Peter A. F Acione 2000

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 24

The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking: Its

Character, Measurement, and Relationship to


Critical Thinking Skill
PETER A. F ACIONE Santa Clara University

Abstract: Theorists have hypothesized that Resume: Les theoriciens ont suppose qu'iI
skill in critical thinking is positively corre- y a une correlation positive entre les
lated with the consistent internal motiva- habiletes de la pcnsee critique et Ia motiva-
tion to think and that specific critical think- tion constante a penser; et que des habiletes
specifiques de Ia pensee critique se relient a
ing skills are matched with specific critical
des dispositions specifiques de la pensee cri-
thinking dispositions. If true, these assump- tique. 51 ces suppositions sont vraies, elles
tions suggest that a skill-focused curriculum suggerent qu'un programme scolaire centre
would lead persons to be both willing and sur des habiletes menerait les etudiant(e)s a
able to think. This essay presents a research- vouloir et apouvoir bien penser. On presente
based expert consensus definition of critical une definition de la pensee critique fondee
thinking, argues that human dispositions are sur un consensus parmi des experts qui font
neither hidden nor unknowable, describes a de la recherche; avance que les dispositions
humaines sont ni cachees, ni
scientific process of developing conventional
inconnaissables; decrit un procede
testing tools to measure cognitive skills and scientifique pour developper des moyens
human dispositions, and summarizes recent pour mesurer des habiletes cognitives et des
empirical research findings that explore the dispositions humaines ; resume des resultats
possible relationship of critical thinking skill des recherches empiriques recentes qui
and the consistent internal motivation, or explorent Ie rapport possible entre les
disposition, to use that skill. Empirical stud- habiletes de la pensee critique et la motiva-
ies indicate that for all practical purposes tion ou la disposition a employer ces
habiletes, Des etudes empriques indiquent
the hypothesized correlations are not evi-
qu'a toute fin printique les correlations
dent. It would appear that effective teach- admises comme hypotheses ne sont pas
ing must include strategies for building intel- evidentes. II semblerait qu'un enseignement
lectual character rather than relying exclu- efficace inelurait des strategies qui forment
sively on strengthening cognitive skills Ie caractCre intellectuel et qu i ne
concentrent pas exclusivement sur
I'amelioration des habiletes cognitives,

Keywords: critical thinking, disposition, assessment, test development, CCTST, CCTDI

"We have warp drive capability, Yes,


But where can warp drive take us?"
Anji, Star Trek Insurrection

1. Two Questions
The general consensus is that critical thinking (CT) per se is judging in a reflective
way what to do or what to believe. The cognitive skills of analysis, interpretation,

©lnformal Logic Vol. 20, No.1 (2000): pp.61-84.


62 Peter A. Faciane

inference, explanation, evaluation, and of monitoring and correcting one's own


reasoning are at the heart of critical thinking (APA, 1990). Through practice, and
with guidance from a good instructor, we can develop our thinking skills (like our
artistic, athletic, or leadership skills) to the extent our natural abilities allow. But
we should take care not to confuse the component skills with the activity itself.
Critical thinking (CT) is judgment, reflective and purposive.
For decades college text books and K-12 educational policy emphasized learn-
ing skills (Marshall and Tucker, 1992). US government policy, which first recog-
nized college level CT in its 1994 articulation of national educational goals, used
the language of skills and abilities in that legislation (US Congress, 1994). So much
attention has been paid to the skills used in CT that we risk trivializing CT, charac-
terizing it as merely an assortment of techniques rather than as a complex, thoughtful,
purposeful process offormingjudgments using reasons and evidence (Paul, 1990).
What might be described as an over-emphasis on skills has been countered
recently by a rebirth of interest in the dispositional side of thinking (Siegel, 1988;
Paul, 1990; Esterle, 1993; Facione & Facione, 1992; Tishman and Andrade 1996,
Ennis, 1996). The empirical identification of two factors as being involved when
persons engage in tests of their CT tends to support the philosophical distinction
between the skill dimension and the disposition dimension of the use of CT
(Taube, 1997). However, some theorists, like Paul, Tavris and Wade, include the
disposition to use CT skills as part of their definition of CT, (Paul, 1990; Esterle,
1993). Carole Wade explains saying, "Carol Tavris and I ... wanted to get in the
willingness as well as the ability because a person can master CT skills without
being the least bit disposed to use them." (Esterle, 1993). By analogy to the proper-
ties of physical objects, some have argued that just like a cup that breaks must
have been breakable, the fact that a person uses a skill is evidence that the person
is disposed to use that skill, (Ennis, 1994; Perkins, 1993). Perkins and Tishman
include the ability to exercise a given thinking skill as part of the meaning of being
disposed to use that thinking skill (Perkins, 1993).
Hence the obvious first question, one with implications for teaching as well as
for theory: Does demonstrable overall skill at CT correlate positively with the
overall disposition toward CT? A second question goes further: Do any specific
CT skills correlate with specific CT dispositions? Since more or less incompatible
conceptualizations, as well as listings ofCT dispositions, exist in the literature (see
Baron, 1987; Lipman, 1987; Facione and Facione, 1992; Perkins, 1993; Ennis
1996), to clarify these questions let us first consider some commonplace exam-
ples of human dispositions, other than CT.

2. The Nature of Human Dispositions: Experiential Insights


Common experience shows that some people are courageous, others cowardly.
Some are tenacious, others give up too easily. Some are trustworthy, others are
unreliable. Some are more or less compassionate than others. At times we name
The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking 63

both the positive human disposition and its antithesis; at times we speak as if a
human disposition might be stronger in one person than in another.
We think of courage, compassion, trustworthiness and the like as part of a
person's character. Describing someone in terms of their dispositions expresses
the person's habitual ways of acting. For example, an expert soccer player might
be offensive-minded (prone to attack with the intent of scoring goals) or defen-
sive-minded (prone to fall back with the intent of preventing the opponents from
scoring) in her style of play. One parent might be permissive and another authori-
tative in their approach to discipline. A taxi driver might be talkative or sullen. An
administrator, entrepreneurial or risk-averse. These are things people can know
about other people. They are things we can say about ourselves. Knowing a per-
son's dispositions allows us to predict, more or less, how the person is most likely
to act or react in a wide variety of circumstances.
Consider compassion. In saying someone is compassionate, we mean that the
person is sensitive to the needs of others, open to being touched by their suffer-
ing, and moved to act with the intent of ameliorating that suffering. There is a
certain sensitivity, vulnerability, and responsiveness wrapped up in our everyday
understanding of human compassion. This example does not imply that every
human disposition has three elements; rather it shows that key components, if
there are any, of a human disposition may, at times, be open to analysis and under-
standing. But not every compassionate person is a Mother Teresa. Truly saying
that one person is more or less compassionate (reliable, collaborative, aggressive,
studious, self-serving, communicative, easy-going, violent, decisive, playful, de-
ceitful, etc.) than another is revealing. It shows that in everyday life, including in
serious contexts like employment, family relations, and the courts, we have the
ability to discern, comprehend, and compare human dispositional strengths and
weaknesses.
Common human experience supports a theory of human dispositions that would
characterize them as knowable tendencies, readily accessible to description, evalu-
ation, and comparison by oneself and others. Namable and describable as a nexus
of attitudes, intentions, values, and beliefs, one's dispositions are among the distin-
guishing features of one's character or personality. As such, human dispositions
are not mysterious, unaccessible, hidden qualities.
John Dewey described the dispositional aspects of thinking as "personal at-
tributes" (Dewey, 1933). In social psychology a disposition, when the word is
used at all, is generally conceived of as an attitude or attitudinal tendency (Nunnally,
1978; Cronbach, 1990). In the physical sciences, where the word is almost never
used, a disposition is a molecular property that objects manifest under certain
conditions. For example, at a certain air pressure and temperature, hydrogen is
disposed to boil. Ennis takes the physical science approach as paradigmatic (Ennis,
1996). Our research team takes the social science approach, with a nod toward
Dewey, as our starting point. We propose to use the word 'dispositions' as ap-
64 Peter A. Facione

plied to humans to refer to characterological attributes of individuals. As such, a


human disposition is a person's consistent internal motivation to act toward, or to
respond to, persons, events, or circumstances in habitual, and yet potentially mal-
leable, ways.

