Behavioral Neurogenetics
Behavioral Neurogenetics
Behavioral Neurogenetics
Series Editors
Mark A. Geyer, La Jolla, CA, USA
Bart A. Ellenbroek, Wellington, New Zealand
Charles A. Marsden, Nottingham, UK
Editors
Behavioral Neurogenetics
123
Editors
John F. Cryan Prof. Dr. Andreas Reif
Department of Anatomy and Neuroscience Department of Psychiatry
University College Cork and Psychotherapy
Western Gateway Building University of Würzburg
Western Rd., Cork Füchsleinstrasse 15
Ireland 97080 Würzburg
Germany
The field of behavioural neurogenetics has developed significantly over the past
two decades. This has been largely driven by technical advances in the field of
molecular genetics both in model systems and in clinical analysis. Indeed, it is
hoped that the elucidation and ongoing functionalisation of the human genome
may provide new insights into the aetiology, course and, ultimately, treatment of
psychiatric illnesses.
This book covers a wide array of topics relevant to behavioural genetics from
both a preclinical and clinical standpoint. Indeed in juxtaposing both areas of
research the reader will appreciate the true translational nature of the field. Topics
covered range from technical advances in genetic analysis in humans and animals
to specific descriptions of advances in schizophrenia, attention disorders, depres-
sion and anxiety disorders, autism, aggression, neurodegeneration and neurode-
velopmental disorders. The importance of gene–environment interactions is
emphasised and the role of neuroimaging in unravelling the functional conse-
quences of genetic variability described.
Part I of this book focuses on advances in the basic sciences of behavioural
neurogenetics with a strong emphasis on animal models of psychiatric illness. It
opens with a chapter by Robert Gerlai highlighting the use of model organisms and
specifically zebrafish (Danio rerio) in modelling complex human psychiatric
disease and its applicability for behavioural genetic studies. Lisa Tarantino follows
by giving the state of the art on forward genetic approaches to elucidate the
contribution of genetic variation to complex behavioural phenotypes. Carola and
Gross illuminate the relative contribution of environment and genetics to psy-
chopathology and how this is informing translational studies in animals and
humans. Miczek and colleagues discuss the neurobiological mechanisms under-
lying aggression and how it can be modified in genetically modified mice. We-
gener, Mathe and Neumann describe the utility of various selectively bred rodent
strains to ask key questions regarding the underlying pathophysiology of depres-
sion and anxiety. Next up is a chapter on how genetic manipulations in rodent
models have allowed for analysis of the impact of specific roles of glutamate
receptors and transporters in cognitive and emotional behaviours shown to be
v
vi Preface
altered by stress. The final two chapters are relevant to schizophrenia. First,
O’Tuathaigh and colleagues describe how phenotypic characterisation of genetic
models of candidate risk genes and/or putative pathophysiological processes
implicated in schizophrenia, as well as examination of epidemiologically relevant
gene 9 environment interactions in these models, can illuminate molecular and
pathobiological mechanisms involved in schizophrenia. Powell, on the other hand,
reviews the literature on genetic models of sensorimotor gating as they apply to
schizophrenia and other neuropsychiatric disorders and discusses the utility of
prepulse inhibition as a tool in phenotyping mutant mouse models.
Part II of this volume focuses on advances in clinical genetic analysis as applied
to various neuropsychiatric disorders. Bayes and colleagues describe how second
generation sequencing technologies are generating unprecedented amounts of
sequence data very rapidly and at relatively limited costs. They also describe the
challenges associated with such data generation in terms of data interpretation,
analysis and management in addition to highlighting where such technologies are
moving to in the future. Hakonarson and colleagues follow on with a description of
the role of copy number variations in a number of neurodevelopmental disorders
including autism, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and schizophrenia. They
also discuss relevant methodological considerations for such analysis. The appli-
cation of neuroimaging has transformed modern neuroscience research, thus Hariri
and colleagues describe how such approaches can be used to understand the
interplay between genes and behaviour in shaping individual variability in brain
function. Christine Freitag, Philip Asherson and Joahnnes Hebebrand discuss in
detail the behavioural neurogenetics of childhood disorders including autism
spectrum disorders, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, nocturnal enuresis and
obesity. A chapter follows this on the use of new technologies to identify genes
relevant to Schizophrenia by Dan Rujescu. As highlighted in the preclinical sec-
tion there is growing appreciation of gene–environment as an emerging area in
psychiatry research. Katja Karg and Srijan Sen give a comprehensive introduction
to the field from a clinical context emphasising theoretical and practical problems
that are worth considering. The behavioural neurogenetics of affective and anxiety
disorders is expertly reviewed by Katharina Domschke and Andreas Reif in the
subsequent chapter. This is followed up by a discussion by Quinn and colleagues
of the contribution of variable number tandem repeat polymorphisms to a range of
psychiatric disorders. Individual variability in response to stimulant drugs has long
been known and Amy Hart, Harriet De Wit and Abraham Palmer examine the
evidence for the contribution of genetic polymorphisms to this response. The
penultimate chapter in the book focuses on the cognitive genetics of psychiatric
disorders and reviews evidence for the heritability of the main cognitive pheno-
types and early progress in the field using cytogenetic, linkage and candidate gene-
based research methodologies. The book closes with a chapter from Daniela
Galimberti and Elio Scarpini on the behavioural genetics of neurodegenerative
disorders with a special focus on susceptibility genes for Alzheimer’s Disease and
Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration.
Preface vii
ix
x Contents
Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 633
Part I
Preclinical Behavioral Genetics
Using Zebrafish to Unravel the Genetics
of Complex Brain Disorders
Robert Gerlai
R. Gerlai (&)
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga,
3359 Mississauga Road North, Rm 3035,
Mississauga, ON L5L 1C6, Canada
e-mail: robert_gerlai@yahoo.com
of the genetics and behavioral approaches is rare with zebrafish, I argue that the
rapid accumulation of knowledge in both of these disciplines will make zebrafish a
prominent research tool for the genetic analysis of complex brain disorders.
Keywords Zebrafish High throughput behavioral screening Fetal alcohol
syndrome Alcoholism Learning and memory Fear and anxiety
Contents
Numerous laboratory organisms are available for one to employ in the analysis of
how genes influence behavior or how certain biochemical processes affect the
functioning of the brain. For example, rats have been traditionally utilized to screen
libraries of compounds to identify drugs that may be beneficial in a range of human
brain and behavioral disorders both in academic and biopharmaceutical preclinical
research. The house mouse has been employed in a range of behavior genetic
applications including those that probe the effects of specific genes (reverse
genetics) and their roles in behavior and brain function [e.g. Gerlai et al. (1995),
Pekhletski et al. (1996)]. But other, simpler, laboratory organisms including the flat
worm (Giles and Rankin 2009), the sea slug (Bailey and Kandel 2008), or the fruit
fly (Sokolowski 2001) have also been successfully utilized to study the biological
bases of brain function and behavior. Compared to these laboratory model organ-
isms the zebrafish is quite novel. I use the term ‘‘model’’ here in a very loose sense
and only mean that the reason for the use of animal species in the laboratory is to
isolate and mimic some aspects of complex biological phenomena, a reductionist
approach that may yield results faster than in human due to the simpler features of
Using Zebrafish to Unravel the Genetics 5
the studied organism and to the precisely controlled laboratory conditions. In this
sense, zebrafish may be an ideal model organism.
The zebrafish strikes an optimal compromise between system complexity and
practical simplicity. It is a vertebrate species with a physiology (Alsop and Vijayan
2008), brain anatomy (Tropepe and Sive 2003), and neurochemistry (Chatterjee and
Gerlai 2009) characteristic of the prototypical vertebrate, and thus translationally
relevant to our own species. Most importantly, the nucleotide sequence of zebrafish
genes is often found highly similar (70–80% homology) to the mammalian (and
human) counterparts, and the amino acid sequence of functionally relevant domains
of its proteins has been found to be even more evolutionarily conserved, i.e. similar
to mammalian sequences (Reimers et al. 2004; Renier et al. 2007). It is thus quite
probable that a gene identified in zebrafish as involved in particular functions/dys-
functions of its brain, will have a human homolog serving similar functions and vica
versa. Briefly, the translational relevance of zebrafish research is expected to be high.
The number of animals one can screen is a crucial factor in forward genetics
where one does not know which and how many genes may influence the pheno-
typical function in question. In case of brain disorders or behavioral function, the
number of such genes may be quite large and thus one may have to analyze
thousands of mutants to tackle this complexity and identify appropriate mutations
and thus the genes involved. One can, of course, generate the same number of
zebrafish and mice for screening purposes but there are several reasons why
zebrafish may be preferred. First, a single female zebrafish can produce 200
offspring at every spawning and can spawn multiple times a week. Second,
zebrafish is small (4 cm long) and is highly social and thus a large number of
subjects may be housed cheaply in a small animal holding room. For example, a
standard zebrafish stand-alone high density rack system (e.g. Aquatic Ecosystems
Inc, FL, or Aquaneering Inc. CA) with six shelves and about 23 liter tanks per shelf,
can house about 2,000 zebrafish, and a 40 m2 standard vivarium room may be fitted
up with up to 10 such racks. Briefly, the same room that may house a couple of
hundred mice can have about 20,000 zebrafish in it. Therefore, when it comes to
large scale screening, the zebrafish has a definite cost advantage. Given the relative
simplicity of this vertebrate species and the fact that it is a phylogenetically older
‘‘design’’ compared to mammals, one may also argue that it may allow the analysis
of fundamental core mechanisms of the chosen brain function. Last, adding zebrafish
to the list of already well studied vertebrates (e.g. the mouse and the rat) should
facilitate cross species comparison and finding common characteristics and mech-
anisms, which should also enhance our ability to translate the findings to human.
Genetics is one of the strengths of zebrafish and excellent reviews have been
published on numerous genetic techniques developed for this species (Amsterdam
and Hopkins 2006; Chen and Ekker 2004; Lekven et al. 2000; Patton and Zon 2001).
6 R. Gerlai
Here I will discuss these techniques only briefly. In addition to sophisticated gene
expression analyses including quantitative reverse transcriptase polymerase
chain reaction (q-RT-PCR) and DNA microarrays (gene chip), both reverse genetic
and forward genetic methods are available with zebrafish, although the former, the
forward genetic approaches, have been more prevalent in zebrafish research. Reverse
genetic analysis allows one to study the phenotypical effects of targeted manipulation
of known genes. The main goal of forward genetic studies, on the other hand, is to
discover novel genes by the introduction of random mutations.
Among the reverse genetic tools, TILLING has been employed successfully in
zebrafish (Moens et al. 2008). Targeting induced local lesions in genomes
(TILLING) allows the identification of mutations in specific genes of interest in
chemically mutagenized zebrafish populations. The method was first described for
mutation detection in Arabidopsis about a decade ago but since then it has
successfully been adopted for zebrafish too (Moens et al. 2008). The essence of the
TILLING method is the screening of chemically mutagenized populations of
zebrafish using the polymerase chain reaction (PCR) for mutations in the gene
of interest. Unlike in gene targeting with the use of homologous recombination in
embryonic stem cells employed in the mouse (Capecchi 1989), and most recently
in the rat (Tong et al. 2010), the actual mutagenesis conducted in TILLING is
random, i.e. not targeted. The ‘‘trick’’ of TILLING is then the identification of the
mutation(s) in the target gene. The identification of the mutation may be achieved
using two different approaches. One is the resequencing of every single mutage-
nized genome (the gene of interest and its sequence is known and thus alterations
in the sequence can be detected), a brute force approach that is becoming
increasingly feasible with the ever improving speed of sequencing methods. The
other method is based upon the use of cell, a plant-specific extracellular glyco-
protein that cleaves heteroduplex DNA at single nucleotide mismatches (resulting
from the introduced point mutation). Using fluorescently labeled primers to
detect cell cleavage products on a LiCor acrylamide slab gel, cell can identify a
heterozygous single nucleotide mismatch [for further details of the TILLING
methodology and its use, see Moens et al. (2008)].
Another approach that is principally a reverse genetic method, i.e. it is also
aimed at the characterization of the function of known genes, is a knock down
method using morpholinos (Bill et al. 2009). Morpholinos are antisense oligonu-
cleotides (usually 25 bases long) composed of a phosphorodiamidate backbone
with a morpholine ring and the same bases as DNA (Bill et al. 2009). Morpholinos
are injected into the target cells and act by steric hindrance to block ribosome entry
and hence prevent protein production. The antisense technology has been long
employed in mammalian species but the efficiency and specificity of the oligo-
nucleotide approach has been controversial. In zebrafish, however, the morpholino
Using Zebrafish to Unravel the Genetics 7
approach has been successfully employed. For example, due to the altered
backbone of the morpholinos they are not affected by nucleases and are, therefore,
highly resistant to breakdown in vivo. Furthermore, because of their small size and
unusual chemistry, morpholinos remain undetected for the immune system.
Morpholinos are traditionally introduced into the yolk of 1–8 cell-stage embryos in
which the cytoplasmic bridges connecting the embryonic cells allow rapid diffu-
sion of the hydrophilic morpholinos leading to ubiquitous delivery. The analysis of
the result of the gene expression knock down is usually limited to the embryonic
stage of zebrafish. However, examples already exist suggesting that modified
morpholino chemistry (the VIVO-morpholino, which allows penetration of the
oligonucleotide into adult zebrafish cells) may be successfully employed in adult
zebrafish as well [e.g. Kim et al. (2010)]. This is a crucial novel development
considering that most complex brain disorders and higher behavioral functions can
be best modeled, observed, and analyzed in the adult zebrafish.
RNA-interference, or RNAi, is yet another intriguing possibility for targeted
modification of gene expression in zebrafish. RNAi is a transcriptional gene
silencing mechanism-induced by short (21–23 bases long) double stranded
RNA whose main mechanisms are believed to be gene expression regulation via
miRNA’s (endogeneous microRNA’s) and defense against viral genetic material
mediated by dsRNAs (double stranded RNAs), terms that represent structurally
indistinguishable mRNA species. Irrespective of the physiological function, a few
years ago it was realized that the cell’s RNAi mechanism could be utilized for the
induction of targeted knock down of gene expression by delivering double
stranded short RNA sequences specific to the chosen target gene. Although
zebrafish cells have been shown to possess the RNAi machinery, the functional
consequences, especially the specificity of the RNAi-based manipulation, have
been questioned [for review see Skromne and Prince (2008)] and thus whether this
technology will lead to success in zebrafish remains to be seen. Another tech-
nology, transgenic methods, however, may offer a currently existing true and tried
alternative.
In the mammalian neurobehavioral genetics field, transgenic methods have
been perhaps the most fruitful reverse genetic approaches. Transgenic technologies
have also been successfully employed with zebrafish [for a recent review see
Skromne and Prince (2008)]. These techniques make use of a variety of methods
(e.g. enzymatic approaches, transposons, and retroviruses) to enable the delivery
and increase the efficiency of incorporation of foreign DNA into the genome of
zebrafish thereby generating stable transgenic fish lines in which the foreign DNA
is expressed. Irrespective of the mode of delivery, transgenic zebrafish may be
divided into two main classes: transgenic overexpressors and dominant negative
transgenics. In the former, overexpression of the transgene is achieved, for
example, by delivering multiple copies of the transgene or using a strong promoter,
and is used to test the functional consequences of the excess amount of translated
gene product. In the latter, the expressed transgene product interferes with or
blocks the function of the endogenous gene and thus allows one to test the effect of
loss-of-function at the phenotypical level.
8 R. Gerlai
screens were conducted about 15 years ago and set the stage for subsequent
screening studies. Since then, most forward genetic studies have utilized a
chemical mutagen, ethyl-nitroso-urea (ENU), which is expected to induce single
nucleotide point mutations, and when dosed appropriately, one mutation per
genome on average. The advantage of ENU mutagenesis is that ENU is efficient
and with it one can achieve a good coverage of the entire genome, i.e. can expect
to hit a large proportion of genes as long as a large enough number of animals
(thousands) are mutagenized and analyzed. The Achilles heel of ENU mutagen-
esis, however, is the subsequent linkage analysis, which requires several genera-
tions of crosses and cumbersome mapping. Nevertheless, due to the availability of
high resolution markers developed for zebrafish, the genes carrying the induced
mutations can be successfully identified using linkage analysis-based positional
cloning (Knapik 2000; Patton and Zon 2001). Another concern with ENU-based
forward genetic analysis is that the point mutation induced by ENU is often
recessive and thus may not be observable unless bred into a homozygous form,
which requires three generations of breeding to create an F3 or a backcross seg-
regating population (Patton and Zon 2001). Although not without technical
complications, an alternative that can speed up the generation of recessive
homozygous mutants does exist, it is gynogenesis. For gynogenesis one may use
heat shock or pressure to manipulate the cell division cycle at the earliest
embryonic stage leading to haploid to diploid genome conversion and allowing the
generation of homozygous fish in essentially one step without the cumbersome
breeding [for review of experimental examples using gynogenesis, see Patton and
Zon (2001)].
ENU has been the most frequently employed mutagen for zebrafish, but other
mutagenesis methods have also been successfully utilized. Perhaps the most
promising among them is retrovirus mediated insertional mutagenesis (Amsterdam
and Hopkins 2006). This method has the advantage over ENU because insertion of
the viral genetic material into the zebrafish genome not only induces a mutation
but by leaving the unique viral sequences in the genome allows fast and efficient
localization and cloning of the mutated gene (Amsterdam and Hopkins 2006).
A potential concern with forward genetic approaches in zebrafish is that this
species has undergone a partial genome duplication event in its evolutionary past.
The concern is that the genes whose locus is on the duplicated region of the
genome (approximately 20% of the genome) may be able to compensate for the
induced mutation and could mask its effects if the sister gene remained active and
if its function has not changed much since the gene duplication event. However,
some argue that the partial ‘‘tetraploidy’’ may be viewed as an advantage in
zebrafish for forward genetics especially when it comes to the analysis of the
genetics of complex traits: the induced mutations are not expected to be lethal and
may only have small quantitative effects on the phenotype, thus making functional
phenotypical identification and characterization feasible. Whether these arguments
turn out to be correct will have to be seen. But the few already existing examples
suggest that we have some reason for optimism. Below I review such examples
organized according to their disease relevance.
10 R. Gerlai
5 Parkinson’s Disease
worldwide (Querfurth and LaFerla 2010). The histological hallmarks of the disease
are neurofibrillary tangles (mentioned above) and, perhaps even more character-
istically, amyloid plaques. Although the familial (heritable) form of the disease
only makes up 4–5% of all cases, clearly demonstrating the importance of
environmental factors, the genetic analysis of Alzheimer’s disease has led to major
discoveries and clarified some of the biological mechanisms core to the disease.
The genes encoding amyloid-b precursor protein (APP), and presenilins PS1 and
PS2 have been found to underlie familial Alzheimer’s disease, and the e4 allele of
the gene encoding apolipoprotein E (ApoE) has been found to be a risk factor in
sporadic (non-familial) Alzheimer’s cases. The APP, PS1, PS2, and ApoE genes
have all been identified in zebrafish and have been found to be highly homologous
to their mammalian, and human counterparts, with certain functionally relevant
regions approaching 100% identity with human (e.g. the transmembrane region of
APP). Furthermore, some of the components of the c-secretase complex mediating
the processing and cleavage of APP, which may lead to the generation of the toxic
Ab (40, 42) peptide have started to be examined in zebrafish (reviewed in Xia
2010). To characterize the involvement of APP in zebrafish embryonic develop-
ment, the expression of the APP gene was reduced using morpholinos, which
led to significant shortening of the body length of the embryos (Joshi et al. 2009).
The involvement of overexpression of APP (as in Down’s patients, for example,
who also develop Alzheimer’s disease) or of the expression of mutant forms of
APP (identified in familial Alzheimer’s cases), or the role of presinilins have not
been investigated in zebrafish, nor has been any studies conducted for the potential
behavioral consequences of transgenic manipulations of these genes. The function
of ApoE and its potential role in Alzheimer’s disease related abnormalities also
has not been investigated with zebrafish.
Perhaps the largest cluster of human brain disorders in terms of prevalence are
neuropsychiatric conditions including anxiety and stress disorders, depression,
obsessive compulsive disorders, and several forms of phobias. Although both
academic and pharmaceutical and biotechnology companies have been studying
the potential mechanisms of these disorders and several treatment options have
been developed, these diseases still represent an enormous unmet medical need.
This is because the causative factors (both genetic and environmental) behind
these diseases are difficult to trace and the mechanisms of the diseases are also
complex. A recent review paints an optimistic picture as to the potential use of
zebrafish in the analysis and modeling of neuropsychiatric conditions (Mathur and
Guo 2010) and I share this optimism. For example, zebrafish possess a gluco-
corticoid and a mineralocorticoid receptor that have been cloned and sequenced,
and has a corticoid signaling pathway highly similar to that of mammals
(Amsterdam and Hopkins 2006; Denver 2009). Also, the major components
12 R. Gerlai
necessary for success, including the genetic tools discussed above and the novel
behavioral paradigms, some of which are presented below, already exist for
zebrafish. Nevertheless, one must also acknowledge that these studies with zeb-
rafish are only starting now and thus have not produced major breakthroughs.
The zebrafish has also been suggested for the analysis of the mechanisms of autism
spectrum disorders (Tropepe and Sive 2003). The number of genetic factors
underlying autism spectrum disorders is believed to be much less than in such
neuropsychiatric conditions as anxiety or schizophrenia, and thus animal genetic
models have been generated with much hope [for a review, see e.g. Gerlai and
Gerlai (2003)]. Importantly, several of the genes implicated in the human disease
have been shown to have homologs in zebrafish [for a most recent review, see
Mathur and Guo (2010)]. It may therefore be possible to recapitulate some aspects
of autism spectrum disorders by selectively targeting these genes and testing the
effect of the genetic or pharmacological manipulations on developmental as well
as behavioral characteristics in zebrafish. It is also notable that the zebrafish is a
highly social species, and the novel behavioral paradigms that are being developed
to induce and quantify social behavioral responses in zebrafish may also contribute
to this research, for example, by allowing large scale mutaganesis screening-based
identification of molecular mechanisms involved in vertebrate social behavior.
9 Schizophrenia
response to tactile stimulation in the zebrafish larva (Gauthier et al. 2010). It may
be noted, however, that the specificity of the morpholino-induced changes may be
questionable given that the attempt to rescue the phenotype by injection of wild
type or mutant SHANK3 mRNA sequences led only to partial success at best.
Last, psychopharmacological experiments have already started to be utilized with
zebrafish in the analysis and modeling of schizophrenia. One behavioral endo-
phenotype often argued to be an important aspect of schizophrenia is reduced
prepulse inhibition, or PPI. Prepulse inhibition is believed to be a measure of
sensory gating. It is induced by employing a weak stimulus (the prepulse), which is
expected to inhibit the reaction to a subsequent stronger startling stimulus (the
pulse). Larval zebrafish exhibit PPI of the acoustic startle response similarly to
what has been demonstrated in rodents [reviewed in Mathur and Guo (2010)]. PPI
can be disrupted by dopamine agonists in the zebrafish larvae, an alteration that is
reversed by antipsychotic drugs similarly to the mammalian situation (Braff et al.
2001). In addition to these promising psychopharmacology results, a forward
genetic screen has already isolated a mutant ‘‘Ophelia’’, which exhibited reduced
PPI (Burgess and Granato 2007). In summary, the first examples showing how
zebrafish may be utilized in the investigation of the genetic mechanisms of
complex brain disorders already exist. However, in most of these studies the
behavioral consequences of the employed experimental manipulations were not
analyzed or were studied in a rudimentary manner. This is, in general, the current
weakness of the zebrafish as an experimental tool: its genetics and neurobiology
have been traditionally powerful but its behavioral characteristics are largely
unmapped because only a few behavioral test paradigms are available (Sison et al.
2006).
Sophisticated behavioral paradigms may be crucial for two main reasons: first, the
construct and face validity of the genetic (reverse genetics) or pharmacological
models may only be fully established using behavioral tests; and two, behavioral
paradigms may represent unbiased screening tools for forward genetic applications
(Gerlai 2002). Arguably, behavioral analysis can efficiently probe a broad spec-
trum of brain functions in a large number of subjects. It is not limited to particular
brain regions or neurobiological mechanisms, and it is simple and cheap to
conduct (Gerlai and Clayton 1999). Arguably, high throughput behavioral screens
may be able to systematically reveal mutation or drug-induced functional changes
in the brain. Fortunately, for the past few years a clear upsurge of zebrafish
behavioral studies is evident, indicating that behavioral neuroscience and behavior
genetics has started to acknowledge the utility of this species. Below I discuss
some of these recent studies focusing on the question of how behavioral analysis
may be utilized for the discovery of novel genes and compounds affecting brain
14 R. Gerlai
dysfunction associated with complex human brain disorders. Three main behav-
ioral focus areas will be represented below: learning and memory, which is rele-
vant for a number of neurodegenerative diseases including Alzheimer’s disease;
fear and anxiety, which are important behavioral responses and behavioral states
relevant for a spectrum of neuropsychiatric conditions; and social behavior, whose
abnormalities may be important in the analysis of autism spectrum disorders and
schizophrenia, to mention but the two most important diseases in this domain.
Learning and memory has been extensively studied by scholars of several scien-
tific fields. Numerous mechanistic questions related to how learning occurs, and
what memory is, have been successfully tackled. For example, by now a large
number of genes and biochemical mechanisms underlying learning and memory
have been identified (Sweatt 2010). Can zebrafish add anything to this wealth of
knowledge? Although hundreds of genes involved in learning and memory have
been identified, the mechanisms of these complex processes are far from being
understood. It is likely that the number of undiscovered genes that play roles in
learning and memory is large. For example, according to conservative estimates,
most vertebrate genomes contain about 30,000 genes. Recent microarray studies
suggest that at least 50% of all the genes of the genome are expressed in the
vertebrate brain (e.g. in zebrafish), i.e. about 15,000 genes (Pan et al. 2010). Given
that plasticity is perhaps the most complex aspect of brain function, it is likely that
a large proportion of these genes, i.e. potentially thousands of them, are involved
in some mechanisms subserving plasticity, i.e. learning and memory.
A number of laboratories have realized that the cheap and easy to breed zeb-
rafish may offer a solution for high throughput screening which would be costly
with traditional laboratory rodents. Investigators have started to characterize the
cognitive capabilities of zebrafish and have already developed several test methods
that can measure learning and memory efficiently and fast [for examples see Sison
et al. (2006), Sison and Gerlai (2010)]. The key in these paradigms concerns
automatability. Even if one needs to employ several repeated training trials, as is
the case in most learning paradigms, if these trials are administered in an auto-
mated manner, and if the behavioral responses that reflect learning and memory
performance are easy to measure and do not require the constant presence and
attention of an experimenter, the paradigm may be run in multiple test apparati in
parallel and thus become high throughput.
A successful high throughput learning task design utilizes moving (animated)
images of conspecifics, which are shown on a computer screen placed by each side
of the experimental tank (Pather and Gerlai 2009). Previously, access to view a
shoal (group) of zebrafish has been shown to represent a reward for experimental
zebrafish and that this visual stimulus (the sight of a group of zebrafish) can support
good learning performance (Al-Imari and Gerlai 2008). Subsequently, it has been
Using Zebrafish to Unravel the Genetics 15
exploration phase of the paradigm also used the right tunnel during the probe trial,
those fish that experienced the left tunnel open used the left tunnel during the
probe trial and those fish that experienced both the left and right tunnel open chose
randomly. Why is this paradigm high throughput? Although the exploration phase
of the paradigm took 16 days, because the fish were not monitored and their
behavior was not analyzed, one could set up a large number of mazes and train a
large number of fish every day. The probe trial was conducted for every fish
separately, but it lasted only for 10 min per fish and the swim path of the fish could
be quantified using automated video-tracking techniques (Blaser and Gerlai 2006).
Thus this phase of the paradigm could also be made high throughput. Furthermore,
given the spatial nature of the task, this paradigm is likely to be capable of tapping
into complex forms of learning and memory.
There are again many questions about this novel paradigm. What motivates the
fish to learn the maze? In other words, why fish remember the tunnel they explored
before? This form of learning is termed latent learning because apparently there is
no external experimenter controlled motivator (positive or negative reinforcement)
presented. However, it has been argued (Gómez-Laplaza and Gerlai 2010), based
on prior supporting evidence, that exploration of novelty itself is rewarding in this
task and the novel aspect of the maze is what kept the fish motivated to explore and
learn. The results of this study also suggested that learning in this paradigm was
likely based upon acquiring and remembering external visual cues, i.e. spatial
learning, a hypothesis that will need to be proven in the future. But again, despite
the novel aspect of the task and the fact that there may be numerous questions one
could explore with it, the paradigm does appear to be appropriate for high
throughput screening of learning and mnemonic characteristics of zebrafish and
mutation-induced changes in these characteristics.
There are numerous human disorders associated with memory loss and/or
impairment of cognitive function, perhaps the most devastating and prevalent is
Alzheimer’s disease discussed above. But milder forms of memory problems, mild
cognitive impairment (MCI) and age-related memory decline also affect a large
percentage of the aging human population in the twenty-first century. Given
the large unmet medical need associated with these diseases and the potential
complexity of the genetic mechanisms underlying them (Haffter et al. 1996), the
importance of appropriate screening tools with which mutation-induced changes in
learning and memory processes may be identified is unquestionable.
Fear (induced by particular negative stimuli) and anxiety (a more diffuse and
prolonged behavioral state not associated with particular induction stimuli) affects
a large percentage of the human population (Weisberg 2009) and despite concerted
efforts by pharmaceutical research companies and academic laboratories and
despite the existence of several drugs, proper treatment is still not available for a
18 R. Gerlai
proportion of patients. It has been argued by several researchers that zebrafish may
be successfully utilized to study and model some of the mechanisms of vertebrate
fear and/or anxiety (Gerlai 2010). For example, fear responses have been reliably
induced in zebrafish using a chemical cue, the alarm substance (Speedie and Gerlai
2008). Alarm substances have been shown to elicit fear and panic reactions in a
broad range of fish species (Speedie and Gerlai 2008). These substances, which are
produced by epidermal club cells in the skin of many fish species, are released
when the skin is cut or damaged. In nature, the alarm substance is believed to
signal danger, perhaps the presence of an actively hunting predator (piscivore fish
species or a bird of prey). In the laboratory, the alarm substance has been suc-
cessfully utilized to experimentally induce fear responses. Zebrafish have also
been shown to reliably respond to this chemical cue with alarm reactions that
include erratic movements (zig-zagging), jumping (or leaping) and freezing
(complete immobility) (Speedie and Gerlai 2008). From the perspective of muta-
genesis screening, however reliable these responses may seem, the alarm substance
approach suffers from a major disadvantage. This substance has to be extracted
from the skin of conspecifics which entails cutting or homogenizing the skin of
freshly sacrificed fish and washing, diluting the extract. Because of the variability
inherent in this extraction process, the exact dose and potency of the substance can
not be ascertained across multiple experiments (multiple extractions). Recently,
however, zebrafish has been shown to respond to a synthetic alarm substance that
shares a key chemical structural element with that of natural alarm substances from
several fish species (Parra et al. 2009). Hypoxantin-3-N-Oxide, H3NO, has been
found to induce alarm reactions in zebrafish similar to those elicited by the natural
alarm substance (Parra et al. 2009). Thus, it is now possible to precisely control the
dose of the alarm substance and reduce unwanted experimental error variation, a
crucial requirement for high throughput mutagenesis screens.
Although the synthetic alarm substance, H3NO, now allows precise and rep-
licable fear induction, this method suffers from a drawback. Olfactory cues are
notoriously difficult to work with. The onset (delivery) of the cue and now also its
dose can be precisely controlled, however, its offset (washout) is difficult to
achieve. In most behavioral paradigms, experimenters want to introduce the
subject to its test chamber (tank) and let the subject habituate, establish a stable
baseline behavior before administering the cue (the alarm substance in this case).
This allows pre- and post-cue delivery periods to be compared and thus is a more
powerful experimental design. Ideally, after the delivery of the cue and recording
the effects of this delivery, one would like to turn it off and again compare periods
during and after cue delivery. But this is quite cumbersome with olfactory cues.
Furthermore, even if the experimental paradigm does not require turning off the
cue during the behavioral session, the cue may be difficult to remove from the tank
for the next subject. Residual amounts of the alarm substance may remain in the
test tank even after emptying and refilling the test tank. As even trace amounts of
the alarm substance may influence the behavior of the fish, this olfactory cue is
difficult to work with especially when one wants to run a large number of fish as
required for mutagenesis screening.
Using Zebrafish to Unravel the Genetics 19
To circumvent the above issues, cues of other modalities have been tried for the
induction of fear responses. Zebrafish, being a diurnal vertebrate, has excellent
vision and respond well to visual cues. Zebrafish have been demonstrated to
respond differentially to the sight of live fish according to whether the fish species
shown were predatory or harmless and whether they were sympatric (coinhabiting
the geographical region) or allopatric with zebrafish (Bass and Gerlai 2008). The
latter study also demonstrated that zebrafish uniquely responded to a sympatric
predator, the Indian leaf fish (Nandus nandus) and that the sight (solely visual
stimuli) of the predator was sufficient to induce a maximal fear response (erratic
movements and jumps). Utilizing this finding, subsequently zebrafish have been
found to exhibit significant antipredatory responses not only to the sight of live
Indian leaf fish but also to animated (moving) computer images of this species
(Gerlai et al. 2009b). In this latter paradigm, both the presentation of stimuli and
the recording and analysis of the fear responses were conducted in an automated
computerized manner, i.e. the test paradigm was scalable and thus potentially
appropriate for high throughput screening. Although numerous parameters of this
automated fear paradigm will have to be optimized (e.g. size of the test tank, size
and speed of movement of the predator image, presence or absence of hiding
places, level of illumination, etc.), the results demonstrate the feasibility of high
throughput screening for agents (mutations or pharmaceutical compounds) that
may have fear altering properties.
13 Social Behavior
The last behavioral focus area I discuss in this paper is social behavior. Social
behavior is a common term for a range of complex behavioral phenomena from
agonistic (aggressive) encounters to reproductive (courtship) behaviors. Here I
focus on a behavior within this broad range termed affiliative behavior, social
cohesion or group forming. Affiliative or group forming behaviors are character-
istic of our own species. Humans tend to form groups, which in modern history led
to the development of the complex society where a set of rules govern. We are
particularly sensitive to social signals and tend to spontaneously follow a large
number of complex social rules. Briefly, being social is an inherent human trait.
There are numerous human disorders that are associated with abnormalities in
social behavior, one prominent example is the autism spectrum disorders (ASD).
Treatment for ASD and other forms of abnormal social behaviors is lacking for
two main reasons. One, the mechanisms underlying these diseases are unclear.
Two, the mechanisms underlying social behaviors in general are not understood.
Laboratory model organisms have been proposed to speed up the discovery of such
mechanisms [for review see Gerlai and Gerlai (2003)]. The question as to whether
autism may be modeled using animals is not trivial, however. Some may be skep-
tical and say that in order to model autism in animals one would need to understand
its mechanism first in humans, so what is the use of animal research? Nevertheless, it
20 R. Gerlai
is becoming clearer that even such complex phenomena as social behavior has not
only face but also construct validity in animal models, i.e. not only looks similar in
animals but may also be mechanistically similar to that of our own species. Briefly,
it may make sense to study social behavior in vertebrates other than humans,
discover the underlying mechanisms in the laboratory organism, and look for
translational aspects of the work, i.e. human homologs. Zebrafish is perhaps the most
social vertebrate model organism currently under study in the laboratory. Zebrafish
are found swimming in groups in nature, a behavior that they maintain under the
artificial confines of the laboratory (Engeszer et al. 2007). It is this swimming
together response, or shoaling, that may be an excellent behavioral phenomenon
to study from a translational perspective. Answering such questions as to what
neurobiological mechanisms (circuits, synaptic processes, biochemical interactions)
underlie group forming or social cohesion in zebrafish may help us understand
human social behavior and ultimately perhaps the mechanisms of the abnormalities
of human social behavior. The first step in this research could be the characterization
of social behavior in zebrafish followed by the development of behavioral test
paradigms that could detect mutation or drug-induced changes in brain function at
the level of social behavior. Below I present some examples of recent discoveries
with zebrafish that may be useful to make the first steps in this direction.
Zebrafish forms groups and swims in group formation but due to unavailability
of appropriate behavioral quantification methods, the complexities of this behavior
were not properly described in the past. By now, however, methods have been
designed that allow the quantification of numerous parameters of shoaling
behavior, including moment to moment changes of the distances among every
possible pairs of fish within the shoal (Miller and Gerlai 2007, 2008). A periodic
(cyclical) fluctuation of shoal cohesion has been discovered in zebrafish (Miller
and Gerlai 2008). Analysis of shoaling is now further developed to allow high
throughput automated tracking of multiple fish and thus the precise description of
how the entire shoal behaves. This method may enable one to screen for mutations
but would require the use of a group of fish that carry the same mutation, which
would necessitate breeding an extra generation (i.e. testing not the individual
mutant fish but its offspring). Perhaps a faster behavioral screening method may be
to test single fish and its response to social stimuli. The disadvantage of the latter
approach is that complex group dynamics may not be detected but the advantage is
that the test would save the extra generation of breeding.
Testing responses of individual fish to social stimuli has been achieved with an
experimental set up similar to the predator visual stimulus paradigm (Gerlai et al.
2009a). Here the computer monitor placed on the side of the experimental tank
shows animated (moving) images of zebrafish (five fish in this case). Each fish on
the monitor moves independently and in different randomized directions and with
a speed that changes from second to second while remaining within the range of
the speed of normally swimming zebrafish. This artificial ‘‘shoal’’ elicits a robust
behavioral response. The single experimental fish placed in the test tank usually
does not exhibit a preference for any sides of the tank, explores the entire tank,
and thus its position when averaged over a period of time (e.g. for one minute
Using Zebrafish to Unravel the Genetics 21
intervals) ends up to be in the middle of the tank, which is 25 cm away from the
computer screen in case of a 50 cm long tank. However, as soon as the computer
screen shows the artificial shoal, the experimental zebrafish moves closer to the
computer screen and on average stays about 10 cm away from it, a distance that is
similar to what has been obtained with freely moving zebrafish in a real shoal
(Miller and Gerlai 2008). Given that the visual stimulus that elicits the response
is computer controlled and the subject’s distance from the stimulus screen is
recorded using computerized video-tracking, the entire test paradigm is automated,
i.e. does not require the presence of the experimenter during the behavioral
recording session. The paradigm therefore is high throughput and has utility in
screening for mutation or drug-induced changes in social behavior. Indeed, this
paradigm has been already used to detect strain (genetic) differences between
populations of zebrafish, and alcohol and dopamine receptor antagonist-induced
changes in social behavior [Gerlai et al. (2009a) and unpublished results].
14 Concluding Remarks
The excellent genetic tools developed for zebrafish have already provided promising
results in the analysis of complex brain disorders. Importantly, several genes impli-
cated in a number of human brain disorders have been shown to have zebrafish
homologs. The function of these genes in embryonic development, and in a few cases
in behavioral responses has started to be investigated. The genetic tools are constantly
refined. Increasing number of genetic markers is becoming available for linkage
analysis-based gene localization in random mutagenesis. Reverse genetic tools are
also rapidly developing. In addition, but also very importantly, numerous novel
behavioral paradigms are being developed and our understanding of the behavioral
responses and capabilities of zebrafish has been exponentially increasing over the past
few years suggesting that efficient and high throughput phenotypical screening
applications (Gerlai 2002) are becoming reality for zebrafish. Given the fact that
behavior is the output of the brain and that vertebrates share numerous biological
features, including nucleotide sequence homologies, it is likely that zebrafish
neurobehavioral genetics will facilitate the identification of numerous genes and
compounds leading to the understanding and better treatment of human brain disorders.
References
Al-Imari L, Gerlai R (2008) Conspecifics as reward in associative learning tasks for zebrafish
(Danio rerio). Behav Brain Res 189:216–219
Alsop D, Vijayan MM (2008) Development of the corticosteroid stress axis and receptor
expression in zebrafish. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 294:R711–R719
22 R. Gerlai
Giles AC, Rankin CH (2009) Behavioral and genetic characterization of habituation using
Caenorhabditis elegans. Neurobiol Learn Mem 92:139–146
Gómez-Laplaza LM, Gerlai R (2010) Latent Learning in Zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Brain
Res 208:509–515
Haffter P, Nüsslein-Volhard C (1996) Large scale genetics in a small vertebrate, the zebrafish.
Int J Dev Biol 40:221–227
Haffter P, Granato M, Brand M, Mullins MC, Hammerschmidt M, Kane DA, Odenthal J,
Van Eeden FJM, Jiang YJ, Heisenberg CP, Kelsh RN, Furutaniseiki M, Vogelsang E, Beuchle D,
Schach U, Fabian C, Nüsslein-Volhard C (1996) The identification of genes with unique and
essential function in the development of the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Development 123:1–36
Huang CJ, Jou TS, Ho YL, Lee WH, Jeng YT, Hsieh FJ, Tsai HJ (2005) Conditional expression
of a myocardium-specific transgene in zebrafish transgenic lines. Dev Dyn 233:1294–1303
Joshi P, Liang JO, Dimonte K, Sullivan J, Pimplikar SW (2009) Amyloid precursor protein is
required for convergent-extension movements during zebrafish development. Dev Biol 335:1–11
Kim S, Radhakrishnan UP, Rajpurohit SK, Kulkarni V, Jagadeeswaran P (2010) Vivo-Morpholino
knockdown of alpha IIb: a novel approach to inhibit thrombocyte function in adult zebrafish.
Blood Cells Mol Dis 44:169–174
Knapik EW (2000) ENU mutagenesis in zebrafish—from genes to complex diseases. Mamm
Genome 11:511–519
Langenau DM, Feng H, Berghmans S, Kanki JP, Kutok JL, Look AT (2005) Cre/lox-regulated
transgenic zebrafish model with conditional myc-induced T cell acute lymphoblastic
leukemia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:6068–6073
Lee KY, Huang H, Ju B, Yang Z, Lin S (2002) Cloned zebrafish by nuclear transfer from long-
term-cultured cells. Nat Biotech 20:795–799
Lekven AC, Helde KA, Thorpe CJ, Rooke R, Moon RT (2000) Reverse genetics in zebrafish.
Physiol Genomics 2:37–48
Mathur P, Guo S (2010) Use of zebrafish as a model to understand mechanisms of addiction and
complex neurobehavioral phenotypes. Neurobiol Dis 40:66–72
McEchron MD, Disterhoft JF (1999) Hippocampal encoding of non-spatial trace conditioning.
Hippocampus 9:385–396
Miller N, Gerlai R (2008) Oscillations in shoal cohesion in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain
Res 193:148–151
Miller N, Gerlai R (2007) Quantification of shoaling behaviour in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav.
Brain Res 184:157–166
Moens CB, Donn TM, Wolf-Saxon ER, Ma TP (2008) Reverse genetics in zebrafish by
TILLING. Brief Funct Genomic Proteomic 7:454–459
Pan Y, Razak Z, Mo K, Westwood JT, Gerlai R (2010) Chronic alcohol exposure induced gene
expression changes in the zebrafish brain. Genes Brain Behav (in press)
Parra KV, Adrian JC Jr, Gerlai R (2009) The synthetic substance hypoxanthine 3-N-oxide elicits
alarm reactions in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res 205:336–341
Paquet D et al (2009) A zebrafish model of tauopathy allows in vivo imaging of neuronal cell
death and drug evaluation. J Clin Invest 119:1382–1395
Pather S, Gerlai R (2009) Shuttle box learning in zebrafish. Behav Brain Res 196:323–327
Patton EE, Zon LI (2001) The art and design of genetic screens: zebrafish. Nat Rev Genet 2:956–966
Pekhletski R, Gerlai R, Overstreet L, Huang X-P, Agopyan N, Slater NT, Roder J, Hampson DR
(1996) Impaired motor learning and short-term synaptic plasticity in mice lacking mGluR4
metabotropic glutamate receptors. J Neurosci 16:6364–6373
Querfurth HW, LaFerla FM (2010) Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 362:329–344
Reimers MJ, Hahn ME, Tanguay RL (2004) Two zebrafi sh alcohol dehydrogenases share
common ancestry with mammalian class I, II, IV, and V alcohol dehydrogenase genes but
have distinct functional characteristics. J Biol Chem; 279:38303–38312
Renier C, Faraco JH, Bourgin P, Motley T, Bonaventure P, Rosa F, Mignot E (2007) Genomic and
functional conservation of sedative-hypnotic targets in the zebrafish. Pharmacogen Genomics
17:237–253
24 R. Gerlai
Salas C, Rodríguez F, Vargas JP, Durán E, Torres B (1996) Spatial learning and memory deficits
after telencephalic ablation in goldfish trained in place and turn maze procedures. Behav
Neurosci 110:965–980
Scott EK, Mason L, Arrenberg AB, Ziv L, Gosse NJ, Xiao T, Chi NC, Asakawa K, Kawakami K,
Baier H (2007) Targeting neural circuitry in zebrafish using GAL4 enhancer trapping.
Nat Methods 4:323–326
Sison M, Gerlai R (2010) Associative learning in zebrafish (Danio rerio) in the plus maze. Behav
Brain Res 207:99–104
Sison M, Cawker J, Buske C, Gerlai R (2006) Fishing for genes of vertebrate behavior: zebra fish
as an upcoming model system. Lab Animal 35:33–39
Sivasubbu S, Balciunas D, Davidson AE, Pickart MA, Hermanson SB, Wangensteen KJ,
Wolbrink DC, Ekker SC (2006) Gene-breaking transposon mutagenesis reveals an essential
role for histone H2afza in zebrafish larval development. Mech Dev 123:513–529
Skromne I, Prince VE (2008) Current perspectives in zebrafish reverse genetics: moving forward.
Dev Dyn 237:861–882
Sokolowski MB (2001) Drosophila: genetics meets behaviour. Nat Rev Genet 2:879–890
Speedie N, Gerlai R (2008) Alarm substance induced behavioral responses in zebrafish
(Danio rerio) Behav. Brain Res 188:168–177
Sweatt JD (2010) Mechanisms of memory. 2nd edn, Elsevier, Amsterdam, p 343
Tong C, Li P, Wu NL, Yan Y, Ying QL (2010) Production of p53 gene knockout rats by
homologous recombination in embryonic stem cells. Nature 467:211–213
Tropepe V, Sive HL (2003) Can zebrafish be used as a model to study the neurodevelopmental
causes of autism? Genes Brain Behav 2:268–281
Vargas JP, López JC, Portavella M (2009) What are the functions of fish brain pallium? Brain Res
Bull 79:436–440
Weisberg RB (2009) Overview of generalized anxiety disorder: epidemiology, presentation,
and course. J Clin Psychiatry 70(Suppl 2):4–9
Xia W (2010) Exploring Alzheimer’s disease in zebrafish. J Alzheimer’s Dis 20:981–990
Forward Genetic Approaches
to Understanding Complex Behaviors
Abstract Assigning function to genes has long been a focus of biomedical research.
Even with complete knowledge of the genomic sequences of humans, mice and other
experimental organisms, there is still much to be learned about gene function and
control. Ablation or overexpression of single genes using knockout or transgenic
technologies has provided functional annotation for many genes, but these tech-
nologies do not capture the extensive genetic variation present in existing experi-
mental mouse populations. Researchers have only recently begun to truly appreciate
naturally occurring genetic variation resulting from single nucleotide substitutions,
insertions, deletions, copy number variation, epigenetic changes (DNA methylation,
histone modifications, etc.) and gene expression differences and how this variation
contributes to complex phenotypes. In this chapter, we will discuss the benefits and
limitations of different forward genetic approaches that capture the genetic variation
present in inbred mouse strains and present the utility of these approaches for
mapping QTL that influence complex behavioral phenotypes.
Contents
Animal models, and rodents in particular, have been used extensively to study
behaviors that model human psychiatric diseases. While rats have been particu-
larly well-suited to this research, the mouse has recently emerged as the primary
model for studying genetic and genomic aspects of human disease (Cryan and
Holmes 2005). This is due, in large part, to the availability of a vast array of
genetic and genomic resources for such endeavors in the mouse.
The ability to introduce genes into the mouse (transgenics) and retain tissue-
specific expression patterns as well as the ability to disrupt gene function
(knockouts) represented a significant advance in a field that has now become
known as ‘‘functional genomics.’’ Reverse genetic approaches such as these have
been useful for identification and analysis of genes believed to increase risk for
schizophrenia (Desbonnet et al. 2009), anxiety (Holmes et al. 2003; Lesch et al.
2003), depression (Savitz et al. 2009; Cryan and Slattery 2010) and autism (Sudhof
2008) but are not without limitations. The types of genetic lesions introduced are
often severe, completely knocking out gene function over the course of the life-
span, and may not accurately reflect the genetic basis of many complex human
diseases. The ability to make conditional knockouts that limit loss-of-function to
specific tissues or developmental timepoints has overcome some of these limita-
tions. However, the reverse genetic approach is generally hypothesis-driven,
depending on some prior knowledge regarding a gene’s role in a disease or
pathway. Understandably, the genes interrogated are often those in pathways
that are already targeted by current drug treatments, making the approach inher-
ently biased. More comprehensive phenotyping might result in identification of
gene function not yet attributed to a specific gene. Recently, large-scale reverse
genetic projects have been launched in the United States (knockout mouse
project (KOMP), Texas A&M Institute for Genomic Medicine (TIGM)), Europe
(European Conditional Mouse Mutagenesis (EUCOMM)) and Canada (North
American Conditional Mouse Mutagenesis (NorCOMM)) to systematically
interrogate the role of every gene by producing targeted knockouts and analyzing
the resulting mutants in a variety of phenotypic assays (Collins et al. 2007;
Gailus-Durner et al. 2009). These efforts to catalog gene function in a non-
hypothesis-driven, unbiased manner will surely provide new insights into gene
Forward Genetic Approaches to Understanding Complex Behaviors 27
In the earliest days of mouse genetics, the phenotype was the primary starting
point for genetic analysis. Observations of spontaneous, chemical- or radiation-
induced mutations progressed to identification of linkage groups and, eventually,
to chromosomal assignment (for excellent reviews, see (Lyon 2002; Paigen
2003)). As molecular tools advanced, starting with the identification of genetic
markers and progressing to complete sequencing of the mouse genome, the means
for exact gene localization were realized. Knowledge regarding the organization of
the genome increased along with an appreciation for the complexities therein,
resulting in a renewed interest in the study of complex traits from a systems
biology perspective. Recent years have witnessed a resurgence of forward genetic
approaches that consider naturally occurring genetic and phenotypic variation and
place more emphasis on the genetic complexities that are likely to explain a large
portion of human phenotypic variation.
As opposed to reverse genetics, forward genetic studies start with the mea-
surement or observation of a phenotype followed by mapping of the causative loci
or genes. A major advantage of this approach is that the role of the gene or genes
in a specific biological process is/are proven a priori. In addition, forward genetic
approaches are, by definition, unbiased in terms of gene identification since the
phenotype is the primary level of measurement. The term ‘‘forward genetics’’
applies to any phenotype-driven mapping approach, including the use of standard
inbred strain crosses and haplotype association analysis—both of which result in
the identification of quantitative trait loci (QTL).
In this chapter, we will describe forward genetic approaches for gene identi-
fication and functional analysis, detailing the advantages, disadvantages, successes
and failures of several commonly used forward genetic models.
Early mouse geneticists, including pioneers of the field William Castle and
Clarence Little, utilized visible characteristics such as coat color and tumor sus-
ceptibility to study Mendelian inheritance. These studies eventually led to the
28 L. M. Tarantino and A. F. Eisener-Dorman
recombination frequency and genetic distance between two markers. A LOD score
is calculated to indicate the strength of the association between the phenotype and
genotype. This technique has been refined over the years—Jansen (1993) and Zeng
(1993) developed an algorithm for composite interval mapping (CIM) that allows
for interval mapping in combination with multiple-regression analysis to account
for background QTL. The specifics of QTL mapping algorithms are beyond the
scope of this chapter, but several books are available that provide a thorough
discussion (Broman and Sen 2009; Wu et al. 2010). Linkage and mapping pro-
grams are freely available, including mapmaker QTL (Lander et al. 1987), QTL
Cartographer (Basten et al. 1994, 2002), map manager QTX (Manly et al. 2001)
and R/qtl (Broman et al. 2003) or its graphical user interface, J/qtl (Smith et al.
2009). Most mapping programs were designed to analyze the basic mapping
populations described below, but special attention must be given to populations
frequently used for fine mapping QTL.
Donald Bailey and Benjamin Taylor developed RI lines at the Jackson Laboratory
with the goal of creating a recombinant population for linkage mapping. RI lines
are created by intercrossing the F1 hybrids of two inbred progenitor strains to
generate F2 progeny, which are then sib-mated for at least 20 generations,
resulting in homozygosity at each locus and a mixture of the parental alleles
providing genetic variation necessary for QTL mapping. Each RI line consists of
animals that are genetically identical but distinct from other RI lines in the set
(Fig. 1c) (Bailey 1971).
The first murine RI panel, the CXB line, was constructed by Bailey from a
BALB/cBy 9 C57BL/6By cross. Bailey used the seven lines resulting from the
initial crosses to identify loci associated with histocompatibility factors and coat
color (Taylor 1978). Twelve CXB lines now exist and have been used to map
QTL that influence exploratory activity (Blizard and Bailey 1979; Crabbe et al.
1982; Neiderhiser et al. 1992), circadian rhythms of locomotion (Schwartz and
Zimmerman 1990), avoidance (Neiderhiser et al. 1992) and sleep behaviors (Tafti
et al. 1997). Taylor developed more RI panels, such as the BXH (C57BL/6J 9
C3H/HeJ) and AKXL (AKR/J 9 C57L/J) RI sets, which were used in the study of
murine leukemia virus (Taylor et al. 1971), drug-induced seizures (Taylor 1976)
and lipopolysaccharide response (Watson et al. 1977). There are now RI panels for
crosses between A/J and C57BL/6J (AXB and BXA), B6(Cg)-Tyrc-2 J/J and C3H/
HeJ (BXH) and C57BL/6J and DBA/2J (BXD), as well as RI lines derived from
mice selectively bred for righting response to ethanol—the Long Sleep and Short
Sleep mice, ILS/IbgTejJ and ISS/IbgTejJ (ILSXISS) (http://jaxmice.jax.org).
30 L. M. Tarantino and A. F. Eisener-Dorman
The BXD RI panel is by far the most frequently used, beginning with an
initial set of 25 lines (Taylor et al. 1977) but now consisting of 79 strains (Peirce
et al. 2004) (http://www.genenetwork.org). QTL for alcohol (Cunningham
1995; Phillips et al. 1994, 1995; Rodriguez et al. 1994, 1995) and drug-related
(Alexander et al. 1996; Grisel et al. 1997; Jones et al. 1999; Mogil et al. 1997;
Phillips et al. 1998) behaviors were among the first to be dissected using BXD
lines. More recently, concerted efforts have been made to characterize the full
BXD panel for neurobehavioral phenotypes, particularly those relating to pain,
Forward Genetic Approaches to Understanding Complex Behaviors 31
b Fig. 1 a–h Breeding schemes of various genetic mapping populations. Backcross mice (a) are
generated by crossing two inbred strains to recover F1 mice and then backcrossing the F1 mice to
one of the parental strains, resulting in one copy of a chromosome that has undergone meiotic
recombination and another copy from the inbred strain to which the F1 was backcrossed.
Intercross mice (b) result from crossing F1 mice, resulting in two recombined chromosomes.
Recombinant inbred strains (c) are produced by continuous brother-sister mating among F2s,
resulting in homozygosity at each locus but with a reassortment of alleles from each parental
strain. Advanced intercross lines (d) are also produced by crossing F2s, but inbreeding is avoided
by restricting breeding partners to exclude common parents or grandparents and a large breeding
population is maintained to avoid allelic drift. Congenic strains (e) have a small portion of one
chromosome from a donor strain introgressed onto a recipient background strain, while
chromosome substitution strains (f) have an entire donor chromosome introgressed onto a
recipient background strain (examples of a congenic interval on Chr 9 (e) and a Chr 9 CSS (f) are
shown). Heterogeneous stocks (g) are similar to advanced intercross lines but are derived from
eight different inbred strains to increase genetic diversity. The collaborative cross (h) also has
eight parental strains, but the breeding scheme is similar to recombinant inbred lines to maximize
recombination while breeding each CC line to homozygosity
drug addiction, anxiety, stress and locomotor activity (Philip et al. 2010). One
innovative approach uses brain-specific gene expression data from a microarray
analysis of the BXD panel to map neurobehavioral QTL (Bao et al. 2007).
RI lines offer several advantages for QTL mapping. RI lines are genetic
reference populations—genetically stable over time and infinitely reproducible.
Therefore, each line must be genotyped only once, and that data becomes
available to anyone using the line in the future (Taylor 1978). Comparisons of
phenotypic data can be made across laboratories enabling genetic correlations
and investigation of similarities among phenotypes. Phenotypic data can be
examined retrospectively as marker density increases and more phenotypes are
collected. This process has been automated by Williams and colleagues, who
established the GeneNetwork database that provides tools for RI-QTL mapping
and correlations among more than 1,200 phenotype datasets, including gene
expression (Wang et al. 2003). RI lines are also particularly well-suited for
mapping QTL for phenotypes for which genetic variance is small compared
to environmental variance. This feature of RI lines is derived from the ability to
conduct repeated sampling from individuals with the same genetic background,
resulting in strain means that reduce environmental noise and more accurately
reflect the strain phenotype.
The ability to map QTL for complex traits is dependent upon the availability of
a sufficiently large RI panel that provides enough power to detect QTL of small
effect size (Gora-Maslak et al. 1991). However, the RI strains were developed to
investigate Mendelian traits, and the small number of strains per line has limited
their usefulness for identifying QTL for complex phenotypes. Prior to the devel-
opment of dense sets of genetic markers, the utility of RIs was also limited by the
availability of loci for mapping. The expansion of the BXD line, along with the
development of a more dense set of polymorphic markers, now allows for mapping
of QTL responsible for 10% of the phenotypic variance to approximately 1–2
megabases (Mb) (Peirce et al. 2004).
32 L. M. Tarantino and A. F. Eisener-Dorman
However, even the largest RI sets have several characteristics that mitigate
their usefulness in complex trait analysis. Existing RI panels were produced by
crossing only two inbred strains and from only a limited number of inbred
strains, resulting in limited genetic variability both within and across panels.
Both the advantages and disadvantages of RI lines were considered when
planning the collaborative cross (CC) (Churchill et al. 2004), an experimental
population akin to RI strains but with added genetic variability. The CC is
discussed in more detail below.
The limited power for mapping provided by existing RI lines led to the use of other
types of mapping populations—either for replication of RI-identified QTL or as
alternative crosses for initial QTL identification. The most commonly used QTL
mapping populations are intercross (F2) or backcross (BC) lines produced by
breeding two inbred strains known to differ for the complex trait(s) of interest. The
resulting F1 hybrid progeny are either intercrossed to produce an F2 generation
(Fig. 1b) or backcrossed to one of the parental strains to produce a BC generation
(Fig. 1a). At each locus, an F2 animal will have one of three genotypes: homo-
zygous for either of the parental alleles or heterozygous, possessing one allele
from each of the parental strains. A BC animal has only two genotype possibilities:
heterozygous or homozygous for the allele of the parental strain to which the F1
progeny were backcrossed.
Unlike RI animals, intercross and backcross progeny are genetically hetero-
geneous; each F2 or BC individual within the population is characterized by a
unique pattern of genome-wide recombination. Therefore, each set of animals
must be genotyped and the informativeness of these genotypes is limited to the set
of phenotypes collected for any particular set of animals. When genotyping costs
were prohibitive, selective genotyping of animals from the top and bottom 15% of
the phenotypic distribution was frequently performed using DNA pools (Darvasi
and Soller 1994; Wang and Paterson 1994). The genetic (DNA) contribution of
each individual to the pool was often weighted toward individuals at the pheno-
typic extremes (Taylor et al. 1999, 2001). Individuals in each pool were genotyped
only at loci for which a significant or suggestive QTL were identified, and the
entire mapping population was only genotyped for those loci that proved signifi-
cant. As genotyping costs have decreased, the use of pooling has waned.
The number of markers required to genotype either an F2 or BC is relatively
low based on the limited number of recombinations in these populations.
A polymorphic marker every 40 Mb provides adequate coverage, as each marker
will sweep 20 Mb in each direction. The choice of which type of cross to use is
dependent on the genetic architecture of the phenotype of interest. Using an F2
mapping population is recommended to obtain an overview of the number, loca-
tion and estimated effect sizes of both additive and dominant QTL segregating in
Forward Genetic Approaches to Understanding Complex Behaviors 33
closely-linked, small-effect QTL may fail to be detected if the loci have opposing
phenotypic effects. Consequently, the fine mapping necessary to narrow QTL
intervals and, ultimately, identify quantitative trait genes (QTG) must be carried
out in specialized populations.
Takada et al. produced a CSS panel with MSM/Ms introgressed onto C57BL/6J
(Takada et al. 2008) that has been used to identify QTL for emotionality
(Takahashi et al. 2008), social interactions (Takahashi et al. 2010) and home cage
activity (Nishi et al. 2010). Finally, Gregorova et al. (Gregorova et al. 2008) have
introgressed PWD/Ph onto C57BL/6J and have characterized the lines for blood
chemistry-related phenotypes.
onto the D2 recipient background (D2.B6-Mop2), and the D2 QTL interval was
introgressed onto the B6 recipient background (B6.D2-Mop2) (Ferraro et al. 2005).
Heterozygous D2.B6-Mop2 congenic mice were backcrossed to D2 mice, thereby
fragmenting the 28.8-Mb QTL interval of the congenic into smaller, discrete
intervals of 9.5 and 17.2 Mb in the resulting subcongenic strains (Doyle et al.
2008). Mice from one of these two subcongenic strains exhibited morphine
preferences similar to both the D2.B6-Mop2 congenic and the B6 parental strain
mice, thereby narrowing the Chr 10 QTL interval to a 9.5-Mb region containing 39
genes (Doyle et al. 2008). In another study, the obesity-related QTL Fob3 on Chr
15 was identified in an F2 cross originating from two outbred mouse lines selected
for either high or low body fat (fat and lean lines) (Horvat et al. 2000). One mouse,
selected because it was recombinant within the Fob3 interval, was backcrossed for
10 generations to the Fat line to create two subcongenic strains with the Lean line
Fob3 QTL interval introgressed onto the Fat line recipient background (Stylianou
et al. 2004). Further backcrossing of the Fob3 subcongenic strains to the Fat
line yielded additional recombinants, allowing the isolation of two smaller QTL
(Fob3a and Fob3b) within the original Fob3 QTL interval (Stylianou et al. 2004).
Continued backcrossing of select recombinants within the 22.39-Mb Fob3b interval
generated six additional subcongenic strains with overlapping donor intervals. Four
subcongenic strains were intercrossed with the Fat line to generate four F2 mapping
populations, which identified two closely-linked QTL, Fob3b1 (4.98 Mb) and
Fob3b2 (7.68 Mb), within the Fob3b QTL interval (Prevorsek et al. 2010).
However, QTL effects often disappear during the construction of congenics or
subcongenics. Reasons for this loss of QTL effect could be the removal of the QTL
from a heterogeneous background, thereby eliminating epistatic interactions that
increase QTL effect size. It is also likely that QTL that disappear during congenic
production might actually be groups of two or more smaller effect size QTL that
appear as a single peak in an F2 or BC but do not have sufficient power alone to
result in a statistically significant change in phenotype (Legare et al. 2000; Legare
and Frankel 2000).
Although the ability to restrict genetic heterogeneity to only two strains has
advantages (Cheng et al. 2010), the limited number of recombinations present in
F2 and BC mice also limits the resolution of these mapping resources. AILs are
produced by intercrossing two parental strains to obtain an F2 and then inter-
crossing each successive generation (Fig. 1d). AILs have the advantage of
expanding the genetic map by increasing the number of breakpoints, thereby
increasing the recombination fraction between markers. The increased number of
recombinations in an F10 can reduce the size of a QTL interval by five-fold in
comparison with a similarly-sized population of F2 animals (Darvasi and Soller
1995). However, the increase in resolution comes at the cost of power to detect
Forward Genetic Approaches to Understanding Complex Behaviors 37
QTL. This loss in power is due to the increased number of markers necessary to
account for the reduction in linkage disequilibrium between markers and genetic
drift that results in altered allele frequencies. The latter can be controlled some-
what by choosing an appropriate breeding strategy (Rockman and Kruglyak 2008).
In general, maintaining large population sizes and controlling for inbreeding can
mitigate concerns about power in an AIL.
One of the largest AILs is the LG/J 9 SM/J (LG, SM) AIL produced
by Cheverud and colleagues at Washington University in St. Louis, Missouri
(Norgard et al. 2008). The F9-F10 LG, SM AILs have been used successfully to
identify dozens of QTL for bone-length (Kenney-Hunt et al. 2008; Norgard et al.
2008, 2009). More recently, F34 LG, SM AILs were used to replicate and refine
QTL from previous studies. Norgard et al. were able to replicate almost 80% of
QTL identified in both F2-F3 and F9-F10 populations. However, QTL interval
reduction in the F34 was less than expected compared to previous studies. QTL
intervals ranged from 0.6 to 14 Mb with half of the QTL having confidence
intervals from 2 to 5 Mb. The authors postulated that family structure bias inflated
the QTL peaks and decreased the confidence interval sizes reported in earlier
studies. An alternative explanation is the presence of multiple linked loci at the
QTL peaks—a problem that is encountered with any fine mapping population.
Cheng et al. (2010) took advantage of the power of an F2 and the mapping
resolution of the LG, SM AIL to identify QTL for methamphetamine sensitivity.
QTL were identified for both methamphetamine- and saline-induced locomotor
behavior in a large LG 9 SM F2 and the F34 LG, SM AIL. Both populations were
also combined and analyzed as one large intercross. Cheng et al. found that the
population structure present in AILs must be considered when mapping to reduce
Type I errors. By using a mixed model that considered relatedness of individuals in
the AIL for both mapping and significance threshold determination, the authors were
able to identify several QTL intervals at sub-centimorgan resolution—one of which
contains only a single gene that is now being assessed for its role in methamphet-
amine sensitivity. Samocha et al. (2010) used a similar approach to map QTL for
acoustic startle response, habituation and prepulse inhibition of the startle response.
A large set of F2 identified QTL for multiple behaviors. The population size of the
AIL used in this study did not provide enough power to detect significant QTL on its
own, but combined analysis of both the F2 and AIL populations narrowed QTL
intervals identified initially in the F2. Taken together, these studies indicate that the
approach of using both an F2 and AILs, along with taking into account population
structure, is an effective method for identification and fine-mapping of QTL.
Outbred stocks are closed populations of genetically variable animals that are bred
to maintain maximum heterozygosity (Chia et al. 2005). Dozens of outbred stocks
exist, and like HS mice, they offer the advantage of increased mapping resolution
but suffer some of the same drawbacks—increased sample size and marker density
are required for QTL detection. In addition, outbred stocks (with the exception of
HS lines) do not have the advantage of progenitor allele information to derive the
origin of alleles in the offspring. This limitation introduces difficulties in the use of
these stocks for QTL mapping. Only one example of a behavioral QTL has been
published thus far using outbred stocks. The Rgs2 gene that influences anxiety in
mice was identified using outbred MF1 mice (Yalcin et al. 2004). Yalcin et al.
determined that the haplotype patterns in MF1 mice were very similar to those
found in standard inbred strains. Thus, they were able to use similar techniques for
mapping as those used with HS mice.
Aldinger et al. (2009) recently examined genetic variation and population
structure in CD-1 outbred stocks and determined that CD-1 mice are reasonably
outbred, polymorphic at a significant number of loci and resemble human popu-
lations in terms of complex genetic history. Although CD-1 mice have population
substructure that may affect mapping results, the authors believe this structure
could be exploited and that the CD-1 mice represent a valuable genetic mapping
resource. Williams et al. (2009) used CD-1 mice to confirm the effect of a
duplication of the Glo1 gene on anxiety-related behavior in mice. However, out-
bred mice have not yet been used for genome-wide association mapping of
behavioral traits. Perhaps as genomic and analytical resources are developed, these
untapped resources of genetic variability will become utilized more frequently in
the identification and fine mapping of QTL.
40 L. M. Tarantino and A. F. Eisener-Dorman
In the late 1990s and early 2000s, the discussion in the mouse genetics community
started to focus on the problem of QTG identification; it was determined that
current mouse resources were not suitable for tackling such a difficult problem.
Various standard mapping populations had the features necessary for identifying
complex trait loci, but not one mapping resource had all of the features that were
necessary if such an endeavor was to be successful down to the point of gene
identification. Mouse geneticists began to envision a reference population of mice,
the CC, that would capitalize on the advantages of existing resources by offering
genetic diversity, mapping power and high-resolution. The CC would also provide
a platform for systems genetic studies and modeling of complex networks,
including gene by gene and gene by environment interactions. The CC was to be
derived from eight divergent inbred strains (A/J, C57BL/6J, 129S1/SvImJ, NOD/
LtJ, NZO/HlLt, CAST/EiJ, PWK/PhJ and WSB/EiJ) to maximize genetic diversity
and ensure that the resulting population would provide the phenotypic diversity
necessary to study any trait of interest. Like the AIL and HS lines, the CC would
be subjected to multiple generations of breeding to increase meiotic recombination
and provide the necessary mapping resolution for fine mapping QTL (Fig. 1h).
However, the CC would be inbred and, upon completion, would provide a fully-
genotyped reference population that could be provided to individual investigators.
Finally, the CC would be sufficiently large (*1,000 lines) to provide the power
necessary to map QTL of relatively small-effect size.
The initial and largest crosses to produce the CC were begun in 2005 at the Oak
Ridge National Laboratory (ORNL). Separate, smaller breeding populations were
located in Tel Aviv, Israel and Western Australia. In late 2008, the Department of
Energy announced that the ORNL mouse genetics program was being phased out,
and the ORNL CC population was transferred to the University of North Carolina
(UNC). The Tel Aviv population has recently joined the ORNL lines at UNC.
Several CC lines have already reached the inbred state due to an acceleration of the
process using marker-assisted inbreeding. At least 100 lines will be completed by
the end of 2012, and additional lines will follow over the next several years
(personal communication, Darla Miller).
It is likely that the final population of the CC will be closer to 500 rather than
1,000 lines, as first proposed. Simulation studies have shown that 500 lines provide
both adequate power and fine resolution for QTL mapping (Broman 2005; Valdar
et al. 2006a). A multi-stage strategy has also been proposed using an initial
mapping panel of 100 strains followed by a second set of 100 strains that have
informative allele combinations and recombination events based on QTL locali-
zation from the first stage (Churchill et al. 2004). This strategy will make the
CC more accessible for smaller laboratories. The completion of fully-genotyped
CC-RI lines would also enable the production of many more CC recombinant
inbred intercrosses (CC-RIX) produced by crossing different CC lines. Each
CC-RIX mouse will inherit a chromosome from each CC parent and, thus,
Forward Genetic Approaches to Understanding Complex Behaviors 41
Inbred strain surveys are an important initial step in understanding the genetic and
phenotypic architecture of complex traits. Until recently, however, strain surveys
had become less frequently used, meaning that researchers had a limited set of data
on which to base experiments. In 2000, the Mouse Phenome Project was started as
an effort to collect phenotypic data from a core set of inbred strains. These data
would be stored in a central location, be publicly available on the Mouse Phenome
Database (MPD; http://phenome.jax.org/ and Bogue et al. (2007)) and would offer
investigators a wide range of phenotypes with which to inform research decisions
for complex trait analysis (Bogue 2003; Paigen and Eppig 2000).
The effort to collect phenotype data for inbred strains coincided with efforts to
increase the number of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) identified in
inbred mouse lines and construct a SNP haplotype map of the mouse similar to the
HapMap being constructed in humans (2003) (http://hapmap.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/).
This convergence of phenotype data and SNP genotype data presented an obvious
opportunity to take advantage of both the phenotypic and genotypic diversity
among inbred strains. In the context of genetic mapping, inbred strains offer
several advantages over existing mapping populations. Inbred strains are, of
course, a reference population of mice enabling collection and storage of pheno-
typic and genetic data that can be utilized repeatedly. In addition, inbred strains,
like RIs, allow for repeated sampling within a strain to reduce environmental
variance. Inbred strains also have increased genetic and phenotypic diversity in
comparison to RI or standard F2 or BC populations, as well as a dense genetic map
due to increased recombination.
In 2001, Grupe et al. used existing inbred strain phenotype data from the MPD
and over 3,000 SNPs identified in their laboratory and at the Whitehead Institute
(Lindblad-Toh et al. 2000) to conduct ‘‘in silico QTL mapping’’ in mouse. The
basic approach is straightforward—as described previously, inbred strains are
genetically identical within a strain but genetically diverse across strains. There-
fore, associating SNP genotypes across the genome with strain phenotypes, similar
to an RI-QTL study, can be expected to yield chromosomal regions that are
associated with the trait of interest. The in silico mapping technique was successful
in identifying QTL that overlapped with previously published reports using
standard crosses (Grupe et al. 2001). Nevertheless, subsequent commentary on the
method exposed several weaknesses, including lack of power, insufficient genetic
variability and failure to control for both Type I and II errors (Chesler et al. 2001;
Darvasi 2001). Darvasi (2001) estimated that between 40 and 150 strains would be
necessary to identify a QTL affecting a quantitative trait with a heritability of 50%,
42 L. M. Tarantino and A. F. Eisener-Dorman
and the number of strains used in the Grupe study ranged from 4 to 8. However,
Grupe et al. argued that this estimate was based on power calculations using
Lander and Schork significance levels based on an infinite density of genetic
markers and was not applicable to their study.
Regardless, it was generally agreed that in silico mapping, more recently
termed haplotype association mapping (HAM), might be a useful tool for identi-
fication of QTL. Three factors were necessary for its successful implementation:
dense SNP coverage across the genome for more than just a few strains, phenotype
data for multiple strains and appropriate analysis tools for genotype/phenotype
associations. Pletcher et al. (2004) addressed two of these problems by identifying
over 10,000 SNPs in 48 strains and developing a HAM algorithm called SNPster
(http://snpster.gnf.org) to perform genotype/phenotype associations across the
genome using a three-SNP sliding haplotype window. Using this method, the
authors were able to identify QTL peaks for several Mendelian traits, including
coat color and retinal degeneration. They were also able to replicate previously
identified QTL for more complex traits such as saccharin preference, high-density
lipoprotein (HDL) levels and gallstone formation. In addition, most map locations
spanned intervals of 1 Mb or less—a significant improvement over standard F2 or
BC mapping.
However, several hurdles still remained for HAM. As more dense SNP panels
were genotyped and more was known about the genetic structure of the inbred
strains, it became clear that an extensive family structure existed that could lead to
spurious associations. In addition, only a limited number of strains existed for which
there was both phenotype and genotype data, resulting in limited power to detect
QTL for complex phenotypes. McClurg et al. (2007) attempted to address family
structure by calculating a genetic similarity matrix along with a weighted bootstrap
method to decrease the significance of nonspecific associations. In addition, gene
expression data were used to optimize the power of the SNPster algorithm to
identify cis-acting expression QTL that are considered, by some, to be a highly-
enriched set of true positives with a low false-positive rate (Chesler et al. 2006).
Additional analysis programs have been developed to perform association mapping
in inbred strains, including Efficient Mixed-Model Association (EMMA; (Kang
et al. 2008)) and hmmSNP, which uses a hidden Markov model (HMM)-based
algorithm (Tsaih and Korstanje 2009). Each program uses a different algorithm and
has a slightly different way of dealing with population structure. However, most
programs give qualitatively similar results (unpublished data, Tim Wiltshire).
The full power of HAM has not yet been realized, as the resources for
performing such analyzes are still being compiled. The quantity of SNPs in mice
now number in the millions across more than 100 inbred strains. The number of
phenotypes in the mouse phenome database continue to grow, with over 2,000
phenotypes stored and an average of 19 mouse strains tested per phenotype.
However, the HAM technique has been used successfully to narrow down previ-
ously identified QTL intervals (Burgess-Herbert et al. 2009; Cervino et al. 2005;
Harrill et al. 2009; Park et al. 2003; Wang et al. 2005) and to identify new QTL
(Bopp et al. 2010; Liao et al. 2004; Liu et al. 2006; Pletcher et al. 2004).
Forward Genetic Approaches to Understanding Complex Behaviors 43
Few HAM studies have been published for behavior. Webb et al. conducted a
whole genome association study for prepulse inhibition and identified QTL on
Chrs 1 and 13 (Webb et al. 2009) that overlap with genes that alter PPI when
knocked out or regions of the human genome that contain genes that have been
implicated in schizophrenia. Miller et al. (2010) measured tail suspension, an
animal model of behavioral despair or depression, in 33 inbred strains of mice and
identified four QTLs, including one that overlapped a human region that contains a
locus associated with major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder.
Initial reports of QTL mapping using HAM resulted in concern about the fate of
standard QTL studies (Chesler et al. 2001; Darvasi 2001). However, these con-
cerns have not been realized. In general, HAM is viewed as one step in QTL
identification that can be used for initial identification of QTL regions that must
then be replicated using standard approaches. More commonly, HAM can be used
to both replicate and narrow QTL that were identified using standard approaches
(Burgess-Herbert et al. 2008, 2009; DiPetrillo et al. 2005). This technique is
particularly effective when using strains that are genetically similar. Although
genetic similarity may result in phenotypic similarity, QTL can be detected even
when using parental strains that are phenotypically similar (Bailey et al. 2008;
Eisener-Dorman et al. 2010). This is due to transgressive segregation, the
reshuffling of alleles that occurs when two strains are crossed (Rieseberg et al. 1999).
QTL identified in these crosses may be fine mapped to relatively small genomic
regions using haplotype comparisons since regions of shared haplotype can be
eliminated from consideration (Bailey et al. 2008; Eisener-Dorman et al. 2010).
As more phenotype data are collected for larger numbers of inbred strains and
more information is gathered on the genomic structure of the laboratory mouse,
HAM analysis will become an even more vital tool with which to identify complex
disease genes.
7 Mutagenesis
In the late 1990s, with QTL accumulating in the literature and very few identified
genes, the scientific community was looking for alternative approaches to iden-
tification of complex disease genes. Mutagenesis, the induction of mutations by
chemicals or radiation, was particularly attractive. Mutagenesis in the mouse was
not new—radiation-induced mutagenesis experiments to determine the genetic
effects of radiation on mammals had been ongoing at the Department of Energy’s
Oak Ridge National Laboratory (ORNL) since just after World War II. The use of
N-ethyl-N-nitrosourea (ENU) as a chemical mutagen in mice began in 1978 when
it was shown to have a mutation rate 12 times higher in male spermatogonia than
that of X-rays (Justice 2004).
ENU is an alkylating agent that causes single base pair mutations—usually
A–T to T–A transversions or A–T to G–C transitions. ENU induces random
heritable mutations at a rate of approximately 1.4 9 10-6 per nucleotide site
44 L. M. Tarantino and A. F. Eisener-Dorman
(Takahasi et al. 2007), although this rate varies depending on the protocol and the
species or inbred strain background. Because of the random nature of mutations
resulting from ENU, it was proposed as an unbiased way to induce a single
mutation that would affect a complex phenotype. Thus, unlike transgenic and
knockout mouse technologies, which target a specific gene of interest, a strength of
the ENU mutagenesis approach is that it is not hypothesis-driven and is not limited
to the study of known genes previously associated with specific biological path-
ways. Downstream analysis for ENU studies included mapping of the causative
mutation followed by identification of the genetic lesion. The presence of only one
causative mutation was anticipated to render the gene easier to identify.
In 1997, several large-scale ENU mutagenesis centers were set up in the UK
and Germany to produce and screen ENU-generated mutants using batteries of
phenotypic tests, including dysmorphologies, behavior, neurological abnormali-
ties, immunology, clinical chemistry and hearing (Hrabe de Angelis et al. 2000;
Nolan et al. 2000). In 2000, the NIH awarded grant funding to three large-scale
mutagenesis centers in the United States at the University of Tennessee at
Memphis, Northwestern University and the Jackson Laboratory with a mandate on
production, identification and dissemination of ENU-induced mutants displaying
alterations in nervous system function and behavior (collectively called the
Neuromice.org Consortium). Additional ENU mutagenesis centers were formed
outside the auspices of NIH, both small and large-scale, in academia and industry
(for a complete list see (Cordes 2005)).
The mutagenesis centers quickly began producing hundreds of mutants for
dozens of disease-related domains. However, it quickly became obvious that
identification of the causative mutation would not be as straightforward as hoped.
There have been many successes in gene identification for ENU-induced mutants
in the areas of immunology (Hoebe and Beutler 2008; Sandberg et al. 2005; Tabeta
et al. 2006; Theodoratos et al. 2010), development (Herron et al. 2002; Garcia–
Garcia et al. 2005; Stottmann et al. 2009) and metabolism (Lloyd et al. 2005, 2006,
2010; Wilkes et al. 2009). ENU-disrupted genes responsible for neurological traits
that might be considered less susceptible to environmental fluctuations have also
been successfully identified in the areas of deafness (Grillet et al. 2009; Mackenzie
et al. 2009; Parker et al. 2010; Schwander et al. 2009a, b), ataxia (Sharkey et al.
2009; Swanson et al. 2010; Xie et al. 2010) and epilepsy (Frankel et al. 2009;
Tokuda et al. 2011). Mapping mutants for more complex behavioral phenotypes,
on the other hand, has proven problematic.
The basic steps in ENU mutagenesis (Fig. 2) include induction of mutations by
injection of the mutagen in male mice. These G0 mice are then bred with wildtype
females, and the offspring of that cross, the G1, can be screened for dominant
mutations. Further breeding of the G1 males to wildtype females produces G2
females that can be bred back to the G1 male. The resulting G3 animals will carry
recessive mutations at a rate of one per eight G3s. Once an outlier is identified, the
standard course of action is to prove heritability by crossing the animal back
to a wildtype, produce F1 mice and then intercross to recover the mutation in the
F2 progeny—one-quarter of which should exhibit a fully-penetrant, recessive
Forward Genetic Approaches to Understanding Complex Behaviors 45
ENU
G0: . .
random point
mutations
G1:
dominant
. . .
mutations
detected
G2:
dominant
. .
mutations
detected
G3:
recessive . . . .
mutations
detected
Fig. 2 Standard ENU mutagenesis. The standard ENU mutagenesis scheme includes injecting
male mice (G0) with ENU. Doses vary depending upon strain, but generally, three weekly
injections of 85–100 mg/kg ENU works well in B6 mice. ENU-treated males become infertile
following treatment but many will regain fertility after 10–12 weeks. Loss of fertility is often
used as an indicator that the ENU was effective. After regaining fertility, ENU-treated males are
bred to wildtype females of the same strain or a different strain. G1 animals produced from this
cross can be tested for dominant mutations. G1 mice will inherit a mutagenized genome from the
father and the mutations will differ between G1s. To recover recessive mutations, the G1 females
are crossed back to the G1 father to generate G2s. Two copies of any mutations that the father and
daughter share will be recovered in the G3 at a rate of one mutation per eight G3s. For this reason,
G3 pedigrees are often screened for phenotypes in order to recover at least one or two affected
animals with which to propagate the new mutant colony
One way to deal with a non-robust phenotype is to cross the mutant to a number
of inbred strains in order to find a heterogeneous background that allows for the
recovery of the mutant phenotype in the mapping cross, but this increases mapping
costs. Alternatively, sufficient SNP data are now available that allow mapping
to strains that are closely related to the background strain and might reduce
the number of interacting QTL (Bailey et al. 2008; Eisener-Dorman et al. 2010;
Xia et al. 2010). The mutant phenotype might also be preserved by limiting the
amount of the mapping strain background by backcrossing the F1 animals to a
mutant. This strategy also increases the number of animals in the mapping cross
that carry the causative mutation. Finally, a contemporaneous population of mice
not carrying the mutation can be produced, phenotyped and genotyped to provide a
control for both choosing outliers in the mutant cross and identifying background
QTL.
Even with these strategies, however, identification of genes in ENU mutants
that exhibit behavioral anomalies has been painfully slow, even when the phe-
notype appears robust. Many mutants with abnormal behavioral profiles have been
reported (Rastan et al. 2004; Reijmers et al. 2006; Hamre et al. 2007; Mathews
et al. 2009), yet gene identification for ENU behavioral mutants has been slow to
materialize.
Recent publications, however, indicate that ENU-induced behavioral mutants
are beginning to yield genes. In 2007, Keays et al. identified an ENU-induced
mutation in the guanosine triphosphate (GTP) binding pocket of alpha-1 tubulin
(Tuba1). Tuba1 mice exhibited hyperactive behavior but also had a secondary
correlated phenotype—body weight. The presence of a secondary phenotype that
was more stable and reproducible than the hyperactivity phenotype aided in fine
mapping and identifying the gene. Speca et al. (2010) recently reported on the
identification of an ENU-induced nonsense mutation in the Unc-79 gene in a
mouse mutant (Lightweight) initially identified in a screen for animals with
enhanced locomotor activity. The screen was conducted on a sensitized genetic
background (mice heterozygous null for dopamine transporter), but the Light-
weight mutation acted independently of this background. As the name implies, this
mutant also had a correlated phenotype of low body weight. Peaks for both weight
and locomotor activity overlapped, and the use of both phenotypes contributed to
the identification of the causative ENU-induced mutation. Finally, an ENU-
generated missense mutation in the Grin1 gene was identified in a mouse mutant
that displayed increased spontaneous locomotor activity (Furuse et al. 2010). It
should be noted that all three of these mutants were identified in screens for
dominant ENU mutations, and all were initially generated on a mixed genetic
background (i.e. ENU mutagenized males were crossed to females of a different
inbred strain). These results suggest that dominant mutations are ultimately easier
to identify than recessive mutations. Furthermore, using a mixed genetic back-
ground in the primary ENU screen may increase the chance of recovering mutant
mice in the mapping cross, thereby hastening identification of the mutated gene.
The goals of the NIH-funded ENU centers included generation and distribution
of mutagenized mice with the idea that individual researchers could follow up on
Forward Genetic Approaches to Understanding Complex Behaviors 47
phenotypes of interest and map the mutated genes. Although gene identification of
ENU behavioral mutants has been slow, improvements in next-generation
sequencing technologies and decreased costs may accelerate the process. Deep
sequencing of ENU-generated mutants, in combination with or independent of
mapping information, could yield the causative mutations. Several large libraries
of cryopreserved sperm and/or embryos from G1 mice are also available at RIKEN
(Yoshiki et al. 2009), Harwell (Glenister and Thornton 2000), the Australian
Phenomics Network and the German ENU mutagenesis center. As sequencing
prices decrease, these libraries could be screened to identify mutations genome-
wide. Cryo-recovery and phenotyping of mice with mutations in specific genes
would complement efforts such as the KOMP.
As knowledge of the genomic sequence and organization of both the human and
mouse genomes increases, an appreciation for the complexities that will likely
explain the genetic components of complex behaviors has become more apparent.
Single-gene, reverse genetic approaches like knockouts and transgenics still have
48 L. M. Tarantino and A. F. Eisener-Dorman
References
Abiola O, Angel JM, Avner P, Bachmanov AA, Belknap JK, Bennett B, Blankenhorn EP,
Blizard DA, Bolivar V, Brockmann GA, Buck KJ, Bureau JF, Casley WL, Chesler EJ,
Cheverud JM, Churchill GA, Cook M, Crabbe JC, Crusio WE, Darvasi A, de Haan G,
Dermant P, Doerge RW, Elliot RW, Farber CR, Flaherty L, Flint J, Gershenfeld H, Gibson JP,
Gu J, Gu W, Himmelbauer H, Hitzemann R, Hsu HC, Hunter K, Iraqi FF, Jansen RC,
Johnson TE, Jones BC, Kempermann G, Lammert F, Lu L, Manly KF, Matthews DB,
Medrano JF, Mehrabian M, Mittlemann G, Mock BA, Mogil JS, Montagutelli X, Morahan G,
Mountz JD, Nagase H, Nowakowski RS, O’Hara BF, Osadchuk AV, Paigen B, Palmer AA,
Peirce JL, Pomp D, Rosemann M, Rosen GD, Schalkwyk LC, Seltzer Z, Settle S, Shimomura K,
Shou S, Sikela JM, Siracusa LD, Spearow JL, Teuscher C, Threadgill DW, Toth LA, Toye AA,
Vadasz C, Van Zant G, Wakeland E, Williams RW, Zhang HG, Zou F (2003) The nature and
identification of quantitative trait loci: a community’s view. Nat Rev Genet 4:911–916
Aldinger KA, Sokoloff G, Rosenberg DM, Palmer AA, Millen KJ (2009) Genetic variation and
population substructure in outbred CD-1 mice: implications for genome-wide association
studies. PLoS One 4:e4729
Forward Genetic Approaches to Understanding Complex Behaviors 49
Alexander RC, Wright R, Freed W (1996) Quantitative trait loci contributing to phencyclidine-
induced and amphetamine-induced locomotor behavior in inbred mice. Neuropsychophar-
macology 15:484–490
Atkins JKBAL (2001) The replicability of QTLs for murine alcohol preference drinking behavior
across eight independent studies. Mamm Genome 12: 893–899
Bailey DW (1971) Recombinant-inbred strains. An aid to finding identity, linkage, and function
of histocompatibility and other genes. Transplantation 11:325–327
Bailey JS, Grabowski-Boase L, Steffy BM, Wiltshire T, Churchill GA, Tarantino LM (2008)
Identification of QTL for locomotor activation and anxiety using closely-related inbred
strains. Genes Brain Behav 7:761–769
Bao L, Peirce JL, Zhou M, Li H, Goldowitz D, Williams RW, Lu L, Cui Y (2007) An integrative
genomics strategy for systematic characterization of genetic loci modulating phenotypes.
Hum Mol Genet 16:1381–1390
Basten CJ, Weir BS, Zeng Z-B (1994) Zmap—a QTL cartographer. In: Smith C, Gavora JS,
Benkel B, Chesnais J, Fairfull W, Gibson JP, Kennedy BW, Burnside EB (eds) 5th world
congress on genetics applied to livestock production: computing strategies and software.
Organizing Committee, 5th world congress on genetics applied to livestock production,
Guelph, Ontario, Canada, pp 65–66
Basten CJ, Weir BS, Zeng Z-B (2002) QTL Cartographer, version 1.16. Department of Statistics,
North Carolina State University, Raleigh, NC
Belknap JK (2003) Chromosome substitution strains: some quantitative considerations for
genome scans and fine mapping. Mamm Genome 14:723–732
Berrettini WH, Ferraro TN, Alexander RC, Buchberg AM, Vogel WH (1994) Quantitative trait
loci mapping of three loci controlling morphine preference using inbred mouse strains. Nat
Genet 7:54–58
Blizard DA, Bailey DW (1979) Genetic correlation between open-field activity and defecation:
analysis with the CXB recombinant-inbred strains. Behav Genet 9:349–357
Bogue M (2003) Mouse phenome project: understanding human biology through mouse genetics
and genomics. J Appl Physiol 95:1335–1337
Bogue MA, Grubb SC, Maddatu TP, Bult CJ (2007) Mouse phenome database (MPD). Nucleic
Acids Res 35:D643–D649
Bopp SE, Ramachandran V, Henson K, Luzader A, Lindstrom M, Spooner M, Steffy BM,
Suzuki O, Janse C, Waters AP, Zhou Y, Wiltshire T, Winzeler EA (2010) Genome wide
analysis of inbred mouse lines identifies a locus containing Ppar-gamma as contributing to
enhanced malaria survival. PLoS One 5: e10903
Boyle AE, Gill KJ (2008) Confirmation of provisional quantitative trait loci for voluntary alcohol
consumption: genetic analysis in chromosome substitution strains and F2 crosses derived
from A/J and C57BL/6J progenitors. Pharmacogenet Genomics 18:1071–1082
Boyle AE, Gill KJ (2009) A verification of previously identified QTLs for cocaine-induced
activation using a panel of B6.A chromosome substitution strains (CSS) and A/J 9 C57Bl/6J
F2 mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 207(2):325–334
Broman KW (2005) The genomes of recombinant inbred lines. Genetics 169:1133–1146
Broman KW, Sen S (2009) A guide to QTL mapping with R/qtl. Springer, New York
Broman KW, Wu H, Sen S, Churchill GA (2003) R/qtl: QTL mapping in experimental crosses.
Bioinformatics 19:889–890
Buchner DA, Burrage LC, Hill AE, Yazbek SN, O’Brien WE, Croniger CM, Nadeau JH (2008)
Resistance to diet-induced obesity in mice with a single substituted chromosome. Physiol
Genomics 35:116–122
Burgess-Herbert SL, Cox A, Tsaih SW, Paigen B (2008) Practical applications of the
bioinformatics toolbox for narrowing quantitative trait loci. Genetics 180:2227–2235
Burgess-Herbert SL, Tsaih SW, Stylianou IM, Walsh K, Cox AJ, Paigen B (2009) An
experimental assessment of in silico haplotype association mapping in laboratory mice. BMC
Genet 10:81
50 L. M. Tarantino and A. F. Eisener-Dorman
Caldarone B, Saavedra C, Tartaglia K, Wehner JM, Dudek BC, Flaherty L (1997) Quantitative
trait loci analysis affecting contextual conditioning in mice. Nat Genet 17:335–337
Cervino AC, Li G, Edwards S, Zhu J, Laurie C, Tokiwa G, Lum PY, Wang S, Castellani LW,
Lusis AJ, Carlson S, Sachs AB, Schadt EE (2005) Integrating QTL and high-density SNP
analyses in mice to identify Insig2 as a susceptibility gene for plasma cholesterol levels.
Genomics 86:505–517
Cheng R, Lim JE, Samocha KE, Sokoloff G, Abney M, Skol AD, Palmer AA (2010) Genome-
wide association studies and the problem of relatedness among advanced intercross lines and
other highly recombinant populations. Genetics 185:1033–1044
Chesler EJ, Bystrykh L, De Haan G, Cooke MP, Su AI, Manly KF, Williams RW (2006) Reply to
normalization procedures and detection of linkage signal in genetical-genomics experiments.
Nat Genet 38:856–858
Chesler EJ, Rodriguez-Zas SL, Mogil JS (2001) In silico mapping of mouse quantitative trait loci.
Science 294:2423
Chia R, Achilli F, Festing MF, Fisher EM (2005) The origins and uses of mouse outbred stocks.
Nat Genet 37:1181–1186
Churchill GA, Airey DC, Allayee H, Angel JM, Attie AD, Beatty J, Beavis WD, Belknap JK,
Bennett B, Berrettini W, Bleich A, Bogue M, Broman KW, Buck KJ, Buckler E, Burmeister M,
Chesler EJ, Cheverud JM, Clapcote S, Cook MN, Cox RD, Crabbe JC, Crusio WE, Darvasi A,
Deschepper CF, Doerge RW, Farber CR, Forejt J, Gaile D, Garlow SJ, Geiger H, Gershenfeld H,
Gordon T, Gu J, Gu W, de Haan G, Hayes NL, Heller C, Himmelbauer H, Hitzemann R,
Hunter K, Hsu HC, Iraqi FA, Ivandic B, Jacob HJ, Jansen RC, Jepsen KJ, Johnson DK,
Johnson TE, Kempermann G, Kendziorski C, Kotb M, Kooy RF, Llamas B, Lammert F,
Lassalle JM, Lowenstein PR, Lu L, Lusis A, Manly KF, Marcucio R, Matthews D, Medrano JF,
Miller DR, Mittleman G, Mock BA, Mogil JS, Montagutelli X, Morahan G, Morris DG, Mott R,
Nadeau JH, Nagase H, Nowakowski RS, O’Hara BF, Osadchuk AV, Page GP, Paigen B,
Paigen K, Palmer AA, Pan HJ, Peltonen-Palotie L, Peirce J, Pomp D, Pravenec M, Prows DR,
Qi Z, Reeves RH, Roder J, Rosen GD, Schadt EE, Schalkwyk LC, Seltzer Z, Shimomura K,
Shou S, Sillanpaa MJ, Siracusa LD, Snoeck HW, Spearow JL, Svenson K, Tarantino LM,
Threadgill D, Toth LA, Valdar W, de Villena FP, Warden C, Whatley S, Williams RW,
Wiltshire T, Yi N, Zhang D, Zhang M, Zou F (2004) The collaborative cross, a community
resource for the genetic analysis of complex traits. Nat Genet 36:1133–1137
Churchill GA, Doerge RW (1994) Empirical threshold values for quantitative trait mapping.
Genetics 138:963–971
Collins FS, Rossant J, Wurst W (2007) A mouse for all reasons. Cell 128:9–13
Cordes SP (2005) N-ethyl-N-nitrosourea mutagenesis: boarding the mouse mutant express.
Microbiol Mol Biol Rev 69:426–439
Crabbe JC, Rigter H, Kerbusch S (1982) Analysis of behavioural responses to an ACTH analog in
CXB/By recombinant inbred mice. Behav Brain Res 4:289–314
Crow JF (2002) C. C. Little, cancer and inbred mice. Genetics 161:1357–1361
Cryan JF, Holmes A (2005) The ascent of mouse: advances in modelling human depression and
anxiety. Nat Rev Drug Discov 4:775–790
Cryan JF, Slattery DA (2010) GABAB receptors and depression. Current status. Adv Pharmacol
58:427–451
Cunningham CL (1995) Localization of genes influencing ethanol-induced conditioned place
preference and locomotor activity in BXD recombinant inbred mice. Psychopharmacology
(Berl) 120:28–41
Darvasi A (1998) Experimental strategies for the genetic dissection of complex traits in animal
models. Nat Genet 18:19–24
Darvasi A (2001) In silico mapping of mouse quantitative trait loci. Science 294:2423
Darvasi A, Soller M (1994) Selective DNA pooling for determination of linkage between a
molecular marker and a quantitative trait locus. Genetics 138:1365–1373
Darvasi A, Soller M (1995) Advanced intercross lines, an experimental population for fine
genetic mapping. Genetics 141:1199–1207
Forward Genetic Approaches to Understanding Complex Behaviors 51
stereociliary protein, disrupt hair cell function in mice and cause progressive hearing loss in
humans. Am J Hum Genet 85:328–337
Grisel JE, Belknap JK, O’Toole LA, Helms ML, Wenger CD, Crabbe JC (1997) Quantitative trait
loci affecting methamphetamine responses in BXD recombinant inbred mouse strains.
J Neurosci 17:745–754
Grupe A, Germer S, Usuka J, Aud D, Belknap JK, Klein RF, Ahluwalia MK, Higuchi R, Peltz G
(2001) In silico mapping of complex disease-related traits in mice. Science 292:1915–1918
Hamilton BA, Frankel WN (2001) Of mice and genome sequence. Cell 107:13–16
Hamre KM, Goldowitz D, Wilkinson S, Matthews DB (2007) Screening for ENU-induced
mutations in mice that result in aberrant ethanol-related phenotypes. Behav Neurosci 121:
665–678
Harrill AH, Watkins PB, Su S, Ross PK, Harbourt DE, Stylianou IM, Boorman GA, Russo MW,
Sackler RS, Harris SC, Smith PC, Tennant R, Bogue M, Paigen K, Harris C, Contractor T,
Wiltshire T, Rusyn I, Threadgill DW (2009) Mouse population-guided resequencing reveals
that variants in CD44 contribute to acetaminophen-induced liver injury in humans. Genome
Res 19:1507–1515
Henderson ND, Turri MG, DeFries JC, Flint J (2004) QTL analysis of multiple behavioral
measures of anxiety in mice. Behav Genet 34:267–293
Herron BJ, Lu W, Rao C, Liu S, Peters H, Bronson RT, Justice MJ, McDonald JD, Beier DR
(2002) Efficient generation and mapping of recessive developmental mutations using ENU
mutagenesis. Nat Genet 30:185–189
Hitzemann B, Dains K, Kanes S, Hitzemann R (1994) Further studies on the relationship between
dopamine cell density and haloperidol-induced catalepsy. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 271:969–976
Hitzemann R, Cipp L, Demarest K, Mahjubi E, McCaughran J Jr (1998) Genetics of ethanol-
induced locomotor activation: detection of QTLs in a C57BL/6J 9 DBA/2J F2 intercross.
Mamm Genome 9:956–962
Hitzemann R, Dains K, Bier-Langing CM, Zahniser NR (1991) On the selection of mice for
haloperidol response and non-response. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 103:244–250
Hoebe K, Beutler B (2008) Forward genetic analysis of TLR-signaling pathways: an evaluation.
Adv Drug Deliv Rev 60:824–829
Holmes A, Murphy DL, Crawley JN (2003) Abnormal behavioral phenotypes of serotonin
transporter knockout mice: parallels with human anxiety and depression. Biol Psychiatry
54:953–959
Horvat S, Bunger L, Falconer VM, Mackay P, Law A, Bulfield G, Keightley PD (2000) Mapping
of obesity QTLs in a cross between mouse lines divergently selected on fat content. Mamm
Genome 11:2–7
Hrabe de Angelis MH, Flaswinkel H, Fuchs H, Rathkolb B, Soewarto D, Marschall S, Heffner S,
Pargent W, Wuensch K, Jung M, Reis A, Richter T, Alessandrini F, Jakob T, Fuchs E,
Kolb H, Kremmer E, Schaeble K, Rollinski B, Roscher A, Peters C, Meitinger T, Strom T,
Steckler T, Holsboer F, Klopstock T, Gekeler F, Schindewolf C, Jung T, Avraham K,
Behrendt H, Ring J, Zimmer A, Schughart K, Pfeffer K, Wolf E, Balling R (2000) Genome-
wide, large-scale production of mutant mice by ENU mutagenesis. Nat Genet 25:444–447
Iancu OD, Darakjian P, Walter NA, Malmanger B, Oberbeck D, Belknap J, McWeeney S,
Hitzemann R (2010) Genetic diversity and striatal gene networks: focus on the heterogeneous
stock-collaborative cross (HS-CC) mouse. BMC Genomics 11:585
International HapMap Consortium (2003) The international HapMap project. Nature 426:789–796
Jansen RC (1993) Interval mapping of multiple quantitative trait loci. Genetics 135:205–211
Jones BC, Tarantino LM, Rodriguez LA, Reed CL, McClearn GE, Plomin R, Erwin VG (1999)
Quantitative-trait loci analysis of cocaine-related behaviours and neurochemistry. Pharma-
cogenetics 9:607–617
Justice MJ (2004) From the atomic age to the genome project. Genetica 122:3–7
Kang HM, Zaitlen NA, Wade CM, Kirby A, Heckerman D, Daly MJ, Eskin E (2008) Efficient
control of population structure in model organism association mapping. Genetics 178:
1709–1723
Forward Genetic Approaches to Understanding Complex Behaviors 53
Keays DA, Tian G, Poirier K, Huang GJ, Siebold C, Cleak J, Oliver PL, Fray M, Harvey RJ,
Molnar Z, Pinon MC, Dear N, Valdar W, Brown SD, Davies KE, Rawlins JN, Cowan NJ,
Nolan P, Chelly J, Flint J (2007) Mutations in alpha-tubulin cause abnormal neuronal
migration in mice and lissencephaly in humans. Cell 128:45–57
Kelly MA, Low MJ, Phillips TJ, Wakeland EK, Yanagisawa M (2003) The mapping of
quantitative trait loci underlying strain differences in locomotor activity between 129S6 and
C57BL/6J mice. Mamm Genome 14:692–702
Kenney-Hunt JP, Wang B, Norgard EA, Fawcett G, Falk D, Pletscher LS, Jarvis JP, Roseman C,
Wolf J, Cheverud JM (2008) Pleiotropic patterns of quantitative trait loci for 70 murine
skeletal traits. Genetics 178:2275–2288
Kile BT, Hilton DJ (2005) The art and design of genetic screens: mouse. Nat Rev Genet
6:557–567
Kono DH, Park MS, Theofilopoulos AN (2003) Genetic complementation in female
(BXSB 9 NZW) F2 mice. J Immunol 171:6442–6447
Koyner J, Demarest K, McCaughran J Jr, Cipp L, Hitzemann R (2000) Identification and time
dependence of quantitative trait loci for basal locomotor activity in the BXD recombinant
inbred series and a B6D2 F2 intercross. Behav Genet 30:159–170
Lander E, Kruglyak L (1995) Genetic dissection of complex traits: guidelines for interpreting and
reporting linkage results. Nat Genet 11:241–247
Lander ES, Botstein D (1989) Mapping mendelian factors underlying quantitative traits using
RFLP linkage maps. Genetics 121:185–199
Lander ES, Green P, Abrahamson J, Barlow A, Daly MJ, Lincoln SE, Newberg LA (1987)
MAPMAKER: an interactive computer package for constructing primary genetic linkage
maps of experimental and natural populations. Genomics 1:174–181
Legare ME, Bartlett FS 2nd, Frankel WN (2000) A major effect QTL determined by multiple
genes in epileptic EL mice. Genome Res 10:42–48
Legare ME, Frankel WN (2000) Multiple seizure susceptibility genes on chromosome 7 in
SWXL-4 congenic mouse strains. Genomics 70:62–65
Lesch KP, Zeng Y, Reif A, Gutknecht L (2003) Anxiety-related traits in mice with modified
genes of the serotonergic pathway. Eur J Pharmacol 480:185–204
Leussis MP, Frayne ML, Saito M, Berry EM, Aldinger KA, Rockwell GN, Hammer RP Jr,
Baskin-Hill AE, Singer JB, Nadeau JH, Sklar P, Petryshen TL (2009) Genomic survey of
prepulse inhibition in mouse chromosome substitution strains. Genes Brain Behav 8:806–816
Liao G, Wang J, Guo J, Allard J, Cheng J, Ng A, Shafer S, Puech A, McPherson JD, Foernzler D,
Peltz G, Usuka J (2004) In silico genetics: identification of a functional element regulating
H2-Ealpha gene expression. Science 306:690–695
Lindblad-Toh K, Winchester E, Daly MJ, Wang DG, Hirschhorn JN, Laviolette JP, Ardlie K,
Reich DE, Robinson E, Sklar P, Shah N, Thomas D, Fan JB, Gingeras T, Warrington J,
Patil N, Hudson TJ, Lander ES (2000) Large-scale discovery and genotyping of single-
nucleotide polymorphisms in the mouse. Nat Genet 24:381–386
Liu P, Wang Y, Vikis H, Maciag A, Wang D, Lu Y, Liu Y, You M (2006) Candidate lung tumor
susceptibility genes identified through whole-genome association analyses in inbred mice. Nat
Genet 38:888–895
Lloyd DJ, Bohan S, Gekakis N (2006) Obesity, hyperphagia and increased metabolic efficiency in
Pc1 mutant mice. Hum Mol Genet 15:1884–1893
Lloyd DJ, Hall FW, Tarantino LM, Gekakis N (2005) Diabetes insipidus in mice with a mutation
in Aquaporin-2. PLoS Genet 1:e20
Lloyd DJ, Wheeler MC, Gekakis N (2010) A point mutation in Sec61alpha1 leads to diabetes and
hepatosteatosis in mice. Diabetes 59:460–470
Lyon MF (2002) A personal history of the mouse genome. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet
3:1–16
Macarthur DG, Tyler-Smith C (2010) Loss-of-function variants in the genomes of healthy
humans. Hum Mol Genet 19:R125–R130
54 L. M. Tarantino and A. F. Eisener-Dorman
Mackenzie FE, Parker A, Parkinson NJ, Oliver PL, Brooker D, Underhill P, Lukashkina VA,
Lukashkin AN, Holmes C, Brown SD (2009) Analysis of the mouse mutant cloth-ears shows a
role for the voltage-gated sodium channel Scn8a in peripheral neural hearing loss. Genes
Brain Behav 8:699–713
Malmanger B, Lawler M, Coulombe S, Murray R, Cooper S, Polyakov Y, Belknap J,
Hitzemann R (2006) Further studies on using multiple-cross mapping (MCM) to map
quantitative trait loci. Mamm Genome 17:1193–1204
Manly KF, Cudmore RH Jr, Meer JM (2001) Map manager QTX, cross-platform software for
genetic mapping. Mamm Genome 12:930–932
Markel P, Shu P, Ebeling C, Carlson GA, Nagle DL, Smutko JS, Moore KJ (1997) Theoretical
and empirical issues for marker-assisted breeding of congenic mouse strains. Nat Genet
17:280–284
Mathews TA, Brookshire BR, Budygin EA, Hamre K, Goldowitz D, Jones SR (2009) Ethanol-
induced hyperactivity is associated with hypodopaminergia in the 22-TNJ ENU-mutated
mouse. Alcohol 43:421–431
McClearn GE, Wilson J, Meredith W (1970) The use of isogenic and heterogenic mouse stock in
behavioral research. In: Lindzey G, Thiessen D (eds) Contributions to behavioral-genetic
analysis: the mouse as a prototype. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, pp 3–22
McClurg P, Janes J, Wu C, Delano DL, Walker JR, Batalov S, Takahashi JS, Shimomura K,
Kohsaka A, Bass J, Wiltshire T, Su AI (2007) Genomewide association analysis in diverse
inbred mice: power and population structure. Genetics 176:675–683
Melo JA, Shendure J, Pociask K, Silver LM (1996) Identification of sex-specific quantitative trait
loci controlling alcohol preference in C57BL/6 mice. Nat Genet 13:147–153
Miller BH, Schultz LE, Gulati A, Su AI, Pletcher MT (2010) Phenotypic characterization of a
genetically diverse panel of mice for behavioral despair and anxiety. PLoS One 5:e14458
Mogil JS, Richards SP, O’Toole LA, Helms ML, Mitchell SR, Kest B, Belknap JK (1997)
Identification of a sex-specific quantitative trait locus mediating nonopioid stress-induced
analgesia in female mice. J Neurosci 17:7995–8002
Mott R, Flint J (2002) Simultaneous detection and fine mapping of quantitative trait loci in mice
using heterogeneous stocks. Genetics 160:1609–1618
Nathan BM, Hodges CA, Palmert MR (2006) The use of mouse chromosome substitution strains to
investigate the genetic regulation of pubertal timing. Mol Cell Endocrinol 254–255:103–108
Neiderhiser JM, Plomin R, McClearn GE (1992) The use of CXB recombinant inbred mice to
detect quantitative trait loci in behavior. Physiol Behav 52:429–439
Nishi A, Ishii A, Takahashi A, Shiroishi T, Koide T (2010) QTL analysis of measures of mouse
home-cage activity using B6/MSM consomic strains. Mamm Genome 21:477–485
Nolan PM, Peters J, Strivens M, Rogers D, Hagan J, Spurr N, Gray IC, Vizor L, Brooker D,
Whitehill E, Washbourne R, Hough T, Greenaway S, Hewitt M, Liu X, McCormack S,
Pickford K, Selley R, Wells C, Tymowska-Lalanne Z, Roby P, Glenister P, Thornton C,
Thaung C, Stevenson JA, Arkell R, Mburu P, Hardisty R, Kiernan A, Erven A, Steel KP,
Voegeling S, Guenet JL, Nickols C, Sadri R, Nasse M, Isaacs A, Davies K, Browne M,
Fisher EM, Martin J, Rastan S, Brown SD, Hunter J (2000) A systematic, genome-wide,
phenotype-driven mutagenesis programme for gene function studies in the mouse. Nat Genet
25:440–443
Norgard EA, Jarvis JP, Roseman CC, Maxwell TJ, Kenney-Hunt JP, Samocha KE, Pletscher LS,
Wang B, Fawcett GL, Leatherwood CJ, Wolf JB, Cheverud JM (2009) Replication of
long-bone length QTL in the F9–F10 LG, SM advanced intercross. Mamm Genome 20:
224–235
Norgard EA, Roseman CC, Fawcett GL, Pavlicev M, Morgan CD, Pletscher LS, Wang B,
Cheverud JM (2008) Identification of quantitative trait loci affecting murine long bone length
in a two-generation intercross of LG/J and SM/J Mice. J Bone Miner Res 23:887–895
Paigen K (2003) One hundred years of mouse genetics: an intellectual history. I. The classical
period (1902–1980). Genetics 163:1–7
Paigen K, Eppig JT (2000) A mouse phenome project. Mamm Genome 11:715–717
Forward Genetic Approaches to Understanding Complex Behaviors 55
Park YG, Clifford R, Buetow KH, Hunter KW (2003) Multiple cross and inbred strain haplotype
mapping of complex-trait candidate genes. Genome Res 13:118–121
Parker A, Hardisty-Hughes RE, Wisby L, Joyce S, Brown SD (2010) Melody, an ENU mutation
in Caspase 3, alters the catalytic cysteine residue and causes sensorineural hearing loss in
mice. Mamm Genome 21:565–576
Peirce JL, Lu L, Gu J, Silver LM, Williams RW (2004) A new set of BXD recombinant inbred
lines from advanced intercross populations in mice. BMC Genet 5:7
Petryshen TL, Kirby A, Hammer RP Jr, Purcell S, O’Leary SB, Singer JB, Hill AE, Nadeau JH,
Daly MJ, Sklar P (2005) Two quantitative trait Loci for prepulse inhibition of startle identified
on mouse chromosome 16 using chromosome substitution strains. Genetics 171:1895–1904
Philip VM, Duvvuru S, Gomero B, Ansah TA, Blaha CD, Cook MN, Hamre KM, Lariviere WR,
Matthews DB, Mittleman G, Goldowitz D, Chesler EJ (2010) High-throughput behavioral
phenotyping in the expanded panel of BXD recombinant inbred strains. Genes Brain Behav
9:129–159
Phillips TJ, Crabbe JC, Metten P, Belknap JK (1994) Localization of genes affecting alcohol
drinking in mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 18:931–941
Phillips TJ, Huson M, Gwiazdon C, Burkhart-Kasch S, Shen EH (1995) Effects of acute and
repeated ethanol exposures on the locomotor activity of BXD recombinant inbred mice.
Alcohol Clin Exp Res 19:269–278
Phillips TJ, Huson MG, McKinnon CS (1998) Localization of genes mediating acute and
sensitized locomotor responses to cocaine in BXD/Ty recombinant inbred mice. J Neurosci
18:3023–3034
Pletcher MT, McClurg P, Batalov S, Su AI, Barnes SW, Lagler E, Korstanje R, Wang X,
Nusskern D, Bogue MA, Mural RJ, Paigen B, Wiltshire T (2004) Use of a dense single
nucleotide polymorphism map for in silico mapping in the mouse. PLoS Biol 2:e393
Prevorsek Z, Gorjanc G, Paigen B, Horvat S (2010) Congenic and bioinformatics analyses
resolved a major-effect Fob3b QTL on mouse Chr 15 into two closely linked loci. Mamm
Genome 21:172–185
Rastan S, Hough T, Kierman A, Hardisty R, Erven A, Gray IC, Voeling S, Isaacs A, Tsai H,
Strivens M, Washbourne R, Thornton C, Greenaway S, Hewitt M, McCormick S, Selley R,
Wells C, Tymowska-Lalanne Z, Roby P, Mburu P, Rogers D, Hagan J, Reavill C, Davies K,
Glenister P, Fisher EM, Martin J, Vizor L, Bouzyk M, Kelsell D, Guenet JL, Steel KP,
Sheardown S, Spurr N, Gray I, Peters J, Nolan PM, Hunter AJ, Brown SD (2004) Towards a
mutant map of the mouse–new models of neurological, behavioural, deafness, bone, renal and
blood disorders. Genetica 122:47–49
Reijmers LG, Coats JK, Pletcher MT, Wiltshire T, Tarantino LM, Mayford M (2006) A mutant
mouse with a highly specific contextual fear-conditioning deficit found in an N-ethyl-N-
nitrosourea (ENU) mutagenesis screen. Learn Mem 13:143–149
Rieseberg LH, Archer MA, Wayne RK (1999) Transgressive segregation, adaptation and
speciation. Heredity 83(Pt 4):363–372
Rockman MV, Kruglyak L (2008) Breeding designs for recombinant inbred advanced intercross
lines. Genetics 179:1069–1078
Rodriguez LA, Plomin R, Blizard DA, Jones BC, McClearn GE (1994) Alcohol acceptance,
preference, and sensitivity in mice. I. Quantitative genetic analysis using BXD recombinant
inbred strains. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 18:1416–1422
Rodriguez LA, Plomin R, Blizard DA, Jones BC, McClearn GE (1995) Alcohol acceptance,
preference, and sensitivity in mice. II. Quantitative trait loci mapping analysis using BXD
recombinant inbred strains. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 19:367–373
Samocha KE, Lim JE, Cheng R, Sokoloff G, Palmer AA (2010) Fine mapping of QTL for
prepulse inhibition in LG/J and SM/J mice using F(2) and advanced intercross lines. Genes
Brain Behav 9:759–767
Sandberg ML, Sutton SE, Pletcher MT, Wiltshire T, Tarantino LM, Hogenesch JB, Cooke MP
(2005) c-Myb and p300 regulate hematopoietic stem cell proliferation and differentiation. Dev
Cell 8:153–166
56 L. M. Tarantino and A. F. Eisener-Dorman
Savitz J, Lucki I, Drevets WC (2009) 5-HT(1A) receptor function in major depressive disorder.
Prog Neurobiol 88:17–31
Schwander M, Lopes V, Sczaniecka A, Gibbs D, Lillo C, Delano D, Tarantino LM, Wiltshire T,
Williams DS, Muller U (2009a) A novel allele of myosin VIIa reveals a critical function for
the C-terminal FERM domain for melanosome transport in retinal pigment epithelial cells.
J Neurosci 29:15810–15818
Schwander M, Xiong W, Tokita J, Lelli A, Elledge HM, Kazmierczak P, Sczaniecka A,
Kolatkar A, Wiltshire T, Kuhn P, Holt JR, Kachar B, Tarantino L, Muller U (2009b) A mouse
model for nonsyndromic deafness (DFNB12) links hearing loss to defects in tip links of
mechanosensory hair cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:5252–5257
Schwartz WJ, Zimmerman P (1990) Circadian timekeeping in BALB/c and C57BL/6 inbred
mouse strains. J Neurosci 10:3685–3694
Sharkey LM, Cheng X, Drews V, Buchner DA, Jones JM, Justice MJ, Waxman SG, Dib-Hajj SD,
Meisler MH (2009) The ataxia3 mutation in the N-terminal cytoplasmic domain of sodium
channel Na(v)1.6 disrupts intracellular trafficking. J Neurosci 29:2733–2741
Silver LM (1995) Mouse genetics concepts and applications. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Singer JB, Hill AE, Burrage LC, Olszens KR, Song J, Justice M, O’Brien WE, Conti DV,
Witte JS, Lander ES, Nadeau JH (2004) Genetic dissection of complex traits with
chromosome substitution strains of mice. Science 304:445–448
Singer JB, Hill AE, Nadeau JH, Lander ES (2005) Mapping quantitative trait loci for anxiety in
chromosome substitution strains of mice. Genetics 169:855–862
Smith R, Sheppard K, DiPetrillo K, Churchill G (2009) Quantitative trait locus analysis using
J/qtl. Methods Mol Biol 573:175–188
Smoller JW, Paulus MP, Fagerness JA, Purcell S, Yamaki LH, Hirshfeld-Becker D, Biederman J,
Rosenbaum JF, Gelernter J, Stein MB (2008) Influence of RGS2 on anxiety-related
temperament, personality, and brain function. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65:298–308
Solberg LC, Valdar W, Gauguier D, Nunez G, Taylor A, Burnett S, Arboledas-Hita C,
Hernandez-Pliego P, Davidson S, Burns P, Bhattacharya S, Hough T, Higgs D, Klenerman P,
Cookson WO, Zhang Y, Deacon RM, Rawlins JN, Mott R, Flint J (2006) A protocol for high-
throughput phenotyping, suitable for quantitative trait analysis in mice. Mamm Genome
17:129–146
Speca DJ, Chihara D, Ashique AM, Bowers MS, Pierce-Shimomura JT, Lee J, Rabbee N,
Speed TP, Gularte RJ, Chitwood J, Medrano JF, Liao M, Sonner JM, Eger EI, 2nd,
Peterson AS, McIntire SL (2010) Conserved role of unc-79 in ethanol responses in
lightweight mutant mice. PLoS Genet 6(8):e1001057
Steinberger D, Reynolds DS, Ferris P, Lincoln R, Datta S, Stanley J, Paterson A, Dawson GR,
Flint J (2003) Genetic mapping of variation in spatial learning in the mouse. J Neurosci
23:2426–2433
Stottmann RW, Tran PV, Turbe-Doan A, Beier DR (2009) Ttc21b is required to restrict sonic
hedgehog activity in the developing mouse forebrain. Dev Biol 335:166–178
Strohl KP, Gallaugher L, Lynn A, Friedman L, Hill A, Singer JB, Lander ES, Nadeau J (2007)
Sleep-related epilepsy in the A/J mouse. Sleep 30:169–176
Stylianou IM, Christians JK, Keightley PD, Bunger L, Clinton M, Bulfield G, Horvat S (2004)
Genetic complexity of an obesity QTL (Fob3) revealed by detailed genetic mapping. Mamm
Genome 15:472–481
Sudhof TC (2008) Neuroligins and neurexins link synaptic function to cognitive disease. Nature
455:903–911
Swanson DJ, Steshina EY, Wakenight P, Aldinger KA, Goldowitz D, Millen KJ, Chizhikov VV
(2010) Phenotypic and genetic analysis of the cerebellar mutant tmgc26, a new ENU-induced
ROR-alpha allele. Eur J Neurosci 32:707–716
Tabeta K, Hoebe K, Janssen EM, Du X, Georgel P, Crozat K, Mudd S, Mann N, Sovath S,
Goode J, Shamel L, Herskovits AA, Portnoy DA, Cooke M, Tarantino LM, Wiltshire T,
Steinberg BE, Grinstein S, Beutler B (2006) The Unc93b1 mutation 3d disrupts exogenous
antigen presentation and signaling via Toll-like receptors 3, 7 and 9. Nat Immunol 7:156–164
Forward Genetic Approaches to Understanding Complex Behaviors 57
Wakeland E, Morel L, Achey K, Yui M, Longmate J (1997) Speed congenics: a classic technique
in the fast lane (relatively speaking). Immunol Today 18:472–477
Wang GL, Paterson AH (1994) Assessment of DNA pooling strategies for mapping of QTLs.
Theor Appl Genet 88:355–361
Wang J, Liao G, Usuka J, Peltz G (2005) Computational genetics: from mouse to human? Trends
Genet 21:526–532
Wang J, Williams RW, Manly KF (2003) WebQTL: web-based complex trait analysis.
Neuroinformatics 1:299–308
Watson J, Riblet R, Taylor BA (1977) The response of recombinant inbred strains of mice to
bacterial lipopolysaccharides. J Immunol 118:2088–2093
Webb BT, McClay JL, Vargas-Irwin C, York TP, van den Oord EJ (2009) In silico whole genome
association scan for murine prepulse inhibition. PLoS One 4:e5246
Wehner JM, Radcliffe RA, Rosmann ST, Christensen SC, Rasmussen DL, Fulker DW, Wiles M
(1997) Quantitative trait locus analysis of contextual fear conditioning in mice. Nat Genet
17:331–334
Wilkes JJ, Lloyd DJ, Gekakis N (2009) Loss-of-function mutation in myostatin reduces tumor
necrosis factor alpha production and protects liver against obesity-induced insulin resistance.
Diabetes 58:1133–1143
Williams RT, Lim JE, Harr B, Wing C, Walters R, Distler MG, Teschke M, Wu C, Wiltshire T,
Su AI, Sokoloff G, Tarantino LM, Borevitz JO, Palmer AA (2009) A common and unstable
copy number variant is associated with differences in Glo1 expression and anxiety-like
behavior. PLoS One 4:e4649
Wu R, Ma C, Casella G (2010) Statistical genetics of quantitative traits: linkage, maps and QTL.
Springer, New York
Xia Y, Won S, Du X, Lin P, Ross C, La Vine D, Wiltshire S, Leiva G, Vidal SM, Whittle B,
Goodnow CC, Koziol J, EM YM, Beutler B (2010) Bulk segregation mapping of mutations in
closely related strains of mice. Genetics 186:1139–1146
Xie G, Harrison J, Clapcote SJ, Huang Y, Zhang JY, Wang LY, Roder JC (2010) A new Kv1.2
channelopathy underlying cerebellar ataxia. J Biol Chem 285:32160–32173
Yalcin B, Willis-Owen SA, Fullerton J, Meesaq A, Deacon RM, Rawlins JN, Copley RR,
Morris AP, Flint J, Mott R (2004) Genetic dissection of a behavioral quantitative trait locus
shows that Rgs2 modulates anxiety in mice. Nat Genet 36:1197–1202
Yoon CK (1996) J. NIH Res. 8:23–24
Yoshiki A, Ike F, Mekada K, Kitaura Y, Nakata H, Hiraiwa N, Mochida K, Ijuin M, Kadota M,
Murakami A, Ogura A, Abe K, Moriwaki K, Obata Y (2009) The mouse resources at the
RIKEN BioResource center. Exp Anim 58:85–96
Zeng ZB (1993) Theoretical basis for separation of multiple linked gene effects in mapping
quantitative trait loci. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90:10972–10976
Zhang S, Gershenfeld HK (2003) Genetic contributions to body weight in mice: relationship of
exploratory behavior to weight. Obes Res 11:828–838
Mouse Models of the 5-HTTLPR 3 Stress
Risk Factor for Depression
V. Carola (&)
Santa Lucia Foundation, Via del Fosso di Fiorano 64/65,
00143 Rome, Italy
e-mail: valeria.carola@uniroma1.it
V. Carola
Department of Psychology, University of Rome,
‘‘La Sapienza’’, Via dei Marsi 78, Rome, Italy
C. Gross
Mouse Biology Unit, European Molecular Biology Laboratory (EMBL),
Via Ramarini 32, Monterotondo, Italy
into the origins of major depression and other mental illnesses with interacting
genetic and environmental risk factors.
Contents
1 Challenges of Designing Mouse Models of the 5-HTTLPR 9 Stress Risk Factor ......... 60
2 Mouse Models of the 5-HTTLPR 9 Early Stress Risk Factor for Depression................ 63
3 Mouse Models of the 5-HTTLPR 9 Adult Stress Risk Factor for Depression ............... 65
4 Physiological and Molecular Substrates of the 5-HTT 9 Stress Risk Factor in Mice .... 66
5 Promises of Mouse Models of Gene 9 Environment Risk Factors
for Clinical Research........................................................................................................... 68
References.................................................................................................................................. 70
short allele
5-HTTLPR
Fig. 1 Mouse models of 5-HTTLPR 9 early and adult stress risk factors for depression.
In humans, the combination of either childhood maltreatment or adult stress and the 5-HTLPR S
allele results in increased risk for major depression (Caspi et al. 2003). In mice, the 5-HTTLPR S
allele was modeled by a heterozygous knockout (-/+) mutation in 5-HTT that shows a similar
twofold decrease in mRNA expression. Childhood maltreatment was modeled by low maternal
care (Carola et al. 2008) and adult stressors were modeled by daily social defeat (Bartolomucci
et al. 2010). In both cases the low-expressing heterozygous knockout mice were more susceptible
to the effects of stress on anxiety and depression-related behavior, mimicking the human findings.
These studies suggest that reduced 5-HTT function is associated with increased responsivity to
environmental stressors in mice and humans (dotted arrows indicate orthologous developmental
time periods)
increased anxiety in the open field, elevated plus maze, and dark-light tests.
Exposure to unfamiliar male odor was also associated with increased anxiety in all
mice. However, the environmental effect was significant in homozygous knockout
mice and not in wild-type mice, indicating greater susceptibility of the knockout to
threatening olfactory cues than the wild-type. This study confirmed a role for
serotonin homeostasis in regulating the long-term effects of early exposure to
environmental stressors. Furthermore, these findings are intriguing because they
highlight a susceptibility that spans embryonic and early postnatal development
and thus points to a relatively non-specific modulation by serotonin of environ-
mental responsivity during development. It will be important to determine whether
environmental pathogens in humans are similarly effective across development in
modulating the effect of 5-HTTLPR genotype.
Mouse Models of the 5-HTTLPR 9 Stress Risk Factor 65
A much larger number of studies have tried to model the interaction between
5-HTTLPR genotype and adult stress. The first study used a chronic mild stress
procedure involving daily exposure to several stressors (forced swimming, wet
bedding, light cycle reversal, unfamiliar cage mate, tilted cage, restraint stress, no
bedding material, etc.) each day for 4 weeks (Joeyen-Waldorf et al. 2009). Chronic
mild stress was associated with increased anxiety in the novelty suppressed
feeding test and this effect was potentiated in both heterozygous and homozygous
5-HTT knockout female mice, while males showed no genotype-by-environment
interaction effect. A similar genotype by stress effect was observed following
exposure of heterozygous and homozygous 5-HTT knockout mice to a winner or
loser experience in a resident-intruder paradigm over a period of 3 days (Jansen
et al. 2010). Both winners and losers exhibited increased anxiety behavior in the
elevated plus maze, dark-light test, and open field demonstrating an anxiogenic
effect of adverse social exposure regardless of outcome. Homozygous, but not
heterozygous 5-HTT knockout mice showed exaggerated anxiety when exposed to
either repeated winning or losing. These data suggest that 5-HT homeostasis
modulates responses to a wider range of social experiences that are previously
thought and warrants closer investigation in future human association studies.
Although heterozygous 5-HTT knockout mice did not show interactions with
repeated winning or losing in the above study, they did show enhanced response to
a continuous psychosocial stress paradigm where mice were exposed to brief daily
defeat by an aggressive male mouse with whom they were housed in continuous
sensory contact (Bartolomucci et al. 2010), possibly due to the greater intensity of
the later stressor. Following 3 weeks of psychosocial stress mice were tested for
social interaction toward an unfamiliar male mouse in a novel environment.
Stressed mice showed strong avoidance of the unfamiliar mouse, but this effect
was significantly greater in heterozygous knockout mice than wild-type litter-
mates. An important feature of this study was the monitoring of behavioral and
physiological responses to stress in the home cage on a daily basis. Heterozygous
5-HTT knockout mice showed similar responses to stress as wild-type littermates,
including decreased home cage locomotion between encounters, increased body
weight, and increased temperature responses to daily social defeat. These data
suggest that 5-HTT genotype modulates the generalization of avoidance behavior
rather than the direct physiological and behavioral consequences of stress. Such an
interpretation is in line with a role of 5-HTT in determining not the physical
responses to stress, but in modifying stress coping strategies. Moreover, these
findings confirm that a 50% decrease in 5-HTT function is sufficient to modulate
the response to environmental stressors if those stressors are sufficiently prolonged
or salient. Continuous exposure to psychosocial stressors in particular was most
effective in interacting with serotonin homeostasis to increase susceptibility to
anxiety and depression behavior.
66 V. Carola and C. Gross
As discussed above, fMRI studies have identified numerous brain regions whose
activity is modulated by 5-HTTLPR genotype and life stress events and which
are thus implicated in mediating the effect of this risk factor on behavioral traits.
In particular, neural activity in both amygdala and hippocampus was shown to be
significantly modulated by 5-HTTLPR 9 stress, with S allele subjects showing a
positive correlation between life stress events and L/L subjects showing a negative
correlation (Canli et al. 2006). Neural activity in the superior temporal gyrus and
superior parietal lobule, on the other hand, showed a positive correlation with life
stress events in L/L subjects, suggesting a possible role of this structure in the
stress resilience of this genotype. In the same study, the authors also presented
evidence that resting state brain activity in hippocampus and amygdala as mea-
sured while the subjects fixated on a spot was also significantly enhanced in
stressed S allele carriers when compared to all other groups. The modulation of
resting state brain activity is important as it suggests that serotonin signaling
moderates the capacity of stress to reset resting brain activity, and raises the
possibility that changes in neural activity homeostasis in selected brain regions
may underlie the 5-HTTLPR 9 stress risk factor. Unfortunately, this hypothesis
has not yet been examined in animal models.
Nevertheless, two mouse model studies did attempt to identify changes in
molecular substrates that might underlie the 5-HTT 9 stress risk factor. In the first
study, where heterozygous 5-HTT knockout mice were shown to be more sus-
ceptible to the anxiogenic and depressogenic effects of low maternal care (Carola
et al. 2008), molecular substrates were identified in the adult that correlated with
the effect of genotype (G), early environment (eE), and genotype 9 environment
(G9eE; Table 1). First, levels of GABA-A receptors (as measured by binding to
the benzodiazepine flunetrazepam) were reduced in the central nucleus of the
amygdala of mice exposed to low maternal care regardless of the genotype. This
finding replicated earlier data in rats exposed to low maternal care (Caldji et al.
2003) and suggested that decreased inhibition in this nucleus might underlie the
increased risk assessment behavior associated with low maternal care. Second,
serotonin turnover as measured by the ratio of the serotonin metabolite 5-hydroxy-
indole-acetic acid (5-HIAA) to serotonin (5-HT) in the hippocampus was
decreased in heterozygous 5-HTT knockout mice regardless of environmental
exposure. This finding was consistent with earlier studies showing decreased
turnover and serotonin neuron firing in this genotype (Mathews et al. 2004). The
absence of an effect of environment on serotonin turnover suggested that stress
acts downstream of serotonin homeostasis to affect neural circuit function. Finally,
levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) mRNA in the CA1 region of
the hippocampus (but not in other parts of the hippocampus) showed an interacting
effect of genotype and environment, with heterozygous 5-HTT knockouts exposed
to low maternal care showing the highest BDNF levels. Intriguingly, elevated
Mouse Models of the 5-HTTLPR 9 Stress Risk Factor 67
Table 1 Physiological and Molecular Correlates of the 5-HTT 9 Stress Risk Factor in Mice.
Summary of the effects of gene (G), environment (E), and gene-by-environment (G9E) on
molecular substrates as reported by studies measuring anxiety and depression-related behavior in
5-HTT +/+ and ± mice exposed to low maternal care (Carola et al. 2008, 2011) and chronic
psychosocial stress (Bartolomucci et al. 2010). Note that molecular substrates following exposure
to low maternal care were studied at two time points, postnatal day 10 and adulthood (– no effect;
n.d. not determined)
Molecular substrate Low maternal care Chronic psychosocial stress
Adult Postnatal day 10
FCtx 5-HT turnover G GxE GxE
AMY GABA-A receptor E - n.d.
HIP 5-HT turnover G G G
BDNF mRNA GxE E -
AMPA receptor - GxE n.d.
BDNF levels in forebrain are associated with elevated anxiety behavior (Yee et al.
2007) and are a candidate molecular substrate for the elevated neural activity seen
in this structure in human 5-HTTLPR S allele carriers exposed to life stress events
(Canli et al. 2006). Moreover, genetic variation in BDNF itself moderated the
long-term effects of maternal care on anxiety behavior (Carola and Gross 2010).
