Doyen 2017
Doyen 2017
Cognitive Development
a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t
Article history: Many studies have shown phonological awareness to be a predictor of reading and spelling
Received 9 March 2016 acquisition, but arguments that motor performance and manual laterality may also be pre-
Received in revised form dictors of literacy are much more controversial. We examined the links between manual
20 November 2016
performance, degree of laterality (absolute difference between the two hands) and literacy
Accepted 14 February 2017
in a group of 73 pupils with typical development observed at the end of kindergarten and
Available online 28 February 2017
in the middle of Grade 1. Phonological awareness and hand skill were assessed in kinder-
garten. Reading comprehension, reading, spelling and graphomotor skills were assessed in
Keywords:
Grade 1. Phonological awareness was confirmed to be a strong predictor for reading and
Literacy
Handedness spelling. Higher performance levels in reading and spelling were found to be associated
Laterality degree with a weaker degree of laterality and with faster peg-moving by the non-dominant hand.
Manual performance These results support the hypothesis that manual motor skills have an impact on spelling
Non-dominant hand and reading acquisition.
© 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
The hypothesis that manual asymmetry and cognitive abilities are related dates back a long time in psychology. A dis-
proportionately large number of left- or non-right handers was observed in people with an intellectual disability at the
beginning of the last century (Gordon, 1921) and was later confirmed for known (e.g. Bishop, 1990a; Carlier et al., 2011)
or unknown etiologies (Papadatou-Pastou & Tomprou, 2015). The putative relationship between laterality and cognitive
ability in typically developing people then stood as an interesting path to investigate. Handedness is one of the most striking
forms of asymmetry and is found across the world, even though the percentage of left-handers may differ from one culture
to another (Bryden, 1982; Perelle & Ehrman, 1994; Scharoun & Bryden, 2014). There is an ongoing debate on the cogni-
tive advantages or disadvantages for left- or right-handedness. Many studies have focused on general cognitive ability (e.g.
Nicholls, Chapman, Loetscher, & Grimshaw, 2010; see Papadatou-Pastou & Tomprou, 2015 for a meta-analysis), or on more
specific skills such as verbal and spatial skills (Somers, Shields, Boks, Kahn, & Sommer, 2015) but few studies have focused
on reading skills (Annett & Manning, 1990; Palmer & Corballis, 1996).
∗ Corresponding author at: 72 rue du Faubourg de Bourgogne, 45044 Orléans cedex 1, France.
E-mail addresses: anne-lise.doyen@univ-orleans.fr (A.-L. Doyen), eric.lambert@univ-poitiers.fr (E. Lambert), florence.dumas@unimes.fr (F. Dumas),
michele.carlier@univ-amu.fr (M. Carlier).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.011
0885-2014/© 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
A.-L. Doyen et al. / Cognitive Development 43 (2017) 80–90 81
Our study was designed to investigate the influence of two complementary aspects of motor behavior − manual perfor-
mance for each of the two hands and level of between-hand asymmetry − on literacy acquisition in typically developing
children, controlling for phonological skills. Previous studies have reported handedness as a discontinuous trait (left- vs.
right-handers, and sometimes left- vs. mixed-handers), which has low sensitivity when screening for individual differences.
For the present study, we chose to assess hand performance (hand skill), giving the possibility of using continuous traits and
regardless of the direction of laterality.
The main predictor of literacy acquisition is clearly phonological awareness (i.e. the ability to perceive, identify and
manipulate sounds of spoken language). The link between phonological awareness, as measured in kindergarten, and reading
and spelling skills, as measured subsequently, has been recognized for a number of years now (see Melby-Lervåg, Lyster, &
Hulme, 2012, for a meta-analysis), and the effect on reading has been confirmed via assessments up to the age of 14 (Roman,
Kirby, Parrila, Wade-Woolley, & Deacon, 2009). This link may be universal, as researchers have observed it in many different
languages [e.g. in French (Casalis, Deacon, & Pacton, 2011), in Dutch (de Graaff, Hasselman, Verhoeven, & Bosman, 2011),
and in non-Roman alphabet languages such as Chinese (Ziegler, Tan, Perry, & Montant, 2000)]. Furthermore, some studies
have reported the link between letter knowledge (name and sound) and reading acquisition (Leppänen, Aunola, Niemi, &
Nurmi, 2008; Shatil, Share, & Levin, 2000), including findings for different languages, showing that letter knowledge helps
children grasp the alphabetic principle (in English, McBride-Chang, 1999; in Hebrew, Levin, Shatil-Carmon, & Asif-Rave,
2006; in French, Labat, Ecalle, & Magnan, 2010; in Portuguese, Cardoso-Martins, Mesquita, & Ehri, 2011).
In addition to these phonological predictors, it has been found that motor and graphomotor skills can be related to
literacy acquisition, and some studies have hypothesized that there is a relation between literacy acquisition and fine motor
abilities. Delayed fine motor skill acquisition was indeed found to correlate with language impairment among kindergarten-
age children (Adi-Japha, Strulovich-Schwarz, & Julius, 2011). There is also a comorbidity between dyslexia and dyspraxia
(Kirby & Sugden, 2007). Haslum and Miles (2007) showed, in a sample of 12,950 10-year-old students, that 51.7% of severely
dyslexic participants (2% of the total) presented motor difficulties. In typical children, a positive link was observed between
handwriting performance and the ability to learn to read (Gentaz, Colé, & Bara, 2003). Berninger et al. (2006) found that
early training in handwriting (with orthographic and/or motor activities) improved not only handwriting skills but also
word reading. Such training is more effective with handwriting than with typing, showing that practice in writing letters by
hand helps children recognize them (Longcamp, Zerbato-Poudou, & Velay, 2005). This difference between handwriting and
typing may be attributable to activation of cerebral zones involved in motor activity when recognizing letters. Functional MRI
scanning of four- to five-year-old children showed that after writing by hand, the “reading circuit” was activated during letter
perception, but this was not the case after single-key typing or tracing (James & Engelhardt, 2012). The authors concluded
that the experience of handwriting might facilitate reading acquisition in children.