3. The Correlation of Willing with Able: Experiential Insights


From weeding the garden to filing tax returns, life abounds with examples where
skill exists but not the consistent internal motivation to use those skills. From
writing bestsellers to being an effective leader, there are things some of us are
strongly disposed toward doing but lack the requisite skiIls to accomplish. But
perhaps CT is a special case. After all, when we consider our friends, co-workers,
and family members, how many people do we know who are skilled at CT but
apathetic or disposed not to approach problem solving by thinking? And how
many people do we know who sincerely value and are well disposed to use CT,
but simply do not possess the strong cognitive skills?
Experienced professionals in accounting, counseling, education, engineering,
government service, health care, law, and management probably would not find
such cases difficult to imagine. Unfortunately, there may be far too many exam~
pIes of people who have the talent for skillful thinking but lack the consistent
internal motivation to use that talent. And, like the weak student, far too many
profess a disposition toward thinking but, regretfully, are not capable of generating
much mental horsepower.
To the open-minded, imaginative, and intellectually adventurous, reflection on
human experience can reveal the a rich array of possibilities. Yet, while we might
wish to base our theories only on what we discern through everyday living, that
basis, as the history of science and the history of philosophy show, can result in
less than adequate understandings and misguided programs. To more fully explore
reality one must move beyond comfortable descriptions of common experience
and attempt to test, to the extent possible, one's hypotheses about the world in
well-executed empirical studies.

4. The Empirical Research on the Concept of CT


Efforts to define and measure CT intensified throughout the last quarter of the
Twentieth Century (Kurfiss, 1988; Norris & Ennis, 1989; Jones, 1993). In 1990,
under the sponsorship of the Committee on Pre-College Philosophy of the Ameri-
can Philosophical Association, a cross-disciplinary international panel of 46 ex-
perts completed a two-year, multi-round, strict-method Delphi research project.
which yielded a robust conceptualization of CT for purposes of instruction and
educational assessment.
We understand critical thinking to be purposeful, self-regulatory judgment
which results in interpretation, analysis, evaluation, and inference, as well as
The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking 65

explanation of the evidential, conceptual, methodological, criteriological, or


contextual considerations upon which that judgment is based .... CT is
essential as a tool of inquiry. As such, CT is a liberating force in education
and a powerful resource in one's personal and civic life. . .. While not
synonymous with good thinking, CT is a pervasive and self-rectifying hu-
man phenomenon. (APA, 1990)
Thus conceived, CT was characterized as a self-adjusting process of judging
what to believe or what to do in a given context. In so doing a person engaged in
CT uses a core set of cognitive skills to form that judgment and to monitor and
improve the quality of that judgment (APA, 1990). The successful application of
these core CT skills requires that one take into reasoned consideration the evi-
dence, methods, contexts, theories, and criteria which, in effect, define specific
disciplines, fields, and areas of human concern.
For many years CT testing focused primarily, if not exclusively, on measures
of skills. To give only one paradigmatic example, The California Critical Thinking
Skills Test, introduced in 1990, provides scores on sub-scales named analysis,
inference, evaluation, deductive reasoning, and inductive reasoning (Facione, P.,
et al. 1990, 1998).
Many participants in the Delphi Project and others as well maintain that focus-
ing only on CT skills is not adequate for instructional purposes. John Chaffee
says, "A critical thinker is not only capable of reflecting, exploring, and analyzing
but chooses to think in these advanced, sophisticated ways" (Esterle, 1993). Carol
Tavris says, "To crystallize this spirit of thinking, to develop an understanding that
there are other was of thinking about problems and other ways of thinking about
our own behavior, including our 'feelings.' The question is: How do we get every-
body to actually do it?" (Esterle, 1993).
While many endorse the importance of the critical spirit or the overall disposi-
tion toward CT, few take an empirical approach to measuring that disposition or
exploring the elements of that overall disposition to value and utilize CT (Ennis,
1994; Facione, 1994; Salomon, 1994; Tishman, 1994, Ennis, 1996). My research
colleagues and I decided to explore whether the disposition toward CT could be
measured and analyzed empirically. In our research we characterize the overall
disposition toward CT as the consistent internal motivation to engage problems
and make decisions by using CT (Facione, Facione, & Giancarlo, 1996). In this
we followed the 1990 Delphi research, which determined that while CT per se is
defined as a form of cognition (judgment), to instill in students the disposition
toward CT required an appreciation of the characterological profile one was try-
ing to nurture. For this purpose the Delphi investigation offered a rich description
ofthe ideal critical thinker.
The ideal critical thinker is habitually inquisitive, well-informed, trustful of
reason, open-minded, flexible, fair-minded in evaluation, honest in facing
personal biases, prudent in making judgments, willing to reconsider, clear
about issues, orderly in complex matters, diligent in seeking relevant infor-
66 Peter A. Facione

mation, reasonable in the selection of criteria, focused in inquiry, and persist-


ent in seeking results which are as precise as the subject and the circum-
stances of inquiry permit. (APA, 1990)
Our research team's empirical refinement of the Delphi concept of the ideal
critical thinker using the methods of experimental psychology, which are described
below, began with the development of the California Critical Thinking Disposi-
tion Inventory (CCTDI) (Facione & Facione, ] 992).
Unexpected support for the validity ofthe Delphi concept ofCT, which grounds
the CCTDI and the CCTST, came in the form of independent research sponsored
by the United States Department of Education. Seeking to understand the Goals
2000 legislation, the Department of Education initiated a national survey of hun-
dreds of educators, employers, and policy-makers to determine their priorities
with regard to the communication and thinking skills expected of all college gradu-
ates (Jones, et al. ] 994). The National Center for Post-secondary Teaching, Learn-
ing, and Assessment used the APA Delphi Research listing of CT skills and the
seven dispositional factors identified in the development of the CCTDI as the
expressions of CT for purposes of the national survey. The results, published in
] 995, found strong national consensus that CT dispositions (and skills), charac-
terized generally as expressed in the APA 1990 Delphi Report, should be essential
outcomes of a college education (Jones, et al. 1995).
The 1990 Delphi description of the ideal critical thinker was further tested by
Giancarlo (1996) using the California Q-sort method (Block, 1961). Twenty na-
tionally recognized experts in adult CT individually sorted the 100 Q-sort items so
that the result would characterize, in their opinions, the ideal critical thinker. The
twenty were merged to form a prototype personality profile of the ideal critical
thinker (r=.80, N=20, p<.OO 1). [This shorthand means that the correlation "r"
which can range from -1.00 to + 1.00 was a very strong .80; the number of people
sampled "N" was 20; and the probability of this result having occurred merely by
chance "p" was less than 1 time out of 1000.] This prototype correlated with
undergraduate students = CCTDI scores (r=.36, N=91, p<.OOl) and with CCTDI
and Q-sort data collected about those students from their peers (r=.32, N=91,
p<.OO 1). These findings further confirm the strength of the 1990 Delphi consen-
sus description of the ideal critical thinker and they support the use of CCTDI as
a valid measurement tool relative to that definition.
Before we can cite empirical evidence in response to the question about the
possible correlation of CT skills and the disposition toward CT, we must first be
sure that we have valid and reliable measures of both. Section 5 below describes
the process of tool development. Section 6 identifies the specific CT skills and
the elements of the disposition toward CT that are described by the two measures
used to gather the data cited in this paper.
The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking 67

