Marshall Mtsu 0170E 10816
Marshall Mtsu 0170E 10816
Marshall Mtsu 0170E 10816
by
Holly B. Marshall
Dissertation Committee:
There are not enough words to express my appreciation to the ones who have supported me
during this long journey. First, without sounding cliché, I give the highest credit and gratitude to
God, with whom I have had many diverse and interesting conversations.
I have some amazing friends, especially during the hibernation and all night marathons of
studying. Your texts and calls to make sure I was still breathing were thoughtful. Your offers to
run chores, bring me food or whatever I needed was kind. I am most grateful for our friendship,
thank you Joni and Sheila.
It is always a good idea to have a support of peers who are going through the experience with
you. I do not want to unintentionally leave anyone’s name out, instead, I will extend a heartfelt
“thank you” and appreciation to fellow doctoral students who gave support and encouragement.
An extended appreciation to the graduate assistants who had the mundane task of editing my
work. (you know who you are)
It is impossible to achieve your goal without the faculty. I want to express the appreciation and
gratitude to Dr. Bass and Dr. Boulware, thank you for your valuable time, experience, and
knowledge reviewing my work. To Dr. Kim, thank you for your wisdom, time, and
encouragement…and let’s not forget about your humor. Your kindness and patience is well-
acknowledged. I would like to extend my gratitude and appreciation to the chair of my
committee, Dr. Amy Elleman. I thank you for your support, guidance, and patience. You
presented me with challenges and I am better for them. Your encouragement and belief stayed
constant and never wavered. I thank you for the experience.
To my parents, Jerry and Marilyn Ross whose support and love I will always cherish. They are
some of my biggest cheerleaders, never doubting that I wouldn’t complete what I set out to do.
(no matter how long it may take). Thank you for your prayers, encouragement, interest, and
loving care. I love you.
I want to thank my sons, Ross, Grant, and Blake. If anyone was the most affected aside from
myself, it was you. Your support and encouragement kept me going, I could not have, or would
not have done it any other way. You never showed frustration when our lives took a long detour.
Thank you for stepping up during the times when I couldn’t. We say, “family first”, and I love
you for the many times that family could not be first. Thank you for asking how I was doing and
showing interest. We have experienced many situations and events over the last few years and I
am so grateful and thank God that I got to go through them with you. I would like to give a
separate “thank you” to my daughter-in-law. Christina, you became part of our family the
moment our son said, “she’s the one.” I enjoyed our talks and stories about our own experiences
as educators. Thank you for your love and for taking care of my son.
ii
Finally, there is not enough words that describe the overwhelming support that my husband
Steve has given me. From the onset, you have endured this journey with me, only to give me
encouragement, hope, confidence, and strength. You have been both mom and dad in the early
stages, to now taking care of every task in our daily lives to keep moving forward. You have
gone way above the call “for better or worse” and it hasn’t gone unnoticed. Thank you for your
unconditional love. I am look forward to experiencing new adventures in life with you and I
thank God for the day we met.
iii
ABSTRACT
Seventy-five percent of third grade students who are at-risk will continue to struggle with
reading through the years into adulthood, never to recover their potential reading
development. Once less-skilled third grade readers reach ninth grade, one in six students
are four times more likely to leave high school before receiving a diploma than those who
are proficient readers. Whether students who read effortlessly or struggle to decode text,
both cannot comprehend. This relationship between reading fluency and comprehension
has educators seeking instructional resources to improve the reading deficit across the
country. Readers’ theatre, as a form of repeated reading, may be one solution to the
exhaustive search in this review found few quantitative studies in readers’ theatre
studies found, results have been mixed on the influence of readers’ theatre on various
reading components. The purpose of the study was to replicate and extend the existing
studies and to further examine the effects of readers’ theatre on fluency and
comprehension using expository text and instruction. This study took place in an
elementary school in the mid-south, with second grade students with diverse reading
abilities. Random assignment was implemented for individual students within two
intervention lasted two weeks. Instruction consisted of expository text focused on social
studies content. Students’ progress was assessed with a standardized measure of reading
iv
motivation measure. Although there were no statistically significant findings in this
study, effect sizes indicated that repeated reading may be more beneficial for reading
comprehension and fluency with samples such as the one used in this study and that
readers’ theatre may be more beneficial for knowledge acquisition. The motivation
results were mixed with the survey indicating a decline in interest among both groups and
interviews indicating that readers’ theatre was interesting and motivating to the students.
Practical implications and future research are also addressed in this study.
v
TABLE OF CONTENTS
LIST OF TABLES……………………………………………………….……... ix
CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION………………...…………………………….. 1
Repeated Reading………………………………………………………. 3
Comprehension ……………………………………………………….... 5
Readers’ Theatre………………………………………………………… 7
History ………………………………………………………….. 7
Research to Practice……………………………………………………. 12
Summary……………………………………………………………….. 13
Case Study……………………………………………..………………. 22
Knowledge Acquisition…………………………………………........... 38
Motivation ………………………….……………………….…………. 39
Summary ……………………………………………………………….. 44
vi
CHAPTER III: METHODOLOGY.…………...……………………………….. 47
Participants……………………………………………………………… 48
Design…………………………………………………………………… 48
Measures………………………………………………………………… 51
Motivational assessment………………………………………... 52
Data Analysis………………………………………………..…………. 56
Motivation ……………………………………………….…….. 71
vii
CHAPTER V: DISCUSSION………………………………...……………. ….. 76
Knowledge acquisition…………………………………......…… 79
Motivation……………………………………………………….………. 80
Conclusion ……………………………………………….……………… 85
REFERENCES…………………………………………………………………… 87
APPENDICES………………………………………………………………...…. 99
viii
LIST OF TABLES
Page
Table 2. Means and Standard Deviations for Pretest and Posttest Measures by
Condition………………………………………………………………………….….. 60
ix
LIST OF FIGURES
Page
Figure 4.1 Graph comparison for the WIAT III oral reading fluency subtest
pretest and posttest means and the easyCBM oral reading fluency pretest
Figure 4.2 Graph of the WIAT III pretest and posttest means for the word
reading subtest and the word reading timed subtest by condition …………....... 66
Figure 4.3 Graph on the WIAT III pretest and posttest means for the reading
Figure 4.4 Means for the knowledge acquisition test one and knowledge
Figure 4.5 Graph of the Likert student reading survey pretest and posttest means
by condition ……………...…………………………………………………...… 73
x
1
CHAPTER I
INTRODUCTION
The ability to speak, listen, and comprehend provide the foundation for reading
acquisition, beginning from birth through the academic years and progressing into adulthood.
Reading is an important skill for an individual to obtain, yet, approximately 75% of less-
skilled readers in third grade continue to have difficulties in the ninth grade, and sadly, into
adulthood (Mercer, Campbell, Miller, Mercer, & Lane, 2000). A national study performed
by the Annie E. Casey Foundation (2011) examined approximately 4,000 students and found
that one in six-third graders were not proficient and four times more likely to leave high
school without receiving a diploma. The percentages are greatest for the below-basic
readers: 23% fail to complete high school (Hernandez, 2011). Literacy education needs
improvement to not only help students achieve functional reading which is limited in skills,
but to develop critical reading which advances students to be prepared to confront social
issues (Jagger, 2008). To improve conditions, in 1997 the National Institute of Child Health
and Human Development (NICHD) was commissioned by the U.S. Congress to create a
education for children. The NICHD formed the National Reading Panel (NRP). The Panel’s
purpose was to review existing studies based on research-based knowledge, including the
efficacy of various techniques to instruct children to read. The NRP’s (2000) report
the nature and importance of reading fluency instruction. The report concluded that for most
of the 20th century and leading into the 21st fluency was thought of and taught primarily as
2
word recognition, disregarding other components of reading (NICHD, 2000). The report
emphasized that fluency is a vital element of reading development (Chard, Vaughn, & Tyler,
healthy reading program (Adams, 1990; Allington, 2012; Kuhn & Stahl, 2003; Reutzel &
necessary component in a well-balanced and rigorous reading curriculum (Deno & Marston,
2006; Dowhower, 1987; Kuhn & Stahl, 2003). Fluency is formally defined as the clear,
distinct, and simple written or verbal relaying of ideas (Harris & Hodge, 1995; Oxford
University Press, 2010). The definition of fluency may dictate its measurement as how one
defines fluency dictates the ways we measure it (Samuels, 2006). The following terms are
ability to recognize words without any forethought, while simultaneously using other reading
skills (Samuels, 1979). Accuracy is the ability to decode printed words without error
(Johnson, 2011). Prosody is the inflection of tonal quality, pitch, and rhythmic patterns of
Although many researchers were concentrating on rate, some noticed that other features of
fluent reading were important to reading growth (Dahl, 1975). While Dahl (1975) and
LaBerge and Samuels (1974) focused on rate and accuracy, other researchers examined
phrasing, intonation, and prosody (Dowhower, 1987; Schreiber, 1980). More recently,
3
simultaneously and includes rate and accuracy, while also using proper intonation, phrasing,
and expression (Keehn, Harmon, & Shoho, 2008; Martinez, Roser, & Stecker, 1999; Zutell &
Rasinski, 1991). Clay and Imlach (1971) examined reading behaviors of beginning readers.
They observed that readers who made the most progress read with intonation, prosody, and
phrasing while increasing their fluency rate, while the readers who read in sequences of one
and two word phrases at a time progressed more slowly. In addition, Clay and Imlach
observed the more skilled readers self-corrected on an average of five to seven times more
miscues than the less skilled readers, suggesting that self-correction, along with an
appropriate focus on phrasing and natural intonation might promote better reading. The
NAEP (2002) investigated the connection between oral reading fluency and comprehension
and found that the higher reading comprehension scores aligned with oral fluency reading
components. There have been many other studies that support this bidirectional relationship
(Fuchs, Fuchs, Hosp, & Jenkins, 2001; NAEP, 2000; Spear-Swerling, 2006) between oral
reading fluency and comprehension (Jenkins, Fuchs, van den Broek, Espin, & Deno, 2003).
Repeated Reading
Oral reading fluency is the execution in verbalizing text with precision and skill to
allow for sufficient comprehension (Rasinski, Blachowicz, & Lems, 2006). Acknowledging
1987; Herman, 1985), the NRP (2000) strongly suggested that effective instructional
methods for fluency include two reading methods: oral reading fluency and repeated reading.
Many studies have been performed on the instruction of repeated reading to improve fluency
4
and in turn, comprehension (Fuchs et al., 2001; Roshotte & Torgeson, 1985; Schreiber,
1980).
advocate of theory to practice, he set out to create an instructional technique that could be
passage to reach a specific rate. To examine his new method, Samuels worked with children
who have intellectual disabilities, explaining to them that to get better in anything, one must
practice (Samuels, 2006). To test his method a story was deconstructed into 150 word
segments. After listening to a model of the reading, the students individually practiced
reading the passage aloud, and then read the passage orally to the teacher who recorded their
reading rate. The students reread the passage until a rate of 85 words per minute was
achieved. Once the criterion was met, the student received a new passage and repeated the
same process. The students could see how their rate increased and the amount of fewer
errors decreased with each reading (Samuels, 1979; Herman, 1985). Samuels (1979)
demonstrated that slow readers could improve their automaticity through repeated readings.
The effect of Samuels’ method solicited mixed views. Even though his method was
favorably acknowledged, it did not explain how gains in reading rate achieved by reading
one passage several times would transfer to passages that contained different and unfamiliar
words. Chomsky (1978) conducted a study that found a similar result. Working with five at-
risk third-grade students, Chomsky asked the students to repeatedly follow and read along
with a text that was simultaneously read on audiotape until a specific rate was achieved.
Chomsky found that in over four months of intervention there were gains in fluency rate,
5
ranging from a few months to a year’s growth. It was also determined that in transitioning
from one text to a new text, there were fewer mistakes and the specific goal was achieved at
a faster rate (Chomsky, 1978). Additional research supports Samuels (1979) and Chomsky’s
(1978) studies and found students who engaged in repeated reading improved in
comprehension when moving from one text to another (Dowhower, 1987; Herman, 1985;
Hiebert, 2005, Millin & Rinehart, 1999; Therrien, 2004). In contrast, other studies found
students’ comprehension was not improved when transferring from text to text (Carrick,
2000; Johnson, 2011; Roshotte & Torgesen, 1985). The method of repeated reading has been
established as a viable reading method, however, additional studies are needed to examine
possible influences that repeated reading has on the relationship between comprehension and
Comprehension
The NAEP (2015) report shows that many children have insufficient reading
comprehension abilities. If not helped, children who experience difficulties in their early
years of reading development can experience lifelong difficulties into adulthood (Cutting,
Materek, Cole, Levine, & Mahone, 2009). Oral reading fluency is the execution in
verbalizing text with precision and skill to allow for sufficient comprehension (Rasinski,
Blachowicz, & Lems, 2006). Research has demonstrated that oral reading fluency predicts
comprehension (Fuchs, Fuchs, Hosp & Jenkins, 2001) moving beyond word identification to
understanding (Kuhn & Stahl, 2003; Pikulski & Chard, 2005; Rasinski, 2010). In Perfetti’s
(1985) explanation of the verbal-efficiency theory, immediate word recognition provides the
attentional resources and working memory, and in turn, affect the construct of meaning.
However, Perfetti (1985) argued that the process can be improved with guidance and
practice. As word recognition increases, so does the available attentional resources and
working memory, increasing the ability to construct meaning from text. In comparison, less
skilled readers who may have difficulty increasing automatic word recognition, evoke greater
demands on attentional resources and working memory, reducing the availability of those
Perfetti’s (1985) theories have found support in subsequent studies (Cutting, et al.,
2009; Jenkins, et al., 2003). In addition, studies have determined that extracting meaning
from text can be interrupted by slow arduous reading, indicating reading rate and not just
accuracy alone, is just as vital in text comprehension (LaBerge & Samuels, 1974; Perfetti &
Hogaboam, 1975). Being able to comprehend a text is an essential skill students must
develop. However, it is an extremely complex activity that skilled readers take for granted.
It is a complex process requiring multiple skills to develop relationship between text and
reader (Durkin, 1992: Harris & Hodges, 1995; NICHD, 2000; Rasinski et al., 2006).
Only within the past fifteen years have studies implied that comprehension requires
more than just fluent reading but is instead a composition of critical thinking, experience,
automaticity and passage-level skills are integral to the development of critical thinking,
emergent stage of reading development (Cain & Oakhill, 2007; Duke et al., 2011; Durkin,
1992; Fielding & Pearson, 1994; Rasinski et al., 2006; Spear-Swerling, 2006).
7
Studies indicate background knowledge is the most important requisite skill based on
its dependency on awareness of the world and its connection with text (Fielding & Pearson,
recognized as a prominent model in comprehension studies holds that knowledge and the
our knowledge of the world along with the text to construct mental representations of what
the text means. This information is integrated as new knowledge, establishing more
information than we knew before we read (Duke et al., 2011). Readers’ theatre not only
provides the opportunity to incorporate multiple reading components between oral reading
fluency and comprehension, but it may also help to translate text into meaning, to connect
with the world, and to relay the information through a visual performance.
Readers’ Theatre
History. Readers’ theatre can be traced back 2,500 years ago to Greece, progressing
through the medieval ages, and rapidly evolving in the early 19th century. In 1806, the author
Gilbert Austin wrote the book, Chironomia. He described a specific type of performance of
multiple persons reading various parts of a story, poem, or play with dramatic expression and
gestures while sitting in chairs (Carrick, 2000, p. 16). In 1945, the term readers’ theatre was
first used when a theatrical group in New York put on a performance of Oedipus Rex. It was
not until 1951, when producer, Charles Laughton asked his peer Paul Gregory, to
conceptualize a “concert version” of George Bernard Shaw’s Man and Superman. Little did
Laughton realize, his kind gesture was just the beginning of what later would be
acknowledged as one of the most prolific and innovative theatre experiences. Many refer to
8
Gregory as the author of the readers’ theatre movement. However, some historians believe
its birth came from Ancient Greece, as it was not unusual for gatherings to meet in public
when his eye caught the glimmer of four diamonds in the window of a prominent jewelry
store. The diamonds were displayed on a small black stool against a black velvet drape. The
vision became the inspiration of the next performances that he and Charles Laughton would
present, as four actors, dressed in black, sat on stools and participated in lively recitation.
This epiphany came to fruition in 1951, with the historical presentation of George Bernard
Shaw’s, Don Juan into Hell. The performance is considered the fundamental framework and
extensively toured the country, a 3 reader, 50 voice choral production of Steven Vincent
Benet’s John Brown’s Body also toured, achieving critical success. What would later
become known as readers’ theatre was not only perceived as a form of entertainment, but as
an innovative, professional event, seen across the country by thousands. The era marked the
newly constructed form of theatre as an education resource, dominantly in the speech and
The 1960s saw the growth of readers’ theatre in popularity, within the collegiate
theater programs and soon into the secondary English education. As interest in readers’
theatre grew, all levels of education began to incorporate it as an integral part of the reading
curriculum, even branching into various subject areas (Coger & White, 1973). While
readers’ theatre was becoming more popular in education, Coger (1963) and Brooks (1962)
9
collaborated to establish the framework for discussions and “rules” that would define
Literacy theorists began to analyze the complexities of literacy texts, as readers theatre
flourished. Theorists, directors, and educators often would debate the significance of the
text, not just as a performance of spoken text, but more relevant as a purposeful artistic
intrepretations of readers’ theatre and how its text influenced the overall experience.
Although the debate was gaining momentum, it would never replace the traditionalists view
supporting its influence on comprehension and appreciation of literature. The content should
conference, Crain and Smith (1976) demonstrated how readers’ theatre in a primary
educational classroom differed from the performance and entertainment factor that existed
within the higher levels of education. Crain and Smith stated that readers’ theatre enhanced
oral fluency skills and reading development, was motivational, and encouraged positive peer
interaction.
development from 1980 to the present. Any reference to readers’ theater is generally defined
as a literacy format and its implications in reading development. Currently, readers’ theatre
10
is still very present in the theatrical world and is performed as an alternative way for the
reading” or if a musical, “concert version or rendition”. Readers’ theatre has played a role
within the classroom for over 30 years. However, it has only been within the last 15 years
that empirical research has been conducted. Research has indicated that readers’ theatre is an
repeated reading enhances the progress of other elements of oral reading fluency: accuracy,
rate, and prosody. Readers’ theatre allows practice for repeated reading, and in turn affects
rate and prosody when implemented into a curriculum daily (Flynn, 2004; Johnson, 2011;
Keehn, 2003; Martinez et al., 1999; Millin & Rinehart, 1999; Young & Vardell, 1993).
