handbag may feel less self-conscious because they personally picked the handbag and

welcome the attention they will receive from others. We examined these possibilities in a

3 (Product Type: Choose Luxury vs. Given Luxury vs. Control) between-subjects design

study. Six participants were excluded from the analysis for failure to follow instructions,

being allergic to chocolate, or choice of the non-luxury handbag.

Brand Pretests and Handbag Selection

Louis Vuitton was selected as the luxury brand for the study based on a pretest

with female students similar to those in the main experiment (N = 28, Mage = 20.31, SD

= .92). We tested eight luxury brands, and Louis Vuitton was rated highly in terms of: (1)

high level of brand familiarity (M = 4.10, 1=not at all familiar to 5=very familiar); 2)

positive brand attitude (M = 5.67, average of two items on a scale of 1=bad/very

undesirable to 7=good/very desirable); and (3) strong brand preference (over 75% of

students picked it as one of the top three luxury handbags brands that they would most

like to buy). A high level of brand familiarity ensures that the luxury item can be

recognized and garner attention from others, which is foundational for feelings of being

78
in a spotlight for luxury users. A strong and positive brand attitude ensures that the luxury

item will garner positive attention, which rules out alternative explanations for the luxury

depletion effect based on embarrassment or social exclusion elicited by using an

unpopular, negatively evaluated, or ugly handbag.

A cross-body clutch (9.4 × 5.5 × 1.6 inches) with the iconic Louis Vuitton

monogram canvas pattern was chosen as the luxury handbag (retail price: $730). This

handbag was given to participants in the Given Luxury condition. In the Choose Luxury

condition, participants were allowed to choose a handbag to carry, either the Louis

Vuitton handbag or a non-luxury cross-body clutch of a similar style (retail price: $45).

In the Control condition, participants were given a non-luxury handbag to carry (retail

price: $75).

Procedure

Participants were told that the lab session consisted of several short unrelated

studies. They were told the first study was about consumers’ opinions of women’s

handbags, which would involve using a handbag and completing a survey. After

providing informed consent, participants were randomly assigned to one of the three

conditions. For participants in the Choose Luxury condition, two handbags were

presented, a Louis Vuitton (luxury) handbag and a non-luxury handbag. Participants were

allowed to choose one of the handbags for the study. Only two participants (out of 32)

chose the non-luxury handbag and were excluded from the analysis. Participants in the

Given Luxury condition were given the Louis Vuitton handbag to use, whereas

participants in the Control condition were given a non-luxury handbag to use. To make

79
the consumption experience as realistic as possible, each participant was asked to put her

personal items into the handbag and to imagine the handbag belonged to her.

Next, each participant was given a list of tasks to complete while using the

handbag for the next 15 minutes, including walking to a nearby building and stopping by

a coffee shop. Several precautions were taken to ensure the consumption experience was

public and visible. First, all sessions of the experiment were scheduled in a lab located in

a school building during class time when there were plenty of students in the building.

Second, participants were specifically asked to go to a coffee shop (located in an adjacent

building) that has a high customer volume. Third, participants were asked to complete a

short survey while sitting in the coffee shop, which provided more time for them to use

the handbag in public. Finally, participants were scheduled so they would avoid seeing

each other in public using the luxury or non-luxury handbags.

After 15 minutes, participants returned to the lab and were directed to individual

rooms. Each room had a computer and a bowl of M&M’s candies on the desk. The

experimenter casually mentioned to participants that they were welcome to have some

complimentary snacks while completing the computer-based survey. The participants

were then left alone in the room to complete a second survey regarding the handbag

experience and several demographic measures. After completing the survey, participants

were debriefed and thanked. As soon as they left the room, the amount of M&M’s

consumed was measured using a digital scale (in grams).

Measures

Food consumption. The weight of M&M’s candy consumed (in grams) served as

the main dependent variable.

80
Spotlight index. At the end of the second survey, participants were asked to recall

their experiences using the luxury or non-luxury handbag, including several questions to

assess the extent to which they felt they were in a spotlight during the experience.

Participants responded to four items: “When you walked around with the handbag, to

what extent did you feel you: (1) attracted attention from others, (2) were being noticed

by others, (3) needed to be more careful in front of others, and (4) felt self-conscious

about carrying the handbag.” Responses were recorded on a 7-point scale ranging from

“not at all” to “very much.” Responses to these four items were averaged to form a

spotlight index (α = .68).

Results

Food consumption. A series of planned contrasts were conducted to examine the

luxury depletion effect. First, as predicted, we found that participants who were given the

Louis Vuitton handbag to use consumed significantly more M&M’s (M = 26.19, SD =

25.39) than participants who were given the control handbag to use (M = 15.21, SD =

12.60, t(82) = 2.11, p = .02). We also found that participants who chose to use the Louis

Vuitton handbag also consumed significantly more M&M’s than participants who were

given a control handbag to use (M = 26.70 vs. 15.21, t(82) = 2.46. p = .009). Further, we

did not find any difference between participants who were given the Louis Vuitton

handbag to use versus those who chose the Louis Vuitton handbag (M = 26.19 vs. 26.70,

t(82) = .09, p = .93, see figure 1).

----------- Figure 1 about here --------------

Spotlight index. The same sets of planned contrasts were conducted with the

spotlight index. Participants who were given the Louis Vuitton handbag to carry had

81
stronger feelings of being in a spotlight (M = 4.19, SD=1.48) than participants who

carried the control handbag (M=3.42, SD=1.41; t(82) = 1.99, p = .025). Similarly,

participants who chose the Louis Vuitton handbag also reported significantly stronger

feelings of being in a spotlight (M = 4.50, SD=1.30) than participants carrying the control

handbag (M=3.42, t(82) = 2.77, p = .003). Again, whether participants were given or

chose the Louis Vuitton handbag did not affect their feelings of being in a spotlight (M =

4.19 vs. 4.50, t(82) = .87, p = .38; see figure 1).

Mediation analysis. Thus far, our findings indicate that using a luxury handbag (1)

heightened participants’ feelings of being in a spotlight; and (2) resulted in higher

M&M’s consumption. We predicted these two effects would be related, such that the

effect of luxury product use on M&M’s consumption would be mediated by feelings of

being in a spotlight when using the luxury product. To test for mediation, we used the

Preacher and Hayes (2008) method for estimating indirect effects. Consistent with

expectations, bootstrap estimates (based on 10,000 samples) indicated that the indirect

effects of condition (Choose Luxury vs. Given Luxury vs. Control) through the spotlight

index had a significant effect on M&M’s consumption (Hayes 2012, Model 4, Kappa-

squared, 95% bias-corrected, CI = [.0007, .0956]). And, after controlling for the spotlight

index, the effect of condition on M&M’s consumption became non-significant (from β =

-5.48, p = .06, to β = -4.79, p = .12; see figure 2).

----------- Figure 2 about here --------------

Discussion

Study 1 provides support for the luxury depletion effect. Consumers who used a

luxury (vs. non-luxury) product in public were less successful in a subsequent self-

82
control task, eating more unhealthy food (M&M’s) offered as a snack. This effect was

observed regardless of whether the luxury product was chosen or was given to the

consumer to use. Further, we found evidence for the proposed process. Using a luxury (vs.

non-luxury) product in public induced stronger feelings of being in a spotlight, and these

feelings mediated the effect of the type of product used (luxury vs. non-luxury) on

consumption of M&M’s.

The next study provides further evidence that luxury consumption depletes self-

regulatory resources by varying the type of snack offered to participants, unhealthy

snacks (M&M’s, Skittles) versus healthy snacks (nuts, raisins). Prior research has shown

that foods considered to be unhealthy and tempting (e.g., chocolate) are more difficult to

resist, and require more self-regulatory resources to resist, than healthy foods (e.g.,

broccoli) (Vohs and Heatherton 2000; Redden and Haws 2013). Thus, if luxury

consumption depletes self-regulatory resources, consumers who use a luxury product

should have more difficulty resisting unhealthy snacks (and should eat more of these

snacks) than consumers who use a non-luxury product. However, there should be no

difference between consumers using a luxury versus non-luxury product in terms of

consumption of healthy snacks, as self-regulatory resources are less necessary for

resisting healthy foods. Further, feelings of being in a spotlight should mediate the

predicted interaction between food type (healthy, unhealthy) and product type (luxury,

non-luxury).

We also include several additional measures in the next study to rule out

alternative explanations for the luxury depletion effect. First, we include a measure of

power. Prior research shows that consumers desire luxury products when they lack power

83
or status (Rucker and Galinsky 2009; Sivanathan and Pettit 2010), and thus, using luxury

products should make consumers feel that they are more powerful or have higher status.

Feelings of status might be associated with a feeling of entitlement (Piff 2014), and these

feelings may lead luxury users to feel they are entitled to eat more of the snacks being

offered. Second, we include a mood measure to rule out a mood explanation for our

results. Using a luxury product leads to feelings of being in a spotlight, which includes

being self-conscious and sensing that others are watching you. If these feelings induce a

more negative mood, luxury users may consume more unhealthy snacks as a means of

repairing the negative mood they are experiencing (Tice, Bratslavsky, and Baumeister

2001). Alternatively, using a luxury product that is highly desirable may put consumers

into a more positive mood, and they may be less inclined to break a positive mood by

exerting self-control to resist a tempting snack offered to them.

STUDY 2: BURBERRY

Participants and Design

Eighty-eight female students (Mage = 20.78, SD = 1.96) from a public university

participated in exchange for partial course credit or a small payment, and were randomly

assigned to one of the conditions in a 2 (Product Type: Luxury vs. Control) by 2 (Food

Type: Healthy vs. Unhealthy) between-subjects design.

Brand Pretests and Handbag Selection

Burberry was selected as the luxury brand for the study based on a pretest with

female students similar to those in the main experiment (N = 28, Mage = 20.31, SD = .92).

Using the same criteria for brand selection described in study 1, we chose Burberry due

to: (1) a high level of brand familiarity (M = 3.80, 1=not at all familiar to 5=very

84
familiar); 2) a positive brand attitude (M = 5.50, average of two items on a scale of

1=bad/very undesirable to 7=good/very desirable); and (3) strong brand preference (over

65% of students picked it as one of the top three luxury handbags brands that they would

most like to buy).

A beige color Burberry tote with the iconic check-print pattern (retail price: $875)

was chosen as the luxury handbag. A beige tote of similar size, with no visible logo was

chosen as the control handbag (retail price: $65).

Procedure

The procedure was similar to the first study. Participants were given either a

luxury handbag or control handbag, and carried this handbag while walking around a

busy building completing several tasks. Afterwards, they returned to the lab to complete a

final survey, and were offered healthy snacks (a bowl of unsalted nuts and a bowl of

raisins) or unhealthy snacks (a bowl of M&M’s and a bowl of Skittles) (adopted from

Redden and Haws 2013). Two bowls of healthy (unhealthy) snacks were provided to

accommodate varying food preferences among respondents. As soon as participants

finished the survey and left the lab, the amount of the food consumed was measured.

Measures

Food consumption. The weight of food consumed (in grams) served as the main

dependent variable. The consumption of M&M’s and Skittles was combined for

participants in the unhealthy food condition, and the consumption of unsalted peanuts and

raisins was combined for participants in the healthy food condition.

Spotlight index. Participants completed the same four items described in study 1,

and responses were averaged to obtain the spotlight index (α = .78).

85
Mood. To examine mood as an alternative explanation, participants completed the

20-item Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS: Watson, Clark, and Tellegen

1988) while sitting in a coffee shop with the handbag. Included in the twenty items were

positive emotions (e.g., interested, excited, proud, and happy) and negative emotions (e.g.,

upset, embarrassed, afraid, and irritable). For each item, participants were asked describe

whether each emotion reflected how they felt at the moment (1 = Very slightly/not at all

to 5 = Extremely).

