MTT5 3 Hughes
MTT5 3 Hughes
MTT5 3 Hughes
Hughes, Philip
SRAsT(M), PhD
ABSTRACT
This paper will first examine the perception of music in a music therapy
session with reference to ecological psychology and music psychology. A
summary is given of the complicating factors such as clients’ past experi-
ence of music, but also associations with musical idioms and the sounds
of the instruments. This serves as a reminder that the theoretical posi-
tions (for instance music psychotherapy and ‘music as therapy’) which
therapists use to inform their work, could lead to idealisations of a
‘messy’ reality. Notwithstanding this, a theoretical model is presented of
what music therapists might be doing when they work in the two media of
words and music, drawn from the author’s experience of mathematical
modelling. The concept of bi-level optimisation is put forward as an
analogy for the way the therapist has to think in two media at once, or at
least in the same session, and ‘optimise’ the distance between therapist
and client in both media, at the current point in the process of the ther-
apy. A separate but related model is given of insight and resistance in
music therapy, using ‘catastrophe theory’. The ideas may be of particu-
lar relevance to music therapists working in psychiatry and/or with psy-
chosis; a clinical vignette is used to illustrate this.
1
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
INTRODUCTION
This paper has two strands, both motivated by the big question “What is
music therapy?” As a relatively recently qualified therapist, I have found
myself inevitably trying to work out my own response to this question, in
the process of finding and consolidating my way of working in clinical
practice. Thus as models of what happens in a music therapy session
they are personal to me, and there is always a danger they will seem
obvious, or “so what?” The ideas will serve their purpose if they provoke
some thought, and perhaps clarify some of the assumptions we
sometimes make.
In this first section I want to point out the ways in which music therapy
doesn’t quite fit into any particular theoretical framework, while in the
second section I fail to resist the temptation to provide my own
theoretical framework. Music therapy has always been a ‘practice’, and
the value of a framework will be in stimulating ideas in practitioners,
whilst the limitations of such frameworks should always be
acknowledged.
INTRODUCTION 2
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
The conclusion is that we use sounds to make sense of our world at quite
a basic level, and it certainly seems likely that those associations will stay
with us even when we know that they are produced by musical
instruments. In my experience, clients can compare the sounds of the
instruments directly to everyday noises such as a door shutting, and this
is perhaps trying to make sense of the somewhat strange experience of a
music therapy session in terms of things they know well. Again, because
the focus in the session is on sound, there may be a heightened awareness
of outside noises which can be used consciously or unconsciously to
distract from the process of therapy. Gaver’s categories of sound
production and source attributes have their parallels in the categories of
musical instrument, although the correspondence is not always exact –
obvious examples are the rain-stick and ocean drum, producing quite
convincing watery noises using non-watery substances.
The other finding of the Palmer et al. experiments was that there were, as
expected, cultural factors: the instruments from the subjects’ own culture
were easier to identify. Clearly, a client will find some instruments in a
music therapy room familiar, and some less so. Leading on from this, the
client’s previous experience of those instruments will influence their
perceptions. Some percussion instruments may seem childish if they
were last seen in school music; the piano or the therapist’s other
instrument may bring associations of unsuccessful music lessons, and
hence feelings of failure. The issue of skills and the perception of the
therapist as a teacher is one of the commonest hurdles for client and
therapist to overcome.
The musical backgrounds of the two parties are only one facet of course
of the cultural issues involved in a therapeutic encounter. Other issues,
which of course overlap with the talking therapies, are verbal cultural
issues – whether therapist and client share the same first language, and
whether there are different cultural assumptions about verbal
interactions. Within British culture, there are many subtle differences in
the way people interact verbally and many clues that we give away about
our social class, ethnic background and regional allegiances – for
instance, people from the North of England often say that they have a
more direct, friendly style of verbal interaction than those from the
South. As music therapists we are interested in musical communication
but there are other non-verbal communications to be aware of, such as
our body-language, and the way we dress.
These are all contexts which might be seen as ‘obstacles’ to the ‘pure’
communication in words or music which we might be aspiring to. Seen
positively, they may provide fruitful work for therapy, in that the
exploration of obstacles to a relationship is the beginning of the
exploration of that relationship. At the least, I suggest they need to be
acknowledged. I defined a context at the beginning of this section which
might be the one which the therapist is ‘aiming’ for: a developing
relationship between therapist and client, expressed through music and
(where appropriate) words, in which the therapist’s interventions are
aimed at helping the client to achieve greater understanding of his or
herself. To arrive at this context, music has to be understood as
communicative, and that what the client plays may have emotional
significance and meaning. Clearly this achieved principally by ‘doing’,
by responding musically to the actions of the client. However, given that
A THEORETICAL MODEL
TRAFFIC AND I spent some years in my first career, developing computerised models of
MUSIC THERAPY
traffic, predicting in particular the way people choose routes, and the
impact on delays on roads. The model of music therapy which follows is
based very loosely on mathematical ideas from that field, but I feel it is
important to stress at the outset what should be obvious, that it is not a
mathematical model in the same way, but an analogy which I have found
helpful and interesting. For myself, of course, there is also a pleasing
symmetry in bringing together ideas from the two diverse parts of my
working life.