Readers’ theatre is text written in the form of a script. Its content is character driven,
prosodic and fluent, while enhancing diction and expression in the deliverance of the
meaning of words and conversation. The story is delivered with the performers standing in
front of the audience with the script and using voice to project the story through their
characters. The script is not memorized as in other theater performances, movement is not
required, sets are not created, actors perform in a concert-type of setting, and many times, the
actors wear black so as not to distract from the dialogue (Flynn, 2004; Griffith & Rasinski,
2004; Groff, 1978; Harris & Hodges, 1995; Martinez et al., 1999; Young & Vardell, 1993;
Readers’ theatre advocates contend that it is a good method for increasing not only
fluency but comprehension as well. As Stayter and Allington (1991) suggested, readers’
theatre accentuates students’ ability to understand and reconstruct text. Students begin to
11
synthesize and develop their characters through each reading. The readers make changes
from their first impression to a more complex character after several readings of the script.
They read multiple parts to experience other characters rather than themselves. By the time
the students analyze the characters’ actions, behavior, and relationships with others (Flynn,
2004; Millin & Rinehart, 1999), they have a relationship and understanding with their
character (Busching, 1981). Students have proclaimed that through their own observation of
their character, and listening to peers, they have gained a wider perspective of their own
knowledge and of the world (Stayter & Allington, 1991). Readers’ theatre allows students to
interact with each other, actively working together in response and interpretation (Carrick,
2000).
Nathan and Stanovich (1991) indicated that incorporating drama within the classroom
has distinctive and defining elements of language development. Readers’ theatre is a reading
method which drama can be implemented, providing a whole class activity for all ability
Educators have the opportunity to use readers’ theatre to promote social awareness through
careful consideration of scripts and creating an atmosphere for discourse about societal
issues. Readers’ theatre transforms the classroom into another time and place. A place
where language has no barrier, where “aesthetic and educational values exit in harmony”
(Combs, 1987), creating drama that allows children to experience literature, through real or
rehearsals of dialogue incorporating expression, tone, pitch, and emotion in developing the
character (Morgan & Saxton, 1988). As students analyze their characters, they discuss
relationships, attitudes, situations, and opinions, while comprehending the text by eliminating
any interferences and gaining a deeper understanding on what they already knew (Booth,
1985). As the performing students participate in the development of language, speech, and
thinking skills, the audience is unknowingly enhancing listening skills (Courtney, 1987).
comprehension skills with early learners by comparing kindergarten children who were
active in dramatic play to children who were minimally active. He found that the children in
dramatic play had significantly higher scores in word and retell skills. A meta-analysis
conducted by Kardash and Wright (1987) suggested that drama overall has a positive effect
skills. Readers’ theatre and drama are widely used in education and performance theatre,
despite the limited amount of research promoting its efficacy. The empirical research that
has been conducted reflect mixed results on readers’ theatre as an effective influence on
Research to Practice
reading process that shows positive improvements for the struggling reader. Even with the
strong probability of improvement, Allington stated that fluency has been neglected and its
and publishers of educational materials. Studies repeatedly emphasize the need for well-
trained teachers who can implement effective reading instruction and ameliorate reading
methods, the NRP recommended that teachers design their instruction built on best evidence-
based research studies (NICHD, 2000). The International Reading Association defines
(International Reading Association, 2015). The panel’s assumption was that evidence-based
methods would increase students’ achievement if properly applied (Eash, 1968). Educators
seem to have a renewed interest in research and its contribution to their classroom instruction
as there is of researcher’s desire to inform them. Yet, the process of establishing a researcher
and teacher relationship has not been without obstacles. Bates (2002) argue that frustration
exists between researchers and teachers, because teachers want solutions to existing
instructional problems, while researchers are searching for new information and knowledge.
Summary
in which actors engage in conversation as they interact with literature to induce an emotion
from the audience (Tanner, 1993). Readers’ theatre contributions and benefits as an
instructional tool for improving reading have been discussed in the literature (Busching,
1981; Coger & White, 1973; Tanner, 1993, Stayter & Allington, 1991). Studies focused on
readers’ theatre generally examine fluency with comprehension. It has been established that
fluency instruction due to the strong correlation between fluency and comprehension (Fuchs
To close the gap between research and the classroom, researchers have enlisted
language, and reading development (Busching, 1981; Carrick, 2000; Corcoran & Davis,
2005; Flynn, 2004; Keehn, 2003; Rinehart, 1999). An abundance of research exists
validating readers’ theatre as a performance genre with educational benefits, yet, there is
CHAPTER II
LITERATURE REVIEW
multiple rehearsals in reading of text. Through repeated practice, a dramatic and effective
theatrical production, readers’ theatre does not require memorization of lines, but instead,
content is read from a script while focusing on the interaction of speech (Griffith & Rasinski,
Prior to 1990, readers’ theatre had long been acknowleged in literature. For example,
Maberry (1975) compared readers’ theatre and solo performance to a commonly used
instructional technique of silent reading. Mayberry’s experiment involved 371 high school
English students in grades nine and eleven. Two groups were created: those who silently
read the content material and those who listened to an oral presentation of the content
material. An assessment followed the task to determine which method scored the highest in
comprehension and literature appreciation. Results showed that readers’ theatre had the most
influence on both variables. Maberry administered the same test ten days after the
intervention to see if the students retained the information. Again, readers’ theatre had a
greater impact on both comprehension and literature appreciation. The study may not have
In a separate study, Walker, Salverson, and French (1983) compared various reading
strategies for children with reading difficulties. The children received approximately 20
hours of tutoring in a reading clinic. The sessions did not focus on one specific reading
strategy, instead, tutors differentiated lessons to meet the strengths and weaknesses of the
student. Methods of instruction used included alternate readings, repeated readings, readers
theatre, cloze method, timed readings, and word games. The tutorials were conducted to
recognition, and syntax. Students who received treatment showed gains in fluency rate, word
recognition, decoding, syntax use, and comprehension (Walker, Salverson, & French, 1983).
Outcomes were based on the individual students and the strategy used most often to improve
his or her reading skills. Collectively, in the category of oral reading fluency, majority of
students increased their rate using the strategies of repeated reading, readers’ theatre, and
timed readings. In addition, repeated reading and readers’ theatre influenced comprehension.
as a reading method to increase reading development. This literature review will explore
exhaustive search of literature confirmed the lack of empirical and quantitative research
examining readers’ theatre and its effectiveness on reading development, with only 12
studies found considering readers’ theatre with primary students. The literature review was
dissertations. Case studies and action research studies were accepted due to the limited
research availability. All studies included participants within grades one to five. All studies
included one or more of the following dependent variables: fluency, comprehension, and
Studies in this review did not include studies with readers’ theatre with techniques with
computer technology or subject areas other than literacy, because the methods used did not
Education Source, Info Search, EBSCO, and ProQuest databases were searched. Additional
information was collected to inform this author of current readers’ theatre practices,
influences and instruction through journals, articles and educational websites as, The Reading
Teacher, Reading Horizons, Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, Review of Research in
the Classroom, Journal of Reading, Reading and Writing Quarterly, Reading Research
Quarterly, and Journal of Educational Psychology. Twelve studies were found with search
terms that included “Readers Theater” spelled in its various forms and combinations
added to each of the above-mentioned readers’ theatre spellings as individual searches, such
Other terms were used in isolation for additional studies related to elements of reading
of Expository content”. Additional sources were found through the articles’ bibliographies,
and appendices of the primary sources (Carrick, 2000; Dixon, 2007; Forney, 2013;
Gummere, 2004; Jagger, 2008; Johnson, 2011; Keehn, 2003; Morris, 2011; Mraz, Nichols,
Caldwell, Beisley, Sargent, & Rupley, 2013; Millin & Rinehart, 1999; Smith, 2011).
The initial search resulted in 1,198 peer-reviewed studies, articles, and dissertations
between the years of 1935-2016. All were reviewed and out of these initial abstracts, 328
were read to discern the contents regarding relevance and to meet the inclusion criteria for
this study. Only 12 studies met the inclusion criteria. The review discusses type of article
with case studies, followed by action research studies, then concludes with unpublished
dissertations. This investigation found 1 case study, 1 action research study, 3 peer reviewed
study, and 7 dissertations. All the studies included one or more of the measures identified in
the search. Table 1 shows readers’ theatre effectiveness on fluency, comprehension, and
fluency, and motivation is relatively new, as majority of current information is written for
instructional and informative purposes. The studies reviewed are the most current research
meeting criteria, ranging from 1999-2016. See Table 1 for the study characteristics of the
Readers’ Theatre Effectiveness on Fluency, Comprehension, and Motivation on Primary Students Study Characteristics
Study Intervention Grade Student Length Study Design Fidelity Standardized Results
Description Reported Assessment
Carrick The effects of readers’ 5th 179 total 12 weeks Quasi- Researcher Pre-and Posttests: Readers’ theatre
2000 theatre on fluency and Large urban 60 minutes experimental observations, TerraNova Level had greater gains in
comprehension on 5th special per day Pre/post test teacher 15 (McGraw-Hill) fluency.
Dissertation grade students in needs Three groups: checklists, Analytical Reading Yet, there were no
regular classrooms district. Readers’ journals, Inventory (Woods & significant
using traditional Intervention theatre evaluations, Moe, 1995) – passages differences for
method, paired took place Paired reading and used for voice fluency or
reading and readers’ in four Control. observations. recordings. comprehension.
theatre different Intact
schools classrooms, ten Motivation
within same classrooms, No effect given,
district. four schools. through
observations RT
increase
motivation.
Dixon Effects readers’ 4th 85% and 6 weeks Non-equivalent External Test of Word Reading Significant gains
2007 theatre has on fluency 87% Experimental evaluators, Efficiency (TOWRE), were made between
and comprehension Caucasian, Pre/post test checklists, Gates-McGinitie treatment and
Dissertation via expository text. 3%/4% observations, Reading Tests control groups for
African Treatment and and walk- (GMRT). fluency and
American, Control groups. thru. comprehension.
11&/8% Intact
Hispanic, classroom, 2
and 1% campuses.
Asian-
Pacific
Islander
Forney Teaching content 4th 66 total 4 weeks Quasi- No No pretest was given. Results show that
2012 material through Researchers experimental information Posttest: there was no
readers’ theatre school using three given. Researcher created significant
Dissertation affiliated groups: multiple-choice test. difference between
with a readers’ theatre Retention test: the three groups.
Florida silent reading multiple-choice test. Scores for retention
University. and. round did show
Intact robin reading significant
classrooms. differences.
19
Table 1 (con’t.)
Study Intervention Grade Student Length Study Design Fidelity Standardized Rests
Description Reported Assessment
Gummere Readers Theater: Its 1st Middle to 7 weeks Quasi- All Multi-Dimensional There were no
2004 impact on fluency, upper experimental intervention Fluency Scale (MFS) significant
retell comprehension, socio- Pre/post test material DORF (DIBELS Oral differences in oral
Dissertation and motivation in first economic Comparison given to Reading Fluency) reading rate.
graders population group. Both teachers Motivation to Read- Comprehension,
Took place groups used before study, Full Scale and motivation.
in one regular journal kept
school, with classroom during
intact curriculum, intervention
classrooms. with the and
addition of researcher
readers’ theatre. observation.
Intact
classrooms.
Jagger The effect of reader’s 5tht Total 82 8 weeks Quasi- Researcher DORF (DIBELS Oral Treatment showed
2008 theater on fifth- Urban Experimental conducted Reading Fluency) greater mean
graders reading school nonequivalent observations Gates McGinitie change scores than
Dissertation fluency and One school, control group. Reading Test (GMRT) the control group in
comprehension based intact Intact all three areas of
on reading levels classrooms. classrooms. measure, yet, none
No other showed to be
information significant.
given.
Johnson The effect of a 2nd 44 total 9 weeks Quasi- Meetings STAR (Renaissance Results show a
2011 readers’ theatre 68% experimental and Learning Center) significant
instruction on second- African Nonequivalent observations. DORF (DIBELS Oral difference for
Dissertation grade student’s American pretest posttest. Reading Fluency) fluency with
fluency and 4% Treatment and treatment. There
comprehension skills European Control. Intact are no significant
Asia classrooms. differences in
9% Latino comprehension
13% Asia
4% multi-
ethnicity
One school,
intact
classrooms.
20
Table 1 (con’t.)
Study Intervention Grade Student Length Study Design Fidelity Standardized Results
Description Reported Assessment
Millin & Rinehart The effects of readers’ 2nd 28 9 weeks Mixed The reading Fluency: Results showed
1999 theatre participants on Low to 45 experimental specialist Qualitative Reading Treatment group
oral reading ability, middle minutes pre/posttests. scored Inventory (QRI) for showed a
comprehension and socio- samples, fluency large gains in oral
daily
motivation economic All participants researcher Comprehension: word recognition
status, took part in observations Whole group retelling and
urban treatment. No Motivation: comprehension.
public Title control. Elementary Reading Study states that
1 school Scale Assessment the pre-and post-
(ERAS) test scores did not
differ.
Morris The effect of readers’ 2nd 22 total 12 weeks Pretest, posttest None Fluency: Pretest showed
2011 theater on reading Suburban Researchers repeated reported Qualitative Reading 85% (N = 15) of
fluency and attitudes elementary class measure Inventory (QRI) higher readers
Action Research towards reading. school in all students Comprehension: reached the 77-
San participated Multi-dimensional wpm ceiling,
Francisco. in treatment Fluency Scale (MFS) Therefore,
Total at the same Motivation: study focused on
student time. Elementary Reading improvement and
population Attitude Survey scores for the at-
492 (ERAS) risk group (N = 7).
398 Results showed
Caucasian improvement
29 Asian
65 Other
Young & Implementing 2ndd 29 Total Full Quantitative Not given Developmental No statistical data
Rasinski readers’ theater as an Title 1 academic and qualitative Reading Assessment analysis was
2009 approach to classroom Mono- year research case (DRA) reported.
fluency instruction lingual 20-25 study Texas Primary Reading All students had
Peer Review Study students minutes 2 Inventory treatment.
times a (TPRI) Researchers
week indicated that
5-10 fluency was
minutes 2 increased.
times a
week
21
22
A typical scenario and pattern of readers’ theatre intervention would consist of the
following activities (Millin & Rinehart, 1999; Martinez, Roser, & Strecker, 1998).
Day 1: Teacher reads script, discussion on character, plot, setting, and teacher gives
out student scripts, teacher guides while students read, teacher assigned parts,
Day 2: Students listen to tape of script reading while reading along. Students are put
Day 3: Teacher listens to groups, they polish reading parts, read aloud, students are
given six questions regarding script, and students take home script.
Day 4: Dress rehearsal with teacher guidance, practice, and take home script.
Day 5: Performance
Case Study
Students with reading problems need multiple occasions and time to read if they are
to attain competency in oral reading fluency. Unfortunately, many at-risk readers become
frustrated, less motivated, and give up on reading. This results in the inability to develop
word recognition skills, word rate, vocabulary, and prosody, working together to extract
meaning from text (Nathan & Stanovich, 1991; Mraz et al., 2013).
In their review, Mraz et al. (2013) discussed recent strategies used to nurture fluency
with struggling readers, emphasizing repeated reading. This case study is based on the
authors involvment in an intervention led by a third-grade teacher. The school was reported
as a high poverty school with 85% of students receiving free and reduced lunch.
All of the 19 participants were African American; 13 lived in single parent homes, 3 were
repeating, and 3 qualified for special services. All of the students began the school year
23
below grade level. The intervention lasted six weeks, with 30-minute daily sessions. All
participants performed identical activities and readings in the same session. The intervention
was duplicated each week, with the exception of new texts. Each day began with a mini-
lesson alternating forms of whole group reading; shared reading, paired reading, echo
reading, and choral reading with expression. The mini-lesson was followed by small group
instruction and individual or partner reading. While the teacher provided feedback and
guidance, the session would conclude with a rehearsal to prepare for the weekly
effective reading method increasing fluency and comprehension scores. The study did not
report data findings in statistical effects. Results showed that prior to the intervention, the
class had an average fluency rate of 55 word-per-minute (WPM). The posttest results
revealed a dramatic increase in reading rate, ranging from 21 to 47 WPM, and a collective
going from a frustrating reading level to almost an independent reading level. Although
readers’ theatre. The study did not include a control group for comparison as all students
participated in the treatment. Since all the students simultaneously took part in the various
reading methods, there was not a definitive way to discern which strategy or skill may have
been the cause of improvement. It is unclear what skills and strategies the class may have
suburban California elementary school. The construct of the 12-week intervention consisted
of three, 4-week units of study. In the first unit, after a pretest was given, four weeks of
regular fluency instruction was implemented. Regular classroom curriculum included a basal
reading program, followed by a posttest. During the following four-week unit, treatment was
applied along with the regular curricululm including a pre- and posttest. The third four-week
unit mimicked the first week using regular curriculum with no treatment (except for the
knowledge learned from the treatment application during the second unit).
reading. Pretest indicated that 68% (n = 15) of students reached the target of 77 WPM. The
higher readers remained consistent through the duration of the intervention, showing little
change in the posttests scores. The result may have been influenced by the use of the same
passage for the pretest and posttest, therefore, repeated practice may have had an effect on
the increase of higher readers. In addition, details were not given on what the instruction
entailed within the regular classroom curriculum that may have enhanced reading skills. The
study did not include a comparison group, but as a whole group, with a subgroup of less-
skilled readers. Morris (2011) indicated the at-risk readers improved their oral rate with
three students meeting the 77 WPM target by the end of the intervention. Three students
exceeded the target rate with an average pretest score of 55 WPM, posttest score of 100
WPM, with an increase of 30 words. The researcher did not conduct a statistical analysis,
but used points and words per minute to achieve outcomes. The researcher reported that
25
readers’ theatre maintained and improved oral reading fluency and comprehension for the
less-skilled students. To assess motivation, Morris used the Elementary Reading Attitude
Survey (ERAS) and indicated that readers’ theatre did not have a strong effect on motivation.
from five Title 1 reading classes in two neighboring elementary schools. The participants
were randomly assigned within their intact classrooms to either the experimental group (i.e.,
readers’ theatre) or control (i.e., regular classroom curriculum) group. Fourteen students that
were assigned to Campus A, served as the experimental group, where they were then divided
into two smaller experimental groups of six to eight. Another group of 14 students from
Campus B or C, comprised the control group, and the remaining students from Campus C
Resource books written for readers’ theatre, trade books, and a basal provided the
reading content for the study. The control group worked with the basal readers used in the
regular reading curriculum. The intervention followed a researcher created protocol, with
data collected at the end of the nine-week period. Results indicated that participation in
readers’ theatre showed a moderate influence in oral reading ability (word recognition,
prosody, phrasing) and word meaning. Outcomes reflected significant effects for oral
reading rate and comprehension, indicating readers’ theatre as an effective tool for
improvement. Millin and Rinehart’s findings support other fluency research and the benefits
of readers’ theatre for at-risk readers (Carrick, 2000; Keehn, 2003; Mraz et al., 2013).