Power. To examine whether feelings of power could constitute an alternative

explanation, participants were asked to recall their experience using the handbag (luxury

or control) and rate the extent to which they felt superior to others, felt powerful, or felt

they had higher status than others. Responses for each item were recorded on a 7-point

scale (1 = not at all to 7 = very much) and the three items were averaged to form a power

index (α = .93).

Results

Food consumption. We predicted that participants who used the luxury (vs.

control) handbag in public would consume more unhealthy, but not healthy, food. An

ANOVA was performed on food consumption with Product Type (Luxury vs. Control)

and Food Type (Healthy vs. Unhealthy) as between-subjects factors. As expected, there

was a significant two-way interaction between Product Type and Food Type (F(1, 84) =

3.88, p =.05).

To test our specific predictions, a series of planned contrasts was performed. As

shown in Figure 3, participants in the Luxury handbag condition ate significantly more

unhealthy food (M = 40.45, SD = 19.25) than participants in the Control handbag

86
condition (M = 28.31, SD = 28.15, t(84) = 1.78, p = .035). However, for healthy food

consumption, there was no difference between participants in the Luxury handbag

condition (M = 16.19, SD = 19.09) and Control handbag condition (M = 23.47, SD =

24.15, t(84) = 1.02, p = .31).

---Figure 3 about here---

Spotlight index. As expected, participants in the Luxury handbag condition felt

more in the spotlight (M = 4.68, SD = 1.68) than participants in the Control handbag

condition (M = 3.67, SD = 1.79, t(87) = 2.73, p = .004). These results confirmed that

using the Burberry handbag made participants feel like they were garnering attention

from others, needed to be careful about their behavior, and felt more self-conscious.

Mediation analysis. We hypothesized that the effect of luxury product

consumption on unhealthy food intake, not healthy food intake, would be mediated by the

feelings of being in a spotlight while using the luxury product. Specifically, we expected

the interaction between Product Type and Food Type would be mediated by the spotlight

index.

To test for mediation, we used the Preacher and Hayes (2008) method for

estimating indirect effects. The mediation analysis was based on two separate multiple

regressions models (Hayes 2012, Model 5, see figure 4). The first model included

Product Type, Food Type, and the interaction as the independent variables, and food

consumption as the dependent variable. This model revealed a significant two-way

interaction (β = 24.27, t(84) = 3.51, p < .001). After controlling for the spotlight index,

this interaction effect was significantly reduced (β = -18.29, t(84) = -1.89, p = .06, 95%

87
bias-corrected, CI = [-8.52, -.45]). Thus, this analysis indicated that the interaction effect

(Product Type x Food Type) was fully mediated by the spotlight index.

---Figure 4 about here---

Additional analyses. We explored two alternative explanations for our findings.

First, we examined whether food consumption was related to the mood elicited by using

different handbags. Results showed no significant differences between participants

carrying a luxury versus control handbag for all twenty positive and negative affect

measures (p-values > .17). Participants in both conditions evinced moderately positive

moods. For example, they generally felt interested (M = 2.97) and attentive (M = 2.88).

Therefore, it is unlikely that the higher (lower) consumption of unhealthy food in the

Luxury (Control) handbag condition was due to negative or positive affect from using the

handbag.

Second, we examined whether greater unhealthy food consumption after using a

luxury (vs. control) handbag was due to feelings of being powerful. Participants who

used the luxury (vs. control) handbag reported greater feelings of power (M = 4.70 vs.

3.45, t(87) = 2.84, p = .006). However, a mediation analysis revealed that power did not

mediate the effects of luxury on unhealthy food consumption. We used the same

mediation analysis described above, but substituted power instead of the spotlight index

as the mediator. After power was entered into the regression, the interaction between

Product Type and Food Type was still significant (β = -19.13, p = .05), and the 1,000

resample of bootstrap also revealed a non-significant mediation effect (95% bias-

corrected, CI = [-5.65, .29]). Thus, greater feelings of power do not account for why

participants using a luxury handbag consumed more unhealthy food.

88
Discussion

Our findings provide additional evidence for the luxury depletion effect. Using a

different luxury brand (Burberry), we replicate the finding from study 1 that using a

luxury product in public depletes self-regulatory resources, which impairs self-control

and results in eating more unhealthy food. Further, study 2 strengthens our theory by

showing that using a luxury product increases unhealthy food consumption (where self-

regulatory resources are needed to resist consumption) but not healthy food consumption

(where self-regulatory resources are less at play). The divergent results for unhealthy

versus healthy food help rule out alternative explanations for why luxury users consume

more unhealthy food, such as simply desiring more food or feeling entitled to take more

food. Alternative explanations related to feelings of power or differential moods related

to luxury use were also ruled out. Only heightened feelings of being in a spotlight proved

to be a mediator for the observed pattern of findings.

The next study provides further evidence that luxury users’ feelings of being in a

spotlight are responsible for self-regulatory depletion. First, we vary the luxury

consumption context to be public or private. In our first two studies, participants carried a

luxury handbag in public, which triggers feelings of being in a spotlight. In study 3, we

asked one group of participants to use a luxury handbag in public and another group to

use the same luxury handbag in private. We used the small Louis Vuitton handbag from

study 1, which participants carried as a regular handbag in view of others (public) or

placed inside a tote bag hidden from others (private) while walking in a busy building. If

feelings of being in a spotlight are responsible for the luxury depletion effect observed in

our first two studies, we should observe the same effect for the public, but not the private,

89
consumption context. When luxury products are used in private, there is no reason to

believe that a user will feel self-conscious and feel the need to watch their behavior—

therefore, the luxury depletion effect should not be observed.

Further, with all participants using the same luxury handbag, we seek to rule out

hedonic goal activation as an alternative explanation for why luxury users consume more

tasty but unhealthy food. In prior studies, we compared participants who used a luxury

handbag versus a control handbag, and it could be argued that carrying the luxury

handbag activated hedonic goal seeking behaviors, such as eating candy. In study 3, all

participants used the same luxury handbag, which would equalize hedonic goal seeking

behaviors (if any) across conditions. If consumption of candy increases only for luxury

users in the public condition, this would effectively rule out hedonic goal seeking as a

plausible explanation for food consumption effects.

Finally, to add to our process evidence, we add a third condition where we

directly manipulate feelings of being in a spotlight mindset. For this condition,

participants use the Louis Vuitton handbag in private, but prior to doing so, we prime

participants with a brief message about how others are always watching them and how

they need to be careful about their behavior in front of others, which is consistent with

how luxury users feel when using these goods in public settings. Thus, with this

manipulation, we expect to see that participants using the luxury handbag in private will

exhibit the same spotlight feelings (and associated depletion and food consumption) as

participants using the luxury handbag in public.

90
STUDY 3: PUBLIC VS. PRIVATE LUXURY CONSUMPTION

Sample and Procedure

Eighty female students (Mage = 20.37, SD = 1.73) participated in exchange for

partial course credit and were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: Luxury

Public vs. Luxury Private vs. Luxury Private with Spotlight Mindset Prime.

Participants were seated in a private room and were told they would complete

several unrelated short studies. First, they were asked to read an article. For participants

in the Spotlight Mindset Prime condition, the article described new psychological

research revealing that people underestimate the amount of attention they receive from

others in public. The article ended with the following statement: “Be careful about what

you do in public because you are being watched by others more than you think!”

Participants in the other two conditions read a control article about new research on high

school students’ participation in extracurricular activities.

Next, all participants were given the Louis Vuitton handbag and were asked to put

their personal items into it. Then, they were asked to walk around the building to

complete the same set of tasks used in study 1. Participants in the Luxury Public

condition completed their tasks while carrying the Louis Vuitton handbag in sight.

Participants in the Luxury Private conditions were instructed to put the Louis Vuitton

handbag into a leather tote bag (without a visible brand name or logo) before setting out

to complete the same set of tasks. Per the procedure used in study 1, participants returned

to the lab after 15 minutes and completed a computer-based survey in their private room.

As before, a bowl of M&M’s candy was placed on the desk in each room and participants

91
were told it was a complimentary snack. After completing the survey, participants were

dismissed, and then the weight of M&M’s consumed was measured.

Measures

Food consumption. The weight of M&M’s candy consumed (in grams) served as

the main dependent variable.

Manipulation Checks. Immediately after reading the spotlight prime or control

article, participants rated how informative and credible the article was on a 7-point scale

(1 = Not at all to 7 = Very much). Then, after carrying the handbag, participants in the

spotlight prime and other conditions completed the four items in the spotlight index used

in prior studies (α =.79). Both measures were taken to ensure that the spotlight prime

article was credible and triggered the intended feelings of being in a spotlight.

Handbag evaluation. At the end of the study, participants rated how much they

liked the handbag they used in the study on a scale from 1= “not at all” to 7=”very

much.” This measure was taken to ensure that the Louis Vuitton handbag was equally

liked in the public and private consumption conditions.

Results

Manipulation checks. The spotlight prime article and control article were rated

equally in terms of being informative (M = 4.73 vs. 4.87, t(77) = .41, p = .69) and

credible (M = 4.50 vs. 4.34, t(77) = .37, p = .72). Ratings for both articles were

significantly higher than the scale midpoint, indicating participants found both articles

informative and credible (p-values < .001).

Next, we compared the spotlight index across conditions, which indicated our

spotlight prime was successful. Participants in the Luxury Public condition and Luxury

92
Private with Spotlight Mindset Prime conditions had similar spotlight feelings (M = 4.32

vs. 4.19, t(77) = .29, p = .77), and both had significantly greater spotlight feelings than

participants in the Luxury Private condition (M = 3.20, t-values > 2.10, p-values < .02).

Food consumption. A one-way ANOVA revealed the predicted main effect of

different conditions on food consumption quantity (F(2,77) = 3.04, p = .05). To test our

specific predictions, a series of planned contrasts was conducted. Consistent with

expectations, participants in the Luxury Public condition consumed significant more

candy (M = 24.11, SD = 20.03) than participants in the Luxury Private condition (M =

14.68, SD = 15.97, t(77) = 1.90, p = .031). Moreover, participants in the Luxury Private

with Spotlight Mindset Prime condition also ate significant more candy (M = 28.70, SD =

25.16) than participants in the Luxury Private condition (M = 14.68, SD = 15.97, t(77) = -

2.42, p = .01). Thus, triggering feelings of self-consciousness and being watched by

others (spotlight prime) increased food consumption. Further, the spotlight prime was so

successful that food consumption in this condition (Luxury Private with Spotlight

Mindset Prime) was no different than food consumption in the Luxury Public condition

(t(77) = -.748, p = .46, see figure 5)

---Figure 5 about here---

Additional analysis. We examined whether candy consumption was related to

how desirable each handbag was. Although all participants carried the same Louis

Vuitton handbag, those in the Luxury Private condition carried the Louis Vuitton

handbag inside a black tote bag. Evaluations of the Louis Vuitton handbag and the black

tote bag were not different from one another (M = 4.39 vs. 4.59, t(77) = .95, p = .95), and

ratings of both handbags were significantly higher than the mid-point of the 7-point scale

93
(ts > 5.38, ps < .001). Therefore, the observed effects were unlikely due to how much

participants liked each handbag.

Discussion

Results of this study provide additional support for our contention that luxury

users’ feelings of being in a spotlight are responsible for self-regulatory depletion,

indicated by a lessened ability to resist tasty but unhealthy food. When a luxury handbag

was carried in private, which vastly reduced feelings of being in the spotlight, food

consumption was significantly lower than when the same luxury handbag was carried in

public. Further, when the luxury handbag was carried in private, but participants were

primed to feel that they were in a spotlight with others watching, food consumption was

equal to when the luxury handbag was carried in public. These patterns of data rule out

hedonic goal seeking behavior as an alternative hypothesis because participants in all

conditions carried the same luxury handbag, yet food consumption amounts varied in

accordance with our expectations.