In humans too a non-synonymous variant in BDNF (Val66Met) with reduced
functionality is able to reverse the neural activity correlates of associated with the
5-HTTLPR S allele (Pezawas et al. 2008). Consistent with a causal role for these
neural substrates in behavior, levels of GABA-A receptor binding, serotonin
turnover, and BDNF expression were significantly correlated with anxiety
behavior in the mice and estimated to each explain about 15–30% of the variance
of relevant behavioral measures (Carola et al. 2008).
A follow-up study by the same group (Carola et al. 2011) investigated the
origins of these changes by examining molecular substrates in young mice during
the period of maternal care (postnatal day 10). Unlike in adult mice, BDNF mRNA
in the hippocampus was elevated in all low maternal care offspring regardless of
genotype (Table 1). This finding suggests that the low functioning 5-HTT variant
may act to maintain elevated BDNF expression induced by low maternal care.
Thus, 5-HTT genotype may not modulate the immediate impact of environment on
neurophysiology, but rather to control its perseverance. As a result, 5-HTTLPR S
allele carriers might be more vulnerable to repeated stressors and, consequently,
recurrent illness, a feature supported by recent epidemiological evidence (Uher
et al. 2011). At the same time, levels of excitatory neurotransmitter receptors
(of the AMPA class) in the hippocampus at postnatal day 10 showed a G9eE
effect and were elevated in heterozygous 5-HTT knockout mice exposed to low
maternal care. These differences did not persist to adulthood (V. Carola, unpub-
lished observations) and thus enhanced excitatory neurotransmission in hippo-
campus may serve as an immediate substrate for early stress and 5-HTT variation
that subsequently triggers long-term G9eE effects in this or other brain circuits.
68 V. Carola and C. Gross
Neural substrates of the 5-HTT 9 adult stress risk factor were investigated in
one study (Bartolomucci et al. 2010). These authors observed that although
genotype alone determined serotonin turnover in most brain regions, in the frontal
cortex serotonin homeostasis showed a significant GxaE effect, with the lowest
serotonin turnover in heterozygous 5-HTT knockout mice experiencing chronic
social defeat (Table 1). Intriguingly, serotonin turnover in the frontal cortex was
best correlated with levels of 5-HTT protein in the amygdala, suggesting cross-talk
between these two connected structures. This finding contrasts with serotonin
turnover data from experiments with 5-HTT 9 early stress (Carola et al. 2008),
where no such interaction was observed. Why frontal cortical serotonin turnover is
more plastic in adulthood than in childhood following stress is not clear, but could
reflect differences in the predominant circuits engaged by stress at these ages or by
the longer time since childhood stress. This difference suggests that the 5-HTTLPR
9 early stress and adult stress risk factors are associated with different patterns of
neural activity and that these forms of depression may show differential thera-
peutic responses. It also suggests that, at least following adult stress, altered
serotonin homeostasis may directly explain the altered activity and/or connectivity
of frontal cortical circuits seen in 5-HTTLPR S carriers by fMRI.
Animal models of gene by environment risk factors for mental illness hold several
promises for the pre-clinical research aimed at developing new diagnostic markers
and therapeutic approaches. One important outcome of a successful animal model
is its ability to point out homologous animal and human behavior. Until now, the
primary avenue to match animal and human behavioral traits associated with
mental illness was pharmacological validation, in which drugs that are used to treat
behavioral disorders in humans induce homologous behavioral changes in animals.
However, pharmacological validation has several problems. The benzodiazepine
class of anxiolytics, for example, is frequently used to validate behavioral tests of
anxiety in rodents, and these drugs cause increased exploration of novel envi-
ronments and reductions in contextual fear (Garner et al. 2009). However, because
these drugs act non-selectively to boost GABA-A receptor function throughout the
brain, they act on a wide range of behaviors beyond anxiety and just because a
particular rodent behavior is modulated by benzodiazepines does not mean it
reflects anxiety. In the field of schizophrenia the situation is even less clear since it
is not obvious how to measure psychotic symptoms in animals. Some behaviors
such as head twitches and scratches selectively induced in mice by hallucinogenics
have been reported (González-Maeso et al. 2007), but the relevance of these
behaviors to the positive symptoms of schizophrenia remain speculative.
For these reasons, a genetic validation approach using a mouse model incor-
porating environmental exposure could offer a parallel avenue for the identification
Mouse Models of the 5-HTTLPR 9 Stress Risk Factor 69
References
Ansorge MS, Zhou M, Lira A, Hen R, Gingrich JA (2004) Early-life blockade of the 5-HT
transporter alters emotional behavior in adult mice. Science 306:879–881
Bartolomucci A, Carola V, Pascucci T, Puglisi-Allegra S, Cabib S, Lesch KP, Parmigiani S,
Palanza P, Gross C (2010) Increased vulnerability to psychosocial stress in heterozygous
serotonin transporter knockout mice. Dis Model Mech 3:459–470
Bengel D, Murphy DL, Andrews AM, Wichems CH, Feltner D, Heils A, Mossner R, Westphal H,
Lesch KP (1998) Altered brain serotonin homeostasis and locomotor insensitivity to 3, 4-
methylenedioxymethamphetamine (‘‘Ecstasy’’) in serotonin transporter-deficient mice. Mol
Pharmacol 53:649–655
Caldji C, Diorio J, Meaney MJ (2003) Variations in maternal care alter GABA(A) receptor subunit
expression in brain regions associated with fear. Neuropsychopharmacology 28:1950–1959
Canli T, Omura K, Haas BW, Fallgatter A, Constable RT, Lesch KP (2005) Beyond affect: a role
for genetic variation of the serotonin transporter in neural activation during a cognitive
attention task. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:12224–12229
Canli T, Qiu M, Omura K, Congdon E, Haas BW, Amin Z, Herrmann MJ, Constable RT, Lesch KP
(2006) Neural correlates of epigenesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:16033–16038
Carola V, Gross C (2010) BDNF moderates early environmental risk factors for anxiety in
mouse. Genes Brain Behav 9:379–389
Carola V, Frazzetto G, Pascucci T, Audero E, Puglisi-Allegra S, Cabib S, Lesch KP, Gross C
(2008) Identifying molecular substrates in a mouse model of the serotonin transporter 9
environment risk factor for anxiety and depression. Biol Psychiatry 63:840–846
Carola V, Pascucci T, Puglisi-Allegra S, Cabib S, Gross C (2011) Effect of the interaction
between the serotonin transporter gene and maternal environment on developing mouse brain.
Behav Brain Res 217:188–194
Mouse Models of the 5-HTTLPR 9 Stress Risk Factor 71
Carroll JC, Boyce-Rustay JM, Millstein R, Yang R, Wiedholz LM, Murphy DL, Holmes A
(2007) Effects of mild early life stress on abnormal emotion-related behaviors in 5-HTT
knockout mice. Behav Genet 37:214–222
Caspi A, Moffitt T (2006) Gene–environment interactions in psychiatry: joining forces with
neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:583–590
Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, McClay J, Mill J, Martin J,
Braithwaite A, Poulton R (2003) Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a
polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 301:386–389
Caspi A, Hariri A, Holmes A, Uher R, Moffitt T (2010) Genetic sensitivity to the environment:
the case of the serotonin transporter gene and its implications for studying complex diseases
and traits. Am J Psychiatry 167:509–527
Clancy B, Finlay BL, Darlington RB, Anand KJ (2007) Extrapolating brain development from
experimental species to humans. Neurotoxicology 28:931–937
Eley TC, Sugden K, Corsico A, Gregory AM, Sham P, McGuffin P, Plomin R, Craig IW (2004)
Gene-environment interaction analysis of serotonin system markers with adolescent
depression. Mol Psychiatry 9:908–915
Ferreira TA, Iacono LL, Gross CT (2010) Serotonin receptor 1A modulates actin dynamics and
restricts dendritic growth in hippocampal neurons. Eur J Neurosci 32:18–26
Garner M, Möhler H, Stein DJ, Mueggler T, Baldwin DS (2009) Research in anxiety disorders:
from the bench to the bedside. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 19:381–390
González-Maeso J, Weisstaub NV, Zhou M, Chan P, Ivic L, Ang R, Lira A, Bradley-Moore M,
Ge Y, Zhou Q, Sealfon SC, Gingrich JA (2007) Hallucinogens recruit specific cortical
5-HT(2A) receptor-mediated signaling pathways to affect behavior. Neuron 53:439–452
Greenberg BD, Tolliver TJ, Huang SJ, Li Q, Bengel D, Murphy DL (1999) Genetic variation in
the serotonin transporter promoter region affects serotonin uptake in human blood platelets.
Am J Med Genet 88:83–87
Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Kolachana B, Fera F, Goldman D, Egan MF, Weinberger DR
(2002) Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala.
Science 297:400–403
Heiming RS, Jansen F, Lewejohann L, Kaiser S, Schmitt A, Lesch KP, Sachser N (2009) Living
in a dangerous world: the shaping of behavioral profile by early environment and 5-HTT
genotype. Front Behav Neurosci 3:26
Heinz A, Braus DF, Smolka MN, Wrase J, Puls I, Hermann D, Klein S, Grüsser SM, Flor H,
Schumann G, Mann K, Büchel C (2005) Amygdala-prefrontal coupling depends on a genetic
variation of the serotonin transporter. Nat Neurosci 8:20–21
Heinz A, Smolka MN, Braus DF, Wrase J, Beck A, Flor H, Mann K, Schumann G, Büchel C,
Hariri AR, Weinberger DR (2007) Serotonin transporter genotype (5-HTTLPR): effects of
neutral and undefined conditions on amygdala activation. Biol Psychiatry 61:1011–1014
Jansen F, Heiming RS, Lewejohann L, Touma C, Palme R, Schmitt A, Lesch KP, Sachser N
(2010) Modulation of behavioural profile and stress response by 5-HTT genotype and social
experience in adulthood. Behav Brain Res 207:21–29
Joeyen-Waldorf J, Edgar N, Sibille E (2009) The roles of sex and serotonin transporter levels in
age- and stress-related emotionality in mice. Brain Res 1286:84–93
Kaufman J, Yang BZ, Douglas-Palumberi H, Houshyar S, Lipschitz D, Krystal JH, Gelernter J
(2004) Social supports and serotonin transporter gene moderate depression in maltreated
children. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:17316–17321
Kaufman J, Gelernter J, Kaffman A, Caspi A, Moffitt T (2010) Arguable assumptions, debatable
conclusions. Biol Psychiatry 67:e19–e20
Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, Benjamin J, Muller CR,
Hamer DH, Murphy DL (1996) Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in
the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 274:1527–1531
Mann JJ, Huang YY, Underwood MD, Kassir SA, Oppenheim S, Kelly TM, Dwork AJ, Arango V
(2000) A serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and prefrontal
cortical binding in major depression and suicide. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57:729–738
72 V. Carola and C. Gross
Mathews TA, Fedele DE, Coppelli FM, Avila AM, Murphy DL, Andrews AM (2004) Gene dose-
dependent alterations in extraneuronal serotonin but not dopamine in mice with reduced
serotonin transporter expression. J Neurosci Methods 140:169–181
Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Drabant EM, Verchinski BA, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS,
Egan MF, Mattay VS, Hariri AR, Weinberger DR (2005) 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts
human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression.
Nat Neurosci 8:828–834
Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Goldman AL, Verchinski BA, Chen G, Kolachana BS,
Egan MF, Mattay VS, Hariri AR, Weinberger DR (2008) Evidence of biologic epistasis
between BDNF and SLC6A4 and implications for depression. Mol Psychiatry 13:709–716
Risch N, Herrell R, Lehner T, Liang KY, Eaves L, Hoh J, Griem A, Kovacs M, Ott J, Merikangas
KR (2009) Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life
events, and risk of depression: a meta-analysis. JAMA 301:2462–2471
Sigurdsson T, Stark KL, Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA, Gordon JA (2010) Impaired hippocampal-
prefrontal synchrony in a genetic mouse model of schizophrenia. Nature 464:763–767
Stark KL, Xu B, Bagchi A, Lai WS, Liu H, Hsu R, Wan X, Pavlidis P, Mills AA, Karayiorgou M,
Gogos JA (2008) Altered brain microRNA biogenesis contributes to phenotypic deficits in a
22q11-deletion mouse model. Nat Genet 40:751–760
Sugden K, Tichopad A, Khan N, Craig IW, D’Souza UM (2009) Genes within the serotonergic
system are differentially expressed in human brain. BMC Neurosci 10:50
Trowbridge S, Narboux-Nême N, Gaspar P (2010) Genetic models of serotonin (5-HT) depletion:
what do they tell us about the developmental role of 5-HT. Anat Rec (Hoboken) 294:
1615–1623
Uher R, Caspi A, Houts R, Sugden K, Williams B, Poulton R, Moffitt TE (2011) Serotonin
transporter gene moderates childhood maltreatment’s effects on persistent but not single-
episode depression: replications and implications for resolving inconsistent results. J Affect
Disord (in press)
Williams LM, Gatt JM, Schofield PR, Olivieri G, Peduto A, Gordon E (2009) ‘Negativity bias’ in
risk for depression and anxiety: brain-body fear circuitry correlates, 5-HTT-LPR and early life
stress. Neuroimage 47:804–814
Yee BK, Zhu SW, Mohammed AH, Feldon J (2007) Levels of neurotrophic factors in the
hippocampus and amygdala correlate with anxiety- and fear-related behaviour in C57BL6
mice. J Neural Transm 114:431–444
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics
of Aggressive Behavior
A. Takahashi
Mouse Genomics Resource Laboratory,
National Institute of Genetics (NIG),
Mishima, Shizuoka, Japan
I. M. Quadros
Departamento de Psicobiologia, Universidade Federal de São Paulo (UNIFESP),
São Paulo-SP, Brazil
R. M. M. de Almeida
Instituto de Psicologia, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS),
Porto Alegre-RS, Brazil
K. A. Miczek (&)
Departments of Psychology, Psychiatry, Pharmacology and Neuroscience,
Tufts University, Bacon Hall, 530 Boston Ave,
Medford and Boston, MA 02155, USA
e-mail: klaus.miczek@tufts.edu
areas can reduce species-typical and other aggressive behaviors, but the same
receptors in the medial prefrontal cortex or septal area promote escalated forms
of aggression. Thus, there are receptor populations in specific brain regions that
preferentially modulate specific types of aggression. Genetic studies have shown
important gene-environment interactions; it is likely that the polymorphisms in
the genes of 5-HT transporters or rate-limiting synthetic and metabolic enzymes
of 5-HT (e.g., MAOA) determine the vulnerability to adverse environmental
factors that escalate aggression. We also discuss the interaction between the
5-HT system and other systems. Modulation of 5-HT neurons in the dorsal
raphe nucleus by GABA, glutamate and CRF profoundly regulate aggressive
behaviors. Also, interactions of the 5-HT system with other neuropeptides
(arginine vasopressin, oxytocin, neuropeptide Y, opioid) have emerged as
important neurobiological determinants of aggression. Studies of aggression in
genetically modified mice identified several molecules that affect the 5-HT
system directly (e.g., Tph2, 5-HT1B, 5-HT transporter, Pet1, MAOA) or indi-
rectly [e.g., BDNF, neuronal nitric oxide (nNOS), aCaMKII, Neuropeptide Y].
The future agenda delineates specific receptor subpopulations for GABA, glu-
tamate and neuropeptides as they modulate the canonical aminergic neuro-
transmitters in brainstem, limbic and cortical regions with the ultimate outcome
of attenuating or escalating aggressive behavior.
Contents
1 Introduction.......................................................................................................................... 75
2 Definition and Measurement of Aggression ...................................................................... 76
3 Aggressive ‘‘Trait’’ Versus ‘‘States’’ .................................................................................. 81
4 5-HT Receptors ................................................................................................................... 84
4.1 5-HT1 Family ............................................................................................................. 84
4.2 5-HT2 Family ........................................................................................................... 100
5 Serotonin Transporter (5-HTT)......................................................................................... 102
5.1 Pharmacological Approach....................................................................................... 102
5.2 Genetic Approach ..................................................................................................... 103
6 Monoamine Oxidase A ..................................................................................................... 104
6.1 Pharmacological Approach....................................................................................... 104
6.2 Genetic Approach ..................................................................................................... 104
7 Modulation of Serotonergic Activity by Other Systems ................................................. 106
7.1 Excitatory and Inhibitory Amino acids ................................................................... 107
7.2 Neuropeptides ........................................................................................................... 109
7.3 Other Molecules that Directly or Indirectly Affect 5-HT Pathways
and Aggression ......................................................................................................... 112
8 Concluding Remarks ......................................................................................................... 114
References................................................................................................................................ 116
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 75
1 Introduction
One of the oldest roots of the neurogenetic analysis of aggressive behavior can be
traced to the domestication of feral animals, as illustrated experimentally in the
elevated brain levels of serotonin in tame silver foxes (e.g., Popova et al. 1991).
No other transmitter system has been more consistently implicated in the neuro-
biological mechanisms mediating impulsive aggressive behavior than serotonin
(Miczek et al. 2002, 2007; Takahashi et al. 2011). Serotonin emerged as the focus in
the initial phase of ‘‘top-down’’ genetics of aggressive behavior, when the brains of
individuals that were selected for high aggressive traits revealed lower levels of
serotonin, its major acidic metabolite and some of the receptor or transporter mol-
ecules upon which this amine acts (Brown et al. 1979; Garattini et al. 1967; Linnoila
et al. 1983). It continues to be the key transmitter system of interest in the more recent
phase of ‘‘bottom-up’’ genetics, when the gene for a specific receptor or transporter
was deleted and the behavioral phenotype indicated a higher than normal level of
aggressive behavior (Bouwknecht et al. 2001a; Saudou et al. 1994).
The current understanding of serotonergic mechanisms in aggressive behavior
has to accommodate two different but interacting sources of influence. Classically,
serotonin has been studied for its role in the predisposition to engage in impulsive
violent and antisocial behavior. The question of interest continues to be which
features of serotonergic transmission—its rate of synthesis, uptake, metabolism,
receptor and transporter expression—are characteristic of an aggressive heritable
trait that runs in families (Brunner et al. 1993a). It has become clear that the
seductively simple serotonin deficiency hypothesis that links low serotonin activity
to the propensity for aggressive behavior is being replaced by more sophisticated
and detailed schemes. Recent clinical and preclinical data point to important allelic
differences in the genes for specific serotonin receptor subtypes, transporter
molecules and metabolic enzymes in impulsively aggressive individuals (de Boer
and Koolhaas 2005; Lesch and Merschdorf 2000; Miczek et al. 2002, 2007; Nelson
and Chiavegatto 2001; Takahashi et al. 2011).
A second role for serotonin is to modulate aggressive behavior by its phasic
activity, particularly during the anticipation of aggressive and defensive acts and at
the termination of an aggressive bout. Superimposed on the serotonergic tone are
transient changes in release, impulse flow and receptor activation that are syn-
chronized with the initiation and termination of bouts of aggressive and defensive
acts (Ferrari et al. 2003). Phasic serotonergic activity appears to be linked to bouts
of several species-normative consummatory behaviors ranging from feeding,
drinking and sexual acts to fighting and to pathological excesses of these activities.
Neuroplastic changes in serotonergic pathways from the dorsal raphé neurons to
the neo- and paleocortical terminals result from repeated aggressive experiences as
reflected in serotonin receptor regulation, firing rate and release. To further our
understanding of the interaction between tonic and phasic serotonergic activities
ideally requires real-time measurements over the course of a circadian cycle and
arranging conditions that provide a challenge behaviorally and neurochemically.
76 A. Takahashi et al.
During the last decade, epigenetic mechanisms for the expression of certain
genes that encode for serotonergic metabolic enzymes, transporter proteins and
receptors have been scrutinized for their role in impulsively violent individuals.
Stressful experiences such as abusive maltreatment during a critical developmental
period can lead young adults to engage in more impulsive and violent behavior,
particularly in those individuals who are characterized by the expression of a
certain allelic variety of Monoamine Oxidase A, an important metabolic enzyme
for serotonin (Caspi et al. 2002).
The last decade has seen also a concerted effort to translate more readily
between preclinical data and clinical observations, and vice versa. This is evident
from both an increasing focus on escalated, atypical forms of aggressive behavior
in animal models, and by efforts to define aggressive behaviors using operational
and functional definitions and assessments in clinical settings (DSM-V).
Most research on the neurogenetics of serotonin and aggression in both human and
veterinary medicine seeks to understand and control pathological aggression
(Volavka et al. 2005), while laboratory experiments using animal models typically
deal with adaptive, species-typical aggressive behavior. When studying aggression
in animals, it is important to consider the ethological significance of the behavior,
including its phylogenetic and ontogenetic development and the functions it serves
for individuals and the species. Aggressive signals, postures and acts are used to help
an animal obtain specific goals, or to defend against threats actual attacks (Miczek
et al. 2002). These behaviors occur when individuals compete for food, water and
other resources necessary for survival and reproduction (resident-intruder aggres-
sion), when they defend their territory or offspring (territorial and maternal
aggression), or in response to frustration or fear (Miczek et al. 2001). Engaging in
aggressive behavior is often beneficial to the individual and the species. For example,
dominance hierarchies are established and maintained through confrontations
between rival males (Fig. 1), and so-called isolation-induced aggression mimics
many elements of the behavior used by resident males to exclude other breeding
males from their home territory and their mates (Brain and Benton 1979; Miczek
et al. 2001, Table 1). However, it should be noted that isolated housing results in
social avoidance in a large proportion of a sample of mice, while aggressive behavior
is seen in varying numbers of isolated animals depending on species and strain.
Aggressive behavior can be classified as offensive or defensive based upon the
distal and proximal conditions that precipitated it, the topography of the behavior,
and its consequences (Blanchard and Blanchard 1977; Brain 1979). Defensive
behaviors occur in response to threatening or fear-inducing stimuli and often result in
escapes (Brain 1979). In male rodents, specific defensive behaviors include escape,
freezing, defensive postures and threats, which occur in response to attacks by either
predators or conspecifics (Blanchard et al. 2003; Pellis and Pellis 1988; Rasia-Filho
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 77
in hamsters, mice, and rats home-cage prior to the resident- Potegal (1991)
intruder encounter Potegal and Tenbrink (1984)
Frustration-heightened A resident male is positively reinforced. Berkowitz (1993)
aggression The reinforcement is abruptly omitted De Almeida and Miczek
before the resident-intruder encounter (2002)
Aggression induced by low Resident rats are adrenalectomized and Haller et al. (2001)
glucocorticoids implanted with a low-dose
corticosterone pellet
Affective defense Electrical stimulation (0.2–0.8 mA, Hissing, arching of the back, Hess (1954)
(‘‘rage’’), mainly in cats 63 Hz, 1 ms per half cycle duration) retraction of the ears, piloerection, Leyhausen (1979)
delivered in medial hypothalamus or unsheathing of the claws, Siegel et al. (1999)
midbrain periaqueductal gray papillary dilatation and paw striking
79
80 A. Takahashi et al.
A frequently reiterated theory, based on early clinical and preclinical studies, links
impulsive, hostile and violent behavior to a serotonin deficiency (Brown and
Goodwin 1986; Goldman et al. 1992; Lesch and Merschdorf 2000; Linnoila and
Virkkunen 1992; Mann 1999; Valzelli 1977). Individuals exhibiting such behavior
may benefit from pharmacological treatments aimed at inhibiting 5-HT trans-
porters (e.g., SSRIs such as fluoxetine or citalopram), activating 5-HT1A receptors
(e.g., buspirone) or blocking 5-HT2A receptors (e.g., risperidone). Acute treatment
with these drugs induces phasic changes in 5-HT function that are associated with
their transient anti-aggressive effects. In vivo microdialysis allows transient
changes in extracellular 5-HT levels to be monitored in anticipation of, during, and
after aggressive encounters in rats. One study reported reduced 5-HT levels in the
prefrontal cortex during and after the aggressive confrontation, while no changes
were detected in the nucleus accumbens, another terminal region (Van Erp and
Miczek 2000, Fig. 2). By contrast, chronic treatment with these anti-aggressive
compounds may promote as yet undefined neuroadaptive changes in 5-HT
function such as autoreceptor desensitization, which may in turn be associated
with the emergence of therapeutic effects.
On the other hand, aggression as a ‘‘trait’’ is the focus of genetic studies. While
these aggressive traits are clearly polygenic, it is remarkable that several studies
have found an interaction between genotypes such as TPH2, MAO-A and 5-HTT
polymorphisms and environmental triggers such as social stress underlying an
increased likelihood of violent outbursts (see below). For example, a SNP in TPH2
gene (A2051C) has been linked to aggressive behavior in rhesus monkeys.
Monkeys with the AA/AC genotype that were reared without their mother (peer-
reared) showed increased aggressive acts compared to those with a CC genotype,
but the difference disappeared when infants of both genotypes were reared by their
mothers (Chen et al. 2010). This review will focus on the interaction between
salient environmental events and genes and the subsequent effects on aggressive
behaviors.
Gene–gene interactions are also of interest and need to be explored. For
example, Passamonti et al. (2008) showed interactions between 5-HTT and
MAOA polymorphisms that affected the activity of the anterior cingulate cortex, a
brain area that has been implicated in impulsivity, including impulsive aggression.
Many other genes may have subtle effects on aggressive phenotypes, and stronger
effects may emerge as a result of complex epistatic interactions between those
82
Table 2 Experimental protocols for assessing 5-HT effects on human aggressive behavior
A. Experimental manipulations
Experimental manipulation Measurement Trait/state References
Aggressive responses toward a Activate buttons at 5–10 settings, State Buss (1961)
competitor are measured in the each corresponding to Godlaski and Giancola (2009)
form of electric shock settings a different intensity or duration
of electric shock
A fictitious instigator or competitor is the target of Setting of electric shock level on State Chermack and Giancola (1997)
aggressive responses that are measured in the a scale from 1–10 Taylor (1967)
form of electric shock deliveries
The subjects are provoked by having points Number of point subtractions from State Cherek and Heistad (1971)
subtracted in a competitive task. The point losses a fictitious competitor Cherek and Lane (1999)
are attributed to a fictitious opponent, but are Gowin et al. (2010)
actually random. Subjects
responded by retaliation of
point subtractions
(=aggressive responses)
Aggression was defined as Uses a modified version of the State Giancola et al. (2009)
delivery of electric shocks Buss aggression machine. Zeichner and Pihl (1979)
to a fictitious opponent Setting of shock level on a
scale from 1–5
B. Psychometric inventories
Psychometric assessment Instrument Trait/state References
Aggression, impulsivity Inventory Trait McKinley et al. (1948)
and hostility are measured Nagtegaal and Rassin (2004)
by Minnesota multiphasic
personality inventory (MMPI)
(continued)
A. Takahashi et al.
Table 2 (continued)
B. Psychometric inventories
Psychometric assessment Instrument Trait/state References
Buss–Durkee hostility inventory Inventory Trait Buss and Durkee (1957)
(BDHI), a self-rating scale of
anger and hostility. Sixty six items
with false/true answers; also contains
seven scales: assault, indirect aggression,
irritability, negativism, resentment,
suspicion and verbal aggression
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics
Anger and anxiety are measured Inventory State/trait Kim et al. (2009b)
by state–trait anger expression Spielberg et al. (1973)
inventory (STAXI)
Aggression is measured by beck Inventory Trait Beck et al. (1961)
anxiety inventory and beck Lamar et al. (2009)
depression inventory
83
84 A. Takahashi et al.
genes (Miczek et al. 2001). In the past 15 years, most rodent studies on genetics
and aggressive behavior have used conventional knockout techniques, in which the
expression of a gene is completely deleted, affecting the whole body through all
developmental stages and inducing compensatory changes in other genes (trait-like
change; see Table 3). Newer, more subtle methods, including conditional knock-
out, viral vector microinfusion, and drug-inducible knockout techniques, can
produce transient and local changes in gene expression, allowing the study of more
‘‘phasic’’ changes in gene expression and how they influence aggression. Such
studies may explain some of the discrepancies in the results from previous genetic
and pharmacological studies of 5-HT function and aggression.
4 5-HT Receptors
The 5-HT1A receptor partial agonist buspirone has been used clinically to reduce
general anxiety. It was shown that buspirone also reduces aggressive behavior in
mentally retarded patients (Kavoussi et al. 1997; Ratey et al. 1991), and this
compound has been used for the management of aggressive outbursts associated
with neuropsychiatric disorders in adults and children (Connor and Steingard
1996; Pabis and Stanislav 1996). Similar findings have been reported in preclinical
studies; systemic administration of 5-HT1A receptor agonists (e.g., 8-OH-DPAT,
repinotan, alnespirone) dose-dependently decrease aggressive behaviors in a wide
range of animal species, including fish, amphibians, birds, rodents, guinea pigs and
non-human primates (Bell and Hobson 1994; Blanchard et al. 1988; Clotfelter
et al. 2007; de Boer et al. 1999; de Boer and Koolhaas 2005; Dompert et al. 1985;
Haug et al. 1990; Joppa et al. 1997; Lindgren and Kantak 1987; McMillen et al.
1988; Miczek et al. 1998b; Muehlenkamp et al. 1995; Nikulina et al. 1992; Olivier
et al. 1992; Sanchez et al. 1993; Sperry et al. 2003; Ten Eyck 2008; Tompkins
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 85
Fig. 2 Dopamine and serotonin during aggression. Measurements of extracellular dopamine and
serotonin via in vivo microdialysis in resident male rats before, during and after a confrontation with
an intruder. a In the nucleus accumbens (top panel), dopamine levels (gray circles) rise and remain
elevated after the confrontation, while serotonin levels (black diamonds) do not significantly
change. b In the prefrontal cortex (bottom panel), dopamine levels rise after the confrontation, while
serotonin decline and remain lower after the confrontation. Samples were collected every 10 min
and levels are expressed as mean percent of baseline ±SEM. Baseline was measured for 50 min
before the fight. The vertical light gray bar indicates the occurrence of the 10-min fight. Asterisks
and double asterisks represent significant differences from baseline (dashed line) at the p \ 0.05 and
0.01 levels, respectively. Reprinted with permission from Van Erp and Miczek (2000)
et al. 1980). One exception are fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster), as they
showed increased aggressive behavior after 8-OH-DPAT treatment (Johnson et al.
2009). Selective antagonists of 5-HT1A receptors such as WAY-100635 success-
fully reversed the effect of 5-HT1A agonists, while the administration of the
antagonist by itself did not show any effect on aggression (de Boer and Koolhaas
2005; Mendoza et al. 1999; Miczek et al. 1998b). Notably, in laboratory studies the
anti-aggressive effects of most of 5-HT1A agonists are accompanied by nonspecific
Table 3 Genes and aggressive behavior in mice
86
Gene Abb Chr Background strain Type of Effects Serotonin function References
(generations of aggression
backcross)
Plasmacytoma expressed transcript 1 Pet1 1 Mix C57BL/6 Isolation- Increased 90% reduction Hendricks et al.
(Fev) and 129 Sv induced of brain 5-HT (2003)
resident
aggression
Arginine vasopressin receptor 1B V1bR 1 Mix C57BL/6 Isloation- Suppressed Wersinger et al.
and 129/SvJ induced (2002)
aggression
Neutral cage Suppressed
aggression
Aadenosine A1 receptor A1AR 1 Mix C57BL Isolation- Increased Gimenez-Llort
and 129/OlaHsd induced et al. (2002)
resident
aggression
Regulators of G protein signaling 2 Rgs2 1 C57BL/6J (N5) Social Suppressed Oliveira-dos-
dominance Santos et al.
test (2000)
v-abl Abelson murine leukemia viral Arg 1 Mix C57BL/6 Resident Suppressed Koleske et al.
oncogene homolog 2 (Abelson- and 129/SvJ aggression (1998)
related gene)
Glutamic acid decarboxylase 2 GAD65 2 Mix C57BL/6 Isolation- Suppressed Stork et al.
and CBA2 induced (2000)
resident
aggression
Calcium channel, voltage-dependent, Cav2.2 2 Mix C57BL/6J Isolation- Increased Increased hypothalamus Kim et al.
N type, a1B subunit and 129S4/SvJae induced 5-HT (2009a)
resident
aggression
A. Takahashi et al.
(continued)
Table 3 (continued)
Gene Abb Chr Background strain Type of Effects Serotonin function References
(generations of aggression
backcross)
Dopamine b-hydroxylase Dbh 2 Mix C57BL/6J Isolation- Suppressed Marino et al.
and 129/SvEv induced (2005)
resident
aggression
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor [±] BDNF 2 C57BL/6J ([N10) Isolation- Increased Decreased brain 5-HT Lyons et al.
induced (1999)
resident
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics
aggression
b2-microglobulin b2 m 2 129S2/SvPas Resident Suppressed Loconto et al.
(Mix C57BL/6J?) aggression (2003)
Oxytocin Oxt 2 Mix C57BL/6J Isolation- Increased Winslow et al.
and 129SvEv induced (2000)
resident
aggression
Resident Increased
aggression Suppressed DeVries et al.
(1997)
Membrane metallo endopeptidase NEP 3 Mix C57BL/6J Resident Increased Fischer et al.
(enkephalinase) and 129 Sv aggression (2000)
Cannabinoid receptor 1 CB1 4 CD1 (N15) Isolation- Increaseda Martin et al.
induced (2002)
resident
aggression
Preproenkephalin Enk 4 Mix CD1 Isolation- Increaseda Konig et al.
and 129 induced (1996)
resident
aggression
87
(continued)
Table 3 (continued)
88
Gene Abb Chr Background strain Type of Effects Serotonin function References
(generations of aggression
backcross)
Endothelial nitric oxide synthase eNOS 5 Mix C57BL/6 Resident Suppressed Increased 5-HT turnover Demas et al.
and 129 Sv aggression (1999)
Neutral cage Suppressed
aggression
Interleukin-6 IL-6 5 Mix C57BL/6, Isolation- Increasedb No difference in brain Alleva et al.
129/SvEv induced 5-HT concentration (1998)
resident
aggression
Adrenergic receptor, a2c Naa2C 5 C57BL/6J (N7) Isolation- Increased Sallinen et al.
induced (1998)
resident
aggression
Neuronal nitric oxide synthase nNOS 5 Mix C57BL/6J, Resident Increased Reduced brain 5-HT Nelson et al.
129 Sv, DBA2 aggression turnover (1995)
Maternal Suppressed Gammie and
aggression Nelson
(1999)
Acetylcholinesterase AChE 5 129S6/SvEvTac Home-cage Suppressed Duysen et al.
hierarchy (2002)
Adenylate cyclase activating PAC1 6 Mix C57BL/6J Resident Suppressed Nicot et al.
polypeptide 1 receptor 1 and 129 Sv aggression (2004)
Tachykinin receptor 1 NK-1r 6 Mix C57BL/6 Isolation- Suppressed De Felipe et al.
and 129 Sv induced (1998)
resident
aggression
A cluster of vomeronasal receptor V1Ra/b 6 129/SvEv Maternal Suppressed Del Punta et al.
genes aggression (2002)
A. Takahashi et al.
(continued)
Table 3 (continued)
Gene Abb Chr Background strain Type of Effects Serotonin function References
(generations of aggression
backcross)
Oxytocin receptor Oxtr 6 Mix C57BL/6 Isloation- Increased Takayanagi
and 129 Sv induced et al. (2005)
aggression
Cage-mate injury Increased
Histamine receptor H1 H1 6 Mix C57BL/6J Isolation- Suppressedb Increased 5-HT turnover Yanai et al.
and 129 induced (1998)
resident
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics
aggression
Amyloid b(A4) precursor protein- X11L 7 C57BL/6 Competitive Subordinate Increased Hypothalamus Sano et al.
binding, family A, member 2 feeding 5-HT (2009)
Transient receptor potential cation TRP2 7 Mix C57BL/6J Resident Suppressed Stowers et al.
channel, subfamily C, member 2 and 129Sv aggression (2002)
Neuropeptide Y receptor Y1 Y1 8 Mix C57BL/6 Resident Increased Reduced TPH mRNA Karl et al.
and 129SvJ aggression expression in the (2004)
raphe nuclei
Prostaglandin E receptor 1 EP1 8 C57BL/6 (N5) Neutral cage Increased No difference in 5-HT Matsuoka et al.
(subtype EP1) aggression turnover (2005)
Norepinephrine transporter NET 8 Mix C57BL/6J Isolation- Increaseda Haller et al.
and 129SvJ induced (2002)
resident
aggression
Neural cell adhesion molecule 1 NCAM1 9 C57BL/6J ([N5) Isolation- Increased Stork et al.
induced (1997)
resident
aggression
(continued)
89
Table 3 (continued)
90
Gene Abb Chr Background strain Type of Effects Serotonin function References
(generations of aggression
backcross)
Aromatase (Cyp19a1) Ar 9 Mix C57BL/6 Resident Suppressed Matsumoto
and 129S/SvEv aggression et al. (2003)
Aggression Increased
toward
female
5-hydroxytryptamine 5-HT1B 9 129S2/SvPas Resident Increased Deleted 5-HT1B receptor Saudou et al.
(serotonin) receptor 1B aggression expression (1994)
Maternal Increased Brunner and
aggression Hen (1997)
Estrogen receptor-a ERa 10 Mix C57BL/6J Resident Suppressed Ogawa et al.
and 129 aggression (1998b)
Female Increased Ogawa et al.
aggression (1998a)
Fyn tryrosine kinase Fyn 10 Mix C57BL/6 and Isolation- Suppressed Miyakawa
CBA induced et al. (2001)
resident
aggression
(continued)
A. Takahashi et al.
Table 3 (continued)
Gene Abb Chr Background strain Type of Effects Serotonin function References
(generations of aggression
backcross)
Nuclear receptor subfamily 2, Nr2e1 10 C57BL/6J ([ N6) Resident Increased Young et al.
group E, member 1 aggression (2002)
Adenosine A2a receptor A2AR 10 CD1 (N4) Isolation- Increased Ledent et al.
induced (1997)
resident
aggression
Tryptophan hydroxylase 2 Tph2 10 FVB/N (N7) Home-cage Increased Reduced brain 5-HT Alenina et al.
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics
injury (2009)
Glutamate receptor, ionotropic, GluR-A 11 C57BL/6J (N5) Isolation- Suppressed No difference in brain 5- Vekovischeva
AMPA1 (a1) induced HT level et al. (2004)
aggression
Neutral cage Suppressed
aggression
5-hydroxytryptamine (serotonin) SERT 11 C57BL/6J (N8) Isolation- Suppressed Increased extracelluler Holmes et al.
transporter induced 5-HT (2002)
resident
aggression
Estrogen Receptor-b ERb 12 Mix C57BL/6J Resident Increaseda Ogawa et al.
and 129 aggression (1999)
Dopamine transporter DAT 13 Mix C57BL/6J Dyadic Increased Rodriguiz et al.
and 129SvJ encounter (2004)
aggression
5-hydroxytryptamine (serotonin) 5-HT1A 13 129S1/Sv Resident Suppressed Deleted 5-HT1A receptor Zhuang et al.
receptor 1A aggression expression (1999)
Catechol-O-methyltransferase 1 [±] COMT 16 Mix C57BL/6J Neutral cage Increased No difference in brain Gogos et al.
and 129SvJ aggression 5-HT level (1998)
(continued)
91
Table 3 (continued)
92
Gene Abb Chr Background strain Type of Effects Serotonin function References
(generations of aggression
backcross)
Calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein aCaMKII 18 Mix C57BL/6, Defensive Increased Reduced 5-HT release in Chen et al.
kinase II alpha [±] 129/OU, aggression the dorsal raphe (1994)
BALB/c nucleus
Melanocortin-5 receptor MC5R 18 C57BL/6 ([N7) Neutral cage Suppressed Morgan et al.
aggression (2004)
Monoamine oxidase A MAOA X C3H/HeJ Resident Increased Increased brain 5-HT up Cases et al.
aggression to ninefold (1995)
Cage-mate injury Increased
Cyclic nucleotide–gated channel a2 Cnga2 X Mix C57BL/6J Resident Suppressed Mandiyan et al.
and 129SvEv aggression (2005)
Guanosine diphosphate (GDP) Gdi1 X C57BL/6J (N5) Isolation- Suppressed D’Adamo et al.
dissociation inhibitor 1 induced (2002)
resident
aggression
Androgen receptorc AR X C57BL/6 and Isolation- Suppressedb Raskin et al.
129SvEv induced (2009)
resident
aggression
b
[±] Data obtained from heterozygote of knockout mouse, a behavioral change only at the first encounter, behavior change after repeated exposure,
c
Nestine-Cre-LoxP conditional knockout mice that selectively lack AR expression in the nervous system
A. Takahashi et al.
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 93
Fig. 3 a Effects of social instigation on aggressive behavior by a resident mouse toward a male
intruder. Bars represent the mean frequency ±SEM (vertical lines) of attack bites under control
(light gray) and instigated (dark gray) conditions. Asterisks denote statistical significance from
control (double asterisks P \ 0.01). b Preferential reduction of instigated aggressive behavior by
the 5-HT1B agonist anpirtoline (left panel, filled circles) and CP-94,253 (right panel, filled
squares). Symbols represent the mean frequency of attack bites, expressed as a percentage of
vehicle (V) baseline, ±SEM. Light gray symbols represent non-instigated fighting and dark gray
symbols represent instigated levels of fighting. Asterisks denote significance from vehicle
baseline (P \ 0.05). Adapted from Fish et al. (1999) and de Almeida and Miczek (2002)
male mice 1.0 lg with reduced motor activity; no effects on alcohol- et al.
5-HT1B: CP-94253, 1.0 lg related aggression (2008)
(agonists)
Schedule-heightened aggression, 5-HT1B: CP-93129, ; escalated aggression; reduced walking behavior Bannai et al.
male mice 0.1–1.0 lg (agonist) (2007)
Alcohol-escalated aggression, 5-HT1B: CP-93129, ; baseline and alcohol-related aggression (0.5–1.0 lg); Faccidomo
male mice 0.1–1.0 lg (agonist) concomitant reduction in motor activity et al.
2012
Maternal aggression, rats 5-HT1A: 8-OH-DPAT, : maternal aggression (0.56 lg); no motor effects Veiga et al.
0.56 lg (agonist) DPAT-escalated aggression prevented by infusion (2011)
of CP-93129 (1.0 lg) into the orbitofrontal cortex
MRN Maternal aggression, rats 5-HT1A: 8-OH-DPAT, ; maternal aggression; no side effects De Almeida
0.2–2.0 lg (agonist) and
Lucion
(1997)
PAG Maternal aggression, rats Dorsal PAG ; maternal aggression (0.2–2.0 lg); no side effects de Almeida
5-HT1A: 8-OH-DPAT, and
0.2–2.0 lg (agonist) Lucion
(1997)
(continued)
95
Table 4 (continued)
96
Brain region Type of aggression, species Target; drugs and doses Pharmacological effects References
Maternal aggression, rats Dorsal PAG a-methyl-5-HT maleate: ; maternal aggression; de Almeida
5-HT2A/2C: a-methyl-5- no motor effects et al.
HT maleate, 0.2–1.0 lg (2005)
(agonist)
5-HT2A/2C: ketanserin, Ketanserin: no effects on aggression,
1.0 lg (antagonist) decreased motor activity
Hypothalamic-stimulated PAG 8-OH-DPAT: ; defensive hissing (0.66–1.0 lg), effect Shaikh et al.
defensive aggression, cats 5-HT1A: 8-OH-DPATa, prevented by antagonist p-MPPI. No motor effect (1997)
0.016 ng–1.0 lg DOI: facilitation of defensive hissing (0.54 lg)
5-HT2C: DOIa, 3.57 ng–
0.54 lg (agonists)
Septal nuclei Maternal aggression, rats Medial septal nucleus : maternal aggression (0.2–0.5 lg); reduced activity de Almeida
5-HT1A: 8-OH-DPAT, only with highest dose (2.0 lg) and
0.2–2.0 lg (agonist) Lucion
(1997)
Maternal aggression, rats Medial septal nucleus No effects on aggressive or non-aggressive behaviors de Almeida
5-HT2A/2C: alpha-methyl- (agonist) et al.
5-HT maleate, Ketanserin: no effects on aggression, but decreased (2005)
0.2–1.0 lg (agonist) motor activity
5-HT2A/2C: ketanserin,
1.0 lg (antagonist)
Resident aggression, male mice Lateral septal nucleus 8-OH-DPAT: no behavioral effects Cologer-
(castrated males with hormonal 5-HT1A: 8-OH-DPATa, CGS: ; aggression with CGS, only in androgen- Clifford
replacement) 0.33 ng replacement condition; no side effects et al.
5-HT1B: CGS12066a, (1997)
18.0 ng
(agonists)
Alcohol-escalated aggression, Lateral septal nucleus No effects on alcohol-related or baseline aggression Faccidomo
male mice 5-HT1B: CP-94253, et al.
1.0 lg (agonist) (2008)
A. Takahashi et al.
(continued)
Table 4 (continued)
Brain region Type of aggression, species Target; drugs and doses Pharmacological effects References
Hypothalamus Resident aggression, male mice Medial pre-optic area 8-OH-DPAT: ; aggression in androgen- and Cologer-
(castrated males with hormonal 5-HT1A: 8-OH-DPATa, estrogen-replacement conditions; no motor effects Clifford
replacement) 0.16 ng CGS: ; aggression in androgen- and estrogen- et al.
5-HT1B: CGS12066a, replacement conditions; no motor effects (1997)
9.0 ng (agonists)
Vasopressin-escalated aggression, Anterior hypothalamus 8-OH-DPAT: ; escalated aggression (0.33, 3.28 ng); Ferris et al.
male hamsters 5-HT1A: 8-OH-DPATa, no apparent motor effects (1999)
0.03–3.28 ng
5-HT1B: CGS12066a, 5-HT1B: no effects on aggression (trends toward
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics
4.5–45.0 ng inhibition)
(agonists)
PAG-stimulated defensive Medial hypothalamus 8-OH-DPAT: ; defensive hissing (0.33–1.0 lg); Hassanain
aggression, cats 5-HT1A: 8-OH-DPATa, effect prevented by antagonist p-MPPI et al.
0.03–1.0 lg (2003)
5-HT2C: DOIa, DOI: facilitates defensive hissing (0.36 lg); effect
0.036–0.36 lg (agonists) prevented by antagonist LY-53,857
Amygdala Maternal aggression, rats Corticomedial amygdaloid ; maternal aggression (0.5–2.0 lg); no side effects De Almeida
nucleus and
5-HT1A: 8-OH-DPAT, Lucion
0.2–2.0 lg (agonist) (1997)
Prefrontal Resident aggression, male mice 5-HT1B: CP-94253, 1.0 lg No effects on aggressive or non-aggressive behaviors de Almeida
cortex (agonist) et al.
(2006)
Alcohol-escalated aggression, 5-HT1B: CP-94253, : alcohol-related aggression (1.0 lg); mild increases Faccidomo
male mice 0.5–1.0 lg (agonist) in activity; no effects on baseline aggression et al.
(2008)
(continued)
97
Table 4 (continued)
98
Brain region Type of aggression, species Target; drugs and doses Pharmacological effects References
Alcohol-escalated aggression, 5-HT1B: CP-93129, ; baseline and alcohol-related aggression (0.1–1.0 lg); Faccidomo
male mice 0.1–1.0 lg (agonist) no motor effects et al.
2012
Orbitofrontal Resident aggression, male mice 5-HT1B: CP-94253, ; aggression (0.56–1.0 lg); no side effects. De Almeida
cortex 0.1–1.0 lg (agonist) CP-94253 effects prevented by 1B antagonist, et al.
GR-127935. (2006)
Maternal aggression, rats 5-HT1B: CP-93129, 1.0 lg CP-93129: ; maternal aggression (1.0 ug); no side Veiga et al.
5-HT1B: CP-94253, effects (2007)
0.56–1.0 lg (agonists) CP-94253: no behavioral effect
Alcohol-escalated aggression, 5-HT1B: CP-94253, No effects on alcohol-related or baseline levels of Faccidomo
male mice 0.5–1.0 lg (agonist) aggression et al.
(2008)
Social instigation-escalated 5-HT1A: 8-OH-DPAT, 8-OH-DPAT: ; instigated aggression (0.56–1.0 lg); no Centenaro
aggression, male mice 0.1–1.0 lg side effects et al.
5-HT1B: CP-93129, CP-93129: ; instigated aggression (only 0.1 lg); no (2008)
0.1–1.0 lg (agonists) side effects
DRN dorsal raphé nucleus, MRN median raphé nucleus, PAG periaqueductal gray area
a
doses calculated based on the following molecular weights (MW) of the compounds (as available at Sigma–Aldrich): 8-OH-DPAT hydrobromide,
MW = 328.29; CGS-12066 maleate salt, MW = 450.41; DOI hydrochloride, MW = 357.62
A. Takahashi et al.
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 99
The 5-HT1B receptor was the first molecule to be linked to aggression by using the
gene knockout technique. Male mice with disrupted 5-HT1B receptor expression
(Htr1b-/-) increased isolation-induced aggressive behavior compared to the wild-
type mice (Bouwknecht et al. 2001b; Saudou et al. 1994). However, the aggressive
behavior of the background strain of mice (129/Sv-ter) was very low, close to zero,
and thus the frequency of attack bites in Htr1b-/- was low and the latency to
initiate fighting was very long compared to other strains of mice. These mice
displayed behavioral disinhibition in other behavioral tests including hyperloco-
motor activity (Brunner et al. 1999; Ramboz et al. 1995), drug intake (Crabbe et al.
1996; Rocha et al. 1998), measures of anxiety-like behavior (Brunner et al. 1999;
Malleret et al. 1999) and autonomic hyperreactivity to novelty (Bouwknecht et al.
2001a). Females of Htr1b-/- increased their aggressive behavior during the
postpartum period (Brunner and Hen 1997). These results suggest a role for
5-HT1B receptors in the inhibition of some aggressive and impulsive behaviors.
The level of tissue 5-HT concentration in Htr1b-/- mice was lower than wild-type
in nucleus accumbens and spinal cord (Ase et al. 2000).
There are several polymorphisms in the human 5-HT1B receptor (HTR1B) gene.
The most frequently studied HTR1B polymorphism in relation to aggressive
phenotype is a single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) G861C. This SNP had
significant linkage with aggressive behavior in antisocial alcoholism (Lappalainen
et al. 1998). Specifically, 861C, the SNP with lower 5-HT1B receptor expression
(Huang et al. 1999), was related to antisocial behavior. This SNP is actually a
100 A. Takahashi et al.
Buitelaar et al. 2001; Czobor et al. 1995; De Deyn et al. 1999; Fava 1997; Keck
et al. 2000; Zarcone et al. 2001). However, some reports cast doubt on the routine
use of antipychotics (Swanson et al. 2008; Tyrer et al. 2008). In one study, the
placebo control group showed the greatest reduction in aggressively challenging
behavior compared to antipsychotic drug treatments in people with intellectual
disability (Tyrer et al. 2008). In animal models, selective 5-HT2A antagonists
(e.g., ketanserin, ritanserin and MDL 100907) reduce aggressive behaviors in a
behaviorally non-specific manner (Rodriguez-Arias et al. 1998; Sakaue et al. 2002;
Shih et al. 1999; White et al. 1991).
Activation of 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors by DOI and other substituted
phenylisopropylamines also reduces aggressive behavior in several species
including flies, amphibians, mice and rats (Bonson et al. 1994; de Almeida and
Lucion 1994; Johnson et al. 2009; Muehlenkamp et al. 1995; Olivier et al. 1995;
Sanchez et al. 1993; Ten Eyck 2008). However, the anti-aggressive effects of
5-HT2 ligands are accompanied by sedative effects in the same dose range. Local
infusion of a 5-HT2A/2C agonist into the PAG reduces maternal aggression in rats
(de Almeida et al. 2005), whereas microinjections into the medial hypothalamus
and into the PAG increased defensive aggression in cats (Hassanain et al. 2003;
Shaikh et al. 1997, see Table 4). This latter effect is likely linked to the role of
5-HT2A/2C receptors in anxiety-like behavior (Lucki and Wieland 1990; Nogueira
and Graeff 1995). The development of a more selectively acting pharmacological
tools will allow a more adequate differentiation of 5-HT2 receptor subtypes, and
promises to dissociate the anti-aggressive and sedative effects.
6 Monoamine Oxidase A
The gene for monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) was the first to be identified as a
possible determinant for pathological aggression in humans, and it has remained
the focus of most genetic and epigenetic studies. A Dutch family with a syndrome
of borderline mental retardation and dysregulated impulsive aggression was
identified by Brunner et al. (1993b). Aggressive outbursts were seen in all affected
males in the kindred, and some exhibited aberrant sexual behavior, attempted
murder and arson. Linkage and sequence analyses identified one missense muta-
tion in the MAOA gene on the X chromosome, so that MAOA function was
completely disturbed; the affected males had more serotonin and lower levels of
norepinephrine, dopamine and 5-HT metabolites in their urine (Brunner et al.
1993a). MAOA is also an important factor in animal aggression. Aggressive
behaviors (skin wounds among cage mates and briefer attack latency in the resi-
dent-intruder test) escalated in male mice with a disrupted MAOA gene on either
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 105
Fig. 4 Means on the composite index of antisocial behavior as a function of MAOA activity and
a childhood history of maltreatment. MAOA activity is the gene expression level associated with
allelic variants of the functional promoter polymorphism, grouped into low and high activity;
childhood maltreatment is grouped into three categories of increasing severity. The antisocial
behavior composite is standardized (z score) to a M = 0 and SD = 1; group differences are
interpretable in SD unit differences (d). Reprinted with permission from Caspi et al. (2002)
individuals with higher MAOA expression (MAOA-H) (Caspi et al. 2002; Foley
et al. 2004; Frazzetto et al. 2007; Kim-Cohen et al. 2006; Weder et al. 2009;
Widom and Brzustowicz 2006). The effect of MAOA genotype disappeared if all
subjects were lumped together regardless of rearing environment (Fresan et al.
2007), and MAOA-H individuals sometimes reported higher aggression in inter-
views and on questionnaires (Manuck et al. 2000, 2002). The finding that indi-
viduals with the MAOA-L allele are vulnerable to environmental factors, showing
a high propensity to engage in aggressive behaviors when in a stressful environ-
ment, is consistent in males but not females (Sjoberg et al. 2007). Rhesus monkeys
have a similar repeat length variation polymorphism in the MAOA gene
(rhMAOA-LPR) which is also linked to aggression. Monkeys with a low-activity
allele who were reared by their mother showed more aggressive behavior and
attained higher dominance rank than monkeys with the low-activity allele who were
peer-reared separately from their parents (Newman et al. 2005). Higley and Suomi
(1986) attributed this inhibition of aggression to increased fear and anxiety in peer-
reared monkeys.
Substantial differences in both volume and activity of limbic system and
neocortical areas between MAOA-L and MAOA-H individuals have been found in
neuroimaging studies [see Buckholtz and Meyer-Lindenberg (2008) for a review].
In healthy male human volunteers with the MAOA-L variant, fMRI showed
smaller limbic and orbitofrontal volumes and higher activity in amygdala and
hippocampus during aversive recall (Meyer-Lindenberg et al. 2006), which may be
related to violent behavior. Lower MAOA activity in cortical and subcortical brain
areas is associated with elevated aggression as measured by a self-report ques-
tionnaire, with no effect of MAOA polymorphism (Alia-Klein et al. 2008). These
data show that the MAOA activity is one determinant of the propensity to
aggression. The interaction between MAOA polymorphism and stressful social
experiences is especially critical, since it can escalate aggression and can also
change relevant brain structures.
The 5-HT neurons in the raphé nuclei are modulated by other amines, acids,
peptides and steroids (Adell et al. 2002). Several efforts have been undertaken to
uncover the nature of the neural systems that modulate 5-HT neurons to promote
escalated aggressive behaviors. Here we will focus briefly on inhibitory and
excitatory neurotransmitters and some neuropeptides in terms of their interaction
with 5-HT system. Especially, dorsal raphé nucleus (DRN), the largest 5-HT
nucleus in the brain, will be highlighted because of its important role for
aggression (Bannai et al. 2007; Faccidomo et al. 2008; Jacobs and Cohen 1976;
Koprowska and Romaniuk 1997; Mos et al. 1993; Sijbesma et al. 1991; Van Der
Vegt et al. 2003; Vergnes et al. 1986).
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 107
150
100
(b) (c)
60
Saline
Bregma * Baclofen
+2.10mm
Bregma
4.36mm
*
Attack bites
40
+1.94mm
4.60mm
20
+1.70mm 4.84mm
0
PFC DRN 10 40 100 min
Fig. 5 Extracellular 5-HT concentration in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) of mice after
GABAB receptor activation in the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN). a Baclofen microinjected into the
DRN increased the 5-HT level in the mPFC whereas saline injection did not change the 5-HT
level. Twenty-minute samples were collected: five samples for baseline, three samples after saline
injection and six samples after baclofen (0.06 nmol) injection. Data are means ± SEM expressed
as percentage of baseline (n = 7); aisterisks = p \ 0.05 compared to baseline. b Histological
representation of probe placement in the mPFC for the microdialysis (vertical bars: 2 mm probe
membrane) and drug injection site in the DRN (circles). c The effect of 0.06 nmol baclofen
(black bars) or saline (gray bars) on attack bites at different post-injection intervals (10, 40 and
100 min, corresponding to fractions 9, 11 and 14 in the microdialysis, respectively). Escalated
attack bites were observed both 10 and 40 min after the intra-DRN baclofen injection. Values are
means ±SEM; asterisks = p \ 0.05compared to corresponding vehicle control. From Takahashi
et al. (2010b)
7.1.2 Glutamate
The DRN receives glutamate input by the descending projections from the lateral
habenula, periaqueductal gray, lateral hypothalamus, interpeduncular nucleus
and medial prefrontal cortex (Aghajanian and Wang 1977; Behzadi et al. 1990;
Kalen et al. 1986; Maciewicz et al. 1981). Both the N-metyl-D-aspartate (NMDA)
and a-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolaproprionate/kainate (AMPA/kainate),
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 109
7.2 Neuropeptides
pro-aggressive effects (Elkabir et al. 1990; Tazi et al. 1987). Under conditions
of escalated aggression promoted by moderate doses of alcohol in male
mice, CRF1 receptors are a promising target for pharmacological intervention.
Systemic administration of the antagonists of CRF1 receptors reduce alcohol-
heightened aggression, but also reduce baseline levels of aggressive behavior
(Quadros et al. 2009b). When locally administered into the DRN, CRF1 antagonists
(e.g., CP-154526 or MTIP) prevent the escalated levels of aggression observed
after consumption of alcohol, with no detectable side effects on other behaviors.
Remarkably, such anti-aggressive effects of CRF1 antagonists can be abolished with
the infusion of 8-OH-DPAT into the DRN, which transiently slows 5-HT impulse
flow. On the other hand, microinfusion of a CRF2 antagonist (e.g., Astressin-2B) into
the DRN escalates aggressive behavior (Quadros et al. 2009a).
The modulation of aggressive behaviors by CRF systems depends on the spe-
cies such as mice or rats, and type of aggression (species-typical, maternal or
escalated aggression). Initial evidence suggests the 5-HT cells in the DRN as one
of the critical sites for such modulation in escalated aggression that is promoted by
alcohol, with presumably opposing roles for CRF1 and CRF2 receptors.
7.2.2 Neuropeptide Y
Neuropeptide Y (NPY) controls primarily food intake, energy balance and meta-
bolic regulation (Herzog 2003). In addition to the critical role for energy
homeostasis, NPY is also implicated in aggressive behavior (Emeson and Mor-
abito 2005). Male mice with deleted expression of Y1 receptor (Y1-/-) showed
obesity and reduced energy homeostasis (Kushi et al. 1998). These animals also
exhibited increased aggressive behaviors in the resident-intruder test (Karl et al.
2004). However, escalated aggression was observed only in the home-cage but not
in the novel environment in Y1-/- mice. Both NPY and its receptors can be found
in the DRN (Martel et al. 1990; O’Donohue et al. 1985; Saria et al. 1984), and
NPY inhibits both hyperpolarizing and depolarizing slow synaptic potentials in the
DRN (Kombian and Colmers 1992). Y1-/- mice showed reduced expression of
tryptophan hydroxylase mRNA in the raphé nuclei, and also 5-HT1A agonists
could suppress the heightened aggression of Y1-/- mice (Karl et al. 2004). These
results suggest that the disruption of Y1 receptor expression specifically increases
territorial aggression by altering 5-HT function.
Goodson 2008; Koolhaas et al. 1990; Neumann et al. 2010; Winslow and Insel
1993). Selective antagonists of vasopressin V1a receptors (SRX251, [d(CH2)
5Tyr(Me)AVP]) inhibited inter-male aggression (Ferris et al. 2006; Ferris and
Potegal 1988), suggesting the involvement of V1a receptors in aggressive
behaviors. Evidence points to a critical role of the interaction between AVP and
5-HT in certain types of aggressive behaviors. In humans, a positive correlation has
been observed between AVP concentrations in the CSF and the life history of
aggression, a composite measure of trait aggression. Also, there was a positive
correlation between AVP concentrations and prolactin responses to a challenge
with d-fenfluramine (Coccaro et al. 1998). This result indicates that individuals that
have higher aggression ratings tend to have a high AVP concentration in the CSF and
a hyporesponsive 5-HT system. Neuronal interactions between AVP containing
neurons and 5-HT neurons are found in the anterior hypothalamus (Ferris et al. 1997,
1999), and this AVP-5-HT link is implicated in aggression. Microinjection of AVP
into the anterior or lateral hypothalamus increased aggressive behavior in hamsters,
and systemic fluoxetine, a 5-HTT inhibitor, blocked the pro-aggressive effect of
AVP (Delville et al. 1996; Ferris et al. 1997) Therefore, 5-HT may have an inhibitory
effect on the AVP-heightened aggression. By contrast, mice with disrupted Ca2+
channel expression (Cav2-/-) showed escalated level of aggressive behavior and
also higher AVP concentration in the CSF. However, these animals showed over-
activation of the dorsal 5-HT neurons and increased 5-HT concentration in the
hypothalamus (Kim et al. 2009a). Further investigation will uncover how AVP and 5-
HT interact and whether there are specific types of aggression that require the activity
of either 5-HT or AVP systems independently.
7.2.4 Oxytocin
7.3.2 Pet-1
VTA-accumbens pathway of mice and rats that are repeatedly attacked and show
evidence for defeat, submission and disrupted social interactions (Berton et al.
2006; Miczek et al. 2011).
Nitric oxide, a free radical gas which diffuses across membranes, is involved in
several cellular functions [for review, see Calabrese et al. (2007)]. Mice lacking
neuronal nitric oxide synthase (nNOS-/-) show various deficits in their physio-
logical development and also behavior (Huang et al. 1993). nNOS-/- males, but
not females, showed higher duration of aggressive behavior and also displayed
much fewer submissive postures compared to wild-types (Nelson et al. 1995).
Serotonergic dysfunctions were observed in the nNOS-/- mice, specifically
reduced 5-HT turnover in the brain and deficient 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptor
function (Chiavegatto et al. 2001). Escalated aggression in the nNOS-/- was
rescued by 5-HTP treatment which increased 5-HT level and turnover. These
findings point to an important role of nitric oxide for the normal 5-HT function,
and thus increased aggression nNOS-/- may be induced by changing 5-HT
activity.
8 Concluding Remarks
(3) the trait characteristics of the human subject or the mouse (i.e., Mus mus-
culus is a pugnacious species and many inbred strains are quite placid).
• Based on genetic studies, it is likely that the 5-HT1B receptor has an important
role in the inhibition of aggression. For other receptor subtypes such as 5-HT1A
or 5-HT2A receptors, the association between aggressive behavior and poly-
morphisms in receptor genes all failed to have a reliable relationship compared
to relatively consistent pharmacological data. It may be important to determine
where in the brain of aggressive individuals the gene expression is changed by
the polymorphisms.
• An important role of MAOA in aggressive behaviors is supported by genetic
studies of deleterious mutation of MAOA gene in humans and knockout mice.
In contrast to the serotonin deficiency hypothesis, these results suggest that
chronically increased 5-HT levels due to reduced MAOA function—trait-like
change—may promote or intensify escalated aggressive displays.
• Consistent anti-aggressive effects of SSRI treatment in several species strongly
point to the serotonin transporter as a promising therapeutic target for man-
agement of impulsive, escalated aggression. More studies are required to
determine the precise neurobiological mechanisms recruited for the anti-
aggressive effects of SSRIs to emerge, especially as a result of chronic treatment
that induces pre- and post-synaptic neuroadaptive processes.
• The genetic studies in human and nonhuman primates also suggest 5-HTT
polymorphisms such as the short allele as a risk factor for violent traits, which
seems to be particularly relevant in combination with environmental stress.
Results from transgenic mice lacking 5-HTT are more difficult to interpret due
to the wide variety of behavioral functions that are affected by the genetic
manipulation.
• Gene polymorphism studies of MAOA, 5-HTT and Tph2 revealed critical gene-
environment interactions as a risk factor for violent traits. Individuals with a
certain allele are particularly prone to engage in violent behavior when they
have a history of early life maltreatment, but the effect disappeared when they
are reared in an environment with low stress.
• Modulatory mechanisms of serotonergic neurons that induce escalated aggres-
sion have gradually been identified. So far, evidence points to the modulations
of activity of dorsal raphé nucleus by GABA, glutamate, CRF and its auto-
receptors as being particularly relevant to the display of escalated aggression.
Genetic analyses of aggressive individuals have identified several molecules that
affect the 5-HT system directly (e.g., Tph2, 5-HT1B, 5-HT transporter, Pet1,
MAOA) or indirectly (e.g., Neuropeptide Y, aCaMKII, NOS, BDNF).
In the current phase of advanced molecular genetic studies, it is necessary to
remind ourselves to match them with more sophisticated behavioral methodologies
of aggression. It is evident that the neurogenetic mechanisms differentiate types
and patterns of aggressive behavior requiring higher resolution. The current focus
116 A. Takahashi et al.
on brain serotonin takes advantage of the most intensively studied neural system
that has been implicated in the neurogenetics of aggression. It remains astounding
how such trace amounts of this indole amine can engender such profound changes
in aggressive traits and states.
References
Abbadie C, Honore P, Fournie-Zaluski MC, Roques BP, Besson JM (1994) Effects of opioids and
non-opioids on c-fos-like immunoreactivity induced in rat lumbar spinal cord neurons by
noxious heat stimulation. Eur J Pharmacol 258:215–227
Abellan MT, Jolas T, Aghajanian GK, Artigas F (2000) Dual control of dorsal raphe serotonergic
neurons by GABAB receptors. Electrophysiological and microdialysis studies. Synapse
36:21–34
Adell A, Celada P, Abellan MT, Artigas F (2002) Origin and functional role of the extracellular
serotonin in the midbrain raphe nuclei. Brain Res Rev 39:154–180
Adell A, Celada P, Artigas F (2001) The role of 5-HT1B receptors in the regulation of serotonin
cell firing and release in the rat brain. J Neurochem 79:172–182
Aghajanian GK, Wang RY (1977) Habenular and other midbrain raphe afferents demonstrated by
a modified retrograde tracing technique. Brain Res 122:229–242
Albers HE, Bamshad M (1998) Role of vasopressin and oxytocin in the control of social behavior
in Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). Prog Brain Res 119:395–408
Alenina N, Kikic D, Todiras M, Mosienko V, Qadri F, Plehm R, Boye P, Vilianovitch L, Sohr R,
Tenner K, Hortnagl H, Bader M (2009) Growth retardation and altered autonomic control in
mice lacking brain serotonin. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:10332–10337
Alia-Klein N, Goldstein RZ, Kriplani A, Logan J, Tomasi D, Williams B, Telang F, Shumay E,
Biegon A, Craig IW, Henn F, Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Fowler JS (2008) Brain monoamine
oxidase a activity predicts trait aggression. J Neurosci 28:5099–5104
Alleva E, Cirulli F, Bianchi M, Bondiolotti GP, Chiarotti F, De Acetis L, Panerai AE (1998)
Behavioural characterization of interleukin-6 overexpressing or deficient mice during
agonistic encounters. Eur J Neurosci 10:3664–3672
Amat J, Baratta MV, Paul E, Bland ST, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2005) Medial prefrontal cortex
determines how stressor controllability affects behavior and dorsal raphe nucleus. Nat
Neurosci 8:365–371
Amat J, Tamblyn JP, Paul ED, Bland ST, Amat P, Foster AC, Watkins LR, Maier SF (2004)
Microinjection of urocortin 2 into the dorsal raphe nucleus activates serotonergic neurons and
increases extracellular serotonin in the basolateral amygdala. Neuroscience 129:509–519
Arnone M, Dantzer R (1980) Effects of diazepam on extinction induced aggression in pigs.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav 13:27–30
Ase AR, Reader TA, Hen R, Riad M, Descarries L (2000) Altered serotonin and dopamine
metabolism in the CNS of serotonin 5-HT1A or 5-HT1B receptor knockout mice. J Neurochem
75:2415–2426
Assal F, Alarcon M, Solomon EC, Masterman D, Geschwind DH, Cummings JL (2004)
Association of the serotonin transporter and receptor gene polymorphisms in neuropsychiatric
symptoms in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 61:1249–1253
Baca-Garcia E, Vaquero C, Diaz-Sastre C, García-Resa E, Saiz-Ruiz J, Fernández-Piqueras J,
de Leon J (2004) Lack of association between the serotonin transporter promoter gene
polymorphism and impulsivity or aggressive behavior among suicide attempters and healthy
volunteers. Psychiatry Res 126:99–106
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 117
Bonson KR, Johnson RG, Fiorella D, Rabin RA, Winter JC (1994) Serotonergic control of
androgen-induced dominance. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 49:313–322
Bonvento G, Scatton B, Claustre Y, Rouquier L (1992) Effect of local injection of 8-OH-DPAT
into the dorsal or median raphe nuclei on extracellular levels of serotonin in serotonergic
projection areas in the rat brain. Neurosci Lett 137:101–104
Bosch OJ, Meddle SL, Beiderbeck DI, Douglas AJ, Neumann ID (2005) Brain oxytocin
correlates with maternal aggression: link to anxiety. J Neurosci 25:6807–6815
Bouwknecht JA, Hijzen TH, van der GJ, Maes RA, Hen R, Olivier B (2001a) Absence of 5-HT1B
receptors is associated with impaired impulse control in male 5-HT1B knockout mice. Biol
Psychiatry 49:557–568
Bouwknecht JA, van der GJ, Hijzen TH, Maes RA, Hen R, Olivier B (2001b) Corticosterone
responses in 5-HT1B receptor knockout mice to stress or 5-HT1A receptor activation are
normal. Psychopharmacology 153:484–490
Bowery NG, Hudson AL, Price GW (1987) GABAA and GABAB receptor site distribution in the
rat central nervous system. Neuroscience 20:365–383
Brain P, Benton D (1979) The interpretation of physiological correlates of differential housing in
laboratory rats. Life Sci 24:99–115
Brain PF (1979) Hormones, drugs and aggression. Eden Press, Montreal
Brown GL, Goodwin FK (1986) Cerebrospinal fluid correlates of suicide attempts and
aggression. In: Mann JJ (ed) Psychobiology of suicidal behavior (Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences, Vol 487). New York Academy of Sciences, New York, pp 175–220
Brown GL, Goodwin FK, Ballenger JC, Goyer PF, Major LF (1979) Aggression in humans
correlates with cerebrospinal fluid amine metabolites. Psychiatry Res 1:131–139
Brunner D, Buhot MC, Hen R, Hofer M (1999) Anxiety, motor activation, and maternal-infant
interactions in 5HT1B knockout mice. Behav Neurosci 113:587–601
Brunner D, Hen R (1997) Insights into the neurology of impulsive behavior from serotonin
receptor knockout mice. Ann N Y Acad Sci 836:81–105
Brunner HG, Nelen M, Breakefield XO, Ropers HH, van Oost BA (1993a) Abnormal behavior
associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science
262:578–580
Brunner HG, Nelen MR, Vanzandvoort P, Abeling NGGM, Vangennip AH, Wolters EC, Kuiper MA,
Ropers HH, Vanoost BA (1993b) X-linked borderline mental-retardation with prominent
behavioral disturbance—phenotype, genetic localization, and evidence for disturbed monoamine
metabolism. Am J Hum Genet 52:1032–1039
Buckholtz JW, Meyer-Lindenberg A (2008) MAOA and the neurogenetic architecture of human
aggression. Trends Neurosci 31:120–129
Buckley PF, Ibrahim ZY, Singer B, Orr B, Donenwirth K, Brar PS (1997) Aggression and
schizophrenia: efficacy of risperidone. J Am Acad Psychiatry Law 25:173–181
Buitelaar JK, van der Gaag RJ, Cohen-Kettenis P, Melman TM (2001) A randomized controlled
trial of risperidone in the treatment of aggression in hospitalized adolescents with subaverage
cognitive abilities. J Clin Psychiatry 62:239–248
Burkhalter JE, Balster RL (1979) The effects of phencyclidine on isolation-induced aggression in
mice. Psychol Rep 45:571–576
Burt SA, Mikolaiewski AJ (2008) Preliminary evidence that specific candidate genes are
associated with adolescent-onset antisocial behavior. Aggress Behav 34:437–445
Buss AH (1961) The psychology of aggression. Wiley and Sons, New York
Buss AH, Durkee A (1957) An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. J Consult
Psychol 21:343–349
Cadoret RJ, Langbehn D, Caspers K, Troughton EP, Yucuis R, Sandhu HK, Philibert R (2003)
Associations of the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism with aggressivity, attention
deficit, and conduct disorder in an adoptee population. Compr Psychiat 44:88–101
Cairns RB, Nakelski JS (1971) On fighting in mice: ontogenetic and experiential determinants.
J Comp Physiol Psychol 74:354–364
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 119
Calabrese V, Mancuso C, Calvani M, Rizzarelli E, Butterfield DA, Stella AM (2007) Nitric oxide
in the central nervous system: neuroprotection versus neurotoxicity. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:
766–775
Caldwell EE, Miczek KA (2008) Long-term citalopram maintenance in mice: selective reduction
of alcohol-heightened aggression. Psychopharmacology 196:407–416
Carrillo M, Ricci LA, Coppersmith GA, Melloni RH Jr (2009) The effect of increased
serotonergic neurotransmission on aggression: a critical meta-analytical review of preclinical
studies. Psychopharmacology 205:349–368
Cases O, Seif I, Grimsby J, Gaspar P, Chen K, Pournin S, Muller U, Aguet M, Babinet C, Shih JC,
De Maeyer E (1995) Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin and
norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA. Science 268:1763–1766
Cases O, Vitalis T, Seif I, DeMaeyer E, Sotelo C, Gaspar P (1996) Lack of barrels in the
somatosensory cortex of monoamine oxidase A-deficient mice: role of a serotonin excess
during the critical period. Neuron 16:297–307
Caspi A, McClay J, Moffitt TE, Mill J, Martin J, Craig IW, Taylor A, Poulton R (2002) Role of
genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science 297:851–854
Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, McClay J, Mill J, Martin J,
Braithwaite A, Poulton R (2003) Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a
polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 301:386–389
Ceglia I, Acconcia S, Fracasso C, Colovic M, Caccia S, Invernizzi RW (2004) Effects of chronic
treatment with escitalopram or citalopram on extracellular 5-HT in the prefrontal cortex of
rats: role of 5-HT1A receptors. Br J Pharmacol 142:469–478
Celada P, Puig MV, Casanovas JM, Guillazo G, Artigas F (2001) Control of dorsal raphe
serotonergic neurons by the medial prefrontal cortex: involvement of serotonin-1A, GABAA,
and glutamate receptors. J Neurosci 21:9917–9929
Centenaro LA, Vieira K, Zimmermann N, Miczek KA, Lucion AB, de Almeida RM (2008)
Social instigation and aggressive behavior in mice: role of 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptors in
the prefrontal cortex. Psychopharmacology 201:237–248
Chalmers DT, Lovenberg TW, De Souza EB (1995) Localization of novel corticotropin-releasing
factor receptor (CRF2) mRNA expression to specific subcortical nuclei in rat brain:
comparison with CRF1 receptor mRNA expression. J Neurosci 15:6340–6350
Chan JP, Unger TJ, Byrnes J, Rios M (2006) Examination of behavioral deficits triggered by
targeting Bdnf in fetal or postnatal brains of mice. Neuroscience 142:49–58
Chen C, Rainnie DG, Greene RW, Tonegawa S (1994) Abnormal fear response and aggressive
behavior in mutant mice deficient for a-calcium-calmodulin kinase II. Science 265:291–294
Chen GL, Novak MA, Meyer JS, Kelly BJ, Vallender EJ, Miller GM (2010) The effect of rearing
experience and TPH2 genotype on HPA axis function and aggression in Rhesus monkeys:
a retrospective analysis. Horm Behav 57:184–191
Chen ZY, Jing DQ, Bath KG, Ieraci A, Khan T, Siao CJ, Herrera DG, Toth M, Yang C, McEwen BS,
Hempstead BL, Lee FS (2006) Genetic variant BDNF (Val66Met) polymorphism alters anxiety-
related behavior. Science 314:140–143
Cherek DR, Heistad GT (1971) Fixed-interval induced aggression. Psychon Sci 25:7–8
Cherek DR, Lane SD (1999) Laboratory and psychometric measurements of impulsivity among
violent and nonviolent female parolees. Biol Psychiatry 46:273–280
Chermack ST, Giancola PR (1997) The relation between alcohol and aggression: an integrated
biopsychosocial conceptualization. Clin Psychol Rev 17:621–649
Chiavegatto S, Dawson VL, Mamounas LA, Koliatsos VE, Dawson TM, Nelson RJ (2001) Brain
serotonin dysfunction accounts for aggression in male mice lacking neuronal nitric oxide
synthase. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:1277–1281
Cichon S, Winge I, Mattheisen M, Georgi A, Karpushova A, Freudenberg J, Freudenberg-Hua Y,
Babadjanova G, Van Den Bogaert A, Abramova LI, Kapiletti S, Knappskog PM, McKinney J,
Maier W, Abou Jamra R, Schulze TG, Schumacher J, Propping P, Rietschel M, Haavik J,
Nothen MM (2008) Brain-specific tryptophan hydroxylase 2 (TPH2): a functional Pro206Ser
120 A. Takahashi et al.
substitution and variation in the 5 ‘-region are associated with bipolar affective disorder. Hum
Mol Genet 17:87–97
Clotfelter ED, O’Hare EP, McNitt MM, Carpenter RE, Summers CH (2007) Serotonin decreases
aggression via 5-HT1A receptors in the fighting fish Betta splendens. Pharmacol Biochem
Behav 87:222–231
Coccaro EF, Kavoussi RJ (1997) Fluoxetine and impulsive aggressive behavior in personality-
disordered subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54:1081–1088
Coccaro EF, Kavoussi RJ, Hauger RL, Cooper TB, Ferris CF (1998) Cerebrospinal fluid
vasopressin levels: correlates with aggression and serotonin function in personality-disordered
subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:708–714
Coccaro EF, Kavoussi RJ, Sheline YI, Berman ME, Csernansky JG (1997) Impulsive aggression
in personality disorder correlates with platelet 5- HT2A receptor binding. Neuropsychophar-
macology 16:211–216
Coccaro EF, Lee R, Kavoussi RJ (2010) Aggression, suicidality, and intermittent explosive
disorder: serotonergic correlates in personality disorder and healthy control subjects.
Neuropsychopharmacology 35:435–444
Cole HF, Wolf HH (1970) Laboratory evaluation of aggressive behavior of the grasshopper
mouse (Onychomys). J Pharm Sci 59:969–971
Colmers WF, Williams JT (1988) Pertussis toxin pretreatment discriminates between pre- and
postsynaptic actions of baclofen in rat dorsal raphe nucleus in vitro. Neurosci Lett 93:300–306
Cologer-Clifford A, Simon NG, Lu SF, Smoluk SA (1997) Serotonin agonist-induced decreases
in intermale aggression are dependent on brain region and receptor subtype. Pharmacol
Biochem Behav 58:425–430
Connor DF, Steingard RJ (1996) A clinical approach to the pharmacotherapy of aggression in
children and adolescents. In: Understanding aggressive behavior in children. Ann N Y Acad
Sci 794:290–307
Crabbe JC, Phillips TJ, Feller DJ, Hen R, Wenger CD, Lessov CN, Schafer GL (1996) Elevated
alcohol consumption in null mutant mice lacking 5-HT1B serotonin receptors. Nat Genet
14:98–101
Crawley JN, Schleidt WM, Contrera JF (1975) Does social environment decrease propensity to
fight in male mice? Behav Biol 15:73–83
Cutler MG, Rodgers RJ, Jackson JE (1997) Behavioural effects in mice of subchronic buspirone,
ondansetron, and tianeptine. I. Social interactions. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 56:287–293
Czobor P, Volavka J, Meibach RC (1995) Effect of risperidone on hostility in schizophrenia.
J Clin Psychopharmacol 15:243–249
D’Adamo P, Welzl H, Papadimitriou S, Raffaele dB, Tiveron C, Tatangelo L, Pozzi L, Chapman PF,
Knevett SG, Ramsay MF, Valtorta F, Leoni C, Menegon A, Wolfer DP, Lipp HP, Toniolo D
(2002) Deletion of the mental retardation gene Gdi1 impairs associative memory and alters
social behavior in mice. Hum Mol Genet 11:2567–2580
D’Anna KL, Stevenson SA, Gammie SC (2005) Urocortin 1 and 3 impair maternal defense
behavior in mice. Behav Neurosci 119:1061–1071
DaVanzo JP, Daugherty M, Ruckart R, Kang L (1966) Pharmacological and biochemical studies
in isolation-induced fighting mice. Psychopharmacologia 9:210–219
Davidson RJ, Putnam KM, Larson CL (2000) Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion
regulation–a possible prelude to violence. Science 289:591–594
de Almeida RM, Rosa MM, Santos DM, Saft DM, Benini Q, Miczek KA (2006) 5-HT1B
receptors, ventral orbitofrontal cortex, and aggressive behavior in mice. Psychopharmacology
185:441–450
de Almeida RMM, Faccidomo S, Fish E, Miczek KA (2001a) Inhibition of alcohol-heightened
aggression by action at post-synaptic 5-HT1b receptors in male mice. Aggress Behav
3:234–235
de Almeida RMM, Giovenardi M, da Silva SP, de Oliveira VP, Stein DJ (2005) Maternal
aggression in Wistar rats: effect of 5-HT2A/2C receptor agonist and antagonist microinjected
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 121
into the dorsal periaqueductal gray matter and medial septum. Braz J Med Biol Res
38:597–602
de Almeida RMM, Lucion A (1994) Effects of intracerebroventricular administration of 5-HT
receptor agonists on the maternal aggression of rats. Eur J Neurosci 264:445–448
de Almeida RMM, Lucion AB (1997) 8-OH-DPAT in the median raphe, dorsal periaqueductal
gray and corticomedial amygdala nucleus decreases, but the medial septal area it can increase
maternal aggressive behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology 134:392–400
de Almeida RMM, Miczek KA (2002) Aggression escalated by social instigation or by
discontinuation of reinforcement (‘‘frustration’’) in mice: inhibition by anpirtoline—a 5-HT1B
receptor agonist. Neuropsychopharmacology 27:171–181
de Almeida RMM, Nikulina EM, Faccidomo S, Fish EW, Miczek KA (2001b) Zolmitriptan–a
5-HT1B/D agonist, alcohol, and aggression in mice. Psychopharmacology 157:131–141
de Boer SF, Koolhaas JM (2005) 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptor agonists and aggression:
a pharmacological challenge of the serotonin deficiency hypothesis. Eur J Pharmacol
526:125–139
de Boer SF, Lesourd M, Mocaer E, Koolhaas JM (1999) Selective antiaggressive effects of
alnespirone in resident-intruder test are mediated via 5-hydroxytryptamine1A receptors:
a comparative pharmacological study with 8-hydroxy-2-dipropylaminotetralin, ipsapirone,
buspirone, eltoprazine, and WAY-100635. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 288:1125–1133
de Boer SF, Lesourd M, Mocaer E, Koolhaas JM (2000) Somatodendritic 5-HT1A autoreceptors
mediate the anti-aggressive actions of 5-HT1A receptor agonists in rats: An ethopharmaco-
logical study with S-15535, alnespirone, and WAY-100635. Neuropsychopharmacology
23:20–33
De Deyn PP, Rabheru K, Rasmussen A, Bocksberger JP, Dautzenberg PL, Eriksson S, Lawlor BA
(1999) A randomized trial of risperidone, placebo, and haloperidol for behavioral symptoms of
dementia. Neurology 53:946–955
De Felipe C, Herrero JF, O’Brien JA, Palmer JA, Doyle CA, Smith AJH, Laird JMA, Belmonte C,
Cervero F, Hunt SP (1998) Altered nocieption, analgesia and aggression in mice lacking the
receptor for substance P. Nature 392:394–397
De Groote L, Olivier B, Westenberg HG (2003) Role of 5-HT1B receptors in the regulation of
extracellular serotonin and dopamine in the dorsal striatum of mice. Eur J Pharmacol
476:71–77
DeBold JF, Miczek KA (1981) Sexual dimorphism in the hormonal control of aggressive
behavior of rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 14:89–93
Dekeyne A, Brocco M, Adhumeau A, Gobert A, Millan MJ (2000) The selective serotonin
5-HT1A receptor ligand, S15535, displays anxiolytic-like effects in the social interaction and
Vogel models and suppresses dialysate levels of 5-HT in the dorsal hippocampus of freely-
moving rats. A comparison with other anxiolytic agents. Psychopharmacology 152:55–66
Del Punta K, Leinders-Zufall T, Rodriguez I, Jukam D, Wysocki CJ, Ogawa S, Zufall F,
Mombaerts P (2002) Deficient pheromone responses in mice lacking a cluster of vomeronasal
receptor genes. Nature 419:70–74
Deltheil T, Guiard BP, Guilloux JP, Nicolas L, Delomenie C, Reperant C, Le Maitre E, Leroux-
Nicollet I, Benmansour S, Coudore F, David DJ, Gardier AM (2008) Consequences of
changes in BDNF levels on serotonin neurotransmission, 5-HT transporter expression and
function: studies in adult mice hippocampus. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 90:174–183
Delville Y, Mansour KM, Ferris CF (1996) Serotonin blocks vasopressin-facilitated offensive
aggression: interactions within the ventrolateral hypothalamus of golden hamsters. Physiol
Behav 59:813–816
Demas GE, Kriegsfeld LJ, Blackshaw S, Huang P, Gammie SC, Nelson RJ, Snyder SH (1999)
Elimination of aggressive behavior in male mice lacking endothelial nitric oxide synthase.
J Neurosci 19:1–5
Denney RM, Koch H, Craig IW (1999) Association between monoamine oxidase A activity in
human male shin fibroblasts and genotype of the MAOA promoter-associated variable
number tandem repeat. Hum Genet 105:542–551
122 A. Takahashi et al.
DeVries AC, Young WS, Nelson RJ (1997) Reduced aggressive behaviour in mice with targeted
disruption of the oxytocin gene. J Neuroendocrinol 9:363–368
Dompert WU, Glaser T, Traber J (1985) 3H-TVX Q 7821: identification of 5-HT1 binding sites as
target for a novel putative anxiolytic. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol 328:467–470
Duysen EG, Stribley JA, Fry DL, Hinrichs SH, Lockridge O (2002) Rescue of the
acetylcholinesterase knockout mouse by feeding a liquid diet; phenotype of the adult
acetylcholinesterase deficient mouse. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 137:43–54
Egan MF, Kojima M, Callicott JH, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Bertolino A, Zaitsev E, Gold B,
Goldman D, Dean M, Lu B, Weinberger DR (2003) The BDNF val66met polymorphism
affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function.
Cell 112:257–269
Eibl-Eibesfeldt I (1950) Beiträge zur Biologie der Haus- und der Ährenmaus nebst einigen
Beobachtungen an anderen Nagern. Z Tierpsychol 7:558–587
Elkabir DR, Wyatt ME, Vellucci SV, Herbert J (1990) The effects of separate or combined
infusions of corticotrophin-releasing factor and vasopressin either intraventricularly or into
the amygdala on aggressive and investigative behaviour in the rat. Regul Pept 28:199–214
Emeson RB, Morabito MV (2005) Food fight: the NPY-serotonin link between aggression and
feeding behavior. Sci STKE 2005:pe12
Faccidomo S, Bannai M, Miczek KA (2008) Escalated aggression after alcohol drinking in male
mice: dorsal raphe and prefrontal cortex serotonin and 5-HT1B receptors. Neuropsychophar-
macology 33:2888–2899
Faccidomo S, Quadros IM, Takahashi A, Fish EW, Miczek KA (2012) Infralimbic and dorsal
raphé microinjection of the 5-HT1B receptor agonist CPG–93,129: attenuation of aggressive
behavior in CFW male mice. Psychopharmacology. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2629-1 (pub-
lished online 12 January 2012)
Fairbanks LA, Fontenot MB, Phillips-Conroy JE, Jolly CJ, Kaplan JR, Mann JJ (1999) CSF
monoamines, age and impulsivity in wild grivet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops aethiops).
Brain Behav Evol 53:305–312
Fairbanks LA, Melega WP, Jorgensen MJ, Kaplan JR, McGuire MT (2001) Social impulsivity
inversely associated with CSF 5-HIAA and fluoxetine exposure in vervet monkeys.
Neuropsychopharmacology 24:370–378
Farrokhi C, Blanchard DC, Griebel G, Yang M, Gonzales C, Markham C, Blanchard RJ (2004)
Effects of the CRF1 antagonist SSR125543A on aggressive behaviors in hamsters. Pharmacol
Biochem Behav 77:465–469
Fava M (1997) Psychopharmacologic treatment of pathologic aggression. Psychiatr Clin North
Am 20:427–451
Ferrari PF, Parmigiani S, Rodgers RJ, Palanza P (1997) Differential effects of chlordiazepoxide
on aggressive behavior in male mice: the influence of social factors. Psychopharmacology
134:258–265
Ferrari PF, Van Erp AMM, Tornatzky W, Miczek KA (2003) Accumbal dopamine and serotonin
in anticipation of the next aggressive episode in rats. Eur J Neurosci 17:371–378
Ferris CF, Lu SF, Messenger T, Guillon CD, Heindel N, Miller M, Koppel G, Robert BF, Simon NG
(2006) Orally active vasopressin V1a receptor antagonist, SRX251, selectively blocks
aggressive behavior. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 83:169–174
Ferris CF, Melloni RH, Koppel G, Perry KW, Fuller RW, Delville Y (1997) Vasopressin/
serotonin interactions in the anterior hypothalamus control aggressive behavior in golden
hamsters. J Neurosci 17:4331–4340
Ferris CF, Pilapil CG, Hayden-Hixson D, Wiley RG, Koh ET (1992) Functionally and
anatomically distinct populations of vasopressinergic magnocellular neurons in the female
golden hamster. J Neuroendocrinol 4:193–205
Ferris CF, Potegal M (1988) Vasopressin receptor blockade in the anterior hypothalamus
suppresses aggression in hamsters. Physiol Behav 44:235–239
Ferris CF, Stolberg T, Delville Y (1999) Serotonin regulation of aggressive behavior in male
golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). Behav Neurosci 113:804–815
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 123
Ferris CF, Stolberg T, Kulkarni P, Murugavel M, Blanchard R, Blanchard DC, Febo M, Brevard M,
Simon NG (2008) Imaging the neural circuitry and chemical control of aggressive motivation.
BMC Neurosci 9:111
Fischer HS, Zernig G, Schuligoi R, Miczek KA, Hauser KF, Gerard C, Saria A (2000) Alterations
within the endogenous opioid system in mice with targeted deletion of the neutral
endopeptidase (‘‘enkephalinase’’) gene. Regul Pept 96:53–58
Fish EW, Faccidomo S, DeBold JF, Miczek KA (2001) Alcohol, allopregnanolone and
aggression in mice. Psychopharmacology 153:473–483
Fish EW, Faccidomo S, Miczek KA (1999) Aggression heightened by alcohol or social instigation in
mice: reduction by the 5-HT1B receptor agonist CP-94,253. Psychopharmacology 146:391–399
Foley DL, Eaves LJ, Wormley B, Silberg JL, Maes HH, Kuhn J, Riley B (2004) Childhood
adversity, monoamine oxidase A genotype, and risk for conduct disorder. Arch Gen Psychiat
61:738–744
Forster GL, Pringle RB, Mouw NJ, Vuong SM, Watt MJ, Burke AR, Lowry CA, Summers CH,
Renner KJ (2008) Corticotropin-releasing factor in the dorsal raphe nucleus increases medial
prefrontal cortical serotonin via type 2 receptors and median raphe nucleus activity. Eur J
Neurosci 28:299–310
Frazzetto G, Di Lorenzo G, Carola V, Proietti L, Sokolowska E, Siracusano A, Gross C, Troisi A
(2007) Early trauma and increased risk for physical aggression during adulthood: the
moderating role of MAOA genotype. PLoS ONE 2:e486
Fresan A, Camarena B, Apiquian R, Aguilar A, Urraca N, Nicolini H (2007) Association study of
MAO-A and DRD4 genes in schizophrenic patients with aggressive behavior. Neuropsycho-
biology 55:171–175
Fuller RW (1996) The influence of fluoxetine on aggressive behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology
14:77–81
Gallager DW, Aghajanian GK (1976) Effect of antipsychotic drugs on the firing of dorsal raphe
cells. II. Reversal by picrotoxin. Eur J Pharmacol 39:357–364
Gammie SC, Bethea ED, Stevenson SA (2007) Altered maternal profiles in corticotropin-
releasing factor receptor 1 deficient mice. BMC Neurosci 8:17
Gammie SC, Hasen NS, Stevenson SA, Bale TL, D’Anna KL (2005) Elevated stress sensitivity in
corticotropin-releasing factor receptor 2 deficient mice decreases maternal, but not intermale
aggression. Behav Brain Res 160:169–177
Gammie SC, Negron A, Newman SM, Rhodes JS (2004) Corticotropin-releasing factor inhibits
maternal aggression in mice. Behav Neurosci 118:805–814
Gammie SC, Nelson RJ (1999) Maternal aggression is reduced in neuronal nitric oxide synthase-
deficient mice. J Neurosci 19:8027–8035
Gammie SC, Stevenson SA (2006) Intermale aggression in corticotropin-releasing factor receptor 1
deficient mice. Behav Brain Res 171:63–69
Garattini S, Giacalone E, Valzelli L (1967) Isolation, aggressiveness and brain 5-hydroxy-
tryptamine turnover. J Pharm Pharmacol 19:338–339
Gervasoni D, Peyron C, Rampon C, Barbagli B, Chouvet G, Urbain N, Fort P, Luppi PH (2000)
Role and origin of the GABAergic innervation of dorsal raphe serotonergic neurons.
J Neurosci 20:4217–4225
Giancola PR, Levinson CA, Corman MD, Godlaski AJ, Morris DH, Phillips JP, Holt JC (2009)
Men and women, alcohol and aggression. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 17:154–164
Giménez-Llort L, Fernández-Teruel A, Escorihuela RM, Fredholm BB, Tobeña A, Pekny M,
Johansson B (2002) Mice lacking the adenosine A1 receptor are anxious and aggressive, but
are normal learners with reduced muscle strength and survival rate. Eur J Neurosci 16:
547–550
Gobert A, Rivet JM, Audinot V, Newman-Tancredi A, Cistarelli L, Millan MJ (1998)
Simultaneous quantification of serotonin, dopamine and noradrenaline levels in single frontal
cortex dialysates of freely-moving rats reveals a complex pattern of reciprocal auto- and
heteroreceptor-mediated control of release. Neuroscience 84:413–429
124 A. Takahashi et al.
Higley JD, Suomi SJ (1986) Parental behavior in primates. In: Sluckin W (ed) Parental behavior
in animals and humans. Blackwell Press, Oxford, pp 152–207
Higley JD, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M (1991) CSF monoamine metabolite concentrations vary
according to age, rearing, and sex, and are influenced by the stressor of social separation in
rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology 103:551–556
Hjorth S, Sharp T (1991) Effect of the 5-HT1A receptor agonist 8-OH-DPAT on the release of
5-HT in dorsal and median raphe-innervated rat brain regions as measured by in vivo
microdialysis. Life Sci 48:1779–1786
Ho HP, Olsson M, Westberg L, Melke J, Eriksson E (2001) The serotonin reuptake inhibitor
fluoxetine reduces sex steroid-related aggression in female rats: an animal model of
premenstrual irritability? Neuropsychopharmacology 24:502–510
Hollander E (1999) Managing aggressive behavior in patients with obsessive-compulsive
disorder and borderline personality disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 60(Suppl 15):38–44
Holmes A, Murphy DL, Crawley JN (2002) Reduced aggression in mice lacking the serotonin
transporter. Psychopharmacology 161:160–167
Homberg JR (2007) Serotonin transporter deficiency in rats improves inhibitory control but not
behavioural flexibility. Eur J Neurosci 26:2066–2073
Huang PL, Dawson TM, Bredt DS, Snyder SH, Fishman MC (1993) Targeted disruption of the
neuronal nitric oxide synthase gene. Cell 75:1273–1286
Huang YY, Grailhe R, Arango V, Hen R, Mann JJ (1999) Relationship of psychopathology to the
human serotonin1B genotype and receptor binding kinetics in postmortem brain tissue.
Neuropsychopharmacology 21:238–246
Hurst JL (1987) Behavioral variation in wild house mice Mus domesticus Rutty: a quantitative
assessment of female social organization. Anim Behav 35:1846–1857
Innis RB, Aghajanian GK (1987) Pertussis toxin blocks 5-HT1A and GABAB receptor-mediated
inhibition of serotonergic neurons. Eur J Pharmacol 143:195–204
Ito W, Chehab M, Thakur S, Li J, Morozov A (2011) BDNF-restricted knockout mice as an
animal model for aggression. Genes Brain Behav 10:365–374
Jacobs BL, Cohen A (1976) Differential behavioral effects of lesions of the median or dorsal
raphe nuclei in rats: open field and pain-elicited aggression. J Comp Physiol Psychol 90:
102–108
Jarrell H, Hoffman JB, Kaplan JR, Berga S, Kinkead B, Wilson ME (2008) Polymorphisms in the
serotonin reuptake transporter gene modify the consequences of social status on metabolic
health in female rhesus monkeys. Physiol Behav 93:807–819
Jensen KP, Covault J, Conner TS, Tennen H, Kranzler HR, Furneaux HM (2009) A common
polymorphism in serotonin receptor 1B mRNA moderates regulation by miR-96 and
associates with aggressive human behaviors. Mol Psychiatry 14:381–389
Johnson DE, Rollema H, Schmidt AW, McHarg AD (2001) Serotonergic effects and extracellular
brain levels of eletriptan, zolmitriptan and sumatriptan in rat brain. Eur J Pharmacol 425:203–210
Johnson O, Becnel J, Nichols CD (2009) Serotonin 5-HT2 and 5-HT1A-like receptors
differentially modulate aggressive behaviors in Drosophila melanogaster. Neuroscience 158:
1292–1300
Jonsson EG, Goldman D, Spurlock G, Gustavsson JP, Nielsen DA, Linnoila M, Owen MJ,
Sedvall GC (1997) Tryptophan hydroxylase and catechol-O-methyltransferase gene poly-
morphisms: relationships to monoamine metabolite concentrations in CSF of healthy
volunteers. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 247:297–302
Joppa MA, Rowe RK, Meisel RL (1997) Effects of serotonin 1A or 1B receptor agonists on social
aggression in male and female Syrian hamsters. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 58:349–353
Judge SJ, Ingram CD, Gartside SE (2004) GABA receptor modulation of 5-HT neuronal firing:
characterization and effect of moderate in vivo variations in glucocorticoid levels. Neurochem
Int 45:1057–1065
Kalen P, Pritzel M, Nieoullon A, Wiklund L (1986) Further evidence for excitatory amino acid
transmission in the lateral habenular projection to the rostral raphe nuclei: lesion-induced
decrease of high affinity glutamate uptake. Neurosci Lett 68:35–40
126 A. Takahashi et al.
Kreiss DS, Lucki I (1994) Differential regulation of serotonin (5-HT) release in the striatum and
hippocampus by 5-HT1A autoreceptors of the dorsal and median raphe nuclei. J Pharmacol
Exp Ther 269:1268–1279
Krsiak M (1974) Isolation-induced timidity in mice as a measure of anxiolytic activity of drugs.
Act Nerv Super (Praha) 16:241–242
Kushi A, Sasai H, Koizumi H, Takeda N, Yokoyama M, Nakamura M (1998) Obesity and mild
hyperinsulinemia found in neuropeptide Y-Y1 receptor-deficient mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA 95:15659–15664
Lamar M, Cutter WJ, Rubia K, Brammer M, Daly EM, Craig MC, Cleare AJ, Murphy DG (2009)
5-HT, prefrontal function and aging: fMRI of inhibition and acute tryptophan depletion.
Neurobiol Aging 30:1135–1146
Lang A, Harro J, Soosaar A, Koks S, Volke V, Oreland L, Bourin M, Vasar E, Bradwejn J,
Mannisto PT (1995) Role of N-methyl-D-aspartic acid and cholecystokinin receptors in
apomorphine-induced aggressive behaviour in rats. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol
351:363–370
Lappalainen J, Long JC, Eggert M, Ozaki N, Robin RW, Brown GL, Naukkarinen H, Virkkunen M,
Linnoila M, Goldman D (1998) Linkage of antisocial alcoholism to the serotonin 5-HT1B
receptor gene in 2 populations. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:989–994
Ledent C, Vaugeois JM, Schiffmann SN, Pedrazzini T, El Yacoubi M, Vanderhaeghen JJ,
Costentin J, Heath JK, Vassart G, Parmentier M (1997) Aggressiveness, hypoalgesia and high
blood pressure in mice lacking the adenosine A2a receptor. Nature 388:674–678
Lee R, Ferris C, Van de Kar LD, Coccaro EF (2009) Cerebrospinal fluid oxytocin, life history of
aggression, and personality disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34:1567–1573
Lerch-Haner JK, Frierson D, Crawford LK, Beck SG, Deneris ES (2008) Serotonergic
transcriptional programming determines maternal behavior and offspring survival. Nat
Neurosci 11:1001–1003
Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, Benjamin J, Mueller CR, Hamer DH,
Murphy DL (1996) Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin
transporter gene regulatory region. Science 264:1537–1551
Lesch KP, Merschdorf U (2000) Impulsivity, aggression, and serotonin: a molecular
psychobiological perspective. Behav Sci Law 18:581–604
Lesch KP, Meyer J, Glatz K, Flugge G, Hinney A, Hebebrand J, Klauck SM, Poustka A, Poustka F,
Bengel D, Mossner R, Riederer P, Heils A (1997) The 5-HT transporter gene-linked
polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) in evolutionary perspective: alternative biallelic variation
in rhesus monkeys. J Neural Transm 104:1259–1266
Leyhausen P (1979) Cat behavior: predatory and social behavior of domestic and wild cats.
Garland STPM Press, New York
Lin YMJ, Chao SC, Chen TM, Lai TJ, Chen JS, Sun HS (2007) Association of functional
polymorphisms of the human tryptophan hydroxylase 2 gene with risk for bipolar disorder in
Han Chinese. Arch Gen Psychiatry 64:1015–1024
Lindgren T, Kantak KM (1987) Effects of serotonin receptor agonists and antagonists on
offensive aggression in mice. Aggress Behav 13:87–96
Linnoila M, Virkkunen M, Scheinin M, Nuutila A, Rimon R, Goodwin FK (1983) Low
cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentration differentiates impulsive from
nonimpulsive violent behavior. Life Sci 33:2609–2614
Linnoila VMI, Virkkunen M (1992) Aggression, suicidality, and serotonin. J Clin Psychiatry
53:46–51
Loconto J, Papes F, Chang E, Stowers L, Jones EP, Takada T, Kumanovics A, Fischer LK, Dulac C
(2003) Functional expression of murine V2R pheromone receptors involves selective
association with the M10 and M1 families of MHC class Ib molecules. Cell 112:607–618
Lonstein JS, Gammie SC (2002) Sensory, hormonal, and neural control of maternal aggression in
laboratory rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26:869–888
Lowry CA, Rodda JE, Lightman SL, Ingram CD (2000) Corticotropin-releasing factor increases
in vitro firing rates of serotonergic neurons in the rat dorsal raphe nucleus: evidence for
128 A. Takahashi et al.
McBride PA, Brown RP, DeMeo M, Keilp J, Mieczkowski T, Mann JJ (1994) The relationship of
platelet 5-HT2 receptor indices to major depressive disorder, personality traits, and suicidal
behavior. Biol Psychiatry 35:295–308
McKenzie-Quirk SD, Girasa KA, Allan AM, Miczek KA (2005) 5-HT3 receptors, alcohol and
aggressive behavior in mice. Behav Pharmacol 16:163–170
McKinley JC, Hathaway SR, Meehl PE (1948) The Minnesota multiphasic personality inventory:
the K-scale. J Consult Psychol 12:20–31
McMillen BA, DaVanzo EA, Scott SM, Song AH (1988) N-alkyl-substituted aryl-piperazine
drugs: relationship between affinity for serotonin receptors and inhibition of aggression. Drug
Dev Res 12:53–62
Mehlman PT, Higley JD, Faucher I, Lilly AA, Taub DM, Vickers J, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M (1994)
Low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations and severe aggression and impaired impulse control in
nonhuman primates. Am J Psychiatry 151:1485–1491
Meloni EG, Reedy CL, Cohen BM, Carlezon WA Jr (2008) Activation of raphe efferents to the
medial prefrontal cortex by corticotropin-releasing factor: correlation with anxiety-like
behavior. Biol Psychiatry 63:832–839
Mendoza DL, Bravo HA, Swanson HH (1999) Antiaggresive and anxiolytic effects of gepirone in
mice, and their attenuation by WAY 100635. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 62:499–509
Meyer JH, Wilson AA, Rusjan P, Clark M, Houle S, Woodside S, Arrowood J, Martin K,
Colleton M (2008) Serotonin2A receptor binding potential in people with aggressive and
violent behaviour. J Psychiatry Neurosci 33:499–508
Meyer-Lindenberg A, Buckholtz JW, Kolachana B, Hariri AR, Pezawas L, Blasi G, Wabnitz A,
Honea R, Verchinski B, Callicott JH, Egan M, Mattay V, Weinberger DR (2006) Neural
mechanisms of genetic risk for impulsivity and violence in humans. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA
103:6269–6274
Miczek KA (1974) Intraspecies aggression in rats: effects of d-amphetamine and chlordiazep-
oxide. Psychopharmacologia 39:275–301
Miczek KA (1987) The psychopharmacology of aggression. In: Iversen LL, Iversen SD, Snyder SH
(eds) Handbook of psychopharmacology, vol 19. New directions in behavioral pharmacology.
Plenum, New York, pp 183–328
Miczek KA, Barros HM, Sakoda L, Weerts EM (1998a) Alcohol and heightened aggression in
individual mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 22:1698–1705
Miczek KA, de Almeida RMM (2001) Oral drug self-administration in the home cage of mice:
alcohol-heightened aggression and inhibition by the 5-HT1B agonist anpirtoline. Psycho-
pharmacology 157:421–429
Miczek KA, de Almeida RMM, Kravitz EA, Rissman EF, de Boer SF, Raine A (2007)
Neurobiology of escalated aggression and violence. J Neurosci 27:11803–11806
Miczek KA, Faccidomo S, de Almeida RMM, Bannai M, Fish EW, DeBold JF (2004) Escalated
aggressive behavior: new pharmacotherapeutic approaches and opportunities. Ann N Y Acad
Sci 1036:336–355
Miczek KA, Fish EW, De Bold JF (2003) Neurosteroids, GABAA receptors, and escalated
aggressive behavior. Horm Behav 44:242–257
Miczek KA, Fish EW, DeBold JF, de Almeida RMM (2002) Social and neural determinants of
aggressive behavior: pharmacotherapeutic targets at serotonin, dopamine and c-aminobutyric
acid systems. Psychopharmacology 163:434–458
Miczek KA, Haney M (1994) Psychomotor stimulant effects of d-amphetamine, MDMA and PCP:
aggressive and schedule-controlled behavior in mice. Psychopharmacology 115:358–365
Miczek KA, Hussain S, Faccidomo S (1998b) Alcohol-heightened aggression in mice:
attenuation by 5-HT1A receptor agonists. Psychopharmacology 139:160–168
Miczek KA, Maxson SC, Fish EW, Faccidomo S (2001) Aggressive behavioral phenotypes in
mice. Behav Brain Res 125:167–181
Miczek KA, Nikulina EM, Takahashi A, Covington HE, III, Yap JJ, Boyson CO, Shimamoto A,
de Almeida RM (2011) Gene expression in aminergic and peptidergic cells during aggression
and defeat: relevance to violence, depression and drug abuse. Behav Genet, Online First
130 A. Takahashi et al.
Miczek KA, O’Donnell JM (1978) Intruder-evoked aggression in isolated and nonisolated mice:
effects of psychomotor stimulants and L-dopa. Psychopharmacology 57:47–55
Miczek KA, O’Donnell JM (1980) Alcohol and chlordiazepoxide increase suppressed aggression
in mice. Psychopharmacology 69:39–44
Miczek KA, Weerts EM, Tornatzky W, DeBold JF, Vatne TM (1992) Alcohol and ‘‘bursts’’ of
aggressive behavior: ethological analysis of individual differences in rats. Psychopharmacology
107:551–563
Mitchell PJ (2005) Antidepressant treatment and rodent aggressive behaviour. Eur J Pharmacol
526:147–162
Mitchell PJ, Fletcher A, Redfern PH (1991) Is antidepressant efficacy revealed by drug-induced
changes in rat behaviour exhibited during social interaction? Neurosci Biobehav Rev
15:539–544
Mitchell PJ, Redfern PH (1992) Acute and chronic antidepressant drug treatments induce
opposite effects in the social behavior of rats. J Psychopharmacol 6:241–257
Mitchell PJ, Redfern PH (1997) Potentiation of the time-dependent, antidepressant-induced
changes in the agonistic behaviour of resident rats by the 5-HT1A receptor antagonist, WAY-
100635. Behav Pharmacol 8:585–606
Miyakawa T, Yagi T, Takao K, Niki H (2001) Differential effect of Fyn tyrosine kinase deletion
on offensive and defensive aggression. Behav Brain Res 122:51–56
Morgan C, Thomas RE, Ma W, Novotny MV, Cone RD (2004) Melanocortin-5 receptor
deficiency reduces a pheromonal signal for aggression in male mice. Chem Senses 29:
111–115
Mos J, Olivier B (1989) Quantitative and comparative analyses of pro-aggressive actions of
benzodiazepines in maternal aggression of rats. Psychopharmacology 97:152–153
Mos J, Olivier B, Poth M, Van Oorschot R, Van Aken H (1993) The effects of dorsal raphé
administration of eltoprazine, TFMPP and 8-OH-DPAT on resident intruder aggression in the
rat. Eur J Pharmacol 238:411–415
Muehlenkamp F, Lucion A, Vogel WH (1995) Effects of selective serotenergic agonists on
aggressive behavior in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 50:671–674
Murphy DL, Lesch KP (2008) Targeting the murine serotonin transporter: insights into human
neurobiology. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:85–96
Musty RE, Consroe PF (1982) Phencyclidine produces aggressive behavior in rapid eye
movement sleep- deprived rats. Life Sci 30:1733–1738
Nagtegaal MH, Rassin E (2004) The usefulness of the thought suppression paradigm in
explaining impulsivity and aggression. Pers Individ Dif 37:1233–1244
Nanopoulos D, Belin MF, Maitre M, Vincendon G, Pujol JF (1982) Immunocytochemical
evidence for the existence of GABAergic neurons in the nucleus raphe dorsalis. Possible
existence of neurons containing serotonin and GABA. Brain Res 232:375–389
Natarajan D, Caramaschi D (2010) Animal violence demystified. Front. Behav Neurosci 4:9
Nelson RJ, Chiavegatto S (2001) Molecular basis of aggression. Trends Neurosci 24:713–719
Nelson RJ, Demas GE, Huang PL, Fishman MC, Dawson VL, Dawson TM, Snyder SH (1995)
Behavioural abnormalities in male mice lacking neuronal nitric oxide synthase. Nature
378:383–386
Neumann ID, Veenema AH, Beiderbeck DI (2010) Aggression and anxiety: social context and
neurobiological links. Front Behav Neurosci 4:12
Neves-Pereira M, Mundo E, Muglia P, King N, Macciardi F, Kennedy JL (2002) The brain-
derived neurotrophic factor gene confers susceptibility to bipolar disorder: evidence from a
family-based association study. Am J Hum Gene 71:651–655
New AS, Buchsbaum MS, Hazlett EA, Goodman M, Koenigsberg HW, Lo J, Iskander L,
Newmark R, Brand J, O’Flynn K, Siever LJ (2004) Fluoxetine increases relative metabolic
rate in prefrontal cortex in impulsive aggression. Psychopharmacology 176:451–458
New AS, Gelernter J, Goodman M, Mitropoulou V, Koenigsberg H, Silverman J, Siever LJ
(2001) Suicide, impulsive aggression, and HTR1B genotype. Biol Psychiatry 50:62–65
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 131
New AS, Gelernter J, Yovell Y, Trestman RL, Nielsen DA, Silverman J, Mitropoulou V, Siever LJ
(1998) Tryptophan hydroxylase genotype is associated with impulsive-aggression measures: a
preliminary study. Am J Med Genet 81:13–17
Newman TK, Syagailo YV, Barr CS, Wendland JR, Champoux M, Graessle M, Suomi SJ, Higley JD,
Lesch KP (2005) Monoamine oxidase A gene promoter variation and rearing experience
influences aggressive behavior in rhesus monkeys. Biol Psychiatry 57:167–172
Nicot A, Otto T, Brabet P, Dicicco-Bloom EM (2004) Altered social behavior in pituitary
adenylate cyclase-activating polypeptide type I receptor-deficient mice. J Neurosci
24:8786–8795
Nielsen DA, Goldman D, Virkkunen M, Tokola R, Rawlings R, Linnoila M (1994) Suicidality
and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentration associated with a tryptophan-hydroxylase
polymorphism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51:34–38
Nikulina EM, Avgustinovich DF, Popova NK (1992) Role of 5HT1A receptors in a variety of
kinds of aggressive behavior in wild rats and counterparts selected for low defensiveness
towards man. Aggress Behav 18:357–364
Nogueira RL, Graeff FG (1995) Role of 5HT receptor subtypes in the modulation of dorsal
periaqueductal gray generated aversion. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 52:1–6
Noirot E, Goyens J, Buhot MC (1975) Aggressive behavior of pregnant mice towards males.
Horm Behav 6:9–17
Nomura M, Kusumi I, Kaneko M, Masui T, Daiguji M, Ueno T, Koyama T, Nomura Y (2006)
Involvement of a polymorphism in the 5-HT2A receptor gene in impulsive behavior.
Psychopharmacology 187:30–35
O’Donohue TL, Chronwall BM, Pruss RM, Mezey E, Kiss JZ, Eiden LE, Massari VJ, Tessel RE,
Pickel VM, DiMaggio DA (1985) Neuropeptide Y and peptide YY neuronal and endocrine
systems. Peptides 6:755–768
Oades RD, Lasky-Su J, Christiansen H, Faraone SV, Sonuga-Barke EJS, Banaschewski T, Chen W,
Anney RJL, Buitelaar JK, Ebstein RP, Franke B, Gill M, Miranda A, Roeyers H, Rothenberger
A, Sergeant JA, Steinhausen HC, Taylor EA, Thompson M, Asherson P (2008) The influence of
serotonin- and other genes on impulsive behavioral aggression and cognitive impulsivity in
children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): findings from a family-based
association test (FBAT) analysis. Behav Brain Funct 4:48
Ogawa S, Chan J, Chester AE, Gustafsson JA, Korach KS, Pfaff DW (1999) Survival of
reproductive behaviors in estrogen receptor b gene-deficient (bERKO) male and female mice.
Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 96:12887–12892
Ogawa S, Eng V, Taylor J, Lubahn DB, Korach KS, Pfaff DW (1998a) Roles of estrogen
receptor-a gene expression in reproduction-related behaviors in female mice. Endocrinology
139:5070–5081
Ogawa S, Washburn TF, Taylor J, Lubahn DB, Korach KS, Pfaff DW (1998b) Modifications of
testosterone-dependent behaviors by estrogen receptor-a gene disruption in male mice.
Endocrinology 139:5058–5069
Oliveira-dos-Santos AJ, Matsumoto G, Snow BE, Bai D, Houston FP, Whishaw IQ, Mariathasan S,
Sasaki T, Wakeham A, Ohashi PS, Roder JC, Barnes CA, Siderovski DP, Penninger JM (2000)
Regulation of T cell activation, anxiety, and male aggression by RGS2. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA
97:12272–12277
Olivier B (2004) Serotonin and aggression. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1036:382–392
Olivier B, Mos J, Rasmussen D (1990) Behavioural pharmacology of the serenic, eltoprazine.
Rev Drug Metab Drug Interact 8:31–83
Olivier B, Mos J, Tulp MTM, van der Poel AM (1992) Animal models of anxiety and aggression
in the study of serotonergic agents. In: Langer SZ (ed) Serotonin receptor subtypes:
pharmacological significance and clinical implications. Karger, Basel, pp 67–79
Olivier B, Mos J, Van der Heyden J, Hartog J (1989) Serotonergic modulation of social
interactions in isolated male mice. Psychopharmacology 97:154–156
Olivier B, Mos J, Van Oorschot R, Hen R (1995) Serotonin receptors and animal models of
aggressive behavior. Pharmacopsychiatry 28:80–90
132 A. Takahashi et al.
Oquendo MA, Russo SA, Underwood MD, Kassir SA, Ellis SP, Mann JJ, Arango V (2006)
Higher postmortem prefrontal 5-HT2A receptor binding correlates with lifetime aggression in
suicide. Biol Psychiatry 59:235–243
Osipova DV, Kulikov AV, Popova NK (2009) C1473G polymorphism in mouse tph2 gene is
linked to tryptophan hydroxylase-2 activity in the brain, intermale aggression, and depressive-
like behavior in the forced swim test. J Neurosci Res 87:1168–1174
Pabis DJ, Stanislav SW (1996) Pharmacotherapy of aggressive behavior. Ann Pharmacother
30:278–287
Palanza P, Della Seta D, Ferrari PF, Parmigiani S (2005) Female competition in wild house mice
depends upon timing of female/male settlement and kinship between females. Anim Behav
69:1259–1271
Pallotta M, Segieth J, Whitton PS (1998) N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors regulate 5-HT release in
the raphe nuclei and frontal cortex of freely moving rats: differential role of 5-HT1A
autoreceptors. Brain Res 783:173–178
Parmigiani S, Ferrari PF, Palanza P (1998) An evolutionary approach to behavioral
pharmacology: using drugs to understand proximate and ultimate mechanisms of different
forms of aggression in mice. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 23:143–153
Passamonti L, Cerasa A, Gioia MC, Magariello A, Muglia M, Quattrone A, Fera F (2008)
Genetically dependent modulation of serotonergic inactivation in the human prefrontal cortex.
Neuroimage 40:1264–1273
Peeke HVS, Figler MH (1981) Modulation of aggressive behavior in fish by alcohol and
congeners. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 14(Suppl 1):79–84
Pellis SM, Pellis VC (1988) Play-fighting in the Syrian golden hamster Mesocricetus auratus
Waterhouse, and its relationship to serious fighting during postweaning development. Dev
Psychobiol 21:323–337
Pineyro G, Blier P, Dennis T, de Montigny C (1994) Desensitization of the neuronal 5-HT carrier
following its long-term blockade. J Neurosci 14:3036–3047
Pinna G, Costa E, Guidotti A (2006) Fluoxetine and norfluoxetine stereospecifically and
selectively increase brain neurosteroid content at doses that are inactive on 5-HT reuptake.
Psychopharmacology 186:362–372
Pinna G, Dong E, Matsumoto K, Costa E, Guidotti A (2003) In socially isolated mice, the
reversal of brain allopregnanolone down-regulation mediates the anti-aggressive action of
fluoxetine. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:2035–2040
Popova NK, Avgustinovich DF, Kolpakov VG, Plyusnina IZ (1998) Specific [H-3]8-OH-DPAT
binding in brain regions of rats genetically predisposed to various defense behavior strategies.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav 59:793–797
Popova NK, Voitenko NN, Kulikov AV, Avgustinovich DF (1991) Evidence for the involvement
of central serotonin in mechanism of domestication of silver foxes. Pharmacol Biochem
Behav 40:751–756
Potegal M (1991) Attack priming and satiation in female golden hamsters: tests of some
alternatives to the aggression arousal interpretation. Aggress Behav 17:327–335
Potegal M, Tenbrink L (1984) Behavior of attack-primed and attack-satiated female golden
hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). J Comp Psychol 98:66–75
Potter E, Sutton S, Donaldson C, Chen R, Perrin M, Lewis K, Sawchenko PE, Vale W (1994)
Distribution of corticotropin-releasing factor receptor mRNA expression in the rat brain and
pituitary. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 91:8777–8781
Price ML, Curtis AL, Kirby LJ, Valentino RJ, Lucki I (1998) Effects of corticotropin-releasing
factor on brain serotonergic activity. Neuropsychopharmacology 18:492–502
Price ML, Lucki I (2001) Regulation of serotonin release in the lateral septum and striatum by
corticotropin-releasing factor. J Neurosci 21:2833–2841
Quadros IM, Miguel TT, DeBold JF, Miczek KA (2009a) Opposing action of CRF1 vs. CRF2
receptors in the dorsal raphé: modulation of alcohol-heightened aggression. 2009
Neuroscience meeting planner. Society for neuroscience, Chicago, Program No.445.5/T8
Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics 133
Quadros IM, Takahashi A, Miczek KA (2009b) Serotonin and aggression. In: Müller CP, Jacobs BL
(eds) Handbook of the behavioral neurobiology of serotonin. Academic Press, pp 687–714
Ragnauth AK, Devidze N, Moy V, Finley K, Goodwillie A, Kow LM, Muglia LJ, Pfaff DW
(2005) Female oxytocin gene-knockout mice, in a semi-natural environment, display
exaggerated aggressive behavior. Genes Brain Behav 4:229–239
Raine A (2002) Biosocial studies of antisocial and violent behavior in children and adults:
a review. J Abnorm Child Psychol 30:311–326
Raj YP (2004) Psychopharmacology of borderline personality disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep
6:225–231
Ramboz S, Saudou F, Amara DA, Belzung C, Segu L, Misslin R, Buhot MC, Hen R (1995)
5-HT1B receptor knock out: behavioral consequences. Behav Brain Res 73:305–312
Rasia-Filho AA, Giovenardi M, de Almeida RM (2008) Drugs and aggression. Recent Pat CNS
Drug Discov 3:40–49
Raskin K, de Gendt K, Duittoz A, Liere P, Verhoeven G, Tronche F, Mhaouty-Kodja S (2009)
Conditional inactivation of androgen receptor gene in the nervous system: effects on male
behavioral and neuroendocrine responses. J Neurosci 29:4461–4470
Ratey J, Sovner R, Parks A, Rogentine K (1991) Buspirone treatment of aggression and anxiety in
mentally retarded patients: a multiple-baseline, placebo lead-in study. J Clin Psychiatry
52:159–162
Reist C, Nakamura K, Sagart E, Sokolski KN, Fujimoto KA (2003) Impulsive aggressive
behavior: open-label treatment with citalopram. J Clin Psychiatry 64:81–85
Rewerski W, Kostowski W, Piechocki T, Rylski M (1971) The effects of some hallucinogens on
aggressiveness of mice and rats. I. Pharmacology 5:314–320
Ricci LA, Grimes JM, Melloni RH Jr (2004) Serotonin type 3 receptors modulate the aggression-
stimulating effects of adolescent cocaine exposure in Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus).
Behav Neurosci 118:1097–1110
Rios M, Lambe EK, Liu RJ, Teillon S, Liu JH, Akbarian S, Roffler-Tarlov S, Jaenisch R,
Aghajanian GK (2006) Severe deficits in 5-HT2A-mediated neurotransmission in BDNF
conditional mutant mice. J Neurobiol 66:408–420
Rocha BA, Scearce-Levie K, Lucas JJ, Hiroi N, Castanon N, Crabbe JC, Nestler EJ, Hen R
(1998) Increased vulnerability to cocaine in mice lacking the serotonin-1B receptor. Nature
393:175–178
Rodgers RJ, Waters AJ (1985) Benzodiazepines and their antagonists: a pharmacoethological
analysis with particular reference to effects on ‘‘aggression’’. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 9:21–35
Rodriguez-Arias M, Minarro J, Aguilar MA, Pinazo J, Simon VM (1998) Effects of risperidone
and SCH 23390 on isolation-induced aggression in male mice. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol
8:95–103
Rodriguiz RM, Chu R, Caron MG, Wetsel WC (2004) Aberrant responses in social interaction of
dopamine transporter knockout mice. Behav Brain Res 148:185–198
Rosell DR, Thompson JL, Slifstein M, Xu X, Frankle WG, New AS, Goodman M, Weinstein SR,
Laruelle M, Abi-Dargham A, Siever LJ (2010) Increased serotonin 2A receptor availability in
the orbitofrontal cortex of physically aggressive personality disordered patients. Biol
Psychiatry 67:1154–1162
Rudissaar R, Pruus K, Skrebuhhova T, Allikmets L, Matto V (1999) Modulatory role of 5- HT3
receptors in mediation of apomorphine-induced aggressive behaviour in male rats. Behav
Brain Res 106:91–96
Rujescu D, Giegling I, Bondy B, Gietl A, Zill P, Moller HJ (2002) Association of anger-related
traits with SNPs in the TPH gene. Mol Psychiatry 7:1023–1029
Rydén E, Thase ME, Straht D, Aberg-Wistedt A, Bejerot S, Landen M (2009) A history of
childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) impacts clinical outcome in adult
bipolar patients regardless of current ADHD. Acta Psychiatr Scand 120:239–246
Sabol SZ, Hu S, Hamer D (1998) A functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene
promoter. Hum Genet 103:273–279
134 A. Takahashi et al.
Spielberg CD, Sharma S, Singh M (1973) Development of Hindi edition of state-trait anxiety
inventory. Indian J Psychol 48:11–20
Spigset O (1999) Adverse reactions of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors—reports from a
spontaneous reporting system. Drug Saf 20:277–287
Sprouse JS, Aghajanian GK (1987) Electrophysiological responses of serotoninergic dorsal raphe
neurons to 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B agonists. Synapse 1:3–9
Steiniger F (1950) Beiträg zur Soziologie und sonstigen Biologie der Wanderratte. Z Tierpsychol
7:356–379
Stoff DM, Vitiello B (1996) Role of serotonin in aggression of children and adolesecents:
biochemical and phamacological studies. In: Stoff DM (ed) Aggression and violence: genetic.
Neurobiological and Biosocial Perspectives, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah,
pp 101–123
Stork O, Ji FY, Kaneko K, Stork S, Yoshinobu Y, Moriya T, Shibata S, Obata K (2000) Postnatal
development of a GABA deficit and disturbance of neural functions in mice lacking GAD65.
Brain Res 865:45–58
Stork O, Welzl H, Cremer H, Schachner M (1997) Increased intermale aggression and
neuroendocrine response in mice deficient for the neural cell adhesion molecule (NCAM). Eur
J Neurosci 9:1117–1125
Stowers L, Holy TE, Meister M, Dulac C, Koentges G (2002) Loss of sex discrimination and
male–male aggression in mice deficient for TRP2. Science 295:1493–1500
Sun HF, Chang YT, Fann CS, Chang CJ, Chen YH, Hsu YP, Yu WY, Cheng AT (2002)
Association study of novel human serotonin 5-HT1B polymorphisms with alcohol dependence
in Taiwanese Han. Biol Psychiatry 51:896–901
Swanson JW, Swartz MS, Van Dorn RA, Volavka J, Monahan J, Stroup TS, Mcevoy JP, Wagner HR,
Elbogen EB, Lieberman JA (2008) Comparison of antipsychotic medication effects on reducing
violence in people with schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 193:37–43
Swanson LW, Sawchenko PE, Rivier J, Vale WW (1983) Organization of ovine corticotropin-
releasing factor immunoreactive cells and fibers in the rat brain: an immunohistochemical
study. Neuroendocrinology 36:165–186
Takahashi A, Kwa C, DeBold JF, Miczek KA (2010a) GABAA receptors in the dorsal raphé
nucleus of mice: escalation of aggression after alcohol consumption. Psychopharmacology
211:467–477
Takahashi A, Quadros IM, de Almeida RM, Miczek KA (2011) Brain serotonin receptors and
transporters: initiation vs. termination of escalated aggression. Psychopharmacology
213:183–212
Takahashi A, Shimamoto A, Boyson CO, DeBold JF, Miczek KA (2010b) GABAB receptor
modulation of serotonin neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus and escalation of aggression in
mice. J Neurosci 30:11771–11780
Takayanagi Y, Yoshida M, Bielsky IF, Ross HE, Kawamata M, Onaka T, Yanagisawa T, Kimura T,
Matzuk MM, Young LJ, Nishimori K (2005) Pervasive social deficits, but normal parturition, in
oxytocin receptor-deficient mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:16096–16101
Tao R, Auerbach SB (2000) Regulation of serotonin release by GABA and excitatory amino
acids. J Psychopharmacol 14:100–113
Tao R, Ma Z, Auerbach SB (1996) Differential regulation of 5-hydroxytryptamine release by
GABAA and GABAB receptors in midbrain raphe nuclei and forebrain of rats. Br J Pharmacol
119:1375–1384
Taylor SP (1967) Aggressive behavior and physiological arousal as a function of provocation and
the tendency to inhibit aggression. J Personality 35:297–310
Tazi A, Dantzer R, Le Moal M, Rivier J, Vale W, Koob GF (1987) Corticotropin-releasing factor
antagonist blocks stress-induced fighting in rats. Regul Pep 18:37–42
Ten Eyck GR (2008) Serotonin modulates vocalizations and territorial behavior in an amphibian.
Behav Brain Res 193:144–147
Tompkins EC, Clemento AJ, Taylor DP, Perhach JL Jr (1980) Inhibition of aggressive behavior
in rhesus monkeys by buspirone. Res Commun Psychol Psychiatr Behav 5:337–352
136 A. Takahashi et al.
Abstract Stress related diseases such as depression and anxiety have a high degree
of co morbidity, and represent one of the greatest therapeutic challenges for
the twenty-first century. The present chapter will summarize existing rodent models
for research in psychiatry, mimicking depression- and anxiety-related diseases.
In particular we will highlight the use of selective breeding of rodents for extremes in
stress-related behavior. We will summarize major behavioral, neuroendocrine and
neuronal parameters, and pharmacological interventions, assessed in great detail
in two rat model systems: The Flinders Sensitive and Flinders Resistant Line rats
(FSL/FRL model), and rats selectively bred for high (HAB) or low (LAB) anxiety
related behavior (HAB/LAB model). Selectively bred rodents also provide an
excellent tool in order to study gene and environment interactions. Although it is
generally accepted that genes and environmental factors determine the etiology
of mental disorders, precise information is limited: How rigid is the genetic
disposition? How do genetic, prenatal and postnatal influences interact to shape adult
disease? Does the genetic predisposition determine the vulnerability to prenatal and
G. Wegener (&)
Centre for Psychiatric Research, Aarhus University Hospital,
8240 Risskov, Denmark
e-mail: wegener@dadlnet.dk
G. Wegener
Unit for Drug Research and Development, School of Pharmacy (Pharmacology),
North-West University, Potchefstroom, South Africa
A. A. Mathe
Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet,
Stockholm, Sweden
I. D. Neumann
Department of Behavioural and Molecular Neuroscience,
University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
Contents
1 Introduction........................................................................................................................ 140
2 Selectively Bred Models on Depression .......................................................................... 143
2.1 The Flinders Sensitive and Resistant Line Rat ....................................................... 143
2.2 Learned Helplessness Rats (cLH/cNLH) ................................................................. 155
2.3 Fawn Hooded Rats ................................................................................................... 155
2.4 Wistar-Kyoto Rats .................................................................................................... 156
2.5 Swim Low-Active/Swim High-Active Rats ............................................................ 156
2.6 High/Low Stress Reactivity Mice ............................................................................ 157
3 Selectively Bred Models on Anxiety................................................................................ 157
3.1 Rats Selectively HAB and LAB Anxiety-Related Behavior .................................. 157
3.2 HAB LAB Mice ....................................................................................................... 169
3.3 Floripa H and L Rats................................................................................................ 170
3.4 Maudsley Reactive and Nonreactive Rats ............................................................... 170
3.5 High/Low Avoidance Rats: RHA/RLA; SHA/Bru and SLA/Bru .......................... 171
4 Conclusions........................................................................................................................ 171
References................................................................................................................................ 172
1 Introduction
It has been estimated that 127 million Europeans out of a population of 466 million
currently live with a brain disorder, with total annual costs (of brain disorders in
Europe) of €386 billion in 2004 (Andlin-Sobocki et al. 2005). Of these, mental
disorders constitute about 60% of the total costs reflecting the large socioeconomic
burden of these diseases. These numbers greatly emphasize the importance of
developing new strategies in treating mental disorders.
Stress-related diseases such as depression and anxiety, having a high degree of
co-morbidity, represent one of the greatest therapeutic challenges for the twenty-
first century. Although it is generally accepted that genes and environmental
factors determine depression, precise information is limited: How rigid is the
genetic disposition? How do genetic, pre- and post-natal influences interact to
shape adult depression? Does the genetic predisposition determine the vulnera-
bility to pre- and post-natal or adult stressors?
When depression, and in some degree anxiety, precipitates, the dominating
etiological hypotheses have focused at a dysregulation in the serotonergic and
noradrenergic system. This is emphasized by the mechanism of action of the
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 141
The Flinders Line rats were established by selective breeding for differential
responses to the anticholinesterase agent, diisopropyl fluorophosphate (DFP), at
Flinders University in Adelaide, Australia. The original rationale was to breed a rat
strain that would be genetically resistant to irreversible anticholinesterase agents,
DFP. However, the selective breeding of Sprague–Dawley (SD) rats, resulted in a
line more sensitive to DFP, the Flinders Sensitive Line (FSL), whereas the Flinders
Resistant Line (FRL) rats were not more resistant than an outbred control
(Overstreet et al. 1979; Russell et al. 1982). Being less tolerant to DFP, the FSL rat
were also found to be more sensitive to drugs targeting the cholinergic system, in
particular effects of directly acting muscarinic receptor agonists (Russell and
Overstreet 1987; Overstreet and Russell 1982; Overstreet 1986) and to have more
muscarinic receptors in several brain regions (Overstreet and Russell 1984).