Outcomes from interviews and observations indicated that readers’ theatre had an impact on
26
about reading while exploring various genres beyond the basal reader. Teachers noticed
improvement on word recognition, vocabulary, and knowledge of word meanings (Millin &
Rinehart, 1999). Less-skilled students self-corrected, read in phrases, and used intonation.
Once read word-by-word, students improved in retelling and comprehension (Millin &
Rinehart, 1999).
reading, Keehn’s (2003) study investigated the effects of explicit instruction on fluency
through different teaching methods. Research has advocated for explicit instruction in
fluency and its components (Zutell & Rasinski, 1993). Keehn based his study on
instructional methods that included modeling, rereading, and discussion. Research has found
the combination of these components promotes oral reading growth and self-awareness of
fluency performance (Keehn, 2003; Rasinski, Padak, Linek & Sturtevant, 1994). The
participants were second graders from a rural Texas school district with a diverse ethnicity
population. Out of the 66 students who received the readers’ theatre intervention, two classes
were randomly assigned to the treatement group with explicit instruction, while two classes
received readers’ theatre without instruction. The readers’ theatre group with explicit
instruction followed the intervention protocol designed by Martinez et al. (1999) which
included steps to create critical thinking skills, teacher coaching, and student assigned parts.
In addition, texts were chosen based on three reading level abilities to ensure students would
be reading within their instructional level. The intervention included multiple readings of all
parts by all the students, teacher modeling, and discussions on character attributes
27
(Martinez et al., 1999). At the conclusion of the nine-week intervention, data was collected
using pretest and posttest measures. Results indicated both treatment groups made
significant growth in oral reading fluency using rereading, modeling, and use of appropriate
texts. However, there was not a significant difference between the two groups in rate,
the study indicates the combination of modeling, repeated reading, and discussion increased
students’ fluency. Keehn’s (2003) treatment group of explicit fluency instruction did not
support the theory that students who receive teacher feedback improves the benefits of
repeated reading fluency development (Therrian, 2004). This may be explained by the
inconsistency of the mini-lessons. After the initial mini-lessons were taught in the first week,
further lessons were not taught again until the seventh and eighth week. Findings revealed
that the most gains were achieved with the lower-ability readers; possibly due to the lower
reading texts being so accessible. It also provided an opportunity to read at a faster rate
producing comprehension. It is also noted that the lower-level readers had “more room to
grow” (Keehn, 2003). Findings indicated that repeated reading of text based on the
individual’s reading level and ability is of crucial importance in fluency improvement. The
primary intention of Keehn’s study was to examine if fluency was increased with or without
explicit intruction, using the method of readers’ theatre. In doing so, both groups practiced
repeated reading, which has been shown in other research to be effective in fluency
development (Chomsky, 1978; Samuels S., 1979; Tyler & Chard, 2000). However, it is
unclear how readers’ theatre was the catalyst for the improvement when both groups used the
28
treatment. Stronger effects may have been produced with the inclusion of a control group
that used the regular daily reading curriculum without readers’ theatre.
Young and Rasinski (2009) also conducted an action research study that examined
how readers’ theatre may improve fluency and comprehension. The one-year- study took
place in a Title 1 school, with a total of 29 second-grade participants. Like Keehn (2003) all
students participated in the readers’ theatre intervention, which became a product of the daily
90-minute reading instruction. During the days of independent reading and workstations, the
primary researcher conducted small-guided reading groups. Outcomes were not reported in
effect sizes and no data was given to calculate effects. Information that was reported reveal
substantial growth was made over the school year in fluency rate, automaticity, and word
recognition. Students increased from 62.7 WPM to 127.6 WPM, reflecting an increase of 65
words. These results based on Hasbrouck and Tindal (2006) puts these students in the 50th
and 75th percentile for second graders. The action research study included many variables
that could have contributed to the fluency increase, finding it difficult to identify the
dominant factor that produced the gains. Young explained that although readers’ theatre,
repeated reading, and fluency rate may not be entirely responsible for the fluency rate
increase, it is likely that reader’s theatre had a major impact. Young found treatment scores
to be greater than the other second grade classes who did not implement readers’ theatre.
Young reports that through observer, student, and parent discussions, motivation improved
Parents responded in dialogue about the positive impact that their child had in
reading, while students claimed they looked forward to reading and enjoyed learning (Young
& Rasinski, 2009). This study may have produced the positive outcomes the researchers had
anticipated, but it does not provide sufficient information to state that readers’ theatre was the
cause of those changes. It is also vague in its description on the role of readers’ theatre, apart
from other forms of reading used within the literacy block and small group instruction. The
alternating of different texts four days a week, did not allow for repetition, which has been
shown to be effective for word recognition, accuracy, and rate (Samuels, 1979). It is
theatre study were included in the mini-lessons or small group sessions. Information is also
lacking on the level of content used during the mini-lesson and small group sessions.
Additional factors that may have compromised the results is the amount of intensity placed
on other methods of reading the students practice, the amount of daily reading of each
diverse fifth-grade students from four different schools within the same district. The study’s
purpose was to examine the use of readers’ theatre to improve reading rate, accuracy, and
(readers’ theatre), a control group (no treatment), and a paired reading group. Groups were
also not clear whether the paired reading partners were matched by ability level, randomly
chosen by teacher, or chosen by students. The experimental groups’ protocol was based on
30
The Elementary Drama Curriculum Guide (1985), resembling protocols created by Millin &
Rinehart (1999) and Martinez et al. (1998). The control group read to each other during each
session and recorded responses in journals on how well they read and how well they listened.
Different texts were given to the paired reading group each day, while the control group had
Findings showed the experimental group significantly improved on both oral reading
fluency and comprehension compared to the control group and paired reading group. Carrick
stated that while it is clear that the experimental group would have greater gains than the
control group in fluency rate, a few intervention elements may have contributed to the
differences found between the experimental and paired repeated reading group in fluency.
The readers’ theatre group read the same script each day, while the paird reading group
passages changed daily. Therefore, the treatment group had the advantage of repititionh
which could influence rate and automaticity. The students kept journals to record thoughts
and ideas on the reading process and intervention. Overall, the readers’ theatre group were
engaged, motivated, and enjoyed performing. Several went as far as to say they were
dissappointed or frustrated if they would miss the sessions, due to pull-outs of other subjects.
In contrast, most of the paired readers’ expressed boredom, were underwhelmed, and tired of
the paired reading process. Some students expressed the desire to withdraw after the first
four weeks of the intervention. Teachers commented that the paired reading groups became
more of a chore to get the students motivated and to follow through with the lessons.
31
It has been suggested that children need to have access to literature based on their
ability level to improve fluency and comprehension skills, and to strengthen their reading
development (Adams, 1990; Hiebert, 2005). Gummere (2004) investigated the effects of
readers’ theatre on oral reading fluency, retell comprehension, and motivation on first grade
students ability levels. This quasi-experimental study took place in two intact first- grade
classrooms. The experimental group (n = 20) used readers’ theatre integrated in the regular
reading curriculum, followed the same intervention protocol as Martinez et al. (1998) which
used scripts based on students’ reading ability levels. The control group (n = 19) continued
the reading program without readers’ theatre. Data collected after the seven-week
intervention showed that the treatment group did not improve on oral reading fluency.
stating that fluency is not only word-per-minute, but is comprised of multiple skills
(Allington, 1983; Martinez et al., 1998; Zutell & Rasinski, 1991). Gummere contended that
the prosodic skills assessment reflects significant improvement. This assertion presents
conflict, as it appears there is a discrepancy in the reported data. The analysis did not report
effect sizes, but included the posttest scores for the treatment with a means of 8.45 (3.35),
control means of 7.63 (2.56), and an alpha of (p = .07). Using the information reported in
Gummere’s Table 4. Childrens Performance on Pretests and Posttests, the results indicated
minimal improvement. To test for comprehension, a student would read a passage followed
by retelling. As the student retold what they read, the assessor would count how many words
were used in the retell for one minute. Results showed that the comparison group had a
higher retell score when compared to the experimental group, but there was not a significant
32
difference between the two groups. Gummere suggests that a power analysis (with power at
.328) may have been to weak to detect effects and would indicate 32.8%, or 13 (1 in 3)
samples failed to detect effects through the statistic (Gummere, 2004). Most research studies
select 80% power, hoping to explain potential effects can be attributed to differences between
the groups to generalize to the population. The Central Limit Theorum suggests the bigger
the sample, the greater the power to obtain greater effects (Field, 2009). This study would
need 31 out of the 39 samples to achieve 80%. This study was underpowered and unable to
substantiate effects for readers’ theatre. It would have been beneficial and informative if this
study would have conducted a statistical analysis to determine any effects. Even if
significant differences were not found, the information might contribute to inform future
Results showed that readers’ theatre was not effective on oral reading fluency or retell
comprehension between the two ability groups. Gummere suggested that the above level
readers had already reached a higher level of fluency, therefore, the comprehension effect
was not as crucial. However, continous instruction in the prosodic elements of reading is
critical. It is possible the short duration of intervention and small sample size failed to detect
differences between these ability groups. In respect to the below or on-level readers, there
Dixon (2007) extended prior readers’ theatre studies and investigated the effect of
readers’ theatre on fluency and reading comprehension with a highly diverse ethnic
population of fourth grade students using expository text. A total of 172 participants from
two campuses took part in the intervention. The experimental group used expository reading
33
as cross subjects of social studies and science. The nine-week intervention utilized four days
per week, with days alternating between the two subjects. Each subject was instructed by
two different teachers who followed the same instructional pattern. Day 1 of both social
studies and science, consisted of listening to the teacher read, model expression, prosody and
individually and in groups while the teacher gave feedback. The session would end with
students blocking the placement of characters during the performances, proceeded by the
performance. During the same six-weeks, the control group continued with district
results showed that readers’ theatre did not reflect gains in fluency over the control group.
Likewise, Dixon (2007) reports no meaningful differences between the two groups in
comprehension.
readers’ theatre enhances fifth graders reading fluency and comprehension. Jagger also set
out to examine if readers’ theatre had differential effects based on reading abilities. This
quasi-experimental control group design employed four 5th-grade classrooms, for a total of
82 participants. Two classrooms became the treatment group and two participated as the
control group. A pretest and posttest were used to form groups based on ability levels. The
treatment group consisted of a systematic intervention of readers’ theatre, as part of the 90-
minute reading block. The weekly routine took only 5 to 20 minutes of instructional time,
with the remaining time dedicated to the basal reading curriculum. The control group
continued with the district’s basal-oriented reading curriculum for the complete 90-minute
34
literacy block. Supporting the district’s adopted reading curriculum, the readers’ theatre
intervention was created as a complimentary tool to enhance the already established 5th-
grade objectives and standards on reading of texts. The weekly intervention emulated
Carrick’s (2000) study using the Elementary Drama Curriculum Guide (1985). The first
week consisted of mini-lessons discussing readers’ theatre elements, fluency skills, effective
rehearsal strategies, and group rehearsal protocol. Each week’s intervention was as follows:
Day 1, students received scripts prepared earlier by teachers who assigned and highlighted
parts based on ability levels. Day 2 consisted of students self-monitoring the reading of
script with expression, accuracy, and rate. The teacher would observe and provide feedback
and modeling, which is a critical component of effective repeated readings during the
transference into other texts (Chomsky, 1978; Samuels, 1979). Students then rehearsed the
script several times. Day 3 was similar to Day 2; students rehearsed while the teacher
stopping to provide their peers with constructive feedback. Day 5 the treatment group
performed the readers’ theatre play. Data was collected after the conclusion of the eight-
week intervention and showed no differences between the readers’ theatre and control group
In summary, Jagger (2008) found the results unexpected due to the amount of
research that supports repeated reading to be highly effective on fluency rate (Dowhower,
1987; Hiebert, 2005; Samuels, 1979). Jagger used findings from Carver’s (1989) study, that
the average student will increase reading rate by 10 to 12 words per school year, 2.5 to 5
WPM per-quarter of the academic year. Using this information, the control group gained
35
5.02 WPM, while the treatment group increased 9.61WPM in the length of eight weeks. In
comparison, Fuchs and Fuchs (1993) found that for the average second grader, a weekly oral
Jagger’s calculation of 9.61 WPM for the treatment group is more aligned with Fuchs and
Fuchs study. An observation to point out, is that Carver’s study is based on silent reading
rates obtained through comprehension, while Jagger’s study is analyzed on oral reading rates.
In using Carver’s values, Jagger contends that a longer intervention period or a larger sample
size may have increased the means in word per minute (Jagger, 2008).
as an instructional tool and a motivation factor in reading. The ten-week study used readers’
theatre as the treatment group and an alternative treatment of repeated reading as the
comparison group. Participants were 85 second-grade students from a small suburban Title 1
school in Pennsylvania. Two classes (n = 43) served as the treatment group, and two other
(n = 42) served as the alternate treatment group. The treatment group supplemented the basal
curriculum with readers’ theatre, while the comparison group utilized the basal curriculum
with repeated reading. Using the Harcourt, Inc. Storytown series, the treatment (i.e., readers’
theatre) followed guidelines established in the basal program, similar protocol used in other
research studies based on readers’ theatre. The alternate comparison group (i.e., repeated
reading) also utilized the Harcourt, Inc. Storytown reading series. The group activities
included students independent reading and paired reading, while switching partners on a
daily basis. During this time, teachers observed the groups and offered feedback. Smith
found there were no significant differences between the treatment and alternative treatment
36
groups in any of the posttest variables with little effects for fluency, attitude, and motivation.
The results in this study did not support the claim that readers’ theatre positively influences
The rationale was to test the theoretical framework of readers’ theatre in a classroom setting to
determine if readers’ theatre can influence automaticity, oral fluency accuracy, and prosody.
Previously explained in chapter one, automaticity is the ability to recognize words without any
forethought while simultaneously using other reading skills (Samuels, 1979). Accuracy is
ability to decode printed words (Johnson, 2011), and prosody is the inflection of tonal quality,
pitch, and rhythmic patterns of spoken language (Harris & Hodges, 1995). Johnson explored
readers’ theatre and its effect on fluency and comprehension through a systematic instructional
intervention. This study was conducted using data collected from a pretest and posttest. The
study also determined if treatment differences were apparent in fluency and comprehension
based on ability reading levels, especially low achieving readers. The nine-week study
consisted of 44 second-grade students, divided into the two groups, readers’ theatre and control.
The treatment group (i.e., readers’ theatre) was divided into four smaller instructional groups
according to reading abilities. The weekly scripts were written on a first through third grade
reading level based on the students reading ablities. The diverse reading materials enabled the
students to read on an independent level, which was hypothesized to produce greater gains
(Hiebert, 2005; Johnson, 2011; Young & Rasinski, 2009). The treatment group followed a
daily protocol that included teacher modeling, role assignments, repeated practice, listening,
teacher coaching and feedback, and performance. On the third day of the intervention, the
37
teacher checked for comprehension by asking questions on the text while students responded in
writing. The business as usual control groups continued regular classroom literacy instruction
using a basal program and activities. Information was not available reflecting the effects,
means, and standard deviations. The study found that the students who participated in the
readers’ theatre group increased their fluency reading rate, but failed to show significant
When reading text, what students learn is based on prior knowledge retrieved from
stored memory to create new knowledge gathered from information in content material (Duke
et al., 2011). Forney (2013) set out to examine readers’ theatre as a method to comprehend and
retain information compared to silent reading and round robin reading. Participants were three
4th-grade classes from a research lab- school affliated with a Florida university. It was
assumed the 66 participants had no knowledge on the topic of the intervention, therefore, a pre-
test was not given. All students in each classroom, took part in all three reading conditions of
silent reading, round robin and readers’ theatre. All of the students read the same text, both
nartraive and expository using one of the three reading formats. Each session was preceded
with a five question multiple-choice test. The protocol for the readers’ theatre group included
lessons, rehearsals, discussions, guidance, and performance. Detailed documentation was not
provided regarding the activities the other two groups engaged in. However, standard
application for silent reading lends itself by the title of the method, while round robin generally
requires guidance. The researcher reported that on the last day of Week 1 the silent reading and
round robin groups took a second retention test from the first day’s reading, while the readers’
theatre group took the same assessment at the start of the following week. The classes
38
alternated the reading approach using different texts each week. The intervention concluded
The studies’ findings were mixed. Results conducted for comprehension showed a
moderate effect for all reading methods. A limitation to the study might be the minimal
training teachers received. Fidelity was not secured to ensure all teaching was identical, the
four-week intervention did not allow adequate time, and students were restricted to each
reading treatment only once. This study did not include adequate practice time for groups
Jagger, 2008; Johnson, 2011; Keehn, 2003). Repeated reading has been shown to be critical for
students reading development along with teacher feedback (Keehn, 2003), neither of which was
present in this study. According to Forney (2013), the findings are not strong enough to
conclude that reader’s theatre is effective and worth incorporating into the reading curriculum.
Knowledge Acquisition
(Harris & Hodges, 1995). Forney (2013) found that readers’ theatre had greater effects on
retention as opposed to alternate reading methods of silent reading and round-robin reading.
The assessments were not given consistently, as two groups were tested at the end of the first
week and the third group was tested three days later. This potentially provided time for
discussion between the students who took the test and those who had yet to take it. The
study does not provide adequate information on the content of the assessments, only to say
that the groups alternated between narrative and expository text within a week’s time
changing texts each week. The final retention assessment was given in a whole group setting
39
at the end of the final week. However, the study’s results are vague regarding the
assessments content, scoring, and results. In addition, an assessment was not given to assess
intervention.