This study also identifies a moderator of the luxury depletion effect. Luxury

consumption in public elicits feelings of being in a spotlight and depletes self-regulatory

resources, as indicated by higher levels of unhealthy food consumption. However, luxury

consumption in private does not trigger the same spotlight feelings, and therefore, we

observe less self-regulatory resource depletion and less unhealthy food consumption.

Note that we used the exact same luxury handbag in both the public and private

conditions, and participants performed the same tasks in both conditions.

We continue to examine moderating factors in the next study. First, we examine

how the level of luxury moderates spotlight feelings and self-regulatory depletion. The

94
luxury market for items such as handbags, watches, and clothes is often viewed as

consisting of different price-quality tiers, with brands such as Louis Vuitton and Prada at

the top (“premier” or “premium” luxury) versus brands such as Coach and Tory Burch at

the bottom (“affordable” or “accessible” luxury). In prior studies, we used premium

luxury brands (Louis Vuitton and Burberry), and we predict that the effects observed with

these brands would be less apparent with affordable luxury brands like Coach. Affordable

luxury brands are lower-priced, purchased by more consumers, and therefore, garner less

attention that premier luxury goods. Consumers carrying affordable luxury goods should

not feel as self-conscious and watched by others, and thus, we expect that they will

experience less self-regulatory resource depletion and will be more able to exert self-

control when tempted by tasty unhealthy food. In short, the luxury depletion effect should

be more evident for premier than affordable luxury goods.

Second, we examine individual differences in chronic levels of self-control as an

important moderator. At a foundational level, people have varying abilities to control

their thoughts, impulses, and behaviors. Although it may be difficult for people to

consistently resist temptations such as eating candy, especially after their self-regulatory

resources have been depleted, people with lower (vs. higher) levels of self-control are

even less likely to overcome this challenge successfully (Muraven, Shmueli, and Burkley

2006; Hofmann, et al. 2012). If self-regulatory resource depletion and lack of self-control

resources are responsible for increasing unhealthy food consumption, then low self-

control participants should be even less successful at resisting the tempting M&M’s

candies than high self-control participants.

95
Both moderators, level of luxury and trait self-control, are examined in the

context of public luxury consumption in study 4. We predict that spotlight feelings and

candy consumption will be greater when individuals use a premier (Prada) versus

affordable (Coach) luxury brand. Further, we predict an interaction between level of

luxury and trait self-control, expecting that the difference in candy consumption between

premier versus affordable luxury brands will be greater for individuals with lower (vs.

higher) trait self-control.

STUDY 4:

LEVEL OF LUXURY AND TRAIT SELF-CONTROL AS MODERATORS

Sample and Procedure

One hundred and seven female students (Mage = 20.09, SD = 1.07) from a public

university participated in the study in exchange for partial course credit and were

randomly assigned to one of the level of luxury conditions: Affordable Luxury (Coach)

vs. Premium Luxury (Prada). The procedure was identical to study 1, except the

computer-based survey administered at the end of the study included a measure of trait

self-control.

Brand Pretests and Handbag Selection

To select affordable versus premium luxury brands, undergraduate female

students (N = 68; Mage= 20.05, SD = 2.32) were surveyed. They were provided with a

detailed definition of an affordable luxury brand and a premium luxury brand, and were

shown a list of 16 popular luxury brands of handbags (e.g., Chanel, Burberry, Prada,

Louis Vuitton, Coach, Marc By Marc Jacobs, Juicy Couture). They were then asked to

put each brand into one of the following categories: (1) affordable luxury brand, (2)

96
premium luxury brand, (3) not a luxury brand, and (4) not familiar with the brand. Coach

was deemed the best example of an affordable luxury brand (85% of students placed it in

this category), while Prada was chosen as a clear example of a premium luxury brand

(88% of students placed it in this category).

Thus, we used Coach and Prada handbags for the study. To minimize style

differences, we chose a Prada handbag in a medium blue Saffiano leather (retail price:

$1890) and a similar sized Coach handbag in a medium blue Saffiano leather (retail price

$295). We pretested both handbags to ensure that the Prada handbag would elicit greater

feelings of being in the spotlight. We asked undergraduate female students (N = 34, Mage

= 20.52, SD = 2.14) to rate pictures of several handbags, including the Prada and Coach

handbags. Students were asked to imagine they were walking around with each handbag

and rated the extent to which they felt that it would: (1) attract attention from others, (2)

be noticed by others, (3) make them feel they would need to be more careful in front of

others, and (4) make them feel self-conscious about carrying the handbag. These four

items were combined into a spotlight index for the Prada handbag (α =.86) and a spotlight

index for the Coach handbag (α = .82). Results confirmed that the selected Prada handbag

elicited significantly greater feelings of being in the spotlight than the selected Coach

handbag (M = 5.57 vs. 4.65, t(33) = -4.89, p < .001).

Measures

Food Consumption. The weight of M&M’s candy consumed (in grams) served as

the main dependent variable.

Trait Self-Control. After several unrelated filler tasks were completed,

participants filled out the self-control trait measures. We used the 12-item short form of

97
Tangney et al.’s (2004) general trait self-control scale, which is the most commonly used

and well-validated measure of trait self-control in various self-control domains (e.g.,

Redden and Haws 2013; de Ridder, et al. 2012). Supported by past research, we used this

general measure because all self-control regulation tends to come from generalized

sources (Vohs and Baumeister 2004). The scale contains items such as “I refuse things

that are bad for me” and “I am good at resisting temptation.” Participants responded on a

7-point scale (1 = Not at all to 7 = Very much), and responses to 12 items were averaged

to form a self-control index (α = .68), with scores mean-centered for regression analyses.

Results

Food consumption. Consistent with predictions, participants who used the Prada

handbag ate significantly more M&M’s (M = 24.13, SD = 21.58) than participants who

used the Coach handbag (M = 13.78, SD = 12.52, t(105) = 2.92, p = .004, see figure 6).

---Figure 6 about here---

Trait self-control. To examine whether differences between affordable versus

premier luxury would be affected by an individual’s trait self-control, a multiple

regression was conducted with Level of Luxury (Premium = 0, Affordable =1),

participants’ mean centered Self-Control index, and their interaction as independent

variables, with food consumption as the dependent variable. The results revealed a

significant main effect of Level of Luxury (β = -45.89, t(103) = -2.25, p = .02), a

significant main effect of Self-Control (β = -8.52, t(103) = -2.95, p = .003), and a near

significant interaction term (β = 8.79, t(103) = 1.84, p = .06). To test our specific

predictions, we performed spotlight analysis for participants who have stronger self-

control (+1 SD above the mean) and weaker self-control (-1 SD below the mean; Aiken

98
and West 1991; Fitzsimons 2008). As depicted in figure 7, for participants with stronger

self-control, using the premium luxury handbag or the affordable luxury handbag did not

affect how much candy they ate (M = 16.83 vs. 12.95, t(103) = -.59, p = .55). As

expected, individuals with stronger self-control were generally better at resisting

tempting unhealthy food. In contrast, participants with weaker self-control ate

significantly more candy when they used the premium compared to the affordable luxury

handbag (M = 29.66 vs. 13.54, t(103) = -3.11, p = .001).

---Figure 7 about here---

Discussion

Study 4 provided evidence of two important moderators of the luxury depletion

effect found in our prior studies: level of luxury and trait self-control. Compared to an

affordable luxury brand handbag, using a premium luxury brand handbag depleted more

of a participant’s self-regulatory resources, as indicated by higher consumption of candy.

Trait self-control was also shown to be a moderator. The effect of using different levels

of luxury handbags was strongest for people who had weaker trait self-control.

GENERAL DISCUSSION

When we use a luxury good, how does it affect the way we think, feel, and behave?

We observed the actual use of luxury goods in our studies, and found that luxury

consumption can affect one’s psychological state and subsequent behavior. Across four

experiments, we found that using a luxury good in public elicits a spotlight mindset,

where consumers feel watched, self-conscious, and needing to manage their behavior in

front of others. This mindfulness requires self-regulation resources, and depletion of

these resources reduces one’s ability to exert self-control in subsequent situations (such

99
as resisting tasty but unhealthy food). We find this luxury depletion effect for different

premium luxury brands (Louis Vuitton, Prada, Burberry) and different acquisition

settings (luxury good is given versus chosen). Further, we find that the effect is stronger

for public (vs. private) luxury usage, for premier (vs. affordable) luxury goods, and for

individuals with low (vs. high) trait self-control. Taken together, our results show that

luxury consumption can have unanticipated negative consequences—it can lower your

ability to exert self-control in everyday life.

Below, we discuss our findings in terms of their contributions to several research

streams, and also suggest directions for future research.

Luxury and Conspicuous Consumption

In contrast to prior work on consumer motivations for acquiring luxury goods, we

examine what the actual luxury consumption experience is like for consumers. To our

knowledge, our work is the first to explore the psychological states and behavioral

consequences of using luxury goods. While much of the prior research suggests that

consumers expect the experience of consuming luxury goods to produce positive feelings

(e.g., power, status, self-enhancement) and positive consequences (e.g.,

attracting/protecting a mate), we find that there can be unanticipated negative aspects to

using luxury goods. In fact, our results show that the luxury consumption experience can

be a mixed bag, with consumers feeling more powerful, yet also feeling self-conscious

and needing to be careful of how they behave in front of others.

These novel effects on consumers’ psychological states and subsequent behaviors

deserve more inquiry, and our research provides an experimental paradigm for doing so.

Actual consumption behavior can be difficult to study in a laboratory setting, yet we

100
show that allowing research participants to use a luxury good for as little as 15 minutes

allows us to capture changes in how they feel, think, and behave. This experimental

paradigm can be utilized and adopted for future research examining these effects in more

detail. For example, one might investigate whether luxury goods deplete self-regulatory

resources if the good is used for longer stretches of time, or how long the depletion effect

lasts for situations throughout the day. For these types of questions, the research

procedures can be adapted by having participants experience multiple episodes of using

the same luxury good, or engaging in multiple tasks requiring self-control.

In this vein, there are a number of interesting questions that can be pursued in

future research. First, we might examine whether our effects generalize to other situations

where self-regulatory resources are required to exert self-control. For example: Does

using a luxury good make consumers more prone to impulse purchases? Does using a

luxury good cause individuals to purchase even more luxury goods? Prior work has

suggested that individuals continue to purchase material goods and luxury items as a way

to combat hedonic adaptation as they seek to raise their level of happiness. Our research

suggests that repeated purchases of luxury items may also be explained by lack of self-

control—that is, shopping while wearing a luxury item can reduce self-control, resulting

in subsequent purchases of more luxury items.

Luxury Consumption and Increased Self-Control

Although we found that luxury consumption reduces self-control, is it possible

that there are conditions under which this effect may be reversed? Could luxury

consumption ever lead to increases in self-control? We believe this possibility exists

based on the following line of reasoning. Our findings from study 2 show that using a

101
luxury product increases perceptions of power and status. These feelings of enhanced

power and status did not mediate the effects of luxury consumption on impaired self-

control in a subsequent situation (resisting tempting but unhealthy food). However,

several researchers have found that power leads to better self-control in that powerful

individuals are more likely to take action rather than being passive (Galinsky, Gruenfeld

and Magee 2003), and they perform better at controlling their attention on a variety of

tasks (Guinote 2007). Finally, particularly relevant to the current investigation, DeWall et

al. (2011) found that powerful people are better at self-control, especially when resources

for self-regulation are low.

How can luxury consumption both impair and enhance self-control? We speculate

that the nature of the self-control task is a determining factor. In our research, the self-

control task is to resist eating tempting but unhealthy candy. Importantly, this task was

incorporated into the experimental procedure in a way that did not draw attention to the

fact that we were going to evaluate the participant’s food consumption. And, we did not

state an explicit goal for participants to resist eating too much food, such as “try not to

each too much of the snacks because they are high in fat and calories.” However, if we

had used a self-control task with an explicit goal communicated in our instructions to

participants, especially a goal that is connected to power or status/achievement, we

believe that the enhanced self-control tendencies associated with power/status (and

luxury use) could result in better self-control for luxury users.

