SpecialEd Ch12
SpecialEd Ch12
SpecialEd Ch12
Chapter 12
Introduction
This report is about learning disabilities (LD), the most frequently identified class of disabilities
among students in public schools in the United States. Despite its apparently high—and rising—
incidence, LD remains one of the least understood and most debated disabling conditions that
affect school-aged children (and adults). Indeed, many
disagree about the definition and classification of LD; the
Given what is now diagnostic criteria and assessment practices used in the
known about LD, it is identification process; the content, intensity, and duration of
irresponsible to instructional practices employed; and the policies and legal
requirements that drive the identification and education of
continue current those with LD.1
policies that dictate
inadequate We take the position that many of these debates can be
informed by converging scientific data. On the basis of this
identification practices. evidence, we contend that many of the persistent difficulties
in developing valid classifications and operational
definitions of LD are due to reliance on inaccurate
assumptions about causes and characteristics of the disorders. Furthermore, we argue that
sufficient data exist to guide the development and implementation of early identification and
prevention programs for children at-risk for LD, particularly reading programs that can benefit
many of these youngsters.
We contend that sound prevention programs can significantly reduce the number of older
children who are identified as LD and who typically require intensive, long-term special
education programs. Moreover, prevention programs will prove more effective than remedial
programs. Finally, we contend that, given what is now known about LD, it is irresponsible to
continue current policies that dictate inadequate identification practices. Instead, we must
develop evidence-based alternatives, specific strategies to implement these alternatives, and a
research and policy agenda to ensure that these changes are phased into practice as quickly as
possible.
We provide a description of the specific instructional needs of children whose low academic
achievement can be strengthened by informed teaching.
The chapter is organized into three sections. In the first, we present the current federal definition
of LD and trace the theoretical, clinical, and political
bases for its construction.2
We estimate that the number
In the second section, we summarize a body of of children who are typically
converging research on reading development,
identified as poor readers
reading disabilities (RD), and reading instruction that
underscores the importance of early identification and and served through either
prevention intervention programs to reduce reading special education or
failure among many children at-risk for limited literacy compensatory education
development. Although RD represents only two (LD in
basic reading skills; LD in reading comprehension) of
programs could be reduced
the seven types of LD that can be identified according by up to 70 percent through
to federal law, our focus on RD is predicated on three early identification and
facts. First, approximately 80 percent of children with
prevention programs.
LD have primary difficulties with reading.3 Second,
learning to read is essential for academic
achievement and accomplishment in all subjects. Third, more is known about deficiencies in
reading than about any other academic domain affected by LD, and much of what is known can
effectively impact policy and instruction.4
We have chosen to combine the following within the RD designation: (1) those children who
meet criteria for LD and typically receive services through special education; and (2) those who
read below the 25th percentile but do not qualify for the diagnosis of LD and often receive
services through compensatory education. Our decision to combine the two groups is predicated
on data indicating little difference between them in the proximal causes of their reading
difficulties. We estimate that the number of children who are typically identified as poor readers
and served through either special education or compensatory education programs (as well as
children with significant reading difficulties who are not formally identified and served) could be
reduced by up to 70 percent through early identification and prevention programs.
In the third section, we examine a number of issues that should be considered when addressing
the educational needs of children at risk for learning failure and children identified as LD at later
ages. Under current policies and practices, the number of older children identified as LD
continues to increase without concomitant improvements in their learning abilities. We explain
why this is the case and provide alternatives for meeting the educational needs of these students.
Finally, we are mindful of the complexity of translating research findings into policy and
practice. Policy can have unintended outcomes. Evidence-based alternatives can have few
benefits or even harmful effects if implementation strategies are not informed by a clear
understanding of specific needs for capacity building at the teacher, school, and system levels.
In response, we outline a series of short- and long- range initiatives designed to optimize
instruction for all students.
The concept of unexpected underachievement has been reported in medical and psychological
literature since the mid-19th century under the rubrics of dyslexia, word blindness, dysgraphia,
dyscalculia, and other terms.6 However, it has only been since 1962, when Samuel Kirk, a
psychologist at the University of Illinois, coined the term learning disabilities, that the concept of
unexpected underachievement attained formal recognition in the education community. Kirk used
the term to refer to a variety of syndromes affecting language, learning, and communication;
like his more medically oriented predecessors, he felt that LD
reflected unanticipated learning problems in a seemingly
The term learning capable child. Writing in 1962, Kirk defined LD as “a
disability gained retardation, disorder, or delayed development in one or
more of the processes of speech, language, reading,
rapid acceptance in spelling, writing, or arithmetic resulting from a possible
the 1960s and 1970s cerebral dysfunction and not from mental retardation,
because it addressed sensory deprivation, or cultural or instructional factors.”7
Speaking at a 1963 conference, Kirk further noted that LD
a critical need of
represented a discrepancy between a child’s achievement
concerned parents and his or her apparent capacity to learn. As in the current
and professionals. federal definition, Kirk recognized that LD represented an
amalgam of disabilities, all grouped under a single label. He
did not feel that the term was synonymous with RD.8
However, RD was the most frequently identified type of LD in Kirk’s day, as today.
The term learning disability gained rapid acceptance in the 1960s and 1970s because it
addressed a critical need of concerned parents and professionals. The concepts represented by
LD also made educational sense.9 Previously, children whose failure to learn could not be
explained by mental retardation, visual impairments, hearing impairments, or emotional
disturbance were disenfranchised from special education. Their learning characteristics simply
did not correspond to existing categories of special education. Thus, the needs of these children
were not being met by the educational system; it was through parental and professional
advocacy efforts that special education services were ultimately made available for them through
the 1969 Learning Disabilities Act.10 The same legislative language later appeared in the
Education for All Handicapped Children Act of 1975 (EAHCA), now the Individuals with
Disabilities Education Act (IDEA).11
The concept of learning disabilities and the need for different specialized educational services
also made intuitive sense to parents, teachers, and policymakers. The term did not stigmatize
children. Specifically, the learning difficulties displayed by youngsters with LD were not due to
mental retardation, poor parenting, or psychopathology. The term likewise reflected optimism.
Students with LD had not yet reached their potential: Their difficulties in learning to read, write,
and/or calculate occurred despite adequate intelligence, sensory integrity, healthy emotional
development, and cultural and environmental advantage. Education programs were needed that
recognized differences among children with LD, those who learned “normally,” and those who
manifested physical, sensory, and intellectual handicaps that affected academic achievement.
Few would disagree that 5 percent or more of our school-age population experience difficulties
with language and other skills that would be disruptive to academic achievement, or that the
factors that led to the concept of LD have lost their salience. The concept of LD is valid, and
there are many children and adults whose difficulties in learning are indeed the result of genuine
learning disabilities. The issues we raise involve whether classifications used for LD identify all
children who would benefit from special education services and/or specialized instruction.
Similarly, we ask whether younger children who have severe (difficult to remediate) forms of LD
are being adequately served given identification rates that point toward disproportionate
representation of older children within this category.
What underlies this disproportionate increase in the prevalence of children with LD, particularly
in the 12-17 age range? Is it because of improvements in diagnostic and identification practices,
or are other factors at work? Is the definition of LD that guides assessment and diagnostic
practices too general and ambiguous to ensure accurate identification of younger students? Are
the constructs and principles inherent in the definition of LD even valid? Are diagnostic practices
biased against the identification of younger, poor, or ethnically different children with LD? Are
some students identified as LD actually underachieving in school because of poor teaching and
inadequate services? Or has the education profession failed to tolerate individual differences in
learning and to properly train regular teachers and special educators to address these
differences? Is teacher preparation an issue in the emergence of a child as LD?