Handwriting, by definition, is a one-hand activity performed by the dominant hand (i.e. a lateralized motor experience
involving the same brain lateralization as for letter recognition). Functional MRI studies have shown that the same region
of the brain is activated for letter recognition and writing letters by hand: the left ventral premotor cortex for right-handed
persons, and the right ventral premotor cortex for left-handed persons (Longcamp, Anton, Roth, & Velay, 2005). The phe-
nomenon of manual asymmetry raises the question of the different roles of the dominant and non-dominant hands in
cognitive development and, in the present case, in literacy acquisition. Data on the magnitude of the difference between
the two hands suggest that it may be the weakness of the non-dominant hand rather than the strength of the dominant
hand that is relevant. Bishop (1984, 1990b) and Hill and Bishop (1998) focused on the role of the non-dominant hand when
investigating left-handedness associated with clumsiness, finding, for example, that children with poor non-dominant hand
scores made more mispronunciations in sentence repetition tasks (Bishop, 1990b). Corriveau and Goswami (2009) assessed
hand dominance using a peg-moving task − the task consists of transferring 10 dowelling pegs from the furthest row of
10 holes to the nearest row of 10 holes as fast as possible. They observed that children with specific language impairment
had lower performance levels with their non-dominant hand compared to age-matched children, but found no significant
difference for dominant hand performance. In addition, non-dominant hand performance explained a significant part of
the variance of one of the two phonological tasks (phoneme deletion, but not for reading and spelling scores). This finding
supports the hypothesis of a link between the performance of the non-dominant hand and some language skills.
Annett’s theory of handedness is based on asymmetry in manual activities, and one of the key assumptions was that being
moderately right-handed (moderate-to-weak difference between the right hand and left hand) could maximize cognitive
advantages, in particular for phonological processes (Annett, 1985; Smythe and Annett, 2006). Crow, unlike Annett, worked
on the hypothesis that weakly lateralized children would be poorer readers (Crow, Crow, Done, & Leask, 1998). The data
supported the hypothesis of Crow et al., as participants with the lowest scores on the cognitive tasks recorded equal hand
skills (i.e. were not lateralized). Mayringer and Wimmer (2002) failed to replicate the results of Crow’s team and found
no association between relative hand skill scores (left-hand minus right-hand difference) and reading and spelling; their
study did not support Annett’s hypothesis either. In contrast, Nettle (2003), using a multiple regression model, observed
that average cognitive ability increased monotonically with increasing strength of laterality (i.e. larger difference between
the right and left hand). Unfortunately Nettle (2003) did not include any data on literacy acquisition. The inconsistency
between these authors’ findings may be explained, at least partially, by the different cognitive and laterality tasks used and
differences in the characteristics of the participants (age, sex and proportion of left-handers). Thus, the debate on the impact
of the non-dominant hand and relative hand skill on literacy acquisition continues.
82 A.-L. Doyen et al. / Cognitive Development 43 (2017) 80–90
There are very few studies investigating motor abilities and language development in typical populations, and even fewer
that have endeavored to establish a predictive link between motor abilities and written language learning. In most studies
that have found an association between manual laterality and literacy acquisition (including Annett’s), it was impossible to
distinguish hand dominance or the degree of laterality (see Doyen & Carlier, 2002 for a discussion of laterality indices). To
distinguish the two parameters, the absolute difference between the two hands − which we call the “degree of laterality”
thereafter − must be measured. The degree of laterality has not been widely used as a dependent variable, even though it
has been reported as a relevant parameter (Bryden, 1982; Carlier, Beau, Marchaland, & Michel, 1994; Doyen, Dufour, Caroff,
Cherfouh, & Carlier, 2008).
A review of the literature on literacy acquisition linked to motor skills shows that manual performance may be a predictor
of literacy acquisition, but most of the experimental procedures were not designed to test long-term prediction. Dellatolas
et al.’s longitudinal study (2003) covered manual performance, manual laterality, and cognitive tasks. They found a link
between manual motor skill and language learning, but reading was not tested. In the studies cited above, motor abilities
and degree of laterality are not included in the experimental design.
The main goal of our study was to examine the contribution of manual performance and of degree of laterality as measured
in kindergarten for predicting literacy acquisition in Grade 1, controlling for age and phonological awareness. Putative and
complementary predictors of literacy acquisition were assessed. The first factor is manual performance, the hypothesis
being that the manual performance of kindergartners could be linked to their reading and spelling skills, as measured in
Grade 1; the underlying argument is that the motor activity of handwriting has an effect on letter recognition (Longcamp,
Zerbato-Poudou, & Velay, 2005), independently of phonological abilities. The second hypothesis is that there is a link between
manual performance and literacy acquisition, as determined by dominant or non-dominant hand performance, rather than
by overall hand performance (both hands). Previous studies in atypical populations found the non-dominant hand to play
a role in literacy acquisition (Bishop, 1984; Dellatolas, Filho, Souza, Nunes, & Braga, 2005), but little is known of typically
developing children. The third hypothesis is that the difference in abilities between the dominant and non-dominant hands
is a predictor of literacy acquisition. Degree of laterality may be linked to literacy, and more specifically to the alphabetic
principle.
The three hypotheses were tested in our study from kindergarten to Grade 1. Phonological and motor predictors were
measured in kindergarten, whereas literacy skills were assessed in Grade 1. Statistical analyses measured and dissociated
the influence of four potential predictors of literacy acquisition: overall manual performance, dominant hand performance,
non-dominant hand performance and degree of laterality.
2.1. Participants
A cohort of 86 French-speaking children (44 girls and 42 boys) was recruited from six kindergarten classes in three public
schools in the area in and around Orléans in France. For the follow-up (Grade 1), the cohort included 73 children (36 girls
and 37 boys). At the start of the study, the ages ranged from 5 years 5 months to 6 years 5 months (mean age = 6 years 0
months). All were typically developing children following the mainstream school curriculum, enrolled in the last year of
kindergarten and in Grade 1. There was no selection criterion. All children in the classroom the day of the assessment were
enrolled if the parents had provided prior written consent. In Grade 1, 13 pupils were excluded from the study as they were
absent the day of the second assessment.
