Intérêt - Krapp
Intérêt - Krapp
Intérêt - Krapp
REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
https://www.jstor.org/stable/23420114?seq=1&cid=pdf-
reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
Springer is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to European
Journal of Psychology of Education
Andreas Krapp
University of the German Forces, Germany
Introduction
Basic conceptualizatio
In general, research
firstly, interest em
(Valsiner, 1992; Oert
person and some topi
(Nuttin, 1984). This i
An important aspect i
well as cognitive comp
positive emotional exp
notion of personal re
-related tasks. Interes
becomes part of a syn
In of these spite
com
researchers use differ
and methodological
research (Krapp, Ren
identified three conce
discussions on motiv
person, (2) interest as
interest as a psycholo
other. Especially, the
characteristics and th
environment.
Characteristics
o as a disposition
ndividual interest
of the person
Characteristics of the
C^Tjnterestingness
learning context
Figure 1. Three approaches to interest research (Krapp, Hidi, & Renninger, 1992).
Basic ideas
The basic ideas of this theoretical approach are summarized in the following statements:
(1) The theoretical framework is based on metatheoretical premises. They refer, for
example, to the question of ultimate aims of an educational theory on motivated
learning. With respect to educational practice, it seems to be necessary not only to
describe and explain the motivational aspects of single learning episodes
(Boekaerts, 1996, this volume), but also to describe and explain the role of
motivation in the course of human development.
(2) In accordance with ideas of Lewin (1951), Nuttin (1984), Renninger (1990, 1992),
Deci and Ryan (1985, 1991), Oerter (1995) and many others, it is postulated that
the individual, as a potential source of action, and the environment as the object of
action, constitute a bipolar unit. Therefore, the interest-construct is conceptualized
as a relational conce
between a person a
Lewin, 1951). It can
(3) It is assumed tha
environment in a m
consists of units tha
We refer to these u
an interest for a sho
concrete things, a t
the cognitively repr
(4) The most import
one's values and fe
value-related and f
The value-related va
of personal signific
states while being e
arousal or feelings
-based interactions w
modes that combin
goals) and positive
Under extremely op
1988). Flow can be
1993, p. 73). A furth
-based activities me
interest-related goa
growing self (Deci &
what a person has to
to do (Dewey, 1913;
(5) An interest-based
analysis: on a first
individual. Here, in
occupied with an ob
level, interest refers
learning activity (ac
(6) Referring to a dy
Deci and Ryan's (19
emergence and dev
interests is often re
self. Developmenta
processes of identi
person encounters
assimilated to the se
are not only (or f
consideration (e.g.,
-related experience
cognitive approac
Heckhausen, 1989,
1996) are not suffici
In addition, concept
psychological proce
concept of basic ps
basis for describing
origin and basic fun
The concept of interest, as defined here, can also be used to specify the meaning of
Empirical approaches
Individual interest and academic achievement. Studies which investigated the relation
between individual interest and learning-outcome have often used a correlational approach.
Dependent variables are mostly grades or test scores. In a meta-analysis covering all studies
of the last 3 decades (Schiefele, Krapp, & Winteler, 1992) it was found that across all school
types, grade levels, and subjects, the best (average) correlative estimate of the "interest
-achievement-relation" is approximately .30. This relationship appears to be a function of
sex, age or grade level, and school subject. Specifically, interest has a greater effect on the
grades of male students than on those of female students. There also seems to be a closer
relationship between interest and academic achievement at higher grade levels. This might be
the result of reciprocal effects: With growing age, specifically after puberty, students begin to
identify more seriously with selected fields of knowledge and competence and tend to reject
others (Todt, Drewes, & Heils, 1991; Fend, 1994). As a consequence, learning efforts are
concentrated on selected topics and yield better learning results.
If we compare the interest-achievement correlations with the results of prediction studies
in which other measures of motivation are used, for example measures of achievement
motivation, the correlations are more stable, and mostly higher (Schiefele, 1996a). However,
the critical question is what they can tell us about the role of interest in learning. Teacher
-ratings of achievement are very poor measures of what really has been learned. Better
indicators are measures of cognitive changes during learning, e.g., changes in the representa
tional structure of domain-specific knowledge.