Such questions must honestly be answered for the sake of our nation’s children. We believe
some of the answers can be found through close examination of the features that comprise the
current definition(s) of LD, as well as those that preceded it. We also propose that the
(a) A team may determine that a child has a specific learning disability if:
(1) The child does not achieve commensurate with his or her age and ability
levels in one or more of the areas listed in paragraph (a) (2) of this section,
when provided with learning experiences appropriate for the child’s age and
ability levels; and
(2) The team finds that a child has a severe discrepancy between achievement
and intellectual ability in one or more of the following areas: (i) Oral expression;
(ii) Listening comprehension; (iii) Written expression; (iv) Basic reading skill; (v)
Reading comprehension; (vi) Mathematics calculation; or (vii) Mathematics
reasoning.
Sources: Assistance to States for the Education of Children with Disabilities Program and Preschool Grants for
Children with Disabilities Final Rule, 34 C.F.R. pts. 300, 301 (1992); see also note 2.
disproportionate increase in the numbers of older children identified as LD during the late
elementary to middle school years is, in part, attributable to the following: (1) the limited
effectiveness of remediation after age nine; (2) measurement practices that are biased against
the identification of children before age nine; and (3) socio-educational factors operating within
the public school enterprise. Within this context, we have organized the rest of this section to
address the scientific integrity of major themes that guide identification and instructional
practices in the field of LD. We conclude it with an examination of the function of the current LD
category within the larger educational enterprise and the effects of this function on education
policies and practices, particularly those involving the definition of LD. It is important to point out
that many of the concerns we address in this chapter (for example, the over use and over
interpretation of discrepancy data, as well as the misinterpretation of disclaimers in the exclusion
language of the IDEA) frequently reflect misinterpretation of the actual regulatory language in the
IDEA by schools determining eligibility for special education. Nevertheless, these concerns are
frequently cited and are predicated on less than optimal translation of the federal law into
identification practices at the school level.
Neurobiological factors have been most closely studied in the area of reading. A considerable
body of evidence indicates that poor readers exhibit disruption primarily, but not exclusively, in
the neural circuitry of the left hemisphere serving language. Both a range of neurobiological
investigations using postmortem brain specimens and, more recently, quantitative assessment of
brain anatomy using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) suggest that there are subtle structural
differences in several brain regions between RD and nonimpaired readers. Converging evidence
from neuroimaging modalities that measure brain function (for instance, a functional MRI)
Of particular interest from the studies of brain function is the possibility that the resultant neural
circuitry reflects not only the individual’s biological makeup, but also environmental influences.
Central among these influences is how reading instruction impacts brain circuitry. The findings
suggest that neural systems develop and are deployed for specific cognitive functions through
the interaction of the brain and the environment (including instruction).
This “interaction” perspective is supported by genetic studies of individuals with RD. It has long
been known that reading problems recur across family generations, with a risk in the offspring
of a parent with RD eight times higher than the general population. Studies with identical and
fraternal twins have shown that a significant portion of this familial risk is due to genetic factors.
Yet such factors account for only about half of the variability in reading skill development;
environmental factors account for the other half and therefore
have a significant influence on reading outcomes. Thus, what
may be inherited is a susceptibility for RD that may manifest itself
No definitional given specific interactions, or lack thereof, with the environment.
element of LD has For example, parents who read poorly may be less likely to read
generated as to their children. The quality of reading instruction provided in the
school may be most critical for children when there is a both a
much controversy genetic risk for poor reading and a family situation giving rise to
as the use of IQ- limited instructional interactions in the home.17
achievement
The Discrepancy Element
discrepancy in the No definitional element has generated as much controversy as
identification of the use of IQ-achievement discrepancy in the identification of
students with LD. students with LD.18 When resources (funding) are limited, a valid
classification must give rise to operational criteria that can guide
the reliable identification of individual cases. Indeed, the
adoption of the concept of an IQ-achievement discrepancy as only one, but clearly the primary,
operational criterion commenced in 1977, shortly after passage of the EAHCA, to “objectively
and accurately” distinguish the child with LD from children with other academic deficiencies.19
When the EAHCA was enacted in 1975, states reported that the definition of LD provided
insufficient criteria for identifying eligible children. In response, the Office of Education
developed more explicit criteria for eligibility and published guidelines for identification which
included a severe discrepancy between achievement and intellectual ability (see Table 1). These
criteria maintained the heterogeneity and exclusionary elements of the 1969 definition, but
added the IQ-discrepancy component as an additional criterion. Inherent in this criterion is an
implicit classification of low-achieving students into those who are LD (those with unexpected
underachievement) and those who simply underachieve (those with expected underachievement).
The idea of using an IQ-achievement discrepancy metric as one way to “objectively determine”
the presence or absence of LD was probably reasonable at the time. Long before “severe
discrepancy” became synonymous with LD, practitioners had been intrigued by the seemingly
paradoxical inability of some children of average and superior intelligence to master academic
concepts. Following repeated reports of this phenomenon in the literature, clinicians and
researchers saw value in distinguishing between a subset of low achievers who displayed
pervasive limitations in cognitive ability (for example, students with mild mental deficiency) and
a subgroup of children with academic deficits displayed against a background of normal
intelligence.20
There are many problems with the concept of an IQ-achievement discrepancy. It not only
embodies sometimes naive and erroneous assumptions about the adequacy of an IQ score as an
index of learning potential, but the actual comparison of academic achievement scores with IQ
scores to derive a discrepancy value is fraught with psychometric, statistical, and conceptual
problems that render many comparisons useless.22 Of even greater significance, the IQ-
achievement discrepancy, when employed “inappropriately” as the primary criterion for the
identification of LD, may well harm more children than it helps. Not only do discrepancy
formulas differ from state to state, making it possible for a student to lose special education
services following a family move, but also reliance on a measurable discrepancy between IQ
and achievement makes early identification of LD difficult. An overreliance on discrepancy
means that children must fail or fall below a predicted level of performance before they are
eligible for special education services. Because achievement failure sufficient to produce a
discrepancy from IQ cannot be reliably measured until a child reaches approximately nine years
of age, the use of IQ-discrepancy constitutes a “wait-to-fail” model.23 Thus, the student has
suffered the academic and emotional strains of failure for two to three years before potentially
effective instruction can be brought to bear. This order of events has devastating, lifelong
consequences. In the area of RD, epidemiological data show clearly that the majority of children
who are poor readers at age nine or older continue to have reading difficulties into adulthood.24
Another potentially serious flaw in the use of an IQ-achievement discrepancy metric concerns the
unsystematic and frequently inequitable provision of educational services and accommodations
based on the presence or absence of a discrepancy. Because there is no strong evidence that
In the area of RD, the issue is further complicated when some individuals score in the average
range on word reading tasks but exhibit significant difficulties when reading connected text.
Here, their reading comprehension is impaired primarily because they read slowly. These
individuals are disabled in reading and clearly require specialized instruction and
accommodations. They do not receive such instruction and accommodations because reading
fluency is rarely assessed in current identification procedures. If a slow-reading student scores
significantly above the average range on a measure of
intelligence, services may be afforded on the basis of the
discrepancy between the average untimed word reading score
No child is born a and the above-average IQ score. However, slow-reading
reader; all students who score within the average range on both the
children in literate untimed reading measures and the IQ test will typically be
denied services because there is no discrepancy—even though
societies have to they also have a disability that requires specialized services
be taught to read. and/or accommodations. The bottom line is that the IQ-
achievement discrepancy formulation provides access to
services for only some individuals and sometimes denies
services without appropriately measuring the fundamental problem.