As it also was reported that depressed individuals were more sensitive to
cholinergic agonists than normal controls, defined by behavior, neuroendocrine
measures and sleep (Janowsky et al. 1980, 1994; Risch et al. 1981), it was sug-
gested that the FSL rat might be a model for depression.
Today there are now breeding colonies of the FSL rats in Australia, Canada,
Denmark, Greece, Israel, Mexico, South Africa, Sweden and United States.
depression can be reproduced in the FSL strain; the more salient characteristics are
shown in Table 1.
Depression-Related Behavior
Several observations of the unmotivated and motivated behavior of the FSL rat
suggest that it exhibits psychomotor retardation, a key behavioral characteristic of
depressed individuals (Lecrubier 2006). In particular, the FSL rat is less active in a
novel open field (Overstreet and Russell 1982; Overstreet et al. 1986), bar-presses
at a low rate for water or food reward (Overstreet and Russell 1982; Bushnell et al.
1995), and does not complete food-motivated nonmatching- to-sample learning
trials in a timely manner (Bushnell et al. 1995).
Importantly, the FSL rats show increased immobility in the forced swim test
(FST), which is the prototypic screening tool for depression-like behavior in
rodents (Overstreet and Russell 1982; Schiller et al. 1992; El Khoury et al. 2006).
Especially of interest for the predictive validity of the model, these behaviors are
reversible by chronic but not acute treatment with antidepressants.
Anhedonia, the inability to experience pleasure, is often regarded as a core
symptom of depression. Interestingly, under basal conditions the FSL compared to
FRL rats did not show signs of anhedonia, and signs of anhedonia were only found
when FSL rats were exposed to chronic mild stress (Pucilowski et al. 1993;
Matthews et al. 1996), supporting this model as being a candidate for Gene 9
Environment studies. We have replicated these findings, and found that group
housed FSL rats display a higher level of anhedonia following chronic mild stress
exposure, when compared with the FRL rats (Mathé et al. unpublished results).
A reduction in appetite is a classical symptom seen in most depressed indi-
viduals, while an increase in appetite and weight gain is observed in fewer.
Appetite and food intake in the FSL and FRL rats has not been studied in detail,
but the FSL rat weighs less than the FRL rat and in a recent study the FSL were
found to consume less food than FRL (Abildgaard et al. 2010). It, therefore,
appears that the FSL rats have a decreased appetite thus resembling the reduced
appetite in depressed individuals.
Anxiety-Related Behavior
Anxiety is not considered a core feature of depression, but there exist a high degree
of co-morbidity of anxiety with depression. Therefore, the behavior of FSL was
examined in the classical test of anxiety-like behavior, the elevated plus maze
(EPM), which is an unconditioned test for anxiety in rodents, and works by cre-
ating a conflict between an animal’s exploratory drive and its fear of open and
brightly-lit areas. Under baseline conditions no differences were discovered
between the FSL and FRL lines (Overstreet et al. 1995). Treatment with a
benzodiazepine exerted a comparable anxiolytic effect in both FSL and FRL rats
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 145
and did not differentiate between the two strains (Schiller et al. 1991; Mathé et al.
unpublished data).
However, our own recent results demonstrated that FSL rats had a reduced level
of unconditioned anxiety on the EPM compared to the FRL rats (Abildgaard et al.
2010). Specifically, the FSL spent more time on the open arms and had a higher
level of full entries onto open arms. Similar findings have been described in young
FSL rats compared to SD rats (Braw et al. 2006). It is, however, of interest to note
that FSL rats did exhibit some anxiogenic behavior in the social interaction task
(Overstreet et al. 2004b), which may reflect enhanced social anxiety, or alterna-
tively, reduced social motivation and social withdrawal.
Taken together, as anxiety does not seem to be a prominent feature of the FSL
strain, the FSL rats seem to be a model for depression without comorbidity of
anxiety. However, as anxiety can be judged from multiple paradigms, further
studies are warranted.
Cognition
Pain
Affective disorders have been repeatedly linked with alterations in thermal and
visceral pain perception (Haug et al. 2004; Vedolin et al. 2009; Robinson et al. 2009).
However, only a limited number of studies have been carried out in the FSL model. In
a model with partial denervation of the sciatic nerve (PSL model), which produces a
chronic decrease in touch and heat withdrawal thresholds (allodynia) and an
increased response to noxious mechanical and heat stimuli (hyperalgesia, Fujioka
et al. 2001; Fumagalli et al. 2007), the FSL rats expressed significantly lower levels of
tactile allodynia and less heat hyperalgesia following PSL injury compared to SD rats
146 G. Wegener et al.
(Shir et al. 2001). These studies have been carried out using a denervation model,
and no results for basal pain parameters are available.
Sleep Patterns
Sleeping disorders are very closely associated with depressive disorders, with both
insomnia and hypersomnia being observed. Two pronounced changes associated
with depression are increases in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and decreases in
slow wave sleep (Jindal et al. 2002; Thase et al. 1995; Benca 1996; Benca et al.
1992; Adrien 2002). Basal sleep recordings in the FSL have demonstrated that the
FSL rat exhibited a reduced latency to––and greater amount of REM sleep than the
FRL rats (Benca et al. 1996; Shiromani et al. 1991), with no differences in slow
wave sleep patterns. Thus, the FSL rat resembled depressed individuals with
regard to the elevated REM sleep, but not with regard to the reduced slow wave
sleep (Jindal et al. 2002; Benca et al. 1992).
Monoamine Metabolism
Fig. 1 Hippocampal constitutive nitric oxide synthase (cNOS) activity data under basal
conditions [FSL (n = 8), FRL (n = 7)], and following escapable stress/inescapable stress
(ES–IS) [FSL (n = 8), FRL (n = 7)]. Following ES–IS, cNOS activity is significantly elevated in
FSL rats compared to unstressed FSL controls (** p \ 0.005) and versus pre- and post-stress FRL
animals (* p \ 0.05). Pre- and post-stress activity levels for FRL rats did not differ from one
another. Values shown are means ? S.E.M. Reprinted from (Wegener et al. 2010) with
permission. Ó Cambridge University Press
Neuropeptides
Fig. 2 Messenger RNA samples from hippocampus and frontal cortex of FSL (%; n = 9) and
FRL (&; n = 9) rats were used for quantification of the expression levels of BDNF using real-
time qPCR. Values for each individual were normalized with the geometric mean of the reference
genes Ywhaz and Hmbs in the hippocampus and Ywhaz and Actb in the frontal cortex. Plotted
data show mean group values ? S.E.M. of mRNA expression as % of FRL rats. * Indicates
significant between-group differences (p \ 0.05). Reprinted from (Elfving et al. 2010a) with
permission. Ó Cambridge University Press
Neurotrophic Factors
Fig. 3 Effect of Imipramine on the non-perforated spine synapses in FSL and FRL rats. The total
number of spine synapses in the CA1 stratum radiatum was significantly smaller in the FSL rats
compared to the FRL rats. Following 3 weeks of Imipramine (Imi, 15 mg/kg/day) there were a
significant increase in the spine synapses in both FSL and FRL, thereby normalizing the FSL
spine synapse numbers. (* p \ 0.05; *** p \ 0.001). Modified from (Chen et al. 2010) with
permission. Ó John Wiley & Sons
Also other aspects of brain remodeling have been proposed to be essential for
development of disease. Thus, both the hippocampal volume (Videbech and
Ravnkilde 2004) and synaptic morphology may play a role (Nestler et al. 2002).
Therefore, we have investigated changes in hippocampal volume, neuron and
synapse numbers in the FSL and FRL following chronic imipramine therapy, using
design-based stereological methods (Chen et al. 2010). We found that the volume
and the number of neurons and synapses were significantly smaller in the FSL
saline group compared with the FRL saline group, a feature which was reversed
following imipramine treatment (Fig. 3). Our experiments illustrate the impor-
tance of using a disease model to study cell proliferation and effects of treatments
that could potentially be translated to human condition.
Interactions with the environment, which can have negative or positive conse-
quences, have also been found to be a major determinant of disease. For example,
psychosocial stress in adulthood impairs the health condition of the individual
(Lupien et al. 2009; Bale et al. 2010; Reber et al. 2007), whereas social support or
exercise exert beneficial effects on somatic and mental health (Brene et al. 2007;
Dishman et al. 2006; Dunn and Dishman 1991; Young 1979; Neumann 2009).
The consequences of acute, subchronic or chronic stress are largely dependent
on the individual (and genetically determined) stress susceptibility, and there is
good evidence that FSL and FRL as well as HAB and LAB rats provide good
models to study gene 9 environment interactions. For example, as mentioned
above, a subchronic adult stress paradigm significantly upregulates the NO sig-
naling pathway in the FSL only (Wegener et al. 2010), and metabolic stress more
severely impacts the FSL compared with the FRL (Abildgaard et al. 2010).
In another series of experiments, we compared adult female FSL and SD rats in a
paradigm of 7 weeks of social isolation at the age of 29 weeks, and observed
increased number of BrdU-IR cells in the FSL, whereas it had no impact in the SD
strain (Bjornebekk et al. 2007). Other environmental stimuli may be experienced
positive. Thus, we have examined the effect of physical activity using running
wheels. We observed that voluntary wheel running had antidepressant effects
and selectively altered NPY and NPY Y1 receptor and opiate expression in the
FSL, but not FRL, rats further supporting a role of NPY in their phenotype
154 G. Wegener et al.
(Bjornebekk et al. 2006, 2010). These findings parallel and support the results from
human studies (Russo-Neustadt et al. 1999; Ransford 1982).
In addition to adult stress exposure, gene 9 environment interactions have been
described with respect to early-life stress, either prenatally or postnatally. Thus, a
large number of human and animal studies show a strong association between an
adverse fetal or immediate postnatal environment and behavioral and emotional
development later in life (Abe et al. 2007; Maccari et al. 2003; Nagano et al. 2008;
O’Connor et al. 2002; Tazumi et al. 2005; Van Den Bergh et al. 2005). Stressful
experiences during early life have been hypothesized to enhance susceptibility
(eventually triggered by adult stress) for mental illness (Cottrell and Seckl 2009;
Fumagalli et al. 2007; Maynard et al. 2001).
Prenatal stress studies have not yet been carried out in FSL/FRL. However, the
classical post-natal stress paradigm, maternal separation, in FSL and FRL have
been demonstrated to exacerbate the depression-like behavior of the FSL, but not
the FRL (El Khoury et al. 2006). Treatment with escitalopram selectively
decreased depression-like behavior in the FST in both maternally non-separated
and separated FSL, but not FRL rats (El Khoury et al. 2006). Maternal separation
in FSL has been also been found to reduce NPY in dorsal hippocampus of both
female and male FSL rats compared with FRL rats (Jimenez-Vasquez et al. 2001;
Wortwein et al. 2006).
In another study, we analyzed hippocampal synaptic transmission and plasticity
in vivo and ionotropic receptors for glutamate in FSL and FRL rats subjected to
maternal separation. A strong inhibition of long-term potentiation (LTP) and lower
synaptic expression of NR1 subunit of the NMDA receptor were found in FSL rats
(Ryan et al. 2009), and unexpectedly maternal separation induced a remodeling of
synaptic plasticity only in FSL rats, reducing inhibition of LTP accompanied by
marked increase of synaptic NR1 subunit and GluR2/3 subunits of AMPA
receptors (Ryan et al. 2009). This finding is in line with the demonstration that
maternal separation increased the hippocampal cell number, while consistently
with this increase, chronic escitalopram treatment reduced the cell number
(Petersen et al. 2008; Husum et al. 2008).
In a study of basal differences in synaptic signaling between FSL and FRL rats,
as well as on consequences of maternal separation in adulthood, it was found that
the FSL rats showed basal differences in the interaction/activation of distinct
synaptic mediators purified hippocampal synaptosomes (Musazzi et al. 2010).
In addition, following maternal separation, the FSL rats displayed a blunted
response of the mediators, suggesting a synaptic dysfunction in the FSL animals
(Musazzi et al. 2010). Escitalopram treatment restored some but not all alterations
observed in FSL rats after early-life stress, suggesting that early gene-environment
interaction may cause life-long synaptic changes affecting the course of depres-
sion-like behavior and response to drugs (Musazzi et al. 2010).
Finally, using an open-ended approach based on a proteomic analysis of serum,
maternal separation was found to induce changes in inflammation and transport
proteins in FSL rats (Carboni et al. 2010), changes that were partly reversed
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 155
A detailed review of the different studies, where the FSL rat has been used to test for
the antidepressant-like effects of drugs lies beyond the scope of this text, and only a
brief overview is given in the Table 3. A detailed review of the classical antide-
pressants and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) as well as a variety of
novel agents that presumably have different actions from the well-characterized
antidepressants, can be found elsewhere (Overstreet 2002, 2005).
The Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) rat strain was developed as the normotensive control
strain for the spontaneously hypertensive rat, and bred from the Wistar strain
starting in 1963 (Okamoto and Aoki 1963). The WKY presents with hormonal,
behavioral, and physiological measures that mimic those found in depressed
patients, such as increased immobility in the FST (Lahmame et al. 1997;
Rittenhouse et al. 2002; Paré 1992, 1994) and dysregulation of the HPA and
hypothalamic–pituitary–thyroid axes (Solberg et al. 2001; Redei et al. 1994;
Gómez et al. 1996). However, the WKY responds with variable degree to anti-
depressants (López-Rubalcava and Lucki 2000; Lahmanie and Armario 1996;
Lahmame et al. 1997), and has therefore been proposed as a model of treatment-
resistant depression (Lahmame et al. 1997). Therefore, the model has been further
developed into ‘WKY most immobile’ (WMI) and ‘WKY least immobile’ (WLI)
rats (Will et al. 2003).
Since low motor activity and a condition of passive stress coping in a swim test
have been proposed to represent depression-like behavior in the rat, SD rats were
bred in accordance with the motor-activities starting in 1987 (Weiss et al. 1998).
Two rat lines have been obtained, Swim Low-Active (SwLo) and Swim High-
Active (SwHi) rats, which differ dramatically in FST behavior. The SwLo rats
show little struggling and much floating, while SwHi rats show the reverse (Weiss
et al. 1998). Importantly, when SwLo rats were given antidepressant, chronic but
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 157
not acute administration increased swim-test activity of SwLo rats (West and
Weiss 1998a). Information on neurotransmitter involvement was limited, but
studies suggest involvement of both glutamatergic (Tabb et al. 2007) and dopa-
minergic (West et al. 1999a, 1999b) mechanisms as well as alterations of the stress
axis (Gutman et al. 2008).
An adequate level of innate anxiety and fear is essential for survival of individuals
and species. Naturally, there exists a wide individual range in trait anxiety: from
extremely low to extremely high. Similarly, in humans, anxiety-related patholo-
gies including generalized anxiety, panic disorders or social phobia, reflect
extremes in trait anxiety with significant contributions of adverse life events
shaping the individual anxiety phenotype. In order to reveal neuroendocrine,
neurochemical and neurogenetic mechanisms of a complex behavioral phenotypes
such as anxiety, and in order to identify potential targets for psychotherapy,
we have established and extensively studied selectively bred HAB and LAB
rats (Landgraf and Wigger 2002; Landgraf et al. 2007; Neumann et al. 2010).
This approach is particularly promising to further our understanding of genetic
mechanisms underlying anxiety-related disorders. Other relevant rodent models for
anxiety-related behavior include exposure to early-life stress (Wigger and
158 G. Wegener et al.
Fig. 4 Anxiety-related behavior of male and female HAB, LAB and non-selected NAB rats on
the elevated plus-maze (EPM) which is consistent over the years between 2003 and 2010
Neumann 1999; Huot et al. 2001), chronic stress in adulthood (Barrot et al. 2005;
Reber et al. 2007; 2008), or transgenic modifications (Bale 2006; Mantella et al.
2003). Recently, the Landgraf group succeeded in establishing also mice lines
selectively bred for high (M-HAB) and low (M-LAB) anxiety-related behavior (see
Table 1; Landgraf et al. 2007).
Since 1993, we have selectively and bi-directionally bred outbred Wistar rats
for high (HAB) versus low (LAB) anxiety-related behavior based on their
behavioral performance on the EPM at the Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry
in Munich and, since 2002, at the University of Regensburg (Landgraf and
Wigger 2002; Landgraf et al. 2007; Neumann et al. 2010; see Fig. 4). Male and
female HAB and LAB rats are selected at the age of 9 weeks for further
breeding only, if the percentage of time spent on the open arms of the elevated
plus maze (EPM) is below 5% and above 40–45%, respectively. For experi-
mental purposes, HAB rats with an anxiety level of less than 10% and LAB
rats with more than 35% time on the open arms during testing at the age of
9 weeks are used.
Anxiety-Related Behavior
The behavioral profile of HAB and LAB rats with respect to the selection criteria
has been reliable and robust over the last 10–15 years (Liebsch et al. 1998b;
Neumann et al. 2010; see Fig. 4), is present over all seasons, independent of sex or
age and could be confirmed in different European laboratories. The extremes in
anxiety could be confirmed in a battery of relevant behavioral tests, including the
EPM, the open field, the light dark box and the holeboard (Slattery and Neumann
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 159
2010; Ohl et al. 2001; Henniger et al. 2000). In addition, HAB rats are unable to
properly extinguish inappropriate fear in tests for conditioned anxiety despite
similar acquisition of fear (Muigg et al. 2008). Thus, although bred for high innate
non-conditioned anxiety on the EPM, this finding reveals that HAB rats also
display exaggerated responses to conditioned fear, which makes them a potentially
useful model for posttraumatic stress disorder. Also, LAB rats were able to
extinguish this fear faster than non-selected Wistar rats (NAB), again showing
robust trait differences between these lines.
Depression-Like Behavior
Cognition
Social Behaviors
Other behavioral differences, which have been established over the years of
selective breeding for high versus low trait anxiety, include a variety of social
behaviors (Neumann et al. 2010), which is of interest as several psychopathologies
often are accompanied by various abnormalities in social interactions including
aggression, social phobia, or impaired social bonding.
Inter-Male Aggression
Male HAB and LAB rats differ in inter-male aggression with LAB males showing
an extreme high level of offensive behavior in the resident-intruder test (Veenema
et al. 2007), a finding which is also reflected by an abnormal aggression of LAB
males displayed towards females and anesthetized males compared with NAB rats
(Neumann et al. 2010). In contrast, HAB males rather display an intermediate level
of intermale aggression (Beiderbeck et al. 2007; Neumann et al. 2010; Veenema
et al. 2007). Thus, the selective breeding for low trait anxiety resulted in a socio-
behavioral phenotype characterized not only by low anxiety and fear responses,
reduced risk assessment and an active stress coping style, but also by abnormal
social behaviors in different social settings, thus providing an excellent model for
studying mechanisms of pathological aggression.
Social Phobia
In a relevant behavioral setup for social phobia/social preference, NAB and HAB
rats show social preference. In this test, rats are allowed to explore a small wired
cage placed into the cage of the experimental rat. Social preference is seen, when
the animal explores the small cage longer and more often, when it contains a
conspecific animal. Here, LABs, in general, do not search for social contact,
display reduced contact to cage mates (Ohl et al. 2001) and do not show social
reference, which can also be interpreted as social phobia (Lukas and Neumann,
unpublished).
Maternal Care
Line-differences in social behavior are also found in females with respect to
maternal care and maternal defense behavior (maternal aggression). Here, HAB
dams are more protective towards their pups, leave the nest less often, show more
arched back nursing and more maternal aggression (Bosch et al. 2005; Bosch and
Neumann 2010; Neumann et al. 2005a). A significant contribution of high activity
of the brain vasopressin system seen in HAB dams to their maternal behavior
profile could recently be identified (Bosch and Neumann 2008, 2010). Interest-
ingly, a correlation between high maternal trait anxiety and the intensity of
162 G. Wegener et al.
maternal care has also been found in mouse dams bred for high and low anxiety
and, again, line-dependent differences in brain vasopressin appear to underlie the
behavioral differences (Kessler et al. 2011).
Pain
Similar to depressed patients, HAB rats have been shown to exhibit decreased
sensitivity to thermal pain (Jochum et al. 2007). Acute administration of diazepam
partly reversed the abnormal pain response. Also, treatment of male HAB rats with
the SSRI citalopram, daily for 8 weeks, reduced anxiety-related behavior as
assessed on the EPM and reversed the abnormal thermal pain response in HAB rats
(Jochum et al. 2007). As, in contrast to the thermal pain sensitivity, the sensitivity
to visceral pain has been found to be elevated in patients suffering from affective
disorders (Haug et al. 2004), it would be of interest to test visceral pain threshold
in HAB versus LAB rats.
Sleep
The behavioral differences between HAB and LAB rats are accompanied by
distinct neuroendocrine underpinnings. Although basal levels of plasma ACTH or
corticosterone reflecting basal activity of the HPA axis do not differ, the respon-
siveness of the HPA axis to a mild emotional (and non-social) stressor is more
pronounced in HAB compared with LAB males (and with non-selected Wistar
rats; Landgraf et al. 1999; Neumann et al. 2010), thus resembling psychiatric
patients (Holsboer 2000). However, the neuroendocrine response to social stimuli
is aggravated in LABs, paralleling their abnormal social behavioral responses
described above (Neumann et al. 2010; Veenema et al. 2007). HAB rats also show
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 163
an aberrant hormonal secretion pattern during the DEX/CRH test (Keck et al.
2002; Neumann et al. 2010), a clinical test used for the neuroendocrine charac-
terization of depressed patients (Ising et al. 2005). Intravenous administration of
DEX revealed DEX-non-suppression and, thus, impairment of negative feedback
regulation in HAB rats. Subsequent administration of CRH resulted in pronounced
ACTH and corticosterone responses, which are absent in LAB and NAB rats.
This indicates a contribution of endogenous vasopressin, which––together with
exogenous CRH––triggered pituitary secretion of ACTH despite (impaired) DEX-
suppression. Consequently, and in support of this hypothesis, an acute intravenous
administration of a vasopressin V1a receptor antagonist abolished the ACTH and
corticosterone hyper-response and normalized the DEX/CRH test outcome in
HAB rats, pointing towards a significant contribution of the endogenous brain
vasopressin system in these neuroendocrine abnormalities (Keck et al. 2002).
Furthermore, chronic administration of paroxetine over 8 weeks prevented the
abnormal DEX/CRH response in HAB rats with a concomitant attenuation of the
vasopressin hyperdrive in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus (PVN, Keck
et al. 2003b).
Neuronal Activity
Neuropeptides
Vasopressin
Given the importance of brain vasopressin in the regulation of anxiety, depression,
and neuroendocrine stress coping (Landgraf and Neumann 2004; Frank and
Landgraf 2008), the vasopressin gene was considered a candidate gene in high trait
anxiety. Indeed, high vasopressin mRNA expression within the parvocellular part
of the PVN both under basal conditions and in response to stressor exposure is a
reproducible characteristics for male and female HAB rats (Keck et al. 2003b;
164 G. Wegener et al.
Wigger et al. 2004; Bosch et al. 2006; Bosch and Neumann 2010; Frank and
Landgraf 2008). Consequently, increased vasopressin immunoreactivity and local
neuropeptide release were both found in the HAB hypothalamus. In addition,
vasopressin V1a receptor binding is also elevated within the lateral septum of
HAB rats, a region relevant for the regulation of anxiety and social behaviors
(Keck et al. 2003b).
In line with our hypothesis of a substantial contribution of high endogenous
vasopressin activity to the high anxiety and depression-like phenotype of HAB rats,
blockade of vasopressin V1a receptors within the hypothalamic PVN reduced their
anxiety level and resulted in a more active coping style (Wigger et al. 2004). Further,
the V1a antagonist (i.v.) normalized the pathological outcome of the DEX/CRH test
in male HABs (Keck et al. 2002). These findings, reflecting both construct and
predictive validity, confirm the involvement of endogenous central vasopressin in
the behavioral and neuroendocrine phenomena of high trait anxiety and depression.
Interestingly, line-dependent differences in brain vasopressin also appear to
contribute to differences in social behavior, i.e. intermale aggression as well as
maternal behavior (Beiderbeck et al. 2007; Veenema et al. 2007; Bosch et al. 2010;
Bosch and Neumann 2008).
Given the robust increase in brain vasopressin activity in HAB rats in parallel to
their anxiogenic and hyperresponsive neuroendocrine phenotypes, underlying
genetic mechanisms are likely. Indeed, 10 single nucleotide polymorphisms
(SNPs) within the vasopressin promoter were found between the lines. In addition,
a single base pair substitution has been identified in the first intron of the
vasopressin gene of HAB rats itself (Murgatroyd et al. 2004). One of the SNPs
identified was found to be embedded in a potential transcription factor binding
site (CArG box), the locus of binding to the transcriptional repressor CBF-A.
Functional relevance of the SNP was identified by in vitro DNA binding assay and
revealed that CBF-A binding to the CArG box derived from the HAB allele was
indeed diminished, resulting in an attenuated transcriptional repression of the
vasopressin gene. Thus, this genetic mechanism may underlie vasopressin over-
expression in the PVN of HAB rats (Murgatroyd et al. 2004; Landgraf et al. 2007).
Interestingly, out of 100 outbred Wistar rats, the HAB allele was found in 3 rats
(heterozygeous) indicating a gene frequency of 1.5% in the general Wistar rat
population.
In addition to vasopressin, other brain neuropeptides such as CRH, oxytocin,
prolactin, NPY, or neuropeptide S are important neuromodulators of emotionality,
in particular of anxiety- and depression-related behaviors. Thus, an endophenotype
of high or low anxiety accompanied by differences in active or passive stress
coping style is likely to be accompanied by differences in the activity of several
endogenous neuropeptide and other neurotransmitter systems.
Corticotropin-Releasing Hormone
Corticotropin-Releasing Hormone (CRH) exerts anxiogenic and depression-like
effects, and CRH mRNA expression has recently been found to be up-regulated
within the PVN of HAB rats compared with LAB rats (Bosch et al. 2006). In contrast,
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 165
Oxytocin
Besides its capacity to modulate complex social behaviors, oxytocin is an estab-
lished anxiolytic neuropeptide of the brain (Blume et al. 2008; Neumann 2008)
and has antidepressive properties (Slattery and Neumann 2010). However, dif-
ferences in central oxytocin expression or release were not found between HAB
and LAB rats, except in lactation (Bosch et al. 2007).
In order to study potential anxiolytic or antidepressive effects of oxytocin in a
psychopathological animal model, HAB and LAB rats were treated i.c.v. with
either oxytocin or an oxytocin receptor antagonist (Slattery and Neumann 2010),
which was only effective in female but not male rats: chronic oxytocin reduced the
high anxiety level of HAB females on the EPM, whereas chronic i.c.v. treatment
with the oxytocin antagonist increased anxiety only in female LAB rats without
any effect in HABs or males. In contrast, acute manipulation of the oxytocin
system did not alter anxiety-related behavior independent of sex and trait anxiety.
Also, passive/active stress coping in the FST was not altered by any manipulation
of the oxytocin system. Thus, chronic oxytocin seems to be a promising thera-
peutic strategy in particular for the treatment of anxiety disorders in women.
Prolactin
Prolactin has distinct anxiolytic properties, and the brain prolactin is involved
in the regulation of anxiety and stress coping (Torner et al. 2004; Bunck et al.
2009; Ditzen et al. 2010). Plasma prolactin levels were found elevated in HAB rats
in response to a mild emotional stressor (Neumann et al. 1998; Landgraf et al.
1999). The behavioral significance of this finding needs to be studied, as plasma
prolactin does not reflect intracerebral prolactin release patterns (Torner et al.
2004), but peripheral prolactin can cross the blood brain barrier. However,
differences in brain prolactin expression, release or receptor binding have not been
studied in HAB and LAB rats until now.
Neuropeptide S
Neuropeptide S (NPS) is another powerful anxiolytic neuropeptide (Xu et al. 2004;
Leonard et al. 2008; Vitale et al. 2008), and our preliminary results indicate substantial
genetic and activity differences of the endogenous brain NPS system between HAB
166 G. Wegener et al.
and LAB rats. For example, the latter express more NPS receptors in the hypothalamus
(Slattery, Wegener, Naik, Mathé, Neumann, unpublished). In order to confirm the
anxiolytic properties described in non-selected male mice (Xu et al. 2004) and rats
(Vitale et al. 2008) in a psychopathological animal model, we acutely treated HAB and
LAB rats with i.c.v. NPS 45 min prior to EPM testing can (Slattery et al. 2008). Indeed,
preliminary evidence suggest that NPS reversed the high trait anxiety of HABs and
exerted modest antidepressant effects (Slattery et al. 2008). Further, i.c.v. NPS
improves consolidation of extinguished learned fear (Sartori et al. 2009).
Hippocampal Neurogenesis
Consistent with the generally elevated stress responsiveness including HPA axis
hyperreactivity and impaired negative feedback functions, we found reduced
hippocampal cell survival and neurogenesis in 43-days old male HAB rats
(Lucassen et al. 2009). Specifically, the number of newly generated surviving
(BrdU-positive) cells in the subgranular cell layer/subgranular zone of the hip-
pocampal dentate gyrus was found to be lower in HAB versus LAB rats. Further,
the number of hippocampal doublecortin-positive cells reflecting neurogenesis is
lower in HAB rats (Lucassen et al. 2009).
These results show that the high level of anxiety and activity of the HPA axis
may affect cell survival in HAB rats, which may, in turn, also be partly responsible
for their behavioral phenotype.
Early life stress, such as prenatal and immediately postnatal stress is a well-
characterized risk factor for the development of affective disorders in adulthood,
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 167
Fig. 5 Gene 9 early environment interactions shape adult behavioral and neuroendocrine stress
responses as seen in rats with genetic determination of trait anxiety
and there are well documented interactions of genetic and environmental factors
(Caspi et al. 2003; Fig. 5, see also Sect. 2.1.2). Selectively bred rodents with clear
genetic determinants are valuable models for studying gene 9 environment
interactions. Both prenatal stress as well as postnatal maternal separation resulted
in line-dependent behavioral effects seen in adult HAB and LAB rats.
Gene 9 prenatal environment interactions: Surprisingly, after exposure to pre-
natal stress between pregnancy days 4 and 18 (exposure of the pregnant dam to
maternal defeat by an unknown lactating resident daily for 45 min between preg-
nancy days 4 and 10, and to restraint between pregnancy days 11 and 18 for 60 min
daily) adult male HAB rats became less anxious (see Fig. 6). This was confirmed in
two independent tests for anxiety-related behavior, i.e. the EPM and the modified
holeboard (Bosch et al. 2006). The opposite behavioral consequences of prenatal
stress were accompanied by opposing effects on central vasopressin and CRH mRNA
expression: whereas the genetically determined high level of hypothalamic vaso-
pressin mRNA expression of HAB rats was not altered by prenatal stress, it was
elevated in early life stressed LAB male offspring (Fig. 6). Similarly, the genetic
difference in CRH expression within the PVN with high levels in unstressed HAB
compared with LAB controls was also at least partly abolished by prenatal stress.
Further, after prenatal stress the high HPA axis response to a mild stressor found in
HAB rats was reduced and found to be indistinguishable from unstressed and pre-
natally stressed LABs (Bosch et al. 2006).
Opposing effects of prenatal stress were also found with respect to hippocampal
neurogenesis, which has been shown to be stress-sensitive and implicated in
depression (Pittenger and Duman 2008). As mentioned above, the survival of
168 G. Wegener et al.
Fig. 6 Opposite effects of prenatal stress on anxiety-related behavior on the EPM (% entries
open arms; left) and basal vasopressin mRNA expression in the hypothalamic PVN (right) in
male HAB and LAB adult offspring indicating gene 9 early environment interactions. # versus
LAB; * versus respective control. Adapted from (Bosch et al. 2006)
Fig. 7 Opposite effects of postnatal stress (periodic maternal separation; PMD) on anxiety-
related behavior on the EPM (% time open arms; left) and plasma corticosterone response to
novel environment exposure (elevated platform, right) in HAB and LAB adult offspring
indicating gene 9 early environment interactions. # versus LAB; * versus respective control.
Adapted from (Neumann et al. 2005b)
Although various selectively bred rat lines are useful tools for studying behavioral
and especially neuroendocrine parameters of depression and anxiety, as well as
environmental factors shaping innate stress coping style, genetic studies such as
the functional analysis of candidate genes underlying, for example, high trait
anxiety or depression-related behavior, are a priori limited in rats. Therefore,
Landgraf and co-workers also generated mice selectively bred for high (M-HAB)
versus low (M-LAB) anxiety-related behavior (Kromer et al. 2005). After 9
generations of continuous breeding (sibling mating), a robust behavioral diver-
gence had been achieved. High trait anxiety has been confirmed on the EPM and in
the light–dark box; further, M-HAB mice pups show more ultrasound vocalization,
which was reversed by diazepam (Kromer et al. 2005). In agreement to what has
been found in HAB and LAB rats, M-LAB mice are more active in several tests for
depression-like behavior (FST, tail suspension). Also, as seen in HAB and LAB
170 G. Wegener et al.
The Floripa H and L rat lines, have been developed based on selection for high and
low locomotion in the central aversive area of an open field, an experimental
measure of fearfulness in rodents (Ramos and Mormede 1998). The Floripa H and
L lines differ from each other not only for the selected behavior, but also for other
experimental indices of anxiety, such as the approach towards the open arms of the
elevated plus maze and the white compartment of the black/white box (Ramos
et al. 2003). In addition, compared with Floripa L, the Floripa H rats show less
depression-like behavior in the FST (Hinojosa et al. 2006), suggesting the Floripa
rats to be a combined model of both anxiety and depression. The Floripa L female
rats consumed more ethanol than their Floripa H counterparts at concentrations of
6 and 10% in a two-bottle choice protocol (Izídio and Ramos 2007), however the
connection to the anxiety-like phenotype remains to be determined.
Based on reactivity in the open field, with defecation and urination being the
central variables, two lines of rats, later termed the Maudsley high reactive (high
defecation, MHR) and low, nonreactive (low defecation, MLR) rats, were bred
from Wistar in the 1950s aiming to model the human personality dimension of
emotionality (Broadhurst 1957, 1960, 1962, 1975). The overall conclusion of the
several studies published were that Maudsley reactive rats yielded higher scores in
several tests of anxiety-like/avoidance behavior than non-reactive animals (Blizard
and Adams 2002). Although this approach was useful to study individual differ-
ences in anxiety-like/avoidance behavior, it has not been pursued intensively in
recent years (Pawlak et al. 2008; Blizard and Adams 2002).
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 171
Several rat breeding lines have been selected for learning to actively avoid foot
shocks in a two-way shuttle-box. The Roman high avoidance (RHA) and
Roman low avoidance (RLA) Wistar rats (Bignami 1965), the Long–Evans rats
(Brush et al. 1979, 1985, 1989; Brush 2003), and Syracuse high and low
avoidance rats (SHA/Bru and SLA/Bru) are the most prominent examples.
Although these attempts differ in nature, some of the common characteristics
include differences in learning and memory, and a number of emotion-related
behavioral and neuroendocrine characteristics. A detailed review can be found
in (Brush 2003).
4 Conclusions
Acknowledgments GW was supported by grants from The Danish Medical Research Council
(grant 271-08-0768) and the Research Foundation of County Midtjylland. AAM was supported
from the Swedish Medical Research Council (grant 10414) and the Karolinska Institutet. IDN
was supported by DFG, BMBF and Bayerische Forschungsstiftung.
172 G. Wegener et al.
References
Benca RM, Overstreet DE, Gilliland MA, Russell D, Bergmann BM, Obermeyer WH (1996)
Increased basal REM sleep but no difference in dark induction or light suppression of REM
sleep in flinders rats with cholinergic supersensitivity. Neuropsychopharmacology 15(1):
45–51
Bendotti C, Samanin R (1987) The role of putative 5-HT(1A) and 5-HT(1B) receptors in the
control of feeding in rats. Life Sci 41(5):635–642
Bignami G (1965) Selection for high rates and low rates of avoidance conditioning in the rat.
Anim Behav 13(2–3):221–227
Bjornebekk A, Mathe AA, Brene S (2005) The antidepressant effect of running is associated with
increased hippocampal cell proliferation. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 8(3):357–368
Bjornebekk A, Mathe AA, Brene S (2006) Running has differential effects on NPY, opiates, and
cell proliferation in an animal model of depression and controls. Neuropsychopharmacology
31(2):256–264
Bjornebekk A, Mathe AA, Gruber SH, Brene S (2007) Social isolation increases number of newly
proliferated cells in hippocampus in female flinders sensitive line rats. Hippocampus
17(12):1193–1200
Bjornebekk A, Mathe AA, Brene S (2010) The antidepressant effects of running and escitalopram
are associated with levels of hippocampal NPY and Y1 receptor but not cell proliferation in a
rat model of depression. Hippocampus 20(7):820–828
Blizard DA, Adams N (2002) The maudsley reactive and nonreactive strains: a new perspective.
Behav Genet 32(5):277–299
Blume A, Bosch OJ, Miklos S, Torner L, Wales L, Waldherr M, Neumann ID (2008) Oxytocin
reduces anxiety via ERK1/2 activation: local effect within the rat hypothalamic paraventric-
ular nucleus. Eur J Neurosci 27(8):1947–1956
Bosch OJ, Neumann ID (2008) Brain vasopressin is an important regulator of maternal behavior
independent of dams’ trait anxiety. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105(44):17139–17144
Bosch OJ, Neumann ID (2010) Vasopressin released within the central amygdala promotes
maternal aggression. Eur J Neurosci 31(5):883–891
Bosch OJ, Meddle SL, Beiderbeck DI, Douglas AJ, Neumann ID (2005) Brain oxytocin
correlates with maternal aggression: link to anxiety. J Neurosci 25(29):6807–6815
Bosch OJ, Kromer SA, Neumann ID (2006) Prenatal stress: opposite effects on anxiety and
hypothalamic expression of vasopressin and corticotropin-releasing hormone in rats
selectively bred for high and low anxiety. Eur J Neurosci 23(2):541–551
Bosch OJ, Musch W, Bredewold R, Slattery DA, Neumann ID (2007) Prenatal stress increases
HPA axis activity and impairs maternal care in lactating female offspring: implications for
postpartum mood disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32(3):267–278
Bosch OJ, Pfortsch J, Beiderbeck DI, Landgraf R, Neumann ID (2010) Maternal behaviour is
associated with vasopressin release in the medial preoptic area and bed nucleus of the stria
terminalis in the rat. J Neuroendocrinol 22(5):420–429
Braw Y, Malkesman O, Dagan M, Bercovich A, Lavi-Avnon Y, Schroeder M, Overstreet DH,
Weller A (2006) Anxiety-like behaviors in pre-pubertal rats of the flinders sensitive line (FSL)
and Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) animal models of depression. Behav Brain Res 167(2):261–269
Breier A, Albus M, Pickar D, Zahn TP, Wolkowitz OM, Paul SM (1987) Controllable and
uncontrollable stress in humans: alterations in mood and neuroendocrine and psychophys-
iological function. Am J Psychiatry 144(11):1419–1425
Brene S, Bjornebekk A, Aberg E, Mathe AA, Olson L, Werme M (2007) Running is rewarding
and antidepressive. Physiol Behav 92(1–2):136–140
Broadhurst PL (1957) Determinants of emotionality in the rat. I. Situational factors. Br J Psychol
(London, England : 1953) 48(1):1–12
Broadhurst PL (1960) Experiments in psychogenetics: applications of biometrical genetics to the
inheritance of behaviour. In: Eysenck HJ (ed) Experiments in personality. Psychogenetics and
psychopharmacology, vol 1. Routledge & Kegan Paul, London, pp 1–102
Broadhurst PL (1962) A note on further progress in a psychogenetic selection experiment.
Psychol Rep 10:65–66
174 G. Wegener et al.
Broadhurst PL (1975) The maudsley reactive and nonreactive strains of rats: a survey. Behav
Genet 5(4):299–319
Brush FR (2003) Selection for differences in avoidance learning: the syracuse strains differ in
anxiety, not learning ability. Behav Genet 33(6):677–696
Brush FR, Froehlich JC, Sakellaris PC (1979) Genetic selection for avoidance behavior in the rat.
Behav Genet 9(4):309–316
Brush FR, Baron S, Froehlich JC, Ison JR, Pellegrino LJ, Phillips DS, Sakellaris PC, Williams VN
(1985) Genetic differences in avoidance learning by rattus norvegicus: escape/avoidance
responding, sensitivity to electric shock, discrimination learning, and open-field behavior. J
comp Psychol (Washington, DC: 1983) 99(1):60–73
Brush FR, Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC (1989) Social dominance and response to a natural
predator by rats selectively bred for differences in shuttle-box avoidance learning.
Ethoexperimental Approaches to the study of behavior, pp 411–417
Bunck M, Czibere L, Horvath C, Graf C, Frank E, Kessler MS, Murgatroyd C, Muller-Myhsok B,
Gonik M, Weber P, Putz B, Muigg P, Panhuysen M, Singewald N, Bettecken T, Deussing JM,
Holsboer F, Spengler D, Landgraf R (2009) A hypomorphic vasopressin allele prevents
anxiety-related behavior. PLoS One 4(4):e5129
Bus BA, Molendijk ML, Penninx BJ, Buitelaar JK, Kenis G, Prickaerts J, Elzinga BM, Voshaar RC
(2011) Determinants of serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Psychoneuroendocrinology
36(2):228–239
Bushnell PJ, Levin ED, Overstreet DH (1995) Spatial working and reference memory in rats bred
for autonomic sensitivity to cholinergic stimulation: acquisition, accuracy, speed, and effects
of cholinergic drugs. Neurobiol Learn Mem 63(2):116–132
Caberlotto L, Hurd YL (2001) Neuropeptide Y Y(1) and Y(2) receptor mrna expression in the
prefrontal cortex of psychiatric subjects. Relationship of Y(2) subtype to suicidal behavior.
Neuropsychopharmacology 25(1):91–97
Caberlotto L, Jimenez P, Overstreet DH, Hurd YL, Mathe AA, Fuxe K (1999) Alterations in
neuropeptide Y levels and Y1 binding sites in the flinders sensitive line rats, a genetic animal
model of depression. Neurosci Lett 265(3):191–194
Campbell DT, Fiske DW (1959) Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-
multimethod matrix. Psychol Bull 56(2):81–105
Carboni L, Becchi S, Piubelli C, Mallei A, Giambelli R, Razzoli M, Mathe AA, Popoli M,
Domenici E (2010) Early-life stress and antidepressants modulate peripheral biomarkers in a
gene-environment rat model of depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry
34(6):1037–1048
Carney RM, Rich MW, Freedland KE, Saini J, TeVelde A, Simeone C, Clark K (1988) Major
depressive disorder predicts cardiac events in patients with coronary artery disease.
Psychosom Med 50(6):627–633
Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, McClay J, Mill J, Martin J,
Braithwaite A, Poulton R (2003) Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a
polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 301(5631):386–389
Cerbone A, Pellicano MP, Sadile AG (1993) Evidence for and against the naples high- and low-
excitability rats as genetic model to study hippocampal functions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
17(3):295–303
Chen B, Dowlatshahi D, MacQueen GM, Wang JF, Young LT (2001) Increased hippocampal
BDNF immunoreactivity in subjects treated with antidepressant medication. Biol Psychiatry
50(4):260–265
Chen F, Madsen TM, Wegener G, Nyengaard JR (2010) Imipramine treatment increases the
number of hippocampal synapses and neurons in a genetic animal model of depression.
Hippocampus 20(12):1376–1384
Clause BT (1998) The Wistar Institute Archives: rats (not mice) and history. http://www.amphilsoc.
org/mendel/1998.htm#Clause. Accessed 9th Sept 2010
Cloninger CR, Reich T, Wetzel R (1979) Alcoholism and affective disorders: familial
associations and genetic models. alcoholism and affective disorders: clinical, genetic and
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 175
biochemical studies. In: Goodwin DW, Erickson CK (eds) Alcoholism and affective disorders.
SP Medical and Scientific Books, New York, pp 57–86
Coenen AML, Van Luijtelaar ELJM (2003) Genetic animal models for absence epilepsy: a
review of the WAG/Rij strain of rats. Behav Genet 33(6):635–655
Costall B, Naylor RJ (1973) The role of telencephalic dopaminergic systems in the mediation of
apomorphine stereotyped behaviour. Eur J Pharmacol 24(1):8–24
Cottrell EC, Seckl JR (2009) Prenatal stress, glucocorticoids and the programming of adult
disease. Front Behav Neurosci 3:19
Crocker AD, Overstreet DH (1991) Dopamine sensitivity in rats selectively bred for increases in
cholinergic function. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 38(1):105–108
Dawson VL, Dawson TM (1996) Nitric oxide in neuronal degeneration. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med
211(1):33–40
De Kloet ER, Vreugdenhil E, Oitzl MS, Joëls M (1998) Brain corticosteroid receptor balance in
health and disease. Endocr Rev 19(3):269–301
Deuschle M, Schweiger U, Weber B, Gotthardt U, Körner A, Schmider J, Standhardt H,
Lammers CH, Heuser I (1997) Diurnal activity and pulsatility of the hypothalamus-pituitary-
adrenal system in male depressed patients and healthy controls. J Clin Endocrinol Metab
82(1):234–238
Dishman RK, Berthoud HR, Booth FW, Cotman CW, Edgerton VR, Fleshner MR, Gandevia SC,
Gomez-Pinilla F, Greenwood BN, Hillman CH, Kramer AF, Levin BE, Moran TH, Russo-
Neustadt AA, Salamone JD, Van Hoomissen JD, Wade CE, York DA, Zigmond MJ (2006)
Neurobiology of exercise. Obesity (Silver Spring) 14(3):345–356
Ditzen C, Varadarajulu J, Czibere L, Gonik M, Targosz BS, Hambsch B, Bettecken T,
Kessler MS, Frank E, Bunck M, Teplytska L, Erhardt A, Holsboer F, Muller-Myhsok B,
Landgraf R, Turck CW (2010) Proteomic-based genotyping in a mouse model of trait anxiety
exposes disease-relevant pathways. Mol Psychiatry 15(7):702–711
Donner N, Bredewold R, Maloumby R, Neumann ID (2007) Chronic intracerebral prolactin
attenuates neuronal stress circuitries in virgin rats. Eur J Neurosci 25(6):1804–1814
Dremencov E, Gispan-Herman I, Rosenstein M, Mendelman A, Overstreet DH, Zohar J, Yadid G
(2004) The serotonin-dopamine interaction is critical for fast-onset action of antidepressant
treatment: in vivo studies in an animal model of depression. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol
Biol Psychiatry 28(1):141–147
Dumbrille-Ross A, Tang SW (1981) Absence of high-affinity [3H]imipramine binding in platelets
and cerebral cortex of Fawn-hooded rats. Eur J Pharmacol 72(1):137–138
Dunn AL, Dishman RK (1991) Exercise and the neurobiology of depression. Exerc Sport Sci Rev
19:41–98
Egede LE, Nietert PJ, Zheng D (2005) Depression and all-cause and coronary heart disease
mortality among adults with and without diabetes. Diabetes Care 28(6):1339–1345
Einat H, Belmaker RH, Zangen A, Overstreet DH, Yadid G (2002) Chronic inositol treatment
reduces depression-like immobility of flinders sensitive line rats in the forced swim test.
DepressAnxiety 15(3):148–151
El Khoury A, Gruber SH, Mork A, Mathe AA (2006) Adult life behavioral consequences of early
maternal separation are alleviated by escitalopram treatment in a rat model of depression.
Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 30(3):535–540
Elfving B, Plougmann PH, Muller HK, Mathe AA, Rosenberg R, Wegener G (2010a) Inverse
correlation of brain and blood BDNF levels in a genetic rat model of depression. Int J
Neuropsychopharmacol 13(5):563–572
Elfving B, Plougmann PH, Wegener G (2010b) Differential brain, but not serum VEGF levels in a
genetic rat model of depression. Neurosci Lett 474(1):13–16
Ellenbroek BA, Cools AR (2000) Animal models for the negative symptoms of schizophrenia.
Behav Pharmacol 11(3–4):223–233
Ellenbroek BA, Cools AR (2002) Apomorphine susceptibility and animal models for
psychopathology: genes and environment. Behav Genet 32(5):349–361
176 G. Wegener et al.
Elzinga BM, Molendijk ML, Oude Voshaar RC, Bus BA, Prickaerts J, Spinhoven P, Penninx BJ
(2011) The impact of childhood abuse and recent stress on serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor
and the moderating role of BDNF val(66)met. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 214(1):319–328
Engelmann M, Wotjak CT, Neumann I, Ludwig M, Landgraf R (1996) Behavioral consequences
of intracerebral vasopressin and oxytocin: focus on learning and memory. Neurosci Biobehav
Rev 20(3):341–358
Frank E, Landgraf R (2008) The vasopressin system––from antidiuresis to psychopathology. Eur
J Pharmacol 583(2–3):226–242
Frank E, Salchner P, Aldag JM, Salome N, Singewald N, Landgraf R, Wigger A (2006) Genetic
predisposition to anxiety-related behavior determines coping style, neuroendocrine responses,
and neuronal activation during social defeat. Behav Neurosci 120(1):60–71
Frasure-Smith N, Lesperance F, Talajic M (1993) Depression following myocardial infarction:
impact on 6-month survival. J Am Med Assoc 270(15):1819–1825
Fujioka T, Fujioka A, Tan N, Chowdhury GMI, Mouri H, Sakata Y, Nakamura S (2001) Mild
prenatal stress enhances learning performance in the non-adopted rat offspring. Neuroscience
103(2):301–307
Fumagalli F, Molteni R, Racagni G, Riva MA (2007) Stress during development: impact on
neuroplasticity and relevance to psychopathology. Prog Neurobiol 81(4):197–217
Gass P, Hellweg R (2010) Peripheral brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) as a biomarker
for affective disorders? Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 13(1):1–4
Geyer MA, Markou A (1995) Animal models of psychiatric disorders. In: Bloom FE, Kupfer DJ
(eds) Psychopharmacology: the fourth generation of progress, Raven Press, New York,
pp 787–798
Gómez F, Lahmame A, De Kloet ER, Armario A (1996) Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
response to chronic stress in five inbred rat strains: differential responses are mainly located at
the adrenocortical level. Neuroendocrinology 63(4):327–337
Gutman DA, Coyer MJ, Boss-Williams KA, Owens MJ, Nemeroff CB, Weiss JM (2008)
Behavioral effects of the CRF1 receptor antagonist R121919 in rats selectively bred for high
and low activity in the swim test. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33(8):1093–1101
Hambsch B, Chen BG, Brenndorfer J, Meyer M, Avrabos C, Maccarrone G, Liu RH, Eder M,
Turck CW, Landgraf R (2010) Methylglyoxal-mediated anxiolysis involves increased protein
modification and elevated expression of glyoxalase 1 in the brain. J Neurochem 113(5):
1240–1251
Haug TT, Mykletun A, Dahl AA (2004) The association between anxiety, depression, and
somatic symptoms in a large population: the HUNT-II study. Psychosom Med 66(6):845–851
Heilig M (2004) The NPY system in stress, anxiety and depression. Neuropeptides 38(4):
213–224
Hemingway H, Marmot M (1999) Psychosocial factors in the aetiology and prognosis of coronary
heart disease: systematic review of prospective cohort studies. Br Med J 318(7196):1460–1467
Henn FA, Vollmayr B (2005) Stress models of depression: forming genetically vulnerable strains.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29(4–5):799–804
Henniger MS, Ohl F, Holter SM, Weissenbacher P, Toschi N, Lorscher P, Wigger A, Spanagel R,
Landgraf R (2000) Unconditioned anxiety and social behaviour in two rat lines selectively
bred for high and low anxiety-related behaviour. Behav Brain Res 111(1–2):153–163
Hinojosa FR, Spricigo L Jr, Izídio GS, Brüske GR, Lopes DM, Ramos A (2006) Evaluation of
two genetic animal models in behavioral tests of anxiety and depression. Behav Brain Res
168(1):127–136
Holsboer F (2000) The corticosteroid receptor hypothesis of depression. Neuropsychopharma-
cology 23(5):477–501
Hou C, Jia F, Liu Y, Li L (2006) CSF serotonin, 5-hydroxyindolacetic acid and neuropeptide y
levels in severe major depressive disorder. Brain Res 1095(1):154–158
Huot RL, Thrivikraman KV, Meaney MJ, Plotsky PM (2001) Development of adult ethanol
preference and anxiety as a consequence of neonatal maternal separation in long evans rats
and reversal with antidepressant treatment. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 158(4):366–373
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 177
from lowered platelet bdnf release unrelated to platelet reactivity. Biol Psychiatry 57(9):
1068–1072
Keck ME, Holsboer F (2001) Hyperactivity of crh neuronal circuits as a target for therapeutic
interventions in affective disorders. Peptides 22(5):835–844
Keck ME, Welt T, Post A, Muller MB, Toschi N, Wigger A, Landgraf R, Holsboer F,
Engelmann M (2001a) Neuroendocrine and behavioral effects of repetitive transcranial
magnetic stimulation in a psychopathological animal model are suggestive of antidepressant-
like effects. Neuropsychopharmacology 24(4):337–349
Keck ME, Welt T, Wigger A, Renner U, Engelmann M, Holsboer F, Landgraf R (2001b) The
anxiolytic effect of the CRH(1) receptor antagonist R121919 depends on innate emotionality
in rats. Eur J Neurosci 13(2):373–380
Keck ME, Wigger A, Welt T, Muller MB, Gesing A, Reul JM, Holsboer F, Landgraf R,
Neumann ID (2002) Vasopressin mediates the response of the combined dexamethasone/CRH
test in hyper-anxious rats: implications for pathogenesis of affective disorders. Neuropsy-
chopharmacology 26(1):94–105
Keck ME, Welt T, Muller MB, Landgraf R, Holsboer F (2003a) The high-affinity non-peptide
crh1 receptor antagonist R121919 attenuates stress-induced alterations in plasma oxytocin,
prolactin, and testosterone secretion in rats. Pharmacopsychiatry 36(1):27–31
Keck ME, Welt T, Muller MB, Uhr M, Ohl F, Wigger A, Toschi N, Holsboer F, Landgraf R
(2003b) Reduction of hypothalamic vasopressinergic hyperdrive contributes to clinically
relevant behavioral and neuroendocrine effects of chronic paroxetine treatment in a
psychopathological rat model. Neuropsychopharmacology 28(2):235–243
Keck ME, Sartori SB, Welt T, Muller MB, Ohl F, Holsboer F, Landgraf R, Singewald N (2005)
Differences in serotonergic neurotransmission between rats displaying high or low anxiety/
depression-like behaviour: effects of chronic paroxetine treatment. J Neurochem 92(5):1170–1179
Keller J, Flores B, Gomez RG, Solvason HB, Kenna H, Williams GH, Schatzberg AF (2006)
Cortisol circadian rhythm alterations in psychotic major depression. Biol Psychiatry 60(3):
275–281
Kessler MS, Murgatroyd C, Bunck M, Czibere L, Frank E, Jacob W, Horvath C, Muigg P,
Holsboer F, Singewald N, Spengler D, Landgraf R (2007) Diabetes insipidus and, partially,
low anxiety-related behaviour are linked to a SNP-associated vasopressin deficit in lab mice.
Eur J Neurosci 26(10):2857–2864
Kessler MS, Bosch OJ, Bunck M, Landgraf R, Neumann ID (2011) Maternal care differs in mice
bred for high vs. low trait anxiety: impact of brain vasopressin and cross-fostering. Soc
Neurosci 6(2):156–168
Knol MJ, Twisk JWR, Beekman ATF, Heine RJ, Snoek FJ, Pouwer F (2006) Depression as a risk
factor for the onset of type 2 diabetes mellitus. A meta-analysis. Diabetologia 49(5):837–845
Kovacevic T, Skelin I, Diksic M (2010) Chronic fluoxetine treatment has a larger effect on the
density of a serotonin transporter in the flinders sensitive line (FSL) rat model of depression
than in normal rats. Synapse 64(3):231–240
Kromer SA, Kessler MS, Milfay D, Birg IN, Bunck M, Czibere L, Panhuysen M, Putz B,
Deussing JM, Holsboer F, Landgraf R, Turck CW (2005) Identification of glyoxalase-i as a
protein marker in a mouse model of extremes in trait anxiety. J Neurosci 25(17):4375–4384
Lahmame A, Del Arco C, Pazos A, Yritia M, Armario A (1997) Are wistar-kyoto rats a genetic
animal model of depression resistant to antidepressants? Eur J Pharmacol 337(2–3):115–123
Lahmanie A, Armario A (1996) Differential responsiveness of inbred strains of rats to
antidepressants in the forced swimming test: are wistar kyoto rats an animal model of
subsensitivity to antidepressants? Psychopharmacology 123(2):191–198
Lancel M, Muller-Preuss P, Wigger A, Landgraf R, Holsboer F (2002) The CRH1 receptor
antagonist R121919 attenuates stress-elicited sleep disturbances in rats, particularly in those
with high innate anxiety. J Psychiatr Res 36(4):197–208
Landgraf R, Neumann ID (2004) Vasopressin and oxytocin release within the brain: a dynamic
concept of multiple and variable modes of neuropeptide communication. Front Neuroendo-
crinol 25(3–4):150–176
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 179
Landgraf R, Wigger A (2002) High vs low anxiety-related behavior rats: an animal model of
extremes in trait anxiety. Behav Genet 32(5):301–314
Landgraf R, Wigger A, Holsboer F, Neumann ID (1999) Hyper-reactive hypothalamo-pituitary-
adrenocortical axis in rats bred for high anxiety-related behaviour. J Neuroendocrinol
11(6):405–407
Landgraf R, Kessler MS, Bunck M, Murgatroyd C, Spengler D, Zimbelmann M, Nussbaumer M,
Czibere L, Turck CW, Singewald N, Rujescu D, Frank E (2007) Candidate genes of anxiety-
related behavior in HAB/LAB rats and mice: focus on vasopressin and glyoxalase-I. Neurosci
Biobehav Rev 31(1):89–102
Lecrubier Y (2006) Physical components of depression and psychomotor retardation. J Clin
Psychiatry 67(Suppl 6):23–26
Leonard SK, Dwyer JM, Sukoff Rizzo SJ, Platt B, Logue SF, Neal SJ, Malberg JE, Beyer CE,
Schechter LE, Rosenzweig-Lipson S, Ring RH (2008) Pharmacology of neuropeptide s in
mice: therapeutic relevance to anxiety disorders. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 197(4):601–611
Lesch KP (1991) 5-HT1A receptor responsivity in anxiety disorders and depression. Prog
Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 15:723–733
Li TK, Lumeng L (1977) Alcohol metabolism of inbred strains of rats with alcohol preference
and nonpreference. Alcohol Aldehyde Metab Sys 3:625–633
Li TK, Lumeng L, Doolittle DP (1993) Selective breeding for alcohol preference and associated
responses. Behav Genet 23(2):163–170
Liebenberg N, Harvey BH, Brand L, Brink CB (2010) Antidepressant-like properties of
phosphodiesterase type 5 inhibitors and cholinergic dependency in a genetic rat model of
depression. Behav Pharmacol 21(5–6):540–547
Liebsch G, Linthorst AC, Neumann ID, Reul JM, Holsboer F, Landgraf R (1998a) Behavioral,
physiological, and neuroendocrine stress responses and differential sensitivity to diazepam in
two Wistar rat lines selectively bred for high- and low-anxiety-related behavior. Neuropsy-
chopharmacology 19(5):381–396
Liebsch G, Montkowski A, Holsboer F, Landgraf R (1998b) Behavioral profiles of two Wistar rat
lines selectively bred for high or low anxiety-related behaviour. Behav Brain Res 94(2):
301–310
Lindsay JR, Baker HJ (2006) Historical foundations. In: Suckow MA, Weisbroth SH (eds) The
laboratory rat. Elsevier Academic Press, Burlington, pp 1–52
López-Rubalcava C, Lucki I (2000) Strain differences in the behavioral effects of antidepressant
drugs in the rat forced swimming test. Neuropsychopharmacology 22(2):191–199
Lucassen PJ, Bosch OJ, Jousma E, Kromer SA, Andrew R, Seckl JR, Neumann ID (2009)
Prenatal stress reduces postnatal neurogenesis in rats selectively bred for high, but not low,
anxiety: Possible key role of placental 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2. Eur J
Neurosci 29(1):97–103
Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C (2009) Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on
the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 10(6):434–445
Maccari S, Darnaudery M, Morley-Fletcher S, Zuena AR, Cinque C, Van Reeth O (2003)
Prenatal stress and long-term consequences: implications of glucocorticoid hormones.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 27(1–2):119–127
Maier SF (1991) Stressor controllability, cognition and fear. In: Madden J (ed) Neurobiology,
learning, emotion and affect. Raven Press, New York, pp 155–193
Mantella RC, Vollmer RR, Li X, Amico JA (2003) Female oxytocin-deficient mice display
enhanced anxiety-related behavior. Endocrinology 144(6):2291–2296
Marescaux C, Micheletti G, Vergnes M (1984) A model of chronic spontaneous petit mal-like
seizures in the rat: comparison with pentylenetetrazol-induced seizures. Epilepsia 25(3):
326–331
Marescaux C, Vergnes M, Depaulis A (1992) Genetic absence epilepsy in rats from strasbourg––
a review. J Neural Transmis Suppl 35:37–69
Mathe AA, Jimenez PA, Theodorsson E, Stenfors C (1998) Neuropeptide y, neurokinin a and
neurotensin in brain regions of fawn hooded ‘‘depressed’’, Wistar, and Sprague Dawley rats.
180 G. Wegener et al.
Neumann ID (2008) Brain oxytocin: a key regulator of emotional and social behaviours in both
females and males. J Neuroendocrinol 20(6):858–865
Neumann ID (2009) The advantage of social living: brain neuropeptides mediate the beneficial
consequences of sex and motherhood. Front Neuroendocrinol 30(4):483–496
Neumann ID, Wigger A, Liebsch G, Holsboer F, Landgraf R (1998) Increased basal activity of
the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis during pregnancy in rats bred for high anxiety-related
behaviour. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23(5):449–463
Neumann ID, Torner L, Wigger A (2000) Brain oxytocin: differential inhibition of
neuroendocrine stress responses and anxiety-related behaviour in virgin, pregnant and
lactating rats. Neuroscience 95(2):567–575
Neumann ID, Kromer SA, Bosch OJ (2005a) Effects of psycho-social stress during pregnancy on
neuroendocrine and behavioural parameters in lactation depend on the genetically determined
stress vulnerability. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30(8):791–806
Neumann ID, Wigger A, Kromer S, Frank E, Landgraf R, Bosch OJ (2005b) Differential effects
of periodic maternal separation on adult stress coping in a rat model of extremes in trait
anxiety. Neuroscience 132(3):867–877
Neumann ID, Veenema AH, Beiderbeck DI (2010) Aggression and anxiety: social context and
neurobiological links. Front Behav Neurosci 4:12
Nikisch G, Mathe AA (2008) CSF monoamine metabolites and neuropeptides in depressed
patients before and after electroconvulsive therapy. Eur Psychiatry 23(5):356–359
Nikisch G, Agren H, Eap CB, Czernik A, Baumann P, Mathe AA (2005) Neuropeptide y and
corticotropin-releasing hormone in CSF mark response to antidepressive treatment with
citalopram. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 8(3):403–410
Nishi K, Kanemaru K, Diksic M (2009) A genetic rat model of depression, flinders sensitive line,
has a lower density of 5-HT(1A) receptors, but a higher density of 5-HT(1B) receptors,
compared to control rats. Neurochem Int 54(5–6):299–307
O’Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, Beveridge M, Glover V (2002) Maternal antenatal anxiety and
children’s behavioural/emotional problems at 4 years. Report from the Avon longitudinal
study of parents and children. Br J Psychiatry 180(JUNE):502–508
Ohl F, Toschi N, Wigger A, Henniger MS, Landgraf R (2001) Dimensions of emotionality in a rat
model of innate anxiety. Behav Neurosci 115(2):429–436
Ohl F, Roedel A, Storch C, Holsboer F, Landgraf R (2002) Cognitive performance in rats
differing in their inborn anxiety. Behav Neurosci 116(3):464–471
Okamoto K, Aoki K (1963) Development of a strain of spontaneously hypertensive rats. Jpn Circ
J 27:282–293
Oliveira RM, Guimaraes FS, Deakin JF (2008) Expression of neuronal nitric oxide synthase in
the hippocampal formation in affective disorders. Braz J Med Biol Res 41(4):333–341
Olsson A, Regnell G, Traskman-Bendz L, Ekman R, Westrin A (2004) Cerebrospinal
neuropeptide Y and substance P in suicide attempters during long-term antidepressant
treatment. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 14(6):479–485
Overmier JB, Seligman ME (1967) Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and
avoidance responding. J Comp Physiol Psychol 63(1):28–33
Overstreet DH (1986) Selective breeding for increased cholinergic function: development of a
new animal model of depression. Biol Psychiatry 21(1):49–58
Overstreet DH (1993) The flinders sensitive line rats: a genetic animal model of depression.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 17(1):51–68
Overstreet DH (2002) Behavioral characteristics of rat lines selected for differential hypothermic
responses to cholinergic or serotonergic agonists. Behav Genet 32(5):335–348
Overstreet DH, Griebel G (2004) Antidepressant-like effects of CRF1 receptor antagonist
SSR125543 in an animal model of depression. Eur J Pharmacol 497(1):49–53
Overstreet DH, Russell RW (1982) Selective breeding for diisopropyl fluorophosphate-
sensitivity: behavioural effects of cholinergic agonists and antagonists. Psychopharmacology
(Berl) 78(2):150–155
182 G. Wegener et al.
Overstreet DH, Russell RW (1984) Selective breeding for differences in cholinergic function: sex
differences in the genetic regulation of sensitivity to the anticholinesterase, DFP. Behav
Neural Biol 40(2):227–238
Overstreet DH, Russell RW, Helps SC, Messenger M (1979) Selective breeding for sensitivity to
the anticholinesterase DFP. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 65(1):15–20
Overstreet DH, Janowsky DS, Gillin JC, Shiromani PJ, Sutin EL (1986) Stress-induced
immobility in rats with cholinergic supersensitivity. Biol Psychiatry 21(7):657–664
Overstreet DH, Double K, Schiller GD (1989) Antidepressant effects of rolipram in a genetic
animal model of depression: Cholinergic supersensitivity and weight gain. Pharmacol
Biochem Behav 34(4):691–696
Overstreet DH, Rezvani AH, Janowsky DS (1990) Impaired active avoidance responding in rats
selectively bred for increased cholinergic function. Physiol Behav 47(4):787–788
Overstreet DH, Russell RW, Hay DA, Crocker AD (1992) Selective breeding for increased
cholinergic function: biometrical genetic analysis of muscarinic responses. Neuropsycho-
pharmacology 7(3):197–204
Overstreet DH, Rezvani AH, Pucilowski O, Gause L, Janowsky DS (1994) Rapid selection for
serotonin-1A sensitivity in rats. Psychiatr Genet 4(1):57–62
Overstreet DH, Pucilowski O, Rezvani AH, Janowsky DS (1995) Administration of antidepres-
sants, diazepam and psychomotor stimulants further confirms the utility of flinders sensitive
line rats as an animal model of depression. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 121(1):27–37
Overstreet DH, Pucilowski O, Retton MC, Delagrange P, Guardiola-Lemaitre B (1998) Effects of
melatonin receptor ligands on swim test immobility. Neuroreport 9(2):249–253
Overstreet DH, Hlavka J, Feighner JP, Nicolau G, Freed JS (2004a) Antidepressant-like effects of
a novel pentapeptide, nemifitide, in an animal model of depression. Psychopharmacology
(Berl) 175(3):303–309
Overstreet DH, Keeney A, Hogg S (2004b) Antidepressant effects of citalopram and CRF receptor
antagonist CP-154,526 in a rat model of depression. Eur J Pharmacol 492(2–3):195–201
Overstreet DH, Friedman E, Mathe AA, Yadid G (2005) The flinders sensitive line rat: a selectively
bred putative animal model of depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29(4–5):739–759
Overstreet DH, Stemmelin J, Griebel G (2008) Confirmation of antidepressant potential of the
selective beta3 adrenoceptor agonist amibegron in an animal model of depression. Pharmacol
Biochem Behav 89(4):623–626
Overstreet DH, Fredericks K, Knapp D, Breese G, McMichael J (2010a) Nerve growth factor
(NGF) has novel antidepressant-like properties in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 94(4):
553–560
Overstreet DH, Naimoli VM, Griebel G (2010b) Saredutant, an NK2 receptor antagonist, has both
antidepressant-like effects and synergizes with desipramine in an animal model of depression.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav 96(2):206–210
Owens MJ, Overstreet DH, Knight DL, Rezvani AH, Ritchie JC, Bissette G, Janowsky DS,
Nemeroff CB (1991) Alterations in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in a proposed
animal model of depression with genetic muscarinic supersensitivity. Neuropsychopharma-
cology 4(2):87–93
Paré WP (1992) Learning behavior, escape behavior, and depression in an ulcer susceptible rat
strain. Integr Physiol Behav Sci 27(2):130–141
Paré WP (1994) Open field, learned helplessness, conditioned defensive burying, and forced-
swim tests in WKY rats. Physiol Behav 55(3):433–439
Pare WP, Redei E (1993) Depressive behavior and stress ulcer in Wistar Kyoto rats. J Physiol
Paris 87(4):229–238
Pawlak CR, Ho YJ, Schwarting RKW (2008) Animal models of human psychopathology based
on individual differences in novelty-seeking and anxiety. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32(8):
1544–1568
Petersen A, Wortwein G, Gruber SH, Mathe AA (2008) Escitalopram reduces increased
hippocampal cytogenesis in a genetic rat depression model. Neurosci Lett 436(3):305–308
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 183
Rugulies R (2002) Depression as a predictor for coronary heart disease: a review and meta-
analysis. Am J Prevent Med 23(1):51–61
Russell RW, Overstreet DH (1987) Mechanisms underlying sensitivity to organophosphorus
anticholinesterase compounds. Prog Neurobiol 28(2):97–129
Russell RW, Overstreet DH, Messenger M, Helps SC (1982) Selective breeding for sensitivity to
DFP: generalization of effects beyond criterion variables. Pharmacol Biochem Behav
17(5):885–891
Russo-Neustadt A, Beard RC, Cotman CW (1999) Exercise, antidepressant medications, and enhanced
brain derived neurotrophic factor expression. Neuropsychopharmacology 21(5):679–682
Ryan B, Musazzi L, Mallei A, Tardito D, Gruber SH, El Khoury A, Anwyl R, Racagni G, Mathe
AA, Rowan MJ, Popoli M (2009) Remodelling by early-life stress of nmda receptor-
dependent synaptic plasticity in a gene-environment rat model of depression. Int J
Neuropsychopharmacol 12(4):553–559
Sadile AG, Gironi Carnevale UA, Vitullo E, Cioffi LA, Welzl H, Bättig K (1988) Maze learning
of the naples high- and low-excitability rat lines. Adv Biosci 70:177–180
Salchner P, Sartori SB, Sinner C, Wigger A, Frank E, Landgraf R, Singewald N (2006) Airjet and
FG-7142-induced fos expression differs in rats selectively bred for high and low anxiety-
related behavior. Neuropharmacology 50(8):1048–1058
Salome N, Salchner P, Viltart O, Sequeira H, Wigger A, Landgraf R, Singewald N (2004)
Neurobiological correlates of high (HAB) versus low anxiety-related behavior (LAB):
Differential FOS expression in HAB and LAB rats. Biol Psychiatry 55(7):715–723
Sandnabba NK (1996) Selective breeding for isolation-induced intermale aggression in mice:
associated responses and environmental influences. Behav Genet 26(5):477–488
Santarelli L, Saxe M, Gross C, Surget A, Battaglia F, Dulawa S, Weisstaub N, Lee J, Duman R,
Arancio O, Belzung C, Hen R (2003) Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the
behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science 301(5634):805–809
Sartori S, Hauschild M, Muigg P, Slattery DA, Biederbeck D, Mathe AA, Wegener G,
Neumann ID, Singewald N (2009) Modulation of impaired fear extinction by neuropeptides in
a psychopathological animal model of trait anxiety In: Society for Neuroscience, Chicago, IL,
2009. Society for Neuroscience, Neuroscience Meeting Planner
Sartorius A, Hellweg R, Litzke J, Vogt M, Dormann C, Vollmayr B, Danker-Hopfe H, Gass P
(2009) Correlations and discrepancies between serum and brain tissue levels of neurotrophins
after electroconvulsive treatment in rats. Pharmacopsychiatry 42(6):270–276
Schildkraut JJ (1965) The catecholamine hypothesis of affective disorders: A review of
supporting evidence. Am J Psychiatry 122(5):509–522
Schiller GD, Daws LC, Overstreet DH, Orbach J (1991) Lack of anxiety in an animal model of
depression with cholinergic supersensitivity. Brain Res Bull 26(3):433–435
Schiller GD, Pucilowski O, Wienicke C, Overstreet DH (1992) Immobility-reducing effects of
antidepressants in a genetic animal model of depression. Brain Res Bull 28(5):821–823
Shimizu E, Hashimoto K, Okamura N, Koike K, Komatsu N, Kumakiri C, Nakazato M,
Watanabe H, Shinoda N, Okada S, Iyo M (2003) Alterations of serum levels of brain-derived
neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in depressed patients with or without antidepressants. Biol
Psychiatry 54(1):70–75
Shir Y, Zeltser R, Vatine JJ, Carmi G, Belfer I, Zangen A, Overstreet D, Raber P, Seltzer Z
(2001) Correlation of intact sensibility and neuropathic pain-related behaviors in eight inbred
and outbred rat strains and selection lines. Pain 90(1–2):75–82
Shiromani PJ, Klemfuss H, Lucero S, Overstreet DH (1991) Diurnal rhythm of core body
temperature is phase advanced in a rodent model of depression. Biol Psychiatry
29(9):923–930
Slattery DA, Neumann ID (2010) Chronic icv oxytocin attenuates the pathological high anxiety
state of selectively bred Wistar rats. Neuropharmacology 58(1):56–61
Slattery DA, Finger BC, Malumby R, Wegener G, Mathe AA, Neumann ID (2008) Neuropeptide
s alters affective behaviour in high (HAB) but not low anxiety-related (LAB) rats In: Society
for neuroscience, Washington, D.C., 2008, Society for Neuroscience 25(10):1295-303
Selectively Bred Rodents as Models of Depression and Anxiety 185
Slattery DA, Neumann I, Cryan JF (2010) Transient inactivation of the infralimbic cortex induces
antidepressant-like effects in the rat. J Psychopharmacol 25(10):1295–1303
Solberg LC, Olson SL, Turek FW, Redei E (2001) Altered hormone levels and circadian rhythm
of activity in the WKY rat, a putative animal model of depression. Am J Physiol––Regul
Integr Comp Physiol 281(3):R786–R794
Solskov L, Lofgren B, Pold R, Kristiansen SB, Nielsen TT, Overstreet DH, Schmitz O,
Botker HE, Lund S, Wegener G (2010) Evaluation of the relationship between hyperinsu-
linaemia and myocardial ischaemia/reperfusion injury in a rat model of depression. Clin Sci
(Lond) 118(4):259–267
Tabb K, Boss-Williams KA, Weiss JM, Weinshenker D (2007) Rats bred for susceptibility to
depression-like phenotypes have higher kainic acid-induced seizure mortality than their
depression-resistant counterparts. Epilepsy Res 74(2–3):140–146
Tazumi T, Hori E, Uwano T, Umeno K, Tanebe K, Tabuchi E, Ono T, Nishijo H (2005) Effects of
prenatal maternal stress by repeated cold environment on behavioral and emotional
development in the rat offspring. Behav Brain Res 162(1):153–160
Thase ME, Kupfer DJ, Buysse DJ, Frank E, Simons AD, McEachran AB, Rashid KF,
Grochocinski VJ (1995) Electroencephalographic sleep profiles in single-episode and
recurrent unipolar forms of major depression: I. Comparison during acute depressive states.
Biol Psychiatry 38(8):506–515
Torner L, Toschi N, Pohlinger A, Landgraf R, Neumann ID (2001) Anxiolytic and anti-stress
effects of brain prolactin: improved efficacy of antisense targeting of the prolactin receptor by
molecular modeling. J Neurosci 21(9):3207–3214
Torner L, Maloumby R, Nava G, Aranda J, Clapp C, Neumann ID (2004) In vivo release and
gene upregulation of brain prolactin in response to physiological stimuli. Eur J Neurosci
19(6):1601–1608
Touma C, Bunck M, Glasl L, Nussbaumer M, Palme R, Stein H, Wolferstatter M, Zeh R,
Zimbelmann M, Holsboer F, Landgraf R (2008) Mice selected for high versus low stress
reactivity: a new animal model for affective disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33(6):
839–862
Tschopp TB, Zucker MB (1972) Hereditary defect in platelet function in rats. Blood 40(2):
217–226
Van Den Bergh BRH, Mulder EJH, Mennes M, Glover V (2005) Antenatal maternal anxiety and
stress and the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: links and possible
mechanisms. A Rev Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29(2):237–258
Van Luijtelaar ELJM, Coenen AML (1986) Two types of electrocortical paroxysms in an inbred
strain of rats. Neurosci Lett 70(3):393–397
Van Luijtelaar ELJM, Coenen AML (1989) The WAG/RIJ model for generalized absence
seizures. Adv Epileptol 17:78–83
van Oortmerssen GA, Bakker TC (1981) Artificial selection for short and long attack latencies in
wild mus musculus domesticus. Behav Genet 11(2):115–126
Vedolin GM, Lobato VV, Conti PC, Lauris JR (2009) The impact of stress and anxiety on the
pressure pain threshold of myofascial pain patients. J Oral Rehabil 36(5):313–321
Veenema AH, Torner L, Blume A, Beiderbeck DI, Neumann ID (2007) Low inborn anxiety
correlates with high intermale aggression: link to acth response and neuronal activation of the
hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus. Horm Behav 51(1):11–19
Vergnes M, Marescaux C, Micheletti G (1982) Spontaneous paroxysmal electroclinical patterns
in rat: a model of generalized non-convulsive epilepsy. Neurosci Lett 33(1):97–101
Videbech P, Ravnkilde B (2004) Hippocampal volume and depression: a meta-analysis of mri
studies. Am J Psychiatry 161(11):1957–1966
Viggiano D, Vallone D, Ruocco LA, Sadile AG (2003) Behavioural, pharmacological, morpho-
functional molecular studies reveal a hyperfunctioning mesocortical dopamine system in an
animal model of attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 27(7):
683–689
186 G. Wegener et al.
Contents
1 Introduction........................................................................................................................ 190
2 Stress-Induced Executive Dysfunction in Humans and Rodents .................................... 190
3 Structural Changes in Response to Stress ........................................................................ 193
4 Molecular Mechanisms of Structural Changes: Focus on Glutamate ............................. 195
5 Glutamate in Rodent Behavioral Correlates of Emotion and Cognition ........................ 196
6 Conclusions........................................................................................................................ 199
References................................................................................................................................ 200
1 Introduction
express greater aversive feelings toward negative stimuli, find themselves preoc-
cupied with worry and are impaired in their ability to suppress feelings of fear
(Blechert et al. 2007; Michael et al. 2007). Individuals with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), depression or schizophrenia have deficits in cer-
tain learning and memory functions such as spatial working memory and cognitive
flexibility (McLean et al. 2004; Murphy et al. 2003; Murray et al. 2008; Rogers
et al. 2004; Waltz and Gold 2007). Using various stress paradigms, animal models
have begun to show evidence for a direct link between stress and emotional or
cognitive dysfunction.
In rodents, exposure to stress can result in increased anxiety- and depression-
related behaviors (Sterner and Kalynchuk 2010). Adult rodents that experienced
maternal separation as pups, a model of early life stress, have heightened corti-
costerone secretion following stress and show greater anxiety in the elevated plus-
maze (e.g. Eiland and McEwen 2012; Holmes et al. 2005; Huot et al. 2001).
Rodents exposed to chronic unpredictable stress (CUS) or repeated restraint stress,
or simply injected with corticosterone, show elevated anxiety- like behaviors in the
light/dark exploration and the elevated plus-maze test, and a heightened startle
response relative to non-stressed controls (e.g. Mozhui et al. 2010; Pego et al.
2008). Cross-strain comparisons indicate the effect of stress is more pronounced in
mouse strains with greater trait anxiety-like behavior, suggesting genetic back-
ground influences stress susceptibility (Mizoguchi et al. 2000). In addition to
heightened anxiety, stress can evoke a behavioral profile reminiscent of depres-
sion. Rodents exposed to a stressor or those that received corticosterone orally or
systemically show reduced social interaction and increased behavioral despair (e.g.
Berton et al. 2006; Gourley et al. 2008; Shirayama et al. 2002; Wood et al. 2008).
History of maternal separation or oral corticosterone delivery can decrease sucrose
consumption or responding in an appetitive progressive ratio test, measures of
anhedonia (Gourley et al. 2009, 2008; Huot et al. 2001).
Also sensitive to stress are fear conditioning and extinction, measures of
emotional learning and regulation (Rodrigues et al. 2009). Stress prior to condi-
tioning has a potentiating effect on fear learning resulting in greater freezing
during fear conditioning and post-conditioning fear recall tests (Rau et al. 2005;
Sandi et al. 2001; Wood et al. 2008; Yamamoto et al. 2009). A series of experi-
ments examining the effect of footshock stress prior to fear conditioning found that
stressed rats showed enhanced fear recall when tested the following day (Rau et al.
2005). Using variants of this basic design, fear generalization and reinstatement
were ruled out as alternate explanations for the increase in fear. Rather it appears
that stress strengthens the fear memory, which may explain the impairing effects of
stress on fear extinction, the learned suppression and the fear response (Corcoran
and Quirk 2007; Quirk and Mueller 2008). Rats exposed to restraint stress or given
oral corticosterone fail to extinguish the fear response, retaining a greater level of
freezing after extinction training as compared to non-stressed subjects (Baran et al.
2009; Gourley et al. 2009; Miracle et al. 2006). In mice, a mere three days of
10 min forced swim stress was sufficient to impair fear extinction (Izquierdo et al.
2006). Stress does not have to immediately precede fear conditioning to affect
192 C. Graybeal et al.
learning. Rat pups exposed to five days of footshock stress and then tested on fear
conditioning as adults fail to extinguish the fear response showing that stress can
have long-lasting repercussions on cognition (Judo et al. 2010).
The effects of stress are not limited to cognitive processes associated with
emotionality (Arnsten 2009; Holmes and Wellman 2009; Sterner and Kalynchuk
2010). Working memory, the ability to transiently store, recall and utilize recently
learned information is vulnerable to stress. Mice exposed to four weeks of cold
water submersion are impaired in a delayed alternation T-maze task (Mizoguchi
et al. 2000). In rats, four weeks or merely six days of CUS result in impaired
spatial working memory in the Morris water maze (MWM) (Cerqueira et al. 2005).
This deficit is replicated by four weeks of daily injections of dexamethasone, an
agonist of the glucocorticoid receptor, one of the two main corticosterone recep-
tors, suggesting that the effects of stress are mediated at least in part by gluco-
corticoid receptor activity (Cerqueira et al. 2005).
Spatial learning itself is disrupted by stress. Rats that experienced maternal
separation were impaired in an object placement task which examines the ability to
discriminate between objects placed in the same or a novel location (Eiland and
McEwen 2012). A single session of restraint stress or a corticosterone injection
resulted in mice having longer escape latencies in circular hole board maze
(Schwabe et al. 2010). In the hidden platform version of the MWM, tail shock
stressed rats had greater escape latencies than non-stressed rats (Kim et al. 2001).
Predator stress, where the rodent is exposed to either the odor of or an actual
predator, impairs spatial working memory in the radial arm version of the MWM
(RAWM) (Diamond et al. 1999; Park et al. 2008). In the RAWM, up to six swim
paths are available from a center start point, one of which leads to the escape
platform. An effect of stress was not present when just four arms were available.
However, increasing the difficulty of the task by presenting six arms reveals an
effect of stress, suggesting that observable effects of stress may be dependent on
task demands (Diamond et al. 1999). Pretreatment with the antidepressants ago-
melatine or tianeptine can rescue this effect (Campbell et al. 2008; Conboy et al.
2009).
Stress also interferes with cognitive flexibility, the ability to adjust previously
learned behavior in response to changing task demands. Rats subjected to either
four weeks of CUS or dexamethasone injections or six just days of CUS were
impaired on a spatial reversal variant of the MWM, where the escape platform is
switched to the quadrant opposite from where it was located during training
(Cerqueira et al. 2007; Cerqueira et al. 2005). Two weeks of CUS impaired
reversal and extra-dimension set shifting in a texture-odor attention set-shifting
task (Bondi et al. 2008). In this rodent adaptation of human cognitive flexibility
tasks, the rodent learns to discriminate between digging textures or odors to locate
a hidden reward. This stress-induced set-shifting impairment is robust, having
been replicated with a number of different stress protocols (Bondi et al. 2008;
Lapiz-Bluhm et al. 2009; Liston et al. 2006) and has been shown to be rescued
with the antidepressants desipramine and citalopram (Bondi et al. 2008;
Lapiz-Bluhm et al. 2009).
Stress-Induced Deficits in Cognition and Emotionality 193
From the subset of work highlighted here, there is strong evidence to support
the link between stress exposure and altered emotionality and cognitive function.
Similar to what is seen in clinical populations, in rodent models stress increases
anxiety- and depression-like behaviors, and facilitates the acquisition of fear while
impairing control over fear expression. Stress also impairs certain cognitive
functions such as spatial learning, working memory and cognitive flexibility. How
stress may be affecting these changes via morphological and molecular alterations
will be discussed in the following sections.
Key substrates mediating emotionality and cognition include the prefrontal cortex,
hippocampus and amygdala. Changes in cortical volume have been recorded in
these regions in human neuropsychiatric patients suggesting possible loci for the
behavioral and cognitive pathologies (Sterner and Kalynchuk 2010; van Harmelen
et al. 2010). These regions are anatomically interconnected, often working in
concert to mediate emotional regulation and cognition (Thierry et al. 2000).
Preclinical studies in rodents have not only shown parallel changes but have
provided more precise information on morphological changes in these areas.
Loss of prefrontal cortex (PFC) function can impair working memory and
cognitive flexibility as well as other higher-order cognitive functions (Chudasama
and Robbins 2006; Dalley et al. 2004; Robbins 2007). In rodents, the prelimbic
and infralimbic cortices are subregions of the PFC thought to mediate fear
expression and fear extinction, respectively (Corcoran and Quirk 2007; Myers and
Davis 2007; Quirk and Mueller 2008). Four weeks of CUS result in neuronal
atrophy, specifically reduced volume, neuron number and apical dendritic length in
layers I–III in both these regions (Cerqueira et al. 2005; Dias-Ferreira et al. 2009).
This stress effect is replicated pharmacologically with four weeks of corticosterone
or dexamethasone, a corticosterone-receptor agonist, injections (Cerqueira et al.
2005). In addition to neuronal atrophy, decreases in spine density and spine surface
area have been observed in the medial PFC following exposure to three weeks of
restraint stress (Liston et al. 2006; Radley et al. 2004, 2006, 2008). Interestingly,
while restraint stress reduced apical dendritic length in the medial PFC, there was a
corresponding increase in the orbitofrontal cortex, suggesting that subregions of
the PFC may respond differently to stress (Liston et al. 2006). Shorter stress
exposures have highlighted the potential sensitivity of the PFC to stress. A reduced
restraint stress paradigm, ten days of two-hour restraint stress, reduced apical
dendritic length in rat infralimbic cortex (Shansky and Morrison 2009). Seven days
of only ten-minute daily restraint stress was effective in reducing apical branch
number and length in layer II/III of the cingulate cortex (Brown et al. 2005). Even
more limited, one bout of ten-minute forced swim stress was suffient to reduce
apical dendritic length in layer II/III of the mouse infralimbic cortex though no
change was seen in the prelimbic cortex (Izquierdo et al. 2006).
194 C. Graybeal et al.
Also key in the fear learning circuitry are the hippocampus and amygdala (Maren
and Quirk 2004). The hippocampus has a well-established role in learning and
memory and is particularly important for spatial learning, as in the MWM or with
contextual fear learning (Bird and Burgess 2008; Ji and Maren 2007; Maren and Holt
2000). Stress has a similar effect on morphology in the hippocampus as it does in the
PFC (Joels et al. 2004; McEwen 2001). Five weeks of social conflict decreased cell
proliferation and survival in the rat dentate gyrus, an effect rescued by concomitant
fluoxetine treatment (Czeh et al. 2007). Rats exposed to either a brief (two days) or
chronic (21 days) social stressor induced morphology changes, though changes were
more robust following chronic stress (Kole et al. 2004). Four weeks of CUS or
pharmacological stress by corticosterone or dexamethasone injections resulted in
reduced CA3 and DG volume and decreased dendritic length of granule cells, CA3
and CA1 pyramidal cells (Cerqueira et al. 2007; Sousa et al. 2000). Shorter durations
of stress, 21 days of restraint stress or ten days of CUS, also results in hippocampal
dendritic retraction which was reversed when tianeptine was given in conjunction
with stress (Vyas et al. 2002; Watanabe et al. 1992b).
In contrast to the PFC and hippocampus, stress induces neuronal hypertrophy in
the amygdala, a region essential for the acquisition and consolidation of aversive
memories (Maren and Quirk 2004; Pare et al. 2004; Roozendaal et al. 2009). While
21 days of restraint stress decreased hippocampal dendritic length, this same stressor
increased dendritic length and branching in neurons of the basolateral amygdala
(BLA) as well as the bed nucleus of stria terminalis (Vyas et al. 2002, 2003). This
effect has been replicated with either CUS or corticosterone injections which lead to
increases in dendritic length and spine density in the BLA and bed nucleus of stria
terminalis (Pego et al. 2008). Stress-induced changes appear to be longer lasting in
the amygdala than in the hippocampus or PFC. Following 21 days of recovery from
stress, BLA pyramidal cells of stressed rats were still longer than controls, whereas
CA3 hippocampal neurons were no different and overextension was seen in proximal
dendritic arbors of IL neurons (Goldwater et al. 2009; Vyas et al. 2004).
As with behavior, stress results in different changes in neuronal structure in the
PFC, hippocampus and amygdala. Generally, stress-induced dendritic retraction in
the PFC and hippocampus but induced extension in the amygdala. This appears to
align with the differing effects of stress on behavior, with impairments in largely PFC
and hippocampus-associated behaviors and enhancements in amygdala-associated
behaviors. However, it is unlikely that structural changes alone are the cause of
stress-induced behavioral changes. More likely stress results in a cascade of changes
on a molecular level of which modification to structure is an observable consequence.
It is clear that exposure to stress can alter neuronal morphology in regions asso-
ciated with the emotional and cognitive changes seen with psychiatric disorders.
Stress can alter multiple molecular signaling cascades that could explain the
Stress-Induced Deficits in Cognition and Emotionality 195
induced by the learned helplessness paradigm (Li et al. 2010). NMDA receptor
antagonists not only block stress-induced atrophy but have also been found to
increase spine density in medial PFC pyramidal cells 24 h after administration in
the absence of stress (Li et al. 2010). While additional data directly studying the
role of glutamate transmission in each region showing stress-induced structural
changes is sparse, much is known about the effects of glutamate transmission in
cognitive and emotional behavioral paradigms mediated by these regions.
knockdown in the BLA still blocks the stress-induced behavioral changes in the
light/dark exploration task.
Prenatal deletion of the NR1 or NR2B subunit is lethal. However, several
genetic models with reduced NR1 or NR2B levels have been successfully used.
With the development of a floxed NR1 mouse line, it has been possible to study the
effects of NR1 deletion in specific regions of the brain. One such model limited
NR1 deletion to hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells and found a deficit in trace, but
not delay, fear conditioning and impaired spatial (MWM, T-maze), but not non-
spatial memory (Huerta et al. 2000; Tonegawa et al. 1996; Tsien et al. 1996a,
1996b). Mice with a deletion of NR1 restricted to hippocampal CA3 pyramidal
cells are impaired in retention of one-trial context discrimination at three h, but not
24 h after avoidance training (Cravens et al. 2006). Similar results were found in
mice with the NR2B subunit deleted in CA1 and cortical pyramidal cells (Brigman
et al. 2010). Mice lacking the NR2B subunit on pyramidal cells of the cortex and
CA1 also showed multiple learning and memory deficits, including in the MWM,
T-maze and trace, but not delay fear conditioning. These same mice additionally
showed decreased spine density in pyramidal cells of the CA1. While they dem-
onstrated normal depressive-like behavior in the six-minute forced swim test, these
mice developed less depressive-like behavior over the course of a novel 10 day
swim stress paradigm, indicating a phenotype specific to repeated stress
(Kiselycznyk et al. 2011).
Ketamine and other NMDA receptor antagonists have acute behavioral effects
in humans and rodent models that appear to resemble many of the symptoms of
schizophrenia. This has in part led to the NMDA receptor hypofunction hypoth-
esis, suggesting that NMDA receptor hypofunction on inhibitory neurons in the
PFC is responsible for excessive neuronal excitability in this region thus disrupting
executive functions. Mice that are viable with only 5–10% of the normal NR1
levels have been used to test the relationship between NR1 subunits and schizo-
phrenia. These mice have decreased prepulse inhibition, sociability and anxiety-
like behavior but increased locomotion, a constellation of behaviors similar to
those seen in patients (Halene et al. 2009). A recent study found that deletion of
NR1 on interneurons during early postnatal development resulted in schizophre-
nia-like phenotypes in mice. This included novelty-induced hyperlocomotion,
impaired nest building and mating, increased anxiety- and anhedonia-like
behaviors, behaviors that were exacerbated by social isolation stress (Belforte et al.
2010). The schizophrenia-like behavior was selective to mice with early postnatal
deletion (approximately equivalent to late gestation to age 2 in humans) and was
not observed in mice with post-adolescence NR1 deletion. This is consistent with
hypotheses proposing a developmental cause in schizophrenia, with stress exac-
erbating deficits in cortical development.
An additional post-synaptic receptor of glutamate, the AMPA receptor, is also
essential for synaptic plasticity. The AMPA receptor is a heteromeric receptor
composed of a combination of four subunits: GluR1-GluR4. Various anti-
depressants with diverse structures have been found to alter the phosphorylation
of GluR1 receptors, therefore altering glutamatergic synaptic transmission
198 C. Graybeal et al.
viable and have a phenotype resembling the positive and negative symptoms of
schizophrenia, including increased novelty-induced hyperlocomotion that is
reversed by the antipsychotic haloperidol, abnormal sociability, reduced acoustic
startle response and impaired visual discrimination (Karlsson et al. 2008, 2009). It
is not known, however, how a deficit in glutamate transporters affects depression-
related behaviors or response to stress. Future studies could investigate the role of
glutamate transporters in buffering pyramidal cells to the increased glutamate
release during stress.
Because of their role in synaptic plasticity, these glutamatergic receptors have
been extensively studied in animal models of cognition. Animals lacking these
receptors often show deficits in cognitive-related tasks that overlap those seen in
models of neuropsychiatric disorders. Their role in emotional-related behaviors
and stress response, however, has not been systematically studied and still remains
untested in many of these mutant models. There have been examples of baseline
alterations in emotionality (as with the NR2A mice) and schizophrenia-related
behaviors, suggesting that dysfunction of the glutamatergic system alone can result
in pathology. A more nuanced approach, however, is seen in those studies ana-
lyzing changes in the normal stress response in animals with alterations in the
glutamatergic system. As mentioned earlier, stress can act as a predisposing factor
for many neuropsychiatric illnesses and it is important to see how stress can lead to
a disruption in cognition and emotional regulation. Here, we briefly discussed the
dysregulated stress response in NMDA, but not AMPA, receptor-related manip-
ulations. In the future, it will be interesting to see studies of stress response applied
to more models of glutamatergic function.
6 Conclusions
In its mature form, BDNF promotes neuronal growth and survival and its release is
dependent on glutamatergic synaptic activity (Carvalho et al. 2008; Lu 2003).
BDNF itself also influences glutamatergic transmission, altering the release of
glutamate, glutamate receptor composition and synaptic plasticity (Carvalho et al.
2008; Kuczewski et al. 2009; Lu 2003). BDNF expression is reduced with stress,
an effect reversed by antidepressants, and has itself been shown to be reduced in
depression-like behaviors (Gourley et al. 2009; Nibuya et al. 1995; Shirayama
et al. 2002; Siuciak et al. 1997). Thus far few studies have collectively examined
the effects of stress, morphology and behavior in relation to BDNF. Preliminary
work has shown that genetically induced BDNF overexpression can prevent stress-
induced hippocampal dendritic retraction and reduced learned helplessness,
making BDNF a particularly relevant molecule in the study of stress and cognition
(Govindarajan et al. 2006). The contributions of BDNF and other downstream
signaling pathways will be important targets elucidating the mechanisms of stress-
induced morphological changes.
In addition, many studies have used genetic techniques to understand the
contribution of glutamate receptors in emotional and cognitive behavioral tasks.
These studies have shown that changes in activity and composition of glutamate
receptors can lead to changes in emotional regulation and cognitive deficits.
However, few studies have used these models to directly investigate the role of
receptors in the context of stress. These receptors are integrally involved in syn-
aptic plasticity mechanisms, thus examining the effects of stress on these receptors
could help explain why stress is a precursor to the development of neuropsychi-
atric disorders.
Furthering our understanding of how stress affects glutamate receptors and
alters downstream signaling pathways will ultimately contribute to the develop-
ment of improved therapeutics for individuals suffering from neuropsychiatric
disorders.
References
Belforte JE, Zsiros V, Sklar ER, Jiang Z, Yu G, Li Y, Quinlan EM, Nakazawa K (2010) Postnatal
NMDA receptor ablation in corticolimbic interneurons confers schizophrenia-like pheno-
types. Nat Neurosci 13:76–83
Berton O, McClung CA, Dileone RJ, Krishnan V, Renthal W, Russo SJ, Graham D, Tsankova
NM, Bolanos CA, Rios M, Monteggia LM, Self DW, Nestler EJ (2006) Essential role of
BDNF in the mesolimbic dopamine pathway in social defeat stress. Science 311:864–868
Bird CM, Burgess N (2008) The hippocampus and memory: insights from spatial processing. Nat
Rev Neurosci 9:182–194
Blechert J, Michael T, Vriends N, Margraf J, Wilhelm FH (2007) Fear conditioning in
posttraumatic stress disorder: evidence for delayed extinction of autonomic, experiential, and
behavioural responses. Behav Res Ther 45:2019–2033
Bondi CO, Rodriguez G, Gould GG, Frazer A, Morilak DA (2008) Chronic unpredictable stress
induces a cognitive deficit and anxiety-like behavior in rats that is prevented by chronic
antidepressant drug treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:320–331
Boyce-Rustay JM, Holmes A (2006) Genetic inactivation of the NMDA receptor NR2A subunit
has anxiolytic- and antidepressant-like effects in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology
31:2405–2414
Brigman JL, Wright T, Talani G, Prasad-Mulcare S, Jinde S, Seabold GK, Mathur P, Davis MI,
Bock R, Gustin RM, Colbran RJ, Alvarez VA, Nakazawa K, Delpire E, Lovinger DM,
Holmes A (2010) Loss of GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors in CA1 hippocampus and
cortex impairs long-term depression, reduces dendritic spine density, and disrupts learning.
J Neurosci 30:4590–4600
Brown SM, Henning S, Wellman CL (2005) Mild, short-term stress alters dendritic morphology
in rat medial prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 15:1714–1722
Campbell AM, Park CR, Zoladz PR, Munoz C, Fleshner M, Diamond DM (2008) Pre-training
administration of tianeptine, but not propranolol, protects hippocampus-dependent memory
from being impaired by predator stress. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 18:87–98
Carvalho AL, Caldeira MV, Santos SD, Duarte CB (2008) Role of the brain-derived neurotrophic
factor at glutamatergic synapses. Br J Pharmacol 153(1):S310–S324
Cerqueira JJ, Mailliet F, Almeida OF, Jay TM, Sousa N (2007) The prefrontal cortex as a key
target of the maladaptive response to stress. J Neurosci 27:2781–2787
Cerqueira JJ, Pego JM, Taipa R, Bessa JM, Almeida OF, Sousa N (2005) Morphological
correlates of corticosteroid-induced changes in prefrontal cortex-dependent behaviors.
J Neurosci 25:7792–7800
Choudary PV, Molnar M, Evans SJ, Tomita H, Li JZ, Vawter MP, Myers RM, Bunney WE Jr,
Akil H, Watson SJ, Jones EG (2005) Altered cortical glutamatergic and GABAergic
signal transmission with glial involvement in depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
102:15653–15658
Chudasama Y, Robbins TW (2006) Functions of frontostriatal systems in cognition: comparative
neuropsychopharmacological studies in rats, monkeys and humans. Biol Psychol 73:19–38
Ciabarra AM, Sullivan JM, Gahn LG, Pecht G, Heinemann S, Sevarino KA (1995) Cloning and
characterization of chi-1: a developmentally regulated member of a novel class of the
ionotropic glutamate receptor family. J Neurosci 15:6498–6508
Conboy L, Tanrikut C, Zoladz PR, Campbell AM, Park CR, Gabriel C, Mocaer E, Sandi C,
Diamond DM (2009) The antidepressant agomelatine blocks the adverse effects of stress on
memory and enables spatial learning to rapidly increase neural cell adhesion molecule
(NCAM) expression in the hippocampus of rats. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 12:329–341
Corcoran KA, Quirk GJ (2007) Activity in prelimbic cortex is necessary for the expression of
learned, but not innate, fears. J Neurosci 27:840–844
Cravens CJ, Vargas-Pinto N, Christian KM, Nakazawa K (2006) CA3 NMDA receptors
are crucial for rapid and automatic representation of context memory. Eur J Neurosci
24:1771–1780
Czeh B, Muller-Keuker JI, Rygula R, Abumaria N, Hiemke C, Domenici E, Fuchs E (2007)
Chronic social stress inhibits cell proliferation in the adult medial prefrontal cortex:
202 C. Graybeal et al.
Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C (2009) Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on
the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:434–445
Magarinos AM, McEwen BS (1995) Stress-induced atrophy of apical dendrites of hippocampal
CA3c neurons: comparison of stressors. Neuroscience 69:83–88
Maren S, Holt W (2000) The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian
conditioning. Behav Brain Res 110:97–108
Maren S, Quirk GJ (2004) Neuronal signalling of fear memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:844–852
Martin KP, Wellman CL (2011). NMDA receptor blockade alters stress-induced dendritic
remodeling in medial prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 21(10):2366–2373
Matsuzaki M, Honkura N, Ellis-Davies GC, Kasai H (2004) Structural basis of long-term
potentiation in single dendritic spines. Nature 429:761–766
McCullumsmith RE, Meador-Woodruff JH (2002) Striatal excitatory amino acid transporter
transcript expression in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depressive disorder.
Neuropsychopharmacology 26:368–375
McEwen BS (2000a) Allostasis and allostatic load: implications for neuropsychopharmacology.
Neuropsychopharmacology 22:108–124
McEwen BS (2000b) The neurobiology of stress: from serendipity to clinical relevance. Brain
Res 886:172–189
McEwen BS (2001) Plasticity of the hippocampus: adaptation to chronic stress and allostatic
load. Ann N Y Acad Sci 933:265–277
McLean A, Dowson J, Toone B, Young S, Bazanis E, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ (2004)
Characteristic neurocognitive profile associated with adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder. Psychol Med 34:681–692
Michael T, Blechert J, Vriends N, Margraf J, Wilhelm FH (2007) Fear conditioning in panic
disorder: Enhanced resistance to extinction. J Abnorm Psychol 116:612–617
Miracle AD, Brace MF, Huyck KD, Singler SA, Wellman CL (2006) Chronic stress impairs
recall of extinction of conditioned fear. Neurobiol Learn Mem 85:213–218
Mizoguchi K, Yuzurihara M, Ishige A, Sasaki H, Chui DH, Tabira T (2000) Chronic stress
induces impairment of spatial working memory because of prefrontal dopaminergic
dysfunction. J Neurosci 20:1568–1574
Moghaddam B (1993) Stress preferentially increases extraneuronal levels of excitatory amino
acids in the prefrontal cortex: comparison to hippocampus and basal ganglia. J Neurochem
60:1650–1657
Mozhui K, Karlsson RM, Kash TL, Ihne J, Norcross M, Patel S, Farrell MR, Hill EE, Graybeal C,
Martin KP, Camp M, Fitzgerald PJ, Ciobanu DC, Sprengel R, Mishina M, Wellman CL,
Winder DG, Williams RW, Holmes A (2010) Strain differences in stress responsivity are
associated with divergent amygdala gene expression and glutamate-mediated neuronal
excitability. J Neurosci 30:5357–5367
Murphy FC, Michael A, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ (2003) Neuropsychological impairment in
patients with major depressive disorder: the effects of feedback on task performance. Psychol
Med 33:455–467
Murray GK, Cheng F, Clark L, Barnett JH, Blackwell AD, Fletcher PC, Robbins TW, Bullmore
ET, Jones PB (2008) Reinforcement and reversal learning in first-episode psychosis.
Schizophr Bull 34:848–855
Myers KM, Davis M (2007) Mechanisms of fear extinction. Mol Psychiatry 12:120–150
Nibuya M, Morinobu S, Duman RS (1995) Regulation of BDNF and trkB mRNA in rat brain by
chronic electroconvulsive seizure and antidepressant drug treatments. J Neurosci
15:7539–7547
Olney JW, Collins RC, Sloviter RS (1986) Excitotoxic mechanisms of epileptic brain damage.
Adv Neurol 44:857–877
Pare D, Quirk GJ, Ledoux JE (2004) New vistas on amygdala networks in conditioned fear.
J Neurophysiol 92:1–9
Stress-Induced Deficits in Cognition and Emotionality 205
Park CR, Zoladz PR, Conrad CD, Fleshner M, Diamond DM (2008) Acute predator stress impairs
the consolidation and retrieval of hippocampus-dependent memory in male and female rats.
Learn Mem 15:271–280
Pego JM, Morgado P, Pinto LG, Cerqueira JJ, Almeida OF, Sousa N (2008) Dissociation of the
morphological correlates of stress-induced anxiety and fear. Eur J Neurosci 27:1503–1516
Pittenger C, Duman RS (2008) Stress, depression, and neuroplasticity: a convergence of
mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:88–109
Quirk GJ, Mueller D (2008) Neural mechanisms of extinction learning and retrieval.
Neuropsychopharmacology 33:56–72
Radley JJ, Rocher AB, Miller M, Janssen WG, Liston C, Hof PR, McEwen BS, Morrison JH
(2006) Repeated stress induces dendritic spine loss in the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Cereb
Cortex 16:313–320
Radley JJ, Rocher AB, Rodriguez A, Ehlenberger DB, Dammann M, McEwen BS, Morrison JH,
Wearne SL, Hof PR (2008) Repeated stress alters dendritic spine morphology in the rat
medial prefrontal cortex. J Comp Neurol 507:1141–1150
Radley JJ, Sisti HM, Hao J, Rocher AB, McCall T, Hof PR, McEwen BS, Morrison JH (2004)
Chronic behavioral stress induces apical dendritic reorganization in pyramidal neurons of the
medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience 125:1–6
Rao VL, Dogan A, Bowen KK, Todd KG, Dempsey RJ (2001a) Antisense knockdown of the glial
glutamate transporter GLT-1 exacerbates hippocampal neuronal damage following traumatic
injury to rat brain. Eur J Neurosci 13:119–128
Rao VL, Dogan A, Todd KG, Bowen KK, Kim BT, Rothstein JD, Dempsey RJ (2001b) Antisense
knockdown of the glial glutamate transporter GLT-1, but not the neuronal glutamate
transporter EAAC1, exacerbates transient focal cerebral ischemia-induced neuronal damage
in rat brain. J Neurosci 21:1876–1883
Rau V, DeCola JP, Fanselow MS (2005) Stress-induced enhancement of fear learning: an animal
model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:1207–1223
Robbins TW (2007) Shifting and stopping: fronto-striatal substrates, neurochemical modulation
and clinical implications. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 362:917–932
Rodrigues SM, LeDoux JE, Sapolsky RM (2009) The influence of stress hormones on fear
circuitry. Annu Rev Neurosci 32:289–313
Rogers MA, Kasai K, Koji M, Fukuda R, Iwanami A, Nakagome K, Fukuda M, Kato N (2004)
Executive and prefrontal dysfunction in unipolar depression: a review of neuropsychological
and imaging evidence. Neurosci Res 50:1–11
Roozendaal B, McEwen BS, Chattarji S (2009) Stress, memory and the amygdala. Nat Rev
Neurosci 10(6):423–433
Rosenmund C, Stern-Bach Y, Stevens CF (1998) The tetrameric structure of a glutamate receptor
channel. Science 280:1596–1599
Rothstein JD, Dykes-Hoberg M, Pardo CA, Bristol LA, Jin L, Kuncl RW, Kanai Y, Hediger MA,
Wang Y, Schielke JP, Welty DF (1996) Knockout of glutamate transporters reveals a major
role for astroglial transport in excitotoxicity and clearance of glutamate. Neuron 16:675–686
Sandi C, Merino JJ, Cordero MI, Touyarot K, Venero C (2001) Effects of chronic stress on
contextual fear conditioning and the hippocampal expression of the neural cell adhesion
molecule, its polysialylation, and L1. Neuroscience 102:329–339
Sapolsky RM (2000a) Glucocorticoids and hippocampal atrophy in neuropsychiatric disorders.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 57:925–935
Sapolsky RM (2000b) The possibility of neurotoxicity in the hippocampus in major depression: a
primer on neuron death. Biol Psychiatry 48:755–765
Schneiderman N, Ironson G, Siegel SD (2005) Stress and health: psychological, behavioral, and
biological determinants. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 1:607–628
Schwabe L, Schachinger H, de Kloet ER, Oitzl MS (2010) Stress impairs spatial but not early
stimulus–response learning. Behav Brain Res 213:50–55
Shansky RM, Morrison JH (2009) Stress-induced dendritic remodeling in the medial prefrontal
cortex: effects of circuit, hormones and rest. Brain Res 1293:108–113
206 C. Graybeal et al.
Keywords Schizophrenia Psychotic illness Susceptibility gene Mutant model
Phenotype Gene 9 environment interaction
Contents
1 Introduction........................................................................................................................ 211
1.1 ‘Missing Heritability’? Evaluating Genetic Evidence for Schizophrenia
Risk Genes ................................................................................................................ 211
1.2 The Influence of Technological Progress on Mutant Modelling of Psychosis ...... 212
1.3 Assessing Construct Validity of Mutant Models of Schizophrenia........................ 213
1.4 Gene 9 Environment Interactions in Schizophrenia: Implications
for Modelling ............................................................................................................ 214
2 Genetic Models Relating to Dopaminergic Dysfunction................................................. 215
2.1 Dopamine Receptors................................................................................................. 215
2.2 Dopamine Transporter .............................................................................................. 216
2.3 Catechol-O-Methyltransferase .................................................................................. 217
2.4 AKT1/GSK-3 ............................................................................................................ 218
3 Genetic Models Relating to Glutamatergic Dysfunction................................................. 219
3.1 NMDAR-Mediated Neurotransmission.................................................................... 219
3.2 Glycine ...................................................................................................................... 220
3.3 D-serine ..................................................................................................................... 221
3.4 NMDAR Signalling Complex .................................................................................. 222
3.5 Metabotropic Glutamate Receptors.......................................................................... 222
4 Genetic Models Relating to Candidate Risk Genes and Copy Number Variations ....... 223
4.1 Disrupted-in-schizophrenia-1.................................................................................... 223
4.2 Dysbindin .................................................................................................................. 225
4.3 Neuregulin................................................................................................................. 226
4.4 Copy Number Variations.......................................................................................... 228
5 Evaluation of G 9 E Hypotheses in Mutant Models of Schizophrenia ......................... 229
5.1 DISC1 9 Immune Challenge ................................................................................... 229
5.2 NRG1/COMT 9 Exposure to Drugs of Abuse ....................................................... 230
5.3 Genes Related to the Pathophysiology of Schizophrenia 9 Exposure
to Prenatal Stress ...................................................................................................... 231
5.4 Overview of G 9 E studies ..................................................................................... 232
6 Discussion .......................................................................................................................... 233
6.1 Divining gene-Phenotype Associations in Schizophrenia Risk Gene Models ....... 233
6.2 Integration of Novel Gene Manipulation Strategies ............................................... 234
6.3 Examination of Gene 9 Gene Interactions in Schizophrenia................................. 238
7 Conclusions........................................................................................................................ 239
References................................................................................................................................ 239
Susceptibility Genes for Schizophrenia 211
1 Introduction
disease features that bridge the gap between the overt manifestations of
schizophrenia and underlying risk genes (Gottesman and Gould 2003; Braff et al.
2007). We have previously noted the utility of endophenotypes in relation to
mutant phenotypes, being relatively closer to the genetic basis of the disorder at
issue and, therefore, less complex to model; it is assumed that while a single
gene can affect multiple behavioural endpoints, an intermediate biological or
behavioural endophenotype is less susceptible to confounding influences and is
therefore more amenable to investigation (O’Tuathaigh et al. 2007a; Desbonnet
et al. 2009; Kirby et al. 2010). An endophenotype-based approach to schizo-
phrenia risk genes may explain how a gene with modest association support may
still have a function of potential relevance to the disorder, insofar as it may be
associated with one or more putative endophenotype in humans, or their
equivalents in mice. Among the most prominent candidate genes implicated in
risk for schizophrenia by association or CNV studies of risk for schizophrenia,
those for which the evidence is most strong and replicable are Disrupted-in-
Schizophrenia-1 (DISC1), dystrobrevin-binding protein 1 (DTNBP1; dysbindin),
and neuregulin 1 (NRG1) (Waddington et al. 2007; Allen et al. 2008; Bertram
2008; Gill et al. 2009; Owen et al. 2010).
Constitutive mutant models involving the construction of mice with gene dis-
ruption, either by deletion [i.e. knockout (KO)] or insertion/over-expression [i.e.
transgenic/knockin] have been used routinely to study the effect of developmental,
whole-body loss-of-target gene function. These techniques have not been without
drawbacks, with extrapolation of gene-phenotype relationships potentially con-
founded by: putative compensatory and redundancy mechanisms; the possibility of
embryonic lethality where the gene is functionally pleiotropic; potentially, whole-
body ablation or overexpression of a given gene may result in phenotypes that
involve both diverse brain regions and extracerebral mechanisms and thus com-
plicate interepretation of phenotypic data. However, each technique presents its
own profile of advantage and disadvantage.
Progress in mouse genetic technology over the last decade has facilitated the
development of conditional genetic models that are differentiated on the basis of
cell-type specificity, regional brain selectivity and temporal activation of the
mutation. Unsurprisingly, the evidence indicates that each of these factors can be
critical determinants of whether a specific gene disruption can give rise to a given
phenotypic outcome or otherwise; indeed, contradictory data have also been
reported for the same gene mutation depending on cell-, regional- and possibly
temporal specificity (e.g. Singer et al. 2011).
Susceptibility Genes for Schizophrenia 213
With ongoing progress in the field of psychiatric genetics and the modifying
influence of environmental factors, current conceptualisations of schizophrenia
have addressed the complex interaction of genes (G) and environment (E) (van
Os et al. 2010). Focusing on studies examining the interaction of epidemiolog-
ically relevant risk factors and candidate gene mutant models, it has been noted
that such an approach has the potential to produce G 9 E models with increased
construct validity (Desbonnet et al. 2009; Kirby et al. 2010). Consistent with the
well-considered ‘stress-vulnerability’ aetiological model, which proposes an
interaction between inherited genetic vulnerability and subsequent exposure to
adverse environmental risk factors, there is a growing body of evidence linking
early pre-l and postnatal life trauma (e.g. maternal infection, childhood abuse),
societal factors (e.g. urbanicity, migration), and drug use (e.g. cannabis exposure)
with risk for schizophrenia (van Os et al. 2010). While clinical studies linking
specific genetic factors with the psychosis-inducing or precipitating effects of
environmental factors have been slow to emerge, there has been some supportive
evidence sensitive enough to detect the interaction of environmental adversity
with a putative risk gene within a young population followed prospectively
(Caspi et al. 2005).
Recent years have seen a movement towards the generation of animal models of
psychosis based on the interaction of genetic mutations and well-characterised
environmental factors (Ayhan et al. 2009; Gray and Hannan 2007). As outlined
elsewhere (Burrows et al. 2011), the utility of such G 9 E models depends upon
the degree to which the environmental models at a preclinical level possess con-
struct validity with respect to environmental factors which have been epidemio-
logically linked with increased risk for schizophrenia; thus, although issues of
validity have most frequently been explored in relation to the target gene mutation
employed in a given model, whether epidemiologically relevant environmental
factors have been successfully translated into current preclinical G 9 E models
constituting a particular challenge (Burrows et al. 2011).
The present chapter will discuss the evidence on (a) mutant models relating to
the putative pathophysiology of schizophrenia, primarily dopaminergic and
glutamatergic dysfunction, (b) mutant models involving candidate risk genes for
schizophrenia, with an emphasis on evaluating the extent to which mutant phe-
notypes recapitulate the totality of the disease phenotype or model selective
endophenotypes, and (c) mutant models addressing G 9 E interactions relevant to
schizophrenia.
Susceptibility Genes for Schizophrenia 215
2.3 Catechol-O-Methyltransferase
2.4 AKT1/GSK-3
Mice deficient in the NMDAR NR1 subunit exhibit deficiencies across several
schizophrenia-related phenotypes: decreased responsivity to the NMDAR antag-
onists PCP and MK-801, hyperactivity in a novel environment and deficits in PPI
that were reversible particularly by second-generation antipsychotics, and social
deficits that were less responsive to antipsychotics (Mohn et al. 1999; Fradley et al.
2005; Duncan et al. 2004, 2006). Mice containing a targeted mutation in NR1
which prevented NR1 phosphorylation at serine 897, shown to be dramatically
reduced in patients with schizophrenia (Emamian et al. 2004), induced a marked
disruption of NMDAR-mediated synaptic transmission; these mice displayed
deficits in both PPI and social interaction, as indexed by a modified sociability and
preference for social novelty paradigm, but no genotypic changes in locomotor
activity (Li et al. 2009). These mutants also displayed decreased AMPA receptor-
mediated transmission and decreased AMPAR GluR1 subunit expression in the
synapse.
A recent study further refined our understanding of the role NMDAR signalling
in modulation of schizophrenia endophenotypes by examining the effect of tar-
geted postnatal or post-adolescent ablation of the NR1 subunit in cortical and
hippocampal GABAergic neurons, based on evidence supporting corticolimbic
GABAergic interneurons as a site of NMDA receptor hypofunction (Belforte et al.
2010). Several schizophrenia-related phenotypes, including novelty-induced hy-
perlocomotion and nest-building behaviour, were found to be present in postna-
tally targeted NR1 mutants when assessed in adulthood but not before
adolescence. These NR1 mutants also displayed social recognition deficits and a
mild anhedonic phenotype, measured in the sucrose preference test, as well as
220 C. M. P. O’Tuathaigh et al.
3.2 Glycine
3.3 D-serine
in the medial prefrontal cortex. In overview, while the endpoint associated with
many of these lines is NMDA receptor hypofunction, the diverse modes for
achieving this outcome carry distinct phenotypic consequences compensatory
mechanisms may account for some of these differences. However, phenotypic
diversity may reflect (a) differential availability of D-serine and glycine at the
glycine modulatory site and (b) whether the mutation affects receptor function
directly or indirectly via limited availability in a subset of glycine modulatory site
ligands.
4.1 Disrupted-in-schizophrenia-1
(Koike et al. 2006; Kvajo et al. 2008); (2) ENU mutagenesis in exon 2 of DISC1 is
characterised by hyperactivity, with antipsychotic-sensitive deficits in PPI and LI
and disrupted working memory in a T-maze task (Clapcote et al. 2007); (3) A
transgenic line with inducible expression of a DISC1 C-terminal fragment evi-
denced impaired spatial working memory using the delayed-non-matched-to-place
task, as well as reduced sociability (Li et al. 2007); (4) A transgenic line with
expression of a dominant-negative truncated form of DISC1 under the CaMKII
promoter, mutants showed hyperactivity but no changes in PPI, social interaction
or cognition (Hikida et al. 2007); (5) An inducible transgenic line with forebrain-
specific expression of mutant human DISC1 (hDISC1) under the CaMKII pro-
moter showed hyperactivity without material disruption to PPI, sex-specific
impairment of working memory using the Morris water maze, with a deficit in
dyadic social interactions but not sociability (Pletnikov et al. 2008); (6) A trans-
genic line with expression of truncated DISC1 exhibited normal exploratory
activity but disruption to LI (Shen et al. 2008).
Interestingly, lentiviral silencing of DISC1 expression in the adult dentate gyrus
was accompanied by an increase in novelty-induced hyperlocomotion; this
behavioural effect was reversed following treatment with the GSK-3b inhibitor
SB-216763, suggesting that increased GSK-3b activity secondary to DISC1 loss-
of-function might be associated with schizophrenia-like behaviours (Mao et al.
2009). Further support for this hypothesis has come from a recent report that
pharmacological or genetic inactivation of GSK-3 in the ENU-generated DISC1
mutant (L100P) reversed phenotypic deficits in PPI and LI, and normalised their
hyperactivity profile (Lipina et al. 2010). This further implicates impaired DISC1-
GSK-3 interplay in schizophrenia-relevant behaviours.
Employing the mutant hDISC1 transgenic model (Pletnikov et al. 2008), a
follow-up study assessed the effect of timing of the DISC1 mutation on the
expression of schizophrenia-related phenotypes (Ayhan et al. 2011). In this study,
the effect of mutant hDISC1 expression was compared during prenatal, postnatal,
or both periods. Regardless of timepoint of expression, hDISC1 mutants exhibited
fewer cortical parvalbumin-positive cells, as well as cortical hypodopaminergia
relative to controls. Combined pre- and postnatal mutant hDISC1 expression was
associated with the most prominent schizophrenia-related abnormalities; these
included decreased social interaction in a test of dyadic interaction, increased
sensitivity to psychostimulants and increased immobility in the tail suspension
test, with lateral ventricular enlargement and morphological abnormalities in
granule cells of the dentate gyrus. In contrast, neither pre- nor postnatal expression
alone fully recapitulated the phenotype associated with combined timepoint
expression.
Extending these findings and introducing regional specificity, Niwa and col-
leagues (2010), using in utero electroporation to produce selective knockdown of
DISC1 in pyramidal neurons in prefrontal cortex of the foetal mouse, reported
post-pubertal emergence of disrupted PPI, impaired object recognition memory
and delayed T-maze alternation; there was disturbed maturation of mesocortical
DAergic projections to and interneurons in the medial prefrontal cortex. Several of
Susceptibility Genes for Schizophrenia 225
4.2 Dysbindin
4.3 Neuregulin
line also showed impaired working memory in the 4- or 8-arm radial maze; a
similar profile has not been observed in NRG1 and ErbB4 mutants (O’Tuathaigh
et al. 2007a, b). These data would implicate parvalbumin-positive neurons as a
target for NRG1/ErbB4 modulation of behaviours relevant to schizophrenia.
Recent experiments demonstrate that ErbB4 also controls the formation and/or
maintenance of excitatory synapses on specific populations of GABAergic inter-
neurons and that postsynaptic ErbB4 function is probably required in this process
(Fazzari et al. 2010).
A potential role for NRG1 in G 9 E interactions related to schizophrenia (see
5.2) is highlighted by a recent study which examined HPA axis activity and stress
responsivity in a line of NRG1 mutant rats [generated using the sleeping beauty
transposon] with disruption of the NRG1 Type II isoform (Taylor et al. 2011).
NRG1 mutant rats displayed increased basal corticosterone levels and decreased
corticosterone levels after 30 min restraint stress relative to basal levels, with
increased expression of glucocorticoid receptor expression in the pituitary and
hippocampus and decreased expression in the paraventricular nucleus. These
mutants also displayed slower habituation in a novel environment, consistent with
that observed in NRG1 hypomorphic mice (O’Tuathaigh et al. 2006), which the
authors interpreted as increased reactivity to environmental stimuli.
agonists at the a7 nicotinic cholinergic receptor may have some efficacy against
both the positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia (Hajós and Rogers
2010).
In recent years, several studies have supported a link between increased sus-
ceptibility for schizophrenia and microdeletions affecting the gene neurexin 1
(NRXN1; Kirov et al. 2008, 2009b; Ikeda et al. 2010; Rujescu et al. 2009;
Levinson et al. 2011). Neurexins are presynaptic proteins that function as synaptic
recognition molecules and may contribute to various aspects of synaptic function
via binding to neuroligins (Ichtchenko et al. 1995). Mice containing deletion of
NRXN1a display behavioural and electrophysiological deficits relevant to
schizophrenia, including disruption to synaptic transmission and PPI (Etherton
et al. 2009). Interestingly, dysregulation of NRXN1 expression throughout
development has been observed in mice containing mutation of the DISC1 gene
(the ENU-generated L100P mutation), providing a measure of functional associ-
ation between two prominent risk genes.
6 Discussion
As others have pointed out, progress in molecular and genomic techniques, and the
implications for genetic modelling in schizophrenia, has not been paralleled by
comparable advances in the generation of murine behavioural paradigms capable
of measuring specific forms of cognition and social and motivational behaviours
which are disrupted-in-schizophrenia; furthermore, these relate to clinical features
that are largely insensitive to existing antipsychotic treatments (Brigman et al.
2010). While the field has made incremental progress in generating novel murine
paradigms to assess these domains, including translating across species from rat to
mouse, significant progress have been made in some areas such as the develop-
ment of tasks employing touchscreen technology to measure executive function
processes (Brigman et al. 2010).
234 C. M. P. O’Tuathaigh et al.
Even where a single gene mutation has been introduced, evidence indicates that a
mutant model bred onto different background genetic strains can show very dif-
ferent phenotypes, suggesting gene 9 gene interactions and/or genetic modifier
effects (Burrows et al. 2011; see Waddington et al. 2005 for further discussion of
implications of strain differences in CNS KO models).
Review of single gene mutant and G 9 E studies over the past few years, which
have increasingly involved the application of conditional transgenic technologies
and alternative gene manipulation strategies, demonstrates the power of examining
the effect of dynamic genetic manipulations throughout development, followed by
multidomain behavioural, cellular, and molecular phenotyping. Further integration
of new strategies to modulate gene expression in a phenotypic and regional manner
will facilitate the development of more accurate in vivo disease models based
on available pathophysiological data for disease states (Garbett et al. 2010).
Table 1 Mutant mouse phenotypes relating to dopaminergic and glutamatergic mechanisms implicated in schizophrenia
Anxiety Novelty- Cognition Sociability Psychopharmacology
induced
Working Attention PPI Social Social Social Increased sensitivity Increased Responsivity to
activity
memory memorya aggression interactionb to psychostimulantsc sensitivity antipsychotics
to stress
Dopamine mutation
D1 Receptor – : ; – = – – – = – –
KO
D2 Receptor – ; ; – = – – – :/; – –
KO
D2L KO – = – – = – – – = – ;
D2 Receptor – – – – – – – – – – –
Susceptibility Genes for Schizophrenia
OE
D3 Receptor – – ; – – – – – – – –
KO
DAT KO – : ; – ; – : ; – – –
COMT Het : = =/: – = = : = – – –
COMT KO :/; = : – = = = = ; : –
COMT-Val Tg ; = ; ; = – – – : ; –
AKT1 KO – = – – ; – – – : – –
GSK-3 KO – – – – – – – – – – –
DVL-1 KO – = – – ;/= = – ; – – –
Glutamate mutation
NR1 KO – : – – ; ; ; ; ; – =/;
NR1 S897 – – – – ; – – ; – – –
NR1 PND : : ; – ; ; – – – – –
GluRe1 KO – : – – – – – – – – :
GlyT1 cKO = – = : = – – – ;/= – –
(continued)
235
Table 1 (continued)
236
DISC1-hu Tg = : ; – = = ; ; – – –
DISC1-(tr) Tg – – – ; – – – – – – –
NRG1–TM Het = : = – ; ; : = : : –
NRG1 III Het – = ; – ; – – – – – –
NRG1 Ig Het – = – ; – – – – – – :
NRG1 EGF Het – : – – = – – – – – –
NRG1 (I) Tg – : – – = – : ; – – –
NRG1 (I-Thy1) Tg = = – – ; – – – – – –
ErbB4 Het – =/: ; – – – – – – – –
ErbB4 PV – : ; – ; – – – – – –
:, increased relative to wildtype; ;, decreased relative to wildtype; =, no difference; /, separates different findings; -, not reported; PPI prepulse inhibition, DAO
D-amino acid oxidase, DISC1 disrupted-in-schizophrenia-1, DISC1 C Tg DISC1 C-terminal fragment transgenic mice, DISC1-DN DISC1 dominant-negative mice,
DISC1-hu Tg DISC1 human form transgenic mice, DISC1-(tr) truncated DISC1 transgenic mice, NRG1-TM neuregulin 1 transmembrane domain, Het heterozygous
mutants, NRG1-EGF neuregulin1 epidermal growth factor domain, NRG1 (I) neuregulin1 type I over-expressing mice, NRG1 (I-Thy1) Tg neuregulin1 type 1 (Thy1
promoter driven) over-expressing mice, ErbB4 PV mice with ablation of parvalbumin-positive ErbB4. a Includes investigation of stationary mouse within a cage and
free social interaction tests. b includes dopamine-enhancing agents, NMDA receptor antagonists
237
238 C. M. P. O’Tuathaigh et al.
In addition to inducible and conditional systems, the in utero gene transfer tech-
nique, with expression and/or shRNA constructs for RNAi, may prove a promising
methodological innovation by enabling the selection of target cell populations for
genetic modulation, while the introduction of inducible expression constructs will
allow better temporal control. Furthermore, experimental designs for future studies
on mutant models of psychosis would be improved, where appropriate, by the
incorporation of a rescue strategy aimed at restoring or compensating for the loss-
of-gene function; this would assist in providing mechanistic insight into the role of
the gene in mediating schizophrenia-related phenotypes and the development of
novel antipsychotic targets. By using the technical advantage of in utero transfer,
in which expression of more than one gene can be modified at one time by
cotransfection of expression and/or RNAi constructs (Shu et al. 2004; Young-
Pearse et al. 2007), it should prove possible to test the synergistic or other epistatic
effects of multiple genetic factors.
Evidence from the emerging field of molecular epigenomics suggests a putative
deficit in microRNA processing in schizophrenia. MicroRNAs are small, non-
coding RNAs that are believed to target at least a third of protein coding genes and
are associated with the regulation of mRNA levels for multiple genes in the human
cerebral cortex (Farh et al. 2005; Zhang and Su 2008). Dysregulation of miRNA
expression may account for some of the neurodevelopmental aspects of psychiatric
disorders and the difficulty in identifying individual causative genes (Dinan 2010;
Miller and Wahlestedt 2010). For example, disruption of miRNA biogenesis, as a
result of conditional deletion of DiGeorge syndrome critical region gene 8
(Dgcr8), has been associated with the expression of behavioural and structural
brain endophenotypes related to schizophrenia (Stark et al. 2008). Increased study
of the role of miRNAs in regulating phenotypic expression in existing mutant
models has the potential to provide further insight into the mechanisms by which
genes may contribute to specific aspects of the schizophrenia phenotype.
Assuming a polygenic basis for schizophrenia, neither partial nor complete loss-
of-function or over-expression of any single gene in mice is likely to produce an
animal model with construct validity for the disease; simultaneous dysregulation
of several susceptibility genes likely better reflects the genetic contribution to risk
for developing the disorder, emphasising the importance of generating and phe-
notypically characterising compound heterozygous mutants. In these animals,
partial loss- or gain-of-function in several components could perhaps mimic more
precisely the etiopathologic mechanisms, as well as the pathophysiological
features of schizophrenia. Evidence from cellular and molecular studies conducted
till date provides an important framework to understand how several common
susceptibility genes and/or multiple, rare CNVs might converge functionally onto
common pathways that may reflect the etiopathobiology of schizophrenia.
Susceptibility Genes for Schizophrenia 239
For example, there is evidence to connect NRG1, AKT, and DISC1 in a common
pathway that may regulate neurodevelopment and contribute to susceptibility to
schizophrenia (Kim et al. 2009) Future characterisation of mice with simultaneous
dysregulation of several risk genes may provide novel insights into whether those
risk genes may contribute to unique phenotypes and/or whether they might con-
tribute in a synergistic manner with other risk genes to the expression of schizo-
phrenia-related endophenotypes.
7 Conclusions
Increasingly, the evidence from mutant studies is showing that targeted and
transient ontogenetic challenges of certain spatial and temporal specificity may be
sufficient to invoke a disease process (Thompson and Levitt 2010). Additionally,
recent technical advances allowing manipulation of a single gene in a regionally or
temporally specific manner has provide the opportunity to assess how specific risk
genes might interact with environmental factors to produced schizophrenia-rele-
vant phenotypes. Clearly, the construct validity of any genetic model is determined
by the degree to which the mouse gene mutation successfully mimics the disease-
associated gene variant or mutation, as well as the available arsenal of accurate
and reliable phenotyping instruments. This is particularly the case for a heterog-
enous and/or polygenic disorder such as schizophrenia; however, single and
compound gene mutant models have enduring potential to inform on the contri-
bution of a given risk gene or networks of genes to the expression of schizo-
phrenia-related endophenotypes (Tables 1, 2).
Acknowledgments The authors’ studies are supported by Science Foundation Ireland Principal
Investigator grant 07/IN.1/B960 and Health Research Board of Ireland Postdoctoral Fellowship
PD/2007/20.
References
Abazyan B, Nomura J, Kannan G et al (2010) Prenatal interaction of mutant DISC1 and immune
activation produces adult psychopathology. Biol Psychiatry 68:1172–1181
Accortt EE, Freeman MP, Allen JJ (2008) Women and major depressive disorder: clinical
perspectives on causal pathways. J Womens Health 17:1583–1590
Adler CM, Malhotra AK, Elman I et al (1999) Comparison of ketamine-induced thought disorder
in healthy volunteers and thought disorder in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 156:1646–1649
Allen NC, Bagade S, McQueen MB et al (2008) Systematic meta-analyses and field synopsis of
genetic association studies in schizophrenia: the SzGene database. Nat Genet 40:827–834
Almond SL, Fradley RL, Armstrong EJ et al (2006) Behavioral and biochemical characterization
of a mutant mouse strain lacking D-amino acid oxidase activity and its implications for
schizophrenia. Mol Cell Neurosci 32:324–334
Amann LC, Gandal MJ, Halene TB et al (2010) Mouse behavioral endophenotypes for
schizophrenia. Brain Res Bull 83:147–161
240 C. M. P. O’Tuathaigh et al.
Arguello PA, Gogos JA (2006) Modeling madness in mice: one piece at a time. Neuron
52:179–196
Arguello PA, Gogos JA (2010) Cognition in mouse models of schizophrenia susceptibility genes.