Motivation
In the early psychology studies, motivation became the dominant topic of research as
theorists differed on motivational framework and mechanisms, while more current studies
have focused on behavior and metacognition (Paris & Paris, 2001; Smith, 2011). While
many factors can determine motivation, Gambrell, Palmer, Codling, and Mazzoni (1996)
listed necessary elements in the classroom that can be considered motivational factors; (a)
access to a plethora of books, (b) socialization with books, (c) exposure to genres, and (d)
choice of books. In addition to providing texts and reading materials, it is equally important
for educators to be conversant in the types of instructional reading methods such as readers’
Readers’ theatre promotes enthusiasm about the tasks involved in performance; from
preparation, rehearsal, and peer interaction (Flynn, 2004). Students understand in order to be
well-prepared for a performance, it is necessary to read the text multiple times to understand
the meaning of the text as they focus on what other characters are saying (Flynn, 2004;
Moran, 2006). Readers’ theatre has been shown to motivate and excite students as they
anticipate new scripts and performances (Casey & Chamberlain, 2006; Griffith & Rasinski,
2004). This literature review found limited research demonstrating that readers’ theatre is
effective for enhancing motivation. Some of the studies in this review chose to administer
40
pretests and posttests surveys to assess differences in motivation, while other studies used
observation.
Morris (2011) used the Elementary Reading Attitude Scale to consider motivation (ERAS;
McKenna and Kerr, 1990). The Garfield pictorial assessment consists of questions
pertaining to reading attitudes. Answers are coded with five Garfield cartoon figures, each
exhibiting a different emotion from happy to mad. The student chooses the figure that best
represents their answer for the specific question. Smith (2011) used the ERAS to examine
motivation between readers’ theatre and repeated reading before the intervention and after.
The study did not show significant differences between the two groups, including across
gender. There are several reasons for the lack of motivational differences: 1) the lack of
focus, 2) inability to hear, read, or understand the question, 3) randomly chosen answers, 4)
Gummer’s (2004) study was the only study that included a control group and a
standardized measure of motivation, while other studies (Forney, 2013, Millin & Rinehart,
1999; Smith, 2011) did not include a control group in their design. The inclusion of a control
group provides a baseline to compare results and isolates the independent variable to rule out
other explanations from the results (Field, 2009). The outcomes of the current study did not
show positive results, and the overall assumption that reader’s theatre is a positive
motivational factor is difficult to determine when both conditions took part in the treatment
with no comparison.
41
In summary, a review of the 12 primary studies concluded that readers’ theatre had
mixed results, adding to similar outcomes of prior research. Seven of the studies utilized
pretest posttest control group design. Out of the seven studies, Carrick (2000), Dixon (2007),
Keehn (2003), and Johnson (2011) showed readers’ theatre had some effect on oral reading
fluency, while Jagger (2008) showed some movement in the mean gains, but no significant
differences between the two groups. Dixon (2007) and Smith (2011) showed no differences
between intervention and control. Only Dixon (2007) showed significant differences
between the two conditions for comprehension. The remaining studies did not use a
comparison group. Millin & Rinehart (1999) administered pretest posttest measures and
found effects for oral reading fluency, while Gummere (2004) failed to produce effects.
Mraz et al. (2013) and Morris (2011) reported positive outcomes, however, they elected not
to use a comparison group and did not conduct a statistical analysis. While the studies may
be informative, the design does not allow conclusions to be drawn about reader theatre’s
Gummere (2004), Millin and Rinehart (1999), and Smith (2011) used standardized
assessments, with two of the three studies determined there were no effects for motivation.
In comparison, all the studies that assessed using informal discussions, observations and
interviews claimed that students became motivated to read, and saw a positive adjustment in
their reading attitude. Here again, there is no evidence to substantiate the claim. The
observations made by the educators are useful, yet, the possibility exists the results become
skewed or subjective in the desire for a positive outcome. There is not adequate information
to determine if reader’s theatre was the cause of any potential increase in motivation in
42
research that did not include a comparison group and without appropriate design to assess a
causal impact.
In this review, most of the researchers agreed that the duration of their intervention
was inadequate to produce anticipated effects, and advise future studies to lengthen the time
to obtain greater effects. Likewise, a majority of the studies concluded that the sample size
was also a factor that affected the low effect sizes. A larger sample size produces greater
statistical power, which enhances researchers’ ability to find an effect if one exists (Field,
2009). In looking at the reviewed studies, there was not one that randomly assigned
individual students into the condition groups. Random assignment allows for the equal
ensures equality between the conditions to establish internal validity, in that, if there should
be threats to the experiment, it would affect all treatment groups. School administrators and
teachers usually prefer to have as little interruption as possible in the classroom, and to
ensure all teachers and students are treated equally. To avoid administrators concern, studies
that utilized classes from multiple locations, assigned all classes at each location as the same
treatment group (Carrick, 2000; Dixon, 2007). Ten studies in this literacy review utilized
multiple classes in one location, each intact classroom randomly assigned to either treatment
group. It is more convenient for the researcher, teacher, and students in the same classrom to
be involved in the same tasks, which possibly eliminates student competition or envy of
seperate group’s activities and lessons. In the case of an treatment and control design, there
would be less training, less interruptions, and possibly less instrutional time with less groups.
Although, assigned intact classrooms can be part of an adequate design, it is not without
43
limitations and biases, such as competition between treatment and control groups. A control
group may feel less adequate than a treatment class, therefore, may be motivated to increase
work and tasks to increase posttest scores. A teacher that has been assigned a control group
may prefer to be a treatment and wants to apply the same lessons and ideas, therefore,
diffusing the study by speaking with a treatment teacher and sharing materials. The obvious
limitation of whole class treatment groups, is that there is not an equal opportunity for each
participant to take part in either treatment group. In choosing to randomly assign the whole
class to a specific treatmenet, it is possible that some classes are formed based on reading
levels, special needs, gifted, and interests. Such predetermined groupings could affect the
outcome of research done with intact classrooms assigned as one group. The benefit of using
random assignment is to ensure equivalence amongst the conditions. This means that the
difference between the groups is contributed to the treatment group on which they were
All but two studies chose to use narrative text, except for Dixon (2007) and Forney
(2013), who used expository text, defined as written information containing facts. These
texts are usually more difficult for students to understand, as expository text contains new
words that are not part of a common daily vocabulary, have difficult concepts to relate to
personal experiences, and are non-sequential in its written format (Hall et al., 2005). Dixon’s
(2007) results did not seem to differ on oral fluency and comprehension using expository
text, compared to studies that used narrative. It is possible effects were not met because the
research implemented both social studies and science only once a week, with the content
material changing weekly. A longer duration of time with each subject may have allowed for
44
the difficult genre to be examined. In addition, it may be beneficial for the expository text to
be on the same topic for a length of time due to its complexity. Further research is needed to
comprehension and motivation, and instructional methods. Considered a relatively new area
of experimental research, readers’ theatre has been explored as a reading technique intended
to impact reading development. Future studies are crucial to investigate various techniques
and methods in using readers’ theatre including the measurement of literacy elements (e.g.,
genres, text complexities) and the response of students with different reading abilities.
Summary
The ability to understand and derive meaning from text is central to reading
development (Durkin, 1992; Rasinski, 2006). LaBerge and Samuels (1974) automaticity
theory specifies that improved fluency may positively impact comprehension. Since the
theory was first introduced, substantial research has been conducted supporting fluency as an
influential component of comprehension (Fuchs et al., 2001; Kuhn &Stahl, 2003; Rasinski,
2010). Driving the urgency for fluency development, the National Reading Panel’s 2000
report suggested oral reading and repeated reading to be included in daily reading instruction.
Developed by Samuels (1979) repeated reading enlists multiple readings of text with a
predetermined goal. This method was to improve fluency, comprehension, motivation, and
attitude. As a form of repeated reading, reader’s theater requires multiple practices of the
script in preparation for the performance (Flynn, 2004; Martinez et al., 1999). Educators
45
recommend readers’ theatre as a strategy to motivate all levels of reading abilities (Corcoran,
2005) and a dramatic art genre interacting with literature to present a vivid performance
while seducing audience members into a spontaneous response (Carrick, 2001; Tanner,
1993), and as a beneficial and valuable instructional tool to enhance reading skills (Craig &
Smith, 1976; Post, 1972). Although there seems to be a plethora of practitioner support for
readers’ theatre as an instructional method to improve skills (Busching, 1981; Tyler & Chard,
2000; Worthy & Prater, 2002), a paucity of experimental research has been conducted to
Purpose of Study
The purpose for this study is to extend current research on readers’ theatre’s effect on
oral reading fluency, comprehension, and motivation. This study utilized expository text due
to the limited experimental studies on readers’ theatre using expository texts. Early
(Duke, 2000). Current research on expository text and its structure is important to students
progressing reading development (Englert & Hiebert, 1984). This study uses expository text
that is aligned with state standards and in cooperation with the classroom teachers (CCSS,
2016). This study is unique in that it is one of the only experimental studies to examine the
motivation.
46
Through the limited amount of studies that have been conducted, research has
indicated mixed-results in support of readers’ theatre and its influence. This research
improving oral reading fluency, comprehension, and motivation. Specifically, this study will
1. What is the effect of using readers’ theatre on the reading fluency of second
graders?
second graders?
reading attitude?
CHAPTER III
METHODOLOGY
improving fluency, comprehension, and motivation (Keehn, 2003; Martinez et al., 1999;
Millin & Rinehart, 1999). This study improves on the methods used in previous studies to
test the effectiveness of readers’ theatre on fluency, comprehension, and motivation. It also
provides information about long-term knowledge retention by testing knowledge two weeks
after the intervention. After receiving approval from the Middle Tennessee State University
IRB, schools and classrooms were approached to participate in the study. A location was
selected, and permission letters were handed out explaining the intent of the study. Only
those students who returned the signed consent form could be participants in the study (See
This study used a pretest posttest control group design to examine the effects of
readers’ theatre using expository text on fluency, comprehension, motivation, and knowledge
acquisition for second-grade elementary students. This chapter provides the methodology for
the study including the intervention, description of the participants, study design, measures,
1. What is the effect of using readers’ theatre on the reading fluency of second
graders?
second graders?
48
reading attitude?
Participants
The participants were from an elementary school located in a southern city in middle
Tennessee. The Title 1 school is part of a rural special school district within the city limits.
The teachers are the primary classroom teachers with their own intact classrooms. The
participants ranged in age from seven to nine years of age and were in intact classrooms
control condition (repeated reading). During the two-week intervention, one student
withdrew from the study due to relocation out of the district zone area. The study maintained
ethnicities.
Design
This experimental study was used to determine if readers’ theatre influences oral
group research design was chosen to test for differences between the intervention and the
dependent variables. The independent variable in this study is readers’ theatre. Readers’
comprehension, and motivation. This design was used so that posttest differences can be
attributed to the treatment and not to initial differences within the sample. This design
method effectively controls for the eight threats of internal validity (e.g., history, maturation,
maturation; Campbell & Stanley, 1963). The school administration selected the classroom
The participants rotated between two classrooms for reading and math instruction.
The classroom teachers divided the students into groups based on reading abilities of either
high to average or low average to low. This study’s 2-week intervention took place in the
classroom where the teacher conducted the reading instruction for two second grade
classrooms. The 2-week intervention took place during the regular 90-minute literacy block
for each group, totaling three hours per day, consisting of 40-minute sessions for each of the
two treatment and two control groups. The experimental pretest posttest was selected,
because random assignment across the selected grade level was not possible. There was total
of two treatment groups and two control groups within each 90-minute literacy block.
Randomizing the participants allows equal opportunity for each participant to take part in
This study’s content material was based on a social studies common core state
standard and school district standards written for the first-quarter grading period. The
expository text was used to determine if readers’ theatre increases comprehension and
50
mastery of informational content. (See Appendices C and D for readers’ theatre lesson plans
and script)
• Day 1: Explicit instruction with introduction of readers’ theatre, discussion for prior
and vocabulary, group reading, discussion on content and check for understanding.
character parts, students highlighted parts, character analysis, and written response of
practiced parts, researcher observed, listened, and gave feedback. Constructive and
staging and blocking a scene, group rehearsed, researcher coached, and group
discussed rehearsal. The last few minutes consisted of group discussion on readers’
theatre and the group’s opinions on the benefits, on content, and scripts. Session
• Day 5: The performance from the week’s work. Researcher and audience provided
Control Group
The researcher instructed the control group prior and after the treatment groups
instruction. The control group instruction included repeated reading passages based on same
content material as the treatment groups, mini-lessons, discussions, and activities. The
instruction was explicit and systematic. A folder was given to each student which included a
world map to label and color, a page to record facts and a reading passage. The control
group would read the reading passage daily using various formats, such as, individual read,
partner read, group read, and instructor read-aloud. The students read the passage on an
average of two to three times per day. See Appendices E and F for the control groups lesson
Measures
WIAT III was used to assess comprehension before the intervention and after. It is a
from K-12. The WIAT-III includes 16 subtests. This study administered the reading
comprehension, oral reading fluency, and word reading subtests only. The assessment
nonfiction, instructional texts, and advertisements. The participant was given the choice of
reading a passage aloud or silent, followed by a verbal response to literal and inferential
questions read by the administrator. The WIAT-III was normative standardized in the United
52
States with 2,775 students. Internal-consistency reliabilities are over .80, excluding a subtest
of Listening Comprehension and Sentence Completion, which report reliabilities of .75 and
.79.
words are written in a purposeful context, as opposed to reading them in a separate word list
(Jenkins et al., 2003; Stanovich, 1980). The easyCBM was used at pretest and posttest to
assess oral reading fluency rate. A one-minute timed reading passage was selected and
scores were obtained according to how many words were read per-minute, minus any
miscues. The easyCBM test-retest reliability for second grade ranged from .88 to .96 with a
median of .94, while reliability for the passage reading fluency report .91 to .95. Further,
generalization showed a G-coefficient from .95 to 97, while predictive and concurrent
validity ranges from .19 to .22, and construct validity for second-grade ranges from .91 to
.98.
Survey was administered as part of the pretest and posttest to measure students’ attitude
towards reading. The participants verbally selected a number from one to four that best
stated their reaction to a specific question about reading. The number scale given reflected
the following: 1–I don’t like it, 2–It’s okay, 3–I like it, and 4–I love it. The assessment was
scored based on the total of each item’s value that the participant selected. The assessment
was given to obtain knowledge of the participants reading attitude and motivation prior to
and after the completion of the intervention. Student interviews were also administered
open-ended questionnaire was used as observations only and not scored (See Appendix G
and H).
each week during the two-week intervention to test the effects of readers’ theatre. This
researcher created assessment consisted of 20-items, based on the weeks’ content material.
The assessment was created using a multiple question format such as, multiple choice, fill-in
the blank, true/false statements, and labeling (See Appendices I and J for the two knowledge
• The test items were similar, so the test scores were combined for analysis.
• To ensure that each test and its items would be at a similar level of difficulty and have
• The tests were scored in the same manner with each quiz having the same number of
items.
To eliminate the least amount of internal errors as possible, and to ensure a stronger
reliability, this study followed criteria established by Cohen and Wollack (2003):
(a) same testing conditions for all participants; (b) test administered verbally; (c) instructions
and questions easily understood, and (d) answers cannot be confusing as incorrect or correct
(Field, 2009).
54
administered four weeks after the intervention to determine if readers’ theatre contributes to
“mastery”, and its effect on student retention of information. The assessment consisted of a
long-term memory and mastery of subject material. The questions were taken from the
weekly comprehension assessments and written in a multiple question format such as,
multiple choice, fill-in the blank, true/false statements, and labeling. To ensure reliability,
the assessment was constructed with same items from the weekly comprehension
assessments, which was created following reliability protocols. The scores of each
participant’s weekly comprehension assessments and the delayed assessment were scored
All materials were provided for both treatment and control groups for the participants.
The content material was entered seven reliability calculators to decide readability level, age,
and grade appropriateness. The calculators are the Flesch-Kincaid Grade Level, Flesch-
Kincaid Reading Ease Formula, FOG Scale, SMOG Scale Index, Coleman-Lieu Index,
Automated Readability Index, and the Linsear Write Formula. The scores were totaled and
averaged to determine an appropriate grade level. The Lexile analyzer was used to assess the
same constructs as the readability calculators. In addition, assessors were asked to determine
reliability and validity of the created assessments and content material. These assessors
specialist, a professor, and doctoral students of literacy to ensure reading text and content
55
received feedback with the assessors through email and verbal communication.
Reliability and validity was decided using a checklist following given criteria. A 90-
percentile was agreed upon to meet expectations. The seven readability calculators were
utilized and came to a consensus that all created assessments met a second-grade readability
level ranging from grade 1.5 to 2.0. This was acceptable and met the criterion goal of the
researcher to reflect the average reading ability of students at the second-week of their
second academic year. Assessors found the content material (readers’ theatre and repeated
Fidelity of implementation. The pretest and posttest measures were given by the
researcher and two doctoral students who had experience administering the individual
Likert student reading survey and the reading attitude questionnaire. To insure fidelity of
implementation, all the test and intervention sessions were audiotaped and 30% (80 tests out
of 260) were randomly selected to be rescored. Fidelity was high on all pretest and posttest
measures with an overall fidelity of 96.25%, including the WIAT III pretests, (100%) and
posttest measures (90.5%), easyCBM pre-and posttest measures, (93.75%), Likert pre- and
posttest measures, (100%). The knowledge acquisition tests were rescored, some
discrepancies were found. The inter-rator and researcher consulted and in agreement scores
implementation of tests with 100%. The researcher conducted the 12 intervention sessions.
Each session was audiotaped and 25% were randomly selected for each condition. Fidelity
56
according to specific criterion. Fidelity met the 90% agreement with 96%.
Data Analysis
Four questions were examined during this study to determine the effectiveness of
readers’ theatre using expository text on oral reading fluency, comprehension achievement,
delayed knowledge acquisition, and motivation. The pretest and posttest scores were
1. What is the effect of using readers’ theatre on the reading fluency of second
graders?
2. What is the effect of using readers’ theatre on the reading immediate and delayed
reading attitude?
One-way ANCOVA’s were conducted for each dependent variable to assess the
for each dependent variable (i.e., fluency, comprehension, and motivation) were analyzed
individually using an analysis of covariance with pretest scores serving as the covariate. A
p < .05 levels was used to test the level of statistical significance.
57
Participants’ pretest and posttest scores from the assessments of motivation and the
WIAT-III subtests (comprehension, oral reading fluency, and word reading) were entered as
dependent variables into the SPSS program. Participants’ scores from the weekly
comprehension assessments served as the covariates and were analyzed using a one-way
ANCOVA with the two-week delayed knowledge acquisition as the dependent variable.
The researcher created motivational assessment utilized the Likert scale format with
answer values of 1, 2, 3, or 4 for each item. Each value represented an emotion the
participant felt best answered a given question. The value of 1–I don’t like it, 2–It’s okay, 3–
I like it, and 4–I love it. A total sum from each item was used to determine any differences in
format. The survey was not scored but used as an observational tool.