Manual preference was assessed via two exercises − handling a toy hammer and drawing with a pencil. The child was
asked to perform physical movements using the tools. Previous data on a French cohort had shown that the hand used for
the hammer was a reliable indicator of manual laterality (Doyen, Duquenne, Nuques, & Carlier, 2001). The cohort had 10.5%
left-handers for the hammer and slightly fewer for drawing with a pencil (9.3%), a proportion to be expected in the French
population.
2.2. Assessments
The authors tested and collected the data at the schools. Phonological awareness skills and manual laterality were indi-
vidually assessed in June, at the end of the kindergarten school year (kindergarten assessments). In February of the following
year (at the beginning of the second semester of Grade 1), reading (including reading comprehension and decoding skills),
spelling and graphomotor performance were assessed; some tests were performed on the individual student, and others
were performed using half-class groups (Grade 1 assessments).
(1) Rhyme matching (eight items). A word is given and the child has to recognize another word with the same ending,
choosing from a list of three words [e.g. word 1 is sapin (fir tree), and the possible answers are bonbon (candy), radio
(radio) and marin (sailor)].
(2) Syllable segmentation (eight items). A two-syllable word is given and the child has to drop the first or last syllable,
repeating only one syllable following the verbal instructions (e.g. for the word marteau (hammer), the child has to
pronounce the second syllable, without saying “mar”).
(3) Phoneme recognition (eight items). A list of four words is read (only three have the same initial phoneme), and the child
has to recognize the one word with a different beginning [e.g. drapeau (flag), tigre (tiger), dent (tooth), doigt (finger)].
A total score for phonological awareness was calculated, with equal weight for each subtest.
Manual laterality was assessed using the peg-moving task (Annett, 1985, 2002) that had been successfully used with
French populations (see Dellatolas et al., 2003; Doyen & Carlier, 2002; Doyen et al., 2008). We followed the standard procedure
(with board and pegs generously provided by Marian Annett). The participants stood facing the long side of the board and
were asked to move 10 pegs as fast as they could from the back row to the front row, one by one, using only one hand for
each trial. Participants chose which hand to begin with. Six trials were conducted, alternating hands for each trial. The score
for a trial was the time taken to move the 10 pegs (measured in seconds). Four indices were calculated. Manual performance
was the sum of the mean time of the three trials of each hand. Dominant hand was the mean of the three trials moving the
10 pegs with the faster hand (in s). Non-dominant hand was the mean of the three trials taken to move the 10 pegs with the
slower hand (in s). Finally, the degree of laterality measured the absolute difference of performance between the two hands
and was calculated as follows: the absolute difference between the fastest trial of the dominant hand and the fastest trial of
the non-dominant hand. The goal was to assess the degree of laterality at the best performance level tested and to eliminate
within-child variability.
Scores can be described as follows: the faster the participant, the lower the values for manual performance, the dominant
hand and the non-dominant hand; and the less lateralized the participant, the lower the value of the degree of laterality.
Reliability was estimated using Cronbach’s alpha: right hand = 0.91, left hand = 0.93.
In the present study, characteristics of the children were confounded with classroom characteristics, as the individuals
were not randomly assigned to groups. Such multilevel data structures should be analyzed with multilevel random coefficient
models (MRCM; see Kreft & de Leeuw, 1998; Nezlek, 2008; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). These techniques provide more accu-
rate parameter estimates than do comparable estimates provided by within-person correlations or regression coefficients
(see Nezlek, 2008 for a demonstration). The first step in any MRCM analysis consists of running what is called a null or totally
unconditional model in which no predictors are specified at either level of analysis. Such models provide useful information
about how much variation in each of the outcomes lies within and between groups. Here, the computer program HLM 6
(Raudenbush, Bryk, Cheong, Congdon, & Du Toit, 2004) was used to test six unconditional models, one for each outcome
variable. The intra-class correlation coefficients are reported in Table 1 with descriptive statistics. Chi-square tests showed
84 A.-L. Doyen et al. / Cognitive Development 43 (2017) 80–90
Table 1
Estimation of unconditional model and descriptive statistics for each outcome variable.
Table 2
Pearson intercorrelations between the predictor variables and the outcome variables (N = 73).
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
1. Phonological awareness 1
2. Age −0.18 1
3. Manual performance −0.23* 0.13 1
4. Dominant hand −0.23* 0.12 0.95*** 1
5. Non-dominant hand −0.22* 0.12 0.97*** 0.84*** 1
6. Degree of laterality −0.09 0.01 0.49*** 0.24* 0.64*** 1
7. Reading comprehension 0.61*** −0.05 −0.24* −0.23* −0.24* −0.26* 1
8. Word reading 0.59*** 0.14 −0.27* −0.26* −0.26* −0.24* 0.83*** 1
9. Pseudoword reading 0.46*** 0.10 −0.24* −0.24* −0.23 −0.19 0.76*** 0.85*** 1
10. Word spelling 0.56*** 0.16 −0.25* −0.30** −0.20 −0.09 0.57*** 0.64*** 0.67*** 1
11. Pseudoword spelling 0.52*** −0.04 −0.26* −0.23 −0.27* −0.30** 0.73*** 0.79*** 0.81*** 0.52*** 1
12. Symbol copying −0.01 0.04 −0.43*** −0.42*** −0.41*** −0.21 0.11 0.16 0.12 0.08 0.01
that the proportion of the between-class variance was never statistically significant (ns, ps > 0.11), except for symbol copying
(p < 0.01). Eighteen percent of the total variance was due to between-class differences for this last outcome, whereas no more
than 6% of the total variance was explained by between-class differences for the other outcomes. In other words, classes in
the present sample did not differ substantially according to achievement levels, at least in reading and spelling. Therefore,
modeling variance at level 2 was not really required, and since we had only six classes, we decided to develop conditional
models in which predictors were only specified at Level 1 (child-level).