Schizophr Bull 36:289–300
Arseneault L, Cannon M, Witton J et al (2004) Causal association between cannabis and
psychosis: examination of the evidence. Br J Psychiatry 184:110–117
Ayhan Y, Sawa A, Ross CA (2009) Animal models of gene-environment interactions in
schizophrenia. Behav Brain Res 204:274–281
Ayhan Y, Abazyan B, Nomura J et al (2011) Differential effects of prenatal and postnatal
expressions of mutant human DISC1 on neurobehavioral phenotypes in transgenic mice:
evidence for neurodevelopmental origin of major psychiatric disorders. Mol Psychiatry
16:293–306
Babovic D, O’Tuathaigh CM, O’Connor AM et al (2008) Phenotypic characterization of
cognition and social behavior in mice with heterozygous versus homozygous deletion of
catechol-O-methyltransferase. Neuroscience 155:1021–1029
Babovic D, O’Tuathaigh CM, O’Sullivan GJ et al (2007) Exploratory and habituation phenotype
of heterozygous and homozygous COMT knockout mice. Behav Brain Res 183:236–239
Ballard TM, Pauly-Evers M, Higgins GA et al (2002) Severe impairment of NMDA receptor
function in mice carrying targeted point mutations in the glycine binding site results in drug-
resistant nonhabituating hyperactivity. J Neurosci 22:6713–6723
Balu DT, Coyle JT (2011) Neuroplasticity signaling pathways linked to the pathophysiology of
schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35:848–870
Barros CS, Calabrese B, Chamero P et al (2009) Impaired maturation of dendritic spines without
disorganization of cortical cell layers in mice lacking NRG1/ErbB signaling in the central
nervous system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:4507–4512
Bassett AS, Scherer SW, Brzustowicz LM (2010) Copy number variations in schizophrenia:
critical review and new perspectives on concepts of genetics and disease. Am J Psychiatry
167:899–914
Basu AC, Tsai GE, Ma CL et al (2009) Targeted disruption of serine racemase affects
glutamatergic neurotransmission and behavior. Mol Psychiatry 14:719–727
Bauer D, Gupta D, Harotunian V et al (2008) Abnormal expression of glutamate transporter and
transporter interacting molecules in prefrontal cortex in elderly patients with schizophrenia.
Schizophr Res 104:108–120
Bay-Richter C, O’Tuathaigh CM, O’Sullivan G et al (2009) Enhanced latent inhibition in
dopamine receptor-deficient mice is sex-specific for the D1 but not D2 receptor subtype:
implications for antipsychotic drug action. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 17:1–12
Beaulieu JM, Sotnikova TD, Yao WD et al (2004) Lithium antagonizes dopamine-dependent
behaviors mediated by an AKT/glycogen synthase kinase 3 signaling cascade. Proc Natl Acad
Sci U S A 101:5099–5104
Beaulieu JM, Sotnikova TD, Marion S (2005) An Akt/beta-arrestin 2/PP2A signaling complex
mediates dopaminergic neurotransmission and behavior. Cell 122:261–273
Beaulieu JM, Gainetdinov RR, Caron MG (2009) Akt/GSK3 signaling in the action of
psychotropic drugs. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol 49:327–347
Belforte JE, Zsiros V, Sklar ER et al (2010) Postnatal NMDA receptor ablation in corticolimbic
interneurons confers schizophrenia-like phenotypes. Nat Neurosci 13:76–83
Ben Abdallah NM, Fuss J, Trusel M et al (2011) The puzzle box as a simple and efficient
behavioral test for exploring impairments of general cognition and executive functions in
mouse models of schizophrenia. Exp Neurol 227:42–52
Benneyworth MA, Basu AC, Coyle JT (2011) Discordant behavioral effects of psychotomimetic
drugs in mice with altered NMDA receptor function. Psychopharmacology 213:143–153
Bertram L (2008) Genetic research in schizophrenia: new tools and future perspectives.
Schizophr Bull 34:806–812
Bhardwaj SK, Baharnoori M, Sharif-Askari B et al (2009) Behavioral characterization of
dysbindin-deficient sandy mice. Behav Brain Res 197:435–441
Susceptibility Genes for Schizophrenia 241
Cox MM, Tucker AM, Tang J et al (2009) Neurobehavioral abnormalities in the dysbindin-1
mutant, sandy, on a C57BL/6 J genetic background. Genes Brain Behav 8:390–397
Coyle JT (2006) Glutamate and schizophrenia: beyond the dopamine hypothesis. Cell Mol
Neurobiol 26:365–384
Craddock N, Owen MJ, O’Donovan MC (2006) The catechol-O-methyl transferase (COMT)
gene as a candidate for psychiatric phenotypes: evidence and lessons. Mol Psychiatry
11:446–458
Deakin IH, Law AJ, Oliver PL et al (2009) Behavioural characterization of neuregulin 1 type I
overexpressing transgenic mice. Neuroreport 20:1523–1528
DeRosse P, Hodgkinson CA, Lencz T et al (2007) Disrupted in schizophrenia 1 genotype and
positive symptoms in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 61:1208–1210
Desbonnet L, Waddington JL, O’Tuathaigh CM (2009) Mice mutant for genes associated with
schizophrenia: common phenotype or distinct endophenotypes? Behav Brain Res
204:258–273
DeSteno DA, Schmauss C (2009) A role for dopamine D2 receptors in reversal learning.
Neuroscience 162:118–127
Devito LM, Balu DT, Kanter BR et al (2011) Serine racemase deletion disrupts memory for order
and alters cortical dendritic morphology. Genes Brain Behav 10:210–222
Dinan TG (2010) MicroRNAs as a target for novel antipsychotics: a systematic review of an
emerging field. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 23:1–10
Drew MR, Simpson EH, Kellendonk C et al (2009) Transient overexpression of striatal D2
receptors impairs operant motivation and interval timing. J Neurosci 27:7731–7739
Duan X, Chang JH, Ge S et al (2007) Disrupted-In-Schizophrenia 1 regulates integration of
newly generated neurons in the adult brain. Cell 130:1146–1158
Duffy L, Cappas E, Scimone A et al (2008) Behavioral profile of a heterozygous mutant mouse
model for EGF-like domain neuregulin 1. Behav Neurosci 122:748–759
Duffy L, Cappas E, Lai D et al (2010) Cognition in transmembrane domain neuregulin 1 mutant
mice. Neuroscience 170:800–807
Duncan GE, Moy SS, Perez A et al (2004) Deficits in senosrimotor gating and tests of social
behavior in a genetic model of reduced NMDA receptor function. Behav Brain Res
153:507–519
Duncan GE, Moy SS, Lieberman JA et al (2006) Effects of haloperidol, clozapine, and quetiapine
on sensorimotor gating in a genetic model of reduced NMDA receptor function.
Psychopharmacology 184:190–200
El-Ghundi M, O’Dowd BF, George SR (2007) Insights into the role of dopamine receptor
systems in learning and memory. Rev Neurosci 18:37–66
Emamian ES, Hall D, Birnbaum MJ (2004) Convergent evidence for impaired AKT1-GSK3beta
signaling in schizophrenia. Nat Genet 36:131–137
Etherton MR, Blaiss CA, Powell CM et al (2009) Mouse neurexin-1 alpha deletion causes
correlated electrophysiological and behavioral changes consistent with cognitive impairments.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:17998–18003
Farh KK, Grimson A, Jan C et al (2005) The widespread impact of mammalian MicroRNAs on
mRNA repression and evolution. Science 310:1817–1821
Fatemi SH, Folsom TD (2009) The neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia, revisited.
Schizophr Bull 35:528–548
Fazzari P, Paternain AV, Valiente M et al (2010) Control of cortical GABA circuitry
development by Nrg1 and ErbB4 signalling. Nature 464:1376–1380
Feng YQ, Zhou ZY, He X et al (2008) Dysbindin deficiency in sandy mice causes reduction of
snapin and displays behaviors related to schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 106:218–228
Feyder M, Wiedholz L, Sprengel R et al (2007) Impaired associative fear learning in mice with
complete loss or haploinsufficiency of AMPA GluR1 receptors. Front Behav Neurosci 1:4
Fitzgerald PJ, Barkus C, Feyder M et al (2010) Does gene deletion of AMPA GluA1 phenocopy
features of schizoaffective disorder? Neurobiol Dis 40:608–621
Susceptibility Genes for Schizophrenia 243
Fradley RL, O’Meara GF, Newman RJ et al (2005) STOP knockout and NMDA NR1
hypomorphic mice exhibit deficits in sensorimotor gating. Behav Brain Res 163:257–264
Gainetdinov RR (2008) Dopamine transporter mutant mice in experimental neuropharmacology.
Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol 377:301–313
Gajendran N, Kapfhammer JP, Lain E et al (2009) Neuregulin signaling is dispensable for
NMDA- and GABA(A)-receptor expression in the cerebellum in vivo. J Neurosci
29:2404–2413
Garbett KA, Horváth S, Ebert PJ et al (2010) Novel animal models for studying complex brain
disorders: BAC-driven miRNA-mediated in vivo silencing of gene expression. Mol
Psychiatry 15:987–995
Gill M, Donohoe G, Corvin A (2009) What have the genomics ever done for psychoses? Psychol
Med 40:529–540
Giros B, Jaber M, Jones SR et al (1996) Hyperlocomotion and indifference to cocaine and
amphetamine in mice lacking the dopamine transporter. Nature 379:606–612
Glickstein SB, Hof PR, Schmauss C (2002) Mice lacking dopamine D2 and D3 receptors have
spatial working memory deficits. J Neurosci 22:5619–5629
Gogos JA, Morgan M, Luine V et al (1998) Catechol-O-methyltransferase-deficient mice exhibit
sexually dimorphic changes in catecholamine levels and behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
95:9991–9996
Goodwin RD, Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ (2004) Panic attacks and psychoticism. Am J
Psychiatry 161:88–92
Gottesman II, Gould TD (2003) The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and
stategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry 160:636–645
Gray L, Hannan AJ (2007) Dissecting cause and effect in the pathogenesis of psychiatric
disorders: genes, environment and behaviour. Curr Mol Med 7:470–478
Gray L, van den Buuse M, Scarr E et al (2009) Clozapine reverses schizophrenia-related
behaviours in the metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 knockout mouse: association with
N-methyl-D-aspartic acid receptor up-regulation. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 12:45–60
Grozeva D, Kirov G, Ivanov D et al (2010) Rare copy number variants: a point of rarity in genetic
risk for bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67:318–327
Guo X, Hamilton PJ, Reish NJ et al (2009) Reduced expression of the NMDA receptor-
interacting protein SynGAP causes behavioral abnormalities that model symptoms of
schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 34:1658–1672
Haasio K, Huotari M, Nissinen E (2003) Tissue histopathology, clinical chemistry and behaviour
of adult Comt-gene-disrupted mice. J Appl Toxicol 23:213–219
Hajós M, Rogers BN (2010) Targeting alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in the treatment
of schizophrenia. Curr Pharm Des 16:538–554
Halberstadt AL, Geyer MA (2009) Habituation and sensitization of acoustic startle: opposite
influences of dopamine D1 and D2 family receptors. Neurobiol Learn Mem 92:243–248
Hall FS, Sora I, Uhl GR (2003) Sex-dependent modulation of ethanol consumption in vesicular
monoamine transporter 2 (VMAT2) and dopamine transporter (DAT) knockout mice.
Neuropsychopharmacology 28:620–628
Harrison PJ, Law AJ (2006) Neuregulin 1 and schizophrenia: genetics, gene expression, and
neurobiology. Biol Psychiatry 60:132–140
Harrison PJ, Tunbridge EM (2008) Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): a gene contributing
to sex differences in brain function, and to sexual dimorphism in the predisposition to
psychiatric disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:3037–3045
Harvey PD, Wingo AP, Burdick KE, Baldesarini RJ (2010) Cognition and disability in bipolar
disorder: lessons from schizophrenia research. Bipolar Disord 12:364–375
Hashimoto A, Yoshikawa M, Niwa A et al (2005) Mice lacking D-amino acid oxidase activity
display marked attenuation of stereotypy and ataxia induced by MK-801. Brain Res
1033:210–215
244 C. M. P. O’Tuathaigh et al.
Li Z, Mulligan MK, Wang X (2010) A transposon in comt generates mRNA variants and causes
widespread expression and behavioral differences among mice. PLoS One 5:e12181
Lipina TV, Niwa M, Jaaro-Peled H et al (2010) Enhanced dopamine function in DISC1-L100P
mutant mice: implications for schizophrenia. Genes Brain Behav 9:777–789
Lijam N, Paylor R, McDonald MP et al (1997) Social interaction and sensorimotor gating
abnormalities in mice lacking Dvl1. Cell 90:895–905
Long JM, LaPorte P, Paylor R et al (2004) Expanded characterization of the social interaction
abnormalities in mice lacking Dvl1. Genes Brain Behav 3:51–62
Long JM, LaPorte P, Merscher S et al (2006) Behavior of mice with mutations in the conserved
region deleted in velocardiofacial/DiGeorge syndrome. Neurogenetics 7:247–257
Long LE, Chesworth R, Arnold JC et al (2010) A follow-up study: acute behavioural effects of
Delta (9)-THC in female heterozygous neuregulin 1 transmembrane domain mutant mice.
Psychopharmacology 211:277–289
Low NC, Hardy J (2007) What is a schizophrenic mouse? Neuron 54:348–349
Maekawa M, Okamura T, Kasai N et al (2005) D-amino-acid oxidase is involved in D-serine-
induced nephrotoxicity. Chem Res Toxicol 18:1678–1682
Malhotra AK, Adler CM, Kennison SD et al (1997a) Clozapine blunts N-methyl-D-aspartate
antagonist-induced psychosis: a study with ketamine. Biol Psychiatry 42:664–668
Meechan DW, Maynard TM, Gopalakrishna D et al (2007a) When half is not enough: gene
expression and dosage in the 22q11 deletion syndrome. Gene Expr 13:299–310
Manolio TA, Rodriguez LL, Brooks L et al (2007) New models of collaboration in genome-wide
association studies: the Genetic Association Information Network. Nat Genet 39:1045–1051
Malhotra AK, Pinals DA, Adler CM et al (1997b) Ketamine-induced exacerbation of psychotic
symptoms and cognitive impairment in neuroleptic-free schizophrenics. Neuropsychophar-
macology 17:141–150
Mao Y, Ge X, Frank CL et al (2009) Disrupted in schizophrenia 1 regulates neuronal progenitor
proliferation via modulation of GSK3beta/beta-catenin signaling. Cell 136:1017–1031
Mathews TA, John CE, Lapa GB (2006) No role of the dopamine transporter in acute ethanol
effects on striatal dopamine dynamics. Synapse 60:288–294
Meechan DW, Maynard TM, Gopalakrishna D et al (2007b) When half is not enough: gene
expression and dosage in the 22q11 deletion syndrome. Gene Expr 13:299–310
Mei L, Xiong WC (2008) Neuregulin 1 in neural development, synaptic plasticity and
schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:437–452
Meyer U, Feldon J, Fatemi SH (2009) In vivo rodent models for the experimental investigation of
prenatal immune activation effects in neurodevelopmental brain disorders. Neurosci Biobehav
Rev 33:1061–1079
Miller BH, Wahlestedt C (2010) MicroRNA dysregulation in psychiatric disease. Brain Res
1338:89–99
Miyamoto Y, Nabeshima T (2002) Analysis of neuronal functions in mice lacking the NMDA
receptor epsilon 1 subunit. Nippon Yakurigaku Zasshi 119:327–335
Mohn AR, Gainetdinov RR, Caron MG et al (1999) Mice with reduced NMDA receptor
expression display behaviors related to schizophrenia. Cell 98:427–436
Moore TH, Zammit S, Lingford-Hughes A (2007) Cannabis use and risk of psychotic or affective
mental health outcomes: a systematic review. Lancet 370:319–328
Muir WJ, Pickard BS, Blackwood DH (2008) Disrupted-in-Schizophrenia-1. Curr Psychiatry Rep
10:140–147
Murotani T, Ishizuka T, Hattori S et al (2007) High dopamine turnover in the brains of Sandy
mice. Neurosci Lett 421:47–51
Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, van Os J (2005) Behavioural sensitization to daily life stress in
psychosis. Psychol Med 35:733–741
Myin-Germeys I, Oorschot M, Collip D et al (2009) Experience sampling research in
psychopathology: opening the black box of daily life. Psychol Med 39:1533–1547
Susceptibility Genes for Schizophrenia 247
Niwa M, Kamiya A, Murai R et al (2010) Knockdown of DISC1 by in utero gene transfer disturbs
postnatal dopaminergic maturation in the frontal cortex and leads to adult behavioral deficits.
Neuron 65:480–489
Numakawa T, Yagasaki Y, Ishimoto T (2004) Evidence of novel neuronal functions of dysbindin,
a susceptibility gene for schizophrenia. Hum Mol Genet 13:2699–2708
Ognibene E, Adriani W, Macrì S et al (2007) Neurobehavioural disorders in the infant reeler
mouse model: interaction of genetic vulnerability and consequences of maternal separation.
Behav Brain Res 177:142–149
Oliver PL, Davies KE (2009) Interaction between environmental and genetic factors modulates
schizophrenic endophenotypes in the Snap-25 mouse mutant blind-drunk. Hum Mol Genet
18:4576–4589
O’Sullivan, GJ, O’Tuathaigh C, Tomiyama K, Koshikawa N, Waddington JL (2010) Dopamine
receptor subtypes and behaviour: from psychopharmacology to mutant models. In: Neve K
(ed) The dopamine receptors, pp 323–371. Humana Press, Totowa
O’Tuathaigh CM, O’Sullivan GJ, Kinsella A et al (2006) Sexually dimorphic changes in the
exploratory and habituation profiles of heterozygous neuregulin-1 knockout mice. Neurore-
port 17:79–83
O’Tuathaigh CMP, Babovic D, O’Meara G et al (2007a) Susceptibility genes for schizophrenia:
phenotypic characterisation of mutant models. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 31:60–78
O’Tuathaigh CM, Babovic D, O’Sullivan GJ et al (2007b) Phenotypic characterization of spatial
cognition and social behavior in mice with ‘knockout’ of the schizophrenia risk gene
neuregulin 1. Neuroscience 147:18–27
O’Tuathaigh CM, O’Connor AM, O’Sullivan GJ et al (2008) Disruption to social dyadic interactions
but not emotional/anxiety-related behaviour in mice with heterozygous ‘knockout’ of the
schizophrenia risk gene neuregulin-1. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32:462–466
O’Tuathaigh CM, Desbonnet L, Waddington JL (2009) Neuregulin-1 signalling in schizophrenia:
‘Jack of all trades’ or master of some? Expert Rev Neurother 9:1–3
O’Tuathaigh CMP, Waddington JL (2010) Mutant mouse models: phenotypic relationships to
domains of psychopathology and pathobiology in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 36:243–245
O’Tuathaigh CMP, Kirby BP, Moran PM et al (2010a) Mutant mouse models: genotype-
phenotype relationships to negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 36:271–288
O’Tuathaigh CMP, Harte M, Tighe O et al (2010b) Schizophrenia-related endophenotypes in
heterozygous neuregulin-1 ‘knockout’ mice: NMDA-receptor antagonist effects, neurochem-
istry and brain structure. Eur J Neurosci 31:349–58
O’Tuathaigh CM, Hryniewiecka M, Behan A et al (2010c) Chronic adolescent exposure to delta-
9-tetrahydrocannabinol in COMT knockout mice: Impact on phenotypes relevant to
psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:2262–2273
Owen MJ, Craddock N, O’Donovan MC (2010) Suggestion of roles for both common and rare
risk variants in genome-wide studies of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67:667–673
Papaleo F, Crawley JN, Song J et al (2008) Genetic dissection of the role of catechol-O-
methyltransferase in cognition and stress reactivity in mice. J Neurosci 28:8709–8723
Papaleo F, Yang F, Garcia S et al (2012) Dysbindin-1 modulates prefrontal cortical activity and
schizophrenia-like behaviors via dopamine/D2 pathways. Mol Psychiatry 17:85–98
Patil ST, Zhang L, Martenyi F et al (2007) Activation of mGlu2/3 receptors as a new approach to
treat schizophrenia: a randomized Phase 2 clinical trial. Nat Med 13:1102–1107
Paylor R, Glaser B, Mupo A et al (2006) Tbx1 haploinsufficiency is linked to behavioral disorders
in mice and humans: implications for 22q11 deletion syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
103:7729–7734
Pilowsky LS, Bressan RA, Stone JM et al (2007) First in vivo evidence of an NMDA receptor
deficit in medication-free schizophrenic patients. Mol Psychiatry 11:118–119
Pinard E, Alanine A, Alberati D et al (2010) Selective GlyT1 inhibitors: discovery of
[4-(3-fluoro-5-trifluoromethylpyridin-2-yl)piperazin-1-yl][5-methanesulfonyl-2-((S)-2,2,2-tri-
fluoro-1-methylethoxy)phenyl]methanone (RG1678), a promising novel medicine to treat
schizophrenia. J Med Chem 53:4603–4614
248 C. M. P. O’Tuathaigh et al.
Pletnikov MV, Ayhan Y, Nikolskaia O et al (2008) Inducible expression of mutant human DISC1
in mice is associated with brain and behavioural abnormalities reminiscent of schizophrenia.
Mol Psychiatry 13:173–186
Ralph RJ, Varty GB, Kelly MA et al (1999) The dopamine D2, but not D3 or D4, receptor
subtype is essential for the disruption of prepulse inhibition produced by amphetamine in
mice. J Neurosci 19:4627–4633
Ralph-Williams RJ, Lehmann-Masten V, Otero-Corchon V et al (2002) Differential effects of
direct and indirect dopamine agonists on prepulse inhibition: a study in D1 and D2 receptor
knockout mice. J Neurosci 22:9604–9611
Rimer M, Barrett DW, Maldonado MA, Vock VM et al (2005) Neuregulin-1 immunoglobulin-
like domain mutant mice: clozapine sensitivity and impaired latent inhibition. Neuroreport
16:271–275
Rodriguiz RM, Chu R, Caron MG et al (2004) Aberrant responses in social interaction of
dopamine transporter knockout mice. Behav Brain Res 148:185–198
Ross CA, Margolis RL, Reading SA et al (2006) Neurobiology of schizophrenia. Neuron
52:139–153
Rujescu D, Ingason A, Cichon S et al (2009) Disruption of the neurexin 1 gene is associated with
schizophrenia. Hum Mol Genet 18:988–996
Sagata N, Iwaki A, Aramaki T et al (2010) Comprehensive behavioural study of GluR4 knockout
mice: implication in cognitive function. Genes Brain Behav 9:899–909
Savelieva KV, Caudle WM, Findlay GS (2002) Decreased ethanol preference and consumption in
dopamine transporter female knock-out mice. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 26:758–764
Seamans JK, Yang CR (2004) The principal features and mechanisms of dopamine modulation in
the prefrontal cortex. Prog Neurobiol 74:1–58
Shen S, Lang B, Nakamoto C et al (2008) Schizophrenia-related neural and behavioural
phenotypes in transgenic mice expressing truncated DISC1. J Neurosci 28:10893–10904
Shu T, Ayala R, Nguyen MD et al (2004) Ndel1 operates in a common pathway with LIS1 and
cytoplasmic dynein to regulate cortical neuronal positioning. Neuron 44:263–277
Singer P, Boison D, Möhler H (2007) Enhanced recognition memory following glycine
transporter 1 deletion in forebrain neurons. Behav Neurosci 121:815–825
Singer P, Feldon J, Yee BK et al (2009) Are DBA/2 mice associated with schizophrenia-like
endophenotypes? A behavioural contrast with C57BL/6 mice. Psychopharmacology
206:677–698
Singer P, Boison D, Möhler H et al (2011) Modulation of sensorimotor gating in prepulse
inhibition by conditional brain glycine transporter 1 deletion in mice. Eur Neuropsycho-
pharmacol 21:401–413
Smith RE, Haroutunian V, Davis KL et al (2001) Expression of excitatory amino acid transporter
transcripts in the thalamus of subjects with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 158:1393–1399
Stark KL, Xu B, Bagchi A et al (2008) Altered brain microRNA biogenesis contributes to
phenotypic deficits in a 22q11-deletion mouse model. Nat Genet 40:751–760
Stefansson H, Sigurdsson E, Steinthorsdottir V et al (2002) Neuregulin 1 and susceptibility to
schizophrenia. Am J Hum Genet 71:877–892
Steinberg S, Mors O, Børglum AD et al (2011) Expanding the range of ZNF804A variants
conferring risk of psychosis. Mol Psychiatry 16:59–66
Stone JM, Morrison PD, Pilowsky LS (2007) Glutamate and dopamine dysregulation in
schizophrenia-a synthesis and selective review. J Psychopharmacol 21:440–452
Szeszko PR, Hodgkinson CA, Robinson DG et al (2008) DISC1 is associated with prefrontal
cortical gray matter and positive symptoms in schizophrenia. Biol Psychol 79:103–110
Takao K, Yamasaki N, Miyakawa T (2007) Impact of brain-behavior phenotyping of genetically-
engineered mice on research of neuropsychiatric disorders. Neurosci Res 58:124–132
Takao K, Toyama K, Nakanishi K et al (2008) Impaired long-term memory retention and
working memory in sdy mutant mice with a deletion in Dtnbp1, a susceptibility gene for
schizophrenia. Mol Brain 1:11
Susceptibility Genes for Schizophrenia 249
Takashima A (2009) Drug development targeting the glycogen synthase kinase-3beta (GSK-
3beta)-mediated signal transduction pathway: role of GSK-3beta in adult brain. J Pharmacol
Sci 109:174–178
Talbot K (2009) The sandy (sdy) mouse: a dysbindin-1 mutant relevant to schizophrenia research.
Prog Brain Res 179:87–94
Tammimäki A, Forsberg MM, Karayiorgou M et al (2008) Increase in free choice oral ethanol
self-administration in catechol-o-methyltransferase gene-disrupted male mice. Basic Clin
Pharmacol Toxicol 103:297–304
Tandon R, Nasrallah HA, Keshavan MS (2009) Schizophrenia, ‘‘just the facts’’ 4 Clinical features
and conceptualization. Schizophr Res 110:1–23
Tang J, LeGros RP, Louneva N et al (2009) Dysbindin-1 in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of
schizophrenia cases is reduced in an isoform-specific manner unrelated to dysbindin-1 mRNA
expression. Hum Mol Genet 18:3851–3863
Taylor SB, Taylor AR, Markham JA et al (2011) Disruption of the neuregulin 1 gene in the rat
alters HPA axis activity and behavioral responses to environmental stimuli. Physiol Behav
104:205–214
Thompson BL, Levitt P (2010) The clinical-basic interface in defining pathogenesis in disorders
of neurodevelopmental origin. Neuron 67:702–712
Tsai G, Ralph-Williams RJ, Martina M et al (2004) Gene knockout of glycine transporter 1:
characterization of the behavioral phenotype. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:8485–8490
Tsien JZ (2000) Linking Hebb’s coincidence-detection to memory formation. Curr Opin
Neurobiol 10:266–273
Tunbridge EM, Harrison PJ, Weinberger DR (2006) Catechol-o-methyltransferase, cognition, and
psychosis: Val158Met and beyond. Biol Psychiatry 60:141–151
Tuominen HJ, Tiihonen J, Wahlbeck K (2005) Glutamatergic drugs for schizophrenia: a
systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res 72:225–234
van den Buuse M, Wischhof L, Lee RX et al (2009) Neuregulin 1 hypomorphic mutant mice:
enhanced baseline locomotor activity but normal psychotropic drug-induced hyperlocomotion
and prepulse inhibition regulation. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 12:1383–1393
Van den Buuse M (2010) Modelling the positive symptoms of schizophrenia in genetically-
modified mice: pharmacology and methodology aspects. Schizophr Bull 36:246–270
van Os J, Kenis G, Rutten BP (2010) The environment and schizophrenia. Nature 468:203–212
Waddington JL, O’Tuathaigh C, O’Sullivan G et al (2005) Phenotypic studies on dopamine
receptor subtype and associated signal transduction mutants: insights and challenges from
10 years at the psychopharmacology-molecular biology interface. Psychopharmacology
181:611–638
Waddington JL, Corvin AP, Donohoe G, O’Tuathaigh CMP, Mitchell KJ, Gill M (2007)
Functional genomics and schizophrenia: endophenotypes and mutant models. Psychiat Clin N
Amer 30:365–399
Waddington JL, Hennessy RJ, O’Tuathaigh CMP, Owoeye O, Russell V (2011) Schizophrenia
and the lifetime trajectory of psychotic illness: developmental neuroscience and pathobiology,
redux. In: Brown AS, Patterson PH (eds.) The Origins of Schizophrenia. Columbia University
Press, New York (in press)
Walsh T, McClellan JM, McCarthy SE et al (2008) Rare structural variants disrupt multiple genes
in neurodevelopmental pathways in schizophrenia. Science 320:539–543
Walss-Bass C, Liu W, Lew DF et al (2006) A novel missense mutation in the transmembrane
domain of neuregulin 1 is associated with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 60:548–553
Wang Y, Xu R, Sasaoka T et al (2000) Dopamine D2 long receptor-deficient mice display
alterations in striatum-dependent functions. J Neurosci 20:8305–8314
Wang Q, Jaaro-Peled H, Sawa A (2008) How has DISC1 enabled drug discovery? Mol Cell
Neurosci 37:187–195
Wen L, Lu YS, Zhu XH et al (2010) Neuregulin 1 regulates pyramidal neuron activity via ErbB4
in parvalbumin-positive interneurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:1211–1216
250 C. M. P. O’Tuathaigh et al.
Weickert CS, Straub RE, McClintock BW et al (2004) Human dysbindin (DTNBP1) gene
expression in normal brain and in schizophrenic prefrontal cortex and midbrain. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 61:544–555
Wiedholz LM, Owens WA, Horton RE et al (2008) Mice lacking the AMPA GluR1 receptor
exhibit striatal hyperdopaminergia and ‘schizophrenia-related’ behaviors. Mol Psychiatry
13:631–640
Xing B, Kong H, Meng X et al (2010) Dopamine D1 but not D3 receptor is critical for spatial
learning and related signaling in the hippocampus. Neuroscience 169:1511–1519
Xu R, Hranilovic D, Fetsko LA et al (2002) Dopamine D2S and D2L receptors may differentially
contribute to the actions of antipsychotic and psychotic agents in mice. Mol Psychiatry
7:1075–1082
Xu B, Roos JL, Levy S et al (2008) Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with
sporadic schizophrenia. Nat Genet 40:880–885
Xu B, Woodroffe A, Rodriguez-Murillo L (2009) Elucidating the genetic architecture of familial
schizophrenia using rare copy number variant and linkage scans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
106:16746–16751
Yee BK, Balic E, Singer P et al (2006) Disruption of glycine transporter 1 restricted to forebrain
neurons is associated with a procognitive and antipsychotic phenotypic profile. J Neurosci
26:3169–3181
Young JW, Crawford N, Kelly JS et al (2007) Impaired attention is central to the cognitive
deficits observed in alpha 7 deficient mice. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 17:145–155
Young-Pearse TL, Bai J, Chang R et al (2007) A critical function for beta-amyloid precursor
protein in neuronal migration revealed by in utero RNA interference. J Neurosci
27:14459–14469
Zhang R, Su B (2008) MicroRNA regulation and the variability of human cortical gene
expression. Nucleic Acids Res 36:4621–4628
Zhang M, Ballard ME, Basso AM et al (2011) Behavioral characterization of a mutant mouse
strain lacking D-amino acid oxidase activity. Behav Brain Res 217:81–87
Zhou X, Nie Z, Roberts A et al (2010) Reduced NMDAR1 expression in the Sp4 hypomorphic
mouse may contribute to endophenotypes of human psychiatric disorders. Hum Mol Genet
19:3797–3805
Zhuang X, Oosting RS, Jones SR et al (2001) Hyperactivity and impaired response habituation in
hyperdopaminergic mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:1982–1987
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating:
Relevance to Neuropsychiatric Disorders
Keywords Prepulse inhibition Startle Mouse models Schizophrenia Genetic
Mutant
Contents
degree relatives (Cadenhead et al. 2000), and patients with schizotypal personality
disorder (Cadenhead et al. 1993). Additionally, several other neuropsychiatric
disorders are associated with decreased PPI, including Obsessive–Compulsive
Disorder (Swerdlow et al. 1993), Tourette’s syndrome (Swerdlow et al. 2001b),
Huntington’s disease (Swerdlow et al. 1995), manic bipolar patients (Perry et al.
2001), Panic Disorder (Ludewig et al. 2002), Fragile X syndrome (Frankland et al.
2004; Hessl et al. 2009), and adults with autism (Perry et al. 2007). Although the
core symptoms of these disorders are diverse, a feature common to all of them is
deficient gating, with a gating deficit predominating in the cognitive sphere in
some disorders and in the sensory or motor domains in others. Thus, deficient
gating has been reported across a variety of neuropsychiatric disorders, with PPI
deficits in schizophrenia patients being the best characterized and the most widely
replicated (Braff et al. 2001; Kumari et al. 2008; Ludewig et al. 2003; Mackeprang
et al. 2002; Swerdlow et al. 2008).
Sensorimotor gating abnormalities, as measured by PPI, are being used as an
endophenotype in genetic studies of schizophrenia (Braff et al. 2007; Greenwood
et al. 2011), and meet the criteria outlined for a viable endophenotype (Turetsky
et al. 2007). With an increased focus on observable and measurable behaviors as
rational approaches to genetic studies of heterogeneous neuropsychiatric diseases
such as schizophrenia (Gottesman and Gould 2003), large-scale genetic studies are
examining neurophysiological measures such as PPI, P50 auditory evoked sup-
pression, antisaccade eye movement, mismatch negativity, and P300 event-related
potential (Turetsky et al. 2007). Recent studies suggest that PPI is heritable
(Hasenkamp et al. 2010) and associated with polymorphisms in the CHRNA3
gene (Petrovsky et al. 2010), neuregulin 1 (Roussos et al. 2011), and COMT
(Giakoumaki et al. 2008; Quednow et al. 2008; Roussos et al. 2008). The use of
PPI as an endophenotype in genetic studies of schizophrenia, combined with the
observation that PPI has a strong genetic component in mice (Francis et al. 2003),
indicates that PPI may be a useful behavioral phenotype to consider in genetic
mouse models related to neuropsychiatric disease, particularly schizophrenia.
While there are certainly many other symptoms, behavioral traits, and neuro-
physiological deficits observed across the heterogeneous group of patients with
schizophrenia, PPI appears to be a viable endophenotype for human genetic
studies and thus a reasonable approach to investigate in genetic animal models.
Additionally, considering that PPI measures a fundamental component of infor-
mation processing and is observable across many species, it is a useful endpoint
with which to understand the more general impact of specific genes on neurobe-
havioral function and on the neural substrates underlying this function.
Mutant mouse models related to schizophrenia have been based primarily on
the pathophysiology of schizophrenia, the known effects of antipsychotic drugs,
and candidate genes for schizophrenia. In this review, we provide an overview of
PPI in genetic mouse models, concentrating on the time period since the Swerdlow
et al. (2008) review. We discuss the contribution and usefulness of PPI as a
phenotype in the context of genetic mouse models and speculate about the sig-
nificance of PPI deficits and future directions for the field. There have been
254 S. B. Powell et al.
previous reviews on candidate genes for schizophrenia (Arguello and Gogos 2010;
O’Tuathaigh et al. 2007), and reviews focusing specifically on PPI, which sum-
marized studies of strain differences, genetic mutants, and the pharmacology of
PPI in mice (Geyer et al. 2002; Powell et al. 2009; Swerdlow et al. 2008). In this
review, we highlight the approaches to genetic mouse models of neuropsychiatric
disease, discuss some of the important caveats to these approaches, and provide a
comprehensive table covering the genetic models that have evaluated PPI since the
Swerdlow et al. (2008) review. To that end, we discuss PPI as a phenotype in
genetic mouse models generated to address hypotheses regarding the pathophys-
iology of neuropsychiatric disease, candidate genes, and basic proteins involved in
neural development or synaptic function. We also discuss the usefulness of PPI in
phenotype-driven approaches in which a PPI phenotype could lead to ‘‘bottom up’’
approaches of identifying novel genes of relevance to PPI and/or neuropsychiatric
disease [i.e. hypothesis-generating; or phenotype-genotype approaches (Jacobson
and Cryan 2010)].
Attempts to relate PPI deficits to the positive, negative, and cognitive symptoms of
schizophrenia have yielded mixed results (Thaker 2007). PPI negatively correlates
with thought disorder (Meincke et al. 2004; Perry and Braff 1994; Perry et al.
1999) and distractibility (Karper et al. 1996) in schizophrenia. PPI increases
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating 255
A PPI deficit in a genetic mutant could indicate that the gene may be involved in
the neural circuitry known to modulate PPI [e.g. cortical, limbic, striatal
(Swerdlow et al. 2001a)]; in other words it could function as a ‘‘surrogate measure
for neural processes’’ as Swerdlow et al. (2008) suggest. While a PPI deficit per se
is not indicative of altered striatal or limbic circuitry, the presence of the deficit
may suggest that these brain regions are affected by the genetic manipulation and
provide a reasonable starting place for further hypothesis testing regarding the
neurobiological implications of the genetic manipulation. Additionally, mutant
mouse models offer the opportunity to screen putative antipsychotics that may
256 S. B. Powell et al.
involve a novel target and avoid the problems of receptor tautology inherent in
many pharmacological studies [e.g. dopamine agonist-induced disruption blocked
by a dopamine antagonist; as discussed in Powell et al. (2009). Using mutant mice
to screen for putative antipsychotics may provide a means to develop novel drug
targets not achieved with current pharmacological disruptions of PPI (see Table 1
for examples).
(continued)
257
Table 1 (continued)
258
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale manipulations typically treatments
used to induce PPI-deficits
Reelin
Barr et al. (2008) Reelin KO, M Reelin levels in brains of SZ are [(unimodal acoustic) PPI,
reduced but ;crossmodal PPI at
100 ms PP intervals,
PPP at PP intervals B
60 ms, ;PA
habituation & :PA at
100–110 dB(A)
intensity for +/-
versus WT
Neuregulin
Chen et al. (2008) Type III NRG1 mutants, M NRG1 may represent a SZ ;PPI & [PA in +/- versus Chronic NIC (:PPI in +/-; [PPI in WT)
susceptibility gene. Involved in WT
cell-cell interaction; signals via
erbB TK
Duffy et al. (2008) EGF-like domain NRG1 The EGF-like domain of NRG1 is [PPI in -/- versus WT MK801 (;PPI in -/-
mutants, M sufficient for NRG’s activity versus WT); AMPH
(;PPI in -/-and WT;
no significant
differences between
-/- & WT)
1-Tg
Deakin et al. (2009) NRG1Type Type I NRG1 increased in Hpc and ;PPI & :PA in Tg versus
PFC of SZ patients WT
Barros et al. (2009) ERBB2/B4-CNS-specific NRG1 and ERBB4 are candidate ;PPI in KO versus WT (M CLO :PPI in KO; [PPI in WT
KO, M & F genes for SZ only; [PPI in F); CLO : spine density
PA data not shown
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 1 (continued)
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale manipulations typically treatments
used to induce PPI-deficits
Wen et al. (2010) PV-Cre; ErbB4 KO In adulthood NRG1 and ErbB4 may ;PPI & [PA in -/- DIAZ: :PPI in -/- mice; [PPI in WT; PA
regulate neurotransmission. versus WT data not shown
Evidence that ErbB4 is located
at the postsynaptic density of
excitatory neurons, likely on
GABA interneurons
Kato et al. (2010) NRG1 TG5, TG7, M & F Postmortem studies indicate [PPI & [PA in TG versus
increased NRG1 in SZ brain. WT
Transgenic mutants to examine
hyper NRG1 signaling
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
COMT
Papaleo et al. (2008) COMT KO, COMT Val-Tg Polymorphisms in COMT gene [PPI in -/- or TG versus
reported in SZ; mice WT; :PA in -/-
overexpress human COMT-Val versus WT, ;PA in TG
variant versus WT
Tammimäki et al. (2010) S-COMT KO, M & F Two isoforms of COMT, S-COMT [PPI & :PA in -/-
and MB-COMT, differentially versus WT (although
contribute to DA function in the lack of :PA with : dB
frontal cortex in WT mice)
G-alpha
Kelly et al. (2009) Forebrain-specific Galpha :mRNA G-protein subunit Galpha ;PPI & [PA in +/- versus HAL: :PPI in OE; [PPI in WT
OE, M, F levels, genetically linked to SZ WT; developmental or Rolipram (:cAMP levels): :PPI in OE;
adult expression [PPI in WT
[PPI; ;PPI correlates
with ;CTX & HPC
cAMP levels
(continued)
259
Table 1 (continued)
260
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale manipulations typically treatments
used to induce PPI-deficits
G72/G30
Otte et al. (2009) G72Tg (G72/G30 G72/G30 gene locus implicated in ;PPI & [PA in Tg versus HAL :PPI in Tg; [PPI in WT (slight :PPI at
‘‘humanized’’ BAC SZ, BD, and panic disorder WT low PP intensity in WT); PA data not
transgenic) reported
SMARCA
Koga et al. (2009) SMARCA2 KO, M & F SMARCA encodes BRM, a ;PPI in -/- versus WT;
chromatin remodeling multi- PA data not shown
protein complex; highly
expressed in differentiating
neurons; SNP in gene found in
SZ (this paper)
Neurexin
Etherton et al. (2009) Neurexin-1a KO, M & F CNVs for neurexin-1 a identified in ;PPI in -/- versus WT;
SZ & Autism. Neurexins bind :PA to stimuli 80–100
to neuroligins and function as dB in -/- versus WT
synaptic recognition molecules
TCF4
Brzózka et al. (2010) TCF4 Tg, M TCF4, a helix-loop-helix ;PPI & [PA in Tg versus
transcription factor, identified WT
in several GWAS studies as
susceptibility gene for SZ
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 1 (continued)
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale manipulations typically treatments
used to induce PPI-deficits
AKT1
Chen and Lai (2011) AKT1 KO, M & F AKT1 has been associated with F: ;PPI & [PA in -/- RAC: [PPI & [PA in -/- or WT (F only)
schizophrenia in genetic studies versus WT; PA CLOZ: [PPI & [PA in -/- or WT (F
as well as postmortem brain M: [PPI & [PA in only)
analyses. AKT1 signal -/- versus WT 8-OH-DPAT: [PPI in -/- versus WT
transduction pathway important (attenuation weak; no genotype 9 drug
in diverse biological functions interaction)
SB216763: [PPI in -/- versus WT
(attenuation weak; no genotype 9 drug
interaction)
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
Abbreviations: 5-HT serotonin, AAV adeno-associated virus, Ab amyloid b- peptide, ACE angiotensin converting enzyme, ACH acetylcholine (receptor), AD Alzheimer’s disease, ADX adrenalectomy, AIL
advanced intercross line, AMP amphetamine, AMY amygdala, APD antipsychotic drug, APP amyloid precursor protein, ARIP aripiprazole, APO apomorphine, AT angiotensin, BAC bacterial artificial
chromosome, BACE b-site APP cleaving enzyme, BD bipolar disorder, BG background, CaMKIV calcium-calmodulin-dependent protein kinease IV, cAMP cyclic adenosine monophosphate, Ckr chakragati,
CLO clozapine, CNS central nervous system, COC cocaine, COMT catechol-O-methyltransferase, CORT corticosterone, CRF corticotropin releasing factor, CTR controls, CTX cortex, DA dopamine
(receptor), DAT dopamine transporter, dB decibel, DCC deleted in colorectal cancer, DIAZ diazepam, DIZ dizocilpine, DN dominant-negative, DDO D-aspartate oxidase, E embryonic day, EE
environmental enrichment, EGF epidermal growth factor, ENU N-ethyl-N-nitrosourea, f frontal, F female, FABP fatty acid binding protein, FGF fibroblast growth factor, Fmr1 fragile 9 mental retardation
1 gene, FMRP fragile 9 mental retardation protein, FXS fragile 9 Syndrome, GLAST glutamate and aspartate transporter, GLU glutamate, GluR glutamate receptor, GLUT glucose transporter, GR
glucocorticoid receptor, GRIP glutamate receptor interacting protein, GSK glycogen synthase kinase, GWAS genome wide association studies, HAL haloperidol, HD Huntington’s disease, HPC hippo-
campus, IC imprinted cluster, IL interleukin, ISI interstimulus interval, KI knock-in, KO knock-out, M male, m metabotropic, MDB methyl-CpG binding protein, mo month, NIC nicotine, MPEP 2-methyl-6-
(phenylethylyn)-pyridine hydrocholoride, METH metamphetamine, Mgat N-acetylglucosaminyltransferase, NAA N-acetyl-aspartate, NAAG N-acetylalpha L-aspartyl-L-glutamate, NAC nucleus accumbens,
NCAM neural cell adhesion molecule, NIS nisoxetine, nNOS neuronal nitric oxide synthase, NO nitric oxide, NPS neuropeptide S (receptor), NPY neuropeptide Y, NR NMDA receptor subunit, NRG
neuregulin, NRL neuroligin, ns not significant(ly), NSE neuron-specific enolase, NT neurotensin, OE overexpressor, OXO oxotremorine, PA magnitude of response to pulse alone, PACAP pituitary
adenylate-cyclase-activating polypeptide, PD Parkinson’s disease, PDE phosphodiesterase, PET-1 plasmocytoma expressed transcript-1, PND postnatal day, PLC phospholipase C, PNS prenatal stress, poly
I:C polyinosinic: polycytidylic acid, PP prepulse, PPI prepulse inhibition of startle, PPP prepulse potentiation, PS1 presinilin1, PWS Prader–Willi syndrome, QTL quantitative trait locus, QUET quetiapine,
RAC raclopride, RAGE receptor for advanced glycation end-products, RasGAP Ras GTPase-activating protein, RE renin-enhancer, RIS risperidone, SCOP scopolamine, SI social isolation, SNP single
nucleotide polymorphism, SOCS suppressor of cytokine signaling; SREB superconserved receptor expressed in brain; STR striatum, SYN synapsin, SynGAP synaptic GTP-ase-activating protein, SZ
schizophrenia, TAAR trace amine-associated receptor, TG transgenic, TGF-b transforming growth factor beta, TK typosine kinase, TMS transcranial magnetic stimulation, V1b vasopressin receptor 1b, WT
wild-type
; decreased, : increased, [ unchanged, -/- homozygous mice, +/- heterozygous mice
261
262 S. B. Powell et al.
(continued)
263
Table 2 (continued)
264
References Mouse Strain, Sex Model description/ background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale typically used to induce PPI- treatments
deficits
B. GLUTAMATE
NR1
Duncan et al. (2010) NR1 hypomorph Developmental effects of NR1 ;PPI & :PA in hypomorph LY382884, kainic acid antagonist, ;PA in
(NR1 neo/neo), hypomorph on other Glu versus WT hypomorphs & [PA in WT; :PPI in
receptors could contribute to hypomorphs & WT
the SZ-like phenotype.
Evidence for alterations in
kainic acid receptors in SZ.
Belforte et al. (2010) Early postnatal NMDAR hypofunction, Early postnatal NR1
NR1 reduction specifically in GABA reduction: ;PPI & [PA
in GABA interneurons, may contribute in KO versus Ctrl lines
interneurons to SZ pathophysiology. NR1 (Flox-A & Cre), in
(Ppp1r2- is an obligatory subunit of isolated & group-housed
cre +/-; NMDAR. mice
NR1loxP/loxP- Adult NR1 reduction:
line A [KO]), [PPI & [PA
M
AMPA
Sagata et al. (2010) GluR4 KO, M & F GluR4 subunit of AMPA receptor ;PPI & ;PA in -/- versus WT
involved in synaptic plasticity;
genetic association between
GluR4 and SZ
mGLU
Gray et al. (2009) mGlu5 KO, M, F mGlu5 receptor interacts with ;PPI in -/- versus WT; [PA Chronic (8 wks) CLO (:PPI in -/- & [PPI in WT);
NMDA receptor and has been in -/- versus WT :NMDA receptors with CLO treatment
implicated in behavioral
endophenotypes of SZ
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 2 (continued)
References Mouse Strain, Sex Model description/ background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale typically used to induce PPI- treatments
deficits
Fendt et al. (2010) mGlu8 KO, M mGlu8 is a presynaptic receptor [PPI and ;PA in -/- versus PCP: ;PPI in -/- and WT; [PA in
which regulates Glu release; WT -/- or WT
may be important for
neuropsychiatric disorders
with altered Glu function (e.g.
SZ).
Chen et al. (2010) mGlu5 KO, M & F Evidence of NMDA hypofunction ;PPI & [PA in -/- versus WT Sarcosine: :PPI in -/-; [PPI in WT; [PA
in mGlu5 KO mice; mGlu5 in -/- or WT
KO mice may be useful as LY379268: [PPI & [PA in -/- or WT
novel screen of putative N-acetylcysteine: :PPI in -/- & WT; :PPI in
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
(continued)
265
Table 2 (continued)
266
References Mouse Strain, Sex Model description/ background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale typically used to induce PPI- treatments
deficits
Glycine
Benneyworth et al. SR KO, M D-serine is an agonist and NAG an [PPI & [PA in SR -/- versus PCP: ;PPI in SR -/-, GCP2 +/-, &
(2011) GCP2 HET, M antagonist at the glycine WT; [PPI & [PA in WT; inverted U-shaped dose
modulatory site on NMDA GCP2 +/- versus WT response function on PA: high
receptors. Serine racemate dose (6 mg/kg) [PA in WT,
(SR) and glutamic acid :PA in SR -/-; 3 mg/kg dose
decarboxylase 2 (GCP2) :PA in WT, [PA in GCP2 +/-
regulate D-serine and NAG, AMPH: ;PPI in Sr-/-, GCP2
respectively. +/-, & WT; U-shaped dose
response function on PA; no
difference in SR -/-, GCP2 +/-,
& WT
Other glutamate related
Karlsson et al. (2009) GLAST (EAAT1) Glutamate dysfunction associated [PPI in -/- or +/- versus WT;
KO, M, F with SZ; glutamate transporter ;PA in -/- versus +/- &
may be useful WT
pharmacological target for SZ.
Guo et al. (2009) SynGAP +/- gene SynGAP is a RasGAP and ;PPI and :PA in +/- versus CLO: [PPI in +/- or WT; ;PA in +/- and WT
deletion NMDAR-associated protein; WT
interacts with NMDA
receptors
Han et al. (2009) GCPII +/-, M & F Glutamate carboxypeptidase II [PPI & [PA in +/- versus
(GCPII) hydrolyzes NAAG WT
into glutamate and NAA
Wang et al. (2009) Norbin KO Norbin interacts with mGluR5 ;PPI & [PA in -/- versus WT
receptors and positively
regulates mGluR5 signaling.
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 2 (continued)
References Mouse Strain, Sex Model description/ background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale typically used to induce PPI- treatments
deficits
C. SEROTONIN (5-HT)
Semenova et al. 5-HT7 KO, M Antipsychotics have high affinity [PPI across PP intensities PCP ;PPI in WT, attenuated in -/-;
(2008) for 5-HT7 receptors. and ISI, [PA Apo and Amp ;PPI in WT &
-/-; [PA with Apo, Amp, PCP
Shanahan et al. (2009) 5-HTT KO, F OCD patients have PPI deficits; [PPI in -/-, +/- versus WT RU24969 (5HT1B agonist) ;PPI in
gain of function 5-HTT alleles WT, [PPI in -/-, intermediate
associated with OCD effect in +/-
Schaefer et al. (2009) Pet-1 KO, M &F Pet-1 is a transcription factor :PA & [PPI in -/- versus WT
restricted to 5-HT neurons.
Gene deletion results in loss of
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
5-HT.
Hill et al. (2011) 5HT2C KO 5HT and BDNF interact; :BDNF [PPI & [PA in -/- versus
levels 5HT2C KO mice (this WT
study)
Groenink et al. (2011) 5HT1A KO, M 5HT1A gene may interact with [PPI & [PA in -/- versus 129S6: Maternal separation ;PPI
(129S6, Swiss adverse life events. WT & :PA in -/- & WT
Webster [SW] SW: Maternal separation
backgrounds) [PPI & [PA in -/- & WT
van den Buuse et al. 5HT1A KO, M & F 5HT1A receptor may mediate [PPI & [PA in -/- versus MDMA: ;PPI in -/- at lower doses
(2011a) effects of MDMA on PPI WT than WT at 100 ms ISI (M
only); ;PA in -/- & WT
van den Buuse et al. 5HT1A KO, M & F 5HT1A receptor may interact with [PPI & [PA in -/- versus APO: ;PPI & [PA in -/- & WT
(2011b) neurotransmitter systems WT
involved in SZ
(continued)
267
Table 2 (continued)
268
References Mouse Strain, Sex Model description/ background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale typically used to induce PPI- treatments
deficits
D. ACETHYLCHOLINE (ACh)
Nicotinic
Darvas et al. (2009) LYPD6 Tg LYPD6 modulates nicotinic ACh :PPI in Tg versus WT; [PA
receptors; LYPD6 Tg show in Tg versus WT
enhanced nicotinic tone
Young et al. (2011a) a7-nAChR KO, M a7-nAChR is being investigated as [PPI in -/-, +/-, and WT;
&F a pro-cognitive target for SZ; :PA in -/- versus +/- &
comparison of KO mice WT;
across several cognitive tests
Muscarinic
Turner et al. (2010) M2, M4 KO, M M2 and M4 muscarinic [PPI in M2 or M4 -/- versus
acetylcholine receptors differ C57BL/6 N mice; [PA
in tissue distribution and in M2 -/- versus M4 -/-;
physiology; may play a role in :PA in M2 -/- versus
sleep and arousal C57; [PA in M4 -/-
versus C57 (store bought
mice; not littermates)
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 2 (continued)
References Mouse Strain, Sex Model description/ background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale typically used to induce PPI- treatments
deficits
Thomsen et al. (2010) M1, M4 KO, F Muscarinic receptors may be F: ;PPI & [PA in M1-/-/ SCOP: ;PPI in WT & M4-/- mice Xanomeline: blocked SCOP-induce ;PPI in WT &
M1KO/ related to pathophysiology of M4-/- versus WT; [PPI (F) & slight ;PPI in M1-/-/ M1-/-, but not in M4-/- mice; data in M1-/-/
M4KO,M,F SZ, and muscarinic agonists & [PA in M1 -/- versus M4-/- (M); [PPI in M1-/- (F) M4-/- difficult to interpret because [PPI with
have been proposed as targets WT; [PPI & :PA in M4 or M1-/-/M4-/- (F) SCOP; blocked SCOP-induced :PA in M1-/-
for antipsychotics and -/-WT; :PA in all groups; M1-/-/ & M1-/-/M4-/-; [PA in SCOP-treated M4-/-
cognition enhancers M: [PPI & [PA in M4-/- (F) and M4-/- (F) more OXO (versus SCOP): blocked SCOP-induced
M1-/-/M4-/- versus WT sensitive to :PA ;PPI in all genotypes (although SCOP effect
not significant in M1-/-/M4-/-); trend for
reversal of SCOP-induced :PA
OXO (alone): ;PPI in WT & :PPI in M1-/-/
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
(continued)
269
Table 2 (continued)
270
References Mouse Strain, Sex Model description/ background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale typically used to induce PPI- treatments
deficits
E. (NEURO)PEPTIDES
Angiotensin/Renin
Martin et al. (2008) RE KO, NSE-AT1 AT/Renin may be involved in [PPI (RE -/- & NSE-AT1 APO, AMP & DIZ (all ;PA (RE
TG, M brain (DA) signaling. ACE TG vs. WT); ;PA (RE KO vs. WT; main effect
levels were reported to be KO (trend)) & [PA in across treatment conditions);
altered in SZ patients. REKO NSE-AT1 versus WT ;PPI in RE -/-, NSE-AT1 TG
have reduced renin signaling; & WT (no genotype x drug
NSE-AT1 TG have over- interactions); APO (;PA in
expression of AT1 receptors. RE -/-, NSE-AT1 TG & WT);
AMP (:PA in RE -/- & WT
(trend), [PA in NSE-AT1 TG
& WT;) DIZ (:PA in RE -/- &
WT; more pronounced in RE
-/-, :PA (trend) in NSE-AT1
TG & WT;
Neurotensin
Feifel et al. (2010a) NT1 KO, M & F Neurotensin (NT) may play a role [PPI in -/- versus WT; [PA AMP ;PPI & [PA in -/- & WT; PD149163, NT1 receptor agonist, :PPI in WT,
in SZ pathophysiology. NT in -/- versus WT MK801 ;PPI; MK801 :PA [PPI in -/-;
levels decreased in CSF from (low dose only; more
SZ patients, and APDs pronounced in WT than -/-)
increase NT levels.
Feifel et al. (2010b) NT2 KO, M & F NT2 receptors highly expressed in :PPI & ;PA in -/- versus WT
brain but not well studied (both sexes)
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 2 (continued)
References Mouse Strain, Sex Model description/ background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale typically used to induce PPI- treatments
deficits
Oliveros et al. (2010) NT1 KO & NT2 NT receptors may play a role in [PPI & [PA in NT1 -/-, AMP: ;PPI in NT2 -/- & WT, CLO: blocks AMP-induced ;PPI in WT & NT2 -/-
KO, M psychotomimetic and NT2 -/-, & WT [PPI in NT1 -/-; ;PA in WT, ; blocks DIZ-induced ;PPI in WT, does not
antipsychotic effects of drugs [PA in NT1 -/- or NT2 -/- block DIZ-induced ;PPI in NT2 -/-
DIZ: ;PPI in NT2 -/- & WT, NT69L: blocks AMP-induced ;PPI in WT &
[PPI in NT1 -/- NT2 -/-; blocks DIZ-induced ;PPI in NT2 -/-,
does not block DIZ-induced ;PPI in WT;
;PA in WT & NT2-/-
CRF
Groenink et al. (2008) CRF-OE, M Stress physiology, particularly ;PPI in CRF-OE versus WT CORT implant: [PPI in CRF-OE CRF1 antagonists CP154,526 & DMP695
CRF and glucocorticoids, at lower PP intensities; or WT mice. normalized PPI deficit in CRF-OE mice; GR
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
implicated in neuropsychiatric [PA in CRF-OE versus antagonists and ADX did not; CP154,526
disorders WT ;PA
Oxytocin
Caldwell et al. (2009) Oxt KO, M, F Oxytocin has shown antipsychotic [PPI & [PA in -/- versus PCP: Exacerbated ;PPI in -/-
potential in animal models of WT versus WT, [PA; Apo &
SZ and in some preliminary Amp: ;PPI in -/- & WT; [PA
clinical trials
Arginine Vasopressin
Egashira et al. (2009) V1aR KO, V1bR Arginine vasopressin (AVP), a ;PPI in V1aR & V1bR -/-
KO diuretic peptide with receptors versus WT; :PA in V1aR
widely expressed in the CNS, & V1bR -/- versus WT
has been implicated in
neuropsychiatric disorders.
Neuropeptide Y
Karl et al. (2010a) Y2 KO, M & F NPY levels altered in SZ patients. M: :PPI & [PA in -/- versus MK801: ;PPI & :PA in -/- & WT
NPY Y2 receptor expressed in WT AMP: ;PPI & :PA in -/- &
HPC and AMY F: [PPI in -/- versus WT WT
(continued)
271
Table 2 (continued)
272
References Mouse Strain, Sex Model description/ background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale typically used to induce PPI- treatments
deficits
Karl et al. (2010b) Y1 KO, M Decreased Y1 receptor expression [PPI, ;PA, & ;HAB in -/- MK801: ;PPI & :PA in -/- & WT
in lymphocytes of SZ patients versus WT AMP: ;PPI & [PA in -/- &
WT
(effects are ISI & PP
dependent)
Neuropeptide S
Zhu et al. (2010) NPSR1 KO, M & F NPSR1 is a G protein coupled M: ;PA & [PPI in -/- versus
receptor located in brain WT;
regions involved in anxiety F: [PA & [PPI in -/-
and learning and memory, versus WT
whose endogenous ligand is
NPS. NPSR1 SNPs identified
in panic disorder.
Fendt et al. (2011) NPSR KO, M NPS implicated in regulation of [PPI in -/- or +/- versus WT; HAL: :PPI & ;PA in -/-, +/-, & WT
emotional behavior ;PA in -/- & +/- versus
WT
PACAP
Hashimoto et al. PACAP KO, M PACAP affects neurotransmission; ;PPI in -/- versus WT RIS (:PPI in -/-; [ in WT)
(2007) potential SZ susceptibility
gene
Ishihama et al. (2010) PACAP KO, M Genetic susceptibility plus ;PPI in -/- versus WT (PA SI: ;PPI & [PA in -/- & WT EE: ;PPI in -/- & WT
environmental factors may data not shown)
contribute to SZ
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 2 (continued)
References Mouse Strain, Sex Model description/ background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale typically used to induce PPI- treatments
deficits
Relaxin-3
Smith et al. (2009) Relaxin-3 KO, M & Relaxin-3 modulates feeding, [PPI in -/- versus WT; PA
F stress response, exploratory data not reported
behavior, and arousal
F. ORPHAN RECEPTORS
Matsumoto et al. SREB2 KO, SREB (= GPR85) highly ;PPI & [PA in TG versus
(2008) SREB2 TG, M conserved GPCR; expressed WT; [PPI & [PA in -/-
in brain regions of high versus WT
plasticity; overtransmission of
2 SNPs in SZ (this study)
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
Logue et al. (2009) GPR88 KO, M & F GPR88 is highly expressed in brain ;PPI in -/- versus WT; [PA HAL: :PPI in -/-, [PPI in WT; ;PA in -/- & WT
regions implicated in SZ and in -/- versus WT RISP: :PPI in -/- & WT; ;PA in -/- & WT
regulates DA function (this
paper).
Kim et al. (2010) TAK1 KO, M TAK1 is a nuclear orphan receptor ;PPI, ;PA, ;HAP in -/-
that regulates transcription; versus WT
TAK1 deletion delays
migration of cerebellar
granular neurons
(continued)
273
Table 2 (continued)
274
References Mouse Strain, Sex Model description/ background/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
rationale typically used to induce PPI- treatments
deficits
G. TRACE AMINES
Karmacharya et al. TAAR1 KO, M Trace amines are very similar to [PPI in -/- versus WT (PA CLO: :PPI in WT, [PPI in -/-
(2011) biogenic amines and are data not shown)
implicated in neuropsychiatric
disease; trace amine receptors
may mediate effects of
antipsychotic drugs
Abbreviations: 5-HT serotonin, AAV adeno-associated virus, Ab amyloid b- peptide, ACE angiotensin converting enzyme, ACH acetylcholine (receptor), AD Alzheimer’s disease, ADX adrenalectomy, AIL
advanced intercross line, AMP amphetamine, AMY amygdala, APD antipsychotic drug, APP amyloid precursor protein, ARIP aripiprazole, APO apomorphine, AT angiotensin, BAC bacterial artificial
chromosome, BACE b-site APP cleaving enzyme, BD bipolar disorder, BG background, CaMKIV calcium-calmodulin-dependent protein kinease IV, cAMP cyclic adenosine monophosphate, Ckr
chakragati, CLO clozapine, CNS central nervous system, COC cocaine, COMT catechol-O-methyltransferase, CORT corticosterone, CRF corticotropin releasing factor, CTR controls, CTX cortex, DA
dopamine (receptor), DAT dopamine transporter, dB decibel, DCC deleted in colorectal cancer, DIAZ diazepam, DIZ dizocilpine, DN dominant-negative, DDO D-aspartate oxidase, E embryonic day, EE
environmental enrichment, EGF epidermal growth factor, ENU N-ethyl-N-nitrosourea, f frontal, F female, FABP fatty acid binding protein, FGF fibroblast growth factor, Fmr1 fragile 9 mental retardation
1 gene, FMRP fragile 9 mental retardation protein, FXS fragile 9 Syndrome, GLAST glutamate and aspartate transporter, GLU glutamate, GluR glutamate receptor, GLUT glucose transporter, GR
glucocorticoid receptor, GRIP glutamate receptor interacting protein, GSK glycogen synthase kinase, GWAS genome wide association studies, HAL haloperidol, HD Huntington’s disease, HPC hippo-
campus, IC imprinted cluster, IL interleukin, ISI interstimulus interval, KI knock-in, KO knock-out, M male, m metabotropic, MDB methyl-CpG binding protein, mo month, NIC nicotine, MPEP 2-methyl-6-
(phenylethylyn)-pyridine hydrocholoride, METH metamphetamine, Mgat N-acetylglucosaminyltransferase, NAA N-acetyl-aspartate, NAAG N-acetylalpha L-aspartyl-L-glutamate, NAC nucleus accumbens,
NCAM neural cell adhesion molecule, NIS nisoxetine, nNOS neuronal nitric oxide synthase, NO nitric oxide, NPS neuropeptide S (receptor), NPY neuropeptide Y, NR NMDA receptor subunit, NRG
neuregulin, NRL neuroligin, ns not significant(ly), NSE neuron-specific enolase, NT neurotensin, OE overexpressor, OXO oxotremorine, PA magnitude of response to pulse alone, PACAP pituitary
adenylate-cyclase-activating polypeptide, PD Parkinson’s disease, PDE phosphodiesterase, PET-1 plasmocytoma expressed transcript-1, PND postnatal day, PLC phospholipase C, PNS prenatal stress, poly
I:C polyinosinic: polycytidylic acid, PP prepulse, PPI prepulse inhibition of startle, PPP prepulse potentiation, PS1 presinilin1, PWS Prader–Willi syndrome, QTL quantitative trait locus, QUET quetiapine,
RAC raclopride, RAGE receptor for advanced glycation end-products, RasGAP Ras GTPase-activating protein, RE renin-enhancer, RIS risperidone, SCOP scopolamine, SI social isolation, SNP single
nucleotide polymorphism, SOCS suppressor of cytokine signaling; SREB superconserved receptor expressed in brain; STR striatum, SYN synapsin, SynGAP synaptic GTP-ase-activating protein, SZ
schizophrenia, TAAR trace amine-associated receptor, TG transgenic, TGF-b transforming growth factor beta, TK typosine kinase, TMS transcranial magnetic stimulation, V1b vasopressin receptor 1b, WT
wild-type
; decreased, : increased, [ unchanged, -/- homozygous mice, +/- heterozygous mice
S. B. Powell et al.
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating 275
For a detailed description of the methods of acoustic startle and PPI in mice
see Geyer and Dulawa (2003). When conducting an initial analysis of startle
and PPI in a genetic mutant, several considerations should be made when
evaluating PPI. Typically, startle sessions involve variable prepulse intensities
(e.g. 3, 6, 12 dB above background), which should produce an incremental
increase in PPI with increasing prepulse intensities. Of course any evaluation of
a PPI phenotype should be considered in the context of a thorough assessment
of physical and sensory abnormalities (e.g. hearing loss), as pointed out in
Geyer et al. (2002). In a study examining strain differences in hearing and PPI,
Willot et al. (1994) showed a relationship between PPI levels and hearing
impairments. Hearing loss is also exacerbated by age, particularly in specific
strains of mice such as C57s, which experience high frequency hearing loss
with age (Willott et al. 1994). One way to avoid relying solely on auditory
processing is to assess multimodal PPI using light as a prepulse and/or airpuffs
as a startling stimulus (Brody et al. 2004; Young et al. 2010). This multi-
sensory approach can avoid potential confounds of hearing loss and still enable
the evaluation of PPI. It should be noted, however, that some degree of noise is
often produced with the delivery of airpuff stimuli, therefore, data on airpuff-
tactile startle should be interpreted with this consideration in mind. Other tactile
stimuli, such as mild footshock could provide an alternative approach to this
issue. Another way to provide a gross measure of hearing in genetic mutants is
to incorporate a ‘‘startle threshold’’ block into the session, in which increasing
decibels of acoustic stimuli are presented to measure startle magnitude across
these intensities (Brody et al. 2004). The threshold at which the mouse begins
to startle can then be evaluated. Additionally, differences in baseline startle
magnitude can confound effects on PPI. Although dissociations between startle
and PPI have been reported in mice (Brody et al. 2004) and rats (Sipes and
Geyer 1994), large differences in baseline startle magnitude can complicate
interpretations of changes in PPI. If large differences in baseline startle are
observed, startle magnitude can be ‘‘matched’’ either by using a range of startle
stimuli (e.g. 110, 120 dB) paired with the prepulse stimuli or doing a post hoc
analysis of only those animals across genotypes that have comparable levels of
startle. We have included results on startle magnitude [pulse alone (PA)] in
Tables 1–5 when the data were provided and indicated studies that did not
report data on startle magnitude. A good example of testing over a wide range
of startle amplitudes can be found in Savonenko et al. (2009) in their studies of
EP2 KO mice. The background strain on which genetic mutants are made
should be considered as well. For a discussion of the issue surrounding
background strain the reader is referred to Crawley (2007) and Geyer et al.
(2002).
276 S. B. Powell et al.
Broadly defined, behavioral genetic approaches can be divided into two main
approaches either beginning with the gene of interest or the behavior of interest
(Jacobson and Cryan 2010). Gene-based or ‘‘reverse genetics’’ approaches begin
with the targeted gene being manipulated in the animal and the resultant behavior
evaluated. Phenotype-based or ‘‘forward genetics’’ approaches begin with the
targeted behavioral phenotype (trait) and then involve subsequent genetic analysis
of the trait. Most genetic studies of sensorimotor gating have focused on gene-
based approaches, and as such, these approaches are discussed in more detail than
phenotype-based approaches, which are discussed more extensively in Tarantino
and Eisener-Doman (2011). The goals for any genetic approach can be very dif-
ferent—some approaches seek to understand the more global role of a specific
gene in a particular phenotype related to a disease, while others may be designed
to elucidate the role of a particular gene in a cellular process, in neural circuit
abnormalities, or in brain development. Studies representing all of these goals are
well represented in the literature on genetic models of sensorimotor gating (see
Tables 1–5). The techniques used to generate molecular genetic models using the
gene-based (reverse genetic) approach include constitutive gene deletion (knock-
out), insertion of exogenous DNA into the genome (transgenic), and insertion of
gene at a particular locus via homologous recombination (knockin) [reviewed in
(Crawley 2007; Tecott and Wehner 2001)]. Conditional genetic manipulations,
which restrict the expression of a targeted gene either temporally or regionally, are
useful in avoiding complications of in utero lethality or compensatory brain
changes, or when specific hypotheses exist regarding the developmental expres-
sion of a gene or the specific neuroanatomical function of the gene. Examples
of conditional genetic manipulations include the Cre-LoxP system in which the
targeted gene is floxed with lox-p sites. When combined with Cre recombinase, the
floxed gene is expressed, giving regional, cell-specific, or temporal control over
expression or deletion of targeted gene (van der Neut 1997; Wang 2009).
Restriction to specific brain regions can be achieved through the use of specific
promoters used to drive expression. For example, the CaMKIIa promoter restricts
expression of a genetic mutation to the forebrain (Mayford et al. 1996). Additional
examples of tools to achieve temporal control over gene function involve the
Tet-On and Tet-Off systems. With these systems, the target gene is linked to either
the tetracycline-controlled transactivator (tTA) or the reverse tTA (rtTA) and can
be either turned ‘‘on’’ with doxycycline administered (tTA; Tet-On) or turned
‘‘off’’ in the presence of doxycycline (rtTA; Tet-Off) (Mansuy and Bujard 2000).
Thus, by combining some of these approaches, conditional and inducible gene
expression in mouse brain can be achieved. Additionally, insertion of large seg-
ments of DNA into the genome can be achieved through the use of bacterial
artificial chromosomes (BAC transgenics) (Heintz 2001). The BAC transgenic
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating 277
technique is a useful approach when a large segment of DNA has been implicated
in a disease (e.g. 22q11.2 microdeletion or microduplications). For example, BAC
transgenic technology was used to generate two lines of transgenic mice
expressing different genes located on the 22q11.2 segment of the chromosome.
Interestingly, mice overexpressing Prodh and Vpreb2 showed increased PPI, while
mice overexpressing Zdhhc8, Ranbp1, Htf9c, T10, Arvcf, COMT showed no
differences in PPI (Stark et al. 2009). More recent techniques, such as in utero gene
transfer, are being developed to modulate the expression of genes during specific
stages of embryonic development (Niwa et al. 2010).
‘‘Phenotype-based’’ approaches to behavioral genetics include ‘‘forward genetic
screens’’ such as mutagenesis via radiation or chemical means [e.g. ENU muta-
genesis; (Tarantino and Bucan 2000) and quantitative trait loci (QTL) studies on
crosses of inbred mouse strains with distinct phenotypes or on mice or rats
selectively bred for PPI levels (Hitzemann et al. 2008; Tarantino and Eisener-
Doman 2011). Thus far, most of the PPI mutants identified through ENU muta-
genesis screens have had some amount of hearing loss, and thus the specificity of
the PPI ‘‘phenotype’’ was most likely confounded by deafness (Lisa Tarantino,
personal communication). Other molecular genetic tools such as siRNA, in which
double stranded RNA homologous to the targeted gene is made and then inserted
into the cell to block expression of that gene, and viral transfection, in which genes
are inserted into viral vectors and injected into the nucleus of cells, are additional
techniques used in neuroscience research assessing sensorimotor gating, but will
not be discussed in this chapter. Due to the increasingly large number of genetic
models generated in which PPI was evaluated, we will provide some examples in
the text of different genetic approaches undertaken to study sensorimotor gating,
but the reader is referred to Tables 1–5 for a comprehensive review of the recent
genetic models assessing PPI.
(continued)
279
Table 3 (continued)
280
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations typically Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
used to induce PPI-deficits treatments
Ohgake et al. Mdk KO, M & F Midkine involved in neurodevelopment and ;PPI in -/- versus WT, HAL: :PPI in -/-; [PPI in WT
(2009) adult neuroplasticity; abnormal serum +/- intermediate CLO: :PPI in -/-; [PPI in WT
levels in SZ and AD; KO produces (adult); [PPI in -/- Overall ;PA with APD; CLO (3 mg/
;DA and DAR in striatum (this paper) versus WT (4 wk old); kg) ;PA in WT
[PA in -/-, +/-,
WT (adult, 4wk)
Sun et al. Forebrain-specific SMAD4 is an intracellular transducer of ;PPI & [PA in -/-
(2010) SMAD4 KO, TGF-b signaling; TGF-b signaling versus WT
M important for neurodevelopment and
shown to be abnormal in SZ
Nguyen et al. PDGFR-b f/f;Nestin Platelet derived growth factor (PDGF) is ;PPI & [PA in -/-
Cre+
(2011) KO, M important for embryogenesis and CNS versus WT
(neuron- development. PDFGR-b has been
specific associated with SZ and ASD
PDGFR KO)
Protein kinase
Bersudsky GSK3b KO, M GSK3b levels were reported to be reduced [PPI & [PA in +/-
et al. in SZ patients. Li inhibits GSK3b in versus WT
(2008) vitro
Kaidanovich- GSK3a KO, M & F GSK3 implicated in several neuronal :PPI & [PA in -/-
Beilin signaling pathways versus WT
et al.
(2009)
Takao et al. CaMKIV KO, M CaMKIV is a protein kinase that activates [PPI & [PA in -/-
(2010) transcription factor CREB and thus versus WT
may play a role in synaptic plasticity
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 3 (continued)
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations typically Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
used to induce PPI-deficits treatments
Siuta et al. Rictor f/f;Nestin Cre+ Rictor is a component of mTOR2C, the ;PPI & [PA in -/- NIS: (:PPI in -/-; no data in WT; PA data
(2010) KO (neuron- kinase responsible for phosphorylation versus WT not shown)
specific Rictor of Akt. Akt deficiencies associated CLO: ([PPI in -/-; PA data not
KO) with SZ shown)
Proteases
Horii et al. Neuropsin KO, M The serine protease neuropsin may be [PPI & PA in -/- versus
(2008) involved in neuronal plasticity/ WT
degeneration
Savonenko BACE1 KO, M BACE1 is critical for APP cleavage/ ;PPI in -/- & [PPI in CLO: (amelioration of PPI deficits in -/-;
et al. amyloid b production. May also play a +/- versus WT; [PA [PPI in +/- & WT)
(2008) role in SZ via proteolysis of NRG & in -/- & +/- versus
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
(continued)
281
Table 3 (continued)
282
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations typically Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
used to induce PPI-deficits treatments
Savonenko EP2 KO, F EP2 is a prostaglandin receptor involved in ;PPI in -/- versus WT;
et al. activity-dependent synaptic plasticity [PA in -/- versus
(2009) WT
Oliver and SNAP-25 Bdr SNARE protein SNAP-25 important for ;PPI & [PA in Bdr mutant CLO: :PPI in BDR/NS & BDR/PNS; [PPI
Davies (Blind drunk synaptic function and neurotransmitter versus WT, ;PPI in WT/NS or WT/PNS
(2009) mutant), release; SNAP-25 genomic region exacerbated by PNS; HAL: :PPI in Bdr/PNS at 86dB PP
SNAP-25 +/-, linked to SZ [PPI & [PA in +/- only; [PPI in WT/NS, Bdr/NS, or WT/
combined with versus WT, no effect PNS
prenatal stress of PNS in +/-
(PNS), M
Blundell et al. Rim1a KO, M Presynaptic proteins involved in Rim1a: ;PPI &[PA in
(2010) Rab3A KO, M neurotransmitter release and associated -/- versus WT
Syt1R233Q with SZ in genetic studies Rab3A: ;PPI & [PA
(point in -/-versus WT
mutation) KI, Syt1R233Q: ;PPI &
M&F [PA in KI versus WT
Rim1aS413A Rim1aS413A: [PPI &
KI, M [PA in KI versus WT
Calcium Signaling
Matsuo et al. RyR3 KO Ryanodine receptor 3 is an intracellular ;PPI in -/- versus WT at
(2009) calcium release channel preferentially 78 dB PP + 110 dB
expressed in HPC and STR and startle pulse only;
involved in synaptic transmission and [PA in -/- versus
plasticity. RyR3 forms a signaling WT
complex with calcineurin, a potential
susceptibility gene for SZ and BD
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 3 (continued)
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations typically Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
used to induce PPI-deficits treatments
Nakagawasai Cav2.2 KO, M N-type calcium channels are important for ;PPI & [PA in -/-
et al. neurotransmitter release, brain versus WT
(2010) development, and activity-depdendent
plasticity
Potassium channel signaling
Kapfhamer Ppp2r5d KD, M & KCNQ2 gene encodes an M-type potassium Ppp2r5dGt: ;PPI & [PA
et al. F channel subunit and may be a substrate in GT/+ versus WT
(2010) GSK3b KD, M for GSK3b; PP2A regulates GSK3b; GSK3b: ;PPI & [PA
KCNQ2 KD, GSK3b and M-type potassium in +/- versus WT
M&F channels may be linked to SZ KCNQ2: ;PPI & [PA
KD created via in +/- versus WT
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
(continued)
283
Table 3 (continued)
284
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations typically Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
used to induce PPI-deficits treatments
López-Ramos TgIE96, M Mice expressing pseudorabies virus ;PPI & [PA in Tg versus
et al. immediate-early protein IE180 WT (C57BL/6J WT
(2010) (TgIE96) have cerebellar abnormalities mice purchased from
Jackson Laboratories)
Glucose metabolism
Schmidt et al. GLUT3 (Slc2a3) GLUT is widely expressed in brain. Role in ;PPI at lower PP intensities
(2008) KO, M neuronal glucose homeostasis and & :PA in +/- versus
likely role in diverse behaviors WT
Neuromodulatory functions
Errico et al. DDO KO, M The enzyme DDO degrades D-aspartate, an [PPI & [PA in -/- AMP ([PPI in DDO -/-; ;PPI in WT) Chronic D-aspartate ([PPI & PA)
(2008) amino acid with elusive function in the versus WT DIZ (;PPI in -/- & WT, more antagonized AMP & DIZ-induced ;PPI
CNS. A neuromodulatory function at pronounced in WT) in WT (not tested in -/-)
NMDA receptors has been implied
Tanda et al. nNOS KO, M nNOS synthesizes NO from L-arginine; [PPI & [PA in -/- SKF81297 (DA D1 agonist): ;PPI & [PA
(2009) involved in many intracellular versus WT in -/-; [PPI & ;PA in WT
signaling processes; ;nNOS expression
in STR and HPC of SZ;
polymorphisms of nNOS associated
with SZ
Altered myelination
Tanaka et al. PLP1(tg/-) Mylein and oligodendrocyte dysfunction ;PPI in Tg versus WT at
(2009) may contribute to schizophrenia higher PP intensity
pathogenesis. Myelin proteolipid (78dB), PA data not
protein (plp1) decreased in brains of SZ shown
patients
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 3 (continued)
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations typically Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
used to induce PPI-deficits treatments
Spine morphogenesis
Cahill et al. KALRN KO Kalrinin, a guanine-nucleotide exchange ;PPI in -/- versus WT;
(2009) factor (GEF) for Rac-like GTPases, PA data not shown
regulates spine morphogenesis.
KALRN -/- have decreased spine
density (this paper)
Oxidative stress
Benoit et al. QR2 KO, M QR2 enhances production of quinine and [PPI in -/- versus WT;
(2010) reactive oxygen species (ROS) and is PA data not shown
upregulated in cognitively impaired
aged rats; polymorphism in QR2
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
(continued)
285
Table 3 (continued)
286
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations typically Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
used to induce PPI-deficits treatments
Takata et al. XBP1 KO, M XBP1 gene encodes transcription factor in :PPI & [PA in +/- versus
(2010) the ER unfolded protein response; WT
involved in brain development and
potentially pathogenesis of bipolar
disorder and depression
Glycoproteins
Sakatani et al. RAGE KO, M RAGE is a receptor for multiple ligands :PPI in -/- versus WT
(2009) including Ab, HMGB1, and S100B, :PA in -/- versus
which contribute to the pathology of WT at low sound
AD, epilepsy, and ischemia pressure levels (70-
100 dB); ;PA in -/-
versus WT at 120 dB
Fukuda et al. Fut8 KO, M a1,6 fucosyltransferase (Fut8)2 transfers ;PPI & [PA in -/- After restraint stress: ;PPI & [PA in +/-
(2011) fucose from a GDP-fucose to position 6 versus +/- & WT versus WT
of N-acetylglucosamine to form N-
linked oligosaccharides of
glycoproteins. Fucosylated
glycoproteins widely distributed in
brain
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 3 (continued)
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations typically Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
used to induce PPI-deficits treatments
Sex hormones
Chavez et al. ArKO, M, F Gender differences in the age of onset, [PPI in -/- versus WT AMP ;PPI in -/- & WT (;PPI slightly
(2009) severity, and response to treatment in attenuated in F -/-), ;PA in -/- &
SZ. Second increase in SZ incidence in WT
older age for women when estrogen Apo: ;PPI in -/- & WT (;PPI
levels decline. Thus, estrogen may be slightly attenuated in F -/-), ; PA in
neuroprotective against development of -/- & WT
SZ. Aromatase (Ar) converts MK801: ;PPI & :PA in -/- & WT
androgens to estrogens
Abbreviations: 5-HT serotonin, AAV adeno-associated virus, Ab amyloid b- peptide, ACE angiotensin converting enzyme, ACH acetylcholine (receptor), AD Alzheimer’s disease, ADX adrenalectomy, AIL
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
advanced intercross line, AMP amphetamine, AMY amygdala, APD antipsychotic drug, APP amyloid precursor protein, ARIP aripiprazole, APO apomorphine, AT angiotensin, BAC bacterial artificial
chromosome, BACE b-site APP cleaving enzyme, BD bipolar disorder, BG background, CaMKIV calcium-calmodulin-dependent protein kinease IV, cAMP cyclic adenosine monophosphate, Ckr chakragati,
CLO clozapine, CNS central nervous system, COC cocaine, COMT catechol-O-methyltransferase, CORT corticosterone, CRF corticotropin releasing factor, CTR controls, CTX cortex, DA dopamine
(receptor), DAT dopamine transporter, dB decibel, DCC deleted in colorectal cancer, DIAZ diazepam, DIZ dizocilpine, DN dominant-negative, DDO D-aspartate oxidase, E embryonic day, EE
environmental enrichment, EGF epidermal growth factor, ENU N-ethyl-N-nitrosourea, f frontal, F female, FABP fatty acid binding protein, FGF fibroblast growth factor, Fmr1 fragile 9 mental retardation
1 gene, FMRP fragile 9 mental retardation protein, FXS fragile 9 Syndrome, GLAST glutamate and aspartate transporter, GLU glutamate, GluR glutamate receptor, GLUT glucose transporter, GR
glucocorticoid receptor, GRIP glutamate receptor interacting protein, GSK glycogen synthase kinase, GWAS genome wide association studies, HAL haloperidol, HD Huntington’s disease, HPC hippo-
campus, IC imprinted cluster, IL interleukin, ISI interstimulus interval, KI knock-in, KO knock-out, M male, m metabotropic, MDB methyl-CpG binding protein, mo month, NIC nicotine, MPEP 2-methyl-6-
(phenylethylyn)-pyridine hydrocholoride, METH metamphetamine, Mgat N-acetylglucosaminyltransferase, NAA N-acetyl-aspartate, NAAG N-acetylalpha L-aspartyl-L-glutamate, NAC nucleus accumbens,
NCAM neural cell adhesion molecule, NIS nisoxetine, nNOS neuronal nitric oxide synthase, NO nitric oxide, NPS neuropeptide S (receptor), NPY neuropeptide Y, NR NMDA receptor subunit, NRG
neuregulin, NRL neuroligin, ns not significant(ly), NSE neuron-specific enolase, NT neurotensin, OE overexpressor, OXO oxotremorine, PA magnitude of response to pulse alone, PACAP pituitary
adenylate-cyclase-activating polypeptide, PD Parkinson’s disease, PDE phosphodiesterase, PET-1 plasmocytoma expressed transcript-1, PND postnatal day, PLC phospholipase C, PNS prenatal stress, poly
I:C polyinosinic: polycytidylic acid, PP prepulse, PPI prepulse inhibition of startle, PPP prepulse potentiation, PS1 presinilin1, PWS Prader–Willi syndrome, QTL quantitative trait locus, QUET quetiapine,
RAC raclopride, RAGE receptor for advanced glycation end-products, RasGAP Ras GTPase-activating protein, RE renin-enhancer, RIS risperidone, SCOP scopolamine, SI social isolation, SNP single
nucleotide polymorphism, SOCS suppressor of cytokine signaling; SREB superconserved receptor expressed in brain; STR striatum, SYN synapsin, SynGAP synaptic GTP-ase-activating protein, SZ
schizophrenia, TAAR trace amine-associated receptor, TG transgenic, TGF-b transforming growth factor beta, TK typosine kinase, TMS transcranial magnetic stimulation, V1b vasopressin receptor 1b, WT
wild-type
; decreased, : increased, [ unchanged, -/- homozygous mice, +/- heterozygous mice
287
Table 4 Genetically engineered model organisms (ca. 07/01/2007-06/28/2011) based on genes related to: models for specific disorders
288
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
manipulations treatments
typically used to
induce PPI-
deficits
Alzheimer’s disease
Dejaegere Alpha 1B/C c- c-secretases have been implicated in the cleavage ;PPI (most pronounced at high PA HAL: (:PPI & [PA in -/- & WT) CLO: (:PPI
et al. secretase KO of membrane proteins incl. APP, & NRG (a intensities) & [PA in -/- in -/-; [PPI in WT; [PA in -/- & WT)
(2008) SZ vulnerability gene) & in NOTCH versus WT
signaling
Gruart et al. APP, PS1, APP+PS1 Neuropathological changes in brain regions ;PPI in 18 mo old WT, APP KO,
(2008) TG, M regulating PPI may occur in AD models & PS1 KO & APP+PS1 TG versus
may affect PPI 3 mo old WT. Most pronounced
;PPI in 18 mo APP+PS1 TG.
PA data not shown
Tsujimura KF-1 KO, M KF-1 gene increased in AD patients; localized in :PPI in -/- versus WT; ;PA in
et al. hippocampus, cerebellum; modulates protein -/- versus WT
(2008) levels as a ubiquitin ligase; increased in
frontal ctx after chronic antidepressant
treatment, electroconvulsive therapy, and
TMS
Takeuchi P301S Tg, M Tau, a microtubule-associated protein, can cause :PPI & ;PA at 120 dB only in Tg
et al. cell death and is associated with AD and other versus WT
(2011) neurodegenerative diseases. FTDP17
(frontotemporal dementia and PD linked to
chromosome 17) is linked to a point mutation
in tau (P301S)
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 4 (continued)
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
manipulations treatments
typically used to
induce PPI-
deficits
Autism
Allan et al. MDB1 KO, M & F Via DNA methylation, MDB mediate epigenetic ;PPI & [PA in -/- versus WT
(2008) gene regulation, which has been linked to
neurodevelopmental disorders, including
autism
Chadman NL3 KI, M & F Neuroligin-3 (NL3) is a cell adhesion molecule [PPI in KI versus WT; ;PA in KI
et al. involved in synapse development & versus WT
(2008) implicated in autism; ‘‘knock in’’ of point
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
(continued)
289
Table 4 (continued)
290
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
manipulations treatments
typically used to
induce PPI-
deficits
Fragile X-syndrome (FXS)
deVrij et al. FMR1 KO FXS involves mental retardation & is caused by ;PPI at most PP intensities in -/- MPEP (mGluR5 antagonist; :PPI in -/- & WT)
(2008) the absence of FMRP. PPI is reduced in FXS versus WT
patients
Paylor et al. FMR1 KO, FMR1 KO :PPI & ;PA (trend) in -/- versus Addition of human FMR1 gene normalized, i.e.
(2008) with a yeast WT ;PPI & :PA in FMR1 KO to WT levels, &
artificial ;PPI & [PA in WT
chromosome
containing human
FMR1 gene, M
Baker et al. FMR1 KO, M & F Background strain may affect behavioral :PPI & ;PA in -/- versus WT (both
(2010) phenotype in FMR1 KO mice; bred to albino sexes)
C57BL/6J-Tyr(c-Brd) background
Levenga et al. FMR1 KO, M FMRP involved in translation of mRNA at the ;PPI in -/- versus WT; PA data not AFQ056, mGluR5 antagonist: :PPI in -/-;
(2011) synapse, downstream from mGlu5 signaling reported [PPI in WT
(startle measured via eyeblink)
Veeraragavan FMR1 KO, M Increased M1 muscarinic signaling in FMR1 KO [PPI & [PA in -/- versus WT Dicyclomine: [PPI & [PA in -/- or WT
et al. mice
(2011)
Thomas et al. mG1.FMR1 (mGlu1 FMR1 may increase signaling through Group1 :PPI & ;PA in FMR1-/- &
(2011a) HZ + FMR1 KO); mGluRs (mGlu1, mGlu5) mG5.FMR1 versus WT; :PPI &
mG5.FMR1 ;PA in FMR1-/- &
(mGlu5 HZ + mG1.FMR1 versus WT
FMR1 KO);
FMR1 KO
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 4 (continued)
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
manipulations treatments
typically used to
induce PPI-
deficits
Thomas et al. FMR1 KO, M & F Group 1 mGluRs may be pharmacological target ;PA & [PPI (trend for predicted :) JNJ (mGlu1
(2011b) in Fragile X in -/- versus WT antagonist):
;PPI in WT,
[PPI in -/-;
[PA in -/-
& WT
MPEP
(mGlu5
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
antagonist):
[PPI & [PA
in -/- & WT
Huntington’s disease
Menalled R6/2 TG, R6/2B TG, HD results from a known genetic CAG repeat R6/2: ;PPI in TG versus WT
et al. YAC128 TG, mutation; ;PPI in HD patients; HD mutant beginning at 12 wks; ;PA
(2009) YAC128B TG, mice show varying degrees of neuropathology R6/2B: ;PPI in TG versus WT
BACHD TG, and behavioral abnormalities; comparison BACHD: ;PPI beginning at 24
HDHQ111 KI, M & controlling for many factors including sex, wks; [PA
F background strain, husbandry, and handling YAC128B: ;PPI at 54 wks; [PA
YAC128: [PPI; [PA
HDHQ111 KI: [PPI; [PA
Brooks et al. Hdh(CAG)150 KI, M & F Longitudinal characterization of KI mouse ;PPI & ;PA in KI versus WT
(2010) carrying 150 CAG repeats on the mouse Htt
locus
Brooks et al. R6/1 TG, M & F R6/1 TG mouse contains *115 CAG repeats on ;PPI & ;PA in TG versus WT
(2011) exon 1 of the HD gene. Characterization of
R6/1 TG mice on C57BL/6 background is
needed
291
(continued)
Table 4 (continued)
292
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
manipulations treatments
typically used to
induce PPI-
deficits
Pietropaolo R6/1 TG, M & F Investigate neuropsychiatric symptoms in pre- [PPI & [PA in TG versus WT
et al. motor stage of pathology
(2011)
Hearing disorders
Allen et al. Kv1.1 KO PPI in rodents is a powerful means to identify ;PPI (for long gaps only)
(2008) hearing deficits. The role of Kv1.1 channels
in PPI gap detection paradigms was assessed
22q11 deletion & duplication syndromes
Suzuki et al. SEPT5 (Septin5) KO, 22q11.2 microdeletions result in cognitive & :PPI & [PA in -/- versus WT
(2009b) M behavioral abnormalities and are associated
with SZ and autism; SEPT5 gene located on
200kb region spanning the deletion
Stark et al. BAC Tg-1: 22q11.2 microduplications are also associated Tg-1: :PPI & [PA in Tg-1 versus
(2009) Overexpress with behavioral problems and learning WT
Prodh & Vpreb2 disabilities; gain of function mutations can Tg-2: [PPI & [PA in Tg-2
BAC Tg-2: also be informative for the deletion syndrome versus WT
overexpress
Zdhhc8, Ranbp1,
Htf9c, T10, Arvcf,
& COMT
(continued)
S. B. Powell et al.
Table 4 (continued)
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
manipulations treatments
typically used to
induce PPI-
deficits
Suzuki et al. BAC Tg, M: 22q11.2 is a hotspot for CNVs. 97% of children [PPI & [PA in Tg versus WT
(2009a) overexpress with 22q11.2 duplications show cognitive
190 kb segment of deficits
22q11.2 including
TXNRD2,
COMT, ARVCF
Down syndrome
Ortiz-Abalia TG DyrK1A DyrK1A overexpression has been implicated in ;PPI at higher PP intensities in TG Intrastriatal infusion of AAV iRNA against
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating
et al. overexpressors, M Down Syndrome versus WT (trend only); PA not Dyrk1A: PP intensity-dependent :PPI in TG
(2008) shown treated with functional versus scrambled
viral vectors & pre versus post-treatment
with functional vector; PA not shown
Glycosilation IIa congenital disorders
Soleimani Mgat5 KO, M & F Mgat5 is involed in glycosylation of cell surface [PPI & PA in -/- versus WT; :PA
et al. glycoproteins, including receptors & in M versus F (main effect)
(2008) transporters. Abundant in the CNS
(continued)
293
Table 4 (continued)
294
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/background/rationale Basal PPI Effects of drugs or Effects of (presumed) antipsychotics/other
manipulations treatments
typically used to
induce PPI-
deficits
Prader-Willi syndrome
Relkovic et al. PWS-IC (+/-), M & F Prader-Willi Syndrome (PWS) ;PPI & :PA in +/- versus WT in M
(2010) neurodevelopmental disorder resulting from & F (only at P105 in M)
deletion of paternally expressed imprinted
genes of chromosome 15q11-q13, a region
also implicated in autism and SZ
Abbreviations: 5-HT serotonin, AAV adeno-associated virus, Ab amyloid b- peptide, ACE angiotensin converting enzyme, ACH acetylcholine (receptor), AD Alzheimer’s disease, ADX adrenalectomy, AIL
advanced intercross line, AMP amphetamine, AMY amygdala, APD antipsychotic drug, APP amyloid precursor protein, ARIP aripiprazole, APO apomorphine, AT angiotensin, BAC bacterial artificial
chromosome, BACE b-site APP cleaving enzyme, BD bipolar disorder, BG background, CaMKIV calcium-calmodulin-dependent protein kinease IV, cAMP cyclic adenosine monophosphate, Ckr chakragati,
CLO clozapine, CNS central nervous system, COC cocaine, COMT catechol-O-methyltransferase, CORT corticosterone, CRF corticotropin releasing factor, CTR controls, CTX cortex, DA dopamine
(receptor), DAT dopamine transporter, dB decibel, DCC deleted in colorectal cancer, DIAZ diazepam, DIZ dizocilpine, DN dominant-negative, DDO D-aspartate oxidase, E embryonic day, EE
environmental enrichment, EGF epidermal growth factor, ENU N-ethyl-N-nitrosourea, f frontal, F female, FABP fatty acid binding protein, FGF fibroblast growth factor, Fmr1 fragile 9 mental retardation 1
gene, FMRP fragile 9 mental retardation protein, FXS fragile 9 Syndrome, GLAST glutamate and aspartate transporter, GLU glutamate, GluR glutamate receptor, GLUT glucose transporter, GR
glucocorticoid receptor, GRIP glutamate receptor interacting protein, GSK glycogen synthase kinase, GWAS genome wide association studies, HAL haloperidol, HD Huntington’s disease, HPC hippo-
campus, IC imprinted cluster, IL interleukin, ISI interstimulus interval, KI knock-in, KO knock-out, M male, m metabotropic, MDB methyl-CpG binding protein, mo month, NIC nicotine, MPEP 2-methyl-6-
(phenylethylyn)-pyridine hydrocholoride, METH metamphetamine, Mgat N-acetylglucosaminyltransferase, NAA N-acetyl-aspartate, NAAG N-acetylalpha L-aspartyl-L-glutamate, NAC nucleus accumbens,
NCAM neural cell adhesion molecule, NIS nisoxetine, nNOS neuronal nitric oxide synthase, NO nitric oxide, NPS neuropeptide S (receptor), NPY neuropeptide Y, NR NMDA receptor subunit, NRG
neuregulin, NRL neuroligin, ns not significant(ly), NSE neuron-specific enolase, NT neurotensin, OE overexpressor, OXO oxotremorine, PA magnitude of response to pulse alone, PACAP pituitary adenylate-
cyclase-activating polypeptide, PD Parkinson’s disease, PDE phosphodiesterase, PET-1 plasmocytoma expressed transcript-1, PND postnatal day, PLC phospholipase C, PNS prenatal stress, poly I:C
polyinosinic: polycytidylic acid, PP prepulse, PPI prepulse inhibition of startle, PPP prepulse potentiation, PS1 presinilin1, PWS Prader–Willi syndrome, QTL quantitative trait locus, QUET quetiapine,
RAC raclopride, RAGE receptor for advanced glycation end-products, RasGAP Ras GTPase-activating protein, RE renin-enhancer, RIS risperidone, SCOP scopolamine, SI social isolation, SNP single
nucleotide polymorphism, SOCS suppressor of cytokine signaling; SREB superconserved receptor expressed in brain; STR striatum, SYN synapsin, SynGAP synaptic GTP-ase-activating protein, SZ
schizophrenia, TAAR trace amine-associated receptor, TG transgenic, TGF-b transforming growth factor beta, TK typosine kinase, TMS transcranial magnetic stimulation, V1b vasopressin receptor 1b, WT
wild-type
; decreased, : increased, [ unchanged, -/- homozygous mice, +/- heterozygous mice
S. B. Powell et al.
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating 295
addition to other behavioral abnormalities (Guo et al. 2009). For a review of other
neurotransmitter-related genetic mutants [e.g. acetylcholine, serotonin, dopamine
(see Table 4)].
In addition to classic neurotransmitters, the important role of neuropeptides in
neuropsychiatric disease is becoming more widely appreciated. PPI has been
assessed in genetic mutants for many neuropeptides including angiotensin, neu-
rotensin, corticotropin releasing factor (CRF) overexpression, oxytocin, arginine
vasopressin, neuropeptide Y, PACAP, and Relaxin-3 (Table 4). Stress physiology,
particularly CRF and glucocorticoids, has been implicated in neuropsychiatric
disease. Mice overexpressing a CRF transgene exhibit deficits in PPI (at low
prepulse intensities), which are normalized by CRF1 antagonists, whereas glu-
cocorticoid receptor antagonists did not reverse the PPI deficit (Groenink et al.
2008). Moreover, several genetic mutants have been created to determine the role
of specific proteins in brain processes, including second messenger signaling,
synaptic proteins, synaptic vesicles, protein kinases, etc. (Table 3). While the
direct link for many of these proteins has not necessarily been established for
specific neuropsychiatric diseases, a thorough behavioral evaluation of these mice
including an assessment of PPI can help elucidate the functional implications of
the target protein. For example, gene deletion of PDE4B, which catalyzes the
degradation of cAMP and is widely expressed in brain, is associated with PPI
deficits (Siuciak et al. 2008). Decreased presynaptic proteins such as synapto-
physin, SNAP-25, and complexin II have been observed in postmortem brains of
schizophrenia patients (Harrison and Weinberger 2005). These data, combined
with evidence of problems with neuronal migration and abnormal neuronal pro-
cesses, have led investigators to conceptualize schizophrenia as a disease of
functional ‘‘dysconnectivity’’ (Friston and Frith 1995; McGlashan and Hoffman
2000; Weinberger et al. 1992) or a ‘‘disorder of the synapse’’ (Frankle et al. 2003;
Mirnics et al. 2001) reviewed in (Harrison and Weinberger 2005). Thus many genetic
mutants have been created for specific synaptic proteins (Table 3). For example,
SYNII, a vesicle-linked phosphoprotein that plays a role in neuronal development
and neurotransmitter release (Cesca et al. 2010), is genetically associated with
schizophrenia, decreased in brain of schizophrenia patients, and increased with
chronic antipsychotic drug treatment (Chen et al. 2004; Chong et al. 2002). SYNII
KO mice exhibit decreased PPI, providing more evidence for the essential role of
SYNII in synaptic function and behavior (Dyck et al. 2007, 2009). Similarly, a
mutation of SNAP-25, a SNARE protein that is integral to synaptic function and
neurotransmitter release, is associated with PPI deficits (Oliver and Davies 2009).
As the field of neuropsychiatric genetics has grown, several gene targets have been
identified consistently for particular disorders and have thus been modified in
mouse models through gene deletion, the addition of a transgene, etc. When
evaluating candidate genes for mouse models, one should consider that many
single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) identified in genetic screens for neuro-
psychiatric disease involve mutations in introns of genes. Thus, it is very important
to consider (1) whether or not functional mutations in the gene have been
identified before embarking on mutant mouse models, and (2) the impact of
species–specific alterations in gene function in the mutant mouse (Low and Hardy
2007). Table 1 summarizes genetic mouse models of vulnerability genes for
schizophrenia, and Table 2 summarizes genetic models of other neuropsychiatric
disorders in which PPI deficits have been observed. Two approaches in human
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating 297
Mice with in utero knockdown of DISC1 show reduced PPI when tested in
adulthood (Niwa et al. 2010). As we have discussed previously, modeling the
human DISC1 mutation in mice is difficult because most of the models have
assumed that the mutation in neuropsychiatric disease is due to the formation of a
truncated DISC1 protein (Powell et al. 2009). Another gene, Boymaw, is also
disrupted on chromosome 11 in the Scottish family. Two fusion transcripts are
generated between DISC1 and Boymaw genes in the translocation carriers in the
Scottish schizophrenia family (Zhou et al. 2008). Thus, expressing the two fusion
transcripts may be a better strategy for creating mutant DISC1 mice for the
study of neuropsychiatric disorders. The increasing identification of copy number
variants (CNVs) associated with schizophrenia and large-scale GWAS studies
identifying novel candidate genes will lead to even greater potential to create new
mouse models (see Tables for examples). Additionally, PPI is being used more
widely to characterize mutant mouse models of candidate genes for other neuro-
psychiatric disorders such as autism and Huntington’s Disease (Table 2).
et al. BALBc/J and high and low PPI between PPI & PA initially. Breeding line high PPI line, but [PPI in low PPI line dose only, no line 9
(2008) LP/J crosses, M 9 generation effects on PPI (low vs. high at highest METH dose). DIZ (;PPI; dose interaction)
&F PPI line) after five generations. No line 9 main effect of dose only, no line 9
generation effects on PA, but :PA in low dose interaction)
PPI line versus ;PA in high PPI line (main
effect only). QTL on chromosomes 11 &
16 in low PPI versus high PPI line
Torkamanzehi A/J, C57Bl6/J and Screening for PPI and PA Comparable PPI, but not PA in parental strains.
et al. congenic crosses phenotypes using Significant variation for congenic strains
(2008) thereof, M light prepulses and for PA for both BG and for PPI in C57Bl6
tactile startle pulses BG strains. Common markers with
acoustic PA, but not acoustic PPI from
previous study (Joober et al. 2002)
Samocha et al. LG/J x SM/J F2 cross PPI and PA phenotyping QTLs found at chromosome 12 for all PP
(2010) & F34 advanced followed by QTL in intensities; QTLs for PP6 & PP12 on
intercross line F2 generation and chromosome 7; QTLs on chromosomes 7
(AIL), M, F genotyped F34 line at & 17 for startle response; QTLs on
3000 SNP markers chromosome 4 for startle habituation; AIL
mice reduced size of some QTLs to less
than 5 cm
(continued)
299
Table 5 (continued)
300
References Mouse strain, sex Model description/ Basal PPI Effects of drugs or manipulations typically Effects of (presumed)
background/rationale used to induce PPI-deficits antipsychotics/other
treatments
Insertional mutagenesis
Verma et al. Ckr mouse, M, F Ckr serendipidous ;PPI (modest) at all PP intensities in Ckr versus CLO (:PPI in Ckr; [PPI in
(2008) mutation resulted in (+/- & WT); [PA in Ckr versus -/+ or WT; RIS ([PPI in Ckr
mouse with abnormal WT & WT), HAL ([PPI in
circling behavior Ckr & WT); PA data
not shown
Abbreviations: 5-HT serotonin, AAV adeno-associated virus, Ab amyloid b- peptide, ACE angiotensin converting enzyme, ACH acetylcholine (receptor), AD Alzheimer’s disease, ADX
adrenalectomy, AIL advanced intercross line, AMP amphetamine, AMY amygdala, APD antipsychotic drug, APP amyloid precursor protein, ARIP aripiprazole, APO apomorphine, AT
angiotensin, BAC bacterial artificial chromosome, BACE b-site APP cleaving enzyme, BD bipolar disorder, BG background, CaMKIV calcium-calmodulin-dependent protein kinease IV, cAMP
cyclic adenosine monophosphate, Ckr chakragati, CLO clozapine, CNS central nervous system, COC cocaine, COMT catechol-O-methyltransferase, CORT corticosterone, CRF corticotropin
releasing factor, CTR controls, CTX cortex, DA dopamine (receptor), DAT dopamine transporter, dB decibel, DCC deleted in colorectal cancer, DIAZ diazepam, DIZ dizocilpine, DN dominant-
negative, DDO D-aspartate oxidase, E embryonic day, EE environmental enrichment, EGF epidermal growth factor, ENU N-ethyl-N-nitrosourea, f frontal, F female, FABP fatty acid binding
protein, FGF fibroblast growth factor, Fmr1 fragile 9 mental retardation 1 gene, FMRP fragile 9 mental retardation protein, FXS fragile 9 Syndrome, GLAST glutamate and aspartate
transporter, GLU glutamate, GluR glutamate receptor, GLUT glucose transporter, GR glucocorticoid receptor, GRIP glutamate receptor interacting protein, GSK glycogen synthase kinase,
GWAS genome wide association studies, HAL haloperidol, HD Huntington’s disease, HPC hippocampus, IC imprinted cluster, IL interleukin, ISI interstimulus interval, KI knock-in, KO knock-
out, M male, m metabotropic, MDB methyl-CpG binding protein, mo month, NIC nicotine, MPEP 2-methyl-6-(phenylethylyn)-pyridine hydrocholoride, METH metamphetamine, Mgat N-
acetylglucosaminyltransferase, NAA N-acetyl-aspartate, NAAG N-acetylalpha L-aspartyl-L-glutamate, NAC nucleus accumbens, NCAM neural cell adhesion molecule, NIS nisoxetine, nNOS
neuronal nitric oxide synthase, NO nitric oxide, NPS neuropeptide S (receptor), NPY neuropeptide Y, NR NMDA receptor subunit, NRG neuregulin, NRL neuroligin, ns not significant(ly), NSE
neuron-specific enolase, NT neurotensin, OE overexpressor, OXO oxotremorine, PA magnitude of response to pulse alone, PACAP pituitary adenylate-cyclase-activating polypeptide, PD
Parkinson’s disease, PDE phosphodiesterase, PET-1 plasmocytoma expressed transcript-1, PND postnatal day, PLC phospholipase C, PNS prenatal stress, poly I:C polyinosinic: polycytidylic
acid, PP prepulse, PPI prepulse inhibition of startle, PPP prepulse potentiation, PS1 presinilin1, PWS Prader–Willi syndrome, QTL quantitative trait locus, QUET quetiapine, RAC raclopride,
RAGE receptor for advanced glycation end-products, RasGAP Ras GTPase-activating protein, RE renin-enhancer, RIS risperidone, SCOP scopolamine, SI social isolation, SNP single nucleotide
polymorphism, SOCS suppressor of cytokine signaling; SREB superconserved receptor expressed in brain; STR striatum, SYN synapsin, SynGAP synaptic GTP-ase-activating protein, SZ
schizophrenia, TAAR trace amine-associated receptor, TG transgenic, TGF-b transforming growth factor beta, TK typosine kinase, TMS transcranial magnetic stimulation, V1b vasopressin
receptor 1b, WT wild-type
; decreased, : increased, [ unchanged, -/- homozygous mice, +/- heterozygous mice
S. B. Powell et al.
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating 301
creation of a mutant mouse are the SP4 and the SREB2 genes. SP4 is a member of
the Sp1 family of transcription factors. Hypomorphic Sp4 mice showed vacuoli-
zation in the hippocampus, age-dependent decrease in neurotrophin-3 expression
in the dentate granule cells, and robust deficits in both PPI and contextual memory
(Zhou et al. 2004). These studies revealed a novel Sp4 pathway that is important
for hippocampal development and essential to many behaviors, including PPI,
relevant to schizophrenia (Zhou et al. 2004). Zhou et al. (2009) have gone on to
examine the role of the human SP4 gene in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.
Several SNPs from the human SP4 gene are found to associate with both bipolar
disorder and schizophrenia in both Caucasian and Chinese samples. This work
represents an example in which a PPI phenotype, in combination with other
behavioral abnormalities, suggested the association of this gene with neuropsy-
chiatric disorders. A similar finding was also observed with SREB2 Tg mice,
which display PPI deficits. Follow-up genetic studies in schizophrenia found a
genetic association between SREB2 and schizophrenia (Matsumoto et al. 2008).
4 Discussion
argues, too many models that are not based on either ‘‘etiologic or pathogenic’’
theories, may result in too many ‘‘false positives’’ in drug screens. Rather, refined
models that more closely mimic the etiological risk factors and/or neuropathology
of disease (e.g. schizophrenia) may generate more predictive models for drug
development (Moore 2010). While this approach might be very useful if the goal is
to use the mutant mice for drug development, we have illustrated the benefits of
careful behavioral characterization in genetic models of basic brain processes as
well. As mentioned above, it is unlikely that all aspects of a heterogeneous disease
will be recapitulated in another species with a genetic mutation. Investigators must
rely on convergence of behavioral and neuroanatomical or neurochemical data in a
given mutant mouse to support clinical data on the link between the candidate gene
and disease. No single phenotype such as PPI should be considered as being either
necessary or sufficient to substantiate a model as having relevance to neuropsy-
chiatric disease.
Animal studies addressing neuropsychiatric disease would benefit greatly from
more neurobiologically based biomarkers for these disorders. Such an approach
has been taken in the CNTRICS initiative, in an explicit attempt to incorporate
more cognitive neuroscience-based testing into treatment trials of putative cog-
nitive therapies for schizophrenia. Along the same lines, genetic studies of
schizophrenia have focused on psychophysiological endophenotypes such as PPI
instead of the broader, more heterogeneous diagnosis of schizophrenia. In fact,
many laboratories are now using PPI as an endophenotype in genetic studies of
schizophrenia (Braff et al. 2007; Greenwood et al. 2007; Greenwood et al. 2011;
Hokyo et al. 2010). Some of these genetic studies have generated further support
for a genetic contribution to PPI (Greenwood et al. 2007, 2011), and other studies
have suggested that genetic variants in COMT (Quednow et al. 2008; Roussos
et al. 2008), CHRNA3 (Petrovsky et al. 2010), and neuregulin 1 (Roussos et al.
2011) directly affect PPI levels (as reviewed above). Thus, as human studies
materialize with more neurobiologically defined behavioral measures, the ability
to translate these measures or ‘‘endophenotypes’’ into animal models should
improve dramatically. PPI in genetic models offers a behavioral endpoint that has
shown predictive validity in rat pharmacological models, cross-species homology
with the same measure in humans, and alterations in response to genetic manip-
ulations implicated in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. While these mutant
mouse models are not without shortcomings, they offer some of the best attempts
at etiological models that are possible in rodents. Merging the genetic etiological
models with a second hit approach may strengthen some of the mutant mouse
models. Hence, consideration for other factors, such as the importance of envi-
ronmental risk factors (e.g. prenatal infection) and the role of epigenetics (e.g.
DNA methylation) in the etiology of schizophrenia, should also be incorporated
with genetic models.
Acknowledgments This work was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental
Health (R01MH042228, R01MH052885, R01MH091407) and by the Veterans Affairs VISN 22
Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center.
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating 303
References
Abel KM, Allin MP, Hemsley DR, Geyer MA (2003) Low dose ketamine increases prepulse
inhibition in healthy men. Neuropharmacol 44:729–737
Allan AM, Liang X, Luo Y, Pak C, Li X, Szulwach KE, Chen D, Jin P, Zhao X (2008) The loss of
methyl-CpG binding protein 1 leads to autism-like behavioral deficits. Hum Mol Genet 17:
2047–2057
Allen PD, Schmuck N, Ison JR, Walton JP (2008) Kv1.1 channel subunits are not necessary for
high temporal acuity in behavioral and electrophysiological gap detection. Hear Res
246:52–58
Arguello PA, Gogos JA (2010) Cognition in mouse models of schizophrenia susceptibility genes.
Schizophr Bull 36:289–300
Asp L, Holtze M, Powell SB, Karlsson H, Erhardt S (2010) Neonatal infection with neurotropic
influenza A virus induces the kynurenine pathway in early life and disrupts sensorimotor
gating in adult Tap -/- mice. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 13:475–485
Baker KB, Wray SP, Ritter R, Mason S, Lanthorn TH, Savelieva KV (2010) Male and female
Fmr1 knockout mice on C57 albino background exhibit spatial learning and memory
impairments. Genes Brain Behav 9:562–574
Barr AM, Fish KN, Markou A, Honer WG (2008) Heterozygous reeler mice exhibit alterations in
sensorimotor gating but not presynaptic proteins. Eur J Neurosci 27:2568–2574
Barros CS, Calabrese B, Chamero P, Roberts AJ, Korzus E, Lloyd K, Stowers L, Mayford M,
Halpain S, Muller U (2009) Impaired maturation of dendritic spines without disorganization
of cortical cell layers in mice lacking NRG1/ErbB signaling in the central nervous system.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:4507–4512
Behrens MM, Ali SS, Dao DN, Lucero J, Shekhtman G, Quick KL, Dugan LL (2007) Ketamine-
induced loss of phenotype of fast-spiking interneurons is mediated by NADPH-oxidase.
Science 318:1645–1647
Belforte JE, Zsiros V, Sklar ER, Jiang Z, Yu G, Li Y, Quinlan EM, Nakazawa K (2010) Postnatal
NMDA receptor ablation in corticolimbic interneurons confers schizophrenia-like pheno-
types. Nat Neurosci 13:76–83
Benes FM, Berretta S (2001) GABAergic interneurons: implications for understanding
schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 25:1–27
Benneyworth MA, Basu AC, Coyle JT (2011) Discordant behavioral effects of psychotomimetic
drugs in mice with altered NMDA receptor function. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 213:
143–153
Benoit CE, Bastianetto S, Brouillette J, Tse Y, Boutin JA, Delagrange P, Wong T, Sarret P,
Quirion R (2010) Loss of quinone reductase 2 function selectively facilitates learning
behaviors. J Neurosci 30:12690–12700
Bersudsky Y, Shaldubina A, Kozlovsky N, Woodgett JR, Agam G, Belmaker RH (2008)
Glycogen synthase kinase-3beta heterozygote knockout mice as a model of findings in
postmortem schizophrenia brain or as a model of behaviors mimicking lithium action:
negative results. Behav Pharmacol 19:217–224
Bitsios P, Giakoumaki SG, Theou K, Frangou S (2006) Increased prepulse inhibition of the
acoustic startle response is associated with better strategy formation and execution times in
healthy males. Neuropsychologia 44:2494–2499
Blackwood DH, Fordyce A, Walker MT, Clair DM, Porteous DJ, Muir WJ (2001) Schizophrenia
and affective disorders—cosegregation with a translocation at chromosome 1q42 that directly
disrupts brain-expressed genes: clinical and P300 findings in a family. Am J Hum Genet
69:428–433
Blundell J, Kaeser PS, Sudhof TC, Powell CM (2010) RIM1alpha and interacting proteins
involved in presynaptic plasticity mediate prepulse inhibition and additional behaviors linked
to schizophrenia. J Neurosci 30:5326–5333
304 S. B. Powell et al.
Braff DL (2010) Prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex: a window on the brain in schizophrenia.
Curr Top Behav Neurosci 4:349–371
Braff DL (2011) Gating in schizophrenia: from genes to cognition (to real world function?). Biol
Psychiatry 69:395–396
Braff DL, Freedman R, Schork NJ, Gottesman II (2007) Deconstructing schizophrenia: an
overview of the use of endophenotypes in order to understand a complex disorder. Schizophr
Bull 33:21–32
Braff DL, Geyer MA, Swerdlow NR (2001) Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle:
normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl)
156:234–258
Braff DL, Grillon C, Geyer MA (1992) Gating and habituation of the startle reflex in
schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49:206–215
Brody SA, Conquet F, Geyer MA (2003) Disruption of prepulse inhibition in mice lacking
mGluR1. Eur J Neurosci 18:3361–3366
Brody SA, Dulawa SC, Conquet F, Geyer MA (2004) Assessment of a prepulse inhibition deficit
in a mutant mouse lacking mGlu5 receptors. Mol Psychiatry 9:35–41
Brody SA, Geyer MA (2004) Interactions of the mGluR5 gene with breeding and maternal factors
on startle and prepulse inhibition in mice. Neurotoxic Res 6:79–90
Brooks S, Higgs G, Jones L, Dunnett SB (2010) Longitudinal analysis of the behavioural
phenotype in Hdh ((CAG)150) Huntington’s disease knock-in mice. Brain Res Bull. May 8.
[Epub ahead of print]
Brooks SP, Janghra N, Workman VL, Bayram-Weston Z, Jones L, Dunnett SB (2011)
Longitudinal analysis of the behavioural phenotype in R6/1 (C57BL/6J) Huntington’s disease
transgenic mice. Brain Res Bull. Jan 25. [Epub ahead of print]
Brzózka MM, Radyushkin K, Wichert SP, Ehrenreich H, Rossner MJ (2010) Cognitive and
sensorimotor gating impairments in transgenic mice overexpressing the schizophrenia
susceptibility gene Tcf4 in the brain. Biol Psychiatry 68:33–40
Burdick KE, Hodgkinson CA, Szeszko PR, Lencz T, Ekholm JM, Kane JM, Goldman D,
Malhotra AK (2005) DISC1 and neurocognitive function in schizophrenia. Neuroreport
16:1399–1402
Cadenhead KS, Geyer MA, Braff DL (1993) Impaired startle prepulse inhibition and habituation
in patients with schizotypal personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 150:1862–1867
Cadenhead KS, Swerdlow NR, Shafer KM, Diaz M, Braff DL (2000) Modulation of the startle
response and startle laterality in relatives of schizophrenic patients and in subjects with
schizotypal personality disorder: evidence of inhibitory deficits. Am J Psychiatry 157:
1660–1668
Cahill ME, Xie Z, Day M, Photowala H, Barbolina MV, Miller CA, Weiss C, Radulovic J,
Sweatt JD, Disterhoft JF, Surmeier DJ, Penzes P (2009) Kalirin regulates cortical spine
morphogenesis and disease-related behavioral phenotypes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:
13058–13063
Caldwell HK, Stephens SL, Young WS 3rd (2009) Oxytocin as a natural antipsychotic: a study
using oxytocin knockout mice. Mol Psychiatry 14:190–196
Callicott JH, Straub RE, Pezawas L, Egan MF, Mattay VS, Hariri AR, Verchinski BA,
Meyer-Lindenberg A, Balkissoon R, Kolachana B, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR (2005)
Variation in DISC1 affects hippocampal structure and function and increases risk for
schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:8627–8632
Cannon TD, Hennah W, van Erp TG, Thompson PM, Lonnqvist J, Huttunen M, Gasperoni T,
Tuulio-Henriksson A, Pirkola T, Toga AW, Kaprio J, Mazziotta J, Peltonen L (2005)
Association of DISC1/TRAX haplotypes with schizophrenia, reduced prefrontal gray matter,
and impaired short- and long-term memory. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62:1205–1213
Cesca F, Baldelli P, Valtorta F, Benfenati F (2010) The synapsins: key actors of synapse function
and plasticity. Prog Neurobiol 91:313–348
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating 305
Chadman KK, Gong S, Scattoni ML, Boltuck SE, Gandhy SU, Heintz N, Crawley JN (2008)
Minimal aberrant behavioral phenotypes of neuroligin-3 R451C knockin mice. Autism Res
1:147–158
Chavez C, Gogos A, Jones ME, van den Buuse M (2009) Psychotropic drug-induced locomotor
hyperactivity and prepulse inhibition regulation in male and female aromatase knockout
(ArKO) mice: role of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors and dopamine transporters.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 206:267–279
Chen HH, Stoker A, Markou A (2010) The glutamatergic compounds sarcosine and N-
acetylcysteine ameliorate prepulse inhibition deficits in metabotropic glutamate 5 receptor
knockout mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 209:343–350
Chen Q, He G, Qin W, Chen QY, Zhao XZ, Duan SW, Liu XM, Feng GY, Xu YF, Clair D, Li M,
Wang JH, Xing YL, Shi JG, He L (2004) Family-based association study of synapsin II and
schizophrenia. Am J Hum Genet 75:873–877
Chen Y-JJ, Johnson MA, Lieberman MD, Goodchild RE, Schobel S, Lewandowski N,
Rosoklija G, Liu R-C, Gingrich JA, Small S, Moore H, Dwork AJ, Talmage DA, Role LW
(2008) Type III Neuregulin-1 is required for normal sensorimotor gating, memory-related
behaviors, and corticostriatal circuit components. J Neurosci 28:6872–6883
Chen YW, Lai WS (2011) Behavioral phenotyping of v-akt murine thymoma viral oncogene
homolog 1-deficient mice reveals a sex-specific prepulse inhibition deficit in females that can
be partially alleviated by glycogen synthase kinase-3 inhibitors but not by antipsychotics.
Neuroscience 174:178–189
Chong VZ, Young LT, Mishra RK (2002) cDNA array reveals differential gene expression
following chronic neuroleptic administration: implications of synapsin II in haloperidol
treatment. J Neurochem 82:1533–1539
Cleva RM, Olive MF (2011) Positive allosteric modulators of type 5 metabotropic glutamate
receptors (mGluR5) and their therapeutic potential for the treatment of CNS disorders.
Molecules 16:2097–2106
Cole TB, Giordano G, Co AL, Mohar I, Kavanagh TJ, Costa LG (2011) Behavioral
characterization of GCLM-knockout mice, a model for enhanced susceptibility to oxidative
stress. J Toxicol 2011:157687
Cook MN, Dunning JP, Wiley RG, Chesler EJ, Johnson DK, Miller DR, Goldowitz D (2007)
Neurobehavioral mutants identified in an ENU-mutagenesis project. Mamm Genome
18:559–572
Coyle JT (2004) The GABA-glutamate connection in schizophrenia: which is the proximate
cause? Biochem Pharmacol 68:1507–1514
Crawley JN (2007) What’s wrong with my mouse? Behavioral phenotyping of transgenic and
knockout mice, Wiley
Csomor PA, Stadler RR, Feldon J, Yee BK, Geyer MA, Vollenweider FX (2008) Haloperidol
differentially modulates prepulse inhibition and p50 suppression in healthy humans stratified
for low and high gating levels. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:497–512
Darvas M, Morsch M, Racz I, Ahmadi S, Swandulla D, Zimmer A (2009) Modulation of the
Ca2+ conductance of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors by Lypd6. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol
19:670–681
de Vrij FM, Levenga J, van der Linde HC, Koekkoek SK, De Zeeuw CI, Nelson DL, Oostra BA,
Willemsen R (2008) Rescue of behavioral phenotype and neuronal protrusion morphology in
Fmr1 KO mice. Neurobiol Dis 31:127–132
Deakin IH, Law AJ, Oliver PL, Schwab MH, Nave KA, Harrison PJ, Bannerman DM (2009)
Behavioural characterization of neuregulin 1 type I overexpressing transgenic mice.
Neuroreport 20:1523–1528
Dejaegere T, Serneels L, Schafer MK, Van Biervliet J, Horre K, Depboylu C, Alvarez-Fischer D,
Herreman A, Willem M, Haass C, Hoglinger GU, D’Hooge R, De Strooper B (2008)
Deficiency of Aph1B/C-gamma-secretase disturbs Nrg1 cleavage and sensorimotor gating
that can be reversed with antipsychotic treatment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:9775–9780
306 S. B. Powell et al.
DeLorey TM, Sahbaie P, Hashemi E, Li WW, Salehi A, Clark DJ (2011) Somatosensory and
sensorimotor consequences associated with the heterozygous disruption of the autism
candidate gene, Gabrb3. Behav Brain Res 216(1):36–45
Doherty JM, Masten VL, Powell SB, Ralph RJ, Klamer D, Low MJ, Geyer MA (2008)
Contributions of dopamine D1, D2, and D3 receptor subtypes to the disruptive effects of
cocaine on prepulse inhibition in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:2648–2656
Duffy L, Cappas E, Scimone A, Schofield PR, Karl T (2008) Behavioral profile of a heterozygous
mutant mouse model for EGF-like domain neuregulin 1. Behav Neurosci 122:748–759
Dulawa SC, Hen R, Scearce-Levie K, Geyer MA (1997) Serotonin1B receptor modulation of
startle reactivity, habituation, and prepulse inhibition in wild-type and serotonin1B knockout
mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 132:125–134
Duncan GE, Inada K, Koller BH, Moy SS (2010) Increased sensitivity to kainic acid in a genetic
model of reduced NMDA receptor function. Brain Res 1307:166–176
Duncan GE, Moy SS, Perez A, Eddy DM, Zinzow WM, Lieberman JA, Snouwaert JN, Koller BH
(2004) Deficits in sensorimotor gating and tests of social behavior in a genetic model of
reduced NMDA receptor function. Behav Brain Res 153:507–519
Dyck BA, Skoblenick KJ, Castellano JM, Ki K, Thomas N, Mishra RK (2007) Synapsin II
knockout mice show sensorimotor gating and behavioural abnormalities similar to those in the
phencyclidine-induced preclinical animal model of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 97:292–293
Dyck BA, Skoblenick KJ, Castellano JM, Ki K, Thomas N, Mishra RK (2009) Behavioral
abnormalities in synapsin II knockout mice implicate a causal factor in schizophrenia.
Synapse 63:662–672
Eells JB, Lipska BK, Yeung SK, Misler JA, Nikodem VM (2002) Nurr1-null heterozygous mice
have reduced mesolimbic and mesocortical dopamine levels and increased stress-induced
locomotor activity. Behav Brain Res 136:267–275
Eells JB, Misler JA, Nikodem VM (2006) Early postnatal isolation reduces dopamine levels,
elevates dopamine turnover and specifically disrupts prepulse inhibition in Nurr1-null
heterozygous mice. Neuroscience 140:1117–1126
Egashira N, Mishima K, Iwasaki K, Oishi R, Fujiwara M (2009) New topics in vasopressin
receptors and approach to novel drugs: role of the vasopressin receptor in psychological and
cognitive functions. J Pharmacol Sci 109:44–49
Errico F, Rossi S, Napolitano F, Catuogno V, Topo E, Fisone G, D’Aniello A, Centonze D,
Usiello A (2008) D-aspartate prevents corticostriatal long-term depression and attenuates
schizophrenia-like symptoms induced by amphetamine and MK-801. J Neurosci 28:
10404–10414
Etherton MR, Blaiss CA, Powell CM, Sudhof TC (2009) Mouse neurexin-1alpha deletion causes
correlated electrophysiological and behavioral changes consistent with cognitive impairments.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:17998–18003
Feifel D, Macdonald K, Nguyen A, Cobb P, Warlan H, Galangue B, Minassian A, Becker O,
Cooper J, Perry W, Lefebvre M, Gonzales J, Hadley A (2010a) Adjunctive intranasal oxytocin
reduces symptoms in schizophrenia patients. Biol Psychiatry 68:678–680
Feifel D, Pang Z, Shilling PD, Melendez G, Schreiber R, Button D (2010b) Effects of
neurotensin-2 receptor deletion on sensorimotor gating and locomotor activity. Behav Brain
Res 212:174–178
Feifel D, Pang Z, Shilling PD, Melendez G, Schreiber R, Button D (2010c) Sensorimotor gating
in neurotensin-1 receptor null mice. Neuropharmacology 58:173–178
Fendt M, Buchi M, Burki H, Imobersteg S, Ricoux B, Suply T, Sailer AW (2011) Neuropeptide S
receptor deficiency modulates spontaneous locomotor activity and the acoustic startle
response. Behav Brain Res 217:1–9
Fendt M, Burki H, Imobersteg S, van der Putten H, McAllister K, Leslie JC, Shaw D, Holscher C
(2010) The effect of mGlu8 deficiency in animal models of psychiatric diseases. Genes Brain
Behav 9:33–44
Francis DD, Szegda K, Campbell G, Martin WD, Insel TR (2003) Epigenetic sources of
behavioral differences in mice. Nat Neurosci 6:445–446
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating 307
Frankland PW, Wang Y, Rosner B, Shimizu T, Balleine BW, Dykens EM, Ornitz EM, Silva AJ
(2004) Sensorimotor gating abnormalities in young males with fragile X syndrome and Fmr1-
knockout mice. Mol Psychiatry 9:417–425
Frankle WG, Lerma J, Laruelle M (2003) The synaptic hypothesis of schizophrenia. Neuron
39:205–216
Fridlund AJ, Cacioppo JT (1986) Guidelines for human electromyographic research.
Psychophysiology 23(5):567–589
Friston KJ, Frith CD (1995) Schizophrenia: a disconnection syndrome? Clin Neurosci 3:89–97
Frost WN, Tian LM, Hoppe TA, Mongeluzi DL, Wang J (2003) A cellular mechanism for
prepulse inhibition. Neuron 40:991–1001
Fukuda T, Hashimoto H, Okayasu N, Kameyama A, Onogi H, Nakagawasai O, Nakazawa T,
Kurosawa T, Hao Y, Isaji T, Tadano T, Narimatsu H, Taniguchi N, Gu J (2011) {alpha}1,
6-Fucosyltransferase-deficient mice exhibit multiple behavioral abnormalities associated with
a schizophrenia-like phenotype: importance of the balance between the dopamine and
serotonin systems. J Biol Chem 286:18434–18443
Galici R, Boggs JD, Miller KL, Bonaventure P, Atack JR (2008) Effects of SB-269970, a 5-HT7
receptor antagonist, in mouse models predictive of antipsychotic-like activity. Behav
Pharmacol 19:153–159
Gallinat J, Bajbouj M, Sander T, Schlattmann P, Xu K, Ferro EF, Goldman D, Winterer G (2003)
Association of the G1947A COMT (Val(108/158)Met) gene polymorphism with prefrontal
P300 during information processing. Biol Psychiatry 54:40–48
Geyer MA, Braff DL (1987) Startle habituation and sensorimotor gating in schizophrenia and
related animal models. Schizophr Bull 13:643–668
Geyer MA, Dulawa SC (2003) Assessment of murine startle reactivity, prepulse inhibition, and
habituation. Curr Protoc Neurosci Chapter 8: Unit 8, p 17
Geyer MA, Markou A (2002) The role of preclinical models in the development of psychotropic
drugs. In: Davis KL, Charney D, Coyle JT, Nemeroff CB (eds) Neuropsychopharmacology:
the fifth generation of progress. Lippincott, Williams Wilkins, pp 445–455
Geyer MA, McIlwain KL, Paylor R (2002) Mouse genetic models for prepulse inhibition: an
early review. Mol Psychiatry 7:1039–1053
Giakoumaki SG, Bitsios P, Frangou S (2006) The level of prepulse inhibition in healthy
individuals may index cortical modulation of early information processing. Brain Res 1078:
168–170
Giakoumaki SG, Roussos P, Bitsios P (2008) Improvement of prepulse inhibition and executive
function by the COMT inhibitor tolcapone depends on COMT Val158Met polymorphism.