To explore this possibility, we conducted an additional experiment with

procedures similar to our prior studies. We provided participants with a luxury or a non-

luxury handbag, which they used in a public setting for 15 minutes. After using the

102
handbag, they returned to the lab and were told they would be completing an unrelated

task, which was a series of word puzzles. They read a cover story about how performance

on these types of tasks has been shown to predict academic and career success, and were

then instructed to start working on the word puzzles for as long as they wanted to.

Unknown to the participants, the word puzzles were non-solvable. We measured how

long participants persisted in working on the word puzzles, with longer time indicating

stronger self-control efforts. The results showed that participants in the luxury condition

persisted for a longer time on the self-control task than participants in the control

condition (M = 550.52 sec vs. 386.07 sec, t(41)=1.99, p = .053).

These findings of better self-control for luxury users can be incorporated into our

basic story about luxury consumption and self-control. Using a luxury product does

increase thoughts about impression management, which depletes self-regulatory

resources and leads to poorer self-control unless there is an explicit goal that is salient

and instrumental. When an explicit goal of this nature is activated, the additional self-

control associated with power/status and luxury use can be harnessed to meet this goal,

resulting in better self-control. This is consistent with research showing that when self-

control resources are depleted, power motivates selective self-control towards goals that

are important (DeWall et al. 2011).

Self-Regulation of Behavior

Our work also makes important contributions to self-regulation research and

provides novel empirical evidence supporting the self-regulatory resource depletion

model. First, we examine self-regulatory depletion that results from an interpersonal

context, using luxury goods in public. The superior ability to regulate one’s cognitive,

103
emotional, and behavioral activity sets humans apart from other species and plays an

important role in humanity’s evolutionary history. Given the important role of

interpersonal relationships in human being’s survival and reproduction (Baumeister and

Leary 1995; Dunbar 1998; Leary 1999), it is plausible to argue that self-regulation

evolved, at least in part, to improve interpersonal success. However, most prior work in

self-regulation has focused on intrapsychic processes, with less exploration of

interpersonal processes. Only a few papers have examined how interpersonal processes,

such as challenging self-presentation contexts or social feedback situations, are related to

self-regulatory performances (DeWall, Baumeister, and Vohs 2008; Vohs, Baumeister,

and Ciarocco 2005).

Second, we demonstrate that even tasks generally considered to be interesting and

enjoyable, such as using a luxury product, can draw upon and deplete self-regulatory

resources. Prior work in self-regulation includes depletion tasks that can be characterized

as challenging, tedious, or uncomfortable. Our findings provide evidence that even

positive depleting tasks (using a luxury product) can produce negative depletion

consequences, proving that self-regulatory depletion occurs in the absence of depleting

tasks that are negative in valence.

Finally, in contrast to prior self-regulation research, we provide direct process

evidence to support the self-regulatory resource depletion model. Researchers have

supplied evidence for this model by comparing performance on a self-control task for

individuals who completed a prior task that required or did not require self-regulation. If

performance is worse for individuals who completed a prior task requiring self-regulation,

researchers infer that self-regulatory resources have been depleted. Thus, there is no

104
direct process evidence that depletion has occurred. Although we used the same basic

experimental approach, we also measured a phenomena related to resource depletion

(spotlight mindset) and showed that it mediated the relationship between the source of

resource depletion (using a luxury good) and the effects of resource depletion (candy

consumption). Our mediation results (study 1 & study 2) confirm that stronger spotlight

mindsets result in stronger depletion of self-regulatory resources, which leads to a greater

lack of self-control in a subsequent task (resisting tempting unhealthy snacks).

Materialism and Consumer Well-Being

Materialistic individuals view material possessions as a means for reaching

desired end states, such as happiness and success (Richins and Dawson 1992). However,

materialism has been consistently found to be associated with a variety of negative

outcomes, such as lower levels of psychological well-being and life satisfaction (Belk

1985; Mick 1996; Richins and Dawson 1992), depression (Wachtel and Blatt 1990),

using addictive substances (Kasser and Ryan 2001), and less productive interpersonal

relationships (Kasser 2002).

Among types of material possessions, luxury goods are often mentioned in

discussions of materialism, presumably because luxury goods are such a visible symbol

of status or success. Thus, we would expect that luxury consumption would be associated

with many of the negative long-term consequences of materialism. Our findings add to

this picture by demonstrating that luxury goods can also have immediate negative effects

on consumer well-being. We show that using luxury goods depletes self-regulatory

resources that are often needed for other tasks in one’s daily life. In effect, there is an

opportunity cost of using luxury goods—self-regulatory resources are less available for

105
tasks that would increase an individual’s well-being, such as resisting unhealthy snacks,

quitting smoking, and devoting time to exercise.

In future research, it would be interesting to examine the effect of repeated luxury

consumption on self-regulatory resource depletion. If consumers use luxury goods

frequently, would they become habituated to the experience and cease to experience the

negative effects we observed? We might predict that feelings of being in a spotlight

would abate as consumers become more accustomed to using luxury goods in public,

much like an actor that becomes accustomed to being in a spotlight. Perhaps individuals

from wealthy backgrounds, who have grown up with luxury goods, become used to the

attention that luxury cars, watches, and handbags garner in public. However, it is also

possible that repeated consumption of luxury goods may exacerbate the spotlight mindset,

with stronger feelings of self-consciousness and mindfulness of one’s behavior. Or,

perhaps the social environment in which luxury consumption takes place is more

important. The spotlight mindset is triggered by using a luxury good in public, but in our

studies, the public setting is one in which luxury products are not that visible. Perhaps

this mindset would not be activated in environments where luxury goods are common

among one’s peers. Thus, ironically, luxury consumption may be more damaging to well-

being in social environments with few luxury users, as opposed to social environments

where luxury use is relatively common among many individuals.

Research along these lines would provide a fuller understanding of the effects of

luxury consumption on consumer well-being. The negative impact of luxury consumption

is not limited to our pocketbooks, and further research on the negative consequences in

106
terms of psychological states and behaviors holds the promise of helping consumers

understand the trade-offs they make when they embrace a luxury consumption lifestyle.

107
ANCILLARY STUDIES
STUDY A: LUXURY CONSUMPTION AND BETTER SELF-CONTROL

Sample and Procedure


Forty two female students (Mage = 20.05, SD = 1.83) from a public university
participated in the study in exchange for partial course credit and were randomly assigned
to use either a luxury brand handbag (Prada) or a non-luxury handbag. The procedure
was identical to study 4, except a different dependent measure.

Dependent Measure
Similar to study 4, after 15 minutes of using the handbag, all participants returned
to their individual lab room. Next, participants were told that they would complete a task
that in an unrelated study and read a cover story about how performance on verbal tasks
has been shown to predict academic and career success. Next, participants were asked to
start working on a series of word puzzles for as long as they want to. They were told they
could decide to quit anytime by clicking a button on the computer screen and then
proceed to the rest of the survey. The focal task included finding 24 words embedded in a
20 by 20 matrix of letters. Unknown to the participants, some of the words were not
included in the matrix, and thus the task was unsolvable. Per previous literature (Dewall
et al. 2011), the longer participants persist on an unsolvable task, the better self-control
performance it suggests. Therefore, the time (in seconds) each participant spent on this
task served as the main dependent measure, with longer time indicating stronger self-
control efforts.

Results
The results showed that participants in the luxury condition persisted for a longer
period of time (M = 550.52, SD = 268.18) on the self-control task than participants in the
control condition (M = 386.08, SD = 266.66, t(41) = 1.99, p = .053).

STUDY B: LUXURY CONSUMPTION AND BETTER SELF-CONTROL

Sample and Procedure


Thirty three female students (Mage = 19.88, SD = .89) from a public university
participated the study in exchange for partial course credit and were randomly assigned to
use either a luxury brand handbag (Prada) or a non-luxury handbag. The smaller number
of subjects in this study was due to an unexpected low show up rate. The procedure was
identical to study 4, except for a different dependent measure.

108
Dependent Measure
Similar to study 4, after 15 minutes of using the handbag, all participants returned
to their individual lab room. Next, participants were told that they would complete a task
for an unrelated study. They read a cover story about how performance on the task
requires mathematic ability and a sense of concentration, which have been shown to be
the best index of general intelligence. Next, participants were asked to start working on a
series of math matrices for as long as they wanted to. They were told that could decide to
quit anytime by clicking a button on the computer screen and proceeding to the rest of the
survey. The focal task included 20 matrices, each containing a set of 12 three-digit
number (e.g., 2.13). Participants were instructed to write down the two numbers in each
matrix that added up to exactly 10. Unknown to the participants, some of the matrices
were unsolvable. Similar to the previous study, the time (in seconds) each participant
spent on this task serves as the main dependent measure, with longer time indicating
stronger self-control efforts.

Results
The results showed that participants in the luxury condition persisted for a longer
period time (M = 470.26, SD = 179.43) on the self-control task than participants in the
control condition (M = 407.61, SD = 200.27, t(31)=.94, p = .35). Although the difference
between the two conditions was not statistically significant due to lower statistical power
(small sample size), it was on the same direction as the previous study.

109
REFERENCES

Aiken, Leona S. and Stephen G. West (1991), Multiple Regression: Testing and

Interpreting Interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Bagwell, Laurie S. and B. Douglas Bernheim (1996), “Veblen Effects in a Theory of

Conspicuous Consumption,” The American Economic Review, 86, 3 (June), 349-

73.

Banfield, Jane F., Carrie L. Wyland, C. Neil Macrae, Thomas F. Munte, and Todd F.

Heatherton (2004), “The Cognitive Neuroscience of Self-Regulation,” in The

Handbook of Self-Regulation, eds. Roy F. Baumeister and Kathleen D. Vohs,

New York: Guilford, 62-83.

Baumeister, Roy F., Ellen Bratslavsky, Mark Muraven, and Dianne M. Tice (1998), “Ego

Depletion: Is the Active Self a Limited Resource?” Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology 74 (5), 1252-65.

Baumeister, Roy F., and Mark R. Leary (1995), “The Need to Belong: Desire for

Interpersonal Attachments as a Fundamental Human Motivation,” Psychological

Bulletin, 117 (3), 497-529.

Baumeister Roy F. and Todd F. Heatherton (1996), “Self-Regulation Failure: An

Overview,” Psychological Inquiry, 7 (1), 1-15.

Baumeister Roy F., Todd F. Heatherton, and Dianne M. Tice (1994), Losing Control:

How and Why People Fail at Self-Regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Baumeister Roy F., Kathleen D. Vohs, and Dianne M. Tice (2007), “The Strength Model

of Self-Control,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16 (6), 351-55.

110
Belk, Russell W. (1985), “Trait Aspects of Living in the Material World,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 12 (December), 265-80.

Berger, Jonah and Morgan Ward (2010), “Subtle Signals of Inconspicuous

Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (December), 555-69.

Carver, Charles S. and Michael F. Scheier (2001), On the Self-Regulation of Behavior.

New York: Cambridge University Press.

Charles, Kerwin Kofi, Erik Hurst, and Nikolai Roussanov (2009), “Conspicuous

Consumption and Race,” The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 124 (2), 425-67.

D’Arpizio, Claudia (2012), “Luxury Goods Worldwide Market Study, Fall 2013,” Bain

& Company, accessed Jan 24, 2014,

http://www.bain.com/publications/articles/luxury-goods-worldwide-market-study-

fall-2013.aspx

de Ridder, Denise T. D., Gerty Lensvelt-Mulders, Catrin Finkenauer, F. Marijn Stok, and

Roy F. Baumeister (2012), “Taking Stock of Self-Control: A Meta-Analysis of

How Trait Self-Control Relates to a Wide Range of Behaviors,” Personality and

Social Psychology Review, 16(1), 76-99.

DeWall, C. Nathan, Roy F. Baumeister, Nicole L. Mead and Kathleen D. Vohs (2011),

“How Leaders Self-Regulate Their Task Performances: Evidence That Power

Promotes Diligence, Depletion, and Disdain,” Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 100 (1), 47-65.