5. Tool Development, Validity and Reliability


The process for developing valid and reliable tools to measure cognitive skills and
dispositions in given population has been an established part of social and behavioral
science research methodology since the middle ofthe Twentieth Century (Nunnally,
1978; Cronbach, 1990; Miller, 1991; Rosenthal and Rosnow, 1991). Decades of
research undergirds the now familiar empirical paper and pencil methods used to
gather data about a person's cognitive aptitudes and skills (e.g. multiple choice
tests), and measures of a person's values, beliefs, intentions, opinions, attitudes,
or inclinations (using the Likert style "agree-disagree" prompt). While there are
potentially other valid and reliable ways to gather good educational assessment
data, the use of a well-developed paper and pencil measure remains one funda-
mentally sound and sensible approach.
No approach to educational outcomes assessment is immune from the prob-
lems of establishing validity and reliability. Beyond the rigors of tool development
and the challenges of executing a well-conceived assessment design, there are
other problems: bad data resulting from the use of poorly developed tools or poorly
executed educational assessment programs, honest concerns about the possible
culture-bias or gender-bias of some instruments, appreciation of the test-taking
problems of students with learning disabilities, misuse of modest testing results to
bolster proposals for substantial changes in educational or employment policies,
the national politics around high stakes educational testing, etc. It is no wonder
that talk of testing leads quickly to many worries, some of which are quite reason-
able.
The process of developing a good educational assessment tool of any kind
begins with the construct, or idea, that one seeks to measure. The construct-
validity of the instrument depends on how well that idea has been articulated and
how well the tool captures that idea.The Delphi Report provided a wonderful
opportunity for tool development, since it expressed a consensus construct of CT.
The conceptualization used to structure an instrument, thus, was much more than
simply a single individual's notion of CT. Further, the Delphi Report went well
beyond a brief definition, for it offered lists of core CT skills and sub-skills and a
robust expression of the positive side of the dispositional aspect of CT in its de-
scription of the ideal critical thinking.
The basic idea behind the design of educational assessment tools is that of
representative sampling. The tasks, items, questions, used are developed as repre-
sentative of the learning one is seeking to measure. Regardless of how valid it
might or might not appear to be, typically, no one item on a test stands alone.
Rather, it is the overall collection of items that is conceived of as covering the
various elements that are most central to that learning. Gradations in item difficulty
allow comparisons between those who have achieved a greater mastery of the
learning and those who have a lesser mastery. So, if everyone is getting an item
right or if everyone is getting it wrong, the item is probably of Iittle value, since it
68 Peter A. Faciane

is failing to differentiate and might as well be eliminated. Another indication that an


item should not be used is that students with a poorer overall mastery of the
material tend to do better on that item than do students with a stronger mastery.
With the idea of representative sampling in mind, the next step in tool develop-
ment is to write candidate items. In the case of a skills test, like the CCTST, they
should be questions that are intended to elicit the use CT skills by persons in the
target population to arrive at the designated answer. The items on the CCTST
emerged from teaching CT since 1967 to thousands of students, having developed
and used for that purpose many hundreds of educational skills test items, and
having discussed with students their understanding and interpretation of the items.
In the case of personality measures, like the CCTOI, one approach is to solicit
"agree-disagree" responses to statements that vocalize or express the personal
attitudes, intentions, values, and beliefs that are either central to or the antithesis of
the disposition toward CT (Nunnally, 1978; Cronbach, 1990; Rosenthal and Rosnow,
1991). In 1990, using the rich description in the Oelphi Report, our research team
began the development of the CCTOI with the articulation of 225 statements ex-
pressive of one's disposition toward or away from using CT.
Before presenting a measurement tool, candidate test items are validated using
a qualitative method involving talk-aloud and conversational strategies with per-
sons in the target population. Many candidate test items are discarded or their
wording is modified in this process. Typically this is done through focus group
conversations with special attention paid to the exact phrasing persons in the tar-
get population use to express the attitudes, values, intentions, and beliefs (Triandis,
1980, Ferketich et al. 1993). This strategy provides a second form of validation,
for it establishes whether or not the persons in the target population understand the
test items in the way intended.
The next step is to pilot test the instrument in the target population. The pilot
version often contains more items than will eventually be retained. We used 150 at
this stage in developing the CCTOI. Statistical analyses of the responses of a
sufficiently large and representative sample of test-taker allows for the elimination
of items that fail adequately to discriminate among test-takers, items where the
responses are inversely correlated with the overall scores on the test, and, in the
interest of brevity, items that added little or nothing by way of further refinement
of overall scores. This step yields the final tool, which is both more efficient and
more reliable. In its final form the CCTDI includes 75 items. No single item, no
matter how valid it mayor may not appear on its face, can be thought of as
standing entirely on its own, for it is always possible that a given test taker
interpreted a given item in a way other than the one the test-maker intended.
A test is valid ifit measures the thing it is intended to measure. For example, a
bathroom scale measures weight. A test is reliable if it consistently gives the same
results. (Cronbach, 1990). For example, a bathroom scale that gives different
weights ~ach time you stand on it, is not a reliable measure. All tests admit of many
The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking 69