In sum, the use of an IQ-achievement discrepancy to identify children with LD appears to move
many students further away from the education they need. Because the discrepancy hinges on
the IQ level of students rather than on their specific academic needs, the emphasis is on
eligibility rather than instruction. This situation reflects the orientation of special education in
public schools toward compliance with federal regulations rather than positive educational
outcomes. Such an emphasis is unfortunate since we have little evidence that the special
education remediation services provided to children with LD help them catch up to their peers in
academic skills. This issue is addressed below.
invoked. Third, and most important, many of the conditions excluded as potential influences on
LD are themselves factors in impeding the development of cognitive and linguistic skills that lead
to the academic deficits frequently observed in RD children.
One exclusion criterion for LD that is especially difficult to reconcile is the student’s instructional
history. All definitions of LD exclude children from
consideration if their learning problems are primarily a
product of inadequate instruction. Of all the different From its inception as a
assumptions in the concept of LD, this one is the least category, LD has served
examined yet perhaps the most important. Some would
interpret this exclusion feature to indicate that children
as a sociological sponge
who profit from instruction do not have a biologically that attempts to wipe up
based LD, yet functional imaging studies suggest that in general education’s spills
the area of reading this is not so. Instruction may be
and cleanse its ills.
necessary to establish the neural networks that support
reading. No child is born a reader; all children in literate
societies have to be taught to read. The ability to read and write is explicitly built upon our
natural capacities for developing oral language.28
Similarly, most definitions exclude children from the LD category whose learning difficulties may
be primarily related to environmental, cultural, or economic disadvantage. Yet these very
conditions place some children at significant risk for weaker neural development and secondary
learning difficulties. Given the emphasis within current definitions of LD on the causal role of the
central nervous system in academic skills disorders, it seems unwise to reject the possibility that
the environment (including social and cultural factors) can affect brain development and function,
and thus affect learning. Poor socio-economic conditions are related to a number of factors—
including malnutrition, limited pre- and post-natal care, exposure to teratogens and substance
abuse—all of which can place children at risk for neurological dysfunction, leading to cognitive,
linguistic, and academic deficits.29
In sum, the brain and the environment operate in reciprocal fashion, pushing or limiting
development according to the frequency, timing, and quality of the interactions. To exclude
children from specialized services because of instructional, environmental, social, and cultural
factors ignores the importance of these factors in shaping the central nervous system and the
child’s cognitive and linguistic repertoire. Many lives can be improved significantly by
identifying those children most susceptible to possible cognitive and academic difficulties. These
children need the best instruction at the earliest possible time. To do this will require
reconsideration of current definitions of LD. Unfortunately, this is easier said than done because
of the sociological role that LD has come to play within the larger educational enterprise.
served as a sociological sponge that attempts to wipe up general education’s spills and cleanse
its ills. Today’s classrooms are heterogeneous and teachers are expected to address a wide
range of individual differences in cognitive, academic, and behavioral development.
Unfortunately, many regular classroom teachers have not been trained to accommodate different
students’ learning needs, and they understandably seek assistance that typically takes the
responsibility of educating the child away from the classroom teacher.31
There is no doubt that, because of limitations in training, many general education and special
education teachers are not prepared to address and respond to these individual differences in
an informed manner. For example, a large number of teachers report that their training
programs did not adequately prepare them to impart effective reading instruction, particularly to
children with limited oral language and literacy experiences or to children with the most severe
forms of reading disabilities.32 This is a significant concern, given that many children at risk for
reading failure come from disadvantaged backgrounds, where early childhood education and
preschool experiences are less available. Many of these children fail to read because they did
not receive effective instruction in the early grades. Some may then, in later grades, require
special education services to make up for this early failure in reading instruction.
Senf’s metaphor is particularly apt as one observes the “sponge” expand or contract when
standards for academic accountability stiffen, demographics of school communities change,
administrative concerns increase because LD students are being
over- or under-identified, or parental pressures are brought to
Children who get off bear on behalf of their struggling children. In general, the LD
to a poor start in sponge has expanded since the advent of the EAHCA because it
reading rarely catch has been able to absorb a diversity of educational, behavioral,
and socioemotional problems irrespective of their causes, their
up. We wait—they responses to good teaching, or their prognosis.
fail. But it does not
The effects of these practices on our scientific understanding of
have to be this way.
LD have been devastating and insidious. It is important to
understand that, for the most part, knowledge about LD has been
obtained by studying heterogeneous samples of children identified by their schools as LD without
attention to how or why the diagnosis was applied. The differences observed within and across
samples have been so extensive that the research data are often difficult to interpret.
We contend, therefore, that it is not in the best interest of children to continue to use present
policies and practices as the primary means to provide appropriate instruction to children with
LD, particularly students with reading difficulties. A strong statement? Yes, but it is one that is
based on research indicating that, without early intervention, the poor first-grade reader almost
invariably becomes a poor middle school reader, high school reader, and adult reader. In short,
children who get off to a poor start in reading rarely catch up. We wait—they fail. But it does
not have to be this way. It is a tragedy that both general and special education practices and
policies continue unchanged even as extensive converging evidence makes clear that one major
solution to the problem of school failure in general, and reading failure in particular, is early
identification and prevention.
As mentioned before, children identified as RD after Grade 2 rarely catch up to their peers.
Thus, the long-term development of reading skills appears to be set early and is difficult to alter.
Figure 1 shows the development of reading skills of children in the Connecticut Longitudinal
Study, which followed them from kindergarten through Grade 12.34
Three groups are depicted, including children who are not reading impaired (NRI) as well as
groups defined in Grade 3 as RD using either IQ-discrepancy (RDD group) or low-achievement
(LA group) definitions. Thus, one group of children would qualify for special education as LD
under federal guidelines (RDD), while the other group (LA) would not qualify despite the fact that
Figure 1 shows the RDD and LA groups are comparably impaired in reading ability. The overall
pattern depicts large differences in the development of the NRI and two RD groups. However,
the two RD groups are almost indistinguishable and neither catches up to the NRI group despite
the fact that schools identified at least half the children as eligible for special education services.
Particularly sobering is the finding that over 70 percent of the group identified as RD in Grade 3
was still identified as RD in Grade 12. Regardless of how they are defined, reading disabilities
are often chronic, lifelong incapacities that lead to problems in a variety of social and
vocational areas in adolescence and adulthood.
These and other longitudinal studies indicate that early reading difficulties portend later reading
difficulties. Further, these studies tell us that children do not typically “catch up” on their own.
Unless addressed with well-designed instruction, struggling readers stay that way. Historically,
schools have opted to address these persistent reading difficulties through the provision of
remedial and special education services typically beginning in second grade and beyond. Yet
the majority of children provided such services fail to become skilled readers. We will now
examine specific attempts to improve reading skills, first through remedial efforts, then through
preventative efforts.
Remediation
The importance of early intervention is clearly apparent from studies of typical special education
remediation services for reading and math skill development. Perhaps most revealing is an
analysis of a large data set by Eric Hanushek and colleagues. They found that placement in
special education was associated with a gain of 0.04 standard deviations in reading and 0.11
in math. Unfortunately, these gains are so small that children are not closing the gap between
their academic performance and the performance of their higher achieving classmates. Thus,
many of these children remain for lengthy periods of time in special education programs that
were ostensibly meant to close the academic gaps.37
Remediation models for older children have been ineffective for several reasons, but two stand
out. First, the standard instruction provided through remediation is frequently too little, too
general, and too unsystematic. Second, even if the instruction were of high quality, it may be too
late given that many children are already far behind and less motivated to learn to read
following a year or more of reading failure.