Neuropsychopharmacology 33:3058–3068
Gogos JA, Morgan M, Luine V, Santha M, Ogawa S, Pfaff D, Karayiorgou M (1998) Catechol-O-
methyltransferase-deficient mice exhibit sexually dimorphic changes in catecholamine levels
and behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:9991–9996
Golimbet V, Gritsenko I, Alfimova M, Lebedeva I, Lezheiko T, Abramova L, Kaleda V,
Ebstein R (2006) Association study of COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism with auditory
P300 and performance on neurocognitive tests in patients with schizophrenia and their
relatives. World J Biol Psychiatry 7:238–245
Gottesman II, Gould TD (2003) The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and
strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry 160:636–645
Gottesman II (1991) Schizophrenia Genesis. WH Freeman
Gould TD, Gottesman II (2006) Psychiatric endophenotypes and the development of valid animal
models. Genes Brain Behav 5:113–119
Graham FK (1975) Presidential address, 1974. The more or less startling effects of weak
prestimulation. Psychophysiology 12:238–248
Grant A, Hoops D, Labelle-Dumais C, Prevost M, Rajabi H, Kolb B, Stewart J, Arvanitogiannis
A, Flores C (2007) Netrin-1 receptor-deficient mice show enhanced mesocortical dopamine
transmission and blunted behavioural responses to amphetamine. Eur J Neurosci 26:
3215–3228
308 S. B. Powell et al.
Gray L, van den Buuse M, Scarr E, Dean B, Hannan AJ (2009) Clozapine reverses schizophrenia-
related behaviours in the metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 knockout mouse: association with
N-methyl-D-aspartic acid receptor up-regulation. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 12:45–60
Green MF, Butler PD, Chen Y, Geyer MA, Silverstein S, Wynn JK, Yoon JH, Zemon V (2009)
Perception measurement in clinical trials of schizophrenia: promising paradigms from
CNTRICS. Schizophr Bull 35:163–181
Greenwood TA, Braff DL, Light GA, Cadenhead KS, Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, Freedman R,
Green MF, Gur RE, Gur RC, Mintz J, Nuechterlein KH, Olincy A, Radant AD, Seidman LJ,
Siever LJ, Silverman JM, Stone WS, Swerdlow NR, Tsuang DW, Tsuang MT, Turetsky BI,
Schork NJ (2007) Initial heritability analyses of endophenotypic measures for schizophrenia:
the consortium on the genetics of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 64:1242–1250
Greenwood TA, Lazzeroni LC, Murray SS, Cadenhead KS, Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, Green MF,
Gur RE, Gur RC, Hardiman G, Kelsoe JR, Leonard S, Light GA, Nuechterlein KH, Olincy A,
Radant AD, Schork NJ, Seidman LJ, Siever LJ, Silverman JM, Stone WS, Swerdlow NR,
Tsuang DW, Tsuang MT, Turetsky BI, Freedman R, Braff DL (2011) Analysis of 94
candidate genes and 12 endophenotypes for schizophrenia from the consortium on the
genetics of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry
Groenink L, Bijlsma EY, van Bogaert MJ, Oosting RS, Olivier B (2011) Serotonin1A receptor
deletion does not interact with maternal separation-induced increases in startle reactivity and
prepulse inhibition deficits. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 214:353–365
Groenink L, Dirks A, Verdouw PM, de Graaff M, Peeters BW, Millan MJ, Olivier B (2008) CRF1
not glucocorticoid receptors mediate prepulse inhibition deficits in mice overexpressing CRF.
Biol Psychiatry 63:360–368
Gruart A, Lopez-Ramos JC, Munoz MD, Delgado-Garcia JM (2008) Aged wild-type and APP,
PS1, and APP+PS1 mice present similar deficits in associative learning and synaptic plasticity
independent of amyloid load. Neurobiol Dis 30:439–450
Grunze HC, Rainnie DG, Hasselmo ME, Barkai E, Hearn EF, McCarley RW, Greene RW (1996)
NMDA-dependent modulation of CA1 local circuit inhibition. J Neurosci 16:2034–2043
Guo X, Hamilton PJ, Reish NJ, Sweatt JD, Miller CA, Rumbaugh G (2009) Reduced expression
of the NMDA receptor-interacting protein SynGAP causes behavioral abnormalities that
model symptoms of Schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 34:1659–1672
Han L, Picker JD, Schaevitz LR, Tsai G, Feng J, Jiang Z, Chu HC, Basu AC, Berger-Sweeney J,
Coyle JT (2009) Phenotypic characterization of mice heterozygous for a null mutation of
glutamate carboxypeptidase II. Synapse 63:625–635
Harrison PJ, Weinberger DR (2005) Schizophrenia genes, gene expression, and neuropathology:
on the matter of their convergence. Mol Psychiatry 10: 40–68, image 5
Hasenkamp W, Epstein MP, Green A, Wilcox L, Boshoven W, Lewison B, Duncan E (2010)
Heritability of acoustic startle magnitude, prepulse inhibition, and startle latency in
schizophrenia and control families. Psychiatry Res 178:236–243
Hashimoto R, Hashimoto H, Shintani N, Chiba S, Hattori S, Okada T, Nakajima M, Tanaka K,
Kawagishi N, Nemoto K, Mori T, Ohnishi T, Noguchi H, Hori H, Suzuki T, Iwata N, Ozaki N,
Nakabayashi T, Saitoh O, Kosuga A, Tatsumi M, Kamijima K, Weinberger DR, Kunugi H,
Baba A (2007) Pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating polypeptide is associated with
schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 12:1026–1032
Heintz N (2001) BAC to the future: the use of BAC transgenic mice for neuroscience research.
Nat Rev Neurosci 2:861–870
Hennah W, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Paunio T, Ekelund J, Varilo T, Partonen T, Cannon TD,
Lonnqvist J, Peltonen L (2005) A haplotype within the DISC1 gene is associated with visual
memory functions in families with a high density of schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 10:
1097–1103
Hessl D, Berry-Kravis E, Cordeiro L, Yuhas J, Ornitz EM, Campbell A, Chruscinski E, Hervey C,
Long JM, Hagerman RJ (2009) Prepulse inhibition in fragile X syndrome: feasibility,
reliability, and implications for treatment. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet
150B:545–553
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating 309
Lipina TV, Niwa M, Jaaro-Peled H, Fletcher PJ, Seeman P, Sawa A, Roder JC (2010) Enhanced
dopamine function in DISC1-L100P mutant mice: implications for schizophrenia. Genes
Brain Behav 9:777–789
Logue SF, Grauer SM, Paulsen J, Graf R, Taylor N, Sung MA, Zhang L, Hughes Z, Pulito VL,
Liu F, Rosenzweig-Lipson S, Brandon NJ, Marquis KL, Bates B, Pausch M (2009) The
orphan GPCR, GPR88, modulates function of the striatal dopamine system: a possible
therapeutic target for psychiatric disorders? Mol Cell Neurosci 42:438–447
López-Ramos JC, Tomioka Y, Morimatsu M, Yamamoto S, Ozaki K, Ono E, Delgado-García JM
(2010) Motor-coordination-dependent learning, more than others, is impaired in transgenic
mice expressing pseudorabies virus immediate-early protein IE180. PLoS One 5(8):e12123
Low NC, Hardy J (2007) What is a schizophrenic mouse? Neuron 54:348–349
Lu BY, Martin KE, Edgar JC, Smith AK, Lewis SF, Escamilla MA, Miller GA, Canive JM
(2007) Effect of catechol O-methyltransferase val(158)met polymorphism on the p50 gating
endophenotype in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 62:822–825
Ludewig K, Geyer MA, Vollenweider FX (2003) Deficits in prepulse inhibition and habituation
in never-medicated, first-episode schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 54:121–128
Ludewig S, Ludewig K, Geyer MA, Hell D, Vollenweider FX (2002) Prepulse inhibition deficits
in patients with panic disorder. Depress Anxiety 15:55–60
Mackeprang T, Kristiansen KT, Glenthoj BY (2002) Effects of antipsychotics on prepulse
inhibition of the startle response in drug-naive schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry 52:
863–873
Mackie S, Millar JK, Porteous DJ (2007) Role of DISC1 in neural development and
schizophrenia. Curr Opin Neurobiol 17:95–102
Malhotra AK, Adler CM, Kennison SD, Elman I, Pickar D, Breier A (1997a) Clozapine blunts
N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist-induced psychosis: a study with ketamine. Biol Psychiatry
42:664–668
Malhotra AK, Pinals DA, Adler CM, Elman I, Clifton A, Pickar D, Breier A (1997b) Ketamine-
induced exacerbation of psychotic symptoms and cognitive impairment in neuroleptic-free
schizophrenics. Neuropsychopharmacol 17:141–150
Mansuy IM, Bujard H (2000) Tetracycline-regulated gene expression in the brain. Curr Opin
Neurobiol 10:593–596
Martin S, Markus MA, Morris BJ, Davisson RL, Lawrence AJ, van den Buuse M (2008) Does
angiotensin interact with dopaminergic mechanisms in the brain to modulate prepulse
inhibition in mice? Neuropharmacol 54:399–404
Martinez-Gras I, Rubio G, del Manzano BA, Rodriguez-Jimenez R, Garcia-Sanchez F, Bagney A,
Leza JC, Borrell J (2009) The relationship between prepulse inhibition and general
psychopathology in patients with schizophrenia treated with long-acting risperidone.
Schizophr Res 115(2–3):215–221
Matsumoto M, Straub RE, Marenco S, Nicodemus KK, Matsumoto S, Fujikawa A, Miyoshi S,
Shobo M, Takahashi S, Yarimizu J, Yuri M, Hiramoto M, Morita S, Yokota H, Sasayama T,
Terai K, Yoshino M, Miyake A, Callicott JH, Egan MF, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Kempf L,
Honea R, Vakkalanka RK, Takasaki J, Kamohara M, Soga T, Hiyama H, Ishii H, Matsuo A,
Nishimura S, Matsuoka N, Kobori M, Matsushime H, Katoh M, Furuichi K, Weinberger DR
(2008) The evolutionarily conserved G protein-coupled receptor SREB2/GPR85 influences
brain size, behavior, and vulnerability to schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:
6133–6138
Matsuo N, Tanda K, Nakanishi K, Yamasaki N, Toyama K, Takao K, Takeshima H, Miyakawa T
(2009) Comprehensive behavioral phenotyping of ryanodine receptor type 3 (RyR3) knockout
mice: decreased social contact duration in two social interaction tests. Front Behav Neurosci
3:3
Mayford M, Bach ME, Huang YY, Wang L, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER (1996) Control of memory
formation through regulated expression of a CaMKII transgene. Science 274:1678–1683
McGlashan TH, Hoffman RE (2000) Schizophrenia as a disorder of developmentally reduced
synaptic connectivity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57:637–648
312 S. B. Powell et al.
McOmish CE, Burrows E, Howard M, Scarr E, Kim D, Shin HS, Dean B, van den Buuse M,
Hannan AJ (2008) Phospholipase C-beta1 knockout mice exhibit endophenotypes modeling
schizophrenia which are rescued by environmental enrichment and clozapine administration.
Mol Psychiatry 13:661–672
Meincke U, Morth D, Voss T, Thelen B, Geyer MA, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E (2004) Prepulse
inhibition of the acoustically evoked startle reflex in patients with an acute schizophrenic
psychosis–a longitudinal study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 254:415–421
Mejias R, Adamczyk A, Anggono V, Niranjan T, Thomas GM, Sharma K, Skinner C,
Schwartz CE, Stevenson RE, Fallin MD, Kaufmann W, Pletnikov M, Valle D, Huganir RL,
Wang T (2011) Gain-of-function glutamate receptor interacting protein 1 variants alter GluA2
recycling and surface distribution in patients with autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
108:4920–4925
Menalled L, El-Khodor BF, Patry M, Suarez-Farinas M, Orenstein SJ, Zahasky B, Leahy C,
Wheeler V, Yang XW, MacDonald M, Morton AJ, Bates G, Leeds J, Park L, Howland D,
Signer E, Tobin A, Brunner D (2009) Systematic behavioral evaluation of Huntington’s
disease transgenic and knock-in mouse models. Neurobiol Dis 35:319–336
Meyer U, Feldon J, Schedlowski M, Yee BK (2005) Towards an immuno-precipitated
neurodevelopmental animal model of schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:913–947
Mirnics K, Middleton FA, Lewis DA, Levitt P (2001) Analysis of complex brain disorders with
gene expression microarrays: schizophrenia as a disease of the synapse. Trends Neurosci
24:479–486
Moghaddam B, Adams B, Verma A, Daly D (1997) Activation of glutamatergic neurotransmis-
sion by ketamine: a novel step in the pathway from NMDA receptor blockade to
dopaminergic and cognitive disruptions associated with the prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci
17:2921–2927
Moore H (2010) The role of rodent models in the discovery of new treatments for schizophrenia:
updating our strategy. Schizophr Bull 36:1066–1072
Moy SS, Nonneman RJ, Young NB, Demyanenko GP, Maness PF (2009) Impaired sociability
and cognitive function in Nrcam-null mice. Behav Brain Res 205:123–131
Nakagawasai O, Onogi H, Mitazaki S, Sato A, Watanabe K, Saito H, Murai S, Nakaya K,
Murakami M, Takahashi E, Tan-No K, Tadano T (2010) Behavioral and neurochemical
characterization of mice deficient in the N-type Ca2+ channel alpha1B subunit. Behav Brain
Res 208:224–230
Neuner I, Stocker T, Kellermann T, Ermer V, Wegener HP, Eickhoff SB, Schneider F, Shah NJ
(2010) Electrophysiology meets fMRI: neural correlates of the startle reflex assessed by
simultaneous EMG-fMRI data acquisition. Hum Brain Mapp 31:1675–1685
Nguyen PT, Nakamura T, Hori E, Urakawa S, Uwano T, Zhao J, Li R, Bac ND, Hamashima T,
Ishii Y, Matsushima T, Ono T, Sasahara M, Nishijo H (2011) Cognitive and socio-emotional
deficits in platelet-derived growth factor receptor-beta gene knockout mice. PLoS One
6:e18004
Niwa M, Kamiya A, Murai R, Kubo K, Gruber AJ, Tomita K, Lu L, Tomisato S, Jaaro-Peled H,
Seshadri S, Hiyama H, Huang B, Kohda K, Noda Y, O’Donnell P, Nakajima K, Sawa A,
Nabeshima T (2010) Knockdown of DISC1 by in utero gene transfer disturbs postnatal
dopaminergic maturation in the frontal cortex and leads to adult behavioral deficits. Neuron
65:480–489
Nusbaum MP, Contreras D (2004) Sensorimotor gating: startle submits to presynaptic inhibition.
Curr Biol 14:R247–R249
O’Tuathaigh CM, Babovic D, O’Meara G, Clifford JJ, Croke DT, Waddington JL (2007)
Susceptibility genes for schizophrenia: characterisation of mutant mouse models at the level
of phenotypic behaviour. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 31:60–78
O’Tuathaigh CM, Desbonnet L, Moran PM, Kirby BP, Waddington JL (2011) Molecular genetic
models related to schizophrenia and psychotic illness: heuristics and challenges. Curr Top
Behav Neurosci 7:87–119
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating 313
Ralph RJ, Paulus MP, Fumagalli F, Caron MG, Geyer MA (2001) Prepulse inhibition deficits and
perseverative motor patterns in dopamine transporter knock-out mice: differential effects of
D1 and D2 receptor antagonists. J Neurosci 21:305–313
Ralph RJ, Varty GB, Kelly MA, Wang YM, Caron MG, Rubinstein M, Grandy DK, Low MJ,
Geyer MA (1999) The dopamine D2, but not D3 or D4, receptor subtype is essential for the
disruption of prepulse inhibition produced by amphetamine in mice. J Neurosci 19:4627–4633
Ransome MI, Turnley AM (2008) Growth hormone signaling and hippocampal neurogenesis:
insights from genetic models. Hippocampus 18:1034–1050
Relkovic D, Doe CM, Humby T, Johnstone KA, Resnick JL, Holland AJ, Hagan JJ,
Wilkinson LS, Isles AR (2010) Behavioural and cognitive abnormalities in an imprinting
centre deletion mouse model for Prader–Willi syndrome. Eur J Neurosci 31:156–164
Rose PK, Scott SH (2003) Sensory-motor control: a long-awaited behavioral correlate of
presynaptic inhibition. Nat Neurosci 6:1243–1245
Roussos P, Giakoumaki SG, Adamaki E, Bitsios P (2011) The influence of schizophrenia-related
neuregulin-1 polymorphisms on sensorimotor gating in healthy males. Biol Psychiatry 69:
479–486
Roussos P, Giakoumaki SG, Rogdaki M, Pavlakis S, Frangou S, Bitsios P (2008) Prepulse
inhibition of the startle reflex depends on the catechol O-methyltransferase Val158Met gene
polymorphism. Psychol Med :1–8
Sagata NA, Iwaki A, Aramaki T, Takao K, Kura S, Tsuzuki T, Kawakami R, Ito I, Kitamura T,
Sugiyama H, Miyakawa T, Fukumaki Y (2010) Comprehensive behavioral study of GluR4
knockout mice, implication in cognitive function. Genes Brain Behav
Sakatani S, Yamada K, Homma C, Munesue S, Yamamoto Y, Yamamoto H, Hirase H (2009)
Deletion of RAGE causes hyperactivity and increased sensitivity to auditory stimuli in mice.
PLoS One 4:e8309
Samocha KE, Lim JE, Cheng R, Sokoloff G, Palmer AA (2010) Fine mapping of QTL for
prepulse inhibition in LG/J and SM/J mice using F2 and advanced intercross lines. Genes
Brain Behav 9:759–767
Savonenko A, Munoz P, Melnikova T, Wang Q, Liang X, Breyer RM, Montine TJ, Kirkwood A,
Andreasson K (2009) Impaired cognition, sensorimotor gating, and hippocampal long-term
depression in mice lacking the prostaglandin E2 EP2 receptor. Exp Neurol 217:63–73
Savonenko AV, Melnikova T, Laird FM, Stewart KA, Price DL, Wong PC (2008) Alteration of
BACE1-dependent NRG1/ErbB4 signaling and schizophrenia-like phenotypes in BACE1-null
mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:5585–5590
Scearce-Levie K, Roberson ED, Gerstein H, Cholfin JA, Mandiyan VS, Shah NM, Rubenstein JL,
Mucke L (2008) Abnormal social behaviors in mice lacking Fgf17. Genes Brain Behav
7:344–354
Schaefer TL, Vorhees CV, Williams MT (2009) Mouse plasmacytoma-expressed transcript 1
knock out induced 5-HT disruption results in a lack of cognitive deficits and an anxiety
phenotype complicated by hypoactivity and defensiveness. Neuroscience 164:1431–1443
Schmid S, Azzopardi E, De Jaeger X, Prado MA, Prado VF (2011) VAChT knock-down mice
show normal prepulse inhibition but disrupted long-term habituation. Genes Brain Behav
Schmidt S, Richter M, Montag D, Sartorius T, Gawlik V, Hennige AM, Scherneck S,
Himmelbauer H, Lutz SZ, Augustin R, Kluge R, Ruth P, Joost HG, Schürmann A (2008)
Neuronal functions, feeding behavior, and energy balance in Slc2a3+/- mice. Am J Physiol
Endocrinol Metab 295(5):E1084–E1094
Schneider M, Spanagel R, Zhang SJ, Bading H, Klugmann M (2007) Adeno-associated virus
(AAV)-mediated suppression of Ca2+/calmodulin kinase IV activity in the nucleus
accumbens modulates emotional behaviour in mice. BMC Neurosci 8:105
Schwabe K, Freudenberg F, Koch M (2007) Selective breeding of reduced sensorimotor gating in
wistar rats. Behav Genet 37:706–712
Semenova S, Geyer MA, Sutcliffe JG, Markou A, Hedlund PB (2008) Inactivation of the 5-HT(7)
receptor partially blocks phencyclidine-induced disruption of prepulse inhibition. Biol
Psychiatry 63:98–105
Genetic Models of Sensorimotor Gating 315
Shanahan NA, Holick Pierz KA, Masten VL, Waeber C, Ansorge M, Gingrich JA, Geyer MA,
Hen R, Dulawa SC (2009) Chronic reductions in serotonin transporter function prevent
5-HT1B-induced behavioral effects in mice. Biol Psychiatry 65:401–408
Shi L, Fatemi SH, Sidwell RW, Patterson PH (2003) Maternal influenza infection causes marked
behavioral and pharmacological changes in the offspring. J Neurosci 23:297–302
Sipes TA, Geyer MA (1994) Multiple serotonin receptor subtypes modulate prepulse inhibition of
the startle response in rats. Neuropharmacology 33:441–448
Siuciak JA, McCarthy SA, Chapin DS, Martin AN (2008) Behavioral and neurochemical
characterization of mice deficient in the phosphodiesterase-4B (PDE4B) enzyme. Psycho-
pharmacology (Berl) 197:115–126
Siuta MA, Robertson SD, Kocalis H, Saunders C, Gresch PJ, Khatri V, Shiota C, Kennedy JP,
Lindsley CW, Daws LC, Polley DB, Veenstra-Vanderweele J, Stanwood GD, Magnuson MA,
Niswender KD, Galli A (2010) Dysregulation of the norepinephrine transporter sustains
cortical hypodopaminergia and schizophrenia-like behaviors in neuronal rictor null mice.
PLoS Biol 8:e1000393
Smith CM, Lawrence AJ, Sutton SW, Gundlach AL (2009) Behavioral phenotyping of mixed
background (129S5:B6) relaxin-3 knockout mice. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1160:236–241
Smith SE, Li J, Garbett K, Mirnics K, Patterson PH (2007) Maternal immune activation alters
fetal brain development through interleukin-6. J Neurosci 27:10695–10702
Soleimani L, Roder JC, Dennis JW, Lipina T (2008) Beta N-acetylglucosaminyltransferase V
(Mgat5) deficiency reduces the depression-like phenotype in mice. Genes Brain Behav
7:334–343
Stark KL, Burt RA, Gogos JA, Karayiorgou M (2009) Analysis of prepulse inhibition in mouse
lines overexpressing 22q11.2 orthologues. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol: 1–7
Sun M, Gewirtz JC, Bofenkamp L, Wickham RJ, Ge H, O’Connor MB (2010) Canonical TGF-
beta signaling is required for the balance of excitatory/inhibitory transmission within the
hippocampus and prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle. J Neurosci 30:6025–6035
Suzuki G, Harper KM, Hiramoto T, Sawamura T, Lee M, Kang G, Tanigaki K, Buell M,
Geyer MA, Trimble WS, Agatsuma S, Hiroi N (2009a) Sept5 deficiency exerts pleiotropic
influence on affective behaviors and cognitive functions in mice. Hum Mol Genet 18:
1652–1660
Suzuki G, Harper KM, Hiramoto T, Funke B, Lee M, Kang G, Buell M, Geyer MA,
Kucherlapati R, Morrow B, Mannisto PT, Agatsuma S, Hiroi N (2009b) Over-expression
of a human chromosome 22q11.2 segment including TXNRD2, COMT and ARVCF
developmentally affects incentive learning and working memory in mice. Hum Mol Genet
18:3914–3925
Swerdlow NR, Benbow CH, Zisook S, Geyer MA, Braff DL (1993) A preliminary assessment of
sensorimotor gating in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 33:
298–301
Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Braff DL (2001a) Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of
startle in the rat: current knowledge and future challenges. Psychopharmacology (Berl)
156:194–215
Swerdlow NR, Karban B, Ploum Y, Sharp R, Geyer MA, Eastvold A (2001b) Tactile prepuff
inhibition of startle in children with Tourette’s syndrome: in search of an ‘‘fMRI-friendly’’
startle paradigm. Biol Psychiatry 50:578–585
Swerdlow NR, Light GA, Cadenhead KS, Sprock J, Hsieh MH, Braff DL (2006) Startle gating
deficits in a large cohort of patients with schizophrenia: relationship to medications,
symptoms, neurocognition, and level of function. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63:1325–1335
Swerdlow NR, Paulsen J, Braff DL, Butters N, Geyer MA, Swenson MR (1995) Impaired
prepulse inhibition of acoustic and tactile startle response in patients with Huntington’s
disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 58:192–200
Swerdlow NR, Weber M, Qu Y, Light GA, Braff DL (2008) Realistic expectations of prepulse
inhibition in translational models for schizophrenia research. Psychopharmacology (Berl)
199:331–388
316 S. B. Powell et al.
van den Buuse M, Becker T, Kwek P, Martin S, Ruimschotel E, Risbrough V (2011a) Disruption
of prepulse inhibition by 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA): comparison
between male and female wild-type and 5-HT1A receptor knockout mice. Int J Neuropsy-
chopharmacol: 1–6
van den Buuse M, Ruimschotel E, Martin S, Risbrough VB, Halberstadt AL (2011b)
Enhanced effects of amphetamine but reduced effects of the hallucinogen, 5-MeO-DMT, on
locomotor activity in 5-HT(1A) receptor knockout mice: implications for schizophrenia.
Neuropharmacology
van der Neut R (1997) Targeted gene disruption: applications in neurobiology. J Neurosci
Methods 71:19–27
Veeraragavan S, Bui N, Perkins JR, Yuva-Paylor LA, Carpenter RL, Paylor R (2011) Modulation
of behavioral phenotypes by a muscarinic M1 antagonist in a mouse model of fragile X
syndrome. Psychopharmacology (Berl)
Verma V, Tan CH, Ong WY, Grigoryan GA, Jones CA, Stolzberg D, Salvi R, Gross KW, Ratty
AK, Dawe GS (2008) The chakragati mouse shows deficits in prepulse inhibition of acoustic
startle and latent inhibition. Neurosci Res 60:281–288
Wallén-Mackenzie A, Nordenankar K, Fejgin K, Lagerstrom MC, Emilsson L, Fredriksson R,
Wass C, Andersson D, Egecioglu E, Andersson M, Strandberg J, Lindhe O, Schioth HB,
Chergui K, Hanse E, Langstrom B, Fredriksson A, Svensson L, Roman E, Kullander K (2009)
Restricted cortical and amygdaloid removal of vesicular glutamate transporter 2 in
preadolescent mice impacts dopaminergic activity and neuronal circuitry of higher brain
function. J Neurosci 29:2238–2251
Wang H, Westin L, Nong Y, Birnbaum S, Bendor J, Brismar H, Nestler E, Aperia A, Flajolet M,
Greengard P (2009) Norbin is an endogenous regulator of metabotropic glutamate receptor 5
signaling. Science 326:1554–1557
Wang X (2009) Cre transgenic mouse lines. Methods Mol Biol 561:265–273
Watanabe A, Toyota T, Owada Y, Hayashi T, Iwayama Y, Matsumata M, Ishitsuka Y, Nakaya A,
Maekawa M, Ohnishi T, Arai R, Sakurai K, Yamada K, Kondo H, Hashimoto K, Osumi N,
Yoshikawa T (2007) Fabp7 maps to a quantitative trait locus for a schizophrenia
endophenotype. PLoS Biol 5:e297
Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Suddath R, Torrey EF (1992) Evidence of dysfunction of a
prefrontal-limbic network in schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging and regional
cerebral blood flow study of discordant monozygotic twins. Am J Psychiatry 149:890–897
Wen L, Lu YS, Zhu XH, Li XM, Woo RS, Chen YJ, Yin DM, Lai C, Terry AV Jr, Vazdarjanova A,
Xiong WC, Mei L (2010) Neuregulin 1 regulates pyramidal neuron activity via ErbB4 in
parvalbumin-positive interneurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:1211–1216
Willi R, Weinmann O, Winter C, Klein J, Sohr R, Schnell L, Yee BK, Feldon J, Schwab ME
(2010) Constitutive genetic deletion of the growth regulator Nogo-A induces schizophrenia-
related endophenotypes. J Neurosci 30:556–567
Willott JF, Carlson S, Chen H (1994) Prepulse inhibition of the startle response in mice:
relationship to hearing loss and auditory system plasticity. Behav Neurosci 108:6
Yavich L, Forsberg MM, Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA, Mannisto PT (2007) Site-specific role of
catechol-O-methyltransferase in dopamine overflow within prefrontal cortex and dorsal
striatum. J Neurosci 27:10196–10209
Young JW, Meves JM, Tarantino IS, Caldwell S, Geyer MA (2011a) Delayed procedural learning
in alpha7-nicotinic acetylcholine receptor knockout mice. Genes Brain Behav
Young JW, Powell SB, Scott CN, Zhou X, Geyer MA (2011b) The effect of reduced dopamine
D4 receptor expression in the 5-choice continuous performance task: Separating response
inhibition from premature responding. Behav Brain Res 222:183–192
Young JW, Wallace CK, Geyer MA, Risbrough VB (2010) Age-associated improvements in
cross-modal prepulse inhibition in mice. Behav Neurosci 124:133–140
Zhou X, Geyer MA, Kelsoe JR (2008) Does disrupted-in-schizophrenia (DISC1) generate fusion
transcripts? Mol Psychiatry 13:361–363
318 S. B. Powell et al.
Zhou X, Long JM, Geyer MA, Masliah E, Kelsoe JR, Wynshaw-Boris A, Chien KR (2004)
Reduced expression of the Sp4 gene in mice causes deficits in sensorimotor gating and
memory associated with hippocampal vacuolization. Mol Psychiatry 10:393–406
Zhou X, Tang W, Greenwood TA, Guo S, He3 L, Geyer MA, Kelsoe JR (2009) Transcription
factor SP4 is a susceptibility gene for bipolar disorder. PlosOne In Press
Zhu H, Mingler MK, McBride ML, Murphy AJ, Valenzuela DM, Yancopoulos GD,
Williams MT, Vorhees CV, Rothenberg ME (2010) Abnormal response to stress and
impaired NPS-induced hyperlocomotion, anxiolytic effect and corticosterone increase in mice
lacking NPSR1. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35:1119–1132
Part II
Clinical Behavioral Genetics
Applications of Second Generation
Sequencing Technologies in Complex
Disorders
Contents
1 Sequencing Technologies
In 1977 two very different methods were presented for directly sequencing DNA:
the Maxam–Gilbert chemical cleavage method (Maxam and Gilbert 1977) and the
Sanger chain-termination method (Sanger et al. 1977). The Sanger sequencing
method relies on base-specific termination of the growing chain during DNA
synthesis caused by the incorporation of dideoxy-nucleotides (ddNTPs) that lack
the hydroxyl group necessary for DNA chain elongation. The labeled DNA
fragments are then separated according to their length on a gel matrix. Sanger and
Gilbert shared the Nobel prize in 1980. Sanger’s method became widely used first
in slab-gel implementations with radioactive detection and later in capillary
electrophoretic systems using fluorescent-labeled ddNTPs. It revolutionized the
genetics field and eventually led to the sequencing of the human (Lander et al.
2001; Venter et al. 2001) and many other genomes. However, the effort to
sequence the human genome required many hundreds of automated capillary
sequencers over a ten-year period and cost over $10 M. At the present time,
automated capillary sequencing instruments with up to 384 capillaries can generate
around 1 Mbase of sequence every 24 h.
In 2005 there was a paradigm shift in DNA sequencing with the introduction of
the first second generation sequencing (2ndGS) technologies (for a review see
Ansorge 2009). While Sanger sequencing relies on the individual analysis of DNA
fragments (one fragment per capillary), second generation sequencers are able to
analyze millions of DNA templates in parallel (Fig. 1, Table 1). This is achieved
first by placing single DNA molecules into individual and miniaturized reaction
vessels for amplification. In emulsion PCR, template DNA molecules are indi-
vidually captured on beads, and compartmentalized and clonally amplified in
droplets within an oil emulsion (Diehl et al. 2006). An alternative approach is to
spread out the DNA fragments on a planar surface and generate clonally amplified
Applications of Second Generation Sequencing Technologies 323
Genomic DNA
Fragmentation
Adapter ligation
Clonal amplification
DNA
Sequencing polymerase
primer
Sequencing
chemistry
Imaging
Conversion
to sequence
Fig. 1 Schematic representation of the principles of 2ndGS technologies. Genomic DNA (gray)
is randomly sheared and ligated to universal adapter sequences (black). Clonal amplification of
the fragments is achieved either in droplets within an oil emulsion (emulsion PCR; left) or on the
surface of a solid substrate (solid-phase amplification; right). The sequencing process consists of
iterative cycles of enzyme-driven nucleotide extension and imaging-based data acquisition
324 M. Bayés et al.
templates in situ (Bentley et al. 2008). Once the clonal amplification of the original
templates is completed, millions of DNA ‘‘polymerase clones’’ or ‘‘polonies’’ are
subjected to a sequencing chemistry in a microfluidic device coupled with a high-
resolution imaging system. For each DNA ‘‘polony’’, the resulting images are
converted into sequence reads.
The Roche/454 system (www.454.com) was the first 2ndGS system released to the
market. The templates are amplified by emulsion PCR and the sequences are
obtained by iterative pyrosequencing (Magulies et al. 2005). Pyrosequencing
(Ronaghi et al. 1996) is a sequencing-by-synthesis method that relies on an
enzymatic cascade in which the release of pyrophosphate during the incorporation
of a nucleotide drives a chemiluminescent reaction that can be detected with a
high-resolution charge-coupled device (CCD) camera. It uses regular deoxy-
nucleotides that are flowed sequentially in a known order. The amount of light
emitted is proportional to the number of base incorporations. Because dNTPs are
used, homopolymers in the template DNA are the main source of errors due to the
difficulty to unambiguously assign the number of bases incorporated. The current
454/Roche platform provides reads that are several hundred bases, more than any
other 2ndGS technology. Roche markets two different systems, the Genome
Sequencer (GS) FLX instrument that can generate about 0.7 Gbase of sequence per
run, and the GS Junior that produces about 35 Mbases (Table 1).
Recently Ion Torrent introduced an instrument that uses the same underlying
methods as the Roche system for clonal amplification and parallel sequencing but
Applications of Second Generation Sequencing Technologies 325
The first Solexa sequencing platform (Genome Analyzer 1G) was commercialized
in late 2006 and Illumina acquired the company soon after the launch of this
instrument (www.illumina.com). In this technology templates are amplified in near
vicinity by solid-surface bridge amplification using a microfluidic cluster station
(Bentley et al. 2008) (Fig. 1). Sequencing proceeds on the same surface using four
reversible terminator nucleotides each labeled with a different fluorescent dye. The
identity of each incorporated nucleotide is determined by recording the fluores-
cence with a CCD camera. After each imaging cycle the fluorescent moiety is
cleaved from the base and the 30 end is regenerated to enable next nucleotide
incorporation. The technology has undergone several technical and chemical
upgrades to give read lengths over 100 bases. The new version of the Illumina
instrument, the HiSeq2000, can run two slides simultaneously and generate over
600 Gbases of sequence in 11 days of operation (Table 1). This equates to
30x coverage of six human genomes in a single run.
sequence of bases 3 and 4, 8 and 9, and 13 and 14. The same procedure is repeated
for the two-base, three-base and four-base offset primers, which completes the
sequence. Because in the whole process each base is interrogated twice, the SOLiD
technology shows the greatest accuracy. The latest version of the SOLiD instru-
ment, 5500xl, can generate 60 base reads and process two slides simultaneously to
produce more than 300 Gbases of sequence in a 7-day run (Table 1).
All of the three 2ndGS systems described above can be used with paired-end
protocols that enable the sequence to be obtained from the two extremities of the
templates (for a review see Holt and Jones 2008). Paired-end protocols partially
compensate for the shortcoming of short read lengths of these systems compared to
Sanger sequencing by providing linking information. Illumina offers a paired-end
protocol to read 2 9 100 bases at a distance of up to 600 bases and a mate-pair
procedure in which the insert size can be up to 5 kbases.
There are several companies and academic groups working in the development of
new methodologies and instruments to enable sequencing of the complete human
genome in less than a day at a cost of less than $1,000 (for a review see Schadt
et al. 2010). Oxford Nanopore Technologies is developing a nanopore-based
sequencing platform (www.nanoporetech.com). The principle of the method is
the detection of nucleotides as they are driven by an electrophoretic field pass
through an a–hemolysin membrane nanopore that is coupled with an exonuclease.
Applications of Second Generation Sequencing Technologies 327
The change in the membrane potential at the nanopore is used to determine the
identity of the nucleotide passing through. The system offers the potential to
identify individual nucleotide modifications including 5-methylcytosine.
The unprecedented resolution of the new generation of sequencing technologies
will allow high-throughput single cell DNA and RNA sequencing in the near
future. The underlying concept is that a target sequence of DNA is generated in all
of the cells of a histological section simultaneously. Several studies have already
shown the feasibility of sequencing the transcriptome of individual cells with
current instruments using whole-genome or transcriptome amplification proce-
dures (Tang et al. 2010).
Current sequencing methods do not provide long-range haplotype information
than can be very important for applications such as disease mapping or population
history analysis. Molecular procedures for the determination of haplotypes will
become more important when the methods for the determination of simple
sequence motifs are well established. There are several approaches to stretch and
immobilize long lengths of single genomic DNA molecules on a surface, allowing
molecular sequencing tests to be carried out in situ that will provide haplotype
information.
The three second generation platforms described in this chapter have their own
advantages and disadvantages. Currently the HiSeq2000 and 5500xl SOLiD
platforms have the higher sequencing capacity, 25–50 Gbases per day, while the
GS FLX can generate up to 1 Gbase per day (Table 1). In addition, the reagent
cost per Gbase on the GS FLX is more than two orders of magnitude higher than
on the other platforms. However, these numbers have to be taken with caution
because the rapid increase in throughput at constant reagent consumption has
raised the importance of other cost components such as instrument depreciation,
labor and data management that need to be considered.
The GS FLX system provides intermediate read length, with an average of 700
bases. In contrast, the 5500xl SOLiD and HiSeq2000 platforms read many more
DNA molecules in parallel (up to 3000 million) but have shorter read lengths (up
to 100 bp) (Table 1). This would be a severe limitation if the companies were not
offering paired-end reads, which facilitate their mapping onto the reference
sequence.
Error rates largely dictate the read length limits of different sequencing plat-
forms. However, the different systems are difficult to compare in terms of accuracy
because base quality values are not comparable across platforms. Raw accuracies
of [99.94, [99 and [98.5% are reported in 5500xl SOLiD, GS FLX and
HiSeq2000 specifications, respectively. In the GS FLX platform, the most com-
mon errors are insertions and deletions in homopolymer segments. By contrast, the
5500xl SOLiD and HiSeq2000 instruments have a very distinct error profile, with
328 M. Bayés et al.
the vast majority of the sequencing errors being base substitutions. The Life-
Technologies two-base encoding system shows the highest individual base call
accuracy, although it needs to be pointed out that the method uses the reference
sequence to make corrections to the raw base calls. At present, the error distri-
bution of third generation technologies based on single-molecule sequencing is
much higher than that of PCR-based methods.
As with genotyping technologies, it is essential to use the right 2ndGS instru-
ment for the application in hand. For applications requiring long reads, such as
metagenomic sequencing, and de novo sequencing of genomes, GS FLX is the
technology of choice. The long reads guarantee best possible assembly of the data.
The 5500xl SOLiD and HiSeq2000 systems work better for tag counting appli-
cations such as mRNA and smallRNA profiling and identification of DNA–protein
binding sites, because they provide very high number of individual reads with just
enough sequence to map them to the reference genome. Because of the lower
reagent cost, HiSeq 2000 and 5500xl SOLiD are also the ideal solution for rese-
quencing of genomes or exomes where reference sequences are available. The use
of the SOLiD platform with the very accurate two-base encoding system is rec-
ommended for low coverage projects and for detecting rare variants and somatic
mutations. In some cases, the use of various 2ndGS platforms is recommended to
take the advantages of each technology: error profile, read length, paired-end
reads, number of tags, etc.
The use of 2ndGS technologies that produce hundreds of millions of short reads
brings with it a collection of informatics and bioinformatics challenges. The most
immediate issue is that of data storage, with even small projects needing tens of
terabytes of storage, and sequencing centers having storage requirements in the
petabyte range. The next concerns the assessment of the quality of the produced
sequence data. Each sequencing platform produces its own quality metrics, but it is
often advisable to add additional quality checks to check the consistency of the
results. The final challenge that will be addressed in this chapter is that of the
primary data analysis, which will generally include assembly and/or alignment of
the individual sequence reads followed by subsequent analyses that will depend on
the particular sequencing experiment being performed. A survey of all the analyses
used for the different types of sequencing experiments that have been described
using 2ndGS technologies is beyond the scope of this chapter, but a short
description of variant calling for the case of the resequencing of genomic data will
be given. A list of available 2ndGS software has not been included as such lists
become quickly out of date with the development of new packages and the
improvement of existing packages. Up-to-date information on analysis packages
Applications of Second Generation Sequencing Technologies 329
The principal data produced by the sequencers are the sequence reads and asso-
ciated quality measures, which alone can require a large amount of storage space.
For example, WGS of a human genome at 309 coverage will require almost
100 Gbases of sequence. The common data formats used for this type of data
require 2 bytes per base (1 for the base and 1 for the quality) with some extra to
store the read identity, so 100 Gbases of sequence will require [200 Gbytes of
storage, although this can be reduced to \50 Gbytes using data compression
techniques. The sequence reads, however, are only a part of the story, and to get
full picture of the storage requirements it is necessary to look at the complete
analysis pipeline from the initial data produced by the sequencers (depending on
the platform these could be the sequence reads or could be unprocessed data such
as images) to the final products of the pipeline (i.e., called variants). Each stage of
the pipeline will require space to calculate and store the results of the analysis, and
a working space of at last several Tbytes will typically be required to work on
WGS data from higher organisms.
An important decision to be made that has a large impact on the total storage
requirements is how long the results files from different stages of the pipeline
should be kept. Many of the stages in the analysis pipeline (particularly the early
stages) involve data reduction and some consequent data loss, so that it is often not
possible to take the results of a particular analysis stage and recreate the results of
an earlier stage. A balance must therefore be struck between the cost of recreating
the data from a particular stage if necessary, and the costs of storing them.
For example, a useful metric to assess the overall quality of a sequencing run is the
proportion of sequenced bases that have quality scores that exceed a certain
threshold. For downstream analyses there are two basic approaches to use quality
scores: (a) reads with low quality bases can be trimmed so that all remaining bases
are high quality and (b) the analyses explicitly take account of the quality scores
by weighing bases according to their qualities, with low quality bases being down
weighted and so having less effect on the analyses.
In addition to errors in the base calling, another potential source of problems is
sample mix-ups, leading to the sample that is being analyzed not being what is
expected. Such mix-ups can occur at many points in the process, for example at the
collection facility, during library preparation, at the moment the samples are
placed on the sequencer or during the analyses themselves. It is difficult to com-
pletely eliminate the possibility of sample mix-ups, but a combination of good
laboratory practice and analysis checks can both greatly reduce the chances of
mix-ups and detect the majority of mix-ups that do occur. For example, a common
practice is to perform systematically SNP genotyping on the same samples that are
sequenced using one of the genome-wide SNP array platforms. This provides a set
of reference genotypes that can be checked against genotypes predicted from the
sequence data (Koboldt et al. 2010).
For species for which a good reference sequence exists, the first analysis to be
performed after base calling and initial quality control is to align the short
sequence reads to the reference. When 2ndGS technologies were first developed,
existing sequence aligners were not adapted to handle efficiently the very large
number of short reads. In recent years a large number of aligners have been
developed explicitly for short sequence reads, and now aligning hundreds of
Gbases of sequence for WGS studies is a matter of hours rather than days or weeks
(Li and Homer 2010; Koboldt et al. 2010). The aim of this section is not to give an
exhaustive survey of available alignment software but rather to discuss the features
that a good aligner should have, that may itself depend on the type of analysis that
is required. All aligners report for each read the number of matches that have been
found with an indication of the differences between the read and the reference at
the match position. To reduce the number of possible matches, aligners only
consider matches that are close to the reference, although how ‘close’ is defined
can vary between aligners, and can often be adjusted by the user. A typical
definition of close would be where the read differs from the reference at n or less
positions, possibly only considering high quality bases in the comparison with the
reference. Important considerations when choosing an aligner are whether the
search for matches is exhaustive (i.e., is there a guarantee that all close matches
will be found), what the behavior is with reads that map to repetitive regions (are
all matches returned in these cases, or a subset of matches or a single match
Applications of Second Generation Sequencing Technologies 331
selected using some criterion), whether the aligner can find gapped or split
alignments (discussed below), and the computational requirements. For variant
calling (discussed below) it is important that only uniquely mapping reads are used
otherwise there is a high risk of false calls being made, but for different analyses
(i.e. of CNVs), nonuniquely mapping reads give important information and should
be made available by the aligner.
The alignment of reads from paired-end libraries requires a post-processing step
after alignment where the potential matches from both ends of the same fragment are
examined to see if a consistent pair of matches exists where the orientation and
distance of the two ends is as expected given the library construction method. In this
way a nonuniquely mapping read can be ‘rescued’ if the corresponding pair is itself
uniquely mapped. Paired-end reads are useful for the detection of structural variation
as large deletions/insertions or translocations and inversions will create sets of
paired-reads with incoherent matches (i.e., with the wrong distance between the pairs
or with the two ends mapping in the wrong orientation or on different chromosomes).
This approach is powerful for larger structural variants in the order of several
hundred base pairs or larger, but for smaller variants the distortion of the distance
between paired-reads is too small to allow detection by this method. Smaller
insertion/deletion variants can be detected using gapped and split alignments, if the
aligner can generate these. Gapped alignments allow a short insertion/deletion
(typically \30 bp to be present) within the reads, while split alignments are a
generalization of gapped alignments where an arbitrary gap can exist in a read,
with the two extremities of the same read being potentially mapped to different
chromosomes. The use of gapped/split alignments along with paired-end reads
allows the detection of deletions across the entire scale of the genome from single
bases up to entire chromosomes. Other types of structural rearrangements can also
be detected across most of the range, although with current technologies there is a
gap between very short features (\30 bp) and features [*200 bp where rear-
rangements cannot be reliably detected, although increasing read lengths should
reduce this gap in the near future. It should also be mentioned here that gapped/
split alignments are also very useful in the mapping of reads from the transcrip-
tome on to the genomic reference, allowing the mapping of exon/intron boundaries
(Trapnell et al. 2009; Bryant et al. 2010).
confidence for the call is calculated. Simple callers use heuristic filters to deter-
mine thresholds for calling different genotypes, while more sophisticated callers
use explicit statistical models that allow statements to be made about the precision
of the estimation, and that permit the incorporation of prior information such as the
base quality scores and knowledge about the frequency and location of known
variants (Li et al. 2009; Koboldt et al. 2009).
Today, 2ndGS technologies are being applied to sequence complete human gen-
omes in order to unravel the complexity of the normal and disease genomes in
terms of single-nucleotide variants (SNVs), small insertions and deletions (indels),
structural variants (SVs) and copy number variations (CNVs). The number of
individual genomes that has been publicly announced to be sequenced is rapidly
increasing. However, it is difficult to identify variants contributing to disease from
individual sequences (Fig. 2b). As for the Genome-Wide Association Studies
(GWAS) performed with SNP arrays, sample size needs to be very large to
establish genotype-phenotype relationships for most disorders.
There are several ongoing large-scale projects that aim to sequence the whole
genome of several hundreds or even thousands of individuals. The 1,000 Genome
Project (www.1000genomes.org) aims to sequence the genomes of a large number
of people to provide a comprehensive catalog of human genetic variation,
including rare variants (present in \5% in the population) that are thought to play
a major role in the genetic basis of complex diseases and that are not
readily screened by current genotyping technologies. The International Cancer
Genome Consortium (ICGC, www.icgc.org) has been organized as an interna-
tional effort to obtain a comprehensive description of genomic, transcriptomic and
epigenomic changes in 50 different tumor types (The International Cancer Genome
Consortium 2010).
Applications of Second Generation Sequencing Technologies 333
Fig. 2 Examples of the sequencing reads obtained in different applications. When appropriate
the genome reference sequence (Ref) is shown above, with exons marked with different colors.
Sequence variants in the reads (SNVs and indels) are shown in red (b, c). Paired-end reads
mapping to different chromosomal locations are shown in d. Positions displaying complete
methylation (C), no methylation (T) and partial methylation (T or C) after bisulfite treatment are
depicted in blue (e)
334 M. Bayés et al.
Array with capture probes Genomic DNA Array with capture probes
Fig. 3 Strategies for selecting specific regions of interest. Genomic DNA (gray, with regions of
interest in red) is randomly sheared and ligated to universal adapter sequences (black). The
fragments of interest are then captured by hybridization to target-specific probes either on a
microarray surface (left) or in solution (right). The hybrid-selected enriched output library is
eluted and sequenced
(Mamanova et al. 2010). Robust and reproducible protocols are currently being
worked out in order to minimize variations in capture uniformity across regions.
phenotypes, identifying all variants with respect to the reference sequence, filtering
out common variation present in public SNV databases and selecting and vali-
dating the putative pathogenic variants shared by the affected individuals.
In the near future, exome sequencing will probably trigger the discovery of non-
recurring variants with large effects on complex phenotypes such as schizophrenia,
autism, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and X-linked mental retardation (McCarthy
2009).
Several GWAS in complex disorders have identified regions of the genome that
are significantly associated with the risk of the disease (www.genome.gov/
26525384). However, the majority of these reported associations involve variants
that lie outside known genes, with no obvious functional consequences, and it
remains a challenge to pinpoint the true causative variants.
Using targeted capture and 2ndGS methods, large contiguous stretches of
genome (up to few Mbases) identified through GWAS can be sequenced to help
refine association signals and find the biologically causative mutation among these
correlated variants. Following this approach, Yeager et al. (2008) resequenced the
gene-poor 8q24 region that is associated to breast, prostate and colorectal cancer
and generated a detailed map of common and rare genetic variation across the
region. Hopefully, this kind of data together with the development of assays that
test directly the effect of the newly discovered variants will shed some light on the
biologically relevant variants that predispose to complex disorders.
In a similar way, other groups have used targeted capture methods to quickly
and cost-effectively sequence the genomic intervals identified by linkage analysis
in rare Mendelian disorders (Brkanac et al. 2009; Volpi et al. 2010 and Nikopoulos
et al. 2010). In both applications, a major limitation of this approach is that in all
hybridization-based capture methods, repetitive regions are masked during the
probe design process leaving only the unique fraction of the genomes represented.
Like SNVs, CNVs and SVs may account for the modulation of many complex
phenotypic traits and disease susceptibility in humans. 2ndGS has been quickly
applied to obtain single-base resolution data on CNVs and SVs partially sup-
planting array Comparative Genome Hybridization (aCGH) and SNP genotyping
arrays.
An adequate capture of SVs can be obtained with the analysis of sequence reads
from both ends of contiguous DNA fragments that are few hundred base pairs long
(paired-reads) or from captured distal ends of larger fragments (mate pairs). Long
mate paired-reads are more sensitive for detecting large events, although they have
338 M. Bayés et al.
a broader size distribution and therefore have less resolution for breakpoint
localization. The analysis of the separation distance of the paired-reads with
respect to the reference genome enables the identification of genomic insertions
and deletions. In addition and unlike microarrays, 2ndGS can also provide data on
copy neutral variations such as translocations and inversions by searching for read
pairs with ends mapping to different genomic locations or in opposite orientation,
respectively (Fig. 2). The formation of chimeric clones during the library prepa-
ration and the sequencing errors resulting in incorrectly mapped reads are the
major limitations (Medvedev et al. 2009).
In a massively parallel sequencing experiment with several millions of reads,
the number of sequences aligning to a genomic region is proportional to the
number of times this region appears in the genome. Therefore, CNVs can be
detected at high sensitivity by determining the depth of coverage across the
genome after correcting for differences in GC content and uniqueness across the
genome. Reports on WGS of individual genomes have identified several thousands
of CNVs which is in several fold more than the numbers that were reported in
array-based studies (Ku et al. 2010).
However, paired-end WGS may be not meaningful for those patients in which
cytogenetic techniques have previously localized the disease-associated chromo-
some rearrangement. In these cases, sequencing of sorted derivative chromosomes or
microdissected chromosomal regions has proven to be a feasible approach for the
characterization of chromosomal breakpoints (Chen et al. 2008; Weise et al. 2010).
Eukaryote genomes carry relatively stable chemical marks that are added to either
DNA or its packing histones. These epigenetic changes are known to play an
important role in the regulation of gene expression and to contribute to the
understanding of some cancers, congenital anomaly syndromes and other complex
non-neoplastic disorders, including some metabolic and cardiovascular diseases.
2ndGS using slightly modified sample preparation protocols allows for an unbi-
ased exploration of DNA methylation and histone modifications and has sub-
stantially accelerated epigenetic research.
Standard methodology for the detection of cytosine methylation, the most
commonly studied epigenetic modification, is based on bisulfate treatment that
converts the cytosine residues to uracils while leaving 50 -methylcytosines
unchanged. Bisulfite Sequencing (BS-Seq) combines bisulfite treatment with
2ndGS technology, providing a digital measure of the frequency that a cytosine is
methylated (Lister and Ecker 2009) (Fig. 2e). Other methods such as reduced
representation BS sequencing or MeDIP sequencing allow saving some costs by
targeting only CpG-rich or highly methylated regions, respectively. In the near
future, novel single-molecule sequencing approaches, such as the ones being
developed by Pacific Biosciences (www.pacificbiosciences.com) and Oxford
Applications of Second Generation Sequencing Technologies 339
2ndGS technologies have been proven to be very useful for sequencing RNA
(RNA-Seq) and are currently replacing microarray-based methods. They not only
provide a digital overview of gene expression profiles but also enable the char-
acterization of splice variants, antisense transcription, fusion genes, allele-specific
expression, RNA editing and other forms of sequence variation in the transcribed
portion of genes (Fig. 2f).
In RNA-Seq, the expression level of a gene is deduced from the total number of
reads each gene sequence appears and normalized by the length of the gene. Low
abundant transcripts that would escape detection by microarray-based methods can
be detected by increasing sequencing depth. In this regard, short read sequencing
technologies provide much deeper coverage at lower cost although they may be
problematic when characterizing novel exon junctions (Morozova et al. 2009).
Aberrant transcriptional events resulting from chromosomal rearrangements
play a causative role in many human cancers. In recent years several studies have
successfully used paired-end RNA-Seq to detect fusion transcripts in cancer at
single-nucleotide resolution (Maher et al. 2009; Zhao et al. 2009). In addition,
sequencing the transcriptome is an alternative to exome sequencing for identifying
expressed genetic variants when performed in the appropriate tissue type (Cirulli
et al. 2010). For example, a recurrent somatic mutation in FOXL2 gene has been
found in granulosa-cell tumors using this approach (Shah et al. 2009).
A related application of 2ndGS technologies is the discovery and profiling of
non-coding RNAs (ncRNA) in order to elucidate their role in health and disease.
ncRNAs are small RNA molecules involved in post-transcriptional regulation of
340 M. Bayés et al.
gene expression that have been suggested to modulate parameters such as disease
onset, severity or progression. The short length of ncRNAs makes 2ndGS plat-
forms ideal for ncRNA characterization on a genome-wide basis, as illustrated by a
recent study by Uziel et al. (2009) that demonstrated how miRNA overexpression
collaborates with the Sonic Hedgehog pathway in medulloblastoma. Similarly,
other groups have reported the involvement of specific microRNAs in the etiology
of ovarian and breast cancer that could be used as biomarkers for tumor classifi-
cation and prognosis (Wyman et al. 2009; Nygaard et al. 2009).
2ndGS has also been used for the rapid identification of pathogens in chronic as
well as acute disease replacing conventional Sanger sequencing methods. This is
typically accomplished by extracting all nucleic acids from the sample of interest,
sequencing the complex DNA or RNA mixture and then assembling the non-
human sequence reads derived from the infectious agents. In 2008, a new arena
virus and a new polyomavirus were associated with febrile illness after visceral
organ transplantation and Merkel cell carcinoma, respectively, by sequencing
RNA from affected tissues (Palacios et al. 2008; Feng et al. 2008).
For known pathogens, the high sensitivity of 2ndGS has proven to be ideal to
detect rare variants that might confer to the pathogen some resistance to treatment
or other virulence markers. Given the high throughput of second GS technologies,
hundreds of samples individually tagged with a molecular barcode can be analyzed
simultaneously. This approach has been used to identify drug-resistant HIV strains
in patients at the earliest stages (Hofmann et al. 2007).
The Human Microbiome project (http://nihroadmap.nih.gov/hmp/) aims to
analyze the entire collection of microbes (microbiome) that are present in the inner
and outer surfaces of our body, including the skin, the oral cavity, the vagina and
the intestine, and analyze its role in human health and disease. It uses a metage-
nomic approach that allows the study of native microbial or viral communities
with no initial cloning steps combined with the pyrosequencing technology that
provides with long ([300 bp) and highly accurate reads ([99.5%) (MacLean et al.
2009). In the near future, sequencing the microbiome at intermediate stages from
health to disease will greatly increase our understanding of some human conditions
such as obesity and cancer. In addition, WGS of the host genomes will allow us to
investigate the genetic basis of susceptibility to microbial infection.
References
Wyman SK, Parkin RK, Mitchell PS et al (2009) Repertoire of microRNAs in epithelial ovarian
cancer as determined by next generation sequencing of small RNA cDNA libraries. PLoS One
4(4):e5311
Yeager M, Xiao N, Hayes RB et al (2008) Comprehensive resequence analysis of a 136 kb region
of human chromosome 8q24 associated with prostate and colon cancers. Hum Genet
124(2):161–170
Zhao Q, Caballero OL, Levy S et al (2009) Transcriptome-guided characterization of genomic
rearrangements in a breast cancer cell line. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106(6):1886–1891
Rare Genomic Deletions and Duplications
and their Role in Neurodevelopmental
Disorders
Abstract Copy number variations (CNVs) are deletions and duplications of DNA
sequences that vary in length from a few base pairs to several million. While
these structural variations are often benign, they can disrupt vital biological
functions and result in disease. CNVs have been identified as causal in a number
of neurodevelopmental disorders (NDs), including but not limited to, autism,
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and schizophrenia. Here, we
examine CNV research into these disorders, and discuss relevant methodological
considerations. By identifying specific rare deletions and duplications, we may be
better able to determine the etiology of neurodevelopmental disorders and identify
appropriate treatments.
Keywords Copy number variation Neurodevelopmental disorder Autism
Schizophrenia Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
Contents
1 Introduction........................................................................................................................ 346
2 Methodological Considerations......................................................................................... 346
2.1 CNV Detection and Validation ................................................................................ 346
2.2 Interpretation ............................................................................................................. 350
3 Large Constitutional Deletions and Duplications in Neurodevelopmental Disease ....... 351
4 Common Neurodevelopmental Disorders......................................................................... 353
4.1 Autism ....................................................................................................................... 354
4.2 Schizophrenia............................................................................................................ 354
4.3 ADHD ....................................................................................................................... 355
5 Conclusion ......................................................................................................................... 356
References................................................................................................................................ 357
1 Introduction
2 Methodological Considerations
Fig. 1 CNV detection methods and data. A Two-color FISH shows large CNV, deletion shown.
B Based on a control value, a ratio of observed to control for specific loci signal CNV. C SNP
arrays or aCGH assay contiguous SNPs or CN probes are bound to the genome allowing for
detection of variations from normal diploid
350 J. T. Glessner et al.
2.2 Interpretation
When CNVs are observed in a parent but not in a child, this may indicate an error
in the CNV call. The converse is not necessarily the case, and de novo CNVs have
been strongly associated with a number of NDs (e.g. Sebat et al. 2007). De novo
CNVs may be particularly informative for pronounced NDs such as brain mal-
formations, where the parent is often annotated as healthy. De novo mutations may
also be of special relevance to heterogeneous NDs such as autism and ADHD,
where etiologies are complex and a specific disease label can have very different
phenotypes and combinations (Geschwind and Levitt 2007). Further, CNVs may
have prospective diagnostic value in differentiating behaviorally similar NDs. In
certain cases, the parent may have an attenuated ND or an additional protective
Rare Genomic Deletions and Duplications 351
variant, which has obvious implications for treatment. Rare private mutations in
certain families are expected from a population genetics perspective without
detrimental effect. This necessitates observation of recurrent rare CNVs in unre-
lated individuals from diverse geographic and ethnic populations, which can be
detected by different array technologies.
Proximal to CNV regions, highly homologous low-copy-number repeats (LCRs)
are frequently observed, which can predispose to recombination between non-allelic
LCRs (non-allelic homologous recombination) (Britten and Davidson, 1976; Lupski
1998). Evolution and genome condensation occur through various mechanisms,
including chromosome splicing of highly similar sequences known as homologous
recombination (HR). In somatic cells, HR is needed to repair extreme DNA damage
such as double strand breaks (DSB). If spliced incorrectly, CNVs and genomic
instability can result. An intermediate state is formed between two DNA strands,
which proceeds by crossover (two-way sharing, meiosis, and DSB) or gene
conversion (one-way sharing, DSB). The human genome has much segmental
duplication, which provides similar sequences for HR to occur. HR may expand
gene families with diverse functions but also leads to more mistakes. Segmental
duplications can masquerade as allelic sequences during meiosis and lead to
erroneous splicing, termed non-allelic homologous recombination. Gene conversion
can result in inserts of non-expressed poor function elements into homologous
expressed genes. Segmental duplications are also referred to as low-copy repeats.
Non-homologous end-joining (NHEJ) (Moore and Haber, 1996), fork stalling, and
template switching (FoSTeS) (Lee et al. 2007) can result in less typical CNV generation.
The widespread and, in some regions, high prevalence of CNVs is somewhat
counterintuitive for such an aberration from the classical genetics of a maternal
and paternal diploid chromosome set. However, it is likely that CNVs may con-
tribute to evolutionary mechanisms, whereby gene families with homologous
sequences are extended into complementary functions over time. Perhaps CNVs
involving genes responsible for behavior, cognition, and psychological state are
most active in humans representing a successful expansion of gene functions based
on ancestral duplications.
Taken together, multiple rare CNVs have been implicated in the pathogenesis of
NDs. However, a few common variant models have also been implicated in pre-
disposing to several of the NDs, including autism and schizophrenia (Wang et al.
2009; International Schizophrenia Consortium 2009). While common SNP geno-
types have been implicated to a greater degree in inflammatory and metabolic
disorders, such as type 1 diabetes, inflammatory bowel disease and type 2 diabetes,
it is conceivable that with greater sample sizes that many more common SNP
variants will be uncovered that predispose to ND susceptibility. In addition,
common CNVs are also hypothetically possible but current methods often under
call their true frequency. Thus, rare CNVs have been most successful in NDs
lending support to a common disease-rare variant model that is disparate from the
more typical common disease-common variant model, previously implicated by
many (Schork et al. 2009).
354 J. T. Glessner et al.
4.1 Autism
4.2 Schizophrenia
studies above, rare CNVs were not overrepresented in cases versus controls. This
is largely consistent with the literature, though (Walsh et al. 2008) found novel
large rare deletions and duplications of genes observed in 15% of cases versus 5%
of controls. A study of CNVs in Chinese schizophrenia patients detected no sig-
nificant difference in rare CNVs between cases and controls (Shi et al. 2008).
Another study of 1,013 cases and 1,084 controls of European ancestry also failed
to find more rare CNVs [100 kb in cases or enrichment for neurodevelopmental
pathways (Need et al. 2009).
Previous studies have associated various CNVs with schizophrenia, including
deletions of 22q11.2 (Liu et al. 2002), NRXN1 (Kirov et al. 2007), APBA2 (Kirov
et al. 2007), and CNTNAP2 (Friedman et al. 2008). However, each of these CNVs
is rare and they account for a relatively small proportion of the overall genetic risk
in schizophrenia. Large rare CNVs impacting many different genes enriched in
neurodevelopmental pathways have been reported elsewhere (Walsh et al. 2008;
International Schizophrenia Consortium 2008; Stefansson et al. 2008). Specific
loci exhibiting runs of homozygosity (ROHs) have also been associated with
schizophrenia (Lencz et al. 2007), and de novo CNVs (P = 7.8 9 10-4) were
recently observed in sporadic schizophrenia cases in comparison with controls
(Xu et al. 2008). Comparison of genomic findings in schizophrenia and autism has
suggested a diametric etiology (Crespi et al. 2010).
4.3 ADHD
GRM7 deletion was also observed in cases and not in controls (Elia et al., 2010).
Rare inherited structural variations play an important role in ADHD development
and indicate a set of putative candidate genes for further study in the etiology of
ADHD.
5 Conclusion
In summary, both common and rare variants have been implicated in NDs. While
considerable progress has been made over the past few years in unveiling the
genetic causes of NDs, genome-wide studies explain roughly 10–15% of the
heritability of NDs. A recent simulation study suggests that some of the ‘missing’
heritability might be accountable for synthetic associations—a term used to
describe the relationship between a (common) causal variant and a (rarer) non-
causal variant. Dickson et al. (2010) simulated output from a genome-wide
association study (GWAS), where some individuals had rare variants (0.5–2% of
the population) that likely caused a disease. They found that the GWAS technique
erroneously identified the synthetically associated variant as causal, essentially
obscuring the signal from the rarer ‘‘at risk’’ variant. In other words, synthetic
associations infer that many people share a variant that confers some small risk of
disease. In fact, only a few of these individuals share a rarer variant that carries a
much higher risk.
CNV studies, which are sensitive to specific rare deletions and duplications,
provide a potential route around this problem. However, as noted above, this
approach is relatively insensitive to second-hit variants because of the limits of
sample size. A combination of the two approaches is arguably the most attractive.
The explosion of GWAS publications in the past several years has generated
numerous loci putatively associated with NDs. Perhaps the most important repli-
cation that can be made without the need for genotyping multitudes of additional
samples is in functional gene clusters of associated loci. This also boosts confi-
dence in association signals of rare CNVs, which singularly may have arisen by
chance. The CNVs were recently identified at CNTN4 and NRXN1 have well-
established associations with autism (Glessner et al. 2009).
Some of the missing heritability should also disappear as arrays with increas-
ingly powerful resolutions are developed. Although much progress has been made
to this end, further resolution of data is needed to gain confidence in CNV calls,
and to establish clear breakpoints. Forty-two million probes have already been
achieved with genome-tiling set of multiple arrays (Conrad et al. 2010). The best
resolution, of course, is sequencing the genome for every contiguous base, which
has also been investigated (Kidd et al. 2008).
Many of the issues allied to structural studies will doubtless be resolved by
technological advances. Nevertheless, additional complexity in the CNV theme
exists in mosaicism, translocations, and inversions. Neither translocations nor
inversions can be detected with a SNP array, and clone paired-end sequencing
Rare Genomic Deletions and Duplications 357
References
Arajärvi R et al (2005) Prevalence and diagnosis of schizophrenia based on register, case record
and interview data in an isolated Finnish birth cohort born 1940–1969. Soc Psychiatry
Psychiatr Epidemiol 40:808–816
Britten RJ, Davidson EH (1976) Studies on nucleic acid reassociation kinetics: empirical
equations describing DNA reassociation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 73:415–419
Chamberlain SJ, Lalande M (2010) Neurodevelopmental disorders involving genomic imprinting
at human chromosome 15q11-q13. Neurobiol Dis 39(1):13–20
Conrad DF, Pinto D, Redon R, Feuk L, Gokcumen O, Zhang Y, Aerts J, Andrews TD, Barnes C,
Campbell P, Fitzgerald T, Hu M, Ihm CH, Kristiansson K, Macarthur DG, Macdonald JR,
Onyiah I, Pang AW, Robson S, Stirrups K, Valsesia A, Walter K, Wei J; Wellcome Trust
Case Control Consortium, Tyler-Smith C, Carter NP, Lee C, Scherer SW, Hurles ME (2010)
Origins and functional impact of copy number variation in the human genome. Nature,
464(7289):704–12. Epub 2009 Oct 7
Crespi B, Stead P, Elliot M (2010) Evolution in health and medicine Sackler colloquium:
Comparative genomics of autism and schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 107 Suppl
1:1736–41. Epub 2009 Dec 1
Diskin SJ, Li M, Hou C, Yang S, Glessner J, Hakonarson H, Bucan M, Maris JM, Wang K (2008)
Adjustment of genomic waves in signal intensities from whole-genome SNP genotyping
platforms. Nucleic Acids Res, 36(19):e126. Epub 2008 Sep 10
Dobyns WB, Reiner O, Carrozzo R, Ledbetter DH (1993) Lissencephaly. A human brain
malformation associated with deletion of the LIS1 gene located at chromosome 17p13. JAMA
270:2838–2842
Elia J, Devoto M (2007) ADHD genetics: 2007 update. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 9:434–439
Elia J, Gai X, Xie HM, Perin JC, Geiger E, Glessner JT, D’arcy M, deBerardinis R, Frackelton E,
Kim C, Lantieri F, Muganga BM, Wang L, Takeda T, Rappaport EF, Grant SF, Berrettini W,
Devoto M, Shaikh TH, Hakonarson H, White PS (2010) Rare structural variants found in
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder are preferentially associated with neurodevelopmental
genes. Mol Psychiatry. 15(6):637–46. Epub 2009 Jun 23
Fernandez T et al (2008) Disruption of Contactin 4 (CNTN4) results in developmental delay and
other features of 3p deletion syndrome. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 82:1385
Fishman I, Yam A, Bellugi U, Lincoln A, Mills D (2010) Contrasting patterns of language-
associated brain activity in autism and Williams syndrome. Social Cognitive and Affective
Neuroscience, Aug 27. [Epub ahead of print, doi:10.1093/scan/nsq075]
358 J. T. Glessner et al.
Fodor SP, Read JL, Pirrung MC, Stryer L, Lu AT, Solas D (1991) Light-directed, spatially
addressable parallel chemical synthesis. Science 251:767–773
Francke U (1999) Williams syndrome: genes and mechanisms. Hum Mol Genet 8:1947–1954
Friedman JI, Vrijenhoek T, Markx S, Janssen IM, van der Vliet WA et al (2008) CNTNAP2 gene
dosage variation is associated with schizophrenia and epilepsy. Mol. Psychiatry 13:261–266
Garbern JY (2006) Pelizaeus–Merzbacher disease: Genetic and cellular pathogenesis. Cell Mol
Life Sci: CMLS 64(1):50–65
Geschwind DH, Levitt P (2007) Autism spectrum disorders: developmental disconnection
syndromes. Current Opin Neurobiol 17(1):103–111
Gillberg C, Steffenburg S, Wahlström J, Gillberg IC, Sjöstedt A, Martinsson T, Liedgren S,
Eeg-Olofsson O (1991) Autism associated with marker chromosome. J Am Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 30(3):489–494
Glessner JT, Hakonarson H (2009) Common variants in polygenic schizophrenia. Genome Biol,
10(9):236. Epub 2009 Sep 29
Glessner JT, Reilly MP, Kim CE, Takahashi N, Albano A, Hou C, Bradfield JP, Zhang H,
Sleiman PM, Flory JH, Imielinski M, Frackelton EC, Chiavacci R, Thomas KA, Garris M,
Otieno FG, Davidson M, Weiser M, Reichenberg A, Davis KL, Friedman JI, Cappola TP,
Margulies KB, Rader DJ, Grant SF, Buxbaum JD, Gur RE, Hakonarson H (2010) Strong
synaptic transmission impact by copy number variations in schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA, 107(23):10584–10589. Epub 2010 May 20
Glessner JT, Wang K, Cai G, Korvatska O, Kim CE et al (2009) Autism genomewide copy
number variation reveals ubiquitin and neuronal genes. Nature 459:569–573
Greenberg F, Guzzetta V, Montes de Oca-Luna R, Magenis RE, Smith AC, Richter SF, Kondo I,
Dobyns WB, Patel PI, Lupski JR (1991) Molecular analysis of the Smith-Magenis syndrome:
a possible contiguous-gene syndrome associated with del(17)(p11.2). Am. J. Hum. Genet,
49:1207–1218
Inoue K, Osaka H, Thurston VC, Clarke JTR, Yoneyama A, Rosenbarker L, Bird TD, Hodes ME,
Shaffer LG, Lupski JR (2002) Genomic rearrangements resulting in PLP1 deletion occur by
nonhomologous end joining and cause different dysmyelinating phenotypes in males and
females. Am. J. Hum. Genet 71:838–853
International Schizophrenia Consortium, Purcell SM, Wray NR, Stone JL, Visscher PM,
O’Donovan MC, Sullivan PF, Sklar P (2009) Common polygenic variation contributes to risk
of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature, 460(7256):748–752. Epub 2009 Jul 1
International Schizophrenia Consortium, Stone JL, O’Donovan MC, Gurling H, Kirov GK et al
(2008) Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications increase risk of schizophrenia. Nature
455:237–241
Korn JM et al (2008) Integrated genotype calling and association analysis of SNPs, common copy
number polymorphisms and rare CNVs. Nat Genet 40:1253–1260
Karayiorgou M, Simon TJ, Gogos JA (2010) 22q11.2 microdeletions: linking DNA structural
variation to brain dysfunction and schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 11(6):402–416
Kidd JM, Cooper GM, Donahue WF, Hayden HS, Sampas N, Graves T, Hansen N, Teague B,
Alkan C, Antonacci F, Haugen E, Zerr T, Yamada NA, Tsang P, Newman TL, Tüzün E,
Cheng Z, Ebling HM, Tusneem N, David R, Gillett W, Phelps KA, Weaver M, Saranga D,
Brand A, Tao W, Gustafson E, McKernan K, Chen L, Malig M, Smith JD, Korn JM,
McCarroll SA, Altshuler DA, Peiffer DA, Dorschner M, Stamatoyannopoulos J, Schwartz D,
Nickerson DA, Mullikin JC, Wilson RK, Bruhn L, Olson MV, Kaul R, Smith DR, Eichler EE
(2008) Mapping and sequencing of structural variation from eight human genomes. Nature
453(7191):56–64
Kim HG et al (2008) Disruption of neurexin 1 associated with autism spectrum disorder. Am.
J. Hum. Genet 82:199–207
Kirov G, Gumus D, Chen W, Norton N, Georgieva L et al (2007) Comparative genome
hybridization suggests a role for NRXN1 and APBA2 in schizophrenia. Hum. Mol. Genet.
17(3):458–465
Rare Genomic Deletions and Duplications 359
Roohi J et al (2008) Disruption of contactin 4 in three subjects with autism spectrum disorder.
J Med Genet 46:176–182
Schiff M, Delahaye A, Andrieux J, Sanlaville D et al (2010) Further delineation of the 17p13.3
microdeletion involving YWHAE but distal to PAFAH1B1: Four additional patients. Eur J
Med Genet 53(5):303–308
Schork NJ, Murray SS, Frazer KA, Topol EJ (2009) Common vs. rare allele hypotheses for
complex diseases. Curr Opin Genet Dev 19(3):212–219
Sebat J, Lakshmi B, Malhotra D, Troge J, Lese-Martin C, Walsh T, Yamrom B, Yoon S, Krasnitz
A, Kendall J, Leotta A, Pai D, Zhang R, Lee YH, Hicks J, Spence SJ, Lee AT, Puura K,
Lehtimaki T, Ledbetter D, Gregersen PK, Bregman J, Sutcliffe JS, Jobanputra V, Chung W,
Warburton D, King MC, Skuse D, Geschwind DH, Gilliam TC, Ye K, Wigler M (2007)
Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with autism. Sci 316:445–449
Shaikh TH, Kurahashi H, Emanuel BS (2001) Evolutionarily conserved low copy repeats (LCRs)
in 22q11 mediate deletions, duplications, translocations, and genomic instability: an update
and literature review. Genet Med 3(1):6–13
Shashi V, Keshavan MS, Howard TD, Berry MN, Basehore MJ, Lewandowski E, Kwapil TR
(2006) Cognitive correlates of a functional COMT polymorphism in children with 22q11.2
deletion syndrome. Clin Genet 69:234–238
Shi YY, He G, Zhang Z, Tang W, Zhang J Jr et al (2008) A study of rare structural variants in
schizophrenia patients and normal controls from Chinese Han population. Mol Psychiatry
13:911–913
Skuse DH (2007) Rethinking the nature of genetic vulnerability to autistic spectrum disorders.
Trends in Genetics 23(8):387–395
Steemers FJ, Gunderson KL (2007) Whole genome genotyping technologies on the BeadArray
platform. Biotechnol J 2:41–49
Stefansson H, Rujescu D, Cichon S, Pietiläinen OP, Ingason A et al (2008) Large recurrent
microdeletions associated with Schizophrenia. Nature 455:232–236
Walsh T, McClellan JM, McCarthy SE, Addington AM, Pierce SB et al (2008) Rare Structural
Variants Disrupt Multiple Genes in Neurodevelopmental Pathways in Schizophrenia. Science
320:539–543
Wang K, Zhang H, Ma D, Bucan M, Glessner JT et al (2009) Common genetic variants on 5p14.1
associate with autism spectrum disorders. Nature 459:528–533
Wang K et al (2007) PennCNV: an integrated hidden Markov model designed for high-resolution
copy number variation detection in whole-genome SNP genotyping data. Genome Res
17:1665–1674
Weiss LA, Arking DE (2009) Gene Discovery Project of Johns Hopkins & the Autism
Consortium, Daly MJ, Chakravarti A. A genome-wide linkage and association scan reveals
novel loci for autism. Nature, 461(7265):802–808
Weiss LA et al (2008) Association between microdeletion and microduplication at 16p11.2 and
autism. N Engl J Med 358:667–675
Xu B, Roos JL, Levy S, van Rensburg EJ, Gogos JA et al (2008) Strong association of de novo
copy number mutations with sporadic schizophrenia. Nat Genet 40:880–885
Toward a Mechanistic Understanding
of How Variability in Neurobiology
Shapes Individual Differences in Behavior
Keywords Neurogenetics Amygdala Striatum Threat Reward Aggression
Stress
Contents
1 Introduction........................................................................................................................ 362
2 Trait Anxiety, the Amygdala and Serotonin .................................................................... 364
3 Aggression, the Amygdala and Testosterone ................................................................... 368
4 Impulsivity, the Ventral Striatum and Dopamine ............................................................ 371
5 Endocannabinoids, Threat- and Reward-Related Brain Functions.................................. 373
6 Stress, the HPA Axis and the Mineralocorticoid Receptor ............................................. 377
7 Summary and Future Directions ....................................................................................... 380
References................................................................................................................................ 384
1 Introduction
Fig. 1 Trait anxiety is indirectly predicted by HTR1A genotype (rs6295) through amygdala
reactivity (adapted from Fakra et al 2009; Hariri 2009). Lines are labeled with unstandardized
path coefficients and standard errors in parentheses. Bolded coefficients outside of the lines
represent values from the trimmed model. Unbolded coefficients presented internally represent
values from the full model with all paths included. Significant indirect effects of HTR1A genotype
on trait anxiety were observed (ab = -1.60, SE = 0.73, P \ 0.05) while direct effects were
nonsignificant and dropped from the model. E1 and e2 represent the residual variances not
explained by variables included in the model. *P \ 0.05, **P \ 0.01
Patients with borderline personality disorder, who are prone to engage in reactive
aggression, demonstrate relatively increased amygdala reactivity to facial
expressions depicting threat (Mauchnik and Schmahl 2010) and decreased
amygdala-PFC coupling (New et al. 2007). Finally, spousal abusers characterized
by reactive (but not proactive) aggression display heightened amygdala reactivity
and also demonstrate attentional biases for aggressive words (Lee et al. 2008;
Chan et al. 2010).
Studies in non-clinical samples indicate that even normal variation in constructs
linked to aggressive behavior maps onto variability in threat-related neural
responses. For instance, Beaver et al. (2008) reported that individual differences in
approach motivation, a construct linked to reactive aggression (Harmon-Jones
2003), were positively correlated with amygdala reactivity to angry facial
expressions. Other research has found that individual differences in approach
motivation were associated with decreased ventral ACC-amygdala coupling dur-
ing processing of angry facial expressions (Passamonti et al. 2008). Given the
important role of highly interconnected prefrontal regions (e.g., ventral ACC,
OFC) in mediating top-down regulation of amygdala driven emotional reactivity
(see Davidson et al. 2000 for review), such decreased functional coupling may, in
part, explain the positive link between approach motivation and aggressive
behavior. Also, individual differences in trait anger are positively correlated with
amygdala reactivity to angry faces, but only among men with relatively elevated
trait anxiety scores (Carré et al. in press-a). Finally, in a more direct test of the
hypothesis that amygdala reactivity to facial cues of threat represents a neuro-
biological marker for reactive aggression, we have found that variation in self-
reported physical aggression is positively correlated with amygdala reactivity to
neutral and angry facial expression in two independent samples of healthy men
(Carré et al. in press-b). Together, these findings converge to suggest that amyg-
dala hyper-reactivity and/or decreased amygdala-OFC coupling during processing
of threat-related stimuli may represent a distinct neural signature for one’s pro-
pensity to engage in reactive aggression (Siever 2008). It is important to note that
individuals characterized by callous-unemotional traits and proactive aggression
(e.g., conduct disorder, psychopathy) display amygdala and OFC hypo-reactivity
to facial signals of threat (see Blair 2010 for review).
As described above, it is important to consider the underlying molecular sub-
strates that give rise to individual variation in threat-related amygdala reactivity
and amygdala-OFC coupling. Testosterone, the end-product of the hypothalamic
pituitary gonadal axis, is one prime candidate. The physiological effects of
testosterone occur mainly through binding to intra-cellular steroid hormone
receptors (i.e., androgen and estrogen receptors) to ultimately influence gene
transcription. Importantly, androgen (and estrogen) receptors are abundantly
located in the amygdala and interconnected limbic structures involved in medi-
ating aggressive behavior (see Newman 1999; Simon 2002 for reviews). Thus,
through stimulation of steroid hormone receptors and subsequent modulation of
cell function (see Adkins-Regan 2005 for review) testosterone can influence the
functioning of neural circuits implicated in the expression of human aggression.
370 R. Bogdan et al.
which aggressive behavior is measured directly during fMRI are needed to confirm
the causal role of testosterone in modulating human aggressive and threat-related
neural responses.
signaling, amygdala reactivity and trait anxiety, the selection of our candidate
polymorphism was driven by available in vitro and/or in vivo assays demonstrating
significant impact of the variant on aspects of biological function related to DA
neurotransmission and not on available data from association studies with behav-
ioral (e.g., impulsivity) or clinical (e.g., alcoholism) phenotypes. While association
studies are necessary for understanding the ultimate contribution of genetic poly-
morphisms to variability in behavioral and clinical phenomena, they do not readily
allow for inferences regarding polymorphic effects on gene or protein function.
Such inferences are instrumental for the development of biologically plausible and
tractable hypotheses regarding the impact of genetic variation on inter-individual
variability in brain function and associated behaviors such as those pursued in our
current work (Hariri et al. 2006b; Hariri and Weinberger 2003).
The dopamine transporter is responsible for the active clearance of synaptic DA
and, thus, plays a critical role in regulating the duration of postsynaptic DA
signaling, especially in the striatum (Sesack et al. 1998). Accumulating evidence
indicates that a 40-base pair variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) poly-
morphism in the 30 untranslated region of the DAT gene (SLC6A3) impacts the
expression and availability of DAT (Bannon et al. 2001). Although a genotype
effect has not been consistently observed across all studies (Martinez et al. 2001;
Michelhaugh et al. 2001; Mill et al. 2005; van Dyck et al. 2005), several suggest
that in comparison to the 9-repeat allele, the 10-repeat is associated with relatively
increased levels of DAT both in vivo (Cheon et al. 2005; Heinz et al. 2000) and in
vitro (Mill et al. 2002; Van Ness et al. 2005). We hypothesized that there would
be relatively greater VS reactivity associated with the 9-repeat allele, which is
linked with reduced DAT expression and presumably greater striatal synaptic DA,
in comparison with the 10-repeat allele. Consistent with our hypothesis, the DAT1
9-repeat allele was associated with relatively greater VS reactivity and accounted
for nearly 12% of the inter-individual variability. In contrast, genetic variation
directly affecting DA signaling only in the prefrontal cortex (i.e., COMT
Val158Met) was not associated with variability in VS reactivity. These results
highlight an important role for a genetic polymorphism affecting striatal DA
neurotransmission in mediating inter-individual differences in reward-related VS
reactivity. They further suggest that altered VS reactivity may represent a key
neurobiological pathway through which these polymorphisms contribute to vari-
ability in behavioral impulsivity and related risk for substance use disorders.
physiological (Calignano et al. 1998; Meng et al. 1998) and advanced behavioral
responses (Maldonado et al. 2006; Scherma et al. 2008; Viveros et al. 2005).
Experimental manipulation of these mechanisms has revealed significant behav-
ioral effects, especially in threat- and reward-related domains, which are generally
consistent with the effects of Cannabis intoxication, which are largely driven by
the constituent chemical D9-tetrahydrocannabinol (Robson 2005). The elucidation
of molecular mechanisms regulating eCB signaling, akin to that for serotonin and
dopamine, has motivated attempts to understand its possible contribution to the
emergence of variability in brain circuit function and related individual differences
in behavioral attributes (e.g., anxious or impulsive temperament) associated with
increased risk for psychiatric disorders.
After their biosynthesis from arachidonic acid, eCBs such as anandamide
(AEA) and 2-arachidonoylglycerol (2-AG) typically modulate synaptic neuro-
transmission through stimulation of CB1, the principal CNS cannabinoid receptor
widely expressed on multiple neuronal subtypes and their distributed circuitries.
In turn, the duration and intensity of eCB signaling, especially for AEA, is regu-
lated by two complementary mechanisms: enzymatic degradation via fatty acid
amide hydrolase (Cravatt et al. 1996) and active synaptic clearance via the AEA
transporter (Piomelli et al. 1999). The psychotropic and THC-like effects of AEA,
however, appear to be coupled with fatty acid amide hydrolase (FAAH), but not
AEA transporter function (Solinas et al. 2007). Thus, FAAH, an integral membrane
enzyme, may uniquely regulate behaviorally relevant eCB signaling by mediating
the hydrolytic breakdown of AEA into arachidonic acid and ethanolamine.
Again, common genetic variation (i.e., polymorphisms) affecting the functioning
of components involved in eCB neurotransmission (e.g., AEA, CB1, FAAH) may
represent a significant potential source of inter-individual variability in eCB sig-
naling that mediates emergent differences in emotion- and reward-related behaviors
(Onaivi et al. 2002). Because of its critical role in regulating the signaling duration
and intensity of AEA (Cravatt et al. 1996), and its selective contribution to the
psychotropic effects of AEA (Solinas et al. 2007), we have recently examined the
neurobiological and behavioral effects of a common functional nonsynonymous
SNP resulting in the conversion of a conserved proline residue to threonine (P129T)
in the amino acid sequence of FAAH (Hariri et al. 2009). In vitro, FAAH 385A is
associated with normal catalytic properties, but reduced cellular expression of
FAAH, possibly through enhanced sensitivity to proteolytic degradation (Chiang
et al. 2004; Sipe et al. 2002). Moreover, the C385A is the only common mutation in
FAAH (Flanagan et al. 2006) and the 385A, which putatively augments AEA sig-
naling via decreased enzymatic degradation, has been associated with reward-
related pathologies including street drug use and problem drug/alcohol abuse, as
well as being overweight and obese (Flanagan et al. 2006; Sipe et al. 2002).
In animal models, both pharmacologic and genetic disruption of FAAH function
result in decreased anxiety-like behaviors, as well as increased consumption and
preference for ethanol (Basavarajappa et al. 2006; Blednov et al. 2007; Kathuria
et al. 2003; Moreira et al. 2008; Solinas et al. 2007). Moreover, a recent pharma-
cologic fMRI study in human subjects has reported that acute oral administration of
Toward a Mechanistic Understanding 375
(c)
(a) (b)
(f)
(d) (e)
Fig. 2 Effects of FAAH genotype (rs324420) on threat- and reward-related brain activation
(adapted from Hariri et al. 2009; Hariri 2009). Statistical parametric maps displaying the
correlation between threat-related amygdala reactivity and trait anxiety in a FAAH 385A carriers
and b C385 homozygotes. c Plots of the correlation between threat-related amygdala reactivity
and trait anxiety according to FAAH C385 genotype. Statistical parametric maps displaying the
correlation between reward-related ventral striatal (VS) reactivity and delay discounting in d
FAAH 385A carriers and e C385 homozygotes (no significant correlation observed). f Plots of the
correlation between reward-related VS reactivity and delay discounting according to FAAH
C385A genotype. FAAH: fatty acid amide hydrolase
numerous complex interactions between signaling pathways and that more than
one pathway contributes to the regulation of any brain circuitry. For example, we
know that DA plays an important role in modulating amygdala function and
anxiety (Hariri et al. 2002a; Tessitore et al. 2002), and that 5-HT can influence
reward-related brain circuitry and impulsivity (Manuck et al. 1998). However,
existing studies lack the power and sophistication to model such complex inter-
actions while effectively controlling for other important modulatory factors (e.g.,
age, gender, stress exposure) in the context of BOLD fMRI, pharmacologic fMRI
or multimodal PET/fMRI protocols. To do so, we must aggressively expand the
scale and scope of our studies to include hundreds and, preferably, thousands of
subjects. This will afford opportunities to effectively examine interactions between
signaling pathways (e.g., 5-HT and DA) on brain function and behavior through
modeling of multiple functional polymorphisms (e.g., HTR1A -1019 and DAT1),
and examine the effects of genetically driven variation in signaling pathways on
multiple behaviorally relevant brain circuitries.
A second important consideration is that existing studies have been largely
conducted in ethnically and racially homogenous populations. Thus, the observed
effects may not generalize to other populations. This is especially true of studies
utilizing functional genetic polymorphisms because the potential effect of any
single genetic variant on a complex biological and behavioral phenotype is likely
to be small against the background of the approximately 20,000–25,000 human
genes and the multitude of other neurobiologically relevant functional variants
they likely harbor. In fact, we have already seen that the well-replicated effects of a
common functional polymorphism affecting 5-HT signaling on amygdala reac-
tivity in Caucasian subjects may be reversed in those of Asian ancestry (Lee and
Ham 2008; Munafo et al. 2008). Importantly, our most recent studies have
experimentally controlled for occult genetic stratification independent of self-
reported race or ethnicity as well as the independence of the target genotype from
other functional polymorphisms impacting the brain functions under study. While
such efforts allow for the attribution of emergent variability in brain and behavior
to the candidate variant of interest and not to other possible polymorphisms or
more general differences between genotype groups in genetic background, it is
important to explicitly test the independence of functional polymorphisms through
rigorous statistical modeling in larger samples and also to test the validity of any
associations derived in one sample population (e.g., Caucasian) to populations
with different genetic backgrounds (e.g., Asian or African).
A third important consideration for the future of this research is the need to
conduct large-scale prospective studies beginning in childhood to determine any
developmental shifts in neurogenetic pathways mediating individual differences in
behavior as well as their predictive utility in identifying risk for psychopathology
as a function of environmental or other stressors. All of the studies described
above and most of the studies available in the literature as a whole have been
conducted in adults carefully screened for the absence of psychopathology.
Because of this, these findings identify mechanisms contributing to variability in
the normative range of behavior only. The utility of these markers of individual
382 R. Bogdan et al.
Difficulties Schedule; Brown and Harris 1978; Monroe 2008; Williamson et al.
2003). Furthermore, in light of theoretical arguments that genotypes associated with
adversity may reflect plasticity to the environment and not just vulnerability (e.g.,
Belsky et al. 2009; Manuck 2010), it is critical for these assessments to also assess
positive effects of the environment such as social support (e.g., Hyde et al. 2011).
Finally, there is tremendous potential in developing large databases (again
preferably thousands of subjects) with detailed measures of behavioral traits,
neuroimaging-based measures of multiple brain circuitries and extensive geno-
typing. One of the most exciting applications of molecular genetics is in identi-
fying novel biological pathways contributing to the emergence of complex traits
(Gibson and Goldstein 2007; McCarthy et al. 2008). The continued refinement of a
detailed map of sequence variation across the entire human genome (i.e., SNPs
that ‘‘tag’’ every gene) and production of technologies supporting efficient high-
throughput identification of such variation in individuals have dramatically
accelerated the discovery of genes involved in the emergence of complex disease
processes (Fellay et al. 2007; Link et al. 2008) as well as normal variability in
continuous traits (Lettre et al. 2008). Many of the genes identified in such studies
have illuminated novel pathways not previously implicated in these processes or
traits, spurring intensive efforts to understand the potential biological effects of the
proteins produced by these genes. As such, these ‘‘genome-wide’’ screens repre-
sent an opportunity to leap forward beyond the available pool of candidate mol-
ecules and pathways in parsing the mechanisms of complex biological processes.
Because neuroimaging-based measures of brain function reveal key mechanisms
involved in the emergence of individual differences in behavioral traits and are
closer to the biological effects of functional genetic polymorphisms, they are ideal
substrates for genome-wide screens. For example, BOLD fMRI estimates of
amygdala reactivity predicting variability in trait anxiety can be used as the
continuous trait in a genome-wide screen. Any significant associations that emerge
between genetic variation and amygdala reactivity may confirm existing rela-
tionships (e.g., the importance of genes biasing 5-HT signaling) or, more impor-
tantly, reveal unexpected candidate molecules or pathways (e.g., a gene producing
a molecule that is expressed in the brain and may function in second-messenger
signaling cascades). Once identified and, ideally, replicated in large-scale
databases that effectively address confounds common to genome-wide screens
(e.g., controlling for multiple comparisons resulting from testing the association
of a phenotype with hundreds of thousands or millions of SNPs), the impact of
variation in novel genes associated with amygdala reactivity can be explored at
each level of the biological cascade leading to trait anxiety (i.e., be fed back into
the discovery loop outlined in the introduction). In addition to exponentially
improving our understanding of neurobiological pathways leading to individual
differences in complex behavioral traits these efforts may lead to the discovery of
novel therapeutic strategies targeting related disease processes.
References
Abercrombie HC, Giese-Davis J, Sephton S, Epel ES, Turner-Cobb JM, Spiegel D (2004)
Flattened cortisol rhythms in metastic breast cancer patients. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29:
1082–1092
Adkins-Regan E (2005) Hormones and animal social behavior. Princeton University Press,
Princeton
Alessi SM, Petry NM (2003) Pathological gambling severity is associated with impulsivity in a
delay discounting procedure. Behav Process 64:345–354
Amat J, Matus-Amat P, Watkins LR, Maier SF (1998) Escapable and inescapable stress
differentially alter extracellular levels of 5-HT in the basolateral amygdala of the rat. Brain
Res 812:113–120
Amat J, Tamblyn JP, Paul ED, Bland ST, Amat P et al (2004) Microinjection of urocortin 2 into
the dorsal raphe nucleus activates serotonergic neurons and increases extracellular serotonin
in the basolateral amygdala. Neuroscience 129:509–519
Arai K, Nakagomi Y, Iketani M, Shimura Y, Amemiya S, Ohyama K, Shibasaki T (2003)
Functional polymorphisms in the mineralocorticoid receptor and amirolide-sensitive sodium
channel genes in a patient with sporadic pseudohypoaldosteronism. Hum Genet 112:91–97
Bannon MJ, Michelhaugh SK, Wang J, Sacchetti P (2001) The human dopamine transporter
gene: gene organization, transcriptional regulation, and potential involvement in neuropsy-
chiatric disorders. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 11:449–455
Baron RA, Richardson D (1994) Human aggression. Plenum, New York
Bartholome B, Spies CM, Gaber T, Schuchmann S, Kunkel D et al (2004) Membrane
glucocorticoid receptors (mGCR) are expressed in normal human peripheral blood
mononuclea cells and up-regulated after in vitro stimulation and in patients with rheumatoid
arthritis. FASEB J 18:70–80
Basavarajappa BS, Yalamanchili R, Cravatt BF, Cooper TB, Hungund BL (2006) Increased
ethanol consumption and preference and decreased ethanol sensitivity in female FAAH
knockout mice. Neuropharmacology 50:834–844
Beaver JD, Lawrence AD, Passamonti L, Calder AJ (2008) Appetitive motivation predicts the
neural response to facial signals of aggression. J Neurosci 28:2719–2725
Belsky J, Jonassaint C, Pluess M, Stanton M, Brummett B, Williams R (2009) Vulnerability
genes or plasticity genes? Mol Psychiatry 14:746–754
Berridge KC, Robinson TE (2003) Parsing reward. Trends Neurosci 26:507–513
Bickel WK, Odum AL, Madden GJ (1999) Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting
in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 146:447–454
Bigos KL, Pollock BG, Aizenstein H, Fisher PM, Bies RR, Hariri AR (2008) Acute 5-HT reuptake
blockade potentiates human amygdala reactivity. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:3221–3225
Binder (2009) The role of FKBP5, a co-chaperone of the glucocorticoid receptor in the pathogenesis
and therapy of affective and anxiety disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(Suppl 1),
S186–195
Binder EB, Nemeroff CB (2010) The CRF system, stress, depression and anxiety-insights from
human genetic studies. Mol Psychiatry 15:574–588
Bishop SJ, Duncan J, Lawrence AD (2004) State anxiety modulation of the amygdala response to
unattended threat-related stimuli. J Neurosci 24:10364–10368
Blair RJ (2010) Neuroimaging of psychopathy and antisocial behavior: a targeted review. Curr
Psychiatry Rep 12:76–82
Blednov YA, Cravatt BF, Boehm SL 2nd, Walker D, Harris RA (2007) Role of endocannabinoids
in alcohol consumption and intoxication: studies of mice lacking fatty acid amide hydrolase.
Neuropsychopharmacology 32:1570–1582
Bogdan R, Perlis RH, Fagerness J, Pizzagalli DA (2010) The impact of mineralocorticoid
receptor iso/val genotype (rs5522) and stress on reward learning. Genes Brain Behav 9:
658–667
Toward a Mechanistic Understanding 385
Bogdan R, Williamson DE, Hariri AR (in press) Mineralocorticoid receptor iso/val genotype
(rs5522) moderates the association between priori childhood emotional neglect and amygdala
reactivity. Am J Psychiatry
Brady LS, Whitfield HJ, Fox RJ, Gold PW, Herkenham M (1991) Long-term antidepressant
administration alters corticotropin-releasing hormone, tyrosine hydroxylase, and mineralo-
corticoid receptor gene expression in rat brain. Therapeutic implications. J Clin Invest
87:831–837
Brown GW, Harris TO (1978) Social origins of depression: a study of psychiatric disorder in
women. Tavistock, London
Buchanan TW, Driscoll D, Mowrer SM, Sollers JJ, Thayer JF et al (2010) Medial prefrontal
cortex damage affects physiological and psychological stress responses differently in men and
women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35:56–66
Burghardt NS, Sullivan GM, McEwen BS, Gorman JM, LeDoux JE (2004) The selective
serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram increases fear after acute treatment but reduces fear
with chronic treatment: a comparison with tianeptine. Biol Psychiatry 55:1171–1178
Burghardt NS, Bush DEA, McEwen BS, LeDoux JE (2007) Acute SSRIs increase conditioned
fear expression: blockade with a 5-HT2C receptor antagonist. Biol Psychiatry 62:1111–1118
Burns HD, Van Laere K, Sanabria-Bohorquez S, Hamill TG, Bormans G et al (2007) [18F]MK-
9470, a positron emission tomography (PET) tracer for in vivo human PET brain imaging of
the cannabinoid-1 receptor. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:9800–9805
Calignano A, La Rana G, Giuffrida A, Piomelli D (1998) Control of pain initiation by endogenous
cannabinoids. Nature 394:277–281
Cardinal RN, Winstanley CA, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ (2004) Limbic corticostriatal systems and
delayed reinforcement. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1021:33–50
Carré JM, Fisher PM, Manuck SB, Hariri AR (in press-a) Interaction between trait anxiety and
trait anger predict predict amygdala reactivity to angry faces in men but not women. Soc Cogn
Affect Neurosci
Carré JM, McCormick CM (2008) Aggressive behaviour and change in salivary testosterone
concentrations predict willingness to engage in a competitive task. Horm Beh 54:403–409
Carré JM, Murphy KR, Hariri AR (in press-b) What lies beneath the face of aggression. Soc Cogn
Affect Neurosci
Carré JM, Putnam SK, McCormick CM (2009) Testosterone responses to competition predict
future aggressive behaviour at a cost to reward in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 343:
561–570
Carré JM, Gilchrist JG, Morrissey MD, McCormick CM (2010) Motivational and situations
factors and the relationship between testosterone dynamics and human aggression during
competition. Biol Psychol 84:346–353
Caspi A, Moffitt TE (2006) Gene-environment interactions in psychiatry: joining forces with
neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:583–590
Chakrabarti B, Kent L, Suckling J, Bullmore E, Baron-Cohen S (2006) Variations in the human
cannabinoid receptor (CNR1) gene modulate striatal responses to happy faces. Eur J Neurosci
23:1944–1948
Chamberlain NL, Driver ED, Miesfeld RL (1994) The length and location of CAG trinucleotide
repeats in the androgen receptor N-terminal dsomain affect transactivation function. Nucleic
Acids Res 22:3181–3186
Chan SC, Raine A, Lee TM (2010) Attentional bias toward negative affective stimuli and reactive
aggression in male batterers. Psychiatry Res 176:246–249
Cheon KA, Ryu YH, Kim JW, Cho DY (2005) The homozygosity for 10-repeat allele at
dopamine transporter gene and dopamine transporter density in Korean children with attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder: relating to treatment response to methylphenidate. Eur
Neuropsychopharmacol 15:95–101
Chiang KP, Gerber AL, Sipe JC, Cravatt BF (2004) Reduced cellular expression and activity of
the P129T mutant of human fatty acid amide hydrolase: evidence for a link between defects in
the endocannabinoid system and problem drug use. Hum Mol Genet 13:2113–2119
386 R. Bogdan et al.
Choong CS, Wilson EM (1998) Trinucleotide repeats in the human androgen receptor: a
molecular basis for disease. J Mol Endocrinol 21:235–257
Coccaro EF, McCloskey MS, Fitzgerald DA, Phan KL (2007) Amygdala and orbitofrontal
reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression. Biol Psychiatry 62:
168–178
Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Miller GE (2007) Psychological stress and disease. JAMA 298:
1685–1687
Cooney RE, Atlas LY, Joormann J, Eugene F, Gotlib IH (2006) Amygdala activation in the
processing of neutral faces in social anxiety disorder: is neutral really neutral? Psychiatry Res
148:55–59
Cowen PJ, Power AC, Ware CJ, Anderson IM (1994) 5-HT1A receptor sensitivity in major
depression.A neuroendocrine study with buspirone. Br J Psychiatry 164:372–379
Cravatt BF, Giang DK, Mayfield SP, Boger DL, Lerner RA, Gilula NB (1996) Molecular
characterization of an enzyme that degrades neuromodulatory fatty-acid amides. Nature
384:83–87
Czesak M, Lemonde S, Peterson EA, Rogaeva A, Albert PR (2006) Cell-specific repressor or
enhancer activities of Deaf-1 at a serotonin 1A receptor gene polymorphism. J Neurosci
26:1864–1871
David SP, Murthy NV, Rabiner EA, Munafo MR, Johnstone EC et al (2005) A functional genetic
variation of the serotonin (5-HT) transporter affects 5-HT1A receptor binding in humans.
J Neurosci 25:2586–2590
Davidson RJ, Putnam KM, Larson CL (2000) Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion
regulation—a possible prelude to violence. Science 289:591–594
de Wit H, Richards JB (2004) Dual determinants of drug use in humans: reward and impulsivity.
Nebr Symp Motiv 50:19–55
de Kloet ER, Joëls M, Holsboer F (2005) Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat
Rev Neurosci 6:463–475
De Wit H, Flory JD, Acheson A, McLoskey M, Manuck SB (2007) IQ and nonplanning
impulsivity are independently associated with delay discounting in middle-aged adults. Pers
Indiv Differ 42:111–121
Depue RA, Luciana M, Arbisi P, Collins P, Leon A (1994) Dopamine and the structure of
personality: relation of agonist-induced dopamine activity to positive emotionality. J Pers Soc
Psychol 67:485–498
DeRijk RH, Wüst S, Meijer OC, Zennaro MC, Federenko IS, Hellhammer DH, Giacchetti G,
Vreugdenhil E, Zitman FG, de Kloet ER (2006) A common polymorphism in the mineralo-
corticoid receptor modulates stress responsiveness. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 91:5083–5089
DeRijk RH, van Leeuwen N, Klok MD, Zitman FG (2008) Corticosteroid receptor-gene variants:
modulators of the stress-response and implications for mental health. Eur J Pharmacol
585:492–501
Derntl B, Windischberger C, Robinson S, Kryspin-Exner I, Gur RC, Moser E, Habel U (2009)
Amygdala activity to fear and anger in healthy young males is associated with testosterone.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 34:687–693
Desarnaud F, Cadas H, Piomelli D (1995) Anandamide amidohydrolase activity in rat brain
microsomes. J Biol Chem 270:6030–6035
Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME (2004) Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical
integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol Bull 130:355–391
Dickie EW, Armony JL (2008) Amygdala responses to unattended fearful faces: Interaction
between sex and trait anxiety. Psychiatry Res 162:51–57
Dodge KA, Coie JD (1987) Social-information processing factors in reactive and proactive
aggression in children’s peer groups. J Pers Soc Psychol 53:1146–1158
Etkin A, Klemenhagen KC, Dudman JT, Rogan MT, Hen R et al (2004) Individual differences in
trait anxiety predict the response of the basolateral amygdala to unconsciously processed
fearful faces. Neuron 44:1043–1055
Toward a Mechanistic Understanding 387
Evans KC, Wright CI, Wedig MM, Gold AL, Pollack MH, Rauch SL (2008) A functional MRI
study of amygdala responses to angry schematic faces in social anxiety disorder. Depress
Anxiety 25:496–505
Fakra E, Hyde LW, Gorka A, Fisher PM, Munoz KE et al (2009) Effects of HTR1A C(-1019)G
on amygdala reactivity and trait anxiety. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66:33–40
Fellay J, Shianna KV, Ge D, Colombo S, Ledergerber B et al (2007) A whole-genome association
study of major determinants for host control of HIV-1. Science 317:944–947
Fisher PM, Meltzer CC, Ziolko SK, Price JC, Hariri AR (2006) Capacity for 5-HT1A-mediated
autoregulation predicts amygdala reactivity. Nat Neurosci 9:1362–1363
Flanagan JM, Gerber AL, Cadet JL, Beutler E, Sipe JC (2006) The fatty acid amide hydrolase 385
A/A (P129T) variant: haplotype analysis of an ancient missense mutation and validation of
risk for drug addiction. Hum Genet 120:581–588
Forbes EE, Brown SM, Kimak M, Ferrell RE, Manuck SB, Hariri AR (2009) Genetic variation in
components of dopamine neurotransmission impacts ventral striatal reactivity associated with
impulsivity. Mol Psychiatry 14:60–70
Forster GL, Feng N, Watt MJ, Korzan WJ, Mouw NJ et al (2006) Corticotropin-releasing factor
in the dorsal raphe elicits temporally distinct serotonergic responses in the limbic system in
relation to fear behavior. Neuroscience 141:1047–1055
Fox HC, Wilker EH, Kreek MJ, Sinha R (2006) Reliability of salivary cortisol assessments in
cocaine dependent individuals. J Psychopharmacol 20:650–655
Gass P, Reichardt HM, Strekalova T, Henn F, Tronche F (2001) Mice with targetted mutations of
glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptors: models for depression and anxiety? Physiol
Behav 73:811–825
Gibbons JL, McHugh PR (1962) Plasma cortisol in depressive illness. J Psychiat Res 1:162–171
Gibson G, Goldstein DB (2007) Human genetics: the hidden text of genome-wide associations.
Curr Biol 17:R929–R932
Glahn DC, Lovallo WR, Fox PT (2007) Reduced amygdala activation in young adults at high risk
of alcoholism: studies from the Oklahoma family health patterns project. Biol Psychiatry
61:1306–1309
Green L, Myerson J (2004) A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic
rewards. Psychol Bull 130:769–792
Haas BW, Omura K, Constable RT, Canli T (2007) Emotional conflict and neuroticism:
personality-dependent activation in the amygdala and subgenual anterior cingulate. Behav
Neurosci 121:249–256
Hansenne M, Pitchot W, Pinto E, Reggers J, Scantamburlo G et al (2002) 5-HT1A dysfunction in
borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med 32:935–941
Hariri AR (2009) The neurobiology of individual differences in complex behavioral traits. Ann
Rev Neurosci 32:225–247
Hariri AR, Holmes A (2006) Genetics of emotional regulation: the role of the serotonin
transporter in neural function. Trends Cogn Sci 10:182–191
Hariri AR, Weinberger DR (2003) Imaging genomics. Br Med Bull 65:259–270
Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Fera F, Smith WG, Weinberger DR (2002a) Dextroamphetamine
modulates the response of the human amygdala. Neuropsychopharmacology 27:1036–1040
Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Kolachana B, Fera F et al (2002b) Serotonin transporter
genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science 297:400–403
Hariri AR, Brown SM, Williamson DE, Flory JD, de Wit H, Manuck SB (2006a) Preference for
immediate over delayed rewards is associated with magnitude of ventral striatal activity.
J Neurosci 26:13213–13217
Hariri AR, Drabant EM, Weinberger DR (2006b) Imaging genetics: perspectives from studies of
genetically driven variation in serotonin function and corticolimbic affective processing. Biol
Psychiatry 59:888–897
Hariri AR, Gorka A, Hyde LW, Kimak M, Halder I et al (2009) Divergent effects of genetic
variation in endocannabinoid signaling on human threat- and reward-related brain function.
Biol Psychiatry 66:9–16
388 R. Bogdan et al.
Harmon-Jones E (2003) Anger and the behavioural approach system. Pers Ind Diff 35:995–1005
Heinz A, Goldman D, Jones DW, Palmour R, Hommer D et al (2000) Genotype influences in vivo
dopamine transporter availability in human striatum. Neuropsychopharmacology 22:133–139
Hermans E, Ramsey M, van Honk J (2008) Exogenous testosterone enhances responsiveness to
social threat in the neural circuitry of social aggression in humans. Biol Psychiatry 63:263–270
Hyde LW, Gorka A, Manuck SB, Hariri AR (2011) Perceived social support moderates the link
between threat-related amygdala reactivity and trait anxiety. Neuropsychologia 49:651–656
Hyman SE, Malenka RC, Nestler EJ (2006) Neural mechanisms of addiction: the role of reward-
related learning and memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 29:565–598
Jahn AL, Fox AS, Abercrombie HC, Shelton SE, Oakes TR et al (2010) Subgenual prefrontal
cortex activity predicts individual differences in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity
across different contexts. Biol Psychiatry 67:175–181
Joëls M, Karst H, DeRijk R, de Kloet ER (2008) The coming out of the brain mineralocorticoid
receptor. Trends Neurosci 31:1–7
Johnstone T, Somerville LH, Alexander AL, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ et al (2005) Stability of
amygdala BOLD response to fearful faces over multiple scan sessions. Neuroimage 25:
1112–1123
Kalivas PW, Volkow ND (2005) The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and
choice. Am J Psychiatry 162:1403–1413
Karst H, Berger S, Turiault M, Tronche F, Schütz G, Joëls M (2006) Mineralocorticoid receptors
are indispensable for nongenomic modulation of hippocampal glutamate transmission by
corticosterone. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:19204–19207
Kathuria S, Gaetani S, Fegley D, Valino F, Duranti A et al (2003) Modulation of anxiety through
blockade of anandamide hydrolysis. Nat Med 9:76–81
Kelley AE (2004) Memory and addiction: shared neural circuitry and molecular mechanisms.
Neuron 44:161–179
Kern S, Oakes TR, Stone CK, McAuliff EM, Kirschbaum C, Davidson RJ (2008) Glucose
metabolic changes in the prefrontal cortex are associated with HPA axis response to a
psychosocial stressor. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33:517–529
Killgore WD, Yurgelun-Todd DA (2005) Social anxiety predicts amygdala activation in
adolescents viewing fearful faces. Neuroreport 16:1671–1675
Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK (1999) Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed
rewards than non-drug-using controls. J Exp Psychol Gen 128:78–87
Knutson B, Rick S, Wimmer GE, Prelec D, Loewenstein G (2007) Neural predictors of
purchases. Neuron 53:147–156
Kolber BJ, Muglia LJ (2009) Defining brain region-specific glucocorticoid action during stress by
conditional gene disruption in mice. Brain Res 1293:85–90
Kudielka BM, Hellhammer DH, Wüst S (2009) Why do we respond so differently? Reviewing
determinants of human salivary cortisol responses to challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology
34:2–18
Kuningas M, de Rijk RH, Westendorp RG, Jolle J, Slagboom PE, van Heemst D (2007) Mental
performance in old age dependent on cortisol and genetic variance in the mineralocorticoid
and glucocorticoid receptors. Neuropsychopharmacology 32:1295–1301
LeDoux JE (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:155–184
Lee BT, Ham BJ (2008) Serotonergic genes and amygdala activity in response to negative
affective facial stimuli in Korean women. Genes Brain Behav 7:899–905
Lee TM, Chan SC, Raine A (2008) Strong limbic and weak frontal activation to aggressive
stimuli in spouse abusers. Mol Psychiatry 13:655–656
Lemonde S, Turecki G, Bakish D, Du L, Hrdina PD et al (2003) Impaired repression at a 5-
hydroxytryptamine 1A receptor gene polymorphism associated with major depression and
suicide. J Neurosci 23:8788–8799
Lesch KP, Gutknecht L (2004) Focus on The 5-HT1A receptor: emerging role of a gene
regulatory variant in psychopathology and pharmacogenetics. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol
7:381–385
Toward a Mechanistic Understanding 389
Lettre G, Jackson AU, Gieger C, Schumacher FR, Berndt SI et al (2008) Identification of ten loci
associated with height highlights new biological pathways in human growth. Nat Genet 40:
584–591
Link E, Parish S, Armitage J, Bowman L, Heath S et al (2008) SLCO1B1 variants and statin-
induced myopathy—a genomewide study. N Engl J Med 359:789–799
LoVerme J, Gaetani S, Fu J, Oveisi F, Burton K, Piomelli D (2005) Regulation of food intake by
oleoylethanolamide. Cell Mol Life Sci 62:708–716
Madden GJ, Petry NM, Badger GJ, Bickel WK (1997) Impulsive and self-control choices in
opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: drug and monetary
rewards. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 5:256–262
Maier SF, Watkins LR (2005) Stressor controllability and learned helplessness: the roles of the
dorsal raphe nucleus, serotonin, and corticotropin-releasing factor. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
29:829–841
Maldonado R, Valverde O, Berrendero F (2006) Involvement of the endocannabinoid system in
drug addiction. Trends Neurosci 29:225–232
Manuck SB (2010) The reaction norm in gene x environment interaction. Mol Psychiatry
14:746–754
Manuck SB, Flory JD, McCaffery JM, Matthews KA, Mann JJ, Muldoon MF (1998) Aggression,
impulsivity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity in a nonpatient sample.
Neuropsychopharmacology 19:287–299
Manuck SB, Flory JD, Muldoon MF, Ferrell RE (2003) A neurobiology of intertemporal choice.
In: Loewenstein G, Read D, Baumeister RF (eds) Time and decision: economic and
psychological perspectives on intertemporal choice. Sage, New York, pp 139–172
Manuck SB, Brown SM, Forbes EE, Hariri AR (2007) Temporal stability of individual
differences in amygdala reactivity. Am J Psychiatry 164:1613–1614
Manuck SB, Marsland AL, Flory JD, Gorka A, Ferrell RE, Hariri AR (2010) Salivary
testosterone and a trinucleotide (CAG) length polymorphism in the androgen receptor gene
predict amygdala reactivity in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35:94–104
Marques AH, Silverman MN, Sternberg EM (2009) Glucocorticoid dysregulationss and their
clinical correlated: from receptors to therapeutics. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1179:1–18
Márquez C, Nadal R, Armario A (2005) Responsiveness of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
axis to different novel environments is a consistent individual trait in adult male outbred rats.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 30:179–188
Martinez D, Gelernter J, Abi-Dargham A, van Dyck CH, Kegeles L et al (2001) The variable
number of tandem repeats polymorphism of the dopamine transporter gene is not associated
with significant change in dopamine transporter phenotype in humans. Neuropsychopharma-
cology 24:553–560
Mauchnik J, Schmahl C (2010) The latest neuroimaging findings in borderline personality
disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep 12:46–55
McCarthy MI, Abecasis GR, Cardon LR, Goldstein DB, Little J et al (2008) Genome-wide
association studies for complex traits: consensus, uncertainty and challenges. Nat Rev Genet
9:356–369
McEwen BS, Gianaros PJ (2010) Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: links to
socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1186:190–222
McGowan PO, Sasaki A, D’Alessio AC, Gymov S, Labonté B et al (2009) Epigenetic regulation
of the glucocorticoid receptor in human brain associates with childhood abuse. Nat Neurosci
12:342–348
Mehta PH, Beer J (2010) Neural mechanisms of the testosterone-aggression relation: the role of
the orbito-frontal cortex. J Cog Neuro 22:2357–2368
Meng ID, Manning BH, Martin WJ, Fields HL (1998) An analgesia circuit activated by
cannabinoids. Nature 395:381–383
Menon M, Jensen J, Vitcu I, Graff-Guerrero A, Crawley A et al (2007) Temporal difference
modeling of the blood-oxygen level dependent response during aversive conditioning in
humans: effects of dopaminergic modulation. Biol Psychiatry 62:765–772
390 R. Bogdan et al.
Piomelli D (2003) The molecular logic of endocannabinoid signalling. Nat Rev Neurosci
4:873–884
Piomelli D, Beltramo M, Glasnapp S, Lin SY, Goutopoulos A et al (1999) Structural
determinants for recognition and translocation by the anandamide transporter. Proc Natl Acad
Sci U S A 96:5802–5807
Pruessner JC, Dedovic K, Pressner M, Lord C, Buss C et al (2010) Stress regulation in the central
nervous system: evidence from structural and functional neuroimaging studies in human
populations. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35:179–191
Raine A, Buchsbaum M, LaCasse L (1997) Brain abnormalities in murderers indicated by
positron emission tomography. Biol Psychiatry 42:495–508
Rajender S, Pandu G, Sharma JD, Gandhi KP, Singh L, Thangaraj K (2008) Reduced CAG
repeats length in androgen receptor gene is associated with violent criminal behavior. Int J
Legal Med 122:367–372
Ray RD, Ochsner KN, Cooper JC, Robertson ER, Gabrieli JD, Gross JJ (2005) Individual
differences in trait rumination and the neural systems supporting cognitive reappraisal. Cogn
Affect Behav Neurosci 5:156–168
Reul JM, Gesing A, Droste S, Stec IS, Weber A et al (2000) The brain mineralocorticoid receptor:
greedy for ligand, mysterious in function. Euro J Pharmacol 405:235–249
Rhodes RA, Murthy NV, Dresner MA, Selvaraj S, Stavrakakis N et al (2007) Human 5-HT
transporter availability predicts amygdala reactivity in vivo. J Neurosci 27:9233–9237
Robson P (2005) Human studies of cannabinoids and medicinal cannabis. Handb Exp Pharmacol
168:719–756
Rodrigues SM, LeDoux JE, Sapolsky RM (2009) The influence of stress hormones on fear
circuitry. Annu Rev Neurosci 32:289–313
Roney JR, Simmons ZL, Lukaszewski AW (2009) Androgen receptor gene sequence and basal
cortisol concentrations predict men’s hormonal responses to potential mates. Proc Soc Biol
277:57–63
Rozeboom AM, Akil H, Seasholtz AF (2007) Mineralocorticoid receptor overexpression in
forebrain decreases anxiety-like behavior and alters the stress response in mice. Proc Natl
Acad of Sci U S A 104:4688–4693
Sadikot AF, Parent A (1990) The monoaminergic innervation of the amygdala in the squirrel
monkey: an immunohistochemical study. Neuroscience 36:431–447
Scherma M, Medalie J, Fratta W, Vadivel SK, Makriyannis A et al (2008) The endogenous
cannabinoid anandamide has effects on motivation and anxiety that are revealed by fatty acid
amide hydrolase (FAAH) inhibition. Neuropharmacology 54:129–140
Sesack SR, Hawrylak VA, Guido MA, Levey AI (1998) Cellular and subcellular localization of
the dopamine transporter in rat cortex. Adv Pharmacol 42:171–174
Sharp T, Boothman L, Raley J, Queree P (2007) Important messages in the ‘post’: recent
discoveries in 5-HT neurone feedback control. Trends Pharmacol Sci 28:629–636
Siessmeier T, Kienast T, Wrase J, Larsen JL, Braus DF et al (2006) Net influx of plasma 6-
[18F]fluoro-l-DOPA (FDOPA) to the ventral striatum correlates with prefrontal processing of
affective stimuli. Eur J Neurosci 24:305–313
Siever LJ (2008) Neurobiology of aggression and violence. Am J Psychiatry 165:429–442
Simon NG (2002) Hormonal processes in the development and expression of aggressive
behavior. In: Pfaff DW et al (eds) Hormones, brain and behavior. Academic Press, San Diego
Simpson CA, Vuchinich RE (2000) Reliability of a measure of temporal discounting. Psychol
Rec 50:3–16
Sipe JC, Chiang K, Gerber AL, Beutler E, Cravatt BF (2002) A missense mutation in human fatty
acid amide hydrolase associated with problem drug use. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:
8394–8399
Solinas M, Tanda G, Justinova Z, Wertheim CE, Yasar S et al (2007) The endogenous
cannabinoid anandamide produces delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol-like discriminative and
neurochemical effects that are enhanced by inhibition of fatty acid amide hydrolase but not
by inhibition of anandamide transport. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 321:370–380
392 R. Bogdan et al.
Somerville LH, Kim H, Johnstone T, Alexander AL, Whalen PJ (2004) Human amygdala
responses during presentation of happy and neutral faces: correlations with state anxiety. Biol
Psychiatry 55:897–903
Sorice-Gomez E, Matias I, Rueda-Orozco PE, Cisneros M, Petrosino S et al (2007)
Pharmacological enhancement of the endocannabinoid system in the nucleus accumbens
shell stimulates food intake and increases c-Fos expression in the hypothalamus. Br J
Pharmacol 151:1109–1116
Stein MB, Goldin PR, Sareen J, Zorrilla LT, Brown GG (2002) Increased amygdala activation to
angry and contemptuous faces in generalized social phobia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59:
1027–1034
Stein MB, Simmons AN, Feinstein JS, Paulus MP (2007) Increased amygdala and insula
activation during emotion processing in anxiety-prone subjects. Am J Psychiatry 164:318–327
Tarullo AR, Gunnar MR (2006) Child maltreatment and the developing HPA axis. Hormones
Behav 50:632–639
Tessitore A, Hariri AR, Fera F, Smith WG, Chase TN et al (2002) Dopamine modulates the
response of the human amygdala: a study in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurosci 22:9099–9103
Thode K, Walss-Bass C, Hariri AR, Olvera R, Munoz K, et al Functional evidence implicating
the role of the corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor 1 in the development of stress-related
disorders. Am J Psychiatry (unpublished observation)
Tyndale RF, Payne JI, Gerber AL, Sipe JC (2007) The fatty acid amide hydrolase C385A (P129T)
missense variant in cannabis users: studies of drug use and dependence in Caucasians. Am J Med
Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 144:660–666
Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP (2009) Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress
responses. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:397–409
Urry HL, van Reekum CM, Johnstone T, Kalin NH, Thurow ME et al (2006) Amygdala and
ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and
predict diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults. J Neurosci 26:4415–4425
van Dyck CH, Malison RT, Jacobsen LK, Seibyl JP, Staley JK et al (2005) Increased dopamine
transporter availability associated with the 9-repeat allele of the SLC6A3 gene. J Nucl Med
46:745–751
Van Leeuwen N, Kumsta R, Entringer S, de Kloet ER, Zitman FG, DeRijk RH, Wust S (2010)
Functional mineralocorticoid receptor (MR) gene variation influences the cortisol awakening
response after dexamethasone. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35:339–349
Van Ness SH, Owens MJ, Kilts CD (2005) The variable number of tandem repeats element in
DAT1 regulates in vitro dopamine transporter density. BMC Genet 6:55
van Praag HM, de Kloet R, van Os J (2004) Stress the brain and depression. Cambridge
University Press, New York
van Wingen GA, Zylick SA, Pieters S, Mattern C, Verkes RJ, Buitelaar JK, Fernandez G (2008)
Testosterone increases amygdala reactivity in middle-aged women to a young adulthood
level. Neuropsychopharmacology 34:539–547
van Wingen GA, Mattern C, Verkes RJ, Buitelaar J, Fernandez G (2010) Testosterone reduces
amygdala-orbitalfrontal cortex coupling. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35:105–113
Vermeersch H, T’Sjoen G, Kaufman JM, Vincke J, Van Houtte M (2010) Testosterone, androgen
receptor gene CAG repeat length, mood and behaviour in adolescent males. Eur J Endocrinol
163:319–328
Viding E, Williamson DE, Hariri AR (2006) Developmental imaging genetics: challenges and
promises for translational research. Dev Psychopathol 18:877–892
Viveros MP, Marco EM, File SE (2005) Endocannabinoid system and stress and anxiety
responses. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 81:331–342
Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ (1999) Imaging studies on the role of dopamine in cocaine
reinforcement and addiction in humans. J Psychopharmacol 13:337–345
Wei XY, Yang JY, Dong YX, Wu CF (2007) Anxiolytic-like effects of oleamide in group-housed
and socially isolated mice. Prog Neuropsychopharmacolo Biol Psychiatry 31:1189–1195
Toward a Mechanistic Understanding 393
Whalen PJ, Shin LM, Somerville LH, McLean AA, Kim H (2002) Functional neuroimaging
studies of the amygdala in depression. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry 7:234–242
Williamson DE, Birmaher B, Ryan ND, Shiffrin TP, Lusky JA et al (2003) The stressful life
events schedule for children and adolescents: development and validation. Psychiatry Res
119:225–241
Yehuda R (2002) Post-traumatic stress disorder. N Engl J Med 346:108–114
Zitzmann M, Nieschlag E (2003) The CAG repeat polymorphism within the androgen receptor
gene and maleness. Int J Androl 26:76–83
Behavioural Genetics of Childhood
Disorders
Abstract After a general introduction into genetic risk factors for child psychi-
atric disorders, four specific child psychiatric disorders with a strong genetic
component, namely, Autism Spectrum Disorders, Attention Deficit / Hyperactivity
Disorder, Nocturnal Enuresis, and obesity, are discussed in detail. Recent evidence
of linkage, candidate gene, and genome-wide association studies are presented.
This chapter ends with a prospectus on further research needs.
Keywords Autism spectrum disorders Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder
Nocturnal enuresis Obesity BMI Linkage Association Genetic
C. M. Freitag (&)
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Goethe-Universität Frankfurt am Main,
Deutschordenstraße 50, 60528 Frankfurt am Main, Germany
e-mail: C.Freitag@em.uni-frankfurt.de
P. Asherson
MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre,
Institute of Psychiatry, London, UK
e-mail: philip.asherson@kcl.ac.uk
J. Hebebrand
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany
e-mail: johannes.hebebrand@uni-due.de
Contents
1 Introduction........................................................................................................................ 396
2 Selected Child Psychiatric Disorders with a Strong Genetic Component ...................... 401
2.1 Autism Spectrum Disorders ..................................................................................... 401
2.2 Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder................................................................. 405
2.3 Nocturnal Enuresis.................................................................................................... 411
2.4 Obesity ...................................................................................................................... 412
3 Prospectus .......................................................................................................................... 415
References................................................................................................................................ 415
1 Introduction
Over the past 50 years, substantial progress has been made in linking specific
behavioural and psychiatric phenotypes to chromosomal aberrations or genetic
variation at the DNA level. Prerequisites of this development were significant
conceptual, methodological and technical advances in both molecular and statis-
tical genetics as well as in phenotypic assessment.
Advances in cytogenetics allowed the identification of specific syndromes based
on quantitative chromosomal imbalances of complete chromosomes, as in trisomy
21 in 1959, and of chromosomal regions, as in the cri du chat syndrome due to
monosomy 5p, in 1963. Approximately five megabases represent the resolution
limits of banding techniques; fluorescent in situ hybridisation (FISH) allows
detection of deletions as small as 1.5 Mb. Recently, microarray-based comparative
genomic hybridisation (array CGH) has been introduced as a complementary
method with even higher resolution (Miller et al. 2008).
Micro-deletions can result in syndromes with distinct behavioural features such as
the Williams-Beuren Syndrome (7q11.23), Prader-Willi syndrome (paternal 15q11),
Angelman syndrome (maternal 15q11), 16p11.2 deletion, the velocardiofacial/
DiGeorge syndrome (22q11) and 22q13.3 deletion syndrome (Xiang et al. 2010).
Because a single gene on average encompasses 10–15 (kb), the phenotypes of micro-
deletion syndromes result from the loss of a number of genes (partial monosomy).
Corresponding to micro-deletions, several micro-duplication syndromes exist,
e.g., corresponding to the Williams-Beuren Syndrome region at 7q11.23, and the
chromosome 16p11.2-duplication (Shen et al. 2010). The numerous findings of copy
number variations (CNVs) in recent whole genome studies on child psychiatric
disorders, as Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) or Autism Spectrum
Disorders (ASD) (see below) emphasises the relevance of small cytogenetic findings
for psychiatric disorders.
Quantitative imbalances can functionally result in over expression (e.g., trisomy
or partial trisomy) or under expression (monosomy or partial monosomy) of those
genes located on a chromosome or within a specific chromosomal region. Due to the
high proportion of genes expressed in the central nervous system, e.g., *80% in mice
(Lein et al. 2007), brain function is almost always perturbed in such disorders.
Behavioral Genetics of Childhood Disorders 397
Findings in Down syndrome have revealed that of the approximately 200 genes
on chromosome 21, only a subset is responsible for the characteristic phenotype.
One well-known example is the early onset of dementia in subjects with Down
syndrome, which can partially be attributed to the over expression of the amyloid-
ß precursor protein gene (APP) located at 21q21-22. In addition to the quantitative
imbalance, the Down syndrome phenotype is affected by allelic variation; for
example, differences in length of a tetranucleotide repeat in intron 7 of the APP
locus explain substantial variation in age at onset of dementia in subjects with
trisomy 21 (Margallo-Lana et al. 2004).
The first large scale and systematic genotype–phenotype studies with a primary
behavioural, psychological and psychiatric focus centred on sex chromosome
(gonosome) disorders. Long-term follow-up of their development into adulthood
revealed that these disorders are characterised by subtle neuropsychiatric and
neuropsychological symptoms such as an IQ distribution shifted slightly to the left
and elevated rates of attention problems, speech and reading difficulties and
reduced impulse-control. Nevertheless, the dissection of a highly specific behav-
ioural phenotype associated with any one of these sex chromosome disorders was
not possible (Hebebrand 1990; Walzer 1985).
The ability to detect variation at the DNA level, e.g., mutations, formed the
basis for the successful elucidation of the molecular mechanisms underlying many
monogenetic disorders, which can entail more or less specific behavioural phe-
notypes. Examples are shown in Table 1. Frequently, such variation is detectable
in exons of genes underlying monogenetic disorders and simplistically either
entails that the respective gene product is structurally altered or not formed at all.
For example, missense mutations entail the substitution of the regular amino acid
at a specific position of the protein with another; this alteration of the amino acid
sequence of the respective protein can have functional implications at the levels of
the cell, tissue and organism. Two monogenetic disorders, which can cause autism,
tuberous sclerosis (TSC1-/TSC2-gene) and fragile X syndrome, have been studied
thoroughly, and due to the elucidation of the cellular pathways affected by a
lack of hamartin or tuberin (tuberous sclerosis) or the FMR1 proteine (fragile X
syndrome), new treatment options have been developed which are currently tested
in randomised controlled trials (Ehninger et al. 2009; Hagerman et al. 2009).
The majority of DSM-IV TR psychiatric disorders are most likely complex
implying that they only infrequently result from single gene mutations. Instead, it is
assumed that several gene variants interact in a complex manner with environmental
factors to produce the phenotype (Kendler 2005). Complex disorders typically show
higher concordance rates in monozygotic than dizygotic twins; concordance rates in
monozygotic twins are typically below 1 implying that environmental factors play a
role in the manifestation of the disorder. Family studies have shown that complex
psychiatric disorders are characterised by elevated recurrence risks in first and second
degree relatives which are below those expected for classical monogenetic dominant or
recessive traits. Moreover, for the complex psychiatric disorders a steep decline
in recurrence risks is observable between first and second degree family members.
In third degree relatives recurrence risks are usually only minimally elevated above
population based rates of the respective disorder (Hill et al. 2006; Knoblauch 2007).
398
Table 1 Examples of the influence of single gene disorders on cognition and behaviour
Monogenic Mode of inheritance Gene Cognitive phenotype Behavioural phenotype
disorder
Lesch-Nyhan X-chromosomal Hypoxanthine guanine Mental retardation Self-mutilative biting of fingers and lips
syndrome recessive phosphoribosyltransferase
(HPRT)
Tuberous Autosomal dominant Tuberous sclerosis-1 and -2 Learning difficulties, low Behavioural problems, autism, autism spectrum
sclerosis (TSC1/-2) IQ, mental retardation disorder
Fragile X X-chromosomal, triplet Fragile X mental retardation Mental retardation, Autism spectrum disorder, social phobia, ADHD
syndrome repeat expansion protein (FMR1)
Leptin deficiency Autosomal recessive Leptin (lep) Diminished perception of food reward, decreased
response to satiety signals, hyperphagia
Chorea Autosomal dominant, Huntingtin Dementia Depression, schizophrenia, personality change,
Huntington triplet repeat obsessive–compulsive behaviour
expansion
Phenylketonuria Autosomal recessive Phenylalanine hydroxylase Mental retardation Autism
(PAH)
Wilson disease Autosomal recessive Membrane copper-transport Psychiatric symptoms (personality changes,
protein (ATP7B) depression, psychosis)
Neurofibromatosis Autosomal dominant Neurofibromin Mental retardation ADHD
C. M. Freitag et al.
Behavioral Genetics of Childhood Disorders 399
Within child and adolescent psychiatry, most twin, family and adoption studies
have been carried out for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Freitag
et al. 2010a). ADHD is also noteworthy because large population-based twin
studies have analysed quantitative dimensions of the disorder; complex gene-
environment twin studies have also been performed. For many behavioural traits
and developmental milestones heritability estimates based on categorical or
dimensional (quantitative) data indicate that overall approximately half of the
variance is explained by genetic factors (Plomin 2005), the other half by the
environment (Table 2). For many behavioural traits the environmental influences
are predominantly unique effects that explain differences for familial traits; with
some exceptions, such as conduct problems, where the familial environment also
plays an important role. It is worth noting that in the absence of direct evidence for
the impact of the familial environment on a trait, there may still be a role for such
influences through gene–environment interactions, which are part of the heritable
component in genetic model fitting studies.
For most psychiatric disorders, which are usually assessed categorically,
genetic factors have also been shown to play an important role (Table 3).
Heritability estimates typically exceed 0.5. ADHD and autistic disorders have been
shown to be one of the most highly heritable child and adolescent psychiatric
disorders (Asherson et al. 2005; Freitag et al. 2010a, b; Hebebrand et al. 2010).
Knowledge of the magnitude of the genetic basis of a particular disorder is
valuable for interpreting psychiatric findings within a patient’s family and for
probing for specific disorders in relatives of the index patient.
Until recently, candidate gene and linkage studies dominated the attempts to
uncover genetic variation underlying such complex disorders. However, viewed in
400 C. M. Freitag et al.
retrospect it can be concluded that these large scaled efforts were largely unsuc-
cessful; progress in the molecular dissection of complex psychiatric phenotypes
proved to be exceedingly slow for a period of over 20 years (Burmeister et al. 2008).