DeWall, C. Nathan, Roy F. Baumeister, and Kathleen D. Vohs (2008), “Satiated With

Belongingness? Effects of Acceptance, Rejection, and Task Framing on Self-

111
Regulatory Performance,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95 (6),

1367-82.

Dunbar, Robin I.M. (1998), “The Social Brain Hypothesis,” Evolutionary Anthropology,

6, 178-90.

Euromonitor (2013), “Global Luxury Goods Sales Exceed US$318 Billion,” accessed

January 14, 2014, www.marketwired.com/press-release/global-luxury-goods-

sales-exceed-us318-billion-1838681.htm.

Fenigstein, Allan (1979), “Self-Consciousness, Self-Attention, and Social Interaction,”

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 75-86.

_________ (1984), “Self-Consciousness and the Overperception of Self as a Target,” Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 860-70.

Fenigstein, Allan, Michael F. Scheier, and Arnold H. Buss (1975), “Public and Private

Self-Consciousness: Assessment and Theory,” Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 43 (4), 522-27.

Fitzsimons, Gavan J. (2008), “Editorial: Death to Dichotomizing,” Journal of Consumer

Research, 35 (1), 5-8.

Froming, William J. and Charles S. Carver (1981), “Divergent Influence of Private and

Public Self-Consciousness in a Compliance Paradigm,” Journal of Research in

Personality, 15, 159-71.

Galinsky, Adam D., Deborah H Gruenfeld, and Joe C. Megee (2003), “From Power to

Action,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85 (3), 453- 466.

Gilovich, Thomas, Victoria Husted Medvec, and Kenneth Savitsky (2000), “The

Spotlight Effect in Social Judgment: An Egocentric Bias in Estimates of the

112
Salience of One’s Own Actions and Appearance,” Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 78 (2), 211-22.

Gilovich, Thomas and Kenneth Savitsky (1999), “The Spotlight Effect and the Illusion of

Transparency: Egocentric Assessments of How We Are Seen by Others,” Current

Directions in Psychological Science, 8 (6), 165-68.

Griskevicius, Vladas, Joshua M. Tybur, Jill M. Sundie, Robert B. Cialdini, Geoffrey F.

Miller, and Douglas T. Kenrick (2007), “Blatant Benevolence and Conspicuous

Consumption: When Romantic Motives Elicit Strategic Costly Signals,” Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 93 (1), 85-102.

Grinote, Ana (2007), “Power and Goal Pursuit,” Personality and Social Psychology

Bulletin, 33 (8), 1076-87.

Han, Young Jee, Joseph C. Nunes, and Xavier Drèze (2010), “Signaling Status with

Luxury Goods: The Role of Brand Prominence,” Journal of Marketing, 74 (July),

15-30.

Hayes, Andrew F. (2012), “PROCESS: A Versatile Computational Tool for Observed

Variable Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Modeling [White

paper],” Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/

Heatherton, Todd F. (2011), “Neuroscience of Self and Self-Regulation,” The Annual

Review of Psychology, 62, 363-90.

Hofmann, Wilhelm, Roy F. Baumeister, Georg Forster, and Kathleen D. Vohs (2012),

“Everyday Temptations: An Experience Sampling Study of Desire, Conflict, and

Self-Control,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102 (6), 1318-35.

113
Interbrand (2013), “Best Global Brands 2013,” accessed January 14, 2014,

www.interbrand.com/en/best-global-brands/2013/Best-Global-Brands-2013.aspx.

Kasser, Tim (2002), The High Price of Materialism, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Kasser, Tim and Richard M. Ryan (2001), “Be Careful What You Wish For: Optimal

Functioning and the Relative Attainment of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Goals,” in Life

Goals and Wellbeing: Towards a Positive Psychology of Human Striving, eds.

Peter Schmuck and Kennon M. Sheldon, Goetingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber,

116-31.

Krendl, Anne C. and Todd F. Heatherton (2009), “Self Versus Others/Self-Regulation,”

in Handbook of Neuroscience for the Behavioral Sciences, eds. Gary G. Berntson

and John T. Cacioppo, Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, 859-78.

Leary, Mark R. (1999), “Making Sense of Self-Esteem,” Current Directions in

Psychological Science, 8 (1), 32-35.

Mazzocco, Philip. J., Derek D. Rucker, Adam D. Galinsky, and Eric T. Anderson (2012),

“Direct and Vicarious Conspicuous Consumption: Identification with Low-Status

Groups Increase the Desire for High-Status Goods,” Journal of Consumer

Psychology, 22, 520-28.

Mick, David G. (1996), “Are Studies of Dark Side Variable Confounded by Socially

Desirable Responding? The Case of Materialism,” Journal of Consumer Research,

23 (2), 106-119.

Muraven, Mark, Dianne M. Tice, and Roy F. Baumeister (1998), “Self-Control as a

Limited Resource: Regulatory Depletion Patterns,” Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 74 (3), 774-89.

114
Muraven, Mark, Dikla Shmueli, and Edward Burkley (2006), “Conserving Self-Control

Strength,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91 (3), 524-37.

Paton, Elizabeth (2013), “Prada Sales Climb 29% in a Year,” Financial Times, accessed

January 14, 2014, www.ft.com/cms/s/0/7b6bf2d0-7aa6-11e2-9cc2-

00144feabdc0.html.

Patrick Vanessa M. and Henrik Hagtvedt (2009), “Luxury Branding,” in The Handbook

of Brand Relationships, eds. Deborah J. MacInnis, C. Whan Park, and Joseph W.

Priester, New York: M.E. Sharpe, 267-80.

Phau, Ian and Gerard Prendergast (2000). “Consuming Luxury Brands: The Relevance of

the ‘Rarity Principle’,” Journal of Brand Management, 7 (5), 366-75.

Piff, Paul K. (2014), “Wealth and the Inflated Self: Class, Entitlement, and Narcissism,”

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40 (1) 34-43.

Preacher, Kristopher J. and Andrew Hayes (2008), “Asymptotic and Resampling

Strategies for Assessing and Comparing Indirect Effects in Multiple Mediation

Model,” Behavior Research Method, 40 (3), 879-91.

Raichle, Katherine A., Alan J. Christensen, Shawna Ehlers, Patricia J. Moran, Lucy

Karnell, and Gerry Funk (2001), “Public and Private Self-Consciousness and

Smoking Behavior in Head and Neck Cancer Patients,” Annals of Behavioral

Medicine, 23 (2), 120-24.

Redden, Joseph P. and Kelly L. Haws (2013), “Healthy Satiation: The Role of

Decreasing Desire in Effective Self-Control,” Journal of Consumer Research, 39

(February), 1100-14.

115
Richins, Marsha L. (1987), “Media, Materialism, and Human Happiness,” Advances in

Consumer Research, 14 (1), 352-356.

_________ (1994), “Special Possessions and the Expression of Material Values,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 21 (December), 522-33.

Richins, Marsha L. and Scott Dawson (1992), “A Consumer Values Orientation for

Materialism and its Measurement: Scale Development and Validation,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 19 (December), 303-16.

Roberts, Andrew (2013), “LVMH to Adjust Vuitton Strategy amid Stable Leather Sales

Growth,” Bloomberg News, accessed January 14, 2014,

www.bloomberg.com/news/2013-01-31/lvmh-full-year-profit-increases-13-on-

gains-in-u-s-and-asia.html.

Rucker, Derek D. and Adam D. Galinsky (2009), “Conspicuous Consumption Versus

Utilitarian Ideals: How Different Levels of Power Shape Consumer Behavior,”

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(3), 549-55.

Rucker, Derek D., Adam D. Galinsky, and David Dubois (2012), “Power and Consumer

Behavior: How Power Shapes Who and What Consumers Value,” Journal of

Consumer Psychology, 22, 352-68.

Scheier, Michael F. (1980), “Effects of Public and Private Self-Consciousness on the

Public Expression of Personal Beliefs,” Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 39 (3), 514-21.

Sivanathan, Niro and Nathan C. Pettit (2010), “Protecting the Self through Consumption:

Status Goods as Affirmational Commodities,” Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 46, 564-70.

116
Sparshott, Jeffrey (2014), “What Products Drove Consumer Spending? Luxury Items,

Mostly,” The Wall Street Journal, accessed February 11, 2014,

http://blogs.wsj.com/economics/

2014/01/22/what-products-drove-consumer-spending-luxury-items-mostly/

Sundie, Jill M., Douglas T. Kenrick, Vladas Griskevicius, Joshua M. Tybur, Kathleen D.

Vohs, and Daniel J. Beal (2011), “Peacocks, Porsches, and Thorstein Veblen:

Conspicuous Consumption as a Sexual Signaling System,” Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 100 (4), 664-80.

Tangney, June P., Roy F. Baumeister, and Angie Luzio Boone, “High Self‐Control

Predicts Good Adjustment, Less Pathology, Better Grades, and Interpersonal

Success,” Journal of Personality, 72 (2), 271-324.

Tice, Dianne M., Ellen Bratslavsky, and Roy F. Baumeister, (2001), “Emotional Distress

Regulation Takes Precedence Over Impulse Control: If You Feel Bad, Do

It!” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 53-67.

Veblen, Thorstein (1899), The Theory of the Leisure Class, New York: Penguin.

Vigneron, Franck and Lester W. Johnson (2004), “Measuring Perceptions of Brand

Luxury,” Brand Management, 11 (July), 484-506.

Vohs, Kathleen D. and Todd F. Heatherton (2000), “Self-Regulatory Failure: A

Resource-Depletion Approach,” Psychological Science, 11 (3), 249-54.

Vohs, Kathleen D. and Roy F. Baumeister (2004), “Understanding Self-Regulation: An

Introduction,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and

Applications, eds. Roy F. Baumeister and Kathleen D. Vohs, New York: Guilford,

1-9.

117
Vohs, Kathleen D., Roy F. Baumeister, and Natalie J. Ciarocco (2005), “Self-Regulation

and Self-Presentation: Regulatory Resource Depletion Impairs Impression

Management and Effortful Self-Presentation Depletes Regulatory Resources,”

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88 (4), 632-57.

Wachtel, Paul L. and Sidney J. Blatt (1990), “Perceptions of Economic Needs and of

Anticipated Future Income,” Journal of Economic Psychology, 11(3), 403-15.

Wang, Yajin and Vladas Griskevicius (2014), “Conspicuous Consumption, Relationships,

and Rivals: Women’s Luxury Products as Signals to Other Women,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 40 (February), 834-54.

Watson, David, Lee A. Clark, and Auke Tellegen (1988), “Development and Validation

of Brief Measures of Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales,” Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 (6), 1063-70.

Wernerfelt, Birger (1990), “Advertising Content When Brand Choice is a Signal,” The

Journal of Business, 63 (1), 91-98.

Wilcox, Keith, Hyeong Min Kim, and Sankar Sen (2009), “Why Do Consumers Buy

Counterfeit Luxury Brands?” Journal of Marketing Research, 46 (April), 247-59.

118
FIGURE 1

STUDY 1: EFFECT OF LUXURY USE ON SPOTLIGHT MINDSET

AND M&M’S CONSUMPTION

119
FIGURE 2

STUDY 1: MEDIATION ANALYSIS

Spotlight Index

-.53** 2.05
*

Product Type M&M’s Consumed

-.5.48 *
(-.4.79)

Note: Coefficients are unstandardized


***p < .001; ** p < .01; * p < .05

120
FIGURE 3

STUDY 2: EFFECTS OF LUXURY USE ON FOOD CONSUMPTION

121
FIGURE 4

STUDY 2: MEDIATION ANALYSIS

Spotlight Index

.02

Product Type *
Food Consumed
Food Type
24.29 **
(-18.29)

Note: Coefficients are unstandardized


***p < .001: ** p < .01; * p < .05

122
FIGURE 5

STUDY 3: EFFECT OF CONSUMPTION CONTEXT AND SPOTLIGHT MINDSET

PRIME ON M&M’S CONSUMPTION

123
FIGURE 6

STUDY 4: EFFECT OF LUXURY USE ON M&M’S CONSUMPTION

124
FIGURE 7

STUDY 4: EFFECT OF LUXURY USE AND TRAIT SELF-CONTROL

ON M&M’S CONSUMPTION

125
CHAPTER IV

SUMMARY AND FUTURE RESEARCH DIRECTIONS

Prior research has examined people’s attitudes, preferences, and motivations for

desiring luxury goods, but we know little about what happens when consumers actually

use luxury products. My dissertation examines the psychological and behavioral effects

of luxury consumption, and asks the question: Does using a luxury product influence the

way a person feels and behaves? Two essays of my dissertation examining what happens

DURING and AFTER the consumption experience of a luxury product.