Regarding the first reason, Sharon Vaughn and her colleagues studied children with RD who
were served for an entire year in public elementary school special education resource rooms.38
The researchers found that their instruction was characterized primarily by whole group reading
instruction to large groups of children (5-19) who also varied widely in grade level (3-5 grades).
Despite this variation, little individualized or differentiated instruction occurred. Although a
follow-up study two years later showed that more of the teachers were utilizing materials that
supported differentiated instruction, none of these studies found evidence that children made
significant gains in reading. Several earlier studies also failed to find evidence supporting
significant gains in reading skills through specialized reading instruction programs.39
These observations do not represent new findings. Special education classes often reflect what
happens in general education classes; over the past two decades, there has been a gradual
movement away from small-group, differentiated instruction and towards the inclusion of special
education students in general education classes, as well as a gradual trend towards more
undifferentiated whole group instruction, even in pull-out classrooms. Moreover, other studies
show that placement in special education commonly results in less reading instruction for
students with RD because it takes the place of language arts instruction in the general education
classroom. In addition, despite the fact that most students with LD require direct and intensive
instruction in reading, even special education teachers spend little time directly teaching reading
skills, and remedial students spend very little of their time reading in these “specialized”
programs.40 In short, the remediation services for elementary grade children in today’s special
education classrooms are not particularly effective.
These and similar findings which demonstrate a lack of efficacy for conventional “pull-out”
special education instructional-remediation models served as a major impetus for the “inclusion
movement” in special and general education that currently guides instructional practices in many
states. Unfortunately, several studies have documented that inclusion practices are especially
ineffective for older poor readers. For example, in one study, 80 percent of the poorest readers
made no measurable gain over the school year.41 What is clear is that neither traditional “pull-
out” programs nor inclusion practices have been effective in
helping poor readers in Grades 2 and beyond develop the
Several studies critical literacy skills they need.
have documented It is possible that the lack of progress in reading made by these
that inclusion relatively “older” students can be attributed to insufficient
practices are teacher preparation, large class sizes, and the lack of
specialized reading instruction. Yet even where teachers
especially
received professional development and support, the amount of
ineffective for older progress made by the end of the year did not close the reading
poor readers. gap.42 Because the purpose of reading remediation is to close
the achievement gap, these findings, like those obtained by
Hanushek and associates, were not very positive.
Even with as few as eight children in a group, teachers find it difficult to impart the necessary
individualized instruction with appropriate intensity. Many programs have been developed that
provide individualized tutoring. Unfortunately, such programs have been infrequently evaluated,
particularly for older poor readers. They have also been difficult to introduce into public schools
given their cost, the need for specialized professional development, and the sheer number of
children who need to be served. However, there is little evidence that 1:1 (one teacher to one
student) instruction is necessary. Groupings of 3:1 up to 5:1 have been found effective, and
sometimes more effective than a 1:1 teacher/student ratio if the children are similar in their
reading levels.43
With regard to older poor readers, unfortunately, even the best studies using highly intensive
remediation approaches have improved only a subset of critical reading skills. For example,
Torgesen and his group conducted a series of well-designed reading remediation trials with
severely disabled readers in grades 3-5.44 The students were randomly assigned to one of two
remediation approaches. One intervention was the Lindamood Auditory Discrimination in Depth
(ADD) program; the other was labeled “Embedded Phonics” (EP). Both programs provided
explicit instruction in phonics but varied in the amount of phonics instruction and the amount of
practice in reading and writing connected text. Students received 67.5 hours of individualized
instruction in one of these two programs over an eight-week period.
Figure 2 describes the growth in broad reading ability (a combination of word reading
accuracy and reading comprehension) by the children during their 16 months of special
education prior to the research intervention (pre-pretest to pretest, with pre-pretest based on the
school’s assessment). It also shows the growth they made during the eight weeks of intervention
(pretest to posttest) and in the two years following the interventions. The children showed little
change in the 16 months preceding the interventions, major improvements from pretest to
posttest, and maintenance of the gains for two years after
the intervention.
Although time and
Although these results show that appropriate and intensive
expense should not interventions can help older children substantially improve
dictate how we address their reading accuracy and comprehension, almost all the
the educational needs children in the study remained very slow readers. Their
scores on standardized measures of reading fluency
of children, the reality is
remained below the 5th percentile two years after the
that few school systems intervention. In attempting to account for these mixed
presently have access to findings, Torgesen’s group theorized that a major factor in
the necessary resources. the development of fluency is the number of words to
which a child is exposed through frequent reading
practice. Children with word recognition difficulties avoid
reading, so these children build up enormous deficits during the time they remain poor readers
in elementary school. Thus, it is extremely difficult for them to “catch up” to their peers in total
amount of reading practice time, particularly because their normally reading peers are
continuing their high rates of reading practice.
Although the children remained slow readers, Torgesen and colleagues’ study is noteworthy
because the gains in reading accuracy and comprehension were maintained after the
intervention was terminated. The study is also significant because it shows that more intensive
instruction than is typically provided in special education classrooms can have a very significant
effect on some reading skills of children with severe reading disabilities in a relatively short
period of time. Keep in mind that during the 16 months prior to the intervention, the children
made almost no progress toward closing the gap in word reading and comprehension skills.
After the intervention, however, many of the children maintained some reading skills within the
average range. Moreover, despite the reading fluency weaknesses, 40 percent were returned
from special education to regular education classes, which greatly exceeds the 5 percent figure
commonly cited for leaving special education. These remediated students returned from a
program that lasted 8 weeks but was very intense, and they occurred under two different types
of intervention.
It is likely that understanding of how to improve all reading skills among older disabled readers
will increase in the next few years. But we must expect that the reading programs and strategies
found to be effective through this research will continue to require low teacher-student ratios,
highly trained personnel, and a level of instructional intensity and duration that is time-consuming
and expensive. Although time and expense should not dictate how we address the educational
needs of children, the reality is that few school systems presently have access to the necessary
resources. In reading, this is partly because of the sheer number of older children with RD who
require services. The number of students identified
as LD and provided special education services
increases with age for two major reasons. First, as There is substantial evidence
pointed out previously, many children identified at
that early identification and
approximately nine to ten years of age are not
effectively remediated and therefore continue to intervention in kindergarten
receive services. Second, a large number of and Grade 1 may
children are identified as LD during middle and substantially reduce the
high school primarily because their reading
difficulties preclude learning in content areas.
number of children that
might otherwise be eligible
Prevention for special services.
Because most reading remediation efforts have not
been effective, a number of recent studies have
looked at prevention and early intervention
approaches that have the potential to reduce the number of children who eventually qualify for
special education or compensatory education programs. In reading, prevention research efforts
have been especially promising. Both the consensus report of the National Research Panel
(NRP)45 and the evidence-based report of the National Reading Council (NRC)46 concluded that
reading problems for many children could be prevented.
The NRC report suggested that in kindergarten, for example, children could be identified as “at-
risk” for word reading difficulties on the basis of their performance on tasks that assess
phonemic awareness and naming abilities. The types of measures that are most predictive of
later reading ability involve the child’s knowledge of letter sounds, the ability to blend sounds
into words (done orally), and, at the end of kindergarten, the ability to name letters rapidly. By
first grade, the child’s ability to read appropriately leveled words is the best predictor of later
reading success.
The beneficial effects of early identification and intervention are apparent in many studies. The
NRP identified a large body of research showing that explicit teaching of the relation of print
and sounds through phonological awareness training and phonics was especially effective in
kindergarten and Grade 1, with the instructional effects diminishing in subsequent grades when
word recognition skills had been developed to a level that adequately supported reading of
connected text.
There is substantial evidence that early identification and intervention in kindergarten and Grade
1 may substantially reduce the number of children that might otherwise be eligible for special
services. Torgesen recently summarized five such studies, all of which resulted in a reduction in
the number of potentially eligible children.47 In all these studies, children were identified as at-
risk for RD in kindergarten or Grade 1 based on assessment results that identified the children in
the bottom 12-18 percent (depending on the study) of the school population in either reading
The five studies varied in how children were identified as “at-risk,” the types of early
interventions employed, and the student-teacher ratios. All studies followed children for 2-5 years
and all showed that the gains were maintained. Moreover, in contrast to the remedial studies
described above, improvement in both accuracy and fluency
of word recognition skills was apparent, with gains also
The goal of remedial present in comprehension skills.49
reading instruction None of the intervention programs were equally effective for
should be to improve all of the children studied. There may be individual
characteristics of children that predispose them to more or
reading skills as
less success with a particular program. Research examining
quickly as possible so this possibility is underway, but it’s already clear that we
the student can “read need to move away from a “one-size-fits-all” mentality and
to learn” in critical apply continuous assessment approaches that evaluate how
well an instructional program is working with particular
content areas. youngsters. The growth of reading skills can be constantly
measured over time in the classroom, and these “curriculum-
based” approaches can identify children who are not
responding optimally to a particular instructional strategy. This information can then give the
teacher an objective basis for modifying instruction to address a particular student’s needs.50
The prevention and early intervention research has its critics. Some argue that early
identification is fraught with errors leading to misidentification and may incorrectly label a
student as at-risk for academic failure.51 We respond by suggesting that a label is not necessary
for implementation of prevention programs and the costs of delaying intervention are too great
to wait. Indeed, we hope that, at some point, all kindergarten and elementary grade teachers
will have had sufficient training to provide instruction that incorporates prevention into the
normal course of their teaching. Even with this enhanced capacity, however, some children will
need more instructional intensity than they can obtain in a typical classroom.
Other critics identify studies that show greater gains in word recognition than comprehension,
and suggest that many early intervention studies show diminished gains in the later grades.52 We
respond by pointing out that the so-called “fourth-grade” slump does not mean that children
begin to decline in the reading abilities gained; rather, they simply begin to show a reduction in
the rate of growth. The key is to continue reading instruction throughout elementary school with
Not all children will benefit from early intervention. Nevertheless, as stated in the above
discussion of prevention studies, children whose
word recognition skills were not brought into the
average range did improve to some degree in the Even the best evidence-
five studies. These children may require different based recommendations
interventions over time and/or more intensive
programs to foster compensatory learning strategies will not be utilized and
while continuing to work on basic academic skills sustained in practice unless
deficits. The goal of remedial reading instruction careful thought is given to
should be to improve reading skills as quickly as
possible so the student can “read to learn” in critical
identifying the conditions
content areas. To accomplish this, students, that will increase the
particularly at older age ranges, require highly probability of their
intensive and systematic instruction provided in successful implementation.
settings characterized by low teacher-student ratios.
This can only be done when the potential number of
children with reading difficulties has been reduced to manageable levels through early
intervention.
These conditions include our ability to (1) ensure that all recommendations have been sufficiently
tested to acknowledge clearly their strengths and weaknesses and evaluate their specific impact
on the children and adults to be served; (2) anticipate the effects of changes in policies and
practices on federal, state, and school communities and address them effectively; (3) take into
account barriers to change in public school policy and practice; and (4) articulate specific areas
where capacity must be developed to ensure successful implementation. We recognize that our
recommendations will require time and resources to determine whether these essential conditions
can be met. Hasty implementation of these recommendations without full knowledge of the
challenges that confront such significant change could put students in jeopardy. Thus, we
recommend that the relevant government agencies undertake a consensus development initiative
through which the federal definition of LD and attendant eligibility and intervention issues raised
in this chapter are evaluated in an attempt to align policy with research.
With this as background, we now summarize our major recommendations for improvements in
the definition of LD, teacher preparation, and the development and implementation of early
intervention and remediation programs. We conclude by articulating what we consider to be the
significant factors that must be addressed if productive implementation of these recommendations
is to occur.
Third, stop excluding children because of inadequate instruction, cultural and social factors, and
emotional disturbance. These exclusions may be policy-driven, designed to avoid commingling
of funds for compensatory and special education, but we should not allow our
conceptualizations of LD to be driven by policy issues. In the case of inadequate instruction and
cultural/social factors, we have argued that it is just these factors that may lead to inadequacies
in neural and cognitive development that place children at significant risk for LD. Thus, decisions
to maintain distinctions between compensatory and special education services should not drive
our conception of LD. Instruction is the key for all children who are not achieving according to
expectations.
many “spills” in the current system. The complex identification criteria and expensive due
process procedures of special education should be reserved for children who have not
responded to the powerful shorter-term interventions that are presently available. No doubt,
children who do not benefit from these interventions will require even more intensive remediation
programs as well as educational accommodations as they proceed through school.
We doubt that the colleges of education will change their current preparation practices in the
near future. What is clear is that teachers must be provided the critical academic content,
pedagogical principles, and knowledge of learner characteristics that they need in order to
impart systematic and informed instruction to their students. Some states (such as Texas) and
communities (for example, the Houston Independent School District) have considered alternatives
to traditional teacher preparation to ensure that teachers can close the gap between research
and practice. These initiatives should be carefully evaluated to determine whether such large-
scale efforts are effective.
A major problem with such efforts is that special educators who typically provide instruction to
children with LD have not been integrated into the early identification and prevention initiatives
and have not had a role in efforts to design and implement early intervention programs. It is
important that both regular and special education embrace these efforts and view prevention as
part of their mission. The IDEA today allows states to identify 6- to 9-year-old children as eligible
for special education services based on a designation of “developmental delay,” which means
the child is not making progress. Although developmental delay is a fairly meaningless term,
especially given the evidence that reading problems become persistent deficits early in
schooling, the idea that special education funds can be used for early identification and
prevention is critical. Unfortunately, the most recent report to Congress (for 1997-1998) on
implementation of the 1997 revision of the IDEA—which permitted use of “developmental
delay”—indicated that only eight states actually utilized this eligibility category. Children served
under “developmental delay” represented only 1.32 percent of children with disabilities in the 6-
to 9-year-old age group.55
Although special educators may not be directly involved in the provision of classroom instruction
for at-risk children, they should have a clear role in activities related to prevention. This includes
early identification and the implementation of specialized interventions within the classroom and
elsewhere. Such specialized services and programs should not be oriented toward a
determination of eligibility as is presently the case. An important lesson can be drawn from
compensatory education, where entry criteria are relatively simple and much more of the
funding is targeted for intervention programs rather than administrative issues relevant to
eligibility determination.
In particular, we do not know if there are characteristics of the environment, the brain, or
heredity that make it difficult to teach all children to learn to read, write, and/or develop
mathematics competencies. Yet a child who has difficulty learning to read early and whose
reading skills never develop to a level commensurate with stronger reading peers will most likely
show improved reading levels after aggressive attempts to provide sound early intervention.
Such a child may also need extensive modifications of his or her educational environment, more
extensive compensatory techniques, and other changes in curriculum such as those made for
youngsters with more obvious disabilities.
This is by no means an attempt to “write off” children who do not respond to aggressive
instruction, but it is recognition of the role of instruction in the definition of LD. Thus, it may be
reasonable to reserve the term “learning disabled” for individuals who clearly do not respond to
intensive intervention and who may need more significant modifications of their educational
environment in order to maximize their learning experiences in school. It would be important to
identify these children and look more closely at both neurobiological and environmental factors
that characterize them. With the opportunity to conduct research on children who clearly do not
respond to even the best of current interventions, we may be able to understand the causes of
this intractability and develop interventions that may further reduce the prevalence of LD in the
future. Regardless, such efforts should be tied into preventative approaches through an attempt
to implement what is essentially a public health model and reduce the overall prevalence of LD.
Under no circumstances should inadequate instruction be used as an excuse for denying access
to special education services. Poor instruction causes LD and should not be exclusionary.
the majority of those served by their legislation would be children in a relatively new disability
category. Similarly, although changes needed in the LD identification criteria and intervention
practices seem obvious based on the research presented here, we must expect unintended
consequences to follow from any changes. Considering that enhancing local capacity is key to
any change efforts, the fundamental changes we desire in educational practice will require time
and resources to implement.
It is time to more tightly link policy, research, and practice in LD. Programs that are implemented
on the basis of policy should be continuously evaluated for their efficacy. Similarly, instructional
interventions for children with LD should carry with them the expectation that they will be based
on research and evaluated in a serious way. Decisionmaking in education, whether it involves
policy or practice, should be guided by research. Society should have the same expectations for
education policy and practice that it has for medicine. We do not believe that the criteria used
to evaluate evidence are meaningfully different, nor that decisions on how much research is
necessary to implement a particular policy or practice are particularly different in education,
medicine, or related areas.
Such a radical restructuring of education policy and practice can only be successfully
accomplished if we acknowledge the complexity of the task before us. We have a model of
successful inquiry into the nature of reading disorders that can be applied to other learning
disorders. We now need to use what we know about LD and about education change to
construct and evaluate models for successful change in educational policy and practice for
children with learning difficulties. It is time to apply the same care and precision used in
conducting the original research to the task of effecting serious policy change based on that
research. The real tragedy is that conceptualizations of LD have not changed over 30 years
despite the completion of significant research in the past 15 years. What we know from
research now needs to be implemented. Children deserve no less.
_______________________________________
1
See B.K. Keogh, “A Shared Attribute Model of Learning Disabilities,” in Research in Learning Disabilities: Issues
and Future Directions, eds. S. Vaughn and C. Bos (Boston, MA: Little, Brown and Co., 1987), 3-18; and L.C.
Moats and G.R. Lyon, “Learning Disabilities in the United States: Advocacy, Science, and the Future of the
Field,” Journal of Learning Disabilities 26 (1993): 282-294.
2
See Cecil Mercer, et al., “Learning Disabilities Definitions and Criteria Used by State Education Departments,”
Learning Disabilities Quarterly 19 (1996): 217-232; “Definition and Criteria for Defining Students as Learning
Disabled,” 42 Fed. Reg. 65,083 (1977); and “Assistance to States for the Education of Children with
Disabilities Program and Preschool Grants for Children with Disabilities Final Rule,” 34 C.R.F. 300 and 301
(1992).
3
See J. Lerner, “Educational Intervention in Learning Disabilities,” Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry 28 (1989): 326-331.
4
See M. Kamil, et al., Handbook of Reading Research: Volume III (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 2000); National
Institute of Child Health and Human Development, The Report of the National Reading Panel: An Evidence-
Based Assessment of the Scientific Research Literature on Reading and Its Implications for Reading Instruction
(Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Child Health and Human Development, 2000); and C. Snow, et al.,
Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children (Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 1998).
5
See K. Kavale and S. Forness, The Nature of Learning Disabilities (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1995); and J.L.
Wiederholt, “Historical Perspectives on the Education of the Learning Disabled,” in The Second Review of
Special Education, eds. L. Mann and D. Sabatino, (Austin, TX: PRO-ED, 1974), 103-152.
6
See J. Doris, “Defining Learning Disabilities: A History of the Search for Consensus,” in Better Understanding
Learning Disabilities: New Views from Research and Their Implications for Educational and Public Policies, eds.
G.R. Lyon, et al. (Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes, 1993), 97-115.
7
S. Kirk, Educating Exceptional Children (Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin, 1962), 263.
8
See S. Kirk, “Behavioral Diagnosis and Remediation of Learning Disabilities,” in Proceedings of the Conference
on Exploration into the Problems of the Perceptually Handicapped Child (Evanston, IL: Fund for the Perceptually
Handicapped Child, Inc., 1963), 1-7.
9
See N. Zigmond, “Learning Disabilities from an Educational Perspective,” in Better Understanding Learning
Disabilities: New Views from Research and Their Implications for Education and Public Policies, eds. G.R. Lyon,
et al. (Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes, 1993), 251-272.
10
Education of the Handicapped Act, Title VI, Pub. L. No. 91-230 (1969-1970).
11
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act Amendments of 1991, Pub. L. No. 102-119, 20 U.S.C. § 1401 et
seq.
12
See Office of Special Education Programs, Implementation of IDEA.
13
See Individuals with Disabilities Education Act, Pub. L. No. 102-119. Other definitions and their historical
origins are discussed in G.R. Lyon, “Learning Disabilities,” The Future of Children 6 (1996): 54-76.
14
See G.R. Lyon, “Toward a Definition of Dyslexia,” Annals of Dyslexia 45 (1995): 3-30; and K. Stanovich, “The
Construct Validity of Discrepancy Definitions of Reading Disability,” in Better Understanding Learning
Disabilities: New Views from Research and Their Implications for Education and Public Policies, eds. G.R. Lyon,
et al. (Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes, 1993), 273-295.
15
See K. Kavale and S. Forness, The Science of Learning Disabilities (San Diego, CA: College Hill Press, 1985).
16
Literature on post-mortem and structural imaging studies was reviewed in P. Filipek, “Structural Variations in
Measures in the Developmental Disorders,” in Developmental Neuroimaging: Mapping the Development of
Brain and Behavior, eds. R. Thatcher, G. Lyon, J. Rumsey, and N. Krasnegor (San Diego, CA: Academic Press,
1996), 169-186. Functional imaging studies involve several modalities, such as positron emission tomography,
functional magnetic resonance imaging, and magnetoencephalography. See S.E. Shaywitz, K.R. Pugh, A.R.
Jenner, R.K. Fulbright, J.M. Fletcher, J.C. Gore, and B.A. Shaywitz, “The Neurobiology of Reading and Reading
Disability (Dyslexia),” in Handbook of Reading Research, Volume IIi,.eds. M.L. Kamil, P.B. Mosenthal, P.D.
Pearson, and R. Barr (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 2000), 229-249.
17
See Richard K. Olson, “Genes, Environment, and Reading Disabilities,” in Perspectives on Learning Disabilities,
eds. R.J. Sternberg and L. Spear-Swerling (Oxford: Westview Press, 1999), 3-22; and B. Pennington, “Dyslexia
as a Neurodevelopmental Disorder,” in Neurodevelopmental Disorders, ed. H. Tager-Flusberg (Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press, 1999), 307-330.
18
Detailed reviews of the issues with discrepancy models can be found in J.M. Fletcher, D.J. Francis, S.E.
Shaywitz, G.R. Lyon, B.R. Foorman, K.K. Stuebing, and B.A. Shaywitz, “Intelligent Testing and the Discrepancy
Model for Children with Learning Disabilities,” Learning Disabilities Research & Practice 13 (1998): 186-203;
and L.S. Siegel, “Learning Disabilities: The Roads We Have Traveled and the Path to the Future,” in Perspectives
on Learning Disabilities, eds. R.J. Sternberg and L. Spear-Swerling (Oxford: Westview Press, 1999), 159-175.
19
These recommendations can be found in Assistance to States for Education for Handicapped Children:
Procedures for Evaluating Specific Learning Disabilities, 42 Fed. Reg. G1082-G1085 (1977).
20
The 20th century reflects a period of extensive discussion of how to define different groups of low achievers
that culminated in the concept of LD. This history is discussed by J.L. Doris, “Defining Learning Disabilities: A
History of the Search for Consensus,” in Better Understanding Learning Disabilities, eds. G.R. Lyon, D.B. Gray,
J.F. Kavanaugh, and N.A. Krasnegor (Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes, 1993), 97-116; see also M. Rutter,
“Syndromes Attributed to Minimal Brain Dysfunction in Childhood,” American Journal of Psychiatry 139
(1982): 21-33.
21
See R.L. Thorndike, The Concepts of Over and Under Achievement (New York: Columbia University Bureau of
Publications, 1963).
22
The statistical problems with the use of IQ-discrepancy scores have been widely documented. See C.R.
Reynolds, “Critical Measurement Issues in Learning Disabilities,” Journal of Special Education 18 (1974): 451-
476.
23
Longitudinal research has shown that IQ-discrepancy scores are not reliable in Grade 1, but are reliable in
Grade 3 (and most likely in Grade 2, which was not included in the following study). See S.E. Shaywitz, M.D.
Escobar, B.A. Shaywitz, J.M. Fletcher, and R. Makuch, “Distribution and Temporal Stability of Dyslexia in an
Epidemiological Sample of 414 Children Followed Longitudinally,” New England Journal of Medicine 326
(1992): 145-150.
24
See S.E. Shaywitz, J.M. Fletcher, J.M. Holahan, A.E. Schneider, K.E. Marchione, K.K. Stuebing, D.J. Francis,
and B.A. Shaywitz, “Persistence of Dyslexia: The Connecticut Longitudinal Study at Adolescence,” Pediatrics
104 (1999): 1351-1359.
25
See J.K. Torgesen, R.K. Wagner, C.A. Rashotte, E. Rose, P. Lindamood, J. Conway, and C. Garvan, “Preventing
Reading Failure in Young Children with Phonological Processing Disabilities: Group and Individual Responses to
Instruction,” Journal of Educational Psychology 91 (1999): 579-594; and F.R. Vellutino, D.M. Scanlon, and
G.R. Lyon, “Differentiating Between Difficult-to-Remediate and Readily Remediated Poor Readers: More Evidence
Against the IQ-Achievement Discrepancy Definition for Reading Disability,” Journal of Learning Disabilities 33
(2000): 223-238.
26
See A.D. Ross, Psychological Aspects of Learning Disabilities and Reading Disorders (New York: McGraw-Hill,
1976).
27
See M. Rutter, “Prevalence and Types of Dyslexia,” in Dyslexia: An Appraisal of Current Knowledge, eds. A.L.
Benton and D. Pearl (New York: Oxford University Press, 1978), 3-28.
28
See P.B. Gough and M.L. Hillinger, “Learning to Read: An Unnatural Act,” Bulletin of the Orton Society 30
(1980): 179-196; and A.M. Liberman, “How Theories of Speech Effect Research in Reading and Writing,” in
Foundations of Reading Acquisition and Dyslexia: Implications for Early Intervention, ed. B. Blachman
(Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence, 1997).
29
For an excellent discussion of the relationship between environmental, cultural, and economic factors; the
presence of teratogens; neural development; and learning, see D.P. Hallahan, J.M. Kauffman, and J.W. Lloyd,
Introduction to Learning Disabilities (Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon, 1996).
30
See G. Senf, “LD Research in Sociological and Scientific Perspective,” in Psychological and Educational
Perspectives on Learning Disabilities, eds. J. Torgesen and B. Wong (Orlando, FL: Academic Press, 1986), 27-
53.
31
See American Federation of Teachers, Teaching Reading Is Rocket Science: What Expert Teachers of Reading
Should Know and Be Able to Do (Washington, DC: American Federation of Teachers, 1999).
32
See G.R. Lyon, M. Vaasen, and F. Toomey, “Teachers’ Perceptions of their Undergraduate and Graduate
Training,” Teacher Education and Special Education 12 (1989): 164-169; and L.C. Moats, “The Missing
Foundation in Teacher Preparation,” American Educator 19 (1995): 43-51.
33
See J.M. Fletcher, and G.R. Lyon, “Reading: A Research-Based Approach,” in What’s Gone Wrong in
America’s Classrooms, ed. W. M. Evers (Stanford, CA: Hoover Institution Press, 1998), 49-90; and S.
Shaywitz, “Dyslexia,” Scientific American 275 (1996): 98-104.
34
See S.E. Shaywitz, J.M. Fletcher, J.M. Holahan, A.E. Schneider, K.E. Marchione, K.K. Stuebing, D.J. Francis,
and B.A. Shaywitz, “Persistence of Dyslexia: The Connecticut Longitudinal Study at Adolescence,” Pediatrics
104 (1999): 1351-1359.
35
See C. Juel, “Learning to Read and Write: A Longitudinal Study of 54 Children from First through Fourth
Grades,” Journal of Educational Psychology 80 (1988): 437-447.
36
See J.K. Torgesen, “The Prevention and Remediation of Reading Disabilities: Evaluating What We Know from
Research,” Journal of Academic Language Therapy 1 (1997): 11-47.
37
See E.A. Hanushek, J.F. Kain, and S.G. Riukin, “Does Special Education Raise Academic Achievement for
Students with Disabilities?” (National Bureau of Economic Research working paper no. 6469, 1998).
38
See S.W. Moody, S.R. Vaughn, M.T. Hughes, and M. Fischer, “Reading Instruction in the Resource Room: Set
Up for Failure,” Exceptional Children 16 (2000): 305-316; J.S. Schumm, S.W. Moody, and S.R. Vaughn,
“Grouping for Reading Instruction: Does One Size Fit All?” Journal of Learning Disabilities 5 (2000): 477-488;
and S.R. Vaughn, S.W. Moody, and J.S. Shuman, “Broken Promises: Reading Instruction in the Resource
Room,” Exceptional Children 64 (1998): 211-225.
39
See R.L. Allington and A. McGill-Franzen, “School Response to Reading Failure: Instruction for Chapter One
and Special Education Students Grades Two, Four, and Eight,” Elementary School Journal 89 (1989): 529-542;
M.C. Haynes and J.R. Jenkins, “Reading Instruction in Special Education Resource Rooms,” American
Educational Research Journal 23 (1986): 161-190; and A. McGill-Franzen, “Compensatory and Special
Education: Is There Accountability for Learning and Belief in Children’s Potential?” in Getting Reading Right
from the Start: Effective Early Literacy Interventions, eds. E.H. Hiebert and B.M. Taylor (Boston, MA: Allyn and
Bacon, 1994), 13-35.