Candidate gene studies could only be performed for those genes for which an a priori
hypothesis existed as to their relevance for the respective disorder; obviously for
each disorder this represented only a very limited number in light of the totally
known number of human genes. In addition, candidate gene studies in different
psychiatric disorders frequently focussed on the same set of genes of a particular
neurotransmitter system, such as dopamine and serotonin transporters and receptors.
In other words, the candidate gene studies reflected the paucity of hypotheses as to
the underlying pathways involved in complex psychiatric disorders.
In 2006, the first genome-wide association studies (GWAS) based on DNA chip
technology were introduced (Hardy et al. 2009). Whereas it is still too early to judge
the total insight that this novel technology will provide into the pathogenesis of
complex disorders, we can nevertheless already conclude that GWAS have entailed a
paradigm shift, thus justifying the nomination as ‘breakthrough of the year’ by
Science magazine in 2007 (Pennisi 2007). For many complex somatic and neuro-
psychiatric disorders, novel genes have been detected which provide initial insights
into frequently unknown pathways involved in the respective disorders. For many
disorders, different groups pooled their data to come up with several thousand cases
and controls, such numbers had almost never been analysed in the pre-GWAS era.
A major finding has been that the effect sizes of validated trait or disease-related
SNPs are in most cases extremely small. According to a recent synopsis (Hindorff
et al. 2009) the median odds ratio was 1.33 with an interquartile range of 1.2–1.61,
although more recent evidence for very large-scale studies of traits such as height and
body mass index in samples of 100,000–200,000 individuals, indicate effect sizes
equivalent to odds ratios of 1.1 or less, with hundreds or risk alleles involved
(Speliotes et al. 2010).
Behavioral Genetics of Childhood Disorders 401
Chromosome 2
Chromosome 3
Chromosome 6
Chromosome 7
Chromosome 17
single families in NLGN3 and NLGN4 these could not be replicated in larger
samples of individuals with ASD (Freitag 2007). Similarly, two mutations iden-
tified in the ribosomal protein gene RPL10 on Xq28 were not replicated in a
subsequent study (Gong et al. 2009; Klauck et al. 2006).
For a long-time suspected causes of heterogeneity, ASD are chromosomal
abnormalities which can be observed by standard karyotyping (Vorstman et al.
2006b). With a rate of approximately 1%, the most prevalent cytogenetic abnor-
malities are observed on chromosome 15q11-13 (Depienne et al. 2009; Freitag
2007) in most cases maternally, but also paternally inherited duplications. Further,
relatively frequent findings are deletions of chromosome 2q37, chromosome 7q31
and deletions or duplications of chromosome 22q13. In addition, Klinefelter
Syndrome (XXY) as well as duplications of the Williams-Beuren Syndrome region
7q11.23 and deletions of 22q11 (Velo-cardio-facial syndrome) are associated with
increased autistic traits (Berg et al. 2007; van Rijn et al. 2008; Vorstman et al.
2006a).
Due to technological advances, it is now possible to also assess small cyto-
genetic abnormalities (‘copy number variations’ CNVs) not detected by standard
karyotyping (Henrichsen et al. 2009). Recent publications show that some–espe-
cially rare–CNVs are observed more frequently in ASD patients compared to
control subjects (Bucan et al. 2009; Glessner et al. 2009; Kumar et al. 2008;
Marshall et al. 2008; Mefford et al. 2008; Pinto et al. 2010; Sebat et al. 2007;
Weiss et al. 2008). Similar to the cytogenetic findings obtained by standard
karyotyping, most CNVs represent rare, unique events rather than representing
recurrent deletions or duplications. Replicated rare CNVs from genome-wide
studies, which were observed more frequently in ASD compared to control
individuals, are located on the following chromosomes: 1q21, 2p16.3 (NRXN1),
3p25–26 (CNTN4), 7q36.2 (DPP6), 15q11–13 (UBE3A, OR4M2, OR4N4); 16p11.2
(MAPK3, MAZ, DOC2A, SEZ6L2, HIRIP3, IL6); 22q11.2; X (DDX51-PTCHD1;
maternally inherited). Some of these CNVs were observed also more frequently in
individuals with mental retardation or schizophrenia than in controls. ASD specific
CNVs were not exclusively observed in ASD individuals with specific dysmorphic
features or mental retardation but were also present in high functioning patients with
autism with only minor dysmorphology (van der Zwaag et al. 2009).
Molecular genetic studies in ASD have come a long way from the early linkage
studies, which aimed at describing a few loci and subsequently finding one or a
few genes of major effect relevant for all cases of ASD. It has now become clear
that ASD are heterogeneous disorders, caused by several rare—most likely—
monogenetic disorders (as fragile X syndrome, mutations in TSC1/TSC2, LAMB1,
CNTNAP2, PTEN, DHCR7, SHANK3, NLGN3/4 or RPL10). In addition, ‘con-
tiguous gene syndromes’ are likely causes of ASD, as the overall rate of rare
CNVs and large chromosomal deletions, duplications and translocations is
increased in individuals with ASD compared to controls.
Common variants, on the other hand, may shape the phenotype or eventually
may lead to the disorder by interacting with rare mutations or rare CNVs.
A mechanism like this has been shown for PTEN haploinsufficient individuals.
Behavioral Genetics of Childhood Disorders 405
The serotonin-transporter gene SLC6A4 has been discussed as both an ASD sus-
ceptibility gene and a second-site modifier in ASD (Bartlett et al. 2005; Hessl et al.
2008). A study in PTEN haploinsufficient mice (Page et al. 2009) demonstrated
that the phenotypes of these mice were modified in an additive fashion by SLC6A4
haploinsufficiency. In addition, the role of PTEN in the maintenance of genomic
stability (Shen et al. 2007; Stiles 2009) makes it likely that PTEN haploinsuffi-
ciency may increase the probability of a secondary modifying event, such as a
copy number variation in a chromosomal region relevant to ASD. Common
variants also might increase the risk for autistic traits in the general population as
well as for less severe autistic disorders as Asperger Syndrome or PDD-NOS.
From results of current genetic findings in ASD, it is likely that mutations or
common variants in genes coding for gene products involved in (1) cell–cell
interaction and synaptic function, including development of dendritic spines, (2)
neuronal migration and growth or (3) excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmission
are causes of ASD. The pathway influencing cell–cell interaction and synaptic
function includes NRXNs, NLNGs, CNTN3/4, CNTNAP2 and SHANK3. In addi-
tion, the FMR protein, which is missing in fragile X syndrome, modulates den-
dritic spine formation and synaptic plasticity by inhibiting mGluR1/5-mediated
dendritic protein synthesis (Hagerman et al. 2009). Neuronal migration and growth
are influenced by gene products of LAMB1, EN2 or the MET receptor tyrosine
kinase gene. The mTor/PI3-kinase (PI3 K) pathway involves PTEN, TSC1/2 and
several other genes, which were observed in rare CNVs in individuals with ASD
(Cusco et al. 2009). It strongly influences (neuronal) cell growth. Gene products
influencing the regulation of excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmission are
GABA and glutamate receptors. In addition, disbalance of excitatory and inhibi-
tory neurotransmission was also observed in fragile X syndrome. Clearly, this list
of possibly involved pathways is not exhaustive, and other mechanisms or path-
ways may emerge as results of further studies will be published.
The genetic effects on the development of ADHD throughout the life span are
not well understood. The heritability of self-rated ADHD symptoms in adults is far
lower than that reported for children and adolescents, based on parent and teacher
reports; being around 30% (Boomsma et al. 2010) compared to the average
heritability in children of around 76% (Faraone et al. 2005). This may be a
measurement artefact, based on self versus informant ratings, and confounding
with adult onset disorder that might mimic some of the symptoms of ADHD, or
might reflect a growing impact of unique environmental effects on the long-term
course of ADHD into adulthood. Further work is needed to clarify this important
question.
In terms of the genetic architecture, a segregation study resulted in a model
implicating non-Mendelian major gene effects with low penetrance in ADHD
(Maher et al. 1999). However, these findings are partly in contrast to the obser-
vation of twin studies, which point to a model implying multiple common genetic
variants of small effect in the aetiology of the disorder (Levy et al. 1997).
Results of hypothesis free molecular genetic studies are currently supportive of
both models, too, indicating clear genetic heterogeneity in ADHD. Linkage studies
elicited several loci likely containing rare genetic variants of major effect only
present in single large families, but also common variants of smaller effect present
across several families (Romanos et al. 2008). In addition, genome-wide associ-
ation studies indicate a very limited number of common variants of small effect to
date (Franke et al. 2009; McCarthy et al. 2008) and rare copy number variants that
confer greater (moderate) levels of risk (Elia et al. 2010; Williams et al. 2010).
We therefore conclude that on the basis of current data, both types of genetic
effects likely play a role with evidence for both common risk alleles of minor
effect, and variants that are individually rare in the population but have a larger
impact on risk for the disorder. As we shall see much more work is needed to
clarify the balance between these two types of genetic effects and it remains
feasible that at least some of the risk for ADHD is conferred by many individually
rare alleles of minor effect; which would be extremely difficult to detect.
Another question that can be addressed by quantitative genetic approaches is
the aetiological relationship between inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity.
Twin studies that have investigated this have found that these are separable traits
with only partially overlapping genetic influences. While the correlation between
the two domains is largely accounted for by shared genetic effects there are in
additional unique genetic effects acting on each of the two domains (McLoughlin
et al. 2007). Genetic investigation of the two domains would therefore be expected
to provide some evidence for unique as well as shared aetiological pathways.
In support of this general conclusion we see differential relationships between
each of the two domains and comorbid traits. One example is reading disability
(RD) and inattention where the correlation between ADHD and RD was found to
be largely driven by genetic factors not shared with hyperactivity-impulsivity
(Paloyelis et al. 2010). A contrasting example is a recent study of ADHD
and oppositional behaviour that found very highly phenotypic and genetic
correlations with hyperactivity-impulsivity but much lower with inattention
Behavioral Genetics of Childhood Disorders 407
(Wood et al. 2009). These data suggested that in middle childhood ADHD and
conduct problems may represent the same underlying liability, distinct from the
inattentive domain.
Twin studies have found that there are many other examples of overlapping
genetic influences between ADHD symptoms and comorbid disorders and traits
including autism spectrum disorder (Ronald et al. 2008), motor coordination
(Francks et al. 2003), conduct disorder (Thapar et al. 2001) and depression (Cole
et al. 2009) among others. These studies demonstrate the complexity of shared
and unique genetic and other aetiological influences among many mental health
disorders and comorbid traits. At a basic level further work using longitudinal
twin designs can further delineate the causal relationships between ADHD and
co-occurring traits which include pleiotropic effects (multiple effects of individual
sets of genes), mediating effects (brain functions or behaviours that mediate
genetic effects on ADHD) and risk models (one disorder leading to another).
There have now been numerous candidate gene association studies in ADHD
and these are best summarised in the recent review and meta-analysis from Gizer
et al. (2009). They conducted a systematic review of the association literature with
Behavioral Genetics of Childhood Disorders 409
the aim of identifying the most consistent findings in ADHD research to date.
They further tested for heterogeneity across studies because other genes, like
DAT1, might show significant sources of variability due to identifiable moderating
factors. Overall they concluded that the following genes were significantly asso-
ciated with ADHD: DAT1, DRD4, DRD5, 5HTT (serotonin transporter), HTR1B
(serotonin 1B receptor) and SNAP-25 (synaptosomal associated protein); and the
following genes showed significant evidence of heterogeneity: DAT1, DRD4,
DRD5, DBH, ADRA2A (adrenergic 2A receptor), 5HTT, TPH2 (Tryptophan
hydroxylase 2), MAOA (monoamine oxidase A) and SNAP25. For the candidate
genes showing heterogeneity future studies could usefully investigate the potential
moderating factors that lead to variability in effect size across studies.
Despite significant progress, the total estimated impact of the most replicated
candidate gene findings is around 3.3% of the phenotypic variance of ADHD;
accounting for 4.3% of the estimated average heritability of ADHD of 76%
(Kuntsi et al. 2006). Further work is clearly needed to explain the rest of the
genetic influences on ADHD.
to drug abuse and dependence (Uhl et al. 2008). This finding and other hints from
GWAS indicate that genes involved in cell division, cell adhesion, neuronal
migration and neuronal plasticity may also confer risk for ADHD (Franke et al.
2009).
Overall there is a long way to go to delineate the specific genetic factors
that explain the high heritability of the disorder. This is, however, a common
phenomenon in common disorders research and several possible explanations for
the so-called ‘dark-matter’ of (missing) heritability has been put forward—dark
matter in the sense that we know it exists can detect its influences but simply
cannot see it (Manolio et al. 2009). Potential reasons include numerous genes of
very small effect, genetic heterogeneity with risk conferred by many different
genes and variants within genes, higher order interactions between genes and with
environment and aetiological heterogeneity (Manolio et al. 2009).
There is evidence that genes of major effect might lead to ADHD in a few rare
families. Furthermore, variation in some of these genes may have more general
effects on ADHD. There are two main examples.
The first is a gene called latrophilin 3 (LPHN3) that is located within a
linkage region on chromosome 4q13.2. This region was first identified in a large
Behavioral Genetics of Childhood Disorders 411
Conclusion
Overall current molecular genetic studies of ADHD find evidence for both
frequent and rare variants that influence the risk for ADHD. The level of evidence
is sufficient to confirm that identity of some of the genes and gene regions
involved, although the data only explains a very small amount of the total genetic
variation that impacts on risk for ADHD. Future studies will need to include very
much larger sample sizes and take advantage of the improving technology to
screen for both common and rare variants. The specific findings confirm the old
neurotransmitter hypotheses of ADHD, but also extend the focus to many other
neurobiological processes that impact on brain development and brain function.
loci in the respective families. Interestingly, a recent study reported higher rates of
nocturnal enuresis in individuals with schizophrenia, and individuals with enuresis
showed a worse performance on two frontal lobe cognitive tests compared to
individuals without (Hyde et al. 2008).
2.4 Obesity
Numerous twin, adoption and family studies have allowed for the calculation of
heritability estimates for body mass index (BMI), which typically range from 0.4
to 0.7; twin studies have consistently led to higher estimates. Based on complex
statistical modelling single family studies have nevertheless also led to estimates
in the range of 0.7 (Maes et al. 1997). A recent review of 13 longitudinal studies
found a strong genetic continuity in BMI from early childhood to onset of
adulthood (Silventoinen and Kaprio 2009). There was also evidence for an effect
of a common environment on the tracking of BMI during childhood; in adults the
influence of the non-shared environment substantially exceeded that of the shared
environment.
Monogenic Obesity
The cloning and initial detection of mutations in the mouse and human
(Montague et al. 1997; Zhang et al. 1994) leptin gene marked the beginning of the
large-scale elucidation of DNA variation underlying inter-individual differences in
body weight. Currently, several types of monogenic forms of obesity have been
elucidated. All of the respective mutations are rare, some have only been reported
in single cases worldwide; the genes are all expressed in the hypothalamus. The
elucidation of such mutations and the successful treatment of leptin deficient
extremely obese (Farooqi et al. 1999) individuals has firmly established that the
behavioural phenotype hyperphagia/overeating and as a consequence obesity can
be caused by mutations in specific genes.
Several of the detected novel monogenic forms of obesity encompass other
clinically recognisable features. For instance, mutations in the leptin gene entail
hypothalamic hypogonadism, other endocrinological abnormalities and immune
system dysfunction (Hinney et al. 2008). In the original report of the clinical
features of patients with mutations in the pro-opiomelanocortin gene (POMC)
adrenal insufficiency and red hair were characteristic features in addition to
extreme obesity (Krude et al. 1998). Developmental delay was shown to co-occur
with severe obesity in a patient with a mutation in the neurotrophin receptor TrkB
(Yeo et al. 2004).
The detection of mutations in the melanocortin-4 receptor gene (MC4R)
(Hinney et al. 1999; Vaisse et al. 1998; Yeo et al. 1998) marked a turning point in
the sense that for the first time the molecular basis of a subgroup of patients with
idiopathic obesity was identified. The detection of the association between MC4R
Behavioral Genetics of Childhood Disorders 413
mutations and obesity is also noteworthy because the combined frequency of all
functionally relevant mutations is in the range of 1–6% among extremely obese
children and adolescents (Hinney et al. 2003).
Two family-based studies have attempted to quantify the effect of functionally
relevant MC4R mutations (Dempfle et al. 2004; Stutzmann et al. 2008) by com-
paring BMI of mutation and wild-type carriers of relatives ascertained via the
index cases. According to Dempfle et al. (2004) and Stutzmann et al. (2008) adult
male carriers were 4 and 4.3 kg/m2 heavier than their male relatives with the
wildtype genotype. In females the mean effect size of MC4R mutations is larger;
the corresponding values were 9.5 and 8.7 kg/m2.
In the pre-GWAS era, the MC4R was the first gene shown to harbour a common
variant with a small effect size (Geller et al. 2004). The association of the minor
allele, coding for Ile103, with leanness was confirmed in subsequent larger
meta-analyses with up to almost 124,000 probands (Heid et al. 2008; Speliotes
et al. 2010; Wang et al. 2010a; Young et al. 2007). Roughly 3–6% of different
populations are heterozygous for this variant, which according to German popu-
lation-based data for adults entails a 0.5 kg/m2 reduction in mean BMI. This would
qualify this SNP as the one with the strongest effect size of all currently known
common variants including the SNPs in intron 1 of the fat mass and obesity
associated gene (FTO) (Frayling et al. 2007). A second coding MC4R variant also
protects from obesity (Stutzmann et al. 2007). The MC4R is thus currently unique
in that it harbours variants of considerably different effect sizes both within and
outside the coding region whose minor alleles both increase and decrease BMI.
There are over 90 known coding variants in the MC4R that lead to a reduced
receptor function seemingly support the common disease—rare variant hypothesis,
according to which a substantial proportion of the predisposition to complex
disorders is due to rare variants (Bodmer et al. 2008). Indeed, it seems likely to
assume that other such loci exist in the genome that cannot readily be picked up
via linkage or association analyses due to locus heterogeneity and the low overall
combined rate of such mutations at a specific gene locus.
Polygenic Obesity
The per allele change in BMI ranged from 0.06 (MTCH2, KCTD15, PTBP2,
RPL27A, NUDT3) to 0.39 (FTO) kg/m2; a total of 10 SNPs showed per allele
changes\0.1 kg/m2 equivalent to less than 324 and 289 g in males and females of
average heights (1.8 and 1.7 m). Five SNPs with the lowest per allele change of
0.06 kg/m2 each account for approximately one half of the aforementioned
weights of individuals of average height.
The 32 identified variants explained a mere 1.5% of the BMI variance; this
roughly corresponds to 3% of the genetic variance based on an estimated
BMI-heritability of 0.5. Speliotes et al. (2010) estimated that there are approxi-
mately an additional 200 loci (95% CI: 98–350) with similar effect sizes as the
detected 32, which together would account for roughly 3.5% of the variation in
BMI or 7% of the genetic variation.
Sub-analyses based on children and adolescents revealed a substantial overlap
with the genetic predisposition to adult obesity. Directionally consistent effects
were found for 23 of the 32 SNPs. Common variants detected in adults are
seemingly important throughout different developmental stages. In accordance
with the low heritability estimates for BMI in early life, a significant effect of the
FTO genotypes was not detectable in children prior to age 4 in two birth cohort
studies (Rzehak et al. 2010).
Because most obesity genes are expressed in the central nervous system, the
role of the brain in the regulation of body weight is seemingly substantiated.
However, the high proportion of all genes expressed centrally needs to be
considered prior to drawing firm conclusions. Pathway-based analyses based on
the genes identified via the association signals revealed evidence of enrichment for
pathways involved in the platelet-derived growth factor signalling, translation
elongation, hormone or nuclear hormone receptor binding, homeobox transcrip-
tion, regulation of cellular metabolism, neurogenesis and neuron differentiation,
protein phosphorylation and numerous other pathways related to growth, metab-
olism, immune and neuronal processes (Speliotes et al. 2010).
3 Prospectus
The genetics of four childhood conditions was elaborated in more detail in this
chapter. Other common psychiatric disorders of childhood, adolescence and
adulthood also show a heritability [50%, especially anxiety disorders. Gene–
environmental effects are likely for this condition, as it is also the case for ADHD
and obesity.
Research over the last 10 years in the genetic of child psychiatric disorders has
resulted in many promising findings, but also has shown that elucidating the
genetic background of psychiatric disorders is challenging and will require new
technologies. With sequencing approaches, methylation pattern assessment, and
functional studies, new and clinically relevant results are to be expected and will
ultimately result in more individualised and targeted treatment options.
References
Abrahams BS, Geschwind DH (2008) Advances in autism genetics: on the threshold of a new
neurobiology. Nat Rev Genet 5:341–355
Alarcon M, Abrahams BS, Stone JL, Duvall JA, Perederiy JV, Bomar JM, Sebat J, Wigler M,
Martin CL, Ledbetter DH, Nelson SF, Cantor RM, Geschwind DH (2008) Linkage,
association, and gene-expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism-susceptibility
gene. Am J Hum Genet 1:150–159
American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
DSM-IV-TR.
Arcos-Burgos M, Castellanos FX, Pineda D, Lopera F, Palacio JD, Palacio LG, Rapoport JL,
Berg K, Bailey-Wilson JE, Muenke M (2004) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a
population isolate: linkage to loci at 4q13.2, 5q33.3, 11q22, and 17p11. Am J Hum Genet
6:998–1014
Arcos-Burgos M, Jain M, Acosta MT, Shively S, Stanescu H, Wallis D, Domene S, Velez JI,
Karkera JD, Balog J, Berg K, Kleta R, Gahl WA, Roessler E, Long R, Lie J, Pineda D,
Londono AC, Palacio JD, Arbelaez A, Lopera F, Elia J, Hakonarson H, Johansson S,
Knappskog PM, Haavik J, Ribases M, Cormand B, Bayes M, Casas M, Ramos-Quiroga JA,
Hervas A, Maher BS, Faraone SV, Seitz C, Freitag CM, Palmason H, Meyer J, Romanos M,
Walitza S, Hemminger U, Warnke A, Romanos J, Renner T, Jacob C, Lesch KP, Swanson J,
416 C. M. Freitag et al.
Vortmeyer A, Bailey-Wilson JE, Castellanos FX, Muenke M (2010) A common variant of the
latrophilin 3 gene, LPHN3, confers susceptibility to ADHD and predicts effectiveness of
stimulant medication. Mol Psychiatry 11:1053–1066
Arking DE, Cutler DJ, Brune CW, Teslovich TM, West K, Ikeda M, Rea A, Guy M, Lin S,
Cook EH, Chakravarti A (2008) A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily
member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. Am J Hum Genet 1:160–164
Arnell H, Hjalmas K, Jagervall M, Lackgren G, Stenberg A, Bengtsson B, Wassen C,
Emahazion T, Anneren G, Pettersson U, Sundvall M, Dahl N (1997) The genetics of primary
nocturnal enuresis: inheritance and suggestion of a second major gene on chromosome 12q.
J Med Genet 5:360–365
Asherson P, Kuntsi J, Taylor E (2005) Unravelling the complexity of attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder: a behavioural genomic approach. Br J Psychiatry:103–105
Asherson P, Brookes K, Franke B, Chen W, Gill M, Ebstein RP, Buitelaar J, Banaschewski T,
Sonuga-Barke E, Eisenberg J, Manor I, Miranda A, Oades RD, Roeyers H, Rothenberger A,
Sergeant J, Steinhausen HC, Faraone SV (2007) Confirmation that a specific haplotype of
the dopamine transporter gene is associated with combined-type ADHD. Am J Psychiatry
4:674–677
Baieli S, Pavone L, Meli C, Fiumara A, Coleman M (2003) Autism and phenylketonuria.
J Autism Dev Disord 2:201–204
Bakkaloglu B, O’Roak BJ, Louvi A, Gupta AR, Abelson JF, Morgan TM, Chawarska K, Klin A,
Ercan-Sencicek AG, Stillman AA, Tanriover G, Abrahams BS, Duvall JA, Robbins EM,
Geschwind DH, Biederer T, Gunel M, Lifton RP, State MW (2008) Molecular cytogenetic
analysis and resequencing of contactin associated protein-like 2 in autism spectrum disorders.
Am J Hum Genet 1:165–173
Bartlett CW, Gharani N, Millonig JH, Brzustowicz LM (2005) Three autism candidate genes: a
synthesis of human genetic analysis with other disciplines. Int J Dev Neurosci 2–3:221–234
Becker K, El-Faddagh M, Schmidt MH, Esser G, Laucht M (2008) Interaction of dopamine
transporter genotype with prenatal smoke exposure on ADHD symptoms. J Pediatr 2:263–269
Berg JS, Brunetti-Pierri N, Peters SU, Kang SH, Fong CT, Salamone J, Freedenberg D,
Hannig VL, Prock LA, Miller DT, Raffalli P, Harris DJ, Erickson RP, Cunniff C, Clark GD,
Blazo MA, Peiffer DA, Gunderson KL, Sahoo T, Patel A, Lupski JR, Beaudet AL,
Cheung SW (2007) Speech delay and autism spectrum behaviors are frequently associated
with duplication of the 7q11.23 Williams-Beuren syndrome region. Genet Med 7:427–441
Bochukova EG, Huang N, Keogh J, Henning E, Purmann C, Blaszczyk K, Saeed S,
Hamilton-Shield J, Clayton-Smith J, O’Rahilly S, Hurles ME, Farooqi IS (2010) Large,
rare chromosomal deletions associated with severe early-onset obesity. Nature 7281:666–670
Bodmer W, Bonilla C (2008) Common and rare variants in multifactorial susceptibility to
common diseases. Nat Genet 6:695–701
Bolton D, Rijsdijk F, O’Connor TG, Perrin S, Eley TC (2007) Obsessive-compulsive disorder,
tics and anxiety in 6-year-old twins. Psychol Med 1:39–48
Bonora E, Beyer KS, Lamb JA, Parr JR, Klauck SM, Benner A, Paolucci M, Abbott A, Ragoussis I,
Poustka A, Bailey AJ, Monaco AP (2003) Analysis of reelin as a candidate gene for autism. Mol
Psychiatry 10:885–892
Bonora E, Lamb JA, Barnby G, Sykes N, Moberly T, Beyer KS, Klauck SM, Poustka F,
Bacchelli E, Blasi F, Maestrini E, Battaglia A, Haracopos D, Pedersen L, Isager T, Eriksen G,
Viskum B, Sorensen EU, Brondum-Nielsen K, Cotterill R, Engeland H, Jonge M, Kemner C,
Steggehuis K, Scherpenisse M, Rutter M, Bolton PF, Parr JR, Poustka A, Bailey AJ,
Monaco AP (2005) Mutation screening and association analysis of six candidate genes for
autism on chromosome 7q. Eur J Hum Genet 2:198–207
Boomsma DI, Saviouk V, Hottenga JJ, Distel MA, de Moor MH, Vink JM, Geels LM,
van Beek JH, Bartels M, de Geus EJ, Willemsen G (2010) Genetic epidemiology of attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD index) in adults. PLoS One 5:e10621
Brookes KJ, Mill J, Guindalini C, Curran S, Xu X, Knight J, Chen CK, Huang YS, Sethna V,
Taylor E, Chen W, Breen G, Asherson P (2006) A common haplotype of the dopamine
Behavioral Genetics of Childhood Disorders 417
Donaldson ZR, Young LJ (2008) Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality.
Science 5903:900–904
Dudbridge F, Gusnanto A (2008) Estimation of significance thresholds for genomewide
association scans. Genet Epidemiol 3:227–234
Duvall JA, Lu A, Cantor RM, Todd RD, Constantino JN, Geschwind DH (2007) A quantitative
trait locus analysis of social responsiveness in multiplex autism families. Am J Psychiatry
4:656–662
Ehninger D, De Vries PJ, Silva AJ (2009) From mTOR to cognition: molecular and cellular
mechanisms of cognitive impairments in tuberous sclerosis. J Intellect Disabil Res
10:838–851
Eiberg H (1995) Nocturnal enuresis is linked to a specific gene. Scand J Urol Nephrol (Suppl):
15–16
Eiberg H (1998) Total genome scan analysis in a single extended family for primary nocturnal
enuresis: evidence for a new locus (ENUR3) for primary nocturnal enuresis on chromosome
22q11. Eur Urol:34–36
Eiberg H, Berendt I, Mohr J (1995) Assignment of dominant inherited nocturnal enuresis
(ENUR1) to chromosome 13q. Nat Genet 3:354–356
Eiberg H, Shaumburg HL, von GA, Rittig S (2001) Linkage study of a large Danish 4-generation
family with urge incontinence and nocturnal enuresis. J Urol 6:2401–2403
Eley TC, Bolton D, O’Connor TG, Perrin S, Smith P, Plomin R (2003) A twin study of anxiety-
related behaviours in pre-school children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 7:945–960
Elia J, Gai X, Xie HM, Perin JC, Geiger E, Glessner JT, D’arcy M, deBerardinis R, Frackelton E,
Kim C, Lantieri F, Muganga BM, Wang L, Takeda T, Rappaport EF, Grant SF, Berrettini W,
Devoto M, Shaikh TH, Hakonarson H, White PS (2010) Rare structural variants found in
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder are preferentially associated with neurodevelopmental
genes. Mol Psychiatry 6:637–646
Faraone SV, Perlis RH, Doyle AE, Smoller JW, Goralnick JJ, Holmgren MA, Sklar P (2005)
Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 11:1313–1323
Faraone SV, Biederman J, Mick E (2006) The age-dependent decline of attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychol Med 2:159–165
Farooqi IS, Jebb SA, Langmack G, Lawrence E, Cheetham CH, Prentice AM, Hughes IA,
McCamish MA, O’Rahilly S (1999) Effects of recombinant leptin therapy in a child with
congenital leptin deficiency. N Engl J Med 12:879–884
Fisher SE, Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins KE, Monaco AP, Pembrey ME (1998) Localisation of a
gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nat Genet 2:168–170
Francks C, Fisher SE, Marlow AJ, MacPhie IL, Taylor KE, Richardson AJ, Stein JF, Monaco AP
(2003) Familial and genetic effects on motor coordination, laterality, and reading-related
cognition. Am J Psychiatry 11:1970–1977
Franke B, Hoogman M, Arias VA, Heister JG, Savelkoul PJ, Naber M, Scheffer H, Kiemeney
LA, Kan CC, Kooij JJ, Buitelaar JK (2008) Association of the dopamine transporter
(SLC6A3/DAT1) gene 9–6 haplotype with adult ADHD. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr
Genet 8:1576–1579
Franke B, Neale BM, Faraone SV (2009) Genome-wide association studies in ADHD. Hum
Genet 1:13–50
Frayling TM, Timpson NJ, Weedon MN, Zeggini E, Freathy RM, Lindgren CM, Perry JR,
Elliott KS, Lango H, Rayner NW, Shields B, Harries LW, Barrett JC, Ellard S, Groves CJ, Knight
B, Patch AM, Ness AR, Ebrahim S, Lawlor DA, Ring SM, Ben-Shlomo Y, Jarvelin MR,
Sovio U, Bennett AJ, Melzer D, Ferrucci L, Loos RJ, Barroso I, Wareham NJ, Karpe F,
Owen KR, Cardon LR, Walker M, Hitman GA, Palmer CN, Doney AS, Morris AD,
Smith GD, Hattersley AT, McCarthy MI (2007) A common variant in the FTO gene is
associated with body mass index and predisposes to childhood and adult obesity. Science
5826:889–894
Freitag CM (2007) The genetics of autistic disorders and its clinical relevance: a review of the
literature. Mol Psychiatry 1:2–22
Behavioral Genetics of Childhood Disorders 419
Freitag CM, Rohde LA, Lempp T, Romanos M (2010a) Phenotypic and measurement influences
on heritability estimates in childhood ADHD. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 3:311–323
Freitag CM, Staal W, Klauck SM, Duketis E, Waltes R (2010b) Genetics of autistic disorders:
review and clinical implications. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 3:169–178
Gelhorn HL, Stallings MC, Young SE, Corley RP, Rhee SH, Hewitt JK (2005) Genetic and
environmental influences on conduct disorder: symptom, domain and full-scale analyses.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 6:580–591
Geller F, Reichwald K, Dempfle A, Illig T, Vollmert C, Herpertz S, Siffert W, Platzer M, Hess C,
Gudermann T, Biebermann H, Wichmann HE, Schafer H, Hinney A, Hebebrand J (2004)
Melanocortin-4 receptor gene variant I103 is negatively associated with obesity. Am J Hum
Genet 3:572–581
Gizer IR, Ficks C, Waldman ID (2009) Candidate gene studies of ADHD: a meta-analytic review.
Hum Genet 1:51–90
Glessner JT, Wang K, Cai G, Korvatska O, Kim CE, Wood S, Zhang H, Estes A, Brune CW,
Bradfield JP, Imielinski M, Frackelton EC, Reichert J, Crawford EL, Munson J, Sleiman PM,
Chiavacci R, Annaiah K, Thomas K, Hou C, Glaberson W, Flory J, Otieno F, Garris M,
Soorya L, Klei L, Piven J, Meyer KJ, Anagnostou E, Sakurai T, Game RM, Rudd DS,
Zurawiecki D, McDougle CJ, Davis LK, Miller J, Posey DJ, Michaels S, Kolevzon A,
Silverman JM, Bernier R, Levy SE, Schultz RT, Dawson G, Owley T, McMahon WM,
Wassink TH, Sweeney JA, Nurnberger JI, Coon H, Sutcliffe JS, Minshew NJ, Grant SF,
Bucan M, Cook EH, Buxbaum JD, Devlin B, Schellenberg GD, Hakonarson H (2009) Autism
genome-wide copy number variation reveals ubiquitin and neuronal genes. Nature
7246:569–573
Gong X, Delorme R, Fauchereau F, Durand CM, Chaste P, Betancur C, Goubran-Botros H,
Nygren G, Anckarsater H, Rastam M, Gillberg IC, Kopp S, Mouren-Simeoni MC, Gillberg C,
Leboyer M, Bourgeron T (2009) An investigation of ribosomal protein L10 gene in autism
spectrum disorders. BMC Med Genet:7
Hagerman RJ, Berry-Kravis E, Kaufmann WE, Ono MY, Tartaglia N, Lachiewicz A, Kronk R,
Delahunty C, Hessl D, Visootsak J, Picker J, Gane L, Tranfaglia M (2009) Advances in the
treatment of fragile X syndrome. Pediatrics 1:378–390
Hardy J, Singleton A (2009) Genomewide association studies and human disease. N Engl J Med
17:1759–1768
Hebebrand J, Propping P (1990) Genetic aspects of brain maturation and behavior. In:
Rothenberger A (ed) Brain and behavior in child psychiatry, 1st edn. Springer, Berlin
Hebebrand J, Scherag A, Schimmelmann BG, Hinney A (2010) Child and adolescent psychiatric
genetics. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 3:259–279
Heid IM, Vollmert C, Kronenberg F, Huth C, Ankerst DP, Luchner A, Hinney A, Bronner G,
Wichmann HE, Illig T, Doring A, Hebebrand J (2008) Association of the MC4R V103I
polymorphism with the metabolic syndrome: the KORA Study. Obesity (Silver Spring)
2:369–376
Henrichsen CN, Chaignat E, Reymond A (2009) Copy number variants, diseases and gene
expression. Hum Mol Genet R1:R1–R8
Hessl D, Tassone F, Cordeiro L, Koldewyn K, McCormick C, Green C, Wegelin J, Yuhas J,
Hagerman RJ (2008) Brief report: aggression and stereotypic behavior in males with fragile X
syndrome–moderating secondary genes in a ‘‘single gene’’ disorder. J Autism Dev Disord
1:184–189
Hill MK, Sahhar M (2006) Genetic counselling for psychiatric disorders. Med J Aust 9:507–510
Hindorff LA, Sethupathy P, Junkins HA, Ramos EM, Mehta JP, Collins FS, Manolio TA (2009)
Potential etiologic and functional implications of genome-wide association loci for human
diseases and traits. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 23:9362–9367
Hinney A, Hebebrand J (2008) Polygenic obesity in humans. Obes Facts 1:35–42
Hinney A, Hebebrand J (2009) Three at one swoop! Obes Facts 1:3–8
Hinney A, Schmidt A, Nottebom K, Heibult O, Becker I, Ziegler A, Gerber G, Sina M, Gorg T,
Mayer H, Siegfried W, Fichter M, Remschmidt H, Hebebrand J (1999) Several mutations in
420 C. M. Freitag et al.
the melanocortin-4 receptor gene including a nonsense and a frameshift mutation associated
with dominantly inherited obesity in humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 4:1483–1486
Hinney A, Hohmann S, Geller F, Vogel C, Hess C, Wermter AK, Brokamp B, Goldschmidt H,
Siegfried W, Remschmidt H, Schafer H, Gudermann T, Hebebrand J (2003) Melanocortin-4
receptor gene: case-control study and transmission disequilibrium test confirm that
functionally relevant mutations are compatible with a major gene effect for extreme obesity.
J Clin Endocrinol Metab 9:4258–4267
Hinney A, Scherag S, Hebebrand J (2010) Genetic findings in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Prog
Mol Biol Transl Sci:241–270
Holtkamp K, Muller B, Heussen N, Remschmidt H, Herpertz-Dahlmann B (2005) Depression,
anxiety, and obsessionality in long-term recovered patients with adolescent-onset anorexia
nervosa. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2:106–110
Huang CH, Santangelo SL (2008) Autism and serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms: a
systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 6:903–913
Hublin C, Kaprio J, Partinen M, Koskenvuo M (1998) Nocturnal enuresis in a nationwide twin
cohort. Sleep 6:579–585
Hutcheson HB, Olson LM, Bradford Y, Folstein SE, Santangelo SL, Sutcliffe JS, Haines JL
(2004) Examination of NRCAM, LRRN3, KIAA0716, and LAMB1 as autism candidate
genes. BMC Med Genet:12
Hyde TM, Deep-Soboslay A, Iglesias B, Callicott JH, Gold JM, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Honea RA,
Bigelow LB, Egan MF, Emsellem EM, Weinberger DR (2008) Enuresis as a premorbid
developmental marker of schizophrenia. Brain (Pt 9):2489–2498
Jacob S, Brune CW, Carter CS, Leventhal BL, Lord C, Cook EH Jr (2007) Association of the
oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) in Caucasian children and adolescents with autism. Neurosci
Lett 1:6–9
Kahn RS, Khoury J, Nichols WC, Lanphear BP (2003) Role of dopamine transporter genotype
and maternal prenatal smoking in childhood hyperactive-impulsive, inattentive, and
oppositional behaviors. J Pediatr 1:104–110
Kendler KS (2005) ‘‘A gene for…’’: the nature of gene action in psychiatric disorders. Am J
Psychiatry 7:1243–1252
Kessler RC, Adler LA, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners CK, Faraone SV, Greenhill LL, Jaeger S,
Secnik K, Spencer T, Ustun TB, Zaslavsky AM (2005) Patterns and predictors of attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder persistence into adulthood: results from the national comorbidity
survey replication. Biol Psychiatry 11:1442–1451
Klauck SM, Felder B, Kolb-Kokocinski A, Schuster C, Chiocchetti A, Schupp I, Wellenreuther R,
Schmotzer G, Poustka F, Breitenbach-Koller L, Poustka A (2006) Mutations in the ribosomal
protein gene RPL10 suggest a novel modulating disease mechanism for autism. Mol Psychiatry
12:1073–1084
Knoblauch H (2007) Schizophrenia, human genetics and genetic counselling. Human genetic
counselling as part of the psychiatric/psychotherapeutic treatment concept. Nervenarzt 3(243–8):250
Krebs MO, Betancur C, Leroy S, Bourdel MC, Gillberg C, Leboyer M (2002) Absence of
association between a polymorphic GGC repeat in the 5’ untranslated region of the reelin
gene and autism. Mol Psychiatry 7:801–804
Krude H, Biebermann H, Luck W, Horn R, Brabant G, Gruters A (1998) Severe early-onset
obesity, adrenal insufficiency and red hair pigmentation caused by POMC mutations in
humans. Nat Genet 2:155–157
Kumar RA, Karamohamed S, Sudi J, Conrad DF, Brune C, Badner JA, Gilliam TC, Nowak NJ,
Cook EH Jr, Dobyns WB, Christian SL (2008) Recurrent 16p11.2 microdeletions in autism.
Hum Mol Genet 4:628–638
Kuntsi J, Neale BM, Chen W, Faraone SV, Asherson P (2006) The IMAGE project:
methodological issues for the molecular genetic analysis of ADHD. Behav Brain Funct:27
LaHoste GJ, Swanson JM, Wigal SB, Glabe C, Wigal T, King N, Kennedy JL (1996) Dopamine
D4 receptor gene polymorphism is associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Mol Psychiatry 2:121–124
Behavioral Genetics of Childhood Disorders 421
Lasky-Su J, Neale BM, Franke B, Anney RJ, Zhou K, Maller JB, Vasquez AA, Chen W,
Asherson P, Buitelaar J, Banaschewski T, Ebstein R, Gill M, Miranda A, Mulas F, Oades RD,
Roeyers H, Rothenberger A, Sergeant J, Sonuga-Barke E, Steinhausen HC, Taylor E, Daly M,
Laird N, Lange C, Faraone SV (2008) Genome-wide association scan of quantitative traits for
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder identifies novel associations and confirms candidate
gene associations. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 8:1345–1354
Lein ES, Hawrylycz MJ, Ao N, Ayres M, Bensinger A, Bernard A, Boe AF, Boguski MS,
Brockway KS, Byrnes EJ, Chen L, Chen L, Chen TM, Chin MC, Chong J, Crook BE,
Czaplinska A, Dang CN, Datta S, Dee NR, Desaki AL, Desta T, Diep E, Dolbeare TA,
Donelan MJ, Dong HW, Dougherty JG, Duncan BJ, Ebbert AJ, Eichele G, Estin LK, Faber C,
Facer BA, Fields R, Fischer SR, Fliss TP, Frensley C, Gates SN, Glattfelder KJ,
Halverson KR, Hart MR, Hohmann JG, Howell MP, Jeung DP, Johnson RA, Karr PT,
Kawal R, Kidney JM, Knapik RH, Kuan CL, Lake JH, Laramee AR, Larsen KD, Lau C,
Lemon TA, Liang AJ, Liu Y, Luong LT, Michaels J, Morgan JJ, Morgan RJ, Mortrud MT,
Mosqueda NF, Ng LL, Ng R, Orta GJ, Overly CC, Pak TH, Parry SE, Pathak SD, Pearson OC,
Puchalski RB, Riley ZL, Rockett HR, Rowland SA, Royall JJ, Ruiz MJ, Sarno NR,
Schaffnit K, Shapovalova NV, Sivisay T, Slaughterbeck CR, Smith SC, Smith KA, Smith BI,
Sodt AJ, Stewart NN, Stumpf KR, Sunkin SM, Sutram M, Tam A, Teemer CD, Thaller C,
Thompson CL, Varnam LR, Visel A, Whitlock RM, Wohnoutka PE, Wolkey CK, Wong VY,
Wood M, Yaylaoglu MB, Young RC, Youngstrom BL, Yuan XF, Zhang B, Zwingman TA,
Jones AR (2007) Genome-wide atlas of gene expression in the adult mouse brain. Nature
7124:168–176
Lerer E, Levi S, Salomon S, Darvasi A, Yirmiya N, Ebstein RP (2008) Association between the
oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and autism: relationship to Vineland Adaptive Behavior
Scales and cognition. Mol Psychiatry 10:980–988
Lesch KP, Timmesfeld N, Renner TJ, Halperin R, Roser C, Nguyen TT, Craig DW, Romanos J,
Heine M, Meyer J, Freitag C, Warnke A, Romanos M, Schafer H, Walitza S, Reif A,
Stephan DA, Jacob C (2008) Molecular genetics of adult ADHD: converging evidence from
genome-wide association and extended pedigree linkage studies. J Neural Transm 11:1573–1585
Lesch KP, Selch S, Renner TJ, Jacob C, Nguyen TT, Hahn T, Romanos M, Walitza S, Shoichet S,
Dempfle A, Heine M, Boreatti-Hummer A, Romanos J, Gross-Lesch S, Zerlaut H, Wultsch T,
Heinzel S, Fassnacht M, Fallgatter A, Allolio B, Schafer H, Warnke A, Reif A, Ropers HH,
Ullmann R (2010) Genome-wide copy number variation analysis in attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder: association with neuropeptide Y gene dosage in an extended pedigree.
Mol Psychiatry
Levy F, Hay DA, McStephen M, Wood C, Waldman I (1997) Attention-deficit hyperactivity
disorder: a category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. J Am Acad
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 6:737–744
Li J, Nguyen L, Gleason C, Lotspeich L, Spiker D, Risch N, Myers RM (2004) Lack of evidence
for an association between WNT2 and RELN polymorphisms and autism. Am J Med Genet B
Neuropsychiatr Genet 1:51–57
Li D, Sham PC, Owen MJ, He L (2006) Meta-analysis shows significant association between
dopamine system genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Hum Mol Genet
14:2276–2284
Lichtenstein P, Carlstrom E, Rastam M, Gillberg C, Anckarsater H (2010) The Genetics of
Autism Spectrum Disorders and Related Neuropsychiatric Disorders in Childhood. Am J
Psychiatry
Loeys B, Hoebeke P, Raes A, Messiaen L, De PA, Vande WJ (2002) Does monosymptomatic
enuresis exist? A molecular genetic exploration of 32 families with enuresis/incontinence.
BJU Int 1:76–83
Lowe N, Kirley A, Hawi Z, Sham P, Wickham H, Kratochvil CJ, Smith SD, Lee SY, Levy F,
Kent L, Middle F, Rohde LA, Roman T, Tahir E, Yazgan Y, Asherson P, Mill J, Thapar A,
Payton A, Todd RD, Stephens T, Ebstein RP, Manor I, Barr CL, Wigg KG, Sinke RJ,
Buitelaar JK, Smalley SL, Nelson SF, Biederman J, Faraone SV, Gill M (2004) Joint analysis
422 C. M. Freitag et al.
Montague CT, Farooqi IS, Whitehead JP, Soos MA, Rau H, Wareham NJ, Sewter CP, Digby JE,
Mohammed SN, Hurst JA, Cheetham CH, Earley AR, Barnett AH, Prins JB, O’Rahilly S
(1997) Congenital leptin deficiency is associated with severe early-onset obesity in humans.
Nature 6636:903–908
Neale BM, Lasky-Su J, Anney R, Franke B, Zhou K, Maller JB, Vasquez AA, Asherson P, Chen W,
Banaschewski T, Buitelaar J, Ebstein R, Gill M, Miranda A, Oades RD, Roeyers H,
Rothenberger A, Sergeant J, Steinhausen HC, Sonuga-Barke E, Mulas F, Taylor E, Laird N,
Lange C, Daly M, Faraone SV (2008) Genome-wide association scan of attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 8:1337–1344
Neale BM, Medland SE, Ripke S, Asherson P, Franke B, Lesch KP, Faraone SV, Nguyen TT,
Schafer H, Holmans P, Daly M, Steinhausen HC, Freitag C, Reif A, Renner TJ, Romanos M,
Romanos J, Walitza S, Warnke A, Meyer J, Palmason H, Buitelaar J, Vasquez AA,
Lambregts-Rommelse N, Gill M, Anney RJ, Langely K, O’Donovan M, Williams N,
Owen M, Thapar A, Kent L, Sergeant J, Roeyers H, Mick E, Biederman J, Doyle A,
Smalley S, Loo S, Hakonarson H, Elia J, Todorov A, Miranda A, Mulas F, Ebstein RP,
Rothenberger A, Banaschewski T, Oades RD, Sonuga-Barke E, McGough J, Nisenbaum L,
Middleton F, Hu X, Nelson S (2010) Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies of
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 9:884–897
Page DT, Kuti OJ, Prestia C, Sur M (2009) Haploinsufficiency for Pten and Serotonin transporter
cooperatively influences brain size and social behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 6:1989–1994
Paloyelis Y, Rijsdijk F, Wood AC, Asherson P, Kuntsi J (2010) The genetic association between
ADHD symptoms and reading difficulties: the role of inattentiveness and IQ. J Abnorm Child
Psychol 8:1083–1095
Pennisi E (2007) Breakthrough of the year. Human genetic variation. Science 5858:1842–1843
Persico AM, D’Agruma L, Maiorano N, Totaro A, Militerni R, Bravaccio C, Wassink TH,
Schneider C, Melmed R, Trillo S, Montecchi F, Palermo M, Pascucci T, Puglisi-Allegra S,
Reichelt KL, Conciatori M, Marino R, Quattrocchi CC, Baldi A, Zelante L, Gasparini P,
Keller F (2001) Reelin gene alleles and haplotypes as a factor predisposing to autistic
disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2:150–159
Pinto D, Pagnamenta AT, Klei L, Anney R, Merico D, Regan R, Conroy J, Magalhaes TR, Correia C,
Abrahams BS, Almeida J, Bacchelli E, Bader GD, Bailey AJ, Baird G, Battaglia A, Berney T,
Bolshakova N, Bolte S, Bolton PF, Bourgeron T, Brennan S, Brian J, Bryson SE,
Carson AR, Casallo G, Casey J, Chung BH, Cochrane L, Corsello C, Crawford EL,
Crossett A, Cytrynbaum C, Dawson G, de JM, Delorme R, Drmic I, Duketis E, Duque F,
Estes A, Farrar P, Fernandez BA, Folstein SE, Fombonne E, Freitag CM, Gilbert J, Gillberg C,
Glessner JT, Goldberg J, Green A, Green J, Guter SJ, Hakonarson H, Heron EA, Hill M, Holt R,
Howe JL, Hughes G, Hus V, Igliozzi R, Kim C, Klauck SM, Kolevzon A, Korvatska O,
Kustanovich V, Lajonchere CM, Lamb JA, Laskawiec M, Leboyer M, Le CA, Leventhal BL,
Lionel AC, Liu XQ, Lord C, Lotspeich L, Lund SC, Maestrini E, Mahoney W, Mantoulan C,
Marshall CR, McConachie H, McDougle CJ, McGrath J, McMahon WM, Merikangas A,
Migita O, Minshew NJ, Mirza GK, Munson J, Nelson SF, Noakes C, Noor A, Nygren G,
Oliveira G, Papanikolaou K, Parr JR, Parrini B, Paton T, Pickles A, Pilorge M, Piven J,
Ponting CP, Posey DJ, Poustka A, Poustka F, Prasad A, Ragoussis J, Renshaw K, Rickaby J,
Roberts W, Roeder K, Roge B, Rutter ML, Bierut LJ, Rice JP, Salt J, Sansom K, Sato D,
Segurado R, Sequeira AF, Senman L, Shah N, Sheffield VC, Soorya L, Sousa I, Stein O, Sykes N,
Stoppioni V, Strawbridge C, Tancredi R, Tansey K, Thiruvahindrapduram B, Thompson AP,
Thomson S, Tryfon A, Tsiantis J, van EH, Vincent JB, Volkmar F, Wallace S, Wang K, Wang Z,
Wassink TH, Webber C, Weksberg R, Wing K, Wittemeyer K, Wood S, Wu J, Yaspan BL,
Zurawiecki D, Zwaigenbaum L, Buxbaum JD, Cantor RM, Cook EH, Coon H, Cuccaro ML,
Devlin B, Ennis S, Gallagher L, Geschwind DH, Gill M, Haines JL, Hallmayer J, Miller J,
Monaco AP, Nurnberger JI, Jr., Paterson AD, Pericak-Vance MA, Schellenberg GD,
Szatmari P, Vicente AM, Vieland VJ, Wijsman EM, Scherer SW, Sutcliffe JS, Betancur C
(2010) Functional impact of global rare copy number variation in autism spectrum disorders.
Nature 7304:368–372
424 C. M. Freitag et al.
Plomin R (2005) Finding genes in child psychology and psychiatry: when are we going to be
there? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 10:1030–1038
Purper-Ouakil D, Wohl M, Mouren MC, Verpillat P, Ades J, Gorwood P (2005) Meta-analysis of
family-based association studies between the dopamine transporter gene and attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr Genet 1:53–59
Ribases M, Ramos-Quiroga JA, Sanchez-Mora C, Bosch R, Richarte V, Palomar G, Gastaminza X,
Bielsa A, Arcos-Burgos M, Muenke M, Castellanos FX, Cormand B, Bayes M, Casas M (2010)
Contribution of LPHN3 to the genetic susceptibility to ADHD in adulthood: a replication study.
Genes Brain Behav
Romanos M, Freitag C, Jacob C, Craig DW, Dempfle A, Nguyen TT, Halperin R, Walitza S,
Renner TJ, Seitz C, Romanos J, Palmason H, Reif A, Heine M, Windemuth-Kieselbach C,
Vogler C, Sigmund J, Warnke A, Schafer H, Meyer J, Stephan DA, Lesch KP (2008)
Genome-wide linkage analysis of ADHD using high-density SNP arrays: novel loci at 5q13.1
and 14q12. Mol Psychiatry 5:522–530
Ronald A, Simonoff E, Kuntsi J, Asherson P, Plomin R (2008) Evidence for overlapping genetic
influences on autistic and ADHD behaviours in a community twin sample. J Child Psychol
Psychiatry 5:535–542
Rzehak P, Scherag A, Grallert H, Sausenthaler S, Koletzko S, Bauer CP, Schaaf B, von BA,
Berdel D, Borte M, Herbarth O, Kramer U, Illig T, Wichmann HE, Hebebrand J, Heinrich J
(2010) Associations between BMI and the FTO gene are age dependent: results from the GINI
and LISA birth cohort studies up to age 6 years. Obes Facts 3:173–180
Scerri TS, Schulte-Korne G (2010) Genetics of developmental dyslexia. Eur Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 3:179–197
Schellenberg GD, Dawson G, Sung YJ, Estes A, Munson J, Rosenthal E, Rothstein J, Flodman P,
Smith M, Coon H, Leong L, Yu CE, Stodgell C, Rodier PM, Spence MA, Minshew N,
McMahon WM, Wijsman EM (2006) Evidence for multiple loci from a genome scan of
autism kindreds. Mol Psychiatry 11(1049–60):979
Sebat J, Lakshmi B, Malhotra D, Troge J, Lese-Martin C, Walsh T, Yamrom B, Yoon S,
Krasnitz A, Kendall J, Leotta A, Pai D, Zhang R, Lee YH, Hicks J, Spence SJ, Lee AT,
Puura K, Lehtimaki T, Ledbetter D, Gregersen PK, Bregman J, Sutcliffe JS, Jobanputra V,
Chung W, Warburton D, King MC, Skuse D, Geschwind DH, Gilliam TC, Ye K, Wigler M (2007)
Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with autism. Science 5823:445–449
Serajee FJ, Zhong H, Mahbubul Huq AH (2006) Association of Reelin gene polymorphisms with
autism. Genomics 1:75–83
Sha BY, Yang TL, Zhao LJ, Chen XD, Guo Y, Chen Y, Pan F, Zhang ZX, Dong SS, Xu XH,
Deng HW (2009) Genome-wide association study suggested copy number variation may be
associated with body mass index in the Chinese population. J Hum Genet 4:199–202
Shen WH, Balajee AS, Wang J, Wu H, Eng C, Pandolfi PP, Yin Y (2007) Essential role for
nuclear PTEN in maintaining chromosomal integrity. Cell 1:157–170
Shen Y, Dies KA, Holm IA, Bridgemohan C, Sobeih MM, Caronna EB, Miller KJ, Frazier JA,
Silverstein I, Picker J, Weissman L, Raffalli P, Jeste S, Demmer LA, Peters HK, Brewster SJ,
Kowalczyk SJ, Rosen-Sheidley B, McGowan C, Duda AW III, Lincoln SA, Lowe KR,
Schonwald A, Robbins M, Hisama F, Wolff R, Becker R, Nasir R, Urion DK, Milunsky JM,
Rappaport L, Gusella JF, Walsh CA, Wu BL, Miller DT (2010) Clinical genetic testing for
patients with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 4:e727–e735
Sikora DM, Pettit-Kekel K, Penfield J, Merkens LS, Steiner RD (2006) The near universal
presence of autism spectrum disorders in children with Smith-Lemli-Opitz syndrome. Am J
Med Genet A 14:1511–1518
Silventoinen K, Kaprio J (2009) Genetics of tracking of body mass index from birth to late
middle age: evidence from twin and family studies. Obes Facts 3:196–202
Silverman JM, Buxbaum JD, Ramoz N, Schmeidler J, Reichenberg A, Hollander E, Angelo G,
Smith CJ, Kryzak LA (2008) Autism-related routines and rituals associated with a
mitochondrial aspartate/glutamate carrier SLC25A12 polymorphism. Am J Med Genet B
Neuropsychiatr Genet 3:408–410
Behavioral Genetics of Childhood Disorders 425
Skaar DA, Shao Y, Haines JL, Stenger JE, Jaworski J, Martin ER, DeLong GR, Moore JH,
McCauley JL, Sutcliffe JS, Ashley-Koch AE, Cuccaro ML, Folstein SE, Gilbert JR, Pericak-
Vance MA (2005) Analysis of the RELN gene as a genetic risk factor for autism. Mol
Psychiatry 6:563–571
Sousa I, Clark TG, Toma C, Kobayashi K, Choma M, Holt R, Sykes NH, Lamb JA, Bailey AJ,
Battaglia A, Maestrini E, Monaco AP (2009) MET and autism susceptibility: family and
case-control studies. Eur J Hum Genet 6:749–758
Speliotes EK, Willer CJ, Berndt SI, Monda KL, Thorleifsson G, Jackson AU, Allen HL,
Lindgren CM, Luan J, Magi R, Randall JC, Vedantam S, Winkler TW, Qi L, Workalemahu T,
Heid IM, Steinthorsdottir V, Stringham HM, Weedon MN, Wheeler E, Wood AR, Ferreira T,
Weyant RJ, Segre AV, Estrada K, Liang L, Nemesh J, Park JH, Gustafsson S, Kilpelainen TO,
Yang J, Bouatia-Naji N, Esko T, Feitosa MF, Kutalik Z, Mangino M, Raychaudhuri S,
Scherag A, Smith AV, Welch R, Zhao JH, Aben KK, Absher DM, Amin N, Dixon AL,
Fisher E, Glazer NL, Goddard ME, Heard-Costa NL, Hoesel V, Hottenga JJ, Johansson A,
Johnson T, Ketkar S, Lamina C, Li S, Moffatt MF, Myers RH, Narisu N, Perry JR, Peters MJ,
Preuss M, Ripatti S, Rivadeneira F, Sandholt C, Scott LJ, Timpson NJ, Tyrer JP, van WS,
Watanabe RM, White CC, Wiklund F, Barlassina C, Chasman DI, Cooper MN, Jansson JO,
Lawrence RW, Pellikka N, Prokopenko I, Shi J, Thiering E, Alavere H, Alibrandi MT,
Almgren P, Arnold AM, Aspelund T, Atwood LD, Balkau B, Balmforth AJ, Bennett AJ,
Ben-Shlomo Y, Bergman RN, Bergmann S, Biebermann H, Blakemore AI, Boes T,
Bonnycastle LL, Bornstein SR, Brown MJ, Buchanan TA, Busonero F, Campbell H,
Cappuccio FP, Cavalcanti-Proenca C, Chen YD, Chen CM, Chines PS, Clarke R, Coin L,
Connell J, Day IN, Heijer M, Duan J, Ebrahim S, Elliott P, Elosua R, Eiriksdottir G,
Erdos MR, Eriksson JG, Facheris MF, Felix SB, Fischer-Posovszky P, Folsom AR,
Friedrich N, Freimer NB, Fu M, Gaget S, Gejman PV, Geus EJ, Gieger C, Gjesing AP,
Goel A, Goyette P, Grallert H, Grassler J, Greenawalt DM, Groves CJ, Gudnason V, Guiducci C,
Hartikainen AL, Hassanali N, Hall AS, Havulinna AS, Hayward C, Heath AC, Hengstenberg C,
Hicks AA, Hinney A, Hofman A, Homuth G, Hui J, Igl W, Iribarren C, Isomaa B, Jacobs KB,
Jarick I, Jewell E, John U, Jorgensen T, Jousilahti P, Jula A, Kaakinen M, Kajantie E,
Kaplan LM, Kathiresan S, Kettunen J, Kinnunen L, Knowles JW, Kolcic I, Konig IR,
Koskinen S, Kovacs P, Kuusisto J, Kraft P, Kvaloy K, Laitinen J, Lantieri O, Lanzani C,
Launer LJ, Lecoeur C, Lehtimaki T, Lettre G, Liu J, Lokki ML, Lorentzon M, Luben RN,
Ludwig B, Manunta P, Marek D, Marre M, Martin NG, McArdle WL, McCarthy A, McKnight B,
Meitinger T, Melander O, Meyre D, Midthjell K, Montgomery GW, Morken MA, Morris AP,
Mulic R, Ngwa JS, Nelis M, Neville MJ, Nyholt DR, O’Donnell CJ, O’Rahilly S, Ong KK,
Oostra B, Pare G, Parker AN, Perola M, Pichler I, Pietilainen KH, Platou CG, Polasek O,
Pouta A, Rafelt S, Raitakari O, Rayner NW, Ridderstrale M, Rief W, Ruokonen A,
Robertson NR, Rzehak P, Salomaa V, Sanders AR, Sandhu MS, Sanna S, Saramies J,
Savolainen MJ, Scherag S, Schipf S, Schreiber S, Schunkert H, Silander K, Sinisalo J,
Siscovick DS, Smit JH, Soranzo N, Sovio U, Stephens J, Surakka I, Swift AJ, Tammesoo ML,
Tardif JC, Teder-Laving M, Teslovich TM, Thompson JR, Thomson B, Tonjes A, Tuomi T,
van Meurs JB, van Ommen GJ (2010) Association analyses of 249,796 individuals reveal 18 new
loci associated with body mass index. Nat Genet 11:937–948
Stiles BL (2009) Phosphatase and tensin homologue deleted on chromosome 10: extending its
PTENtacles. Int J Biochem Cell Biol 4:757–761
Stutzmann F, Vatin V, Cauchi S, Morandi A, Jouret B, Landt O, Tounian P, Levy-Marchal C,
Buzzetti R, Pinelli L, Balkau B, Horber F, Bougneres P, Froguel P, Meyre D (2007) Non-
synonymous polymorphisms in melanocortin-4 receptor protect against obesity: the two facets
of a Janus obesity gene. Hum Mol Genet 15:1837–1844
Stutzmann F, Tan K, Vatin V, Dina C, Jouret B, Tichet J, Balkau B, Potoczna N, Horber F,
O’Rahilly S, Farooqi IS, Froguel P, Meyre D (2008) Prevalence of melanocortin-4 receptor
deficiency in Europeans and their age-dependent penetrance in multigenerational pedigrees.
Diabetes 9:2511–2518
426 C. M. Freitag et al.
Sullivan PF, Neale MC, Kendler KS (2000) Genetic epidemiology of major depression: review
and meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 10:1552–1562
Sullivan PF, Kendler KS, Neale MC (2003) Schizophrenia as a complex trait: evidence from a
meta-analysis of twin studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry 12:1187–1192
Swanson J, Castellanos FX, Murias M, LaHoste G, Kennedy J (1998a) Cognitive neuroscience of
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and hyperkinetic disorder. Curr Opin Neurobiol
2:263–271
Swanson JM, Sunohara GA, Kennedy JL, Regino R, Fineberg E, Wigal T, Lerner M, Williams L,
LaHoste GJ, Wigal S (1998b) Association of the dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene with a
refined phenotype of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a family-based approach.
Mol Psychiatry 1:38–41
Szatmari P, Paterson AD, Zwaigenbaum L, Roberts W, Brian J, Liu XQ, Vincent JB, Skaug JL,
Thompson AP, Senman L, Feuk L, Qian C, Bryson SE, Jones MB, Marshall CR, Scherer SW,
Vieland VJ, Bartlett C, Mangin LV, Goedken R, Segre A, Pericak-Vance MA, Cuccaro ML,
Gilbert JR, Wright HH, Abramson RK, Betancur C, Bourgeron T, Gillberg C, Leboyer M,
Buxbaum JD, Davis KL, Hollander E, Silverman JM, Hallmayer J, Lotspeich L, Sutcliffe JS,
Haines JL, Folstein SE, Piven J, Wassink TH, Sheffield V, Geschwind DH, Bucan M,
Brown WT, Cantor RM, Constantino JN, Gilliam TC, Herbert M, Lajonchere C,
Ledbetter DH, Lese-Martin C, Miller J, Nelson S, Samango-Sprouse CA, Spence S,
State M, Tanzi RE, Coon H, Dawson G, Devlin B, Estes A, Flodman P, Klei L, McMahon WM,
Minshew N, Munson J, Korvatska E, Rodier PM, Schellenberg GD, Smith M, Spence MA,
Stodgell C, Tepper PG, Wijsman EM, Yu CE, Roge B, Mantoulan C, Wittemeyer K, Poustka A,
Felder B, Klauck SM, Schuster C, Poustka F, Bolte S, Feineis-Matthews S, Herbrecht E,
Schmotzer G, Tsiantis J, Papanikolaou K, Maestrini E, Bacchelli E, Blasi F, Carone S, Toma C,
van Engeland H, de Jonge M, Kemner C, Koop F, Langemeijer M, Hijimans C, Staal WG,
Baird G, Bolton PF, Rutter ML, Weisblatt E, Green J, Aldred C, Wilkinson JA, Pickles A,
Le Couteur A, Berney T, McConachie H, Bailey AJ, Francis K, Honeyman G, Hutchinson A,
Parr JR, Wallace S, Monaco AP, Barnby G, Kobayashi K, Lamb JA, Sousa I, Sykes N,
Cook EH, Guter SJ, Leventhal BL, Salt J, Lord C, Corsello C, Hus V, Weeks DE, Volkmar F,
Tauber M, Fombonne E, Shih A (2007) Mapping autism risk loci using genetic linkage and
chromosomal rearrangements. Nat Genet 3:319–328
Thapar A, Harrington R, McGuffin P (2001) Examining the comorbidity of ADHD-related
behaviours and conduct problems using a twin study design. Br J Psychiatry:224–229
Thapar A, Rice F, Hay D, Boivin J, Langley K, van den Bree M, Rutter M, Harold G (2009)
Prenatal smoking might not cause attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: evidence from a
novel design. Biol Psychiatry 8:722–727
Trikalinos TA, Karvouni A, Zintzaras E, Ylisaukko-Oja T, Peltonen L, Jarvela I, Ioannidis JP
(2006) A heterogeneity-based genome search meta-analysis for autism-spectrum disorders.
Mol Psychiatry 1:29–36
Uhl GR, Drgon T, Liu QR, Johnson C, Walther D, Komiyama T, Harano M, Sekine Y, Inada T,
Ozaki N, Iyo M, Iwata N, Yamada M, Sora I, Chen CK, Liu HC, Ujike H, Lin SK (2008)
Genome-wide association for methamphetamine dependence: convergent results from 2
samples. Arch Gen Psychiatry 3:345–355
Vaisse C, Clement K, Guy-Grand B, Froguel P (1998) A frameshift mutation in human MC4R is
associated with a dominant form of obesity. Nat Genet 2:113–114
van der Zwaag B, Franke L, Poot M, Hochstenbach R, Spierenburg HA, Vorstman JA, van DE,
De Jonge MV, Verbeek NE, Brilstra EH, van ‘t SR, Ophoff RA, van Es MA, Blauw HM,
Veldink JH, Buizer-Voskamp JE, Beemer FA, van den Berg LH, Wijmenga C, van Amstel HK,
van EH, Burbach JP, Staal WG (2009) Gene-network analysis identifies susceptibility genes
related to glycobiology in autism. PLoS ONE 5:e5324
van Rijn S, Swaab H, Aleman A, Kahn RS (2008) Social behavior and autism traits in a sex
chromosomal disorder: Klinefelter (47XXY) syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 9:1634–1641
Von Gontard A, Hollmann E, Eiberg H, Benden B, Rittig S, Lehmkuhl G (1997) Clinical enuresis
phenotypes in familial nocturnal enuresis. Scand J Urol Nephrol (Suppl):11–16
Behavioral Genetics of Childhood Disorders 427
Altshuler D, Caulfield MJ, Chanock SJ, Farooqi IS, Ferrucci L, Guralnik JM, Hattersley AT,
Hu FB, Jarvelin MR, Laakso M, Mooser V, Ong KK, Ouwehand WH, Salomaa V, Samani NJ,
Spector TD, Tuomi T, Tuomilehto J, Uda M, Uitterlinden AG, Wareham NJ, Deloukas P,
Frayling TM, Groop LC, Hayes RB, Hunter DJ, Mohlke KL, Peltonen L, Schlessinger D,
Strachan DP, Wichmann HE, McCarthy MI, Boehnke M, Barroso I, Abecasis GR, Hirschhorn JN
(2009) Six new loci associated with body mass index highlight a neuronal influence on body
weight regulation. Nat Genet 1:25–34
Williams NM, Zaharieva I, Martin A, Langley K, Mantripragada K, Fossdal R, Stefansson H,
Stefansson K, Magnusson P, Gudmundsson OO, Gustafsson O, Holmans P, Owen MJ,
O’Donovan M, Thapar A (2010) Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications in attention-
deficit hyperactivity disorder: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet 9750:1401–1408
Wood AC (2010) Rethinking shared environment as a source of variance underlying attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms: comment on Burt (2009). Psychol Bull
Wood AC, Rijsdijk F, Asherson P, Kuntsi J (2009) Hyperactive-impulsive symptom scores and
oppositional behaviours reflect alternate manifestations of a single liability. Behav Genet 5:447–460
Wu S, Jia M, Ruan Y, Liu J, Guo Y, Shuang M, Gong X, Zhang Y, Yang X, Zhang D (2005)
Positive association of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) with autism in the Chinese Han
population. Biol Psychiatry 1:74–77
Xiang B, Zhu H, Shen Y, Miller DT, Lu K, Hu X, Andersson HC, Narumanchi TM, Wang Y,
Martinez JE, Wu BL, Li P, Li MM, Chen TJ, Fan YS (2010) Genome-wide oligonucleotide
array comparative genomic hybridization for etiological diagnosis of mental retardation: a
multicenter experience of 1499 clinical cases. J Mol Diagn 2:204–212
Yang B, Chan RC, Jing J, Li T, Sham P, Chen RY (2007) A meta-analysis of association studies
between the 10-repeat allele of a VNTR polymorphism in the 3’-UTR of dopamine transporter
gene and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet
Yang P, Lung FW, Jong YJ, Hsieh HY, Liang CL, Juo SH (2008) Association of the homeobox
transcription factor gene ENGRAILED 2 with autistic disorder in Chinese children.
Neuropsychobiology 1–2:3–8
Yeo GS, Farooqi IS, Aminian S, Halsall DJ, Stanhope RG, O’Rahilly S (1998) A frameshift
mutation in MC4R associated with dominantly inherited human obesity. Nat Genet 2:111–112
Yeo GS, Connie Hung CC, Rochford J, Keogh J, Gray J, Sivaramakrishnan S, O’rahilly S,
Farooqi IS (2004) A de novo mutation affecting human TrkB associated with severe obesity
and developmental delay. Nat Neurosci 11:1187–1189
Young EH, Wareham NJ, Farooqi S, Hinney A, Hebebrand J, Scherag A, O’Rahilly S, Barroso I,
Sandhu MS (2007) The V103I polymorphism of the MC4R gene and obesity: population
based studies and meta-analysis of 29 563 individuals. Int J Obes (Lond) 9:1437–1441
Zhang Y, Proenca R, Maffei M, Barone M, Leopold L, Friedman JM (1994) Positional cloning of
the mouse obese gene and its human homologue. Nature 6505:425–432
Zhang H, Liu X, Zhang C, Mundo E, Macciardi F, Grayson DR, Guidotti AR, Holden JJ (2002) Reelin
gene alleles and susceptibility to autism spectrum disorders. Mol Psychiatry 9:1012–1017
Zhou K, Chen W, Buitelaar J, Banaschewski T, Oades RD, Franke B, Sonuga-Barke E,
Ebstein R, Eisenberg J, Gill M, Manor I, Miranda A, Mulas F, Roeyers H, Rothenberger A,
Sergeant J, Steinhausen HC, Lasky-Su J, Taylor E, Brookes KJ, Xu X, Neale BM, Rijsdijk F,
Thompson M, Asherson P, Faraone SV (2008a) Genetic heterogeneity in ADHD: DAT1 gene
only affects probands without CD. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 8:1481–1487
Zhou K, Dempfle A, Arcos-Burgos M, Bakker SC, Banaschewski T, Biederman J, Buitelaar J,
Castellanos FX, Doyle A, Ebstein RP, Ekholm J, Forabosco P, Franke B, Freitag C, Friedel S,
Gill M, Hebebrand J, Hinney A, Jacob C, Lesch KP, Loo SK, Lopera F, McCracken JT,
McGough JJ, Meyer J, Mick E, Miranda A, Muenke M, Mulas F, Nelson SF, Nguyen TT,
Oades RD, Ogdie MN, Palacio JD, Pineda D, Reif A, Renner TJ, Roeyers H, Romanos M,
Rothenberger A, Schafer H, Sergeant J, Sinke RJ, Smalley SL, Sonuga-Barke E, Steinhausen HC,
van der Meulen E, Walitza S, Warnke A, Lewis CM, Faraone SV, Asherson P (2008b) Meta-
analysis of genome-wide linkage scans of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet
B Neuropsychiatr Genet 8:1392–1398
Schizophrenia Genes: On the Matter
of Their Convergence
Dan Rujescu
Contents
1 Schizophrenia..................................................................................................................... 430
2 Structural Genetic Variants ............................................................................................... 430
3 Common Genetic Variants................................................................................................ 434
References................................................................................................................................ 436
D. Rujescu (&)
Department of Psychiatry,
Ludwig-Maximilians-University,
Nußbaumstr. 7, 80336 München, Germany
e-mail: Dan.Rujescu@med.uni-muenchen.de
1 Schizophrenia
Several approaches have been used to find causative genetic variants including
linkage studies, candidate gene or genome wide association studies as well as
studies on structural genetic variants. It is not a new assumption that structural
genetic variants including deletions or duplications may play a role in
schizophrenia. However, the assumption that rare genetic variants with large
effects may account for a significant number of schizophrenia cases has been
somehow neglected. This occurred despite the fact that rare variants with large
effects on schizophrenia risk are already well established, with the 22q11 deletion
syndrome and DISC1 being among the more prominent findings.
David St Clair et al. (1990) identified a balanced 1q43:11q14 translocation
associated with major mental illness in a large pedigree showing several psychi-
atric phenotypes associated with this translocation (St Clair et al. 1990). Within
this translocation a disrupted gene (DISC1) was identified which is now an
important risk gene for psychosis. Although the mutation is very rare, its discovery
boosted a series of studies on the potential pathobiology underlying psychosis.
Twenty years of intensive work went by and much research has been undertaken
to define the biological function of the DISC1 protein and to further understand
how it contributes to the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. There is evidence for
Schizophrenia Genes 431
coincides with peak forebrain synaptogenesis shortly after birth. Furthermore, their
protein products are associated with brain mitochondria, including those in syn-
aptic terminals. The authors concluded that 22q11 deletion may alter metabolic
properties of cortical mitochondria during early post-natal life, and that several
22q11 mitochondrial genes, particularly during early post-natal cortical develop-
ment, may disrupt neuronal metabolism or synaptic signalling which seems highly
interesting for the pathophysiology of schizophrenia (Maynard et al. 2008).
These two regions were the most prominent large structural variants associated
with schizophrenia until recently. However, the rapid technological development
which paved the way for genome wide association studies provided new unex-
pected possibilities for the detection of shorter structural variants, namely for copy
number variants (CNVs). The first report on submicroscopic micodeletions or
microduplications in schizophrenia was provided by Walsh et al. (2008) and was
the starting point for the revival of structural aberrations in schizophrenia. Novel
deletions and duplications of genes were present in 5% of controls versus 15% of
cases and 20% of young onset cases. These mutations disrupted genes dispro-
portionately more often from signalling networks controlling neurodevelopment,
including neuregulin and glutamate pathways. Although the sample size for the
detection of these rare events was small, Walsh et al. (2008) suggested that
multiple, individually rare mutations altering genes in neurodevelopmental path-
ways contribute to schizophrenia.
The first well powered study on CNVs associating with schizophrenia was
performed by the SGENE+ consortium. A population based sample was used to
identify de novo CNVs by analysing 9,878 transmissions from parents to offspring
(Stefansson et al. 2008). The 66 de novo CNVs identified were tested for asso-
ciation in a sample of 1,433 schizophrenia cases and 33,250 controls. Three
deletions at 1q21.1, 15q11.2 and 15q13.3 showing nominal association with
schizophrenia in the first sample were followed up in a second sample of 3,285
cases and 7,951 controls and all three deletions were significantly associated with
schizophrenia and related psychoses in the combined sample. Interestingly, two of
these regions (1q21.1 and 15q13.3) could be replicated independently by
the International Schizophrenia Consortium in 3,391 patients with schizophrenia
and 3,181 controls (ISC 2008). The third (15q11.2) association has also been
replicated independently by Kirov et al. (2009a). These studies have brought
highest interest in human genetics of complex diseases and encouraged other
groups to analyse CNVs.
Another main finding in schizophrenia is that deletions within the neurexin 1
gene (NRXN1; 2p16.3) are associated with schizophrenia (Kirov et al. 2008;
Walsh et al. 2008). The SGENE+ consortium examined Neurexin 1 for CNVs in
2,977 schizophrenia patients and 33,746 controls from seven European populations
using microarray data. The association analysis was restricted to CNVs that disrupt
exons. These were significantly associated with a high odds ratio (OR 8.97),
showing that Neurexin 1 deletions disrupting exons confer risk of schizophrenia
(Rujescu et al. 2009). A meta-analysis which included 8,789 cases and 42,054
controls provided further evidence for the involvement of Neurexin 1 in Schizophrenia.
Schizophrenia Genes 433
and 6,000 additional CNVs. Over half of them could be mapped to nucleotide
resolution, which facilitates analysing their origin and functional impact. This
study shows that CNVs are abundant in humans, differing from other forms of
variation in extent, origin and functional impact (Milles et al. 2011). Therefore it is
very possible that based on these new findings, rare new genomic disorders will be
defined and that our understanding of neurodevelopmental disorders including
schizophrenia will hopefully be highly enlarged.
Also common polymorphisms (SNPs) were analysed using genome wide associ-
ation studies. The first study of this type included 178 cases and 144 controls and
was performed by Lencz et al. (2007). The best associated SNP out of 500,000
SNPs was located in CSF2RA (colony stimulating factor, receptor 2 alpha).
A second genome wide association (GWA) study did not achieved genome wide
significance Sullivan et al. (2008). Both studies were clearly underpowered to
provide conclusive result. Therefore, O’Donovan (2008) performed an initial
GWAS on a much larger sample including 479 cases and 2,937 controls. Loci
surpassing p \ 10-E5 were followed up in 6,829 cases and 9,897 controls. Of 12
of these loci, 3 had strong independent support and the overall pattern of repli-
cation was unlikely to occur by chance. The evidence for association for the top
SNP in the ZNF804A gene strengthened when the affected phenotype included
bipolar disorder (O’Donovan et al. 2008). Interestingly, this result was replicated
independently in the Irish Case Control Study of Schizophrenia (ICCSS) sample
(Riley et al. 2010) as well as in an independent sample of 5,164 schizophrenia
cases and 20,709 controls (Steinberg et al. 2011). A fine-mapping, replication and
meta-analysis study of 18,945 schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder patients,
21,274 schizophrenia plus bipolar disorder cases and 38,675 controls showed
further evidence for this SNP (Williams et al. 2010). Additionally, there is evi-
dence for the involvement of this SNP in neuropsychological phenotypes. Walters
et al. (2010) investigated whether the identified risk allele of the SNP rs1344706 is
associated with variations in neuropsychological performance in patients and
controls. Patients with DSM-IV diagnosed schizophrenia and healthy controls
from independent samples of Irish (n = 297 cases and n = 165 controls) and
German (n = 251 cases and n = 1472 controls) nationality were included. In the
Irish samples ZNF804A genotype was associated with differences in episodic and
working memory in patients but not controls. These findings replicated in the same
direction in the German sample. Furthermore, in both samples, when patients with
lower IQ were excluded the association between ZNF804A and schizophrenia
strengthened (Walters et al. 2010). Taken together, the ZNF804A gene is one of
the most promising genes for schizophrenia to date.
The largest GWA study to date on schizophrenia was led by Stefansson et al.
(2009). 2,663 schizophrenia cases and 13,498 controls from eight European
Schizophrenia Genes 435
locations were studied within the SGENE+ consortium. Findings from the top
1,500 markers were combined with results for these markers from both the
International Schizophrenia Consortium (ISC 2,602 cases/2,885 controls) and the
European–American portion of the Molecular Genetics of Schizophrenia Con-
sortium (MGS 2,687 cases/2,656 controls). The best markers were followed up in
5,013 cases and 15,559 controls from four sets of additional samples from Europe.
This approach could identify three novel schizophrenia loci: Neurogranin
(NRGN), TCF4 and the HLA region (Stefansson et al. 2009). Interestingly, the
HLA associations were also found in the large GWAS by the International
Schizophrenia Consortium ISC (2009) studying 3,322 European individuals with
schizophrenia and 3,587 controls as well as by (Shi et al. 2009) suggesting further
evidence for the inflammation theory of schizophrenia. The second gene, TCF4
(transcription cell factor 4), is of high interest too as the Pitt–Hopkins Syndrome’s
gene was identified by unrelated groups by comparative genomic hybridization
(CGH) in 2007 showing that the haploinsufficiency of the TCF4 gene (18q21.2) is
due to an autosomal dominant de novo mutation, which is considered to be
causative (for review see Taddeucci et al. 2010). Finally, NRGN (neurogranin), the
third gene is highly promising. It is a calmodulin-binding protein expressed
exclusively in the brain. It is the main postsynaptic protein regulating the avail-
ability of calmodulin, binding to it in the absence of calcium (Stefansson et al.
2009). Neurogranin belongs to the calpacitin family and contains an IQ domain
that interacts with the Ca2+ free form of calmodulin (apo-calmodulin). Depending
on the intracellular Ca2+ concentration, neurogranin releases calmodulin, so that it
can bind Ca2+ and activate downstream signalling molecules. Calmodulin fur-
thermore binds different proteins including CaMKII, the calcium/calmodulin-
dependent protein kinase II gamma (Hayashi 2009). Zhong et al. (2009) show that
neurogranin is concentrated in dendritic spines and that the number of neurogranin
molecules available determines the efficiency of calmodulin signalling in the
synapse and the strength of AMPA receptor transmission (Zhong et al. 2009).
An exogenous overexpression of neurogranin is sufficient to trigger a Ca2+ signal
that normally induces Long Term Potentiation (LTP) which is crucial for learning
and memory. This neurogranin-induced synaptic potentiation and LTP share
common signalling mechanisms. Like LTP, neurogranin-induced potentiation
requires the activity of NMDA receptors and CaMKII. Furthermore, in such
neurons that overexpress neurogranin and where the AMPA receptor response is
therefore already potentiated, LTP can no longer be electrophysiologically
induced. Taken together, these observations in turn imply the involvement of
neurogranin in the signalling pathway of LTP (Hayashi 2009).
Further large genome wide association studies on schizophrenia are currently
under way and will hopefully add new relevant knowledge on schizophrenia.
Based on this, functional studies including cell and animal models will be
necessary. There is new hope that these new avenues will help understanding the
neurobiology of schizophrenia in more depth leading to the development of new
innovative diagnostic tools and therapies.
436 D. Rujescu
References
Abrahams BS, Geschwind DH (2008) Advances in autism genetics: on the threshold of a new
neurobiology. Nat Rev Genet 9(5):341–355
Bradshaw NJ, Porteous DJ (2010) DISC1-binding proteins in neural development, signalling and
schizophrenia. Neuropharmacology. (Epub ahead of print)
Cardno AG, Marshall EJ, Coid B, Macdonald AM, Ribchester TR, Davies NJ, Venturi P, Jones
LA, Lewis SW, Sham PC, Gottesman II, Farmer AE, McGuffin P, Reveley AM, Murray RM
(1999) Heritability estimates for psychotic disorders: the Maudsley twin psychosis series.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 56:162–168
Cook EH Jr, Scherer SW (2008) Copy-number variations associated with neuropsychiatric
conditions. Nature 455(7215):919–923
de Vries BB, Pfundt R, Leisink M, Koolen DA, Vissers LE, Janssen IM, Reijmersdal S, Nillesen
WM, Huys EH, Leeuw N, Smeets D, Sistermans EA, Feuth T, Ravenswaaij-Arts CM, van
Kessel AG, Schoenmakers EF, Brunner HG, Veltman JA (2005) Diagnostic genome profiling
in mental retardation. Am J Hum Genet 77(4):606–616
Dickinson D, Elvevåg B (2009) Genes, cognition and brain through a COMT lens. Neuroscience
164(1):72–87
Gill M, Donohoe G, Corvin A (2010) What have the genomics ever done for the psychoses?
Psychol Med Apr 40(4):529–540
Gothelf D, Schaer M, Eliez S (2008) Genes, brain development and psychiatric phenotypes in
velo-cardio-facial syndrome. Dev Disabil Res Rev 14(1):59–68
Hayashi Y (2009) Long-term potentiation: two pathways meet at neurogranin. EMBO J
28(19):2859–2860
International Schizophrenia Consortium (ISC) (2008) Rare chromosomal deletions and
duplications increase risk of schizophrenia. Nature 455(7210):237–241
International Schizophrenia Consortium (ISC), Purcell SM, Wray NR, Stone JL, Visscher PM,
O’Donovan MC, Sullivan PF, Sklar P (2009) Common polygenic variation contributes to risk
of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature 460(7256):748–752
Ingason A, Rujescu D, Cichon S, Sigurdsson E, Sigmundsson T, Pietiläinen OPH, Buizer J,
Francks C, Muglia P, Gylfason A, Gustafsson O, Olason PI, Steinberg S, Hansen T, Giegling
I, Möller HJ, Hartmann AM, Shianna KV, Ge D, Crombie C, Fraser G, Walker N, Lonnqvist
J, Suvisaari J, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Picchioni M, Kiemeney LA, Franke B, Murray R, Vassos
E, Ettinger U, Mühleisen TW, Tosato S, Ruggeri M, Djurovic S, Andreasson OA, GROUP,
Werge T, Ophoff R, Rietschel M, Goldstein D, Noethen M, Petursson H, Stefansson K,
Peltonen L, Collier DA, Stefansson H, St Clair D (2011a) Copy number variations of
chromosome 16p13.1 region associated with schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry 16(1):17-25
Ingason A, Kirov G, Giegling I, Hansen T, Isles AR, Jakobsen KD, Kristinsson KT, le Roux L,
Gustafsson O, Craddock N, Möller HJ, McQuillin A, Muglia P, Cichon S, Rietschel M,
Ophoff RA, Djurovic S, Andreassen OA, Pietiläinen OP, Peltonen L, Dempster E, Collier DA,
St Clair D, Rasmussen HB, Glenthøj BY, Kiemeney LA, Franke B, Tosato S, Bonetto C,
Saemundsen E, Hreidarsson SJ; GROUP Investigators, Nöthen MM, Gurling H, O’Donovan
MC, Owen MJ, Sigurdsson E, Petursson H, Stefansson H, Rujescu D, Stefansson K, Werge T.
Maternally (2011b) Derived microduplications at 15q11–q13: Implication of imprinted genes
in psychotic illness. Am J Psychiatry
Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA (2004) The molecular genetics of the 22q11-associated schizophrenia.
Brain Res Mol Brain Res 132(2):95–104
Kirov G, Gumus D, Chen W, Norton N, Georgieva L, Sari M, O’Donovan MC, Erdogan F, Owen
MJ, Ropers HH, Ullmann R (2008a) Comparative genome hybridization suggests a role for
NRXN1 and APBA2 in schizophrenia. Hum Mol Genet 17(3):458–465
Kirov G, Grozeva D, Norton N, Ivanov D, Mantripragada KK, Holmans P (2009a) International
Schizophrenia Consortium; Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium, Craddock N,
Schizophrenia Genes 437
Owen MJ, O’Donovan MC. Support for the involvement of large copy number variants in the
pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Hum Mol Genet 18(8):1497–1503
Kirov G, Rujescu D, Ingason A, Collier DA, O’Donovan MC, Owen MJ (2009b) Neurexin 1
(NRXN1) deletions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 35(5):851–854
Lencz T, Morgan TV, Athanasiou M, Dain B, Reed CR, Kane JM, Kucherlapati R, Malhotra AK
(2007) Converging evidence for a pseudoautosomal cytokine receptor gene locus in
schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 12(6):572–580
Maynard TM, Meechan DW, Dudevoir ML, Gopalakrishna D, Peters AZ, Heindel CC, Sugimoto
TJ, Wu Y, Lieberman JA, Lamantia AS (2008) Mitochondrial localization and function of a
subset of 22q11 deletion syndrome candidate genes. Mol Cell Neurosci 39(3):439–451
Meechan DW, Tucker ES, Maynard TM, LaMantia AS (2009) Diminished dosage of 22q11
genes disrupts neurogenesis and cortical development in a mouse model of 22q11 deletion/
DiGeorge syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106(38):16434–16445
Mefford HC, Sharp AJ, Baker C, Itsara A, Jiang Z, Buysse K, Huang S, Maloney VK, Crolla JA,
Baralle D, Collins A, Mercer C, Norga K, de Ravel T, Devriendt K, Bongers EM, de Leeuw
N, Reardon W, Gimelli S, Bena F, Hennekam RC, Male A, Gaunt L, Clayton-Smith J,
Simonic I, Park SM, Mehta SG, Nik-Zainal S, Woods CG, Firth HV, Parkin G, Fichera M,
Reitano S, Lo Giudice M, Li KE, Casuga I, Broomer A, Conrad B, Schwerzmann M, Räber L,
Gallati S, Striano P, Coppola A, Tolmie JL, Tobias ES, Lilley C, Armengol L, Spysschaert Y,
Verloo P, De Coene A, Goossens L, Mortier G, Speleman F, van Binsbergen E, Nelen MR,
Hochstenbach R, Poot M, Gallagher L, Gill M, McClellan J, King MC, Regan R, Skinner C,
Stevenson RE, Antonarakis SE, Chen C, Estivill X, Menten B, Gimelli G, Gribble S, Schwartz
S, Sutcliffe JS, Walsh T, Knight SJ, Sebat J, Romano C, Schwartz CE, Veltman JA, de Vries
BB, Vermeesch JR, Barber JC, Willatt L, Tassabehji M, Eichler EE (2008) Recurrent
rearrangements of chromosome 1q21.1 and variable pediatric phenotypes. N Engl J Med
359(16):1685–1699
Mills RE, Walter K, Stewart C, Handsaker RE, Chen K, Alkan C, Abyzov A, Yoon SC, Ye K,
Cheetham RK, Chinwalla A, Conrad DF, Fu Y, Grubert F, Hajirasouliha I, Hormozdiari F,
Iakoucheva LM, Iqbal Z, Kang S, Kidd JM, Konkel MK, Korn J, Khurana E, Kural D, Lam
HY, Leng J, Li R, Li Y, Lin CY, Luo R, Mu XJ, Nemesh J, Peckham HE, Rausch T, Scally A,
Shi X, Stromberg MP, Stütz AM, Urban AE, Walker JA, Wu J, Zhang Y, Zhang ZD, Batzer
MA, Ding L, Marth GT, McVean G, Sebat J, Snyder M, Wang J, Ye K, Eichler EE, Gerstein
MB, Hurles ME, Lee C, McCarroll SA, Korbel JO (2011) 1000 Genomes Project. Mapping
copy number variation by population-scale genome sequencing. Nature 3 470(7332):59–65
Muir WJ, Pickard BS, Blackwood DH (2008) Disrupted-in-Schizophrenia-1. Curr Psychiatry Rep
10(2):140–147
O’Donovan MC, Craddock N, Norton N, Williams H, Peirce T, Moskvina V, Nikolov I,
Hamshere M, Carroll L, Georgieva L, Dwyer S, Holmans P, Marchini JL, Spencer CC, Howie
B, Leung HT, Hartmann AM, Möller HJ, Morris DW, Shi Y, Feng G, Hoffmann P, Propping
P, Vasilescu C, Maier W, Rietschel M, Zammit S, Schumacher J, Quinn EM, Schulze TG,
Williams NM, Giegling I, Iwata N, Ikeda M, Darvasi A, Shifman S, He L, Duan J, Sanders
AR, Levinson DF, Gejman PV, Cichon S, Nöthen MM, Gill M, Corvin A, Rujescu D, Kirov
G, Owen MJ, Buccola NG, Mowry BJ, Freedman R, Amin F, Black DW, Silverman JM,
Byerley WF, Cloninger CR (2008) Molecular genetics of schizophrenia collaboration.
identification of loci associated with schizophrenia by genome-wide association and follow-
up. Nat Genet 40(9):1053–1055
Okochi T, Ikeda M, Kishi T, Kawashima K, Kinoshita Y, Kitajima T, Yamanouchi Y, Tomita M,
Inada T, Ozaki N, Iwata N (2009) Meta-analysis of association between genetic variants in
COMT and schizophrenia: an update. Schizophr Res 110(1–3):140–148
Paylor R, Glaser B, Mupo A, Ataliotis P, Spencer C, Sobotka A, Sparks C, Choi CH, Oghalai J,
Curran S, Murphy KC, Monks S, Williams N, O’Donovan MC, Owen MJ, Scambler PJ,
Lindsay E (2006) Tbx1 haploinsufficiency is linked to behavioral disorders in mice and
humans: implications for 22q11 deletion syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103(20):
7729–7734
438 D. Rujescu
Contents
1 Introduction........................................................................................................................ 442
1.1 What is a G 9 E Interaction? .................................................................................. 442
1.2 Other Forms of Gene–Environment Co-Action:
Gene–Environment Correlations .............................................................................. 444
2 Theoretical Considerations for G 9 E Interaction Studies ............................................. 444
2.1 Can We Model G 9 E Interaction in Statistics? .................................................... 444
1 Introduction
One of the oldest and most enduring questions in psychiatry is whether mental illness is
caused by nature (genes) or nurture (environment). Decades of epidemiology studies
have tried to answer this question through twin and adoption studies. These studies
have demonstrated a moderate genetic component for some disorders (depression and
alcohol dependence) and a high genetic component for others (schizophrenia and
autism). The relatively high heritability of psychiatric disorders has prompted
investigators to look deeply for direct connections between genes and mental illness.
Over the past 20 years, thousands of studies have been performed assessing the direct
relationship between genes and mental illness in the form of candidate gene associ-
ation studies, linkage studies and more recently, genome-wide association studies
(GWAS). Despite the intense effort, very few direct genetic effects have been iden-
tified (Moffitt et al. 2005; Rutter et al. 2006). Therefore, researchers have increasingly
directed their attention to the investigation of interactions between genes and envi-
ronment, a possibility that has traditionally been understudied in behavioral and
psychiatric genetics (Caspi 1998; Scarr 1992). In contrast, G 9 E interactions have
been demonstrated consistently in other branches of medicine (van Os et al. 2008).
Hence, G 9 E interaction research is an emerging discipline in psychiatric genetics
with growing numbers of novices in need of a comprehensive introduction to the field.
In this chapter we aim to give such an introduction, starting with a detailed definition of
G 9 E interaction. We then discuss two fundamental, but controversial theoretical
questions about the validity of statistical models for biological interaction and the
utility of G 9 E interaction research for the field of psychiatric genetics. Finally, we
discuss practical aspects of studying G 9 E interactions, with an emphasis on study
designs and assessment methods that may affect the success of G 9 E interaction
studies, and present relevant examples from the field.
The term ‘‘G 9 E interaction’’ stems from regression models that seek to divide
the population variance for disorder risk into environmental and genetic parts.
Effects of these factors that are independent from one another are called
Gene 9 Environment Interaction Models in Psychiatric Genetics 443
Fig. 1 Illustration of main and interaction effects of genes and environmental exposure on
disorder risk. Solid line Genotype A, dashed line Genotype B. a Genetic main effect
b Environmental main effect c Additive effect of genes and environmental exposure
d Quantitative interaction effect e Qualitative interaction effect
main effects. The main effect of either the genetic or the environmental factor can
explain the variance for the disorder entirely (Fig. 1a, b) or both factors can coact and
explain the variance additively, operating independently alongside each other
(Fig. 1c). Consider a child with a retinoblastoma, a malignant tumor of the retina
caused by an inherited mutation in one allele of the tumor suppressor gene Rb1. If the
patients’ unaffected eye gets injured through an accident and the eyesight of this
patient becomes worse, the genetic and the environmental factor operate together on
the same outcome (eyesight), but are not involved in the same biological pathway and
fully independent factors. In contrast, in an interaction effect, the factors depend from
each other (Fig. 1d, e). In biological terms, such a G 9 E interaction effect occurs
when the effect of exposure to an environmental factor on the disorder status depends
on the person’s genotype (Moffitt et al. 2006). In other words, a G 9 E interaction is
defined by differences of genotypes in susceptibility to environmental exposure
(Kendler and Eaves 1986). For example, our patient with retinoblastoma has an
impaired DNA repair system causing her to be markedly more susceptible to UV
light compared to an individual without the mutation. By exposure to UV light,
tumors develop and worsen the patient’s eyesight. Thus, the effect of the exposure to
the environmental factor (radiation) on the outcome (eyesight) depends on the per-
son’s genotype, constituting an example for G 9 E interaction. G 9 E interactions
can be quantitative, i. e. the exposure to the environmental pathogen increases the
disorder risk for all genotypes, but to different extends (Fig. 1d) or they can
be qualitative, i.e. the exposure to the environmental factor increases the risk for one
genotype, but decreases it for another (Fig. 1e) (Ottman 1990). With respect to our
previous example, a qualitative interaction would occur if UV radiation decreases the
risk for retinoblastoma for one genotype, whereas it would increase it for another.
444 K. Karg and S. Sen
Genes and environmental factors can co-act in different ways, and not all of them
are G 9 E interactions [see (Moffitt et al. 2006) for details]. Gene–environment
correlations (rGE) are of particular importance, because they can produce false-
positive findings in G 9 E interaction research. rGE can occur when a person’s
genotype influences her probability of exposure to environmental risks (Plomin
et al. 1977; Rutter and Silberg 2002). Several mechanisms have been proposed to
drive rGE (Plomin et al. 1977; Jaffee and Price 2007). In active rGE an individual
actively selects her environment according to her (genetically influenced) traits
and behaviors. For instance, an individual characteristically risk-seeking and
impulsive may be much more prone to risk environments than a cautious indi-
vidual. The presence of rGE has been demonstrated through twin and adoption
studies for a wide range of factors, including the occurrence of life events, such as
divorce, job loss and serious accidents (Rutter et al. 2006; Rutter and Silberg
2002). The common nature of rGE underscores the danger in the independence
assumption of genotype and environment in G 9 E interaction research. This
assumption can be a major problem for some study designs, in particular case-only
studies (Jaffee and Price 2007) (further details below).
effects model. This model is used for continuous outcomes, such as depression
scores. The multiplicative model constitutes a G 9 E interaction when the disorder
risk if exposed to both G and E differs from the product of the risks if exposed only
to G or to E. This is used for categorical outcomes, e.g. diagnosis of depres-
sion with the two categories ‘‘depressed’’ and ‘‘non-depressed’’. The biological
meaning of a multiplicative model is hard to grasp and most researchers argue that
the additive model better reflects biological concepts (Rutter et al. 2009). The
problem is that in some cases, a study result might deviate significantly from a
multiplicative model, but not from an additive model, and vice versa (Kendler and
Gardner 2010) (Table 1a, b). This is particularly problematic as continuous out-
comes can be converted to categorical outcomes by setting an arbitrary threshold.
Given sufficient statistical power, this threshold can be chosen so that either of
both models indicate a significant interaction effect. Some researchers argue that
this model-dependency renders positive G 9 E interaction findings arbitrary
(Zammit et al. 2010a) and testing for interactions across multiple models is
therefore ‘‘no different from trawling through many statistical tests looking for
those that are significant’’ (Kendler and Gardner 2010). Therefore, the statistical
model to be tested should be carefully selected a priori, based on biological
background considerations, and thresholds for categorical data should be set before
the analysis. Unfortunately, our current knowledge about neurological pathways is
very limited, and, as a result, it is still unclear which statistical model is appro-
priate (Thompson 1991). This situation has caused some leaders to conclude that
we might be unable to move back and forth between statistical and biological
446 K. Karg and S. Sen
interaction models (Kendler and Gardner 2010). The debate remains controversial
(Rutter et al. 2006; Zammit et al. 2010a; Caspi and Moffitt 2006; Munafo et al.
2009). One way that investigators have used to circumvent this statistical problem
is utilizing new study designs such as case-only or exposed-only designs. These
designs do not rely on testing statistical interactions, but directly test differences in
exposure rate (case-only design) or in disorder status (exposed-only design)
between genotype groups. To date, these designs have mostly been applied in
psychiatric G 9 E interaction research to investigate the interaction between a
serotonin transporter gene promoter variant (5-HTTLPR) and stress on the risk of
depression (Caspi et al. 2003), with mostly positive results (Karg et al. 2010).
There are three primary arguments for why the identification of G 9 E interaction
effects will substantially advance the field. First, they can help identify new
genetic and environmental main effects associated with psychiatric disorders
(Kraft et al. 2007). Some risk genes and environments might be masked from
detection in scans for direct genes-to-disorder or environment-to-disorder associ-
ations because of genotype-specific environmental effects on the disorder status
due to G 9 E interactions. Second, knowledge about the interaction effect of gene
and environment on a psychiatric disorder might enhance the identification of the
biological pathway underlying the interaction by revealing the genetic and envi-
ronmental factors involved and thus channel neuroscience studies in a productive
direction (Caspi and Moffitt 2006). Third, G 9 E interaction findings may have
clinical relevance and drive the development toward personalized medicine or
individual lifestyle recommendations based on the genetic profile (Dempfle et al.
2008; Uher and McGuffin 2007). They could explain differences in response to
pharmacological and psychological treatments by differences in the susceptibility
of genotypes to environmental factors. Individuals with high-susceptibility geno-
types could be identified and prevented from suffering exposure to the relevant
environmental pathogens.
Several researchers have criticized this optimistic view, pointing out that the
G 9 E interaction effects identified to date are small, with odds ratios generally
between 0.67 and 1.5 (Manolio et al. 2008), limiting the potential influence of G 9 E
interaction on advances in psychiatric genetics and clinical practice (Zammit et al.
2010a, b; Hunter et al. 2008). In particular, the power for finding main effects
might only marginally increase by including G 9 E interaction effects in the
statistical model (Munafo et al. 2009). In addition, G 9 E findings might help
identify the underlying biological pathway only through the detection of qualita-
tive G 9 E interactions, a case known to be rare in epidemiology (Thompson
1991). Thus, there is an ongoing debate about the benefit of G 9 E interaction
research and the considerable amounts of resources spent in the field (Kendler and
Gardner 2010; Uher and McGuffin 2007; Zammit et al. 2010b).
Gene 9 Environment Interaction Models in Psychiatric Genetics 447
The third major problem in G 9 E interaction research is recall bias. Recall bias
occurs when subjects cannot accurately recall past events or when particular events
become more or less important in retrospect than when they occurred. In partic-
ular, patients often overcount potential environmental causes for their disorder,
a phenomenon termed mood-congruent memory revision (Joormann et al. 2009;
Schwarz and Clore 1983). Recall bias tends to become greater with the greater
length of time between the environmental exposure and its report. However, this
retrospective forgetting is often selective and its magnitude and character differs
between affected and unaffected individuals (Monroe 2008). The difficulties in
overcoming the problem of recall bias in retrospective studies provide the impetus
for specific novel study designs that we will discuss in later sections.
rate (e.g., genotype A with low environmental exposure vs. genotype B with low
environmental exposure), and disorder frequencies can be compared between these
groups. If high follow-up rates are obtained, the prospective cohort design can
provide high-quality data because it efficiently handles the three major methodo-
logical issues facing G 9 E studies: it minimizes selection bias, because the
disorder usually occurs after subjects are selected (Yang and Khoury 1997),
it minimizes population stratification by sampling from a defined cohort and it
reduces recall bias to a minimum if the baseline information is assessed early in
life of the cohort and when it can be followed several times over years (Hunter
2005).
Three of the most important findings in psychiatric G 9 E interaction research
were produced by utilizing through a study a prospective cohort study design, the
Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study (Dunedin Longitudinal
Study) (Caspi et al. 2002, 2003, 2005). The Dunedin Longitudinal Study inves-
tigated a large birth cohort of 1,037 children born in 1972–73 in Dunedin,
New Zealand. The cohort was first assessed at age three and since then followed up
every two years for two decades (Silva 1990). Data from this cohort demonstrated
significant G 9 E interaction effects on violent behavior (Caspi et al. 2002),
depression (Caspi et al. 2003) and adult psychosis (Caspi et al. 2005). These
landmark studies provide evidence supporting the strength and accuracy of the
prospective cohort design.
The downside of this study design is the long time frame necessary to conduct
these studies. For instance, the Dunedin Longitudinal Study was started 30 years
before the first G 9 E interaction finding was published. In addition, large samples
are needed because the environmental exposure and/or the disorder might be at
low prevalence in the cohort (Hunter 2005). As a result, many investigators opt for
quicker and less expensive designs. The cross-sectional modification of the cohort
study assesses cohort information only once. Although this design loses some of
the advantages of a prospective study, the cost and time frame necessary to carry
out the study is often more feasible.
Retrospective case-control. Another inexpensive and popular alternative to the
prospective cohort design is the retrospective case-control design. Here, affected
subjects with the disorder are selected and matched with subjects who do not have
the disorder (‘controls’). This procedure allows for the controlled sampling of
subjects with disorder and/or environmental exposure, yielding the advantage of
increased power compared to cohort studies (McClelland and Judd 1993). Infor-
mation about past exposure is gathered and the exposure rates and genotype
frequencies are compared between cases and controls. Due to the selection and
matching process, this design is particularly prone to selection bias and population
stratification, especially when the source population for controls is hard to define
(Hunter 2005). The prospective-nested case-control design is a more sophisticated
study design that addresses these methodological problems by selecting cases and
controls from a predetermined longitudinal cohort. As cases and controls stem
from the same cohort, confounding from selection bias and population stratifica-
tion is avoided. In addition, recall bias is eliminated because exposure is assessed
452 K. Karg and S. Sen
before the diagnose. Compared to a full cohort approach, this design offers sub-
stantial reductions in costs and efforts.
Case-only. Recently, investigators have proposed a study design that eliminates
the use of control subjects (Khoury and Flanders 1996; Piegorsch et al. 1994).
In the case-only design, affected subjects are selected from the population and
grouped according to their genotype and then compared for their exposure rates.
In the presence of G 9 E interaction, some genotypes are more susceptible to the
environmental pathogen than others, resulting in an overrepresentation of subjects
with environmental exposure in this genotype group. Therefore, differential
distributions of exposure rates across genotype groups can be interpreted as a G 9 E
interaction effect. As an example, Mandelli et al. (2006) utilized the case-only
design to investigate the interaction effect of 5-HTTLPR and stress on depression.
They studied a sample of 686 patients diagnosed for major depression or bipolar
disorder and classified them into six groups according to their genotype and the
presence or absence of environmental exposure to life stress in the year before
depression onset. On comparing the proportion of the sample exposed between
each genotype group, they found higher proportions of previously exposed
subjects in the genotype groups carrying the short allele. The authors interpreted
this finding as evidence for higher stress susceptibility of short allele carriers.
However, this conclusion has to be viewed with some caution because the case-
only design is prone to confounding. Differential distributions in exposure rates
across genotypes can also emerge through G-E correlation, with specific genotypes
being more likely to be exposed to the environmental factor than others (Khoury
and Flanders 1996). In this study, it is possible that short allele carriers are more
prone to experience stressful situations and that this causes their overrepresenta-
tion in the exposed group. The only safe way to rule out this potential bias is
through the verification of the underlying assumption of gene–environment
independency. Therefore, the case-only design should be used only if the inde-
pendency assumption is verified or for exploratory studies (Albert et al. 2001).
Exposed-only. A related, but subtly different approach that has become
increasingly popular is the exposed-only design. Here, subjects exposed to the
same environmental factor are selected, grouped according to their genotype and
compared for their disorder status. In the presence of G 9 E interaction, disorder
frequencies should be higher in the genotype group with higher susceptibility to
the environmental exposure. However, as we discussed concerning the case-only
design, this conclusion is only valid in the absence of G-E correlation. An example
might illustrate this problem. A recent study utilized an exposed-only design to
explore a moderating effect of the FKBP5 (FK506 binding protein 5) gene on the
relationship between severe injury and peritraumatic dissociation (Koenen et al.
2005). Peritraumatic dissociation is a evolutionary conserved response to life-
threatening events and a risk factor for the development of post-traumatic stress
disorder (Ozer et al. 2003). The study sample consisted of 46 severely injured
hospitalized children who were genotyped and compared for their peritraumatic
dissociation scores with logistic regression analysis. The study revealed a signif-
icant G 9 E interaction effect of FKBP5 genotype and severe injury on the
Gene 9 Environment Interaction Models in Psychiatric Genetics 453
or maternal alcohol use (Brookes et al. 2006) to factors relevant at birth [e.g. season
of birth (Seeger et al. 2004), birth weight (Thapar et al. 2005)] and early devel-
opment [e.g. childhood maltreatment (Caspi et al. 2003), childhood trauma
(Bradley et al. 2008)] to factors affecting adolescence [e.g. cannabis use (Caspi
et al. 2005)] and adulthood [e.g. stress (Blomeyer et al. 2008), physical inactivity
(Andreasen et al. 2008)]. However, track record of replications has often been poor,
casting doubt on the validity of these findings (Thomas 2010). Nonreplication can
be due to false-negative results, false-positive results or true heterogeneity between
studies. False-negative results in psychiatry studies are most often caused by
insufficient power, either due to a small sample size or suboptimal phenotyping or
genotyping quality. False-positive results can often result from multiple testing and
population stratification. True heterogeneity occurs if the interaction exists in some
populations studied or with some environmental factors studied but not with others.
Here, we present the most heavily investigated example in psychiatric G 9 E
interaction research, a G 9 E interaction between a polymorphism in the promoter
region of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) and both adult stressful life
Gene 9 Environment Interaction Models in Psychiatric Genetics 455
events and childhood maltreatment on the risk of depression (Caspi et al. 2003). We
will discuss conflicting results between studies exploring this interaction and
potential reasons for the conflict.
The original study exploring this interaction utilized a prospective-longitudinal
cohort design with almost 1,000 children and found that individuals homozygous
or heterozygous for the low-expressing short variant of 5-HTTLPR are more
susceptible to depression after stress than individuals homozygous for the alternate
long variant. The same pattern was found for childhood maltreatment. This study
caused a great deal of excitement in G 9 E interaction research and encouraged
further research on this issue. To date, there have been 55 follow-up studies with
some confirming the original finding, some finding evidence of higher stress
susceptibility of individuals with the alternate long allele, and others finding no
interaction effect at all (Karg et al. 2010). This inconsistency might be due to the
heterogeneity of studies in many relevant aspects. First, studies exploring the
relationship between 5-HTTLPR, stress and depression have utilized very different
research designs, including longitudinal, cross-sectional, case-control, case-only,
exposure-only and family-based designs. Second, studies have measured many
different depression phenotypes using diverse assessment strategies, including
clinical interviews and self-report checklists, and diverse depression scales, vari-
ously yielding both categorical and continuous outcome measures. Third, studies
have investigated an extraordinarily varied set of stressors with various assessment
methods. For instance, stressors counted in different studies for stressful life events
ranged from becoming homeless, and the death of a parent or spouse to growing up
in a household with siblings who quarreled or as the child of a father in an
unskilled occupation. Other studies used more specific, but highly diverse stressors
such as stroke survival, hurricane exposure, bullying victimization or childhood
maltreatment. To clarify this confusion, three meta-analyses have been carried out
to date. The first two (Uher and McGuffin 2007; Risch et al. 2009) concluded that
there was no evidence supporting the presence of an interaction. However, these
analyses investigated only small subsamples of all 55 studies due to methodo-
logical constraints. The latest meta-analysis (Karg et al. 2010) included all relevant
studies and detected stressor type (stressful life events, childhood maltreatment,
and specific medical conditions) and stress assessment method (questionnaire,
interview, objective) as two critical sources for variability in study outcomes.
In particular, studies with childhood maltreatment or specific medical conditions
as environmental stressor were more likely to find a significant G 9 E effect than
studies with broader defined stressful life events, as were studies with objective or
interview assessment methods for environmental stressors. This again supports the
assumption that measurement quality can affect results in G 9 E research.
Since this original study, further evidence from various fields has emerged
(Caspi et al. 2010). First, several empirical studies link the short 5-HTTLPR
variant to stress-sensitive phenotypes such as post-traumatic stress disorder
(Xie et al. 2009), post-trauma suicide (Roy et al. 2007), stress-related sleep
disturbance (Brummett et al. 2007) and anxiety (Stein et al. 2008). Second,
a multitude of neuroimaging studies confirmed increased and faster amygdala
456 K. Karg and S. Sen
reactivity following threat in carriers of the short allele e.g. (Furman et al. 2010;
Heinz et al. 2005) and linked it to specific brain anatomy characteristics
e.g. (Pacheco et al. 2009; Pezawas et al. 2005). Third, Rhesus macaques carrying
the short variant exhibit greater anxiety-related behaviors in response to adverse
rearing conditions compared to their conspecific with the long alternate (Barr et al.
2004; Spinelli et al. 2007). Fourth, in addition to 5-HTT knockout mice, 5-HTT
knockout rats showed increased anxiety levels in response to stress (Homberg et al.
2007). Taken together, these outcomes across a wide variety of techniques, models
and species as well as the numerous positive G 9 E studies robustly demonstrate
the interaction effect between stress and 5-HTTLPR genotype on depression and
are to date the most intriguing finding of G 9 E interaction in psychiatric genetics.
5 Future Directions
Although much progress has been made in the past two decades, many questions in
G 9 E interaction research in psychiatric genetics remain open. New, more
carefully conducted epidemiological studies could shed light on these questions.
Another major step for clarification is the identification of the biological mecha-
nisms underlying interaction effects. Not much is known about how environmental
factors can interact with a person’s genotype and her nervous system to moderate
the disorder risk. Therefore, joining forces with neuroscience is an important step
in making progress in the field (Caspi and Moffitt 2006). Many epidemiological
studies on G 9 E interaction in psychiatric genetics were motivated by findings of
neuroscience research and positive epidemiological findings, in turn, can stimulate
new studies in neuroscience. The interaction between 5-HTTLPR and life stress on
depression provides an example where neuroscience studies can illuminate the
black box between genes, environment and disorder (Merikangas and Risch 2003)
and confirm and explain epidemiological findings. Another fruitful approach for
advances in the understanding G 9 E interaction might be the collaboration with
epigenetic research. Many environmental risk factors operate early in develop-
ment, and fine-tuning of neuronal pathways is known to be affected by environ-
mental factors (Abdolmaleky et al. 2004). If these epigenetic modifications depend
on the person’s genotype, a plausible mechanism is constituted for G 9 E inter-
action in psychiatric genetics. Epigenetic studies for psychiatric disorders are still
in their infancy, and new exciting insights in the interplay of genes and environ-
ment on the development of mental disorders are to be awaited.
6 Summary
Although the fundamental questions about the validity of statistical models for
biological interaction and the utility of G 9 E interaction findings for advances in
psychiatric genetics are still highly debated, novel study designs such as case-only
Gene 9 Environment Interaction Models in Psychiatric Genetics 457
and exposed-only designs can overcome at least some of the statistical concerns.
Study designs differ broadly in their strengths and limitations regarding selection
bias, population stratification and recall bias. Previously undetermined study
characteristics that might additionally affect the outcome of G 9 E interaction
studies are the assessment methods for environmental exposure and disorder sta-
tus, as shown for the G 9 E interaction effect between the serotonin transporter
promoter variant and stress on depression. New insights into the interplay between
genes and environment on the development of mental disorders may emerge
through more carefully conducted G 9 E interaction studies as well as through
collaboration with neuroscience and epigenetic research.
References
Abdolmaleky HM, Smith CL, Faraone SV, Shafa R, Stone W, Glatt SJ, Tsuang MT (2004)
Methylomics in psychiatry: modulation of Gene-environment interactions may be through
DNA methylation. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 127B(1):51–59. doi: 10.1002/
ajmg.b.20142
Albert PS, Ratnasinghe D, Tangrea J, Wacholder S (2001) Limitations of the case-only design for
identifying Gene-environment interactions. Am J Epidemiol 154(8):687–693
Andreasen CH, Stender-Petersen KL, Mogensen MS, Torekov SS, Wegner L, Andersen G,
Nielsen AL, Albrechtsen A, Borch-Johnsen K, Rasmussen SS, Clausen JO, Sandbaek A,
Lauritzen T, Hansen L, Jorgensen T, Pedersen O, Hansen T (2008) Low physical activity
accentuates the effect of the FTO rs9939609 polymorphism on body fat accumulation.
Diabetes 57(1):95–101. doi: db07-0910 [pii], 10.2337/db07-0910
Barr CS, Newman TK, Shannon C, Parker C, Dvoskin RL, Becker ML, Schwandt M, Champoux M,
Lesch KP, Goldman D, Suomi SJ, Higley JD (2004) Rearing condition and rh5-HTTLPR interact
to influence limbic-hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis response to stress in infant macaques.
Biol Psychiatry 55(7):733–738. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.12.008 S000632230 4000046
[pii]
Binder EB, Bradley RG, Liu W, Epstein MP, Deveau TC, Mercer KB, Tang Y, Gillespie CF,
Heim CM, Nemeroff CB, Schwartz AC, Cubells JF, Ressler KJ (2008) Association of fkbp5
polymorphisms and childhood abuse with risk of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in
adults. JAMA 299(11):1291–1305. doi: 299/11/1291 [pii], 10.1001/jama.299.11.1291
Blomeyer D, Treutlein J, Esser G, Schmidt MH, Schumann G, Laucht M (2008) Interaction
between crhr1 gene and stressful life events predicts adolescent heavy alcohol use. Biol
Psychiatry 63(2):146–151. doi: S0006-3223(07)00375-7 [pii], 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.
04.026
Bradley RG, Binder EB, Epstein MP, Tang Y, Nair HP, Liu W, Gillespie CF, Berg T, Evces M,
Newport DJ, Stowe ZN, Heim CM, Nemeroff CB, Schwartz A, Cubells JF, Ressler KJ (2008)
Influence of child abuse on adult depression: moderation by the corticotropin-releasing
hormone receptor gene. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65(2):190–200. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.
2007.26
Brookes KJ, Mill J, Guindalini C, Curran S, Xu X, Knight J, Chen CK, Huang YS, Sethna V,
Taylor E, Chen W, Breen G, Asherson P (2006) A common haplotype of the dopamine
transporter gene associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and interacting with
maternal use of alcohol during pregnancy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63(1):74–81. doi: 63/1/74
[pii] 10.1001/archpsyc.63.1.74
Brummett BH, Krystal AD, Ashley-Koch A, Kuhn CM, Zuchner S, Siegler IC, Barefoot JC,
Ballard EL, Gwyther LP, Williams RB (2007) Sleep quality varies as a function of
458 K. Karg and S. Sen
Hutchison KE, LaChance H, Niaura R, Bryan A, Smolen A (2002b) The drd4 vntr polymorphism
influences reactivity to smoking cues. J Abnorm Psychol 111(1):134–143
Jaffee SR, Price TS (2007) Gene-environment correlations: a review of the evidence and
implications for prevention of mental illness. Mol Psychiatry 12(5):432–442. doi: 10.1038/
sj.mp. 4001950
Joormann J, Hertel PT, LeMoult J, Gotlib IH (2009) Training forgetting of negative material in
depression. J Abnorm Psychol 118(1):34–43. doi: 2009-01738-017 [pii], 10.1037/a0013794
Kahn RS, Khoury J, Nichols WC, Lanphear BP (2003) Role of dopamine transporter genotype
and maternal prenatal smoking in childhood hyperactive-impulsive, inattentive, and
oppositional behaviors. J Pediatr 143(1):104–110. doi: S0022-3476(03)00208-7 [pii],
10.1016/S0022-3476(03)00208-7
Karg K, Burmeister M, Shedden K, Sen S (2010) The serotonin transporter promoter variant
(5-HTTLPR), stress, and depression meta-analysis revisited: evidence of genetic moderation.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 12(8):786–792
Kendler KS, Eaves LJ (1986) Models for the joint effect of genotype and environment on liability
to psychiatric illness. Am J Psychiatry 143(3):279–289
Kendler KS, Gardner CO (2010) Interpretation of interactions: guide for the perplexed. Br J
Psychiatry 197(3):170–171. doi: 197/3/170 [pii], 10.1192/bjp.bp. 110.081331
Khoury MJ, Flanders WD (1996) Nontraditional epidemiologic approaches in the analysis of
Gene-environment interaction: case-control studies with no controls! Am J Epidemiol
144(3):207–213
Khoury MJ, Wacholder S (2009) Invited commentary: from genome-wide association studies to
Gene-environment-wide interaction studies-challenges and opportunities. Am J Epidemiol
169 (2):227–230; discussion 234–225. doi: kwn351 [pii], 10.1093/aje/kwn351
Koenen KC, Saxe G, Purcell S, Smoller JW, Bartholomew D, Miller A, Hall E, Kaplow J,
Bosquet M, Moulton S, Baldwin C (2005) Polymorphisms in FKBP5 are associated with
peritraumatic dissociation in medically injured children. Mol Psychiatry 10(12):1058–1059
Kraft P, Yen YC, Stram DO, Morrison J, Gauderman WJ (2007) Exploiting Gene-environment
interaction to detect genetic associations. Hum Hered 63(2):111–119. doi: 000099183 [pii],
10.1159/000099183
Laucht M, Skowronek MH, Becker K, Schmidt MH, Esser G, Schulze TG, Rietschel M (2007)
Interacting effects of the dopamine transporter gene and psychosocial adversity on attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms among 15-year-olds from a high-risk community
sample. Arch Gen Psychiatry 64(5):585–590. doi: 64/5/585 [pii], 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.585
Mandelli L, Serretti A, Marino E, Pirovano A, Calati R, Colombo C (2006) Interaction between
serotonin transporter gene, catechol-o-methyltransferase gene and stressful life events in
mood disorders. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 10(04):437–447. doi: 10.1017/s146114570
6006882
Manolio TA, Brooks LD, Collins FS (2008) A hapmap harvest of insights into the genetics of
common disease. J Clin Invest 118(5):1590–1605. doi: 10.1172/JCI34772
McClelland GH, Judd CM (1993) Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator
effects. Psychol Bull 114(2):376–390
Merikangas KR, Risch N (2003) Will the genomics revolution revolutionize psychiatry? Am J
Psychiatry 160(4):625–635
Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M (2005) Strategy for investigating interactions between measured
genes and measured environments. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62(5):473–481. doi: 62/5/473 [pii],
10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.473
Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M (2006) Measured Gene-environment interactions in psychopa-
thology: concepts, research strategies, and implications for research, intervention, and public
understanding of genetics. Perspect Psychol Sci 1(1):5–27
Monroe SM (2008) Modern approaches to conceptualizing and measuring human life stress.
Annu Rev Clin Psychol 4:33–52. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141207
460 K. Karg and S. Sen
Munafo MR, Durrant C, Lewis G, Flint J (2009) Gene 9 environment interactions at the serotonin
transporter locus. Biol Psychiatry 65(3):211–219. doi: S0006-3223(08)00731-2 [pii], 10.1016/
j.biopsych.2008.06.009
Nuevo R, Lehtinen V, Reyna-Liberato PM, Ayuso-Mateos JL (2009) Usefulness of the beck
depression inventory as a screening method for depression among the general population of
finland. Scand J Public Health 37(1):28–34. doi: 37/1/28 [pii], 10.1177/1403494808097169
Olsson CA, Byrnes GB, Lotfi-Miri M, Collins V, Williamson R, Patton C, Anney RJL (2005)
Association between 5-HTTLPR genotypes and persisting patterns of anxiety and alcohol use:
results from a 10-year longitudinal study of adolescent mental health. Mol Psychiatry
10(9):868–876
Onkamo P, Toivonen H (2006) A survey of data mining methods for linkage disequilibrium
mapping. Hum Genomics 2(5):336–340
Ottman R (1990) An epidemiologic approach to Gene-environment interaction. Genet Epidemiol
7(3):177–185. doi: 10.1002/gepi.1370070302
Ottman R (1994) Epidemiologic analysis of Gene-environment interaction in twins. Genet
Epidemiol 11(1):75–86. doi: 10.1002/gepi.1370110108
Ozer EJ, Best SR, Lipsey TL, Weiss DS (2003) Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and
symptoms in adults: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 129(1):52–73
Pacheco J, Beevers CG, Benavides C, McGeary J, Stice E, Schnyer DM (2009) Frontal-limbic
white matter pathway associations with the serotonin transporter gene promoter region
(5-HTTLPR) polymorphism. J Neurosci 29(19):6229–6233. doi: 29/19/6229 [pii], 10.1523/
JNEUROSCI.0896-09.2009
Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Drabant EM, Verchinski BA, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, Egan MF,
Mattay VS, Hariri AR, Weinberger DR (2005) 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human
cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nat
Neurosci 8(6):828–834. doi: nn1463 [pii], 10.1038/nn1463
Piegorsch WW, Weinberg CR, Taylor JA (1994) Non-hierarchical logistic models and case-only
designs for assessing susceptibility in population-based case-control studies. Stat Med
13(2):153–162
Plomin R, DeFries JC, Loehlin JC (1977) Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in
the analysis of human behavior. Psychol Bull 84(2):309–322
Risch N, Herrell R, Lehner T, Liang KY, Eaves L, Hoh J, Griem A, Kovacs M, Ott J, Merikangas
KR (2009) Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life
events, and risk of depression: a meta-analysis. JAMA 301(23):2462–2471. doi: 10.1001/
jama.2009.878
Roy A, Hu XZ, Janal MN, Goldman D (2007) Interaction between childhood trauma and
serotonin transporter gene variation in suicide. Neuropsychopharmacology 32(9):2046–2052.
doi: 1301331 [pii], 10.1038/sj.npp. 1301331
Rutter M (2002) The interplay of nature, nurture, and developmental influences: the challenge
ahead for mental health. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59(11):996–1000. doi: ysa21000 [pii]
Rutter M, Silberg J (2002) Gene-environment interplay in relation to emotional and behavioral
disturbance. Annu Rev Psychol 53:463–490. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135223
53/1/463 [pii]
Rutter M, Moffitt TE, Caspi A (2006) Gene-environment interplay and psychopathology: multiple
varieties but real effects. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 47(3–4):226–261. doi: 10.1002/da.20641
Rutter M, Thapar A, Pickles A (2009) Gene-environment interactions: biologically valid pathway
or artifact? Arch Gen Psychiatry 66(12):1287–1289. doi: 66/12/1287 [pii], 10.1001/
archgenpsychiatry.2009.167
Scarr S (1992) Developmental theories for the 1990s: development and individual differences.
Child Dev 63(1):1–19
Schaid DJ (1999) Case-parents design for Gene-environment interaction. Genet Epidemiol
16(3):261–273. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2272(1999)16:3\261::AID-GEPI3[3.0.CO;2-M
[pii], 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2272(1999)16:3\261:AID-GEPI3[3.0.CO;2-M
Gene 9 Environment Interaction Models in Psychiatric Genetics 461
Schwarz N, Clore GL (1983) Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: informative and
directive functions of affective states. J Pers Soc Psychol 45(3):513–523. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.45.3.513
Sebastiani P, Ramoni MF, Nolan V, Baldwin CT, Steinberg MH (2005) Genetic dissection and
prognostic modeling of overt stroke in sickle cell anemia. Nat Genet 37(4):435–440.
doi: ng1533 [pii], 10.1038/ng1533
Seeger G, Schloss P, Schmidt MH, Ruter-Jungfleisch A, Henn FA (2004) Gene-environment
interaction in hyperkinetic conduct disorder (HD ? CD) as indicated by season of birth
variations in dopamine receptor (DRD4) gene polymorphism. Neurosci Lett 366(3):282–286.
doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.05.049 S0304394004006305 [pii]
Silva PA (1990) The dunedin multidisciplinary health and development study: a 15 year
longitudinal study. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 4(1):76–107
Sonuga-Barke EJ, Oades RD, Psychogiou L, Chen W, Franke B, Buitelaar J, Banaschewski T,
Ebstein RP, Gil M, Anney R, Miranda A, Roeyers H, Rothenberger A, Sergeant J,
Steinhausen HC, Thompson M, Asherson P, Faraone SV (2009) Dopamine and serotonin
transporter genotypes moderate sensitivity to maternal expressed emotion: the case of conduct
and emotional problems in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
50(9):1052–1063. doi: JCPP2095 [pii], 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02095.x
Spinelli S, Schwandt ML, Lindell SG, Newman TK, Heilig M, Suomi SJ, Higley JD, Goldman D,
Barr CS (2007) Association between the recombinant human serotonin transporter linked
promoter region polymorphism and behavior in rhesus macaques during a separation
paradigm. Dev Psychopathol 19(4):977–987. doi: S095457940700048X [pii], 10.1017/
S095457940700048X
Stein MB, Schork NJ, Gelernter J (2008) Gene-by-environment (serotonin transporter and
childhood maltreatment) interaction for anxiety sensitivity, an intermediate phenotype for
anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 33(2):312–319. doi: 1301422 [pii], 10.1038/
sj.npp. 1301422
Stevens SE, Kumsta R, Kreppner JM, Brookes KJ, Rutter M, Sonuga-Barke EJ (2009) Dopamine
transporter gene polymorphism moderates the effects of severe deprivation on adhd
symptoms: developmental continuities in gene-environment interplay. Am J Med Genet B
Neuropsychiatr Genet 150B(6):753–761. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31010
Thapar A, Langley K, Fowler T, Rice F, Turic D, Whittinger N, Aggleton J, Van den Bree M,
Owen M, O’Donovan M (2005) Catechol o-methyltransferase gene variant and birth weight
predict early-onset antisocial behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62(11):1275–1278. doi: 62/11/1275 [pii], 10.1001/
archpsyc.62.11.1275
Thomas D (2010) Gene-environment-wide association studies: emerging approaches. Nat Rev
Genet 11(4):259–272. doi: nrg2764 [pii], 10.1038/nrg2764
Thompson WD (1991) Effect modification and the limits of biological inference from
epidemiologic data. J Clin Epidemiol 44(3):221–232. doi: 0895-4356(91)90033-6 [pii]
Uher R, McGuffin P (2007) The moderation by the serotonin transporter gene of environmental
adversity in the aetiology of mental illness: review and methodological analysis.
Mol Psychiatry 13(2):131–146. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp. 4002067
Uher R, McGuffin P (2010) The moderation by the serotonin transporter gene of environmental
adversity in the etiology of depression: 2009 update. Mol Psychiatry 15(1):18–22.
doi: mp2009123 [pii], 10.1038/mp. 2009.123
van Os J, Rutten BP, Poulton R (2008) Gene-environment interactions in schizophrenia: review
of epidemiological findings and future directions. Schizophr Bull 34(6):1066–1082.
doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn117
van Winkel R, Henquet C, Rosa A, Papiol S, Faňanás L, De Hert M, Peuskens J, van Os J,
Myin-Germeys I (2008) Evidence that the COMTVal158Met polymorphism moderates
sensitivity to stress in psychosis: an experience-sampling study. Am J Med Genet Part B:
Neuropsychiatr Genet 147B(1):10–17. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30559
462 K. Karg and S. Sen
Abstract As shown by clinical genetic studies, affective and anxiety disorders are
complex genetic disorders with genetic and environmental factors interactively
determining their respective pathomechanism. Advances in molecular genetic
techniques including linkage studies, association studies, and genome-wide asso-
ciation studies allow for the detailed dissection of the genetic influence on the
development of these disorders. Besides the molecular genetic investigation of
categorical entities according to standardized diagnostic criteria, intermediate
phenotypes comprising neurobiological or neuropsychological traits (e.g., neuro-
nal correlates of emotional processing) that are linked to the disease of interest and
that are heritable, have been proposed to be closer to the underlying genotype than
the overall disease phenotype. These intermediate phenotypes are dimensional and
more precisely defined than the categorical disease phenotype, and therefore have
attracted much interest in the genetic investigation of affective and anxiety dis-
orders. Given the complex genetic nature of affective and anxiety disorders with an
interaction of multiple risk genes and environmental influences, the interplay of
genetic factors with environmental factors is investigated by means of gene-
environment interaction (GxE) studies. Pharmacogenetic studies aid in the dis-
section of the genetically influenced heterogeneity of psychotropic drug response
and may contribute to the development of a more individualized treatment of
affective and anxiety disorders. Finally, there is some evidence for genetic factors
potentially shared between affective and anxiety disorders pointing to a possible
overlapping phenotype between anxiety disorders and depression.
Contents
1 Affective Disorders
The group of affective disorders comprises both major depressive disorder (MDD;
unipolar depression) with various subtypes as well as bipolar disorder (BPD). The
latter displays by changes between (hypo-)manic and depressive phases, with
intermittent euthymic phases, while the course of MDD is characterized by
depression and euthymia. As patients rarely develop their first manic phase only
years after their first depression, they might well be initially mischaracterized as
MDD patients (so-called ‘‘hidden bipolars’’), which is a challenge for genetic
studies on affective disorders. To overcome this problem, several indicators for the
presence of BPD in depression have been suggested, e.g. subthreshold hypomanic
symptoms (Angst et al. 2010; Fiedorowicz et al. 2011). This however has not been
incorporated in current genetic studies and thus one should always consider that
MDD studies might well include a substantial amount of ‘‘hidden bipolar’’
patients, obfuscating MDD-specific findings. Furthermore, in the following section
more recent approaches such as genome-wide association studies (GWAS) and
copy-number variant (CNV) analyses are reviewed, as there are already plenty of
scholarly review articles on linkage and association studies. These issues will
therefore be touched upon more briefly; with respect to intermediate phenotype
and gene x environment (GxE) studies in affective disorders, the reader is referred
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 465
to more specialized reviews for the sake of space, as this extensive topic is beyond
the scope of this article.