The first essay demonstrates that the experiences of using a luxury product

boosted women’s self-perceptions of social status, and this state then triggered self-

interested behaviors, with women making choices to benefit themselves. I found that

most of the time self-interested behavior manifested as selfish behavior, such as women

taking more money for themselves and donating less money to charity in private.

However, I found that self-interested behavior manifested as generous behavior when the

situation explicitly afforded people an opportunity to enhance their reputation by acting

generously. For example, I found that wearing luxury products led women to donate

more money to charity when donations were made in public in front of other people. Note

that the types of selfish and generous behavior assessed across six studies all constitute

self-interested behavior. Both selfish and generous behavior benefitted the person by

either enabling them to acquire more resources or by enabling them to gain a boost in

reputation. Indeed, both selfish and generous behaviors were triggered by an increased

sense of social status that resulted from consuming luxury goods.

126
The second essay examines how using luxury products reduces individuals’

ability to exert self-control in food consumption. Across four experiments, I found that

using a luxury good in public elicits a spotlight mindset, where consumers feel they are a

focus of attention and need to manage their behavior in front of others. This mindfulness

requires self-regulation resources, and depletion of these resources reduces one’s ability

to exert self-control in subsequent situations (such as resisting tasty but unhealthy food). I

found this luxury self-control effect for different premium luxury brands (Louis Vuitton,

Prada, Burberry) and different acquisition settings (luxury good is given versus chosen).

Further, I found that the effect is stronger for public (vs. private) luxury usage, for

premier (vs. affordable) luxury goods, and for individuals with low (vs. high) trait self-

control. Taken together, the results of Essay 2 show that luxury consumption can have

unanticipated negative consequences—it can lower your ability to exert self-control in

everyday life.

In summary, ten studies across two essays provide novel and compelling evidence

that using luxury goods affects how consumers feel and behave. Below, I discuss the

contribution of the findings and suggest avenues for future research.

Potential Significance and Contribution

I make two major contributions. First, to my knowledge, my dissertation is the

first examination of how luxury consumption affects individuals in terms of its

psychological and social consequences. I have developed a new experimental paradigm

to examine the effects of luxury consumption within a lab setting, opening up a new area

for research in my field. I provide luxury goods (handbags, scarves) to research

participants, ask them to use it in a naturalistic setting (walking around campus), and then

127
evaluate the consequences of this experience through subsequent tasks (eating candy,

donating to charity).

Second, my research will be the first to show negative psychological and

behavioral consequences of using luxury goods. Although there are obvious financial

costs for luxury consumption, I shed light on ways that luxury consumption negatively

affects the psychological functioning and well-being of individuals. For example, using

luxury products leads people to engage in self-interested behaviors, which suggest that

there are significant interpersonal and societal consequences of luxury consumption.

Furthermore, using luxury products also decreases one’s ability to exert self-control,

which is needed for many day-to-day situations, such as curbing overeating, engaging in

exercise, and resisting overspending for one’s budget.

Limitations and Future Directions

Although all the studies in my dissertation were focused on women, I expect that

luxury consumption should also have similar effects on men. In fact, given that previous

research has shown that men exhibit a stronger preference for signaling power and status

(Hays 2013; Melnyk and Osselaer 2012), it is reasonable to argue that the effects

demonstrated in my dissertation should even be stronger among male consumers. Future

research can expand the product type to be more male relevant and explore how luxury

consumption can influence male consumers’ social behaviors. For example, thinking

about possible consequences associated with the spotlight mindset for male consumers

suggests some interesting topics. Prior research has suggested that men use luxury goods

as a way to signal to prospective mates that they have financial resources to be a good

provider for the family. Presumably, this signal is appealing to women, which makes the

128
male a more desirable partner (Griskevicius et al. 2007; Sundie et al. 2011). However, an

interesting possibility is that men using luxury goods are also more mindful and more

careful about their behavior in front of others. If this results in more “gentlemanly”

behavior—being more polite and more gracious—these behaviors can also be very

appealing to a prospective mate. As this example suggests, the spotlight mindset may also

trigger positive behaviors, in contrast to the more negative behaviors I highlight in my

dissertation.

A limitation of the current studies is that participants engaged in luxury

consumption for only 15 minutes due to the experimental nature of the studies. On one

hand, this procedure shows the robustness of the obtained luxury effects given that they

emerged in such a short period of time. On the other hand, it opens up several interesting

avenues for future research. For example, if consumers use luxury goods for a longer

period of time, would they become habituated to the experience and cease to show the

negative effects I observed? Similarly, all the subjects in the experiments were

undergraduate students from the business school. Although the subject population is

pretested to be luxury consumers, they still are likely to own relatively fewer and less

expensive luxury products compared to affluent consumers, who own and frequently use

high-end luxury goods. Future research is needed to examine the repeated consumption

of luxury goods and the effects of luxury consumption among consumers with different

social economic backgrounds.

Third, the findings across all ten experiments indicate that, on average,

participants who used a luxury product exhibited more self-interested behaviors (Essay 1)

and less self-control (Essay 2). It is notable that the observed effects were quite strong,

129
regardless of the participants’ initial attitudes and desires for luxury products. Future

research is needed to explore how individual differences can moderate the effects. For

example, the role of the audience may be an important factor that affects preferences for

luxury consumption (e.g., Berger and Ward 2010; Han et al. 2010; Wang and

Griskevicius 2014), so it is possible that consumers’ feelings elicited by the luxury

consumption experiences are affected by the audience they are in front of.

Another interesting avenue for future research is to consider the level of

conspicuousness of the luxury product. Past research has suggested that luxury products

can be conspicuous or inconspicuous (Berger and Ward 2010), and luxury brands can be

loud or quiet (Han et al. 2010). As discussed earlier, all the products used in my

experiments were pretested to be well liked by the population I studied, and included

both relative conspicuous/loud and inconspicuous/quiet luxury products. Therefore, I

observed the effects of luxury consumption across different types of luxury products.

Future research can explore whether using a quiet versus a loud luxury product might

affect the types of psychological responses and behavioral consequences I observed in

my studies.

Finally, another limitation of the current studies is that they relied on women from

one culture – the United States. Given that the phenomena of consuming luxury goods

has persisted across history and human cultures, the fundamental psychological states and

consequences of luxury consumption are likely to generalize to consumers in other

cultures. However, culture will, of course, play an important role in determining the kind

of behaviors people engage in to enhance reputation or to create a good impression. In

fact, there is reason to believe that luxury consumption might even have stronger effects

130
in non-Western cultures where the hierarchy of society is more salient. Therefore, we

might expect consumers in such cultures to experience even stronger feelings of higher

perceived status (Essay 1) or impression management concerns (Essay 2).

131
References

Berger, Jonah and Morgan Ward (2010), “Subtle Signals of Inconspicuous

Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (December), 555-69.

Griskevicius, Vladas, Joshua M. Tybur, Jill M. Sundie, Robert B. Cialdini, Geoffrey F.

Miller, and Douglas T. Kenrick (2007), “Blatant Benevolence and Conspicuous

Consumption: When Romantic Motives Elicit Strategic Costly Signals,” Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 93 (1), 85-102.

Han, Young Jee, Joseph C. Nunes, and Xavier Drèze (2010), “Signaling Status with

Luxury Goods: The Role of Brand Prominence,” Journal of Marketing, 74 (July),

15-30.

Hays, Nicholas A. (2013), “Fear and Loving in Social Hierarchy: Sex Differences in

Preferences for Power versus Status,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,

49(6), 1130-1136.

Melnyk, Valentyna, and Stijn MJ van Osselaer (2012), “Make Me Special: Gender

Differences in Consumers’ Responses to Loyalty Programs,” 23(3), 545-559.

Sundie, Jill M., Douglas T. Kenrick, Vladas Grikevicius, Joshua M. Tybur, Kathleen D.

Vohs, and Daniel J. Beal (2011), “Peacocks, Porsches, and Thorstein Veblen:

Conspicuous Consumption as a Sexual Signaling System,” Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 100 (4), 664-80.

Wang, Yajin and Vladas Griskevicius (2014), “Conspicuous Consumption, Relationships,

and Rivals: Women’s Luxury Products as Signals to Other Women,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 40 (February), 834-54.

132
BIBLIOGRAPHY

Aiken, Leona S. and Stephen G. West (1991), Multiple Regression: Testing and

Interpreting Interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Archer, John (2006), “Testosterone and Human Aggression: An Evaluation of the

Challenge Hypothesis,” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 319-45.

Bagwell, Laurie S. and B. Douglas Bernheim (1996), “Veblen Effects in a Theory of

Conspicuous Consumption,” The American Economic Review, 86, 3 (Jun), 349-73.

Banfield, Jane F., Carrie L. Wyland, C. Neil Macrae, Thomas F. Munte, and Todd F.

Heatherton (2004), “The Cognitive Neuroscience of Self-Regulation,” in The

Handbook of Self-Regulation, eds. Roy F. Baumeister and Kathleen D. Vohs,

New York: Guilford, 62-83.

Baumeister, Roy F., Ellen Bratslavsky, Mark Muraven, and Dianne M. Tice (1998), “Ego

Depletion: Is the Active Self a Limited Resource?” Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology 74 (5), 1252-65.

Baumeister, Roy F., and Mark R. Leary (1995), “The Need to Belong: Desire for

Interpersonal Attachments as a Fundamental Human Motivation,” Psychological

Bulletin, 117 (3), 497-529.

Baumeister Roy F. and Todd F. Heatherton (1996), “Self-Regulation Failure: An

Overview,” Psychological Inquiry, 7 (1), 1-15.

Baumeister Roy F., Todd F. Heatherton, and Dianne M. Tice (1994), Losing Control:

How and Why People Fail at Self-Regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Baumeister Roy F., Kathleen D. Vohs, and Dianne M. Tice (2007), “The Strength Model

of Self-Control,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16 (6), 351-55.

133
Belk, Russell W. (1985), “Trait Aspects of Living in the Material World,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 12 (December), 265-80.

Belk, Russell W., Kenneth D. Bahn, and Robert N. Mayer (1982), "Developmental

Recognition of Consumption Symbolism,” Journal of Consumer Research, 9

(June), 4-16.

Berger, Jonah and Morgan Ward (2010), “Subtle Signals of Inconspicuous

Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (December), 555-69.

Boksem, Maarten A. S., Pranjal H. Mehta, Bram Van den Bergh, Veerie van Son, Stefan

T. Trautmann, Karin Roelofs, Ale Smidts, and Alan G. Sanfey (2013),

“Testosterone Inhibits Trust but Promotes Reciprocity,” Psychological Science,

24(11), 2306-14.

Carver, Charles S. and Michael F. Scheier (2001), On the Self-Regulation of Behavior.

New York: Cambridge University Press.

Charles, Kerwin Kofi, Erik Hurst, and Nikolai Roussanov (2009), “Conspicuous

Consumption and Race,” The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 124 (2), 425-67.

Chartrand, Tanya L., Joel Huber, Baba Shiv, and Robin J. Tanner (2008), “Nonconscious

Goals and Consumer Choice,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (August), 189-

201.