Individual interest in specific subject areas and acquired knowledge. A few studies have
tried to investigate the interest-achievement-relation at this level of analysis. Most of them
have been done in the domain of text-learning (cf., Schiefele, 1990, 1991; Schiefele & Krapp,
1996). In these studies high-interest and low-interest students were compared with regard to
different indicators o
questionnaire to meas
series of tests to mea
were also administered
Finally, after reading
All studies displayed
controlling for previo
enhance the quantity
the quality of learning
go beyond the text's su
(for a summary see S
Situational interes
instruction suggest t
achievement (Travers
will automatically lead
to get involved with t
some correlational stu
text-learning, which
interestingness and
Anderson (1992), H
methodology has usu
investigation is rated
relationship between t
interestingness turned
experiments it was fou
(e.g., readability) to ex
Interestingness, how
shown in experimenta
1992). The backgroun
interestingness of le
anecdotes, which are n
the basic information
influence of importan
interestingness were r
were asked to read a
operationally defined
information. Another
effects were measure
and, in addition, som
reported by Garner, G
important informatio
who read the text wi
rated as most import
43% of these ideas. I
read and recall inform
have a powerful effe
interest plays a key
interestingness is the
information is of no re
Studies referring to p
Table 1
Zero-order correlatio
Wild, &Krapp, 1995)
Dependent Extrins
Interest
Variables
Study Time
During Semester .52** .09
Week before Exam .17 .14
Interest was highly related to study time during the semester but weakly related to study
time before the exam. In contrast, extrinsic motivation was not significantly related to study
time during the semester. When looking at the relations between the two aspects of learning
motivation (interest vs. extrinsic motivation) and strategy use, we found nearly the same
pattern for the general as well as for the specific dimension of learning: interest was closely
related to elaboration strategies which are aspects of deep processing approach. Extrinsic
motivation was associated with rehearsal (and metacognitive) strategies, but not with
elaboration strategies. Even though it is well established that interest as well as other
motivation factors play an important role in selecting and using specific kinds of learning
strategies, we still do not know to what degree learning-strategies mediate the causal
relationship between interest and learning outcome (Schiefele & Krapp, 1996; Schiefele,
1996a; Wild, 1996).
Feeling-related states and experiences. The effectiveness of learning is not only a matter
of cognitive factors; feeling-related states and the quality of experience during a learning
episode also play an important role (Csikszentmihalyi, 1988; Christianson, 1992; Boekaerts,
1995). Quality of experience is a multidimensional construct. According to Schiefele
(1996b), the core dimensions of this construct include activation or arousal (e.g., feeling
vigorous), affect (e.g., feeling happy), and concentration. Measuring feeling-related variables
retrospectively is a problem, because the quality of emotional experiences changes very
quickly and subjects do not always correctly remember their emotional states during the
process of an activity. Therefore it is important to use methods which provide valid
information about the emotional states experienced during an interest-related action.
In a recent study Schiefele (1996b) asked high school seniors, what kind of feelings and
experiential states they had while reading a text. The quality of the subjective experience was
assessed by means of rating-scales that were inserted into the text at different points. Topic
interest was found to be significantly related to involvement (activation), enjoyment (happi
ness) and concentration.
Another empirical approach is the "Experience Sampling Method" (ESM;
Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987). Subjects are provided with an electronic pager which is
individually programmed with randomly selected signal-times. Whenever the subject
receives a signal he or she is asked to respond immediately to a small number of self-report
3.8
Feeling of autonomy
—LG-1
3.7 company
3.6
—s-LG-2
3.5 company
3.4 :
3.3
3.2
school ^school
3.1
3
phase 1 phase 2
Feeling of competence
3.8
company school
3.5 company
3.4
—LG-1
3.3
school —b— LG-2
3.2
3.1
3
phase 1 phase 2
4
3.9 . —n school
3.8
3.7
3.6
company
3.5
3.4
3.3
school —LG-1
3.2
3.1 —s- LG-2
3
phase 1 phase 2
Interest as a dependen
Findings on empirica
interest-based motiv
learning (Schiefele, 1
perspective, it seems
educational goals. It
individualinterest d
school can become an
Descriptive studies.