More than 40 linkage scans for BPD have been published to date which generated
plenty of disparate findings. However, quite a number of loci meeting significance
criteria were described by two or more groups: 3p12-14, 4p16, 4q31-35, 5q31-33,
6q16/21-25, 8q21-24, 10q25-26, 11p15.5, 12q23-24, 13q14-32, 18p11, 18q21-22
20q13, 21q21-22, 22q11-12, and Xq24-28. Confirmed positional candidate genes
however are yet to emerge from these studies. An initially highly promising
linkage peak on chromosome 11p15 is considered meanwhile to be due to type I
error (Egeland et al. 1987; Berrettini 2001). Also, other loci which have been
initially promising could subsequently not be confirmed. A paradigmatic case in
this respect is for e.g. the 12q23-24 locus (Dawson et al. 1995; Ewald et al. 1998,
2002; Green et al. 2005), which is noteworthy due to the co-segregation of Darier’s
disease with BPD (Maziade et al. 2001). It has been shown that, in some families
suffering from both disorders, the BPD locus lies outside of the Darier’s disease
causing ATP2A2 gene, yet is in linkage disequilibrium with this variant (Jones
et al. 2002). Hence, there might indeed be intermediately penetrant variants in this
region which are exclusive to only a few families and which are lost in noise when
combining many families, or cases, respectively. Similarly, a functional mutation
in the gene encoding the brain-specific tryptophan hydroxylase 2 (TPH2) has
been described which segregates with BPD in three families (Cichon et al. 2008;
Grigoroiu-Serbanescu et al. 2008). Findings like these argue for a ‘‘common
disease, multiple rare variant’’ model (McCarthy et al. 2008) and underscore the
clinical and genetic heterogeneity of BPD. This however does not argue against
the concurrent existence of a ‘‘common disease, common variant model’’. As both
models most likely are present in clinical samples, this additional level of com-
plexity further hampers the identification of BPD risk genes.
Different meta-analyses found the strongest evidence for BPD susceptibility
loci on 13q and 22q (Badner and Gershon 2002; 1228 patients from 353 families),
or 9p22.3-21.1, 10q11.21-22.1 and 14q24.1-32.12 (948 to 2437 patients; Segurado
et al. 2003). The latter study used the rank-based genome scan (GSAM) method
which, together with sample heterogeneity, might account for the different findings
as compared to the first study. Finally, in a combined analysis, 6q21-q25 and 8q24
showed genome-wide significance (5179 patients from 1067 families; McQueen
et al. 2005). Again, the underlying genes have not yet been identified. Further
scholarly reviews on this topic have been provided by Schulze and McMahon
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 467
(2003), Serretti and Mandelli (2008), Barnett and Smoller (2009), and Craddock
und Sklar (2009).
Also in MDD, numerous linkage scans were carried out (reviewed e.g. by
Lohoff (2010)) yet did not point to clear regions of susceptibility, as expected from
the lower heritability rate of MDD as compared to BPD. No meta-analysis has
been performed to date, which is surprising given the recent efforts to uncover the
genetic basis of MDD and hence there is a clear need for further research. There is
no meaningful overlap of linkage peaks between studies, although it is noteworthy
that two linkage signals have previously been implicated in BPD: one on chro-
mosome 18q (Camp et al. 2005) and the locus mentioned above on 12q23-24
(McGuffin et al. 2005; Abkevich et al. 2003). Therefore, this region appears to be a
promising region for affective disorders, yet most likely carries more than just one
risk gene.
With regard to candidate gene studies, many genes were shown to be associated
with BPD, but none of them has been established as a specific BPD susceptibility
gene. Among the best replicated genes are DAOA/G72 (which was associated in a
case–control study, but not in a meta-analyses; Muller et al. 2011; Shi et al. 2008),
BDNF (again, meta-analyses provided differing data: Kanazawa et al. 2007; Fan
and Sklar 2008), DISC1, NRG1, ARNTL/CLOCK, FAT, and GSK3B (Barnett and
Smoller 2009; Serretti and Mandelli 2008; Luykx et al. 2010). Not surprisingly,
many genes encoding for components of neurotransmitter pathways have been
tested for an association with BPD (such as SLC6A3, HTR2A, TPH2, MAOA,
COMT, DRD1, and SLC6A4). Coming from GWAS on schizophrenia, the risk
gene ZNF804A was demonstrated not to be specific for this disorder, but rather
was also associated with BPD (O’Donovan et al. 2008; Williams et al. 2011;
Steinberg et al. 2011). The same was true for the GWAS schizophrenia risk loci
around the MHC region and NRGN (Williams et al. 2011) and other genes which
initially have been described as schizophrenia risk genes: the above-mentioned
DISC1 and DAOA/G72 genes, but also NRG1, DTNB1, and NPAS3 (Huang et al.
2010; Pickard et al. 2009).
Most of the association studies published to date suffer from the drawback of
small sample sizes and lack of replication, and hence, the combination of large,
well described international samples (as done in the Psychiatric GWAS consor-
tium [PGC]) is paramount especially in the search for risk variants assuming a
‘‘common variant, common disease’’ model. Furthermore, meta-analytic treatment
of existing data might shed some light on the contribution of suggested BPD risk
genes. Such have been performed on considerably sized samples on only a few
genes. The gene encoding for methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase (MTHFR) has
been tested for an analysis with mood disorders several times. While the first study
on 1222 MDD patients yielded negative results (Gaysina et al. 2008), the latest
analysis comprising 9648 cases (MDD, BPD and schizophrenia combined) yielded
468 K. Domschke and A. Reif
again a significant yet unspecific association of MTHFR with mood disorders (best
OR = 1.26; Peerbooms et al. 2011). This is in line with a positive meta-analysis
on BPD by Gilbody et al. (2007), but in discrepancy to three further meta-analyses
on MTHFR in BPD (Zintzaras 2006, 1415 cases; Chen et al. 2009, 1260 cases;
Cohen-Woods et al. 2010, 2584 cases). Given that the original genotyping data
which was presented in the Cohen-Woods study (n = 897 BPD patients) was
negative, but not included in the Peerbooms study, which demonstrated a signif-
icant effect only when all mood disorders were combined, the role of MTHFR in
affective disorders seems to be rather unspecific and small.
A meta-analysis on all mood disorders found borderline evidence for an
association of the dopamine receptor 2 (DRD2) Taq1 polymorphism (which in fact
localizes to the neighboring gene ANKK1) with affective disorders, yet two more
SNPs in DRD2 proved to be negative in much larger data sets so that there is only
weak evidence for this gene being associated with BPD or MDD (Zou et al. 2010).
The gene for catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) that degrades dopamine,
features a well described functional polymorphism resulting in a Val to Met
transition and which has been shown to be linked to BPD in a meta-analysis on
2944 cases (Zhang et al. 2009b), although this seemed to be more pronounced in
Asian populations. Obviously, also genes coding for components of the serotonin
system were subjected to meta-analyses, which often were combined with genuine
genotyping efforts. A functional SNP in the promoter region of the serotonin
receptor gene HTR1A (rs6295) was demonstrated to be significantly associated
with BPD (1148 cases, Kishi et al. 2011). In the MAOA gene, three polymor-
phisms (sample sizes mostly [1000 cases) were meta-analyzed and the main
finding was an association of an intronic CA repeat with BPD in Caucasians
(Fan et al. 2010). The SLC6A4 promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR), which has
mainly been studied in MDD GxE (see below) was also included in several meta-
analyses that conclusively demonstrated a small yet significant association of the
short variant with BPD (Lasky-Su et al. 2005; Cho et al. 2005, 1712 cases). Two
meta-analyses focused on the TPH1 gene (Chen et al. 2008, 2011) and in unison
came to the conclusion that TPH1 is not associated with MDD (2340 and 1812
patients, respectively), but with BPD (1951 and 2083 cases). Given that in the
brain only the TPH2 isoform of tryptophan hydroxylase is expressed, this finding
is rather surprising; however, as the foetal brain depends on maternal 5-HT pro-
duction which is accomplished by placental TPH1 the observed association might
in fact be true but rather due to maternal and not case genotype as has been
described for rare TPH1 mutations in ADHD (Halmoy et al. 2010).
The lower heritability of MDD as compared to BPD notwithstanding, plenty of
case–control association studies have been published thereon as well. A scholarly
overview on candidate gene studies is provided by Lohoff (2010). In order to
separate the wheat from the chaff, replication is key and meta-analytic aggregation
of data is a possible route to success. Accordingly, a thorough meta-analysis on the
data available until June 2007 examined 183 papers on 393 polymorphisms
(Lopez-Leon et al. 2008). Twenty-two of these polymorphisms have been tested in
at least three studies and were thus subjected to further meta-analysis. Here,
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 469
significant association was demonstrated for the genes APOE2 (827 cases), GNB3
(375 cases), MTHFR (875 cases), SLC6A4 (3752 cases), and SLC6A3 (as little as
151 cases). Negative results were obtained for ACE, BDNF, COMT, DRD3,
GABRA3, HTR1A, HTR1B, HTR2A, HTR2C, MAOA, SLC6A2, and TPH1.
Further meta-analyses which have been published before this study were per-
formed on ACE, DRD4, HTR2A, MTHFR, SLC6A4, and TH; positive findings
were obtained for DRD4 (Lopez Leon et al. 2005; 917 cases). In the years fol-
lowing the meta-analysis by Lopez Leon, only few other meta-analytic studies
have been published including those on MTHFR and TPH1 cited above. Fur-
thermore, Franke and associates recently conducted a meta-analysis on the func-
tional BDNF Val66Met polymorphism. As BDNF has been implicated both in the
pathogenesis of depression as well as the mechanism of action of anti-depressant
treatment (Duman and Monteggia 2006), it is an obvious candidate gene and
accordingly was shown to be associated with depression in this meta-analysis of
2812 cases, although the association is sex-specific and only detectable in males
(Verhagen et al. 2010).
The largest body of evidence, and by far the largest sample sizes, exists for the
gene encoding the serotonin transporter (SLC6A4). Following the seminal finding
by Caspi et al. (2003) that environmental influences interact with SLC6A4
genotype to increase the risk toward depression, emphasis has been put on studies
aimed to test such GxE interactions adding a further level of complexity. Fol-
lowing positive meta-analyses confirming a main gene effect of SLC6A4 in
depression (Furlong et al. 1998; Lopez-Leon et al. 2008; Clarke et al. 2010), also
meta-analyses on GxE interaction studies yielded support for the notion that
SLC6A4 has a role in the etiology of depression (Karg et al. 2011). As Karg and
Sen elaborate in depth on this topic in this book, the reader is referred to their
contribution as well as the review articles by Uher and McGuffin (2008, 2010).
Also, genes encoding components of the cortisol pathway have proven to be
interesting candidates for GxE in MDD. The glucocorticoid receptor-regulating
co-chaperone FKBP5 has first been associated with recurrence of depression and
response to antidepressant treatment in 2004 (Binder et al. 2004). This has later
been replicated (Lekman et al. 2008) and FKBP5 was shown to interact with
HTR2A and GRIK4 in moderating the response to antidepressant treatment
(Horstmann et al. 2010). Most interestingly, FKBP5 has been shown to interact
with severity of childhood abuse on later-life PTSD symptoms (Binder et al. 2008)
which however might be confined to African Americans (Xie et al. 2010). One of
the involved SNPs (rs1360780) was later replicated to interact with childhood
maltreatment to affect depression measures (BDI-II) in adult life (Appel et al.
2011). This SNP also displayed a main gene effect on suicidal events in depressive
adolescents (Brent et al. 2010), a categorical diagnosis of depression (Lekman
et al. 2008) and general depressive symptoms (Lavebratt et al. 2010; Zobel et al.
2010; Velders et al. 2011) and interestingly is associated with decreased cortisol
levels (Velders et al. 2011) providing a possible pathophysiological mechanism for
the association data. Also, rs1360780 went along with smaller right hippocampal
volume in patients with depression (Zobel et al. 2010). Taken together, there is
470 K. Domschke and A. Reif
good evidence (Binder 2009) that risk genotypes in FKBP5 (especially rs1360780)
interact with early life adversity on later life depression, possibly by long-term
adaptive changes of the HPA axis and subsequent morphological changes of the
hippocampus increasing vulnerability to disease. In line with these findings, it was
also shown that rs110402 in the corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor
(CRHR1) interacted with child abuse to increase the risk toward later life
depression (Bradley et al. 2008), most interestingly in interaction with the sero-
tonin transporter risk genotype mentioned above (Ressler et al. 2010).
It seems to be a common phenomenon that candidate genes rarely replicate
when tested for in GWAS (see below) data sets. A thorough study on MDD
candidate genes (Bosker et al. 2011) tested 57 genes in the Genetic Association
Information Network (GAIN) MDD sample (n = 1862 cases). From 93 selected
candidate SNPs, only 18 were present on the array, and a further 47 were imputed.
Of those, only five (including an SNP in NPY) were associated in the GAIN
sample, all with p[0.03. When candidates were tested on a gene-based level,
analyzing 4870 SNPs, the TNF and NET genes yielded suggestive evidence.
In general, heavy use of imputation might have introduced a further source of
noise in this particular study. Likewise, when candidate genes were targeted in
other individual GWAS, rarely more than expected by chance replicated.
Taken together, numerous association studies on MDD and BPD—actually too
many to mention in this overview—hitherto only presented few convincing find-
ings. Amongst them, associations of affective disorders with MTHFR, BDNF, and
SLC6A4 seem to be robust. A length variant in the latter gene shows solid evi-
dence for GxE effect; FKBP5 is another promising candidate for the moderating
effects of early life stress regarding depression in adult life. Lessons that can be
learned from the plethora of false-positive findings to date are that large and well-
characterized samples have to be accrued, with careful evaluation of life events
and the assessment of biological measures such as neuroimaging endophenotypes
or therapy response.
genetic variation. The authors assumed that further 730,000 individuals would
have to be genotyped to uncover 95% of these variants (Speliotes et al. 2010).
Likewise, a recent study on body height (Lango Allen et al. 2010) examined
180,000 subjects and found 180 loci explaining 16% of phenotypic variants.
As body weight and height are for sure somewhat easier to determine than
depression, which is a heterogeneous condition from the start, one can easily see
the obstacles one has to face when dealing with these kinds of studies. Having said
this, and thereby also lowering the bar of expectations somehow, the above-men-
tioned studies are also encouraging and can be seen as proof-of-principle: GWAS
can detect novel pathways and provide meaningful results, and thus larger scale
studies should be encouraged in order to identify the molecular determinants of
affective disorders, as previous linkage and association studies fell short in con-
clusively delineating these. Concluding these introductory remarks, the recent
debate on the missing—or, rather, hidden—heritability has to be mentioned. The
discussion whether the major endogenous psychoses are due to the sum of multiple
common alleles with small individual effects (e.g. Purcell et al. 2009) or due to many
rare variants, also including copy number variants (CNV), and causing ‘‘synthetic
associations’’ (Dickson et al. 2010) and resulting in phenocopies (Gershon et al.
2011) is held lively and far from being resolved. These authors’ personal view is that
both models might exist—which however complicates matters even more.
BPD
GWA studies on BPD, which has a higher heritability and presumably less
GxE effects as compared to depression, have (probably due to these facts)
provided stronger findings than MDD GWAS. The following section elaborates
on the most interesting findings from BPD GWAS at the time of writing
(04/2011) with focus on replicated risk genes and pathways, while issues like
population admixture, microarray technology, and statistical comments are not
further commented upon.
In one of the first published GWAS on BPD, Baum et al. (2008a) reported
genome-wide significance of rs10120253 in intron 1 of diacylglycerol kinase eta
(DGKH) in a German and US American population using a pooling approach. The
gene product of DGKH metabolizes diacylglycerol (DAG), which is produced
upon cleavage of PIP2 into IP3 and DAG by phospholipase C. DAG, in turn,
activates protein kinase C which phosphorylates a variety of proteins including
Dishevelled, an inhibitor of GSK3b (which itself has been considered an out-
standing candidate gene for BPD due to several lines of molecular genetic evi-
dence; Luykx et al. 2010). Furthermore, DGKH knockdown in HeLa cells
impaired the MEK/ERK pathway, while overexpression activated the pathway
(Yasuda et al. 2009). DGKH might therefore be involved in crucial pathways for
psychiatric disorders and especially the mechanism of action of lithium. However,
replication of DGKH failed in three studies on BPD and/or lithium response,
respectively (Manchia et al. 2009; Tesli et al. 2009; Takata et al. 2011) while four
other studies were ambiguous or positive (Baum et al. 2008b; Ollila et al. 2009;
Squassina et al. 2009; Zeng et al. 2011). A recent study provided evidence for
472 K. Domschke and A. Reif
an association of a DGKH risk haplotype with MDD, BPD, and adult ADHD (Weber
et al. 2011). Meta-analyses on Caucasian (Weber et al. 2011) as well as Asian (Takata
et al. 2011) samples however demonstrated that DGKH is significantly associated
with BPD. Furthermore, increased expression of DGKH in BPD (Moya et al. 2010)
was demonstrated in human post-mortem tissue, so that DGKH represents one of the
most promising candidate genes for BPD to date. Other candidates from the Baum
et al. GWAS include NXN, VGCNL1, DFNB31, and SORCS2, the latter two of
which were replicated in a later study (Ollila et al. 2009).
The UK Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium (WTCCC 2007) aims at the
investigation of several complex genetic disorders with high prevalence. As evi-
dent from the first glance on the Manhattan plots in this paper, there are no
‘‘skyscraping’’ BPD risk SNPs as compared to very clear signals in cardiovascular
or metabolic disorders. The WTCCC BPD GWAS provided genome-wide evi-
dence for a non-gene marker next to PALB2, NDUFAB1, and DCTN5; other
signals were observed for KCNC2, GABRB1, GRM7, and SYN3, all of which are
in pathways previously implicated in BPD. Shortly after the WTCCC report, Sklar
et al. reported on the STEP-UCL study and provided significant findings for
MYO5B, TSPAN8, CDH7, and EGFR (Sklar et al. 2008). Some of those genes
were attempted to replicate using a targeted approach; both TSPAN8 (Scholz et al.
2010) and CDH7 (Soronen et al. 2010) were confirmed in doing so. When the
significant signals from the WTCCC and Baum data sets were tested in Sklar’s
STEP-BD/UCL sample, negative findings were observed for DGKH and PALB2,
however, this analysis provided further support for CACNA1C and DFNB31
arguing for the rationale to combine large data sets. Accordingly, the fourth
GWAS study, ED-DUB-STEP2 (Ferreira et al. 2008), investigated another 1000
patients and included meta-analytic treatment of the WTCCC and STEP-UCL data
sets (total n = 4387 cases). In doing so, the holy grail of genome-wide significance
was reached for markers in two genes: CACNA1C (alpha-1 subunit of a voltage
dependent calcium channel) and ANK3 (ankyrin 3). Other interesting candidate
genes from this study include SYNE1, SPRED1, CMTM8 (which interacts with
EGFR), NPAS3 (which has previously been suggested to be associated with
schizophrenia and bipolar disorder; Pickard et al. 2009), and ARNT2. In a sub-
sequent meta-analysis including two samples from Nordic countries, the breast
cancer risk genes PALB2 and BRCA2 were followed up in these as well as the
WTCCC and STEP-UCL/ED-DUB-STEP2 samples (total case n = 5547). In doing
so, variants in both genes were shown to be associated with BPD (Tesli et al.
2010). Not surprisingly nevertheless, CACNA1C and ANK3 drew most attention
in follow-up studies.
For both ANK3 and CACNA1C, it is noteworthy that replication attempts not
only provided evidence that these genes are associated with BPD; rather, they were
demonstrated to be associated with a broad range of disorders across diagnostic
boundaries arguing for a more unspecific role of these genes in psychiatric dis-
orders. For example, ANK3 was not only replicated in BPD (Lee et al. 2010;
Schulze et al. 2009; Scott et al. 2009; Smith et al. 2009), but also associated with
schizophrenia (Athanasiu et al. 2010). Likewise, CACNA1C was again found to be
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 473
associated with BPD (Keers et al. 2009), but also with schizophrenia (Green et al.
2009; Moskvina et al. 2009; Nyegaard et al. 2010), MDD (Green et al. 2009), and
psychopathological features (e.g., agitation) therein (Casamassima et al. 2010).
Neuroimaging studies demonstrated an effect of the CACNA1C risk variant
rs1006737 on brain structure (Franke et al. 2010; Kempton et al. 2009) as well as
function (Erk et al. 2010; Krug et al. 2010; Wessa et al. 2010).
In the last 2 years, several other GWAS and meta-analyses on BPD have been
published. A small study from Japan (Hattori et al. 2009), which applied a two-stage
design, provided nominal although not corrected significance for markers within
AUTS2 (previously implicated in autism), SNAP25 (which is a schizophrenia and
ADHD candidate gene), PLXNA2 (which has been found in schizophrenia and
anxiety GWAS) and CSMD1, which was already one of the candidates from the Baum
et al. study (Baum et al. 2008a). No other top hits from previous BPD GWAS however
were replicated. In Han Chinese patients suffering from BPD type I, Lee et al. (2010)
likewise did not provide findings on the genome-wide level, yet interestingly
also found suggestive association of BPD with SNPs in another voltage-dependent
calcium channel subunit, CACNB2 (other highly significant SNPs were located in
KCTD12, SP8, and ST8SIA2) pointing again to calcium signalling having a role in
BPD. Targeted investigation of previously identified GWAS candidate genes yielded
a p = 10-5 for an SNP near ANK3, yet no other gene has been replicated.
Scott and colleagues combined two GWAS studies from the US, Canada, and
UK (the NIMH/Pritzker and GSK GWAS) and analyzed them separately as well as
in conjunction with the WTCCC study (Scott et al. 2009). In doing so, no genome-
wide significant finding was observed; yet three regions with a p around 10-7 were
reported encompassing the genes MCTP1 (which encodes a high-affinity calcium
binding protein which is highly expressed in the brain), ITIH1 and GLN3.
Furthermore, CTNNA2 was amongst the top hits which also gained support from
other hypothesis-free approaches in psychiatric disorders such as ADHD (Lesch
et al. 2008). Neither DGKH, ANK3 nor CACNA1C were confirmed in this
GWAS, however the latter yielded convincing support upon a fixed-effects meta-
analysis including the Ferreira and Schulze studies. In 2009, Kelsoe and associates
reported on two GWAS examining US Americans of European and African
ancestry (Smith et al. 2009), respectively, with a combined n = 1346 BPD cases.
Again, no genome-wide significant findings emerged and interestingly, significant
findings were discrepant for each subsample. One of the promising top hits in this
study is NTRK2, as this gene which encodes a neurotrophin receptor has been
implicated in mood disorders previously. When previous GWAS risk genes
were tested for, ANK3 yielded further support, while CACNA1C was negative.
It should be noted that this sample overlaps with the one tested by Baum et al.;
generally, the sample overlap between different studies will become the rule rather
than the exception due to the need for international cooperation and large sample
sizes. Furthermore, a small GWAS from Norway (n = 194), which however
was followed up in a larger Icelandic sample (Djurovic et al. 2010), provided
suggestive evidence for several interesting candidate genes (e.g. GUCY1B2,
SHANK, and CNTNAP5), none of which however was amongst the top hits in
474 K. Domschke and A. Reif
previous studies. The largest study to date has employed a two-stage design using
a discovery cohort of 682 BPD patients and carried over the top 48 SNPs to
replication samples; SNPs surviving this procedure were subjected to a meta-
analysis with previous BPD GWAS data sets (Cichon et al. 2011). The total case
number investigated in this study was thus n = 8441 BPD patients. In doing so, the
neurocan (NCAN) gene was identified as a susceptibility factor for BPD with the
best SNP yielding a p = 2.1 9 10-9, i.e. genome-wide significance.
In conclusion, GWAS on BPD and subsequent meta-analysis provided evidence
that BPD shares risk variants with schizophrenia, MDD, and ADHD; furthermore,
calcium and GABA signalling pathways were repeatedly found to be associated
with disease, along with genes modifying neuronal plasticity. At the time of
writing, CACNA1C, ANK3, and DGKH can be considered the risk genes with the
most compelling body of evidence. Accordingly, those are scrutinized more
thoroughly and first functional studies already provided evidence for changes in
brain function in risk allele carriers.
MDD
While the prevalence of MDD is five to tenfold higher as compared to BPD, its
heritability is lower and presumably heterogeneity is even higher. These issues
complicate GWAS on this phenotype, probably explaining the lack of genome-
wide findings despite the fact that studies on MDD and schizophrenia feature the
largest of all disorders analyzed in the PGC.
The NIH sponsored Genetic Association Information Network (GAIN) studies
also featured major depression and these studies were amongst the first published
GWAS on MDD (Sullivan et al. 2009). Discovery sample patients came from two
Dutch longitudinal studies (NESDA and NTR, combined n [1700). The top 25
SNPs featured four SNPs in the PCLO gene, which encodes for a subunit of the
presynaptic vesicle fusion complex, although none of them met the criteria for
genome-wide significance. Considerable overlap was noted for the mood disorder
candidate genes CACNA1C, ANK3, GRM7, and DGKH. While PCLO did not
clearly replicate in the Sullivan et al. study, a later reanalysis questioned this initial
notion and argued for an association of a non-synonymous coding SNP with MDD
in the very same replication cohorts (Bochdanovits et al. 2009). Furthermore, a
later population-based study demonstrated an association of PCLO rs2522833 with
depressive disorders (Hek et al. 2010), which also held true when a meta-analysis
of all published data was conducted especially when only population-based studies
were considered (p = 1.9 9 10-9). Most interestingly, in a hypothesis-free
approach, PCLO was demonstrated to be differentially expressed and associated
with BPD again questioning the diagnostic specificity of GWAS candidate genes
(Choi et al. 2011). However, meta-analysis of an MDD and a BPD GWAS did not
support a role of PCLO in BPD (Liu et al. 2011).
In a medium-sized GWAS from Germany (Rietschel et al. 2010), there was
a suggestive finding for HOMER1 which replicated in an independent sample.
In this study, the authors also conducted a genomic imaging study and demonstrated
decreased dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation in the n-back task as well as
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 475
was amongst the top ten hits in this analysis. From the pre-selected candidate genes,
IL10, OPRM1 (being a candidate stemming from the GAIN MDD GWAS), HTT,
HTR1B, GRIN1, and the apparently pleiotropic risk gene CACNA1C were associ-
ated with disease. Meta-analysis did not yield significant findings and in particular
did not support PCLO as a risk gene for MDD.
Trait depression, as assessed with the NEO-PI personality questionnaire (where
‘‘depression’’ is a subscale of the Neuroticism domain) was assessed in two GWAS
in the general population from Sardinia and the US (combined n = 4811; Terr-
acciano et al. 2010). The top hit, at a p = 6 9 10-7, was an intronic SNP in the
RORA gene. Two other noteworthy high-ranking candidates include the glutamate
receptor gene GRM8, which hitherto has mainly been associated with cognitive
phenotypes, and CDH13, which has been identified in an ADHD linkage scan
meta-analysis (Zhou et al. 2008) as well as GWAS on ADHD (Lesch et al. 2008)
and substance use disorders.
Cross-Disorder Analyses
In order to yield larger samples, several meta-analyses have been conducted.
The largest meta-analysis combined the WTCCC, STEP-BD, NIMH-BD, and the
German BPD sample, as well as the GAIN-MDD GWAS on MDD. The total
number of cases exceeded 6600, compared against [9000 controls (McMahon
et al. 2010). An inherent problem with this kind of study is the use of different
genotyping platforms, diagnostic heterogeneity, as well as ethnic heterogeneity;
to minimize these limitations, only subjects of European descent have been
analyzed. In doing so, six SNPs which were located in the PBRM1 gene met the
criteria for genome-wide significance. The best SNP was also significant in the
replication sample, yielding a final p = 1.7 9 10-9. When the ED-DUB-STEP2
GWAS was analyzed together with an MDD GWAS (Liu et al. 2011), CAC-
NA1C SNPs passed the hurdle of genome-wide significance while ANK3 was
not supported in the meta-analysis, probably suggesting that this gene is more
specific to BPD. SYNE1 was one of the candidates where the significance level
actually increased upon meta-analysis, and which interestingly also turned up in
the primary PGC BPD GWAS meta-analysis. Not surprisingly, also schizo-
phrenia and BPD were treated meta-analytically (Wang et al. 2010). In this
study, meta-analysis provided evidence for the genes ASTN2 and CNTNAP2,
both of which have been implicated in ADHD, as well as the GABA receptors
GABRR1 and GABRR2. When the three large US American GWAS on psy-
chiatric disorders and treatment efficacy—namely, STEP-BD (BPD), CATIE
(schizophrenia), and STAR*D (MDD) were analyzed jointly (yielding a total
ncases[3000; (Huang et al. 2010), one locus met the criteria for being genome-
wide significant (near the ADM gene, and apparently being specific for bipolar II
disorder). A total of 24 more SNPs reached the defined Omnibus GWAS Test
Threshold; however, more than half of them were imputed. Promising candidates
are again CTNND2, SP8, ODZ4, and NPAS3.
An alternative rationale is to search for risk variants influencing phenotypic
features of mood disorders. Suitable phenotypes include, for example, therapy
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 477
1.3 Pharmacogenetics
non-responders have suggested a variety of risk genes such as the SLC6A4 (seven
studies, mixed findings) and other hypothesis-driven targets mainly of neuro-
transmitter pathways, intracellular signal transduction pathways involved in the
mechanism of action of lithium, and circadian clock genes. GSK3B and CREB1
are amongst the candidates with the best empirical support and also good face
validity and hence should be further tested, as evidence is far from being solid.
Samples were often rather small and again, most genes lack replication. Scholarly
overviews on published association studies can be obtained from McCarthy et al.
(2010) and Smith et al. (2010). Although not specifically designed for this ques-
tion, the STEP-BD trial tested for lithium response as well by means of a GWA
study, which was replicated in a second, independent sample (n = 458, and n = 359
bipolar I or II patients, respectively). Not surprisingly, there were no genome-wide
significant findings, however five SNPs associated in the STEP-BD cohort repli-
cated in the second sample including a polymorphism within the GRIA2 gene
(Perlis et al. 2009). To specifically search for lithium response genes, the
ConLiGen consortium (Schulze et al. 2010) has gathered more than 1200 lithium
treated BPD patients whose treatment response has been evaluated using the Alda
scale. Genome-wide genotyping has been accomplished and initial data are
expected for the second half of 2011.
2 Anxiety Disorders
Panic disorder has been found to be highly familial with an up to three to fivefold
increased prevalence of the disorder in first-degree relatives of patients with panic
disorder (Horwath et al. 1995; Maier et al. 1993; Hettema et al. 2001). In relatives
of the subgroup of patients with panic disorder and suffocation anxiety, an even
higher familiarity has been discerned (Horwath et al. 1997). Furthermore, famil-
iarity of panic disorder seems to depend on the age of onset in the index patient
with an onset before the age of 20 years predicting a 17-fold increased risk of
panic disorder in first-degree relatives (Goldstein et al. 1997). Also for generalized
anxiety disorder and specific phobias a significant familial aggregation was
reported (Hettema et al. 2001; Marks and Herst 1970).
Twin studies have identified up to 2–3 times higher concordance rates for panic
disorder in monozygotic as compared to dizygotic twins (Skre et al. 1993), with an
even higher concordance rate for the subgroup of patients with carbon dioxide-
sensitive panic disorder (Bellodi et al. 1998). According to a comprehensive meta-
analysis, the contribution of genetic factors has been calculated to be as high as up
to 48%, with the remaining 52% being attributable to individual environmental
factors. Generalized anxiety disorder has been estimated to have a heritability of
about 32%, while the common heritability of phobias was reported to be about
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 481
In panic disorder, linkage studies have yielded a variety of potential risk loci on
chromosomes 1p, 4q, 7p, 9q, 11p, 15q, und 20p (Crowe et al. 1987, 2001; Knowles
et al. 1998; Gelernter et al. 2001; Hamilton et al. 2003; Thorgeirsson et al. 2003;
Fyer et al. 2006; Kaabi et al. 2006). In subgroups of patients with panic disorder
with comorbid bipolar disorder or kidney/bladder dysfunction, respectively, risk
loci on chromosomes 2, 12, 13, and 18 or 13 and 22, respectively, have been
described (MacKinnon et al. 1998; Logue et al. 2009; Weissman et al. 2000;
Hamilton et al. 2003). In social or specific phobia, linkage studies have excluded a
major influence of HTR2A and SLC6A4 loci, with however, some evidence for
potential risk loci on chromosomes 16q and 14p (Stein et al. 1998; Gelernter et al.
2003, 2004).
In panic disorder, a variety of association studies has been published so far. Most
studies have investigated variation in classical candidate genes for panic disorder
as suggested by animal models (e.g. knock-out mice), challenge experiments (e.g.
cholecystokinin [CCK] challenge, caffeine challenge), or pharmacological obser-
vations (e.g. clinical efficacy of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs),
monoamine oxidase (MAO) inhibitors). Most significant evidence has been yiel-
ded for association of variants in the CCKBR (Kennedy et al. 1999; Hösing et al.
2004), MAOA, particularly in female patients (Deckert et al. 1999; Samochowiec
et al. 2004; Maron et al. 2005b), COMT (again restricted to female patients,
Hamilton et al. 2002; Domschke et al. 2004; Woo et al. 2002; Woo et al. 2004;
Domschke et al. 2007; Zintzaras and Sakelaridis 2007), HTR1A (Rothe et al.
2004; Huang et al. 2004) and ADORA2A (Deckert et al. 1998; Hamilton et al.
482 K. Domschke and A. Reif
2004a; Hohoff et al. 2010). Furthermore, there is some evidence for several other
potential risk variants to be involved in the pathogenesis of panic disorder such as
polymorphisms in HTR2A (Inada et al. 2003; Rothe et al. 2004; Maron et al.
2005a; Unschuld et al. 2007; Yoon et al. 2008), SLC6A4 (Ohara et al. 1998;
Maron et al. 2005a, b; Strug et al. 2010; but: Deckert et al. 1997; Hamilton et al.
1999; Blaya et al. 2007), TPH2 (Maron et al. 2007; Kim et al. 2009; but: Mössner
et al. 2006), NET (Lee et al. 2005; but: Sand et al. 2002a), CCK (Wang et al. 1998;
Hattori et al. 2001), ACE (Olsson et al. 2004; Erhardt et al. 2008; Bandelow et al.
2010; but: Shimizu et al. 2004), the transcription factor CREM (Domschke et al.
2003; Hamilton et al. 2004b), ‘regulator of G-protein signaling’ (RGS2, RGS7)
(Leygraf et al. 2006, Smoller et al. 2008b; Hohoff et al. 2009b) and several
hormone receptors (Sand et al. 2002b; Ho et al. 2004; Keck et al. 2008; Hodges
et al. 2009). However, since most of these studies either did not withstand repli-
cation in independent samples or still warrant replication, these results have to be
considered preliminary. The role of the GABA-ergic system in panic disorder
remains to be further elucidated on a molecular genetic level with only little
evidence so far for the glutamate decarboxylase (GAD) or GABA receptors and
transporters, respectively (Crowe et al. 1997; Sand et al. 2000; Hettema et al.
2006; Nakamura et al. 2006; Kobayashi et al. 2007; Thoeringer et al. 2007, 2009;
Unschuld et al. 2009). Recently, besides the classic neurotransmitter systems much
attention has been paid to the role of neuropeptides in the mediation of anxiety.
Significant association of anxiety or panic disorder in particular have been reported
for variants in genes for galanin (Unschuld et al. 2008), the neuropeptide Y (NPY)
system (Domschke et al. 2008b) and the neuropeptide S receptor (NPSR)
(Domschke et al. 2011). Finally, there is preliminary support for possible inter-
active effects of several genetics variants in the mediation of the genetic risk for
panic disorder, e.g. for HTR1A and COMT (Freitag et al. 2006). For social phobia
and generalized anxiety disorder, association has been reported with variation in
the dopamine transporter (SLC6A3) gene (Rowe et al. 1998), while DRD2 variants
seem to play a role in the pathogenesis of posttraumatic stress disorder (Segman
and Shalev 2003). Further associations were observed for COMT in specific
phobias (McGrath et al. 2004), for HTR2A in social phobia (Lochner et al. 2007)
and MAOA in generalized anxiety disorder (Tadic et al. 2003).
In summary, consistent with findings from clinical genetic and linkage studies,
molecular genetic association studies point to a complex genetic etiology of
anxiety disorders with an additive or rather interactive effect of multiple risk
variants.
replicated in a follow-up GWAS by the same group (Otowa et al. 2010). Another
genome-wide association study in three German samples points to a potential role
of the TMEM132D gene in the pathogenesis of panic disorder (Erhardt et al.
2011). Currently, another large GWAS on a homogenous sample from Germany is
under analysis (Reif et al., in preparation).
2.5 Pharmacogenetics
In anxiety disorders, so far only three exemplary studies have investigated the
impact of genetic variants on response to a pharmacological treatment regime.
Two groups reported significant association of 5-HTTLPR with response to SSRI
treatment in panic disorder as well as in generalized anxiety disorder (Perna et al.
2005; Stein et al. 2006). Furthermore, SSRI treatment in panic disorder might in
part be driven by variation in HTR1A (Yevtushenko et al. 2010).
3 Overlapping Phenotypes
Depression and anxiety are highly comorbid with up to 60% of patients with
depression also displaying anxiety (Leckman et al. 1983) and about 58% of those
patients actually meeting DSM criteria for anxiety disorders (see review by
Lydiard 1991; e.g. de Graaf et al. 2002; Kessler et al. 1996; Zimmerman et al.
2002). Comorbidity of affective and anxiety disorders has a significant impact on
the course and treatment of the respective leading disease with a more chronic
course and a significantly detrimental effect on treatment response (e.g. Clayton
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 485
et al. 1991; Liebowitz 1993; Lydiard 1991). Patterns of occurrence allow for both
affective and anxiety disorders preceding the respective other disease.
Besides either simultaneous or sequential true comorbidity of anxiety disorders
and major depression there is a continuous debate about a possible overlapping
phenotype between anxiety disorders and depression. The clinical phenotype of
‘‘anxious depression’’ (Overall et al. 1966; Overall and Zisook 1980) capturing
major depression with subthreshold anxious features has been suggested to con-
stitute a diagnostic entity of its own requiring specific diagnostic and therapeutic
attention (see Levine et al. 2001; Lydiard and Brawman-Mintzer 1998; Silverstone
and von Studnitz 2003). Dual action drugs acting as reuptake inhibitors on both
transporters (serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, SNRI) have been
suggested to be superior to SSRI only or tricyclic antidepressants TCA in the
treatment of anxious depression (Rudolph et al. 1998; Silverstone and Ravindran
1999). While three large meta-analyses discerned similar response rates to anti-
depressant treatment in highly anxious and less anxious patients with major
depression (Levine et al. 2001; Nelson et al. 2009; Papakostas et al. 2008; see
Nelson 2008), there is accumulating evidence for anxious features of depression
potentially complicating the course of antidepressant treatment (e.g. Altamura
et al. 2004; Domschke et al. 2010; Fava et al. 2008; Joffe et al. 1993; see review by
Bagby et al. 2002).
Apart from the individual genetic risk for affective and anxiety disorders, both
disease entities also exhibit a common familial risk (as reviewed by Middeldorp
et al. 2005). There is evidence from twin studies that depression and general
anxiety disorder, panic disorder, and post-traumatic stress disorder share a con-
siderable proportion of their genetic risk (Kendler et al. 1992, 2007; Kendler 1996;
Roy et al. 1995). Consistently, molecular genetic studies have yielded evidence for
specific genetic loci that may generally influence susceptibility across the anxiety-
depression spectrum, e.g. on chromosome 18q (cf. Camp et al. 2005; Hettema
2008). In particular, the combined clinical phenotype of anxious depression has
been suggested to constitute a specific subtype with an increased familial risk of
depression (Clayton et al. 1990, 1991), which points to a possibly increased her-
itability of anxious depression with a specific set of genetic risk factors mediating
the vulnerability for the development of anxious depression. First imaging and
pharmacogenetic studies in anxious depression have implied CNR1, NPY, and
SLC6A4 to confer parts of antidepressant treatment response particularly in the
clinical phenotype of anxious depression, potentially via a dysfunctional cortico-
limbic interaction underlying distorted emotional processing (e.g. Baffa et al.
2010; Domschke et al. 2008a; 2010).
These molecular and imaging genetic findings of overlapping genetic variants
as well as common brain networks of emotional processing partly driving both
clinical phenotypes of anxiety and affective disorders point to similar neurobio-
logical mechanisms underlying these disorders and therefore possibly a common
clinical sub-phenotype shared by anxiety and affective disorders. Particularly,
the clinical phenotype of ‘‘anxious depression’’ might thus possibly constitute a
diagnostic entity of its own requiring specific diagnostic and therapeutic attention
486 K. Domschke and A. Reif
(cf. Lydiard and Brawman-Mintzer 1998; Silverstone and von Studnitz 2003).
So, back from bench to bedside, genetic and imaging studies might inspire a
re-evaluation and refinement of DSM-IV categorized nosological concepts of
depression and anxiety. Alternatively, the current and still emerging body of
knowledge in the field of neurobiological research in anxiety and depression might
have even more far-reaching consequences in the future by challenging the DSM
concept in itself in favor of a more neurobiologically oriented taxonomy of mental
disorders. As suggested by Smoller et al. (2008a), genetic and imaging research
revealing etiological mechanisms of mental disorders might infer a novel noso-
logical concept based on pathogenesis more than phenomenology. To date,
however, despite first essential steps having been made, neurobiological knowl-
edge about the pathomechanism of depression and anxiety has still not progressed
far enough to provide a reliable and valid fundament for diagnostic decisions in
daily clinical practice. So, in summary the presently known vulnerability genes
and patterns of affective and anxiety disorders are slowly beginning to challenge
the DSM-defined nosological boundaries and might have the potential to evolve
into a valuable tool to more precisely delineate the diagnostic system of mental
disorders in the future.
4 Outlook
Future research with respect to the genetic dissection of affective and anxiety
disorders will have to comprise technical as well as clinical aspects. On a
molecular genetic level, more comprehensive analyses such as tagging SNP
approaches, haplotype analyses, as well as the investigation of epistasis of several
genes constituting relevant biochemical pathways or cascades are warranted. Here,
novel genomic techniques such as duplication/deletion analysis using genotyping
arrays and next-generation sequencing of the whole exome or genome for point
mutation identification might have a large impact on risk gene identification.
Furthermore, it will be of utmost importance to analyze the functional conse-
quences of the associated genetic variants and thereby gain more knowledge about
the pathomechanism of the disease of interest. Additionally, there is a need for
more detailed gene-environment interaction studies potentially also in a genome-
wide fashion (cf. Poulton et al. 2008; Thomas 2010) in order to disentangle the
interactive effect of genetic and environmental factors conferring risk or resilience,
respectively, to affective and anxiety disorders. In this respect, epigenetic studies
investigating e.g. DNA methylation or histone modifications regulating gene
activity will tremendously contribute to the elucidation of the interplay between
environmental and genetic factors in the pathogenesis of affective and anxiety
disorders (cf. for bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: Abdolmaleky et al. 2006,
2008).
Besides the more technical aspects as detailed above, future research in the
genetics of affective and anxiety disorders will greatly benefit from clinical
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 487
Acknowledgments This paper was supported by the DFG (Grant RE1632/1-1,/1-3 and/5 to AR,
KFO 125 TP4 to AR, DE357/4-1 to AR, SFB TRR 58 C2 and Z2 to AR and KD, respectively;
RTG 1252, to AR) and the BMBF (Panik-Net Subproject 6, to AR). We thank A. Alttoa for
valuable discussions and her kind help in the preparation of the manuscript.
References
Angst J, Cui L, Swendsen J et al (2010) Major depressive disorder with subthreshold bipolarity in
the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am J Psychiatry 167:1194–1201
Antypa N, Van der Does J (2010) Serotonin transporter gene, childhood emotional abuse and
cognitive vulnerability to depression. Genes Brain Behav 9:615–620
Appel K, Schwahn C, Mahler J, Schulz A, Spitzer C, Fenske K, Stender J, Barnow S, John U,
Teumer A, Biffar R, Nauck M, Völzke H, Freyberger HJ, Grabe HJ (2011) Moderation of
adult depression by a polymorphism in the FKBP5 gene and childhood physical abuse in the
general population. Neuropsychopharmacology 36(10):1982–1991
Athanasiu L, Mattingsdal M, Kahler AK et al (2010) Gene variants associated with schizophrenia
in a Norwegian genome-wide study are replicated in a large European cohort. J Psychiatr Res
44:748–753
Badner JA, Gershon ES (2002) Meta-analysis of whole-genome linkage scans of bipolar disorder
and schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 7:405–411
Baffa A, Hohoff C, Baune BT et al (2010) Norepinephrine and serotonin transporter genes:
impact on treatment response in depression. Neuropsychobiology 62:121–131
Bagby RM, Ryder AG, Cristi C (2002) Psychosocial and clinical predictors of response to
pharmacotherapy for depression. Rev Psychiatr Neurosci 27:250–257
Bandelow B, Saleh K, Pauls J et al (2010) Insertion/deletion polymorphism in the gene for
angiotensin converting enzyme (ACE) in panic disorder: A gender-specific effect? World J
Biol Psychiatry 11:66–70
Barnett JH, Smoller JW (2009) The genetics of bipolar disorder. Neuroscience 164:331–343
Baum AE, Akula N, Cabanero M et al (2008a) A genome-wide association study implicates
diacylglycerol kinase eta (DGKH) and several other genes in the etiology of bipolar disorder.
Mol Psychiatry 13:197–207
Baum AE, Hamshere M, Green E et al (2008b) Meta-analysis of two genome-wide association
studies of bipolar disorder reveals important points of agreement. Mol Psychiatry 13:466–467
Baune BT, Hohoff C, Roehrs T et al (2008) Serotonin receptor 1A–1019C/G variant: Impact on
antidepressant pharmacoresponse in melancholic depression? Neurosci Lett 436:111–115
Bellodi L, Perna G, Caldirola D et al (1998) CO2-induced panic attacks: a twin study. Am J
Psychiatry 155:1184–1188
Berrettini WH (2001) Molecular linkage studies of bipolar disorders. Bipolar Disord 3:276–283
Binder EB (2009) The role of FKBP5, a co-chaperone of the glucocorticoid receptor in the
pathogenesis and therapy of affective and anxiety disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology
34(Suppl 1):186–195
Binder EB, Bradley RG, Liu W et al (2008) Association of FKBP5 polymorphisms and childhood
abuse with risk of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in adults. JAMA 299:1291–1305
Binder EB, Salyakina D, Lichtner P et al (2004) Polymorphisms in FKBP5 are associated with
increased recurrence of depressive episodes and rapid response to antidepressant treatment.
Nat Genet 36:1319–1325
Blaya C, Salum GA, Lima MS et al (2007) Lack of association between the Serotonin Transporter
Promoter Polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and panic disorder: a systematic review and meta-
analysis. Behav Brain Funct 3:41
Bochdanovits Z, Verhage M, Smit AB et al (2009) Joint reanalysis of 29 correlated SNPs
supports the role of PCLO/Piccolo as a causal risk factor for major depressive disorder. Mol
Psychiatry 14:650–652
Bosker FJ, Hartman CA, Nolte IM et al (2011) Poor replication of candidate genes for major
depressive disorder using genome-wide association data. Mol Psychiatry 16:516–532
Bradley RG, Binder EB, Epstein MP et al (2008) Influence of child abuse on adult depression:
moderation by the corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor gene. Arch Gen Psychiatry
65:190–200
Brent D, Melhem N, Ferrell R et al (2010) Association of FKBP5 polymorphisms with suicidal
events in the treatment of resistant depression in adolescents (TORDIA) study. Am J
Psychiatry 167:190–197
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 489
de Graaf R, Bijl RV, Smit F et al (2002) Risk factors for 12-month comorbidity of mood, anxiety,
and substance use disorders: findings from the Netherlands mental health survey and
incidence study. Am J Psychiatry 159:620–629
Deckert J, Catalano M, Heils A et al (1997) Functional promoter polymorphism of the human
serotonin transporter: lack of association with panic disorder. Psychiatr Genet 7:45–47
Deckert J, Catalano M, Syagailo YV (1999) Excess of high activity monoamine oxidase A gene
promoter alleles in female patients with panic disorder. Hum Mol Genet 8:621–624
Deckert J, Nothen MM, Franke P (1998) Systematic mutation screening and association study of
the A1 and A2a adenosine receptor genes in panic disorder suggest a contribution of the A2a
gene to the development of disease. Mol Psychiatry 3:81–85
Dickson SP, Wang K, Krantz I (2010) Rare variants create synthetic genome-wide associations.
PLoS Biol 8:e1000294
Djurovic S, Gustafsson O, Mattingsdal M (2010) A genome-wide association study of bipolar
disorder in Norwegian individuals, followed by replication in Icelandic sample. J Affect
Disord 126:312–316
Domschke K, Braun M, Ohrmann P et al (2006) Association of the functional -1019C/G 5-HT1A
polymorphism with prefrontal and amygdala activation measured with 3T fMRI in panic
disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 9:349–355
Domschke K, Dannlowski U (2010) Imaging genetics of anxiety disorders. Neuroimage 53(3):
822–831
Domschke K, Zwanzger P (2008) GABAergic and endocannabinoid dysfunction in anxiety—
future therapeutic targets? Curr Pharm Des 14:3508–3517
Domschke K, Dannlowski U, Hohoff C (2010) Neuropeptide Y (NPY) gene: impact on emotional
processing and treatment response in anxious depression. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol
20:301–309
Domschke K, Dannlowski U, Ohrmann P (2008a) Cannabinoid receptor 1 (CNR1) gene: impact
on antidepressant treatment response and emotion processing in major depression. Eur
Neuropsychopharmacol 18:751–759
Domschke K, Deckert J, O’Donovan MC (2007) Meta-analysis of COMT val158met in panic
disorder: ethnic heterogeneity and gender specificity. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr
Genet 144:667–673
Domschke K, Freitag CM, Kuhlenbäumer G (2004) Association of the functional V158M
catechol-O-methyl-transferase polymorphism with panic disorder in women. Int J
Neuropsychopharmacol 7:183–188
Domschke K, Hohoff C, Jacob C (2008b) Chromosome 4q31–34 panic disorder risk locus:
Association of neuropeptide Y Y5 receptor variants. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr
Genet 147B:510–516
Domschke K, Hohoff C, Mortensen LS (2008c) Monoamine oxidase A variant influences
antidepressant treatment response in female patients with major depression. Prog Neuropsy-
chopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32:224–228
Domschke K, Kuhlenbaumer G, Schirmacher A (2003) Human nuclear transcription factor gene
CREM: genomic organization, mutation screening, and association analysis in panic disorder.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 117:70–78
Domschke K, Ohrmann P, Braun M et al (2008d) Influence of the catechol-O-methyltransferase
val158met genotype on amygdala and prefrontal cortex emotional processing in panic
disorder. Psychiatry Res 163:13–20
Domschke K, Reif A, Weber H, Richter J, Hohoff C, Ohrmann P, Pedersen A, Bauer J, Suslow T,
Kugel H, Heindel W, Baumann C, Klauke B, Jacob C, Maier W, Fritze J, Bandelow B,
Krakowitzky P, Rothermundt M, Erhardt A, Binder EB, Holsboer F, Gerlach AL, Kircher T,
Lang T, Alpers GW, Ströhle A, Fehm L, Gloster AT, Wittchen HU, Arolt V, Pauli P,
Hamm A, Deckert J (2011) Neuropeptide S receptor gene—converging evidence for a role in
panic disorder. Mol Psychiatry 16(9):938–948
Domschke K, Stevens S, Beck B (2009) Blushing propensity in social anxiety disorder: influence
of serotonin transporter gene variation. J Neural Transm 116:663–666
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 491
Duman RS, Monteggia LM (2006) A neurotrophic model for stress-related mood disorders. Biol
Psychiatry 59:1116–1127
Egeland JA, Gerhard DS, Pauls DL (1987) Bipolar affective disorders linked to DNA markers on
chromosome 11. Nature 325:783–787
Erhardt A, Czibere L, Roeske D, Lucae S, Unschuld PG, Ripke S, Specht M, Kohli MA, Kloiber S,
Ising M, Heck A, Pfister H, Zimmermann P, Lieb R, Pütz B, Uhr M, Weber P, Deussing JM,
Gonik M, Bunck M, Kebler MS, Frank E, Hohoff C, Domschke K, Krakowitzky P, Maier W,
Bandelow B, Jacob C, Deckert J, Schreiber S, Strohmaier J, Nöthen M, Cichon S, Rietschel M,
Bettecken T, Keck ME, Landgraf R, Müller-Myhsok B, Holsboer F, Binder EB (2011)
TMEM132D, a new candidate for anxiety phenotypes: evidence from human and mouse studies.
Mol Psychiatry 16(6):647–663
Erhardt A, Lucae S, Kern N et al (2008) Association of polymorphisms in the angiotensin-
converting enzyme gene with syndromal panic attacks. Mol Psychiatry 13:242–243
Erk S, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Schnell K et al (2010) Brain function in carriers of a genome-wide
supported bipolar disorder variant. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67:803–811
Ewald H, Degn B, Mors O et al (1998) Significant linkage between bipolar affective disorder and
chromosome 12q24. Psychiatr Genet 8:131–140
Ewald H, Flint T, Kruse TA et al (2002) A genome-wide scan shows significant linkage between
bipolar disorder and chromosome 12q24.3 and suggestive linkage to chromosomes 1p22–21,
4p16, 6q14–22, 10q26 and 16p13.3. Mol Psychiatry 7:734–744
Fan J, Sklar P (2008) Genetics of bipolar disorder: focus on BDNF Val66Met polymorphism.
Novartis Found Symp 289:60-72; discussion 72-63, 87-93
Fan M, Liu B, Jiang T et al (2010) Meta-analysis of the association between the monoamine
oxidase-A gene and mood disorders. Psychiatr Genet 20:1–7
Fava M, Davidson KG (1996) Definition and epidemiology of treatment-resistant depression.
Psychiatr Clin North Am 19:179–200
Fava M, Rush AJ, Alpert JE (2008) Difference in treatment outcome in outpatients with anxious
versus nonanxious depression: a STAR*D report. Am J Psychiatry 165:342–351
Ferreira MA, O’Donovan MC, Meng YA et al (2008) Collaborative genome-wide association
analysis supports a role for ANK3 and CACNA1C in bipolar disorder. Nat Genet 40:1056–1058
Fiedorowicz JG, Endicott J, Leon AC (2011) Subthreshold hypomanic symptoms in progression
from unipolar major depression to bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 168:40–48
Franchini L, Serretti A, Gasperini M et al (1998) Familial concordance of fluvoxamine response
as a tool for differentiating mood disorder pedigrees. J Psychiatr Res 32:255–259
Franke B, Vasquez AA, Veltman JA, Brunner HG, Rijpkema M, Fernández G (2010) Genetic
variation in CACNA1C, a gene associated with bipolar disorder, influences brainstem rather
than gray matter volume in healthy individuals. Biol Psychiatry 68(6):586–588
Freitag CM, Domschke K, Rothe C et al (2006) Interaction of serotonergic and noradrenergic
gene variants in panic disorder. Psychiatr Genet 16:59–65
Furlong RA, Ho L, Walsh C et al (1998) Analysis and meta-analysis of two serotonin
transporter gene polymorphisms in bipolar and unipolar affective disorders. Am J Med
Genet 81:58–63
Furmark T, Appel L, Henningsson S et al (2008) A link between serotonin-related gene
polymorphisms, amygdala activity, and placebo-induced relief from social anxiety. J Neurosci
28:13066–13074
Furmark T, Henningsson S, Appel L (2009) Genotype over-diagnosis in amygdala responsive-
ness: affective processing in social anxiety disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci 34:30–40
Furmark T, Tillfors M, Garpenstrand H et al (2004) Serotonin transporter polymorphism related
to amygdala excitability and symptom severity in patients with social phobia. Neurosci Lett
362:189–192
Fyer AJ, Hamilton SP, Durner M (2006) A third-pass genome scan in panic disorder: evidence for
multiple susceptibility loci. Biol Psychiatry 60:388–401
Garriock HA, Hamilton SP (2009a) Genetic studies of drug response and side effects in the
STAR*D study, part 1. J Clin Psychiatry 70:1186–1187
492 K. Domschke and A. Reif
Garriock HA, Hamilton SP (2009b) Genetic studies of drug response and side effects in the
STAR*D study, part 2. J Clin Psychiatry 70:1323–1325
Gaysina D, Cohen S, Craddock N et al (2008) No association with the 5, 10-methylenetetra-
hydrofolate reductase gene and major depressive disorder: results of the depression case
control (DeCC) study and a meta-analysis. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet
147B:699–706
Gelernter J, Bonvicini K, Page G et al (2001) Linkage genome scan for loci predisposing to panic
disorder or agoraphobia. Am J Med Genet 105:548–557
Gelernter J, Page GP, Bonvicini K et al (2003) A chromosome 14 risk locus for simple phobia:
results from a genomewide linkage scan. Mol Psychiatry 8:71–82
Gelernter J, Page GP, Stein MB et al (2004) Genome-wide linkage scan for loci predisposing to
social phobia: evidence for a chromosome 16 risk locus. Am J Psychiatry 161:59–66
Gershon ES, Alliey-Rodriguez N, Liu C (2011) After GWAS: searching for genetic risk for
schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 168:253–256
Gilbody S, Lewis S, Lightfoot T (2007) Methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase (MTHFR) genetic
polymorphisms and psychiatric disorders: a HuGE review. Am J Epidemiol 165:1–13
Goldstein RB, Wickramaratne PJ, Horwath E (1997) Familial aggregation and phenomenology of
‘early’-onset (at or before age 20 years) panic disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54:271–278
Green E, Elvidge G, Jacobsen N et al (2005) Localization of bipolar susceptibility locus by
molecular genetic analysis of the chromosome 12q23–q24 region in two pedigrees with
bipolar disorder and Darier’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 162:35–42
Green EK, Grozeva D, Jones I et al (2009) The bipolar disorder risk allele at CACNA1C also
confers risk of recurrent major depression and of schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 15:
1016–1022
Grigoroiu-Serbanescu M, Diaconu CC, Herms S et al (2008) Investigation of the tryptophan
hydroxylase 2 gene in bipolar I disorder in the Romanian population. Psychiatr Genet 18:
240–247
Grozeva D, Kirov G, Ivanov D et al (2010) Rare copy number variants: a point of rarity in genetic
risk for bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67:318–327
Gunthert KC, Conner TS, Armeli S et al (2007) Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-
HTTLPR) and anxiety reactivity in daily life: a daily process approach to gene-environment
interaction. Psychosom Med 69:762–768
Halmoy A, Johansson S, Winge I et al (2010) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms
in offspring of mothers with impaired serotonin production. Arch Gen Psychiatry
67:1033–1043
Hamilton SP, Fyer AJ, Durner M et al (2003) Further genetic evidence for a panic disorder
syndrome mapping to chromosome 13q. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:2550–2555
Hamilton SP, Heiman GA, Haghighi F et al (1999) Lack of genetic linkage or association
between a functional serotonin transporter polymorphism and panic disorder. Psychiatr Genet
9:1–6
Hamilton SP, Slager SL, de Baisre LA et al (2004a) Evidence for genetic linkage between a
polymorphism in the adenosine 2A receptor and panic disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology
29:558–565
Hamilton SP, Slager SL, Heiman GA et al (2002) Evidence for a susceptibility locus for panic
disorder near the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene on chromosome 22. Biol Psychiatry
51:591–601
Hamilton SP, Slager SL, Mayo D (2004b) Investigation of polymorphisms in the CREM gene in
panic disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 126:111–115
Hasler G, Drevets WC, Manji HK et al (2004) Discovering endophenotypes for major depression.
Neuropsychopharmacology 29:1765–1781
Hattori E, Ebihara M, Yamada K et al (2001) Identification of a compound short tandem repeat
stretch in the 50 -upstream region of the cholecystokinin gene, and its association with panic
disorder but not with schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 6:465–470
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 493
Lango Allen H, Estrada K, Lettre G et al (2010) Hundreds of variants clustered in genomic loci
and biological pathways affect human height. Nature 467:832–838
Lasky-Su JA, Faraone SV, Glatt SJ et al (2005) Meta-analysis of the association between two
polymorphisms in the serotonin transporter gene and affective disorders. Am J Med Genet B
Neuropsychiatr Genet 133B:110–115
Laucht M, Treutlein J, Blomeyer D et al (2009) Interaction between the 5-HTTLPR serotonin
transporter polymorphism and environmental adversity for mood and anxiety psychopathol-
ogy: evidence from a high-risk community sample of young adults. Int J Neuropsychophar-
macol 12:737–747
Lavebratt C, Aberg E, Sjoholm LK et al (2010) Variations in FKBP5 and BDNF genes are
suggestively associated with depression in a Swedish population-based cohort. J Affect Disord
125:249–255
Leckman JF, Weissman MM, Merikangas KR et al (1983) Panic disorder and major depression.
Increased risk of depression, alcoholism, panic, and phobic disorders in families of depressed
probands with panic disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 40:1055–1060
Lee MT, Chen CH, Lee C et al (2010) Genome-wide association study of bipolar I disorder in the
Han Chinese population. Mol Psychiatry 16:548–556
Lee YJ, Hohoff C, Domschke K et al (2005) Norepinephrine transporter (NET) promoter and
50 -UTR polymorphisms: association analysis in panic disorder. Neurosci Lett 377:40–43
Lekman M, Laje G, Charney D et al (2008) The FKBP5-gene in depression and treatment
response–an association study in the sequenced treatment alternatives to relieve depression
(STAR*D) cohort. Biol Psychiatry 63:1103–1110
Lesch K, Bengel D, Heils A et al (1996) Association of anxiety-related traits with a
polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 274:1527–1531
Lesch KP, Timmesfeld N, Renner TJ et al (2008) Molecular genetics of adult ADHD: converging
evidence from genome-wide association and extended pedigree linkage studies. J Neural
Transm 115:1573–1585
Levine J, Cole DP, Chengappa KN et al (2001) Anxiety disorders and major depression, together
or apart. Depress Anxiety 14:94–104
Lewis CM, Ng MY, Butler AW et al (2010) Genome-wide association study of major recurrent
depression in the U.K. population. Am J Psychiatry 167:949–957
Leygraf A, Hohoff C, Freitag C et al (2006) Rgs 2 gene polymorphisms as modulators of anxiety
in humans? J Neural Transm 113:1921–1925
Lichtenstein P, Yip BH, Bjork C et al (2009) Common genetic determinants of schizophrenia and
bipolar disorder in Swedish families: a population-based study. Lancet 373:234–239
Liebowitz MR (1993) Depression with anxiety and a typical depression. J Clin Psychiatry
54:10–14
Liu Y, Blackwood DH, Caesar S et al (2011) Meta-analysis of genome-wide association data of
bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder. Mol Psychiatry 16:2–4
Lochner C, Hemmings S, Seedat S et al (2007) Genetics and personality traits in patients with
social anxiety disorder: a case-control study in South Africa. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol
17:321–327
Logue MW, Durner M, Heiman GA et al (2009) A linkage search for joint panic disorder/bipolar
genes. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 150:1139–1146
Lohoff FW (2010) Overview of the genetics of major depressive disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep
12:539–546
Lopez-Leon S, Janssens AC, Gonzalez-Zuloeta Ladd AM et al (2008) Meta-analyses of genetic
studies on major depressive disorder. Mol Psychiatry 13:772–785
Lopez Leon S, Croes EA, Sayed-Tabatabaei FA et al (2005) The dopamine D4 receptor gene
48-base-pair-repeat polymorphism and mood disorders: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry
57:999–1003
Luykx JJ, Boks MP, Terwindt AP et al (2010) The involvement of GSK3beta in bipolar disorder:
integrating evidence from multiple types of genetic studies. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol
20:357–368
496 K. Domschke and A. Reif
Lydiard RB (1991) Coexisting depression and anxiety: special diagnostic and treatment issues.
J Clin Psychiatry 52:48–54
Lydiard RB, Brawman-Mintzer O (1998) Anxious depression. J Clin Psychiatry 59:10–17
MacGillivray S, Arroll B, Hatcher S et al (2003) Efficacy and tolerability of selective serotonin
reuptake inhibitors compared with tricyclic antidepressants in depression treated in primary
care: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ 326:1014
MacKinnon DF, Xu J, McMahon FJ et al (1998) Bipolar disorder and panic disorder in families:
an analysis of chromosome 18 data. Am J Psychiatry 155:829–831
Maier W, Lichtermann D, Minges J et al (1992) Personality traits in subjects at risk for unipolar
major depression: a family study perspective. J Affect Disord 24:153–163
Maier W, Lichtermann D, Minges J (1993) A controlled family study in panic disorder.
J Psychiatr Res 27:79–87
Manchia M, Squassina A, Congiu D et al (2009) Interacting genes in lithium prophylaxis:
preliminary results of an exploratory analysis on the role of DGKH and NR1D1 gene
polymorphisms in 199 Sardinian bipolar patients. Neurosci Lett 467:67–71
Marks IM, Herst ER (1970) A survey of 1200 agoraphobics in Britain. Soc Psychiatry
5:16–24
Maron E, Lang A, Tasa G et al (2005a) Associations between serotonin-related gene
polymorphisms and panic disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 8:261–266
Maron E, Nikopensius T, Koks S et al (2005b) Association study of 90 candidate
gene polymorphisms in panic disorder. Psychiatr Genet 15:17–24
Maron E, Toru I, Must A et al (2007) Association study of tryptophan hydroxylase 2 gene
polymorphisms in panic disorder. Neurosci Lett 411:180–184
Maziade M, Roy MA, Rouillard E et al (2001) A search for specific and common susceptibility
loci for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a linkage study in 13 target chromosomes. Mol
Psychiatry 6:684–693
McCarthy MI, Abecasis GR, Cardon LR et al (2008) Genome-wide association studies for
complex traits: consensus, uncertainty and challenges. Nat Rev Genet 9:356–369
McCarthy MJ, Leckband SG, Kelsoe JR (2010) Pharmacogenetics of lithium response in bipolar
disorder. Pharmacogenomics 11:1439–1465
McGrath M, Kawachi I, Ascherio A et al (2004) Association between catechol-O-methyltrans-
ferase and phobic anxiety. Am J Psychiatry 161:1703–1705
McGuffin P, Knight J, Breen G et al (2005) Whole genome linkage scan of recurrent depressive
disorder from the depression network study. Hum Mol Genet 14:3337–3345
McMahon FJ, Akula N, Schulze TG et al (2010) Meta-analysis of genome-wide association data
identifies a risk locus for major mood disorders on 3p21.1. Nat Genet 42:128–131
McQueen MB, Devlin B, Faraone SV et al (2005) Combined analysis from eleven linkage studies
of bipolar disorder provides strong evidence of susceptibility loci on chromosomes 6q and 8q.
Am J Hum Genet 77:582–595
Mendlewicz J, Rainer JD (1977) Adoption study supporting genetic transmission in manic–
depressive illness. Nature 268:327–329
Middeldorp CM, Cath DC, Van Dyck R et al (2005) The co-morbidity of anxiety and depression
in the perspective of genetic epidemiology. A review of twin and family studies. Psychol Med
35:611–624
Montag C, Buckholtz JW, Hartmann P et al (2008) COMT genetic variation affects fear
processing: psychophysiological evidence. Behav Neurosci 122:901–909
Mosing MA, Gordon SD, Medland SE et al (2009) Genetic and environmental influences on the
co-morbidity between depression, panic disorder, agoraphobia, and social phobia: a twin
study. Depress Anxiety 26:1004–1011
Moskvina V, Craddock N, Holmans P et al (2009) Gene-wide analyses of genome-wide
association data sets: evidence for multiple common risk alleles for schizophrenia and bipolar
disorder and for overlap in genetic risk. Mol Psychiatry 14:252–260
Mössner R, Freitag CM, Gutknecht L et al (2006) The novel brain-specific tryptophan
hydroxylase-2 gene in panic disorder. J Psychopharmacol 20:547–552
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 497
Moya PR, Murphy DL, McMahon FJ, Wendland JR (2010) Increased gene expression of
diacylglycerol kinase g in bipolar disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 13(8):1127–1128
Muglia P, Tozzi F, Galwey NW et al (2010) Genome-wide association study of recurrent major
depressive disorder in two European case-control cohorts. Mol Psychiatry 15:589–601
Muller DJ, Zai CC, Shinkai T et al (2011) Association between the DAOA/G72 gene and bipolar
disorder and meta-analyses in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Bipolar Disord 13:198–207
Nakamura K, Yamada K, Iwayama Y et al (2006) Evidence that variation in the peripheral
benzodiazepine receptor (PBR) gene influences susceptibility to panic disorder. Am J Med
Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 141B:222–226
Nelson JC (2008) Anxious depression and response to treatment. Am J Psychiatry 165:297–299
Nelson JC, Delucchi K, Schneider LS (2009) Anxiety does not predict response to antidepressant
treatment in late life depression: results of a meta-analysis. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 24:
539–544
Nyegaard M, Demontis D, Foldager L et al (2010) CACNA1C (rs1006737) is associated with
schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 15:119–121
O’Donovan MC, Craddock N, Norton N et al (2008) Identification of loci associated with
schizophrenia by genome-wide association and follow-up. Nat Genet 40:1053–1055
Ohara K, Nagai M, Suzuki Y et al (1998) Association between anxiety disorders and a functional
polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene. Psychiatry Res 81:277–279
Ollila HM, Soronen P, Silander K et al (2009) Findings from bipolar disorder genome-wide
association studies replicate in a Finnish bipolar family-cohort. Mol Psychiatry 14:351–353
Olsson M, Annerbrink K, Westberg L et al (2004) Angiotensin-related genes in patients with
panic disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 127:81–84
O’Reilly RL, Bogue L, Singh SM (1994) Pharmacogenetic response to antidepressants in a
multicase family with affective disorder. Biol Psychiatry 36:467–471
Otowa T, Tanii H, Sugaya N et al (2010) Replication of a genome-wide association study of
panic disorder in a Japanese population. J Hum Genet 55:91–96
Otowa T, Yoshida E, Sugaya N et al (2009) Genome-wide association study of panic disorder in
the Japanese population. J Hum Genet 54:122–126
Overall JE, Hollister LE, Johnson M et al (1966) Nosology of depression and differential response
to drugs. JAMA 195:946–948
Overall JE, Zisook S (1980) Diagnosis and the phenomenology of depressive disorders. J Consult
Clin Psychol 48:626–634
Papakostas GI, Stahl SM, Krishen A et al (2008) Efficacy of bupropion and the selective
serotonin reuptake inhibitors in the treatment of major depressive disorder with high levels of
anxiety (anxious depression): a pooled analysis of 10 studies. J Clin Psychiatry 69:1287–1292
Pare CM, Mack JW (1971) Differentiation of two genetically specific types of depression by the
response to antidepressant drugs. J Med Genet 8:306–309
Peerbooms OL, van Os J, Drukker M, Kenis G, Hoogveld L, MTHFR in Psychiatry Group,
de Hert M, Delespaul P, van Winkel R, Rutten BP (2011) Meta-analysis of MTHFR gene
variants in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and unipolar depressive disorder: evidence for a
common genetic vulnerability? Brain Behav Immun 25(8):1530–1543
Perlis RH, Huang J, Purcell S et al (2010) Genome-wide association study of suicide attempts in
mood disorder patients. Am J Psychiatry 167:1499–1507
Perlis RH, Smoller JW, Ferreira MA et al (2009) A genomewide association study of response to
lithium for prevention of recurrence in bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 166:718–725
Perna G, Favaron E, DiBella D et al (2005) Antipanic efficacy of paroxetine and polymorphism
within the promoter of the serotonin transporter gene. Neuropsychopharmacol 30:2230–2235
Perroud N, Uher R, Ng MY, Guipponi M, Hauser J, Henigsberg N, Maier W, Mors O, Gennarelli M,
Rietschel M, Souery D, Dernovsek MZ, Stamp AS, Lathrop M, Farmer A, Breen G,
Aitchison KJ, Lewis CM, Craig IW, McGuffin P (2010) Genome-wide association study of
increasing suicidal ideation during antidepressant treatment in the GENDEP project. Pharmac-
ogenomics J (in press)
498 K. Domschke and A. Reif
Pickard BS, Christoforou A, Thomson PA et al (2009) Interacting haplotypes at the NPAS3 locus
alter risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Mol Psychiatry 14:874–884
Poulton R, Andrews G, Millichamp J (2008) Gene-environment interaction and the anxiety
disorders. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 258:65–68
Purcell SM, Wray NR, Stone JL et al (2009) Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of
schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature 460:748–752
Ressler KJ, Bradley B, Mercer KB et al (2010) Polymorphisms in CRHR1 and the serotonin
transporter loci: gene x gene x environment interactions on depressive symptoms. Am J Med
Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 153B:812–824
Rietschel M, Mattheisen M, Frank J et al (2010) Genome-wide association-, replication-, and
neuroimaging study implicates HOMER1 in the etiology of major depression. Biol Psychiatry
68:578–585
Psychiatric GWAS Consortium (2009) A framework for interpreting genome-wide association
studies of psychiatric disorders. Mol Psychiatry 14:10–17
Rosenbaum JF, Biederman J, Hirshfield DR et al (1991) Further evidence of an association
between behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders: results from a family study of children
from a non-clinical sample. J Psychiatr Res 25:49–65
Rothe C, Gutknecht L, Freitag C et al (2004) Association of a functional 1019C[G 5-HT1A
receptor gene polymorphism with panic disorder with agoraphobia. Int J Neuropsychophar-
macol 7:189–192
Rowe DC, Stever C, Gard JM et al (1998) The relation of the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1)
to symptoms of internalizing disorders in children. Behav Genet 28:215–225
Roy MA, Neale MC, Pedersen NL et al (1995) A twin study of generalized anxiety disorder and
major depression. Psychol Med 25:1037–1049
Rudolph RL, Entsuah R, Chitra R (1998) A meta-analysis of the effects of venlafaxine on anxiety
associated with depression. J Clin Psychopharmacol 18:136–144
Samochowiec J, Hajduk A, Samochowiec A et al (2004) Association studies of MAO-A, COMT,
and 5-HTT genes polymorphisms in patients with anxiety disorders of the phobic spectrum.
Psychiatry Res 128:21–26
Sand P, Lesch KP, Catalano M et al (2000) Polymorphic MAO-A and 5-HT-transporter genes:
analysis of interactions in panic disorder. World J Biol Psychiatry 1:147–150
Sand PG, Mori T, Godau C et al (2002a) Norepinephrine transporter gene (NET) variants in
patients with panic disorder. Neurosci Lett 333:41–44
Sand PG, Schlurmann K, Luckhaus C et al (2002b) Estrogen receptor 1 gene (ESR1) variants in
panic disorder. Am J Med Genet 114:426–428
Schmidt NB, Storey J, Greenberg BD et al (2000) Evaluating gene x psychological risk factor
effects in the pathogenesis of anxiety: a new model approach. J Abnorm Psychol 109:308–320
Scholz CJ, Jacob CP, Buttenschon HN et al (2010) Functional variants of TSPAN8 are associated
with bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 153B(4):
967–972
Schruers K, Esquivel G, van Duinen M, Wichers M, Kenis G, Colasanti A, Knuts I, Goossens L,
Jacobs N, van Rozendaal J, Smeets H, van Os J, Griez E (2011) Genetic moderation of
CO2-induced fear by 5-HTTLPR genotype. J Psychopharmacol 25(1):37–42
Schulze TG, Alda M, Adli M et al (2010) The International Consortium on Lithium Genetics
(ConLiGen): an initiative by the NIMH and IGSLI to study the genetic basis of response to
lithium treatment. Neuropsychobiology 62:72–78
Schulze TG, Detera-Wadleigh SD, Akula N et al (2009) Two variants in Ankyrin 3 (ANK3) are
independent genetic risk factors for bipolar disorder. Mol Psychiatry 14:487–491
Schulze TG, McMahon FJ (2003) Genetic linkage and association studies in bipolar affective
disorder: a time for optimism. Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet 123C:36–47
Scott LJ, Muglia P, Kong XQ et al (2009) Genome-wide association and meta-analysis of bipolar
disorder in individuals of European ancestry. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:7501–7506
Segman RH, Shalev AY (2003) Genetics of posttraumatic stress disorder. CNS Spectr 8:693–698
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 499
Stein MB, Schork NJ, Gelernter J (2008) Gene-by-environment (serotonin transporter and
childhood maltreatment) interaction for anxiety sensitivity, an intermediate phenotype for
anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:312–319
Stein MB, Seedat S, Gelernter J (2006) Serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism
predicts SSRI response in generalized social anxiety disorder. Psychopharmacology (Berl)
187:68–72
Steinberg S, Mors O, Borglum AD et al (2011) Expanding the range of ZNF804A variants
conferring risk of psychosis. Mol Psychiatry 16:59–66
Strug LJ, Suresh R, Fyer AJ et al (2010) Panic disorder is associated with the serotonin
transporter gene (SLC6A4) but not the promoter region (5-HTTLPR). Mol Psychiatry
15:166–176
Sullivan PF (2010) The psychiatric GWAS consortium: big science comes to psychiatry. Neuron
68:182–186
Sullivan PF, de Geus EJ, Willemsen G et al (2009) Genome-wide association for major depressive
disorder: a possible role for the presynaptic protein piccolo. Mol Psychiatry 14:359–375
Sullivan PF, Neale MC, Kendler KS (2000) Genetic epidemiology of major depression: review
and meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 157:1552–1562
Tadic A, Rujescu D, Szegedi A et al (2003) Association of a MAOA gene variant with
generalized anxiety disorder, but not with panic disorder or major depression. Am J Med
Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 117:1–6
Takata A, Kawasaki H, Iwayama Y et al (2011) Nominal association between a polymorphism in
DGKH and bipolar disorder detected in a meta-analysis of East Asian case-control samples.
Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 65:280–285
Terracciano A, Tanaka T, Sutin AR et al (2010) Genome-wide association scan of trait
depression. Biol Psychiatry 68:811–817
Tesli M, Athanasiu L, Mattingsdal M et al (2010) Association analysis of PALB2 and BRCA2 in
bipolar disorder and schizophrenia in a scandinavian case-control sample. Am J Med Genet B
Neuropsychiatr Genet 153B:1276–1282
Tesli M, Kahler AK, Andreassen BK et al (2009) No association between DGKH and bipolar
disorder in a Scandinavian case-control sample. Psychiatr Genet 19:269–272
Thoeringer CK, Binder EB, Salyakina D et al (2007) Association of a Met88Val diazepam
binding inhibitor (DBI) gene polymorphism and anxiety disorders with panic attacks.
J Psychiatr Res 41:579–584
Thoeringer CK, Ripke S, Unschuld PG et al (2009) The GABA transporter 1 (SLC6A1): a novel
candidate gene for anxiety disorders. J Neural Transm 116:649–657
Thomas D (2010) Gene-environment-wide association studies: emerging approaches. Nat Rev
Genet 11:259–272
Thorgeirsson TE, Oskarsson H, Desnica N et al (2003) Anxiety with panic disorder linked to
chromosome 9q in Iceland. Am J Hum Genet 72:1221–1230
Tsuang M, Domschke K, Jerskey BA et al (2004) Agoraphobic behavior and panic attack: a study
of male twins. J Anxiety Disord 18:799–807
Uher R, McGuffin P (2008) The moderation by the serotonin transporter gene of environmental
adversity in the aetiology of mental illness: review and methodological analysis. Mol
Psychiatry 13:131–146
Uher R, McGuffin P (2010) The moderation by the serotonin transporter gene of environmental
adversity in the etiology of depression: 2009 update. Mol Psychiatry 15:18–22
Uher R, Perroud N, Ng MY et al (2010) Genome-wide pharmacogenetics of antidepressant
response in the GENDEP project. Am J Psychiatry 16:555–564
Unschuld PG, Ising M, Erhardt A et al (2007) Polymorphisms in the serotonin receptor gene
HTR2A are associated with quantitative traits in panic disorder. Am J Med Genet B
Neuropsychiatr Genet 144:424–429
Unschuld PG, Ising M, Erhardt A et al (2008) Polymorphisms in the galanin gene are associated
with symptom-severity in female patients suffering from panic disorder. J Affect Disord
105:177–184
Behavioral Genetics of Affective and Anxiety Disorders 501
Yevtushenko OO, Oros MM, Reynolds GP (2010) Early response to selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitors in panic disorder is associated with a functional 5-HT1A receptor gene polymorphism.
J Affect Disord 123:308–311
Yoon HK, Yang JC, Lee HJ et al (2008) The association between serotonin-related gene
polymorphisms and panic disorder. J Anxiety Disord 22:1529–1534
Zeng Z, Wang T, Li T et al (2011) Common SNPs and haplotypes in DGKH are associated with
bipolar disorder and schizophrenia in the Chinese Han population. Mol Psychiatry
16:473–475
Zhang D, Cheng L, Qian Y et al (2009a) Singleton deletions throughout the genome increase risk
of bipolar disorder. Mol Psychiatry 14:376–380
Zhang Z, Lindpaintner K, Che R et al (2009b) The Val/Met functional polymorphism in COMT
confers susceptibility to bipolar disorder: evidence from an association study and a meta-
analysis. J Neural Transm 116:1193–1200
Zhou K, Dempfle A, Arcos-Burgos M et al (2008) Meta-analysis of genome-wide linkage scans
of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet
147B:1392–1398
Zimmerman M, Chelminski I, McDermut W (2002) Major depressive disorder and axis I
diagnostic comorbidity. J Clin Psychiatry 63:187–193
Zintzaras E (2006) C677T and A1298C methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase gene polymorph-
isms in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and depression: a meta-analysis of genetic association
studies. Psychiatr Genet 16:105–115
Zintzaras E, Sakelaridis N (2007) Is 472G/A catechol-O-methyl-transferase gene polymorphism
related to panic disorder? Psychiatr Genet 17:267–273
Zobel A, Schuhmacher A, Jessen F et al (2010) DNA sequence variants of the FKBP5 gene are
associated with unipolar depression. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 13:649–660
Zou YF, Wang F, Feng XL, Li WF, Tian YH, Tao JH, Pan FM, Huang F (2010) Association of
DRD2 gene polymorphisms with mood disorders: a meta-analysis. J Affect Disord (in press)
Behavioural Genetics of the Serotonin
Transporter
however often the lack of reproducibility in different cohorts has led to debate on
the actual association of the polymorphisms with this extensive range of
neurological conditions. Here we review these two polymorphic VNTRs in depth
and relate that to pharmaceutical response, their ability to regulate differential
transporter expression, their core involvement in gene-environment interaction and
their genetic association with specific disorders.
Keywords Mood disorders Genetic polymorphism Serotonin Serotonin
transporter Variable number tandem repeat Gene regulation Monoaminergic
signaling
Contents
1 Introduction...................................................................................................................... 504
2 Investigating Polymorphic Variation .............................................................................. 506
3 Serotonin Transporter: Variable Number Tandem Repeats and Clinical
Association Studies. The Linked Polymorphic Region (5-HTT LPR) ......................... 507
4 The Serotonin Transporter Intron 2 VNTR (STin2 VNTR).......................................... 512
5 Pharmacogenetics of the 5-HTT Polymorphisms and Selective Serotonin
Reuptake Inhibitor (SSRI) Response.............................................................................. 515
6 Gene 9 Environment (G 9 E) Interaction Role in the SLC6A4 Polymorphism ........ 515
7 Haplotype Analysis of the SLC6A4 Polymorphisms, Gene 9 Gene (G 9 G)
and Gene 9 Gene 9 Environment (G 9 G 9 E) Interactions..................................... 516
8 Nuclear Imaging of SLC6A4 Polymorphic Variants in Affective Disorders ............... 517
9 Effects of 5-HTTLPR on Serotonin Signalling Systems (MRI) ................................... 518
10 Effects of 5HTTLPR on Serotonin Transporter Binding .............................................. 520
11 Effect of the Serotonin Transporter Polymorphisms on Functional Gene Expression... 522
12 Lessons from the 5-HTT Polymorphisms and Their Application to Other
Gene Polymorphisms....................................................................................................... 524
13 Summary.......................................................................................................................... 524
References................................................................................................................................ 526
1 Introduction
Fig. 1 Schematic representation of the 5-HTTLPR, STin2VNTR and rs25531/2 SNP in the
SLC6A4 gene
which a VNTR driving marker gene expression for example, b-galactosidase, may
be transiently expressed to identify tissues in which the VNTR may be active
in response to certain challenges or at particular developmental stages
(Mackenzie and Quinn 1999); (3) Formaldehyde assisted identification of regu-
latory elements (FAIRE) that identifies genome-wide open regions of chromatin
allowing assessment of changes in chromatin structure over polymorphic regions
in response to challenge (Esquivel et al. 2011); (4) DNA methylation that identifies
regions which are not accessible to regulation by transcription factors; (5) Nuclear
imaging including single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) and
positron emission tomography (PET) that allow resolution of events at a molecular
level (for example transporter/receptor binding potentials and density) and func-
tional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) that allows analysis of a whole system
activity and connectivity approach between brain regions (see Sect. 6).
Used in combination these techniques provide a powerful armoury for delin-
eating the functional mechanism of action of polymorphisms with the observed
clinical associations for such polymorphisms.
as 14 allelic variations of this locus. Most studies focus on the more common
biallelic 14 (s) and 16 (l) alleles of the LPR, however, since the discovery of a
functional SNP (rs25531) within the long variant of the LPR (an A to G substi-
tution) this is now referred to as the triallelic LPR variant. Inclusion of this SNP
(lg) has conferred clinical properties normally associated with the s-allele onto the
l-allele (Nakamura et al. 2000; Hu et al. 2005; Kraft et al. 2005; Wendland et al.
2006). However, no significant variation was observed between la and lg variants
in a study employing the use of AEI (Martin et al. 2007). The distribution of these
genotypes varies, with individuals Hm for the s-allele appearing at lower or similar
frequencies to those Hm for the l-allele in the Caucasian population (Heils et al.
1995, 1996). However, allelic frequencies vary among different ethnic groups.
The frequency of the alleles for the Caucasian population has been shown to be
27.4–28.3% (l/l), 43.4–51.0% (s/l) and 21.6–28.3% (s/s), whilst that of Asian
population has been shown to be 4.2–10.0% (l/l), 30.0–39.2% (s/l) and
55.6–60.0% (s/s) (Smits et al. 2004).
We have previously discussed the clinical and functional associations of this
variant with a number of affective disorders and behaviour traits (Table 1) and levels
of gene expression in vitro and in vivo (Table 2) (extensively reviewed in Haddley
et al. 2008). In general the l-allele of 5-HTTLPR has been associated with increased
levels of reporter gene activity, mRNA abundance and transporter binding. We will
now extrapolate on these associations using more recent data and meta-analyses.
Early and promising associations were made between 5-HTT polymorphisms
and a number of affective disorders with the Hm s-allele believed to be predisposing
to affective disorders. However, these initial findings were not always consistent and
even the result of meta-analysis found some of these association to be lacking in
power. More recent investigations have sought to clarify the specifics of these
associations including more detailed parameters such as age, population, environ-
ment and co-morbidities, as well as the mechanisms by which these variants may
mediate their effect. Recent meta-analysis found a significant association of the Hm
s-allele genotype with an increased risk of major depressive disorder (MDD) in
Caucasians but not in Asians (Kiyohara and Yoshimasu 2010). In agreement with
the latter, when studied in a Thai population no association could be made between
5-HTTLPR genotype and MDD (Tencomnao and Wongpiyabovorn 2010). A recent
investigation in a Northern Swedish population could not find any association of the
5-HTTLPR with bipolar depression (Alaerts et al. 2009).
Overall the picture remains unclear on the exact role of the LPR polymorphism
risk associations particularly in Asian populations, where the s-allele is present at a
higher frequency than the l-allele, but taking into account other factors, especially
gene x environment (G 9 E) interactions and treatment response, may go some
way to clarifying current understanding.
Homozygosity for the s-allele has conferred threefold higher odds of a patient
suffering from post-stroke depression (PSD) compared to those with one or more
l-allele (Kohen et al. 2008). Similarly, in a Chinese population, homozygosity for
the s-allele has been associated with a higher incidence of PSD (Fang et al. 2011).
The mechanism(s) by which these alleles may confer susceptibility to mood
Behavioural Genetics of the Serotonin Transporter 509
Table 1 A sample of previously reported clinical associations for the 5-HTT LPR polymorphism
Author Disorder Cohort/ethnicity Predisposing
genotype
5-HTTLPR genotype positively associated with affective disorder
Cervilla et al. (2006) Depression European (Spanish) s/s
Zalsman et al. (2006) Depression Caucasian (European) s/s
Collier et al. (1996a) Unipolar depression European s/s
Hauser et al. (2003) Unipolar depression European s/s
Collier et al. (1996a) Bipolar depression European s/s
Cho et al. (2005)a Bipolar depression Mixed s/s
Lotrich and Pollock Bipolar depression – s/s
(2004)a
Lasky-Su et al. (2005)a Bipolar depression Mixed s/s
Hauser et al. (2003) Bipolar depression European s/s
Lotrich and Pollock Major depressive – s/s
(2004)a disorder
Lesch et al. (1996) Anxiety Caucasian (German) s/s
Mazzanti et al. (1998) Anxiety European (Finnish) s/s
Katsuragi et al. (1999) Anxiety Japanese s/s
Du et al. (2000) Anxiety Caucasian s/s
(predominantly)
Melke et al. (2001) Anxiety Caucasian s/s
Feinn et al. (2005)a Substance abuse Mixed s
(alcohol)
Caspi et al. (2003) Suicide Caucasian (non-Maori) s/s
Bengel et al. (1999) OCD Caucasian l/l
Malhotra et al. (1998) Psychosis/ Mixed (N.American) l/l
hallucinations
Dubertret et al. (2005) Schizophrenia Mixed I
5-HTTLPR genotype not positively associated with affective disorder
Lasky-Su et al. (2005)a Unipolar depression Mixed
Mendlewicz et al. (2004) Unipolar depression European
Mansour et al. (2005) Bipolar depression Caucasian (N.America)
Meira-Lima et al. (2005) Bipolar depression Brazilian
Mendlewicz et al. (2004) Bipolar depression European
Ospina-Duque et al. Bipolar depression Columbian
(2000)
Arango et al. (2003) Major depressive –
disorder
Frisch et al. (1999) Major depressive European (Jewish)
disorder
Middledorp et al. (2007) Anxiety European (Dutch)
Deary et al. (1999) Anxiety British
Mendlewicz et al. (2004) Suicide European
Arango et al. (2003) Suicide –
Geijer et al. (2000) Suicide Caucasian (European)
Serretti et al. (2002) Major Psychosis European
Dickel et al. (2007) OCD Mixed
(continued)
510 K. Haddley et al.
Table 1 (continued)
Author Disorder Cohort/ethnicity Predisposing
genotype
Rao et al. (1998) Schizophrenia Caucasian (N.
American)
Rao et al. (1998) Schizophrenia African-American
Rao et al. (1998) Schizophrenia Caucasian (Swedish)
Where cohort states European or British, ethnicity was either mixed or not stated
a
meta-analysis
disorder (ADHD) in a Norwegian sample (Landaas et al. 2010). Other studies have
also shown a lack of association in schizophrenia, neurocognition or core psy-
chotic symptoms (Konneker et al. 2010), migraine in Europeans and Asians
(Schurks et al. 2010a) and bulima nervousa (Lee and Lin 2010). Recent associa-
tions have been made between the presence of an s-allele and insomnia (Deuschle
et al. 2010) and anorexia nervosa (Lee and Lin 2010).
Table 4 A sample of previously reported functional effects for the STin2VNTR polymorphism
Author Model Experimental detail Gene expression
bias
Fiskerstrand et al. (1999) Embryonic stem cells VNTR in isolation cloned into a reporter vector showed 12 [ 10
responses following differentiation
Lovejoy et al. (2003) Murine embryonic stem cells Trimeric oligonucleotides composed of individual repeat
elements cloned into reporter vector showed varied
responses following differentiation.
Lovejoy et al. (2003) JAr human clonal cell line VNTR in isolation 9 [ 12 [ 10
Klenova et al. (2004) Monkey Cos 7 clonal cells Human Differential expression and regulation by transcription factors 12[10[9 9 [ 10 [
HEK293 clonal cells CTCFand YB1 12
MacKenzie and Quinn Transient transgenic mice Differential expression driven by the human StlN2VNTR 10 or
(1999) 12 variant supporting LacZ expression
Hranilovic et al. (2003) Lymphoblasts Allelic expression imbalance/semi-quantitative real time PCR 12/12 [10
Roberts et al. (2007) Stably transfected human JAr clonal cell Differential binding of CTCF and YB-1 transcription factors to
line. Human HEK293T clonal cells the allelic variants in response to lithium chloride
Kaiser et al. (2002) Human blood platelets The maximum rate of 5-HT uptake by platelets was unaffected 12/12 \ 9/10
although the affinity of uptake varied as a function of
genotype
K. Haddley et al.
Behavioural Genetics of the Serotonin Transporter 515
example SLC6A4 Knockout (KO) mice displayed more fearful behaviour compared
to wild-type (WT) only in response to stress (Murphy et al. 2001; Wellman et al.
2007). The potential interaction between the 5-HTTLPR and early experiences was
demonstrated in non-human primates; rhesus macaques with an s-variant analogous
to that of human s-allele, displayed decreased serotonergic function but only in
animals reared under stressful conditions (Bennett et al. 2002). Furthermore, peer-
reared females with the s-variant were more vulnerable to alcoholism and showed
more aggressive behaviour whereas those reared by their mothers were not differ-
entiated by genotype (Champoux et al. 2002; Barr et al. 2003, 2004). Furthermore, in
agreement with a seminal study by Caspi et al. demonstrating a greater risk of
depression and suicide in those individuals Hm for the s-allele dependant on the
number of past stressful life events and childhood maltreatment, it has been found
that individuals in a Spanish population Hm for the s-allele acquired a higher risk of
depression after considerably fewer stressful life episodes (Caspi et al. 2003; Cervilla
et al. 2007). Moreover, in an adult twin study, individuals Hm for the s-allele dis-
played increased sensitivity of depressogenic effects of stressful-life events than
those with one copy of the l-allele (Kendler et al. 2005).
Recently, a potential role for the 5-HTTLPR polymorphisms has been inves-
tigated in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) as a function of a gene 9 envi-
ronment interaction. Having one or more copies of the s-allele predisposed
individuals who had suffered both childhood adversity and adult traumatic events
to develop lifetime PTSD (Xie et al. 2009). A similar effect has also been seen in
patients with a copy of the la-allele with more than 60% of carriers developing
PTSD following three or more stressful life events (Grabe et al. 2009). At first,-
these results would seem conflicting, however, other environmental factors may
need to be considered, for example the s-allele has been associated with decreased
risk of PTSD in low-risk environments (low crime/unemployment rates) but
increased risk of PTSD in high-risk environments (Koenen et al. 2009).
These studies emphasise the significant impact of the environment and early
experiences on genetic polymorphisms and suggest that with a particular genotype
an individual may be more vulnerable to environmental stress and have higher
tendency to develop psychiatric disorders, depending on their genotype. The
interaction between SLC6A4 polymorphisms and stressful life events is striking
and one that has stood up to scrutiny particularly in longitudinal studies (reviewed
in Uher and McGuffin 2008).
Greater associations have been made between genotype and phenotype now
that focus has begun to shift towards investigating polymorphisms in gene-gene
interactions or as haplotypes rather than as standalone entities. According to the
Behavioural Genetics of the Serotonin Transporter 517
international HapMap project the SLC6A4 gene exists as two haplotype blocks.
A recent study in a Chinese Han population found that when assessed alone no
association could be made between the STin2VNTR and schizophrenia, however,
significant association was found when the locus was studied as a haplotype in
conjunction with a number of tagging SNPs, identifying two haplotypes with positive
association (Lin et al. 2009). Similar results were also found in a North European
population, in that schizophrenia was associated with a haplotype block but not with
single loci (Zaboli et al. 2008). Furthermore, haplotype analysis has revealed that
having the l-allele and the STin2VNTR 10-allele predisposes Russian females, but
not males to suicide (Gaysina et al. 2006). In a Turkish population predisposition
towards suicidal behaviour was not observed for either the 5-HTTLPR or
STin2VNTR polymorphisms, however when combined it was found that those with
an s/10 or l/12 genotype were predisposed to suicidal behaviour (Akar et al. 2010).
Moreover, in a study of German alcoholics the sa/10 and lg/12 haplotypes were more
prevalent in type 2 alcoholics compared with controls (Reese et al. 2010).
Polymorphic interactions are not limited to those in-cis and evidence is emerging
for in-trans interaction between genes as predisposing factors for disease. A recent
investigation by Lorenzi et al. (2010) found that in an Italian sample of patients with
Alzhiemers linked dementia that s-allele homozygosity was particularly abundant and
the 5-HTTLPR synergistically interacted with a polymorphism in the saitohin gene
(Q7R) to increase susceptibility. In addition, interactions between the Hm s-allele
5-HTTLPR genotype and polymorphisms within the cannabinoid receptor 1 locus
have been associated with increased anxiety scores in Caucasian Hungarians (Lazary
et al. 2009). Finally, Armbruster et al. found a significant interaction between the
5-HTTLPR and DRD4 polymorphisms in response to stress in a German population
with individuals Hm for the 5-HTTLPR la-allele and one copy of the DRD4 7R allele
having a lower cortisol response compared to other genotypes (Armbruster et al. 2009).
In addition to two-way interactions many studies have identified three-way
interactions, so called G 9 G 9 E interactions. Ressler et al. conducted an analysis
of SLC6A4 interaction with corticotropin-releasing hormone haplotypes as a
function of child abuse in an African-American cohort. They found that the
5-HTTLPR s-allele interacted with CRHR1 haplotypes and child abuse to predict
current depressive symptoms (Ressler et al. 2010). Similarly, Conway et al. (2010)
found that in individuals Hm for the catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) val158
mutation, a Hm 5-HTTLPR l-allele genotype exerted a protective effect in
adolescents in response to stress-related depression.
Over the last decade advances in in vivo imaging techniques have enabled further
investigation into the in vitro and genome-wide associations made between
polymorphic variation, changes in brain activity and affective disorders, in the
518 K. Haddley et al.
hope of clarifying not only whether these associations are accurate but also in the
hope of determining biological endophenotypes associated with genotype. Such
investigations have included examining differences in brain structure, brain
metabolism, transporter and receptor-ligand binding and the signaling interactions
between brain regions.
The three most widely used techniques in brain imaging for affective disorders
are SPECT, positron emission tomography (PET) and fMRI. MRI is used to give a
whole system overview of brain activity, whereas SPECT and PET provide res-
olution of events at a molecular level allowing the investigation of binding of a
radiolabelled ligand to its target receptor or transporter. MRI is rapid but gives low
resolution and is generally used as a functional measure of activity. Both SPECT
and PET are used to determine binding potential, target density or distribution, and
volume ratios (Innis et al. 2007). The most commonly used ligands for assessing
binding potentials and distribution for the monoamine transporters are summarised
in Table 5. Many studies concentrating on affective disorders and polymorphisms
using these techniques analyse multiple brain regions, whereas others focus on
specific regions for example the midbrain or striatum which are both rich in
serotonin or the amygdala and limbic systems which are central regions involved
in the control of mood, anxiety and emotion. For this reason and in combination
with the different techniques available there is still conflict over the role of
polymorphisms and their associations however a clearer picture is emerging. The
results of these investigations, with a focus on the serotonin LPR are discussed
below.
Functional MRI studies have been undertaken to determine the effects of the
5-HTTLPR polymorphism on brain organisation and structure in brain regions
associated with affective disorders. Healthy individuals with an s or lg-allele
displayed reduced grey matter volumes, when compared to those with a la-allele,
suggesting that the polymorphism plays a role in development of brain structure.
Interestingly, the same study found that patients with depression suffered from
reduced bilateral hippocampal volumes and this was particularly pronounced in
those who were Hm for the la-allele, compared to those who were lg/lg, lg/s or s/s
suggesting that individuals Hm for the la-allele are more susceptible to morpho-
logical changes during depressive episodes (Frodl et al. 2004, 2008a, b). A further
study assessed hippocampal volumes in an elderly cohort in a similar manner but
on the basis of the age of onset of depression. This study determined that those
individuals Hm for the l-allele and with late-onset depression had significantly
smaller hippocampal volumes than those with early-onset depression or healthy
controls. In addition they found that in individuals Hm for the s-allele early onset
of depression was associated with smaller hippocampal volumes suggesting that
genotype and age interact to affect the timing of depression (Taylor et al. 2005).
Tracers used in nuclear imaging studies of the 5-HTT
Ligand A.K.A Selectivity Application
[123l](-)-2b-Carbomethoxy-3b-(4-iodophenyl) [123I]b-CIT Cocaine analogue DAT1 SPECT
tropane 5-HTT
NET
[l-123]-N-omega-fluoropropyl-2beta -carbomethoxy- [1231] FP-CIT [123I]-b-CIT analogue DAT1 SPECT
3beta -(4-iodophenyl)nortropane 5-HTT
NET
Behavioural Genetics of the Serotonin Transporter
Functional MRI has been used by both Hariri et al. and Heinz et al. in healthy
individuals to demonstrate that those with at least one s-allele where found to have
stronger amygdala reactivity and stronger amygdala-prefrontal cortex coupling in
response to aversive pictures or uncertain contexts, which could be considered
stressful (Hariri et al. 2006; Heinz et al. 2005, 2007). Furthermore, in individuals
exposed to fearful faces those Hm for the s-allele showed greater activity in the
right fusiform gyrus (FG), and greater positive functional connectivity between
right amygdala and FG and between right FG and right ventrolateral prefrontal
cortex, compared to those with other genotypes (Surguladze et al. 2008). These
replications are a good indicator that the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism can contribute
to differential hyper-responsiveness of the amygdala, the control centre for emo-
tions, and go some way towards defining a mechanism for the clinical and in vitro
observations that the s-allele is associated with a predisposition to anxiety-
depression spectrum disorders. It has also been suggested that these findings may
also be key in other psychiatric disorders because those Hm for the l-allele dis-
played what could be considered ‘‘hypo-responsiveness’’ a trait that may be
important where a lack of emotion is a key factor e.g., psychopathy (Surguladze
et al. 2008; reviewed in Glenn 2011). The effect of 5-HTTLPR genotype on
connectiveness between the ventral anterior cingulate cortex and the amygdala has
also been examined as this pathway is hyporesponsive in patients with bipolar
disorder. When patients with bipolar depression or healthy controls were presented
with happy and fearful facial imagery those with bipolar disorder showed
decreased activity in this pathway as expected. Furthermore, in healthy individuals
and in those with bipolar depression Hm for the s-allele, this pathway was found to
be hypoactive compared to those with an l-allele (Shah et al. 2009). Taken together
these data strongly suggest an effect of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism, in particular
the s-allele, on amygdala responsiveness and connectivity although the molecular
mechanisms behind these differences cannot be delineated using MRI.
The regulation of expression of a given gene can vary between individuals because
of epigenetics and polymorphic variation in regulatory domains where for example,
transcription factors may bind. Many studies have shown that cis-regulatory loci
(regulatory polymorphisms) in promoters and other non-coding regions of a gene, in
addition to transcription factors (trans-acting modulators) regulate allele-specific
expression (Heils et al. 1996; Yan 2002; Bray et al. 2003; Lo et al. 2003; Klenova
et al. 2004; Roberts et al. 2007). In the absence of these cis-regulatory domains,
paternal and maternal alleles of a gene are equally expressed, unless one allele is
imprinted. However, when an individual is heterozygous for a cis-regulatory
domain, mRNA expression level may vary from each allele; termed differential
allelic gene expression. Different combinations of these polymorphisms within our
genome create the ‘genetic fingerprint’ that contributes to determining phenotypic
diversity and leads in part to individuality amongst us. Nearly three decades ago,
King and Wilson pointed out that changes in the mechanisms controlling the gene
expression, rather than the DNA sequence itself account for the morphological,
behavioural and cognitive differences between human beings and other primates
(King and Wilson 1975). Therefore, it is suggested that the person we are is not
solely determined by our genes but how we control their expression, hence drugs
such as cocaine vary our behaviour in part by modulating gene expression.
We and others have previously demonstrated the transcriptional capacity of the
VNTRs to support tissue-specific and stimulus inducible gene expression (Table 4)
(reviewed in Haddley et al. 2008). We further extended our studies to investigate
the possible combinatorial interaction of these polymorphisms with one another in
regulating gene expression and recently demonstrated that the 5-HTTLPR and
STin2VNTR can act in concert, in the context of a transient or stable reporter gene
assay in human 5-HTT-expressing JAr cells and primary rodent neurons, to
demonstrate differential reporter gene expression which was not a simplistic
additive effect of the individual domains (Ali et al. 2010a).
Behavioural Genetics of the Serotonin Transporter 523
correlated with an increase in the association of positive histone marks and RNA
polymerase II binding within the SLC6A4 promoter. Similarly, we have also
demonstrated that the mood stabiliser lithium chloride alters the binding of CTCF to
the STin2VNTR dependent on genotype (Roberts et al. 2007). These observations
suggest a potent mechanism by which genotype can effect gene regulation and
indirectly the level of 5-HTT protein present by altering the transcription factor
complexes bound by allelic variants under basal conditions and in response to stimuli
and may provide further insight into the mechanism(s) behind genotypic influence on
susceptibility to affective disorders.
We have focused in this chapter on two of the many polymorphisms present in the
SLC6A4 gene there are many others including SNPs and other mini-satellites present
and more are being identified as deep sequencing of the human genome takes place.
All genes contain what could be considered functional SNPs and these are not only
present in protein coding regions. SLC6A4 is obviously not the only gene involved in
the plethora of psychosis and affective disorders and many other genes contain
functional and disorder-associated polymorphisms. For example, the widely studied
DAT1 (SLC6A3) gene from the same family as the 5-HTT contains VNTRs in non-
coding regions that have been associated with a range of disorders including ADHD
and drug addiction (Guindalini et al. 2006; Yang et al. 2007). The SLC6A3 poly-
morphisms have thus been investigated with as much vigour as the SLC6A4 and the
conclusions of such studies have been similar with association both negatively and
positively correlated with disorders, conflicting imaging studies and differential
frequency distribution of alleles in different populations. Nevertheless, molecular
models have shown that these polymorphisms support differential gene expression
and the lessons that can be learnt from studies with one group of polymorphisms can
be applied to other polymorphisms for example the SLC6A4 and SLC6A3 VNTRs
share similar transcription factor binding sites within their sequences implying that
they may be functional on similar pathways and in response to similar challenges
such as stress. The same can be said for many other VNTRs within other neuronal
genes such as those in the dopamine R4 receptor (DRD4), monoamine oxidase A
(MAOA), neuronal restrictive silencer factor (NRSF) and N-methyl-D-aspartic acid
(NMDA) glutamate receptor (GRIN1).
13 Summary
After almost two decades of research the importance of the link between individual
genetic polymorphisms as standalone agents and their relation to disease suscepti-
bility remains somewhat controversial and tenuous for many polymorphisms. Initial
Behavioural Genetics of the Serotonin Transporter 525
twin studies pointed towards a genetic component for many psychiatric disorders
making the polymorphisms a likely candidate. Early indications linking the serotonin
transporter polymorphisms to a range of psychiatric disorders including depression,
OCD, anxiety, schizophrenia and treatment response while at first positive have
fallen under recent scrutiny in the light of many negative associations. Amongst most
researchers, however, there is little doubt that these functional polymorphisms do
have a role to play in the aetiology of psychiatric disorders but the key to unlocking
these roles is more complex than a simple ‘one polymorphism equals one disorder
model’. It has been apparent for a long time that the same polymorphism occurs
at different frequencies within distinct populations and that an ‘at risk’ allele in a
Caucasian population of European descent will not be an at risk allele for the same
disorder in, for example, a Chinese Han population. This points to a more complex
network of other factors being involved and recent investigations have begun to
expand on this idea looking not only for single polymorphism associations but
examining haplotype associations where one polymorphism may only exert an effect
in the presence of a distinct polymorphism in the same or another gene, as has been
shown on many occasions for the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism (Miyajima et al.
2008; Wells et al. 2010; Hiio et al. 2011). In addition to haplotype analysis it is also
apparent that G 9 G 9 E interactions play as big a role as the polymorphisms
themselves. A polymorphism can be a marker or predictor for susceptibility but its
effect may only become apparent under certain circumstances; it may only have an
adverse (or in some cases positive) effect within a specific context for example
following stressful-life events (Caspi et al. 2003).
Advances in sequencing and array technologies are allowing rapid and cost
effective analysis of the thousands of human polymorphisms that exist allowing
large scale mapping projects worldwide in different populations such as the 1,000
man genome project and the international HAPMAP project. Genome wide
association studies are being supported by functional genome wide studies such as
ChIP-Geneseq and FAIRE which expand current knowledge on how polymor-
phisms may regulate gene expression at a molecular level by interaction with
transcription factors and by epigenetic remodelling. Perhaps most useful in human
functional studies are the advances made in nuclear medicine. Imagining is cer-
tainly an effective tool for assessing genotype effects on binding and possible
function although like all methods compounding factors must also be considered
and not just those inherent in the experimental design e.g., tracer sensitivity or
brain region under study. Environmental effects on endogenous transporter
expression must also be taken into account for example, underlying pathologies,
seasonal changes, age and lifestyle, as these may mask any underlying mecha-
nisms apparent due to polymorphic variation. A large proportion of imaging
studies failed to detect any effect of a polymorphism on transporter function, not
solely in the serotonin transporter but also in other monoaminergic transporters
such as the dopamine transporter. This could be due to the heterogeneity of such
studies, as of yet there are still no clear parameters in place that make comparing
studies wholly effective. Studies have, however, found differences in brain region
volumes, connectivity and activity and it has been suggested by Keers and
526 K. Haddley et al.
References
Dubertret C, Hanoun N et al (2005) Family-based association study of the 5-HT transporter gene
and schizophrenia. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 8(1):87–92
Esquivel UP, Haddley K et al (2011) Finding gene expression regulators implicated in major
depression using formaldehyde assisted isolation of regulatory elements (FAIRE). Neuro-
psychopharmacology 21:S28–S29, Abstract
Evans J, Battersby S et al (1997) Association of short alleles of a VNTR of the serotonin
transporter gene with anxiety symptoms in patients presenting after deliberate self harm.
Neuropharmacology 36(4–5):439–443
Fang J, Yan WH et al (2011) Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism in Chinese patients with
poststroke depression a case-control study. Stroke 42(5):1461–1463
Feinn R, Nellissery M et al (2005) Meta-analysis of the association of a functional serotonin
transporter promoter polymorphism with alcohol dependence. Am J Med Genet B
Neuropsychiatr Genet 133B(1):79–84
Fiskerstrand CE, Lovejoy EA et al (1999) An intronic polymorphic domain often associated with
susceptibility to affective disorders has allele dependent differential enhancer activity in
embryonic stem cells. FEBS Lett 458(2):171–174
Frisch A, Postilnick D et al (1999) Association of unipolar major depressive disorder with genes
of the serotonergic and dopaminergic pathways. Mol Psychiatry 4(4):389–392
Frodl T, Meisenzahl EM et al (2004) Reduced hippocampal volumes associated with the long
variant of the serotonin transporter polymorphism in major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry
61(2):177–183
Frodl T, Koutsouleris N et al (2008a) Reduced gray matter brain volumes are associated with variants
of the serotonin transporter gene in major depression. Mol Psychiatry 13(12):1093–1101
Frodl T, Zill P et al (2008b) Reduced hippocampal volumes associated with the long variant of
the tri- and diallelic serotonin transporter polymorphism in major depression. Am J Med
Genet B Neuropsychiatr Gene 147B(7):1003–1007
Furlong RA, Ho L et al (1998) Analysis and meta-analysis of two serotonin transporter gene
polymorphisms in bipolar and unipolar affective disorders. Am J Med Genet 81(1):58–63
Gaysina D, Zainullina A et al (2006) The serotonin transporter gene: polymorphism and
haplotype analysis in Russian suicide attempters. Neuropsychobiology 54(1):70–74
Geijer T, Frisch A et al (2000) Search for association between suicide attempt and serotonergic
polymorphisms. Psychiatr Genet 10(1):19–26
Gillihan SJ, Rao H et al (2010) Serotonin transporter genotype modulates amygdala activity
during mood regulation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 5(1):1–10
Glenn AL (2011) The other allele: exploring the long allele of the serotonin transporter gene as a
potential risk factor for psychopathy: a review of the parallels in findings. Neurosci Biobehav
Rev 35(3):612–620
Grabe HJ, Spitzer C et al (2009) Serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) promoter polymorphisms
and the susceptibility to posttraumatic stress disorder in the general population. Am J
Psychiatry 166(8):926–933
Graff-Guerrero A, De La Fuente-Sandoval C et al (2005) Frontal and limbic metabolic differences
in subjects selected according to genetic variation of the SLC6A4 gene polymorphism.
Neuroimage 25(4):1197–1204
Greenberg BD, Tolliver TJ et al (1999) Genetic variation in the serotonin transporter promoter
region affects serotonin uptake in human blood platelets. Am J Med Gent 88(1):83–87
Guindalini C, Howard M et al (2006) A dopamine transporter gene functional variant
associated with cocaine abuse in a Brazilian sample. Proc Natl Acad Sci (USA) 103(12):
4552–4557
Gutierrez B, Arranz MJ et al (1998a) Serotonin transporter gene and risk for bipolar affective
disorder: an association study in Spanish population. Biol Psychiatry 43(11):843–847
Gutierrez B, Pintor L et al (1998b) Variability in the serotonin transporter gene and increased risk
for major depression with melancholia. Hum Genet 103(3):319–322
Haddley K, Vasiliou AS et al (2008) Molecular genetics of monoamine transporters: relevance to
brain disorders. Neurochem Res 33(4):652–667
Behavioural Genetics of the Serotonin Transporter 529
Hall FS, Sora I et al (2004) Molecular mechanisms underlying the rewarding effects of cocaine.
Ann NY Natl Acad Sci 1025:47–56
Ham BJ, Lee MS et al (2005) No association between the tryptophan hydroxylase gene
polymorphism and major depressive disorders and antidepressant response in a Korean
population. Psychiatr Genet 15:299–301
Hanna GL, Himle JA et al (1998) Serotonin transporter and seasonal variation in blood serotonin
in families with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 18(2):102–111
Hariri AR, Mattay VS et al (2002) Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the
human amygdala. Science 297(5580):400–403
Hariri AR, Drabant EM et al (2006) Imaging genetics: perspectives from studies of genetically
driven variation in serotonin function and corticolimbic affective processing. Biol Psychiatry
59(10):888–897
Hauser J, Leszczynska A et al (2003) Association analysis of the insertion/deletion polymorphism
in serotonin transporter gene in patients with affective disorder. Eur Psychiatry 18(3):129–132
Heils A, Teufel A et al (1995) Functional promoter and polyadenylation site mapping of the
human serotonin (5-HT) transporter gene. J Neural Transm Gen Sect 102(3):247–254
Heils A, Teufel A et al (1996) Allelic variation of human serotonin transporter gene expression.
J Neurochem 66(6):2621–2624
Heinz A, Jones DW et al (2000) A relationship between serotonin transporter genotype and in
vivo protein expression and alcohol neurotoxicity. Biol Psychiatry 47(7):643–649
Heinz A, Braus DF et al (2005) Amygdala-prefrontal coupling depends on a genetic variation of
the serotonin transporter. Nat Neurosci 8(1):20–21
Heinz A, Smolka MN et al (2007) Serotonin transporter genotype (5-HTTLPR): effects of neutral
and undefined conditions on amygdala activation. Biol Psychiatry 61(8):1011–1014
Hiio K, Merenakk L et al (2011) Effects of serotonin transporter promoter and BDNF Val66Met
genotype on personality traits in a population representative sample of adolescents. Psychiatr
Genet 21(5):261–264
Hoehe MR, Wendel B et al (1998) Serotonin transporter (5-HTT) gene polymorphisms are not
associated with susceptibility to mood disorders. Am J Med Genet 81(1):1–3
Hranilovic D, Schwab SG et al (2000) Serotonin transporter gene and schizophrenia: evidence for
association/linkage disequilibrium in families with affected siblings. Mol Psychiatry 5(1):
91–95
Hranilovic D, Stefulj J et al (2003) Serotonin transporter gene promoter (5-HTTLPR) and intron 2
(VNTR) polymorphisms in Croatian suicide victims. Biol Psychiatry 54(9):884–889
Hranilovic D, Stefulj J et al (2004) Serotonin transporter promoter and intron 2 polymorphisms:
relationship between allelic variants and gene expression. Biol Psychiatry 55(11):1090–1094
Hu X, Oroszi G, Chun J et al (2005) An expanded evaluation of the relationship of four alleles to
the level of response to alcohol and the alcoholism risk. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 29:8–16
Hu XZ, Rush AJ, Charney D et al (2007) Association between a functional serotonin transporter
promoter polymorphism and citalopram treatment in adult outpatients with major depression.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 64(7):783–792
Innis RB, Cunningham VJ et al (2007) Consensus nomenclature for in vivo imaging of reversibly
binding radioligands. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 27(9):1533–1539
Ishihara K, Oshimura M et al (2006) CTCF-dependent chromatin insulator is linked to epigenetic
remodeling. Mol Cell 23(5):733–742
Ito K, Yoshida K et al (2002) A variable number of tandem repeats in the serotonin transporter
gene does not affect the antidepressant response to fluvoxamine. Psychiatry Res 111:235–239
Jacobsen LK, Staley JK et al (2000) Elevated central serotonin transporter binding availability in
acutely abstinent cocaine-dependent patients. Am J Psychiatry 157(7):1134–1140
Jeffreys AJ, Wilson V et al (1985) Hypervariable ‘minisatellite’ regions in human DNA. Nature
314(6006):67–73
Jernej B, Stefulj J et al (2004) Intronic polymorphism of tryptophan hydroxylase and serotonin
transporter: indication for combined effect in predisposition to suicide. J Neural Transm
111(6):733–738
530 K. Haddley et al.
Joshi G, Pradhan S et al (2010) No direct association of serotonin transporter (STin2 VNTR) and
receptor (HT 102T[C) gene variants in genetic susceptibility to migraine. Dis Markers 29(5):
223–229
Kaiser R, Tremblay PB et al (2001) Serotonin transporter polymorphisms: no association with
response to antipsychotic treatment, but associations with the schizoparanoid and residual
subtypes of schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 6(2):179–185
Kaiser R, Muller-Oerlinghausen B et al (2002) Correlation between serotonin uptake in human
blood platelets with the 44-bp polymorphism and the 17-bp variable number of tandem repeat
of the serotonin transporter. Am J Med Genet 114(3):323–328
Kalbitzer J, Frokjaer VG et al (2009) The personality trait openness is related to cerebral 5-HTT
levels. Neuroimage 45(2):280–285
Kalin NH, Shelton SE et al (2008) The serotonin transporter genotype is associated with
intermediate brain phenotypes that depend on the context of eliciting stressor. Mol Psychiatry
13(11):1021–1027
Katsuragi S, Kunugi H et al (1999) Association between serotonin transporter gene polymor-
phism and anxiety-related traits. Biol Psychiatry 45(3):368–370
Keers R, Aitchison KJ (2010) Gender differences in antidepressant drug response. Int Rev
Psychiatry 22(5):485–500
Keers R, Aitchison KJ (2011) Pharmacogenetics of antidepressant response. Expert Rev
Neurother 11(1):101–125
Keers R, Uher R et al (2011) Interaction between serotonin transporter gene variants and life
events predicts response to antidepressants in the GENDEP project. Pharmacogenomics J
11(2):138–145
Kendler KS, Kuhn JW et al (2005) The interaction of stressful life events and a serotonin
transporter polymorphism in the prediction of episodes of major depression: a replication.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 62(5):529–535
Kim DK, Lim SW et al (2000) Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism and antidepressant
response. Neuroreport 11:215–219
King MC, Wilson AC (1975) Evolution at two levels in humans and chimpanzees. Science
188(4184):107–116
Kinnally EL, Capitanio JP et al (2010) Epigenetic regulation of serotonin transporter expression
and behavior in infant rhesus macaques. Genes, Brain Behav 9(6):575–582
Kirov G, Rees M et al (1999) Bipolar disorder and the serotonin transporter gene: a family-based
association study. Psychol Med 29(5):1249–1254
Kiyohara C, Yoshimasu K (2010) Association between major depressive disorder and a
functional polymorphism of the 5-hydroxytryptamine (serotonin) transporter gene: a meta-
analysis. Psychiatr Genet 20(2):49–58
Klenova E, Scott AC et al (2004) YB-1 and CTCF differentially regulate the 5-HTT polymorphic
intron 2 enhancer which predisposes to a variety of neurological disorders. J Neurosci
24(26):5966–5973
Koenen KC, Aiello AE et al (2009) Modification of the association between serotonin transporter
genotype and risk of posttraumatic stress disorder in adults by county-level social
environment. Am J Epidemiol 169(6):704–711
Kohen R, Cain KC et al (2008) Association of serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms with
poststroke depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65(11):1296–1302
Konneker TI, Crowley JJ et al (2010) No association of the serotonin transporter polymorphisms
5-HTTLPR and RS25531 with schizophrenia or neurocognition. Am J Med Genet B
Neuropsychiatr Genet 153B(5):1115–1117
Kraft JB, Slager SL et al (2005) Sequence analysis of the serotonin transporter and associations
with antidepressant response. Biol Psychiatry 58(5):374–381
Kreek MJ, Bart G et al (2005a) Pharmacogenetics and human molecular genetics of opiate and
cocaine addictions and their treatments. Pharmacol Rev 57(1):1–26
Kreek MJ, Nielsen DA et al (2005b) Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress
responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nat Neurosci 8(11):1450–1457
Behavioural Genetics of the Serotonin Transporter 531
Mann JJ, Huang YY et al (2000) A serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR)
and prefrontal cortical binding in major depression and suicide. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57(8):
729–738
Mansour HE, Talkowski ME et al (2005) Serotonin gene polymorphisms and bipolar I disorder:
focus on the serotonin transporter. Ann Med 37(8):590–602
Martin J, Cleak J et al (2007) Mapping regulatory variants for the serotonin transporter gene
based on allelic expression imbalance. Mol Psychiatry 12(5):421–422
Martinez D, Slifstein MR et al (2009) Positron emission tomography imaging of the serotonin
transporter and 5-HT1A receptor in alcohol dependence. Biol Psychiatry 65(2):175–180
Maze I, Covington JH 3rd et al (2010) Essential role of the histone methyltransferase G9a in
cocaine-induced plasticity. Science 327(5962):213–216
Mazzanti CM, Lappalainen J et al (1998) Role of the serotonin transporter promoter
polymorphism in anxiety-related traits. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55(10):936–940
McHugh RK, Hofmann SJ et al (2010) The serotonin transporter gene and risk for alcohol
dependence: a meta-analytic review. Drug Alcohol Depend 108(1–2):1–6
McLaughlin DP, Little KY et al (1996) Expression of serotonin transporter mRNA in human
brainstem raphe nuclei. Neuropsychopharmacology 15(5):523–529
Meira-Lima I, Michelon L et al (2005) Allelic association analysis of the functional insertion/
deletion polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene in bipolar
affective disorder. J Mol Med 27(2):219–224
Melke J, Landen M et al (2001) Serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms are associated with
anxiety-related personality traits in women. Am J Med Genet 106(1):458–463
Mendlewicz J, Massat I et al (2004) Serotonin transporter 5HTTLPR polymorphism and affective
disorders: no evidence of association in a large European multicenter study. Eur J Hum Genet
12(5):377–382
Merenakk L, Maestu J et al (2011) Effects of the serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) and a(2A)-
adrenoceptor (C-1291G) genotypes on substance use in children and adolescents: a
longitudinal study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 215(1):13–22
Middledorp CM, de Geus EJC et al (2007) Family based association analyses between the serotonin
transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and neuroticism, anxiety and depression. Behav
Genet 37(2):294–301
Mileva-Seitz V, Kennedy J et al (2011) Serotonin transporter allelic variation in mothers predicts
maternal sensitivity, behavior and attitudes toward 6-month-old infants. Genes Brain Behav
10(3):325–333
Min W, Li T et al (2009) Monoamine transporter gene polymorphisms affect susceptibility to
depression and predict antidepressant response. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 205:409–417
Miyajima F, Quinn JP et al (2008) Additive effect of BDNF and REST polymorphisms is
associated with improved general cognitive ability. Genes Brain Behav 7(7):714–719
Mortensen OV, Kristensen AS et al (1999a) Molecular cloning, expression and characterization
of a bovine serotonin transporter. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 71(1):120–126
Mortensen OV, Thomassen M et al (1999b) Functional analysis of a novel human serotonin
transporter gene promoter in immortalized raphe cells. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 68(1–2):141–148
Mrazek DA, Rush AJ et al (2009) SLC6A4 variation and citalopram response. Am J Med Genet
B Neuropsychiatr Genet 150B:341–351
Murphy DL, Li Q et al (2001) Genetic perspectives on the serotonin transporter. Brain Res Bull
56(5):487–494
Myung W, Lim SW et al (2010) Serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms and chronic illness of
depression. J Korean Med Sci 25(12):1824–1827
Nakamura Y, Koyama K et al (1998) VNTR (variable number of tandem repeat) sequences as
transcriptional, translational, or functional regulators. J Hum Genet 43(3):149–152
Nakamura M, Ueno S et al (2000) The human serotonin transporter gene linked polymorphism
(5-HTTLPR) shows ten novel allelic variants. Mol Psychiatry 5(1):32–38
Naslund K, Saetre P et al (2005) Genome-wide prediction of human VNTRs. Genomics
85(1):24–35
Behavioural Genetics of the Serotonin Transporter 533
protein 1, through the polymorphic intron 2 variable number tandem repeat. J Neurosci 27(11):
2793–2801
Ruhe HG, Booij J et al (2009) Serotonin transporter binding with [123I]beta-CIT SPECT in major
depressive disorder versus controls: effect of season and gender. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging
36(5):841–849
Sakai K, Nakamura M et al (2002) The silencer activity of the novel human serotonin transporter
linked polymorphic regions. Neurosci Lett 327(1):13–16
Schloesser RJ, Manji HK et al (2009) Suppression of adult neurogenesis leads to an increased
hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis response. Neuroreport 20(6):553–557
Schurks M, Rist PM et al (2010a) 5-HTTLPR polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene and
migraine: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Cephalalgia 30(11):1296–1305
Schurks M, Rist PM et al (2010b) STin2 VNTR polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene
and migraine: pooled and meta-analyses. J Headache Pain 11(4):317–326
Selvaraj S, Murthy NV et al (2011) Diminished brain 5-HT transporter binding in major
depression: a positron emission tomography study with [11C]DASB. Psychopharmacology
(Berl) 213(2–3):555–562
Serretti A, Lilli R et al (2002) Serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) and major psychoses. Mol
Psychiatry 7(1):95–99
Serretti A, Kato M et al (2007) Meta-analysis of serotonin transporter gene promoter
polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) association with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor efficacy
in depressed patients. Mol Psychiatry 12(3):247–257
Shah MP, Wang F et al (2009) Role of variation in the serotonin transporter protein gene
(SLC6A4) in trait disturbances in the ventral anterior cingulate in bipolar disorder.
Neuropsychopharmacology 34(5):1301–1310
Shioe K, Ichimiya T et al (2003) No association between genotype of the promoter region of
serotonin transporter gene and serotonin transporter binding in human brain measured by
PET. Synapse 48(4):184–188
Smeraldi E, Serretti A et al (2006) Serotonin transporter gene linked polymorphic region:
possible pharmacogenetic implications of rare variants. Psychiatr Genet 16:153–158
Smits KM, Smits LJ et al (2004) Influence of SERTPR and STin2 in the serotonin transporter
gene on the effect of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in depression: a systematic
review. Mol Psychiatry 9(5):433–441
Smits K, Smits L et al (2007) Serotonin transporter polymorphisms and the occurrence of adverse
events during treatment with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Int Clin Psychopharma-
col 22:137–143
Soeby K, Larsen SA et al (2005) Serotonin transporter: evolution and impact of polymorphic
transcriptional regulation. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 136B(1):53–57
Stober G, Heils A et al (1996) Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism and affective disorder.
Lancet 347(9011):1340–1341
Surguladze SA, Elkin A et al (2008) Genetic variation in the serotonin transporter modulates
neural system-wide response to fearful faces. Genes Brain Behav 7(5):543–551
Taylor WD, Steffens DC et al (2005) Influence of serotonin transporter promoter region
polymorphisms on hippocampal volumes in late-life depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry
62(5):537–544
Taylor MJ, Sen S et al (2010) Antidepressant response and the serotonin transporter gene-linked
polymorphic region. Biol Psychiatry 68(6):536–43
Tencomnao T, Wongpiyabovorn J (2010) An investigation of the relationship between serotonin
transporter gene promoter polymorphism and psoriasis susceptibility in a Thai population.
Genet Mol Res 9(4):2275–2282
Tibrewal P, Kumar HB et al (2010) Association of serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms
with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) in a south Indian population. Indian J Med Res
132(6):690–695
Tsankova NM, Kumar A et al (2004) Histone modifications at gene promoter regions in rat
hippocampus after acute and chronic electroconvulsive seizures. J Neurosci 24(24):5603–5610
Behavioural Genetics of the Serotonin Transporter 535
Uher R, McGuffin P (2008) The moderation by the serotonin transporter gene of environmental
adversity in the aetiology of mental illness: review and methodological analysis. Mol
Psychiatry 13(2):131–146
Uhl GR, Hall FS et al (2002) Cocaine, reward, movement and monoamine transporters. Mol
Psychiatry 7(1):21–26
Van der Zwaluw CS, Engels RC et al (2010) A serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR)
predicts the development of adolescent alcohol use. Drug Alcohol Dependance 112(1–2):
134–139
Van Dyck CH, Malison RT et al (2004) Central serotonin transporter availability measured with
[123I]beta-CIT SPECT in relation to serotonin transporter genotype. Am J Psychiatry
161(3):525–531
Van Ijzendoorn MH, Caspers K et al (2010) Methylation matters: interaction between
methylation density and serotonin transporter genotype predicts unresolved loss or trauma.
Biol Psychiatry 68(5):405–407
Vasiliou SA, Ali FR et al (2011) The SLC6A4 VNTR genotype determines transcription factor
binding and epigenetic variation of this gene in response to cocaine in vitro. Addict Biol
17(1):156–170
Wang XJ, Zhong SR et al (2011) Association of polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene
promote with the susceptibility to alcohol dependence in Yunnan Han population. Yi Chuan
33(1):48–53
Wellman CL, Izquierdo A et al (2007) Impaired stress-coping and fear extinction and abnormal
corticolimbic morphology in serotonin transporter knock-out mice. J Neurosci 27(3):684–691
Wendland JR, Martin BJ et al (2006) Simultaneous genotyping of four functional loci of human
SLC6A4, with a reappraisal of 5-HTTLPR and rs25531. Mol Psychiatry 11:224–6
Wendland JR, Kruse MR et al (2007) A large case–control study of common functional SLC6A4 and
BDNF variants in obsessive compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 32:2543–2551
Wells TT, Beevers CG et al (2010) Serotonin transporter and BDNF genetic variants interact to
predict cognitive reactivity in healthy adults. J Affect Disord 126(1–2):223–229
Willeit M, Stastny J et al (2001) No evidence for in vivo regulation of midbrain serotonin
transporter availability by serotonin transporter promoter gene polymorphism. Biol Psychiatry
50(1):8–12
Xie P, Kranzler HR et al (2009) Interactive effect of stressful life events and the serotonin
transporter 5-HTTLPR genotype on posttraumatic stress disorder diagnosis in 2 independent
populations. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66(11):1201–1209
Yan QS (2002) Reduced serotonin release and serotonin uptake sites in the rat nucleus accumbens
and striatum after prenatal cocaine exposure. Brain Res 929(1):59–69
Yang B, Chen RC et al (2007) A meta-analysis of association studies between the 10-repeat allele
of a VNTR polymorphism in the 30 -UTR of dopamine transporter gene and attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 144B(4):541–550
Zaboli G, Joensson EG et al (2008) Haplotype analysis confirms association of the serotonin
transporter (5-HTT) gene with schizophrenia but not with major depression. Am J Med Genet
B Neuropsychiatr Genet 147B(3):301–307
Zalsman G, Huang YY et al (2006) Association of a triallelic serotonin transporter gene promoter
region (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism with stressful life events and severity of depression. Am J
Psychiatry 163(9):1588–1593
Zamorano R, Suchindran S et al (2006) 30 -untranslated region of the type 2 bradykinin receptor is
a potent regulator of gene expression. Am J Physiol Renal Physiol 290(2):F456–F464
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response
to Stimulant Drugs in Humans
Abstract Individuals vary in their responses to stimulant drugs, and several lines
of evidence suggest that the basis for this variation is at least partially genetic in
origin. Association studies have examined the effects of polymorphisms in specific
genes on acute and chronic responses to stimulant drugs. Several of these genetic
polymorphisms are also associated with other psychiatric dimensions and disor-
ders. This chapter examines the evidence for genetic associations between the
genes that have been most carefully examined for their influence on the response
to stimulant drugs.
Keywords Stimulants Inter-individual variation Drug response Candidate
gene Genetic association Genetic polymorphism
Contents
1 Introduction...................................................................................................................... 538
2 Twin Studies of Stimulant Drug Phenotypes ................................................................. 539
3 Association Studies ......................................................................................................... 540
4 Monoamine Transporters................................................................................................. 541
5 Neurotransmitter Receptors............................................................................................. 558
1 Introduction
(Yang et al. 2010). We will discuss two types of association studies: candidate
gene studies and genome-wide association studies. Candidate gene studies draw on
prior pharmacological knowledge of how stimulants affect the brain to select
‘candidate’ genes that are likely to be the source of genetic differences. In these
studies, polymorphisms within or near the candidate gene are tested to see if they
are statistically associated with relevant phenotypes. Phenotypes might include
measures of acute response, patterns of drug use or therapeutic response in
patients, or clinical diagnoses of drug dependence or abuse. Other studies use a
genome-wide association (GWAS) approach to examine many or most of the
common polymorphisms in the genome; these studies do not depend on prior
hypotheses about which genes might be important.
In all of the studies discussed: twin, candidate gene association, and genome-
wide association, we will address two types of genetic polymorphisms: single
nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and variable number tandem repeats (VNTRs).
SNPs are sites at which a single nucleotide differs among individuals within a
population. SNPs can either occur in the coding sequence of a gene and thus alter
amino acid sequence (termed non-synonymous) or, more commonly, they may be
outside the coding sequence and alter gene regulation. In both cases, a SNP may
alter a biological function itself (coding or gene expression), or be linked to
another polymorphism that is functionally significant. VNTRs are polymorphisms
in which a variable number of short repetitive sequences (tandem repeats) are
present at a given locus; as with SNPs they may have direct functional conse-
quences or may be linked to some other functionally significant polymorphism.