Cole, Douglas and Ira Chaikin (1990). An Iron Band upon the People – The Law Against

the Potlatch on the Northwest Coast, Seattle, WA: University of Washington

Press.

Cottrell, Catherine A., Steven L. Neuberg, and Norman P. Li (2007), “What do People

Desire in Others? A Sociofunctional Perspective on the Importance of Different

134
Valued Characteristics,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(2),

208-31.

D’Arpizio, Claudia (2013), “Luxury Goods Worldwide Market Study, Fall 2013,” Bain

& Company, accessed April 27, 2014, http://www.bain.com/publications/articles/

luxury-goods-worldwide-market-study-fall-2013.aspx

D’Arpizio, Claudia (2014), “Luxury Goods Worldwide Market Study, Winter 2014,”

Bain & Company, accessed April 27, 2014, http://www.bain.com/publications/

articles/luxury-goods-worldwide-markey-study-winter-2014.aspx

Forbes (2013), “The World’s Most Valuable Brands,” Thomson Reuters Fundamentals

via FactSet Research System, accessed April 27, 2014, http://www.forbes.com/

powerful-brands/list/#page:1_sort:6_direction:asc_search: releases/worldwide-

luxury-goods-continues-double-digit-annual-growth.aspx

Dabbs, James M. and Marian F. Hargrove (1997), “Age, Testosterone, and Behavior

Among Female Prison Inmates,” Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 477-80.

Dabbs, James M. and Robin Morris (1990), “Testosterone, Social Class, and Antisocial

Behavior in a Sample of 4,462 Men,” Psychological Science, 209-11.

de Graaf, John, David Wann, and Thomas H. Naylor (2001). Affluenza: The All-

Consuming Epidemic. San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler.

de Ridder, Denise T. D., Gerty Lensvelt-Mulders, Catrin Finkenauer, F. Marijn Stok, and

Roy F. Baumeister (2012), “Taking Stock of Self-Control: A Meta-Analysis of

How Trait Self-Control Relates to a Wide Range of Behaviors,” Personality and

Social Psychology Review, 16(1), 76-99.

135
DeWall, C. Nathan, Roy F. Baumeister, Nicole L. Mead and Kathleen D. Vohs (2011),

“How Leaders Self-Regulate Their Task Performances: Evidence That Power

Promotes Diligence, Depletion, and Disdain,” Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 100 (1), 47-65.

DeWall, C. Nathan, Roy F. Baumeister, and Kathleen D. Vohs (2008), “Satiated With

Belongingness? Effects of Acceptance, Rejection, and Task Framing on Self-

Regulatory Performance,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95 (6),

1367-82.

Dreze, Xavier and Joseph C. Nunes (2009), “Feeling Superior: The Impact of Loyalty

Program Structure on Consumers’ Perceptions of Status,” Journal of Consumer

Research, 35 (April), 890- 905.

Dubois, David and Nailya Ordabayeva (in press), “Social Hierarchy, Social Status, and

Status Consumption,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Consumer Psychology, eds.

Michael Norton, Derek Rucker, and Cait Lamberton.

Dunbar, Robin I.M. (1998), “The Social Brain Hypothesis,” Evolutionary Anthropology,

6, 178-90.

Eastman, Jacqueline K., Ronald E. Goldsmith, and Leisa Reinecke Flynn (1999), “Status

Consumption in Consumer Behavior: Scale Development and Validation,”

Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 7(Summer), 41-52.

Euromonitor (2013), “Global Luxury Goods Sales Exceed US$318 Billion,” accessed

January 14, 2014, www.marketwired.com/press-release/global-luxury-goods-

sales-exceed-us318-billion-1838681.htm.

136
Fenigstein, Allan (1979), “Self-Consciousness, Self-Attention, and Social Interaction,”

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 75-86.

_________ (1984), “Self-Consciousness and the Overperception of Self as a Target,” Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 860-70.

Fenigstein, Allan, Michael F. Scheier, and Arnold H. Buss (1975), “Public and Private

Self-Consciousness: Assessment and Theory,” Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 43 (4), 522-27.

French, John R. P. Jr. and Bertram Raven (1959), The Bases of Social Power, Ann Arbor,

MI: University of Michigan Press.

Froming, William J. and Charles S. Carver (1981), “Divergent Influence of Private and

Public Self-Consciousness in a Compliance Paradigm,” Journal of Research in

Personality, 15, 159-71.

Fitzsimons, Gráinne M, Tanya L. Chartrand, and Gavan Fitzsimons (2008), “Automatic

Effects of Brand Exposure on Motivated Behavior: How Apple Makes You

“Think Different”,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (June), 21-35.

Fuchs, Christoph, Emanuela Prandelli, Martin Shreier, and Darren W. Dahl (2013), “All

That Is Users Might Not Be Gold: How Labeling Products as User Designed

Backfires in the Context of Luxury Fashion Brands,” Journal of Marketing, 77

(September), 75-91.

Galinsky, Adam D., Deborah H Gruenfeld, and Joe C. Megee (2003), “From Power to

Action,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85 (3), 453- 466.

Galinksy, Adam D., Joe C. Magee, M. Ena Inesi, and Deborah H. Gruenfeld (2006),

“Power and Perspective Not Taken,” Psychological Science, 17 (2), 1068-74.

137
Gilovich, Thomas, Victoria Husted Medvec, and Kenneth Savitsky (2000), “The

Spotlight Effect in Social Judgment: An Egocentric Bias in Estimates of the

Salience of One’s Own Actions and Appearance,” Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 78 (2), 211-22.

Gilovich, Thomas and Kenneth Savitsky (1999), “The Spotlight Effect and the Illusion of

Transparency: Egocentric Assessments of How We Are Seen by Others,” Current

Directions in Psychological Science, 8 (6), 165-68.

Griskevicius, Vladas and Douglas T. Kenrick (2013), “Fundamental Motives for Why

We Buy: How Evolutionary Needs Influence Consumer Behavior,” Journal of

Consumer Psychology, 23(3), 372-386.

Griskevicius, Vladas, Joshua M. Tybur, Jill M. Sundie, Robert B. Cialdini, Geoffrey F.

Miller, and Douglas T. Kenrick (2007), “Blatant Benevolence and Conspicuous

Consumption: When Romantic Motives Elicit Strategic Costly Signals,” Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 93 (1), 85-102.

Griskevicius, Vladas, Joshua M. Tybur, and Bram Van den Bergh (2010) “Going Green

to Be Seen: Status, Reputation, and Conspicuous Conservation,” Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 392-404.

Gurven, Michael, Wesley Allen-Arave, Kim Hill, and Magdalena Hurtado (2000), ““It's a

Wonderful Life”: Signaling Generosity among the Ache of Paraguay." Evolution

and Human Behavior 21(4), 263-82.

Grinote, Ana (2007), “Power and Goal Pursuit,” Personality and Social Psychology

Bulletin, 33 (8), 1076-87.

138
Han, Young Jee, Joseph C. Nunes, and Xavier Drèze (2010), “Signaling Status with

Luxury Goods: The Role of Brand Prominence,” Journal of Marketing, 74 (July),

15-30.

Hardy, Charlie L. and Mark Van Vugt (2006), “Nice Guys Finish First: The Competitive

Altruism Hypothesis,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1402-13.

Hayes, Andrew F. (2012), “PROCESS: A Versatile Computational Tool for Observed

Variable Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Modeling [White

paper],” Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/

Hays, Nicholas A. (2013), “Fear and Loving in Social Hierarchy: Sex Differences in

Preferences for Power versus Status,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,

49(6), 1130-1136.

Heatherton, Todd F. (2011), “Neuroscience of Self and Self-Regulation,” The Annual

Review of Psychology, 62, 363-90.

Hofmann, Wilhelm, Roy F. Baumeister, Georg Forster, and Kathleen D. Vohs (2012),

“Everyday Temptations: An Experience Sampling Study of Desire, Conflict, and

Self-Control,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102 (6), 1318-35.

Interbrand (2013), “Best Global Brands 2013,” accessed January 14, 2014,

www.interbrand.com/en/best-global-brands/2013/Best-Global-Brands-2013.aspx.

Iredale, Wendy, Mark Van Vugt, and Robin Dunbar (2008), “Showing Off in Humans:

Male Generosity as a Mating Signal,” Evolutionary Psychology, 6(3), 386-92.

Irmak, Caglar, Lauren G. Block, and Gavan J. Fitzsimons (2005), “The Placebo Effect in

Marketing: Sometimes You Just Have to Want It to Work,” Journal of Marketing

Research, 42 (November), 406-09.

139
Jones, Liz (2008), “The Luxury Brand with a Chequered Past, Burberry’s Shaken off Its

Chav Image to Become the Fashionistas’ Favourite Once More,” Mail Online,

accessed February 10, 2014, http://www.dailymail.co.uk/femail/article-

1023460/Burberrys-shaken-chav-image-fashionistas-favourite-more.html.

Jones, Lyle V. (1952), “Tests of Hypotheses: One-Sided vs. Two-Sided Alternatives,”

Psychological Bulletin, 49 (1), 43-6.

Kasser, Tim (2002). The High Price of Materialism. Mass.: MIT Press.

Kasser, Tim and Richard M. Ryan (2001), “Be Careful What You Wish For: Optimal

Functioning and the Relative Attainment of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Goals,” in Life

Goals and Wellbeing: Towards a Positive Psychology of Human Striving, eds.

Peter Schmuck and Kennon M. Sheldon, Goetingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber,

116-31.

Kenrick, Douglas T., Vladas Griskevicius, Steven L. Neuberg, and Mark Schaller (2010),

“Renovating the Pyramid of Needs: Contemporary Extensions Built Upon

Ancient Foundations,” Perspective on Psychological Science, 5(3),292-314.

Krendl, Anne C. and Todd F. Heatherton (2009), “Self Versus Others/Self-Regulation,”

in Handbook of Neuroscience for the Behavioral Sciences, eds. Gary G. Berntson

and John T. Cacioppo, Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, 859-78.

Kuksov, Dmitri and Ying Xie (2012), “Competition in a Status Goods Market,” Journal

of Marketing Research, 49(October), 609-23.

Leary, Mark R. (1999), “Making Sense of Self-Esteem,” Current Directions in

Psychological Science, 8 (1), 32-35.

140
Lee, Jaehoon and L. J. Shrum (2012), “Conspicuous Consumption versus Charitable

Behavior in Response to Social Exclusion: A Differential Needs Explanation,”

Journal of Consumer Research 39 (October), 530-44.

Lindberg, Nina, Pekka Tani, Bjorn Appelberg, Hannu Naukkarinen, Ranan Rimon, Tarja

Porkka-Heiskanen, and Matti Vrikkunen (2003), “Human Impulsive Aggression:

A Sleep Research Perspective,” Journal of Psychiatric Research, 37, 313-24.

Magee, Joe C. and Adam D. Galinsky (2008), “Social Hierarchy: The Self-Reinforcing

Nature of Power and Status,” The Academy of Management Annals, 2(1), 351-98.

Maner, Jon K., Matthew T. Gailliot, David A. Butz, and B. Michelle Peruche (2007),

“Power, Risk, and the Status Quo: Does Power Promote Riskier or More

Conservative Decision Making?” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,

33(4), 451-62.

Maner, Jon K. and Nicole L. Mead (2010), “The Essential Tension Between Leadership

and Power: When Leaders Sacrifice Group Goals for the Sake of Self-Interest,”

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99 (3), 482-97.

Mazzocco, Philip. J., Derek D. Rucker, Adam D. Galinsky, and Eric T. Anderson (2012),

“Direct and Vicarious Conspicuous Consumption: Identification with Low-Status

Groups Increase the Desire for High-Status Goods,” Journal of Consumer

Psychology, 22, 520-28.

Melnyk, Valentyna, and Stijn MJ van Osselaer (2012), “Make Me Special: Gender

Differences in Consumers’ Responses to Loyalty Programs,” 23(3), 545-559.