We computed five separate MRCM analyses (Model 1 through Model 5) to test each motor predictor independently. The
same set of MRCM analyses (Model 1 through Model 5) was repeated for each outcome variable. The first set of analyses
concerned reading comprehension. The second set of analyses to the sixth set concerned word reading, pseudoword reading,
word spelling, pseudoword spelling and symbol copying, respectively (see Table 3). The analyses focused on child-level
(within-class) relationships. The global score of phonological awareness and age were systematically entered as predictors
in each conditional model and alone in the first one (Model 1). In the early stages of reading, age can explain differences
in acquisition of the alphabetic principle (Bentin, Hammer, & Cahan, 1991). In France, the difference in age within one
kindergarten class is up to 11 months, which, at the age of five, means a large difference in development, particularly for
motor skills. The age influence was therefore controlled for in the statistical analyses. Preliminary analyses were conducted
examining phonological awareness, sex and age as predictors of reading and spelling outcomes. Sex was never significant
except for pseudoword spelling (ˇ = −0.33, t = −2.04, p = 0.045), with boys outperforming girls. However, this sex effect did
not remain significant when one or the other of the motor predictors were used in the same model. We decided not to use
the variable sex in the subsequent analyses.
Model 2 introduced manual performance, Model 3 the dominant hand, Model 4 the non-dominant hand and Model 5 the
degree of laterality. All variables were standardized (z scoring) to have M = 0, SD = 1 across the entire sample (see Raudenbush
& Bryk, 2002) prior to the analysis, which is an advantage for interpretation.
3. Results
Mean scores (and SD) of the predictor variables in kindergarten were as follows: phonological awareness: 13.45 (4.45);
manual performance: 29.91 (4.24); dominant hand: 14.11 (1.93); non-dominant hand: 15.80 (2.49); degree of laterality:
1.56 (1.20). Mean scores in the peg-moving task were slightly lower (faster) in our sample of participants than were those
Annett (1985, p. 419) obtained for six-year-old British children.
Table 2 reports the Pearson correlations. As could be expected, the three correlations between manual performance,
dominant hand, and non-dominant were very high (rs ≥ 0.84). Correlations between each of these three variables and the
degree of laterality were lower (.49, 0.24, and 0.64, respectively, ps ≤ 0.02). The degree of laterality did not correlate sig-
nificantly with phonological awareness (r = −0.09, p = 0.42, ns), whereas the three others motor predictors did. A significant
and negative correlation was obtained between phonological awareness and each of these three variables (-0.23, −0.23,
Table 3
Summary of the MRCM analyses (Model 1 through Model 5) for each outcome variables.
Reading comprehension Word reading Pseudoword reading Word spelling Pseudoword spelling Symbol copying
Fixed effect ˇ t ˇ t ˇ t ˇ t ˇ t ˇ t
Model 1
Intercept 0.02 0.25 −0.01 −0.06 0.01 0.06 0.02 0.21 0.02 0.16 −0.06 −0.33
Model 2
Intercept 0.03 0.31 0.00 0.02 −0.01 −0.14 0.03 0.27 0.03 0.23 −0.03 −0.18
Manual performance −0.13 −1.54 −0.15 −1.79 −0.30 −1.85 −0.15** −2.82 −0.17 −1.47 −0.38*** −3.97
Phonological awareness 0.59*** 11.21 0.59*** 7.32 .43*** 7.48 0.54*** 15.93 0.50*** 6.24 −0.03 −0.57
Age −0.01 −0.10 0.16* 2.16 0.15 1.51 .21** 2.88 −0.02 −0.22 0.05 1.12
Model 3
Intercept 0.03 0.27 −0.00 −0.03 −0.02 −0.16 0.03 0.25 0.02 0.18 −0.05 −0.27
Dominant hand −0.10 −1.27 −0.13 −1.54 −0.27 −1.86 −0.20** −2.91 −0.12 −0.95 −0.37*** −4.45
Phonological awarenes .60*** 11.76 0.59*** 7.38 .42*** 6.36 .52*** 18.07 0.51*** 6.61 −0.04 −0.56
Age −0.01 −0.09 0.17* 2.18 0.15 1.46 .22** 3.02 −0.02 −0.20 0.05 1.19
Model 4
Intercept 0.03 0.35 0.01 0.05 0.02 0.18 0.03 0.27 0.03 0.28 −0.02 −0.13
Non-dominant hand −0.14 −1.73 −0.15* −2.08 −0.16 −1.26 −0.11* −2.14 −0.20* −2.07 −0.36*** −3.79
Phonological awareness .60*** 11.49 .60*** 7.55 .45*** 10 .55*** 13.61 0.50*** 6.43 −0.02 −0.34
Age −0.01 −0.13 .16* 2.13 0.14 1.36 .21** 2.86 −0.02 −0.26 0.04 0.96
Model 5
Intercept 0.04 0.39 0.01 0.08 0.02 0.20 0.03 0.25 0.04 0.34 −0.05 −0.25
Degree of laterality −0.21** −3.35 −0.16** −3.23 −0.14* −1.99 −0.03 −0.46 −0.27*** −4.89 −0.15 −1.64
Phonological awareness .60*** 11.20 .61*** 7.93 .46*** 10.38 .58*** 13.13 0.51*** 7.12 0.04 0.86
Age −0.03 −0.31 .16* 2.13 0.13 1.32 .22* 2.97 −0.04 −0.51 0.03 0.59
85
86 A.-L. Doyen et al. / Cognitive Development 43 (2017) 80–90
−0.22, respectively, ps ≤ 0.05). All correlations between manual predictors and literacy scores were significant and negative:
the faster the fine motor skills, the higher the literacy skills. The correlation between symbol copying and phonological
awareness was almost null (r = −0.01, p = 0.93, ns).
Table 3 summarizes the set of MRCM analyses (Model 1 through Model 5) that were repeated for each outcome. Model
1 is comprised of only the phonological awareness and age variables. Phonological awareness was a positive and significant
predictor of all five reading and spelling variables, with a coefficient ranging from 0.48 for pseudoword reading to 0.63 for
word reading. For symbol copying, neither phonological awareness nor age were significant predictors. Age was a positive
and significant predictor, but only for word spelling (ˇ = 0.21) and word reading (ˇ = 0.16).
Because all variables were standardized in the same metric (z scoring) before analysis, the coefficients from MRCM
analyses are to be interpreted like the standardized coefficients from traditional ordinary-least-squares regression analyses.