descriptive. For exam
different groups, and
Findings from studi
1992) indicate that al
young age interests
differentiated over t
have been studied usi
negative trend. Helm
children. This negativ
(cf., Todt, 1978; Lehr
this time, especially
degree, a similar decl
Cross-sectional stud
groups can only show
differences and some
caused by individual a
be found when one considers the relation between interest and the context in which interest
-related information is conveyed. Gender really is the only moderator variable that has been
carefully studied (cf., for a summary, Hoffmann et al., 1998). Many suggest that the decline
of subject-matter-interest in the natural sciences is most apparent among female students. For
example, in biology, girls' interests have been found to increase in anthropology and ecology,
while they decrease in zoology and botany. Differential effects of this kind have also been
found in other domains, such as sociology, politics and physics (Birnstengel, 1989; Todt &
Schreiber, 1998). In an extensive study of 5th to 10th graders developing interests in physics
(cf., Hoffmann & Lehrke, 1986), both the different subject areas of physics (e.g., optics,
mechanics) and the contexts within which the topics were cove
laws of physics vs. the application of these laws to real-world
account. An undifferentiated (global) data-analysis revealed a gen
was more distinct for girls than it was for boys. Separate consider
the types of contextual integration resulted in a more differen
(Hoffmann, Hàupier, Peters-Haft, 1997). In classes primarily
demonstrate general rules in physics, the interest for topics in phy
both boys and girls. A much stronger interest can be seen with
however, if cross-references are made to the students' interest (e.
devices). This effect is particularly pronounced for girls.
References
Amelang, M., & Bartussek, D. (1985). Différentielle Psychologie und Persônlichkeitsforschung. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.
Anastasi, A., & Foley, J.P. (1949). Differential psychology. New York: Macmillan.
Anderson, R.C. (1982). Allocation of attention during reading. In A. Flammer & W. Kintsch (Eds.), Discourse
processing (pp. 287-299). Amsterdam: North Holland.
Anderson, R.C., Shirey, L.L., Wilson, P.T., & Fielding, L.G. (1987). Interestingness of children's reading material.
In R.E. Snow & M.J. Farr (Eds.), Aptitude, learning, and instruction: Cognitive and affective process analyses
(pp. 287-299). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Arnold, F. (1906). The psychology of interest. II. Psychological Review, 13, 291-315.
Berlyne, D.E. (1949). Interest as a psychological concept. The British Journal of Psychology, 39, 184-195.
Berlyne, D.E. (1960). Conflict, arousal and curiosity. New York: Grove Press.
Birnstengel, U. (1989). Die Interessen von Hauptschülern des achten bis zehnten Schuljahres. Bad Heilbrunn:
Klinkhardt.
Boekaerts, M. (1996). Personality and the psychology of learning. European Journal of Personality, 10, 377-404.
Christianson, S.A. (Ed.) (1992). The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). The flow experience and its significance for human psychology. In M. Csikszentmihalyi
& I.S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 15-33).
New York: University Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. The Journal of
Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 526-536.
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum
Press.
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (1991). A motivational approach to self: Integration in personality. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.),
Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 38 Perspectives on Motivation (pp. 237-288). Lincoln, NE: University
of Nebraska Press.
Entwistle, N. (1988). Motivational factors in students' approaches to learning. In R.R. Schmeck (Ed.), Learning
strategies and learning styles (pp. 21-51). New York: Plenum Press.
Epstein, S. (1990). Cognitive-experiential self theory: Implications for developmental psychology. In M. Gunnar &
L.A. Sroufe (Eds.), Minnesota Symposia on child psychology: Self processes and Development (pp. 79-123).
Hilsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Fend, H. (1994). Die Entdeckung des Selbst und die Verarbeitung der Pubertal. Bern: Huber.