potential sources of measurement error. A statistic that expresses the overall reli-
ability of the test is called the Cronbach-alpha. A test that attempts to measure
many different things where it is reasonable to expect that some students might
have mastered some parts of the material but not other parts, will have a lower
reliability. A test aimed at a clear, singular construct such that mastering that ma-
terial would necessitate mastering all of its parts, should be expected, ifit is a good
test, to have an Cronbach-alpha in the neighborhood .80. Tests with alphas below
.50 are suspect. Consider the construct "CT." To the extent that CT is a list of
desperate skills, where it is possible to be good at some (say interpretation and
analysis) but not others (say evaluation or explanation), what sort of alpha should
we expect of a test of CT skills? Ennis and Norris at one time suggested .67
(Norris, 1989). To the extent that the disposition toward CT is not a singular thing,
but actually a long list of 7, 12, 72 or more different dispositions and correlative
dispositions, such that a person might have some and not others, one might won-
der what an acceptable alpha should be for a measure of large sets of CT disposi-
tions.
But there is a different approach. One that begins with the overall idea, and
goes to constituent elements a posteriori. Given a valid and reliable test of a given
construct, one can statistically explore that construct to see if there are different
variables or factors working within it. In other words, instead of offering a priori,
speculative analyses and lists of putative candidates, one can use factor analytic
techniques on test data to achieve an a posteriori analysis. Factor analysis is "a
broad category of approaches to conceptualizing groupings (or clusterings) of
variables and an even broader collection of mathematical procedures for determin-
ing which variables belong to which groups" (Nunnally, 1978, p. 327). Factor-
analytic techniques can yield lists of test items that cluster together because they
are the responses to those items are so highly correlated with one another. By
examining the test items in a given cluster, one can discern the conceptual meaning
of that factor. Factors can thus be described and, if desired, named.
Each sub-set of items that form the factors on a given test can be thought of as
a sub-scale which measures one of the elements of the overall construct that the
test as a whole is measuring. The Cronbach-alphas of each sub-scale can be
calculated. Often items are eliminated from the overall test so that the alpha of a
given sub-scale can be increased. Because the sub-scales contain relatively few
items as compared to the overall test, this usually does no serious damage to the
alpha ofthe overall test. In this way scores can be reported as an overall result and
in terms of sub-scales (Nunnally, 1978; Cronbach, 1990). This is how the seven
scales for the CCTDI emerged, (Facione, 1992; Facione, N., Facione, P. &
Giancarlo, 1994).
The question of validity can be expressed this way, "To what extent does a
given test measure the right thing, namely the construct it purports to measure?"
Starting with a well-articulated, research-based definition of the construct is very
70 Peter A. Faciane

important. Having independent confirmation ofthe validity of that definition gives


further confidence that one has embarked on the right path. Then, based on under-
standing and experience, carefully writing well-targeted candidate items advances
the process. Validating those items qualitatively, by asking representative mem-
bers of the test's target population of test-takers how they interpret or understand
the candidate items further strengthens test validity. If these earlier steps have
been well executed so that the tool is focused on the right target, pilot testing the
instrument and eliminating individual items that negatively correlate with the over-
all instrument scores contributes to our confidence in the validity of the remaining
items. All of these steps were used in the development of the CCTST and the
CCTDI.
After a test is developed, other support for its validity can be derived by
examining the correlation of results of that test with previously existing, valid
measures of the same construct. High correlations of CCTST scores with the
analytical and verbal sections of the Graduate Record Exam, for example, suggest
that the two are measuring very similar constructs (See Table I). As indicated, the
Giancarlo Q-Sort research lends additional conceptual and experimental support to
the validity ofthe CCTDI. Additional independent support for the two testing tools
can be found in studies that compare them to other commercial tests (Pendarvis,
1996).
Table 1
CCTST Correlations with Other Measures
Variable N Group p-value
GRETotal .719 143 A <.001
GRE Analytic .708 143 A <.001
GREVerbsal .716 143 A <.001
GRE Quantitative .582 143 A <.001
ACT .402 446 B <.001
CCTDITotal .41 193 D <.05
CCTDITotal .201 1557 B <.001
Watson-Glaser CTA .405 139 B <.001
Watson-Glaser CTA .544 65 C <.001
SAT Verbal .545 123 B <.001
SAT Verbal .55 333 E <.001
SAT Math .422 123 B <.001
SAT Math .44 333 E <.001
CollegeGPA .20 473 E <.001
Nelson-Denny .49 42 E <.001
Age -.006 479 E .449
Units Earned .03 473 E .262
A - Nursing Graduate Students - CCTST D English Speaking 10th Graders - CCTST
B ~ Nursing Students CCTST Entry E = 1990 Validation Study CCTST Pretest
C Nursing Students- CCTST Exit
The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking 71

Even if the tool one is using is both reliable and valid, the practical context
within which social science research occurs presents several potential pitfalls to
arriving at a valid and reliable repeatable result in any given study. Educational
assessment of learning outcomes is a prime example. The context is replete with
circumstances that can confound our measurement efforts. For example, one
worry with low-stakes tests of cognitive skills are that test-takers may not be
motivated to give their best efforts. For a variety of reasons students might guess
at the items, perhaps arriving at the correct answers but not by thinking them
through properly. One worry with "agree-disagree" response tests is that test-
wise persons will discern the desired response and mark that choice even if it is
not their honest response.
While worries, like social-desirability bias, cannot be overlooked, in behavioral
science research these concerns are not necessarily insurmountable. Text books
on research methods in the social sciences and educational testing discuss offer
strategies for discovering whether a given potential threat is of genuine concern in
a given case. They suggest ways to minimize, if not neutralize, those threats that
are of concern. For example, one way to determine whether a given test is in fact
being compromised because test-takers are simply responding with the social-
desirable answer is to use the Crowne-Marlowe Social Desirability Scale (Crowne,
1960; Rosenthal, 1991). For example, ten items used on the CCTDI were tested
with 902 women aged 18 through 90, and the responses to those items were
found to have no relationship to social desirability (F acione, N. et ai., 1998).
In social science testing, the context can make a major difference. The above
evidence was gathered in the context of women responding to a health survey
concerning their attitudes toward help-seeking for breast cancer symptoms. There
is no evidence that the women did not answer truthfully. On the other hand, a
Japanese translation of the CCTDI used with Japanese engineering and nursing
students appears vulnerable to problems of social desirability. In Japanese culture
tateme is responding to questions by saying what a person should do or should be.
Honne is responding by saying what a person really wants to do or to be (July
1999, conversations with Professors Kiyoko Makimoko and Ariko Noji at UCSF.)
Their translation of some CCTDI items into Japanese appears to elicits the tateme
response. To improve the performance of the Japanese version of the CCTDI items
must be phrased to evoke the honne response.
The most telling evidence that the CCTDI is not significantly subject to social-
disability bias when used with North Americans is the surprisingly consistent and
unflatteringly low score that emerge in response to the Truth-Seeking sub-scale.
We describe truth-seeking as the courageous desire for best knowledge in any
given situations, asking the tough questions, and being willing to follow reasons
and evidence wherever they lead even if the result is contrary to one's own pre-
conceptions and interests. The socially desirable responses to some of the items
on that scale are so blatant that faculty frequently are amused by the item's candor.
How could anyone fail to know that the desired response, particularly in the con-
72 Peter A. Facione

text of higher education, would be to disagree with statements like: "To get people
to agree with me I would give any reason that worked." "Everyone always argues
from their own self-interest, including me." "If there are four reasons in favor and
one against, I'll go with the four." However, with a national mean on this scale
falling solidly in the ambivalent range, it is clear from tens of thousands of cases
that college students, whether or not they know the desired response, assert that
they agree with these sorts of statements.
Frankly, it is not the validity of the test thatworries those of us who have seen
these data over the past seven years, rather it is the repeated evidence of ambiva-
lence toward truth-seeking that most worries us.