The positive effect of phonological awareness remained strong and significant across all models with reading and
spelling outcome variables (i.e. whatever motor predictor was added to the model − manual performance, dominant hand,
non-dominant hand or degree of laterality). Models 2–5 show that when the motor predictors reached (or approached)
significance, they were always negatively associated with reading and spelling achievement variables. Negative associations
can be described as follows: the slower the participant, the longer the duration of motor task execution and the weaker the
literacy scores. The effects of manual performance (Model 2) and dominant hand (Model 3) were statistically significant only
for word spelling and symbol copying. The non-dominant hand (Model 4) significantly predicted four of six achievement
variables (word reading, word spelling, pseudoword spelling, and symbol copying), with a coefficient ranging from −0.11 to
−0.36. Degree of laterality (Model 5) was also a good predictor for four of six achievement variables (reading comprehension,
word reading, pseudoword reading and pseudoword spelling), with a coefficient ranging from −0.14 to −0.27.
4. Discussion
The present study was designed to test four complementary but non-exclusive fine motor predictors of literacy acquisi-
tion in a developmental view. The study does not follow a repeated-measure longitudinal design, but rather, it documents a
predictive relation between manual performance and literacy acquisition in a sample of typically developing children. The
motor skills tested in kindergarten were: manual performance, dominant hand performance, non-dominant hand perfor-
mance, and absolute degree of laterality. Phonological awareness was also assessed in kindergarten. Literacy was assessed
in Grade 1, with reading comprehension and reading and spelling of words and pseudowords. Symbol copying was used in
Grade 1 to assess graphomotor abilities implied in handwriting tasks.
As might be expected, phonological skills were the strongest predictor of reading and spelling acquisition. Our data were
in line with past research studies from the 1970s that ranked phonological awareness as one of the best predictors of literacy
achievement (Bus & van IJzendoorn, 1999; Melby-Lervåg et al., 2012). However, certain motor variables are also predictors
of literacy acquisition, independently of phonological skills. Our study investigated motor variables that were often pooled
in previous studies, testing them separately.
The first hypothesis was that manual performance of kindergartners could be linked to their reading and spelling skills
when assessed later in Grade 1. Manual performance proved to be a significant predictor for word spelling only. The hypoth-
esis could therefore only be partially validated: the ability of both hands did not significantly predict literacy acquisition in
the next school year, except for the word spelling test, with a no significant link between manual performance and the three
reading tasks. This result may seem surprising given the studies quoted above with data on embodied cognition showing
that handwriting helps children memorize the shape of letters (Longcamp, Zerbato-Poudou, & Velay, 2005) and helps with
reading acquisition (James & Engelhardt, 2012). Studies have reported that graphomotor training enhanced letter recogni-
tion and pseudoword reading (Gentaz et al., 2003; Longcamp, Zerbato-Poudou, & Velay, 2005). It could be that a high level of
manual proficiency can help a child to learn to write letters, while it is not a positive factor for reading unless there is specific
training, such as haptic training (Gentaz et al., 2003), visuomotor training (Vinter & Chartrel, 2010) or a motor strategy to
distinguish between mirror letters (Dehaene, 2010).
In our study, manual performance predicted word spelling, an activity involving not only orthographical skills but also
graphomotor processing, as the pupil has to write the words by hand. One difficulty here is the composite nature of the manual
performance variable that is measured as the mean performance score of the two hands together. The interpretation of a link
between manual performance and word spelling is reinforced by the fact that dominant hand performance presents the same
profile, with the only link being with word spelling. This relation may depend on the level of graphomotor automatization
(Pontart et al., 2013). The dominant hand is usually the writing hand, so with a more efficient dominant hand, graphomotor
processes are easier to be automated, and more cognitive resources can be devoted to spelling processes. Evidence for this
is provided by symbol copying, which was predicted by manual performance and dominant hand performance, the variable
having exactly the same profile as word spelling. Symbol copying and written spelling require graphomotor abilities, and both
exercises are linked to dominant hand proficiency. Spelling also requires phonological skills for orthographic representations.
Word spelling can be explained by phonological awareness, which is not the case for symbol copying. Taken together, these
A.-L. Doyen et al. / Cognitive Development 43 (2017) 80–90 87
observations stand as evidence for phonological skills and manual performance (in particular, dominant hand performance)
to be used as two distinct predictors for spelling achievement.
The literature appears to have very few articles devoted to the role played by the non-dominant hand. Our study analyzed
manual skills separately for each hand, and the data supported the hypothesis that non-dominant hand performance was
a better motor predictor of reading and spelling skills in Grade 1 than was dominant hand performance. Dominant hand
performance was significant for word spelling ability only, while non-dominant hand performance was a significant motor
predictor for three literacy variables (word reading, word spelling and pseudoword spelling). The faster the non-dominant
hand in kindergarten, the greater the literacy achievement in Grade 1, independently of the phonological skills. This is a key
finding for understanding the link between manual motor performances and literacy acquisition. Corriveau and Goswami
(2009) studied a population of children with specific language impairment and found that the non-dominant score in a
pegboard task was a significant source of variation in a phoneme deletion task (but not in reading and spelling tasks).
However, to the best of our knowledge, the impact of the non-dominant hand has not been investigated in a study of literacy
acquisition and motor tasks in typically developing children.