58
CHAPTER IV
RESULTS
comprehension, oral reading fluency, and motivation with primary students. This study
examined readers’ theatre impact on student mastery using expository content material. Four
quantitative questions were addressed as follows: (a) What is the effect of using readers’
theatre on the reading fluency of second graders? (b) What is the effect of using readers’
theatre on the immediate and delayed comprehension on second graders? (c) Does an
intervention using readers’ theater motivate second-graders’ reading attitude? (d) What is the
effect of using readers’ theatre on students’ knowledge acquisition four weeks after the
completion of the intervention? This chapter includes participant demographic data, pretest
measures, the outcomes of each quantitative research question independently, and discussion.
Demographic Data
The intervention research took place August through early September of 2016, at a
rural elementary school in middle Tennessee. The public elementary school qualified as a
Title 1 school, meeting all required eligibilities by the state and district departments of
education. The total student body enrollment was 630, with 72% Caucasian, 13.5% African
consented to be in the study. During the two-week intervention, one student withdrew from
the study due to relocation out of the district zone area. The study maintained a total of 26
subtests of oral reading fluency, word reading, and reading comprehension; an additional
standardized fluency measure; four researcher created measures (three comprehensions and
one motivation); and one non-scored reading attitude questionnaire. All tests were conducted
using raw scores from each assessment (Table 2). To ensure inter-rater reliability and
fidelity, 25% of all test protocols were reviewed and rescored by a second trained researcher,
Table 2
Means and Standard Deviations for Pretest and Posttest Measures by Condition
Treatment Control
(n = 13) (n = 13)
Variable M SD M SD M SD M SD
WIAT III
Oral Reading Fluency 69.6 30.1 88.0 33.3 74.0 36.3 90.7 39.8
Word Reading 30.1 9.6 31.4 8.3 29.6 6.8 30.7 6.6
Word Reading timed 23.6 4.8 28.1 5.2 24.7 7.8 27.1 7.0
Student Reading Survey 59.0 11.9 51.3 12.2 61.3 9.2 60.6 10.9
Treatment Control
M SD M SD
Oral reading fluency. The Wechsler individual achievement test III (WIAT III)
was used to help answer the research question, what is the effect of using readers’ theatre
on the reading fluency of second graders? The WIAT III was used for both pretest and
posttest in administering the subtest of oral reading fluency and the subtest of word
reading. The WIAT III subtest of oral reading consisted of two independent reading
The experimental and control groups were assessed prior to the research to
determine if there was a significant pretest differences on the WIAT III measure. An
independent samples t-test was conducted using the pretest scores on the dependent
variable. According to the t-test in oral reading fluency, student scores in the control
group were not significantly different from the experimental group (t(24) = -.335,
p = .205).
Standardized residuals for the intervention was not normally distributed between
the experimental group (p = .590), and control group (p = .008) as assessed by the
results also showed normality was not met, however, the researcher proceeded to conduct
the ANCOVA analysis despite the lack of normality. A test for homogeneity of
regression slopes confirmed a non-significant interaction effect between the WIAT III
oral reading fluency pretest scores and the group for the WIAT III oral reading posttest
scores, F(1, 22) = .081 p = .778, validating that the pretest scores could be used as the
It was hypothesized that the intervention would show a significant effect between
the readers’ theatre group and the control group. To investigate possible statistical
differences between the experimental and control groups on the WIAT III oral reading
fluency measure, a one-way ANCOVA was performed between the two groups through
SPSS, using the intervention (readers’ theatre) as the independent variable, the WIAT III
oral reading fluency posttest as the dependent variable, and the oral reading fluency
pretest as a covariate. After adjustments were made for pretest oral reading fluency
scores, results indicated that there was no significant difference between the control
group and experimental group on the post-intervention oral reading fluency scores
second oral fluency measure, is an independent reading passage, timed for one-minute.
Scores were configured as total words read after subtracting potential miscues. To
determine if any pretest differences existed between the easyCBM experimental and
control groups, an independent samples t-test was conducted using the pretest scores on
the dependent variable. According to the t-test, student scores for the control group
did not differ from the experimental group scores (t (24) = -.573, p = .147). A test for
homogeneity of regression slopes confirmed that there was not a significant interaction
effect between the easyCBM pretest scores and the groups, F(1,22) = .128,
p = .724, validating that the pretest scores could be used as the covariate in the
ANCOVA test. Standardized residuals for the interventions were normally distributed, as
the easyCBM pretest and posttest group means. To examine possible statistical
differences between the experimental group and control group on the easyCBM oral
reading fluency measure, an ANCOVA was conducted using the intervention (readers’
theatre) as the independent variable, the easyCBM oral reading fluency posttest as the
dependent variable, and the easyCBM pretest as a covariate. After adjustments were
made for pretest oral reading fluency scores, results indicated that there was no
oral reading fluency scores F(1,24) = 1.23, p = .279, partial 2 = .051. The ANCOVA
findings indicated no significant differences for the easyCBM oral reading fluency
between experimental and control pretest and posttest scores, therefore the hypothesis
120 120
90.70 99.4 d = -0.14
100 d = 0.01 100
84.5
74.00 99
Raw Scores
80 80
Raw Scores
88.00
76.4
60 60
69.60
40 Experimental Experimental
40
Control Control
20 20
0 0
Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest
Oral Oral easyCBM easyCBM
Reading Reading
Fluency Fluency
Figure 4.1. Graph comparison for the WIAT III oral reading fluency subtest pretest and
posttest means and the easyCBM oral reading fluency pretest and posttest means by
condition.
64
The WIAT III word reading subtest was used to examine oral reading rate using a
word list beginning with one-syllabic words graduating to more difficult multisyllabic
words. The treatment and control groups were assessed prior to the research to determine
if there was a significant pretest differences on the WIAT III measure. An independent
samples t-test was conducted using the pretest scores on the dependent variable.
According to the t-test in word reading, student scores in the control group were not
that the intervention would show a significant group effect between the WIAT III word
reading pretest and posttest scores. To investigate potential statistical differences between
the treatment and control groups on the WIAT III word reading measure, a one-way
ANCOVA was conducted between the two groups, using treatment (readers’ theatre) as
the independent variable, the WIAT III word reading subtest as the dependent variable, and
the subtest word reading pretest as a covariate. A test for homogeneity of regression slopes
confirmed the interaction between the WIAT III pretest scores and the groups for the WIAT
III posttest scores was not significant, F(1,22) = .346, p = .562, confirming that the pretest
scores could be used as the covariate in the ANCOVA test. After adjustments were made
for pretest word reading scores, ANCOVA results indicated that there was not a significant
difference between the treatment- and control-group on the post-intervention word reading
scores F(1,23) = .039, p = .849, partial 2 = .002, therefore, the hypothesis was not
accepted.
65
The WIAT III word reading subtest included a supplemental score which was a
timed component of 30 seconds. The time was recorded by the administrator and was not
revealed to the student as not to detour from reading the word list until the required four
consecutive missed words were met. The first word read within 30 seconds was “an”
with the hardest word read within 30 seconds was “custodian”. The treatment and
control groups were assessed prior to the intervention to determine if there was a
significant pretest differences on the WIAT III word reading supplemental measure. An
independent samples t-test was conducted using the pretest scores on the dependent
variable. According to the t-test, student scores in the control group were not
pretest scores and the supplemental posttest scores did not differ as the interaction term
was not statistically significant, F(1,22) = .525, p = .476, confirming that the pretest
scores could be used as the covariate in the ANCOVA test. It was hypothesized that the
intervention would find a significant effect between the treatment and control groups for
groups on the word reading supplemental score, an ANCOVA test was performed
between the two groups using treatment (readers’ theatre) as the independent variable, the
word reading supplemental posttest scores as the dependent variable, and the word
66
reading supplemental pretest scores as a covariate. After adjustments were made for
pretest word reading supplemental scores, ANCOVA results indicated that there was not
supplemental word reading scores F(1,23) = 2.25, p = 147, partial 2 = .089, therefore,
Pretest Word
Reading 29.62
untimed 30.15
d = -0.03
Posttest Word
Reading 30.77
untimed 31.46
Pretest Word
24.77
Reading timed 23.69
d = 0.29
Posttest Word
27.08
Reading timed 28.15
0 10 20 30 40
Control Experimental
Figure 4.2. Graph of the WIAT III pretest and posttest means for the word reading subtest
and the word reading timed subtest by condition.
Reading comprehension. The WIAT III was utilized to help answer the research
question, what is the effect of using readers’ theatre on the reading comprehension of
repeated as a posttest. The WIAT III subtest of reading comprehension consisted of three
The treatment and control groups were assessed prior to the research to determine
if there was a significant pretest differences on the WIAT III measure. An independent
samples t-test was conducted using the pretest scores on the dependent variable.
According to the t-test, in reading comprehension, student scores in the control group did
not indicate substantial differences from the experimental group (t(24) = -.741, p = .452).
the WIAT III reading comprehension raw pretest scores and the WIAT III reading
comprehension raw posttest scores, F(1,22) = . 53, p = .508, confirming that the pretest
scores could be used as the covariate in the ANCOVA test. It was hypothesized that the
groups for the WIAT III reading comprehension measure, a one-way ANCOVA was
performed between the two groups. The condition of experimental (readers’ theatre) was
used as the independent variable, the WIAT III reading comprehension subtest as the
After adjustments were made for pretest reading comprehension scores, ANCOVA
results indicated that there was not a significant effect on the WIAT III posttest scores
after controlling for the WIAT III reading comprehension pretest as a covariate. The
WIAT III reading comprehension means did not indicate a statistical difference
68
F(1,23) = .2.32, p = .141, partial 2 = .092, therefore, the hypothesis was not confirmed
35
28.8
30
23.3 d = - 0.35
25
Raw Score
25.4
20
21.2
15
10 Experimental
Control
5
0
Pretest Reading Posttest Reading
Comprehension Comprehension
Figure 4.3. Graph on the WIAT III pretest and posttest means for the reading
comprehension subtest by condition.
comprehension assessments were utilized to help answer the research questions: (a) What
is the effect of using readers’ theatre on the reading immediate and delayed
comprehension so second graders? and (b) What is the effect of using readers’ theatre on
students’ knowledge acquisition two weeks after the completion of the intervention.
The comprehension tests were administered at the end of each week during the
the intervention. The research hypothesis predicted that the intervention would show
significant effects between the treatment and control groups delayed acquisition scores.
An independent samples t-test was conducted using the knowledge acquisition test one
scores on the dependent variable. According to the t-test, student scores in the control
group did not indicate substantial differences from the experimental group (t(24) = 3.16,
p = .131).
between the treatment and control groups on the knowledge acquisition test one reading
the knowledge acquisition delayed reading comprehension test as the dependent variable,
that the intervention would find a significant effect on knowledge acquisition test one
between the treatment and control groups. Standardized residuals for the interventions
were normally distributed, as assessed by the Shapiro-Wilke’s test (p >.05). There was
F(1,24) = 2.32, p = .141. A test for homogeneity of regression slopes confirmed the
relationship between the knowledge acquisition delayed scores and the knowledge
acquisition test one scores did not differ as the interaction term was not statistically
significant F(1,22) = 1.28, p = .269, validating that the knowledge acquisition test one
scores could be used as the covariate in the ANCOVA test. After adjustments were made
for the knowledge acquisition test one covariate, results indicated that there was not a
significant difference between the treatment and control groups for the knowledge
acquisition test one and the delayed knowledge acquisition reading comprehension
70
F(1,23) = 2.214, p = .150, partial 2 = .088, therefore the hypothesis could not be
confirmed.
independent samples t-test was conducted using the knowledge acquisition test two
scores on the dependent variable. According to the t-test, student scores in the control
group indicated differences from the experimental group (t(24) = 2.74, p = .036).
the dependent variable, and the knowledge acquisition comprehension test two as a
assessed by the Shapiro-Wilke’s test, (p > .05). There was homogeneity of variance, as
A test for homogeneity of regression slopes revealed the relationship between the
delayed knowledge acquisition test scores and the knowledge acquisition test two scores
did not differ and were statistically significant F(1,22) = 2.33, p = .141, validating that
the knowledge acquisition test two scores could be used as the covariate in the ANCOVA
test. It was hypothesized that the intervention would find significant effects on the
knowledge acquisition delayed performance scores after controlling for the knowledge
acquisition test two as a covariate. After adjustments were made for knowledge
acquisition test two covariate, ANCOVA results indicated that there was not a significant
difference between the treatment and control groups for the delayed knowledge
acquisition reading comprehension test F(1,23) = 3.23, p = .085, partial 2 = .123, after
71
controlling for the covariate of knowledge acquisition test two, therefore the hypothesis
30 30
d = 1.25 d = 1.09 d = 1.32
d = 1.32
25 25
19.85 19.85
20 20
Raw Score
Raw Score
15 15
9.69
10 10 9.31 9.69
10.00
5 Experimental 5 Experimental
4.92 Control
4.62 Control
0 0
Knowledge Knowledge Knowledge Knowledge
Acquisition Acquisiton Acquisition Acquisition
Test One Delayed Test Two Delayed
Figure 4.4. Means for the knowledge acquisition test one and knowledge acquisition test
two with knowledge acquisition delayed (retention) by condition.
Motivation. Students enter second grade aware of their reading abilities gained
from prior knowledge and experience. Students who are aware of their reading abilities
develop attitudes and opinions toward the reading experience. To interpret students’
literacy attitudes, a Likert student reading survey was administered to help answer the
research question, how does an intervention using readers’ theatre motivate second grade
students reading attitude? A 20-item survey was administered prior to intervention and
repeated after intervention. The questionnaire was based on a Likert scale of 1-4, coded
as: (1) I don’t like it, (2) It’s okay, (3) I like it, and (4) I love it. Students would say the
72
number that best represented their opinion in relation to the specific question. Table 2
shows means and standard deviations for pretest and posttest for each of the 20-item
questionnaire.
The treatment and control groups were assessed prior to the intervention to
determine if there was a significant pretest differences on the student reading survey
measure. An independent samples t-test was conducted using the pretest scores on the
dependent variable. According to the t-test, student scores in the control group
were not significantly different from the experimental group (t(24) = -.459, p = .610). It
was hypothesized that the intervention would find a significant effect between the
groups on the reading attitude measure, an ANCOVA test was performed between the
two groups using treatment (readers’ theatre) as the independent variable, the student
reading survey posttest scores as the dependent variable, and the student reading survey
pretest scores as a covariate. Standardized residuals for the interventions were normally
distributed for treatment, (p = .405) but not for control (p = .010) as assessed by the
homogeneity of regression slopes affirmed the relationship between the student reading
survey pretest scores and the student reading survey posttest scores did not differ as the
interaction term was not statistically significant, F(1, 22) = .851, p = .366, confirming
that the pretest scores could be used as the covariate in the ANCOVA test. After
adjustments were made for pretest scores as the covariate, ANCOVA results indicated
73
that were no significant differences between the student reading survey pretest and
posttest reading survey scores F(1,23) = 2.11, p = .159, partial 2 = .084, therefore, the
80
70 61.3 d = -0.8
60 60.6
50 59.4 51.3
Raw Scores
40
30
20 Experimental
10 Control
0
Pretest Student Posttest Student
Reading Survey Reading Survey
Figure 4.5. Graph for the Likert student reading survey pretest and posttest means by
condition.
When looking at the 20-item survey responses, 54% of the 20 questions were
answered “I love it. This study will discuss the top five questions that were answered “I
love it!” A complete list is provided with the means and standard deviations for each of
the twenty questions (see Appendix L). When students were asked how they felt about
going to the library, 61.6% of students responded, “I love it” at pretest and 50%
responded “I love it” at posttest. When asked how they felt about reading, students
answered “I love it” with 73.1% on the pretest and 53.9% on the posttest”. These
questions obtained the highest percentage for the response “I love you”, yet percentages
decreased from pretest to posttest. The question, how do you feel about reading during
74
free time at school, showed the highest increase for the answer “I love it”, with 34.6% at
pretest to 50% at posttest, an increase of 23.4%. The question with the least amount of
change in percentages from pretest to posttest asked, how do you feel about reading one-
on-one with your teacher. At pretest, 42.3% of students answered, “I love it” and 42.3%
answered “I love it” at posttest. When asked how they felt about using their reading
strategies, students responded “I love it” with 34.6 % at pretest and 34.6% at posttests.
post-intervention to help answer the research question, how does an intervention using
readers’ theatre motivate second grade students reading attitude? Pre-interviews and
post-interviews were not scored and were not entered into a data analysis program, but
served as observations. When looking at the student responses, patterns of answers can
be seen in both the treatment and control groups. When asked about how they felt about
reading, many of the student responses included “learn”, “fun”, “pictures”, “too hard”,
“quiet”, “levels”, and “nothing”. Students also mentioned they read at home and named
favorite book titles. When asked pre-intervention what they knew about readers’ theatre,
all but one student answered “nothing”, and the one student responded they had
When asked post-intervention what they knew about readers’ theatre, the
treatment group could describe in detail: (a) what readers’ theatre looks like, (b) the
elements of readers’ theatre, and (c) the subject material written in script form. When
asked what they knew about readers’ theatre post-intervention, majority of the students in
the control group answered “nothing” and some students responded readers’ theatre was
“a play, it was practiced, and it was performed”. When asked about the subject matter,
75
majority of the students in the control group accurately responded with details and facts.
In comparison, the intervention groups answers revealed less details and facts than the
control group.
76
CHAPTER V
DISCUSSION
This research study was conducted to examine readers’ theatre and its influence
on oral reading fluency, comprehension, and motivation with primary students. This
study also investigated readers’ theatre and its influence on mastery of content material.
To add to the existing research, the discussion presents each dependent variable with its
methods and results; comparing the findings to existing and historical research presented
earlier in the dissertation. The chapter will provide a discussion of the limitations,
Students who took part in the instruction of readers’ theatre did not make
significant gains in fluency rate compared to the students who did not take part in
readers’ theatre. Although, both conditions showed gains from pre- to posttest, the
difference did not show statistical differences between the groups, which is comparable
with current research of Carrick (2000), Dixon (2007) and Johnson (2011). There is a
vast amount of research that has shown repeated reading significantly increases oral
reading rate (Fuchs et al., 2001; Hiebert, 2005; Kuhn & Stahl, 2003; Mraz et al., 2013;
Reutzel et al., 199; Schreiber, 1980; Smith, 2011; Therrien, 2004). As a form of repeated
reading, it is surprising that readers’ theatre enhanced reading fluency, yet, has not shown
In 1989, Carver established norms showing that typical students average a yearly
growth rate of 10-20 WPM. Jagger (2008) used the norms to examine word growth after
a standardized measure did not obtain significant differences for oral reading fluency.
77
Jagger used the norms to illustrate the treatment groups increase of 9.61 WPM within
one-quarter of the time set by Carver’s established norms. In similar research, Hasbrouck
and Tindal (2006) found the average yearly rate for gains in word reading to range from
36 to 38 WPM. In this study, the potential word increase for the WAIT III subtest shows
4 to 59 WPM for the treatment group (4 words pertained to an already high level of
words read at 107 WPM) and 8 to 46 WPM for the control group. The easyCBM showed
4 to 54 WPM for the treatment group and 8 to 25 for the control group. Using Carver’s
norms, this present study indicates words gained in four-weeks’ time, exceeded the
established rate of 2.5 to 5.0 words in an eight-week period. It should be noted that
Carver set up grade-equivalent norms in 1989, using norms written 20 years earlier by
Taylor (1965), and showed that the norms stayed fairly the same during the 20-year gap
(Hiebert, et al., 2012). In comparison to Harris and Touck (2006), the WIAT III and
easyCBM, met the required 9 to 10-word increase in one-quarter of the school year,
equating to 2 to 2.5 words per week. This research supports Fuchs and Fuchs (1993)
study that found 1.5 to 2.0 to be enough for an average second graders weekly word
growth, and with research by Young and Rasinki (2009) that found 62.7 to 127 WPM
(1.24-3.6 per week), with an increase of 65 words in one school year. In looking at this
information, it shows improvement for both types of treatment, though did not
Reading Comprehension
comprehension (Carrick, 2000; Dixon, 2007; Forney, 2013; Gummere, 2004; Jagger,
2008; Johnson, 2011; Keehn, 2003; Morris, 2011; Smith, 2011; Young & Rasinski,
78
2009). These findings contrast with past research that proved reading acquisition occurs
when readers have achieved adequate fluency and can extract meaning from text (Fuchs
et al., 2001; Kuhn & Stahl, 2003; Pikulski & Chard, 2005; Rasinski, 2010). LaBerge and
Samuels (1974) theory of automaticity showed that automatic decoding of text enables
finding significant main effects in oral reading fluency, but not in comprehension,
therefore, the study did not support LeBerge and Samuels theory of automaticity. Only
two studies (Miller & Rinehart, 1999: Mraz et al., 2013) showed strong effects for both
oral reading fluency and comprehension, yet, both studies used whole groups as
standardized assessment for the pretest and posttest, and did not show effects. It is
possible the two-week intervention was not long enough to develop the skills needed to
gain effects.
verbal and written language skills contribute to reading comprehension outcomes: critical
thinking, vocabulary, syntax, and inference skills, explain discrete differences in the
Oakhill, 2016; Duke et al., 2011; Durkin, 1992; Fielding & Pearson, 1994; Rasinski et al.,
also prove inadequate for substantiating comprehension gains. This may help in
understanding the mixed results of repeated reading research in which some studies found
increased acquisition was a product of repeated reading instruction (Dixon, 2007; Forney,
2013; Gummer, 2004; Jagger, 2008; Hiebert, 2005; Millin & Rinehart, 1999; Ruetzel &
79
Hollingsworth, 1993), while other studies did not detect significant differences in
comprehension (Carrick, 2000; Johnson, 2011; Roshotte & Torgesen, 1985). Future
research on readers’ theatre needs to go beyond the basic reading elements and deeper
into other reading skills such as; critical thinking, response, and engagement. If such
skills were incorporated with readers’ theatre, it would provide creative and differentiated
significant differences and low effects. It appeared that conditions did not influence
retention of content material assessed with the knowledge delayed assessment, two weeks
after the completion of the intervention. When looking at the means, there was not a lot
of movement from test one to test two. However, both the treatment and control groups
showed gains for the knowledge acquisition delayed assessment, with the treatment
showing the highest gain. This suggests that possible movement did occur through the
intervention, due to the multiple readings of the script and the repeated reading passage.
Multiple readings of the same passage allow the connection between the text and short-
term working memory to store the information for later retrieval (Durkin, 1992; Fielding
and Pearson, 1994). However, the means did not show a statistical differences between
the tests one and two and knowledge acquisition test. This may have occurred due to the
shorter intervention time and the duration between intervention and delayed assessment.
It is also possible that while the regular classroom curriculum continued during the two
weeks between the intervention and delayed assessment, some students did not have the
ability for long-term memory. Students may have found it difficult as they had moved
forward with the regular curriculum, to retrieve what was taught two to three weeks prior.
80
It is also possible with the small sample size, that significant differences are difficult to
detect.
Motivation
This research found the student reading survey showed a moderate effect size, but
did not show any significant differences between the treatment and control groups. This
agrees with other studies that failed to determine significance for motivation (Gummere,
2004; Millin & Rinehart, 1999; Smith, 2011). The common link between the studies was
the use of a Likert formatted test. This study used a Likert scale of 1 to 4, with 4
representing the most positive answer choice. The highest rated questions reveal students
like starting new books and reading on their own or with a partner, as opposed to
individual reading with the teacher, questioning, and small group time, which showed the
largest drop in means. This may explain the drop in means as the intervention included
the instructional methods that were least favored. This researcher observed a reoccurring
pattern during the pretest as some students typically chose the greater valued response
choices. This would explain potential response biases, such as, a desire to please the
assessor, a lack of focus, mimicking peer choice, and realizing a positive response
generates a greater score. (I like it, I love it) A comfort level may have been established
after the intervention, as posttest answers were more diverse than at pretest. Although the
student reading survey did not produce results, it still offered information on students’
variable discovered outcomes using observations, student journals, and interviews. This
present study also conducted student interviews at pretest and posttest to examine
observations and interviews indicated that readers’ theatre positively influenced reading
motivation (Carrick, 2000; Gummere, 2004; Millin & Rinehart, 1999; Smith, 2011).
Researchers and teachers who observed the students expressed that readers’ theatre had a
positive impact on students reading behavior with renewed interest, a desire to read, and
overall excitement for literacy. Observers witnessed student accountability for their own
learning and self-monitoring. This study did not find any statistical differences between
the two groups as pretest scores to posttest decreased. However, through individual
reading, going to the library and reading during free time. On the other perspective,
majority of students were not as enthused when asked questions that pertained to reading
with teacher, retelling, and small group instruction. Studies that reported motivation as a
positive outcome (Forney, 2013, Millin & Rinehart, 1999, and Smith, 2011) used
This study used a comparison group and found there were no differences between
the two groups, while Gummere (2004) also used a comparison group and found
statistical effects. Both studies used a Likert Scale format, however, Gummere used a
work will need to be done to determine its validity, as an indicator of motivation and to
One strength was the experimental pretest posttest control group design that
manages internal validity eliminating any extraneous variables, and threats to validity.
This experimental design allows for testing to detect potential causal interactions between
82
eliminate alternate explanations from the results (Field, 2009). Another strength was the
use of a statistical analysis of pre- and posttests that had established reliability and
validity.
Limitations of this study are aligned with concerns with much of the past readers’
theatre research. One limitation in this present study, was that there were only two
classrooms each with small student enrollment. The small sample size served as the
dominate factor explaining the lack of effects throughout the research analysis. The
sample size predicts the statistical power, the greater the power, the great probability that
effects will be detected if one exists (Field, 2009). Although analysis found no
relationships between the groups, except for the knowledge acquisition assessment, other
An additional limitation was the length and the timing of the intervention. It is
possible that the length of the 2-week intervention was not adequate to obtain strong
effects. Although this study was designed to examine if treatment would increase
mastery and retention of a specified learned standard. To extend the length of the
research would not comply with the duration the standard is normally taught to achieve
mastery within the normal classroom curriculum sequence. Current studies duration of
interventions ranged from 4-12 weeks, with an average of 9-weeks, which may indicate
This research contributes to prior studies that contend readers’ theatre can be used
readers’ theatre for literacy development. This study struggled to show statistical effects,
primarily due to the small sample size. Future research would benefit by recruiting a
larger sample size that might potentially determine greater effects and generalize to the
This study incorporated text based on the CCSS (2010) second-grade standards
including social studies and expository text. Dixon (2007) and Forney (2013) used
expository text but interchanged the content material with other expository texts or with
narrative text. Neither study produced adequate results to state that expository text
contributed greatly to readers’ theatres outcomes. Expository texts can be more difficult
for students to comprehend due to the complexity of words that are not part of their
everyday vocabulary and difficult concepts to relate to (Hall et al., 2005). Future studies
would benefit by continuing readers’ theatre with scripts that pertain to social events or
issues, historical events or persons, and topics that are conducive to be constructed in
including English language learners, providing a whole class experience for all
84
linguistically diverse children. A place where language has no barrier, where “aesthetic
This research was conducted based on the average second graders abilities, with
one readers’ theatre script and one control repeated reading curriculum. It might be
advantageous to create a script written for various reading abilities. The lack of diverse
reading materials may have contributed to the small or no effects found in this study, as
this study’s content material was created on an average second grade reading level for the
specific time of the academic year. There is a very limited amount of current studies that
considered ability levels. Keehn’s (2003) study included texts written on three ability
levels based on different content material. Keehn did not report adequate information to
calculate means or effects, but indicated that the lower ability reading students showed
the most gains in oral reading fluency. Likewise, Young and Rasinski (2009) did not
report statistical information, but through observations and word gains, suggested that all
the students in the lower reading ability class increased their oral reading fluency.
Reading materials written to meet the diverse needs of students enable them to read on an
independent level which may increase oral reading fluency (Hiebert, 2005; Young &
Rasinski, 2009).
Prior and current studies have mixed results concerning readers’ theatre and its
influence on reading skills. A majority of the studies state that their sample size was
either too small or too large to obtain the effects desired (Carrick, 2000; Gummer, 2004;
a measure of the interventions efficacy. The lack of a comparison group eliminates the
85
ability to apply outcomes to the general population. Although many studies show a lack
of effectiveness for readers’ theatre, prior research has shown it has many
positive attributes (Flynn, 2005; Johnson: 2011; Millin & Rinehart, 1999; Young &
Vardell, 1993) that make it worthwhile for at least limited use in classrooms. It also a
worthwhile endeavor to continue research using larger samples, more varied measures,
and longer intervention lengths to determine the impact of readers’ theatre on literacy
development. This work will help determine the efficacy of readers’ theatre and what
Conclusion
Educators across the nation’s schools are seeking effective instructional strategies
to affect literacy development. The NRP (2000) highly suggested that teachers
empirical data (Shanahan, 2006). This study is consistent with current studies providing
study contributes to the existing studies on readers’ theatre and its influence on oral
delayed retention assessment to test for mastery. This study’s results indicate that
readers’ theatre was not influential on the WIAT III oral reading fluency and
comprehension tests, likewise, for the knowledge acquisition tests. Although differences
could not be found, some effect sizes indicated possible movement from pretest to
posttest scores. It is possible that effects might be found if the sample size was larger.
Readers’ theatre did not promote motivation per the student reading survey, however,
86
research examine readers’ theater and its influence on additional elements of oral fluency
and comprehension, and its influence using various genres, including real world
REFERENCES
Technology.
Allington, R. (2012). What really matters for struggling readers: Designing research-
Allington, R. L. (1983). Fluency: The neglected goal. The Reading Teacher, 36, 556-561.
Booth, D. (1985). Imaginary gardens with real toads: Reading and drama in education.
Brooks, K. (1962). Readers' theatre: Some questions and answers. Dramatics, 24(3),
14-27.
Busching, B. (1981, March). Readers theatre; An education for language and life.
Language Arts, 8(3), 330-338. Retrieved November 29, 2015, from JSTOR:
www.jstor.org/stable/41961305
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2007). Reading comprehension difficulties: Correlates, causes,
problems in oral and written language. (pp. 41-70). New York City, NY: The
Guilford Press.
88
Campbell, D., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for
Carrick, L. (2000). The effects of readers’ theatre on fluency and comprehension on fifth
Carver, J. (1989). Silent reading rates in grade equivalents. Journal of Reading Behavior,
21(2), 155-166.
Casey, S., & Chamberlain, S. (2006). Bringing reading alive through readers' theatre.
CCSS. (2016). Common Core State Standards Initiative. Retrieved Jan. 11, 2016, from
Chard, D., Vaughn, S., & Tyler, B. (2002). A synthesis of research on effective
Chomsky, C. (1978). When you still can't read in third grade: After decoding, what? In S.
J. Samuels, & A. E. Farstrup (Eds.), What research has to say about reading
Clay, M. M., & Imlach, R. H. (1971). Juncture, pitch, and stress as reading behavior
Coger, L., & White, M. R. (1973). Readers’ theatre handbook, a dramatic approach to
Cohen, A., & Wollack, J. A. (2003). Handbook on test development: Helpful tips for
Combs, C. (1987). Theatre and drama in education: A laboratory for actual, virtual or
Corcoran, C., & Davis, A. D. (2005). A study of the effects of readers' theater on second
and third grade special education students' fluency growth. Reading Improvement,
42(2), 1105-11.
Crain, I. Z., & Smith, W. F. (1976). Readers theatre reaches the young. Readers Theatre
Cutting, L., Materek, A., Cole, C. A., Levine, T. M., & Mahone, E. M. (2009, January).
Dahl, P. (1975). A mastery based experimental program for teaching high speed word
research has to say about fluency instruction (pp. 179-203). Newark, DE:
Denton, C., Vaughn, S., & Fletcher, J. M. (2003). Bringing research-based practice in
201-211.
Dixon, O. J. (2007). Content area readers' theater: The effect on fluency and
Duke, N. K. (2000). 3.6 minutes per day: The scarcity of informational texts in first
Duke, N., Pearson, P. D., Strachen, S. L., & Billiman, S. K. (2011). Essential elements of
(Eds.), What research has to say about reading instruction (4th ed., pp. 51-93).
Durkin, D. (1992). Teaching them to read (6th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Eash, M. (1968). Bringing research findings into classroom practice. The Elementary
Englert, C., & Hiebert, E. H. (1984). Children's developing awareness in text structures in
Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS (3rd ed.). (L. SAGE Publications, Ed.)
Flynn, R. (2004). Curriculum-based readers’ theatre: Setting the stage for reading and
10.1598/RT.58.4.5
Fuchs, L., & Fuchs, D. (1993). Formative evaluation of academic progress: How much
Fuchs, L., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., & Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an
Gall, M., Gall, J. P., & Borg, W. R. (2007). Education research: An introduction (8th
ed.). (I. Pearson Education, Ed.) Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Gambrell, L. B., Palmer, B. M., Codlng, R. M., & Mazzoni, S. A. (1996). Assessing
Griffith, L., & Rasinski, T. V. (2004). A focus on fluency: How one teacher incorporated
fluency with her reading curriclum. The Reading Teacher, 49(7), 126-137.
Groff, P. (1978). Readers theatre for children. Elementary School Journal, 78(1), 15-22.
Gummere, S. L. (2004). Readers theatre: Its impact on oral reading fluency, retell
Hall, K., Sabey, B. L., & McClellan, M. (2005). Expository test comprehension: Helping
Harris, T. L., & Hodges, R. E. (1995). The literacy dictionary: The vocabulary of reading
Hasbrouck, J., & Tindal, G. A. (2006). Oral reading fluency norms: A valuable
assessment tool for reading teachers. The Reading Teacher, 59(7), 636-644.
Herman, P. (1985). The effect of repeated readings on reading rate, speech pauses, and
Hernandez, E. (2011). Double Jeopardy: How third-grade readings skills and poverty
influence high school graduation. Retrieved Dec. 2015, from The Annie E. Casey
Foundation: www.aecf.org
Institute of Educational Sciences. (2002, 2013). National Center for Education Statistics.
(NAEP): nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard
Jagger, T. (2008). The effect of readers' theatre on fifth grader's reading fluency and
Jenkins, J., Fuchs, L. S., van de Broek, P., Espin, C., & Deno, S. L. (2003). Sources of
46(3), 266-275.
Kardash, C., & Wright, L. (1987). Does creative drama benefit elementary school
Keehn, S. (2003). The effect of instruction and practice through readers theater on young
Keehn, S., Harmon, J., & Shoho, A. (2008). A study of readers' theatre in eighth grade:
Kuhn, M., & Stahl, S. (2003). Fluency: A review of developmental and remedial
LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic processing in reading.
69-70.
Martinez, M., Roser, N. L., & Strecker, S. (1999). "I never thought I could be a star": A
McKenna, M., & Kear, D. J. (1990). Measuring attitude toward reading: A new tool for
Mercer, C. D., Campbell, K. U., Miller, M. D., Mercer, K. D., & Lane, H. B. (2000).
Merriam-Webster.
Millin, S. K., & Rinehart, S. D. (1999). Some of the benefits of readers theater
39(1), 71-88.
Moran, K. (2006). Nurturing emergent readers through readers' theatre. Early Childhood
Morgan, N., & Saxton, J. (1988). Enriching language through drama. Language Arts, 65,
35-40.
Morris, E. W. (2011). The effects of readers' theater on reading fluency and attitudes
Mraz, M., Nichols, W., Caldwell, S., Beisley, R., Sargent, S., & Rupley, W. (2013).
Improving oral reading fluency through readers' theatre. Reading Horizons, 52(2),
163-180.
Nathan, R. G., & Stanovich, K. E. (1991). The causes and consequences of differences in
National Institute for Child Health and Human Development (NICHD). (2000). Report of
the National Reading Panel. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Oxford University Press. (2010). Oxford American Dictionary (Vol. 3). CT: Oxford
University Press.
Pachtman, A., & Wilson , K. A. (2006). What do the kids think? The Reading Teacher,
59(7), 680-684.
www.tandfonline.com
Perfetti, C., & Hogaboam, T. (1975). Relationship between single word decoding and
Pikulski, J., & Chard, D. J. (2005). Fluency: Bridge between decoding and reading
Prescott, J. O. (2003). The power of readers' theater: An easy way to make dramatic
changes in kids' fluency, writing, listening, and social skills. Instructor, 112(5),
Rasinski, T. V., Blackowicz, C., & Lems, K. (2006). Fluency instruction (2nd ed.). New
Rasinski, T., Linek, W., Sturtevant, E., & Padak, N. (1994). Effects of fluency
Renaissance Learning. (2006). Standardized test for the assessment of reading. Retrieved
Rinehart, S. (1999). "Don't think for a minute that I'm getting up there": Opportunities for
Roshotte, C., & Torgeson, J. K. (1985). Repeated reading and reading fluency in disabled
www.jstor.org/stable747754
325-331.