Gardner, P.L. (1998). The development of males' and females' interests in science and technology. In L. Hoffmann,
A. Krapp, A.K. Renninger, & J. Baumert (Eds.), Interest and Learning. Proceedings of the Seeon-Conference on
Interest and Gender (pp. 41-57). Kiel: IPN.
Garner, R., Brown, R., Sanders, S., & Menke, D.J. (1992). "Seductive details" and learning from text.
In K.A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 239-254).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Garner, R., Gillingham, M.G., & White, C.S. (1989). Effects of "seductive details" on macroprocessing and
microprocessing in adults and children. Cognition and Instruction, 6, 41-57.
Helmke, A. (1993). Die Entwicklung der Lernfreude vom Kindergarten bis zur 5. Klassenstufe. Zeitschrift fiir
Pàdagogische Psychologie, 7, 77-86.
Hidi, S. (1990). Interest and its contribution as a mental resource for learning. Review of Educational Research,
60, 549-571.
Hidi, S. (1995). A reexamination of the role of attention in learning from text. Educational Psychological Review,
7, 323-350.
Hidi, S., & Anderson, V. (1992). Situational interest and its impact on reading and expository writing. In
K.A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 215-238).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hidi, S., & Berndorff (1998). Situational interest and learning. In L. Hoffmann, A. Krapp, A. Renninger, & J. Baumert
(Eds.), Interest and learning. Proceedings of the Seeon-Conference on Interest and Gender (pp. 74-90).
Kiel: IPN.
Hoffmann, L., HàuBler, P., & Peters-Haft, S. (1997). An den Interessen von Màdchen
Physikunterricht. Kiel: IPN.
Hoffmann, L., Krapp, A., Renninger, A., & Baumert, J. (1998). Interest and learning
-conference on interest and gender. Kiel: IPN.
Hoffmann, L., & Lehrke, M. (1986). Eine Untersuchung über Schülerinteressen an Physik
Pàdagogik, 32, 189-204.
Krapp, A. (1993). The construct of interest: Characteristics of indvidual interests and inter
perspective of a person-object-theory (Studies in Educational Psychology). Munich: Un
Krapp, A., & Fink, B. (1992). The development and function of interests during the critical transition from home to
preschool. In K.A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development
(pp. 397-429). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Krapp, A., Hidi, S., & Renninger, K.A. (1992). Interest, learning and development. In K.A. Renninger, S. Hidi, &
A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 3-25). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Krapp, A., & Prenzel, M. (Eds.). (1992). Intéressé, Lernen, Leistung. Neuere Ansàtze einerpàdagogisch-psychologischen
Interessenforschung. Münster: Aschendorff.
Krapp, A., Renninger, A., & Hoffmann, L. (1998). Some thoughts about the development of a unifying framework for
the study of individual interest. In L. Hoffmann, A. Krapp, A. Renninger, & J. Baumert (Eds.), Interest and
learning. Proceedings of the Seeon-Conference on Interest and Gender (pp. 455-468). Kiel: IPN.
Lehrke, M., Hoffmann, L. & Gardner, P.L. (Eds.). (1985). Interests in science and technology education. Kiel: IPN.
Lewalter, D., Krapp, A., Schreyer, I., & Wild, K.-P. (1998). Die Bedeutsamkeit des Erlebens von Kompetenz,
Autonomie und sozialer Eingebundenheit fiir die Entwicklung berufsspezifischer Interessen. In K. Beck &
R. Dubs (Eds.), Kompetenzentwicklung in der Berufserziehung - Kognitive, motivationale und moralische
Dimensionen kaufmànnischer Qualifizierungsprozesse. Zeitschrift fur Berufs- und Wirtschaftspàdagogik
(Supplement 14, pp. 143-168). Stuttgart: Steiner.
Lewin, K. (1951). Fieldtheory in social science. New York: Harper & Row.
Markus, H., & Wurf, E. (1987). The dynamic self-concept: A social pschological perspective. Annual Review of
Psychology, 38, 299-337.