As noted above, strong laterality does not necessarily mean that the dominant hand is proficient, but rather that the non-
dominant hand is weaker (Annett, 1985; Annett & Manning, 1990; Resch et al., 1997). In our study, age variability was smaller
with an age range of one year. In a typical child population with small within-group variability in age, individual differences
are nevertheless the rule. Differential maturational changes can be expected when comparing the motor development of the
non-dominant hand and dominant hand, in particular for the less trained, non-dominant hand. Slowness or clumsiness of the
non-dominant hand as a predictor of poor literacy performance is a key finding. There are now extensive reports describing
dyslexic populations, observing that in addition to the phonological deficit, motor-skill deficits are more prevalent in persons
with dyslexia (Kirby & Sugden, 2007; Nicolson & Fawcett, 2011). The causes of the associated motor deficits are still under
debate. It can be hypothesized that cerebellar involvement in dyslexia affects cerebellar morphology and function (Nicolson
& Fawcett, 2011; Nicolson, Fawcett, & Dean, 2001). The level of cerebellar symmetry has been related to the severity of
the phonological decoding deficit in dyslexia (Sun, Lee, & Kirby, 2010), but Raberger and Wimmer (2003) reported, in a
population of children, that motor deficits in dyslexia related more strongly to co-morbid ADHD problems than to dyslexia
itself. Rae et al. (2002) investigated cerebellar morphology and compared a group of 11 adult males with a history of dyslexia
and a matched-age control group, testing pseudoword reading and Annett’s peg-moving task (using an absolute non-relative
degree of laterality index). A strong negative relationship was found for the entire group for the left/right grey matter volume
ratio in the cerebellum and the difference in time required for peg-moving (left hand minus right hand). Greater differences
between the hands were associated with a larger ratio of left-to-right grey matter in the cerebellum. Unfortunately, the
authors did not present results for each hand or use absolute indices (to describe manual/cerebellar asymmetries).
Our results extend the link between non-dominant hand performance and literacy acquisition to a general child popu-
lation, not just children with specific language impairment. The non-dominant hand develops more slowly (Annett, 1985)
and the effect was confirmed (Bryden & Roy, 2005; Roy, Bryden, & Cavill, 2003). It could be hypothesized that non-dominant
hand performance in young children would be a better indicator of development compared to the dominant hand (Van Mier,
2006). The slowness of the non-dominant hand could be an indicator of developmental delay, with consequences on literacy
acquisition.
Our third hypothesis was that in kindergartners, the absolute degree of laterality (the absolute difference between the
two hands) could be linked to their reading and spelling skills in Grade 1. The degree of laterality proved to be a significant
predictor for four out of five literacy variables (reading comprehension, word reading, pseudoword reading and pseudoword
spelling). The weaker the degree of laterality in kindergarten, the better the literacy achievement in Grade 1. The result seems
counterintuitive because studies showed a link between hemispheric dominance and language acquisition (Bishop, 2013).
However, it is worth mentioning that there was no direct assessment of hemispheric lateralization here. Handedness was
frequently used as a proxy measure of language lateralization. Brain imaging demonstrates that handedness measures cannot
be considered as a reliable indicator of cerebral lateralization for language (Groen, Whitehouse, Badcock, & Bishop, 2013;
Mazoyer et al., 2014).
Comparing our results to the findings of Annett (2002) and Crow et al. (1998), we note that the inclusion/addition of the
“right minus left percent” index to the MRCM analysis (results not presented) was much less predictive of literacy acqui-
sition when compared to the degree of laterality (Model 5): the right minus left percent index was significantly predictive
for pseudoword spelling only. This provides confirmation that the degree of laterality is a more informative variable than is
the relative difference between each hand performance. In our study, the degree of laterality is the second most important
factor in literacy acquisition, after phonological awareness. Furthermore degree did not correlate significantly with phono-
logical awareness (whereas phonological awareness correlated significantly with the three other motor predictors): when
phonological awareness was entered in the MRCM analysis, a significant part of variance remained explained by degree of
88 A.-L. Doyen et al. / Cognitive Development 43 (2017) 80–90
laterality. Weak manual lateralization was associated with better scores for reading and spelling. Our results showed that
this effect does not come from global manual performance.
Our data are in line with Annett’s prediction (2002) that a weak degree of laterality could be an indicator of easier
acquisition of reading and spelling processes. But neither of the two better predictors (non-dominant hand and degree of
laterality) can be said to be totally independent: the weakness of the degree of laterality depended, in part, on the velocity
of the non-dominant hand. Future research should investigate the separate roles of each of these two factors.
The theory of Embodied Cognition hypothesizes that all cognition, including high-level processes like language process,
relies on an internal simulation of the sensory–motor experience, providing a new look at the interplay between language,
motor acts and the motor system (Barsalou, 2010). Several studies have shown a close link between motor skills and cognitive
information processing. For example, the SNARC effect (Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes) can be explained
by finger counting habits during mathematical learning and then indirectly by manual abilities (Fischer & Brugger, 2011).
Future studies should investigate links between manual performance and literacy, and deepen the probable role played by
the degree of laterality as a factor of bimanual coordination in learning and writing learning.
5. Conclusion
Phonological awareness in kindergarten was the strongest predictor of reading and spelling acquisition, but other pre-
dictors of motor development can be put forward, specifically the performance of the non-dominant hand and the degree
of laterality. In our study, the two indices significantly predicted several aspects of literacy acquisition, as observed from
one school year to the next. Our findings can suggest educational applications. The assessment of basic skills needed to
acquire written language in kindergarten is very useful: particularly phonological skills for screening children at risk of
language disorders. Initial language assessments in school should be combined with motor assessments to improve the
diagnosis, covering manual skills and specifically the degree of laterality and non-dominant hand performance. Such motor
assessments require only limited time and equipment. Future research will need to explore possible links between poor
motor development and reading disabilities. Our study is limited by the relatively small sample size, even though it is in
the range of most studies published. The results should be extended to other populations and using a longitudinal design
before considering the possibility of training the non-dominant hand, as well as the dominant hand (Berninger et al., 2006),
to facilitate literacy acquisition in young children.
Acknowledgements
We wish to thank the schoolchildren, their parents, and the teachers who agreed to take part in the study. We are very
grateful to Isabelle Régnier for her support and helpful advices during the revision phase of the manuscript.
References
Adi-Japha, E., Strulovich-Schwarz, O., & Julius, M. (2011). Delayed motor skill acquisition in kindergarten children with language impairment. Research in
Developmental Disabilities, 32, 296–371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.05.005
Annett, M., & Manning, M. (1990). Reading and a balanced polymorphism for laterality and ability. Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 31(4),
511–529. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb00795.x
Annett, M. (1985). Left, right, hand and brain: The right shift theory. London, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Annett, M. (2002). Handedness and brain asymmetry: The right shift theory. Hove, East Sussex, U.K: Psychology Press.
Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded cognition: Past, present, and future. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 716–724.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x
Bentin, S., Hammer, R., & Cahan, S. (1991). The effects of aging and first grade schooling on the development of phonological awareness. Psychological
Science, 2, 271–274. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991. tb00148.x
Berninger, V. W., Rutberg, J. E., Abbott, R. D., Garcia, N., Anderson-Youngstrom, M., . . . & Fulton, C. (2006). Tier 1 and Tier 2 early intervention for
handwriting and composing. Journal of School Psychology, 44, 3–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2005.12.003
Bishop, D. V. M. (1984). Using non-preferred hand skill to investigate pathological left-handedness in an unselected population. Developmental Medicine &
Child Neurology, 26, 214–226. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1984. tb04434.x
Bishop, D. V. M. (1990a). Handedness and developmental disorder. London, England: Mac Keith Press.
Bishop, D. V. M. (1990b). Handedness, clumsiness and developmental language disorders. Neuropsychologia, 28, 681–690.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(90)90123-6
Bishop, D. V. M. (2013). Cerebral asymmetry and language development: Cause, correlate or consequence? Science, 340(6138), 1230531.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1230531
Bryden, P. J., & Roy, E. A. (2005). Unimanual performance across the age span. Brain and Cognition, 57, 26–29.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.016
Bryden, M. P. (1982). Laterality: Functional asymmetry in the intact brain. New York, U.S.A: Academic Press.
Bus, A. G., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: a meta-analysis of experimental training studies. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 91, 403–414. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.403
Cardoso-Martins, C., Mesquita, T. C. L., & Ehri, L. (2011). Letter names and phonological awareness help children to learn letter-sound relations. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 25–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.12.006
Carlier, M., Beau, J., Marchaland, C., & Michel, F. (1994). Sibling resemblance in two manual laterality tasks. Neuropsychologia, 32, 741–746.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(94)90033-7
Carlier, M., Desplanches, A. G., Philip, N., Stefanini, S., Vicari, S., Volterra, V., & Swillen, A. (2011). Laterality Preference and Cognition: Cross-Syndrome
Comparison of Patients with Trisomy 21 (Down), del7q11.23 (Williams–Beuren) and del22q11.2 (DiGeorge or Velo-Cardio-Facial) Syndromes.
Behavior Genetics, 413–432. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10519-011-9465-2
A.-L. Doyen et al. / Cognitive Development 43 (2017) 80–90 89
Casalis, S., Deacon, S. H., & Pacton, S. (2011). How specific is the connection between morphological awareness and spelling? A study of French children.
Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 499–511. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S014271641100018X
Corriveau, K. H., & Goswami, U. (2009). Rhythmic motor entrainment in children with speech and language impairments: Tapping to the beat. Cortex, 45,
119–130. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2007.09.008
Crow, T. J., Crow, L. R., Done, D. J., & Leask, S. (1998). Relative hand skill predicts academic ability: global deficits at the point of hemispheric indecision.
Neuropsychologia, 36, 1275–1282. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00039-6
De Graaff, S., Hasselman, F., Verhoeven, L., & Bosman, A. M. T. (2011). Phonemic awareness in Dutch kindergartners: effects of task, phoneme position, and
phoneme class. Learning and Instruction, 21, 163–173. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.02.001
Dehaene, S. (2010). Reading in the brain. New York, U.S.A: Penguin Viking.
Dellatolas, G., De Agostini, M., Curt, F., Kremin, H., Letierce, A., Maccario, J., & Lellouch, J. (2003). Manual skill, hand skill asymmetry, and cognitive
performances in young children. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain, and Cognition, 8, 317–338. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650
Dellatolas, G., Filho, G. N., Souza, L., Nunes, L. G., & Braga, L. W. (2005). Manual skill, hand skill asymmetry, and neuropsychological testing in
school-children with spastic cerebral palsy. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 10, 161–182.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13576500442000012
Doyen, A.-L., & Carlier, M. (2002). Measuring handedness: A validation study of the Bishop’s card-reaching test. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and
Cognition, 7, 115–130. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13576500143000140
Doyen, A.-L., Duquenne, V., Nuques, S., & Carlier, M. (2001). What can be learned from a lattice analysis of a laterality questionnaire? Behavior Genetics, 31,
193–207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1010205528113
Doyen, A.-L., Dufour, T., Caroff, X., Cherfouh, A., & Carlier, M. (2008). Hand preference and hand performance: Cross-sectional developmental trends and
family resemblance in degree of laterality. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 13, 179–197.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13576500701764124
Fischer, M. H., & Brugger, P. (2011). When digits help digits: spatial-numerical associations point to finger counting as prime example of embodied
cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(260), 41–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00260
Gentaz, Colé, P., & Bara, F. (2003). Évaluation d’entraînements multisensoriels de préparation à la lecture pour les enfants en grande section de maternelle:
Une étude sur la contribution du système haptique manuel. = Evaluating multisensory preparatory training for reading in kindergarten-age children:
A study on the contribution of the manual haptic mode. L’Année Psychologique, 103, 561–584. http://dx.doi.org/10.3406/psy.2003.29652
Gordon, H. (1921). Left-handedness and mirror writing, especially among defective children. Brain, 43, 313–368.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00216-0
Groen, M. A., Whitehouse, A. J. O., Badcock, N. A., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2013). Association between handedness and cerebral lateralization for language: A
comparison of three measures in children. PLoS One, 8, e64876. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064876
Haslum, M. N., & Miles, T. R. (2007). Motor performance and dyslexia in a national cohort of 10-year-old children. Dyslexia, 13, 257–275.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.350
Hill, E. L., & Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). A reaching test reveals weak hand preference in specific language impairment and developmental co-ordination
disorder. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 3, 295–310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/135765098397160
James, K. H., & Engelhardt, L. (2012). The effects of handwriting experience on functional brain development in pre-literate children. Trends in
Neuroscience and Education, 1, 32–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2012.08.001
Khomsi, A. (1999). LMC-R Epreuve d’évaluation de la compétence en lecture = LMC-R Test for assessing reading skills. Paris: ECPA.
Kirby, A., & Sugden, D. A. (2007). Children with developmental coordination disorders. Journal of Royal Society of Medecine, 100, 182–186.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1258/jrsm.100.4.182
Kreft, I. G. G., & de Leeuw, J. (1998). Introducing multilevel modeling. Newbury Park, CA, U.S.A: Sage Publications.
Labat, H., Ecalle, J., & Magnan, A. (2010). Effet d’entraînements bimodaux à la connaissance des lettres. Étude transversale chez des enfants de trois et cinq
ans. = Effect of two-mode trainings of letter knowledge: A cross-sectional study with 3- and 5-year-old children. Psychologie Française, 55, 113–127.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psfr.2009.12.002
Leppänen, U., Aunola, K., Niemi, P., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2008). Letter knowledge predicts Grade 4 reading fluency and reading comprehension. Learning and
Instruction, 18, 548–564. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.11.004
Levin, I., Shatil-Carmon, S., & Asif-Rave, O. (2006). Learning of letter names and sounds and their contribution to word recognition. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 93, 139–165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.08.002
Longcamp, M., Anton, J.-L., Roth, M., & Velay, J.-L. (2005). Premotor activations in response to visually presented single letters depend on the hand used to
write: A study on left-handers. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1801–1809. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.020
Longcamp, M., Zerbato-Poudou, M.-T., & Velay, J.-L. (2005). The influence of writing practice on letter recognition: A comparison between handwriting
and typing. Acta Psychologica, 119, 67–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2004.10.019
Mayringer, H., & Wimmer, H. (2002). No deficits at the point of hemispheric indecision. Neuropsychologia, 40, 701–704.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00191-9
Mazoyer, B., Zago, L., Jobard, G., Crivello, F., Jolio, M., Perchey, G., . . . & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2014). Gaussian mixture modeling of hemispheric
lateralization for language in a large sample of healthy individuals balanced for handedness. PLoS One, 9(6), e101165.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101165
McBride-Chang, C. (1999). The ABCs of the ABCs: The development of letter-name and letter-sound knowledge. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 285–308.
http://digitalcommons.wayne.edu/mpq/vol45/iss2/7
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S.-A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin,
138, 322–352. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026744
Nettle, D. (2003). Hand laterality and cognitive ability: A multiple regression approach. Brain and Cognition, 52, 390–398.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00187-8
Nezlek, J. B. (2008). An introduction to multilevel modelling for social and personality psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 842–860.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00059.x
Nicholls, M. E. R., Chapman, H. L., Loetscher, T., & Grimshaw, G. M. (2010). The relationship between hand preference, hand performance, and general
cognitive ability. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 585–592. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617710000184
Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2011). Dyslexia, dysgraphia, procedural learning and the cerebellum. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous
System and Behavior, 47, 117–127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.016
Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: The cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences, 24, 508–511.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01896-8
Palmer, R. E., & Corballis, M. C. (1996). Predicting reading ability from handedness measures. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 609–620.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1996. tb02611.x
Papadatou-Pastou, M., & Tomprou, D.-M. (2015). Intelligence and handedness: Meta-analyses of studies on intellectually disabled, typically developing,
and gifted individuals. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 56, 151–165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.017
Perelle, I. B., & Ehrman, L. (1994). An international study of human handedness: The data. Behavior Genetics, 24, 217–227.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01067189
Pontart, V., Bidet-Ildei, C., Lambert, E., Morisset, P., Flouret, L., & Alamargot, D. (2013). Influence of handwriting skills during spelling in primary and lower
secondary grades. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 818. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00818
90 A.-L. Doyen et al. / Cognitive Development 43 (2017) 80–90
Raberger, T., & Wimmer, H. (2003). On the automaticity/cerebellar deficit hypothesis of dyslexia: balancing and continuous rapid naming in dyslexic and
ADHD children. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1493–1497. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00078-2
Rae, C., Harasty, J. A., Dzendrowskyj, T. E., Talcott, J. B., Simpson, J. M., Blamire, A. M., . . . & Stein, J. F. (2002). Cerebellar morphology in developmental
dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1285–1292. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00216-0
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA, U.S.A: Sage.
Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., Cheong, Y. F., Congdon, R. T., & Du Toit, M. (2004). HLM6: Linear and nonlinear modeling. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software
International.
Resch, F., Haffner, J., Parzer, P., Pfueller, U., Strehlow, U., & Zerahn-Hartung, C. (1997). Testing the hypothesis of the relationships between laterality and
ability according to Annett’s right-shift theory: Findings in an epidemiological sample of young adults. British Journal of Psychology, 88, 621–635.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1997. tb02661.x
Roman, A. A., Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. K., Wade-Woolley, L., & Deacon, S. H. (2009). Toward a comprehensive view of the skills involved in word reading in
Grades 4, 6, and 8. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 96–113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.01.004
Roy, E. A., Bryden, P., & Cavill, S. (2003). Hand differences in pegboard performance through development. Brain and Cognition, 53, 315–317.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00133-7
Scharoun, S. M., & Bryden, P. J. (2014). Hand preference, performance abilities, and hand selection in children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 82.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00082
Shatil, E., Share, D. L., & Levin, I. (2000). On the contribution of kindergarten writing to Grade 1 literacy: A longitudinal study in Hebrew. Applied
Psycholinguistics, 21, 1–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400001016
Smythe, P., & Annett, M. (2006). Phonology and handedness in primary school: Predictions of the right shift theory. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 47, 205–212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01463.x
Somers, M., Shields, L. S., Boks, M. P., Kahn, R. S., & Sommer, I. E. (2015). Cognitive benefits of right-handedness: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience &
Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 48–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.003
Sun, Y.-F., Lee, J.-S., & Kirby, R. (2010). Brain imaging findings in dyslexia. Pediatrics and Neonatology, 51, 89–96.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1875-9572(10)60017-4
Van Mier, H. (2006). Developmental differences in drawing performance of the dominant and non-dominant hand in right-handed boys and girls. Human
Movement Science, 25, 657–677. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2006.06.004
Vinter, A., & Chartrel, E. (2010). Effects of different types of learning on handwriting movements in young children. Learning and Instruction, 20, 476–486.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.07.001
Ziegler, J. C., Tan, L. H., Perry, C., & Montant, M. (2000). Phonology matters: the phonological frequency effect in written Chinese. Psychological Science, 11,
234–238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00247