141
Melnyk, Valentyna, Stijn MJ van Osselaer and Tammo HA Bijmolt (2009), “Are Women

More Loyal Customers Than Men? Gender Differences in Loyalty to Firms and

Individual Service Providers,” Journal of Marketing, 73(4), 82-96.

Mick, David G. (1996), “Are Studies of Dark Side Variable Confounded by Socially

Desirable Responding? The Case of Materialism,” Journal of Consumer Research,

23 (2), 106-119.

Miller, Geoffrey F. (2007), “Sexual Selection for Moral Virtues,” Quarterly Review of

Biology, 82, 97-125.

Murdock, George Peter (1970), “Rand and Potlatch among the Haida,” Yale University

Publications in Anthropology, 13, New Haven, CT: Human Relations Area Files

Press.

Muraven, Mark, Dianne M. Tice, and Roy F. Baumeister (1998), “Self-Control as a

Limited Resource: Regulatory Depletion Patterns,” Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 74 (3), 774-89.

Nelissen, Rob M.A. and Marijn H.C. Meijers (2012), “Social Benefits of Luxury Brands

as Costly Signals of Wealth and Status,” Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(5),

343-55.

Ordabayeva, Nailya and Pierre Chandon (2011), “Getting ahead of Joneses: When

Equality Increases Conspicuous Consumption Among Bottom-Tier Consumers,”

Journal of Consumer Research, 38 (June), 27-41.

Park, Ji Kyung and Deborah Roedder John (2014), “I Think I Can, I Think I Can: Brand

Use, Self-Efficacy, and Performance,” Journal of Marketing Research, 51(April),

233-47.

142
Paton, Elizabeth (2013), “Prada Sales Climb 29% in a Year,” Financial Times, accessed

January 14, 2014, www.ft.com/cms/s/0/7b6bf2d0-7aa6-11e2-9cc2-

00144feabdc0.html.

Patrick Vanessa M. and Henrik Hagtvedt (2009), “Luxury Branding,” The Handbook of

Brand Relationships. NY: Society for Consumer Psychology, 267-80.

Phau, Ian and Gerard Prendergast (2000), “Consuming Luxury Brands: The Relevance of

the ‘Rarity Principle’,” Journal of Brand Management, 7 (5), 366-75.

Phillips, Jordan (2012), “The Lure of Luxe: Climbing The Luxury Consumption

Pyramid,” North Charleston, SC: CreateSpace Independent Publishing Platform.

Piff, Paul K. (2014), “Wealth and the Inflated Self Class, Entitlement, and Narcissism,”

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(1), 34-43.

Piff, Paul K., Michael W. Kraus, Stéphane Côté, Bonnie Hayden Cheng, and Dacher

Keltner (2010) “Having Less, Giving More: The Influence of Social Class on

Prosocial Behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 771-84.

Piff, Paul K, Daniel M. Stancato, Stephane Cote, Rodolfo Mendoze-Denton, and Dacher

Keltner (2012), “Higher Status Class Predicts Increased Unethical Behavior,”

Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 109(11), 4086-91.

Plourde, Aimee M. (2008), “The Origins of Prestige Goods as Honest Signals of Skill

and Knowledge,” Human Nature, 19 (4), 374-88.

Preacher, Kristopher J. and Andrew Hayes (2008), “Asymptotic and Resampling

Strategies for Assessing and Comparing Indirect Effects in Multiple Mediation

Model,” Behavior Research Method, 40 (3), 879-91.

143
Raichle, Katherine A., Alan J. Christensen, Shawna Ehlers, Patricia J. Moran, Lucy

Karnell, and Gerry Funk (2001), “Public and Private Self-Consciousness and

Smoking Behavior in Head and Neck Cancer Patients,” Annals of Behavioral

Medicine, 23 (2), 120-24.

Redden, Joseph P. and Kelly L. Haws (2013), “Healthy Satiation: The Role of

Decreasing Desire in Effective Self-Control,” Journal of Consumer Research, 39

(February), 1100-14.

Richins, Marsha L. (1987), “Media, Materialism, and Human Happiness,” Advances in

Consumer Research, 14 (1), 352-56.

———--- (1994), “Special Possessions and the Expression of Material Values,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 21 (December), 522-33.

Richins, Marsha L. and Scott Dawson (1992), “A Consumer Values Orientation for

Materialism and its Measurement: Scale Development and Validation,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 19 (December), 303-16.

Ridgeway, Cecilia L. and Henry A. Walker (1995), “Status Structure,” in Karen S. Cook,

Gary Alan Fine, and James S. House, eds. Sociological Perspectives on Social

Psychology. Needham Heights: Allyn and Bacon.

Roberts, Andrew (2013), “LVMH to Adjust Vuitton Strategy amid Stable Leather Sales

Growth,” Bloomberg News, accessed January 14, 2014,

www.bloomberg.com/news/2013-01-31/lvmh-full-year-profit-increases-13-on-

gains-in-u-s-and-asia.html.

144
Rucker, Derek D., David Dubois, and Adam D. Galinsky (2011), “Generous Paupers and

Stingy Princes: Power Drives Consumer Spending on Self versus Others,”

Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (April), 1015-29.

Rucker, Derek D. and Adam D. Galinsky (2009), “Conspicuous Consumption Versus

Utilitarian Ideals: How Different Levels of Power Shape Consumer Behavior,”

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(3), 549-55.

Rucker, Derek D. and Adam D. Galinsky (2008), “Desire to Acquire: Powerlessness and

Compensatory Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35(2), 257-67.

———--- (2009), “Conspicuous Consumption versus Utilitarian Ideals: How Different

Level of Power Shape Consumer Behavior,” Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 45(3), 549-55.

Rucker, Derek D., Adam D. Galinsky, and David Dubois (2012), “Power and Consumer

Behavior: How Power Shapes Who and What Consumers Value,” Journal of

Consumer Psychology, 22, 352-68.

Saad, Gad and John G. Vongas (2009), “The Effect of Conspicuous Consumption on

Men’s Testosterone Levels,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 110, 80-92.

Scheier, Michael F. (1980), “Effects of Public and Private Self-Consciousness on the

Public Expression of Personal Beliefs,” Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 39 (3), 514-21.

Schor, Juliet B. (2004), Born to Buy, New York: Scribner.

145
Semmann, Dirk, Hans-Jürgen Krambeck, and Manfred Milinski (2005), “Reputation is

Valuable Within and Outside One’s Own Social Group,” Behavioral Ecology and

Sociobiology, 57(6), 611-16.

Sivanathan, Niro and Nathan C. Pettit (2010), “Protecting the Self through Consumption:

Status Goods as Affirmational Commodities,” Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 46, 564-70.

Smith, Eric Alden and Rebecca Bird (2000), “Turtle Hunting and Tombstone Opening:

Public Generosity as Costly Signaling,” Evolution and Human Behavior, 21(4),

245-61.

Solomon, Michael R. (1983), “The Role of Products as Social Stimuli: A Symbolic

Interactionism Perspective,” Journal of Consumer Research, 10 (December), 319-

29.

Sparshott, Jeffrey (2014), “What Products Drove Consumer Spending? Luxury Items,

Mostly,” The Wall Street Journal, accessed February 11, 2014,

http://blogs.wsj.com/economics/2014/01/22/what-products-drove-consumer-

spending-luxury-items-mostly/

Statista (2014), “Value of the Personal Luxury Goods Market Worldwide From 1995 to

2013,” The Statistics Portal, accessed September 2014, http://www.statista.com/

statistics/266503/value-of-the-personal-luxury-goods-market-worldwide/

Stellar, Jennifer E., Vida M. Manzo, Michael W. Kraus, and Dacher Keltner (2012),

“Class and Compassion: Socioeconomic Factors Predict Responses to Suffering,”

Emotion,12 (3), 449-59.

146
Stiff, Chris and Mark Van Vugt (2008), “The Power of Reputations: The Role of Third

Party Information in the Admission of New Group Members,” Group Dynamics:

Theory, Research, and Practice 12, 155-66.

Sundie Jill M., Douglas T. Kenrick, Vladas Grikevicius, Joshua M. Tybur, Kathleen D.

Vohs, and Daniel J. Beal (2011), “Peacocks, Porsches, and Thorstein Veblen:

Conspicuous Consumption as a Sexual Signaling System,” Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 100 (4), 664-80.

Tangney, June P., Roy F. Baumeister, and Angie Luzio Boone, “High Self‐Control

Predicts Good Adjustment, Less Pathology, Better Grades, and Interpersonal

Success,” Journal of Personality, 72 (2), 271-324.

Tice, Dianne M., Ellen Bratslavsky, and Roy F. Baumeister, (2001), “Emotional Distress

Regulation Takes Precedence Over Impulse Control: If You Feel Bad, Do

It!” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 53-67.

Van Vugt, Mark and Charlotte L. Hardy (2009), “Cooperation for Reputation: Wasteful

Contributions as Costly Signals in Public Goods,” Group Processes Intergroup

Relations, 13(1), 101-11.

Veblen, Thorstein (1899), The Theory of the Leisure Class, New York: Penguin.

Vigneron, Franck and Lester W. Johnson (2004), “Measuring Perceptions of Brand

Luxury,” Brand Management, 11 (July), 484-506.

Vohs, Kathleen D. and Todd F. Heatherton (2000), “Self-Regulatory Failure: A

Resource-Depletion Approach,” Psychological Science, 11 (3), 249-54.

Vohs, Kathleen D. and Roy F. Baumeister (2004), “Understanding Self-Regulation: An

Introduction,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and

147
Applications, eds. Roy F. Baumeister and Kathleen D. Vohs, New York: Guilford,

1-9.

Vohs, Kathleen D., Roy F. Baumeister, and Natalie J. Ciarocco (2005), “Self-Regulation

and Self-Presentation: Regulatory Resource Depletion Impairs Impression

Management and Effortful Self-Presentation Depletes Regulatory Resources,”

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88 (4), 632-57.

Vohs, Kathleen D. and Todd F. Heatherton (2000), “Self-Regulatory Failure: A

Resource-Depletion Approach,” Psychological Science, 11 (3), 249-54.

Wachtel, Paul L. and Sidney J. Blatt (1990), “Perceptions of Economic Needs and of

Anticipated Future Income,” Journal of Economic Psychology, 11(3), 403-15.

Wang, Yajin and Vladas Griskevicius (2014), “Conspicuous Consumption, Relationships,

and Rivals: Women’s Luxury Products as Signals to Other Women,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 40 (February), 834-54.

Watson, David, Lee A. Clark, and Auke Tellegen (1988), “Development and Validation

of Brief Measures of Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales,” Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 (6), 1063-70.

Wedekind, Claus and Victoria A. Braithwaite (2002), “The Long-Term Benefits of

Human Generosity in Indirect Reciprocity,” Current Biology, 12(12), 1012-15.

Wernerfelt, Birger (1990), “Advertising Content When Brand Choice is a Signal,” The

Journal of Business, 63 (1), 91-8.

Wilcox, Keith, Hyeong Min Kim, and Sankar Sen (2009), “Why Do Consumers Buy

Counterfeit Luxury Brands?” Journal of Marketing Research, 46 (April), 247-59.

148
Wong, Jimmy and Sharon Shavitt (2010), “Be Rude to Me and I Will Buy a Rolex:

Effects of Culture Orientation on Responses to Power Threat in a Service

Setting,” Advances in Consumer Psychology, 2, 95.

Zak, Paul J., Robert Kruzban, Sheila Ahmadi, Ronald S. Swerdloff, Jang Park, Levan

Efremidze, Karen Redwine, Karla Morgan, and William Matzner (2009),

“Testosterone Administration Decreases Generosity in the Ultimatum Game,”

PLoS One, 4 (12), e8330.

Zitek, Emily M., Alexander H. Jordan, Benoit Monin, and Frederick R. Leach (2010),

“Victim Entitlement to Behave Selfish,” Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 98(2), 245-55.

149

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy