Hooper 2011
Hooper 2011
doi: 10.1111/j.1752-0606.2011.00258.x
Kirsten Doehler
Elon University
Despite the frequent use of the Parentification Questionnaire (PQ) and the Parentification
Scale (PS) in research studies, scant attention has been directed toward the psychometric
properties of the scores derived from these measures. Moreover, given the importance of
parentification as a clinical topic for treatment, it is surprising that more attention has not
been paid to the accuracy and psychometric properties of parentification instruments. The
current study was the first to compare the psychometric properties of these two commonly
used parentification instruments (i.e., PQ and PS). This study also examined another
parentification instrument, newly developed: the Parentification Inventory (PI). More
specifically, the psychometric properties of the PI were compared comprehensively with
those of the PQ and PS in a sample of college students (N = 787). Scores derived from
the three measures were found to be comparable, valid, and reliable. Implications for
couple and family system practice, and directions for future research are proffered.
Lisa M. Hooper, PhD, Department of Educational Studies in Psychology, Research Methodology, and
Counseling, University of Alabama; Kirsten Doehler, PhD, Department of Mathematics and Statistics, Elon
University.
Address correspondence to Lisa M. Hooper, Department of Educational Studies in Psychology, Research
Methodology, and Counseling, University of Alabama, Box 870231, Tuscaloosa, Alabama 35487; E-mail:
lhooper@bamaed.ua.edu
BACKGROUND
Previous research using the PQ (Jurkovic & Thirkield, 1998) and PS (Mika et al., 1987)
has suggested that the consequences of parentification fall along a continuum from mild to
great depending on the demands placed on the child, the length of time in which the responsi-
bilities are carried out, the extent to which the parent-like roles and responsibilities are valued,
to whom the parentification roles and responsibilities are directed (i.e., parent, sibling, grand-
parent and so forth), and the personality characteristics that the child possesses. The next sec-
tion describes the accumulated indirect evidence of validity and reliability of the PQ and PS
and empirical evidence that has been informed by these measures.
According to Aldridge (2006), Hooper (2011), and Mika et al. (1987), the caretaking role
can become detrimental both in childhood and throughout the life span when it becomes a
long-term role that requires more of the child, physically and emotionally, than the child can
give because of age and maturity levels. Godsall, Jurkovic, Emshoff, Anderson, and Stanwyck
(2004), and Mika et al. also asserted that the caretaking role can become harmful to a child if
the child’s needs are neglected because of the responsibilities placed upon him or her, if the par-
ent takes on a child-like role in relation to the child and if the child is not recognized for fulfill-
ing the role or is punished for performing its duties. Because of the additional concerns and
responsibilities of caretaking, the parentified child may not learn ‘‘developmentally appropriate
tasks that build self-concept,’’ which can negatively affect a child (Godsall et al., 2004, p. 799)
and the adult that he or she later becomes (Hooper, DeCoster, White, & Voltz, 2011).
An important point highlighted by child development and family system scholars is that
childhood relationships and attachments can greatly influence relational patterns during adult-
hood (Byng-Hall, 2008a; Hooper, 2003). Parentification has been associated with higher levels
of difficulty in establishing ‘‘a separate and authentic sense of self in adult relationships and
work’’ (Chase et al., 1998, p. 106). Cree (2003) posited that some caregivers, especially younger
children, can experience difficulties because of their young caregiving roles when they transition
from adolescence to adulthood. Cree also suggested that certain problems experienced by young
caregivers increase in severity as the caregivers grow older. The problems that Cree found to
worsen with age in her young caregiving participants included the following: ‘‘sleeping difficul-
ties, eating problems, truancy, trouble with the police, substance abuse, self-harm and worries
about not having friends’’ (p. 306). Alarmingly, the risk of suicide also appears to increase as
the caregiving children grow older. In Cree’s study, four of the 61 participants between the ages
of 10 and 12 years reported having had suicidal ideation, and Cree suggested that the risk of
suicide for all young caregivers should be closely monitored.
Parentification can include duties other than emotionally focused caretaking, such as help-
ing to provide financially for one’s family. Longest and Shanahan (2007) suggested that adoles-
cents living in families with only one parent may have to work an excessive number of hours to
help support their families, which can lead to higher levels of stress and a higher likelihood of
substance use. In their study of 3,290 US adolescents, Longest and Shanahan found that ado-
lescent participation in paid work was associated with adolescent use of alcohol and sometimes
Three specific aims motivated this study. Aim 1 sought to examine the convergent validity
of the three assessments of parentification: PI, PQ, and PS. Aim 2 sought to examine the inter-
nal consistency (Cronbach’s alphas) of the PI, PQ, and PS. Finally, aim 3 sought to investigate
the divergent and predictive validity of the PI, PQ, and PS by relating the scores from these
measures to those from the SF-36 health outcome measure.
Three corresponding hypotheses were, therefore, examined in this study:
Hypothesis 1. The PI, PQ, and PS will be strongly positively correlated with one another.
Hypothesis 2. Scores from the PI will result in reliability coefficients that (a) are similar to
the PQ and PS and (b) are 0.70 or greater.
Hypothesis 3. The higher scores on the parentification measures will be correlated with and
predictive of scores derived from a measure of psychological and physical health (i.e., SF-36).
METHOD
Survey Procedure
Following Institutional Review Board approval, participants were recruited to take part in
a study investigating the link between childhood roles and responsibilities and adult functioning.
The survey was administered online using a web-based methodology. Specifically, participants
were sent an electronic invitation to participate in the study. The electronic invitation included a
description of the study and a direct link to the electronic survey and the informed consent form.
Four instruments and a demographic information sheet were used in the current study. On aver-
age, participants took approximately 35–40 min to complete the questionnaires. All instruments
were in English. Extra course credit was provided as an incentive for participating in the study.
Participants received reminder e-mails 2 weeks after they expressed an interest in the study.
Measures
Demographic information sheet. This instrument, created for the study, asked survey par-
ticipants to respond to background questions. Questions covered the participant’s year in
school, academic program, age, gender, and racial and ethnic background.
Parentification Inventory. The PI (Hooper, 2009) is a retrospective 22-item self-report mea-
sure that assesses caregiving roles and responsibilities usually reserved for adults but carried out
by children. The PI is also designed to measure the perceived benefits of performing caregiving
roles in one’s family of origin. Participants respond to the 22 items using a five-point Likert-type
scale, ranging from 1 (never true) to 5 (always true). The PI consists of three subscales: parent-
focused parentification, sibling-focused parentification, and perceived benefit finding of parentifi-
cation. Items associated with parent-focused parentification include, for example, ‘‘I was expected
to comfort my parents when they were sad or having emotional difficulties’’ and ‘‘My parent(s)
often shared secrets with me about other family members.’’ Items associated with sibling-focused
parentification include, for example, ‘‘I was responsible for making sure that my siblings went to
bed every night’’ and ‘‘I was the primary person who disciplined my siblings.’’ Items associated
with perceived benefit finding of parentification include ‘‘I really enjoyed my role in the family.’’
Scores can fall in the range of 1–5, with higher total and subscale scores reflecting greater
perceived levels of parentification or perceived benefits of the parentification process.
RESULTS
Sample Characteristics
The convenience study sample consisted of 787 student volunteers recruited from under-
graduate classes in a southeastern university. Participants’ ages ranged from 19 to 48 years
(M = 20.86, SD = 3.55). The sample primarily consisted of White Americans (82.5%,
n = 649) and females (76%, n = 598). Self-reported races of the remaining participants were
American Indian (0.05%, n = 4), Asian American (1.0%, n = 8), African American (12.5%,
n = 98), Hispanic–Latino (0.04%, n = 3), mixed race (3.0%, n = 21), and those who failed to
report a race (0.05%, n = 4).
Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. PI (PFP) –
2. PI (SFP) 0.421** –
3. PI (PBP) 0.264** 0.147** –
4. PQ (IM) 0.540** 0.538** 0.352** –
5. PQ (EM) 0.683** 0.310** 0.455** 0.617** –
6. PQ (PF) 0.466** 0.259** 0.688** 0.637** 0.694** –
7. PS (SR) 0.561** 0.244** 0.352** 0.440** 0.632** 0.479** –
8. PS (PR-P) 0.580** 0.248** 0.298** 0.430** 0.631** 0.449** 0.841** –
9. PS (PR-S) 0.375** 0.564** 0.260** 0.538** 0.382** 0.371** 0.521** 0.537** –
10. PS (NS) 0.394** 0.387** 0.272** 0.567** 0.409** 0.394** 0.447** 0.460** 0.503** –
Note. PI = Parentification Inventory (PFP = parent-focused parentification; SFP = sibling-focused parentification; PBP = perceived benefits of
parentification); PQ = Parentification Questionnaire (IM = instrumental parentification; EM = emotional parentification; PF = perceived fairness);
Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
1. PI (PFP) –
2. PI (SFP) 0.421** –
3. PI (PBP) 0.264** 0.147** –
4. PQ (IM) 0.540** 0.538** 0.352** –
5. PQ (EM) 0.683** 0.310** 0.455** 0.617** –
6. PQ (PF) 0.466** 0.259** 0.688** 0.637** 0.694** –
7. PS (SR) 0.561** 0.244** 0.352** 0.440** 0.632** 0.479** –
8. PS (PR-P) 0.580** 0.248** 0.298** 0.430** 0.631** 0.449** 0.841** –
9. PS (PR-S) 0.375** 0.564** 0.260** 0.538** 0.382** 0.371** 0.521** 0.537** –
10. PS (NS) 0.394** 0.387** 0.272** 0.567** 0.409** 0.394** 0.447** 0.460** 0.503** –
11. SF-36 MENT 0.210** 0.091* 0.418** 0.186** 0.281** 0.399** 0.256** 0.225** 0.148** 0.163** –
12. SF-35 PHYS 0.244** 0.133** 0.280** 0.253** 0.206** 0.294** 0.288** 0.240** 0.217** 0.128** 0.447** –
M 1.90 1.545 4.10 17.81 22.57 17.87 32.67 24.83 51.38 15.04 975.67 1794.00
SD 0.585 0.560 0.840 6.59 7.15 7.56 6.40 4.73 8.41 3.52 238.77 293.76
Cronbach’s Alpha 0.85 0.82 0.76 0.83 0.85 0.90 0.88 0.81 0.91 0.83 0.80 0.90
PI scale scores B SE B b
subscale scores). Once again, because of the exploratory nature of this study, all variables were
entered simultaneously. As shown in Table 3, the first overall regression model (test 1) estab-
lished the existence of a significant relationship between the three predictor variables and the
criterion variable of psychological health: F(3, 787) = 52.93, p < .0001. The R value
(r = .411) for this model meets the criterion for a medium effect size, as defined by Cohen
DISCUSSION
This study compared the psychometric properties of three measures that assess childhood
parentification retrospectively. We found evidence of reliability and validity for all three mea-
sures. In addition, the results of the study suggest that scores from the newly created PI have
satisfactory reliability and validity. The main results also reveal that the PI generates findings
comparable to those of the PQ and PS. The main results of the study can be described as fol-
lows: First, all three measures of parentification are capturing a multi-dimensional construct
similarly. Second, all three measures’ scores are reliable, as measured by Cronbach’s alphas.
Third, all three measures’ scores demonstrate sound predictive, convergent, and divergent valid-
ity in theoretically expected ways.
Moreover, although we expected all three measures to reveal similar relations between
study variables, we also expected the parentification measures to reveal differences. For exam-
ple, an important finding from these results is the slight incremental validity (Haynes & Lench,
2003) evinced in the PI scores compared with the PQ and PS scores. The slight difference
between the three parentification instruments, as evidenced in these results, may point to an
advantage of the PI in comparison with the PQ and PS, although we are cautious in this preli-
minary finding. Further, because of the conditional nature of incremental validity (Haynes &
Lench, 2003), many more studies comprising diverse study samples are needed to fully clarify
this tentative assertion.
The prevalence of parentification in childhood has implications for the individuals that
children become in adulthood (DiCaccavo, 2006). Moreover, the construct of parentification
has relevance to science and practice. Couple and family therapists as well as other mental
health care providers would benefit from assessing for parentification. DiCaccavo outlines
important issues relevant to the assessment of signs and symptoms in individuals and couples
who report a childhood history of parentification. Specifically, she reports there are three areas
that should be examined in a comprehensive assessment of a client with a possible history of
parentification: (a) the type of role assignment (i.e., instrumental and emotional parentification)
and the level of support received when the client performed the parentification role; (b) the age
of the client when the parentification process took place and the extent of the parentification-
related responsibilities in which the client engaged; and (c) ‘‘the consideration of the object of
concern and degree of internalization’’ (DiCaccavo, 2006, p. 472). Clients who have internalized
the parentified role and responsibilities may describe issues and concerns about others but not
about him or herself. Deparentification based on a comprehensive assessment may include
explorations related to issues of shame, splitting, and projective identification, for example (see
DiCaccavo, 2006).
Because parentification is a common clinical phenomenon likely to be observed by family
therapists as well as other mental health care providers (Byng-Hall, 2008b; Chase, 1999;
Hooper, 2007b), it is critical that useful measures with sound psychometric properties be avail-
able to assess for parentification. Additionally, a panoply of evidence (summarized earlier in
this article) supports the correlation between parentification and psychological health. The cur-
rent study adds to the clinical literature base by considering the link between parentification
and physical health in addition to the link between parentification and psychological health.
Thus, these findings are pertinent to both the physical and the psychological health of patients
with whom couple and family therapists work. The results also add to the current literature
base by describing the psychometric properties of three instruments that produce reliable and
valid scores. We hope that these results will engender therapists’ confidence in the measures
that capture parentification, such that therapists will use these instruments in their practice.
These findings strengthen the validity and empirical evidence that underpins the construct
of parentification. These results suggest some implications for future research. Over the past
30 years, an extensive amount of empirical research on parentification and possible correlates,
antecedents, predictors, and outcomes has been conducted. Less work has focused on the mea-
surement of parentification. There remains a lack of information on the test–retest reliability
and social desirability of all three instruments included in the current study. Future research
should include a measure that examines the extent to which respondents answer questions in a
way that they believe is favorable (e.g., Social Desirability Scale; Crowne & Marlowe, 1960).
Certainly, the stability and validity of the scores derived from these instruments could be
affected if respondents alter answers based on their desire to appear in a positive light.
Additionally, few studies have advanced ideas about how parentification measures may be
used in process and outcome research. Additional value—specifically clinical value—could be
garnered from establishing the clinical utility of the PI, PQ, and PS instruments. For example,
important clinical information could be uncovered if measures of parentification included in
randomized trials focused on the efficacy and effectiveness of couple and family system thera-
pies. With regard to process research, it could be that a history of parentification might impede
the joining process or building of rapport in therapy. Similar to the link between attachment
style and the relationship patients have with important individuals in their life (e.g., significant
romantic other, therapist, primary care physician), so too could a history of childhood parentif-
ication impinge upon the ability of patients to form therapeutic relationships with their thera-
pist and thereby have an impact therapeutic outcomes. Finally, few studies have investigated
potential mediators and moderators between childhood parentification and adult functioning.
STUDY LIMITATIONS
The single-informant, cross-sectional design of the current study yielded several limitations.
First, given that the current sample consisted of undergraduate students, the results may not
generalize to other populations. Second, the study sample was homogenous. Because reliability
and validity are context dependent (Schmidt & Hunter, 2003), it is important to continue to
accumulate evidence related to cultural validity and generalizability in studies of diverse popu-
lations. Researchers (e.g., Bravo, 2003) suggest that validity and reliability of an instrument’s
scores in one culture do not portend validity and reliability in another culture. Researchers
should examine the cross-cultural, linguistic, and translational equivalence of the PI, PQ, and
PS. Findings from such studies would inform needed changes in measures of parentification.
Further replications of the current study’s findings are needed to determine the extent to which
these findings may be applicable to and clinically meaningful for a range of populations.
The fact that all data in the study were based on self-report measures is another limitation
of the study. Future studies should employ multi-method and direct observation strategies to
further clarify the validity and reliability of the PI, PQ, and PS. Other considerations and possi-
ble limitations related to the measurement of parentification include retrospective recall. Mem-
ory or motivational factors may affect accurate reporting of retrospective parentification. For
example, the stability of reporting of adverse events, particularly when based on family of ori-
gin or lifetime experiences, may be confounded by memory loss, underreporting, overreporting,
or minimizing (Baker, 2009; Briere & Conte, 1993; Hooper & Wallace, 2010). Alternately, indi-
viduals may be unwilling to report events such as childhood neglect, adversity, or mistreatment
(Baker, 2009; Baker & Festinger, 2011; Brewin, Andrews, & Gotlib, 1993). Moreover, studies
that include assessments that are cross-situational sources will enhance our understanding of
parentification, the process, and the correlates and predictors of parentification (Kuperminc,
Jurkovic, & Casey, 2009).
CONCLUSION
Despite the frequent use of the PQ and the PS in research studies, scant attention has been
directed toward the psychometric properties of the scores derived from these measures. The cur-
rent study was the first to compare the psychometric properties of these two commonly used
parentification instruments. This study also examined another parentification instrument, newly
developed: the PI. The psychometric properties of the PI were compared comprehensively to
those of the PQ and PS. The current study adds to the literature base because, in addition to
being often studied, parentification is a ubiquitous clinical phenomenon that most family thera-
pists and other mental health care providers will face (DiCaccavo, 2006). Findings demon-
strated that parentification is relevant to adults’ psychological and physical health and warrants
the attention of practitioner–scientists in clinical settings. Therefore, continued examination of
psychometrically sound measures is needed to further our understanding of the parentification
process in both clinical and research settings, the outcomes across the life span that are associ-
ated with parentification and interventions that are culturally relevant and linguistically appro-
priate for racially and ethnically diverse populations.
REFERENCES
Aldridge, J. (2006). The experiences of children living with and caring for parents with mental illness. Child Abuse
Review, 15, 79–88. DOI: 10.1002/car.904.
Alexander, P. C., Teti, L., & Anderson, C. L. (2000). Childhood sexual abuse history and role reversal in parent-
ing. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24, 829–838.
Baker, A. J. L. (2009). Adult recall of childhood psychosocial maltreatment: Definitional strategies and chal-
lenges. Children and Youth Services Review, 31, 703–714.