97
Samuels, S. (2006). Toward a model of reading fluency. In S. Samuels and A.E. Farstrup
(Eds.) What research has to say about fluency instruction (pp. 24-46). Newark,
Samuels, S. J. (1979). The method of repeated readings. The Reading Teacher, 50(5),
376-381.
12, 177-186.
Shanahan, T. (2006). What reading resarch says: The promises and limitations of
What research has to say about reading instruction (3rd ed., pp. 8-24). Newark,
3483738).
16(1), 32-71.
Stayter, F., & Allington, R. L. (1991, Summer). Fluency and the understanding of texts.
Tanner, F. A. (1993). Readers theatre fundamentals. (2nd, Ed.) Topeka, KS: Clark
Publishing, Inc.
Taylor, S.E. (1965). Movements in reading: Facts and fallacies. American Educational
Tyler, B., & Chard, D. J. (2000). Using readers' theatre to foster fluency in struggling
readers: A twist on the repeated reading strategy. Reading and Writing Quarterly,
16(2), 163-168.
Walker, B., Salverson, L., & French, K. (1983, April). Instructional intervention that
works: Case study research. Paper presented at the Annual Montana Symposium
Worthy, J., & Prater, K. (2002). "I thought about it all night": Readers' theatre for reading
Young, T., & Vardell, S. (1993). Weaving reader's theatre and non-fiction into the
Zutell, J., & Rasinski, T. V. (1991). Training teachers to attend to their students' oral
APPENDICES
100
APPENDIX A
Date:
Student Principal Investigator: Holly Marshall
Faculty Advisor: Dr. Amy Elleman
Institution faculty advisor: (Principal)
Purpose: Participation in the 4-week research study is voluntary. Your decision whether to allow
your child to participate will not affect the services normally provided to your child by the school.
There will be two groups of studies, one of Readers’ theatre and one that will study the same
subject matter through other alternate reading activities. Readers’ Theater is an alternate reading
method where reading is performed from scripts that are created based on various genres of
literacy and subjects. It is a form of theatre arts without memorization, scenery, and costumes.
The instruction provides alternate activities and strategies to learn and understand the subject
material that may be more difficult to master. The readers’ theatre group activities will include
character analysis, genre study, the reading elements of fluency, expression, comprehension, plot
and subject study, vocabulary study, and repeated reading. The activities for group two will
consist of group and partner reading, creative writing, discussions, small projects on subject
matter, and other reading activities. The lesson material used for both groups is created from
required state and district curriculum standards
Your child’s involvement in this study will not lead to the loss of any benefits to which her or she
is otherwise entitled.
a. Even if you give permission for your child to participate, your child is free to refuse.
b. If your child agrees to participate, he or she is free to end participation anytime.
c. You and your child are not waiving any legal claims, rights, or remedies because of
your child’s participation in this research study.
d. There is no cost to you or your child for participation in this study.
e. There is no compensation for participation in study.
f. There is no compensation in case of study related injury.
g. If your child does not take part in the study, they will continue with regular
curriculum and activities.
Confidentiality: Any information that is obtained about this study and that can be identified with
you will remain confidential and will be disclosed only with your permission or as required by
law. Confidentiality will be maintained by means of assigning numbers to the children who
participate to eliminate using names.
101
a. All materials will be collected each day by myself and kept safely for confidentiality;
no one will have access to the materials except for authorized personnel if necessary.
Such personnel would be: teachers, approved assistants from the university, principal,
reading specialist. Sharing information would be only to use for educational reasons and
possibly to incorporate into the curriculum depending on outcomes of research study.
Procedures: The activities will be explained to your child clearly in a way they will
understand.
a. The lessons and activities are created to enhance the study and activity of each group.
The lessons include cooperative learning and decision making, group discussions,
readings, group response in writing, partner readings, individual readings, individual
response in writing, and varied ways of learning to meet the diverse needs of the students.
b. At times, the researcher (myself) and/or the teacher may observe or assist your child
while he or she takes part in activities. Observations may, consist of listening to your
child read, discussions between teacher and your child and teacher to child instruction.
c. The sessions with a whole group and/or individual child may be audiotaped to check
and ensure the study reliable and valid.
d. The first and last week of the experiment will consist of testing. The pretest before the
actual experiment of two-weeks, is to determine ability levels of fluency, comprehension
and motivation. The same testing will be administered again, after the completion of the
experiment to see any differences before and after the instruction. The tests include tasks
of one of the following: multiple choice, open-ended questions, 1-5 rating scale, timed
reading passages, reading passages and retelling.
Educators are required to teach curriculum standards in diverse ways to motivate, encourage, and
guide students to master what they are learning. This study will provide an opportunity to
discover if an alternate reading method that encompasses many elements of reading development
improves reading abilities.
If you should have any questions, please feel free to contact your child’s classroom teacher or
myself.
The university of which I am obtaining my degree from, from the district that your child’s school
resides, from the principal of your school has approved this study.
Please see attached Consent Form, please fill form out, sign, and return by _____ 2016.
Thank you,
Holly Marshall
Candidate for Ph.D. in Literacy Studies
Middle Tennessee State University
102
APPENDIX B
I have read the information letter concerning the research project entitled The
Effectiveness of Readers Theatre on the Fluency, Comprehension, and Motivation of
Primary Students. This study is to be conducted by Holly Marshall, a doctoral student
for a Ph.D. in Literacy Studies from Middle Tennessee State University.
I have had the opportunity to ask questions and receive and additional details I wanted
about the study.
I acknowledge that all information gathered on this project will be used for research
purposes only and will be considered confidential. I am aware that permission may be
withdrawn as any time without penalty by advising the researcher.
I realize that this project has been reviewed by and approved by the Institutional Review
Board at MTSU, the district level, and principal of the school.
If I have any questions about this study, I can feel free to contact the researcher, Holly
Marshall, hbm2n@mtmail.mtsu.edu. Or Dr. Amy Elleman, Department of Literacy
Studies, Middle Tennessee State University, Amy.Elleman@mtsu.edu
Parent or Guardian
Signature____________________________________________________Date_______
Researcher’s Signature_____________________________________________________Date_________
Researcher’s Title Candidate for Ph.D. in Literacy Studies _____ Department _MTSU DOE
103
APPENDIX C
a. Discuss a few of the plays that were kind, action, events, scenery, story,
characters, etc.
b. Reinforce what they saw and emphasis attributes of a traditional play.
c. Does anyone know what readers’ theatre is? (answers vary, may be guesses,
play, reading books in front of others, etc.)
d. Define and explain readers’ theatre.
= Like a play you may have seen, RT is a play performed by actors who
are different characters telling you a story. However, RT is different in many
ways. RT actors read their parts, there is no costumes, and or scenery. (expand on
the three points for clarification)
= Discuss what to “memorize” means. RT does not memorize the parts.
= Discuss limited movement and props.
e. History Trivia:
= A condensed version of Paul Gregory and the Tiffany diamonds on how
he came to vision what readers’ theatre is.
f. Recap what RT is and put on chart.
Instruction
a. Read Act 1, Scene One to introduce beginning of script to also model
characters, voice, expression, etc. without them really knowing it.
b. Discuss the reading. What are the ways it was read? Was it interesting? What
made it interesting or not? (word questions depending on how they are asked or
answered)
c. Hand out scripts in folders. (NO assigning parts, NO markings, discuss how to
handle scripts.)
Characters:
a. Discuss reasons for different voices.
b. Describe what they know so far about the characters. How do they know this?
c. What is happening in the RT up to this point?
d. predicts what may happen in story.
Content:
a. Subject: Earth’s makeup
b. Prior knowledge
Reading:
Group Read in parts, but no specific part given for the performance yet.
=they read Act 1, Scene One
Word Analyze: Chart any difficult words, discuss.
= rotate reading different parts.
Conclusion:
a. recap mini-lesson
b. Discuss what they liked and what they found difficult.
c. Write any ideas to improve for the next day’s work. (may include RT,
scripts, words, and reading behaviors, respect, etc.)
b. Collect folders
Greeting. Recap previous day’s lesson with students, give guidance, but allow students to
provide as much information as possible.
a. Pull out ICharts and review and see what was remembered and not.
Instruction:
a. Guidance of retelling of Act 1, Scene One.
b. hand out scripts.
c. Model Act 1, Scene Two.
• Stop reading before the first continent. Discuss events
up to this point.
• Predict what is going to happen.
• Analyze characters since there is more to know. (write
on chart)
• Continue modeling and read rest of Scene Two as they
follow along.
• Discuss content. Bring out blank Content and Ocean
map. (not the one used for the performance).
105
Introduction:
Instruction:
• Expression:
o Model a few character’s line in a monotone voice.
Discuss what is interesting and not interesting about it.
o Why is expression important in reading and
performance? (so the characters come to life, real,
interesting, to keep the audience attention)
o Expression not to be overdone.
Props: not many are used, and if so, to add to point of story, not
distract.
b. Read through script stopping to add the above elements. Try to let students
decide, with guidance. Repeat.
c. Put into partners to read their own line and polish any issues as pronunciation,
etc.
d. Whole group practices for the rest of session up to the last 5 minutes.
= rehearse with all blocking, expression, etc.
Conclude:
Discuss how they feel about their practice.
Introduction:
Instruction:
SCHEDULE OF DAY:
1. RT rehearsal (15 min.)
2. Control Lesson (15 min.)
3. RT Performance (15-20 min.)
5. Control presentation (10 min.)
6. Both groups: Knowledge acquisition assessment based on the week one
material (20-25 min) Collect.
RT (15 min.)
Introduction:
Instruction:
APPENDIX D
“What on Earth?”
(A readers’ theatre play on The Seven Continents and Oceans)
2nd Grade
The following readers’ theatre script is written to include the Common Core State Standards (CCSS)
based on the Seven Continents and Five Oceans. The play was created and incorporated to serve as an
intervention treatment for a doctoral study to examine “Readers’ Theatre and its effectiveness on
Fluency, Comprehension, and Motivation”. The standards are being taught using Readers’ Theater
within the allotted time frame that a standard is generally taught to determine mastery.
CCSS Standards:
2.14 Construct a globe depicting the four hemispheres, seven continents, and five oceans using the equator
and prime meridian. *(The script will not incorporate the actual making of a globe, but the learning of
the items. The constructing of a globe will be done during another time in the class schedule)
2.15 Create a map depicting the current boundaries of the United States, Canada, and Mexico and recognize
they are part of the North American continent.
109
“What on Earth?”
Synopsis: A group of Alien friends set out to discover how planet Earth is designed and some interesting facts about
the Earth.
Earth Citizens:
Emilio: (boy) From Antarctica who becomes their guide through the alien friends journey.
or Emme (girl)
Act One
(week one)
Setting: A classroom on a planet called Arkon, far out in space.
Zeela: Oh. Wow…. This is super far out! I can’t wait until we begin our homework! Ork, Ork, Ork (an alien giggle)
Babeet: I agree, this should be an interesting, however, I have no idea how to begin.
Gordeet: No problem, I can handle this, I’ve got it. We just…… uh…… well….
Zeela: We begin by looking at the map of the planet that is called Earth. Now, our job is to find out what it looks
like and how it is made. That shouldn’t be too hard.
Orto: Oh…. I don’t know, looking at something we don’t know that much about? I don’t know.
Babeet: (Sniffle, snort…) I agree with Zeela, we can do this. Now, let’s have a look that ma…ah…ah..achoo! Oh
goodness, sorry again.
Gordeet: Great. Looks like the map is a mess of squiggly lines and objects, it shows us where it is, but there is nothing
to tell us what Earth looks like or anything.
Orto: Oh… no…. don’t tell me things like that, there is nothing? How are we going to figure this out? We aren’t
going to get the project done in time.
Ahbit: Well now… this isn’t a problem, if we work together, we can get this done.
Gordeet: Calm down Orto, hey, if we must, we’ll just go to Earth…. (laughs kiddingly)
Zeela: Gordeet! Yes! That’s it! What a great idea, can we take your Astroplane and see Earth for ourselves! This is
perfect! I better write all of this down….
Gordeet: Uh… I was just kidding Zeela, I really didn’t mean it.
Orto: Oh, my…. This is not going to be good for me… I can just tell it.
Babeet: Do you think there will be food down there? I’m getting hungry.
111
Ahbit: Oh well…. I guess is we are going to do this; we might as well do it all the way!
Narrator: The friends decided to fly to earth the next day, since they didn’t have school.
Zeela: Okay, Gordeet, I have a plan, let’s start to fly up to this spot, here at the top of the map.
Gordeet: Whatever Zeela, we should have enough fuel to get wherever that is.
Narrator: As the five friends fly through the universe, they come upon what they have been told is earth.
Orto: I hope we don’t land in the blue; it doesn’t look very safe to me. Huh… do you notice that Earth looks like
a ball? Its round? Do they not fall off the planet? Interesting.
Gordeet: It does look cool from up here. Let’s see if I can land where we think… ah oh….
Orto: What! No, no ah oh…. Not here, not anywhere. No, we don’t say ah oh.
Gordeet: I was wrong, our fuel is lower than I thought, we will have to land where I can.
Babeet: I hope so; my stomach is making those gurgling sounds when I haven’t had my fuel yet.
Narrator: Gordeet and his friends held on tight as he carefully landed the Astroplane on a body of land.
Zeela: Hey, where are we? It looks all white out there. Kind of strange.
Ahbit: Well there is only one way to find out. Let’s go!
Narrator: As they stepped out of the plane, there was nothing to find except for a few small buildings and very few
people.
Ahbit: Oh, come on, this will be fun. Let’s see if there are any life forces here, there doesn’t seem to be much of
anything else.
Narrator: As the friends walked to a small building, a door opened and in front of them stood a life force…. A human
person.
Gordeet: Oh whoa… whoa… uh… we are students from planet Arkon and we are here to work on a project. (whispers
to the others) This life force looks rather strange.
112
Emilio: Okay…. Seriously, guys, who put you up to this, huh? (laughing) Good, hey, did the guys doing the research
on ice caps send you over? Okay, got me, you can take your costumes off.
Zeela: What?
Emilio: What? Are you kidding me? You are not the guys from research lab 12? Those… are… your… real clothes
and you really look like that? Okay then…. Uh…. (stammering) wh…wh... what can I do for you and for
how long are you planning on being here?
Zeela: Thank you, human life force for asking. We are in need to know more about this planet of yours called Earth.
Our project is to understand what it looks like and any other facts we find.
Babeet: Well, since you ask, my stomach is making some strange sounds.
Gordeet: We need fuel for our plane and I believe Babeet is asking for food.
Emilio: Got it. Your plane needs fuel and she is hungry. Yea, I can help with both of those.
Narrator: Once the plane and friends were all fueled up, they began to ask questions and explore the unique land they
were on.
Emilio: You are on a land called Antarctica. It is one of the Seven Continents of the Earth’s makeup.
Zeela: Oh! Oh! Wait a minute, this is so important, I must write this down. Oh, Orto, pull out the map, maybe he
can show us where this body of land is.
Emilio: Antarctica is right here, (showing on map) it is at the southernmost part of the Earth, down here at the bottom
of the Earth. As you can see, what you landed on was frozen water called ice.
Ahbit: Yea, I found that out the hard way (as he rubs his rear end)
Emilio: Sorry, I had to get used to walking on it when I first got here. If you notice, there are not many people, uh…
life forces as you call them, around. The only ones here are doing what you are doing, science, experiments,
trying to find out more about our earth.
Zeela: Very interesting. I am going to write all of this down so we can share it when we get back to Arkon.
Babeet: I would like to know more too, and I also would like to have another snow cone please. These are so good.
Emilio: Well, the most ice that the earth is covered with, is right here in Antarctica.
Emilio: (laughing). See the black and white one? That is called a penguin, they are a type of bird species. Those over
there on the ice hill are seals. They are just a few animals that can survive here.
Zeela: Wow. You know, this is so great, all this is good information. Would you be able to be our guide and take us
around the Earth to show us other places? I mean, if you can leave right now.
Gordeet: Hey, that is a good idea and I can focus on driving while you tell us about Earth.
Emilio: You know, that would be fun. I have don’t have to be back here until tomorrow at this time, we have 24 hours
to get it done, I think we can do it. Let’s go.
Narrator: The Aliens and Emilio begin to fly up from Antarctica going Northeast to another body of land.
Gordeet: Hey Emilio, do I need to worry, we are flying over that blue stuff we saw coming in.
Emilio: No, there is no worry if you have all the fuel you need.
Orto: Seriously? We are going to talk about that AFTER we have taken off?
Ahbit: Orto, it is fine, we have enough fuel. Look at the window it is pretty.
Zeela: Emilio, what is that blue stuff anyway? I need to write it down.
Emilio: That is the Southern Ocean, it is a large body of water that surrounds Antarctica. There will be four other
oceans we will fly over. The five oceans make up 2/3 of the whole Earth surface, and the seven continents
make up 1/3.
Ahbit: So, the earth is made up of more water than solid surfaces….
Emilio: Land.
Ahbit: Yea, land. Huh, more water than land on the earth. Crazy.
Babeet: Uh… hey, Emilio? Just for planning sake, how many stops will we be making? You know, just in case…
uh… we may need more food?
Emilio: Ah, oh yes, Babeet, you already explored one of the seven continents, Antarctica, so now we are going to
visit the other six. Will that work for you Babeet?
Babeet: Yes, I believe so, I will just have to plan for it.
Emilio: Alright! Land right there, yea, that’s it. Friends we just landed on the continent of Australia. Don’t be afraid
of the kangaroos.
Orto: The what? What did you say? What are kangaroos and do they speak our language? Should we be afraid?
Emilio: No, no, Orto, they are an animal that is popular here. Australia is known for several animals that you can
only find here.
Emilio: Gordeet, let’s continue our path and fly up North and to the left or west just a bit.
Ahbit: Whoa! Look down fellow aliens! That is a lot of land down there.
Orto: What?
Gordeet: We’re not going to get lost Orto, Emilio knows what he is doing…. Uh… you do, don’t you Emilio?
Emilio: Oh sure, we are doing fine. Hmmm… let’s see, this is quite a large continent,
Narrator: The Alien friends and Emilio have explored four of the seven continents and are on their way to the next
continent to see more of Earth.
115
Narrator: The Alien friends and Emilio have explored four of the seven continents and have landed on the next
continent to explore.
Emilio: We are in Asia and yes, Babeet, I think you will find some of the food here very interesting.
Emilio: Asia is the largest of the seven continent and, it is also where the most people live. Asia has many different
sections of land they divide called countries. In those countries people speak differently from each other
and have different ways of living.
Orto: Doesn’t that make it hard to have discussions with each other if they do not speak the same way?