40
See R.L. Allington, H. Stuetzel, M.C. Shake, and L. Lamarch, “What is Remedial Reading? A Descriptive
Study,” Reading Research and Instruction 26 (1986): 15-30; and G. Leinhardt, C. Weidman, and K.
Hammond, “Introduction and Integration of Classroom Routines by Expert Teachers,” Curriculum Inquiry 17
(1987): 135-176.
41
See J.K. Klingner, S.R. Vaughn, J.S. Schumm, M. Hughes, and B. Elbaum, “Outcomes for Students with and
Without Learning Disabilities in Inclusive Classrooms,” Learning Disabilities Research & Practice 13 (1997):
153-161. These results are consistent with other studies. See, for example, N. Zigmond and J.M. Baker,
“Concluding Comments: Current and Future Practices in Inclusive Schooling,” The Journal of Special Education
29 (1995): 245-250.
42
See B. R. Foorman, D.J. Francis, T. Beeler, D. Winikates, and J.M. Fletcher, “Early Interventions for Children
with Reading Problems: Study Designs and Preliminary Findings,” Learning Disabilities 8 (1997): 63-71.
43
See B.E. Elbaum, S. Vaughn, M.T. Hughes, and S.W. Moody, “How Effective are One-to-One Tutoring Programs
in Reading for Elementary Students At-Risk for Reading Failure? A Meta-Analysis,” Journal of Educational
Psychology (in press).
44
See J.K. Torgesen, A.W. Alexander, R.K. Wagner, C.A. Rashotte, K. Voeller, T. Conway, and E. Rose, “Intensive
Remedial Instruction for Children with Severe Reading Disabilities: Immediate and Long-Term Outcomes from
Two Instructional Approaches,” Journal of Learning Disabilities 34 (2001): 33-58.
45
See National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, The Report of the National Reading Panel: An
Evidence-Based Assessment of the Scientific Research Literature on Reading and Its Implications for Reading
Instruction (Bethesda, MD: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000).
46
C. Snow, et al., Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children (Washington, DC: National Academy Press,
1999).
47
See J.K. Torgesen, “Individual Responses in Response to Early Interventions in Reading: The Lingering Problem
of Treatment Resisters,” Learning Disabilities Research & Practice 15 (2000): 55-64. The five studies are as
follows: R.H. Felton, “Effects of Instruction on the Decoding Skills of Children with Phonological-Processing
Problems,” Journal of Learning Disabilities 26 (1993): 583-589; B.R. Foorman, D.J. Francis, J.M. Fletcher, C.
Schatschneider, and P. Mehta, “The Role of Instruction in Learning to Read: Preventing Reading Failure in At-
Risk-Children,” Journal of Educational Psychology 90 (1998): 37-55; J.K. Torgesen, R.K. Wagner, C.A.
Rashotte, and J. Herron, “The Effectiveness of Teacher Supported Computer Assisted Instruction in Preventing
Reading Problems in Young Children: A Comparison of Two Methods” (unpublished manuscript, Florida State
University, Tallahassee, FL); and J.K. Torgesen, R.K. Wagner, C.A. Rashotte, E. Rose, P. Lindamood, and C.
Galvan, “Preventing Reading Failure in Young Children with Phonological Processing Disabilities: Group and
Individual Responses to Instruction,” Journal of Educational Psychology 91 (1999): 1-15; and F.R. Vellutino,
D.M. Scanlon, E. Sipay, S. Small, A. Pratt, R. Chen, and M. Denckla, “Cognitive Profiles of Difficult-to-
Remediate and Readily Remediated Poor Readers: Early Intervention as a Vehicle for Distinguishing Between
Cognitive and Experiential Deficits as Basic Causes of Specific Reading Disabilities”, Journal of Educational
Psychology 88 (1996): 601-638.
48
The reduction in eligible children is calculated by multiplying the criterion for designating a child at-risk by the
number of children who would continue to need services. In Foorman, et al., “The Role of Instruction in Learning
to Read,” this is .18 times .30 equals .054.
49
See J.K. Torgesen, C.A. Rashotte, and A.W. Alexander, “Principles of Fluency Instruction in Reading:
Relationships with Established Empirical Outcomes,” in Time, Fluency, and Dyslexia, ed. M. Wolf (Parkton, MD:
York Press, in press).
50
See L. Fuchs and D. Fuchs, “Treatment Validity: A Simplifying Concept for Reconceptualizing the Identification
of Learning Disabilities,” Learning Disabilities Research and Practice 4 (1998): 204-219.
51
This issue has been discussed since the advent of the idea of early identification. See P. Satz, H.G. Taylor, J.
Friel, and J.M. Fletcher, “Some Developmental and Predictive Precursors of Reading Disabilities: A Six Year
Follow-up,” in Dyslexia: An Appraisal of Current Knowledge, eds. A.L. Benton and D. Pearl (New York: Oxford
Press, 1978), 313-348; A.A. Silver, “Prevention,” ibid. at 349-377; and J.J. Jansky, “A critical review of ‘Some
Developmental and Predictive Precursors of Reading Disabilities,’” ibid. at 377-395. A more contemporary
discussion is in D. Speece and L.P. Case, “Classification in Context: An Alternative Approach to Identifying
Early Reading Difficulty” (unpublished manuscript).
52
See M. Pressley and R. Allington, “What Should Reading Instructional Research Be the Research Of?” Issues in
Education 5 (1999): 1-35. See the responses in the same issue by B.R. Foorman, J.M. Fletcher, and D.J.
Francis, “Beginning Reading is Strategic and By Design Multi-Level”; G.R. Lyon, “In Celebration of Science in
the Study of Reading Development, Reading Difficulties, and Reading Instruction: The NICHD Perspective”; J.K.
Torgesen, “Placing NICHD-Supported Research on Reading With the Proper Context”; and F.R. Vellutino and
D.M. Scanlon, “Focus, Funding, Phonics—What’s the Point?”
53
See M. Fullan and M. Miles, “Getting Reform Right: What Works and What Doesn’t,” Phi Delta Kappan 74
(1992): 745-752; B. Rowan and L.F. Guthrie, “The Quality of Chapter 1 Instruction: Results From a Study of
Twenty-four Schools,” in Effective Programs for Students at Risk, eds. Robert E. Slavin, N. L. Karweit, and N.A.
Madden (Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon, 1989), 195-219; and R. Weatherly and M. Lipsky, “Street-level
Bureaucrats and Institutional Innovation: Implementing Special Education Reform,” Harvard Educational Review
47 (1977): 171-197.
54
See S.L. Hall and L.C. Moats, Straight Talk About Reading (Chicago, IL: Contemporary Books, 1999).
55
See U.S. Department of Education, Twenty-first Annual Report to Congress on the Implementation of the
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (Washington DC: U.S. Department of Education, 1999).
56
These issues are extensively discussed by V.W. Berninger and R.D. Abbott, “Redefining Learning Disabilities:
Moving Beyond Aptitude Achievement Discrepancies to Failure to Respond to Validated Treatment Protocols,” in
Frames of Reference for the Assessment of Learning Disabilities, ed. G.R. Lyon (Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes,
1994), 143-162; and F.R. Vellutino, D.M. Scanlon, and E.R. Sipay, “Towards Distinguishing Between Cognitive
and Experimental Deficits as Primary Sources of Difficulty in Learning to Read: The Importance of Early
Intervention in Diagnosis Specific Reading Disability,” in Foundations of Reading Acquisition and Dyslexia, ed.
B. Blachman (Mahwah NJ: Erlbaum, 1997), 347-380.