Nenniger, P. (1987) How stable is motivation by contents? In E. DeCorte, H. Lodewijks, R. Parmentier, & P. Span
(Eds.), Learning and Instruction (vol. 1, pp. 159-168). Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Nenniger, P. (1988). Das Pàdagogische Verhàltnis als motivationales Konstrukt. Weinheim: Deutscher Studien Verlag
Nenniger, P. (1993). Von der summativen zur strukturellen Betrachtung des Unterrichts. Zu den theoretischen Folgen
des methodologischen Zugangs in der Unterrichtsforschung. Empirische Pàdagogik, 7, 21-35.
Nenniger, P, Eigler, G., & Macke, G. (1993). Studien zur Mehrdimensionalitat von Lehr-Lern-Prozessen. Bern: Lang.
Nenniger, P., Straka, G.A., Spevacek, G., & Wosnitza, M. (1996). Die Bedeutung motivationaler Einflussfaktoren fiir
selbstgesteuertes Lernen. Unterrichtswissenschaft, 3, 250-265.
Oerter, R. (1995). Entwicklung der Motivation und Handlungssteuerung. In R. Oerter & L. Montada (Eds.),
Entwicklungspsychologie (pp. 758-822). Weinheim: PVU.
Pintrich, P.R., & Schunk, D.H. (1996). Motivation in education. Theory, research and applications. Englewood Cliffs:
Prentice-Hall.
Rathunde, K. (1998, in press). Undivided and abiding interest. Comparisons across studies of talented adolescents and
creative adults. In L. Hoffmann, A. Krapp, A. Renninger, & J. Baumert (Eds.), Interest and learning.
Proceedings of the Seeon-Conference on Interest and Gender (pp. 367-376). Kiel: IPN.
Renninger, K.A. (1990). Children's play interest(s), representation, and activity. In R. Fivush & J. Hudson (Eds.),
Knowing and remembering in young children (pp. 127-147). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Renninger, K.A. (1992). Individual interest and development: Implications for theory and practice. In K.A. Renninger,
S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 361-395). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Renninger, K.A. (1998). The role of individual interests and gender in learning: An overview of research on preschool
and elementary school-aged children/students. In L. Hoffmann, A. Krapp, A. Renninger, & J. Baumert (Eds.),
Interest and learning. Proceedings of the Seeon-conference on interest and gender (pp. 165-174). Kiel: IPN.
Renninger, K.A., & Hidi, S., & Krapp, A. (Eds.). (1992). The role of interest in learning and development. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Renninger, K.A., & Leckrone, T.G. (1991). Continuity in young children's actions: A consideration of interest and
temperament. In L. Oppenheimer & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The origins of action: Interdisciplinary and international
perspectives (pp. 205-238). New York: Springer.
Reynolds, R.E. (1992). Selective attention and prose learning: Theoretical and empirical research. Educational
Psychological Review, 4, 345-391.
Schiefele, H., Krapp, A., Prenzel, M., Heiland, A., & Kasten, H. (1983). Principles of an educational theory of interest.
(Paper presented at the 7th Meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development in
Munich).
Schiefele, U. (1990). The influence of topic interest, prior knowledge, and cognitive capabilities on text comprehension.
In J.M. Pieters, K. Breuer, & P.R.J. Simons (Eds.), Learning environments: Contributions from Dutch and
German research (pp. 323-338). Berlin: Springer.
Schiefele, U. (1991). Interest, learning and motivation. Educational Psychologist, 26, 299-323.
Schiefele, U. (1996b). Topic interest, text representation, and quality of experience. Contemporary Educational
Psychology, 21, 3-18.
Schiefele, U., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1994). Interest and the quality of experience in classrooms. European Journal of
Psychology of Education, 9, 251-270.
Schiefele, U., & Krapp, A. (1996). Topic interest and free recall of expository text. Learning and Individual Differences,
8, 141-160.
Schiefele, U., Krapp, A., & Winteler, A. (1992). Interest as a predictor of academic achievement: A meta-analysis of
research. In K.A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interes
(pp. 183-212). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schiefele, U., & Rheinberg, F. (1997). Motivation and knowledge acquisition: Sea
In M.L. Maehr & P.R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievemen
Press.