Ahbit: I bet it is kind of like where we live, there are ways to learn how the other Alien forms speak so we can
understand each other, is that the same here Emilio?
Emilio: Yes, there are ways to learn to speak each other’s language so that we can talk to each other.
Ahbit: See, that is all I wanted to hear, how we all can get along even though we may be different in some ways. I
think I may like this earth so far. Can we continue?
Zeela: Hang on here, (writing quickly) I am a bout done with the last point here, largest continent, different
countries, most human forms of all continents on earth. Okay, let’s go.
Gordeet: Alright, let’s get back in the Astroplane and take off for more places!
Emilio: We are going to fly down southwest a bit to the continent of Africa.
Emilio: It is an interesting place, Africa. It is the second largest continent and the second largest with the most
people. There are 1,000 different languages here that are spoken.
Ahbit: Excuse me? 1,000 different types of speaking? Okay, now that may be stretching it in trying to learn all of
them.
116
Emilio: Africa has many, many types of animals that get to run around in the open spaces on their own. It is really
a great thing to see.
Orto: Huh… sounds good, but … what happens if these animals come up to you?
Emilio: Well, you don’t get that close to them if you can help it. Africa also has the longest river that in the whole
world.
Emilio: Oh, sorry. The Nile River. It is a running body of water that begins in Egypt, a country of Africa.
Zeela: I can’t believe that we have already been to four of the seven continents. This is so great. Are we going to
be able to finish the trip?
Babeet: We will be stopping for a longer time at the next continent? Just asking.
Gordeet: Emilio, now where to and yes, Babeet, I will need to refuel the Astroplane again as well.
Babeet: Oh, no matter to me, I was just asking, but since you do have to stop to fuel the Astroplane…
Emilio: Europe, just direct the plane up North right above Africa.
Narrator: Emilio and the Alien friends take off for Europe to learn more about Earth.
Emilio: Oh yes, Europe is my favorite continent to go to, it also has many countries on the continent. Yet, like Asia,
the countries are very close to each other, but the people have different ways of living and speaking.
Emilio: Europe is full of things to see, a lot of history, art, music, very old churches and good food.
Babeet: What? Excuse me, I am not sure if I caught that last statement.
Emilio: Ha! Yes, many of the countries in Europe are famous for wonderful food. Other facts to write down Zeela
are…
Zeela: ready…
Emilio: Europe has no deserts like several of the other continents, but they do have the famous mountains called the
Alps.
Zeela: Oooo… now that is interesting and I will go back to Arkon and explore more on that fact. However, tell us
more about Europe.
Emilio: Well, like all the continents, there is so much more that can be told, we only have time to see where they
are on the earth and a few facts.
Gordeet: Emilio, we are going to run out of time. We may want to keep going.
117
Emilio: Got it. Thanks for keeping watch on our time. If you noticed, we haven’t crossed over any water for some
time. We crossed over the Southern Ocean from Antarctica to Australia, from Australia to Asia, we crossed
over the Indian ocean.
Zeela: Oh, Ahbit, did you get that? You are keeping up by marking the map with the continents and oceans, aren’t
you?
Ahbit: Yea, yea, got it, it’s looking good, not much more to see.
Emilio: That is correct, we’re almost done. We are flying across the Atlantic Ocean now from Europe to North
America.
Zeela: I like the sound of that. What are a few facts about North America?
Orto: What? Really? do you want me to just lose it right here, right now? No thanks.
Gordeet: Emilio, go ahead and tell us some about North America, but first where do I land?
Emilio: Land right there on The United States of America. It is one of the countries of the North America
continent. North America is the third largest continent.
Zeela: Well if it is the third largest, and there are few countries with it, what are the others?
Emilio: Well, like I said, the United States is here in the middle, and above is Canada, and below is Mexico. North
American is the only continent surrounded by three oceans, the Pacific, the Arctic, and the Atlantic
Zeela: Oh, give me a moment please, you are giving really good information but I need to catch up please.
(writing) Whoo…. Okay, keep going.
Gordeet: Well, we have seen six of the seven continents alien friends, I guess we need to head to the last one and get
Emilio back to where he needs to be.
Orto: Or maybe where WE need to be as well? What a trip, but I am beginning to worry about the interspace
traffic as we fly back home.
Ahbit: It will be fine, we have enjoyed the day, let’s hit the last continent Emilio.
Emilio: Gordeet, just fly south, that’s it. South from North America.
Gordeet: Wow. Here already? That did not take very long.
Emilio: I know. This continent is easy to remember because it is also named an “America”, but SOUTH America.
Since it is south or below North America. However, they are different in their own ways.
Emilio: Yes, Babeet, very good, that is the Amazon River, the second longest after the Nile River. The Amazon
carries more water, so much that it can be seen from space.
Gordeet: This is pretty. I wish we had more time to really explore more about the earth and more about the human
life forces that inhabit the earth.
Zeela: Oh, that would be wonderful! We can do that for our next project!
Babeet: Oh, no, really? Can’t we just give it a rest for a while?
Orto: Yes, it has been a very nice trip, however, I do think it is time, yes, it is time to return to our planet Arkon
to present our report to our alien friends at school.
Zeela: I suppose you are all correct in your ideas. We will return to get our report ready.
Gordeet: Emilio, let us take you back to Antarctica so you may begin your study.
Emilio: You know guys, I was thinking, that I should study more about earth from your point of view. I would like
to come with you and help you with your report. You know, there is still more I could teach you.
Zeela: Really? You would do that? Oh, that is so great! Yes, come with us.
Gordeet: Okay Alienators! We are on our way back to Arkon! Hang on!
Orto: Oh, oh no, a bit too fast there, can we slow down please? I am still not feeling that great.
APPENDIX E
Time: 40 min.
Day One
Introduction:
Greeting.
“How are you today? (answers) I am excited to have you part of this experiment
in reading. You are in a group that will be reading the same subject but in different ways
than another group. We are going to see which way of reading works the best. Are you
up for this? I hope so, it is going to be fun.”
Instruction:
1. Read Aloud: Read Passage One, Week One. (group listens, they have
no materials in front of them)
2. Discuss what the passage was about. Minimal Retell.
“What was this passage about?” (put on IChart)
3. Pass out passage. Read aloud as they follow.
“Is there anything else that we can add to the chart?”
4. Put them in partners. “Now, before you read with your partner, I want to
teach you how to “I read, you read.”
Group Share:
a. “How did it go? Were there any words that were hard to say or understand?
Let’s write them down on a chart and discuss them together.”
b. chart and discuss.
c. “Great, okay, now go back with your partner and read the passage again, you
read I read and if you are having a hard time with saying the words, here is what I
want you to do.
120
1. If the reader can’t say a word, the listener gives the reader a moment to
try for themselves and then asks, “do you want help?” and the listener can
help the reader with the word. “What should you do if neither one of you
know the word?” (ask Mrs. Marshall)
Conclude: do not retell to the full extent. Only minimal. Collect folders.
Fact Finders
Week One: Day Two:
Introduction:
Go over previous day’s steps of reading the passage. Go over the word chart.
Instruction:
(hand out folders)
1. Model and read passage while they follow in folders.
2. After reading: “can you tell me what kind of story this is?” (nonfiction)
a. Discuss what Non-fiction is. Expository. (tells facts, teaches)
b. How is this different than Fiction? How could this passage be made into
a fiction story? (characters, speaking, etc.)
c. “You are right, this is a reading with facts about the continents and
oceans. Let’s look at the map here and see where they are. I find it easier
to understand when I can see where the places are.”
*look in the back of your folder. You have a map that is also labled
with the continents and oceans. You can look at it as you read the passage
to understand where the continent or ocean is.
Reading:
a. “I am going to have you read by yourself now and then we will together
again and discuss. I would like for you to write down any questions you
may have on the passage in your folder. I would like you to read the
passage one time.” “How many?” (1x)
*have someone repeat the directions so everyone gets another chance to
hear them and to make sure they heard them correctly.
b. Individual reading around room if at possible, if not, spread apart not to
distract each other.
Group:
a. “How did it go? What questions do you have on the passage?”
b. Discuss.
c. “Can someone tell me what you have learned from the passage so far?”
e. “Now, let’s get back with your partner from yesterday and read the
passage together. This is different from yesterday.”
121
= “You will read a different way today. You will read the same
passage but take turns reading different parts. Let me show you
what I mean.” (ask for a volunteer)
= “Now, I am going to read first, (read a short section and stop).
Now, (John) is going to continue from where I stopped.”
= “After you finish reading, talk about how you read it and what it
said. = “Ask each other to name the continents and oceans without
looking at the passage or the maps.
Conclude: Group. Collect folders. If time, “How did you do? Did you find things out you
didn’t know before?”
Instruction:
Pass out folders.
1. Review map discussing the continents in the order the they are written in the
reading passage.
B. Reading-Partners
1. they read whole page. Assign paragraph passages for each student.
2. Read complete reading passage in partners.
3. In partners, reread Asia and write down two facts.
4. Discuss as a group their findings. Write on chart. See if there are any
that were missed.
5. Model reading paragraph on Asia.
= during modeling, underline important facts of the continent.
6. Students mimic what was modeled. They are to reread passage and
underline what they consider important facts. Give feedback as they do so
in order to direct them in understanding.
7. Group discussion on findings. They write down any fact they may have
not had and may hear from peer.
C. “Now, that you have read the passage, we have written down facts, discussed
them, you are doing very well understanding the passage.” “Do you have any
questions on the passage, facts, how you read it?”
D. Make sure individual maps has the continent of the day colored .
Collect Folders
122
Control Group
Week One: Day Four
(40 min.)
Introduction:
“How are you doing? I am excited because we are finishing up this passage today,
but I hope you are remembering facts from it, because you will need to know them for
other readings. Are you ready to start today’s activities?”
Instruction:
Collect Folders
SCHEDULE OF DAY:
1. RT rehearsal (15 min.)
2. Control Lesson (15 min.)
3. RT Performance (15-20 min.)
5. FF presentation (10 min.)
6. Both groups: Knowledge acquisition assessment based on the week one
material (20-25 min) Collect.
123
FF:
Introduction:
Instruction:
Practice.
APPENDIX F
The Earth is made up of a solid mass called “land” and a liquid called “water”. The
water covers 2/3 of the earth and the land covers 1/3 of the earth. So, the earth is covered
with more water than land.
The water that covers the earth is called Oceans, there are five of them. The Pacific,
Atlantic, Southern, Indian, and Artic.
The land is divided into parts. These parts are called Continents. There are seven
continents of land. On most of the land, people live, eat, play, and work. Not all the
people are the same. Their food, homes, languages, and lives may be different from each
other. The seven continents are named: North America, South America, Europe, Asia,
Africa, Australia, and Antarctica.
There are four continents that begin and end with the letter “A”. The first is Antarctica.
Antarctica is located at the bottom of the earth and is very cold and is covered with ice. In
fact, Antarctica covers the earth with the most ice than any other continent. It is not a
continent where many people live. However, there are some animals that can live in the
very cold. These animals are called seals and penguins. They can find fish in the water
below the ice for food.
Australia is North of Antarctica. It is the smallest of the Seven Continents. It has known
for the Great Barrier Reef. The Great Barrier Reef is a water community in the Southern
Ocean of thousands of different water plants and animals. The Outback is an area of land
that many people do not live in because it is very dry, very hot, and does not get much
rain. Several animals that live in Australia, are the kangaroos and koala bears.
Asia is the largest of the Seven Continents. Asia is the continent where the most people
live on the whole planet of Earth. It is so big, that the land is divided into parts called
countries. People live, eat, work, and play just like they do in the other continents. Asia
has a wall called the Great Wall of China that is more than 2,300 years old and is over
13,000 miles long.
Africa is South of Asia and is also divided into different countries. Africa is the second
largest continent there is. There are so many people living in different countries that there
are over 1,000 types of languages. Africa can be dry in parts of the continent and very
hot. It has many wild animals that walk around free in open parts of land. The longest
river on earth is in Africa, in the country of Egypt. It is called the Nile River.
125
The Earth is made up of a solid mass called “land” and a liquid called “water”. The
water covers 2/3 of the earth and the land covers 1/3 of the earth. So, the earth is covered
with more water than land.
The land is divided into parts. These parts are called Continents. There are seven
continents of land. The seven continents are named: North America, South America,
Europe, Asia, Africa, Australia, and Antarctica.
North America is the third largest continent of the earth. North America is made of the
United States of America, Canada, and Mexico. The United States is in the middle of
North America with Canada north and Mexico south of the continent. Three oceans touch
North America, the Pacific Ocean which is west of the United States, Atlantic Ocean,
east of North America and Arctic, which is north. The United States has smaller parts
called states.
South America is below or south of North America. It has many parts called countries.
South America has the second longest river called the Amazon river. The Amazon runs
so much water, that it can be seen from space. South America is also known for its
rainforests and has the highest waterfall called Angel Falls.
There are five oceans on the earth. They are the Pacific, Atlantic, Arctic, Southern, and
Indian. These oceans plus a few other bodies of water called lakes and rivers, cover
2/3rd of the earth. The oceans are the largest bodies of water and surround the seven
continents. They are filled with sea life and plants.
APPENDIX G
ADMINISTRATOR: ___________________________________________
Read each question and circle the number of the answer you choose. The numbers mean the following:
1-I don’t like it 2-It’s Okay 3-I like it 4-I love it
APPENDIX H
Script:
“I am going to ask you a few questions about how you feel about reading. There is no “right” or “wrong”
answer with these questions. I want to know what you think about reading now. I will read some questions
to you and write down what you say. Do you have any questions?”
Pre-Test
Questions:
Post test
Questions:
2. What did you like about the group you were in?
APPENDIX I
Name__________________________ ID______________
Instructions: Listen to the following questions read to you aloud. Do not choose an answer until the
question and choices are read to you completely. If you are not sure of the answer, choose and circle the
best answer you think it may be. If you do not choose any answer at all, then the question will be marked
wrong. Do your best! You can do it! ☺
15. The four continents that you have learned so far are the four “A’s”. Can you list them?
1. ___________________________________
2. ___________________________________
3. ___________________________________
4. ___________________________________
131
16. From the four continents that you have learned so far, which continent is the smallest one?
a. Antarctica
b. Asia
c. Australia
d. Africa
17. Which continent covers the Earth with the most ice?
a. Africa
b. Antarctica
c. Asia
d. Australia
20. On the map, put the number of the continent that matches the continent.
a. Write the number 1 on Antarctica.
b. Write the number 2 on Australia
c. Write the number 3 on Asia
d. Write the number 4 on Africa
Bonus:
So far, which continent is your favorite? ___________________________
132
APPENDIX J
Name__________________________ ID______________
Instructions: Listen to the following questions read to you aloud. Do not choose an answer until the question
and choices are read to you completely. If you are not sure of the answer, choose and circle the best answer you
think it may be. If you do not choose any answer at all, then the question will be marked wrong. Do your best!
You can do it! ☺
4. The land that makes up the Earth is divided into how many parts? ________
5. What country is between Asia and Africa and has a country that is shaped like a lady’s boot?
a. Australia
b. North America
c. Europe
d. Antarctica
17. Name the Continents of the Earth: (1 pt. ea.) 18. Name the 5 oceans. (1 pt. ea.)
a.___________________________________ a. _____________________________
b. __________________________________ b. _____________________________
c.___________________________________ c. _____________________________
d.___________________________________ d. _____________________________
e.___________________________________ e. _____________________________
f.____________________________________
134
APPENDIX K
Name__________________________ ID_________________________
Instructions: Listen to the following questions read to you aloud. Do not choose an answer until the question and
choices are read to you completely. If you are not sure of the answer, choose and circle the best answer you think it
may be. If you do not choose any answer at all, then the question will be marked wrong. Do your best! You can do
it! ☺
2. From our lessons, the earth is made up of what two 7. There are _____ parts of water.
things? a. 3
a. plants and trees b. 7
b. gas and air c. 4
c. land and water d. 5
d. people and animals
4. The large parts of water on earth are called _______. 9. Which continent is the smallest?
a. states a. North America
b. oceans b. Europe
c. continents c. Africa
d. countries d. Australia
5. “Continents” is the name given to the pieces of 10. Which continent has a country called Italy that
__________ that makeup the earth. looks like a ladies’ boot?
a. water a. Africa
b. sky b. Europe
c. land c. Asia
d. mountains d. Antarctica
11. Which is a name of an ocean? 19. How many oceans makeup the earth’s surface?
a. Pacific
b. Australia ____________
136
c. Tennessee
d. Northern
12. How many continents are there on earth? 20. Which is a name of a continent?
a. Atlantic
______________ b. Antarctica
c. United States
d. Arctic
13. There are 4 oceans that makeup the earth. 21. The land covers how much of the earth?
a. True a. 2/3
b. False b. 1/3
c. 1/2
d. the whole earth
14. Which continent is covered with the most ice? 22. The Arctic Ocean is at the top of Earth by the
a. Asia North Pole.
b. Antarctica a. True
c. Africa b. False
d. South America
16. Which is a name of an ocean? 24. North America is ______ largest continent.
a. Northern a. 2nd
b. Southern b. 4th
c. Asia c. 3rd
d. Australia d. 7th
17. What is the name of the city you live in? 25. People live, eat, work and play the same way on
a. Tennessee every continent.
b. United States a. True
c. Lebanon b. False
d. North America
18. Which is a name of a continent? 26. Which continent is south of North America?
a. Europe a. Africa
b. United States b. Europe
c. Tennessee c. Asia
d. Atlantic d. South America
28. The United States is made up of continents or states? 34. Which is a country of North America?
_______________ a. Europe
b. Tennessee
c. Arctic
d. United States
29. How many countries makeup the continent of North 35. What is the land called that makes up the earth?
America? a. water
a. 4 b. countries
b. 6 c. states
c. 3 d. continents
d. 7
30. Which is a name of a country that is part of North 36. Oceans surround the continent of North America.
America? a. True
a. Tennessee b. False
b. Canada
c. Africa
d. Lebanon
Write the names of the oceans: Write the names of the continents:
1. ______________________________ 1.______________________________
2._______________________________ 2.______________________________
3._______________________________ 3.______________________________
4. _______________________________ 4.______________________________
5. _______________________________ 5.______________________________
6.______________________________
7._____________________________
138
1 on Antarctica
2 on Australia
3 on Asia
4 on Africa
5 on Europe
6 on North America
7 on South America
APPENDIX L
Pretest Posttest
(N = 26) (N = 26)
Survey Question M SD M SD
Writing about what you have read 2.73 0.75 2.38 1.24
APPENDIX M