A THEORETICAL MODEL 8
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
DISTANCE IN A The quality of a relationship clearly has many facets, positive and
RELATIONSHIP
negative, which we might be able to call ‘variables’; I would like to
project these many dimensions onto one, and call it ‘closeness’. A client
is often referred because they find it difficult to achieve closeness in
words, so the scenario where the musical relationship is closer than the
verbal one may be common and appropriate, especially in the early
stages of therapy; however, I suggest that this imbalance becomes
uncomfortable if it is sustained. On the other hand, if the verbal
relationship is much closer than the musical one, it is possible there is a
temporary resistance to the client expressing themselves musically.
Again though, there are implications if this position is sustained:
presumably the sessions are then operating more as a talking therapy, and
should be acknowledged as such. In figures 34-36, these possibilities are
shown by the lengths of the arrows joining the client and the therapist
A THEORETICAL MODEL 9
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
A THEORETICAL MODEL 10
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
A THEORETICAL MODEL 11
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
If the ‘distances’ in words and music were plotted, we could show the
three positions mentioned above in graphs:
A THEORETICAL MODEL 12
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
A THEORETICAL MODEL 13
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
A THEORETICAL MODEL 14
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
A THEORETICAL MODEL 15
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
A THEORETICAL MODEL 16
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
A THEORETICAL MODEL 17
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
HIERARCHICAL Without stretching the traffic analogy too far, there is one more
CONSTRAINTS
comparison which may be interesting, or indeed we may decide that the
analogy breaks down here. In bi-level optimisation problems, the two
problems are often not completely ‘equal’; in the traffic example, once
the demands are fixed, the flows can only take a certain range of values,
so they are ‘constrained’ by the demands. In music therapy, an
analogous statement might be that what is possible in the verbal
relationship is determined by what has happened in the musical
relationship. Some therapists will agree with this statement more than
others; I am not sure I agree with it, if it were to be taken as operating in
every moment of every session. There must surely be times when the
musical relationship is not progressing, and a verbal intervention moves
A THEORETICAL MODEL 18
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
CATASTROPHE Catastrophe theory was developed in the 1970’s first by Rene Thom
THEORY, INSIGHT
(1975), and then by others including Christopher Zeeman (1977), my
AND RESISTANCE
supervisor for an M.Sc. in mathematics. The arresting title refers to
sudden (rather than particularly negative) change; in nature many step-
changes occur as a result of continuous changes in other variables, which
were difficult to model mathematically before catastrophe theory. As
catastrophe theory was popularised, it was used to model phenomena in
the social sciences, where the variables are less quantifiable; a layman’s
summary of such applications is given in Woodcock and Davis (1978).
A THEORETICAL MODEL 19
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
NON-VERBAL Clearly the ideas described above apply most naturally to clients who are
CLIENTS
physically able to communicate verbally. So therapists working in
psychiatry may find the ideas more interesting, compared for instance
those working with clients with severe learning disabilities, or advanced
dementias. However, often clients’ receptive language is much better
than their spoken language; the communication may be ‘one-way’ and as
therapists we make judgments as to what kind or level of verbal
relationship may be possible. One valid position is that the musical
relationship is the one which empowers the client and puts them on an
equal footing with the therapist - to use many words oneself emphasises
A THEORETICAL MODEL 20
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
the client’s disability. This position does not of course preclude the fact
that we are thinking in words about the relationship when we reflect
afterwards, and in supervision.
CONCLUSIONS
This paper has attempted to point out the ways in which clients’ and
therapists’ perceptions impinge on the process of music therapy. The
pure ‘transference’ model of psychoanalysis and the purity of the
experience of musical communication may be ideals which we try to
facilitate, but do not achieve, even though we may come close. The
second half of the paper used some mathematical ideas to give pictorial
views of how music therapists may be working within music and words
at the same time. The writing of this paper been enjoyable, in bringing
together ideas from my previous career with my present one, and helpful,
CONCLUSIONS 21
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
The author is indebted to Dr Philip Barnard of the Cognitive Brain
Sciences Unit, Medical Research Council, Cambridge, for some
illuminating conversations in the early stages of writing this paper, in
particular, in pointing out relevant parts of the psychology literature and
helping to clarify my thoughts as to how they might apply to music
therapy.
REFERENCES
Gaver W.W. (1988) Everyday listening and auditory icons. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of California, San Diego.
Gaver W.W. (1993) What in the world do we hear?: An ecological
approach to auditory event perception. Ecological Psychology
5(1), 1-29, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gibson J.J. (1977) The theory of affordances. In R. Shaw and J.
Bransford (eds.), Perceiving, acting and knowing: Toward an
ecological psychology, (pp. 67-82), Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.
Henderson H. (1991) Improvised song stories in the treatment of a
thirteen-year-old sexually abused girl from the Xhosa tribe in
South Africa. In K. Bruscia (ed.), Case Studies in Music Therapy,
(pp. 207-217), Pennsylvania: Barcelona Pubs.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT 22
Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V (3) May 2004, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
This article can be cited as: Hughes, P. (2004) What happens in music
therapy: An ecological approach and a theoretical model. Music Therapy
Today (online) Vol. V, Issue 3, available at http://musictherapyworld.net
REFERENCES 23