6. Measuring Dispositions and Skills


Skills are manifest in performance. Persons with stronger skills tend to be able to
perform a range of tasks requiring those skills with fewer mistakes. Lesser skilled
persons make more errors on those same tasks. There are many ways to assess
CT, including using performance appraisals, rating forms, rubrics, and portfolios
(Facione & Facione, 1996a; Facione and Facione, 1996b).Research on psycho-
logical and educational testing indicates that well crafted multiple choice tests can
validly and reliably measure higher order cognitive skills (Haldyna, 1994). The ball
diagram with the names of the core CT skills is a useful reminder that we do not
know the order in which one uses those skills in forming the judgment about what
to believe or what to do. It also is a reminder that CT skills can apply not only to
the question or evidence at hand, but to the products of the work of other CT
skills. For example, we can interpret a set of data. We can analyze or evaluate that
interpretation We can explain the analysis of that evaluation, or, using self-regula-
tion, correct that analysis or that interpretation. We can revise the correction,
interpret the analysis, etc. Furthermore, in this process we can revise our interpre-
tation of the standards of evaluation, we can explain the problem in different ways,
and we can re-evaluate our methodological assumptions. At times the complexi-
ties of good CT are evident when CT is carried on by groups, as when a manage-
ment team is considering a business problem. Observation of the progression (and
digressions) of the group's thinking, as manifest by what the participants say to
one another during the deliberations, can show that sound, reflective judgments
often come about in non-linear ways. Because there is so much we do not yet
know about the cognitive complexity of CT in practice, instruction or assessment
that emphasizes the exercise of a single CT skill in isolation from the possible
influence of any of the others is problematic.
On the CCTST higher scores indicates greater skill at CT. Each CCTST item
requires the test taker to form a judgment about the best response, from among
those provided, to a question involving a more or less everyday situation or prob-
lem. No specialized knowledge is required, other than what can reasonably be
expected of a person who has received a modest high school education and can
The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking 73

Core Critical Thinking Skills


I
ANALYSIS CRITICAL INFERENCE

INTERPRETAnON THINKING EXPLANATION

SELF-REGULA nON EVALUATION

read at a 10th grade level. No technical vocabulary is used. To consistently answer


correctly, the person must be skilled at inductive and deductive reasoning and at
making correct analyses, inferences, and evaluations. The CCTST is a challenging
test of CT, but it is not a test about CT (Facione et al., 1990, 1998).Thus, a
person's overall skill at CT can be arrayed along a continuum of test scores,
which, in tum, permits statistical analyses, such as correlations. This provides a
way to gather empirical data with regard to hypotheses such as CT skill is corre-
lated with age, number of college credits earned, OPA, or scores on other educa-
tional or psychological tests. [Table I on p. 70 presents some of the correlations
discovered to date.]
We characterized the disposition toward CT as the consistent internal motiva-
tion to use CT skills to decide what to believe and what to do. Some approach
problems confident in their own ability to reason them through; others mistrust
themselves as decision-makers, thinkers, or problem-solvers. Some people are
open-minded about divergent ideas; others intolerant. Some are inclined to ap-
proach problems in diligent, focused, and systematic ways; others tend to be
scattered, disorganized, and easily distracted. Some seek for evidence and reasons
as they consider what to do; others eschew data and principled approaches, pre-
ferring rather to decide on the basis of impulse, whim, fashion, pressure, or ca-
price. Some seek answers as objectively as possible; others seem unable to step
past personal bias, fear, self-interest, or preconception. Some see the complexity
and subtlety of problems, noting multiple possible resolutions; others see things in
stark, dualistic terms, as good or bad, right or wrong, true or false. Some people
are curious as to the workings of things, wanting to know more about the prob-
lem than simply how to solve it; others are content just to know what to do, and
prefer not deal with why or what if.
In general, people might be either positively or negatively disposed with regard
to CT. And a third possibility is that they might not have formed a strong habit one
way or the other; that is, they might be rather ambivalent in their tendencies or
inclinations. Thus, one desirable feature of any measure of the overall CT disposi-
tion would be that it reflect the extent to which a person might be positive,
ambivalent, or negative toward the use of CT.
Seven elements or aspects of the overall disposition toward CT emerged when
statistical factor analytic techniques were applied in the initial development of the
CCTDI. In their positive manifestations, these seven characterological attributes
74 Peter A. Facione

were given the names truth-seeking, open-mindedness, analyticity, systematicity,


CT self-confidence, inquisitiveness, and maturity ofjudgment (Facione & Facione,
1992). Seven intellectual bad habits can be thought of as the negative poles of each

The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking

Inquisitive
Systematic Judicious
Analytical Truthseeking
Open-minded Confident in Reasoning

intellectual virtue. The antithesis of the ideal would be a person who habitually
approached problems, ideas, decisions, or issues being intellectually dishonest
(e.g. in the use of data), intolerant (e.g. of opposing ideas), inattentive (e.g. to
implications of proposals), haphazard (e.g. procedurally), mistrustfol of reason
(e.g. hostile toward sound scientific inquiry), indifferent (e.g. toward new find-
ings), and simplistic (e.g. naively dualistic). In the final analysis, CT is about
reflectively making sound judgments. So, while the intellectual virtues are obvi-
ously assets, the intellectual vices are even more obviously liabilities.The picture of
the group talking with the names of the disposition sub-scales suggests that we
can characterize the deliberations of groups of people, not just individuals, using
these positive or negative attributions. Using 75 Likert style items to measure the
seven characteristics and the overall disposition toward CT, the CCTDI gives us a
means of gathering evidence that profiles the CT dispositions of individuals and
groups.

7. The Hypotheses to be Tested


For purposes of empirical exploration, the two questions motivating this essay
should be expressed as null hypotheses. Hypothesis #1: There is no correlation,
positive or negative, between the persons' overall disposition toward CT and their
strength or weakness in the CT skills. Hypothesis #2: No specific CT disposition
is correlated, positively or negatively, with any specific CT skill. Using the CCTST
to measure the skills and the CCTDI to measure the dispositions, the potential
exists to gather some useful data that would tend to confirm or to disconfirm one
or both of these null hypotheses.

8. Findings: Tenth Graders, Accounting and Nursing Professionals,


and College Students
One early study provided evidence in support of a positive correlation between
skills and dispositions at the macro level. It was a study of 10th grade high school
The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking 75

students in a school district in the Southwestern United States. In a sample of 193