Schiefele, U., & Schreyer, I. (1994). Intrinsische Lernmotivation und Lernen. Ein Überblick zu Ergebnissen der
Forschung. Zeitschrift far Pàdagogische Psychologie, 8, 1-13.
Schiefele, U., Wild, K.P., & Krapp, A. (1995). Course-specific interest and extrinsic motivation as predictors of specific
learning strategies and course grades. Paper presented at the 6th EARLI Conference in Nijmegen.
Shirey, L.L., & Reynolds, R.E. (1988). Effect of interest on attention and learning. Journal of Educational Psychology,
80, 159-166.
Thorndike, E.L. (1935). The psychology of wants, interests, and attitudes. New York: Appleton-Century.
Todt, E. (1985). Elements of a theory of science interests. In M. Lehrke, L. Hoffmann, & P.L. Gardner (Eds.), Interests
in science and technology (pp. 59-69). Kiel: IPN.
Todt, E., Drewes, R., & Heils, S. (1991). The development of interests during adolescence: Social context, individual
differences, and individual significance. In R.K. Silbereisen & E. Todt (Eds.), Adolescence in context: The
interplay of family, school, peers and work in adjustment (pp. 82-95). New York: Springer.
Todt, E., & Schreiber, S. (1998). Development of interests. In L. Hoffmann, A. Krapp, A.K. Renninger, & J. Baumert
(Eds.), Interest and Learning. Proceedings of the Seeon-Conference on Interest and Gender (pp. 25-40). Kiel: IPN.
Travers, R.M.W. (1978). Children's interests. Kalamazoo, Michigan: Michigan University, College of Education.
Valsiner, J. (1992). Interest: A Meta-Theoretical Perspective. In K.A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of
interest in learning and development (pp. 27-41). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wade, S.E., & Adams, R.B. (1990). Effects of importance and interest on recall of biographical text. JRB: A Journal of
Literacy, 22, 331-353.
Wild, K.P. (1996). Die Beziehung zwischen Lernmotivation und Lernstrategien als Funktion personaler und situativer
Faktoren. In R. Duit & C.V. Rhôneck (Eds.), Lernen in den Naturwissenschaften (pp. 69-86). Kiel: IPN.
Wild, K.P., & Krapp. A. (1996). Die Qualitàt subjektiven Erlebens in schulischen und betrieblichen Lernumwelten.
Untersuchungen mit der Erlebens Stichproben Methode. Unterrichtswissenschaft, 24, 195-216.
Wild, K.-P., Krapp, A., Schreyer, I., & Lewalter, D. (1998, in press). The development of interest and motivational ori
entations: Gender differences in vocational education. In L. Hoffmann, A. Krapp, A.K. Renninger, & J. Baumert
(Eds.), Interest and Learning. Proceedings of the Seeon-Conference on Interest and Gender (pp. 441-454). Kiel:
IPN.
Wild, K.-P., Schiefele, U., & Krapp, A. (1996). Moods, motivations, thoughts, and external events in the classroom and
in the workplace as assessed by the experience sampling method. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Educational Research Association 1996 in New York.
hypothétiques pou
l'intérêt (par exem
réactions émotionn
essaient d'explorer
environnement éd
discutées par référ
par l'éducation.
Andreas Krapp. Fakultât für Sozialwissenschaften, Universitàt der Bundeswehr München, 85577
Neubiberg, Germany, Tel./Fax: +049-89-6004-3128.
Krapp, A. (1993). Psychologie der Lernmotivation - Perspektiven der Forschung und Problème ihrer pâdagogischen
Rezeption. Zeitschriftfur Pàdagogik, 39, 187-206.
Krapp, A., & Prenzel, M. (Eds.). (1992). Intereresse, Lernen, Leisiung. Neuere Anzàtze einer pàdagogisch
-psychologischen Interessenforschung. Münster: Aschendorff.
Renninger, K.A., Hidi, S., & Krapp, A. (Ed.). (1992). The role of interest in learning and development. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Weidenmann, B., & Krapp, A. (1993). Pàdagogische Psychologie - ein Lehrbuch. Weinheim: PVU.