We will attempt to synthesize genetic studies of acute, sub-chronic, and chronic
administration of stimulant drugs in this review. First, we will discuss overall
heritability of stimulant drug-related phenotypes based on twin studies. Next, we
will discuss candidate gene and genome-wide association studies that implicate
specific genes in modulating responses. Last, we will highlight the key conclusions
and identify future directions for study.
Two early twin studies provide strong evidence for the heritability of acute
responses to stimulant drugs (Nurnberger et al. 1982; Crabbe et al. 1983). Twin
studies estimate heritability by comparing the concordance rate between mono-
zygotic twins, who share a familial environment and all genes, to dizygotic twins,
who share the same environment but only half of their genes. Typically, biometric
modeling is used to explain variability due to genetic or environmental effects.
Heritability estimates can range from 0 (no variation contributed by genetic
sources) to 1 (all variation contributed by genetic sources). Nurnberger et al.
(1982) administered d-amphetamine intravenously (0.3 mg/kg) to 13 pairs of
monozygotic twins and 3 pairs of dizygotic twins and measured physiological and
subjective effects of the drug. Responses to the drug in monozygotic twins were
540 A. B. Hart et al.
3 Association Studies
4 Monoamine Transporters
SLC6A3
Strong
Candidate
Genes
Gene name Locus Administration Study Phenotype Number Ethnicity Group Dose Associated Notes
symbol type polymorphisms
Dopamine SLC6A3; DAT1; Chronic Bellgrove Methylphenidate 43 Irish Children with – 30 -UTR VNTR 10-repeat allele
transporter DAT et al. therapeutic ADHD associated
(2005) response with better
treatment
response
Acute Cheon et al. Methylphenidate 11 Korean Children with Up to 0.7 30 -UTR VNTR 10/10 associated
(2005) therapeutic ADHD mg/kg/day with poor
response treatment
response
Chronic Gelernter et al. Cocaine-induced 58 Caucasian, Cocaine users – 30 -UTR VNTR 9-repeat allele
(1994) paranoia African– associated
American with increased
risk
Acute Hamidovic Amphetamine subjective 152 Caucasian Healthy Placebo, rs460000 C/C increased
et al. response volunteers 10 mg, Euphoria and
(2010a) 20 mg Stimulation at
10 and 20 mg
Acute Kooij et al. Methylphenidate 42 – Adults with Placebo, 0.5 mg/ 30 -UTR VNTR 10/10 associated
(2008) therapeutic ADHD kg/day by with poor
response week 1, response
0.75 mg/kg/
day by
week 2, up
to 1.0 mg/
kg/day by
week 3
(continued)
A. B. Hart et al.
Table 1 (continued)
Strong
Candidate
Genes
Gene name Locus Administration Study Phenotype Number Ethnicity Group Dose Associated Notes
symbol type polymorphisms
Sub-chronic Joober et al. Methylphenidate 159 – Children with Placebo, 0.5 mg/ 30 -UTR VNTR 9/9 associated with
(2007) therapeutic ADHD kg/day poor treatment
response response
Acute Lott et al. Amphetamine subjective 101 67 Caucasian, 20 Healthy volunteers Placebo, 10 mg, 30 -UTR VNTR 9/9 associated
(2005) response African– 20 mg with poor
American, response at
12 Asian, 2 20 mg
unknown
Acute Purper-Ouakil Methylphenidate 141 132 Caucasian; 8 Children with 10–60 mg/day 30 -UTR VNTR 10/10 associated
et al. therapeutic Other ADHD with poor
(2008) response treatment
response
Acute Rohde et al. Methylphenidate 8 – Children with 0.35–0.7 mg/kg 30 -UTR VNTR 10/10 associated
(2003) therapeutic ADHD (male) with higher
response cerebral blood
flow
Sub-chronic Stein et al. Methylphenidate 47 – Children with Placebo, 18 mg, 30 -UTR VNTR 9/9 associated with
(2005) therapeutic ADHD 36 mg, poor response
response 54 mg
Chronic Ujike et al. Methamphetamine- 124 cases 160 Japanese Methamphetamine – 30 -UTR VNTR Possession of allele
(2003) induced psychosis controls dependence/ with 8 = 9 or
psychosis fewer repeats
patients associated
with
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response to Stimulant Drugs in Humans
prolonged
psychosis
(continued)
543
Table 1 (continued)
544
Strong
Candidate
Genes
Gene name Locus Administration Study Phenotype Number Ethnicity Group Dose Associated Notes
symbol type polymorphisms
Serotonin SLC6A4; Acute Lott et al. Amphetamine 101 67 Caucasian, Healthy volunteers Placebo, 10 mg, 5-HTTLPR & L/L and 12/12
transporter 5-HTT (2006) subjective 20 20 mg Intron 2 genotype
response African– VNTR combination
American, haplotype associated
12 Asian, 2 with weak
unknown subjective
response
Chronic Ezaki et al. Methamphetamine- 166 cases Japanese Methamphetamine- – 5-HTTLPR Frequency of S
(2008) induced 197 induced allele higher
psychosis controls psychosis in patients
patients with
prolonged
psychosis
Sub-chronic Thakur et al. Methylphenidate 157 – Children with Placebo, 0.5 mg/ 5-HTTLPR LG/LG associated
(2010) therapeutic ADHD kg/day triallelic with best
response treatment
response
(continued)
A. B. Hart et al.
Table 1 (continued)
Strong
Candidate
Genes
Gene name Locus Administration Study Phenotype Number Ethnicity Group Dose Associated Notes
symbol type polymorphisms
Norepinephrine SLC6A2; Acute Dlugos et al. Amphetamine 99 65 Caucasian, Healthy Placebo, 10 mg, rs36017, rs47958 rs36017 C/C, G–C–
transporter NET (2007) subjective 20 volunteers 20 mg rs36017- C haplotype
response African– rs2270935- associated with
American, rs47958 increased
12 Asian, 2 haplotype positive mood;
unknown rs47958 C/C
associated with
increased
elation
Acute Dlugos et al. Amphetamine 159 Caucasian Healthy Placebo, 10 mg, rs36017, rs36017 C/C,
(2009a) subjective volunteers 20 mg rs1861647 rs1861647 A/A
response rs36017- associated with
rs10521329- increased
rs3785155 elation and
haplotype vigor; C–C–G
haplotype
associated with
increased vigor
(continued)
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response to Stimulant Drugs in Humans
545
Table 1 (continued)
546
Strong
Candidate
Genes
Gene name Locus Administration Study Phenotype Number Ethnicity Group Dose Associated Notes
symbol type polymorphisms
Dopamine DRD2 Sub-chronic David et al. Bupropion 722 Caucasian Smokers Placebo, Taq1A A2/A2 3x more
D2 (2007) therapeutic 150 mg/day (rs1800497; likely to
receptor response first 3 days, T/C) abstain from
then 300 smoking after
mg/day treatment; no
effect for
A1/A1, A1/A2
Acute Hamidovic et al. Impulsivity 93 Caucasian Healthy Placebo, 10 mg, rs12364283 rs12364283
(2009) amphetamine volunteers 20 mg A/G, G/G
response increased
reaction time
at 10 mg
Chronic Ujike et al. Methamphetamine- 202 cases 243 Japanese Methamphetamine – Taq1A A2/A2, A1/A2
(2009) induced controls dependence (rs1800497; associated with
psychosis patients T/C) DRD2- prolonged
141 Ins/Del methamphetamine-
induced
psychosis
Del/Del,
Ins/Del
associated
with rapid onset
(continued)
A. B. Hart et al.
Table 1 (continued)
Strong
Candidate
Genes
Gene name Locus Administration Study Phenotype Number Ethnicity Group Dose Associated Notes
symbol type polymorphisms
Dopamine DRD4 Acute Lee et al. Amphetamine 100 66 Caucasian, 20 Healthy Placebo, Exon III VNTR Single 7-repeat allele
D4 (unpublished subjective African– volunteers 10 mg, associated with
receptor data) response American, 12 20 mg increased elation,
Asian, 2 friendliness, heart
unknown rate, decreased
dysphoria and
anxiety at 10 and
20 mg
Chronic Chen et al. Methamphetamine 416 cases 435 Taiwanese Methamphetamine – Exon III Higher frequency of
(2004a) abuse controls abusers VNTR 6-repeat allele in
cases
Chronic Hamarman Methylphenidate 45 – Children with – Exon III Subjects with 7-repeat
et al. therapeutic ADHD VNTR allele require higher
(2004) response doses of
methylphenidate
Chronic Li et al. Methamphetamine 416 cases 435 Taiwanese Methamphetamine – Promoter Higher
(2004) abuse controls abusers VNTR ? exon frequency
III of 7-repeat
VNTR allele in
haplotype cases
l-opioid OPRM1 Acute Dlugos et al. Amphetamine 162 Caucasian Healthy volunteers Placebo, rs510769, rs510769 G/G A/G,
receptor (2010) subjective 10 mg, rs2281617 rs2281617 C/C
response 20 mg associated increased
euphoria and
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response to Stimulant Drugs in Humans
energy at 10 mg
Chronic Ide et al. (2004) Methamphetaime 138 cases 213 Japanese Methamphetamine – A118G (rs1799971) G/G associated with
psychosis controls dependence/ psychosis within
psychosis 3 years of first use
patients
Chronic Ide et al. (2006) Methamphetamine 128 cases 232 Japanese Methamphetamine – IVS2 ? G691C G/G associated with
abuse/ controls dependence/ (rs2075572) methamphetamine
psychosis psychosis abuse and psychosis
patients
547
(continued)
Table 1 (continued)
548
Strong
Candidate
Genes
Gene name Locus Administration Study Phenotype Number Ethnicity Group Dose Associated Notes
symbol type polymorphisms
Adenosine A2A ADORA2A Acute Hohoff et al. Amphetamine 99 65 Caucasian, Healthy volunteers Placebo, 1976C/T rs5751876 & rs3032740
receptor (2005) subjective 20 African– 10 mg, (rs5751876) associated with
response American, 12 20 mg 2592C/T ins increased anxiety at
Asian, 2 (rs3032740) 10 & 20 mg
unknown
Catechol O- COMT Acute Hamidovic et al. Cognitive response 161 Caucasian Healthy volunteers Placebo, val158met Val/Val, Val/Met
methyl (2010b) to 10 mg, (rs4680; performance
transferase amphetamine 20 mg G/A) improves with
amphetamine;
Met/Met no change
Chronic Li et al. (2004) Methamphetamine 416 cases 435 Taiwanese Methamphetamine – val158met Val allele frequency
abuse controls abusers (rs4680; higher in abusers
G/A)
Chronic Lohoff et al. Cocaine dependence 330 African–American Cocaine dependence – val158met Met allele frequency
(2008) patients (rs4680; higher in abusers
G/A)
Acute Mattay et al. Amphetamine brain 123 – Healthy volunteers Placebo, val158met Val/Val performance
(2003) response 0.25 mg/ (rs4680; improves with
kg of G/A) amphetamine; Met/
body Met perform worse
weight with amphetamine
Chronic Suzuki et al. Methamphetamine- 143 cases 200 Japanese Methamphetamine- – val158met Met allele frequency
(2006) induced controls induced (rs4680; higher in cases with
psychosis psychosis G/A) spontaneous relapse
patients
(continued)
A. B. Hart et al.
Table 1 (continued)
Strong
Candidate
Genes
Gene name Locus Administration Study Phenotype Number Ethnicity Group Dose Associated Notes
symbol type polymorphisms
Monoamine MAOA Chronic Nakamura Methamphetamine- 118 cases 199 Japanese Methamphetamine- – MAOA-u VNTR 4-repeat allele associated
oxidase A et al. induced controls induced with prolonged (vs.
(2009) psychosis psychosis transient) psychosis
patients in males
Dopamine beta- DBH Chronic Cubells et al. Cocaine-induced 45 European– Cocaine – Del-A haplotype Frequency higher in cases
hydroxylase (2000) paranoia American dependence/
paranoia
patients
DBH Acute Kalayasiri Cocaine-induced 31 Euopean– Cocaine dependence 0, 8, 16, and -1021C?T T/T genotype associated
et al. paranoia American, patients 32 mg/ (rs1611115) with increased risk
(2007) African– 70 kg for paranoia
American body
weight
Tryptophan TPH2 Acute Manor et al. Methylphenidate 498 – Children with 0.3–1 mg/kg rs1386488- C–G–C–A– A–G–A–C
hydroxylase (2008) therapeutic ADHD rs2220330- ‘‘Yang’’ haplotype
2 response rs1386495- associated with
rs1386494- better response to
rs6582720- methylphenidate
rs1386492-
rs4760814-
rs1386497
haplotype
Fatty acid amide FAAH Acute Dlugos et al. Amphetamine 72 Caucasian Healthy volunteers Placebo, rs3766246, C/C genotype for both
hydrolase (2009b) subjective 10 mg, rs2295633; SNPs associated
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response to Stimulant Drugs in Humans
(continued)
549
Table 1 (continued)
550
Strong
Candidate
Genes
Gene name Locus Administration Study Phenotype Number Ethnicity Group Dose Associated Notes
symbol type polymorphisms
Brain-derived BDNF Acute Flanagin et al. Amphetamine 100 66 Caucasian, Healthy Placebo, val66met Val/Val associated
neurotrophic (2006) subjective 20 African– volunteers 10 mg, (rs6265; with higher
factor response American, 12 20 mg G/A) response at
Asian, 2 10 mg
unknown
Casein kinase 1 CSNK1E Acute Veenstra- Amphetamine 101 67 Caucasian, 20 Healthy Placebo, 10 mg, rs135745 (C/G) C/C associated with
epsilon VanderWeele subjective African– volunteers 20 mg greater
et al. (2006) response American, 12 sensitivity at
Asian, 2 10 mg
unknown
Adenosine A2A ADORA2A Acute Hohoff et al. Amphetamine 99 65 Caucasian, 20 Healthy Placebo, 10 mg, 1976C/T rs5751876 &
receptor (2005) subjective African– volunteers 20 mg (rs5751876) rs3032740
response American, 12 2592C/T ins associated
Asian, 2 (rs3032740) with increased
unknown anxiety at 10
& 20 mg
Cytochrome CPY2D6 Chronic Otani et al. Methamphetamine 202 cases 337 Japanese Methamphetamine – CYP2D6*10, *10 and *14
P450 2D6 (2008) dependence controls dependence CYP2D6 alleles at
patients *14 lower
frequency
in cases
(continued)
A. B. Hart et al.
Table 1 (continued)
Strong
Candidate
Genes
Gene name Locus Administration Study Phenotype Number Ethnicity Group Dose Associated Notes
symbol type polymorphisms
Glycoprotein MANEA Chronic Farrer et al. Cocaine-induced 1,612 individuals Euopean–American, Cocaine – rs9387522 and rs9387522 A allele
endo-alpha-1, (2009) paranoia for family-based; African–American dependence/ rs9387522 in associated with
2-mannosidase 1,921 unrelated paranoia all cocaine induce
subjects for case– patients populations; paranoia in all
control replication rs6937479 populations
from
family based
Genes are discussed in the following order: transporters, receptors, biosynthetic enzymes, and miscellaneous
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response to Stimulant Drugs in Humans
551
Table 2 Association studies of less commonly studies genes with responses to stimulant drugs
552
Possible
candidate
genes
Gene name Locus Administration Study Phenotype Number Ethnicity Group Dose Associated Notes
symbol type polymorphisms
Glutathione GSTM1 Chronic Nakatome Methamphetamine 100 cases Japanese Methamphetamine – Null Null/null
S-transferase et al. abuse 150 users allele associated
Mu 1 (2009) controls with abuse in
females
Glutathione GSTT1 Chronic Nakatome Methamphetamine 100 cases Japanese Methamphetamine – Null allele Higher liablity to abuse
S-transferase et al. abuse 150 users in combination
theta-1 (2009) controls with GSTM1 null
genotype
Glutathione S- GSTP1 Chronic Hashimoto Methamphetamine 189 cases Japanese Methamphetamine- – Ile105Val G/G, A/G associated
transferase P et al. abuse/ 199 induced (rs947894; A/G) with abuse and
(2004) psychosis controls psychosis psychosis
patients
Prodynorphin PDYN Chronic Nomura Methamphetamine 143 cases Japanese Methamphetamine – Promoter VNTR 3- and 4-repeat alleles
et al. dependence 206 dependence associated with
(2006) controls patients increased risk
Prokineticin PROKR2 Chronic Kishi et al. Methamphetamine 199 cases Japanese Methamphetamine – rs3746684, rs6085086, Individual SNPs and
receptor 2 (2010) dependence 337 dependence rs3746684 and G–G–G–C–G
controls patients rs17721321-rs6085086- haplotype
rs3746684-rs3746682- associated with
rs4815787 haplotype dependence
Solute carrier SLC22A3 Chronic Aoyama Methamphetamine 213 cases Japanese Methamphetamine- – rs509707-rs4709426 Associated with
family 22 et al. dependence 443 induced haplotype polysubstance
member 3 (2006) controls psychosis abuse
patients
Glycine GLYT1 Chronic Morita Methamphetamine 204 cases Japanese Methamphetamine – IVS3 ? 411C [ T T/T and C/T genotypes
transporter et al. dependence/ 210 users (rs2486001); associated with
(2008) psychosis controls rs2486001-rs2248829 methamphetamine
haplotype use disorder
Alpha-synuclein SNCA Chronic Kobayashi Methamphetamine 170 cases Japanese Methamphetamine – rs1372520, rs3756063, Association only in
et al. dependence/ 161 dependence/ rs3756059 females
(2004) psychosis controls psychosis
patients
Gamma- GABRG2 Chronic Nishiyama Methamphetamine 178 cases Japanese Methamphetamine – 315 C [ T-1128-99C [ A Increased susceptibility
aminobutyric et al. use disorder 288 use disorder haplotypes to
acid receptor (2005) controls patients methamphetamine
subunit use disorder
gamma-2
(continued)
A. B. Hart et al.
Table 2 (continued)
Possible
candidate
genes
Gene name Locus Administration Study Phenotype Number Ethnicity Group Dose Associated Notes
symbol type polymorphisms
Dystrobrevin- DTNBP1 Chronic Kishimoto Methamphetamine- 197 cases Japanese Methamphetamine- – rs3213207, rs2619538; rs3213207 G allele,
binding et al. induced 243 induced rs2619539- rs3213207 T allele
protein 1 (2008a) psychosis controls psychosis rs3213207- at higher frequency
patients rs2619538 in cases
haplotype
Frizzled-3 FZD3 Chronic Kishimoto Methamphetamine- 288 cases Japanese Methamphetamine- – rs2241802-rs2323019- G–A–T–G and A–G–C–
et al. induced 240 induced rs352203-rs880481 A haplotypes
(2008b) psychosis controls psychosis haplotype negative risk
patients factors for
psychosis
D-amino acid G72 Chronic Kotaka Methamphetamine- 209 cases Japanese Methamphetamine- – rs778293 (A/G); rs778293 G/G
oxidase et al. induced 291 induced rs3916965- associated with
activator (2009) psychosis controls psychosis rs2391191 (G-A) Methamphetamine-
patients and rs947267- induced psychosis
rs1421292 (T–T)
Glutamate GRM2 Chronic Tsunoka Methamphetamine- 196 cases Japanese Methamphetamine – rs3821829–rs12487957– C–C–A, C–T–T
receptor, et al. induced 802 dependence/ rs4687771 haplotypes
metabotropic 2 (2010) psychosis controls psychosis haplotype associated with
patients Methamphetamine-
induced psychosis
NAD(P)H NQO2 Chronic Ohgake Methamphetamine- 191 cases Japanese Methamphetamine – Promoter indel Del/Del associated with
dehydrogenase, et al. induced 207 dependence/ prolonged
quinone 2 (2005) psychosis controls psychosis psychosis
patients
PRKCA-binding PICK1 Chronic Matsuzawa Methamphetamine- 208 cases Japanese Methamphetamine – rs713729, rs2076369 rs713729
protein et al. induced 218 abusers methamphetamine
(2007) psychosis controls abuse; rs713729
and rs2076369
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response to Stimulant Drugs in Humans
spontaneous
relapse of
psychosis
(continued)
553
Table 2 (continued)
554
Possible
candidate
genes
Gene name Locus Administration Study Phenotype Number Ethnicity Group Dose Associated Notes
symbol type polymorphisms
Superoxide SOD2 Chronic Nakamura Methamphetamine- 116 cases Japanese Methamphetamine- – Ala/Val exon Ala/Val associated with
dismutase 2 et al. induced 189 induced 2 (rs4880) prolonged
(2006) psychosis controls psychosis psychosis; Ala allele
patients at higher frequency
Carboxylesterase 1 CES1 Sub-chronic Nemoda Methylphenidate 122 Hungarian Children 10–30 mg Gly143Glu Glu/Glu, Glu/Gly
et al. therapeutic with ADHD (by body (rs71647871) associated with
(2009) response weight) better treatment
twice response
daily (0.22 to
0.95 mg/kg/
day)
The genes here are not as well studied in relation to the phenotypes discussed in this review; they are therefore denoted ‘possible candidate genes’. The associations described have primarily been performed
for methamphetamine use-related phenotypes
A. B. Hart et al.
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response to Stimulant Drugs in Humans 555
sample from the same study, Hamidovic et al. (2010a) tested 4 additional SNPs
near the 50 end of SLC6A3 that were not in linkage disequilibrium with the 30 -UTR
VNTR, and found a significant association between the rs460000 C/C genotype
and increased scores on the ARCI Euphoria and Stimulation composite scales after
10 and 20 mg amphetamine administration, when compared to A/A and A/C
individuals. Interestingly, this SNP is in perfect linkage disequilibrium with
rs463379, a SNP that has been associated with increased risk of ADHD (Friedel
et al. 2007), which suggests that variants underlying acute response to amphet-
amine may also underlie disease risk. Overall, these results suggest that both the
30 -UTR VNTR and SNPs in SLC6A3 affect acute response to amphetamine.
The 30 -UTR VNTR polymorphism has also been studied in relation to acute
responses to methylphenidate in children that have been diagnosed with ADHD. In
a pilot study, eight children received acute doses of methylphenidate before
SPECT neuroimaging (Rohde et al. 2003). Children homozygous for the 10-repeat
allele exhibited greater cerebral blood flow, which may reflect higher transporter
activity, in the frontal and basal ganglia brain areas in response to methylphenidate
than children who were not homozygous for the 10-repeat allele.
SLC6A3 has also been the analyzed for its role in modulating therapeutic
responses to sub-chronic (8 weeks) administration of methylphenidate. Similar to
the Rohde et al. (2003) study discussed above, Cheon et al. (2005) used SPECT to
measure dopamine transporter density in several brain regions (basal ganglia, right
basal ganglia, and occipital cortex) of 11 children diagnosed with ADHD treated
with varying doses of methylphenidate, and measured both clinical response and
transporter density. Children with at least one copy of the 9-repeat allele (n = 4)
showed a better therapeutic response to methylphenidate than 10/10 individuals (4
out of 4 responders versus 2 out of 7 responders in the 10/10 group), and signif-
icantly lower dopamine transporter density than 10/10 individuals. This suggests
that 10/10 individuals may require more methylphenidate due to increased dopa-
mine transporter density within the brain.
The association between the 30 -UTR VNTR and response to sub-chronic
methylphenidate has also been examined in a larger cohort of children and
adolescents with ADHD (Purper-Ouakil et al. 2008). Individuals received placebo
and varying doses of methylphenidate until no further clinical improvement or
limiting side effects occurred (mean dose 31.19 mg/day). They were phenotyped
with the ARS, the Stroop test (Stroop 1935), the Trail Making Test (Reitan 1958),
and the Continuous Performance Test (Rosvold et al. 1956). Subjects that were
homozygotes for the 10-repeat allele had significantly lower treatment responses.
However, the final methylphenidate doses reached did not differ across genotype
group. These results suggest that the maximal response to methylphenidate may be
lower in 10/10 homozygotes and cannot be overcome with larger doses. Similarly,
another study of adults with ADHD found that the 10/10 genotype was associated
with non-statistically significant lower therapeutic response to 3 weeks of treat-
ment with methylphenidate as assessed by the CGI-S and ARS (Kooij et al. 2008).
Although the results discussed above suggest that the 10-repeat allele may be
associated with poor treatment response to methylphenidate, other studies have
556 A. B. Hart et al.
reported that therapeutic responses are poorer in patients with the 9-repeat allele.
In a double-blind study, Stein et al. (2005) found that children with the 9/9
genotype showed poor response to methylphenidate treatment as measured by
ARS and CGI-S when they received higher (36 and 54 mg) but not lower (placebo
or 18 mg) doses of methylphenidate. These results suggest that individuals with at
least one copy of the 10-repeat allele responded better when given higher doses of
methylphenidate. Joober et al. (2007) also performed a double-blind, placebo-
controlled fixed-dose study, in which children diagnosed with ADHD were treated
with methylphenidate for two weeks (0.5 mg/kg/day) and the effect of 30 -UTR
VNTR was assessed. Similar to the results of Stein et al. (2005), children with the
9/10 and 10/10 genotypes showed significant improvement in ADHD symptoms
following treatment as measured by the CGI-Parents scale when compared to 9/9
children; no difference was observed between the 9/10 and 10/10 groups. The
10-repeat allele was also associated with better treatment response to methyl-
phenidate following longer term treatment in children diagnosed with ADHD
using family-based association testing (Bellgrove et al. 2005). In addition to these
studies, three studies have observed no association between the 30 -UTR VNTR
polymorphism and methylphenidate response (da Silva et al. 2010; McGough et al.
2006; Roman et al. 2001). Therefore, the reported effects of the 30 -UTR VNTR on
treatment response are contradictory and warrant further investigation.
The dopamine transporter has also been studied in relation to stimulant drug
abuse. In these studies, the exact doses of drug are unknown and presumed to be
high relative to laboratory or clinical studies. A case–control study examined
methamphetamine dependence and drug-induced psychosis in a cohort of indi-
viduals with methamphetamine use disorder and psychosis (Ujike et al. 2003). No
associations were found between the four polymorphisms tested and metham-
phetamine dependence or psychosis. However, when the patients with prolonged
psychosis were analyzed separately from those with transient psychosis, there was
a strong association indicating that individuals with nine or fewer repeat alleles of
the 30 -UTR VNTR had prolonged psychosis. The 9-repeat allele was also mar-
ginally associated with increased risk of cocaine-induced paranoia in cocaine
dependent subjects when the frequency of the 9-repeat allele was compared in
individuals with and without psychosis (Gelernter et al. 1994). The reasons for
these associations are unclear, given the lack of information about the doses that
were ingested. The findings may indicate that drug users with the 9-repeat allele of
the 30 -UTR VNTR polymorphism are more sensitive to stimulant-induced
psychotic phenotypes, or, alternatively, they may have ingested higher doses of the
drug to achieve their desired effect, thus increasing their risk for psychosis.
Lastly, the effects of an additional polymorphism in SLC6A3 and cocaine-
related behaviors have also been examined. Guindalini et al. (2006) examined the
SLC6A3 Intron 8 VNTR (Int8 VNTR), which consists of either five or six repeats
and is in moderate to low linkage disequilibrium with the 30 -UTR VNTR
(Asherson et al. 2007), and found that the 6-repeat allele was associated with
cocaine abuse in a case-control study of cocaine abusers. When the 5- and 6-repeat
alleles were cloned into expression vectors and transfected into a dopaminergic
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response to Stimulant Drugs in Humans 557
cell line, the 6-repeat allele showed increased SLC6A3 expression following
treatment with various stimuli (including cocaine), while the 5-repeat allele
showed no change in expression. This study, along with Hamidovic et al. (2010a),
suggests that polymorphisms other than the 30 -UTR VNTR may also contribute to
responses to stimulant drugs.
SLC6A4
The serotonin transporter (SLC6A4; 5-HTT) is another direct target of stimulant
drugs, and has been associated with numerous psychiatric phenotypes including
obsessive-compulsive disorder (Bloch et al. 2008), autism (Huang and Santangelo
2008), and depression (Brown and Harris 2008; Kato and Serretti 2008; Risch
et al. 2009). The 5-hydroxytryptamine transporter gene-linked polymorphic region
(5-HTTLPR), which may be the most widely studied polymorphism in all of
psychiatric genetics, has at least two common alleles: a short (S) 14-repeat and a
long (L) 16-repeat allele (Nakamura et al. 2000; Rausch 2005). The 5-HTTLPR S
allele has been associated with reduced gene expression (Hranilovic et al. 2004)
and increased risk for psychiatric phenotypes like depression. The long allele has
been further refined into two alleles (LA and LG) distinguished by an A?G
polymorphism within the first repeat; the LG allele is reported to have equivalent
expression to the S allele (Hu et al. 2006). In additional to 5-HTTLPR, a VNTR in
Intron 2 of SLC6A4 has also been described, which consists of either a 10- or 12-
repeat allele. The 12-repeat allele has been associated with increased gene
expression (Hranilovic et al. 2004).
We have examined both of the SLC6A4 polymorphisms mentioned above to
determine whether they influence acute responses to amphetamine using our
sample of healthy volunteers described in the previous section (Lott et al. 2006).
When these two polymorphisms were analyzed separately, individuals homozy-
gous for the 10-repeat allele of the Intron 2 VNTR showed a stronger euphoric
response. When the polymorphisms were analyzed jointly, no significant associ-
ation was observed, but trends in the predicted directions were observed—subjects
homozygous for the low expressing alleles (S and 10-repeat) had the strongest
responses to amphetamine (for the POMS Anxiety, DEQ Feel Drug, and ARCI
Euphoria scales). These data identify a non-significant trend towards decreased
expression of the serotonin transporter being associated with increased responses
to stimulants.
The serotonin transporter has also been investigated for its role in sub-chronic
methylphenidate treatment response in children diagnosed with ADHD (Thakur
et al. 2010). In a 2 week trial, children received placebo and methylphenidate at
varying doses. Subjects homozygous for the higher expressing LA had the worst
response to placebo but the best response to methylphenidate, heterozygotes were
intermediate, and individuals homozygous for the LS and LG alleles exhibited the
best response to placebo but deterioration with methylphenidate treatment as mea-
sured by the CGI-Parents subscale. Thus, lower expression of SLC6A4 is associated
with stronger euphoric, but weaker therapeutic responses to stimulant drugs.
558 A. B. Hart et al.
The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism has also been associated with adverse responses
in methamphetamine abusers. The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism was tested for
association with prolonged methamphetamine-induced psychosis in a case-control
study of methamphetamine abusers (Ezaki et al. 2008), utilizing the sample and
methods described in the previous section (Ujike et al. 2003). SLC6A4 was chosen
as a candidate gene for this phenotype based on previous neuroimaging findings
from the same group showing that methamphetamine abusers had lower serotonin
transporter density than non-abusers (Sekine et al. 2006). The lower expressing
S allele was significantly associated with methamphetamine psychosis, and in
particular with prolonged psychosis. Thus, the S allele (and the LG allele in the one
instance where it was differentiated from the LA allele) appears to be associated
with more intense acute and chronic responses to various different stimulants.
Furthermore, it appears that the lower expressing alleles of both the dopamine and
the serotonin transporters are associated with greater propensity to methamphet-
amine-induced psychosis.
SLC6A2
The norepinephrine transporter (SLC6A2; NET) is a third direct target of stimulant
drugs (Sulzer et al. 2005). Variants in SLC6A2 have been associated with psy-
chiatric phenotypes including depression (Haenisch et al. 2009; Min et al. 2009),
antidepressant response (Min et al. 2009), and panic disorder (Lee et al. 2005).
Using our sample of healthy adults (Dlugos et al. 2007) we found that rs36017 (C/
C genotype) was associated with increased positive mood, as measured by a
composite scale composed of ‘‘elation’’ minus ‘‘depression’’, following amphet-
amine administration (20 mg), while rs47958 (C/C genotype) was associated with
increased elation. Haplotypes containing these SNPs (rs36017-rs10521329-
rs3785155, G–C–C and C–C–A) were also associated with increased positive
mood. A follow-up study with a larger number of individuals replicated the
association between rs36017 and the POMS Elation scale, as well as with the
POMS Vigor scale, and identified a new association between the A/A genotype at
rs1861647 and these scales (Dlugos et al. 2009a). In addition, a different haplotype
than those mentioned above, which was also constructed from rs36017,
rs10521329, and rs3785155 (C–C–G), was associated with increased vigor fol-
lowing amphetamine administration. In sum, these studies suggest that multiple
variants in SLC6A2 influence responses to acute amphetamine administration.
5 Neurotransmitter Receptors
DRD2
Jimenez et al. 2006). In particular, the historically named Taq1A SNP (also known
as rs1800497) has been widely studied. Historical nomenclature describes the A1
(T) and the A2 alleles (C). This polymorphism was initially described as being
located in DRD2, but is now recognized to be located in a neighboring gene,
ANNK1 (Neville et al. 2004). However, this polymorphism appears to influence the
expression of the DRD2 gene and thereby alter DRD2 function. In a study of
DRD2 polymorphisms and gene expression in postmortem human brain tissues,
rs1800497 was not associated with DRD2 expression, but the G allele of another
SNP in DRD2, rs12364283, was found to be associated with enhanced expression
(Zhang et al. 2007).
Variation in DRD2 has been investigated in relation to amphetamine-induced
impulsive behavior. Amphetamine increases behavioral inhibition, and the degree
of this inhibition varies across individuals (de Wit et al. 2000). We examined the
role of several SNP polymorphisms in DRD2 and the effects of amphetamine on
behavioral inhibition utilizing the sample of healthy human subjects that is
described above. Individuals were phenotyped with the stop task (Logan et al.
1984), a measure of behavioral inhibition, and genotyped at 12 SNPs in DRD2 (but
not the Taq1A polymorphism). One SNP, rs12364283, was significantly associated
with better performance on the stop task following 10 mg amphetamine admin-
istration in the G/G and A/G groups. This G allele of this SNP, as discussed earlier,
is associated with increased DRD2 expression (Zhang et al. 2007); increased
DRD2 expression may be related to better task performance (Cropley et al. 2006).
Variation in DRD2 has been investigated for its effect on bupropion treatment
response (David et al. 2007). David et al. (2007) found that smokers with the
A2/A2 genotype at Taq1A who received bupropion (150 mg/day for the first
3 days, then 300 mg/day) were three times more likely to abstain from smoking at
the end of the trial compared to A2/A2 subjects receiving placebo. This difference
was not observed for the other genotypic groups.
Finally, DRD2 has also been investigated for association with methamphet-
amine dependence and methamphetamine-induced psychosis. Šerý et al. (2001)
found no association between polymorphisms in DRD2 and methamphetamine
dependence. Ujike et al. (2009) tested 3 SNPs in DRD2: 141C insertion/deletion
(rs1799732; -/C), Ser311Cys (rs1801028; C/G) and Taq1A, and found that the
Taq1A A2 (C) allele, which was associated with good response to bupropion for
smoking cessation, was associated with prolonged methamphetamine psychosis
(A2/A2, A1/A2 genotypes). Additionally, they found that the Del/Del and Ins/Del
genotypes of 141C insertion allele (rs1799732) were associated with rapid onset of
psychotic symptoms (within 3 years of initial abuse). Therefore, the Taq1A
polymorphism, along with other polymorphisms in DRD2, may influence devel-
opment of stimulant-induced psychosis as well as drug dependence.
DRD4
The dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) has been associated with smoking behavior
(Laucht et al. 2008), schizophrenia (Shi et al. 2008), novelty seeking (Munafò
560 A. B. Hart et al.
OPRM1
The l-opioid receptor, OPRM1, has been investigated for a role in the subjective
response to acute amphetamine. Stimulation of l-opioid receptors by endogenous
beta-endorphins in the ventral tegmental area increases dopamine release
(Spanagel et al. 1992); therefore, variation in OPRM1 may influence dopamine
release and therefore response to stimulants. A commonly studied coding poly-
morphism within OPRM1, Asp40Asn (A118G; rs1799971; A/G), was first
described by Bergen et al. (1997) and has been associated with numerous phe-
notypes, including heroin addiction (Drakenberg et al. 2006), schizophrenia (Šerý
et al. 2010), and naltrexone treatment response (Oroszi et al. 2009). This SNP has
also been shown to alter binding of beta-endorphin to the receptor (Bond et al.
1998). We examined associations between several SNPs (including rs1799971)
and the acute responses to amphetamine on the ARCI Euphoria, Energy and
Stimulation scales (Dlugos et al. 2010), and found that two polymorphisms
(rs510769 G/G, A/G and rs2281617 C/C) were associated with increased euphoric
responses to amphetamine at 10 mg; rs510769 is in strong linkage disequilibrium
with rs1799971. The results indicate that variation in OPRM1 may be related to
variation in positive, euphoric responses to amphetamine.
Rs1799971 has been associated with the duration of methamphetamine-induced
psychosis in a case-control study of individuals with methamphetamine depen-
dence/psychosis; G/G individuals were more likely to become psychotic within
3 years of their first methamphetamine intake (Ide et al. 2004). In a more recent
study using the same population, the previous association did not replicate, but the
G/G genotype of an additional SNP in OPRM1 (IVS2 ? G691C; rs2075572; G/C)
was associated with both methamphetamine dependence and psychosis (Ide et al.
2006). These results suggest the possibility that additional variants underlie
chronic methamphetamine response, although rs1799971 has been the main target
of the literature in investigating acute and chronic stimulant responses.
ADORA2A
The adenosine A2A receptor (ADORA2A) is a major target of caffeine (Daly and
Fredholm 1998) and forms a heterodimer with the dopamine D2 receptor (Fuxe
et al. 2005). We investigated the effect of ADORA2A polymorphisms on the
anxiety response to amphetamine in healthy human subjects (Hohoff et al. 2005)
and found that two polymorphisms (rs5751876; T/T and rs3032740; Tins/Tins),
were associated with increased anxiety at the 10 and 20 mg doses.
6 Biosynthetic Enzymes
COMT
Various enzymes involved in the synthesis and breakdown of neurotransmitters
have also been analyzed for associations with acute response to stimulant drugs.
562 A. B. Hart et al.
One major candidate gene is catechol O-methyl transferase (COMT). This gene
codes for an enzyme that preferentially metabolizes dopamine in the prefrontal
cortex, rather than in the limbic and striatal brain regions, where the dopamine
transporter is more important for clearance (Chen et al. 2004b). COMT contains a
val?met coding polymorphism (val158-met; rs4680; G(val)/A(met)) that has been
associated with a variety of phenotypes including personality, cognition, risk for
psychiatric disorders (Tunbridge et al. 2006), and pain sensitivity (Andersen and
Skorpen 2009). The met allele has been associated with 3–4 times lower activity
compared to the val allele (Lachman et al. 1996).
Polymorphisms in COMT have been associated with acute amphetamine
response. In a landmark study (Mattay et al. 2003), healthy volunteers completed
the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Heaton et al. 1993) after placebo or
amphetamine, and underwent fMRI while performing the N-back working memory
task (Kirchner 1958). Amphetamine administration reduced prefrontal cortical
activity at all working memory loads when compared to placebo in individuals
homozygous for the val allele. This was interpreted as a more efficient physiological
response; this reduction was associated with improved reaction time and no
decrease in accuracy on the task. Conversely, amphetamine increased prefrontal
cortical activity in the most difficult part of the working memory task in met
homozygotes. This was interpreted as a reduction in efficiency, because increased
prefrontal cortical activity was associated with increased reaction time and
decreased accuracy. The authors proposed that amphetamine increased dopamine
levels above an optimum level in met/met individuals and thus negatively impacted
cortical function. In contrast, in val/val individuals, amphetamine increased the
lower pre-drug dopamine levels so that they were closer to optimal and thus
enhanced function. The WCST also showed genotypic effects, with val/val subjects
improving following amphetamine administration and met/met individuals per-
forming worse. Taken together, the results of this study suggest that an optimum
level of dopamine in the prefrontal cortex is necessary for efficient prefrontal cortex
function, and that this efficiency is in part mediated by val158-met genotype. Another
study (Hamidovic et al. 2010b) evaluated the effect of val158-met on a processing
speed task (Digit Symbol Substitution Test; Wechsler 1958) and a reaction time test
measuring attention lapses (Deviation from the Mode; de Wit 2009) and reported
that when compared to placebo, amphetamine improved DSST performance in val
homozygotes and heterozygotes, but not in met homozygotes. However, val
homozygotes and heterozygotes exhibited more lapses in attention under placebo
conditions, suggesting that the drug improved a preexisting deficit in one genotypic
group. Taken together, these findings suggest that individuals with the met allele are
less sensitive to the cognitive enhancing effects of stimulant drugs.
COMT has been investigated for association with chronic stimulant drug
phenotypes. The val allele has been associated with polysubstance abuse
(Vandenbergh et al. 1997), and in a later case–control study, Li et al. (2004) found
that the val/val genotype was significantly associated methamphetamine abuse.
In addition, Lohoff et al. (2008) found that cocaine-dependent individuals possessed the
lower activity met allele more often than controls, suggesting that this polymorphism may
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response to Stimulant Drugs in Humans 563
play some role in dependence. These results differ from the Vandenbergh et al. (1997) and
Li et al. (2004) studies described above; Lohoff et al. suggest their finding of the met allele
association may also be due to linkage disequilibrium or population stratification.
MAOA
Monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) degrades monoamine neurotransmitters and has
been most commonly associated with aggressive behavior (reviewed in Gri-
gorenko et al. 2010). MAOA contains a 30 bp VNTR polymorphism within its
promoter that consists of 3, 3.5, 4, or 5 repeats (MAOA-u VNTR); the 3.5 and 4
repeat alleles are transcribed more efficiently than the 3- or 5-repeat alleles (Sabol
et al. 1998). Nakamura et al. (2009) tested the MAOA-u VNTR polymorphism for
association with methamphetamine-induced psychosis, and found that in males,
the 4-repeat allele was associated with having prolonged psychosis rather than
transient; this effect was not seen in females.
DBH
Dopamine beta-hydroxylase (DBH) catalyzes the conversion of dopamine to nor-
epinephrine (Kaufman and Friedman 1965) and has been associated with ADHD
(reviewed in Banaschewski et al. 2010); additionally, a locus near DBH was
recently associated with smoking cessation (Furberg et al. 2010). Cubells et al.
(2000) investigated the effects of DBH polymorphisms on cocaine-induced para-
noia in patients with cocaine dependence/paranoia, and identified a haplotype found
more frequently in cocaine-dependent individuals reporting paranoia, that was also
associated with low DBH plasma levels in normal subjects (-4784–4803del-
444A?G; del-A haplotype). Individuals from the same group later sequenced DBH
in eight individuals at the phenotypic extremes for DBH plasma levels, and iden-
tified a putative functional polymorphism (-1021C?T; rs1611115; C/T)
accounting for 35–52% of phenotype variance in DBH activity (Zabetian et al.
2001); the T allele was associated with low DBH plasma levels, and found to be in
positive linkage disequilibrium with the del and A alleles associated with low DBH
plasma levels and paranoia in the Cubells et al. (2000) study (Zabetian et al. 2003).
A controlled laboratory study was later conducted in which 31 cocaine-using
volunteers acutely self-administered intravenous doses of the cocaine (0, 8, 16, and
32 mg/70 kg body weight) over four sessions (Kalayasiri et al. 2007). The previ-
ously identified -1021C?T polymorphism was tested for association with
cocaine-induced paranoia, and the low DBH plasma level T/T genotype group
reported higher paranoia when compared to the C/T and C/C groups.
TPH2
7 Miscellaneous
BDNF
CSNK1E
Another enzyme showing association with acute stimulant drug responses is
casein kinase 1 epsilon (CSNK1E). CSNK1E encodes an enzyme that phosphor-
ylates dopamine and cAMP-regulated phosphoprotein (DAARP-32; PPP1R1B), a
second messenger that integrates dopaminergic and glutamatergic signaling
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response to Stimulant Drugs in Humans 565
CYP2D6
Cytochrome P450 2D6 (CYP2D6) is a p450 enzyme that metabolizes metham-
phetamine (Lin et al. 1997). Otani et al. (2008) investigated the effect of CYP2D6
polymorphisms in methamphetamine dependent patients in a case-control study,
and found the lower activity CYP2D6*10 and CYP2D6*14 alleles were under-
represented in patients, suggesting that these alleles are protective against devel-
opment of methamphetamine dependence.
MANEA
An association between rs1133503, located in the 30 -UTR of glycoprotein endo-
alpha-1,2-mannosidase (MANEA), and cocaine-induced paranoia was the strongest
result in a genome-wide scan for variants associated with cocaine dependence and
cocaine-induced paranoia (Yu et al. 2008). In a later study from the same group, 11
SNPs in MANEA were studied further using a family-based approach in two
separate populations (African-–American; AA and European–American; EA).
Nine of these SNPs were at least nominally associated with cocaine-induced
paranoia in the AA population, whereas six of them at least nominally associated
in the EA population (Farrer et al. 2009). Additionally, the rs9400554-rs6937479-
rs9387522 haplotype (T–T–A) was associated with cocaine-induced paranoia in
the pooled AA/EA family-based sample, and the C–A–C haplotype was associated
with decreased risk of cocaine-induced paranoia in the pooled sample. No sig-
nificant associations were found for the cocaine dependence phenotype. Two
separate case–control samples (EA and AA) were used for replication. The indi-
vidual SNP associations from the family-based studies did not replicate in either
population sample. However, two associated haplotypes in the replication study
contained SNPs that were found in associated haplotypes in the family-based
analysis (rs900554 and rs9387522; C–A; T–A); the C–A haplotype was associated
with increased risk of cocaine-induced paranoia and cocaine dependence in EA,
566 A. B. Hart et al.
and the T–A haplotype was associated with increased risk of cocaine dependence
in the AA replication sample.
8 Exploratory Studies
A number of association studies have focused on genes that are not as well studied as
those mentioned in the previous section; these more exploratory studies are sum-
marized in Table 2. These studies mostly focus on methamphetamine abuse-related
phenotypes. These genes include CES1 (Nemoda et al. 2009); DTNBP1 (Kishimoto
et al. 2008a); FZD3 (Kishimoto et al. 2008b); G72 (Kotaka et al. 2009); GABRG2
(Nishiyama et al. 2005); GLYT1 (Morita et al. 2008); GRM2 (Tsunoka et al. 2010);
GSTM1 (Nakatome et al. 2009); GSTP1 (Hashimoto et al. 2004); GSTT1 (Nakatome
et al. 2009); NQO2 (Ohgake et al. 2005); PDYN (Nomura et al. 2006); PICK1
(Matsuzawa et al. 2007); PROKR2 (Kishi et al. 2010); SLC22A3 (Aoyama et al.
2006); SNCA (Kobayashi et al. 2004) and SOD2 (Nakamura et al. 2006).
10 Closing Remarks
The main focus of this review was to investigate sources of genetic variation
across a range of different phenotypes and prototypic stimulant drugs. Some of
the data are specific to acute or chronic administration and therapeutic or
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response to Stimulant Drugs in Humans 567
Acknowledgements This work was supported by NIH grants DA02812, DA027545 and
DA021336.
References
Faraone S, Doyle A, Mick E, Biederman J (2001) Meta-analysis of the association between the
7-repeat allele of the dopamine D4 receptor gene and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Am J Psychiatry 158:1052–1057
Farrer L, Kranzler H, Yu Y, Weiss R, Brady K, Anton R, Cubells J, Gelernter J (2009)
Association of variants in MANEA with cocaine-related behaviors. Arch Gen Psychiatry
66:267
Fergusson D, Horwood L, Lynskey M, Madden P (2003) Early reactions to cannabis predict later
dependence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 60:1033–1039
Filbey F, Schacht J, Myers U, Chavez R, Hutchison K (2009) Individual and additive effects of
the CNR1 and FAAH Genes on brain response to marijuana cues. Neuropsychopharmacology
35:967–975
Flanagin B, Cook E Jr, de Wit H (2006) An association study of the brain-derived neurotrophic
factor Val66Met polymorphism and amphetamine response. Am J Med Genet Part B
Neuropsychiatr Genet 141:576–583
Friedel S, Saar K, Sauer S, Dempfle A, Walitza S, Renner T, Romanos M, Freitag C, Seitz C,
Palmason H (2007) Association and linkage of allelic variants of the dopamine transporter
gene in ADHD. Mol Psychiatry 12:923–933
Fuke S, Suo S, Takahashi N, Koike H, Sasagawa N, Ishiura S (2001) The VNTR polymorphism
of the human dopamine transporter (DAT1) gene affects gene expression. Pharmacogenomics
J 1:152–156
Furberg H, Kim Y, Dackor J, Boerwinkle E, Franceschini N, Ardissino D, Bernardinelli L,
Mannucci P, Mauri F, Merlini P (2010) Genome-wide meta-analyses identify multiple loci
associated with smoking behavior. Nat Genet 42:441–447
Fuxe K, Ferre S, Canals M, Torvinen M, Terasmaa A, Marcellino D, Goldberg S, Staines W,
Jacobsen K, Lluis C (2005) Adenosine A2A and dopamine D2 heteromeric receptor
complexes and their function. J Mol Neurosci 26(2):209–220
Gelernter J, Kranzler HR, Satel SL, Rao PA (1994) Genetic association between dopamine
transporter protein alleles and cocaine-induced paranoia. Neuropsychopharmacology
11:195–200
Greengard P (2001) The neurobiology of slow synaptic transmission. Science 294:1024–1030
Grigorenko E, DeYoung C, Eastman M, Getchell M, Haeffel G, af Klinteberg B, Koposov R,
Oreland L, Pakstis A, Ponomarev O (2010) Aggressive behavior, related conduct problems,
and variation in genes affecting dopamine turnover. Aggress Behav 36:158–176
Guindalini C, Howard M, Haddley K, Laranjeira R, Collier D, Ammar N, Craig I, O’Gara C,
Bubb VJ, Greenwood T, Kelsoe J, Asherson P, Murray RM, Castelo A, Quinn JP, Vallada H,
Breen G (2006) A dopamine transporter gene functional variant associated with cocaine abuse
in a Brazilian sample. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:4552–4557
Guy W (1976) Clinical global impressions. In: ECDEU assessment manual for psychopharma-
cology, revised. National Institute of Mental Health, Rockville
Haenisch B, Linsel K, Brüss M, Gilsbach R, Propping P, Nöthen M, Rietschel M, Fimmers R,
Maier W, Zobel A (2009) Association of major depression with rare functional variants in
norepinephrine transporter and serotonin1A receptor genes. Am J Med Genet Part B
Neuropsychiatr Genet 150:1013–1016
Haertzen C, Kocher T, Miyasato K (1983) Reinforcements from the first drug experience can
predict later drug habits and/or addiction: results with coffee, cigarettes, alcohol, barbiturates,
minor and major tranquilizers, stimulants, marijuana, hallucinogens, heroin, opiates and
cocaine. Drug Alcohol Depend 11:147–165
Hamarman S, Fossella J, Ulger C, Brimacombe M, Dermody J (2004) Dopamine receptor 4
(DRD4) 7-repeat allele predicts methylphenidate dose response in children with attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder: a pharmacogenetic study. J Child Adolescent Psychopharma-
cology 14:564–574
Hamidovic A, Dlugos A, Skol A, Palmer AA, de Wit H (2009) Evaluation of genetic variability
in the dopamine receptor D2 in relation to behavioral inhibition and impulsivity/sensation
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response to Stimulant Drugs in Humans 571
Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao UMA, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, Ryan N (1997)
Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and
lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 36:980–988
Kendler K, Karkowski L, Prescott C (1999) Hallucinogen, opiate, sedative and stimulant use and
abuse in a population-based sample of female twins. Acta Psychiatr Scand 99:368–376
Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Neale MC, Prescott CA (2000) Illicit psychoactive substance use,
heavy use, abuse, and dependence in a US population-based sample of male twins. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 57:261–269
Kendler KS, Jacobson KC, Prescott CA, Neale MC (2003) Specificity of genetic and
environmental risk factors for use and abuse/dependence of cannabis, cocaine, hallucinogens,
sedatives, stimulants, and opiates in male twins. Am J Psychiatry 160:687–695
Kendler K, Gardner C, Jacobson K, Neale M, Prescott C (2005) Genetic and environmental
influences on illicit drug use and tobacco use across birth cohorts. Psychol Med 35:1349–1356
Kirchner WK (1958) Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information.
J Exp Psychology 55:352–358
Kishi T, Kitajima T, Tsunoka T, Okumura T, Okochi T, Kawashima K, Inada T, Ujike H,
Yamada M, Uchimura N, Sora I, Iyo M, Ozaki N, Iwata N (2010) PROKR2 is associated with
methamphetamine dependence in the Japanese population. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol
Psychiatry 34:1033–1036
Kishimoto M, Ujike H, Motohashi Y, Tanaka Y, Okahisa Y, Kotaka T, Harano M, Inada T,
Yamada M, Komiyama T, Hori T, Sekine Y, Iwata N, Sora I, Iyo M, Ozaki N, Kuroda S
(2008a) The dysbindin gene (DTNBP1) is associated with methamphetamine psychosis. Biol
Psychiatry 63:191–196
Kishimoto M, Ujike H, Okahisa Y, Kotaka T, Takaki M, Kodama M, Inada T, Yamada M,
Uchimura N, Iwata N (2008b) The frizzled 3 gene is associated with methamphetamine
psychosis in the Japanese population. Behav Brain Funct 4:37
Kobayashi H, Ide S, Hasegawa J, Ujike H, Sekine Y, Ozaki N, Inada T, Harano M, Komiyama T,
Yamada M, Iyo M, Shen H-W, Ikeda K, Sora I (2004) Study of association between alpha-
synuclein gene polymorphism and methamphetamine psychosis/dependence. Ann N Y Acad
Sci 1025:325–334
Kooij J, Boonstra A, Vermeulen S, Heister A, Burger H, Buitelaar J, Franke B (2008) Response
to methylphenidate in adults with ADHD is associated with a polymorphism in SLC6A3
(DAT1). Am J Med Genet Part B: Neuropsychiatr Genet 147:201–208
Kotaka T, Ujike H, Okahisa Y, Takaki M, Nakata K, Kodama M, Inada T, Yamada M, Uchimura
N, Iwata N, Sora I, Iyo M, Ozaki N, Kuroda S (2009) G72 gene is associated with
susceptibility to methamphetamine psychosis. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry
33:1046–1049
Kreek M, Nielsen D, Butelman E, LaForge K (2005) Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk
taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nat Neurosci
8:1450–1457
Lachman H, Papolos D, Saito T, Yu Y, Szumlanski C, Weinshilboum R (1996) Human catechol-
O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: description of a functional polymorphism and its
potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenet Genomics 6:243–250
Lamb RJ, Henningfield JE (1994) Human d-amphetamine drug discrimination: methampheta-
mine and hydromorphone. J Exp Anal Behav 61:169–180
Laucht M, Becker K, Frank J, Schmidt MH, Esser G, Treutlein J, Skowronek MH, Schumann G
(2008) Genetic variation in dopamine pathways differentially associated with smoking
progression in adolescence. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 47:673–681
Lee Y, Hohoff C, Domschke K, Sand P, Kuhlenbäumer G, Schirmacher A, Freitag C, Meyer J,
Stöber G, Franke P (2005) Norepinephrine transporter (NET) promoter and 50 -UTR
polymorphisms: association analysis in panic disorder. Neurosci Lett 377:40–43
Genetic Factors Modulating the Response to Stimulant Drugs in Humans 573
Lee A, Kim SJ, Lott DC, Cook EH, de Wit H, McGough JJ (2006) DRD4-7R mediated response
to amphetamine in normal subjects. In: Scientific proceedings of the 53rd annual meeting of
the American Academy of child and adolescent Psychiatry, San Diego
Levran O, Londono D, O’Hara K, Nielsen D, Peles E, Rotrosen J, Casadonte P, Linzy S, Randesi
M, Ott J (2008) Genetic susceptibility to heroin addiction: a candidate gene association study.
Genes Brain Behav 7:720–729
Li T, Chen C, Hu X, Ball D, Lin S, Chen W, Sham P, Loh E, Murray R, Collier D (2004)
Association analysis of the DRD4 and COMT genes in methamphetamine abuse. Am J Med
Genet Part B Neuropsychiatr Genet 129:120–124
Li D, Sham P, Owen M, He L (2006) Meta-analysis shows significant association between
dopamine system genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Hum Mol Genet
15:2276–2284
Lichter J, Barr C, Kennedy J, Van Tol H, Kidd K, Livak K (1993) A hypervariable segment in the
human dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene. Hum Mol Genet 2:767–773
Lin L, Di Stefano E, Schmitz D, Hsu L, Ellis S, Lennard M, Tucker G, Cho A (1997) Oxidation
of methamphetamine and methylenedioxymethamphetamine by CYP2D6. Drug Metab
Dispos 25:1059–1064
Logan G, Cowan W, Davis K (1984) On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time
responses: a model and a method. J Exp Psychology 10:276–291
Lohoff F, Weller A, Bloch P, Nall A, Ferraro T, Kampman K, Pettinati H, Oslin D, Dackis C,
O’Brien C (2008) Association between the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met
polymorphism and cocaine dependence. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:3078–3084
Lott D, Kim S, Cook E, de Wit H (2005) Dopamine transporter gene associated with diminished
subjective response to amphetamine. Neuropsychopharmacology 30:602–609
Lott D, Kim S, Cook E Jr, de Wit H (2006) Serotonin transporter genotype and acute subjective
response to amphetamine. Am J Addict 15:327–335
Madsen MV, Peacock L, Werge T, Andersen MB (2006) Effects of the cannabinoid CB1 receptor
agonist CP55, 940 and antagonist SR141716A on d-amphetamine-induced behaviours in
Cebus monkeys. J Psychopharmacol (Oxford) 20:622–628
Manor I, Laiba E, Eisenberg J, Meidad S, Lerer E, Israel S, Gritsenko I, Tyano S, Faraone S,
Ebstein R (2008) Association between trypotphan hydroxylase 2, performance on a
continuance performance test and response to methylphenidate in ADHD participants. Am
J Med Genet Part B Neuropsychiatr Genet 147:1501–1508
Martin WRSJW, Sapira JD, Jainski DR (1971) Physiologic, subjective and behavioral effects of
amphetamine, methamphetamine, ephedrine, phenmetrazine and methylphenidate in man.
Clin Pharmacol Therap 12:245–258
Matsuzawa D, Hashimoto K, Miyatake R, Shirayama Y, Shimizu E, Maeda K, Suzuki Y,
Mashimo Y, Sekine Y, Inada T, Ozaki N, Iwata N, Harano M, Komiyama T, Yamada M, Sora
I, Ujike H, Hata A, Sawa A, Iyo M (2007) Identification of functional polymorphisms in the
promoter region of the human PICK1 gene and their association with methamphetamine
psychosis. Am J Psychiatry 164:1105–1114
Mattay V, Goldberg T, Fera F, Hariri A, Tessitore A, Egan M, Kolachana B, Callicott J,
Weinberger D (2003) Catechol O-methyltransferase val158-met genotype and individual
variation in the brain response to amphetamine. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:6186–6191
McGough J, McCracken J, Swanson J, Riddle M, Kollins S, Greenhill L, Abikoff H, Davies M,
Chuang S, Wigal T, Wigal S, Posner K, Skrobala A, Kastelic E, Ghuman J, Cunningham C,
Shigawa S, Moyzis R, Vitiello B (2006) Pharmacogenetics of methylphenidate response in
preschoolers with ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 45:1314–1322
McKinney MK, Cravatt BF (2005) Structure and function of fatty acid amide hydrolase. Annu
Rev Biochem 74:411–432
McNair D, Lorr M, Droppleman L (1971) POMS manual for profile of mood states. EDITS, San
Diego
574 A. B. Hart et al.
Warwick JM (2004) Imaging of brain function using SPECT. Metab Brain Dis 19:113–123
Wechsler D (1958) The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence. Acad Med 33:706
Yang A, Palmer A, de Wit H (2010) Genetics of caffeine consumption and responses to caffeine.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 211:245–257
Yu Y, Kranzler HR, Panhuysen C, Weiss RD, Poling J, Farrer LA, Gelernter J (2008) Substance
dependence low-density whole genome association study in two distinct American
populations. Hum Genet 123:495–506
Zabetian C, Anderson G, Buxbaum S, Elston R, Ichinose H, Nagatsu T, Kim K, Kim C, Malison
R, Gelernter J (2001) A quantitative-trait analysis of human plasma-dopamine [beta]-
hydroxylase activity: evidence for a major functional polymorphism at the DBH locus. Am J
Human Genet 68:515–522
Zabetian C, Buxbaum S, Elston R, Köhnke M, Anderson G, Gelernter J, Cubells J (2003) The
structure of linkage disequilibrium at the DBH locus strongly influences the magnitude of
association between diallelic markers and plasma dopamine [beta]-hydroxylase activity. Am J
Human Genet 72:1389–1400
Zhang X, Beaulieu J, Gainetdinov R, Caron M (2006) Functional polymorphisms of the brain
serotonin synthesizing enzyme tryptophan hydroxylase-2. Cell Mol Life Sci 63:6–11
Zhang Y, Bertolino A, Fazio L, Blasi G, Rampino A, Romano R, Lee M, Xiao T, Papp A, Wang
D (2007) Polymorphisms in human dopamine D2 receptor gene affect gene expression,
splicing, and neuronal activity during working memory. Proc Nat Acad Sci 104:20552–20557
The Cognitive Genetics
of Neuropsychiatric Disorders
their role in cognitive function at a genome-wide level. We review the results of early
genomic studies and discuss the novel insights that they are starting to provide.
Finally, we review the analysis of whole-genome DNA sequence data and the
challenges that this will bring for cognitive genomics research.
Keywords Genetics Phenotypes Cognition Genomics
Contents
1 Introduction........................................................................................................................ 580
2 Selection of Cognitive Deficits in Neuropsychiatric Genetics........................................ 581
2.1 Social Cognition and Psychiatric Disorder.............................................................. 582
3 Testing Cognitive Deficits in Neuropsychiatric Genetics ............................................... 588
4 Cytogenetics and Cognitive Phenotypes .......................................................................... 589
4.1 Cytogenetics of Childhood-Onset Neuropsychiatric Disorders .............................. 589
4.2 Cytogenetics of Adult-Onset Neuropsychiatric Disorders ...................................... 591
5 Linkage and Candidate Gene-Based Approached to Cognitive Genetics....................... 593
5.1 Linkage and Candidate Genes Studies for Disorders of Childhood....................... 594
5.2 Linkage and Candidate Genes Studies for Disorders of Adult-Onset.................... 597
6 Application of Genomics Methods ................................................................................... 599
6.1 Genome-Wide Association Studies ........................................................................ 600
6.2 Studies of Structural Genomic Variation in Neuropsychiatric Disorders .............. 603
7 Future Directions: New Phenotypes and New Approaches............................................. 605
References................................................................................................................................ 606
1 Introduction
novel insights that they are starting to provide. Finally, we review the analysis of
whole-genome DNA sequence data and the challenges that this will bring for
cognitive genomics research.
Historically, no aspect of cognition (or any human trait for that matter) has
received more attention in terms of its underlying genetics basis than general
cognitive ability or intelligence. While this history has not been without contro-
versy (cf. the eugenics movement), the genetic basis of intelligence has been
studied widely in both the general population and across psychiatric disorders.
A key question in the selection of specific cognitive phenotypes is what extent
performance on a given task is heritable. Differing degrees of relationships within
families, associated with more or less sharing of genetic material (e.g. monozygotic
twins share 100% of genes, dizygotic twins/siblings 50%, and half-siblings 25%)
allow estimation of the proportion of individual differences in performance in a
population at a given time that are due to genetic differences (termed heritability
(h2)). The availability of twin and population-based disease register data has con-
firmed the importance of heritability for many psychiatric disorders. Less infor-
mation has been available to allow interpretation of the heritability of cognitive
deficits within twin samples. Because of this practical sampling constraint, most
epidemiological studies have used a family-based design to investigate cognitive
deficit in healthy relatives of patients. The finding that this group has higher rates of
a particular cognitive deficit is taken as suggestive evidence of heritability.
The total variance in general intelligence that can be attributed to genetic influ-
ences range from 30 to 80%. Broad domains of cognitive ability, such as verbal and
perceptual abilities show similar measures of genetic influence (Posthuma et al.
2001), although the genetic influence on subdomains such as memory tends to be
smaller, due to measurement error and variance, but being highly correlated with
general intelligence, genetic effects overlap. The heritability of general intelligence
increases with age to 70–80% in adulthood. Variations in brain structures such as the
density and the volume of grey and white matter, amygdala, and hippocampus, and
overall brain volume are thought to be endophenotypes for intelligence. Therefore,
genes involved in intelligence might be more closely associated with variations in
brain structure and function than to measures of intelligence.
By comparison, selection of specific cognitive functions for analysis in genetic
studies has been heavily influenced by the kinds of deficits observed within specific
disorders. This selection has also been strongly influenced by the perceived potential
of a cognitive function or process as ‘intermediate phenotypes’ or ‘endophenotypes’
for the disorder. The ‘endophenotype’ concept in psychiatry (described by Gottesman
and Gould 2003) relates to the identification of heritable quantifiable characteristics,
which may be useful targets for genetic studies as they represent some intermediate
stage between genotype and clinical disorder. The authors suggested that the potential
utility of an endophenotype can be judged against a set of criteria:
582 A. Corvin et al.
(continued)
585
Table 2 (continued)
586
(continued)
A. Corvin et al.
Table 2 (continued)
Test First author N Associated with Heritability/ State Co-segregates
illness Present in other independent with illness
family members
Cavedini et al. (2010) 35 pairs of OCD probands and unaffected first- Yes Yes
degree relatives and 31 pairs of HC
subjects without a known family history of
OCD and their relatives
Implicit association Egloff and Schmukle (2002) 41 introductory psychology students (33 Yes
test women and 8 men)
Sasaki et al. (2010) Individuals scoring High (n = 26) and Low Yes
(n = 18) on Social Anxiety
Glashouwer and de Jong Patients (n = 2,329) and non-clinical controls Yes Yes
(2010) (n = 652)
While many tests show good evidence that poorer performance is both associated with illness and heritable (at least based on assessments in first-degree relatives) both classical twin studies
and studies of co-segregation in families are currently lacking
The Cognitive Genetics of Neuropsychiatric Disorders
587
588 A. Corvin et al.
suggesting a heritability of 0.46–0.72 for this measure (Groot et al. 2004). Yet many
proposed endophenotypes in ADHD have not been examined in classical population-
based twin studies. It is not yet clear if these impairments are related or perhaps have
separate aetiological pathways. Kuntsi et al. (2010) examined this in a large sample
of ADHD probands and their siblings using a multivariate familial factor analysis
approach. The results suggest that two familial phenotypes, mean reaction time/
reaction time variability and omission/commission errors on the go/no-go task reflect
85–98 and 13% of the familial variance of ADHD, respectively. The findings for
response time variability reflect recent population twin data (Wood et al. 2010).
There are a number of ways in which loss of genes at this locus could contribute
to the expression of these cognitive phenotypes. First, under-expression of a single
gene at this locus could exert a major effect on the phenotype. Second, the effect
could be the result of under-expression of a number of proximally located genes.
Finally, the microdeletion could unmask the effects of one or more recessive
mutations. How these effects can be functionally investigated in an animal (e.g.
mouse model) is beyond the scope of this chapter, but is reviewed in Karayiorgou
et al (2010). The example of 22q11.2DS identified several key issues in cognitive
genetics. Unlike the risk variants reported in the association studies of DISC1,
22q11.2DS is uncommon, so ascertainment of sufficient samples for neurocogni-
tive studies is challenging. Compared to DISC1, we are more informed as to the
potential molecular mechanisms involved making this locus more tractable for
functional studies in model systems. Unlike the DISC1 example, which implicates
one gene, the 22q11.2DS locus potentially involves more than 25 genes, which is
more biologically challenging. In the next section, we consider how advances in
genotyping technology have expanded the range of risk loci available for inves-
tigation in cognitive genetic studies of neuropsychiatric disorders.
regarding the biological aetiology of the disorder, and limited study power to
detect common alleles of small effects. This power issue becomes even more
relevant for the genome-wide association studies (GWAS) described in the
Genomics section.
5.1.1 ADHD
Genetic studies of the ADHD clinical phenotype have followed the traditional
pathway of twin and adoption studies to establish heritability, followed by genetic
linkage studies, association studies based on candidate genes and more recently
genome wide association studies (GWAS) and analysis of rare structural variants
(detailed below).
Results from the handful of published linkage studies using the ADHD diag-
nosis as a phenotype (Fisher et al. 2002; Arcos-Burgos et al. 2004; Asherson et al.
2008; Rommelse et al. 2008), show some degree of overlap for regions on chro-
mosomes 5p, 9q, 16q, and 17p if nominally significant findings are considered;
however, no regions have achieved genome-wide significance using strict criteria.
Attempts have been made to include neurocognitive measures in the linkage
analysis of ADHD to identify quantitative trait loci linked to these traits. This
approach assumes that the neurocognitive impairments in ADHD index a latent
trait, or traits, that overlap, at least in part, with the heritable pathophysiology of
ADHD. Taking this approach, Rommelse et al. (2008) examined candidate
endophenotypes in a genome-wide search for susceptibility loci for ADHD. This
study found strong evidence for linkage to 2q21.1 and 13q12.11 for measures of
motor timing and digit span measures, respectively, incorporating ADHD symp-
toms as covariates. Doyle et al. (2008) identified a region on 3q13 showing sug-
gestive evidence for linkage to several neurocognitive traits and inattention
symptoms in ADHD. None of these studies have produced convincing evidence
for linkage, making the presence of one or a small number of gene variants with a
large effect on a given trait measure unlikely.
Early candidate gene studies focused on a range of candidate genes with some
a priori evidence for a potential role in ADHD pathophysiology. As with the linkage
studies described above, putative ADHD risk variants at candidate genes have been
tested for association against clinical and cognitive variables. Kebir et al. (2009)
reviewed 29 studies examining 10 genes (DRD4, DAT1, COMT, DBH, MAOA,
DRD5, ADRA2A, GRIN2A, BDNF and TPH2) in relation to neuropsychological
traits relevant for ADHD. For DAT1, there are conflicting results in relation to
omission and commission errors, but more consistent findings that increased
reaction time variability (Bellgrove et al. 2005a) and abnormalities in spatial
attention (Bellgrove et al. 2005b) are associated with the ADHD associated
The Cognitive Genetics of Neuropsychiatric Disorders 595
10-repeat variant. Against what might have been expected, several studies (Manor
et al. 2002; Bellgrove et al. 2005a, b, c), reported better performance on tests of
attention in children with the 7-repeat DRD4 variant previously shown to be
associated with ADHD. This is a similar finding to studies in psychoses where the
GWAS identified ZNF804A risk variant (discussed below) may identify a patient
subgroup with relatively spared cognitive performance, suggesting that the DRD4
risk variant indexes a pathophysiological pathway to ADHD not mediated by poor
performance on cognitive measures. The effect of the 7-repeat variant was confined
to children with ADHD and not seen in controls and in a more recent study (Johnson
et al. 2008), spectral analysis of reaction time variability supported the hypothesis
that the association of greater variability was with the absence of the 7-repeat allele
and was also specific to ADHD. Other ADHD candidate genes examined in relation
to cognitive function include the X-linked steroid sulfatase gene where case reports
of deletions have been found in cases with neurodevelopmental disorders associ-
ated with abnormal cognitive function and ADHD (Doherty et al. 2003).
Stergiakouli et al. (2011) showed that ADHD associated risk variants were asso-
ciated with inattentive symptoms and poor performance on verbal IQ and
comprehension subtests in ADHD subjects but not controls.
A number of linkage regions, replicated in two or more studies have been identified
in autism 2q21–33, 3q25–27, 3p25, 4q32, 6q14–21, 7q22, 7q31–36, 11p12–13,
17q11–21 (reviewed by (Freitag et al. 2010). The chromosome 2q region had
marginally stronger evidence for linkage in individuals with language delays
(Buxbaum et al. 2001). A further study stratifying linkage analyses found nonsig-
nificant evidence of linkage in individuals with IQ [ 70 and those with delayed
language (Liu et al. 2008). Multiple candidate gene studies have been conducted but
none have been reliably replicated. Consequently, we have focused here on genetic
studies in social cognition where a convergence of evidence appears to support the
role of neuropeptides oxytocin and vasopressin.
Oxytocin (OXT) and vasopressin (AVP) are highly conserved neuropeptides with
marked diversity in the regulation of their receptors. Modulated significantly by
sex steroids, they are likely to have sexually dimorphic effects and are therefore
of interest for further investigation in disorders such as autism, which show
marked gender bias (M:F*4:1). Considerable evidence from animal literature
has implicated nonapeptides oxytocin and vasopressin in social behaviour (Insel
2010) relating particularly to pair bonding, maternal care, social recognition, and
response to threat (reviewed by (Donaldson and Young 2008). Administration of
oxytocin to humans is associated with reduced anxiety, alteration in parenting
behaviour, increases in prosocial behaviour (e.g. trust, generosity, altruism and
596 A. Corvin et al.
Notwithstanding the high heritability of intelligence, little progress has been made
in identifying loci reliably linked or associated with intelligence in normal pop-
ulation samples. There are exceptions, such as the association, predominantly in
older people, between ApoE variants and general cognitive ability, episodic
memory, and executive function, and the weak associations reported with COMT
and BDNF variants accounting, if true for only a very small proportion of the
variance in intelligence. In contrast, several hundred genes are known to be
associated with intellectual disability (Chelly et al. 2006).
Genetic forms of ID are divided into syndromic ID, characterised by associated
clinical, radiological, metabolic or biological features, and non-syndromic ID in
which cognitive impairment represents the only manifestation of the condition.
The distinction might be helpful for clinical purposes, but recent phenotype–
genotype studies are blurring the distinction. Causes of ID are extremely hetero-
geneous and include environmental forms (e.g. premature birth, perinatal brain
ischaemia or foetal alcohol syndrome), disorders due to chromosomal abnormal-
ities (including sub-microscopic copy number variation discussed below) and
conditions due to monogenic causes or dysregulation of imprinted genes. About
50% of cases with moderate or severe ID have a definable cause with a lower
percentage in milder cases. Taken together, the emerging genetic findings in ID are
suggesting a neurobiology around synaptogenesis, synaptic activity, and plasticity
with aberrant length and density of dendritic spines a frequent histological finding.
Genetic defects and biochemical abnormalities have been described in several
pathways that feed into synaptic function including the RhoGTPase signal
The Cognitive Genetics of Neuropsychiatric Disorders 597
5.2.1 Schizophrenia
Association between an increased risk for Alzheimers disease and the e4 allele of
the apolipoprotein E (ApoE) gene is one of the most robust findings in complex
disorder genetics. Carriers of one copy of this allele are 3–4 times more likely to
develop late-onset Alzheimers disease (LOAD), but carriers of two copies have a
more than 10-fold increase in risk (Farrer et al. 1997). Multiple studies have shown
that APOE e4 is associated with cognitive decline in patients with AD. More
recently it has been demonstrated that this allele has an effect on cognitive per-
formance in non-patient groups as well. Carriers of the risk allele perform sig-
nificantly poorer on tests of episodic memory, global cognitive ability, executive
functioning, and perceptual speed although the effect sizes are small. There was no
difference between carriers and non-carriers for tests of attention, primary mem-
ory, verbal ability, and visuo-spatial skill. For the domains where differences were
detected these differences became more significant with increased age (Wisdom
et al. 2011). These data lead researchers to explore the effects that other candidate
genes may have on cognitive ageing. A study investigating 10 candidate genes
(including BDNF, COMT and DISC1) for cognitive function failed to identify
association with performance and cognitive ageing in the Lothian birth cohort of
over a 1,000 Scottish 70-year old individuals (Houlihan et al. 2009).
GWAS platforms were designed to assay common genetic variation and SNPs
with a population frequency of at least 5%, however, rare or even unique genetic
variants are much more frequent in the human genome. Until now our ability to
test for involvement of this type of variation in human disease ‘the rare variant
common disease hypothesis’ has been very limited. GWAS platforms and custom-
designed microarrays using comparative genomic hybridization (CGH) have
allowed investigation of one class of rare genetic variation, namely, copy number
variation (CNV). This provides some insight into likely challenges for cognitive
research in analysing rare variants: an issue that will become more relevant with
the increasing availability of whole-genome sequence data.
Schizophrenia
Nine schizophrenia loci have been identified: the zinc finger protein 804A
(ZNF804A) gene on chromosome 2q32; at the major histocompatibility complex
(MHC) region on chromosome 6p21–22; upstream of the neurogranin (NRGN)
gene on chromosome 11q24; at the transcription factor 4 (TCF4) gene on chro-
mosome 18q21; downstream from microRNA miRNA137 on chromosome 1p21.3;
a 0.5 Mb gene-rich region on chromosome 10q24.32; an intronic SNP in the CUB
and sushi multiple domains 1 (CSMD1) gene; and common variants in gene
deserts on chromosomes 2q32.3 and 8p21.3. Additionally, there is substantial
evidence for overlap in particular between schizophrenia and mood disorders, as
the schizophrenia risk variant at ZNF804A has also been implicated in bipolar
disorder (Williams et al. 2011) and the CACNA1C variant, identified in bipolar
disorder has also been implicated in schizophrenia and recurrent major depression
(Green et al. 2010). A logical next step is for cognitive studies to test whether
specific neural mechanisms underlie this susceptibility and its clinical expression.
The psychosis risk variant at gene ZNF804A has received the most attention to
date. Esslinger et al. 2009 investigated the influence of the risk variant (rs1344706)
on cortical activity within, and connectivity between, regions during working
memory (N-back task) and emotional recognition task performance in a sample of
115 healthy controls. Differences in functional connectivity, but not regional
activation, were observed. They reported reduced connectivity in the dorso-lateral
prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) between and within hemispheres, but also increased
connectivity between the hippocampal formation (HF) and the DLPFC, and
between the amygdala and the HF, orbitofrontal cortex and prefrontal cortex. They
have subsequently reported evidence for involvement of the variant in aberrant
brain activation during social information processing using a theory of mind
(TOM) task (Walter et al. 2011) and in state-independent inter-hemispheric
The Cognitive Genetics of Neuropsychiatric Disorders 601
processing (Esslinger et al. 2009). Their interpretation that the risk allele has a
deleterious effect on cognitive performance has been questioned by several more
recent studies. First et al. (2010) found and replicated evidence for better cognitive
performance on working memory and episodic memory tasks—which involve the
DLPFC and HF—in patient carriers of the risk allele. This effect was not present in
controls. In a subsequent study, of a different patient group, the authors found
relatively larger hippocampal volumes in risk allele carriers (Donohoe et al. 2011a,
b). These data suggest that the ZNF804A risk variant may identify a patient sub-
group with relatively spared cognitive performance, but possibly more social
deficits. Although we note that several smaller equivocal studies have also been
reported (Lencz et al. 2010; Balog et al. 2010). Further studies particularly in the
domain of social cognition would be useful.
Of the other identified schizophrenia loci, some are large and implicate many
genes (e.g. the MHC region) and some map to gene deserts which are not obvious
candidates for involvement in cognitive functioning. Of the identified genes,
neurogranin (NRGN) is the most compelling target as it plays an important role in
calcium–calmodulin signaling, is abundantly expressed in hippocampus, and
NRGN knockout mice have severe deficits in hippocampus-dependent tasks.
However, a recent study by Donohoe et al. (2011b) failed to identify a strong
relationship between the risk allele and neuropsychological performance in either
patient or control populations on general cognitive ability, verbal episodic and
working memory, spatial episodic or working memory or attentional control.
Bipolar Disorder
In bipolar disorder the best supported loci are the calcium channel, voltage-
dependent, L type, alpha 1C subunit (CACNA1C), and the ankyrin 3, node of
Ranvier (ANK3) genes (Ferreira et al. 2008). Perceived wisdom is that cognitive
deficits are less prominent in bipolar disorder, although a range of abnormalities
have been reported including altered identification of emotional stimuli (e.g. facial
expression), processing speed, working memory, and impairments in sustained
attention (Arts et al. 2010). Cognitive phenotypes in bipolar disorder may vary
with mood state and have received less attention in family studies (to estimate
heritability) than equivalent studies in schizophrenia. Of the common risk variants
identified in GWAS studies, the CACNA1C gene has received the most attention as
non-synonymous mutations of CACNA1C cause Timothy syndrome, a multi-organ
disorder, which includes cognitive impairments (Splawski et al. 2005). Cacna1c
heterozygous female mice also demonstrate mood-related phenotypes including
reduced risk-taking behaviour and increased anxiety (Dao et al. 2010).
Association studies have been reported in case and control populations between
the risk allele at rs1006737 across different neuropsychological testing paradigms
and imaging studies. The first reported study (Krug et al. 2010) found reduced
semantic verbal fluency with increased activation of the left inferior frontal gyrus
and left precuneus in healthy male subjects who carried the risk variant. The
authors acknowledged that the data to suggest reduced verbal fluency in euthymic
602 A. Corvin et al.
bipolar disorder is limited and their results require independent replication. To date
five imaging studies have been published, with somewhat mixed results. An initial
report of reduced grey matter volume in healthy UK carriers of this risk variant
(Kempton et al. 2009) did not replicate in a much larger German control sample
(Franke et al. 2010). The latter reported association between genetic variation at
the gene and reduced brainstem volume, but this was with different SNPs at the
gene and requires independent replication. Studies using blood-oxygenation level-
dependent (BOLD) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have targeted
circuits potentially implicated in bipolar disorder. A study of patients and controls,
by Bigos et al. (2010) implicated circuits putatively involved in bipolar disorder
and schizophrenia. They identified a trend for increased hippocampal activity
during emotional processing and also greater prefrontal cortical activity during a
working memory paradigm, a pattern previously associated with putative schizo-
phrenia risk variants. A smaller study of healthy controls targeted increased limbic
activity as a bipolar disorder phenotype and identified association with increased
amygdala activity in response to reward. Erk S. Meyer-Lindenberg et al. (2010) in
studying brain activation during a declarative memory task identified reduced
bilateral hippocampal activation during episodic memory recall and reduced
coupling between left and right hippocampal regions in 110 healthy subjects.
Alzheimers Disease
Until recently, APOE was the only gene known to increase risk of the common
form of Alzheimer’s disease with late-onset. A number of new susceptibility
variants have been identified by GWAS including novel loci for late-onset
Alzheimers disease (AD) implicating the genes clusterin (CLU), the phospatid-
lyinositol-binding clathrin assembly protein gene (PICALM), the complement
receptor gene (CR1), the bridging integrator 1 gene (BIN1), the ATP-binding
cassette (ABC) transporter gene (ABCA7), MS4A gene, CD2AP, CD33 and
EPHA1 (Hollingworth et al. 2011). These genes appear to be involved in different
processes, with five being linked to immune function; four to cell membrane
endocytosis; and three being involved in lipid processing. As of yet none have
been tested for involvement in specific aspects of cognitive functioning or their
role in cognitive ageing.
Data from GWAS studies to date have not generally supported the linkage regions
previously identified for childhood-onset disorders. A region on chromosome 5p.14
harbouring cadherin genes CDH9 and CDH10 showed evidence for association in
one study (Wang et al. 2009). A SNP at 5p.15 was located close to a taste receptor
gene (TASR1) and a member of the semaphorin family (SEMA5A), the latter
family of genes are implicated in axonal guidance (Weiss et al. 2009). Genome-
wide evidence for association at the MACROD2 gene, a gene of uncertain function,
The Cognitive Genetics of Neuropsychiatric Disorders 603
was detected in a further study (Anney et al. 2010). Stratification in the latter
analysis based on IQ and verbal status did not reveal statistically significant
genome-wide evidence of association.
Results of the PGC consortium ADHD meta-analysis have yet to be reported
and smaller GWAS studies have yet to provide genome-wide significant evidence
of association.
The results thus far challenge many preconceptions about the clinical entities
being investigated. The same CNVs are being implicated in different disorders.
For example, in autism although 70% of affected individuals have learning disability,
almost all of the implicated CNVs have also been associated with learning disability.
Many of the CNVs identified in schizophrenia have also been implicated in autism:
the duplication reported in ADHD has been reported across all three disorders, being
most common in ADHD patients with co-morbid learning disability. This diverse
phenotypic expression extends beyond psychiatric phenotypes. The 1q21.1 deletion
reported in schizophrenia (International Schizophrenia Consortium 2009a, b) is now
known to be associated with a broad array of paediatric developmental abnormalities
604 A. Corvin et al.
including autism, but also heart defects and cataracts (Mefford et al. 2008). These
findings suggest that at least a subset of patients with clinical disorders have
underlying rare genomic disorders.
Candidate gene studies of cognitive phenotypes have received much wider
attention. This was prompted by studies of functional variants at two candidate
genes, the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) and brain derived neurotrophic
factor (BDNF) genes. From both animal and human studies it is known that
reduced dopamine in prefrontal cortex is associated with impaired performance on
cognitive testing. Deficits in working memory can be reversed with dopamine
agonists, but both very low and very high levels of dopamine activity are asso-
ciated with impaired prefrontal cortex function. The COMT gene appears to be key
to dopamine catabolism in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and is a logical candidate
for investigation in disorders such as schizophrenia as well as for studies of
cognition. Numerous association studies for neuropsychiatric phenotypes have
been performed with equivocal results, with several large meta-analyses failing to
find association with schizophrenia (Munafo et al. 2005). In the first cognitive
study, Egan et al. (2001) reported that the high-activity val allele was associated
with poorer performance on the wisconsin card sort test (WCST) and reduced
efficiency of physiological response of the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex during a
working memory task. This finding has received consistent replication and a meta-
analysis of 12 studies supports the original WCST finding (Barnett et al. 2007).
The WCST is a complex problem solving task with many cognitive components
and several authors have tried to identify simpler tests for specific components of
this task involving cognitive stability and flexibility. Because COMT influences
the ratio of activation of D1/D2 receptors and D4 receptors are known to have an
effect on PFC function, variants in these three genes have also been investigated in
cognitive studies of adults with no clear findings emerging.
Extrapolating from examples of rare genomic disorders that have already
been classified we could suspect that some will share core phenotypic features
(e.g. Williams syndrome and Prader-Willi/Angelman syndrome) but others
(e.g. chromosomal deletions involving 1q21.1 and 22q11.21) may have such a wide
range of phenotypic expression as to encompass several clinical syndromes (Lee and
Scherer 2010). Not all CNVs are causative: some are likely to have more modest
effects on risk and probably interact with other genetic or environmental risk factors.
What does this mean for cognitive studies? We know that some of the
implicated loci can have a profound effect on cognitive functioning leading to
significant general learning disability. Would a general screen of IQ in clinical
populations identify these CNV carriers? Or do some CNVs cause more subtle
cognitive deficits? Plausibly, specific CNVs may impact on, and be extremely
informative about, discrete aspects of cognitive functioning. Performing such
studies is problematic because of the numbers involved.
Lessons can be learned from investigation of chr22q11.21, but many of
the validated CNVs have a frequency of less than one in 500 in case samples,
having large-scale collaboration and common assessment methods will be essen-
tial. Some consensus on batteries of tests is also essential to allow comparison
The Cognitive Genetics of Neuropsychiatric Disorders 605
across CNVs, which may be important in identifying where the phenotypic effects
may be a consequence of involvement of the same molecular mechanism or
pathway. Obvious targets for investigation are loci where a single, or small
number of genes are disrupted as these are currently most tractable for other func-
tional studies. CNVs are often complex and both gain or loss of function at a locus
may need to be considered. For example, mutation of the gene encoding methyl-
CpG-binding protein-2 (MECP2) causes Rett syndrome a neurodevelopmental
disorder almost exclusively found in females, however, duplications or triplications
of the gene are associated with developmental delay or learning disability in males.
References
Adolphs R, Sears L, Piven J (2001) Abnormal processing of social information from faces in
autism. J Cogn Neurosci 13:232–240
Almasy L, Gur RC, Haack K, Cole SA, Calkins ME (2008) A genome screen for quantitative trait
loci influencing schizophrenia and neurocognitive phenotypes. Am J Psychiatry 165:1185–1192
Alonso M, Vianna MR, Izquierdo I, Medina JH (2002) Signaling mechanisms mediating BDNF
modulation of memory formation in vivo in the hippocampus. Cell Mol Neurobiol 22:663–674
Anney R, Klei L, Pinto D, Regan R, Conroy J et al (2010) A genome-wide scan for common
alleles affecting risk for autism. Hum Mol Genet 19(20):4072–4082
Arcos-Burgos M, Castellanos FX, Pineda D, Lopera F, Palacio JD et al (2004) Attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder in a population isolate: linkage to loci at 4q13.2, 5q33.3, 11q22, and
17p11. Am J Hum Genet 75:998–1014
Arts B, Jabben N, Krabbendam L, van Os J (2010) A 2-year naturalistic study on cognitive
functioning in bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand 123:190–205
Asherson P, Zhou K, Anney RJ, Franke B, Buitlaar J et al (2008) A high-density SNP linkage
scan with 142 combined subtype ADHD sib pairs identifies linkage regions on chromosomes
9 and 16. Mol Psychiatry 13:514–521
Balog K, Kiss I, Keri S (2010) ZNF804A may be associated with executive control of attention.
Genes Brain Behav 10:223–227
Barnett JH, Jones PB, Robbins TW, Muller U (2007) Effects of the catechol-O-methyltrasnferase
Val158Met polymorphism on executive function: a meta-analysis of the wisconsin card sort
test in schizophrenia and healthy controls. Mol Psychiatry 12:502–509
Bediou B, Asri F, Brunelin J, Krolak-Salmon P, D’Amato T, Saoud M, Tazi I (2007) Emotion
recognition and genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 191:126–130
Bellgrove MA, Hawi Z, Kirley A, Gill M, Robertson IH (2005a) Dissecting the attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) phenotype: sustained attention, response variability and spatial
attentional asymmetries in relation to dopamine transporter (DAT1) genotype. Neuropsychologia
43:1847–1857
The Cognitive Genetics of Neuropsychiatric Disorders 607
Ferreira MA, O’Donovan MC, Meng YA, Jones IR, Ruderfer DM et al (2008) Collaborative
genome-wide association analysis supports a role for ANK3 and CACNA1C in bipolar
disorder. Nat Genet 40:1056–1058
Ferrero GB, Howald C et al (2010) An atypical 7q11.23 deletion in a normal IQ Williams-Beuren
syndrome patient. Eur J Hum Genet 18(1):33–38
Fisch GS, Carpenter N et al (2010) The course of cognitive-behavioral development in children
with the FMR1 mutation, Williams-Beuren syndrome, and neurofibromatosis type 1: the
effect of gender. Am J Med Genet A 152A(6):1498–1509
Fisher SE, Francks C, McCracken JT, McGough JJ, Marlow AJ (2002) A genomewide scan for
loci involved in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Hum Genet 70:1183–1196
Franke B, Vasques AA, Veltman JA, Brunner HG, Rijpkema M, Fernandez G (2010) Genetic
variation in CACNA1C, a gene associated with bipolar disorder, influences brainstem rather
than gray matter volume in healthy individuals. Biol Psychiatry 68:568–568
Freitag CM, Staal W et al (2010) Genetics of autistic disorders: review and clinical implications.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 19(3):169–178
Frith CD, Frith U (2006) The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50:531–534
Frith CD, Frith U (2007) Social cognition in humans. Curr Biol 17:R724–R732
Frodl T, Bodke AL, Scheuerecker J, Lisiecka D, Schoepf V et al (2009) Functional connectivity
bias in the orbitofrontal cortex in drug-free patients with major depression. Biol Psychiatry
67:161–167
Gao MC, Bellugi U et al (2010) Intelligence in Williams syndrome is related to STX1A which
encodes a component of the presynaptic SNARE complex. PLoS One 5(4):e10292
Gasperoni TL, Ekelund J, Huttenen M, Palmer CG, Tuulio-Henriksson A et al (2003) Genetic
linkage and association between chromosome 1q and working memory function in
schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychaitr Genet 116B:8–16
Gentile JK, Tan WH et al (2010) A neurodevelopmental survey of Angelman syndrome with
genotype-phenotype correlations. J Dev Behav Pediatr 31(7):592–601
Gill M, Donohoe G, Corvin A (2010) What have the genomics ever done for the psychoses?
Psychol Med 40:529–540
Glashouwer K, de Jong PJ (2010) Disorder-specific automatic self-associations in anxiety and
depression: results of the Netherlands study of depression and anxiety. Psychol Med 40:
1101–1111
Goldberg TE, Ragland JD, Torrey EF, Gold JM, Bigelow LB et al (1990) Neuropsychological
assessment of monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 47:
1066–1072
Goldberg TE, Torrey EF, Gold JM, Bigelow LB, Ragland RD et al (1995) Genetic risk of
neuropsychological impairment in schizophrenia: a study of monozygotic twins discordant
and concordant for schizophrenia. Schizopr Res 17:77–84
Gottesman II, Gould TD (2003) The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: ethymology and
strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry 160:636–645
Gratacos M, Gonzalez JR, Mercader JM, de Cid R et al (2007) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor val
66met and psychiatric disorders: meta-analysis of case-control studies confirm association to
substance-related disorders, eating disorders and schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 61:911–922
Green JJ, Hollander E (2010) Autism and oxytocin: new developments in translational
approaches to therapeutics. Neurotherapeutics 7(3):250–257
Green EK, Grozeva D, Jones I, Jones L, Kirov G et al (2010) The bipolar disorder risk allele at
CACNA1C also confers risk of recurrent major depression and of schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry
15:1016–1022
Green MF, Kern RS, Heaton RK (2004) Longitudinal studies of cognition and functional outcome
in schizophrenia: implications for MATRICS. Schizophr Res 72:41–51
Greenwood TA, Braff DL, Light GA, Cadenhead KS, Calkins ME et al (2007) Initial heritability
analyses of endophenotypic measures for schizophrenia: the consortium on the genetics of
schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 64(11):1242–1250
610 A. Corvin et al.
Groot AS, de Sonneville LM, Stins JF, Boomsma DI (2004) Familial influences on sustained
attention and inhibition in preschoolers. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45:306–314
Grozeva D, Kirov G, Ivanov D, Jones IR, Jones L et al (2010) Rare copy number variants: a point
of rarity in the genetic risk for bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry
67:318–327
Gur RE, McGrath C, Chan RM, et al (2002) An fMRI study of facial emotion processing in
patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 159:1992–1999
Hallmayer JF, Kalaydjieva L, Badcock J, Dragovic M, Howell S et al (2005) Genetic evidence for
a distinct subtype of schizophrenia characterized by pervasive cognitive deficit. Am J Hum
Genet 77:468–476
Hariri AR, Goldberg TE, Mattay VS, Kolachana BS, Callicott JH et al (2003) Brain-derived
neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism affects human memory-related hippocampal
activity and predicts memory performance. J Neurosci 23:6690–6694
Hennah W, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Paunio T, Ekelund J, Varilo T et al (2005) A haplotype within
the DISC1 gene is associated with visual memory functions in families with high density
schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 10:1097–1103
Ho BC, Milev P, O’Leary DS, Librant A, Andreasen NC (2006) Cognitive and MRI brain
morphometric correlates of brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met gene polymorphism
in patients with schizophrenia and controls. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63:731–740
Hoeft F, Walter E et al (2011) Neuroanatomical differences in toddler boys with fragile x
syndrome and idiopathic autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 68(3):295–305
Hollingworth P, Harold D, Sims R, Gerrish A, Lambert JC et al (2011) Common variants at
ABCA7, MS4A6A/MS4A4E, EPHA1, CD33 and CD2AP are associated with Alzheimer’s
disease. Nat Genet 43:429–435
Hong D, Scaletta Kent J et al (2009) Cognitive profile of Turner syndrome. Dev Disabil Res Rev
15(4):270–278
Hoogenraad CC, Koekkoek B et al (2002) Targeted mutation of Cyln2 in the Williams syndrome
critical region links CLIP-115 haploinsufficiency to neurodevelopmental abnormalities in
mice. Nat Genet 32(1):116–127
Houlihan LM, Harris SE, Luciano M, Gow AJ, Starr JM et al (2009) Replication study of
candidate genes for cognitive abilities: the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Genes Brain Behav
8:238–247
Insel TR (2010) The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of oxytocin,
vasopressin, and affiliative behavior. Neuron 65(6):768–779
International Schizophrenia Consortium (2009a) Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications
increase risk of schizophrenia. Nature 455:237–241
International Schizophrenia Consortium (2009b) Common polygenic variation contributes to risk
of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature 460:748–752
Janssen I, Krabbendam L, Jolles J, van Os J (2003) Alterations in theory of mind in patients with
schizophrenia and non-psychotic relatives. Acta Psychiatr Scand 108(2):110–117. http://
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed?term=%22Janssen%20I%22%5BAuthor%5D
Janssen I, Vermissen D, Campo JÀ, Myin-Germeys I, Van Os J, Krabbendam L (2006)
Attribution style and psychosis: evidence for an externalizing bias in patients but not in
individuals at high risk. Psychol Med 36:771–778
Johnson KA, Kelly SP, Robertson IH, Barry E, Mulligan A et al (2008) Absence of the 7-repeat
variant at the DRD4 VNTR is associated with drifting sustained attention in children with
ADHD but not controls. Am J Med Genet Neuropsychiatr Genet 147B:927–937
Karayiorgou M, Simon TJ, Gogos JA (2010) 22q11.2 microdeletions: linking DNA structural
variation to brain dysfunction and schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 11:402–416
Kebir O, Tabbane K, Sengupta S, Joober R (2009) Candidate genes and neuropsychological phenotypes
in children with ADHD: review of association studies. J Psychiatry Neurosci 34:88–101
Kempton MJ, Ruberto G, Vassos E, Tatarelli R, Girardi P et al (2009) Effects of the CACNA1C
risk allele for bipolar disorder on cerebral gray matter volume in healthy individuals. Am J
Psychiatry 166:1413–1414
The Cognitive Genetics of Neuropsychiatric Disorders 611
Need AC, Attix DK, McEvoy JM, Cirulli ET et al (2009) A genome-wide study of common SNPs
and CNVs in cognitive performance in the CANTAB. Hum Mol Genet 18:4650–4661
Ng MY, Levinson DF, Faraone SV, Suarez BK, deLisi LE et al (2009) Meta-analysis of 32
genome-wide linkage studies in schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 14:774–785
Penn DL, Sanna LJ, Roberts DL (2008) Social cognition in schizophrenia: an overview.
Schizophr Bull 34:408–411
Pezawas L, Verchinski BA, Mattay VS, Cacllicott JH, Kolachana BS et al (2004) The brani-
derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism and variation in human cortical
morphology. J Neurosci 24:10099–10102
Pinto D, Pagnamenta AT, Klei L, Anney R, Merico D et al (2010) Functional impact of global
rare copy number variation in autism spectrum disorders. Nature 466:368–372
Pober BR (2010) Williams-Beuren syndrome. N Engl J Med 362(3):239–252
Porteous DJ, Thomson P, Brandon NJ, Millar JK (2006) The genetics and biology of DISC1-an
emerging role in psychosis and cognition. Biol Psychiatry 60:123–131
Posthuma D, de Geus EJ, Boomsma DI (2001) Perceptual speed and IQ are associated through
common genetic factors. Behav Genet 313:593–602
Psychiatric GWAS Consortium Coordinating Committee (2009) Genomewide association studies:
history, rationale, and prospects for psychiatric disorders. Am J Psychiatry 166:540–546
Ranheim EA, Kwan HC et al (2005) Frizzled 9 knock-out mice have abnormal B-cell
development. Blood 105(6):2487–2494
Rommelse NN, Arias-Vasquez A, Altink ME, Buschgens CJ, Fliers E et al (2008) Neuropsy-
chological endophenotype approach to genome-wide linkage analysis identifies susceptibility
loci for ADHD on 2q21.1 and 13q12.11. Am J Hum Genet 83:99–105
Sabbagh MA, Seamans EL (2008) Intergenerational transmission of theory-of-mind. Dev Sci
11:354–360
Sasaki S, Iwanaga M, Kanai Y, Seiwa H (2010) Implicit and explicit associations in the fear
structure of social anxiety. Percept Mot Skills 110(1):19–32
Samango-Sprouse C (2010) Expansion of the phenotypic profile of the young child with XXY.
Pediatr Endocrinol Rev 8 (Suppl 1):160–168
Scott RW, Olson MF (2007) LIM kinases: function, regulation and association with human
disease. J Mol Med 85(6):555–568
Scourfield J, Martin N, Lewis G, McGuffin P (1999) Heritability of social cognitive skills in
children and adolescents. Br J Psychiatry 175:559–564
Scourfield J, Martin N, Eley TC, McGuffin P (2004) The genetic relationship between social
cognition and conduct problems. Behavior Genet 34(4):377–383
Sebat J, Lakshmi B, Troge J, Alexander J, Young YJ et al (2004) Large-scale copy number
polymorphism in the human genome. Science 305:525–528
Sebat J, Levy DL, McCarthy SE (2009) Rare structural variants in schizophrenia: one disorder,
multiple mutations; one mutation, multiple disorders. Trends Genet 25:528–535
Shamay-Tsoory SG, Shur S, Barcai-Goodman L, Medlovich S, Harari H, Levkovitz Y (2007)
Dissociation of cognitive from affective components of theory of mind in schizophrenia.
Psychiatry Res 149(1–3):11–23
Slaats-Wilemse D, Swaab-Barneveld H, de Sonneville L, Buitlaar J (2005) Familial clustering of
executive functioning in affected sibling pair families with ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 44:385–391
Skuse DH, Gallagher L (2011) Genetic influences on social cognition. Pediatr Res 69(5 Pt 2):
85R–91R
Splawski I, Timothy KW, Decher N, Kumar P, Sachse FB et al (2005) Severe arrhythmia disorder
caused by cardiac L-type calcium channel mutations. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:8089–8096
Sprong M, Schothorst P, Vos E, Hox J, van Engeland H (2007) Theory of mind in schizophrenia:
meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 191:5–13. Review
Stergiakouli E. Langley K, Willains H, Walters J, Williams NM et al (2011) Steroid sulfatase is a
potential modifier of cognition in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Genes Brain Behav
10:334–344
The Cognitive Genetics of Neuropsychiatric Disorders 613
Straub RE, MacLean CJ, Kendler KS (1996) The putative schizophrenia locus on chromosome
6p: a brief overview of the linkage studies. Mol Psychiatry 1:89–92
Szatmari P, White J et al (2007) The use of genetic epidemiology to guide classification in child
and adult psychopathology. Int Rev Psychiatry 19(5):483–496
Tansey KE, Hill MJ et al (2011) Functionality of promoter microsatellites of arginine vasopressin
receptor 1A (AVPR1A): implications for autism. Mol Autism 2(1):3
Thomas EJ, Elliott R (2009) Brain imaging correlates of cognitive impairment in depression.
Front Hum Neurosci 3:30
Toulopoulou T, Goldberg TE, Mesa IR, Picchioni M, Rijsdijk F et al (2010) Arch Gen Psychiatry
67:905–913
Toulopoulou T, Picchioni M, Rijsdijk F, Hua-Hall M, Ettinger U et al (2007) Substantial genetic
overlap between neurocognition and schizophrenia: genetic modeling in twin samples. Arch
Gen Psychiatry 64:1348–1355
Visscher PM, Montgomery GW (2009) Genome-wide association studies and human disease:
from trickle to flood. J Am Med Assoc 302:2028–2029
Viswanath B, Reddy YCJ, Kumar KJ, Kandavel T, Chandrashekar CR (2009) Cognitive
endophenotypes in OCD: a study of unaffected siblings of probands with familial OCD. Prog
Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 33:610–615
Vuilleumier P, Pourtois G (2007) Distribution and interactive brain mechanisms during emotion
face perception: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia 45:174–194
Walsh CA, Engle EC (2010) Allelic diversity in human developmental neurogenetics: insights
into biology and disease. Neuron 68:245–253
Walsh T, McClellan JM, McCarthy SE, Addington AM, Pierce SB et al (2008) Rare structural
variants disrupt genes in neurodevelopmental pathways in schizophrenia. Science 320:539–543
Walter H, Schnell K, Erk S, Arnold C, Kirsch P et al (2011) Effects of a genome-wide supported
psychosis risk variant on neural activation during a theory-of-mind task. Mol Psychiatry 16:
462–470
Walters J, Corvin A, Owen MJ, Williams H, Dragovic M, Quinn EM (2010) Psychosis susceptibility
gene ZNF804A and cognitive performance in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67:692–700
Wang K, Zhang H et al (2009) Common genetic variants on 5p14.1 associate with autism
spectrum disorders. Nature 459(7246):528–533
Wang Z, Aragona BJ (2004) Neurochemical regulation of pair bonding in male prairie voles.
Physiol Behav 83(2):319–328
Wassink TH, Piven J et al (2004) Examination of AVPR1a as an autism susceptibility gene. Mol
Psychiatry 9(10):968–972
Weiss LA, Arking DE et al (2009) A genome-wide linkage and association scan reveals novel
loci for autism. Nature 461(7265):802–808
Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF (2005) Validity of the executive
function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol
Psychiatry 57:1336–1346
Williams HJ, Craddock N, Russo G, Hamshere ML, Moskvina V, Dwyer S et al (2011) Most
genome-wide significant susceptibility loci for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder reported to
date cross-traditional diagnostic boundaries. Hum Mol Genet 20:387–391
Williams NM, Zaharieva I, Martin A, Langley K, Mantripragada K et al (2010) Rare
chromosomal deletions and duplications in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a genome-
wide analysis. Lancet 376:1401–1408
Wisdom NM, Callahan JL, Hawkins KA (2011) The effects of apolipoprotein E on non-impaired
cognitive functioning: a meta-analysis. Neurobiol Aging 32:63–74
Wood AC, Asherson P, van der Meere JJ, Kuntsi J (2010) Separation of genetic influences
on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms and reaction time performance from
those of IQ. Psychol Med 40:1027–1037
Yirmiya N, Rosenberg C et al (2006) Association between the arginine vasopressin 1a receptor
(AVPR1a) gene and autism in a family-based study: mediation by socialization skills. Mol
Psychiatry 11(5):488–494
Behavioral Genetics of Neurodegenerative
Disorders
Abstract Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common cause of dementia in the
elderly, and is typically characterized by memory loss. In addition, during the
disease progression, most patients develop behavioural and psychiatric symptoms
of dementia (BPSD). Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration (FTLD) is the most
frequent neurodegenerative disorder with a presenile onset. It is characterized
mainly by behavioural disturbances, whereas memory is conserved. The two major
neuropathologic hallmarks of AD are extracellular Amyloid beta (Ab) plaques and
intracellular neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs). Conversely, in FTLD the deposition of
tau has been observed in a number of cases, but in several brains there is no
deposition of tau but instead a positivity for ubiquitin. In some families these
diseases are inherited in an autosomal dominant fashion. Genes responsible for
familial AD include the Amyloid Precursor Protein (b-APP), Presenilin 1 (PS1)
and Presenilin 2 (PS2). The majority of mutations in these genes are often asso-
ciated with a very early onset (40–50 years of age). Regarding FTLD, the first
mutations described are located in the Microtubule Associated Protein Tau gene
(MAPT). Tau is a component of microtubules, which represent the internal support
structures for the transport of nutrients, vesicles, mitochondria and chromosomes
within the cell. Mutations in MAPT are associated with an early onset of the
disease (40–50 years), and the clinical phenotype is consistent with Frontotem-
poral Dementia (FTD). Recently, mutations in a second gene, named progranulin
(GRN), have been identified in some families with FTLD. The pathology associ-
ated with these mutations is most frequently characterized by the immunostaining
of TAR DNA Binding Protein 43 (TDP-43), which is a transcription factor.
Keywords Dementia Frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) Behavioural
disturbances
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) Behavioural and psychological
symptoms of dementia (BPSD)
Contents
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common cause of dementia in the elderly,
with a prevalence of 5% after 65 years of age. The disease was originally described
by Alois Alzheimer and Gaetano Perusini in 1906, and it is clinically characterized
by a progressive cognitive impairment, including impaired judgment, decision-
making and orientation, often accompanied, in later stages, by behavioural and
psychiatric symptoms of dementia (BPSD), including agitation, hallucinations and
delusions, as well as language impairment. Loss of noradrenergic and serotonergic
neurons contributes to the emergence of BPSD. Conversely, loss of cholinergic
neurons is the major contributor to the cognitive impairment of AD (Palmer 1996).
The two major neuropathologic hallmarks of AD are extracellular beta-amyloid
(Ab) plaques and intracellular neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs). The production of
Behavioral Genetics of Neurodegenerative Disorders 617
2 Familial AD
cause amino acid changes in putative sites for the cleavage of the protein, thus
altering the APP processing, such that more pathological Ab42 is produced (Hardy
and Selkoe 2002). Interestingly, the chromosome 21, in which b-APP resides, is
triplicated in Down syndrome and most of the cases manifest also AD by the age
of 50. Post-mortem analyses of Down’s patients who die young show diffuse intra-
neuronal deposits of Ab, suggesting that its deposition is an early event in cog-
nitive decline. The recent discovery of an extra copy of the b-APP gene in
familial AD (Rovelet-Lecrux et al. 2006) provides further support that increased
Ab production can cause the disease.
The other two genes causing familial AD are PS1 (14q24.3) and PS2 (1q31-
q42). Presenilins represent a central component of c-secretase, the enzyme
responsible for originating Ab from the C-terminal fragment of the APP protein.
Mutations in presenilins also alter APP cleavage, leading to an increased pro-
duction of Ab42. So far, 179 mutations in PS1 have been identified and 14
additional mutations have been found in the homologous gene PS2 (http://molgen-
www.uia.ac.be).
Most variants in PS1 are missense mutations resulting in single amino-acid
substitutions. Some are more complex, for example, small deletions or splice
mutations. The most severe mutation in PS1 is a donor–acceptor splice mutation that
causes a two-aminoacid substitution and an in-frame deletion of exon 9. However,
the biochemical consequences of these mutations for c-secretase assembly seem to
be limited (Bentahir et al. 2006; Steiner et al. 1999). All these clinical mutations are
likely to cause a specific gain of toxic function for PS1, determined by an increase of
the ratio between Ab42 and Ab40 amyloid peptides, thus indicating that presenilins
might modify the way in which c-secretase cuts APP.
Mutations in presenilins occur in the catalytic subunit of the protease respon-
sible for determining the length of Ab peptides therefore generating toxic Ab
fragments. However, presenilins have also non-proteolytic functions (Baki et al.
2004; Huppert et al. 2005), the disruption of which might also contribute to
familial AD pathogenesis.
Despite several carriers develop the disease early (40–50 years of age) with a
typical AD phenotype, in some cases patients carrying the same mutation develop
signs and symptoms resembling FTD instead of AD (Bruni et al. 2010). In addi-
tion, other mutations are associated with myoclonus, seizures, bilateral spasticity,
parkinsonian features or ataxia (Larner and Doran 2009).
3 Sporadic AD
this reason, the genetics of sporadic AD has been labeled ‘‘complex’’ (Bertram and
Tanzi 2005).
The gene mainly related to the sporadic forms of AD is the Apolipoprotein E
(APOE) (Corder et al. 1993), which is located at chromosome 19q13.32 and was
initially identified by linkage analysis (Pericack-Vance et al. 1991). The rela-
tionship between APOE and AD has been confirmed in more than 100 studies
conducted in different populations. The gene has three different alleles, APOE*2,
APOE*3 and APOE*4. The APOE*4 allele is the variant associated with AD.
Longitudinal studies in Caucasian populations have shown that carriers for one
APOE*4 allele have a two-fold increase in the risk for AD (Raber et al. 2004). The
risk increases in homozygous for the APOE*4 allele, and this allelic variant is also
associated with an earlier onset of the disease.
Several linkage studies have been performed, giving rise to additional candidate
susceptibility loci at chromosomes 1, 4, 6, 9, 10, 12 and 19. In particular, prom-
ising loci have been found at chromosome 9 and 10 (Grupe et al. 2006; Li et al.
2006). Recently, a wide genome analysis identified variants at CLU (which
encodes clusterin or ApoJ) on chromosome 8 and PICALM in chromosome 11
associated with AD (Harold et al. 2009). Data on CLU were replicated in an
independent study, which, in addition, demonstrated that CR1, encoding the
complement component (3b/4b) receptor 1and locate on chromosome 1, is asso-
ciated with AD (Lambert et al. 2009).
Also, a large number of candidate genes studies have been performed in order
to search a robust risk factor for the sporadic form of the disease. Several studies
were mainly focused in genes clearly involved in the pathogenesis of AD such as
genes encoding for inflammatory molecules or involved in the oxidative stress
cascade, both considered major factors in AD pathology. One of the strongest
evidence of the role played by genetic variants in inflammatory molecules to
increase the risk of AD involves the Interleukin-1 (IL1) complex, which map at
chromosome 2q14-21 and includes IL1-a, IL1-b, and IL1R antagonist protein
(IL-1Ra), all of which have a number of polymorphisms found to be associated
with AD in several case-controls studies carried out in different populations
(Du et al. 2000; Grimaldi et al. 2000; Papassotiropoulous et al. 1999). Several
polymorphisms in IL-6, which is a potent inflammatory cytokine but has also
regulatory functions, have been investigated so far. The IL6 gene is located at
chromosome 7p21 and polymorphisms exist in the -174 promoter region and in
the region of a variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR), which is located in the
30 untraslated region. Both of them have been found associated with AD in case–
controls studies (Licastro et al. 2003; Nicoll et al. 2000). Investigation of Tumor
Necrosis Factor-a (TNFa) polymorphisms was initiated because genome screening
suggested a putative association of AD with a region on chromosome 6p21.3,
which lies within 20 centimorgans of the TNFa gene. Furthermore, other
polymorphisms located in the promoter region of TNFa have been associated with
autoimmune and inflammatory diseases (Collins et al. 2000).
As with TNFa, investigations of the role of a-2macroglobulin (A2M) were
initiated as a result of screening studies of the genome. In this case, linkage was
620 D. Galimberti and E. Scarpini
found in the region of chromosome 1p, where A2 M and its low-density lipoprotein
receptor are found. Blacker et al. (1998) tested for association of polymorphisms
with AD showing a strong involvement of this gene in its pathogenesis.
Moreover, polymorphisms in chemokines have been investigated with regard of
susceptibility of AD. Monocyte Chemoattractant Protein-1 (MCP-1) and RANTES
genes have been widely screened in different neurodegenerative diseases (Huerta
et al. 2004). The distribution of the A-2518G variant was determined in different
AD populations with concordant results (Fenoglio et al. 2004; Combarros et al.
2004a) showing no evidence for association of this variant in AD compared with
controls. Moreover, Fenoglio et al. (2004) found a significant increase of MCP-1
serum levels in AD carrying at least one G polymorphic allele. Therefore, the
A-2518G polymorphism does not seem to be a risk factor for the development of
AD, but its presence correlates with higher levels of serum MCP-1.
RANTES promoter polymorphism -403 A/G, found to be associated with
several autoimmune diseases, was examined in AD population, failing to find
significant differences between patients and controls (Huerta et al. 2004).
CCR2 and CCR5 genes, encoding for the receptors of MCP-1 and RANTES
respectively, have been screened for association with AD. The most promising
variants involve a conservative change of a valine with an isoleucine at codon 64
of CCR2 (CCR2-64I) and a 32-bp deletion in the coding region of CCR5
(CCR5D32), which leads to the expression of a non-functional receptor.
A decreased frequency and an absence of homozygous for the polymorphism
CCR2-64I were found in AD, thus suggesting a protective effect of the poly-
morphic allele on the occurrence of the disease (Galimberti et al. 2004); con-
versely, no different distribution of the CCR5D32 deletion in patients compared
with controls were shown (Galimberti et al. 2004; Combarros et al. 2004b).
A mutation scanning of the Interferon-c-induced protein 10 (IP-10) gene coding
region has been performed in AD patients searching for new variants. The analysis
demonstrated the presence of two previously reported polymorphisms in exon 4 (G/
C and T/C), which are in complete linkage disequilibrium, as well as a novel rare one
in exon 2 (C/T). Subsequently these SNPs have been tested in a wide case–control
study but no differences in haplotype distribution were found (Venturelli et al. 2006).
Other genes under investigation are related to oxidative stress, a process closely
involved in AD pathogenesis. In this regard, genes coding for the nitric oxide
synthase (NOS) complex have been screened. The common polymorphism con-
sisting in a T/C transition (T-786C) in NOS3, previously reported to be associated
with vascular pathologies, has been tested in AD, but no significant differences
with controls were found. Nevertheless, expression of NOS3 in PBMC either from
patients or controls seems to be influenced by the presence of the C polymorphic
allele, and is likely to be dose dependent, being mostly evident in homozygous for
the polymorphic variant. The influence of the polymorphism on NOS3 expression
rate supports the hypothesis of a beneficial effect exerted in AD by contributing to
lower oxidative damage (Venturelli et al. 2005).
An additional variant in NOS3 gene has been extensively investigated in AD
patients, although the results are still controversial. It is a common polymorphism
Behavioral Genetics of Neurodegenerative Disorders 621
4 Familial FTLD
replicated by Cruts et al. (2006), who analyzed other families with a FTLD-U
disease without MAPT pathology, finding a mutation five base pairs into the intron
following the first non coding exon of the GRN gene (IVS0 ? 5G-C). This is
predicted to prevent splicing out of the intron 0, leading the mRNA to be retained
within the nucleus and subjected to nuclear degradation (Cruts et al. 2006).
At present there is no obvious mechanistic link between the mutations in MAPT
and GRN, currently assuming that their proximity on chromosome 17 is simply a
coincidence. Progranulin is known by several different names including granulin,
acrogranin, epithelin precursor, proepithelin and prostate cancer (PC) cell derived
growth factor (He and Bateman 2003). The protein is encoded by a single gene on
chromosome 17q21, which produces a 593 amino acid, cysteine rich protein with a
predicted molecular weight of 68.5 kDa. The full-length protein is subjected to
proteolysis by elastase and this process is regulated by a secretory leukocyte
protease inhibitor (SLPI) (Zhu et al. 2002). Progranulin and the various granulin
peptides are implicated in a range of biological functions including development,
wound repair and inflammation by activating signaling cascades that control cell
cycle progression and cell motility (He and Bateman 2003). Excess progranulin
appears to promote tumor formation and hence can act as a cell survival signal.
Despite the increasing literature on the function of progranulin, its role in neuronal
function and survival remains unclear. In the human brain, GRN is expressed in
neurons but significantly is also highly expressed in activated microglia (Baker
et al. 2006), with the result that GRN expression is increased in many neurode-
generative diseases.
Since the original identification of null-mutations in FTLD in 2006, 68 different
mutations spanning most exons have been reported so far (http://www.molgen.
ua.ac.be/). Interestingly, the majority of mutations identified create functional null
alleles, causing premature termination of the GRN coding sequence. This leads to
the degradation of the mutant RNA by nonsense mediated decay, creating a null
allele (Baker et al. 2006; Cruts et al. 2006). The presence of a null mutation causes
a partial loss of functional progranulin protein, which in turn leads eventually to
neurodegeneration (haploinsufficency mechanism), although how loss of GRN
causes neuronal cell death remains unclear. Estimates of the frequency of GRN
mutations in typical FTD patient populations suggests that they account for about
5–10% of all FTD cases, although numbers vary markedly depending on the nature
of the populations considered (Cruts et al. 2006; Gass et al. 2006; Snowden et al.
2006).
Neuropathology analysis revealed that ubiquitin immunoreactive neuronal
cytoplasmatic and intranuclear inclusions were present in all cases with FTDP-17,
where pathological findings were available (Mackenzie et al. 2006). Furthermore,
soon after the identification of mutations in GRN, biochemical analyses demon-
strated that truncated and hyperphosphorylated isoforms of the TAR-DNA binding
protein (TDP-43) are major components of the ubiquitin-positive inclusions in
families with GRN mutations as well as in idiopathic FTD and a proportion of
Behavioral Genetics of Neurodegenerative Disorders 625
5 Sporadic FTLD
The best well-known risk factor for late onset SAD, Apo E4, has also been con-
sidered as a risk factor for sporadic FTLD. A number of studies suggested an
association between FTLD and APOE*4 allele (Bernardi et al. 2006; Fabre et al.
2001; Farrer et al. 1995; Gustafson et al. 1997; Helisalmi et al. 1996; Stevens et al.
1997). Other Authors however, did not replicate these data (Geschwind et al. 1998;
Riemenschneider et al. 2002; Short et al. 2002). Recent findings demonstrated an
association between the APOE*4 allele and FTLD in males, but not females
(Srinivasan et al. 2006), possibly explaining the discrepancies previously reported.
An increased frequency of the APOE*4 allele was described in patients with SD
compared to those with FTD and PA (Short et al. 2002).
Concerning the APOE*2 allele in the development of FTLD, heterogeneous data
have been obtained in different populations. Bernardi et al. (2006) showed a pro-
tective effect of this allele towards FTLD, whereas other Authors failed to do so
(Engelborghs et al. 2003; Riemenschneider et al. 2002; Short et al. 2002; Srinivasan
et al. 2006). Despite these results, a recent meta-analysis comprising a total of 364
FTD patients and 2671 controls demonstrated an increased susceptibility to FTD in
APOE*2 carriers (Verpillat et al. 2002).
Besides pathogenic mutations, several polymorphisms have been reported to
date, both in MAPT and GRN. An association between Progressive Supranuclear
Palsy (PSP) and a dinucleotide repeat polymorphism in the intron between MAPT
exons 9 and 10 was described in 1997 (Conrad et al. 1997). The alleles at this locus
carry 11–15 repeats. Subsequently, two common MAPT haplotypes, named H1 and
H2, were identified (Baker et al. 1999). They differ in nucleotide sequence and
intron size, but are identical at the amino acid level. Homozygosity of the more
common allele H1 predisposes to PSP and Corticobasal Degeneration (CBD), but
not to AD or Pick Disease (Baker et al. 1999; Di Maria et al. 2000).
Regarding GRN, an association of a SNP located in the promoter and an
increased risk to develop FTLD in patients who did not carry causal mutations has
recent been demonstrated (Galimberti et al. 2010).
In addition, a known polymorphism in MCP-1 (A-2518G) has been shown to
exert a protective effects towards the development of FTLD (Galimberti et al.
2009), whereas NOS3 G894T (Glu298Asp) and NOS1 C276T SNPs likely
Increases the risk to develop FTLD (Venturelli et al. 2008, 2009).
References
Hutton M, Lendon CL, Rizzu P et al (1998) Association of missense and 50 -splice-site mutations
in tau with the inherited dementia FTDP-17. Nature 393:702–705
Lambert JC, Heath S, Even G et al (2009) Genome-wide association study identifies variants at
CLU and CR1 associated with Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Genet 41(10):1094–1099
Larner AJ, Doran M (2009) Genotype-phenotype relationships of presenilin-1 mutations in
Alzheimer’s disease: an update. J Alzheimers Dis 17(2):259–265
Le Ber I, Camuzat A, Berger E et al (2009) Chromosome 9p-linked families with frontotemporal
dementia associated with motor neuron disease. Neurology 72:1669–1676
Lendon CL, Lynch T, Norton J et al (1998) Hereditary dysphasic disinhibition dementia: a
frontotemporal dementia linked to 17q21–22. Neurology 50:1546–1555
Lesch KP, Balling U, Gross J et al (1994) Organization of the human serotonin transporter gene.
J Neural Transm Gen Sect 95(2):157–162
Levy-Lahad E, Wasco W, Poorkaj P et al (1995) Candidate gene for the chromosome 1 familial
Alzheimer’s disease locus. Science 269(5226):973–977
Li Y, Grupe A, Rowland C et al (2006) DAPK1 variants are associated with Alzheimer’s disease
and allele-specific expression. Hum Mol Genet 15(17):2560–2568
Licastro F, Chiappelli M (2003) Brain immune responses cognitive decline and dementia:
relationship with phenotype expression and genetic background. Mech Ageing Dev
124:539–548
Liou YJ, Hong CJ, Liu HC et al (2002) No association between the neuronal nitric oxide synthase
gene polymorphism and Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Med Gen 114:687–688
Lotrich FE, Pollock BG, Ferrell RE (2001) Polymorphism of the serotonin transporter:
implications for the use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Am J Pharmacogenomics
1(3):153–164
Mackenzie IR, Baker M, West G et al (2006) A family with tau-negative frontotemporal
dementia and neuronal intranuclear inclusions linked to chromosome 17. Brain 129:853–867
Monastero R, Cefalu AB, Camarda C et al (2003) No association between Glu298Asp endothelial
nitric oxide synthase polymorphism and Italian sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett
341:229–232
Morita M, Al-Chalabi A, Andersen PM et al (2006) A locus on chromosome 9p confers
susceptibility to ALS and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 66(6):839–844
Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L et al (1998) Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus
on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 51:1546–1554
Neumann M, Sampathu DM, Kwong LK et al (2006) Ubiquitinated TDP-43 in frontotemporal
lobar degeneration and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Science 314:130–133
Nicoll JA, Mrak RE, Graham DI et al (2000) Association of interleukin-1 gene polymorphisms
with Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 47(3):365–368
Palmer AM (1996) Neurochemical studies of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurodegeneration 5:381–391
Papassotiropoulos A, Bagli M, Jessen F et al (1999) A genetic variation of the inflammatory
cytokine interleukin-6 delays the initial onset and reduces the risk for sporadic Alzheimer’s
disease. Ann Neurol 45(5):666-668
Pericak-Vance MA, Bebout JL, Gaskell PC Jr et al (1991) Linkage studies in familial Alzheimer
disease: evidence for chromosome 19 linkage. Am J Hum Genet 48(6):1034–1050
Pesiridis G, Lee VMY, Trojanowski JQ (2009) Mutations in TDP-43 link glycine-rich domain
functions to amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Hum Mol Gen 18(2):R156–162
Poorkaj P, Bird TD, Wijsman E et al (1998) Tau is a candidate gene for chromosome 17
frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 43:815–825
Raber J, Huang Y, Ashford JW (2004) ApoE genotype accounts for the vast majority of AD risk
and AD pathology. Neurobiol Aging 25(5):641–650
Rademakers R, Cruts M, Dermaut B et al (2002) Tau negative frontal lobe dementia at 17q21:
significant finemapping of the candidate region to a 4.8 cM interval. Mol Psychiatry
7:1064–1074
Rademakers R, Cruts M, van Broeckhoven C (2004) The role of tau (MAPT) in frontotemporal
dementia and related tauopathies. Hum Mutat 24(4):277–295
630 D. Galimberti and E. Scarpini
Reif A, Herterich S, Strobel A et al (2006) A neuronal nitric oxide synthase (NOS-I) haplotype
associated with schizophrenia modifies prefrontal cortex function. Mol Psychiatry
11(3):286–300
Reinikainen KJ, Soininen H, Riekkinen PJ (1990) Neurotransmitter changes in Alzheimer’s
disease: implications to diagnostics and therapy. J Neurosci Res 27(4):576–586
Riemenschneider M, Diehl J, Muller U et al (2002) Apolipoprotein E polymorphism in German
patients with frontotemporal degeneration. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 72:639–641
Rocchi A, Micheli D, Ceravolo R et al (2003) Serotoninergic polymorphisms (5-HTTLPR and
5-HT2A): association studies with psychosis in Alzheimer disease. Genet Test 7(4):309–314
Rosen HJ, Hartikainen KM, Jagust W et al (2002) Utility of clinical criteria in differentiating
frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) from AD. Neurology 58:1608–1615
Rosso SM, Kamphorst W, de Graaf B et al (2001) Familial frontotemporal dementia with
ubiquitin-positive inclusions is linked to chromosome 17q21–22. Brain 124:1948–1957
Rovelet-Lecrux A, Hannequin D, Raux G et al (2006) APP locus duplication causes autosomal
dominant early-onset Alzheimer disease with cerebral amyloid angiopathy. Nat Genet
38:24–26
Sánchez-Guerra M, Combarros O, Alvarez-Arcaya A et al (2001) The Glu298Asp polymorphism
in the NOS3 gene is not associated with sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry 70:566–567
Sato N, Ueki A, Ueno H et al (2009) Dopamine D3 receptor gene polymorphism influences on
behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) in mild dementia of Alzheimer’s
type. J Alzheimers Dis 17(2):441–448
Saur D, Vanderwinden JM, Seidler B et al (2004) Single-nucleotide promoter polymorphism
alters transcription of neuronal nitric oxide synthase exon 1c in infantile hypertrophic pyloric
stenosis. Proc Natl Acad Sci 101(6):1662–1667
Scarpini E, Galimberti D, Guidi I et al (2006) Progressive, isolated language disturbance: its
significance in a 65-year-old-man. A case report with implications for treatment and review of
literature. J Neurol Sci 240(1–2):45–51
Seshadri S, Beiser A, Selhub J et al (2002) Plasma homocysteine as a risk factor for dementia and
Alzheimer’s disease. New Engl J Med 346:476-483
Sherrington R, Rogaev EI, Liang Y et al (1995) Cloning of a gene bearing missense mutations in
early-onset familial Alzheimer’s disease. Nature 375(6534):754–760
Short RA, Graff-Radford NR, Adamson J et al (2002) Differences in tau and apolipoprotein E
polymorphism frequencies in sporadic frontotemporal lobar degeneration syndromes. Arch
Neurol 59:611–615
Skibinski G, Parkinson NJ, Brown JM et al (2005) Mutations in the endosomal ESCRTIII-
complex subunit CHMP2B in frontotemporal dementia. Nat Genet 37:806–808
Snowden JS, Neary D, Mann DM (2002) Frontotemporal dementia. Br J Psychiatry 180:140–143
Snowden JS, Pickering-Brown SM, Mackenzie IR et al (2006) Progranulin gene mutations
associated with frontotemporal dementia and progressive non-fluent aphasia. Brain
129:3091–3102
Spillantini MG, Murrell JR, Goedert M et al (1998) Mutation in the tau gene in familial multiple
system tauopathy with presenile dementia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:7737–7741
Srinivasan R, Davidson Y, Gibbons L et al (2006) The apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele
selectively increases the risk of frontotemporal lobar degeneration in males. J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatry 77:154–158
Steiner H, Romig H, Grim MG et al (1999) The biological and pathological function of the
presenilin-1 dExon 9 mutation is independent of its defect to undergo proteolytic processing.
J Biol Chem 274:7615–7618
Stevens M, van Duijn CM, de Knijff P et al (1997) Apolipoprotein E gene and sporadic frontal
lobe dementia. Neurology 48:1526–1529
Sukonick DL, Pollock BG, Sweet RA et al (2001) The 5-HTTPR*S/*L polymorphism and
aggressive behavior in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 58(9):1425–1428
Behavioral Genetics of Neurodegenerative Disorders 631
Sweet RA, Pollock BG, Sukonick DL et al (2001) The 5-HTTPR polymorphism confers liability
to a combined phenotype of psychotic and aggressive behavior in Alzheimer disease.
Int Psychogeriatr 13(4):401–409
Tanzi RE, Gusella JF et al (1987) Amyloid beta protein gene: cDNA, mRNA distribution, and
genetic linkage near the Alzheimer locus. Science 235(4791):880–884
Tedde A, Nacmias B, Cellini E et al (2002) Lack of association between NOS3 polymorphism
and Italian sporadic and familial Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol 249:110–111
Ueki A, Ueno H, Sato N, Shinjo H, Morita Y (2007) Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism
and BPSD in mild Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 12(3):245–253
van der Zee J, Rademakers R, Engelborghs S et al (2006) A Belgian ancestral haplotype harbours
a highly prevalent mutation for 17q21-linked tau-negative FTLD. Brain 129:841–852
Vance C, Al-Chalabi A, Ruddy D et al (2006) Familial amyotrophic lateral sclerosis with
frontotemporal dementia is linked to a locus on chromosome 9p13.2–21.3. Brain 129:868–876
Venturelli E, Galimberti D, Fenoglio C et al (2006) Candidate gene analysis of IP-10 gene in
patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett 404(1–2):217–221
Venturelli E, Galimberti D, Lovati C et al (2005) The T-786C NOS3 polymorphism
in Alzheimer’s disease: association and influence on gene expression. Neurosci Lett 382(3):
300–303
Venturelli E, Villa C, Fenoglio C et al (2009) The NOS3 G894T (Glu298Asp) polymorphism is a
risk factor for frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Eur J Neurol 16(1):37–42
Venturelli E, Villa C, Scarpini E et al (2008) Neuronal nitric oxide synthase C276T
polymorphism increases the risk for frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Eur J Neurol
15(1):77–81
Verpillat P, Camuzat A, Hannequin D et al (2002) Apolipoprotein E gene in frontotemporal
dementia: an association study and meta-analysis. Eur J Hum Genet 10:399–405
Wang Y, Newton DC, Robb GB et al (1999) RNA diversity has profound effects on the
translation of neuronal nitric oxide synthase. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96(21):121505–12155
Wilhelmsen KC, Lynch T, Pavlou E et al (1994) Localization of disinhibition–dementia–
parkinsonism–amyotrophy complex to 17q21–22. Am J Hum Genet 55:1159–1165
Yu CE, Bird TD, Bekris LM et al (2010) The spectrum of mutations in progranulin: a
collaborative study screening 545 cases of neurodegeneration. Arch Neurol 67(2):161–170
Zhu J, Nathan C, Jin W et al (2002) Conversion of proepithelin to epithelins: roles of SLPI and
elastase in host defence and wound repair. Cell 111:867–878
Index
R
Recombinant Inbred Lines, 29 T
Reelin, 258 Tau, 10
Relaxin, 273 Tauopathies, 10
Retroviruses, 7 TBX1 / GNB1L, 431
Reverse genetic, 6 TCF4, 260
Reward, 364, 371–376, 379, 381, 382 Testosterone, 368–370
RNA-interference, or RNAi, 7 Tetracycline transactivator, 8
Tetraploidy, 9
Tilling, 6
S Trace amines, 274
Schizophrenia, 12, 210–223, 234, 237, 238, Trait, 81
239, 345, 354, 429 Transgenic technologies, 7
Screen, 255 Translational, 255
Sea slug, 4 Transposons, 7
Selective breeding, 140, 141, 143, 161 Twin studies, 538
Sensorimotor gating, 252 Tyrosine hydroxylase, 10
Sequencing technologies, 322
Serotonin transporter, 60, 504, 505, 507, 512,
520, 522, 525 V
Serotonin turnover, 67 Variable number tandem repeat, 504, 505,
Serotonin, 66, 81, 84, 267, 364–367, 441, 446, 507, 539
449, 454, 457 Vasopressin, 271
Single Ventral striatum, 364, 371
SMARCA2, 260 VIVO-morpholino, 7
SNPs, 296 Vulnerability, 257, 296
SP4, 301
Spatial learning, 16
Spectrum disorders, 12 Z
Spine, 284 Zinc finger nucleases, 8
Startle magnitude, 275 ZNF804A, 434
Startle, 252
States, 81