students a positive correlation of r=.41 was found between CCTDI total scores
and CCTST total scores (Giancarlo & Facione, N., 1994).This correlation was
statistically significant at the p<.05 level, which tends to disconfirm the first null
hypothesis and support the theory that there is a positive correlation between
overall CT skill and CT disposition.
A statistically significant correlation, if squared, describes the percentage of
the variance in one element of the correlation that might be attributable to the
variance in the other element. Thus a .41 positive correlation between overall CT
skill and the overall disposition toward CT suggests that up to 16.8% of the vari-
ance in CT skills test scores is potentially attributable to the differences in these
students' CT disposition test scores. The same could be said the other way, that as
much as 16.8% of the variance in CT disposition test scores is potentially attrib-
utable to the differences in these students' CT skiIls test scores. In other words,
of all the many different things that could explain the students' performance on
one of these two tests, up to 16.8% might, if we knew how, be explained by
attribution to the element measured by the other of the two tests.
A 1998 unpublished analysis by statisticians at the California Academic Press
of a dataset composed of 133 accounting professionals' scores showed an excel-
lent mean CCTST score of21. 7 and a positive overall CCTDI means on all seven
scales. Yet the correlation of overall skills test scores and overall disposition test
scores as measured in this group of professionals with these instruments was only
.091. Regardless of sample size, a number so close to zero would be interpreted as
showing that there is virtually no correlation of skills and dispositions in this group,
(Blohm, 1998). A study of 328 nursing undergraduates showed a statistically sig-
nificant correlation (p <.01) of .318. This suggests that in this sample about 9% of
the variance in skills test scores can be associated with (social scientists would
say "explained by") the variance in overall CT dispositions (ColuccieIlo, 1997).
By late 1997 sufficient data from multiple sites had been gathered to address
both hypotheses in more than the preliminary ways mentioned above. Beginning in
1992 and on through the summer of 1997 data was collected by on-site investiga-
tors at 50 college level programs of nursing throughout the United States. The
aggregate dataset, comprising 145 predominantly undergraduate samples, when
completed, included information on 7,926 students. Students in this dataset ranged
from entry level freshmen through graduate students, and included working adults
returning to college, as well as traditionaIlyBaged undergraduates. To the best of
this author's knowledge, this aggregate dataset is the largest collected to date
within which it is possible to analyze potential relationships between CT skills, CT
dispositions, and a variety of other academic, institutional, and demographic vari-
ables. And so, it was to this dataset that we turned in search of more substantial
answers (Facione & Facione, 1997).
As with the earlier study of 10th graders, the national aggregate nursing stu-
dent dataset leads us to reject the first null hypothesis. Analyses showed positive
76 Peter A. Facione

correlations between the overall disposition toward CT and strength in CT skills.


For example, 1557 nursing students completed both the CCTST and the CCTDI at
entry to their college programs. The correlation of the total scores on the CCTST
and the CCTDI in this 1557 person aggregation was r=.201, p<.OOI, (Facione &
Facione, 1997). Figure 1 below is a graphic plotting of the correlation. Visually a
strong positive correlation would be manifest more or less as a line running diago-
nally upward from the lower left corner to the upper right.The cloud-like splotch
visible on Figure 1 helps illustrate how weak the correlation is in this large data set.

Figure 1
Correlation of Students' CT Skills and Overall Disposition Toward CT at Program Entry
1557 College Level Nursing Students Represented (r = .201. P < .001 )

400~-------------------------------------------,

300

c
200 c
c cC c c c
c c
UJ
I
a....
8 100~________- r_ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _~_ _ _ _ _ _ _ _~_ _ _ _ _ _ _ _~
o 10 20 30 40

While statistically significant, we should be cautious not to over-interpret this


finding. A correlation of r=.201 tells us that only about 4% of the variance in CT
skills test scores potentially can be attributed to or associated with the variance of
these college students' CT dispositions test scores, or visa versa. In other words,
using these two measurement tools, 96% of the variance in CT skills in this sample
of 1557 college students is not to be associated with or understood in terms of
their CT disposition.
793' students, completed both the CT testing tools at exit from their nursing
programs in the different colleges and universities.Again a positive correlation
The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking 77

(r=.169, p <.001) was evident (Facione & Facione, 1997). Figure 2 below plots
this correlation. In Figure 2, the data points form a rather defuse cloud with only
a hint of the diagonal. As with the entry level findings, however, the discovery of
this statistical significant relationship should not lead us to believe that we have
something of practical significance, for it fails to account for 97% of the variance.
In other words, 97% of the difference between college students' CT disposition at
program exit cannot be explained by reference to their CT skills at program exit,
and vice versa.

Figure 2
Correlation of Students' CT Skills and Overall Disposition Toward CT at Program Exit
=
793 College Level Nursing Students Represented (r .169, P < .001)

50·~------------------------------------~

40G o o
o o
o
o

o
o
30G o

o DO B

20(__------~--------~------~------~
o 10 20 30 40

The evidence from the entry and the exit data indicates even more clearly that
the preponderance of the variance in CT skills is not potentially associated or
attributable to variance in the overall disposition toward CT. The caveats for that
generalization include: as CT is defined by the consensus construct developed in
1990, to the extent that the construct is measured validly and reliably by the two
CT instruments used, insofar as the various studies aggregated into this analysis
78 Peter A. Faciane

were conducted appropriately, and to the extent that the college nursing student
sample is representative of anything other than itself.
Interestingly, and perhaps as experienced instructors would expect, a some-
what stronger relationship was shown between the students' disposition score on
the CCTDI at program entry and their skills test score on the CCTST at exit
(r=.233, p<.OO 1) (Facione & Facione, 1997). Those students with a stronger
initial disposition toward CT showed greater development in CT skills by the end
of their studies than did those with a weaker initial disposition toward CT.
The second question, whether any specific CT skill (e.g. analysis, or inductive
reasoning) is correlated one-to-one with any specific CT dispositional factor (e.g.
open-mindedness, truth-seeking, or systematicity), can be addressed by the data
from the national aggregate sample. Depending on the possible pairings of skills
and dispositional factors, there were between 1325 and 1428 students' cases to
analyze statistically. With five CT skills and seven dispositional factors being tested,
there are 35 possible pairings to consider. In thirty-three ofthe thirty-five possible
relationships explored higher CT skills were generally correlated with stronger CT
dispositions; but in all cases these correlations were very weak, never stronger
than. 194. Only the relationships between CT-self confidence and analysis that and
between CT-self confidence and evaluation did not reach the threshold for statis-
tical significance (Facione & Facione, 1997).
These findings tend to disconfirm the supposition that there is a one-to-one
relationship between each specific CT skill and its supposed correlative disposition
(as measured by the CCTST and the CCTDI). For if that were true, we would
expect to find the skill of analysis, for example, to be strongly correlated to the
disposition of analyticity, and not to the other six dispositional factors; and we
would have expected to find analyticity to be strongly correlated only with skill in
analysis, and not with the other skills.
In the light of these November 1997 empirical findings, it may be unwise to
advance a theory that explicitly or implicitly pairs one and only one CT skill in a
positive correlation with one and only one CT dispositional factor. While Perkins
and Tishman did suggest such an approach, Ennis argued common sense grounds
that it is both implausible and impractical (Perkins and Tishman, 1993; Ennis,
1996).The data presented here support Ennis' contention.
More importantly, what are we to make of the fact that skill at CT does not
appear to be correlated with the disposition toward CT? What do findings like
these imply for teaching CT and for the hope, widely shared among faculty, that
disposition toward CT would assure that CT skills be used beyond the classroom?

9. Nurturing the Disposition and Building the Skin


Leading figures in the history of CT theory, persons who deserve the greatest
credit for shaping the field, for bringing it international attention, and for leading
The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking 79

educational reforms toward a pedagogy based on teaching for and about thinking,
have advanced theories that hypothesize a link between CT skills with CT disposi-
tions. As was seen in the 1990 Delphi Research, some feel so strongly about the
importance of CT dispositions that they want to include the disposition toward CT
as part of the very meaning of 'critical thinking' (APA, 1990; Paul, 1990). The
majority of 46 Delphi Research theoreticians, however, rejected this way of defin-
ing 'critical thinking.' The view that the majority espoused was more or less that
the positive human characteristics of the ideal critical thinker are distinguishable
from the critical thinking process itself. (APA, 1990). Many worried that a person
might be skilled at CT but not fair-minded, and perhaps even unethical, in the use
of those skills. Sensing in the impressive list of characteristics of the ideal critical
thinker the potential for people to confuse "good CT" with "being a good (ethical)
person," the majority of the participating theoreticians preferred a sharper bound-
ary between ethics and CT. Some draw it very crisply precisely in order to distin-
guish between teaching skill at CT as contrasted with motivating students to use
that skill in a consistent way (Fisher and Scriven, 1997).
In presenting a conceptualization of the ideal critical thinker, theoreticians are
promoting a set of values relating to one's intellectual character, what might be
called "intellectual virtues." In 1988 and 1989, the years when the Delphi research
was being conducted, the participating theoreticians did not take the theory of CT
dispositions much further, except that they did speculate that we would discover a
strong positive correlation between CT skill and the disposition toward CT. Using
the instruments currently available, that has not yet happened.
Skill and disposition are two separate things in people. Employers and educa-
tors prize both (Facione, Facione, & Giancarlo, 1996). A developmental perspec-
tive suggests that skills and dispositions are mutually reinforcing; and, hence,
should be explicitly taught and modeled together (Kitchener & King, 1995). Com-
mon sense tells us that a strong overall disposition toward CT is integral to insur-
ing the use of CT skills outside the narrow instructional setting. Motivational theory
(Lewin, 1935) provides the theoretical grounds for the assumption that the dispo-
sition to value and utilize CT would impel an individual to achieve mastery over CT
skills, being motivated to close the gap between what is valued and what is at-
tained.
At the most practical level, learning follows motivation. We are best at learning
what we most need and want to know. Thus, engendering the desire to use CT as
a favored means of problem solving and decision making prepares the ground for
teaching and learning the CT skills. Perhaps it is this linkage between motivation
and learning that responds best to the insight that CT skill and the disposition to
use CT should come together in practical and important ways. Speculation about
a strong positive correlation does not capture the insight. But to assert that there
is a pragmatic connection of tremendous value to learning and to living is not a
philosophical mistake. In fact, the success of the entire enterprise of using reason,
80 Peter A. Facione

rather than myths, magic, and mystery, is predicated on the pragmatic insight that
we must nurture the dispositions even as we teach the skills.

Critical Thinking Pedagogy


Problem-Framing and Problem-Solving
• Model CT Skills and Dispositions
• Reward good CT
• Challenge poor CT
• Create a Climate of Reasoned Inquiry
• Diversify Judgment Contexts
• Engage Students in CT
• Reflect on the Use ofCT

If the disposition toward CT can be described as an intellectual virtue, perhaps


the analogy can be extended a bit and, without minimizing the centrality of devel-
oping CT students' CT skills, teaching CT could be thought of as including the
building intellectual character. We worry that our educational programs at all lev-
els, from K-12 through college and into adult and professional development, will
fail ifthey focus only on skills to the neglect ofthe consistent internal motivation to
use those skills in the appropriate circumstances. While necessary, it will not be
sufficient only to strengthen students' skills on how to interpret, analyze, infer,
explain, or evaluate. To teach for thinking, one must nurture truth-seeking, open-
mindedness, analyticity, systematicity, intellectual curiosity, confidence in the proper
use of reasons and evidence, and maturity of judgment.

If we were compelled to make a choice between these personal attributes


and some degree of technical skill in manipulating special logical
processes, we should decide for the former. (John Dewey, \933)

Even though each person's CT ability has limits, we can still develop our skills
to the extent that our ability permits. We can also expand the arena's within which
we are adept at using our skills. CT is not limited in its content. Rather it is the
process of judging, in a reflective way, what to do or what to believe. Thus its
connections to professional judgement, and to thinking about problems and ques-
tions in science, ethics, and civic life are straightforward. Teaching for and about
CT includes broadening the range of kinds of problems and decision making con-
texts within and about which we are willing and able to exercise our CT. In
everyday situations and in every domain of knowledge or professional practice,
good CT involves attending to the contexts, theories, methods, evidence, and stand-
ards within which problems are framed and decisions formed.
We would suggest that to be maximally effective teaching for and about CT
should be aimed at expanding the opportunities as well as the content areas with
The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking 81

which to engage in informed, reflective problem framing and problem solving.


One powerful tool for nurturing the disposition toward CT in students and co-
workers is by modeling it. Stephen Brookfield says, "something ... has been
absent from a lot of [the] work on how you teach CT, and that is the crucial
importance of educators modeling it in their own actions" (Esterle. 1993). I f we
are not truth-seeking, open-minded, and the rest, in our thinking with students,
colleagues, and family about problems and decisions, is it not unreasonable of us
to expect more of them?

10. Conclusion
We can trace western science and philosophy to the ancient Greeks who created a
culture based on the commercial and civic utility ofusing reason to solve problems
and make decisions. For them the intellectual virtues were as important as the
civic and physical virtues. Although much of the educational and corporate rheto-
ric today is fueled by a pervasive and uncritical faith in high technology as an
educational and economic panacea, wiser voices still whisper that information and
skills alone cannot guarantee success in the workplace or in school. People must
also be disposed to use what they have learned. Educational and professional
success require developing one's thinking skills and nurturing one's consistent in-
ternal motivation to use those skills. To imagine a powerful positive automatic
correlation between CT skills and CT dispositions actually undermines the task at
hand. Ifwe want our students to be both willing and able to engage in CT, and we
do, then we have to include both in school and professional development cur-
ricula, in our instructional assignments, and in our educational outcomes assess-
ments. Why? Because being skilled does not assure one is disposed to use CT.
And, being disposed toward CT does not assure that one is skilled.

References
American Philosophical Association. (1990). Critical Thinking: A Statement of Expert
Consensus for Purposes of Educational Assessment and Instruction. ERIC docu-
ment ED 315-423.
Baron, J. (1987). Evaluating thinking skills in the classroom. In Baron, J. and Sternberg,
R., (eds.), Teaching Thinking Skills: Theory and Practice, 221-47. New York: W. H.
Freeman and Company.
Block, J. (l961).The Q-sort Method in Personality Assessment and Psychiatric Re-
search. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Blohm, S. (1998). Personal correspondence with the author, June.
Colucciello, M. Critical thinking skills and dispositions of Baccalaureate nursing stu-
dents-A conceptual model for evaluation. Journal of Professional Nursing, 13
(4),23645.
82 Peter A. Faciane

Cronbach, L. (1990) Essentials of Psychological Testing. New York: Harper & Row.
Crowne, D. & Marlowe D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of
psychopathology. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 24 (4), 349-54.
Dewey, J. (1933). How We Think: A Restatement of the Relation of Reflective Thinking
to the Educational Process. Lexington, MA: Heath Publishing. Originally published
in 1910.
Ennis, R. (1987). A taxonomy of critical thinking dispositions and abilities. In Baron, J.
and Sternberg, R., (eds.), Teaching Thinking Skills: Theory and Practice. 9-26. New
York: W. H. Freeman and Company.
Ennis, R. (1994). Assessing critical thinking dispositions: Theoretical considerations,
(DRAFT). Presented to the American Educational Research Association Meetings,
New Orleans, LA.
Ennis, R. (1996). Critical thinking dispositions: Their nature and assessability. Informal
Logic, 18 (2&3),165-82.
Esterle, J. & Clurman, D. (1993). Conversations with Critical Thinkers. San Francisco:
The Whitman Institute.
Facione, N., Dodd, M., Miaskowski, c., Paul, S. & Giancarlo, C. (1998). How women
decide to seek evaluation of breast symptoms. (Breast cancer research progress
grant I KB0045.) Paper presented at the International Conference on Cancer Nurs-
ing. Jerusalem, Israel. August.
Facione, N. & Facione, P. (I 996a). Externalizing the critical thinking in knowledge devel-
opment and clinical judgment. Nursing Outlook, 44, 129-36.
Facione, N. & Facione, P. (1996b). Assessment design issues for evaluating critical
thinking in nursing. Holistic Nursing Practice, 10, 44-5\.
Facione, N. & Facione, P. (1997). Critical Thinking Assessment in Nursing Education
Programs: An Aggregate Data Analysis. Millbrae, CA: The California Academic
Press.
Facione, N., Facione, P. & Giancarlo (Sanchez), C. (1994) Critical thinking disposition as
a measure of competent cl inical judgment: The development of the Cal ifornia Critical
Thinking Disposition Inventory. Journal of Nursing Education, 33, (8), 345-350.
Facione, P., et at. (1990, 1998). The California Critical Thinking Skills Test (CCTST):
Forms A and B; and the CCTST Test manual. Millbrae, CA: California Academic
Press.
Facione, P. (1992). Empirical methods of theory and tool development for the assess-
ment of college level critical thinking and problem-solving. Paper prepared for the
Second Department of Education, NCES Study Design Conference on Collegiate
Skills Assessment, Washington D.C.
Facione, P. & Facione, N. (1992). The California Critical Thinking Dispositions Inventory
(CCTDI); and the CCTDI Test manual. Millbrae, CA: California Academic Press.
Facione, P., Facione, N. & Giancarlo, C. (1996). The motivation to think in working and
learning. In E. Jones (ed.), Defining Expectations for Student Learning. San Fran-
cisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Inc.
Facione, P., Giancarlo (Sanchez), c., & Facione, N. (1994). Are college students disposed
to think? Paper presented at the Sixth International Conference on Thinking, Bos-
ton, MA. ERIC Doc: ED 368-311.
The Disposition Toward Critical Thinking 83

Facione, P., Giancarlo (Sanchez), c., Facione, N., & Gainen (Kurfiss), 1. (1995). The
disposition toward critical thinking, Journal of General Education, 44 (l), 1-25.
Ferketich, S., Phillips, L. & Verran, J. (1993). Development and administration of a survey
instrument for cross cultural research. Research in Nursing & Health, 16, 227-30.
Fisher, A. & Scriven, M. (1997). Critical Thinking: Its Definition and Assessment. Point
Reyes, CA: Edgepress.
Giancarlo, C. (1996). The ideal critical thinker: development of an expert q-sort proto-
type. Presented at the American Psychological Association Meetings, Toronto,
Canada.
Giancar10, C. & Facione, N. (1994). A Study of the Critical Thinking Disposition and
Skill of Spanish and English Speaking Students at Camelback High School.
Millbrae, CA: The California Academic Press.
Haldyna, T. (1994). Developing and Validating Multiple Choice Test Items. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Publishers.
Jones, E. (1993). Critical Thinking Literature Review. The National Center for Post-
secondary Teaching, Learning and Assessment. University Park, PA: Pennsylvania
State University Press.
Jones, E., Hoffman, S., Moore, L., Ratcliff, G., Tibbetts, S. & Glick, B. (1994).Essential
Skills in Writing. Speech and Listening. and Critical Thinking for College Gradu-
ates: Perspectives of Faculty, Employers, and Policy Makers. Project summary,
U.S. Department of Education, OERI Contract No. R117GI0037. National Center for
Post-secondary Teaching, Learning and Assessment. University Park, PA: Pennsyl-
vania State University Press.
Jones, E., Hoffman, S., Moore, L., Ratcliff, G., Tibbetts, S. & Click, B. (1995). National
Assessment of College Student Learning: Identifying the College Graduates' Es-
sential Skills in Writing. Speech and Listening, and Critical Thinking. National
Center for Educational Statistics, U.S. Department of Education, Office of Educa-
tional Research and Improvement. Washington, DC: OERI publication NCES 93-001.
King, P. & Kitchener, K. (1995). Developing Reflective Judgment. San Francisco, CA:
Jossey-Bass Publishers.
Kurfiss (Gainen), J. (1988). Critical Thinking: Theory. Research, Practice, and Possi-
bilities. ASHE-ERIC Higher Education Report No.2. Washington DC: ASHE.
Lewin, K. (1935). A DynamiC Theory of Personality: Selected Papers. Translated by D.
K. Adams & K. E. Zener. New York: McGraw Hill.
Lipman, M. (1987). Some thoughts on the foundations of reflective education. In Baron,
J. and Sternberg, R., (eds.), Teaching Thinking Skills: Theory and Practice, 152-61.
New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.
Marshall, R. & Tucker, M. (1992). Thinking for a Living: Education and the Wealth of
Nations. New York: Basic Books.
Miller, D. (1991) Handbook of Research Design and Social Measurement. Newbury
Park, CA: SAGE Publications.
Norris, S. & Ennis, R. (1989). Evaluating Critical Thinking. Pacific Grove, CA: The
Critical Thinking Press.
Nunnally, J. (1978). PsychometriC Theory. New York: McGraw Hill.
84 Peter A. Facione

Pendarvis, F. (1996). Critical Thinking Assessment: Measuring a Moving Target. Rock


Hill, SC: The South Carolina Higher Education Assessment Network.
Paul, R. (1990). Critical Thinking. Rohnert Park, CA: Sonoma State University Center
for Critical Thinking and Moral Critique.
Perkins, D., Jay, E. & Tishman, S. (1993). Beyond abilities: A dispositional theory of
thinking. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39 (I), 1-21.
Rosenthal, R. & Rosnow, R. (1991). Essentials ofBehavioral Research. New York: McGraw
Hill.
Salomon, G. (1994). To Be or Not To Be (Mindful)? Presented to the American Educa-
tional Research Association Meetings, New Orleans, LA.
Siegel, H. (1988). Educating Reason. New York, NY: Routledge.
Taube, K. (1997). Critical Thinking Ability and Disposition as Factors of Performance on
a Written Critical Thinking Test. Journal of General Education.
Tishman, S. (1994). Thinking Dispositions and Intellectual Character. Presented to the
American Educational Research Association Meetings, New Orleans, LA.
Tishman, S. & Andrade, A. (1996). Thinking dispositions: a review of current theories,
practices, and issues. Cambridge, MA. Project Zero, Harvard University.
Triandis, H. (1980). Values, attitudes, and interpersonal behavior. In H. E. Howe (Ed.),
Beliefs, Attitudes, and Values: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1979, 195-259.
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
United States Congress. (1994). Goals 2000: National Goals for Education Act. Wash-
ington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

Peter A. Facione, Dean of the College of Arts and Sciences,


Dean of the Division of Counseling Psychology and Education
Santa Clara UniverSity, Ramos Center, Santa Clara. CA 95053 USA.
pfacione@mailer.scu.edu

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy