Ce Rebelo
Ce Rebelo
Ce Rebelo
The cerebellum – the region of the vertebrate brain that makes smoothly coor-
dinated movements possible – is a remarkable machine. How it accomplishes its
highly complex feats of coordination has been the subject of much inquiry; one
of the preeminent theories places adaptive control at the forefront.
The Cerebellum and Adaptive Control reinforces the view that the cerebellum
functions as an adaptive control system. That is, it automatically adjusts its output
for such eventualities as temporary or lasting weakness of muscle. This text is
the first to synthesize the substantial body of literature on the subject, combining
the neuroscience of the cerebellum with the science of control theory common to
electrical and computer engineers. Organized into four parts, the book examines
cerebellar anatomy and physiology, cerebellar function, and models and theories,
and ends with a summary and conclusions. The author’s clinical perspective offers a
broader view of cerebellar function beyond the basic neuroscience. An appendix
demonstrates evidence supporting the adaptive control model from a detailed
comparison of the cerebellum with an adaptive signal processor of the author’s
design and construction.
John Barlow’s book is the only up-to-date work on this crucial aspect of cere-
bellar function. Researchers and graduate students of neurophysiology, as well
as electrical and computer engineers, will find this integrated model an insightful
and relevant look at the functioning cerebellum.
john s. barlow
Massachusetts General Hospital
Harvard Medical School
CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS
Cambridge, New York, Melbourne, Madrid, Cape Town, Singapore, São Paulo
Published in the United States of America by Cambridge University Press, New York
www.cambridge.org
Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9780521808422
A catalogue record for this publication is available from the British Library
without whose assistance this work could not have been completed.
It seems likely that the cerebellum has some function of proportioning the mus-
cular response to the proprioceptive input, and if this proportioning is disturbed,
a [cerebellar] tremor may be one of the results. [emphasis added]
Norbert Wiener, Cybernetics or Control and Communication in the
Animal and The Machine, 1948, p. 114
It is a lamentable fact that much of the available information regarding the cere-
bellum is still uncorrelated and difficult to understand. Not until neurophysio-
logical and neuroanatomical data are brought in harmony and correlated with
the clinical and neuropathological findings can we expect to be able to form an
all-embracing concept of cerebellar function. It is hoped that the present publica-
tion will contribute toward this end.
From the Preface by Jan Jansen and Alf Brodal to the Festschrift, Aspects of
Cerebellar Anatomy, for Prof. K. E. Schreiner in celebration of his 80th
birthday, Oslo, Norway, 1954, p. 3–4
No one had any idea of how the cerebellar cortex should work or what operations
it should perform with the impulses coming into it from diverse sources.
S. L. Palay, speaking of the era after Ramón y Cajal, in his Introduction to
Cerebellar Cortex, 1974, by S. L. Palay and Victoria Chan-Palay, p. 1
Preface page ix
Acknowledgments xi
vii
viii Contents
Let us consider first the exquisite design of the cerebellar cortex as a laminated
rectangular lattice, a structure built with a precision only exceeded in biology
by the insect eye and its connectivities. A theory of the cerebellar cortex has
to incorporate this design as a key feature, and moreover has to account for
the convergence onto each Purkinje cell of two quite distinctive inputs, that from
the mossy-fiber input with the immense divergence (8,000) and convergence
(100,000) and that from the climbing fibers where the divergence number is about
10 and the convergence number is 1. This extraordinary double innervation has
been maintained through all the exigencies of evolution from primitive cerebella
to the great efflorescence in mammals and birds. It is particularly remarkable
that, when the cerebellar hemispheres were developed in step with the cerebral
hemispheres, the inferior olive hypertrophied also. The cerebral efferents had
to travel down to the medulla oblongata to excite the newly developed inferior
olivary neurons for the essential climbing fiber input to the cerebellar hemispheres.
(Eccles 1982, p. 607)
This book assembles evidence that the requirements of a model to meet the unique
anatomical and functional features that characterize the cerebellum are currently
best met by adaptive control models (or their neural net equivalents), the signal
feature of which is their ability to adjust (i.e., to optimize) their own parameters
automatically.
Adaptive control models – “adaptive” being the operative word – in various forms
and under various names have been discussed since the 1970s, and have reached
increasing levels of sophistication more recently. Adaptive controllers embody prin-
ciples that result in powerful capabilities, for example, prediction of the immediate
future of a signal on the basis of samples of its immediate past and generating a signal
for controlling an object (e.g., a robotic arm) from the desired trajectory of the arm.
These and other features of adaptive signal processors (i.e., adaptive controllers that
do not have a specifically controlled object) are illustrated by means of a specific
ix
x Preface
Readers interested in the manner in which the theme of this book evolved over
a 50-year incubation period can find more details in the Author’s Note at the end of
the book.
Acknowledgments
It has been a special pleasure to have worked on this book with Ellen Carlin, Assistant
Editor, Life Sciences, of Cambridge University Press, New York, and with Michie
Shaw, Books Project Manager, Techbooks, Fairfax, Virginia.
The author is grateful to the following publishers for permission to reproduce
copyrighted figures, and in several instances, also the author’s permission when
specified by the publisher, as noted in the figure captions: Academic Press/Morgan
Kaufmann, Orlando; Almqvist and Wiksell, Stockholm; American Medical Asso-
ciation, Chicago; American Physiological Society, Bethesda; Annual Reviews, Palo
Alto; Blackwell Science, Ltd., Oxford; Cambridge University Press, New York and
Cambridge; Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press, Cold Spring Harbor, NY; Elsevier
Science, Oxford; Freund Publishing House, Ltd., Tel Aviv; Harcourt, Inc., Orlando;
IEEE (Institute of Electrical and Electronic Engineers, Inc.), Piscataway, NJ;
John Wiley & Sons, Inc., New York and Chichester; Kluwer Academic/Plenum
Publishers, New York; Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Philadelphia; Masson Editeur,
Paris; McGraw-Hill Companies, New York; MIT Press, Cambridge, MA; Oxford
University Press, Oxford; Prentice-Hall, Inc., Upper Saddle River, NJ; Springer-
Verlag, New York and Heidelberg; and Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers, Lisse,
Holland.
In addition, the author is grateful to the following authors for permission to
reproduce figures for which they hold the copyright: Prof. József Hámori, Budapest,
Hungary; Prof. John J. Hopfield, Princeton, NJ; and Prof. Rudolph Nieuwenhuys,
Abcoude, The Netherlands.
xi
Midsagittal section of a human brain, showing the cerebellum at the lower right. (From
R. Nieuwenhuys, J. Voogd, and C. van Huijzen (1988). The Human Central Nervous Sys-
tem: A Synopsis and Atlas (3rd ed.), copyright 1988, Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg, reprinted
by permission of the publisher and author.)
part one
Introduction
1.1. History
In the introductory chapter of his magnificent three-volume monograph, The
Comparative Anatomy and Histology of the Cerebellum, Olof Larsell (1967; Larsell
and Jansen, 1972) provides a history of the cerebellum in its gross aspects, which
reads in part as follows (additional historical details can be found in Clarke and
O’Malley 1968):
Herophilus (335–280 b.c.) is usually credited with recognition of the human cere-
bellum as a distinct division of the brain. Aristotle (384–322 b.c.), however, calls
it parencephalis, indicating that he did not regard it as part of the principal mass
of the brain. The great Galen (a.d. 131–200) designated the vermis cerebelli “the
worm-like outgrowth” (epiphysis scolexoides). The arbor vitae [the treelike set
of outlines of white substance seen on a median section of the cerebellum] was
described by Thomas Willis (1664) in his Cerebri Anatome as “ramificatio cere-
belli ad foramen arboris.” The latter author also suggested that the cerebellum
presides over the involuntary movement of the body, whereas the cerebrum con-
trols those movements brought about by volition. The first good drawing of the
vermis was publishes by Heister (1717), but Vesalius (1543) had already included
in his Fabrica rather crude illustrations of the entire cerebellum which are in
striking contrast to his beautiful figures of muscles, bones, and other structures.
Haller (1777) described the cerebellar hemispheres under the name lobi, and
Malacarne (1780) gave a detailed description of the entire organ. Many of the
terms which Malacarne introduced are still in use. He also described the surface
folia or “laminette,” giving their total number as 500 to 780. In the cerebellum of
an idiot, he found only 340 folia, leading him to conclude that intelligence depends
on the number of cerebellar folds. . . .
The earlier studies of the cerebellum in animals were largely experimental
in execution. Rolando (1809) removed the cerebellum in fishes, reptiles, and
mammals, described the disturbances of voluntary movements that resulted, and
3
4 Introduction
pointed out that cerebellar ablation does not affect sensation. Flourens (1844)
confirmed and extended Rolando’s observations, emphasizing the exaggeration
of tendon and antigravity reflexes and the curious stiff-legged locomotion,
with retraction of the head that followed ablation of the cerebellum in birds.
[Purkinje’s original description of the pear-shaped cell somata was made in Prague
in 1837; see also Brazier 1988]. Ferrier (1876) reported his observations on the
responses of the eyes, head, and neck to electrical stimulation of the cerebellum
in dogs. Luciani (1891) described the results in the dog of complete removal of
the organ, and Sherrington (1900) defined the cerebellum as the “head ganglion
of the proprioceptive system,” holding that it functions as a whole because it deals
with the musculature of the body as a whole rather than with individual muscles.
This concept was the dominating influence in cerebellar physiology for more than
forty years.
During the last decade of the nineteenth century a new approach toward an
understanding of the organ was begun by studies on its comparative anatomy and
its embryonic development. The first article to appear in the Journal of Com-
parative Neurology was a comparative paper on the cerebellum by C. L. Herrick
(1891).
There have been many attempts in the past to characterize the essential function
of the cerebellum, of which the one by C. J. Herrick (1924b; the two Herricks were
brothers) can serve as an example:
According to Dow and Moruzzi (1958 p. 4), it was Flourens (1824, 1842) who
introduced the concept of the function of the cerebellum as coordinating movements.
Thus, after cerebellar ablation, the possibility of executing movements remained, but
the coordination of these movements was lost.
Trends in Neurosciences (Vol. 21, No. 9, Sept. 1998), and Trends in Cognitive Sciences
(Vol. 2, No. 9, Sept. 1998). In an interesting short autobiographical note, Ito (1999)
briefly traced the history of the discovery of the inhibitory action of the cerebellum,
the evolution of the concept of synaptic plasticity, and its demonstration experimen-
tally.
processor) of the author’s own design and construction (Chapter 15). A survey of
several adaptive control models of the cerebellum then follows (Chapter 16).
In Part IV (Summary and Conclusions), a selective recapitulation of material in
earlier chapters is presented, together with a detailed comparison between an adap-
tive signal processor and the vestibulo-ocular reflex as an example of the operation of
the cerebellar system (Chapter 17). Some remaining questions are also discussed. In
sum, the marshalled evidence that the cerebellum can be considered at least in part
as an adaptive controller appears to be very strong. At the same time, however, the
cerebellar system itself appears to lack the capability of true prediction, for which a
specific time mechanism would be required. The site of the latter capability, perhaps
distributed in location, remains unclear.
two
If we can discover what functional factors were primitively concerned in the initial
differentiation of the cerebellum from preexisting bulbar structures and some of
the steps by which additional functional systems of diverse kinds were drawn into
the cerebellar complex, some light may be shed on the great problems of the
analysis of higher cerebellar functions. (C. J. Herrick 1924b)
It is from the standpoint that a brief survey of the circumstances under which the
cerebellum and its Purkinje cells developed in phylogeny could shed some light on
the evolution of its organization and functions in higher forms that this review, mostly
from classical sources, is presented. This survey is based primarily on the following
sources: Ariëns Kappers, Huber, and Crosby (1960), Crosby (1969), Herrick (1924a,
1924b), Larsell (1967), Larsell and Jansen (1972), Llinás (1969), Llinás and Hillman
(1969), Nieuwenhuys (1967), Schnitzlein and Faucette (1969), Butler and Hodos
(1996), and Nieuwenhuys, ten Donkelaar, and Nicholson (1998). Some additional
aspects of comparative anatomy of the cerebellum are included in Chapter 12, which
discusses cerebellar-like structures in certain fish, including the valvula of mormyrid
fish, the electrosensory lobe, and the mammalian dorsal cochlear nucleus.
7
8 Comparative Anatomy of the Cerebellum
in the vertebrates above the sluggish urodeles, as well as in the active types of
fishes such as selachians [sharks, skates, and rays] and teleosts [bony fish].
2.2. Fish
In vertebrates, the cerebellum develops from two bilaterally symmetrical formations
located dorsally at the upper end of the medulla oblongata (the rhombencephalon),
2.2. Fish 9
Figure 2.1. Diagrammatic transverse section through the upper medulla oblongata of a lam-
prey, showing (on the right) the similarities between receptor cells of the lateral line nerve and
the vestibular nerve, and (on the left) the crista cerebellum or prototype cerebellum with a
precursor Purkinje cell (1) and granule cell (2). crista cereb., crista cerebellaris; n. lat., nervus
lateralis; tela chor., tela choroidea; s. lim. H., sulcus limitans of His. (From Nieuwenhuys 1967;
reprinted by permission of the author.)
in the region of termination (in their respective nuclear complexes) of the fibers from
the eighth or vestibular nerve and the lateral line nerves. This area is sometimes
termed the area octavolateralis. Thus, the oldest afferent paths to the cerebellum are
those of the vestibular and, in fish (and also in amphibia), of the lateral line systems.
This area is also the area of termination of the trigeminal nerve. This arrangement
is illustrated in Figure 2.1 for the lamprey (a member of the cyclostomes, that is, eel-
like fish having a round suctorial mouth and having a brain length of 1 centimeter).
The lamprey is the lowest form in which there is a clearly distinguishable cerebellum.
Whether still lower forms of vertebrates possess a cerebellum (e.g., the myxinoids, the
most primitive living vertebrates) has been an issue of much debate (Nieuwenhuys
1967).
In bony fish (teleosts), it has been proposed that the cerebellar auricles, which
receive a large input from the vestibulolateral line system, constitute the vestibulo-
cerebellum and are the homologues of the flocculonodular lobe of higher vertebrates,
whereas the corpus cerebelli, receiving spinocerebellar and tectocerebellar fibers, is
the homologue of the vermis of higher vertebrates (Ariëns et al. 1960).
It is relevant to note that the labyrinth (the three semicircular canals together with
the saccule and utricle) and the lateral line organs of lampreys (Petromyzontidae)
have a remarkable structural, as well as functional similarity, which is evident from
the right side of Figure 2.1. An important difference between the two structures
is that the arrangement of the lateral line organs is such that they are sensitive to
relative motion of the fluid surrounding the animal, whereas the labyrinths, having
basically the same sensing mechanism, are sensitive to fluid, the endolymph, which
is trapped in the labyrinths. Thus, by means of detection of the motion of fluid, the
10 Comparative Anatomy of the Cerebellum
lateral line organs detect the occurrence of external currents, principally to detect
and locate other moving animals, whereas the labyrinth serves to provide information
concerning the animals’ own equilibrium of the body and orientation in space (i.e.,
concerning gravity and inertia).
It is evident that these two types of information, which can be considered pro-
prioceptive and exteroceptive, respectively, are complimentary, a point that will be
relevant to a consideration (following) of the question of function of the primitive
cerebellum. It should be noted that another part of this primitive cerebellum receives
additional inputs (e.g., tectocerebellar and spinocerebellar; Nieuwenhuys 1967).
In adult lampreys, the vestibulolateral lobe receives input from the vestibular
apparatus and the lateral line organs, whereas the corpus cerebelli (cerebellar body)
receives spinal, bulbar, and trigeminal fibers. This major division of the cerebellum
into two parts is continued in higher forms. In fishes and amphibia, precursors of the
cerebellar nuclei can be recognized.
Histologically, the area of the brain of lampreys in which the two nerves (vestibular
and lateral line) terminate consists principally of small granular cells that have a few
short dendrites and a laterally directed axon that bifurcates longitudinally (Fig. 2.1,
left side, 2). These bifurcated axons are the forerunners of the parallel fibers. Scattered
among the granular cells are larger neurons having long dendrites extending in the
same direction as the axons of the granule cells (Fig. 2.1, left side, 1), with axons that
may curve downward and medially. These are the precursors of the Purkinje cells.
The neuropil zone of intermixing of the axons of the granular cells and the dendrites
of the larger cells is termed the crista cerebellaris or cerebellar crest, also known
as the molecular layer. The two intermingled cell types form a cell layer. The small
granule cells are equivalent to those in higher forms, and change little in phylogeny.
The larger cells have been considered, as just mentioned, to be the precursors of
Purkinje cells. It should be noted, however, that true Purkinje cells are characterized,
among other features, by the fact that their dendrites branch in a single plane, whereas
the dendritic tree of the larger cerebellar cellular elements is not confined to one plane
(Nieuwenhuys 1967).
The cerebellum of cartilaginous fish (sharks and rays) is considerably larger than,
and much further differentiated than, that of the round-mouth fish (cyclostomes),
and the larger ones display grooves in the cerebellum, the number of which increases
with the size of the body. It is also in this group of fishes that the Purkinje cells
spread their dendrites, which are covered with numerous spines, in a single plane. Of
particular note is the fact that the cartilagenous fishes have an olivocerebellar system,
which accompanies the caudal portion of the spinocerebellar tract toward the body
of the cerebellum (corpus cerebelli). The olivocerebellar fibers originate from the
contralateral inferior olive and terminate in all parts of the body of the cerebellum.
Figure 2.2. Sections showing the cell picture of the cerebellum of some representative ver-
tebrates: (a) lamprey, (b) lungfish, (c) turtle, (d) lizard, and (e) pigeon. Note the progressive
trend toward strict layering of the Purkinje cells from (b) to (e), and the diminution of size of
granule cells after (a). (From Nieuwenhuys 1967; reprinted by permission of the author.)
constitute a distinct zone between the granular and the molecular layers (see Fig. 2.2
for the general trend toward layering). Further, the dendrites of the Purkinje cells are
clearly oriented in a sagittal plane and show a more complex ramification (see Fig. 2.3
for the general trend). In crocodiles, the cerebellar cortical afferent and efferent
Figure 2.3. The progressive elaboration of Purkinje cell dendritic tree from lower vertebrates
to humans. (From Nieuwenhuys 1967; reprinted by permission of the author.)
12 Comparative Anatomy of the Cerebellum
axons are not scattered among granule cells, as in turtles, snakes and lizards, but
rather are concentrated into a deep white matter between the ventricular surface
and the granular layer. Accordingly, it can be said that crocodiles possess a true
cerebellar cortex, as do birds and mammals.
It is of interest that, in the frog, primary vestibular nerve fibers project as climbing
fibers directly onto the Purkinje cells in the vestibulolateral line or auricular lobe
(Precht and Llinás 1969); vestibular fibers thus appear to be the first nonolivary
source for climbing fibers for this type of cerebellar afferent in phylogeny. Primary
vestibular fibers also terminate as mossy fibers on granule cells in the auricular lobe
and activate Purkinje cells through the parallel fiber system in the frog. In contrast,
in the cat cerebellum, for example, primary and secondary vestibular inputs end as
mossy fibers exclusively.
The cerebellum of birds differs from that of reptiles by being more massive and
more complexly fissured.
There has been a successful analysis of the neuronal interactions in the cerebel-
lar cortex (Eccles 1973), but this does not help to any appreciable extent in the
attempt to develop an understanding of the mode of operation of the cerebel-
lum in the control of movement. At the best it can form the basis for building
models. (Eccles 1977b)
In view of the limited scope of this book and the correspondingly limited treatments
of cerebellar anatomy and physiology, the following books and papers may be cited as
supplementary sources: Brodal (1981); Brodal (1998); Eccles (1973, 1977b); Gilman,
Bloedel, and Lechtenberg (1981); Ito (1984); Kandel, Schwartz, and Jessell (1991);
Lechtenberg (1981); and Rothwell (1994).
The anatomical aspects of this chapter are based primarily on Brodal (1981);
Eccles, Ito, and Szentágothai (1967); Ghez (1991); Ito (1984); Paley and Chan-Palay
(1974); Palkovits, Magyar, and Szentágothai (1972); Parent (1996); and Ramón y
Cajal (1995), supplemented in many instances from more recent publications. A
synopsis of cerebellar anatomy appears in Voogd and Glickstein (1998). Further
aspects of cerebellar system anatomy that are particularly relevant to the topic of
adaptive control are considered in subsequent chapters: the mossy fibers (Chapter 4),
the climbing fibers and their source, the inferior olive (Chapter 5), and the cerebellar
nuclei and their efferent pathways (Chapter 6).
14
3.1. Anatomical Aspects of the Cerebellar Cortex 15
Figure 3.1. Schema of the three functional divisions of the cerebellum (the vestibulocere-
bellum, the spinocerebellum, and the cerebrocerebellum) with their outputs (A) and inputs
(B). (From Ghez 1991, in: Kandel, Schwartz, and Jessells (eds.), Principles of Neural Science,
3rd edition, copyright 1991, The McGraw-Hill Companies, reprinted by permission of the
publisher; see also Ghez and Thach [2000].)
outlined in Chapter 2. (In the following paragraphs concerning the major divisions
of the cerebellum, “input” refers to the mossy fiber input, one of the two principal
afferent pathways to the cerebellar cortex, the other being the “climbing fibers,”
which are discussed later.)
16 Anatomy and Physiology of the Cerebellar Cortex
Figure 3.2. Semidiagrammatic representation of part of a cerebellar folium to show the main
elements of the cerebellar cortex and their topographical relationships and orientation. Note
especially the arrangement of the Purkinje cell dendrites (Pd) and basket cell axons (Ba) in
the transverse plane of the folium and the longitudinal arrangement of the parallel fibers (Pf).
Other abbreviations: B, basket cell; Cl, climbing fiber; Coll, recurrent collateral of Purkinje
cell; G, Golgi cell; Gr, granule cell; Mf, mossy fiber; P, Purkinje cell; Pd, Purkinje cell dendrites;
Pf, parallel fibers; S, stellate cell. Inset, lower right: diagrammatic illustration of the relation
between the rosettes (r) of the mossy fibers (Mf) with the dendritic “claws” (cl) of the granule
cells (Gr) in a glomerulus (Gl). The bifurcation of the granule cell axons to form parallel
fibers are shown at the top. Other elements of the glomerulus are not shown. (From Brodal
1981, adapted from Hámori and Szentágothai 1966; reprinted with permission from A. Brodal,
Neurological Anatomy in Relation to Clinical Medicine (3rd ed.), copyright 1981 by Oxford
University Press; and Springer-Verlag and Prof. J. Hámori.)
additional type of cell, the Lugaro cell, is less well established. It lies in the granular
layer close to the Purkinje cell layer, and contacts by recurrent Purkinje collaterals
and by axons of granule cells or parallel fibers have been reported. Its targets have
not been established and its function is unknown (Ito 1984; Palay and Chan-Palay
1974).
3.1. Anatomical Aspects of the Cerebellar Cortex 19
Purkinje Cells. The Purkinje cells number approximately 1,500,000 in cats and
15,000,000 in humans. The cell bodies of these inhibitory cells are flask shaped, and
their axons penetrate through the granular layer (in which recurrent collaterals may
be given off that may branch repeatedly and extend transversely with respect to the
axis of the folia, often for a considerable distance) and continue into the white mat-
ter to terminate in the cerebellar nuclei (where a single Purkinje cell may contact
approximately 35 nuclear cells) or in the vestibular nuclei. The recurrent collater-
als are generally presumed to terminate on the proximal dendrites of the Purkinje
cells and, in cats at least, on basket and Golgi cells. (It may be noted that the recur-
rent inhibition derived from Purkinje cells is monosynaptic because Purkinje cells
are themselves inhibitory, whereas recurrent inhibition derived from the neocortical
pyramidal cells is necessarily disynaptic because pyramidal cells are excitatory.)
The extensively branched apical dendritic tree, which extends outward into the
molecular layer, is quite flattened, being confined almost to a single plane perpendic-
ular to the longitudinal axis of the folium. Its thickness approximates about one twen-
tieth its other dimensions (15–20 mu vs. 300–400 mu). The distal dendritic branches
are covered with spines (thorns), with which, generally speaking, afferent excita-
tory fibers make contact. In contrast, inhibitory inputs (from stellate cells and other
Purkinje cells) usually end on the proximal smooth shafts of dendrites.
To study the formation of cerebellar lamination and differentiation of Purkinje
cells in the absence of their extracerebellar afferents, Tauer, Volk, and Heimrich
(1996) used combined immunocytochemical and Golgi electron microscopic tech-
niques and organotypic cultures of immature cerebellar tissue. It was found that the
lamination (layering) was retained in the majority of cultures, and most Purkinje cells
were aligned. The authors concluded that the Purkinje cells in organotypic cultures
send their axons to the correct target region independently of their local position, but
the orientation of the dendritic tree and differentiation is influenced by the cellular
environment and by specific synaptic interaction.
Basket and Stellate Cells. Of the interneurons in the molecular layer (i.e., basket and
stellate cells), 40% were found by Mann-Metzer and Yarom (2000) to be electrically
(electrotonically) coupled, from which it was suggested that these interneurons form
local networks that give rise to synchronized activity. Such electrical synapses and
intrinsic currents were demonstrated to form a highly modifiable communication
pathway.
Climbing Fiber Synapses onto Purkinje Cells. Climbing fiber synapses have several
features that make for an extremely powerful excitatory effect (Fig. 3.2); activation
of a Purkinje cell by its climbing fiber (Fig. 3.3) invariably results in a (complex)
spike (Fig. 3.8). The disposition, on the Purkinje cell dendritic tree, of the inhibitory
synapses of the stellate cells also has a strong effect, not to mention that of the basket
cells, which synapse primarily around the initial segment of the Purkinje cell axon.
The primary and secondary dendritic branches of Purkinje cells receive climbing
fibers, whereas the tertiary “spiny branchlets” are the site of parallel fiber synapses
20 Anatomy and Physiology of the Cerebellar Cortex
(Ito 1984). The parallel fiber input (see below) induces “conventional” Na+ spikes
(“simple spikes”), whereas climbing fiber activity results in large CA++ spikes with
a long duration (“complex spikes”).
Granular Layer. The granular layer contains the cell bodies of Golgi cells (see below)
together with a large number of the small (cell body: 5–8 mu), densely packed,
granule cells. The total number has been estimated to be of the order of 1010 –1011 ,
more than any other type of neuron. The granule cells are the only neurons in the
cerebellar cortex having an excitatory output (with the exception of the unipolar
brush cells; see below), and they excite all the other cells in the cerebellar cortex.
Structurally, they have 4 or 5 (range 2–7) short dendrites that end with clawlike
expansions, the glomeruli (rosette) or cerebellar isles (Fig. 3.2, inset), which are
contacted (“grasped”) by endings of mossy fibers (see below). Dendrites from more
than one granule cell may contribute to a given glomerulus. However, it is evidently
quite unusual for dendrites to be shared between two mossy fibers. It has been
estimated that there are as many 20 granule cells in synapse with a mossy fiber
rosette.
Unipolar Brush Cells. Recently, the existence of another type of cell in the gran-
ular layer has become identified: the unipolar brush cell (UBC; Fig. 3.4; Mugnaini,
Diño, and Jaarsma 1997), which is characterized by a single dendrite that termi-
nates with a brushlike tip of dendrioles to receive mossy fiber terminals, thus con-
stituting a giant glutaminergic (excitatory) synapse (Nunzi and Mugnaini 2000).
The latter, which represent the main synaptic apparatus of the UBC, articulate
tightly with a single mossy fiber rosette to form a glomerular array having exten-
sive synaptic contact, one of the largest in the vertebrate central nervous system.
3.1. Anatomical Aspects of the Cerebellar Cortex 21
Figure 3.4. Schematic diagram of the mossy fiber–unipolar brush cell–granule cell pathways
using dendrodendritic synapses (left) and axodendritic synapses (right). mf, mossy fiber; UBC,
unipolar brush cell; ax, UBC axon; agca, ascending granule cell axons; pfs, parallel fibers.
Question mark denotes the unresolved question of whether the UBC axon, in addition to
providing local terminal branches, projects outside the granule cell domain. (From Mugnaini,
Diño, and Jaarsma 1997; reprinted from Progress in Brain Research, copyright 1997 by Elsevier
Science; reprinted with permission from the publisher.)
Parallel Fibers. The unmyelinated (or thinly myelinated) axons of granule cells as-
cend into the molecular layer (Fig. 3.2), where they may synapse with Purkinje cells
before bifurcating in a T-shaped manner, the two branches extending as parallel
fibers for a short distance in either direction along the long axis of the folium (thus
the name), across the dendritic trees of the Purkinje and other cells.
The length of parallel fibers (expressed as the sum of the lengths of the two
branches), a parameter that is important for some theories of cerebellar function
(Chapter 13), has long been of interest (e.g., Braitenberg and Atwood 1958;
Braitenberg, Heck, and Sultan 1997; Brand, Dahl, and Mugnaini 1976; Dow 1949;
Mugnaini 1983; Pichitpornchai, Rawson, and Rees 1994; Schild 1980; Smolyaninov
1971; Thach, Goodkin, and Keating 1992). Some of the earlier determinations ranged
appreciably in value (about 2–10 mm), as did the depth in the molecular layer of the
parallel fibers, but greater agreement has occurred in recent years. Taking into ac-
count that the parallel fibers closer to the outer surface of the molecular layer (which
would then make contact with the more distal parts of the Purkinje cell dendritic tree)
are somewhat longer than those near its base (i.e., near the layer of Purkinje cell bod-
ies), Brand, Dahl, and Mugnaini (1976) found an average value of 6 millimeters in
the cat, with a range of 5 to 7 millimeters.
From measurements in chickens and monkeys, Mugnaini (1983) found that the
length of parallel fibers can reach a length of approximately 6 millimeters, which is
consistently longer than the width of single efferent cortical strips (0.5–1 millimeters).
It was also found that in these two species, the longer parallel fibers are situated in
the outer molecular layer, becoming progressively shorter toward the Purkinje cell
layer, a finding opposite those of some earlier authors.
From a survey of determinations, Braitenberg, Heck, and Sultan (1997) provided
a range of 4.7 to 6 millimeters for the combined length of the two parallel fiber
branches of one granule cell axon.
It should be noted, however, that there are variations, not only in length of the
parallel fibers, but also in their diameter and in the density of varicosities (which
correspond to the presence of synapses with Purkinje cells) along the length of a
3.1. Anatomical Aspects of the Cerebellar Cortex 23
Figure 3.5. Summary diagram indicating the relative lengths and diameters of the parallel
fibers in the different thirds of the molecular layer, and the relative size and spacing of the
varicosities along both the parallel fibers and the ascending axons. (From Pichitpornchai,
Rawson, and Rees 1994; in J. Comparative Neurol. Vol. 342, copyright 1994 Wiley-Liss, Inc., a
subsidiary of John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
given parallel fiber, as shown in Figure 3.5. There are also variations in the ascending
segment of the granule cell axons prior to their bifurcation to form parallel fibers.
These variations would of necessity introduce some differences and nonlinearities
in conduction velocities, and perhaps in the local variations of densities of parallel
fiber–Purkinje cell synapses.
The essentially perpendicular arrangement of the transverse input mossy parallel
fibers and the longitudinal Purkinje (as output), basket, and stellate cells (longitudi-
nal) in the cerebellar cortex has been reemphasized by Voogd and Ruigrok (1997).
Cohen and Yarom (2000) pointed out that the parasagittal organization of the
cerebellar cortex that is imposed by the climbing fiber input overlaps that of the
corticonuclear projections, thus suggesting a functional organization.
The parallel fibers make synaptic contact with the dendritic spines of the Purkinje
cells, as well as with stellate, basket, and Golgi cells. According to one estimate,
some 200,000 parallel fibers make contact with a single Purkinje cell in the cat and
250,000 cells in the human. According to another estimate, of the 400,000 parallel
fibers passing through the dendritic tree of a Purkinje cell, only 80,000 make synaptic
contact. In any event, a given Purkinje cell is under the influence of a large number
of granule cells. Conversely, a given parallel fiber has been estimated to pass through
from 450 to 1,100 Purkinje cells along a folium, but if synapses are made with only
one in three to five Purkinje cells, the number could decrease to about 100 or even
less.
Golgi Cells. Already mentioned, the Golgi cells resemble Purkinje cells in some
respects (Fig. 3.2) in that they are large and have branching dendritic trees that
extend outward into the molecular layer; their number approximates that of the
Purkinje cells. The dendritic tree of the Golgi cell, however, extends in all directions
(about three times as far as the Purkinje dendritic tree) rather than being confined to
24 Anatomy and Physiology of the Cerebellar Cortex
Figure 3.6. Matrix of Purkinje neurons (cell body indicated by circle, dendritic tree by bar)
potentially reached by descending axon branches of a basket neuron. The whole matrix and
arborization is seen from the surface. The thin side branches of the basket axon and the
terminal branching, running in the longitudinal direction of the folium, have an ascending
course and probably terminate in the molecular layer. (From Szentágothai 1965; reprinted by
permission of Prof. Szentágothai’s heirs [by courtesy of Prof. Hámori].)
the transverse plane of the folium, as is the case for the Purkinje, stellate, and basket
cells. Parallel fibers form excitatory synapses with the Golgi cell dendrites, which
are also contacted by other afferents (e.g., Golgi cell dendrites that remain in the
granular layer may be contacted by mossy fibers directly). The axons of Golgi cells,
which are extensively branched, make synaptic contact with dendrites of granule
cells at the glomeruli, as already mentioned. Recurrent Purkinje axons synapse onto
Golgi somata. The granule cell to Golgi cell ratio is of the order of 5,000 to 1.
Molecular Layer. The molecular layer, the most superficial layer, is dominated by
fibers; the relatively few neurons in this layer, which are located just above the level
of the Purkinje cell bodies, are the stellate cells and basket cells. The dendrites of
both cell types, like the dendrites of the Purkinje cells, which are in the same layer,
are oriented in the transverse plane of the folium and receive collaterals of climbing
fibers.
Basket Cells. The dendritic tree of the basket cells (Fig. 3.2, B), like that of the
Purkinje cells, is transverse to the folium, but as already mentioned, is not as flattened
as that of the Purkinje cell. At times, the dendrites of the two cell types appear in
regular alternation. The axon of the basket cells (Fig. 3.2, Ba) passes for some distance
across the folium just above the Purkinje cell bodies, giving off descending collaterals
at right angles, which surround Purkinje cell bodies like a basket, making synaptic
contact with an average (in the cat) of about 10 Purkinje cells (Fig. 3.6), and contacting
about 3 Purkinje cells in both directions along the longitudinal axis of the folium
(in the cat), for an average total of some 30 Purkinje cells. Horizontal axonal segments
of the basket cells also contact dendrites of Purkinje, basket, and stellate cells.
The descending axons of the basket cell axons and their arborizations form a
unique plexus, termed pinceau, or (artist’s) paintbrush, around the initial segment
of Purkinje axons, thus forming an axoaxonic (inhibitory) synapse, an uncommon
3.1. Anatomical Aspects of the Cerebellar Cortex 25
structure elsewhere in the vertebrate brain. Although the pinceau would initially
seem to constitute a powerful inhibitory structure, its actual effect has yet to be
clarified completely.
As a result of this arrangement, the basket cells, which are only slightly more
numerous than Purkinje cells (by 15–20%), can act on a series of Purkinje cells
across the folium and to a lesser extent, along it. This arrangement is in contrast to
the parallel fibers, which activate a series of Purkinje cells along, rather than across,
the folium. It is to be emphasized that the inhibitory effects of basket cells are on
Purkinje cells that are arrayed laterally (with respect to the long axis of the folium)
from the locus of the basket cell’s dendritic tree, thus providing inhibition to off-axis
Purkinje cells. In contrast, stellate cells (see the following) provide primarily on-axis
inhibition to Purkinje cells and to the more distal parts of the Purkinje cell dendritic
tree. The parallel fibers that synapse with basket and stellate cells may not necessarily
be the same ones that synapse with Purkinje cells.
Stellate Cells. The stellate cells, which lie in the outer two thirds of the molecular
layer, have a cell body and a dendritic arborization that is smaller than those of
the basket cells. Like the basket cells, the dendritic tree of stellate cells is oriented
transversely to the long axis of folia. Also like basket cells, the stellate cells receive
synapses principally from parallel fibers, and their axons – coursing transversely to
the axis of folia (although not as far as the basket cell axons) – synapse principally with
Purkinje cells in its immediate vicinity, the more so the more superficial the stellate
cell in the molecular layer. The stellate cell axons form synapses only on dendritic
shafts of Purkinje cells, whereas basket cells form baskets around the Purkinje cell
somata and pinceau around their initial segment, as already mentioned. (The more
highly structured basket cell is a more recent development in phylogeny than is the
stellate cell.) The ratio of stellate to Purkinje cells is estimated to be about 1:17.
Inhibitory Mechanisms: Golgi Cells vs. Basket Cells. From the preceding account,
it is evident that Golgi cells are inhibitory to granule cells and that basket cells are
inhibitory to Purkinje cells, but the manner of the inhibition in the two instances
is different. These two types of cells are diagrammed in Figure 3.7, originally from
Ramón y Cajal (1995). The inhibition by the Golgi cells occurs at the level of the
glomeruli; the inhibitory effect of the Golgi synapses counteracts the excitatory ef-
fect of the mossy fiber synapses. The inhibitory synapses of the basket cells on the
Purkinje, in contrast, are on the initial segment of the Purkinje cell axon; the exci-
tatory synapses of (primarily) the parallel fibers are on the Purkinje cell dendritic
tree. It seems likely that there is a significant functional difference between these
two arrangements for delivering inhibition, perhaps related to the extremely small
size and great number of the granule cells. (It should be noted that the Golgi cells
are also inhibitory to stellate and basket cells.)
Starting from the point that Golgi cells, being inhibitory, exert both a feedback
and a feedforward inhibition of granule cell activity, De Schutter, Vos, and Maex
(2000) and Vos et al. (2000) concluded that Golgi cells (in relation to the mossy
26 Anatomy and Physiology of the Cerebellar Cortex
d
c a
c
d a
B B
A A
Figure 3.7. Two diagrams from Ramón y Cajal showing, on the left at top, a Golgi cell with its
synapses onto the glomeruli of three granule cells, and on the right at top, a basket cell with its
synapses onto two Purkinje cells. Both cell types receive their input from the parallel fibers.
Arrows indicate direction of impulses. Legends for left side: A, mossy fiber; a, granule cell;
b, parallel fiber; c, Golgi cell; d, Purkinje cells; B, Purkinje axons (note recurrent collaterals).
Legends for right side: A, mossy fiber; a, granule cell; b, basket cell; B, Purkinje axon; c, Purkinje
cells; d, climbing fibers. See text. (From Eccles, Ito, and Szentágothai 1967; in: Eccles, Ito, and
Szentágothai, The Cerebellum as a Neuronal Machine, copyright 1967, Springer-Verlag New
York, originally from Ramón y Cajal 1911 [Figs. 103 and 104]; [English translation, 1995 by
N. Swanson and L. W. Swanson, Oxford University Press.]; reprinted by permission.)
Climbing Fibers. Moderately thin and myelinated, the climbing fibers pass undivided
from the inferior olive, through the cerebellar white matter and the molecular layer,
and to the level of the Purkinje cells. Electrophysiological and anatomical studies
indicate that branches of a single fiber can divide and innervate different lobules
of the cerebellar cortex in a parasagittal distribution (i.e., longitudinally) to synapse
with 10 to 15 Purkinje cells. In maturity, only a single (branch of a) climbing fiber
innervates a given Purkinje cell, in contrast to the immature state, during which there
is multiple innervation (up to four climbing fibers per Purkinje cell) of which all but
one normally recede in the course of maturation (Chapter 11).
The branch of a climbing fiber for a given Purkinje cell follows closely and winds
ivylike along its dendritic branches, making contact with the dendritic spines of the
latter, thus forming the basis of an extensive and powerful excitatory synaptic action.
These synapses are confined to the inner two thirds of the molecular layer. As previ-
ously mentioned, some of the collaterals of climbing fibers may end on neighboring
Purkinje cells, stellate cells, basket cells, and Golgi cells, although the presumed ex-
citatory synaptic action on Golgi cells, and also on granule cells, has evidently not
been substantiated.
In humans, there is a total of about 1 million neurons for both inferior olives, as
compared with about 15 million Purkinje cells, for a ratio of 1 to 15.
In contrast to the extensive branching of the mossy fibers (described follow-
ing), the climbing fibers ascend, as just mentioned, undivided from the inferior olive
through the white matter of the cerebellum to the Purkinje cell layer.
Mossy Fibers and Their Terminations. These fibers, which derive their name from
their moss like terminals in the granular layer, are thick and heavily myelinated, and
therefore can be expected to possess a conduction velocity higher than that of the
thin climbing fibers. Unlike the climbing fibers, all of which arise from a single source
(the inferior olive, Chapter 5), mossy fibers arise from numerous sources (Chapter
4). In contrast to the above-noted, limited branching by climbing fibers in the vicinity
of the Purkinje cell layer of the cerebellar cortex to a small group of Purkinje cells,
mossy fibers give off collaterals, as many as 20 or 30, along their entire course through
the white matter, not just in the folia in which they largely terminate. Having entered
the cerebellar cortex, mossy fibers branch repeatedly; one fiber may supply two or
even more folia. In their final course, they give off many collaterals, which, like the
final branches, end in the granular layer as a cluster of small endings, forming what
are often referred to as rosettes (glomeruli), as previously described. The mossy
fibers remain (thinly) myelinated up to their preterminal portions. Their terminal
or synaptic endings interdigitate with, and form synaptic contacts with, the clawlike
dendritic terminations of the granule cells (Fig. 3.2, inset), as described above. As
just mentioned, rosettes are surrounded by granule cell dendrites and Golgi cell
axon terminals, forming glomeruli. Thus, mossy fiber rosettes and Golgi cell axon
terminals are the presynaptic elements of glomeruli, and granule cell dendrites are
the postsynaptic elements of glomeruli, the mossy fiber terminations being excitatory
and the Golgi terminals inhibitory. One mossy fiber has been estimated to make
28 Anatomy and Physiology of the Cerebellar Cortex
contact with some 450 granule cells. Mossy fiber terminals also synapse onto the
somata of Golgi cells.
an independent count of the Purkinje dendritic spines of 91,600. (Note that the
assumed length of parallel fibers of 2 mm is now considered an underestimate, as
mentioned earlier in this chapter; today’s estimate of a length of 6–7 mm would bring
the total estimate of parallel fibers per Purkinje cell more closely in line with other
more recent estimates on the order of 200,000.)
These estimates do not, however, take into account the branching of mossy fibers
in the white matter before entering the cerebellar cortex. It is also the case that
granule cells activated by any given mossy fiber are likely to be widely dispersed
in the granular layer within the distribution territory of that mossy fiber. Likewise,
the parallel fibers of these granule cells will be at correspondingly different depths
in the molecular layer, impinging on different branches of the dendritic tree of the
same, or even different, Purkinje cells. Moreover the 2-mm parallel fiber length
is an underestimate, as indicated above. The consequence is that the Purkinje cell
would likely be presented with samples of the same mossy fiber discharge in several
different parts of its dendritic tree at the same time. It is also clear that each mossy
fiber entering the white matter from below distributes to a large field of cortex, which
overlaps extensively with that of its neighbors (Palay and Chan-Palay 1974).
Under certain simplifying assumptions, a ratio of mossy fiber–Purkinje cell within
the folia of 4:1 has been estimated for the cat, which, in view of the very much larger
ratio for parallel fibers to Purkinje cells, could perhaps suggest that an interpolation
or vernier process of some kind takes place in the cerebellar cortex.
There are certain differences among mossy fibers and their terminations according
to their sources of origin (e.g., with respect to their degree of branching within the
granular layer; Brodal 1967, 1981; Brodal and Drabløs 1963).
In addition to the extracerebellar origins of the mossy fibers and the climbing
fibers, a portion of the mossy fibers originate from the cerebellar nuclei, thus con-
stituting a feedback loop to the cerebellar cortex. These nucleocortical fibers are
considered in greater detail in Chapter 6. There are also fibers originating from the
cerebellar nuclei that terminate as inhibitory synapses in the inferior olive, which
completes another feedback loop to the cerebellar cortex, in this instance via the
inferior olive; these are also considered further in Chapter 6.
up by neuroglial cells. The ratio of granule cells to Purkinje cells is of the order of
5,000 to 1, but estimates vary appreciably. In addition to (excitatory) synapses onto
the dendrites of Purkinje cells, parallel fibers also form (excitatory) synapses with
Golgi, stellate, and basket cells.
Efficacy of Parallel Fibers vs. Vertical Segments of Granule Cell Axons. In relation
to possible mechanisms of function of the cerebellar cortex, an important question,
as mentioned earlier, is that of the relative importance of the distribution on Purkinje
cells of the different segments of axons of granule cells (i.e., the ascending [vertical]
segment vs. the parallel fiber segment). If the ascending segment is more important
and the branched parallel fiber from the same granule cell has an appreciably weaker
effect, then the impact of a given granule cell will be primarily on those Purkinje cells
that directly overlie it (i.e., with which the vertical segment of the granule cell has
synaptic contact). However, if the two parallel fibers from such a granule cell have a
major impact on Purkinje cells, then the effect of the granule cell could in principle
make itself felt for the full extent of the parallel fibers. From their study of this
question, Garwicz and Andersson (1992) found that synaptic activity does spread
along the parallel fibers, as far as 1.5 mm outside the mossy fiber termination area.
In this connection, in a study of the spatial effects of excitatory amino acids in
the rat cerebellar cortex using optical imaging of a voltage-sensitive dye (Grinvald
et al. 1988) in conjunction with stimulation of the cerebellar cortex, Elias, Yae, and
Ebner (1993) found discrete “beams” of optical activity consistent with extracellular
recordings. The authors concluded that the optical signal was not due to the evoked
parallel fiber activity but was mainly generated by postsynaptic targets.
Figure 3.8. Simple and complex spikes of three sample Purkinje cells (A, B, C), recorded
extracellularly from the cerebellar cortex of a rhesus monkey. (From Mano, Kanazawa, and
Yamamoto 1989; In: Strata (ed.) The Olivocerebellar System in Motor Control, copyright 1989,
Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
whereas the basket cells inhibit the initial segment of the axon of the latter. The
output of the Purkinje cells, which terminates on the neurons of the cerebellar nuclei
(and on neurons in the vestibular nuclei), is inhibitory. That is, these nuclear neurons
also receive excitatory collaterals from both the climbing fibers and the mossy fibers.
granule cell synapses (i.e., parallel fiber synapses) are restricted to the small spine-
covered tertiary dendrites (the spiny branchlets); (2) each of these systems generates
different postsynaptic effects in the Purkinje cell: climbing fiber activation evokes a
large all-or-none Ca2+ -dependent dendritic action potential, whereas parallel fiber
(granule cell) inputs are considered to generate more classical dendritic EPSPs and
to produce fast Na+ spikes as the Purkinje cell output.
De Schutter and Bower (1994a, 1994b), by adding simulated climbing fiber and
parallel fiber excitatory synaptic inputs to a basic computer model of the Purkinje
cell, were able to replicate both complex spikes and simple spikes, provided there
were inhibitory synaptic inputs to the Purkinje cell from basket and stellate cells.
Using a fluorescent indicator to image voltage in combination with synaptically
activated sodium transients in Purkinje cells, Lasser-Ross and Ross (1992) found that
fast sodium action potentials resulted in large increases in internal sodium concen-
tration in the soma and axon, but not in the dendrites, consistent with the results
of the above-mentioned physiological experiments of Llinás and Sugimori (1980a,
1980b).
Reciprocal Trophic Interaction Between Purkinje Cells and Climbing Fibers. A re-
ciprocal trophic interaction between climbing fiber terminal arborizations and
Purkinje cells was reported by Strata et al. (1997), such that when the climbing fiber
is missing, the Purkinje cell undergoes a hyperspiny transformation and becomes
hyperinnervated by parallel fibers. However, this change was found to be reversible;
the climbing fiber-deprived Purkinje cell is able to elicit sprouting of nearby intact
climbing fibers and the new arbor is able to restore synaptic connections fully.
Bistability in Purkinje Dendrites. Using ion channels, Yuen, Hockberger, and Houk
(1995) modeled the possibility of bistability in Purkinje cell dendrites, pointing out,
among other aspects, that the hysteresis inherent in bistability is equivalent to short-
term memory without changes in synaptic weight (as in long-term depression [LTD]).
Independent bistability in several dendritic branches would, in turn, result in multi-
stability of the Purkinje cell.
localized within the axonal arborization of presynaptic neurons. The concerted re-
lease of several presynaptic vesicles appeared necessary. That is, such multiquantal
events were considered to result from fluctuations in presynaptic depolarization, or
from fluctuations in a regenerative calcium-ion amplification mechanism.
of the granule cell axons, rather than the parallel fiber system, provides the main
drive to Purkinje cells.
Cohen and Yarom (2000) concluded that the basic, independent modules of the
cerebellar cortex consist of a localized group of granule cells and Purkinje cells
above them, with the feedback inhibition of the Golgi cells and the feedforward
inhibition of the stellate cells regulating the activity of each module. In this view,
mutual interactions among the different modules occur in the sagittal axis via the
long-range axons of the molecular layer interneurons, and in the mediolateral axis
via the parallel fibers, the latter interactions, arising from disinhibition, being far
more significant and efficient over a much broader time window in comparison with
the mediolateral interaction via the parallel fibers, which are rather weak and are
efficient only within a narrow time window. Cohen and Yarom (2000) also pointed out
that the parasagittal organization of the cerebellar cortex imposed by the climbing
fiber input overlaps that of the corticonuclear projections and suggested a functional
organization.
four
In this chapter and in Chapter 5, a brief overview is presented of the principal origins
of, respectively, the mossy fibers and the afferent fibers to the inferior olive. The
information below is distilled mainly from Brodal (1981); Bloedel and Courville
(1981); Ekerot, Larson, and Oscarsson (1979); and Brodal (1998).
The termination sites of the climbing fibers from the inferior olive and the mossy
fibers in the cerebellar cortex are, with few exceptions, quite different. Although
both types of excitatory fibers are ultimately derived from a variety of sources, the
climbing fibers, so far as is known, take their immediate origin entirely from the
inferior olivary nucleus (the inferior olive), whereas the mossy fibers do not similarly
pass through a single nuclear structure.
The mossy fibers conduct impulses relatively rapidly, branch extensively, and
influence many Purkinje cells via the granule cells. However, the excitatory effect on
each Purkinje cell is weak so the conjunctive action of many mossy fibers is required
to fire the Purkinje cells at rates of 50–500 per second.
Primary Vestibular Fibers. The primary vestibular fibers are fibers from the vestibu-
lar end-organs (semicircular canals and otolith organs), which have been traced to the
vestibulocerebellum and the fastigial nucleus. The terminals of the primary vestibular
fibers are appreciably more concentrated than mossy fiber terminals elsewhere in the
cerebellar cortex. In addition, they give off fine short collaterals that also terminate
within the granular layer.
37
38 The Mossy Fiber Afferent System
Gibson, and Houk (1993) found that mossy fibers provide the intermediate cere-
bellum with position, velocity, and direction information about movement of in-
dividual forelimb joint, perhaps derived from efference copy and the usual affer-
ence. The discharge patterns of Golgi cells were noted to be quite different from
those of mossy fibers; all Golgi cells showed a phasic discharge without tonic com-
ponents. The output signals from the nucleus interpositus suggested that the inter-
mediate cerebellum incorporates position and velocity information from individual
joints, together with other inputs, into phasic signals relating to coordinated move-
ments of the entire limb. In this connection, Horne and Butler (1995) suggested that
the cerebello–thalamo–cortical pathway receives a form of efference copy from the
motor cortex and compares this message with that derived from peripheral afferents
concerning the actual progress of the movement.
a pH-sensitive dye (an acidic shift indicating increased fluorescence) used to de-
termine pH in neurons was reported by Chen, Hanson, and Ebner (1998). These
authors electrically stimulated parallel fibers in the cerebellar cortex of rats. It was
found that a significant portion of the fluorescence change was of intracellular origin
and reflected activation of parallel fibers and their postsynaptic targets. It was sug-
gested that optical imaging using this dye should prove useful for neuronal activity
mapping.
Reticular Tegmental Pontine Nucleus. The reticular tegmental pontine nucleus re-
ceives input from the cerebral cortex (the primary sensorimotor cortex) and the cere-
bellar nuclei. Quantitatively, the most important input to the nucleus comes from the
cerebellar nuclei via the superior cerebellar peduncle. The nucleus can therefore be
considered a link in a cerebello–reticulo–cerebellar feedback system, although not
point to point. It is the only one of the reticular group of nuclei that does not receive
an input from the spinal cord.
Lateral Reticular Nucleus. Situated just lateral to the inferior olive, the lateral retic-
ular nucleus receives afferents principally from the spinal cord, and also from the
magnocellular part of the red nucleus and the cerebral cortex. Its efferents to the
cerebellum pass via the inferior cerebellar peduncle.
In view of the importance attached to the climbing fibers and the inferior olivary
nucleus (also termed the inferior olivary complex, inferior olive, or simply IO) in a
number of theories of cerebellar function (Chapters 13, 14, 16, and 17), its anatomy
and physiology is surveyed in some detail (see also Chapter 6).
The sequence of topics in this chapter is as follows: organization of the inferior
olivary nucleus itself; its afferent input from the periphery; projections to it from
other structures (e.g., the cerebellar nuclei); and finally, the climbing fibers (i.e., the
output system from the inferior olive to the cerebellar cortex with their collaterals to
the cerebellar nuclei). The chapter closes with a consideration of theories of function
of the inferior olivary nucleus and a discussion of modulation of sensory (peripheral)
input to the inferior olive. For purposes of differentiation, arbitrarily, afferent refers
to input derived from the periphery, whereas projection refers to input derived from
or directed to another brain structure (e.g., cerebellar cortex or nucleus).
Cell Types. Microscopically, the olivary neurons according to Golgi studies in sev-
eral species (Scheibel and Scheibel 1955; Scheibel et al. 1956) are of two principal
42
5.1. The Inferior Olivary System 43
Figure 5.1. The inferior olive (lower right and left) in a drawing of a transverse section through
the caudal part of the medulla oblongata in man. (From Brodal 1981; Neurological Anatomy
in Relation to Clinical Medicine (3rd ed.); copyright 1981, Oxford University Press; reprinted
by permission of the publisher.)
types (Fig. 5.3). The first, the more typical olivary neurons, have highly ramified
and spatially more limited spherical dendritic trees (Type II), and were found in the
phylogenetically newer principal olivary nucleus and rostral halves of the accessory
nuclei. Their dendritic fields were about as extensive as those of Purkinje cells. In-
deed, it could be said that the dendritic arborizations of inferior olivary neurons of
this type are as chaotic as those of Purkinje cells are orderly. The second type of cell,
with large, simple, and relatively unramified dendritic patterns (Type I), was taken
to be more primitive, and tended more to be found in the accessory olives.
Types of Afferent Terminations. Among the afferent terminals (Fig. 5.4), in addition
to a bushy type (the most frequent), a heavy rosette-bearing type and a thinner
bouton type were seen. The overlapping, conelike distribution of terminal axons
(Figs. 5.4A and 5.5a, b, and d) among the tightly packed highly ramifying dendritic
arbors (Fig. 5.3) was considered, on the one hand, to lose the identity of a given
input (i.e., so the dendritic receptive nets function as highly uncritical transmitters of
information) and, on the other hand, enhance the probability of synchronous firing
of rather large numbers of cells (Scheibel and Scheibel 1955). The bushy afferents
were considered to be well suited to provide for activation of relatively large cell
populations, whereas the other two types were thought to perhaps serve to fractionate
activity in such ensembles. A hypothetical diagram showing possible relationships
among the various types of afferent axonal terminals and neurons is reproduced in
Figure 5.6.
According to Armstrong (1974), neighboring arborizations of the most com-
mon bushy type are so heavily overlapped that they preclude a high degree of
44 The Inferior Olivary System and the Climbing Fibers
Figure 5.2. Schematic diagram of lamellar and zonal distribution of olivary afferents and
efferents. The two lamellae (folds) of DAO (dorsal accessory olive; 1 and 2) and the horizontal
lamella of MAO (medial accessory olive; 3) appear to receive afferents mainly from the spinal
cord and dorsal column nuclei while projecting to the anterior vermis and parts of intermediate
cerebellum. The medial MAO (vertical lamella; 4) receives from the vestibular and visual areas,
and projects to the posterior vermis and the flocculus. The rostral lamella of MAO and both
lamellae of PO (principal olive) receive projections from higher centers and send fibers to
the lateral hemispheres. In the lower part of the figure, three drawings of the inferior olive
demonstrate the lamellae corresponding to their sagittal zones of projection in the cerebellum.
(From Azizi and Woodward 1987; J. Comparative Neurol. Vol. 263, copyright 1987, Wiley-Liss,
Inc., a subsidiary of John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
5.1. The Inferior Olivary System 45
Figure 5.3. Portion of main olivary nucleus. A, grey matter; B, white matter; 1, large neuron
with unramified dendrites; 2 and 3, peripherally located cells with fan-shaped arbors; 4 and 5,
centrally located cells with spherical dendrite arbors. Neuron 1 is classified as Type I, neurons
2–5 as Type II. Two-week-old human infant. Golgi × 220. (From Scheibel and Scheibel 1955; in:
J. Comparative Neurol. Vol. 102, copyright 1955, Wiley-Liss, Inc., a subsidiary of John Wiley &
Sons, Inc.; reprinted by permission of the publisher; See also Ruigrok et al. 1990.)
topographical localization within the nucleus. Similarly, from their extensive study on
the inferior olive (i.e., medial accessory olive [MAO]) of the cat, Ruigrok et al. (1990)
reiterated the distinction between two types of cells in the inferior olive drawn earlier
by Scheibel and Scheibel (1955; Fig. 5.3): Type I, with sparsely branched dendrites
that radiate away from a usually small cell body, and Type II (located more rostrally)
in the MAO, with dendrites that branch frequently, forming a ball-like structure that
overlaps with those of perhaps 100 other olivary neurons.
Most, if not all, of the spines of both type I cells (characterized by dendrites that
run away from the soma) and type II cells (characterized by dendrites that tend to turn
Figure 5.4. Terminal patterns of axons afferent to the inferior olive. (A) Bushy arbor formed by
terminating axon 1. Collateral b, from fiber 2 also contributes to the plexus, whereas collateral
a of fiber 1 probably contributes to an adjacent plexus. (B) Rosette-bearing heavy afferent
fiber. (C) Bouton-bearing fiber of moderate or fine caliber. (Rapid Golgi method, 7–10-day-
old kitten material, at same magnification.) (From Scheibel, Scheibel, Walberg, and Brodal
1956; J. Comparative Neurol. Vol. 106, copyright 1956, Wiley-Liss, Inc., a subsidiary of John
Wiley & Sons, Inc.; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
46 The Inferior Olivary System and the Climbing Fibers
Figure 5.5. Patterns of afferent fibers in inferior olive; 1, 2, and 5, parent axons for bushy arbors
a, b, and d; c, collateral that leaves inferior olive without terminating; 3, fine fiber with simple
bouton endings, probably from region of medial longitudinal fasciculus; 4, heavy afferent fiber
with bouton clusters and rosettes. Ten-day-old kitten. Golgi method × 440. (From Scheibel and
Scheibel 1955; in J. Comparative Neurol. Vol. 102, copyright 1955, Wiley-Liss, Inc., a subsidiary
of John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
back toward the soma) were found to be located within olivary glomeruli (complex
synaptic arrangements). Further, all glomerular spines received both a GABAergic
(inhibitory) and non-GABAergic (probably excitatory) synaptic input (de Zeeuw,
Ruigrok, Holstege, Jansen, and Voogd 1990).
It appeared that cells with more compact dendritic arborization are of a more
advanced type (Fig. 5.3, cell types 2–5). Interestingly, no cells of this type were found
in the phylogenetically oldest subdivision of the olivary complex (i.e., the rostral pole
of the medial accessory olive; the cap of Kooy), which projects to the flocculonodular
lobe of the cerebellum. According to Armstrong (1974), citing Kooy, there is an in-
ferior olive in all vertebrates with the possible exception of cyclostomes (Chapter 2),
although in fishes and reptiles the nucleus consists of a diffuse group of cells that do
not have the ovoid form characteristic of the mammalian olivary cell.
Of the three fiber terminal types (Fig. 5.4), it was suggested that the heavy rosette-
bearing afferents may represent recently developed rapidly transmitting systems,
whereas the bushy afferents, distributed more widely within the olive, may represent
a more ancient and generalized type of afferent system (Scheibel et al. 1956). In
humans, there are about 1,000,000 total cells for both olives, as compared with about
15,000,000 Purkinje cells (Armstrong, 1974).
Rutherford and Gwyn (1980) reported, in the squirrel monkey, a pattern similar
to the ones described above (i.e., of two types of dendritic arborization – the one
having dendrites curling around the soma in a “ball-like” pattern and the other
having dendrites streaming away from the soma; Fig. 5.3). By electron microscopy,
these authors found axosomatic synapses to be of the symmetrical (inhibitory) type,
5.1. The Inferior Olivary System 47
Figure 5.6. Hypothetical diagram showing possible relationships among the various types of
afferent axonal terminals and neurons of the inferior olive. A, Bushy afferents arborize in
overlapping patterns within the olivary cell pool, D, Rosette-bearing heavy afferent fiber, C,
and bouton-bearing fiber, B, course obliquely through the pool, each establishing synaptic
relations with small numbers of cells. Cells 1 through 13 are probably capable of being directly
activated by an impulse train in bushy afferent 3. However, of this group, cells 3, 6, and 8 are
also assumed to receive synaptic contributions from fiber C and cells 8 and 12 from fiber B.
It is suggested that such fractionation of olivary cell ensemble 1 through 13 may provide an
anatomical substrate for selective patterns of activations. (From Scheibel and Scheibel 1955,
in: J. Comparative Neurol. Vol. 102, copyright 1955, Wiley-Liss, Inc., a subsidiary of John Wiley
& Sons, Inc.; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
whereas axodendritic synapses, which occurred at all levels of the dendritic tree, were
of the asymmetrical (excitatory) type. Gap junctions (the morphological correlate
of low-resistance pathways between neurons, see below) were encountered in all
three divisions of the inferior olive. Intrinsic neurons (i.e., confined to the olive
itself) were considered to be quite rare, at best. (This finding contrasts with that
for the cerebellar nuclei, where intrinsic neurons [which are inhibitory] are found.)
However, among the conclusions drawn by King (1980) from studies on opossum and
cat were the following: (1) olivary neurons do not receive a primary input on their
cell bodies or proximal dendrites (which differs from the findings just mentioned);
(2) the majority of cerebellar and midbrain/thalamic axons terminate within synaptic
clusters (rosettes), in contrast to spinal afferents, which contact primarily dendritic
shafts; and (3) the locations of neurons that mediate olivary IPSPs were not identified.
which 84% were type II (inhibitory) synapses and only 16% were type I (excita-
tory). Inhibitory terminals were found in the glomeruli characterizing the olivary
neuropil, within which formations of olivary neurons were electrotonically coupled
through dendrodendritic gap junctions (see below). This arrangement suggested to
the authors a synaptic modulation of the electrical coupling such that the release
of the inhibitory transmitter GABA, by increasing nonjunctional membrane con-
ductance, could shunt the coupling between olivary neurons. However, only a small
proportion of the GABA innervation of the olive would be involved in the synaptic
control, because only 14% of the GAD (glutamaic acid decarboxylase, the GABA-
synthesizing enzyme)-positive terminals analyzed were in a glomerular location, with
all the fibers arising presumably from the cerebellar nuclei. Such a functional decou-
pling of selected gap junctions could be responsible for spatial organization of the
olivary electrotonic coupling. These authors (Sotelo, Gotow, and Wassef 1986) also
concluded that the inferior olive appears to be composed solely of projecting neu-
rons, without interneurons. (Concerning the question of intrinsic rhythmic activity
of inferior olivary neurons, see Chapter 13.)
nucleus of Cajal). There are some projections from the caudate nucleus, only ques-
tionably from the globus pallidus, but evidently not from the putamen.
A final, but important, source of projections to the inferior olive derive from the
cerebral cortex, of which all regions contribute (at least in the cat), but preponderantly
the sensorimotor region (see also Chapter 9).
Berkley and Worden (1978) found several regions of overlap in the inferior olive
of somatosensory and motor inputs. Cells were found that responded to activation
of both the cerebral cortex and the spinal cord, a finding of possible relevance to the
concept of the inferior olive as a comparator (see below).
In general, the terminations from this great diversity of sources in the different
parts of the inferior olive (i.e., the principal olive and the dorsal and medial accessory
olives) appears to be topographical (i.e., well localized, although with some overlap).
For example, afferents from spinal cord overlap only partially with those from cere-
bral cortex, whereas afferents from spinal cord overlap extensively with those from
mesencephalic afferents.
From their studies on the rat, Azizi and Woodward (1987) concluded that the in-
ferior olive can be divided into three functional regions on the basis of their common
afferents and efferents (Fig. 5.2): (1) the dorsal accessory olive (DAO) and caudal
medial accessory olive (MAO) form a somatic sensory zone; (2) the medial area of
the MAO is the visual-vestibular zone; and (3) the principal olive (PO) in conjunction
with the rostral MAO is the third zone integrating inputs from higher centers.
and Mugnaini 1989). It follows, in the view of these authors, that GABA modulation
of climbing fiber activity is an important feature of cerebellar control of movement.
Dietrichs and Walberg (1989) pointed out that, if the cerebellar nucleo-olivary
projection is entirely GABAergic and thus inhibitory, then the olivocerebellar neu-
rons may be inhibited by the cerebellar nucleo-olivary fibers, whereas disinhibi-
tion could occur through the Purkinje cells acting on the cerebellar nucleo-olivary
neurons.
De Zeeuw, Ruigrok, Holstege, Jansen, and Voogd (1990) concluded that the cere-
bellar nuclear projection to the IO is derived from rather small cells with thin axons,
whereas larger cerebellar nuclear cells, besides terminating in the thalamus, also
provide a projection to mesodiencephalic regions from which the excitatory olivary
projections are derived. It was further concluded that the combined GABAergic and
non-GABAergic input to glomerular spines are consistent with the hypothesis that
they may modulate the electrotonic coupling of olivary cells (see below), and at the
same time, their firing frequency in a timing-sensitive way.
In addition to a GABAergic inhibitory input to the inferior olive (medial acces-
sory olive MAO) from the cerebellar nuclei (i.e., the interpositus), de Zeeuw et al.
(1989) reported evidence of another GABAergic input of noncerebellar origin and
raised the question of whether the noncerebellar input may regulate the excitability
of IO cells, whereas the cerebellar nuclear input modulates synchronous firing via its
effect on the gap junctions among IO cell dendrites.
Summarizing their own and the work of others, De Zeeuw, Ruigrok, Holstege,
Jansen, and Voogd (1990) pointed out that in the cat there are two major afferent
systems to the medial accessory olive (MAO) and also to the principal olive (PO): a
GABAergic (inhibitory) input derived from cerebellar nuclei, and a non-GABAergic
(excitatory) innervation from the mesodiencephalic junction.
turn, the dorsal cap sends climbing fibers to Purkinje cells in the flocculonodular lobe.
These authors found that the principal electron microscopic features of the dorsal
cap are the same as those of other olivary subdivisions. Thus, the neuropil contains
glomeruli (synaptic clusters) and extraglomerular synaptic fields. The neurons are
coupled by gap junctions, and this nucleus contains numerous GABA-positive and
GABA-negative axon terminals (Fig. 5.7).
In pursuing earlier indications that the cerebellar nuclei provided an (indirect)
excitatory as well as a direct inhibitory (GABAergic) effect on the inferior olive,
De Zeeuw and Ruigrok (1994) concluded that the indirect cerebellar projection to
the inferior olive via the midbrain nucleus of Darkschewitsch is disynaptic and excita-
tory. It was further concluded that the direct GABAergic terminals from the cerebel-
lar nuclei contact the same individual spines in the olivary glomeruli as the excitatory
mesodiencephalic terminals, indicating that the indirect excitatory pathway could act
in conjunction with the direct cerebellar GABAergic input, as shown in Figure 5.8.
5.2. Projections to the Inferior Olive 53
Figure 5.8. Diagram of the neuropil in the medial accessory olive and principal olive (bottom),
and its relation with the cerebellum (top) and mesodiencephalic junction (MJ, left side). All
olivary spines (half circles) are located within glomeruli (dotted circles), and are innervated by
both an excitatory mesodiencephalic and an inhibitory cerebellar terminal (white and black
triangles, respectively). The olivary axons provide climbing fibers to the Purkije cells (PC) in
the cerebellar cortex and collaterals to both the GABAergic and excitatory cerebellar nuclei
(CN) neurons. The GABAergic projection neurons in the cerebellar nuclei project exclusively
to the inferior olive (IO), whereas a substantial part of the excitatory projection neurons
in these nuclei innervate the neurons in the mesodiencephalic junction that in turn project
back to the inferior olive. The excitatory and inhibitory neurons in the cerebellar nuclei can
receive input from the same Purkinje cell axon. Small lines between olivary spines indicate
the dendrodendritic gap junctions by which they are electronically coupled. (From De Zeeuw,
Simpson, et al. 1998; reprinted from Trends in Neurosciences Vol. 21, copyright 1998, Elsevier
Science; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
54 The Inferior Olivary System and the Climbing Fibers
Teune, van der Burg, and Ruigrok (1995) found in rats that cerebellar nuclear
projections to the red nucleus and those to the inferior olive originate from sepa-
rate populations, and that consequently, these two pathways may transmit different
information. These authors confirmed the finding that the neurons projecting to the
magnocellular part of the red nucleus are mainly found in the interposed nuclei,
whereas those projecting to its parvocellular part are predominantly located in the
lateral (dentate) cerebellar nucleus.
Ruigrok and Voogd (1995) emphasized the presence of the two pathways from
cerebellar nuclei to the inferior olive: the first, excitatory, via nuclei at the mesodi-
encephalic junction thence to the inferior olive, and the second, a direct (nucleo-
olivary), GABAergic pathway, the latter having an important role in regulating the
degree of electrotonic coupling among olivary neurons (probably by a shunting mech-
anism), thus rendering them more difficult to activate by incoming afferent volleys.
These authors suggested that such coupled olivary neurons, once activated, however,
may develop membrane potential oscillations that are electrotonically conveyed to
neighboring neurons (see below), and subsequently, during the depolarizing phase
of the oscillation, result in a more easily triggered rebound or long-latency response.
Horn, van Kan, and Ruigrok (1996) found that the inhibitory (GABAergic) effect
from nuclear cells on inferior olivary cells reduced the sensitivity of the latter rather
than reduced the receptive fields, which could be expected were the inhibitory effect
on the regulation of inferior olivary gap junctions.
olive, from which climbing fibers to the Purkinje cells of the flocculonodular lobe
of the cerebellum originate, and which are involved in the visual control of eye
movements, have been shown in the rabbit to receive a GABAergic (inhibitory)
input from the ventral dentate nucleus of the cerebellum and also from a group of
cells known as the dorsal group y. (The nucleus prepositus hypoglossi (PrH) is the
major source of GABAergic input for the dorsal cap of Kooy [De Zeeuw, Gerrits,
et al. 1994]).
5.3. Ultrastructure
In an ultrastructural (electronmicroscopic) study (De Zeeuw, Ruigrok, Holstege,
Schalekramp, and Voogd [1990]), it was found that the GABAergic input to the
olivary cell bodies was partly derived from a noncerebellar source, whereas the
GABAergic terminals apposed to the axons appear to be derived from the cere-
bellar nuclei. Axonal spines, like dendritic spines, receive a combined inhibitory and
excitatory input. Both GABAergic and non-GABAergic terminals were seen on both
type I and type II olivary neurons, but terminations on the axons of type I neurons
were mainly on the axonal shafts, whereas most of the terminations on axons of type
II neurons were on axonal spines.
In another ultrastructural study (De Zeeuw, Holstege, et al. 1990), at least one
third of labeled glomeruli (rosettes) appeared to contain both cerebellar nuclear (i.e.,
inhibitory) terminals and mesodiencephalic (nucleus of Darkschewitsch and retic-
ular tegmental nucleus [of Bechterew]) (i.e., excitatory) terminals. In many cases,
the terminals from both afferent systems contacted the same dendritic spines. The
authors suggested that the timing between these two afferents in the rostral medial
accessory olive (MAO) and principal olive (PO), which project to the cerebellar
hemisphere and give off collaterals to the posterior interposed and dentate nucleus,
may be important in relation to the function of the olivocerebellar system, in partic-
ular, in relation to a timing function.
of complex spikes in different Purkinje cell zones of the flocculus that receive their
climbing fibers from olivary subnuclei having different densities of lamellar bodies.
It was found that complex spike synchrony of Purkinje cell pairs receiving their
climbing fibers from an olivary subnucleus with a high density of lamellar bodies was
significantly higher than that of Purkinje cells receiving their climbing fibers from a
subnucleus with a low density of lamellar bodies. Because the density of DLBs in
the IO is higher than in any other part of the brain, the importance of electrotonic
coupling via dendrodendritic gap junctions in the function of the olivocerebellar
system seemed evident (De Zeeuw, Koekkoek, et al. 1997).
CORTEX:
Purkinje cells
CENTRAL NUCLEI
OUTPUT
INFERIOR OLIVE
D
C3
C2
C1
B
A
A
B
C1
C2
C3
D
Figure 5.9. The cerebellum as a machine: cerebellar efferent modules. Each module con-
sists of a longitudinal strip of Purkinje cells that project to a particular central cerebellar
nucleus. These Purkinje cells and the corresponding nucleus receive fibers from a part of the
inferior olive. Each module has its own nuclear efferents; collateral projections feed back
into the inferior olive. Six symmetrically disposed, independent modules are presented on
each side of the midline. (Note the nucleo-olivary feedback loop; however, the nucleocortical
feedback loops are not shown.) (From Voogd and Bigaré 1980; in: Courville, de Montigny,
and Lamarre (eds.), The Inferior Olivary Nucleus: Anatomy and Physiology, copyright 1980,
Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; reprinted by permission of the publisher and Prof. Voogd, the
author.)
lateral (dentate) and interpositus nuclei (Chan-Palay 1977). In this connection, Chan-
Palay (1977) drew attention to a remarkable resemblance between the neurons of
the nuclear border in the dentate nucleus (Figs. 6.2 and 6.3), and the neurons lo-
cated at the periphery of the principal olivary nucleus (Fig. 5.3), which delineate
the margins of the nucleus by turning their dendritic arborizations internally (com-
pare also Fig. 5.9). The question was raised of whether such a similarity in the ge-
ometry and orientation of dendritic arborizations between the two structures re-
flects their intimate relation in connectivity, structure, and function (Chan-Palay
1977).
The electrophysiological importance of collaterals to the cerebellar nuclei from
climbing fibers (as well as from mossy fibers) was stressed by Llinás and Mühlethaler
(1988), who, following electrical stimulation of the cerebellar cortex or the underlying
white matter, recorded excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (EPSP–
IPSP sequences) in cerebellar nuclear neurons. The EPSPs were elicited by direct
activation of collaterals of climbing (or of mossy) fiber afferents; the IPSPs followed
direct or orthodromic Purkinje cell activation. The integrity of the olivocerebellar
system was tested with harmaline, which had powerful synaptic effects on nuclear
cells.
5.9. Parallels Between Olivocerebellar and Corticonuclear Projections 59
feedback to the cerebellar cortex from nuclear cells via mossy fibers (not included
in Fig. 5.9), is also both excitatory and inhibitory (cf. Chan-Palay 1977, p. 379). (This
question is also considered in Chapter 6.)
Figure 5.10. Oscarsson’s comparator hypothesis for the inferior olive. The diagram is centered
around the functional unit consisting of a sagittal zone with its olivary region and cerebellar
nucleus and the lower motor center controlled by this unit (thick outlines). It is assumed that
the olivary region monitors commands from higher motor centers, the activity these commands
evoke in the lower motor center, and the resulting movement. By comparing information from
these sources, the olive would detect perturbations of the commands introduced in the lower
center by reflex activity and pertubations in the evolving movement due to unexpected changes
in load or resistance. Information about these perturbations might be used by the sagittal zone
to send signals of correction either directly to the lower motor center (path 1) or to the higher
centers (path 2). (Note that the mossy fibers are not included in the figure.) Further explanation
in text. (From Oscarsson 1980; in: Courville, de Montigny, and Lamarre (eds.), The Inferior
Olivary Nucleus: Anatomy and Physiology, copyright 1980, Lippincott Williams & Wilkins;
reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
considered the hypothesis consistent with concepts of the cerebellum as a seat of mo-
tor learning in which the olive plays a fundamental role of evaluating the correctness
of a motor act and, through its action, modifies heretosynaptically the transmission
through the granule cell–Purkinje cell synapses that are coactivated with the climbing
fibers.
generally do not converge on the same olivary neurons. Rather, a comparison would
be much more likely to occur between the ascending and descending inputs (which
are all excitatory), on the one hand, and the inhibitory projection from the hind-
brain, on the other hand. Each dendritic spine of an olivary neuron receives both
an inhibitory input from a hindbrain center (i.e., cerebellar nuclei, vestibular nuclei,
nucleus prepositus hypoglossi, solitary nucleus, dorsal column nuclei) and an excita-
tory input from the spinal cord, brain stem, mesodiencephalic junction, or cerebral
cortex.
In their review of the microcircuitry and possible functions of the IO, includ-
ing learning and timing of movements, and comparison of intended with achieved
movements, De Zeeuw, Simpson, et al. (1998) concluded that the IO is capable of
functioning in both motor learning and motor timing, but not with achieved move-
ments in the sense originally proposed by Oscarsson (1980). However, the IO was
considered to be capable of selecting and transmitting error signals. The authors
based their conclusions on recent findings indicating that the descending and as-
cending projections to the IO generally do not converge on the same olivary neuron,
as mentioned above. However, in view of the (unique) arrangement, whereby each
dendritic spine of an olivary neuron receives an inhibitory input from one of the
hindbrain regions (which include the cerebellar nuclei, vestibular nuclei, nucleus
prepositus hypoglossi, and solitary nucleus) and an excitatory input (from the spinal
cord, brain stem, mesodiencephalic junction, or cerebral cortex), it seemed to the
authors that a comparison within the IO would be much more likely to occur between
the excitatory descending and ascending excitatory inputs, on the one hand, and the
inhibitory projections derived from the hindbrain, on the other hand, as summarized
above.
signal errors to the cerebellum whenever there is a mismatch between intended and
actual movements (Oscarsson 1979).
Ekerot (1999), in a commentary on the above-cited paper of Apps and Lee (1999),
drew attention to (1) the close relationship of climbing fiber cutaneous receptive field
of a module to the associated controlled movement, and (2) the observation that
climbing fibers easily activated by stimuli in a passive animal are not activated when
similar stimuli result from active movement (i.e., during locomotion) but may dis-
charge in response to unexpected stimuli. Thus, the gating of climbing fiber pathways
during locomotion could be an efficient way to eliminate self-generated activation
of climbing fibers during normal locomotion and to facilitate responses to inputs
occurring during movement phases when no peripheral input is expected. Ekerot
(1999) concluded that the results of Apps and Lee (1999) suggested that different
strategies, specifically adapted to the function of the controlled motor system, are
used by climbing fibers to evaluate motor performance.
The concept that the cerebellar cortex is divided into a number of sagittally ori-
ented zones, each defined by its specific afferent connections from the IO and its
efferent connections to the cerebellar (or vestibular) nuclei (thus forming a series
of olivo–cortico–nuclear compartments or “modules”), continues to be explored,
including in the waking animal (Apps 2000; Garwicz 2000).
In a computer modeling approach to the problem of an apparent discrepancy
between anatomical and physiological information concerning the zonal organization
of climbing fibers in the rat cerebellar cortex, Lee and Bower (1990) reported that
their results suggested that several unique features of the olivocerebellar circuit may
contribute to the appearance of zonal organization using anatomical techniques,
but that the detailed pattern of patchy tactile projections seen with physiological
techniques provide a more accurate representation of the organization in the part
modeled (crus IIA of the lateral hemisphere).
controlled by the interplay between climbing and parallel fiber influences on Purkinje
cell dendrites.
Zagrebelsky et al. (1996) found adaptive recovery of collateral sprouting (i.e.,
restoration of topography of projection maps) following IO lesions in the adult rat,
from which it was concluded that topographic cues are available in the adult during
postlesion plasticity to guide the restoration of the olivocerebellar projection map.
six
68
6.1. The Cerebellar Nuclei 69
Cell Types. In the human dentate nucleus, Golgi preparations reveal the presence
of two classes of nerve cells: relatively large projection cells, which predominate, and
small local-circuit neurons, which are scattered throughout the nuclear gray (Braak
and Braak 1984). There are also small GABAergic cells projecting to olivary cells
(Chapter 5). De Zeeuw, Van Alphen, et al. (1997) found that olivary axon collaterals
(i.e., collaterals of climbing fibers) innervate not only non-GABAergic neurons in
the cerebellar nuclei but also GABAergic nucleo-olivary cells, thus establishing a
direct feedback loop to the inferior olive.
For the large dentate neurons, most of their somata, proximal dendrites, and initial
axonal segments are covered by the inhibitory terminals of Purkinje cell axons (Chan-
Palay 1977). The distal receptive surfaces of these cells, however, have numerous ap-
pendages of presumably high and low resistance that participate in multiple glomeru-
lar arrangements with excitatory inputs. The small neurons, however, appear to have
a less sharp spatial division (proximal vs. distal) of excitatory and inhibitory inputs.
The terminal arborization of a single Purkinje axon is approximately conical in
shape (Fig. 6.1), a pattern that is not unlike that in the inferior olive (Fig. 5.4). The
resulting relationship between Purkinje axon terminal field and cells and their den-
drites is depicted in Figure 6.2. As drawn in Figure 6.3 and indicated diagrammatically
in Figure 6.4, collaterals of mossy and climbing fibers (both of which are excitatory),
on their radial path through the dentate nucleus, pass sequentially through the den-
dritic fields or territories of successive neurons, whereas the conical terminal fields
of Purkinje axons enclose entire neurons and parts of many neurons.
70 The Cerebellar Nuclei and Their Efferent Pathways: Voluntary Motor Learning
Figure 6.1. Diagram of the radial arrangement of Purkinje axon conical fields in the dentate
(lateral) nucleus. Axonal plexus A shows the branchlets that define the conical field (of base
diameter of about 100–150 mu) of one Purkinje cell. The remaining cones show variations of
this basic pattern. In actuality, the cones overlap. (From Chan-Palay 1977; Cerebellar Dentate
Nucleus, copyright 1977, Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg; reprinted by permission from publisher
and author.)
Figure 6.2. The relation between neurons (only samples are shown) of different types with
their dendrites and the conical Purkinje axon field. Note that cones B and C (stippled) begin
more deeply than cone A (dashed). Note also the trend to a particular orientation of specific
types of neurons to the direction of the cones. (From Chan-Palay 1977; Cerebellar Dentate
Nucleus, copyright 1977, Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg; reprinted by permission from publisher
and author.)
6.1. The Cerebellar Nuclei 71
Figure 6.3. Diagram of organization of neurons and afferent axons in the lateral nucleus. The
nucleus is encircled by packets of myelinated fibers (stippled). Parts of the nucleus have been
cut away to show the arrangement of cells within. Note: (1) The columnar zone with its large
columnar neurons (black, C) and bridging neurons (white, I); (2) the caudal pole with a large
multipolar neuron (black, M) and small neurons (white, S). Purkinje axons (P) enter the central
zone and may encompass whole columnar neurons. Extracerebellar afferents (NC) radiate
across the nucleus, passing through columns of large neurons in succession. The small neurons
are tilted at an angle and receive a large number of axonal contacts. In the noncolumnar zone,
the Purkinje axon (PX) encompasses parts of the dendrites of several multipolar neurons,
both large (M) and small (S). An extracerebellar axon (NCX) is shown. The relationships
are represented diagrammatically in Figure 6.4. (From Chan-Palay 1977; Cerebellar Dentate
Nucleus, copyright 1977, Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg; reprinted by permission from publisher
and author.)
It is evident from Figure 6.4 that the large columnar neurons (C) and their
dendritic fields tend to fall largely into the territory of one Purkinje axonal cone,
whereas the large and small multipolar (swirl) neurons tend to share their well-
spread dendritic fields into neighboring Purkinje axonal cones. It was suggested
(Chan-Palay 1977) that such a restricted effect of the inhibitory field on single neu-
rons could provide a means of preserving the specificity of the corticonuclear input,
whereas the serial arrangement of the mossy and climbing fiber collaterals could
provide a carefully timed sequence of excitatory stimuli to successive neurons, thus
72 The Cerebellar Nuclei and Their Efferent Pathways: Voluntary Motor Learning
Figure 6.4. Diagram, based on Figure 6.3, of the relation between columnar neurons and swirls
of neurons with their afferent inputs. On the right, the columns are presented; on the left, the
swirls of multipolar neurons are shown. Because of their slender profile, individual columnar
neurons (C) fall under the influence of single Purkinje axonal cones. The extracerebellar (NC1 –
NC2 ) axons cross the columns of several neurons in succession. The latter axons come in three
thicknesses: thick (NC1 ), medium (NC2 ), and fine (NC3 ). The small neurons (I) bridging the
columns are tilted to receive a larger number of contacts with the noncortical afferent axons
as well as the conical fields. In the swirled zones of multipolar cells, the Purkinje axons (PX)
encompass portions of two (or more) large (M) and small (S) neurons. The extracerebellar
axons (NCX) cross neurons in succession and various thicknesses of fibers are indicated. (From
Chan-Palay 1977; Cerebellar Dentate Nucleus, copyright 1977, Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg;
reprinted by permission from publisher and author.)
Purkinje Cell Axons Synapses onto Both Excitatory and Inhibitory Nuclear
Neurons. De Zeeuw and Berrebi (1995, 1996) found that individual Purkinje cells
can innervate both excitatory and inhibitory neurons in the cerebellar and vestibular
nuclei, suggesting that the excitatory cerebellar and vestibular output system and the
6.1. The Cerebellar Nuclei 73
inhibitory feedback to the inferior olive are controlled simultaneously. The authors
pointed out that Purkinje cells thus appear to simultaneously control the inhibitory
feedback to the inferior olive (as part of a closed three-element anatomical path-
way), and also the excitatory cerebellar output system, as part of an open anatomical
pathway influencing motor commands. The authors also suggested that this finding
could explain why olivary neurons appear to be unresponsive to stimuli generated
during active movement (Teune et al. 1998; Chapter 5).
Serotonin and Cerebellar Nuclear Cells. Cerebellar nuclear cells are also subject to
the influence of serotonin from serotoninergic fibers originating in the brain stem,
suppressing amino acid (excitatory) synapses and enhancing GABAergic (inhibitory)
synapses, so the net effect is a decrease in nuclear cell activity and consequently in
cerebellar output (Kitzman and Bishop 1997).
perhaps more likely, through the (contralateral) reticular tegmental nuclei. (See also
Chapter 9, in relation to cognition.)
Fastigial Nucleus. For the fastigial nucleus (the most medial nucleus), the main tar-
gets of the efferents are the pontine and medullary reticular formation and the
vestibular nuclei, although there have also been some reports of terminations in the
thalamus (nucleus ventralis posterior lateralis [VPL] and nucleus ventralis lateralis
[VL]). Fibers passing directly to the spinal cord have been reported but questioned.
The presence of some fibers to the following destinations have also been mentioned:
pontine nuclei, nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis, lateral reticular nucleus, parame-
dian reticular nucleus, and perihypoglossal nuclei.
Nuclei Interpositi (Anterior and Posterior) and Lateral (Dentate) Nucleus. These
nuclei give off fibers that leave the cerebellum via the superior cerebellar peduncle.
A considerable number of the fibers end in the thalamus, many in the red nucleus,
and some in other destinations, particularly the brain stem. The interpositus anterior
projects in a topical pattern to the caudal two thirds of the magnocellular part of
the red nucleus. The large cells in the latter give rise to the relatively coarse fibers
of the rubrospinal tract. The dentate nucleus (and also the interpositus posterior, to
some extent), however, projects topically to the rostral one third of the red nucleus,
the parvicellular part, the small cells of which give rise to the rather fine fibers of the
rubro-olivary tract (see below).
In the monkey, Asanuma, Thach, and Jones (1983a) found that the thalamic
termination zones for the dentate, interposed, and fastigial nuclei were identical and
coincided with the cytoarchitectonically unique cell-sparse region of the thalamic
ventral lateral complex. Asanuma, Thach, and Jones (1983b) also studied the brain
stem and spinal projections of the cerebellar nuclei. The brain stem terminations of
fibers arising in the dentate and interposed nuclei were similar – the red nucleus, the
nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis, and the inferior olivary complex – but there were
some differences. Thus, the dentate nucleus projected only to the parvocellular red
nucleus and the principal olivary nucleus, whereas the interposed nucleus projected
only to the magnocellular red nucleus and to the two accessory olivary nuclei. There
were extensive fastigial projections to the brain stem: nucleus reticularis tegmenti
pontis, the lateral vestibular (Deiter’s) nucleus, portions of the descending vestibular
nucleus, the pontine nuclei, the pontine raphe, and parts of the reticular formation
6.2. Efferent Cerebellar Nuclear Pathways 75
from the midbrain caudally. Projections to the spinal cord arose in the interposed
and fastigial nuclei. No spinal projections were detected from the dentate nucleus.
Brodal (1981) pointed out that, from a functional point of view, it is of interest that
the number of afferents to the red nucleus from the cerebral cortex is far smaller than
the number of cerebellar nuclear afferents. In agreement with this fact, physiological
studies have indicated that the cerebellar input to the red nucleus is more powerful
than that from the cerebral cortex. Evidently little is known, however, about the
corticorubral and rubrospinal tracts in humans.
Mention is made of a projection, in the monkey, passing from the dentate nucleus
directly to the oculomotor nucleus, in a region known in the cat and monkey to
receive primary vestibular fibers. These dentato-oculomotor fibers may therefore be
a link in the vestibulo-ocular pathway.
Finally, the thalamic terminations of fibers from the nuclei interpositi and lateralis
are not identical even if they overlap to some extent. In the cat, most fibers end in the
lateral part of the nucleus ventralis lateralis, but some end laterally in the ventralis
anterior and in the most medial part of the ventralis posterior lateralis.
A history of this question is given by Gould and Graybiel (1976); Tolbert, Bantli, and
Bloedel (1976); Chan-Paley (1977, p. 352); Gould (1979); and Bloedel and Courville
(1981). That there is an orderly nucleocortical projection within the cerebellum for
each of the cerebellar nuclei, as well as for the vestibular complex, was demon-
strated by neuroanatomical techniques in the cat by Gould and Graybiel (1976) and
by Tolbert, Bantli, and Bloedel (1976) (see also Brodal and Kawamura 1980). The
nucleocortical connection appeared to follow (in inverse direction) the plan of the
inhibitory (Purkinje) pathway from the cerebellar cortex to the nuclei, thus providing
an intrinsic feedback loop within the cerebellum.
The existence of a cerebellar nuclear-cortex projection was soon found in the den-
tate and interposed nuclei of a primate (Macaca mulatta), as in the cat, by Tolbert,
Bantli, and Bloedel (1977), using electrophysiological and anatomical techniques.
From their experiments, these authors concluded that the fibers of the nucleocortical
projection arise at least in part as collaterals from nuclear neurons projecting to ex-
tracerebellar structures such as the inferior olive, the ventrolateral thalamic nucleus,
or the red nucleus, depending on their site in the nuclei. In the cat, the fibers appeared
to arise from all the different types of neurons in the cerebellar nuclei (Gould and
Graybiel 1976; McCrea, Bishop, and Kitai 1978; Tolbert, Bantli, and Bloedel 1978),
although in the monkey they appeared to arise principally from large multipolar
neurons (Chan-Palay 1977; Tolbert, Bantli, and Bloedel 1978). Chan-Palay (1977)
considered the neurons of the nuclear border, columnar neurons, and large asym-
metrical neurons to be possible candidates for the nuclear cells of origin.
The nucleocortical projection was classified as a mossy fiber afferent system ter-
minating in the granular layer of the cerebellar cortex, and thus using the indirect
cortical route (i.e., via the granule cell–parallel fiber network) to the Purkinje cells.
Thus, the typical repeated branching of mossy fibers was observed for the nucleocor-
tical fibers along their course to the cerebellar cortex. As already mentioned, it was
suggested that the nucleocortical and corticonuclear projections may be reciprocally
organized (Tolbert, Bantli, and Bloedel 1978), although not completely so (Bloedel
and Courville 1981; Chan-Palay 1977; Tolbert and Bantli 1979) (see Fig. 6.5).
Because the counterpart “nucleus” of the flocculus is the vestibular nuclear com-
plex, it is reasonable to expect a corresponding feedback loop, comparable to that
from the cerebellar nuclei to other parts of the cerebellar cortex. Such evidence of
feedback from vestibular nuclei as mossy fibers to the granular layer in the flocculus
was found by Miles et al. (1980).
The possibility of, in the main, a precise interaction between these two reciprocal
fiber systems was suggested (Haines and Pearson 1979), although a strict one-to-
one reciprocal was questioned (Dietrichs and Walberg 1979). In any case, it has
been proposed that the nucleocortical system is a major component of the cerebellar
cortical afferent system (Tolbert, Bantli, and Bloedel 1977).
Haines (1989) suggested that there are at least three, and possibly four, categories
of nucleocortical (NC) cells: (1) ipsilateral reciprocal (found in, or in the periphery
of, corticonuclear [CN] terminal fields formed by axons originating from the same
cortical area to which the NC cells project); (2) ipsilateral nonreciprocal NC cells
(located outside the CN terminal field); and (3) contralateral NC cells (found in the
6.2. Efferent Cerebellar Nuclear Pathways 77
Figure 6.5. Comparison of the axonal fields of Purkinje cells and dentate nuclear neu-
rons in the monkey. The Purkinje cells (PC) of the cerebellar cortex project single or
multiple conical axon fields onto dentate neurons. Dentatocortical fibers are extended
(perhaps as collaterals from extracerebellar projectional axons, as shown) and have
extensive terminal fields in the overlying cortex. Although the two fields are basically re-
ciprocal, the dentatocortical loop is more diffuse. (From Chan-Palay 1977; Cerebellar Dentate
Nucleus, copyright 1977, Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg; reprinted by permission from publisher
and author.)
opposite cerebellar nuclei). It seemed probable that the different types of NC cells
have individualized functional characteristics (Haines 1989).
From their study of the topography of the nucleocortical projection in the cat,
Trott, Apps, and Armstrong (1990), concluded that, at least for parts of the cerebel-
lum, the nucleocortical projection and the corticonuclear (Purkinje cell) projection
were not entirely reciprocal, a finding that could explain some earlier reports in-
dicating a weak nucleocortical projection to the cortex. Further, confirming earlier
reports, nucleocortical fibers were found to arise both from the small spindle-shaped
(fusiform) cells and the larger, multipolar cell populations within the cerebellar
nuclei (Fig. 6.3), indicating that both of these cell types can give off axon collaterals
to the cerebellar cortex, thus providing a feedback pathway.
vestibular nucleus. Buisseret-Delmas and Angaut (1989) pointed out that the sugges-
tion of Chan-Palay (1977), which is that the nucleocortical projection uses GABA as a
neurotransmitter, may suggest that the projection includes both inhibitory (GABA)
and excitatory fibers, because their termination as mossy fibers in the cerebellar
cortex would imply non-GABAergic (excitatory) terminations.
However, in their study of cat granular layer glomeruli, Hámori and Takács (1989)
found that, in addition to the inhibitory (GABAergic) axon terminals arising from
Golgi cells (Type I terminals), there was a second type (Type II) GABAergic termi-
nal (always accompanied by the first type), which arises from the cerebellar nuclei.
The synapses of Type II terminals, although GABAergic, had some resemblance to
excitatory terminals (i.e., large spheroidal vesicles). The authors indicated that, al-
though the functional significance of the nucleocortical GABAergic terminals was
unclear, this nucleocortical inhibitory innervation of the cerebellar cortex would have
to be taken into account in future modeling of the cerebellum.
From their study of nucleocortical projections in the rat using immunohistochem-
ical methods, Batini et al. (1992) concluded that both excitatory (glutaminergic) and
inhibitory (GABAergic) fibers were present, and both terminate in the cerebel-
lar cortex as mossy fibers some ipsilaterally (“reciprocally”), some contralaterally
(“symmetrically”), to the mossy fiber-type rosettes in the granule cell layer. The
authors characterized this feedback as a short-path, rapidly responding system con-
tributing to fine control of Purkinje cell activity and functioning as a local microin-
tegrator for the longer cerebellofugal pathways.
However, Kolston, Apps, and Trott (1995) did not find GABAergic nucleocortical
fibers in their study using a combined retrograde tracer and GABA-immunochemical
method, possibly for technical reasons, according to the authors.
Species Differences. Attention has also been drawn to species differences (Tolbert,
Bantli, and Bloedel 1978). In the cat, nucleocortical fibers originating in the dentate
nucleus project principally to the lateral hemisphere, whereas in primates, these fibers
diverge over large areas of the ipsilateral cerebellar cortex including the vermis. The
latter development appears to have emerged late in phylogenesis, reaching a signif-
icant development only in primates, which may be associated with the emergence
of a neo- and paleodentate, in the ventrolateral (smaller-neuron), and dorsome-
dial (larger-neuron) parts, respectively, of the dentate nucleus (Tolbert, Bantli, and
Bloedel 1978; for a review, see Tolbert 1982; see also Chapter 9). Umetani (1990)
suggested that the marked differences in primary cortical targets of the interpositus
and dentate nuclei among different species might represent the phylogenetic change
in the development of coordination of more complicated and skilled fine movements.
As mentioned above, it initially appeared that the nucleocortical fibers termi-
nate in the granular layer of the cortex, and hence are a part of the mossy fiber
input system and are thus excitatory (Chan-Palay 1977; Tolbert, Bantli, and Bloedel
1977). Electronmicroscopic studies confirmed the excitatory nature of these synapses
(Tolbert 1982). However, as mentioned, the possibility of an inhibitory nature for the
nucleocortical fibers originating from the dentate nucleus was raised (Tolbert 1982).
6.2. Efferent Cerebellar Nuclear Pathways 79
Indeed, Hámori, Takács, and Petrusz (1990) found two classes of glutamate
(excitatory) terminals in mossy fibers, with two somewhat differing sizes of synaptic
vesicles – the larger ones being in fibers of extracerebellar origin, and the smaller
ones being in fibers of intracerebellar origin (i.e., nucleocortical fibers). As mentioned
above, these authors also found two classes of GABA terminals within cerebellar
glomeruli of differing size of synaptic vesicles – the one of local cortical (i.e., Golgi
cell), and the other originating from cerebellar nuclei. These findings suggested the
existence of both inhibitory and excitatory feedback from cerebellar nuclei to cere-
bral cortex (Hámori, Takács, and Petrusz 1990).
example, nuclei onto which projections from specific regions of the cerebellum con-
verge with other fiber systems involved in the behavior.
the interactions responsible for the learning and performance of a motor sequence
that is optimal for executing the task.
Complementarity of Cerebellar Cortex and Nuclei. Mauk (1997) suggested that the
relative contributions of the cerebellar cortex and cerebellar nuclei are not constant
but instead may depend on the amount and type of training experienced by the ani-
mal, and that such a complementarity could account for apparently differing results
in motor learning, particularly adaptation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) and
blink conditioning.
Optimal Time-Varying Control of Joint Stiffness. Smith (1996) proposed that the
cerebellum has an important role in motor learning by forming and storing associated
muscle activation patterns for the time-varying control of limb mechanics (i.e., of
agonist–antagonist muscle synergies or joint stiffness).
were also found when the results for a joystick task were compared with merely
imagining the same task. The authors considered their results to indicate that the
neocerebellum may be much more concerned with sensory information processing
than previously considered.
Dentate vs. Premotor Area. In a study of writing out unfamiliar nonsense characters,
Seitz et al. (1994) found that during the early phase of learning there was a significant
activation of the ipsilateral dentate nucleus. After overlearning, however, the dentate
nucleus was no longer activated, whereas the premotor cerebral cortical areas were
maximally activated. It was concluded that learning of new movement trajectories
involves the cerebellum, whereas overlearned trajectorial movements are carried out
by the premotor cortex.
CEREBELLAR FUNCTIONS
seven
On a theoretical basis (as summarized in Chapter 13), both Marr (1969) and Albus
(1971) invoked memory storage at the parallel fiber–Purkinje cell dendritic tree in-
terface if the parallel fibers and the climbing fibers (both excitatory) were conjointly
active. In Marr’s theory, the efficacy of these synapses was postulated to become en-
hanced (facilitated) with such conjoint activation, whereas in Albus’s theory, synaptic
efficacy became diminished (depressed). Ekerot and Oscarsson (1981) found that im-
pulses in climbing fibers resulted in a depolarization of Purkinje cell dendrites lasting
about 100 milliseconds. The authors conjectured that this effect might induce plastic
changes in the parallel fiber synapses onto Purkinje cell dendrites, as envisaged in
theories of motor learning by the cerebellum.
The diminished synaptic efficacy predicted by Albus was soon discovered (Ito
and Kano 1982; Ito, Sakurai, and Tongroach 1982) in the form of long-term depres-
sion (LTD; i.e., a significant diminution of the parallel fiber postsynaptic potentials
induced in Purkinje cells and lasting for at least 1 hour).
That there is indeed a capability for plasticity is illustrated for a subject (Fig. 7.1)
throwing darts at a target (Fig. 7.2) before wearing prism spectacles, while wearing
the spectacles, and after removing them. It is the direction of throw of the dart (which
is normally determined from the direction of gaze) that undergoes change followed
by adaptation or recalibration, which is then followed by a rebound and reverse
adaptation upon removal of the prism spectacles. However, if there is impairment
of the inferior olive, as for a patient with hypertrophy of the inferior olive, there
is no adaptation to wearing the prism glasses nor readaptation after their removal
(Fig. 7.3).
Nonetheless, the question of memory for movements in the cerebellum and, in
particular, of LTD as its basis, has continued to be somewhat controversial (see
Lisberger 1995). Thus, there have been questions as to whether, in experimentally
induced LTD, the climbing fiber stimulation rate is too fast in relation to the usual
background firing rate of about 1 per second; whether LTD is sufficiently lasting to
89
90 Cerebellar Memory, LTD, and LTP
Figure 7.1. Throwing darts while wearing wedge prism spectacles (base to the right). The
subject is looking directly at the target toward which she is pointing the dart, but because
the prism bends the optic path 15 degrees to the right, her gaze is deviated 15 degrees to
her left to see the target (she is looking at you). The portion of her face behind the lenses
appears to the viewer to be displaced to her left because of the prism’s bending of the optic
path. The direction of throw is normally in the direction of gaze. The gaze direction has,
however, been calibrated to the throw direction, and the aim of throw is true (at you). (From
Thach, Goodkin, and Keating 1992; in Annual Review of Neuroscience, Volume 15, copyright
1992, Annual Reviews [www.AnnualReviews.org]; reprinted by permission from publisher
and author.)
form the basis of memory; and whether the effect might actually be one of enhance-
ment rather than of LTD (Bloedel, Bracha, and Larson 1993; Bloedel and Kelly 1992).
100
90
80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
cm
−10
−20
−30
−40
−50
−60
−70
−80
−90
−100
0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 38 40 42 44 46 48 50 52 54
Throw
Figure 7.2. A plot of the horizontal location, relative to the target center, of each successive
dart hit. Before introduction of the prism, the darts hit close to the center of the target. Intro-
duction of the prism shifts the hits to the left (down), and with practice they normally return
toward the center of the target as the recalibration of gaze direction–throw direction is made.
After removal of the prism, the hits are normally shifted to the right (up), a result that shows
that the throw direction is still calibrated 15 degrees off the gaze direction (in actuality, the
error is never quite the whole 15 degrees). With practice, the gaze–throw directions readjust
back to the original value. (From Thach, Goodkin, and Keating 1992; in Annual Review of Neu-
roscience, Volume 15, copyright 1992, Annual Reviews [www.AnnualReviews.org]; reprinted
by permission of publisher and author.)
with developing and regenerating neurons, was increased in the Purkinje neurons
following learning, thus indicating that the induction of tPA may play a role in activity-
dependent synaptic plasticity.
92 Cerebellar Memory, LTD, and LTP
100
90
80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
cm
−10
−20
−30
−40
−50
−60
−70
−80
−90
−100
0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 38 40 42 44 46 48 50 52 54
Throw
Figure 7.3. Patient with hypertrophy of the inferior olive, a degenerative disease of the inferior
olive. After introduction of the prism, there is no recalibration of the gaze–throw directions,
and the throws remain to the left of target center (down). After removal of the prisms, the hits
land where they did before the introduction of the prisms, thus indicating no adaptation to the
prisms in this subject. (From Thach, Goodkin, and Keating 1992; in Annual Review of Neuro-
science, Volume 15, copyright 1992 Annual Reviews [www.AnnualReviews.org]; reprinted by
permission of publisher and author.)
7.1.2. Transfer of Plasticity from Purkinje Cells to Stellate and Basket Cells
For more reliable function and more reliable long-term storage, Kenyon (1997) pro-
posed plasticity at parallel fiber synapses onto stellate and basket neurons (which
greatly outnumber Purkinje cells by 10–20 times) as a backup to the modification of
synapses at parallel fiber–Purkinje cell synapses. The relatively short-term effect at
the Purkinje cells would be transferred (after a threshold is exceeded) to the stellate
and basket cells, which would then affect Purkinje cells via their inhibitory influence
on the latter. For Purkinje cells, the model assumes a bidirectional form (about an
equilibrium level) of climbing fiber-dependent LTD/long-term potentiation (LTP) –
decrease in synaptic weight when active during elevated climbing fiber input, and
increase when active during reduced climbing fiber input. Recurrent collaterals from
Purkinje cells to both stellate and basket cells were also assumed.
dendritic tree, but rather as a fine-tuning mechanism having its effect both in space
and in time on synaptic plasticity, not unlike LTD.
Lev-Ram, Jiang, et al. (1997) drew attention to the point that, of the two com-
ponents of LTD, namely, presynaptic activity coincident with postsynaptic depolar-
ization, the first can be replaced by NO (which arises from and mimics parallel fiber
stimulation) and the second by calcium ion, thus indicating that these two messengers
are sufficient for LTD induction.
A number of reviews concerning LTD have appeared (e.g., Crépel et al. 1996;
Daniel, Levenes, and Crépel 1998; Kano 1966; Linden 1996a, 1996b; Linden and
Connor 1995).
irregular background firing rate (of about 1 Hz) of climbing fibers (and therefore of
Purkinje cell complex spikes) in relation to the LTD at parallel fiber–Purkinje cell
synapses arising from conjoint climbing fiber–parallel fiber firing, on the one hand,
and LTP arising from tonic firing of the parallel fibers alone, on the other hand. The
hypothesis being explored was whether the low background firing rate of climbing
fibers (in the absence of movement) represented a mechanism for generating an
amount of LTD that would just balance the LTP induced by parallel fiber firing alone
(see also Kenyon, Medina, and Mauk 1998a, 1998b; Mauk and Donegan 1997). The
hypothesis was tested by analysis of existing recordings of Purkinje cell simple spikes
and complex spikes to search for the predicted increase in simple spike firing rate
prior to complex spikes, the presumed mechanism for which would be the inhibitory
feedback to the inferior olive from the deep cerebellar nuclei. Thus, an increase in
the Purkinje cell simple spike rate would result in greater inhibition of excitatory
and inhibitory deep cerebellar nuclear cells, the effect of the latter being to disin-
hibit olivary cells, resulting in turn in an increase in the frequency of climbing fibers
(the possible effect of the olivonuclear collaterals being ignored in the formulation),
thereby completing the feedback loop, so Purkinje cells would neither fall silent due
to LTD of their inputs nor be driven into excessive activity through LTP.
Miall et al. (1998) found a small but statistically significant increase in simple
spike activity about 150 msec before apparently random complex spikes recorded
during a pause between visually guided movements. This interval was not only much
longer than expected on the basis of axonal and synaptic delays from the Purkinje
cells to the inferior olive and back again, but also close to the estimated visuomotor
feedback delay. Thus, if the Purkinje cell simple spike increased rate represents a
predictive signal and the complex spike represents an error-correcting mechanism,
then the interval between the increased rate of simple spikes and the complex spike
would be expected to depend on the delay between the movement and the feedback
it causes; hence, the observed interval would be appropriate. As for the function of
such a corrective mechanism in the absence of movement, it was suggested that the
predictive nature of the Purkinje cell discharge may act as a feedforward (Chapter
16) motor correction, fine-tuning ongoing processes in higher motor centers.
timing relation between the two stimuli. Depression occurred only if the imposed
postsynaptic spike followed the excitatory postsynaptic potential within 60 msec.
In this connection, the decrease in synaptic effectiveness (i.e., plasticity) in the
cerebellum-like electrosensory lateral line lobe (ELL) of a mormyid electric fish
(Chapter 12) was found by Bell et al. (1997) to occur only if a postsynaptic spike in
the Purkinje-like cell followed the parallel fiber-induced EPSP within 60 msec.
that produced spontaneous drift of synaptic strength. The results further suggested
that the stability of synaptic weights derives from a self-regulating equilibrium of
climbing fiber activity controlling LTP/LTD in the cerebellar cortex and a similar
equilibrium of Purkinje cell activity controlling LTP/LTD in the nucleus. For climb-
ing fibers, this equilibrium was considered to depend on circuitry that allowed the
Purkinje cell to regulate the activity of its single climbing fiber input.
100
8.2. The Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex 101
Figure 8.1. Simplified version according to Robinson (1976) for Ito’s (1972) hypothesed basis
of adaptability of gain of the VOR. Output of semicircular canal (SCC) projects directly to
vestibular nucleus (VN) with gain α and indirectly via mossy fibers (mf), to granule cells (gc),
parallel fibers, and Purkinje cells (Pc) in the vestibulocerebellum (VC) with gain β. Retinal
image slip signal is conveyed from retina via accessory optic tract (aot) through nucleus of optic
tract (not labeled) and inferior olive (IO) to Purkinje cells via climbing fibers (cf). If cf activity
could change mf-Pc synaptic gain β, the gain of the entire reflex could be changed to eliminate
retinal slip during head movements. Ė, eye velocity; Ḣ, head velocity; (gain α = Ė/ Ḣ), OMN,
oculomotor nucleus. (From Robinson 1976; in Journal of Neurophysiology, Vol. 39, copyright
1976, American Physiological Society; reprinted by permission of the publisher and author.)
part of the accessory optic tract to the tectal nucleus of the optic tract, thence via the
central tegmental tract to the inferior olive, and thence to the flocculus via climbing
fibers (Maekawa and Simpson 1973). This pathway conveys direction-specific infor-
mation about movement of large parts of the visual world (Simpson and Alley 1974).
(In a subsequent report, Maekawa and Takeda [1975] found that impulses from the
optic nerve also activated the dorso-rostral portion of the flocculus via mossy fibers.
Thus, it was suggested that the mossy fiber pathway from retina to flocculus acts as a
visual feedback pathway to the VOR system.) Balaban, Kawaguchi, and Watanabe
(1981) reported that neurons in the dorsal cap of Kooy of the inferior olive, which
receive retinal input and project (as climbing fibers) to the flocculus, also send col-
laterals to the vestibular nuclei.
Figure 8.2. Modification of the VOR by in-phase and out-of-phase responses of flocculus
Purkinje cells. E, spike density histogram for the primary vestibular afferent impulses; Ii and
Ia [Io ], same but for in-phase and out-of-phase Purkinje cell discharges, respectively. Note
that when superimposed in second-order vestibular neurons (UN [VN]), Ii cancels E, whereas
Ia [Io ] reinforces E. VO, vestibular organ; OM, oculomotor nucleus. (From Ito 1984, The
Cerebellum and Neural Control, copyright 1984, Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; reprinted
by permission of publisher and author.)
Figure 8.3. Simplified circuit diagram for the VOR according to Raymond and Lisberger
(1997). The putative sites of plasticity are indicated by the shaded regions. Three neural signals
converge at each site: the activity of inputs driven by the vestibular stimulus, the activity of
climbing fibers (CFs) from the inferior olive (IO), and the simple spike activity of Purkinje
cells (P). (FTN), flocculus target neuron in the vestibular nucleus. The dashed climbing fiber
collateral to the FTN indicates that this connection may exist but is not well described. (From
Raymond and Lisberger (1997); in: Learning and Memory, Vol. 3, copyright 1997, Cold Spring
Laboratory Press; reprinted by permission of publisher and author.)
integrate corollary head and eye velocity signals to continuously adjust the setpoint
in the brain stem sites.
In an analysis of recordings from neurons in monkeys concerning the neural basis
for motor learning in the VOR (Lisberger, Pavelko, Bronte-Stewart, and Stone 1994;
Lisberger, Pavelko, and Broussard 1994), which was supplemented by behavioral
studies in monkeys wearing spectacles to vary the gain of the VOR, Lisberger (1994)
concluded that the hypotheses of both Miles and Lisberger (1981; i.e., that the primary
site of learning is in the brain stem) and Ito (1972; i.e., that learning occurs in the
flocculus and is guided by the conjunction of vestibular mossy fiber inputs and visual
climbing fiber inputs) were incomplete. A new hypothesis was proposed in which sites
of motor learning in the VOR may lie both in the brain stem VOR pathways and in
the vestibular inputs to the flocculus and ventral paraflocculus of the cerebellum.
In relation to the critiques offered by du Lac et al. (1995) of Ito’s flocculus hy-
pothesis (and of other models) of the VOR, and of long-term depression (LTD) as
a mechanism for modification of the VOR, a series of experiments were suggested
toward clarification of questions that the authors had raised in this controversial area
(Ito 1993a; Lisberger and Seijnowski 1993).
motion is in the opposite direction from head motion, then the learned change is an
increase in the gain of the VOR.
In examining different subclassifications of Purkinje cells in the floccular complex,
such as “eye contraversive,” “eye ipsiversive,” “vestibular ipsiversive,” and “vestibu-
lar contraversive,” Raymond and Lisberger (1997) concluded that each subclass of
Purkinje cells, which receive inputs from mossy fibers carrying vestibular, eye move-
ment (efference copy or corollary discharge), and visual (image motion) signals,
carried essentially the same information about required changes in the gain of the
VOR. (The authors noted that the latency of the visual signal to the cerebellar cortex
is about 100 milliseconds longer than the latency of the vestibular inputs, in contrast
to about 20 milliseconds for the vestibular inputs themselves.)
Ito (1998) suggested that it is unlikely that VOR adaptation is retained in the
rapidly transmitting (5–15 milliseconds) three-neuron arc, but the question of whether
the late (20–45 milliseconds), modifiable responses are mediated in the flocculus, the
brain stem, or both (as suggested by Raymond, Lisberger, and Mauk 1996) still re-
mained unsolved.
In relation to mechanisms and models of the VOR, Lisberger (1996) listed several
questions (none of which had straightforward answers). These were as follows: is it
appropriate to model the memory mechanism as a change in the strength of synaptic
transmission; is retinal slip required for learning the VOR; are there sites of learning
for the VOR that are not sites of memory (i.e., Purkinje cells and brain stem cells,
respectively); and can mechanisms of plasticity such as LTD function in the intact
brain as well as in slices of brain tissue.
Raymond and Lisberger (2000) summarized their studies in the last few years on
mechanisms of gain change in the VOR as follows. The VOR stabilizes images on the
retina by using vestibular signals to generate compensatory smooth eye movements
in the opposite direction from head motion. When image motion on the retina (retinal
slip – an error signal) is present during head turns, motor learning adjusts the VOR
to reduce image motion during subsequent head turns. The learning depends on the
cerebellar–floccular complex, comprising the flocculus and ventral paraflocculus. The
gain of the VOR is defined as the ratio of eye movement amplitude to head movement
amplitude during head turns in total darkness (i.e., in the absence of visually driven,
tracking eye movements that can occur in the light). For rhesus monkeys, VOR-
driven eye movements are normally opposite in direction and approximately equal
in amplitude to head movements, so the VOR gain is close to 1.0.
Learned changes in the gain of the VOR can be induced by pairing particular
movements of the head with particular movements of a visual stimulus. In general,
paired head and visual stimuli moving in the same direction induce a decrease in gain,
whereas movement of the two stimuli in opposite directions result in an increase in
gain. In this way, gains of 0 and 2.0, for example, can be induced in principle.
Raymond and Lisberger (2000) pointed out that previous hypotheses had pro-
posed two different bases of plasticity during motor learning in the VOR: (1) the
flocculus hypothesis (Ito 1972, 1982a), which suggested that plasticity in the circuit
for the VOR was based on coincident activity in climbing fibers carrying visual signals
8.2. The Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex 107
and parallel fibers carrying vestibular signals (actually a combination of vestibular, vi-
sual, and eye-movement signals); and (2) the coincidence of activity in Purkinje cells
and brain stem vestibular pathways (Miles and Lisberger 1981). Both hypotheses
were based on results obtained with 5 Hertz or less sinusoidal oscillations (velocity
profiles) for the visual and vestibular stimuli.
Extending the frequencies of the velocity profiles upward to 10 Hertz yielded
results that were not in accord with either of the previous hypotheses. Two alternative
new hypotheses were therefore proposed (Raymond and Lisberger 2000): (1) motor
learning (i.e., a gain change) in the VOR is mediated by a single form of plasticity in
which climbing fiber activity induces a change in the strength of the vestibular parallel
fiber synapses about 100 milliseconds earlier (a timing constraint that appeared to
be inconsistent with LTD but perhaps not other cerebellar cortical such as inhibitory
mechanisms); and (2) multiple mechanisms of motor learning occur in the VOR.
In a detailed review, Ito (1998) summarized cerebellar learning in the VOR, in-
cluding the history of the VOR going back to about 1930, when the VOR attracted
the attention of anatomists and physiologists, including Magnus, Lorente de Nó, and
Szentágothai, as a simple neural system mediated by a three-neuron arc. The VOR
evokes eye movements in the direction opposite to head movement, thus serving
to stabilize vision automatically relative to space. The complexity of eye movement
directions in the VOR is matched by the complexity of the 12 muscles in the two eyes
and of the 10 vestibular receptors (three semicircular canals and two otolith organs
in each of the two labyrinths). Ito’s review primarily considered the horizontal VOR.
More recently, the VOR arc has been reexamined, Ito (1998) indicated, because
of its close relationship with the cerebellum, in particular, with the flocculus, an evolu-
tionarily older part of the cerebellum. The VOR, therefore, provides a simple model
system for studying the functions of the cerebellum. In about 1970, Ito proposed the
flocculus to be a center of learning for changes in the VOR gain (Ito 1982a).
There are species differences: in rabbits and cats, the flocculus has 5 to 6 major
folia, and in rats, usually only 1 folium (Ito 1998). In the classic anatomy of the mon-
key cerebellum, 10 folia have been described for the flocculus, but recent studies on
neuronal connectivity have revealed that the rostral 5 belong to the ventral parafloc-
culus rather than to the flocculus itself. Thus, from study of the course of mossy fibers
and climbing fibers to the cerebellar cortex in monkeys, Nagao et al. (1997) concluded
that Purkinje cells in the ventral paraflocculus behave differently from those in the
flocculus and are related to ocular movements following movement of the visual field
and smooth pursuit (see below) of a moving target, but not to the VOR. Ito (1998)
stressed the importance of identifying (e.g., by local electrical stimulation) a narrow
zone of Purkinje cells of the flocculus, the H (for ipsilateral horizontal canal) zone,
in relating data, obtained from the flocculus, to the horizontal VOR.
At the conclusion of his review of cerebellar learning in the VOR, Ito (1998)
appended a series of three outstanding questions. First, does LTD account for long-
term or even permanent memory? This question has not been answered because
of technical difficulties that currently do not allow the time course of LTD to be
followed for more than 3 hours. But this question is central to the cerebellar learning
108 The Vestibulocerebellum and the Oculomotor System
theories and, to answer it, new technologies for marking LTD are required. Second,
is the memory in VOR adaptation controlled by the flocculus alone, by brain stem
pathways, or both? Although the available evidence consistently indicates that the
flocculus, with LTD mechanisms, plays a crucial role in induction of the VOR adapta-
tion, a role of the flocculus in its retention still remains to be verified. If a brain stem
pathway is involved, how can the adaptation induced in the flocculus be transferred
to the brain stem pathway? Third, how can the functional role of oculomotor signals
in the VOR be determined? The eye velocity and position signals in Purkinje cells in
the flocculus might reflect feedback from the oculomotor system to the flocculus, or it
might encode the inverse dynamics of the eyeballs. How might these two alternative
possibilities be distinguished experimentally?
It is evident that, even for the relatively simple VOR, questions still remain; all
the more so for trunk and limb movements, not to mention the question of cognition.
8.2.9. Roles of Mossy and Climbing Fibers in the Plasticity of the VOR
Given that the plasticity of the gain of the VOR is dependent on the vestibulocere-
bellum and on both mossy fiber and climbing fiber input, the question arises as to
whether this control mechanism (for which the term “recalibration” has been used
by some authors) sheds light generally on cerebellar cortical mechanisms (Ito 1984).
The two fiber systems, mossy and climbing, convey distinctly different information
from distinctly different sources to the cerebellar cortex. Mossy fibers convey infor-
mation about head movement (actually, head velocity), and climbing fibers convey
visual information about retinal slip.
For eye movement, Stone and Lisberger (1989) proposed a synergistic combi-
nation of visual input to the flocculus both via climbing fiber and via mossy fiber
pathways to the flocculus. In this formulation, the high-gain mossy fiber pathways
would respond to and act on large errors such as those encountered during the initi-
ation of movement or during the response to sudden changes in trajectory, whereas
the low-gain climbing fiber pathways would respond to and minimize the small errors
encountered during steady-state motor performance. These authors considered that
such a role for climbing fiber pathways did not exclude the possibility of a role for
climbing fibers in long-term changes in mossy fiber–Purkinje cell synapses.
In other systems involving the cerebellum, the distinction between the origins of
mossy fiber input and climbing fiber input are less clear, particularly when the diverse
and overlapping origins of the two are taken into account, as is apparent from the
listing of their respective sources (Chapters 4 and 5). Ito (1984, pp. 335–336) made
the distinction between a feedforward (open-loop) system (e.g, the VOR), in which
the climbing fiber system should have a signal source separate from that of the mossy
fiber system, and a feedback (closed-loop) control system (e.g., optokinetic nystag-
mus), in which both climbing fiber and mossy fiber systems could have the same
(retinal-slip) signal source, although the two could have separate transfer character-
istics appropriate to their own roles.
In relation to the role of climbing fibers, Ito (1984, p. 338) viewed the inferior
olive not so much as a comparator for generating an error signal from two different
8.2. The Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex 109
inputs, but rather as an encoder for error signals generated further downstream or
upstream so as to generate irregular, low-rate climbing fiber discharges.
Against the background of a tentative general hypothesis that the principal role of
the cerebellum, through its influence on motor systems, is to monitor and optimize
the quality of sensory information entering the brain, Paulin, Nelson, and Bower
(1989) considered the VOR to be a specific case in point, and suggested that the
transfer function for the VOR should be capable of continuous and rapid change
during head movements.
Finally, in relation to the question of “motor learning” (plasticity), Ito (1984)
pointed out that this term implies both “adaptation” and “learning,” which are not
always clearly distinguished from one another. He also emphasized that “learning”
implies more than “adaptation” and, borrowing from control theory terminology
(in which both terms imply self-optimization of parameters), suggested that “learn-
ing” indicated an accelerating process with repetition of trials that is not imp-
lied by “adaptation” (compare the section on adaptation vs. skill learning in
Chapter 6).
Figure 8.4. Time course of VO gain after cats begin to wear reversing prisms (arrows) without
(lower curve) and with (upper curve) vestibulocerebellectomy. Note, in the latter, the lack
of change of gain, which is also higher. (From Robinson 1981; in Brooks (ed.), Handbook of
Physiology, Vol. II, Motor Control, Part 2, 1981, copyright American Physiological Society,
Bethesda; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
(see also Robinson 1989). The model includes an equal number (14) of excitatory
and inhibitory neurons. The integration employs positive feedback through lateral
inhibition conveyed by an inhibitory commissure.
The integrator action of the semicircular canals themselves, however, is imperfect,
forming a leaky integrator with a relatively short time constant (Ito 1984). Thus, if
a steplike increment of angular velocity is applied to the head, the discharge rate in
primary vestibular afferents rises rapidly to a level determined by the magnitude of
the velocity step. The discharge rate then begins to fall, returning to the baseline level
after 10 to 12 seconds, thus signaling to the brain that the head is stationary even
though it is still in motion. Another neural integrator (a velocity-storage integrator)
thus becomes necessary, having an output lasting 30–60 seconds, which would result
in sustained compensatory eye movements (Ito 1984), thus compensating for the
leaky integrator action of the semicircular canals.
nystagmus from which can disappear in about 3 days (Robinson 1981). A role of the
vestibulocerebellum (although not necessarily of the fastigial nucleus), as opposed
to purely brain stem mechanisms, was not clearly established.
Figure 8.5. Saccadic plasticity. A monkey is trained to follow a spot that jumps by 10 degrees.
(A) Its left eye is weakened by tenectomy and patched. That eye subsequently makes hypo-
metric saccades one-third as large as those of the normal eye and with a backward postsaccadic
slip (top left). (B) Three days after switching the patch, the weakened eye has regained ability
to make correct (orthometric) saccades, whereas the good eye, under cover, makes hypermet-
ric saccades with postsaccadic slip in the opposite direction (bottom right). This demonstrates
that the central nervous system can repair dysmetria (created by a peripheral lesion) in this
case by increasing gain (saccade size/retinal error) of the central part of saccadic system.
(From Robinson 1981; in Brooks (ed.), Handbook of Physiology, Vol. II, Motor Control Part,
2, 1981, copyright American Physiological Society, Bethesda; reprinted by permission of the
publisher. Modified after Optican and Robinson 1980; Journal of Neurophysiology, Vol. 44,
1980.)
(i.e., the pulse-generating mechanism) and for the positioning of the eyes after a
saccade (i.e., the stepgenerating mechanism).
Using a different experimental paradigm, Goldberg et al. (1993) also obtained
failure of saccadic adaptation as a result of a lesion in the cerebellar nuclei
(primarily the fastigius). It was concluded that the cerebellum provided the basis
of the adaptation.
Kawano and Shidara (1993) concluded from several lines of evidence that the
Purkinje cells in the ventral paraflocculus of the monkey play a role in the short-
latency (of the order of 50 milliseconds), ocular-following response induced by
constant-velocity motion of a random dot pattern on a backlit screen.
Starting from the point that damage to the cerebellar vermis results in permanent
loss of accuracy of saccades, and also from the proposal that the vermis adjusts the
gain of the saccadic internal feedback loop in response to information about the
amplitude of the intended saccade, Dean, Mayhew, and Langdon (1994) and Dean
(1995) described a model for producing fast and accurate saccades that includes
burst firing in the fastigial nuclei (the output nuclei for the vermis) in conjunction
with brain stem mechanisms. It was suggested that the same principle may underlie
the cerebellum in generating fast movements of other parts of the body.
Their et al. (2000) reported, on the basis of single-unit recordings from saccade-
related Purkinje cells in the cerebellar vermis in monkeys, that unlike individual
Purkinje cells, the population response of large groups of Purkinje cells gives a precise
temporal signature of saccade onset and offset, thus helping to determine saccade
duration. In turn, modifying the time course of the population response by changing
the weights of the contributing individual Purkinje cells, discharging at different times
relative to the saccade, would directly translate into changes in saccade amplitude.
Figure 8.6. Architecture of the cerebellar model for predictive smooth pursuit eye movements.
Mossy fibers are randomly connected to granule units. Dotted lines: hypothesized influence
of Golgi (Go) cell receptive fields that allow only those granule cells with the highest activity
to generate outputs. Axons from granule cells bifurcate into two parallel fibers that excite
horizontal (HP) and vertical (VP) Purkinje units. Basket (B) and stellate (S) units receive
excitatory parallel fiber input and inhibit Purkinje cells. The action of these unit types is
collapsed into “Purkinje units” that allow both positive and negative “parallel fiber” weights.
Climbing fiber (cf) inputs are used to train the network. (From Kettner et al. 1997; in Journal
of Neurophysiology, Vol. 77, 1997, copyright American Physiological Society; reprinted by
permission of the publisher.)
Kettner et al. (1997). The architecture of the model is shown in Figure 8.6, a block
diagram in Figure 8.7, and results in Figure 8.8. Prediction was made possible by the
inclusion of a temporary storage mechanism (“eligibility trace”) at the site of alter-
ation of synaptic weights (i.e., at the parallel fiber–Purkinje–dendrite interface) of
the order of 100 milliseconds, which in effect compensated for the delay in arrival (via
the climbing fibers) of the error signals arising from retinal slip, relative to the arrival
times of the mossy fiber signals. Two Purkinje cells (in the flocculus/paraflocculus)
were modeled, one each for horizontal and for vertical eye movements (Fig. 8.7).
Results from testing the model, including with unperturbed (superimposed sinusoids)
and perturbed (abrupt changes in direction) trajectories, were compared with results
from monkeys using the same test trajectories, with close correspondence between
the two. Tracking time lags of 8 milliseconds (model) and 20 milliseconds (monkey),
increasing to 80 and 90 milliseconds, respectively, after perturbations of the trajec-
tories. For computational simplicity, the parallel fiber-to-Purkinje unit weights were
allowed to assume both positive and negative values, to simulate the combined direct
positive activation via parallel fiber input and indirect negative activation via basket
and stellate cells. The authors pointed out some similarities to other models.
8.6. Smooth (Predictive) Pursuit Eye Movements 115
Figure 8.7. Block diagram of the cerebellar model for predictive smooth pursuit eye move-
ments. (Although the brain stem integrator and the eye plant are modeled by the same set
of equations in the model, these two functions are distinguished in the diagram to emphasize
their different neural substrates and the idea that both proprioceptive and efference copy sig-
nals may provide eye position and velocity position.) All lines indicate the flow of multivariate
information, with the heavier arrow indicating the wider bandwidth associated with the ex-
pansive recoding of mossy fiber inputs. Smaller boxes: pure delays. Open arrowhead: indirect
action that climbing fiber training signals have on information throughput by the climbing fiber
training signals as a result of the alteration of network weights via the learning rule. Visual
input to the system is assumed to take the form of retinal error signals that are obtained by
a subtraction at the node labeled S of target and eye position signals. (From Kettner et al.
1997; in Journal of Neurophysiology, Vol. 77, 1997, copyright American Physiological Society;
reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
Figure 8.8. Comparison between tracking by the predictive smooth pursuit cerebellar model
for eye tracking (A, B) and monkey eye tracking (C, D) along complex trajectories. Further
details of the trajectories (Lissajous figures) are given in the original paper. (From Kettner
et al. 1997; in Journal of Neurophysiology, Vol. 77, 1997, copyright American Physiological
Society; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
116 The Vestibulocerebellum and the Oculomotor System
that complex spikes may contribute to long-term interactions between parallel and
climbing fiber inputs, such as LTD and/or potentiation.
The importance of the cerebellum in relation to movement control has long been
established, but in recent years, the question has been raised as to whether the
cerebellum may also have a role in cognition or thought processes. In this chapter, this
question, which has been at once intriguing and controversial (Barinaga 1996), will
be considered, especially in view of the uniformity of cerebellar anatomy. From this
uniformity, it would follow that the same basic type of computation that is performed
in the vestibulocerebellum and the spinocerebellum would also be carried out in the
neocerebellum (lateral cerebellum, cerebellar hemispheres). Assuming that the neo-
cerebellum does participate in cognitive phenomena or is essential for such mental
activities, interfacing the cerebellum with the cerebrum (cerebral cortex), and the
specific nature of their joint transactions, is formidable. It is far beyond the reach
of this text to venture solutions to these questions. Rather, this chapter summarizes
some of the history and findings in relation to the question of the cerebellum and
cognition. If signal adaptive control is indeed an appropriate model for the vestibu-
locerebellum and the spinocerebellum, and for the evidently motor functions of the
cerebrocerebellum, then because cerebellar anatomy is essentially uniform through-
out the “cognitive cerebellum,” there would seem to be a high probability that those
parts of the lateral cerebellum that appear to be involved with cognition are also
functioning as adaptive controllers.
An extensive review of the field is provided in the text edited by Schmahmann
(1997), comprising 28 papers by some 60 contributors. In this collection, cognition is
understood to include thought processes such as executive function, learning, mem-
ory, visual analysis, and language. The topics of emotion, personality, and behavior
are also included.
118
9.2. Anatomical and Physiological Studies 119
receives input from a distinct region of the respective cerebellar nucleus (i.e., the
dentate). It was suggested that cerebellar output channels to the prefrontal cortex
may be involved in cognitive aspects of behavior such as working memory. Such
a view would challenge the traditional one that the cerebellum is confined to the
coordination of motor activities, the authors concluded.
According to Middleton and Strick (1998), the cerebellum, via its nuclei, projects
not only to the primary motor cortex, but also to the premotor and prefrontal cortex.
Further, there is evidence for reciprocal projections from neocortical areas to the
cerebellum. These authors have given the name “output channels” to the distinct
clusters of neurons in the cerebellar nuclei from which the projections to the cerebral
cortex originate. In addition, it was considered that the neuroanatomical substrate
exists for cerebellar dysfunction to contribute to some of the cognitive deficits in
neuropsychiatric disorders (Middleton and Strick 1998).
In a review challenging the view that basal ganglia and cerebellum are solely
concerned with motor control, Middleton and Strick (2000) concluded that it is now
apparent (from anatomical studies in animals and from imaging studies in humans)
that there are multiple cortical areas that are the target of basal ganglia and cere-
bellar output, including not only the primary motor cortex but also subdivisions of
premotor (assumed to be cognitive in function), oculomotor, and inferotemporal
areas. For the cerebellum, the prefrontal areas, including areas 9 (planning, work-
ing memory) and 46 (spatial working memory, but not the inferotemporal cortex),
were considered by the authors to receive projections from the ventromedial dentate
nucleus.
Concerning the question of possible cerebellar learning mechanisms in cognitive
activity, Schmahmann (1996) pointed out a problem related to the origins of mossy
fibers and climbing fibers, the ultimate interaction of which is presumed to lie at the
basis of synaptic plasticity according to the Marr–Albus–Ito theory (Chapter 13).
Thus, the input from the cerebrum to the red nucleus–inferior olive system (and
hence, to the climbing fibers) appears to be predominantly motor in origin, whereas
the pontine system (and thus the mossy fibers) receives motor and sensory, as well as
associative and paralimbic, information from the cerebrum. The question therefore
arises as to whether the red nucleus–olivary system can be involved in cognitive
processing or whether additional pathways from the cerebrum to the red nucleus in
humans will be demonstrated (Schmahmann 1996).
An increase in the time interval required for voluntary shifts of selective at-
tention (i.e., between auditory and visual stimuli) was found by Akshoomoff and
Courchesne (1992) in patients with damage to the cerebellum, in comparison with
a control group of normal subjects. The authors theorized that this function of the
cerebellum may operate via its previously described sensory modulation properties
(e.g., Crispino and Bullock 1984). The increased time required for voluntary atten-
tion shifts was compared with the increased reaction times to voluntary movement
responses to sensory cues in patients with neocerebellar damage.
Silveri, Leggio, and Molinari (1994) reported a case of a right cerebellar infarct
with right hemicerebellar syndrome and agrammatic speech consisting of production
of infinitive forms instead of inflected forms, and omission of free-standing gram-
matic morphemes. The authors suggested that the cerebellum provides the temporal
interplay among neural structures responsible for production of sentences.
the muscles that would be employed to execute it, the details of which are unclear
(Stein and Glickstein 1992).
In the conclusion to their review of the role of the cerebellum in visual guidance of
movement, Stein and Glickstein (1992) remarked that the basic processing function
of the cerebellar cortex is to set up a plastic internal representation of the sensory-
motor system under the benevolent instruction of the climbing fiber input.
From study of a patient with massive ipsilateral cerebellar stroke, Bloedel, Bracha,
and Larson (1993) found that the data supported the notion that the cerebellum is
essential, not for the initial learning of a tracing movement, but rather for performing
the learned movement with the required rotation of the original image.
Jeannerod et al. (1995) pointed out the complexities entailed in the grasping part
of reaching and grasping for an object. Its size and shape must be coded and trans-
formed into a pattern of distal (wrist and finger) movements, a visuomotor transfor-
mation that in monkeys is carried out conjointly by the inferior parietal lobule (more
globally) and by the inferior premotor area (in greater detail). The latter contains
a basic “vocabulary” from which many dexterous movements can be constructed as
coordinated-control programs. Correct execution of grasping, the authors pointed
out, requires an intact (downstream) primary motor cortex (area 4). In primates,
lesions of this area, as well as of the pyramidal tract, result in a profound deficit in
the control of individual fingers, in contrast to lesions of the inferior parietal lobule,
which typically result in misreaching with an awkward grasp that fails to take on the
correct shape.
Timmann et al. (1996) reported that patients with cerebellar lesions are capable
of substantially improving their performance of a complex motor task involving
the recall of memorized shapes and the visuomotor control of a tracing movement.
The authors concluded that it is highly feasible that the cerebellum plays a critical
role during the acquisition of a task, but that this role is not necessarily linked with
the exclusive storage of the critical memory engram within the cerebellum itself.
There is evidence (e.g., Wang et al. 1999) indicating the existence of two cere-
bral visual pathways in humans (i.e., a dorsal stream for spatial and motion vision,
and a ventral stream for object and form vision). In this connection, Schmahmann
and Pandya (1997a, 1997b) pointed out that primary or associative visual cortical
areas concerned with the peripheral visual field, visual spatial parameters, and visual
motion project to the pontine nuclei (and thence to the cerebellar hemispheres),
whereas regions concerned with the central visual field and visual object identifica-
tion do not. These differences may suggest that the former type of visual informa-
tion requires some type of processing or transformation (e.g., of coordinates; see
below) by the cerebellar hemispheres whereas the latter type of visual information
does not.
Glickstein (1997) emphasized that damage to the massive dorsal stream of
extrastriate visual areas projecting to the pontine nuclei, either at a neocortical level
or en route, produces profound disturbance in visuomotor guidance. It was concluded
that the visual guidance of the limb that survives occipitofrontal disconnection is ev-
idently mediated via the cerebellum.
9.5. Imaging Studies 123
From a series of experiments with monkeys designed to clarify the putative role
of the cerebellum in cognition (e.g., in relation to working memory), Nixon and
Passingham (1999) concluded that, unlike the dorsal prefrontal cortex (area 46),
the cerebellum is not essential for working memory or for the executive processes
necessary for correct performance, although it may contribute to the preparation of
responses.
In this connection, Eskandar and Assad (1999) reported on the dissociation of
visual, motor, and predictive signals in the parietal cortex in monkeys trained to
use a joystick to guide a spot to a target. Neurons in the medial superior temporal
(MST) area were selectively modulated by the direction of visible moving stimuli,
whereas neurons in the medial intraparietal (MIP) area were selectively modulated
by the direction of hand movement. In contrast, the selectivity of cells in the lateral
intraparietal (LIP) area did not directly depend on either visual input or motor
output; instead, these cells appeared to encode a predictive representation of stimulus
movement.
PET Study of Cerebellar Timing Functions. Jueptner et al. (1995), using PET to ex-
amine regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) in a cerebellar timing function, found that
124 The Cerebellum and Cognition
during simple finger movements there was increased activation of the inferior parts
of the ipsilateral cerebellar hemisphere in comparison with the resting condition, but
that during similar finger movements carried out to signal lengths of time intervals,
additional activations of the cerebellar vermis and both cerebellar hemispheres, in-
dicating to the authors that the cerebellum is involved in timing tasks separable from
motor tasks (finger movement). (See also Chapter 10.)
Finger Movement Rate and rCBF. Evidence of a relation between rCBF and the
frequency of timed repetitive flexion of the right index finger against the right thumb
was reported by Sadato et al. (1996). It was found that the left primary sensory
cortex and the right (superior) cerebellum showed no significant activation at very
slow rates (0.25 and 0.5 Hertz), a rapid increase of rCBF with increasing rates up to
2 and 2.25 Hertz), but no further increase at 3 and 4 Hertz. In contrast, the posterior
supplementary motor area (SMA) shows a reverse trend – highest activation at very
slow rates but no significant activation at the very fast rates. In a related study using
rCBF with PET, resting activation levels were compared with those for increasingly
complex sequential finger movements at 0.5 Hertz (Catalan et al. 1998). A large
increase in activation from rest to simple repetitive movements was found, with the
shortest sequence found in the contralateral primary sensory and premotor cortex,
supplementary motor area, and ipsilateral cerebellar cortex, suggesting an executive
role by these areas in running sequences. In particular, it was concluded that for
voluntary limb movements, the cerebellum is clearly important for the temporal
order of and precision in the execution of motor programs.
MRI During Cognitive Processing with Pegboards. Kim, Ugurbil, and Strick (1994)
employed MRI, with its greater resolution than PET, to explore activation of the
dentate nucleus in normal subjects while carrying out a visually controlled task of
simple shifting of a line of pegs, and while carrying out the task of solving a diffi-
cult pegboard puzzle. A large bilateral activation of the dentate was found during
attempts to solve the pegboard puzzle, several times greater in area than that acti-
vated during simple movement of the pegs. The authors concluded that these results
provided support for the concept that the computational power of the cerebellum is
applied to cognitive functions as well as to the control of movement.
fMRI and Verbal Fluency. With fMRI of the human brain during tests of verbal flu-
ency (nonvocal generation of words starting with a given initial letter) in right-handed
normal subjects, Schlösser et al. (1998) found activation in the right cerebellum (and
left prefrontal cortex) in comparison with a baseline task of simple counting.
PET During Processing of Single Words. In a study using PET for the processing
of single words, Peterson et al. (1989) found activation in the right lateral inferior
cerebellum, a region distinct from those associated with motor tasks, suggesting to the
authors an involvement with a “cognitive” rather than sensory or motor computation.
129
130 Timing Functions, Classical Conditioning, and Instrumental Conditioning
the basal ganglia have a role in internal timing: the cerebellum operating over a
relatively short temporal window and the timing functions of the basal ganglia being
used in tasks spanning longer durations. As for mechanisms, the author mentioned
the emphasis on network models, in which time is distributed across a set of neural
elements the different elements of which provide an interval-based representation, as
compared with the older concept that temporal codes rely on endogenous oscillatory
processes.
In their review of the biological basis of time estimation and temporal order,
Lalonde and Hannequin (1999) considered that it had been established, on the basis
of tests of normal subjects and patients with brain lesions, that the cerebellum, the
basal ganglia, and the prefrontal cortex are involved in time estimation (as evaluated
by temporal discrimination, verbal estimation, temporal production, and temporal
reproduction). In particular, the authors cited the hypothesis that the central timer
is located in the cerebellum, while the planning abilities subserving the estimation of
longer intervals are mediated by the prefrontal cortex.
Possible mechanisms underlying time perception and temporal processing were
reviewed by Wittmann (1999), who associated temporal processing of intervals of a
fraction of a second up to a few seconds with the cerebellum, and temporal processing
of intervals from seconds to minutes with the basal ganglia. Correspondingly, an
interval-based timing mechanism would apply to the cerebellum, and a clock-plus-
counter system to the basal ganglia.
In contrast to the preceding reports, Harrington and Haarland (1999) concluded
from a review of focal lesion, pharmacological, and functional imaging studies that
there was less evidence for the proposal that timekeeping operations are supported
by the cerebellum than for the basal ganglia.
In a review of the basis of movement initiation (e.g., in relation to reaction time
[RT] determinations), Lalonde and Botez-Marquard (1997) suggested that the den-
tate nucleus triggers the onset of movement through the dentate-ventroanterior and
ventrolateral (VA-VL) thalamic-motor cortex pathway. However, when this pathway
is inactivated (e.g., by cooling), or perhaps even in intact animals under some con-
ditions, alternate pathways, such as dentato-intralaminar thalamo-cortical, dentato–
reticulo–spinal, and interposito–rubro–spinal pathways may take over.
To investigate possible differences between cerebellum and prefrontal cortex in
temporal processing tasks, Casini and Ivry (1999) tested control participants and
patients with either prefrontal or cerebellar lesions on temporal and nontemporal
perceptual tasks under two levels of attentional load. Each trial involved a com-
parison between a standard tone and a subsequent comparison tone that varied in
frequency, duration, or both. When participants had to make concurrent judgments
on both dimensions, patients with frontal lobe lesions were significantly impaired on
both tasks, whereas the variability of cerebellar patients increased in the duration
task only. The authors concluded that the difference suggests that deficits in temporal
processing tasks observed in frontal patients can be related to the attention demands
of such tasks, whereas cerebellar patients have a more specific problem related to
timing.
10.3. Conditioned Eyeblink Reflex: Learning in Cerebellar Cortex and Nuclei 131
conditioned eyeblink response, and considering that a slow neuron response seemed
to be the most likely candidate, Fiala, Grossberg, and Bullock (1996) constructed
a mathematical model based on activation of a metabotropic glutamate receptor
second messenger system in Purkinje cells as a substrate for adaptive timing. In this
way, the interstimulus interval between the onset of parallel fiber activity associated
with the CS and climbing fiber activity associated with the US onset could be bridged.
Moore, Desmond, and Berthier (1989) employed a tapped delay line in conjunc-
tion with variable weights (at parallel fiber–Purkinje cell synapses, and also at parallel
fiber–Golgi cell synapses) in their neural network model for the conditioned nictitat-
ing membrane (eyeblink) response in rabbits. The site of the delay line was assumed
to be the pontine nuclei.
From their study of conditioned eyeblink responses in ferrets, Svensson, Ivarsson,
and Hesslow (1997) concluded that the CS is transmitted through the mossy fibers
and that the mechanism for timing the CR is situated in the cerebellum.
Considering the mechanism of eyelid conditioning (in which a neutral stimulus
such as a tone is repeatedly paired with a reinforcing or US to promote the acquisition
of a CR; the eyelids close in response to the tone alone), and saccadic eye move-
ments (the rapid eye movements used to shift gaze from one direction to another),
Mauk et al. (2000) concluded that the cerebellum influences movement execution
by feedforward use of sensory information via temporally specific learning.
eyeblink conditioning might occur in both the cerebellar cortex and the interpositus
nucleus).
Thompson et al. (1998) concluded that the reinforcement or “instructive” or
“teaching” pathway for eyeblink (nictitating membrane) conditioning is the inferior
olive–climbing fiber projection system to the cerebellar cortex. (The authors deter-
mined that this conclusion applied in general to classical conditioning of behavioral
responses learned with aversive US and was relevant to any behavioral response so
learned [e.g., limb flexion, eyeblink, head turn] for all mammals, including humans.)
This view was considered by the authors to be consonant with the classic theories
of the cerebellum as a learning machine (e.g., Albus 1971; Brindley 1964; Ito 1972;
Marr 1969), in which theories information about movement errors and aversive US
were provided by the inferior olive–climbing fiber system, and information about
movements, contexts, and other types of sensory information was held to be pro-
jected to the cerebellum via the mossy fibers, coming from the pontine nuclei and
other sources that synapse onto granule cells in the cerebellar cortex, giving rise
to parallel fibers. Blocking in eyeblink conditioning was considered by the authors
(Thompson et al. 1998) to result from inhibitory feedback to the inferior olive from
the interpositus nucleus.
Green and Woodruff-Pak (2000) concluded that the cerebellum – in particular,
the ipsilateral interpositus nucleus in rabbits (and also apparently in humans) – is
essential for the formation of the simple association between the CS and the US
necessary in all eyeblink classical conditioning paradigms.
137
138 Cerebellar Pathology in Humans and Animals: Genetic Alterations
Figure 11.1. Tracings of rapid pronation-supination of the forearm from a patient with uni-
lateral cerebellar symptoms. The movements of the affected arm (lower trace) were initially
regular though slower and of smaller amplitude than the normal side (top trace), but became
irregular and the arm became more or less fixed in supination. Muscular contractions appeared
to spread aimlessly over the whole of the affected limb, including unwanted shoulder and finger
movements. (From Holmes, 1939, in Brain Vol. 62, 1939, copyright by Oxford University Press;
reprinted by permission; modified by Rothwell, 1994; Control of Human Voluntary Movement
(2nd ed.) copyright 1994; reprinted by permission of the copyright holders.)
Figure 11.2. Wrist position during successive trials of extension before (left) and after (right)
microinjection of muscimol (a temporary inactivator) into the interpositus nucleus of a mon-
key. After the injection, the movement becomes broken up (note the prominent tremor at
about 3 Hertz in the frequency spectrum). Compare Figure 11.1. (From Thach et al. 1992,
in Llinás and Sotelo (eds.): The Cerebellum Revisited, copyright 1992, Springer, New York;
reprinted by permission of publisher and author.)
In this connection, Wiener and Berthoz (1993) pointed out that the “vestibular”
nuclei receive visual, proprioceptive, and vestibular information about head motion,
and that these nuclei are involved in spatiotemporal transformation, allowing the
sensing of head motion in space.
Figure 11.3. Delayed anatagonist EMG activity (bottommost of lower set of traces) associated
with hypermetria (upper set of traces) in a patient tested for ballistic flexion movements 3
years after infarction of the left cerebellar hemisphere. Averaged records of 15–20 flexions
on the normal side and on the cerebellar affected side made about the elbow (A), wrist (B),
and finger (C). Records synchronized to start of movement (time 0 milliseconds). EMG (Ag,
agonist; Ant., antagonist) records from the two sides were scaled to give approximately the
same magnitudes. Target distance, 30 degrees. Calibrations: position, 30 degrees; velocity, 300
degrees per second; acceleration, 2,000 degrees per second2 . (From Hore, Wild, and Diener
1991; in Journal of Neurophysiology, Vol. 65, 1991, copyright American Physiological Society;
reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
were more variable, less accurate, and performed more slowly; the paths (of finger
tips) were more circuitous and longer than normal irrespective of whether visual
cues were available; and there were large constant errors at the end of movement,
but only in darkness (Fig. 11.4). The authors concluded that the spatial errors arise
because the cerebellum contributes to preparatory motor processes that, based on
limb proprioceptive and visual information, compute the pattern of muscle activity
required to launch the limb accurately toward a target.
Figure 11.4. Perspective view of movement paths of one healthy subject (top) and two patients
with Friedreich’s ataxia. All movements within a block of trials have been superimposed.
Panels on the left show movements performed with vision of target and finger. Panels on the
right show movements performed in darkness. The circles indicate the position of the targets.
The dotted grid projected onto each of the three planes consists of squares with 10 centimeter
sides. (From Day et al. 1998; Brain 121, copyright 1998, Oxford University Press; reprinted by
permission of publisher and author.)
144 Cerebellar Pathology in Humans and Animals: Genetic Alterations
avenues of exploration of the functional role of granule cells; Purkinje cell degener-
ation (Ross 1997); impaired LTD and motor coordination (Funabiki, Mishina, and
Hirano 1995); blockade of LTD and of adaptation of the VOR (De Zeeuw et al.
1998).
An example of interfacing of genetics with the cerebellum is the modeling, in
transgenic mice, of the pathogenesis of the human disease spinocerebellar ataxia
type 1 (SCA1), in which the most frequently seen and most severe alterations are
loss of Purkinje cells and neurons in the inferior olive (Clark and Orr 2000; Cummings,
Orr, and Zoghbi 1999).
disturbances observed in PCD mutant mice. The authors further suggest that such
functional compensation, leading to only moderate motor impairment, could help
explain the apparent discrepancy between clinical signs of ataxia in humans and the
histopathological changes.
In mutant mice lacking the glutamate receptor δ2 subunit (GluRδ2) in Purkinje
cells, there is impaired parallel fiber–Purkinje cell synapse stabilization during cere-
bellar development, which results in fewer parallel fiber synapses (Kurihara et al.
1997).
inferior olivary nucleus, but whose deep cerebellar nuclei are essentially intact). It
was found that the deep cerebellar nuclei sufficed for motor learning, provided the
task was not too difficult (rate of rotation of the rod: 20 rpm). However, the cere-
bellar cortex was required when the task was more difficult (30 rpm). The authors
concluded that the adaptive motor capabilities of lurcher mice are more limited than
those of control animals.
twelve
There are cerebellum-like structures, including the valvula and the electroreceptive
lateral line lobe (ELL), that are found in certain species of fish, as well as the mam-
malian dorsal cochlear nucleus, the architecture and molecular biology of which in
some respects strikingly resemble that of the cerebellum of vertebrates. These merit
separate discussion from the topics in Chapter 2 on comparative anatomy of the
cerebellum.
148
12.1. The Valvula 149
olfactory bulb
The cerebellum in mormyrid fish is not only larger than any other among verte-
brates in relative size, but the extraordinary valvula cerebelli is also almost certainly
histologically the most regular, geometrically ordered (crystalline) structure in the
central nervous system in vertebrates. The valvula in mormyrid fish consists of a re-
peatedly folded, continuous single ribbon of 300 mu width and an estimated 1 meter
in length (i.e., about the same length as the unfolded Purkinje cell layer of the human
cerebellum but far narrower in width; Bell and Szabo 1986; Braitenberg and Atwood
1958).
Of particular interest concerning the valvula is the fact that its ridges or corru-
gations (Figs. 12.2, 12.3) are made up of only the molecular layer (i.e., the layer of
parallel fibers and the dendritic trees of the Purkinje cells) and the bodies of the
Purkinje cells, but not the layer of granule cells. Such folds are absent in the body of
the cerebellum (Fig. 12.4). Further, because the granular layer does not participate in
the superimposed folds of the valvula, the axons of the granule cells do not bifurcate
as in the body of the cerebellum, and the horizontal fibers arise directly from the
granule cells. As a result, successive Purkinje cell dendritic trees are activated in a
strict temporal sequence.
The intersections of parallel fibers with Purkinje dendrites are strictly ordered
(Fig. 12.5), forming an almost crystal-like array (Kaiserman-Abramof and Palay
1969), which is indicative of a precise processing of the input data arriving on the
parallel fibers, perhaps providing the capability for evaluating brief time differences.
The overall arrangement in the valvula has some resemblance to an adaptive phased-
array arrangement for a spatiotemporal (or temporospatial) transformation (Widrow
and Stearns 1985; c.f., also Braitenberg 1967), which, to be adaptive or modifiable,
would likely require a climbing fiber input to the Purkinje cells. It appears that
150 Specialized Cerebellum-Like Structures
Figure 12.2. The cerebellum of the mormyrid, Petrocephalus bovei: section from valvula of the
cerebellum. Key: gr, granular layer; P, Purkinje layer; m, molecular layer. (From Nieuwenhuys
1967; reprinted by permission of the author.)
climbing fibers are not apparent in the valvula by light microscopy (Nieuwenhuys and
Nicholson 1969b) but are identifiable by electron microscopy (Kaiserman-Abramof
and Palay 1969).
It has been suggested that both the anatomy and the physiology of a part of the
valvula (i.e., having mormyromast-ampullary connections) indicates that its major
role relates to the electrosensory system in modulating the sensory input of the latter,
particularly in view of its large relative size and histological specialization, but the
existence and the nature of such a role have remained unsettled (Bell 1986; Bell
and Szabo 1986; Bullock 1986; Nieuwenhuys, ten Donkelaar, and Nicholson 1998,
p. 839; Russell and Bell 1978). Thus, other electrosensitive species do not have such
a large valvula, and hence such an association has been questioned; nonetheless, at
12.1. The Valvula 151
least part of the valvula (in mormyrids at least) appears to be involved in processing
electrosensory information (Finger, Bell, and Russell 1981).
Wullimann and Northcutt (1990) pointed out that, whereas electroreception
evolved anew in mormyrids, vision was reduced concomitantly in this group of fishes,
because primary visual projections are less extensive in mormyrids than in most other
teleosts.
153
154 Specialized Cerebellum-Like Structures
Figure 12.7. Summarizing scheme of some basic differences in the organization of extrinsic
cerebellar connections in teleosts (fish) and amniote tetrapods (i.e., mammals, birds, reptiles).
ccn, central cerebellar nucleus; e, eurydendroid cell; alv, nucleus lateralis valvulae. (From
Meek 1992a; European Journal of Morphology, Vol. 30, copyright 1992, Swets & Zeitlinger
Publishers; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
with a coextensive distribution in the transverse plane (Fig. 12.8, valv.), and different
Purkinje cell arrangements, including a dendritic palisade pattern (in the valvula
[Fig. 12.6, morymids; Fig. 12.9]). Meek (1992a) suggested that these configurations
reinforce the notion that a single main cerebellar function may exist (i.e., coincidence
detection of parallel fiber activity by Purkinje cells, not only in mormyrids but also
perhaps in tetrapods; Meek 1992b).
Earlier, mention had been made that the particular arrangement of the valvula
had suggested that it could provide the capability of a precise evaluation of brief
time differences that may be encountered in the sensory information reported from
lateral line organs, and of the subtle differences in electric field strengths that may be
valv. I.t. C1
MORMYRIDS
Figure 12.8. Transverse views of the granular cell–parallel fiber–Purkinje configurations en-
countered in the mormyrid valvula cerebelli (valv), lobus transitorius (l.t.), and lobe C1 com-
pared with the mammalian one. (From Meek 1992a; European Journal of Morphology, Vol.
30, copyright 1992, Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
12.1. The Valvula 155
parallel
fibers 75,000
contacts
Purkinje
axons
16 boutons
'climbing'
fibers 130 contacts
mossy A (sagittal)
fibers
B (transverse)
Figure 12.9. Summarizing scheme of the morphological and synaptic organization of Purkinje
cells and their input in lobe C1 (a part of the valvula). (A) Sagittal view showing the distribution
of input, the regularity, the gradients in spine and parallel fiber density, and the numbers of
synaptic contacts observed or estimated in lobe C1 . Medially lying granule cells give rise
to symmetrical parallel fibers, but more laterally lying granule cells have asymmetrical or
even unidirectional parallel fibers (not shown). (B) Transverse view showing the similarity
in length of the parallel fibers contacting Purkinje cells in lobe C1 , but their variability in
diameter (superficially: thin; deeply: thick) and site of origin: medially located granule cells
give rise to symmetrical parallel fibers (a; i.e., a left and right branch of equal length), but
more laterally located granule cells have asymmetrical (b) or even unidirectional (c) parallel
fibers. Magnifications: A: × 150; B: × 75. (From Meek and Nieuwenhuys 1991; J. Comparative
Neurol., Vol. 306, copyright 1991, Wiley-Liss, Inc., a subsidiary of John Wiley & Sons, Inc.;
reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
156 Specialized Cerebellum-Like Structures
The mossy fibers probably do not originate from the spiral ganglion in the ear
but may originate from higher auditory centers. In humans, unlike cat, the DCN is
not stratified, the pyramidal cells (see below) have lost their orientation, and there
is only a vestige of a molecular layer. The dorsal cochlear nucleus in humans cannot
therefore be regarded as a structural analog of the cerebellar cortex (Brodal 1981,
pp. 616–617).
Lorente de Nó (1981) pointed out that the total number of local fibers, association
and centrifugal, entering the DCN is greater than the number of cochlear nerve
afferents, and hence the cochlear nuclei cannot be regarded as an ensemble of simple
relay stations. Indeed, this author concluded that the primary (dorsal and ventral)
acoustic nuclei constitute a miniature brain that has a cerebellum of its own, the
dorsal cochlear nucleus.
Parallel fiber
(glutamate)
Basket/Stellate cell
Purkinje cell (GABA)
(GABA)
Golgi Cell
(GABA)
(glycine)
Granule cell
(glutamate)
Climbing fibers
from medulla
(CRF ?)
Parallel fiber
(glutamate)
Stellate cell
(GABA)
Cartwheel cell
(GABA) Golgi cell
(glycine) (GABA)
(glycine)
Granule cell
to pyramidal (glutamate)
cells
Mossy fibers
from brainstem
DCoN
Figure 12.10. Diagram highlighting the differences between Purkinje cells of the cerebellum
(top) and cartwheel neurons of the DCN (bottom). Details in text. (From Berrebi and Mugnaini
1993; reprinted from Merchán et al. (eds.): The Mammalian Cochlear Nuclei: Organization
and Function, copyright 1993, Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers; reprinted by permission
of publisher and author.)
p.f.
p.f.
LAYER I
p.f.
3
p.f.
2
LAYER II
1
LAYER III
4
1
2
3
3
2
5
5 1 2
1 3
1
4 3
4 1
IV
III
II
I 3
2
B 4 5
1 C
Figure 12.11. Schematic diagram showing GABAergic (black) and non-GABAergic (light)
neurons in the superficial portion of the DCN. (A) Possible synaptic relations between the
cells are indicated; (B) the orientation of parallel fibers (p.f.) along the DCN major axis and
the anisotropy of the apical and basal dendritic arbors of pyramidal cells (5) are indicated by
the block diagram. Dendrites of granule (1), Golgi (4), stellate (3), and cartwheel cells (2) (see
below) are orientated in all directions. (C) The relative frequency of GAD-positive (black dots)
and GAD-negative (open circles) boutons on the five categories of cells is indicated. Cartwheel
cell bodies (2) are the only cell bodies contacted exclusively by GABAergic boutons. (From
Mugnaini 1985; J. Comparative Neurol., Vol. 235, copyright 1985, Wiley-Liss, Inc., a subsidiary
of John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
160 Specialized Cerebellum-Like Structures
contact with terminals with round (excitatory) vesicles, many of which are axons of
granule cells (Mugnaini 1985). The sources of the other terminals were described by
these authors as unknown. Potential sources mentioned were other small cells and
any source of projections to the superficial layers.
spiny arborization of pyramidal cells, the output cells of the DCN. Thus, cartwheel
and stellate cells have several similarities to cerebellar Purkinje cells and stellate
cells, respectively. Further, Golgi, Purkinje, and stellate cells in the cerebellum, and
Golgi, cartwheel, and stellate neurons in the DCN are GABAergic, and granule cells
evidently use an excitatory amino acid neurotransmitter in both the cerebellum and
DCN. In addition, the neuropeptides cerebellin and PEP-19 have been found to be
markers both for Purkinje cells in the cerebellum and also the cartwheel cell, the
homologue cell in the DCN, the primary targets of which are the DCN pyramidal
cells (Berrebi and Mugnaini 1991; Mugnaini and Morgan 1987; Mugnaini et al. 1987).
The pyramidal cells, in turn, can be viewed as the homologue of the cerebellar nuclear
cells, as previously mentioned.
12.3.2. Granule Cell Mutation Weaver, Purkinje Cells, and Cartwheel Cells
Complementing the results summarized above, Berrebi, Morgan, and Mugnaini
(1990) found that the cerebellar granule cell mutation, weaver, which primarily af-
fects granule cells but spares most Purkinje cells in the lateral cerebellum, also spares
cartwheel cells. These findings were considered to support the notion that the cere-
bellar germinative zone extends to the caudal portion of the rhombic lip, which gives
rise to the dorsal cochlear nucleus.
Reviews of some aspects of the genetic basis for normal and abnormal devel-
opment of the cerebellum and of its cell types (e.g., global and subcellular levels
of compartmentalization, sagittal versus rostrocaudal compartmentalization) have
appeared (Goldowitz and Hamre 1998; Oberdick, Baader, and Schilling 1998).
basket cells. Further, the cartwheel neurons are not arranged in a single layer, their
dendritic fields do not lie in a single plane, and their axons are relatively short, as are
those of Purkinje cells. These features vary somewhat among different species.
Berrebi and Mugnaini (1993) reiterated that cartwheel neurons represent homo-
logues of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar-like circuit of the DCN superficial layers
(Fig. 12.10), and that cartwheel neurons are members of the Purkinje cell family that
have become adapted to carry out cerebellar-like processing of auditory inputs, or
perhaps play a role in the regulation of acoustic reflexes.
Surface ep.
gr. ca.
par. f.
st.
?
layer 1
par. f. par. f.
go.
? st.
?
gr.
layer 2
py. py.
v.
mossy f.
layer 3
DCN to inferior
colliculus
PVCN
to VCN
T St.
D St.
ANF
Figure 12.12. Schematic representation of the DCN. The layer of pyramidal cells (py) separates
the superficial layer (layer 1) and the deep layer (layer 3). Layer 3 contains the basal dendrites
of the pyramidal cells, vertical cells (v), and giant cells (not shown). Auditory nerve fibers
(ANF) and projections from the VCN terminate in the deep layer. Type T stellate cells (T St.)
from the PVCN presumably supply excitation to the DCN, whereas type D stellate cells (D St.)
from the PVCN presumably supply inhibition to the DCN, Layer 1 contains parallel fibers (par
f.), which are axons of granule cells. The cartwheel (ca.) and stellate (st.) cells are inhibitory
interneurons of the superficial layer. The superficial stellate cells of the DCN are different
from the stellate cells of the PCVN. The granule cell domains also contain a class of inhibitory
neurons, the Golgi cells (go.). (From Nelken and Young 1996; reprinted from Journal of Basic
and Clinical Physiology and Pharmacology, Vol. 7, copyright 1996, by permission of Freund
Publishing House, Ltd.)
The recognition of the different neuronal populations in the cochlear nucleus has
led to the view that the auditory system is made up of multiple neuronal pathways
(e.g., Warr 1982). Presumably, the different pathways have different functions in audi-
tory signal processing; in some cases, there is insight into what that role is (Cant 1992)
12.4. The Mammalian Dorsal Cochlear Nucleus 165
The inhibitory cartwheel cells of the DCN (the homologue of cerebellar Purkinje
cells) are excited by parallel fibers from granule cells to yield both simple and complex
spikes, the latter without a counterpart of climbing fibers, which are not present in
the DCN (Davis and Young 1997). Somatosensory stimulation tended to induce (via
granule cells) complex spike discharges, whereas stimulation of parallel fibers tended
to induce only simple spikes. It was presumed that somatosensory stimulation to the
granule cells produces greater calcium current (sufficient to activate complex spikes
in the cartwheel cells) than stimulation of the parallel fibers.
expected sensory input within the principal cell array, which, when added to actual
sensory input, results in the subtraction of expected from actual input. The unex-
pected or novel input thus stands out more clearly. The authors found that such a
process is remarkably similar among the four groups of fishes, and suggested that it
may generalize to cerebellum-like structures in other sensory systems and groups of
species (e.g., the optic tectum in teleosts and the mammalian DCN). Analogous con-
clusions concerning elasmobranchs (cartilagenous fish, including rays and sharks,
as opposed to teleosts or bony fish) were reached by Bodznick and Montgomery
(1993). An alternative characterization of the process is that of an adaptive filter
(Montgomery and Bodznick 1994).
12.4.3. Comment
Although there are several similarities of cell and fiber types, there appears to be one
major difference between the arrangement of the valvula, the ELL, and the DCN,
and the true cerebellum, namely, that the latter possesses the capability (via climbing
fibers) of plasticity at the parallel fiber–Purkinje cell synapse. It is not evident that the
other organs just mentioned have such a capability that is refined to the degree found
in the true cerebellum. Thus, in the valvula of mormyrids, climbing fiber synapses
are largely confined to the Purkinje cell body (Fig. 12.6), and the DCN is evidently
devoid of climbing fibers (Davis and Young 1997).
However, in the absence of a mechanism for changing synaptic efficacy, the ques-
tion can be raised as to whether a continuing capability of such synaptic plasticity is
not needed in these organs and, for relatively fixed patterns of behavior, is even un-
necessary. Were this the case, the synaptic efficacy could perhaps be fixed by means of
some other mechanism (e.g., simply by repeated use), without the need for a formal
inferior olive-climbing fiber mechanism. In any case, it would presently appear that
only the true cerebellum can be considered fully adaptive (i.e., capable of adjusting
its own parameters [synaptic weights] to a refined degree).
part three
In this chapter and in the following chapters, the terms “theory,” “hypothesis,” and
“model” are used more or less interchangeably, although “theory” (or “hypothesis”)
could imply a mental construct, whereas “model” could refer to an actual imple-
mentation, in terms of a mathematical model, a device, or on a computer (thus, a
computer model).
This group of chapters on models and theories (this chapter and Chapters 14,
15, and 16) presents a limited survey, in roughly chronological order, of cerebellar
theories that have been advanced since the 1960s. A comprehensive detailed, com-
parative, critical evaluation is not attempted, rather, an overview of the principal
trends in modeling the cerebellum is the objective. In several instances, critiques of
particular theories by proponents of other theories have been included. Some of the
theories summarized in this chapter are only of historical interest, but other older
theories were forerunners of adaptive control models that are considered further in
Chapters 14 through 16. Discussions of cerebellar theories (primarily in its motor
aspects) can also be found in, for example, Llinás (1981b), Ito (1984), Miall et al.
(1993), Houk, Buckingham, and Barto (1996), and Thach (1998a, 1998b, 1998c).
169
170 Nonadaptive Models, Forerunners of Adaptive Models
problem is “well-posed” when its solution exists, is unique, and depends continuously
on the initial data. An ill-posed problem fails to satisfy one or more of these criteria.
Most motor-control (movement-control) problems are ill posed in the sense that
their solutions (i.e., their trajectories, as in touching a finger tip to the nose) are not
unique. Any computational theory or neural-network model of the cerebellum that is
to have biological relevance should possess capability to resolve the ill-posed aspect
of these problems.
To indicate the pace of development of models of the cerebellum, it is interest-
ing to compare the minimal mention of models in the book on the cerebellum by
Eccles, Ito, and Szentágothai (1967): “It is generally believed that in some way the
cerebellum functions as a type of computer that is particularly concerned with the
smooth and effective control of movement” (p. 300), which can be said to indicate
design specifications for models, with the more extensive sections on modeling in
the book by Ito (1984) on the cerebellum. In Ito’s book, there are two chapters
(Chapter 10, Neuronal Network Model, and Chapter 24, Control System Model of
the Cerebellum) on this topic.
Historically, it may be noted that, after the appearance of the book by Eccles, Ito,
and Szentágothai (1967), a tutorial paper by Szentágothai (1968), primarily on the
detailed structure (anatomy) of the cerebellum, appeared in the Proceedings of the
Institute of Electrical Engineers. Interestingly, the three authors whose theories are
described immediately below (Marr, Albus, Fujita) cite the original book of Eccles,
Ito, and Szentágothai (1967) rather than Szentágothai’s tutorial paper.
Most papers on cerebellar theories and models have been multiauthored, but for
convenience and brevity and only for convenience and brevity, the survey in this and
subsequent chapters refers to the first or senior author or to the leader of a group,
or to the author whose name is usually associated with a given theory or model.
so a given parallel fiber would make contact with some 130 Purkinje cells in a time
interval of the order of 20 milliseconds, for a 10-millimeters long parallel fiber. Cor-
respondingly (Braitenberg 1967), a chain of parallel fibers reaching from one side of
the human cerebellum to the other (about 100 millimeters) could be expected to give
a total maximum delay of 200 milliseconds. Braitenberg (1987) indicated that time
differences of such small orders of magnitude as just mentioned could be expected
in the execution of complex movements such as playing a violin.
In Braitenberg’s view, the notion of a timing organ that transforms spatial into
temporal patterns and vice versa (a simple mathematical treatment appears in
Braitenberg [1961]) applies to the entire range of vertebrates that have a cerebellum.
Thus, he stated (1977, p. 78):
in its rudimentary form, in Cyclostomes, [see Chapter 2] the cerebellum is nothing
more than a bridge of nerve tissue with parallel fibers and Purkinje cells between
the right and left vestibular nuclei. In view of this connection to the obviously
more static than dynamic function of equilibrium, can the idea of the cerebellum
as a timing device still be upheld? I believe that one can show that in swimming
aquatic animals (which prehistoric vertebrates that first developed a cerebellum
certainly were) the problem of stabilization against rotation around a longitudinal
axis could best be solved by a mechanism involving temporal coding.
precise delay. Freeman (1969) also described a simple model for the precise se-
quential firing of Purkinje cells that might form the basis of a physiological timing
mechanism as suggested by Braitenberg. In discussing Freeman’s results, Bennett
(1969) pointed out that there are several animals for which precise measurement of
time intervals is necessary for processing of exteroceptive signals, including species
that have electroreceptors for measuring small differences in electric field strength,
such as electric fish, cetaceans (dolphins), and perhaps whales and, of course, bats in
echolocation (see also Chapter 12, concerning the valvula).
Kornhuber (1971, 1974) also proposed the parallel fibers of the cerebellar cor-
tex as a delay line for calculation of the burst duration for rapid preprogrammed
movements.
The difficult problem in Braitenberg’s theory of a maximum delay in one parallel
fiber of not much more than 10 milliseconds in time resulted in a reformulation of the
concept (Braitenberg 1983), which entailed chaining of elementary motor acts (con-
trolled perhaps by the motor cortex), whereas the sequencing within the elementary
episodes would be controlled by the cerebellum. In this connection, a given Purkinje
cell in combination with the parallel fibers that synapse with it was envisaged as be-
ing able to perform velocity detection, by virtue of staggered firing of granule cells
along a beam of parallel fibers (Braitenberg 1993). This capability would permit the
cerebellum to respond to sequences of events, which, by their successive activation
of Purkinje cells, would provide a series of inhibitory influences on the motor system
via the inhibitory output of the Purkinje cells to the cerebellar nuclei (Braitenberg
and Preissl 1992). Braitenberg (1993) considered this view of the cerebellum as an
events-sequencer to be compatible with the cerebellar adjustible pattern generator of
Houk (1987).
Figure 13.2. Schema of “tidal wave.” (A) The effect of a short local stimulus (∗) to the cere-
bellum in time and space. The laterolateral direction of the cerebellum is plotted horizontally;
time is plotted vertically downward, the horizontal lines representing a succession of points
in time. The wave set up by the stimulus travels in both directions and dies out after it has
traveled a length l, corresponding to one branch of the parallel fiber. (B) If a succession of
stimuli (asterisks) imitating movement along the folium is presented, summation of activity
in the parallel fiber system occurs. A wave of maximum amplitude builds up when the stimuli
follow each other at the conduction velocity in the parallel fibers. Upon cessation of the stim-
uli, the tidal wave continues for the length l with diminishing amplitude and then dies out.
(From Braitenberg, Heck, and Sultan 1997, in Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Vol. 20, 1997,
copyright by Cambridge University Press (NY); reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
signals it emits may have a duration (e.g., up to 200 milliseconds) considerably ex-
ceeding the 10 milliseconds limit of a parallel fiber.
In their reformulation (which includes a synopsis of cerebellar anatomy),
Braitenberg, Heck, and Sultan (1997) drew attention to certain contrasts between
cerebellar cortex and cerebral cortex: (1) in humans, the anteroposterior length of
the flattened cerebellar cortex is more than 2 meters, as compared with 0.3 meter for
the flattened cerebral cortex; (2) the surface area of the cerebellar cortex is about
that of one cerebral hemisphere; (3) the number of neurons is about the same in both;
(4) in the cerebellar cortex itself, the only fibers running transversely (the parallel
fibers) are excitatory, whereas axons running perpendicularly to them arise from in-
hibitory interneurons, suppressing activity on either side of an active beam of parallel
fibers (an exception being the inhibitory Golgi neurons, whose axons do not exhibit
a preferred direction); (5) in contrast to a relatively well-defined trend for cerebellar
surface area for a number of species (including the macaque monkey), the surface
area for the cerebellar cortex for humans is some 10 times higher in relation to body
weight; (6) the thickness of the dendritic tree of a Purkinje cell along the folium is
about one-tenth that of the approximately square enclosure encompassing its other
dimensions (35 mu vs. 350 mu, approximately), and there is essentially no overlap of
the dendritic trees of adjacent Purkinje dendritic trees (in contrast to a much higher
overlap of pyramidal cell dendrites in the cerebral cortex, of the order of 1,000).
The paper by Braitenberg, Heck, and Sultan (1997) includes more than one
quotable quote reflecting the view of a long-time worker in the cerebellum
(Braitenberg):
174 Nonadaptive Models, Forerunners of Adaptive Models
In no other part of the nervous system does the shape of neuronal elements suggest
their functional relations as clearly as in the cerebellar cortex (p. 234).
The whole arrangement seems to imply that for each dendritic tree there is a
predetermined place along the length of the parallel fibers at which it is to receive
its synaptic input. The idea that what really matters is the time at which the synaptics
become active is old but still enticing. (p. 234). [Emphasis added].
which sees parallel fibers as generators of time delays between the activation
of different muscles involved in one movement. And this was also the most vulner-
able form of the timing hypothesis, since with the known length of parallel fibers
of a few millimeters and the measured velocity of conduction of about 0.5 m/sec
(Eccles, Ito, and Szentágothai 1967) the delays generated could hardly be more
than 10 msec. This is not enough for the timing of movements which typically take
about 200 or 300 msec for their completion.
Moreover, the vague hope that some internal circuitry could perhaps relay
signals from one delay line to the next in order to generate the longer delays
was thwarted by the finding (Eccles, Ito, and Szentágothai 1967) that, except for
the granular cells/parallel fibers themselves, all interneurones in the cerebellar
cortex are inhibitory. Today’s estimates for 2l (i.e., the combined length of the
two segments of a parallel fiber) vary between 4.7 and 6 mm (Brand, Dahl, and
Mugnaini 1976; Harvey and Napper 1988; Mugnaini, 1983; Pichitpornchai,
Rawson, and Rees, 1994) (p. 235a).
The authors (Braitenberg, Heck, and Sultan 1997) presumed that the functional
unit is the beam of parallel fibers together with the associated postsynaptic synapses,
the ensemble being excited by a sequence of events spelled out by the order of mossy
fibers along its length.
The individual events are signals from the motor cortex interspersed with sig-
nals from the periphery. The output (to the cerebellar nuclei) is triggered by the
resultant tidal wave, which travels through the excited beam (itself not limited to
the length of the parallel fiber), the resultant being a series of signals produced by
selected Purkinje cells at times specified by the moving wave of excitation. (The
effect of the moving wave of excitation on the inhibitory neurons, i.e., stellate and
basket cells was assumed to be one of inhibition of adjacent or off beam Purkinje
cells.) In the cerebellar nuclei, the inhibitory Purkinje cell output is subtracted from
the excitatory collaterals of mossy and climbing fibers, thus “sculpting” (Eccles
1973) the motor commands to the periphery, either via the motor cortex or the red
nucleus.
Braitenberg, Heck, and Sultan (1997) pointed out that the experimental verifica-
tion of tidal waves, using rat cerebellar slices, indicated that parallel fiber-to-Purkinje
cell synapses were quite effective when the input is put to them in the right way, in
comparison with synapses onto Purkinje cells from the ascending stems of gran-
ular cell axons (i.e., before bifurcation of the latter). It was also pointed out that
the cerebellum is fed at least as much information from the surface of the body
(somatosensory input) as from the deep (muscle, tendon, and joint) senses.
(The authors’ target article [Braitenberg, Heck, and Sultan 1997] appears together
with 26 open peer commentaries, which provide a rich diversity of ideas concerning
13.2. Nonadaptive Theories and Models 175
the cerebellum and its function, ending with the authors’ responses to the commen-
taries.)
That the cerebellum is a clock in the millisecond range can be considered a partial
theory, limited by the fact that the maximum time delays (of about 10 milliseconds)
appear to be too short (in comparison with movements of 200–300 duration), unless
some additional time-delay mechanism becomes evident. It may be pointed out, how-
ever, that any theory that entails prediction (as does adaptive control) necessitates a
capability of prediction (Chapter 17). But the “tidal-wave” hypothesis of sequences
of events in and out of the cerebellar cortex appears to move the strict requirement
on timing to structures outside the cerebellum, which raises its own set of problems
about timing mechanisms such as the nature of the generators.
∗
Kado’s concept was presented during discussions at a Neurosciences Research Program work
session on the cerebellum held near Portland, Oregon, May 3–5, 1966, but the concept does
not appear in the summary proceedings of that meeting (Neurosciences Research Symposium
Summaries, Vol. 2 [1967], pp. 515–616). A projected volume in which this idea was to appear in a
chapter on the cerebellum, although cited more than once by workers on the cerebellum, was not
actually published for various reasons. The projected chapter was to have been: Harman, L.D.,
Kado, R.T., and Lewis, E.R. (1971): “Cerebellar modeling problems.” In L.D. Harmon (Ed.); To
Understand Brains, New York: Prentice Hall. Kado, R.T., personal communication, 1996.
13.2. Nonadaptive Theories and Models 177
Figure 13.3. Allen and Tsukahara’s schema showing proposed roles of several brain struc-
tures in movements. Thin dashed lines represent a pathway of unknown importance. Heavy
dashed lines at A and B represent cooling of the cerebellum and sectioning of the dorsal
roots or dorsal columns, respectively. It was proposed that the basal ganglia and cerebellar
hemispheres are involved with association cortex in programming of volitional movements.
At the time that the motor command descends to motor neurons, engaging the movement, the
pars intermedia of the cerebellum updates the intended movement, based on the motor com-
mand and somatosensory description of limb position and velocity on which the movement is
to be superimposed. Follow-up correction can be performed by motor cortex when cerebel-
lar hemisphere and pars intermedia do not effectively perform their functions. (From Allen
and Tsukahara 1974; Physiological Reviews, Vol. 54, 1974, copyright American Physiological
Society; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
movements, as depicted in Figure 13.3. The schema incorporates the idea that the
association areas participate in the translation of the idea to move into a patterned
activation of certain motor cortical columns and their elemental movements. The
association areas that project to the cerebellar hemispheres are among those in the
premotor chain. Because the cerebellar hemispheres appear to perform their func-
tion without the aid of direct peripheral inputs, it seems that the hemispheres are
more suited for participation in planning the movement than in actual execution and
updating of the movement, which was proposed to be a function of the intermediate
zone. (The fact that the cerebellar hemispheres may receive somatosensory informa-
tion from association areas does not imply that the lateral cerebellum is primarily a
processor for sensory information relayed by the association areas.) Once the move-
ment has been planned within the association cortex, with the help of the cerebellar
hemisphere and basal ganglia, the motor cortex issues the command for movement.
At this point, the intermediate cerebellum updates the movement based on the sen-
sory report of the limb position and velocity on which the intended movement is to be
superimposed. This is a kind of short-term planning as opposed to the longer-range
planning of the association cortex and lateral cerebellum (Fig. 13.3).
Upon cooling of the dentate (Fig. 13.3, line A) of a monkey conditioned to per-
form movements between two target zones: (1) the movement becomes slower; (2)
external cues (e.g., auditory, visual) are used to locate the target zones; and (3) errors
178 Nonadaptive Models, Forerunners of Adaptive Models
appear in rate and range in attempting the movement. Each of these alterations in the
movement was considered to be consistent with the notion that the movement is pri-
marily preprogrammed. However, after sectioning the dorsal columns or dorsal roots
(Fig. 13.3, heavy dashed line B), the movements are still performed markedly well,
indicating preprogramming at the level of the association areas and the cerebellar
hemispheres (Allen and Tsukahara 1974).
In brief, the mossy fiber pathways are taken to be a force-producing agency, whereas
the olivocerebellar (climbing fiber) system is taken to be a directionality-postural
skewing mechanism. Such an arrangement is presumed to exist in Voogd-type com-
partments (Fig. 5.9) so the background or baseline activity (under the influence of
the climbing fibers) could be different in adjacent compartments (by recurrent lat-
eral inhibition). At the same time, mossy fiber activity would produce immediate
“memory-less” changes of activity in the two adjacent compartments without spatial
patterning. The redistribution of activity among a specific class of muscles that would
be affected by such a mechanism when applied to the spino-olivocerebellar system
and the anterior cerebellum would constitute one means whereby intrinsic spinal
locomotor circuits could bias cerebellar outflow to accord with muscle activation in
different gaits (Boylls 1975a).
In a companion report (Boylls 1975b), the author discussed application of the
model to locomotor behavior in the cat and to the VOR, stressing that movement is
centrally programmed, not in terms of individual muscle contractions but rather in
accordance with linkages (ensembles) of many muscles.
Figure 13.4. Mano’s synchronizing pulse hypothesis. Explanation in text. (From Mano,
Kanazawa, and Yamamoto 1989; In Strata (ed.) The Olivocerebellar System in Motor Control,
copyright 1989, Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
(Voogd and Bigaré 1980; see Fig. 5.9), and also of the cerebellar nuclei. The hypoth-
esis was posed in opposition to the hypothesis of the establishment of engrams in the
cerebellar cortex for motor learning (see also Bloedel and Bracha 1995) as the prime
function of the climbing fibers. It was proposed instead that the climbing fibers have
their prime role in relation to real-time operations by the cerebellum during execu-
tion of movements. This view was considered to be supported by a demonstration of
a specific short-term (some 200 milliseconds) action rather than any long-term effect
such as LTD of climbing fibers on simple spike responses. In brief, only a short-lasting
enhancement of the Purkinje cell’s simple spike response (i.e., a gain change) to the
mossy fiber–granule cell–parallel fiber input was envisaged, rather than a prolonged
alteration in Purkinje cell responsiveness resulting from pairing climbing fiber and
parallel fiber inputs.
Thus, according to the “dynamic selection hypothesis,” the distribution of climb-
ing fiber inputs activated by specific patterns of convergence in the inferior olive
determines those populations of sagittally aligned Purkinje cells that will be most
highly modulated by mossy fibers activated under the same behavioral conditions.
The mossy fibers are viewed as conveying critically important quantitative infor-
mation regarding ongoing changes in the periphery or in activity in the descending
pathways.
More generally (Bloedel 1992), the cerebellum is viewed as a “mediator” that
serves to integrate properties of external target space with other movement-related
information so optimum execution coordination of movement occurs.
The hypothesis was considered to build on the tensor hypothesis of the cerebel-
lum elaborated by Pellionsz (see above), and was viewed as being consistent with
182 Nonadaptive Models, Forerunners of Adaptive Models
Llinás’ concept of inherent rhythmicity in the olivocerebellar system and the ex-
pression of this rhythmicity in the activation of sagittal strips of Purkinje cells, which
would serve to modify the correlation among climbing fiber inputs to nearby Purkinje
cells. A detailed critique of Bloedel’s hypothesis was given by Gilbert and Yeo
(1992).
Figure 13.5. Model of an adjustable cerebellar pattern generator. Bursts of RNm (magnocel-
lular red nucleus) are postulated to be produced by regenerative excitatory connections from
NI (nucleus interpositus) to NRTP (nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis) to NI and from NI to
RNm to LRN (lateral reticular nucleus) to NI. Inhibitory input to NI from several Purkinje
cells (three are shown) in the cerebellar cortex is postulated to control the frequency and dura-
tion of bursts of patterened output, which is sent to the spinal cord as a motor command via the
RST (rubrospinal tract). CBCX, cerebellar cortex. (From Houk 1987; in Glickstein, Yeo, and
Stein (eds.) Cerebellum and Neuronal Plasticity, copyright 1987, Kluwer Academic/Plenum
Publishers; reprinted by permission of publisher and author.)
In the adjustable pattern generator model, the programs for generating the motor
commands are learned by adjusting the synaptic weights of parallel fiber to Purkinje
cell synapses, using training signals transmitted by climbing fibers. The model also
included positive feedback loops interconnecting the deep cerebellar nuclei with the
brain stem and motor cortex (Peterson and Houk 1991).
Figure 13.6. Schema of cerebellar circuitry (top), and logic behind implication of cerebellar
circuitry in motor control and points of experimental support (bottom). Top: Model of granule
cell–parallel fiber control of muscular coordination which has the following features: (a) within
each nucleus, there is a use-specific (modal) representation of somatic musculature; (b) the
orientation of the myotome is in the coronal plane; (c) the orientation of the parallel fibers is
also in the coronal plane; (d) the output of the parallel fiber beam of Purkinje cells falls on
13.3. Other Nonadaptive Theories 185
“eligibility trace” that constitutes a short-time memory trace. The learning rule in-
cludes both long-term depression (LTD) and long-term potentiation (LTP), in a for-
mulation similar to that of Albus (1971). The authors related their model to adaptive
control methods of the direct predictive adaptive controller type.
For exploratory purposes, Barto et al. (1999) developed a simplified version of
the APG for adaptive predictive control based on delayed feedback information.
The model consisted of a single module comprising a single unit representing a
Purkinje cell with multistable properties that consisted of a collection of nonlinear
switching elements termed dendritic zones that represented segments of a Purkinje
cell dendritic tree. The model was explored in conjunction with a simulated single
degree-of-freedom limb.
←
Figure 13.6. (contd.)
the nuclear representation of the myotome; (e) different uses of the muscles in a limb may
be coded by different subsets of parallel fibers and their differential effects on the Purkinje
cells (coordination of synergist muscles); and (f) parallel fiber beams that span the nuclei in
their Purkinje cell projection may influence two or more nuclei simultaneously (coordination
of modes of movement). (From Thach, Goodkin, and Keating 1992, in Annual Review of
Neuroscience, Vol. 15, copyright 1992, Annual Reviews; and from Thach 1998a, in Novartis
Symposium 218, copyright 1998, John Wiley & Sons Limited; reproduced by permission of
publishers and author.)
186 Nonadaptive Models, Forerunners of Adaptive Models
body part configurations all being stored at the level of the parallel fiber–Purkinje
cell synapse.
The authors further pointed out that damage of the cerebellum prevents con-
trol of interaction torques during reaching (Bastian et al. 1996). Thus, multijointed
movement was deemed more complex than a summed combination of single-jointed
movements because interaction torques (inertial, centripetal, Coriolis) are generated
by one linkage moving on another. Further, fast compound reaching movements in-
crease the magnitude of the interaction torques, and normally require prediction
and compensation. Mathematical studies of patients and normal control subjects
performing two-jointed reaching movements showed that patients had curved tra-
jectories that overshot the target because of abnormal net torques, due to the patients’
inability to control interaction torques. Patients attempted to eliminate the need to
control interaction torques by reducing velocity and by decomposing the reach into
movements made at each joint in sequence. As reach velocity increased, the errors
increased, consistent with an inability to control interaction torques. Errors were
made in the initial direction of movement, consistent with an inability to predict and
compensate interaction torques.
The data were interpreted as being consistent with views that the cerebellum both
helps to initiate movement and sends predictive signals to prevent errors that other-
wise would occur because of uncontrolled interaction torques (Thach 1998a). It was
speculated that the cerebellum uses motor commands and somatosensory feedback
during early phases of the movement to trigger ad hoc learned patterns of muscle
activity so selected, scaled, and timed to compensate the interaction torques that
develop during later phases of the movement. The learned predictive compensation
requires somatosensory feedback and is apparently removed by damage to the cere-
bellar cortex, from which it is inferred that the learned program is stored within the
cerebellar cortex.
Such mechanisms of the cerebellum, it was suggested, might theoretically also
underlie “cognitive” contributions of the lateral cerebellum, thus allowing multiple
parallel background mental operations (analogous to multiple automatic muscle
actions) concurrently with conscious foreground mental operations (Thach 1998a).
“prefrontal areas” (for thought) would still plan without the help of the cerebellum,
but not so smoothly, efficiently, and effectively (Thach 1996a, 1996b, 1996c).
cerebellar nuclei (output) cells. (The modification rules for Golgi cells were assumed
to be different.) Modification of excitatory transmission between parallel fibers and
Purkinje cells, mossy fibers and granule cells, and mossy fibers and deep cerebellar
nuclei cells, respectively, was considered to depend on inhibition from stellate/basket
cells, Golgi cells, and Purkinje cells, respectively. The nature of modifications of the
different synapses in the three layers of the network was assumed to be determined
by olivary cell activity. Thus, in the absence (or, alternatively, presence) of a signal
from the inferior olive, the long-term potentiation (or, alternatively, depression)
of the efficacy of a synapse between input mossy fiber and output cell could be
induced.
13.3.9. Commentary
Among the theories mentioned to this point, it is perhaps not surprising that there
is little overlap because the theories are very diverse. Thus, for example, emphasis
is laid on tapped delay lines and their modifications (Braitenberg), Purkinje cell
complex spike readout as a measure of inhibition (Eccles), the special coordination
between the association areas of the cerebral cortex and the lateral cerebellum,
(Allen and Tsukahara, Thach), ongoing control by the climbing fibers of Purkinje
cell simple spike rate (Mano, Bloedel, Llinás), transformations of various kinds by the
cerebellar cortex (Calvert and Meno, Hassul and Daniels, Pellionisz), reverberating
circuits (Eccles, Houk), and multimuscle/multijoint synergy (Boylls, Thach). In short,
there is little in the way of themes common to all these theories of the cerebellum.
In contrast, the next group of theories have in common the feature of storage at the
parallel fiber–Purkinje cell synapses, under the conjoint control of the climbing and
mossy fibers.
Figure 13.7. Classical Perceptron. Each sensory cell receives a stimulus of either +1 or 0 (e.g.,
light or dark). This excitation is passed on to the association cells with either a + 1 or −1
multiplying factor. If the input to an association cell exceeds 0, the cell fires and outputs a 1; if
not, it outputs 0. This association layer output is passed on to response cells through weights
Wi, j , which can take any value, positive or negative. Each response cell sums its total input
and, if it exceeds a threshold, the response cell Rj fires, outputting a 1; if not, it outputs 0.
Sensory input patterns are in class 1 for response cell Rj if they cause the response cell to fire,
in class 0 if they do not. By suitable adjustment of the weights Wi, j , various classifications can
be made on a set of input patterns. (From Albus 1971; in Mathematical Biosciences, Vol. 10,
copyright 1971, Elsevier Science; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
In comparison with the Perceptron, the parallel fibers are viewed, in Albus’ theory,
as containing information coded in an ideal manner to serve as the input to the
Perceptron association (intermediate) cells, and the Purkinje cells are considered
to serve a function similar to the Perceptron response (output) cells (Fig. 13.7). In
comparison with the fan-out (divergence) of the Perceptron association cells and
the fan-in (convergence) of the Perceptron response cells, Albus drew attention
to the large fan-out of each granule cell and the enormous fan-in of each Purkinje
cell. Thus, the closely stacked single-plane Purkinje dendritic tree makes possible the
maximum fan-out for each parallel fiber. At the same time, the flat Purkinje dendritic
tree with its input parallel fibers pierces it at right angles, thus maximizing the fan-in
for each Purkinje cell. These layouts were considered optimal for the cerebellum’s
task of recognizing patterns of information from proprioceptive receptors and of
generating the appropriate motor command signals.
In relation to synapse modifiability, Albus viewed the inactivation pause in
Purkinje simple spikes following climbing fiber activation as a classical uncondi-
tioned response (UR), the unconditioned stimulus (US) being the activation of the
climbing fiber. The conditioned stimulus (CS) was taken to be the ongoing mossy
fiber activity at the time of the climbing fiber activation. If so, then the effect of
learning should be that the particular mossy fiber pattern (CS) should elicit a pause
(CR) in Purkinje cell activity similar to the inactivation response (UR) previously
13.4. Cerebellar Learning Theories – Forerunners of Adaptive Control Theories 193
elicited by the climbing fiber activation. Such a pause would require that the relevant
parallel fiber synapses be weakened, rather than strengthened.
In Albus’ formulation, it was postulated that parallel fibers activating the Purkinje
cells toward the end of the inactivation pause in Purkinje cell simple spikes, which
follow climbing fiber activation, generate an error signal that weakens any parallel
fiber synapses that tend to cause the Purkinje cell to fire during the inactivation
response. The more strongly the excitation of that parallel fiber–Purkinje synapse,
the greater the weakening. After learning is complete, the Purkinje cell pauses when
the appropriate constellation of parallel fiber activation occurs, even in the absence
of climbing fiber activation. The parallel with the Perceptron is that, if the response
cell fires or tends to fire when it should not fire, then all synapses from active parallel
fibers would be weakened, whereas if the response cell does not fire improperly, no
adjustments are made.
It was assumed that inhibitory synapses (from basket and stellate cells) are also
subject to the same weakening, because otherwise, firings of Purkinje cells would
cease entirely. Thus, parallel fiber synapses on Purkinje cells, basket cells, and at least
some stellate cells are weakened by incorrectly firing during climbing fiber activity.
(However, other stellate cells, presumed to be subject to climbing fiber influence, were
viewed as exhibiting a strengthening, rather than a weakening, of their parallel fiber
synapses.) Purkinje cell collaterals were viewed as maintaining the average Purkinje
cell activity at a relatively constant level over the entire cortex, as well as engendering
of lateral inhibition (i.e., across different folia). To the recurrent Purkinje collaterals
(and perhaps also to climbing fiber activity) was also attributed a bistable effect in
the firing rate of Purkinje cells (i.e., either spontaneously active or completely silent).
Because, according to Albus’ theory, learning is associated with a decrease in
Purkinje cell inhibitory output, the net effectiveness of the (excitatory) input from
mossy fiber collaterals to the cerebellar nuclei is increased, so less and less (excitatory)
input to the nuclear cells from collaterals of the climbing fibers is necessary to produce
the same amount of nuclear cell response.
Albus (1975a, 1975b; see also Miller, Glanz, and Kraft 1987) described a robotics
application of his theory of the cerebellum in the form of an adaptive system (Chapter
14) in which control functions for many degrees of freedom operating simultaneously
could be computed by referring to a table (the latter analogous to the stored modifi-
cations of parallel fiber–Purkinje cell synaptic weights) rather than by mathematical
solutions of equations.
the latter would be selected in accordance with the climbing fiber territory traversed,
provided the activation of the parallel and climbing fibers approximate one another
quite closely in time. In this way, an originally diffuse cloud of mossy fiber input
would become greatly sharpened.
Figure 13.8. Sidepath connections through the cerebellum of the rabbit. VO, vestibular organ;
FL, flocculus; VN, vestibular nucleus; OM, motorneurons innervating eye muscles. See also
Figure 8.1, of which this is a simplified version. (From Ito 1984; The Cerebellum and Neural
Control, copyright 1984; Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; reprinted by permission of publisher
and author.)
the climbing fiber and the parallel fiber are uncorrelated; when they are correlated,
the synaptic strength could be flexibly adjusted anywhere within its dynamic range.
(Compare Grossberg’s [1969] parallel fiber–climbing fiber correlation model above.)
relay neurons, and spinal neurons. As a specific example, the vestibulocollic (neck)
reflex arc (for holding the head stable) acts through neck and trunk muscles to
reposition the head; this output is sensed by the vestibular organ, thus closing the
loop, in control theory terminology. There is no cerebellar side loop in this instance.
In the case of the VOR, however, the loop is not closed, because movement of the
eye is not detected by the vestibular organ, but rather by the retina, and is reported
to the cerebellar side chain via the inferior olive (Maekawa and Simpson 1973).
The vestibulo-ocular arc, therefore, functions in an open loop manner (i.e., as a
feedforward control system), the cerebellum having the effect of closing the loop by
adjusting the gain in the open loop. This constitutes Ito’s “flocculus hypothesis of the
VOR control” (Ito 1970, 1984).
Ito (1984, p. 328) gave several additional examples of such a modifiable sidepath
model, as well as some examples in which the cerebellum is included not as a side
path, but within a loop itself, and also a hypothetical lateral interaction type in which
there are facilitatory or inhibitory influences from one control system to another. In
general, Ito suggested that the critical requirement for involvement of the cerebellum
in a control system should be lack of an effective feedback loop, but not necessarily
lack of a feedback loop altogether.
An open-loop control system, Ito (1984) indicated, is simpler to design and con-
struct than a closed loop one, because a feedback path is not required. An open-loop
system, however, can be more subject to external disturbances and to changes in
parameters of the control system. These drawbacks of an open-loop control system
could be removed by a computer. Ito pointed out that the cerebellum could have this
role in motor control and, further, it could be the site of the learning process then re-
cently postulated by Marr (1969). Ito then expanded his control system model of the
cerebellum to include, in general terms, adaptive (i.e., self-modification or optimiza-
tion of parameters) and predictive (anticipatory) control, as well as some aspects of
motor learning (Ito 1984).
In more recent formulations (Ito 1990, 1993d) that incorporate more formally the
phenomenon of LTD in Purkinje cells as the basis of long-term storage (Ito, Sakurai,
and Tongroach 1982), Ito (1984) advanced the concept of a cerebellar microcomplex,
consisting of a cortical microzone (Oscarsson 1979) and a small group of cells in a
cerebellar or vestibular nucleus, the ensemble of which acts as an adaptive controller.
The error signals for modification of the Purkinje cells (in relation to parallel fiber
synapses) were considered to arrive via the climbing fibers. Such adaptive-control
microcomplexes, it was postulated, are inserted in reflex arcs, command systems of
voluntary motor control, and probably even in cortical systems performing certain
mental activities (Ito 1990).
The corticonuclear microcomplexes were postulated to function as follows: (1)
Purkinje cells have an inhibitory action on cerebellar nuclear cells, whereas
both mossy and climbing fibers give off excitatory collaterals to nuclear cells. (2)
Climbing fibers convey encoded error signals. (3) These act to induce LTD in those
parallel fiber–Purkinje cell synapses that were coactivated with the climbing fiber
input.
13.5. Early Adaptive Control Models 197
F1(u(t)) x1(t) w1
TRANSDUCER
F2(u(t)) x2(t)
w2
u(t)
Σ y(t)
FM(u(t)) xM(t)
wM
c(t)
COMPARATOR
Figure 13.9. Fujita’s diagram of an adaptive filter. In the counterpart cerebellar model, u(t) is
a mossy fiber signal; F1 . . . FM represent the output, at various delays (phases), of the mossy
fiber–Golgi cell–granule cell phasing system; w1 . . . wM are the weights of the parallel fiber–
Purkinje cell synapses; y(t) is the output of the Purkinje cells; c(t) the training signal from the
climbing fibers, which is compared with the output y(t) in an iterative manner to readjust the
weights to minimize the error (in a “mean square” sense) between y(t) and c(t). (From Fujita
1982a; Biological Cybernetics, Vol. 45, copyright 1982, Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg; reprinted
by permission of publisher and author.)
As theoretical models for this concept, Ito (1990) cited Albus’ (1971) Perceptron
model and Fujita’s (1982a) adaptive filter model (see below). In actuality, Ito (1993c,
1993d) suggested, ordinary voluntary movement is learned during repeated practice
in a feedback mode, but after learning is completed, it is executed in a feedforward
mode.
Figure 13.10. Schematic diagram of the Smith Predictor. The Smith Predictor consists of the
boxes labeled L and D , and lies within a negative feedback loop in which errors (sensed by
Comparator 1) are converted by a controller (C) to torques that are sent to the limb (L). The
feedback loop contains delays on the forward (D1 ) and backward (D2 ) paths. The dynamic
(time-varying) model of the limb (L ) lies on a fast internal feedback loop that receives a
moving average of the torques sent to the muscles (MA). The output of the dynamic model is
sent to Comparator 2, and also through the model delay (D = D1 + D2 ) before comparison
(by Comparator 3) with the delayed feedback. The dashed lines indicate training signals for
modifying the two models to make the Predictor adaptive or time varying. (From Miall and
Wolpert, 1995, in Ferrell and Proske, eds., Neural Control of Movement, copyright 1995,
Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers; reprinted by permission of publisher and author.)
As Marr and Albus assumed in their models, Fujita assumed that a coincidence
of parallel fiber activity and climbing fiber activity onto Purkinje cells resulted in an
alteration in the efficacy of the parallel fiber–Purkinje cell synapses.
For the filtering function, a Golgi cell was considered to act as a (relative) phase-
lag element, so various lead-lag versions of the incoming mossy fiber signals were
available to the parallel fibers from mossy fiber–granule cell–Golgi cell input network
(Fig. 13.9). The parallel fiber signals are gathered together through the adjustable
weights of their synapses onto the Purkinje cells, to form the output signals of the
Purkinje cells.
In a companion paper, Fujita (1982b) applied his adaptive linear filter model to the
VOR (Chapter 8), obtaining good agreement with experimental data on alterations
of the reflex by prisms.
and passive movements suggested that 80–90% of the neocerebellar signal could be
attributed to sensory information processing. The authors concluded that the neo-
cerebellum may be concerned with monitoring the outcome (the afferent sensory
component) and optimizing movements using sensory (feedback) information.
From a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study of normal subjects,
Parsons et al. (1997) concluded that the lateral cerebellum and dentate nucleus is pri-
marily concerned with sensory rather than motor processing (see also Paulin 1993).
Thus, activation of the dentate occurred during sensory stimulation when there were
no accompanying overt finger movements or discrimination, but substantial finger
movements (when not associated with tactile discrimination) did not result in signif-
icant activation. However, dentate activation was greatly enhanced when a sensory
discrimination was required, with or without overt finger movements. The strongest
activation occurred when sensory discrimination was combined with finger move-
ments.
relevant neural system (sensory, motor, memory, attention, language, etc.) that may
be needed in upcoming moments. Cerebellar preparatory actions thus facilitate and
improve sensory processing, as well as mental and motor performance, in response
to subsequent sensory events.
In the survey of models in Chapter 13, several different types of cerebellar models,
including some adaptive control and some neural network models, were treated more
or less equally. In this chapter, by contrast, emphasis is placed on general features
of (the closely interrelated) adaptive control and neural network models, especially
concerning the question of plasticity (memory; i.e., changeability of weights), as a
model for changes of efficacy of mossy fiber–Purkinje cell dendrite synapses under the
influence of the climbing fibers. This chapter paves the way for Chapter 15, in which
the features of adaptive controllers (as well as, in principle, neural network models)
are illustrated with the aid of a specific implementation. Thus, a look is taken inside
the “black box” or schematic diagram. In turn, more advanced and recent adaptive
control and neural network models are considered in Chapter 16.
A brief word should be mentioned about terminology and synonyms, a topic
that will also arise later. Neural nets, nerve nets, or (artificial) neural networks, are
also known as connectionist models (theories) of computation (Rumelhart 1990).
Adaptive controllers are also known as adaptive filters, adaptive signal processors,
state estimators, and Kalman filters or Kalman–Bucy filters. These terms are used
more or less interchangeably in this text. (The term, adaptive controller, has a slightly
different meaning from that of adaptive signal processor; the latter implies that there
is no specifically controlled object; see also Chapter 16.)
Haykin (1996, p. 17) characterizes an (artificial) neural network as consisting
of a large number of interconnected nonlinear processing units called neurons, the
development of which from the beginning has been motivated by the way the human
brain performs its operations, hence the name.
Surveys of the history of and tutorials on the topic of neural networks can be
found, for example, in Sutton and Barto (1981), Cowan and Sharp (1988a, 1988b),
Lockery (1992), Vemuri (1992), Fetz (1993), Gupta and Rao (1994), Arbib (1998),
Rolls and Treves (1998), and Haykin (1995, 1996). For the field of adaptive control,
standard references include Widrow and Stearns (1985), Haykin (1996), and Ioannou
204
14.1. Neural Networks 205
and Sun Jing (1996). A comparative survey of the two fields, with particular emphasis
on historical aspects, is given by Widrow and Lehr (1990).
14.1.1. History
The field of neural networks is often considered to have begun with the publication
of Rosenblatt’s (1958, 1961) description of his Perceptron. As mentioned in Chapter
13, the Perceptron, in its most specific form, was a visual pattern-recognition device.
However, Rosenblatt himself originally intended the term perceptron (not capital-
ized) as a generic name for a variety of theoretical nerve nets (i.e., to refer to a
set of signal-generating units, or “neurons”) connected together to form a network.
Rosenblatt considered the neuron model to be a direct descent of that proposed
by McCulloch and Pitts (1943), in which the fundamental thesis, as summarized by
Rosenblatt, was that all psychological phenomena can be analyzed and understood
in terms of activity in a network of two-state (i.e., all or none, or one bit) logical de-
vices. Indeed, Arbib and Amari (1985) pointed out that Pitts and McCulloch (1947)
modeled the superior colliculus as a distributed controller of eye movements, in
which spatial distribution of activity, rather than localized decision making based on
a lumped variable, provided the appropriate transformation from retinal activity to
muscle contraction.
Werbos (1975) completed a doctoral dissertation at Harvard University enti-
tled, “Beyond Regression: New Tools for Prediction and Analysis in the Behavioral
Sciences” (see also Werbos 1990). Werbos’ dissertation, although unnoticed for a
time, was the forerunner of a blossoming of activity in the neural networks field.
Werbos’ starting point had been to attempt to improve on the work of Box and
Jenkins (1970) concerning the method of prediction known as autoregressive mov-
ing average (ARMA) analysis. The essential contribution of Werbos (1975) was to
implement two separate methods, using dynamic (time-varying) feedback to accel-
erate the estimation of the coefficients in an ARMA process. A brief digression is
made concerning the ARMA approach.
In ordinary autoregressive analysis or linear prediction (see e.g., Barlow 1985;
Haykin 1996; Makhoul 1975), a time series or signal (e.g., a voltage–time graph) is
“regressed on itself,” by which a model of the time series is made. In the equivalent
terms of linear prediction, the immediate past sample points of the signal (i.e., the
most recent set of values resulting from periodic sampling) are used iteratively to
predict the value at the next sample points. The term linear implies that the predicted
output is a sum of weighted portions (weighted, of course, implies multiplication) of
the most recent sample values. The ensemble of weights or coefficients to be applied
to the sequence of the immediately previous sample points must be computed, for
which methods exist (e.g., the so-called least-mean-square [LMS] technique; see e.g.,
Makhoul 1975). The error signal, itself a time series, is just the time sequence of the
difference between the predicted values and the actual values.
206 Neural Networks and Adaptive Control: Neural Network Models
In ARMA analysis (Haykin 1996), the error signal itself (i.e., the prediction
error) is also expressed as a linear combination of its own past. Correspondingly, the
weights for the error-signal sample points must also be computed, so the ARMA pro-
cedure is clearly a more complex computational procedure than is the autoregressive
method.
In both autoregressive (AR) analysis and in ARMA analysis, the original sig-
nal is assumed to be unchanging in its statistical characteristics (e.g., its power
spectrum). However, in Kalman filtering (or Kalman–Bucy filtering, in the ana-
log case; see, e.g., Barlow 1985; Haykin 1996), the original signal can be changing
or nonstationary. The output error signal is fed back iteratively in such a way as
to adjust the weights to maintain a minimum output error, even though the input
itself is nonstationary or changing with time. It is this essential principle of self-
adjustment that lies at the basis of both adaptive control and neural network meth-
ods and makes them powerful (e.g., for prediction of stationary and nonstationary
signals), and at the same time, makes them relevant to the problem of modeling the
cerebellum.
Figure 14.1. Properties of the units and network used for simulating the vestibulo-oculomotor
system. A, Sample intermediate cell, with weighted inputs and summed and shaped (S(Ej ))
output. B, Sigmoidal transfer characteristic for summed output to firing rate. A zero input
result in a spontaneous rate of 0.5. C, General network architecture. Ordinarily, each unit
projects to all units in the subsequent layer (note that direction of flow is from bottom to
top). (From Anastasio and Robinson 1989b; Biological Cybernetics, Vol. 61, copyright 1989,
Springer-Verlag; reproduced by permission of publisher and author.)
the weights for the ones nearest the outputs are processed first, followed by those
for the successively earlier hidden layers. (Fig. 14.1 is considered further below.)
In effect, back-propagation networks have internal (hidden) units that are free to
take on response properties to best accomplish a given computational task (Zipser
and Andersen 1988).
In recurrent or feedback networks (which have their equivalent in more com-
plex arrangements of feedforward networks), multiple hidden layers can propagate
backward as well as forward, with weights for both directions in some instances. An
example of a single-layer feedback neural network is shown in Figure 14.4. (It should
14.1. Neural Networks 209
Figure 14.2. Sigmoid transfer function (solid line) together with linear (dashed line) and
stepfunction or threshold (dotted-dashed line) transfer functions. Note that, in the linear case,
the mean firing rate does not take on negative values. (From Hopfield, 1982; in Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences, Vol. 79; reprinted by permission of the author.)
be noted that the feedback path is separate from the back-propagation, [which can
also be viewed as feedback] for weight adjustment for time-varying inputs].) (A recur-
rent or feedback network can be viewed as the counterpart of a recursive adaptive fil-
ter, that is, one having an infinite impulse response [IIR] – see below and Chapter 15.)
A development in neural networks by Williams and Zipser (1989) made it possible
to run continuously (i.e., for time-varying inputs) recurrent (i.e., using feedback)
Figure 14.3. The operations of conversion (top) between the wave W and pulse P modes of
activity for the single neuron and for the ensemble. At the single unit level, P to W is time
varying and nonlinear, and W to P is time varying (t) but linear between threshold and cathodal
block. At the macroscopic level, W to P is static and nonlinear, whereas P to W is constrained to
a small-signal linear range. (The slope dW/dP changes with learning; the slope dP/dW changes
with arousal.) (From Freeman 1983; in Basar et al. (eds.), Synergetics of the Brain, copyright
1983 Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg; reprinted by permission of publisher and author.)
210 Neural Networks and Adaptive Control: Neural Network Models
Figure 14.4. Single layer feedback neural network. The inputs are x1 , x2 , . . . xn ; the weights
are w12 , w21 , . . . ,wn2 , etc.; the bias inputs are w01 , w02 , . . . w0n ; the transfer characteristic curve
(Fig. 14.2) is indicated by the ψ[.]’s; and the outputs are y1 (k+1), y2 (k+1). . . yn (k+1). Note that
the feedback (recurrent) input, shown at the bottom of the figure, is delayed one unit of time
(indicated by z−1 ) with respect to the x1 , x2 , . . . xn inputs. (This feedback path is independent of
the feedback path employed to determine the weights.) (From Gupta and Rao 1994; reprinted
from Neuro-Control Systems: Theory and Applications, copyright 1994, IEEE; reprinted by
permission of the Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers.)
networks. For such continuously operating neural networks, Williams and Zipser
used the term back-propagation through time, as opposed to back-propagation. Such
dynamic recurrent networks incorporating time-varying activity may include lateral
as well as feedback connections within the same layer (Fetz 1993).
Figure 14.5. Adaptive linear combiner. (The method of computing the weight vector from
the error signal is not shown.) (From Widrow and Lehr, 1990; reprinted from Proc. IEEE,
Vol. 78, copyright 1990, IEEE, by permission of the IEEE.)
dynamic systems (e.g., robot manipulators) based on “neural” nets, provided, how-
ever, stability is ensured and performance is quantified.
Figure 14.7. Adaptive linear element with inclusion of the return loop for automatically ad-
justing the weights by the LMS algorithm. In this instance, provision for a bias (offset) input is
included (top). (From Widrow and Lehr 1990; reprinted from Proc. IEEE, Vol. 78, copyright
1990, IEEE, by permission of the IEEE.)
point on the respective surfaces). For the linear output, the surface is quadratic or
paraboloid – the larger the error, the faster the error decreases. In the sigmoid case,
the smaller the error, the more rapidly the error decreases. (Only the linear output
is used for the illustrations in Chapter 15 [e.g., Fig. 15.2]). In the sigmum (sign only
or binary) case, the error surface is very irregular; in a mathematical sense, the error
function is not differentiable.
A redrawing of the adaptive linear combiner of Figure 14.5 to include the LMS
algorithm, by means of which the weights are determined from the inputs and the
error signal, is shown in Figure 14.7. Note that both linear and binary outputs are
shown.
If the error signal is evaluated as the difference between the training signal and
the binary output, rather than the linear output, the configuration shown in Figure
14.8 results; with the substitution of the Rosenblatt Perceptron Rule or Perceptron
convergence procedure (as previously described) for the LMS algorithm, this config-
uration becomes equivalent to the Perceptron (Widrow and Lehr 1990). However,
if the sigmoid function (Fig. 14.6) is used, the approach becomes mathematically
equivalent to back-propagation (Widrow and Lehr 1990).
The adaptive linear combiner, when fitted with a sigmoid output characteris-
tic, becomes equivalent to the back-propagation method or algorithm. The sigmoid
214 Neural Networks and Adaptive Control: Neural Network Models
Figure 14.8. Adaptive threshold element of the Perceptron. Note that in the adaptive linear
element (Fig. 14.6), the return loop for adjusting the weights (the linear error) is taken before
the threshold device, whereas in the Perceptron, it is taken (as the quantizer error) after the
threshold device. The Perceptron rule or convergence procedure does not change the weights
if the output decision, the yk ,’s are correct (i.e., if the quantizer error is zero.). However, if there
is an error, then the weight vector (the ensemble of weights) is combined either positively or
negatively with the input signal (vector) to diminish the (quantizer) error. (From Widrow and
Lehr, 1990; reprinted from Proc. IEEE, Vol. 78, copyright 1990, IEEE, by permission of the
IEEE.)
shape provides saturation for decision making (i.e., the size of the error is limited
in both directions), yet its mathematically differentiable input–output characteristic
facilitates adaptivity. Widrow and Lehr (1990) gave a comparison of analogue versus
digital implementation of the linear combiner with a sigmoid output characteristic.
As mentioned previously, the sigmoid function has the effect of converting a Gaussian
amplitude distribution to a rectangular (equally probable) amplitude distribution.
To reiterate, in the linear combiner as shown in Figure 14.5, there are multiple
inputs, for the ensemble of which a weight vector (i.e., a set of weights) is computed.
After scaling according to the weights, the inputs are then summed. From comparison
with the desired response or training signal, the error signal is generated, with which
the error is still further minimized.
Such multiple inputs can also be derived from the same source but at a series
of different times (i.e., the input to the linear combiner becomes a time series),
14.2. Adaptive Control 215
Figure 14.9. A time-delay (dynamic) neural network. The weights Wa (k) are automatically
adjusted according to the LMS learning algorithm. (A bias input [x0 ] and weight [w0 ] are
included in the event that the desired output signal has an offset.) (From Gupta and Rao
1994; reprinted from Neuro-Control Systems: Theory and Applications, copyright 1994, by
permission of the IEEE.)
as shown in Figure 14.9. This configuration makes possible the capability of linear
prediction, which is considered and illustrated in Chapter 15. In particular, means
for implementing the LMS algorithm are considered.
In contrast, an IIR filter involves both feedforward and feedback. The pres-
ence of feedback means that portions of the filter output and possibly other in-
ternal variables in the filter are fed back to the input. Consequently, unless an
IIR filter is properly designed, feedback in the filter can indeed make it unstable,
with the result that the filter oscillates; this kind of operation is clearly unac-
ceptable when the requirement is that of filtering for which stability is a “must.”
By itself, the stability problem in IIR filters is manageable in both theoretical
and practical terms. However, when the filter is required to be adaptive, which
brings with it stability problems of its own, the inclusion of adaptivity combined
with feedback that is inherent present in an IIR makes a problem that much
more difficult to handle. It is for this reason that in the majority of applications
requiring the use of adaptivity, the use of an FIR filter is preferred to an IIR
filter even though the latter is less demanding in computational requirements.
(pp. 195–196)
of coding, and the neural net of granule cells (numbering 20,000 in their particular
example) and Purkinje cells in relation to the information-storing capacity of single
parallel fiber–Purkinje cell synapses.
least cocerning the vertical vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) (Anastasio and Robinson
1990).
Robinson (1992a, 1992b) was equally unenthusiastic, as indicated above, about
black box modeling in general, which in his view seldom makes testable predictions
and has largely proven to be a blind alley. On a similar note, Fetz (1993) pointed out
the basic limitation of conventional physiological (and anatomical) data in that they
provide a very selective sample of a complex system. Fetz called for a combination
of unit recording techniques and neural modeling.
Using the VOR as a model, Lisberger and Seijnowski (1992) demonstrated that
changes in the transient component of a neuron’s responses can be transformed into
changes in the steady-state output of a neural network by the use of recurrent (feed-
back) connections. The model was made more general by Qian (1995), by including
two additional time constant variables and two synaptic weight variables.
As mentioned in Chapter 8, Arnold and Robinson (1997) described a neural
network model that learned to simulate the integration necessary for conversion
of eye-velocity commands (e.g., from the semicircular canals) to eye-position com-
mands for the motor neurons of the extraocular muscles process. The model could
also change its gain (by minimization of retinal slip) and, at the same time, compen-
sate for orbital mechanics. The model included an equal number (14) of excitatory
and inhibitory neurons. The integration employs positive feedback through lateral
inhibition conveyed by an inhibitory commissure.
In the static neural net model of the VOR described by Coenen, Seijnowski,
and Lisberger (1992), biologically plausible local learning rules that included both
vestibular nuclei and the cerebellum were explored.
Figure 14.10. Neural network model for control of joint angles by APG. A collection of APGs
(network) controls a simulated two-degree-of-freedom kinematic planar arm with antagonistic
muscles at each joint. The task is to move the arm in the plane from a central starting location
to one of eight symmetrically placed targets. Activation of an APG causes a movement of
the arm that is specific to that APG and the magnitude of an APG’s activity determines the
velocity of that movement. The simultaneous activation of selected APGs determines the
arm trajectory as a superposition of these movements. (From Berthier et al. 1992; in Moody,
Hanson, and Lippmann (eds.), Advances in Neural Information Processing 4, copyright 1992,
Academic Press/Morgan Kaufmann; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
Figure 14.11. Functional neural net model of dorsal spinocerebellar tract DSCT circuitry.
Sensory receptor afferents diverge onto four types of presynaptic pathways: two types of
excitatory (E1 and E2) and two types of inhibitory (I1 and I2) pathways responsible for early-
and late-peaking responses. DSCT cells receive convergent input from each type of pathway.
(From Osborn and Poppele 1992; in Journal of Neurophysiology, Vol. 68, 1992, copyright
American Physiological Society; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
the authors proposed a parallel distributed network model of the DSCT (Fig. 14.11).
A consequence of the distributed organization is that the signals transmitted to the
cerebellum by single DSCT cells do not simply represent inputs from specific recep-
tors. Rather, the information is transmitted in ensemble form, so a key element in
the signaling was considered likely to reside in the temporal patterns of activity of
DSCT neurons.
fifteen
223
224 Special Features of Adaptive Controllers and Adaptive Signal Processors
TRAINING
SIGNAL
INPUT +
SIGNAL
e
−
A
a OUTPUT SIGNAL
INVERT
TEACHING SIGNAL
+
B INPUT SIGNAL
e
−
a OUTPUT SIGNAL
INVERT
TEACHING SIGNAL
+
C INPUT SIGNAL e
−
OUTPUT SIGNAL
a
INVERT
Figure 15.1. A. schema for carrying out the repeated adaptation shown in Figure 15.2, trace
2. An error signal, e, (Fig. 15.2, trace 3) is generated as the difference between the train-
ing signal (teaching signal) and the input signal (differencer, upper right), after the lat-
ter is passed through a multiplier (lower right) for scaling. The error signal is fed back in
such a way as to minimize the difference between the training signal and the output sig-
nal as follows. The training signal and the input signal are in phase so that, if their ampli-
tudes at the differencer are equal, the error signal is zero. If not, the error signal will have
either the same phase as, or be inverted in phase with respect to, the training and input
signals. Multiplication of the error signal by the input signal (multiplier at the lower left)
will yield a signal (Fig. 15.3, trace 2) having positive values if the amplitude of the training
signal is greater than the input signal, and negative values if the training signal is less than
the input signal. The product signal is then passed through an operational-amplifier integra-
tor (integral sign), the output (Fig. 15.3, trace 3.) of which is fed to the second multiplier,
15.1. Adaptive Signal Processors 225
itself. In short, there are, of course, no Purkinje cells in an adaptive control device,
nor are there any electronic multipliers in the cerebellum. However, that there are
principles of operation common to both is the presumption behind this chapter.
←
Figure 15.1 (contd.).
the second input for which is the input signal. The latter is thus scaled up or down to minimize
the amplitude of the error signal, e, from the differencer. Inversion of the scaled input signal
yields the output signal (Fig 15.2, trace 2). B and C show successively more compact forms of
A: in B, the first multiplier has been combined with the integrator; and in C, the two multipliers
and the integrator have been combined. The latter form is used in Figures 15.11, 15.12 and
15.15. (From Barlow 1993; in Barlow, The Electroencephalogram: Its Patterns and Origins;
copyright 1993, The MIT Press; reprinted by permission.)
226 Special Features of Adaptive Controllers and Adaptive Signal Processors
Figure 15.2. Adaptation of amplitude of output signal (trace 2) to a training signal that al-
ternates abruptly in amplitude (trace 1); the error signal (trace 3) is the difference between
traces 1 and 2. (Note that the adaptation curve [i.e., the envelope of trace 2] is a succession of
exponentials, corresponding to the linear error case (Fig. 14.6A, B); for the cases of sigmoid
error and sigmum error (Fig. 14.6C, D), different forms of the adaptation curve would result,
a difference that may be of importance in relation to the possible function of the inferior olive
as a differencer [Chapter 17].)
Integrator Time Constant. The effect of a change of the time constant of the inte-
grator components (resistor and capacitor) is illustrated in Figure 15.5. The adap-
tation rate (traces 3 and 4) is faster for the shorter time constant (by a factor of
approximately 10:1), but the corresponding traces in B are appreciably more ir-
regular than the corresponding ones in A, reflecting greater fluctuations in the
multiplying coefficient (scale factor, weighting factor) for the shorter time
constant.
Figure 15.3. Adaptation of the amplitude of a sine wave (trace 4) to a sine wave of abruptly
changing amplitude (trace 1). Trace 2 shows the error signal multiplied by the constant-
amplitude input sine wave (not shown); trace 3 shows the integrated output of the latter
(i.e., the scaling factor, a). The fixed-amplitude input sine wave is multiplied by the coefficient
to yield the output (trace 4), which adapts exponentially to each of the stepwise changes in the
training signal. Note, especially, the relatively slow time course of the coefficient (trace 3) in
comparison with the much faster time course of the individual oscillations of the training signal
(trace 1). Such a difference in frequency range also characterizes the signals in the climbing
fibers and the mossy fibers, respectively (Chapter 17).
multipliers, which corresponds to operating on the linear output of Figure 14.6A, with
its associated quadratic (paraboloid) mean square error (MSE) surface, depicted in
Figure 14.6B. It should be noted that use of the sigmoid (signal compression) output
results in a different MSE surface (i.e., Fig. 14.6C), and that use of the signum (sign
only) output results in yet another MSE surface (Fig. 14.6D). Thus, for the linear
output, the greater the error, the greater the rate of decrease of error (i.e., of the
“strength of the restoring force”), whereas for the sigmoid output, the greater the
error, the lesser the rate of decrease of error (i.e., of the “strength of the restoring
force”). It may be mentioned that the sigmum (sign only) can be implemented by
using the principle of synchronous detection (synchronous rectification), which im-
plementation yielded rather satisfactory results in the author’s experience, despite
the rather complex MSE surface shown in Figure 14.6D.
Effect of Fixed (Frozen) Coefficient. Illustrated in Figure 15.7, this effect could result
from an interruption of the input to the integrator, or of either input (or both) to the
first multiplier (Fig. 15.2A, lower left).
In principle, Purkinje cell climbing fiber impairment could have an analogous
effect.
15.1. Adaptive Signal Processors 229
Figure 15.5. Effect on the adaptation process (removal of random noise from a random noise–
sine wave mixture) of integrator time constant change from 10 sec (A) to 1 sec (B). Trace 1:
random noise. Trace 2: sine wave. Trace 3: mixture of noise and sine wave (sum of traces 1 and
2). Trace 4: multiplying coefficient. Note that the latter is much more irregular for the shorter
integrator time constant. (In both A and B, the coefficient increments negatively after having
been set to zero. For these results, the training signal is the sine wave (trace 2), and the input
signal is the mixture of sine wave and noise. Note that the noise component of the mixture is
progressively eliminated – a process termed adaptive interference canceling.)
230 Special Features of Adaptive Controllers and Adaptive Signal Processors
Figure 15.6. Effect on output (trace 2) of intermittent zeroing of the multiplying coefficient
(trace 3). Trace 1: training and also input signal. Trace 4: error signal. Note that traces 2 and 4
summate to yield the same as trace 1.
Figure 15.7. Effect of “freezing” the weight (multiplying coefficient, a) at a nonzero level
(trace 4). Trace 1: training signal; trace 2: output signal; trace 3: error signal. (The coefficient
was “frozen” in the higher-amplitude phase of the training signal.) Under these conditions,
the error signal is generated, but it is ineffective because the connection to the input to the
integrator (Fig. 15.1A) is open; hence, the output remains constant in amplitude, showing no
adaptation (compare Fig. 7.3).
15.3. Linear Prediction – Adaptation to a Time Series 231
Input w0k
Pattern +1
Vector Output
w1k
x1k
sk
Xk
x2k
w2k Σ
wnk −
xnk ek Σ
Error +
Wk dk
Weight Vector Desired Response
Figure 15.8. Schema of a three-input adaptive linear combiner (same as Fig. 14.5). The weights,
the ensemble of which is termed the weight vector, are determined as indicated in Figure 15.1,
each independently, from the common error signal. (From Widrow and Lehr, 1990; Proc.
IEEE, Vol. 78, copyright 1990, IEEE, by permission of the IEEE.).
Figure 15.9. Sample results from a three-input linear combiner. A. Adaptation of output (trace
2) to a 50% decrease of amplitude of a single, 12.5 Hz, sine wave training signal (trace 1), the
input signal remaining constant in amplitude (not shown); trace 3: error signal (difference
15.3. Linear Prediction – Adaptation to a Time Series 233
Carrying out the process that is performed with a linear combiner on the ensemble
of delayed signals, yk−1 , yk−2 , yk−n , permits prediction, as follows from the relation,
yk − (+a1 yk−1 + a2yk−2 · · · + anyk−n ) = e. (1)
The equation states that the next (sample) value, yk , of a time series (voltage-time
graph) can be predicted (estimated) as a linear (thus, the + signs) combination of a
small series of its immediate past sample values, multiplied by the respective weights
(predictor coefficients), a1 , a2 , . . . an . The error of the prediction is e (i.e., the dif-
ference between the first term on the left and the sum of the remaining terms on
the left). The sequence of values (i.e., time series) of the error constitutes the error
signal. If the prediction is perfect, as is possible for a sine wave (see below), the error
signal will be zero after the predictor coefficients have become adapted.
A block diagram of an ASP configured as a linear predictor is shown in
Figure 15.11. When such an ASP is arranged as a linear predictor, the input signal is
also used as the training signal (top left).
The error signal (there is only one, not eight) in Figure 15.11 is fed back and
multiplied by the respective versions of the delayed signal from the appropriate tap of
the delay line, for modifying the respective weights (i.e., the as). In turn, the delayed
signal from the respective tap of the delay line is multiplied by its corresponding
weight so computed, and the resultant products are summated to yield the (negative)
of the predicted next sample point. Thus, the schema in Figure 15.11 represents a
straightforward implementation of the predictor equation (1). In brief, the processor
“attempts” to predict the next value of the signal on the basis of a small fixed number
of its immediate past sample values. (Note, in Figure 15.11, that if the “desired [or
training] signal” were taken after the first delay step of the tapped delay line rather
than before it, no prediction would be entailed. Indeed, only the first coefficient
would be necessary and the device would reduce to that depicted in Figure 15.1A.)
The linear predictor of Figure 15.11 is of the finite impulse response (FIR; some-
times termed finite-duration impulse response) type, for which the error signal is fed
back to adjust the weights, from which in turn the next sample point is predicted on
the basis of the immediate past sample points, the latter being obtained from the
tapped delay line. FIR arises because input information traverses the unit only once,
from left to right through the tapped delay line (top part of Fig. 15.11).
←
Figure 15.9 (contd.).
between traces 1 and 2); trace 4: weight (coefficient), which parallels the envelope of trace
3. B. Individual sine waves of 10, 15, and 15.5 Hz (traces 1–3), and their sum (trace 4). C.
Adaptation of output (trace 2) to a 50% decrease of amplitude of the sum of 3 sine waves
as the training signal (trace 1); the input signal (B, trace 4) remained constant in amplitude;
trace 3: error signal; trace 4: weight. Note that with each of the two changes in the amplitude
of the training signal (trace 4), the error signal (trace 3) exhibits a sudden increase followed
by an exponential decrease. In C, each of the 3 sine waves was weighted independently (only
one weight is shown as trace 4) and adapts independently of the others. Correspondingly, the
percentage changes of their amplitudes could have been made independent of one another.
234 Special Features of Adaptive Controllers and Adaptive Signal Processors
Figure 15.10. Output of four successive taps of a tapped delay line for A: 5 Hz sine wave; B:
random noise (10 Hz narrow-band); note the slower oscilloscope sweep speed in B. The output
signals from the successive taps are progressively shifted to the right (i.e., later in time).
If there are separate input and training signals (as in Fig. 15.1A), ASP can be
used; such a unit (which is of the FIR type) is depicted in Figure 15.12.
Figure 15.13A shows the actual behavior of the eight weights or coefficients from
the ASP operated in the linear predictor (FIR type) model (Fig. 15.11) for a sine
wave abruptly alternating in frequency between 17 and 27 Hertz. The input and
15.3. Linear Prediction – Adaptation to a Time Series 235
TRAINING SIGNAL
INPUT
SIGNAL −1 −1 −1 −1 −1
Z Z Z Z Z
a1 a2 a3 a4 aL-1 aL
ERROR
+ SIGNAL
Σ Σ Σ Σ Σ Σ Σ
− e
INVERT
PREDICTED
SIGNAL OUT
Figure 15.11. Schema of linear predictor (ASP) having a FIR. The chain of boxes at top labeled
z−1 constitute a tapped delay line. Each of the weights or predictor coefficients (a1 . . . aL ) is
computed independently and continuously, according to the principle depicted in Figure 15.1.
Note that, for a predictor, the training signal is the same as the input signal, but the first weight
is ordinarily computed on the input signal after it has been delayed by one step (top left). It
is relative to the output from the first tap of the delay line that the prediction is carried out
and is compared with the actual (desired or training) signal. (Modified from Barlow 1993; in
Barlow, The Electroencephalogram: Its Patterns and Origins, copyright 1993, The MIT Press;
reprinted by permission.)
output signals for the predictor are shown on a compressed time scale in Figure
15.13B. The same coefficients are shown in multiplexed form (i.e., in sequence) in
Figure 15.14.
There is a rather striking behavior of the weights of the ASP as illustrated by the
following example, which cannot very well be conveyed in illustrations. Let the series
of weights (in the present case, eight) be displayed on an oscilloscope screen; the first
one will always be the largest. Then, after the eight coefficients have been manually
zeroed simultaneously, let adaptation take place to an input sine wave of a particular
frequency. The weights will exhibit some pattern (e.g., upper trace of Fig. 15.14).
Then, if the sine wave is changed to another frequency (say 1.2 times higher), the
pattern of weights on the oscilloscope screen will undergo a change (e.g., Fig. 15.14,
lower trace). If now the sine wave frequency is restored to the original, there is a
TRAINING SIGNAL
INPUT
SIGNAL -1
−1 -1
−1 -1
−1 -1
−1 -1
−1
Z Z Z Z Z
a1 a2 a3 a4 aL-1 aL
ERROR
++ SIGNAL
e
Σ
S Σ
S Σ
S Σ
S Σ
S Σ
S -−
Σ
S OUTPUT
SIGNAL
INVERT
Figure 15.12. Schema of ASP with independent input and teaching signals (at the upper left).
(Compare with the linear predictor of Figure 15.11, for which the two signals are the same.)
236 Special Features of Adaptive Controllers and Adaptive Signal Processors
Figure 15.13. Display of ASP weights A. Response of the eight weights (predictor coefficients)
of the ASP (one-step prediction) to a sine wave input alternating between 17 and 27 Hz at a rate
of 0.3 Hz (trace duration: 10 seconds). The short traces at the left indicate the baselines for the
respective weights. Note the largely independent behavior of the weights during adaptation.
B. Input signal (top trace) and predicted signal (bottom trace). (The moiré pattern evident in
the latter results from the periodic sampling.)
further adaptation, but not as pronounced as the previous one. And so on, for a series
of sine waves of different frequencies, so that after several such changes, the amount
of adaptation necessary appears to diminish progressively. It is as though the ASP
had “learned” to generalize from multiple prior trials of adaptation to minimize the
15.3. Linear Prediction – Adaptation to a Time Series 237
Figure 15.14. Sequential (multiplexed) display of weights (same as those in Fig. 15.13) dis-
played in relation to the baseline (unbroken horizontal traces) after adaptation had occurred,
for two frequencies: 25 Hz (top trace) and 35 Hz (bottom trace). For each of the two fre-
quencies, three sequences of the eight weights are shown. Note that the ensemble of predictor
coefficients for the two frequencies are quite different.
extent of succeeding changes. (This behavior is doubtless related to the fact that, for
a sine wave, there is redundancy among the eight weights.) It may be noted in passing
that the weight-determining units (middle portion of Fig. 15.1A) are the counterparts
of the hidden layer of a three-layer neural net, for which not dissimilar behavior has
been described (Chapter 14).
TRAINING SIGNAL
INPUT
Z −1
SIGNAL −1
Z
−1
Z −1 Z −1 Z
a1 a2 a3 a4 aL-1 aL ERROR
+ SIGNAL
Σ Σ Σ Σ Σ Σ −
Σ
e
OUTPUT
SIGNAL
INVERT
bL bL-1 bL-2 bL-3 b1
−1
Z −1 Z
−1
Z −1 Z
Figure 15.15. Schema of an ASP having an IIR, with weights in the feed-forward (upper) chain
(as), and also in the feedback or recursive (lower) chain (bs). Note that the predicted signal
itself is fed back through the lower chain to improve the prediction of itself (compare with
Fig. 15.12). (Modified from Barlow 1993; in Barlow, The Electroencephalogram: Its Patterns
and Origins, copyright 1993, The MIT Press; reprinted by permission.)
attention is paid to limiting signal amplitude. FIR units, in contrast, are inherently
stable, thus their popularity.
To emphasize in a different way the difference between the FIR and the IIR
configurations, in the FIR configuration, only the error signal is fed back, which is not
destabilizing, whereas in the IIR configuration, the error signal and also the predicted
signal itself are fed back. The former is not destabilizing, but the latter is potentially
destabilizing. Thus, it can be said that the IIR format provides better results, but with
some risks. The same is true for an ASP used to duplicate the characteristics of a
system having unknown characteristics (i.e., the determination of “plant” or system
characteristics), which are considered below.
Figure 15.17. Degradation of fidelity of prediction resulting from decrease of the number
of weights (predictor coefficients; equation 1). (Test signal: 10-Hz narrow band electronic
noise, as in Fig. 15.16; prediction interval: 1 sample interval or 4 milliseconds.) The output
of two predictors (both in FIR configuration) is shown in each panel. The upper trace in
each instance shows the result from the unaltered predictor, and the lower trace, that for the
altered one. In A, from top to bottom, respectively: eight out of eight coefficients (a1 − a8 )
intact; last six of eight (a3 − a8 ) intact; last four of eight (a5 = a8 ) intact; last two of eight
(a7 = a8 ) intact; and no intact coefficients. Note that the prediction is progressively degraded
as the number of lower numbered operative coefficients decreases. In B, from top down, the
first four coefficients (a1 , a2 , a3 , and a4 ) and the first two coefficients (a1 , a2 ), respectively, were
left intact. Note the difference between the fourth panel down in A and the first panel in B,
and between the bottom panel in A and the bottom panel in B, in that the lower numbered
weights are more important for (short-lead) prediction than the higher numbered ones, as
expected.
15.4. Adaptive Modeling (System Characterization) 241
UNKNOWN
SYSTEM
TEST + OUTPUT
SIGNAL −
Σ
e SIGNAL
ADAPTIVE
SIGNAL INVERT
PROCESSOR
Figure 15.18. Schema of arrangement for modeling the forward characteristics of an unknown
system. Both the ASP and the unknown system receive the same test signal (a swept-frequency
sine wave or a random noise signal of the appropriate bandwidth) and the ASP receives the
output of the unknown system as a training signal, matching its own characteristics to that of
the unknown. The results were recorded after adaptation. (Modified from Barlow 1993; in
Barlow, The Electroencephalogram: Its Patterns and Origins, copyright 1993, The MIT Press;
reprinted by permission.)
identical predictors are shown in each panel, the top traces from the unaltered predic-
tor, the bottom traces from the predictor in which the number of predictor coefficients
is progressively decreased. It is evident that the prediction error increases (i.e., the
accuracy of prediction decreases) as the number of higher order of intact coefficients
decreases (Fig. 15.17A), but this effect is smaller for inoperative lower numbered
coefficients (Fig. 15.17B).
Figure 15.20. Modeling the response characteristics of two unknown systems: in A, a band-
reject electronic filter; in B, a band-pass filter, both centered at 10 Hz with 24 dB/octave
roll-off), by means of an IIR type of ASP (Fig. 15.15), using the configuration of Figure 15.18.
Trace 1: 1–20 Hz swept sine wave test signal; trace 2: output of the electronic filter, which is the
training signal input for the ASP; trace 3: output of the ASP, which has mimicked the response
characteristic of the unknown system (i.e., the electronic filter).
unknown system is shown in Figure 15.21. The ASP and the unknown system are
cascaded or placed in series (in principle, the order for the two is immaterial). Because
the test signal is also the teaching signal, the ASP adjusts its coefficients such that, after
adaptation, its characteristic is the inverse of the unknown system. (Correspondingly,
244 Special Features of Adaptive Controllers and Adaptive Signal Processors
TEST + OUTPUT
ADAPTIVE SIGNAL
Σ
SIGNAL UNKNOWN − e
SIGNAL
SYSTEM
POCESSOR
INVERT
Figure 15.21. Schema for inverse modeling of an unknown system (i.e., for obtaining the
inverse characteristic of an unknown system). The test signal serves as both the input signal
and the teaching signal. The ASP is placed in series with the unknown system; the order of
the two is in principal immaterial. Figure 15.22 shows an example.
Figure 15.22. Adaptation to the inverse of a system characteristic. The input signal (trace 2)
for the ASP is obtained from the band-reject electronic filter centered at 10 Hz, the input
for which is a 1–20 Hz swept sine wave (trace 1). The latter signal also serves as the training
signal for the ASP. Because the latter converts its input (trace 2) to be the same as the input
to the electronic filter (trace 1), after adaptation, the transfer characteristic (i.e., frequency
response) of the ASP is the inverse of the filter output (i.e., it resembles the middle trace of
Fig. 15.20B, third trace). Stated in another way, the cascaded sequence of Figure 15.20A, trace
2, and Figure 15.20B, trace 2 (or trace 3 in each) is equivalent to a unity transform (i.e., is the
same, except possibly for a scale factor; the output of the cascaded units becomes the same as
the test signal input, that is, a swept sine wave of constant amplitude).
15.5. Summary of Features of Adaptive Signal Processors 245
1. For a particular task (e.g., forward modeling, inverse modeling), there is self-
adjustment of parameters, or better, self-optimization of parameters (i.e., of
weights or multiplying coefficients) by ASPs, a process that is carried out simul-
taneously for all parameters.
2. There is basically one type of ASP, the linear combiner, which comprises multiple
parallel inputs (Fig. 15.8). Some, or all, of the individual inputs can, however, be
in the form of a time series (voltage/time graph), multiple representations of
which can be derived from a tapped delay line (Figs. 14.9, 15.10, and 15.12).
3. ASPs have two basic inputs. For successive prediction of the next sample point,
these are identical (Fig. 15.11), the prediction being based on the most recent
values of the time series. If one time series (the input) is to be made to resem-
ble another (the teaching signal), separate input and training signals are used
(Fig. 15.12).
4. The coefficients, or weights, in an ASP are optimized by generating an error
signal (Fig. 15.2A) in the form of a difference signal between the training signal
and a scaled version of the input signal, the scaling factor (coefficient, weight)
being determined by the sign and magnitude of the running integral (summation)
of the product of the error signal and the input signal. The running integral of
the product of the error signal and the input signal is a slowly varying signal (i.e.,
the coefficient or weight, which can assume either positive or negative values),
in contrast to the (much) higher frequencies (e.g., by a factor of 100) of the input
(and the training) signals.
5. There is a component in the error signal for each component in the input signal
and the training signal (Figs. 14.9 and 15.8), from which the weights are derived.
6. The integrator time constant (Fig. 15.2A) is an important parameter. Too high a
value will result in a slow rate of adaptation, whereas too low a value will result
in a noisy output signal from the ASP (Fig. 15.5).
7. The rate of adaptation is related to the size of the product of the amplitudes of
the input signal and the training signal (Fig. 15.4). To minimize this effect, signal
compression (Figs. 14.1B, 14.2, and 14.3) or (negative) feedback stabilization of
amplitude of the input signal and the training signal may be necessary. In this
connection, there are several negative (and also some positive) feedback loops
in the cerebellar system.
246 Special Features of Adaptive Controllers and Adaptive Signal Processors
8. In the most general case, the input signals for an ASP (and also a neural-
net configured ASP) comprise a mixture of fan-in or convergence (Fig. 15.8;
Fig. 15.11, bottom row) and fan-out or divergence (Fig. 15.11, top row). For a
neural network model, Figure 13.7 shows both fan-out (on the left) and fan-in,
on the right.
9. For a FIR ASP, signals propagate through the ASP only once, yielding stability
(i.e., lack of spurious oscillations) but with somewhat less than optimal per-
formance. In an IIR ASP, the predicted signal may circulate through the ASP
multiple times and, by such positive feedback, disabling oscillations can occur if
careful attention is not given to limiting the amplitudes.
10. Overall malfunction of an ASP can occur because of malfunction at several
points (Fig. 15.1A): the differencer (upper right); the input signal; one or the
other of the two multipliers, the integrator of which or the input for which could
be interrupted, or zeroed; the weight (output of the integrator); and the inverter
for the output signal. In short, malfunction can occur as a result of a defect at
any one of several sites.
11. Failure of prediction could occur as a result of a defect in the initial elements of
the tapped delay line (Fig. 15.12).
15.6. Comment
From the foregoing, it is evident that ASP (and, correspondingly, adaptive controllers,
in which there is a specifically controlled object such as a limb) are extremely pow-
erful and flexible devices. Prior to a detailed examination of cerebellar anatomy
and mechanisms in the light of features of ASPs, some additional, adaptive control
models of the cerebellum of increasing sophistication are surveyed.
sixteen
247
248 Adaptive Control Models
Figure 16.1. Schema of comparison of a conventional controller having fixed parameters (A)
with an adaptive controller (B). Note the recurrent “ringing” (resulting from inadequate
damping) in the conventional controller that results from a change of plant parameters, but
which is eliminated in the adaptive controller after a single transient. (From Landau, Lozano,
and M’Saad 1998; Adaptive Control, copyright 1998, Springer-Verlag; reprinted by permission
of publisher and author.)
A more significant difference is that time delays play a minor role in signal pro-
cessing. It is often permissible to delay a signal without any noticeable difficulty. Be-
cause control systems deal with feedback, even small time delays can result in drastic
deterioration in performance, a difference that can be considered to be related to
the inertia of the controlled object (e.g., a limb).
A third difference is in the settings for the respective technologies. Adaptive
control is used to design control systems that work well in an unknown or changing
environment. Adaptive signal processing, however, is used to process signals whose
characteristics are unknown or changing. Although there have been attempts to
bring the fields closer together, the view has been expressed that much more effort
is needed in this direction (Åström and Wittenmark 1995).
with fixed parameters is used; a change of the plant (model) parameters occurring
at t = 150 results in poor performance as manifested in the repetitive “ringing.” In
Figure 16.1B, an adaptive controller is used, from which it is evident that, after an
adaptation transient, the nominal performance is recovered.
Thus, whereas the design of a conventional feedback control system is oriented
first toward the elimination of the effect of disturbances on the controlled variables,
the design of adaptive control systems is oriented first toward the elimination of the
effect of parameter disturbances on the performance of the control system (Landau,
Lozano, and M’Saad 1998).
output variables that can be measured. (The three real elements can be considered
the counterpart of the three layers in a three-layer neuronal network [Fig. 14.1C]).
Each vector is considered to be defined in its respective space, the dimensions
of which need not necessarily be the same. The output of the system can be ob-
tained by formal multiplication (more exactly, convolution) of the input by the state
vector:
central nervous system (CNS) – the behavior of the controlled systems (Jordan and
Wolpert 2000).
Figure 16.3. An inverse neural model used as a feedforward controller. (From Kawato and
Gomi 1993; in Mano, Hamada, and DeLong; Role of the Cerebellum and Basal Ganglia in
Voluntary Movement, copyright 1993, Elsevier Science; reprinted by permission.)
Starting from the premise (Miall et al. 1993) that the cerebellum provides predic-
tive estimates based on (efferent) copies of motor commands, Miall (1998) reiterated
the view of the cerebellum as a “forward model” such that, in mimicking the forward,
causal responses of the motor effectors of the motor command, its output becomes
a sensory prediction, a signal estimating the outcome of movement (i.e., the sensory
consequences of the motor commands). Thus, the cerebellum as a forward model
was considered to require sensory inputs to update its knowledge of the current state
of the motor system, a copy of the motor commands that are being sent to the motor
system, and a learning mechanism to ensure the forward model accurately reflects
the motor system behavior and adapts over a long time scale (minutes or hours) to
its changes.
Figure 16.4. A. Feedback error learning scheme to acquire the inverse model of the con-
trolled object. B. The same learning scheme using a three-layer feedforward neural network
model as the inverse dynamics model. In A and B, the broken lines indicate information used
for training. (From Kawato 1996a; in Bloedel, Ebner, and Wise (eds.), The Acquisition of
Motor Behavior in Vertebrates, copyright 1996, The MIT Press; reprinted by permission of the
publisher.)
Figure 16.5. A hierarchical neural network model for learning and control of voluntary move-
ment. (For explanation see text.) (From Kawato 1990a; in Miller, Sutton, and Werbos (eds.),
Neural Networks for Control, copyright 1990, The MIT Press; reprinted by permission of the
publisher.)
In this hierarchical neural network (Fig. 16.5), the association cortex sends the
desired movement pattern θ , expressed in body coordinates, to the motor cortex
where the motor command T (i.e., the torque to be generated by muscles is then
computed by some means). The actual movement pattern θ is measured by proprio-
ceptors and is returned to the motor cortex via the transcortical loop. Then feedback
control can be performed using error in the movement trajectory. However, both
feedback delays and small gains limit the controllable speeds of motions.
The cerebrocerebellum and the parvocellular part of the red nucleus system (Fig.
16.5) receive synaptic inputs from wide areas of the cerebral cortex, but they do not
receive peripheral sensory input. That is, they monitor both the desired trajectory
and the motor command but do not receive information about the actual movement.
Within the cerebrocerebellum-parvocellular red nucleus system, an internal neural
model of the inverse dynamics of the musculoskeletal system is acquired. The inverse
dynamics of the musculoskeletal system is defined as the nonlinear system whose
input and output are inverted; that is, the desired trajectory is the input and the
motor command is the output (compare Figs. 15.21 and 15.22). Once the inverse
dynamics model is acquired by motor learning, it can compute an appropriate motor
command Ti directly from the desired trajectory θd .
16.3. Adaptive Control Models of the Cerebellum 255
long-term
2
depression
st
bc
pf CC
go pc NA
gr
5-HT
mf cf LC
1 RP
Inhibition
(GABA) 3
error
IO
signal
CN,VN
I0 RB
CEREBELLAR
CORTICO-
NUCLEAR COPM DN
− AREA 1 + AREA 2
COMMAND
Figure 16.8. Schematic diagram of a neural circuit for voluntary movement learning control by
a cerebrocerebellar loop. CF: climbing fiber; BC: basket cell; GO: Golgi cell; GR: granule cell
MF: mossy fiber; PC Purkinje cell; PF: parallel fiber; ST: stellate cell; DE: dentate nucleus; IO:
inferior olivary nucleus; PN: pontine nuclei, RNp: parvocellular red nucleus; VL: ventrolateral
nucleus of the thalamus. (From Kawato and Gomi 1992a; Biol. Cybern. Vol. 68, copyright 1992,
Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg; reprinted by permission of publisher and author.).
Figure 16.9. Idealized simulation with an ASP (Chapter 15) of the effect of sudden loss (first
arrow), and recovery (second arrow) of cerebellar participation in voluntary motor control,
based on the schema in Figure 16.4A. Top trace: a constant amplitude constant frequency sinu-
soid representing normal functioning of the desired movement. Trace 2: output of cerebellum
as feedforward motor command. Trace 3: output of motor cortex into feedback controller.
Trace 4: output to controlled object (limb). Note that the sum of the output of the cerebel-
lum (trace 2) and of the motor cortex (trace 3) is constant (trace 4). The disablement of
the “cerebellum” (trace 2) was actually accomplished by temporarily zeroing the weights in
the adaptive signal processor (Chapter 15). Upon termination of the zeroing, the coefficients
restore themselves, and the output of the cerebellum restores itself. (See Figure 15.6.)
The depiction does not include such pathological manifestations as cerebellar ataxia
and decomposition of movements that would actually be present during cerebellar
disablement.)
cerebral gating
delayed feedback
A visual input
medial cerebellum
(LTD and eligibility)
brainstem
visual Cerebral Cortex, Basal Ganglia eye eye
Target saccade gaze
input + Superior Colliculus muscle movement
generators
cerebral gating
delayed feedback
B
visual input
thalamus
Figure 16.10. Systems views of cerebellar modulation of the saccadic system and in the adap-
tation of the throw to deviating prisms: (A) The saccadic system must be adaptive. Note that
“delayed feedback” (which is relayed via the IO [not shown] is a form of visual input, but
it is segregated from that which serves as input to the nonadaptive pathway. (B). Putative
mechanisms of adjustment between eye position and synergy of the muscles in the trunk and
arm involved in throwing. Afferent information on eye position arriving in intermediate zone
lobulus simplex is carried over parallel fibers to Purkinje cells, which project to cells in the
dentate nucleus with control eye, neck, arm, and hand muscle synergies. As the saccade model,
the cerebellum provides a correction to the main pathway, but here the correction is further
“upstream,” via the premotor cortex. (From Arbib, Schweighofer, and Thach 1995; reprinted
from Glencross and Pick (eds.), Motor Control and Sensory Motor Integration: Issues and
Directions, copyright 1995, Elsevier Science; reprinted by permission of the publisher.).
motor commands; Kawato and Gomi 1993). The virtual trajectory model, extended
to whole arm reaching movements, accurately reproduced slow movements (of the
order of 1 second), but faster reaching movements deviated significantly from the
planned trajectories faster reaching movements (of the order of 0.5 second) because
the controllers operating for each joint are not coupled and hence this control system
does not generate straight trajectories for rapid movements with large interaction
forces. These results led the authors to propose a new distributed functional model
262 Adaptive Control Models
Figure 16.11. The cerebellar feedback error learning model (CBFELM). (This is a more recent
version of Figure 16.5.) (A) The general feedback-error-learning model. (B) The CBFELM.
The “controlled object” is a physical entity that is to be controlled by the CNS, such as the
eyes, hands, legs, or torso. The controlled object can be considered as a cascade of transforma-
tions between motor command (e.g., joint torques or muscle activations) and linkage motions
(e.g., joint angular position, velocity, acceleration), and between this linkage motion and the
controlled object motion (e.g., spatial position, velocity, acceleration of the hand). Such trans-
formations represent the system dynamics and kinematics, respectively. By “inverse model” is
meant a neural representation of the transformation from the desired movement trajectory of
the controlled object to the motor commands required to attain this movement goal. Because
the inverse model possesses input transfer characteristics that are the inverse of those of the
controlled object, the cascade of the two systems gives an approximate identity function. That
is, if a desired trajectory is given to the inverse model, then at the end of the cascade the
actual trajectory will be fairly close to the desired trajectory. Thus, accurate inverse models
can be used as ideal feedforward controllers. An example of the trajectory error is retinal slip
for the VOR and ocular following responses (OFR). In engineering, a proportional-integral-
derivative controller is often used as a feedback controller. The component of the final motor
command that is generated by a feedback controller is called the feedback motor command.
(From Wolpert, Miall, and Kawato 1998; in Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Vol. 2, copyright 1998;
reprinted by permission from Elsevier Science; illustration courtesy Dr. Daniel Wolpert.)
16.3. Adaptive Control Models of the Cerebellum 263
Figure 16.13. Arm trajectories before learning and after 2000 trials in a successive “zs”
Holmes-like maneuver (Chapter 11) for movements lasting 750 milliseconds. Note that, af-
ter learning, the real trajectory is closer to the desired trajectory. (From Schweighofer et al.
1998b; in European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 10, 1998, copyright by Blackwell Science
Ltd; reprinted by permission of the publisher.)
The authors (Haruno, Wolpert, and Kawato 1999; Kawato and Wolpert 1998;
Wolpert and Kawato 1998; Wolpert, Miall, and Kawato 1998) then proposed a new
computationally intense model in which each inverse (controller) model is aug-
mented with a corresponding forward (predictive) model, the pair being tightly cou-
pled during acquisition, motor learning, and use, through gating dependent on the
behavioral context. Ensembles of such pairs were termed multiple forward inverse
models (MPFIMs; Fig. 16.14). On the basis of simulations of object manipulation,
the authors concluded that the MPFIM model of human motor learning and control,
like the human motor system itself, can learn multiple tasks (e.g., reaching and grasp-
ing), show generalization to new tasks, and exhibit an ability to switch appropriately
between tasks (Haruno, Wolpert, and Kawato 1999).
The following is taken from a detailed description of the MPFIM by the authors
(Wolpert, Miall, and Kawato 1998). (Additional mathematical details are provided
by Wolpert and Kawato [1998].)
Each module consists of three interacting parts. The first two, the forward model
and the responsibility predictor, are used to determine the responsibility (extent
of participation) of the module. This responsibility signal reflects the degree to
which the module captures the current context and hence should participate in
control. The aim is that the multiple forward models learn to divide experience
so at least one forward model can predict the consequence of performed actions
under any given context. The likelihood that a particular forward model captures
266 Adaptive Control Models
Figure 16.14. Schematic of the multiple paired forward-inverse model (MPFIM), including n
paired modules that are depicted as stacked sheets (the dotted lines represent training signals
and X indicates multiplication). The details of the first module are shown and interactions
between modules take place through the Responsibility Estimator (bottom). Further descrip-
tion in text. (From Wolpert and Kawato 1998; in Neural Networks, Vol. 11, copyright 1998,
Elsevier Science; reprinted by permission of the publisher.) (See also Wolpert, Miall, and
Kawato [1998]).
the current behavior is determined from its prediction error. The smaller this er-
ror, the more likely the sensory feedback and efference copy are consistent with
the context captured by the forward model, and hence the higher the module’s
responsibility. However, the forward model can only be used to estimate respon-
sibility once a movement has been initiated and the results of action are known.
To allow sensory contextual signal to alter the responsibility prior to movement,
a responsibility predictor estimates the responsibility before movement onset us-
ing sensory contextual cues and is trained to approximate the final responsibility
16.3. Adaptive Control Models of the Cerebellum 267
Wolpert and Kawato (1998) indicated that the model assumes that the desired
motor command is available to train multiple inverse models; an assumption, how-
ever, that they pointed out is implausible for biological motor learning. For a more
sophisticated physiological computational model, they used the feedback error learn-
ing model (Kawato, Furawaka, and Suzuki 1987; Kawato and Gomi 1992a) in con-
junction with the just-described simple model. In this more sophisticated version,
the total motor command fed to the motor apparatus is the summation of the total
feedforward motor command and the feedback motor command. The latter can be
calculated from the difference between the desired movement pattern and the actual
pattern, in which the feedback motor command is used as the error signal for the
motor command (Wolpert and Kawato 1998).
It was assumed by the authors (Wolpert and Kawato 1998) that the cerebellum is
the most logical site for the location of the forward and inverse models and that a pair
of forward and inverse models were localized within a microzone or functional unit of
the cerebellar context. It was also assumed that both forward and inverse models are
used in mental simulation of movement, as follows. Forward models would be used
(instead of the motor apparatus) to simulate the results of nonperformed actions on
268 Adaptive Control Models
Figure 16.15. Schematic diagram showing how self-consciousness is envisioned as the for-
ward and inverse models of interactions between parallel sensorimotor modules. (From
Kawato 1997; in Ito, Miyashita, and Rolls (eds.), Cognition, Computation, and Consciousness,
copyright 1997, Oxford University Press; reprinted by permission of publisher and author.)
(Compare with Ito’s schema in Fig. 16.6B.)
and pointed out that, if LTD is the only synaptic plasticity that underlies the learning
mechanism of the cerebellar circuitry, then the learning is unlikely to be accompa-
nied by an increase in rate of discharge of Purkinje cells (which may be reflected
as increased local blood flow by fMRI). Therefore, although the original authors
(Imamizu et al. 2000) assumed that both excitatory and inhibitory synaptic trans-
mission to the Purkinje cells are facilitated, and hence that discharge rates of the
Purkinje cells increase, Ito (2000) pointed out that there may be other possibilities
(e.g., the release of nitric oxide, which relaxes blood capillaries).
17.1. Introduction
In this final chapter, a brief retrospective is presented on the history of the cerebellum
as an adaptive controller, a term used as an inclusive term also for adaptive signal
processor (ASP) and adaptive filter, to refer to a system having the capability to
adjust or optimize its own parameters automatically. An alternative but equivalent
view of the cerebellum as a three-layer neural net follows. Then, the significance of
the basic uniformity of the structure of the cerebellar system is briefly reviewed, as a
preliminary to several other topics. (As previously in this book, the term cerebellar
system refers to the cerebellar cortex, cerebellar nuclei, and the inferior olive [the
olivary nucleus], together with their interconnections.) The significance of the two
principal afferent fiber systems of the cerebellum is then considered in relation to
their features and to their counterparts in adaptive controllers.
Next, the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR), which is based on the vestibulocerebel-
lum, is viewed as the simplest and most straightforward manifestation of an adaptive
controller model of the cerebellum. The VOR is then used as a model for the functions
of the remaining parts of the cerebellum, the spinocerebellum and the cerebrocere-
bellum, including the controversial question of the cerebellum and cognition. Then,
some remaining problems are considered, such as the functions of the cerebellar
nuclei and especially the inferior olive, and the function of negative and positive
feedback circuits.
Major questions that arise from an examination of cerebellar anatomy from the
perspective of adaptive controllers are then considered, including the question of
whether the cerebellum functions only in a scaling (multiplicative) mode in contrast
to a fully predictive mode, which necessitates a precise time-delay capability (i.e., a
tapped delay line) and the related question of whether the cerebellum stores motor
patterns as opposed to scaling coefficients. A final conclusion is reached that the
cerebellar system functions as a scaling device but not as a predictor alone.
273
274 The Cerebellum as Adaptive Controller
microzone, which together with the associated cells in the inferior olive and the
vestibular nuclei (or cerebellar nuclei), constitute a microcomplex (in the sense of
Ito), the basic computational unit of the cerebellar system (Chapter 5).
The remainder (right side) of Figure 17.1A is concerned with oculomotor nuclei
(OMN ) and eye movement (EM), and is of such a nature as to compensate for head
turning so the direction of gaze (arrow at the extreme right) remains constant. If
this is not the case, displacement of the visual image on the retina (i.e., retinal slip)
results, and generates an error signal that is transmitted via the accessory optic tract
(aot) and the nucleus of the accessory optic tract, the inferior olive (IO), and the
Purkinje cells via the climbing fibers (cf ). It is the conjoint action of climbing fibers
and parallel fibers that results in a change in the gain (rate of Purkinje cell simple
spikes) presumably through the action of a change in the LTD and LTP at the parallel
fiber–climbing fiber–Purkinje cell dendritic tree interface (Chapter 7) that results in
a change of gain (α − β) (Fig. 17.1A) for the VOR reflex.
Thus, if the eye movement compensatory to head movement is inadequate and
the eye lags behind, the retinal slip error, conveyed by the climbing fibers via the
IO, results in an altered LTD/LTP level at the Purkinje cells, which in turn induces
a decrease in Purkinje cell inhibitory output, so the resultant diminished inhibition
(i.e., disinhibition) of the VN yields an increased output to the oculomotor nuclei
and a greater velocity of compensatory eye movement, which in turn diminishes the
error signal (i.e., retinal slip).
Turning now to the engineering detail of the schema of the basic adaptive control
model depicted in Figure 17.1C (same as Fig. 15.1A), the counterpart of the semi-
circular canal output is the input signal (upper left in Fig. 17.1C), the amplitude of
which is to be made to be the same as that of a training signal (upper left). To carry
out the match, the scaled input signal (multiplier on the right with scale factor, a)
is subtracted from the training signal (differencer at the upper right) to generate
an error signal, e. Actually, the error signal has a magnitude as well as a phase, rel-
ative to the training signal (i.e., the error signal is either of the same phase as, or
opposite in phase to, the training signal, depending on the relative amplitudes of
the training signal and the scaled input signal). In the schema of Figure 17.1C, the
phase, or polarity, is established by multiplication of the error signal by the input
signal (multiplier on the lower left), and the resultant positive (or negative, depend-
ing on the above-mentioned relative amplitudes) signal is integrated (i.e., fine-grain
summated – box with integration sign) to yield the scale or multiplication factor, a.
Assuming that a begins with a value of zero, it builds up to approach a limit, and the
error signal, e, approaches zero as a decaying exponential. (The input signal, now
matched in amplitude to the training signal, is fed to the differencer in inverted form
for the subtraction; therefore, it is inverted to constitute the output signal [right, in
Fig. 17.1C], as a replica of the training signal).
In the engineering version of the adaptive controller shown in Figure 17.1C,
both multipliers can accept either positive or negative inputs, and consequently their
outputs can be either positive or negative. However, such an arrangement is not
17.3. Modeling the VOR with an Adaptive Controller 277
feasible biologically because the biological signals are pulse trains of varying fre-
quency, which can have only positive values. Revamping the basic adaptive con-
troller as depicted in Figure 17.1D overcomes this difficulty, and at the same time,
more closely approximates the actual VOR circuit of Figure 17.1B, the labeling terms
of which have been transferred to Figure 17.1D. As in Figure 17.1B, the input signal
(from the SCC in Fig. 17.1D), is split into two pathways to the vestibular nuclei (VN):
a direct one, via mossy fiber collaterals (mfc), and an indirect one, via mf, gc, pf, Pc,
and inhibitory Pc axons. The scaled signal via the indirect Path B is subtracted from
that via the direct Path A to generate, in the oculomotor nucleus (OMN), the eye
movement (EM) signal. As in Figure 17.1B, if the eye movement does not compen-
sate exactly for head movement (thus maintaining the direction of gaze constant –
arrow at the upper right in Figure 17.1D), an error signal is generated from retinal
slip (e(rs)), transmitted via the accessory optic tract, the IO, and climbing fibers,
and then, in the Purkinje cell–parallel fiber–climbing fiber complex (indicated by the
horizontal bracket below the lettering Pc, the scaling factor, a, is altered in such a
way as to decrease the rate of retinal slip (i.e., so eye movement velocity matches
[is equal and opposite to] that of the head movement, and consequently the error
signal progressively decreases). Figure 17.1D is thus a combination of Figure 17.1B
and Figure 17.1C.
It should be noted that the two inputs to the multiplier in Figure 17.1D (i.e., the
scaling constant, a, and the pf [parallel fiber] rate) are both nominally positive, so
the output of the multiplier itself will be positive. However, because the Purkinje
cell has an inhibitory output, the multiplier output will be subtracted from the input
arriving from the mf collaterals in the VN.
←
Figure 17.1. Comparison of VOR with ASP. A. (same as Fig. 8.1). Simplified version according
to Robinson (1976) for Ito’s (1972) hypothesized basis of adaptability of gain of the VOR.
Output of semicircular canal (SCC) projects directly to vestibular nucleus (VN) with gain α,
and indirectly via mossy fibers (mf), to granule cells (gc), parallel fibers, and Purkinje cells (Pc)
in the vestibulocerebellum (VC) with gain β. Retinal image slip signal is conveyed from retina
via accessory optic tract (aot) through nucleus of optic tract (not labeled) and inferior olive
(IO) to Purkinje cells via climbing fibers (cf). If cf activity could change mf-Pc synaptic gain β,
the gain of the entire reflex could be changed to eliminate retinal slip during head movements.
Ė: eye velocity; Ḣ: head velocity (gain = α − β); OMN: oculomotor nucleus. (From Robinson
1976; in Journal of Neurophysiology, Vol. 39, copyright 1975 American Physiological Society,
reprinted by permission of the publisher and author.) B. Schematized redrawing of A. C. Basic
adaptive signal processor (same as Fig. 15.1A). D. Combination of B and C to show direct and
indirect pathway for the training signal. Labels as in C, except mfc: mossy fiber collaterals;
a: scaling factor; e(rs): error signal (retinal shift). Note that the scaling factor for the indirect
pathway is determined by the conjunctive activity on the Purkinje cells (Pc) of the integrated
climbing activity and the parallel fibers, and that the scaled activity in the indirect pathway,
arising from Purkinje cells, which are inhibitory, is subtracted from the input to the vestibular
nuclei arising from the direct pathway from the SCC.
278 The Cerebellum as Adaptive Controller
Figure 17.2. Schema (greatly simplified) for the cerebellum (spinocerebellum) based on a
combination of adaptive controller and three-layer neural network configurations. Granular
cells (layer 1), Purkinje cells (layer 2), and cerebellar nuclear cells (layer 3). Fan-out and
fan-in between second and third layers not indicated. Abbreviations: excitatory connections:
+; inhibitory connections: −; Mf: mossy fibers; Mfc: mossy fiber collaterals to the nuclei; Gr:
granule cells; Pf: parallel fibers (vertical segment of granule cell axons not shown); Pk: Purkinje
cells; Pk c: Purkinje cell recurrent collateral; IO: inferior olive; C-O: cerebrocortical-olivary
tracts; S-O: spino-olivary tracts; MoCx: motor cortex; Mes: mesencephalon; Cf: climbing fibers;
Cfc: climbing fiber collaterals; Nu: cerebellar nuclear cell; Nu-Ol fb: nuclear-olivary feedback;
R.N.: red nucleus; Th.: thalamus. In the schema, the inferior olive is considered to be a part
of the weight-determining mechanism, and therefore strictly speaking, not part of the third
layer. (The excitatory nucleo-olivary feedback is not shown.)
network of Figure 14.4.) Similarly, the stellate and basket cells would represent a neg-
ative feedforward system to the Purkinje cells. Optionally, the mossy fiber–glomeruli
(rosette) system could constitute a preliminary layer prior to the first layer described
previously.
The neural net model shown in Figure 17.2, which is greatly simplified, de-
picts the cerebellum (specifically, the cerebrocerebellum) and is based on combined
considerations of adaptive controllers and neural nets, specifically, of three layers.
The schema includes, in particular, the most important common feature of neural
nets and adaptive controllers (i.e., self-adjustment of weights; via the climbing fibers).
(In the case of the vestibulocerebellum, the schema would require some modification
because the counterparts of some of the mechanisms are located in the vestibular
nuclei rather than in the cerebellar nuclei.) The schema includes the cerebellar nuclei
and the inferior olive, as well as the nucleo-olivary and the nucleocortical feedback
paths.
In Figure 17.2, the mossy fiber signal input (extreme left) originates, for exam-
ple, from the periphery (e.g., muscle spindles in the limbs after preprocessing in the
spinal cord) and from the cerebral cortex, and is distributed in a relatively widespread
manner among the Purkinje cells via the granule cells and parallel fibers. The nucle-
ocortical feedback pathway is shown at the top in Figure 17.2. A large divergence
(fan-out), not shown because only one Purkinje cell is depicted, and convergence
(fan-in) thus occurs between the first and second layers of the cerebellar cortex
viewed as a neural net, of which the Purkinje cells constitute the second or “hidden”
17.6. An Alternative View: The Cerebellum as a Three-Layer Neural Net 281
Figure 17.3. Same as Figure 17.2, but with the addition (on the left side) of a schema of the
inhibitory Golgi (Go), stellate (St), and basket (Ba) cells.
layer. Changes in the strength of the synapses (entailing changes of long-term de-
pression of Purkinje cells) between parallel fibers and Purkinje cell dendrites (i.e.,
the weights) are determined by the conjoint action of parallel fibers and climbing
fibers from the inferior olive probably acting as an error detector.
The inferior olive also receives an important (inhibitory) input from the cerebellar
nuclei (Fig. 17.2), as previously mentioned in Chapter 6. This input appears to have the
effect of controlling not only the rate of firing of olivary neurons, but also the degree of
electrotonic spread among olivary neurons, and hence controlling the number of infe-
rior olivary cells activated and, in turn, the size of the group of Purkinje cells activated.
A schema in which the two feedback networks within the cerebellar cortex,
namely, the inhibitory (negative feedback) Golgi cell–granule cell network, and the
stellate cell-basket cell–Purkinje cell network (Chapter 3) have been added, is shown
in Figure 17.3. For this discussion, it is assumed that both of these networks serve
the purpose of maintaining the cerebellar cortex in an appropriate (i.e., optimum)
dynamic operating range, as suggested previously by other authors. It seems likely
that the recurrent collaterals of the (inhibitory) Purkinje cell, which may synapse
onto the soma and proximal dendrites of the same and other Purkinje cells (Brodal
1981) also participate in automatic-level control.
At the conclusion of this section on neural networks as models for the cere-
bellum, it is relevant to reiterate the view expressed by Robinson (1992a, 1992b),
previously mentioned in Chapter 14, that experience with neural networks indicates
that attempts to explain how any biological neural network works on a cell-by-cell
(i.e., reductionist) basis may be futile, and that one may have to be content with at-
tempting to understand the brain at higher levels of organization. In a word, it could
be said that the main work in neural networks is done not at the input and output
levels (layers) but by the “hidden units” of the intermediate layers, and although
the self-organization that characterizes the neural net accomplishes the desired task
(e.g., of modeling a certain brain system), the details of this distributed, internal
282 The Cerebellum as Adaptive Controller
17.8. The Two Major Input Systems to Cerebellar Cortex and Their
Counterparts in Adaptive Controllers
The two major input systems to the cerebellar cortex comprise (1) the mossy fiber
system, derived from peripheral sources (e.g., muscle spindles, tendon organs), which,
after modification by the granule cells, is fed to the distal apical dendritic tree of the
Purkinje cells via the parallel fibers, and (2) the climbing fiber system, which originates
in the inferior olive, a presumptive differencing structure in which an error signal is
derived.
The latter error signal, conveyed to the Purkinje cells via the climbing fibers, is
of a relatively low frequency (1–10 Hertz), modulates or scales the firing rate of
the Purkinje cell output (as simple spikes, at 50–500 Hertz) to the cerebellar nuclei
in response to the parallel fiber input to the Purkinje cells. Just such a two-signal
(a fast “throughput” signal and a slow modulating or scaling signal) arrangement also
obtains in an adaptive controller (see below). It is an essential feature of modeling the
cerebellum as an adaptive controller that the error signal be integrated (summated
or accumulated) by the Purkinje cells to derive the scaling factor; long-term depres-
sion (LTD) and long-term potentiation (LTP) provide just such mechanisms. In the
cerebellar nuclei, the indirect, Purkinje cell-scaled mossy fiber inhibitory output is
subtracted from the direct mossy fiber input, conveyed to the nuclei via the collater-
als of the mossy fibers, and constitutes the output from the nuclei. The details of this
arrangement are now summarized comparatively.
17.11. The Vestibulocerebellum 283
17.9. The Relative Rates of Purkinje Cell Simple and Complex Spikes
and Their Counterparts in Adaptive Controllers
Linear combiners and linear predictors share certain important signal characteris-
tics that are also found in the biological adaptive controllers. Thus, an important
characteristic of both the circuit of the VOR (Fig. 17.1A) and of the basic adaptive
controller (Fig. 17.1C) is the difference between the operating frequencies of the
adaptive control mechanism (i.e., the scaling mechanism) and the signal frequencies.
For the basic adaptive controller, the frequencies of the control or scaling mecha-
nism (i.e., of the output of the integrator [Fig. 15.1A], which constitutes the scaling
coefficient, a) are much lower than the frequencies of the signal being controlled or
modulated (compare trace 3 with trace 4 of Fig. 15.3). In the VOR circuit, these are
manifested respectively in the complex spike rate and the simple spike rate of the
Purkinje cell. The frequency range of the former is of the order of 1 to 10 Hertz,
much lower than the simple spike rate of 50 to 500 Hertz. There are corresponding
differences for the respective afferent fibers to the cerebellar cortex: the higher firing
rate mossy fibers (which, via the parallel fibers, and in conjunction with the climbing
fiber-induced Purkinje cell complex spikes, give rise to Purkinje cell simple spikes)
are thick and heavily myelinated (permitting higher conduction velocities and faster
rates), whereas the climbing fibers of lower firing rate are myelinated but thinner.
The faster complex spike rates (i.e., in the range of 10 Hertz) tend to be associated
with a change (an increase or decrease) in the simple spike rate, whereas the slower
complex spike rates tend to be associated with relatively unchanging simple spike
rates, perhaps even fixed (“locked”) rates, commensurate with long-term storage.
(At the level of the cerebellar nuclei, complex spikes have only a minimal effect
because of their relative infrequency as compared with simple spikes.)
Expressed in a different manner, the signal with the lower frequencies can be
viewed as a modulating signal, the (output) signal with the higher frequencies as
the modulated signal. From this perspective, an important corollary arises: adaptive
controllers do not themselves generate new patterns of activity; they only modulate
the amplitude (or frequency) of existing patterns of activity.
the cerebellar nuclei proper. The vestibulocerebellum has been considered in some
detail in Chapter 8, particularly as concerns the VOR, and comparison of it with the
adaptive controller is relatively straightforward.
via the climbing fibers and employed to scale the report of the mossy fibers, thus, with
repetition, correcting the actual movement to agree with the intended movement.
(It should be mentioned that some interaction may occur in the spinal cord be-
tween the spinocerebellar pathways and the spino-olivary pathways via their respec-
tive spinal nuclei.) An additional input, presumably ultimately inhibitory, from the
motor cortex to the IO would in principle permit voluntary modification of reflex
spinocerebellar activity.
Thus, if for the VOR, retinal slip, as an error signal, e, can be viewed as resulting
from a mismatch between head turning and eye turning (in the opposite direction),
then for the spinocerebellum, the counterpart error signal can be viewed as resulting
from a mismatch between the intended movement (represented by, e.g., a corollary
discharge from descending pathways from brain stem spinal reflex centers to the
IO) and the actual movement (as reported to the IO by spino-olivary pathways), the
error being employed by the Purkinje cells to rescale the report of the mossy fibers,
thus tending to diminish the error as the cerebellar nuclear output is fed to brain
stem spinal reflex centers.
the hindbrain, on the other hand. Thus, each dendritic spine of an olivary neuron
receives both an inhibitory input from a hindbrain center (i.e., cerebellar nuclei,
vestibular nuclei, nucleus prepositus hypoglossi, solitary nucleus, dorsal column nu-
clei) and an excitatory input from the spinal cord, brain stem, mesodiencephalic
junction, or cerebral cortex.
The combination of relatively large receptive surfaces of the dendritic trees of
olivary neurons (Fig. 5.3, cells 2–5), in combination with bushy terminals of afferent
fibers (Fig. 5.4) would seem appropriate to receive and propagate axonal terminations
conveying a great diversity of signals, any one of which may give rise to an error
signal indicating necessary modification of one or another parameter of movement
(e.g., a tactile or pressure encounter with an object or surface in the course of a
movement). Such a divergence (fan-out) and convergence (fan-in) in the inferior
olive is reminiscent of that of the parallel fibers and Purkinje cells of the cerebellar
cortex itself.
It was pointed out in Chapter 5 (De Zeeuw et al. 1990) that at least one third of
labeled glomeruli (rosettes, Fig. 5.4B) in the inferior olive appeared to contain both
direct cerebellar nucleo-olivary (i.e., inhibitory) terminals, and mesodiencephalic
(nucleus of Darkschewitsch and reticular tegmental nucleus [of Bechterew]; i.e., ex-
citatory terminals). In many cases the terminals from both afferent systems contacted
the same dendritic spines. It has been suggested that such excitatory–inhibitory ter-
minations may form the basis of the differencing or subtraction, from which an error
signal could be generated in the form of an increased climbing fiber discharge rate.
arrangement of Purkinje cell dendritic tree and parallel fibers in the mammalian
cerebellum (Fig. 3.2), the arrangement in the valvula is extremely regular or palisade-
like, inviting attributes of precise time measurement, and spatial tuning as in a direc-
tional array (Chapter 12; Fig. 16.2). Moreover, in contrast to the region of synapses
of climbing fibers onto the mammalian cerebellum extending over the proximal third
of its dendritic tree, in the valvula, the extent is essentially confined to the cell body
of the Purkinje cell and its immediate vicinity (compare middle and bottom parts of
Fig. 12.6). The mammalian arrangement permits much more interplay between ef-
fects of climbing fibers and parallel fibers than does the mormyrid arrangement, the
latter idealized as a delay device for coincidence detection of time differences (Meek
1992b).
They are not themselves signals for movement. Thus, it is only when the stored
weights operate on an input and generate an output that a movement is gener-
ated. In this sense, the cerebellum cannot be considered to be a (movement) pattern
generator.
In the case of voluntary movements, the original source of commands appears
to be the cerebral cortex, not the cerebellum. It is upon this cerebral signal, and
sensory information from the periphery associated with it, that the cerebellum, with
its existing or altered weights (the latter according to error or modifying signals
from the inferior olive) carries out its transactions (i.e., its remapping operations
or transformations), sculpting signals for dispatch to lower (brain stem/spinal cord)
and/or to higher (motor) centers for further execution. In this sense, the cerebellum
can be said to have the function of “proportioning” the muscular response, as Wiener
(1948, 1961) suggested.
It should be noted that, in this view, the cerebellum is in continuous operation,
regardless of whether the ensemble of weights (coefficients) remains fixed or under-
goes change, continuously modifying its mossy fiber input to generate the signals
for fine control of movements. Evidently, it is only when, for whatever reason, a new
trajectory must be made that the inferior olive–climbing fiber system becomes active,
and the weights become altered.
In the latter connection, a more complete understanding of mechanisms of alter-
ations of weights is contingent on further clarification of mechanisms of modification
of synaptic efficacy (e.g., in terms of LTD/LTP and perhaps other processes).
as a multiplier) and that the additional capability for time delay (e.g., tapped delay
lines), which would be required to perform such complex functions as prediction
and inverse transformations, lies outside the cerebellar system proper, for example,
in brain stem nuclei (cf. Moore, Desmond, and Berthier 1989). Such a capability is
found in a highly specialized part of the cerebellum of certain species (i.e., the valvula
of mormyrid fish), the structure of which is uniquely suited to enable the generation
of precise time delays.
appendix a
293
294 Appendix A: A Hybrid Analogue
Figure A.1. Schema of a 16-tap analog delay line made up of two interfaced synchronously
driven 16-channel analogue multiplexers interconnected by sample-and-hold circuits (S1–
S16). The outputs are available in either individual form or multiplexed form. (From Barlow
1993; in Barlow, The Electroencephalogram: Its Patterns and Origins, copyright 1993, The MIT
Press; reprinted by permission.)
Figure A.2. Use of the summated individual outputs of two cascaded 16-tap analogue mul-
tiplexers used as an interpolator. Top trace: step function; middle trace: output of the first
multiplexer showing linear interpolation; bottom trace, output of second multiplexer with re-
sultant quadratic interpolation. Further explanation in text. (From Barlow 1993; in Barlow,
The Electroencephalogram: Its Patterns and Origins, copyright 1993, The MIT Press; reprinted
by permission.)
Appendix A: A Hybrid Analogue 295
as outputs (e.g., Figs. 16.13, traces 2 and 3, respectively), as are the weights (predictor
coefficients; Figs. 15.13A and 15.14).
Basically, the ASP uses the second 8 taps of the 16-tap delay line to obtain eight
successive sample values (as though through a window), four 8-channel analogue
multiplexers, and 2 analogue multipliers, in combination with eight of the units de-
picted in Figure 15.1A, to generate a predicted signal (Fig. A.3, upper right). (The
first 8 taps of the 16-tap delay line enabled a selectable initial delay [one to eight
steps] against which the predicted output of the ASP can be compared [Fig. 15.16].)
By varying the configuration of the ASP, forward dynamics (prediction), inverse dy-
namics, and “plant” characterization, as illustrated in Chapter 15 (Figs. 15.18 and
15.21) can be carried out. Both finite response impulse response (FIR) (Fig. 15.11)
and infinite response impulse response (IIR) (Fig. 15.15) configurations are possible
(Chapter 15), although the IIR configuration in effect requires the equivalent of a
second ASP essentially identical with the first (including an independent delay line)
because of the feedback path (Fig. 15.15, lower part).
Author’s Note
The origins of this book, in a sense, go back to the mid-1940s, because it was then,
at the MIT Navy Radar School, that I encountered “hunting” or oscillation as a
manifestation of maladjustment of the control system of radar antennae. In reading
Norbert Wiener’s Cybernetics as a first-year medical student, I again encountered
mention of this phenomenon, which Wiener compared with cerebellar tremor. As I
was learning about the elegance of the anatomy of the cerebellum, the choice of a
career in what was later to become known as neuroscience seemed almost assured.
Serious exploration of the cerebellum in relation to movement control was, how-
ever, to wait quite a number of years, because soon after arriving in Boston in the
early 1950s I had the good fortune to join a small group at the Massachusetts Gen-
eral Hospital and the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, including James Casby,
Mary Brazier, Walter Rosenblith, and Norbert Wiener, who were exploring appli-
cations of Wiener’s time-series analysis (Wiener 1950) to the electroencephalogram
(EEG). These activities on my part ranged from the design of equipment to tutorials
by Wiener on time-series analysis, including prediction theory, the concept of which I
found slightly ethereal at the time. More or less at the same time, I was gaining some
experience in clinical neurology, including manifestations of cerebellar disease such
as ataxia (loss of muscular coordination) and tremor, particularly under the tute-
lage of Dr. Raymond D. Adams, chief of neurology at the Massachusetts General
Hospital.
An important part of the EEG studies during those years was equipment of my
own design and construction, some of it related to artifact suppression or elimination
(e.g., eye movement, and electrocardiographic or EKG artifact), in the form of an
automatic nulling device. Another device constructed was a tapped delay line, for
introducing a series of steadily increasing time delays (e.g., for an EEG signal). One
day in the mid-1980s, it occurred to me that, if I combined these two devices, a
predictor similar to the type Wiener had mentioned years earlier should result –
and indeed, that was the case. Some early results and basic schema appear as an
297
298 Author’s Note
appendix in The Electroencephalogram: Its Patterns and Origins (MIT Press, 1993).
But a predictor is one of the formats of a more general adaptive controller, a device
that, at times, can seem almost alive.
Almost immediately, the circuit of the predictor reminded me of the anatomy
of the cerebellar cortex, of the Purkinje cells, in particular, and I wondered if the
cerebellum were an adaptive controller or, more exactly, could be modeled as an
adaptive controller. I soon found that this was not a new idea, but in the interim, evi-
dence from multiple sources has accumulated and the time seemed ripe to assemble
it. After more than 15 years of incubation, this book is the result.
References
Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics or Control and Communication in the Animal and the Machine.
Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Wiener, N. (1950). Extrapolation, Interpolation, and Smoothing of Stationary Time Series.
Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Bibliography
Adams, J.C. (1986). Neuronal morphology in the human cochlear nucleus. Arch. Otolarnyngol.
112: 1253–1261.
Aiba, A., Kano, M., Chen, C., Stanton, M.E., Fox, G.D., Herrup, K., Zwingman, T.A., and
Tonegawa, S. (1994). Deficient cerebellar long-term depression and impaired motor learning
in mGluR1 mutant mice. Cell 79: 377–388.
Akshoomoff, N.A., and Courchesne, E. (1992). A new role for the cerebellum in cognitive
operations. Behav. Neurosci. 106: 731–738.
Albus, J.S. (1971). A theory of cerebellar function. Math. Biosci. 10: 25–61.
Albus, J.S. (1975a). A new approach to manipulator control: the cerebellar model articulation
controller (CMAC). J. Dyn. Sys. Meas. Contr. 97: 220–227.
Albus, J.S. (1975b). Data storage in the cerebellar model articulation controller (CMAC). J.
Dyn. Sys. Meas. Cont. 97: 228–233.
Allen, G.I., and Tsukahara, N. (1974). Cerebrocerebellar communication systems. Physiol.
Rev. 54: 957–1006.
Altman, J., and Bayer, S.A. (1997). Development of the Cerebellar System in Relation to Its
Evolution, Structure, and Functions. New York: CRC Press, pp. 20–22.
Anastasio, T.J. (1992). Simulating vestibular compensation using recurrent back-propagation.
Biol. Cybern. 66: 389–397.
Anastasio, T.J., and Robinson, D.A. (1989a). The distributed representation of vestibulo-
oculomotor signals by brain-stem neurons. Biol. Cybern. 61: 79–88.
Anastasio, T.J., and Robinson, D.A. (1989b). Distributed parallel processing in the vestibulo-
oculomotor system. Neural Comput. 1: 230–241.
Anastasio, T.J., and Robinson, D.A. (1990). Distributed parallel processing in the vertical
vestibulo-ocular reflex: learning networks compared to tensor theory. Biol. Cybern. 63:
161–167.
Andersen, R.A., Snyder, L.H., Li, C.-S., and Stricanne, B. (1993). Coordinate transformation
in the representation of spatial information. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 3: 171–176.
Andersson, G., and Oscarsson, O. (1978). Climbing fiber microzones in cerebellar vermis and
their projection to different groups of cells in the lateral vestibular nucleus. Exp. Brain Res.
32: 565–579.
Apps, R. (2000). Gating of climbing fibre input to cerebellar cortical zones. Progr. Brain Res.
124: 199–211.
Apps, R., and Garwicz, M. (2000). Precise matching of olivo-cortical divergence and
299
300 Bibliography
Barlow, J.S. (1966). Inertial navigation in relation to animal navigation. J. Inst. Navigation
(Lond.) 19: 302–316.
Barlow, J.S. (1985). Methods of analysis of nonstationary EEGs, with emphasis on segmenta-
tion techniques: a comparative review. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2: 267–364.
Barlow, J.S. (1993). The Electroencephalogram: Its Patterns and Origins. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Barto, A.G., Buckingham, J.T., and Houk, J.C. (1996). In D.S. Touretsky, M.C. Mozer, and
M.E. Hasselmo (eds.), Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 8. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press, pp. 138–144.
Barto, A.G., Fagg, A.H., Sitkoff, N., and Houk, J.C. (1999). A cerebellar model of timing and
prediction in the control of reaching. Neural Comput. 11: 565–594.
Bastian, A.J., and Thach, W.T. (1995). Cerebellar outflow lesions: a comparison of movement
deficits resulting from lesions at the levels of the cerebellum and thalamus. Ann. Neurol. 38:
881–892.
Bastian, A.J., Martin, T.A., Keating, J.G., and Thach, W.T. (1996). Cerebellar ataxia: abnormal
control of interaction torques across multiple joints. J. Neurophysiol. 76: 492–509.
Bastian, A.J., Zackowski, K.M., and Thach, W.T. (2000). Cerebellar ataxia: torque deficiency
or torque mismatch between joints? J. Neurophysiol. 83: 3019–3030.
Bastian, J. (1993). Descending control of electroreception in gymnotid fish: contrasting prop-
erties of direct and indirect feedback pathways. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 173: 670–673.
Batini, C., Buisseret-Delmas, C., Compoint, C., and Daniel, H. (1989). The GABAergic neu-
rones of the cerebellar nuclei in the rat: projections to the cerebellar cortex. Neurosci. Lett.
99: 251–256.
Batini, C., Compoint, C., Buisseret-Delmas, Daniel, H., and Guegan, M. (1992). Cerebellar
nuclei and the nucleocortical projections in the rat: retrograde tracing coupled to GABA
and glutamate immunohistochemistry. J. Comp. Neurol. 315: 74–84.
Bäurle, J., Helmchen, C., and Grüsser-Cornehls, U. (1997). Diverse effects of Purkinje cell
loss on deep cerebellar and vestibular nuclei neurons in Purkinje cell degeneration mutant
mice: a possible compensatory mechanism. J. Comp. Neurol. 384: 580–596.
Bell, C., Bodznick, D., Montgomery, J., and Bastian, J. (1997). The generation and subtraction
of sensory expectations within cerebellum-like structures. Brain Behav. Evol. 50 (suppl. 1):
17–31.
Bell, C., Cordo, P., and Harnad, S. (1996). Controversies in neuroscience IV: motor learning
and synaptic plasticity in the cerebellum: introduction. Behav. Brain Sci. 19: v–vi.
Bell, C.C. (1986). Electroreception in mormyrid fish: central physiology. In: T.H. Bullock and
W. Heiligenberg (eds.), Electroreception. New York: Wiley, pp. 423–452.
Bell, C.C. (1993). The generation of expectations in the electrosensory lobe of mormyrid fish.
J. Comp. Physiol. A. 173: 677–680.
Bell, C.C., and Szabo, T. (1986). Electroreception in mormyrid fish: central anatomy. In: T.H.
Bullock and W. Heiligenberg (eds.), Electroreception. New York: John Wiley, pp. 375–
421.
Bell, C.C., Han, V.Z., Sugawara, Y., and Grant, K., (1997). Synaptic plasticity in a cerebellum-
like structure depends on temporal order. Nature 387: 278–281.
Bennett, M. (1969). Discussion of Oscarsson (1969), In R. Llinás (ed.), Neurobiology of Cere-
bellar Evolution and Development. Chicago: American Medical Association Educational
and Research Foundation, p. 536.
Berkley, K.J., and Worden, I.G. (1978). Projections to the inferior olive of the cat: compar-
isons of input from the dorsal column nuclei, the lateral cervical nucleus, the spino-olivary
pathways, the cerebral cortex and the cerebellum. J. Comp. Neurol. 180: 237–252.
Berrebi, A.S., and Mugnaini, E. (1991). Distribution and targets of the cartwheel cell axon in
the dorsal cochlear nucleus of the guinea pig. Anat. Embryol. 183: 427–454.
302 Bibliography
Berrebi, A.S., and Mugnaini, E. (1993). Alterations in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of cerebellar
mutant mice. In M.A. Merchán, J.M. Juiz, D.A. Godfrey, and E. Mugnaini (eds.), The
Mammalian Cochlear Nuclei: Organization and Function. New York: Plenum, pp. 107–
119.
Berrebi, A.S., Morgan, J.I., and Mugnaini, E. (1990). The Purkinje cell class may extend beyond
the cerebellum. J. Neurocytol. 19: 643–654.
Berthier, N.E., Singh, S.P., and Barto, A.G. (1993). Distributed representation of limb motor
programs in arrays of adjustable pattern generators. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5: 56–78.
Berthier, N.E., Singh, S.P., Barto, A.G., and Houk, J.C. (1992). A cortico-cerebellar model that
learns to generate distributed motor commands to control a kinematic arm. In J.E. Moody,
S.J. Hanson, and R.P. Lippmann (eds.), Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems
4. San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann, pp. 611–618.
Blakemore, S.-J., Frith, C.D., and Wolpert, D.M. (1999). Spatio-temporal prediction modulates
the perception of self-produced stimuli. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11: 551–559.
Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D.M., and Frith, C.D. (1999). The cerebellum contributes to so-
matosensory cortical activity during self-produced tactile stimulation. Neuroimage 10: 448–
459.
Bloedel, J.R. (1992). Functional heterogeneity with structural homogeneity: how does the
cerebellum operate? Behav. Brain Sci. 15: 666–678.
Bloedel, J.R. (1993). ‘Involvement in’ versus ‘storage of.’ Trends Neurosci. 16: 451–452.
Bloedel, J.R., and Bracha, V. (1995). On the cerebellum, cutaneomuscular reflexes, movement
control and the elusive engrams of memory. Behavi. Brain Res. 68: 1–44.
Bloedel, J.R., and Bracha, V. (1998). Current concepts of climbing fiber function. Anat. Rec.
253: 118–126.
Bloedel, J.R., and Courville, J. (1981). Cerebellar afferent systems. In V.B. Brooks (ed.),
Handbook of Physiology Vol. II, Motor Control, Part 2. Bethesda: American Physiological
Society, pp. 735–829.
Bloedel, J.R., and Kelly, T.M. (1992). The dynamic selection hypothesis: a proposed function
for cerebellar sagittal zones. In: R. Llinás and C. Sotelo (eds.), The Cerebellum Revisited.
New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 267–282.
Bloedel, J.R., and Roberts, W.J. (1971). Action of climbing fibers in cerebellar cortex of the
cat. J. Neurophysiol. 34: 17–31.
Bloedel, J.R., Bracha, V., and Larson, P.S. (1993). Real time operations of the cerebellar cortex.
Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 20(suppl. 3): S7–S18.
Bloedel, J.R., Bracha, V., and Milak, M.S. (1993). Role of the cerebellar nuclei in the learning
and performance of forelimb movement in the cat. In N. Mano, I. Hamada, and M.R. De-
Long (eds.), Role of the Cerebellum and Basal Ganglia in Voluntary Movement. Amsterdam:
Elsevier, pp. 21–31.
Bloedel, J.R., Bracha, V., Shimansky, Y., and Milak, M.S. (1996). The role of the cerebellum in
the acquisition of complex volitional forelimb movements. In J.R. Bloedel, T.J. Ebner, and
S.P. Wise (eds.), The Acquisition of Motor Behavior in Vertebrates. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press, pp. 320–330.
Blomfield, S., and Marr, D. (1970). How the cerebellum may be used. Nature 227: 1224–1228.
Blond, O., and Crépel, F. (1996). Letter to the editor. Trends Neurosci. 19: 11.
Bodznick, D., and Montgomery, J.C. (1993). The physiology of the dorsal nucleus of elasmo-
branchs and its descending control. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 173: 680–682.
Boose, A., Dichgans, J., and Topka, H. (1999). Deficits in phasic muscle force generation
explain insufficient compensation for interaction torque in cerebellar patients. Neurosci.
Lett. 26: 53–56.
Botez, M.I. (1992). The neuropsychology of the cerebellum: an emerging concept. Arch. Neu-
rol. 49: 321–324.
Bibliography 303
Bower, J.M. (1995). The cerebellum as sensory acquisition controller: commentary on the
underestimated cerebellum by Leiner et al. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2: 255–256.
Bower, J.M. (1997). Is the cerebellum sensory for motor’s sake, or motor for sensory’s sake:
the view from the whiskers of a rat. Progr. Brain Res. 114: 463–496.
Box, G.E.P., and Jenkins, G.M. (1970). Time Series Analysis: Forecasting and Control. San
Francisco: Holden Day.
Boxall, A.R., Lancaster, B., and Garthwaite, J. (1996). Tyrosine kinase is required for long-term
depression in the cerebellum. Neuron 16: 805–813.
Boylls, C.C. (1975a). A Theory of Cerebellar Function with Applications to Locomotion. I.
The Physiological Role of Climbing Fiber Inputs in Anterior Lobe Operation. Amherst:
University of Massachusetts (COINS Tech. Rep. 75C-6).
Boylls, C.C. (1975b). A Theory of Cerebellar Function with Applications to Locomotion. II.
The Relation of Anterior Lobe Climbing Fiber Function to Locomotor Behavior in the Cat.
Amherst: University of Massachusetts (COINS Tech. Rep. 76-1).
Boylls, C.C. (1980). Cerebellar strategies for movement coordination. In G.E. Stelmach
and J. Requin (eds.), Tutorials in Motor Behavior. Amsterdam: North Holland, pp.
83–94.
Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1984). Local circuit neurons in the cerebellar dentate nucleus of
man. In J.R. Bloedel, J. Dichgans, and W. Precht (eds.), Cerebellar Functions. New York:
Springer-Verlag, pp. 324–325.
Bracha, V., Zhao, L., Wunderlich, D.A., Morrissy, S.J., and Bloedel, J.R. (1997). Patients with
cerebellar lesions cannot acquire but are able to retain conditioned eyeblink reflexes. Brain
120: 1401–1413.
Braitenberg, V. (1961). Functional interpretation of cerebellar histology. Nature 190: 539–540.
Braitenberg, V. (1967). Is the cerebellar cortex a biological clock in the millisecond range?
Progr. Brain Res. 25: 334–346.
Braitenberg, V. (1977). On the Texture of Brains (Chapter 7: Analysis of the cerebellum). New
York: Springer-Verlag.
Braitenberg, V. (1983). The cerebellum revisited. J. Theor. Neurobiol. 2: 237–241.
Braitenberg, V. (1987). The cerebellum and the physics of movement: some speculations. In
M. Glickstein, C. Yeo, and J. Stein (eds.), Cerebellum and Neuronal Plasticity. New York:
Plenum, pp. 193–207.
Braitenberg, V. (1990). Reading the structure of brains. Network: Comput. Neural Syst. 1: 1–11.
Braitenberg, V. (1993). The cerebellar network: attempt at a formalization of its structure.
Network 4: 11–17.
Braitenberg, V., and Atwood, R.P. (1958). Morphological observations on the cerebellar cor-
tex. J. Comp. Neurol. 109: 1–27.
Braitenberg, V., and Preissl, H. (1992). Why is the output of the cerebellum inhibitory? Behav.
Brain Sci. 15: 715–717.
Braitenberg, V., Heck, D., and Sultan, F. (1997). The detection and generation of sequences
as a key to cerebellar function: experiments and theory. Behav. Brain Sci. 20: 229–245.
Brand, S., Dahl, A.-L., and Mugnaini, E. (1976). The length of parallel fibers in the cat cere-
bellar cortex. An experimental light and electron microscopic study. Exp. Brain Res. 26:
39–58.
Bravin, M., Rossi, F., and Strata, P. (1995). Different climbing fibers innervate separate den-
dritic regions of the same Purkinje cell in hypogranular cerebellum. J. Comp. Neurol. 357:
395–407.
Brazier, M.A.B. (1988). A History of Neurophysiology in the 19th Century. New York: Raven
Press, pp. 131–133.
Brindley, G.S. (1964). The use made by the cerebellum of the information that it receives from
sense organs (report on symposia and meetings). IBRO Bull. 3(3): 80.
304 Bibliography
Brodal, A. (1967). Anatomical studies of cerebellar fibre connections with special reference
to problems of functional localization. Progr. Brain Res. 25: 135–173.
Brodal, A. (1981). Neurological Anatomy in Relation to Clinical Medicine (3rd ed.). Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press.
Brodal, A., and Drabløs, P.A. (1963). Two types of mossy fiber terminals in the cerebellum
and their regional distribution. J. Comp. Neurol. 121: 173–187.
Brodal, A., and Jansen, J. (1954). Structural organization of the cerebellum. In J. Jansen and
A. Brodal (eds.), Aspects of Cerebellar Anatomy. Oslo: Johan Grundt Tanum Forlag, pp.
285–395.
Brodal, A., and Kawamura, K. (1980). Olivocerebellar projections: a review. Adv. Anat. Em-
bryol. Cell Biol. 64: 1–140.
Brodal, P. (1998). The Central Nervous System: Structure and Function (2nd ed.). Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press.
Brodal, P., and Bjaalie, J.G. (1997). Salient anatomic features of the cortico-ponto-cerebellar
pathway. Progr. Brain Res. 114: 227–247.
Buisseret-Delmas, C., and Angaut, P.E. (1989). Anatomical mapping of the cerebellar nucle-
ocortical projections in the rat: a retrograde labeling study. J. Comp. Neurol. 288: 297–310.
Bullock, D., Fiala, J.C., and Grossberg, S. (1994). A neural model of timed response learning
in the cerebellum. Neural Netw. 7: 1101–1114.
Bullock, T.H. (1969). Discussion of Oscarsson (1969), In R. Llinás (ed.), Neurobiology of Cere-
bellar Evolution and Development. Chicago: American Medical Association and Research
Foundation, p. 536.
Bullock, T.H. (1986). Significance of findings on electroreception for general neurobiology.
In: T.H. Bullock and W. Heiligenberg (eds.), Electroreception. New York: John Wiley, pp.
651–674.
Buonomano, D.V., and Mauk, M.D. (1994). Neural network model of the cerebellum: temporal
discrimination and the timing of motor responses. Neural Comput. 6: 38–55.
Butler, A.B., and Hodos, W. (1996). Comparative Vertebrate Neuroanatomy: Evolution and
Adaptation. (Chapter 12: The cerebellum.) New York: Wiley-Liss, pp. 180–197.
Cabeza, R., and Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and
fMRI studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12: 1–47.
Calne, D.B. (1959). Pathways converging upon Purkinje cells in the frog’s cerebellum. J. Phys-
iol. 146: 459–464.
Calvert, T.W., and Meno, F. (1972). Neural systems modeling applied to the cerebellum. IEEE
Trans. Sys. Man Cybern. SMC-2: 363–374.
Cant, N.B. (1992). The cochlear nucleus: neuronal types and their synaptic organization. In:
D.B. Webster, A.N. Popper, and R. R. Fay (eds.), The Mammalian Auditory Pathway: Neu-
roanatomy. New York: Springer, pp. 66–116.
Caroni, P. (1997). Overexpression of growth-associated proteins in the neurons of adult trans-
genic mice. J. Neurosci. Methods 71: 3–9.
Casini, L., and Ivry, R.B. (1999). Effects of divided attention on temporal processing in patients
with lesions of the cerebellum or frontal lobe. Neuropsychology 13: 10–21.
Caston, J., Vasseur, F., Stelz, T., Chianale, C., Delhaye-Bouchaud, N., and Mariani, J. (1995).
Differential roles of cerebellar cortex and deep cerebellar nuclei in the learning of the
equilibrium behavior: studies in intact and cerebellectomized lurcher mutant mice. Brain
Res. Dev. Brain Res. 86: 311–316.
Catalan, M.J., Honda, M., Weeks, R.A., Cohen, L.G., and Hallett, M. (1998). The functional
neuroanatomy of simple and complex sequential finger movements: a PET study. Brain 121:
253–264.
Chan-Palay (1982). Discussion of Tolbert, D.L. (1982). The cerebellar nucleocortical pathway.
In S.L. Palay and V. Chan-Palay (eds.), The Cerebellum – New Vistas, Berlin: Springer-
Verlag, pp. 296–319.
Bibliography 305
rat inferior olive: an ultrastructural study using a combination of [3 H]leucine and wheat germ
agglutinin coupled horseradish peroxidase anterograde tracing. Neuroscience 34: 645–655.
De Zeeuw, C.I., Koekkoek, S.K.E., Wylie, D.R.W., and Simpson, J.I. (1997). Association be-
tween dendritic lamellar bodies and complex spike synchrony in the olivocerebellar system.
J. Neurophysiol. 77: 1747–1758.
De Zeeuw, C.I., Ruigrok, T.J.H., Holstege, J.C., Jansen, H.G., and Voogd, J. (1990). Intracellular
labeling of neurons in the medial accessory olive of the cat: II. Ultrastructure of dendritic
spines and their GABAergic innervation. J. Comp. Neurol. 300: 478–494.
De Zeeuw, C.I., Ruigrok, T.J.H., Holstege, J.C., Schalekamp, M.P.A., and Voogd, J. (1990).
Intracellular labeling of neurons in the medial accessory olive of the cat: III. Ultrastructure
of axon hillock and initial segment and their GABAergic innervation. J. Comp. Neurol. 300:
495–510.
De Zeeuw, C.I., Simpson, J.I., Hoogenraad, C.C., Galjart, N., Koekkoek, S.K.E., and Ruigrok,
T.J.H. (1998). Microcircuitry and function of the inferior olive. Trends Neurosci. 21: 391–400.
De Zeeuw, C.I., van Alphen, A.M., Hawkins, R.K., and Ruigrok, T.J.H. (1997). Climbing fibre
collaterals contact neurons in the cerebellar nuclei that provide a GABAergic feedback to
the inferior olive. Neuroscience 80: 981–986.
De Zeeuw, C.I., Wentzel, O.P., and Mugnaini, E. (1993). Fine structure of the dorsal cap of the
inferior olive and its GABAergic and non-GABAergic input from the nucleus prepositus
hypoglossi in rat and rabbit. J. Comp. Neurol. 327: 62–82.
De Zeeuw, C.I., Wylie, D.R., DiGiorgi, P.L., and Simpson, J.I. (1994). Projections of individual
Purkinje cells of identified zones in the flocculus to the vestibular and cerebellar nuclei in
the rabbit. J. Comp. Neurol. 349: 428–447.
Dean, P. (1995). Modelling the role of the cerebellar fastigial nuclei in producing accurate
saccades: the importance of burst timing. Neuroscience. 68: 1059–1077.
Dean, P., Mayhew, J.E., and Langdon, P. (1994). Learning and maintaining saccadic accuracy:
a model of brainstem-cerebellar interactions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 6: 117–138.
Decety, J., Sjöholm, H., Ryding, E., Stenberg, G., and Ingvar, D.H. (1990). The cerebellum
participates in mental activity: tomographic measurements of regional cerebral blood flow.
Brain Res. 535: 313–317.
Denk, W., Sugimori, M., and Llinás, R. (1995). Two types of calcium response limited to single
spines in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 92: 8279–8282.
Desmond, J.E., and Fiez, J.A. (1998). Neuroimaging studies of the cerebellum: language, learn-
ing and memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2: 355–362.
Desmurget, M., Pélisson, D., Urquizar, C., Prablanc, C., Alexander, G.E., and Grafton, S.T.
(1998). Functional anatomy of saccadic adaptation in humans. Nat. Neurosci. 1: 524–528.
de’Sperati, C., Montarolo, P.G., and Strata, P. (1993). Effects of inferior olive inactivation and
lesion on the activity of medial vestibular neurons in the rat. Neuroscience 53: 139–147.
Diener, H.C., Dichgans, J., Guschlbauer, B., Bacher, M., and Langenbach, P. (1989). Distur-
bances of motor preparation in basal ganglia and cerebellar disorders. Progr. Brain Res.
880: 481–488.
Dietrichs, E., and Walberg, F. (1979). The cerebellar corticonuclear and nucleocortical pro-
jections in the cat as studied with anterograde and retrograde transport of horseradish
peroxide. Anat. Embryol. 158: 13–39.
Dietrichs, E., and Walberg, F. (1989). Direct bidirectional connections between the inferior
olive and the cerebellar nuclei. In P. Strata (ed.), The Olivocerebellar System in Motor
Control (Exp. Brain Res. Ser. 17), pp. 61–81.
Diño, M.R., Nunzi, M.G., Anelli, R., and Mugnaini, E. (2000). Unipolar brush cells of the
vestibulocerebellum: afferents and targets. Progr. Brain Res. 124: 123–137.
Donoghue, J.P., Hess, G., and Sanes, J.N. (1996). Substrates and mechanisms for learning in
motor cortex. In J.R. Bloedel, T.J. Ebner, and S.P. Wise (eds.), The Acquisition of Motor
Behavior in Vertebrates. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 363–386.
308 Bibliography
Dow, R.S. (1949). Action potentials of cerebellar cortex in response to local electrical stimu-
lation. J. Neurophysiol. 12: 245–256.
Dow, R.S., and Moruzzi, G. (1958). The Physiology and Pathology of the Cerebellum.
Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Doya, K. (1999). What are the computations of the cerebellum, the basal ganglia and the
cerebral cortex. Neural Netw. 12: 961–974.
Droulez, J., and Cornilleau-Pérès, V. (1993). Application of the coherence scheme to the
multisensory fusion problem. In: A. Berthoz (ed.), Multisensory Control of Movement.
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, pp. 485–501.
du Lac, S., Raymond, J.L., Seijnowski, T.J., and Lisberger, S.G. (1995). Learning and memory
in the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18: 409–441.
Dunin-Barkowski, W.L., and Larionova, N.P. (1985a). Computer simulation of a cerebel-
lar compartment. I. General principles and properties of a neural net. Biol. Cybern. 51,
399–406.
Dunin-Barkowski, W.L., and Larionova, N.P. (1985b). Computer simulation of a cerebellar
cortex compartment. II. An information learning and its recall in the Marr’s Memory Unit.
Biol. Cybern. 51: 407–415.
Dunn, M.E., Vetter, D.E., Berrebi, A.S., Krider, H.M., and Mugnaini, E. (1996). The mossy
fiber–granule cell–cartwheel cell system in the mammalian cochlear nuclear complex. In
W.A. Ainsworth, E.F. Evans, and C.M. Hackney (eds.), Advances in Speech, Hearing and
Language Processing, Vol. 3, Part A. London: JAI Press, pp. 63-87.
Ebner, T.J. (1998). A role for the cerebellum in the control of limb movement velocity. Curr.
Opin. Neurobiol. 8: 762–769.
Ebner, T.J., and Chen, G. (1995). Use of voltage-sensitive dyes and optical recordings in the
central nervous system. Progr. Neurobiol. 46: 463–506.
Ebner, T.J., Flament, D., and Shanbhag, S.J. (1996). In J.R. Bloedel, T.J. Ebner, and S.P. Wise
(eds.), The Acquisition of Motor Behavior in Vertebrates. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp.
235–260.
Eccles, J.C. (1966). Functional organization of the cerebellum in relation to its role in motor
control. In: R. Granit (ed.), Muscular Afferents and Motor Control (Proceedings of the First
Nobel Symposium, 1965). Stockholm: Almqvist and Wiksell; New York: John Wiley. pp.
19–36.
Eccles, J.C. (1967). Circuits in the cerebellar control of movement. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
58: 336–343.
Eccles, J.C. (1973). The cerebellum as a computer: patterns in space and time. J. Physiol. 229:
1–32.
Eccles, J.C. (1977a). An instruction-selection theory of learning in the cerebellar cortex. Brain
Res. 127: 327–352.
Eccles, J.C. (1977b). Cerebellar functions in the control of movement (with special reference
to the pioneer work of Sir Gordon Holmes). In F.C. Rose (ed.), Physiological Aspects of
Clinical Neurology. Oxford, England: Blackwell, pp. 157–178.
Eccles, J.C. (1982). The future of studies on the cerebellum. In: S.L. Palay and V. Chan-Palay
(eds.), The Cerebellum – New Vistas. New York: Springer, pp. 607–620.
Eccles, J.C., Ito, M., and Szentágothai, J. (1967). The Cerebellum as a Neuronal Machine. New
York: Springer-Verlag.
Eccles, J.C., Llinás, R., and Sasaki, K. (1966). The excitatory synapse action of climbing fibres
on the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 182: 268-296.
Eccles, J.C., Sabah, N.H., Schmidt, R.F., and Táborı́ková, H. (1972). Mode of operation of the
cerebellum in the dynamic loop control of movement. Brain Res. 40: 73–80.
Eisenman, L.M. (2000). Antero-posterior boundaries and compartments in the cerebellum:
evidence from selected neurological mutants. Progr. Brain Res. 124: 81–93.
Bibliography 309
Ekerot, C.-F. (1999). Climbing fibers – a key to cerebellar function. J. Physiol. 516.3: 629.
Ekerot, C.-F., and Oscarsson, O. (1981). Prolonged depolarization elicited in Purkinje cell
dendrites by climbing fiber impulses in the cat. J. Physiol. 318: 207–221.
Ekerot, C.-F., Garwicz, M., and Jörntell, H. (1997). The control of forelimb movements by
intermediate cerebellum. Progr. Brain Res. 114: 423–429.
Ekerot, C.-F., Larson, B., and Oscarsson, O. (1979). Information carried by the spinocerebellar
paths. Progr. Brain Res. 50: 79–90.
Elias, S.A., Yae, H., and Ebner, T.J. (1993). Optical imaging of parallel fiber activation in the
rat cerebellar cortex: spatial effects of excitatory amino acids. Neuroscience 52: 771–786.
Eskandar, E.N., and Assad, J.A. (1999). Dissociation of visual, motor and predictive signals in
parietal cortex during visual guidance. Nat. Neurosci. 2: 88–93.
Fahle, M., and Braitenberg, V. (1985). Some quantitative aspects of cerebellar anatomy as a
guide to speculation on cerebellar functions. In J.R. Bloedel, J. Dichgangs, and W. Precht
(eds.), Cerebellar Functions. Berlin: Springer, pp. 186–200.
Fetz, E.E. (1993). Dynamic neural network models of sensorimotor behavior. In: D. Gardner
(ed.), The Neurobiology of Neural Networks. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 164–190.
Fetz, E.E., and Shupe, L.E. (1990). Neural network models of the primate motor system. In:
R. Eckmiller (ed.), Advanced Neural Computers. Amsterdam: North Holland, pp. 43–50.
Fiala, J.C., Grossberg, S., and Bullock, D. (1996). Metabotropic glutamate receptor activation
in cerebellar Purkinje cells as substrate for adaptive timing of the classically conditioned
eye-blink response. J. Neurosci. 16: 3760–3774.
Fiez, J.A., Petersen, S.E., Cheney, M.K., and Raichle, M.E. (1992). Impaired non-motor learn-
ing and error detection associated with cerebellar damage (a single case study). Brain 115:
155–178.
Finger, T.E., Bell, C.C., and Russell, C.J. (1981). Electrosensory pathways to the valvula cere-
belli in mormyrid fish. Exp. Brain Res. 42: 23–33.
Flanagan, J.R., Nakano, E., Imamizu, H., Osu, R., Yoshioka, T., and Kawato, M. (1999). Com-
position and decomposition of internal models in motor learning under altered kinematic
and dynamic environments. J. Neurosci. 19: RC34.
Flourens, P. (1824, 1842). Recherches experimentales sur less propriétés nerveux dans les
animaux vertébrés. (Ed. 1), Paris; Crevot, 1824; ed. 2, Paris: Bailliere, 1842, pp. 4, 5, 12, 13,
14, 23.
Freeman, J.A. (1969). The cerebellum as a timing device: an experimental study in the frog. In
R. Llinás (ed.), Neurobiology of Cerebellar Evolution and Development. Chicago: American
Medical Association Educational and Research Foundation, pp. 397–413.
Freeman, J.A. (1970). Space-time transformation in the frog cerebellum through an intrinsic
tapped delay line. Nature (Lond.) 226: 640–642.
Freeman, W.J. (1983). Dynamics of information formation by nerve cell assemblies. In
E. Basar, H. Flohr, H. Haken, and A.J. Mandell (eds.), Synergetics of the Brain. New York:
Springer-Verlag, pp. 102–121.
Frens, M.A., Mathoera, A.L., and Van der Steen, J. (2000). On the nature of gain changes of
the optokinetic reflex. Progr. Brain Res. 124: 247–255.
Friston, K.J., Frith, C.D., Passingham, R.E., Liddle, P.F. and Frackowiak, R.S.J. (1992). Motor
practice and neurophysiological adaptation in the cerebellum: a positron tomography study.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 248: 223–228.
Fu, Q-G. Flament, D., Coltz, J.D., and Ebner, T.J. (1997). Relationship of cerebellar Purkinje
cell simple spike discharge to movement kinematics in the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 78:
478–491.
Fu, Q-G., Mason, C.R., Flament, D., Coltz, J.D., and Ebner, T.J. (1997). Movement
kinematics encoded in complex discharge of primate cerebellar Purkinje cells. Neuroreport
8: 523–529.
310 Bibliography
Fuchs, A.F., Mustari, M.J., Robinson, F.R., and Kaneko, C.R.S. (1992). Visual signals in the
nucleus of the optic tract and their brain stem destinations. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 656:
266–276.
Fuchs, A.F., Robinson, F.R., and Straube, A. (1994). Participation of the caudal fastigial nucleus
in smooth-pursuit eye movements. I. Neuronal activity. J. Neurophysiol. 72: 2714–2728.
Fuhrman, Y., Piat, G., Thomson, M.A., Mariani, J., Delhaye-Bouchaud, N. (1995). Abnormal
ipsilateral functional vibrissae projection onto Purkinje cells multiply innervated by climbing
fibers in the rat. Devel. Brain Res. 87: 172–178.
Fujita, M. (1982a). Adaptive filter model of the cerebellum. Biol. Cybern. 45: 195–206.
Fujita, M. (1982b). Simulation of adaptive modification of the vestibulo-ocular reflex with an
adaptive filter model of the cerebellum. Biol. Cybern. 45: 207–214.
Funabiki, K., Mishina, M., and Hirano, T. (1995). Retarded vestibular compensation in mutant
mice deficient in δ2 glutamate receptor subunit. Neuroreport 7: 189–192.
Gabor, D. (1954). Communication theory and cybernetics. IRE Trans. Circuit Theory CT-1(4):
19–31.
Gabor, D., Wilby, W.P.L., and Woodcock, R. (1961). A universal non-linear filter, predictor
and simulator which optimizes itself by a learning process. Proc. I.E.E. (London) B. 108:
422–438.
Gao, J.-H., Parsons, L.M., Bower, J.M., Xiong. J-h, Li, J-Q. and Fox, P.T. (1996). Cerebellum
implicated in sensory acquisition and discrimination rather than motor control. Science 272:
545–547.
Garwicz, M. (2000). Micro-organisation of cerebellar modules controlling forelimb move-
ments. Progr. Brain Res. 124: 187–199.
Garwicz, M., and Andersson, G. (1992). Spread of synaptic activity along parallel fibres in cat
cerebellar anterior lobe. Exp. Brain Res. 88: 615–622.
Gellman, R., Gibson, A.R., and Houk, J.C. (1985). Inferior olivary neurons in the awake cat:
detection of contact and passive body displacement. J. Neurophysiol. 54: 40–60.
Gell-Mann, M. (1995). Complex adaptive systems. In H. Morowitz and J.L. Singer (eds.),
The Mind, The Brain, and Complex Adaptive Systems. Reading, MA.: Addison-Wesley, pp.
11–23.
Ghez, C. (1991). The cerebellum. In E.R. Kandel, J.H. Schwartz, and T.M. Jessell (Eds.),
Principles of Neural Science (3rd ed.). New York: Elsevier, pp. 627–646.
Ghez, C., Gordon, J., Ghilardi, M.F., Cristakos, C.N., and Cooper, S.E. (1990). Role of pro-
prioceptive input in the programming of arm trajectories. Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant.
Biol. 55: 837–847.
Ghez, C., and Thach, W.T. (2000). The cerebellum. In: E.R. Kandel, J.H. Schwartz, and T.M.
Jessell (eds.), Principals of Neural Science (4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 832–852.
Gibson, J.E. (1963). Nonlinear Automatic Control. New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 491–547.
Gilbert, P.F.C. (1974). A theory of memory that explains the function and structure of the
cerebellum. Brain Res. 709: 1–18.
Gilbert, P.F.C., and Thach, W.T. (1977). Purkinje cell activity during motor learning. Brain Res.
128: 309–328.
Gilbert, P.F.C., and Yeo, C.H. (1992). Cerebellar function: on-line control and learning. Behav.
Brain Sci. 15: 743–744.
Gilman, S., Bloedel, J.R., and Lechtenberg, R. (1981). Disorders of the Cerebellum. Philadel-
phia: F.A. Davis.
Glickstein, M. (1993). Motor skills but not cognitive tasks. Trends Neurosci. 16: 450–451.
Glickstein, M. (1997). Mossy-fibre sensory input to the cerebellum. Progr. Brain Res. 114:
251–259.
Gluck, M.A., Reifsnider, E.S., and Thompson, R.F. (1990). Adaptive signal processing and
the Cb: models of classical conditioning and VOR adaptation. In M.A. Gluck and D.E.
Bibliography 311
Rumelhart (eds.), Neuroscience and Connectionist Theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, pp. 131–185.
Goldberg, M.E., Musil, S.Y., Fitzgibbon, E.J., Smith, M., and Olson, C.R. (1993). The role
of the cerebellum in the control of saccadic eye movements. In N. Mano, I. Hamada, and
M.R. DeLong (eds.), Role of the Cerebellum and Basal Ganglia in Voluntary Movement.
Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers, pp. 203–211.
Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1995). Neurobiology of mental representation. In H. Morowitz and
J.L. Singer (eds.), The Mind, The Brain, and Complex Adaptive Systems. Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley, pp. 51–63.
Goldowitz, D., and Hamre, K. (1998). The cells and molecules that make a cerebellum. Trends
Neurosci. 21: 375–382.
Gomi, H., and Kawato, M. (1992). Adaptive feedback control models of the vestibulocerebel-
lum and spinocerebellum. Biol. Cybern. 68: 105–114.
Gomi, H., Shidara, M., Takemura, A., Inoue, Y., Kawano, K., and Kawato, M. (1998). Temporal
firing patterns of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar ventral paraflocculus during ocular following
responses in monkeys I. Simple spikes. J. Neurophysiol. 80: 818–831.
Gould, B.B. (1979). The organization of afferents to the cerebellar cortex in the cat: projections
from the deep cerebellar nuclei. J. Comp. Neurol. 184: 27–42.
Gould, B.B., and Graybiel, A.M. (1976). Afferents to the cerebellar cortex in the cat: evi-
dence for an intrinsic pathway leading from the deep nuclei to the cortex. Brain Res. 110:
601–611.
Graf, W., Simpson, J.I., and Leonard, C.S. (1989). A synthesis of input–output relationships of
the rabbit flocculus. In: P. Strata (ed.), The Olivocerebellar System in Motor Control. New
York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 338–344.
Gravel, C., Leclerc, N., Rafrafi, J., Sasseville, R., Thivierge, L., and Hawkes, R. (1987). Mono-
clonal antibodies reveal the global organization of the cerebellar cortex. J. Neurosci. Methods
2l: 145–157.
Gray, C., Perciavalle, V., and Poppele, R. (1993). Sensory responses to passive hindlimb joint
rotation in the cerebellar cortex of the cat. Brain Res. 622: 280–284.
Graybiel, A.M., Nauta, H.J.W., Lasek, R.J., and Nauta, W.J.H. (1973). A cerebello-olivary
pathway in the cat: an experimental study using autoradiographic tracing techniques. Brain
Res. 58: 205–211.
Green, J.T., and Woodruff-Pak, D.S. (2000). Eyeblink classical conditioning: hippocampal
formation is for neutral stimulus associations as cerebellum is for association-response.
Psychol. Bull. 126: 138–158.
Grinvald, A., Frostig, R.D., Lieke, E., and Hildesheim, R. (1988). Optical imaging of neuronal
activity. Physiol. Rev. 68: 1285–1366.
Groenewegen, H.J., and Voogd, J. (1977). The parasagittal zonation within the olivocerebellar
projection. I. Climbing fiber distribution in the vermis of cat cerebellum. J. Comp. Neurol.
174: 417–488.
Groenewegen, H.J., Voogd, J., and Freedman, S.L. (1979). The parasagittal zonation within
the olivo-cerebellar projection. II. Climbing fiber distribution in the intermediate and hemi-
spheric parts of cat cerebellum. J. Comp. Neurol. 183: 551–602.
Grossberg, S. (1969). On learning of spatiotemporal patterns by networks with ordered sensory
and motor components 1. Excitatory components of the cerebellum. Stud. Appl. Math 48:
123–132.
Gupta, M.M., and Rao, D.H. (1994). Neuro-Control Systems: Theory and Applications. New
York: The Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers, pp. 1–43.
Haines, D.E. (1989). HRP study of cerebellar corticonuclear-nucleocortical topography of the
dorsal culminate lobule – lobule V – in a prosimian primate (Galago): with comments on
nucleocortical cell types. J. Comp. Neurol. 282: 274–292.
312 Bibliography
Hémart, N., Daniel, H., Jaillard, D., and Crépel, F. (1994). Properties of glutamate receptors
are modified during long-term depression in rat cerebellar Purkinje cells. Neurosci. Res. 19:
213–221.
Hémart, N., Daniel, H., Jaillard, D., and Crépel, F. (1995). Receptors and second messen-
gers involved in long-term depression in rat cerebellar slices in vitro: a reappraisal. Eur. J.
Neurosci. 7: 45–53.
Herndon, R.H. (1963). The fine structure of the Purkinje cell. J. Cell. Biol. 18: 167–180.
Herrick, C.J. (1924a). Neurological Foundations of Animal Behavior. New York: Henry Holt.
Herrick, C.J. (1924b). Origin and evolution of the cerebellum. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 11:
621–652.
Hirano, T. (1990a). Depression and potentiation of the synaptic transmission between a granule
cell and a Purkinje cell in rat cerebellar tissue. Neurosci. Lett. 119: 141–144.
Hirano, T. (1990b). Effects of postsynaptic depolarization in the induction of synaptic depres-
sion between a granule cell and a Purkinje cell in rat cerebellar culture. Neurosci. Lett. 119:
145–147.
Holdefer, R.N., Miller, L.E., Chen, L.L., and Houk, J.C. (2000). Functional connectivity be-
tween cerebellum and primary motor cortex in the awake monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 84:
585–590.
Holland, J.H. (1995). Can there be a unified theory of complex adaptive systems? In: H.
Morowitz and J.L. Singer (eds.), The Mind, The Brain, and Complex Adaptive Systems.
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley, pp. 45–50.
Holmes, G. (1939). The cerebellum of man. Brain 62: 1–30.
Hopfield, J.J. (1982). Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective compu-
tational abilities. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 79: 2554–2558.
Hore, J., Wild, B., and Diener, H.-C. (1991). Cerebellar dysmetria at the elbow, wrist, and
fingers. J. Neurophysiol. 65: 563–571.
Horn, K.M., van Kan, P.L.E., and Ruigrok, T.J.H. (1996). Inferior olive sensitivity is reduced
by increased cerebellar output. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 22: 1092.
Horne, M.K., and Butler, E.G. (1995). The role of the cerebello-thalamo-cortical pathway in
skilled movement. Progr. Neurobiol. 46: 199–213.
Houk, J.C. (1987). Model of the cerebellum as an array of adjustable pattern generators. In:
M. Glickstein, C. Yeo, and J. Stein (eds.), Cerebellum and Neuronal Plasticity. New York:
Plenum, pp. 249–260.
Houk, J.C., and Wise, S.P. (1995). Distributed modular architectures linking basal ganglia,
cerebellum, and cerebral cortex: their role in planning and controlling action. Cereb. Cortex
5: 95–110.
Houk, J.C., Buckingham, J.T., and Barto, A.G. (1996). Models of the cerebellum and motor
learning. Behav. Brain Sci. 19: 368–383.
Houk, J.C., Keifer, J., and Barto, A.G. (1993). Distributed motor commands in the limb pre-
motor network. Trends Neurosci. 16: 27–33.
Houk, J.C., Singh, S.P., Fisher, C., and Barto, A.G. (1990). An adaptive sensorimotor network
inspired by the anatomy and physiology of the cerebellum. In: W.T. Miller, R.S. Sutton, and
P.J. Werbos (eds.), Neural Networks for Control. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 301–348.
Imamizu, H., Miyauchi, S., Tamada, T., Sasaki, Y., Takino, R., Pütz, B., Yoshioka, T., and
Kawato, M. (2000). Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a
new tool. Nature 403: 192–195.
Ioannou, P.A., and Sun, J. (1996). Robust Adaptive Control. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-
Hall, p. 27.
Isermann, R., Lachmann, K.-H., and Matko, D. (1992). Adaptive Control Systems. New York:
Prentice Hall.
Ito, M. (1970). Neurophysiological aspects of the cerebellar motor control system. Int. J.
Neurol. (Montevideo) 7: 162–176.
314 Bibliography
Ito, M. (1972). Neural design of the cerebellar motor control system. Brain Res. 40: 81–84.
Ito, M. (1976a). Adaptive control of reflexes by the cerebellum. Progr. Brain Res. 44: 436–444.
Ito, M. (1976b). Cerebellar learning control of vestibulo-ocular mechanisms. In T. Desiraju
(Ed.), Mechanisms in Transmission of Signals for Conscious Behavior. Amsterdam: Elsevier,
pp. 1–22.
Ito, M. (1979). Is the cerebellum really a computer? Trends Neurosci. 2: 122–126.
Ito, M. (1982a). Cerebellar control of the vestibulo-ocular reflex – around the flocculus hy-
pothesis. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 5: 275–296.
Ito, M. (1982b). Questions in modeling the cerebellum. J. Theoret. Biol. 99: 81–86.
Ito, M. (1984). The Cerebellum and Neural Control. New York: Raven.
Ito, M. (1989). Long-term depression. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 12: 85–102.
Ito, M. (1990). A new physiological concept on cerebellum. Rev. Neurol. (Paris) 146: 564–569.
Ito, M. (1993a). Cerebellar flocculus hypothesis (letter). Nature 363: 24–25.
Ito, M. (1993b). Neurophysiology of the nodulofloccular system. Rev. Neurol. (Paris) 149:
692–697.
Ito, M. (1993c). Movement and thought: identical control mechanisms by the cerebellum.
Trends Neurosci. 16:448–450.
Ito, M. (1993d). Synaptic plasticity in the cerebellar cortex and its role in motor learning. Can.
J. Neurol. Sci. 20 (suppl. 3): S70–S74.
Ito, M. (1996). Letter to the editor. Trends Neurosci. 19: 11–12.
Ito, M. (1998). Cerebellar learning in the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2: 313–321.
Ito, M. (1999). The cerebellum, a gateway to modern neuroscience. Brain Res. Bull. 30: 33.
Ito, M. (2000). Neurobiology: internal model visualized. Nature 403: 153–154.
Ito, M., and Kano, M. (1982). Long-lasting depression of parallel fiber–Purkinje cell transmis-
sion induced by conjunctive stimulation of parallel fibers and climbing fibers in the cerebellar
cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 33: 253–258.
Ito, M., Sakurai, M, and Tongroach, P. (1982). Climbing fibre induced depression of both
mossy fibre responsiveness and glutamate sensitivity of cerebellar Purkinje cells. J. Physiol.
(Lond.) 324: 113–134.
Ivry, R. (1993). Cerebellar involvement in the explicit representation of temporal information.
Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 682: 214–230.
Ivry, R.B. (1996). The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control.
Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6: 851–857.
Ivry, R.B., and Diener, H.C. (1991). Impaired velocity perception in patients with lesions of
the cerebellum. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 3: 355–366.
Ivry, R.B., and Fiez, J.A. (2000). Cerebellar contributions to cognition and imagery. In:
M.S. Gazzaniga (ed.), The New Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp.
999–1011.
Ivry, R.B., and Keele, S.W. (1989). Timing functions of the cerebellum. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1:
136–152.
Jeannerod, M., Arbib, M.A., Rizzolatti, G., and Sakata, H. (1995). Grasping objects: the cortical
mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends Neurosci. 18: 314–320.
Jenkins, I.H., Brooks, D.J., Nixon, P.D., Frackowiak, R.S.J., and Passingham, R.E. (1994).
Motor sequence learning: a study with positron emission tomography. J. Neurosci. 14: 3775–
3790.
Ji Z., Jin Q., and Vogel, M.W. (1997). Evidence of spinocerebellar mossy fiber segregation in
the juvenile staggerer cerebellum. J. Comp. Neurol. 378: 354–362.
Jones, A., Paterlini, M., Wisden, W., and Merlo, D. (2000). Transgenic methods for directing
gene expression to specific neuronal types: cerebellar granule cells. Progr. Brain Res. 124:
69–80.
Jordan, M.I., and Wolpert, D.M. (2000). Computational motor control. In M.S. Gazzaniga
(ed.), The New Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 601–618.
Bibliography 315
Jueptner, M., and Weiller, C. (1998). A review of differences between basal ganglia and cere-
bellar control of movements as revealed by functional imaging studies. Brain 121:1437–1449.
Jueptner, M., Ottinger, S., Fellows, S.J., Adamschewski, J., Flerich, L., Müller, S.P., Diener, H.C.,
Thilmann, A.F., and Weiller, C. (1997). The relevance of sensory input for the cerebellar
control of movements. Neuroimage 5: 41–48.
Jueptner, M.R., Rijntjes M., Weiller, C., Faiss, J.H., Timmann, D., Mueller, S.P., and Diener,
H.C. (1995). Localization of a cerebellar timing process using PET. Neurology 45: 1540–1545.
Kaiserman-Abramof, I.R., and Palay, S.L. (1969). Fine structural studies of the cerebellar
cortex in a mormyrid fish. In: R. Llinás (ed.), Neurobiology of Cerebellar Evolution and De-
velopment. Chicago: American Medical Association Educational and Research Foundation,
pp. 171–205.
Kalil, K. (1979). Projections of the cerebellar and dorsal column nuclei upon the inferior olive
of the rhesus monkey: an autoradiographic study. J. Comp. Neurol. 188: 43–62.
Kalman, R.E. (1960). A new approach to linear filtering and prediction problems. J. Basic
Eng. (ASME Trans. Ser. D) 82: 35–45.
Kalman, R.E., and Bucy, R.S. (1961). New results in linear filtering and prediction theory.
J. Basic Eng. (ASME Trans. Ser. D) 83: 95–108.
Kandel, E.R., Schwartz, J.H., and Jessell, T.M. (1991). Principles of Neural Science (3rd ed.).
New York: Elsevier.
Kano, M. (1996). Long-lasting potentiation of GABAergic inhibitory synaptic transmission
in cerebellar Purkinje cells: its properties and possible mechanisms. Behav. Brain Sci. 19:
354–361.
Kano, M., Hashimoto, K., Chen, C, Abeliovich, A., Aiba, A., Kurihara, H., Watanabe, M.,
Inoue, Y., and Tonegawa, S. (1995). Impaired synapse elimination during cerebellar devel-
opment in PKCgamma mutant mice. Cell 83: 1223–1231.
Kano, M., Hashimoto, K., Kurihara, H., Watanabe, M., Inoue, Y., Aiba, A., and Tonegawa,
S. (1997). Persistent multiple climbing fiber innervation of cerebellar Purkinje cells in mice
lacking mGluRl. Neuron 18: 71–79.
Kano, M., Rexhausen, U., Dreessen, J, and Konnerth, A. (1992). Synaptic excitation produces
a long-lasting rebound potentiation of inhibitory synaptic signals in cerebellar cells. Nature
356: 601–604.
Kashiwabuchi, N., Ikeda, K., Araki, K., Hirano, T., Shibuki, K., Takayama, C., Inoue, Y.,
Kutsuwada, T., Yasgi, T., Kang, Y., Aizawa, S., and Mishina, M. (1995). Impairment of
motor coordination, Purkinje cell synapse formation, and long-term-depression in GluR∂2
mutant mice. Cell 81: 245–252.
Katz, D.B., and Steinmetz, J.E. (1997). Single-unit evidence for eye-blink conditioning in
cerebellar cortex is altered, but not eliminated, by interpositus nucleus lesions. Learn. Mem.
3: 88–104.
Kaufman, G.D., Mustarl, M.J., Miselis, R.R., and Perachio, A.A. (1996). Transneuronal path-
ways to the vestibulocerebellum. J. Comp. Neurol. 370: 501–523.
Kawano, K., and Shidara, M. (1993). The role of the ventral paraflocculus in ocular following
in the monkey. In: N. Mano, I. Hamada, and M.R. DeLong (eds.), Role of the Cerebellum
and Basal Ganglia in Voluntary Movement. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 195–202.
Kawano, K., Shidara, M., Takemura, A., Inoue, Y., Gomi, H., and Kawato, M. (1996). Inverse-
dynamics representation of eye movements by cerebellar Purkinje cell activity during short-
latency ocular-following responses. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 781: 314–319.
Kawano, K., Takemura, A., Inoue, Y., Kitama, T, Kobayashi, Y., and Mustari, M.J. (1996).
Visual inputs to cerebellar ventral paraflocculus during ocular following responses. Progr.
Brain Res. 112: 415–422.
Kawato, M. (1990a). Computational schemes and neural network models for formation and
control of multijoint arm trajectory. In W.T. Miller III, R.S. Sutton, and P.J. Werbos (eds.),
Neural Networks for Control. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 197–228.
316 Bibliography
Kim, J.J., and Thompson, R.F. (1997). Cerebellar circuits and synaptic mechanisms involved
in classical eyeblink conditioning. Trends. Neurosci. 20: 177–181.
Kim, S.-G., Ugurbil, K., and Strick, P.L. (1994). Activation of a cerebellar output nucleus
during cognitive processing. Science 265: 949–951.
King, J.S. (1980). Synaptic organization of the inferior olivary complex. In J. Courville, C. de
Montigny and Y. Lamarre (eds.), The Inferior Olivary Nucleus: Anatomy and Physiology.
New York: Raven Press, pp. 1–32.
Kistler, W.M., and van Hemmen, J.L. (2000). Modeling synaptic plasticity in conjunction with
the timing of pre- and postsynaptic action potentials. Neural Comput. 12: 385–405.
Kitazawa, S., Kimura, T., and Yin, P-B. (1998). Cerebellar complex spikes encode both desti-
nations and error in arm movements. Nature 392: 494–497.
Kitzman, P.H., and Bishop, G.A. (1997). The physiological effects of serotonin on spontaneous
and amino acid-induced activation of cerebellar nuclear cells: an in vivo study in the cat.
Progr. Brain Res. 114: 209–223.
Kobayashi, Y., Kawano, K., Takemura, A., Inoue, Y., Kitama, T., Gomi, H., and Kawato,
M. (1998). Temporal firing patterns of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar ventral parafloccu-
lus during ocular following responses in monkeys II. Complex spikes. J. Neurophysiol. 80:
832–848.
Kolb, F.P., Irwin, K.B., Bloedel, J.R., and Bracha, V. (1997). Conditioned and unconditioned
forelimb reflex systems in the cat: involvement of the intermediate cerebellum. Exp. Brain
Res. 114: 255–270.
Kolston, J., Apps, R., and Trott, J.R. (1995). A combined retrograde tracer and GABA-
immunocytochemical study of the projection from nucleus interpositus posterior to the
posterior lobe c2 zone of the cat cerebellum. Eur. J. Neurosci. 7: 926–933.
Konnerth, A., Dreessen, J., and Augustine, G.J. (1992). Brief dendritic calcium signals initiate
long-lasting synaptic depression in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 89:
7051–7055.
Kornhuber, H.H. (1971). Motor functions of cerebellum and basal ganglia: the cerebellocor-
tical saccadic (ballistic) clock, the cerebellonuclear hold regulator, and the basal ganglia
ramp (voluntary speed smooth movement) generator. Biol. Cybern. 8: 157–162.
Kornhuber, H.H. (1974). Cerebral cortex, cerebellum, and basal ganglia: an introduction to
their motor functions. In F.O. Schmitt and F.G. Worden (eds.), The Neurosciences: Third
Study Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 267–280.
Krupa, D.J., and Thompson, R.F. (1997). Reversible inactivation of the cerebellar interpositus
nucleus completely prevents acquisition of the classically conditioned eye-blink response.
Learn. Mem. 3: 545–556.
Kurihara, H., Hashimoto, K., Kano, M., Takayama, C., Sakimura, K., Mishina, M., Inoue,
Y., and Watanabe, M. (1997). Impaired parallel fiber–Purkinje cell synapse stabilization
during cerebellar development of mutant mice lacking the glutamate receptor ∂2 subunit.
J. Neurosci. 17: 9613–9623.
Lalonde, R. (1994). Cerebellar contributions to instrumental learning. Neurosci. Biobehav.
Rev. 18: 161–170.
Lalonde, R., and Botez-Marquard, T. (1997). The neurobiological basis of movement initiation.
Rev. Neurosci. 8: 35–54.
Lalonde, R., and Hannequin, D. (1999). The neurobiological basis of time estimation and
temporal order. Rev. Neurosci. 10: 151–173.
Landau, I.D., Lozano, R., and M’Saad, M. (1998). Adaptive Control. Berlin: Springer.
Lang, E.J., Sugihara, I., and Llinás, R. (1996). GABAergic modulation of complex spike activity
by the cerebellar nucleoolivary pathway in rat. J. Neurophysiol. 76: 255–275.
Lansner, A., and Ekeberg, Ö. (1994). Neuronal network models of motor generation and
control. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 4: 903–908.
318 Bibliography
Larsell, O. (1967). The Comparative Anatomy and Histology of the Cerebellum (2 vols.) Min-
neapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Larsell, O., and Jansen, J. (1972). The Comparative Anatomy and Histology of the Cerebel-
lum: The Human Cerebellum, Cerebellar Connections, and Cerebellar Cortex. Minneapolis:
University of Minnesota Press.
Lasser-Ross, N., and Ross, W.N. (1992). Imaging voltage and synaptically activated sodium
transients in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 247: 35–39.
Lee, M., and Bower, J.M. (1990). A computer modeling approach to understanding the inferior
olive and its relationship to the cerebellar cortex in rats. In: D.S. Touretsky (ed.), Advances
in Neural Information Processing Systems 2. San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann Publishers,
pp. 117–124.
Le Marec, N., and Lalonde, R. (2000). Treadmill performance of mice with cerebellar lesions
2: lurcher mutants. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 75: 195–206.
Legendre, A., and Courville, J. (1987). Origin and trajectory of the cerebello-olivary projection:
an experimental study with radioactive and fluorescent tracers in the cat. Neuroscience 21:
877–891.
Leiner, H.C., and Leiner, A.L. (1997). How fibers subserve computing capabilities: similarities
between brains and machines. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 41: 535–553.
Leiner, H.C., Leiner, A.L. and Dow, R.S. (1986). Does the cerebellum contribute to mental
skills? Behav. Neurosci. 100: 443–454.
Leiner, H.C., Leiner, A.L., and Dow, R.S. (1987). Cerebro-cerebellar learning loops in apes
and humans. Ital. J. Neurol. Sci. 8: 425–436.
Leiner, H. C., Leiner, A.L., and Dow, R.S. (1991). The human cerebro-cerebellar system: its
computing, cognitive, and language skills. Behav. Brain Res. 44: 113–128.
Leiner, H.C., Leiner, A.L., and Dow, R.S. (1993a). Cognitive and language functions of the
human cerebrum. Trends Neurosci. 16: 444–447.
Leiner, H.C., Leiner, A.L., and Dow, R.S. (1993b). The role of the cerebellum in the human
brain (reply). Trends Neurosci. 16: 453–454.
Lev-Ram, Jiang, T., Wood, J., Lawrence, D.S., and Tsien, R.Y. (1997). Synergies and coinci-
dence requirements between NO, cGMP, and CA2+ in the induction of cerebellar long-term
depression. Neuron 18: 1025–1038.
Lev-Ram, V., Nebyelul, Z., Ellisman, M.H., Huang, P.L., and Tsien, R.Y. (1997). Absence of
cerebellar long-term depression in mice lacking neuronal nitric oxide synthase. Learn. Mem.
3: 169–177.
Linden, D.J. (1996a). A protein synthesis-dependent late phase of cerebellar long-term de-
pression. Neuron 17: 483–490.
Linden, D.J. (1996b). Cerebellar long-term depression as investigated in a cell culture prepa-
ration. Behav. Brain Sci. 19: 339–346.
Linden, D.J. (1998). Synaptically evoked glutamate transport currents may be used to detect
the expression of long-term potentiation in cerebellar culture. J. Neurophysiol. 79: 3151–
3156.
Linden, D.J., and Connor, J.A. (1993). Cellular mechanisms of long-term depression in the
cerebellum. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 3: 401–406.
Linden, D.J., and Connor, J.A. (1995). Long-term synaptic depression. Annu. Rev. Neurosci.
18: 319–357.
Linden, D.J., Dawson, T.M., and Dawson, V.L. (1995). An evaluation of the nitric oxide/
cGMP/cGMP-dependent protein kinase cascade in the induction of cerebellar long-term
depression in culture. J. Neurosci. 15: 5098–5105.
Linden, D.J., Smeyne, M., and Connor, J.A. (1993). Induction of cerebellar long-term depres-
sion in culture requires postsynaptic action of sodium ions. Neuron 11: 1093–1100.
Lisberger, S.G. (1994). Neural basis for motor learning in the vestibulo-ocular reflex of
Bibliography 319
primates. III. Computational and behavioral analysis of the sites of learning. J. Neurophysiol.
72: 974–998.
Lisberger, S.G. (1996). Motor learning and memory in the vestibulo-ocular reflex: the dark
side. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 781: 525–531.
Lisberger, S.G. (1995). A mechanism of learning found? Curr. Biol. 5: 221–224.
Lisberger, S.G., and Fuchs, A.F. (1978a). Role of primate flocculus during rapid behavioral
modification of vestibuloocular reflex. I. Purkinje cell activity during visually guided horizon-
tal smooth-pursuit eye movements and passive head rotation. J. Neurophysiol. 41: 733–763.
Lisberger, S.G., and Fuchs, A.F. (1978b). Role of primate flocculus during rapid behavioral
modification of vestibuloocular reflex. II. Mossy fiber firing patterns during horizontal head
rotation and eye movement. J. Neurophysiol. 41: 764–777.
Lisberger, S.G., and Seijnowski, T.J. (1992). Motor learning in a recurrent network model
based on the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Nature 360: 159–161.
Lisberger, S.G., and Seijnowski, T.J. (1993). Cerebellar flocculus hypothesis (letter). Nature
363: 24–25.
Lisberger, S.G., Pavelko, T.A., and Broussard, D.M. (1994). Neural basis for motor learning
in the vestibuloocular reflex of primates. I. Changes in the responses of brain stem neurons.
J. Neurophysiol. 72: 928–953.
Lisberger, S.G., Pavelko, T.A., Bronte-Stewart, H.M., and Stone, L.S. (1994). Neural basis for
motor learning in the vestibulo-ocular reflex of primates. II. Changes in the responses of
horizontal gaze velocity Purkinje cells in the cerebellar flocculus and ventral paraflocculus.
J. Neurophysiol. 72: 954–973.
Llinás, R. (1969). (ed.). Neurobiology of Cerebellar Evolution and Development. Chicago:
American Medical Association Educational and Research Foundation.
Llinás, R. (1981a). Cerebellar modelling. Nature 291: 279–280.
Llinás, R. (1981b). Electrophysiology of the cerebellar networks. In V.B. Brooks (ed.), Hand-
book of Physiology, Vol. II, Motor Control, Part 2. Bethesda: American Physiological Soci-
ety, pp. 831–876.
Llinás, R. (1982). Discussion of Tolbert, D.L. (1982). The cerebellar nucleocortical pathway. In
S.L. Palay and V. Chan-Palay (eds.), The Cerebellum, New Vistas. Berlin: Springer-Verlag,
pp. 296–319.
Llinás, R. (1995). Thorny issues in neurons. Nature 373: 107–108.
Llinás, R., and Hillman, D.E. (1969). Physiological and morphological organization of the
cerebellar circuits in various vertebrates. In R. Llinás (ed.), Neurobiology of Cerebellar
Evolution and Development. Chicago: American Medical Association Educational and Re-
search Foundation, pp. 43–73.
Llinás, R., and Mühlethaler, M. (1988). Electrophysiology of guinea-pig cerebellar nuclear
cells in the in vitro brain stem-cerebellar preparation. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 404: 241–258.
Llinás, R., and Sugimori, M. (1980a). Electrophysiological properties of in vitro Purkinje cell
somata in mammalian cerebellar slices. J. Physiol. 305: 171–195.
Llinás, R., and Sugimori, M. (1980b). Electrophysiological properties of in vitro Purkinje cell
dendrites in mammalian cerebellar slices. J. Physiol. 305: 197–213.
Llinás, R., and Sugimori, M. (1992). The electrophysiology of the cerebellar Purkinje cell
revisited. In R. Llinás and C. Sotelo (eds.), The Cerebellum Revisited. New York: Springer,
pp. 167–181.
Llinás, R., Baker, R., and Sotelo, C. (1974). Electrotonic coupling between neurons in cat
inferior olive. J. Neurophysiol. 37: 560–571.
Llinás, R., Lang, E.J., and Welsh, J.P. (1997). The cerebellum, LTD, and memory: alternative
views. Learn. Mem. 3: 445–455.
Lockery, S.R. (1992). Realistic neural network models using backpropagation: panacea or
oxymoron? Semin. Neurosci. 4: 47–59.
320 Bibliography
Lorente de Nó, R. (1981). The Primary Acoustic Nuclei. New York: Raven Press, p. 24.
Luebke, A.E., and Robinson, D.A. (1992). Climbing fiber intervention blocks plasticity of the
vestibulo-ocular reflex. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 656 (B. Cohen, D.L. Tomko, and F. Guedry
[eds.]) Sensing and Controlling Motion – Vestibular and Sensorimotor Function, pp. 428–430.
Luebke, A.E., and Robinson, D.A. (1994). Gain changes of the cat’s vestibulo-ocular reflex
after flocculus deactivation. Exp. Brain Res. 98: 379–390.
Lukashin, A.V., Amirikian, B.B., Mozhaev, V.L., and Georgopoulos, A.P. (1995). Neural net-
work modeling of motor cortical operations during mental rotation and memory scanning
tasks. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 21: 2078.
Macchi, G., and Jones, E.G. (1997). Toward an agreement on terminology of nuclear and
subnuclear divisions of the motor thalamus. J. Neurosurg. 86: 670–685.
Maekawa, K., and Simpson, J.I. (1973). Climbing fiber responses evoked in vestibulocerebel-
lum of rabbit from visual system. J. Neurophysiol. 36: 649–666.
Maekawa, K., and Takeda, T. (1975). Mossy fiber responses evoked in the cerebellar flocculus
of rabbits by stimulation of the optic pathway. Brain Res. 98: 590–595.
Makhoul, J. (1975). Linear prediction: a tutorial review. Proc. IEEE 63: 561–580.
Maler, L., and Mugnaini, E. (1993). Organization and function of feedback to the electrosen-
sory lateral line lobe of gymnotiform fish, with emphasis on a searchlight mechanism.
J. Comp. Physiol. A. 173: 667–670.
Maler, L., and Mugnaini, E. (1994). Correlating gamma-aminobutyric acidergic circuits and
sensory function in the electrosensory lateral line lobe of a gymnotiform fish. J. Comp.
Neurol. 345: 224–252.
Mann-Metzer, P., and Yarom, Y. (2000). Electrotonic coupling synchronized interneuron ac-
tivity in the cerebellar cortex.Progr. Brain Res. 124: 107–114.
Mano, N., Kanazawa, I, and Yamamoto, K. (1986). Complex-spike activity of cerebellar P-cells
related to wrist tracking movement in monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 56: 137–158.
Mano, N., Kanazawa, I., and Yamamoto, K. (1989). Voluntary movements and complex-spike
discharges of cerebellar Purkinje cells. In P. Strata (ed.), The Olivocerebellar System in
Motor Control. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, pp. 265–280.
Markram, H., Lübke, J., Frotscher, M., and Sakmann, B. (1997). Regulation of synaptic efficacy
by coincidence of postsynaptic APs and EPSPs. Science 275: 213–215.
Marr, D. (1969). A theory of cerebellar cortex. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 202: 437–470.
Marr, D. (1970). A theory for cerebral neocortex. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B. 176: 161–234.
Marsden, C.D., Merton, P.A., Morton, H.B., Hallett, M., Adam, J., and Rushton, D.N. (1977).
Disorders of movement in cerebellar disease in man. In: F.C. Rose (ed.), Physiological
Aspects of Clinical Neurology. Oxford, England: Blackwell, pp. 179–199.
Martin, J.H., Cooper, S.C., Hacking, A., and Ghez, C. (2000). Differential effects of deep
cerebellar nuclei inactivation on reaching and adaptive control. J. Neurophysiol. 83:
1886–1899.
Mason, C.R., Miller, L.E., Baker, J.F., and Houk, J.C. (1998). Organization of reaching and
grasping movements in the primate cerebellar nuclei as revealed by focal muscimol inacti-
vations. J. Neurophysiol. 79: 537–554.
Mauk, M.D. (1997). Roles of cerebellar cortex and nuclei in motor learning: contradictions or
clues? Neuron 18: 343–346.
Mauk, M.D., and Donegan, N.H. (1997). A model of Pavlovian eyelid conditioning based on
the synaptic organization of the cerebellum. Learn. Mem. 3: 130–158.
Mauk, M.D., Medina, J.F., Nores, W.L., and Ohyama, T. (2000). Cerebellar function: coordi-
nation, learning, or timing? Curr. Biol. 10: R522–R525.
Mauk, M.D., Steinmetz, J.E., and Thompson, R.F. (1986). Classical conditioning using stim-
ulation of the inferior olive as the unconditional stimulus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 83:
5349–5353.
Bibliography 321
McCrea, R.A., Bishop, G.A., and Kitai, S.T. (1978). Morphological and electrophysiologi-
cal characteristics of projection neurons in the nucleus interpositus of the cat cerebellum.
J. Comp. Neurol. 181: 397–420.
McCulloch, W.S., and Pitts, W. (1943). A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous
activity. Bull. Math. Biophys. 5: 115–125.
McCurdy, M.L., Gibson, A.R., and Houk, J.C. (1992). Neuroimage 1: 23–41.
McCurdy, M.L., Houk, J.C., and Gibson, A.R. (1998). Organization of ascending pathways
to the forelimb area of the dorsal accessory olive in the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 392: 115–
133.
McIntyre, J., and Bizzi, E. (1993). Servo hypotheses for the biological control of movement.
J. Mot. Behav. 25: 193–202.
Medina, J.F., and Mauk, M.D. (1999). Simulations of cerebellar motor learning: computational
analysis of plasticity at the mossy fiber to deep nucleus synapse. J. Neurosci. 19: 7140–
7151.
Medina, J.F., Garcia, K.S., Nores, W.L., Taylor, N.M., and Mauk, M.D. (2000). Timing mecha-
nisms in the cerebellum: testing predictions of a large-scale computer simulation. J. Neurosci.
20: 5516–5525.
Meek, J. (1992a). Comparative aspects of cerebellar organization: from mormyrids to mam-
mals Eur. J. Morphol. 30: 37–51.
Meek, J. (1992b). Why run parallel fibers parallel? Teleostean Purkinje cells as possible co-
incidence detectors, in a timing device subserving spatial coding of temporal differences.
Neuroscience 48: 249–283.
Meek, J., and Nieuwenhuys, R. (1991). Palisade pattern of mormyrid Purkinje cells: a corre-
lated light and electron microscopic study. J. Comp. Neurol. 306: 156–192.
Melkonian, D.S., Mkrtchian, H.H., and Fanardjian, V.V. (1982). Simulation of learning pro-
cesses in neuronal networks of the cerebellum. Biol. Cybern. 45: 79–88.
Miall, R.C. (1997). Sequences of sensory predictions. Behav. Brain Sci. 20: 258–259.
Miall, R.C. (1998). The cerebellum, predictive control and motor coordination. Novartis
Found. Symp. 218: 272–290, (discussion) 284–290.
Miall, R.C., and Wolpert, D.M. (1995). The cerebellum as a predictive model of the motor
system: a Smith predictor hypothesis. In W.R. Ferrell and U. Proske (eds.), Neural Control
of Movement. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 215–223.
Miall, R.C., and Wolpert, D.M. (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural
Netw. 9: 1265–1279.
Miall, R.C., Keating, J.G., Malkmus, M., and Thach, W.T. (1998). Simple spike activity predicts
occurrence of complex spikes in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Nature Neurosci. 1: 13–15.
Miall, R.C., Malkmus, M., and Robertson, E.M. (1996). Sensory prediction as a role for the
cerebellum. Behav. Brain Sci. 19: 466–467.
Miall, R.C., Weir, D.J., Wolpert, D.M., and Stein, J.F. (1993). Is the cerebellum a Smith predic-
tor? J. Mot. Behav. 25: 203–216.
Middleton, F.A., and Strick, P.L. (1994). Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia
involvement in higher cognitive function. Science 266: 458–461.
Middleton, F.A., and Strick, P.L. (1997). Dentate output channels: motor and cognitive com-
ponents. Progr. Brain Res. 114: 553–556.
Middleton, F.A., and Strick, P.L. (1998). Cerebellar output: motor and cognitive channels.
Trends Cogn. Sci. 2: 348–354.
Middleton, F.A., and Strick, P. (2000). Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and cognitive
circuits. Brain Res. Rev. 31: 236–250.
Milaihoff, G.A., Kosinski, R.J., Azizi, S.A., Lee, H.S., and Border, B.G. (1992). The expanding
role of the basilar pontine nuclei as a source of cerebellar affferents. In R. Llinás and
C. Sotelo (eds.), The Cerebellum Revisited. New York: Springer, pp. 136–164.
322 Bibliography
Milak, M.S., Bracha, V., and Bloedel, J.R. (1995). Relationship of simultaneously recorded
cerebellar nuclear neuron discharge to the acquisition of a complex, operantly conditioned
forelimb movement in cats. Exp. Brain Res. 105: 325–330.
Milak, M.S., Shimansky, Y., Bracha, V., and Bloedel, J.R. (1997). Effects of inactivating in-
dividual cerebellar nuclei on the performance and retention of an operantly conditioned
forelimb movement. J. Neurophysiol. 78: 939–959.
Miles, F.A., and Lisberger, S.G. (1981). Plasticity in the vestibulo-ocular reflex: a new hypoth-
esis. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 4: 273–299.
Miles, F.A., Fuller, J.H., Braitman, D.J., and Dow, B.M. (1980). Long-term adaptive changes in
primate vestibuloocular reflex. III. Electrophysiological observations in flocculus of normal
monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 41: 1437–1476.
Miller, L.E., and Houk, J.C. (1995). Motor co-ordinates in primate red nucleus: preferen-
tial relation to muscle activation versus kinematic variables. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 488.2:
533–548.
Miller, W.T., III, Glanz, F.H., and Kraft, L.G., III. (1987). Application of a general learning
algorithm to the control of robotic manipulators. Int. J. Robotics Res. 6(2): 84–98.
Montgomery, J.C., and Bodznick, D. (1994). An adaptive filter that cancels self-induced noise
in the electrosensory and lateral line mechanosensory systems of fish. Neurosci. Lett. 174:
145–148.
Moore, J.K., and Osen, K.K. (1979). The cochlear nuclei in man. Am. J. Anat. 154:
393–418.
Moore, J.W., Desmond, J.E., and Berthier, N.E. (1989). Adaptively timed conditioned re-
sponses and the cerebellum: a neural network approach. Biol. Cybern. 62: 17–28.
Morgan, J.I., and Smeyne, R.J. (1997). Transgenic approaches to cerebellar development.
Perspect. Devel. Neurobiol. 5: 33–41.
Mugnaini, E. (1983). The length of cerebellar parallel fibers in chicken and rhesus monkey.
J. Comp. Neurol. 220: 7–15.
Mugnaini, E. (1985). GABA neurons in the superficial layers of the rat dorsal cochlear nucleus:
light and electron microscopic immunocytochemistry. J. Comp. Neurol. 235: 61–81.
Mugnaini, E., and Maler, L. (1993). Comparison between the fish electrosensory lateral line
lobe and the mammalian dorsal cochlear nucleus. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 173: 683–685.
Mugnaini, E., and Morgan, J.I. (1987). The neuropeptide cerebellin is a marker for two similar
neuronal circuits in rat brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 84: 8692–8696.
Mugnaini, E., Berrebi, A.S., Dahl, A.-L., and Morgan, J.I. (1987). The polypeptide PEP-19
is a marker for Purkinje neurons in cerebellar cortex and cartwheel neurons in the dorsal
cochlear nucleus. Arch. Ital. Biol. 126: 41–67.
Mugnaini, E., Diño, M.R., and Jaarsma, D. (1997). The unipolar brush cells of the mam-
malian cerebellum and cochlear nucleus: cytology and microcircuitry. Progr. Brain Res. 114:
131–150.
Mugnaini, E., Osen, K.K., Dahl, A.-L., Friedrich, V.L., Jr., and Korte, G. (1980). Fine struc-
ture of granule cells and related interneurons (termed Golgi cells) in the cochlear nuclear
complex of cat, rat and mouse. J. Neurocytol. 9: 537–570.
Mugnaini, E., Warr, W.B., and Osen, K.K. (1980). Distribution and light microscopic fea-
tures of granule cells in the cochlear nuclei of cat, rat, and mouse. J. Comp. Neurol. 191:
581–606.
Nagao, S., Kitamura, T., Nakamura, N., Hiramatsu, T., and Yamada, J. (1997). Differences
of the primate flocculus and ventral paraflocculus in the mossy and climbing fiber input
organization. J. Comp. Anat. 382: 480–498.
Narendra, K.S. (1995). Adaptive control: neural network applications. In M.A. Arbib (ed.),
The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press,
pp. 69–73.
Bibliography 323
Nelken, I., and Young, E.D. (1996). Why do cats need a dorsal cochlear nucleus? J. Basic Clin.
Physiol. Pharmacol. 7: 199–220.
Nelson, B.J., and Mugnaini, E. (1989). Origins of GABAergic inputs to the inferior olive.
In: P. Strata (ed.), The Olivocerebellar System in Motor Control. Berlin: Springer-Verlag,
(Experimental Brain Research Series 17), pp. 86–107.
Nieuwenhuys, R. (1967). Comparative anatomy of the cerebellum. Progr. Brain Res. 25:
1–93.
Nieuwenhuys, R., and Nicholson, C. (1969a). A survey of the general morphology, the fiber
connections and the possible functional significance of the gigantocerebellum of mormyrid
fishes. In: R. Llinás (ed.), Neurobiology of Cerebellar Evolution and Development. Chicago:
American Medical Association Educational and Research Foundation, pp. 107–134.
Nieuwenhuys, R., and Nicholson, C. (1969b). Aspects of the histology of the cerebellum of
mormyrid fishes. In: R. Llinás (ed.), Neurobiology of Cerebellar Evolution and Development.
Chicago: American Medical Association, pp. 135–169.
Nieuwenhuys, R., ten Donkelaar, H.J., and Nicholson, C. (1998). The Central Nervous System
of Vertebrates, Vol. 2. Berlin: Springer.
Nieuwenhuys, R., Voogd, J., and van Huijzen, C. (1988). The Human Central Nervous System:
A Synopsis and Atlas (3rd ed.). Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.
Nixon, P.D., and Passingham, R.E. (1999). The cerebellum and cognition: cerebellar lesions
do not impair spatial working memory or visual associative learning in monkeys. Eur. J.
Neurosci. 11: 4070–4080.
Nunzi, M.-G., and Mugnaini, E. (2000). Unipolar brush cell axons form a large system
of intrinsic mossy fibers in the postnatal vestibulocerebellum. J. Comp. Neurol. 422:
55–65.
Oberdick, J., Baader, S.L., and Schilling, K. (1998). From zebra stripes to postal zones: deci-
phering patterns of gene expression in the cerebellum. Trends Neurosci. 21: 383–390.
Olivier, J., Coenen, M.D., and Seijnowski, T.J. (1995). A model for how the cerebellum an-
ticipates sensory inputs and modulates the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR). Soc. Neurosci.
21: 915.
Optican, L.M., and Robinson, D.A. (1980). Cerebellar-dependent adaptive control of the
primate saccadic system. J. Neurophysiol. 44: 1058–1076.
Osborn, C.E., and Poppele, R.E. (1992). Parallel distributed network characteristics of the
DSCT. J. Neurophysiol. 68: 1100–1112.
Oscarsson, O. (1969). The sagittal organization of the cerebellar anterior lobe as revealed by
the projection patterns of the climbing fiber system. In: R. Llinás (ed.), Neurobiology of
Cerebellar Evolution and Development. Chicago: American Medical Association Educa-
tional and Research Foundation, pp. 525–537.
Oscarsson, O. (1979). Functional units of the cerebellum – sagittal zones and microzones.
Trends Neurosci. 2: 143–145.
Oscarsson, O. (1980). Functional organization of olivary projection to the cerebellar anterior
lobe. In: J. Courville, C. de Montigny, and Y. Lamarre (eds.), The Inferior Olivary Nucleus:
Anatomy and Physiology. New York: Raven Press, pp. 279–289.
Ozol, K.L., and Hawkes, R. (1997). Compartmentation of the granular layer of the cerebellum.
Histol. Histopathol. 112: 171–184.
Palay, S.L., and Chan-Palay, V. (1974). Cerebellar Cortex. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Palkovits, M., Magyar, P., and Szentágothai, J. (1972). Quantitative histological analysis of the
cerebellar cortex in the cat. IV. Mossy fiber–Purkinje cell numerical transfer. Brain Res.
45: 15–29.
Parent, A. (1996). Carpenter’s Human Neuroanatomy (9th ed.). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Parkins, E.J. (1997). Cerebellum and cerebrum in adaptive control and cognition: a review.
Biol. Cybern. 77: 79–87.
324 Bibliography
Parsons, L.M., Bower, J.M., Gao, J.-H., Xiong, J.-H., Li, J.-Q., and Fox, P.T. (1997). Lateral
cerebellar hemispheres actively support sensory acquisition and discrimination rather than
motor control. Learn. Mem. 4: 49–62.
Pastor, A.M., De la Cruz, R.R., and Baker, R. (1997). Characterization of Purkinje cells in the
goldfish cerebellum during eye movement and adaptive modification of the vestibulo-ocular
reflex. Progr. Brain Res. 114: 359–381.
Paulin, M. (1989). A Kalman filter theory of the cerebellum. In: M.A. Arbib and S.-I. Amari
(eds.), Dynamic Interactions in Neural Networks: Models and Data. New York: Springer,
pp. 239–259.
Paulin, M. (1996). Cerebellar theory out of control. Behav. Brain Sci. 19: 470–471.
Paulin, M. (1997). Neural representations of moving systems. In J.D. Schmahmann (ed.), The
Cerebellum and Cognition. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 515–533.
Paulin, M.G. (1993). The role of the cerebellum in motor control and perception. Brain Behav.
Evol. 41: 39–50.
Paulin, M.G., Nelson, M.E., and Bower, J.M. (1989). Neural control of sensory acquisition: the
vestibulo-ocular reflex. In D.S. Touretsky (ed.), Advances in Neural Information Processing
Systems I. San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann Publishers, pp. 410–418.
Pellegrini, J.J., and Evinger, C. (1997). Role of cerebellum in adaptive modification of reflex
blinks. Learn. Mem. 3: 77–87.
Pellionisz, A. (1970). Computer simulation of the pattern transfer of large cerebellar neuronal
fields. Acta Biochim. Biophys. Acad. Sci. Hung. 5: 71–79.
Pellionisz, A. (1984). Coordination: a vector-matrix description of transformations of over-
complete CNS coordinates and a tensorial solution using the Moore–Penrose generalized
inverse. J. Theoret. Biol. 110: 353–375.
Pellionisz, A., and Graf, W. (1987). Tensor network model of the “three-neuron” vestibulo-
ocular reflex-arc in cat. J. Theoret. Neurobiol. 5: 127–151.
Pellionisz, A. and Llinás, R. (1979). Brain modeling by tensor network theory and computer
simulation. The cerebellum: distributed processor for predictive coordination. Neuroscience
4: 323–348.
Pellionisz, A., and Llinás, R. (1980). Tensorial approach to the geometry of brain function:
cerebellar coordination via a metric tensor. Neuroscience 5: 1125–1136.
Pellionisz, A., and Llinás, R. (1982). Space-time representation in the brain. The cerebellum
as a predictive space-time metric tensor. Neuroscience 7: 2949–2970.
Pellionisz, A., and Szentágothai, J. (1973). Dynamic single unit simulation of a realistic cere-
bellar network model. Brain Res. 49: 83–99.
Pellionisz, A., and Szentágothai, J. (1974). Dynamic single unit simulation of a realistic cere-
bellar network model. II. Purkinje cell activity within the basic circuit and modified by
inhibitory systems. Brain Res. 68: 19–40.
Pellionisz, A., Peterson, B.W., and Tomko, D.L. (1990). Vestibular head-eye coordination:
a geometrical sensorimotor neurocomputer paradigm. In R. Eckmiller, (ed.), Advanced
Neural Computers. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North Holland, pp. 61–68.
Pennacchio, L.A., Boulev, D.M., Higgins, K.M., Scott, M.P., and Noebels, J.L. (1998). Progres-
sive ataxia, myoclonic epilepsy and cerebellar apopotosis in cystatin B-deficient mice. Nat.
Genet. 20: 251–258.
Peterson, B.W., and Houk, J.C. (1991). A model of cerebellar-brainstem interaction in the
adaptive control of the vestibuloocular reflex. Acta Otolaryngol. (Stockh). Suppl. 481:
428–432.
Peterson, S.E., Fox, P.T., Posner, M.I., Mintun, M., and Raichle, M.E. (1989). Positron emission
tomographic studies of the processing of single words. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1: 133–170.
Pichitpornchai, C., Rawson, J.A., and Rees, S. (1994). Morphology of parallel fibers in the
cerebellar cortex of the rat: an experimental light and electron microscopic study with
biocytin. J. Comp. Neurol. 342: 206–220.
Bibliography 325
Pitts, W., and McCulloch, W.S. (1947). How we know universals: the perception of auditory
and visual forms. Bull. Math. Biophys. 9: 127–147.
Poldrack, R.A., and Gabrieli, J. E. (1997). Functional anatomy of long-term memory. J. Clin.
Neurophysiol. 14: 294–310.
Precht, W., and Llinás, R. (1969). Comparative aspects of the vestibular input to the cerebellum.
In R. Llinás, (ed.), Neurobiology of Cerebellar Evolution and Development, Chicago: Amer-
ican Medical Association, pp. 677–702.
Qian, N. (1995). Generalization and analysis of the Lisberger-Seijnowski VOR model. Neural
Comput. 7: 735–752.
Ramón y Cajal, S. (1995). Histology of the Nervous System, 2 Vols. (trans. from the 1909–1911
French edition by N. Swanson and L.W. Swanson). New York/Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Raphan, T., Dai, M.J., and Cohen, B. (1992). Spatial orientation of the vestibular system. Ann.
N. Y. Acad. Sci. 656: 140–157.
Raymond, J.L., and Lisberger, S.G. (1997). Multiple subclasses of Purkinje cells in the primate
floccular complex provide similar signals to guide learning in the vestibulo-ocular reflex.
Learn. Mem. 3: 503–518.
Raymond, J.L., and Lisberger, S.G. (2000). Hypotheses about the neural trigger for plasticity
in the circuit for the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Progr. Brain Res. 124: 235– 246.
Raymond, J.L., Lisberger, S.G., and Mauk, M.D. (1996). The cerebellum: a neuronal learning
machine? Science 272: 1126–1131.
Robinson, D.A. (1976). Adaptive gain control of vestibuloocular reflex by the cerebellum.
J. Neurophysiol. 39: 954–969.
Robinson, D.A. (1981). Control of eye movements. In V.B. Brooks (ed.), Handbook of
Physiology, Vol, II, Motor Control, Part 2. Bethesda: American Physiological Society, pp.
1275–1320.
Robinson, D.A. (1989). Integrating with neurons. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 12: 33–45.
Robinson, D.A. (1992a). Implications of neural networks for how we think about brain func-
tion. Behav. Brain Sci. 15: 644–655.
Robinson, D.A. (1992b). How far into brain function can neural networks take us? Behav.
Brain Sci. 15: 823–828.
Robinson, F.R., Straube, A., and Fuchs, A.F. (1997). Participation of caudal fastigial nucleus
in smooth pursuit eye movements. II. Effects of mucimol inactivation. J. Neurophysiol. 78:
848–859.
Rolls, E.T., and Treves, A. (1998). Neural Networks and Brain Function. Oxford, England:
Oxford University Press.
Rosenblatt, F. (1958). The Perceptron: a probabilistic model for information storage and
organization in the brain. Psychol. Rev. 65: 386–408.
Rosenblatt, F. (1961). Principles of Neurodynamical Perceptrons and the Theory of Brain
Mechanisms. Buffalo, NY: Cornell Aeronautical Laboratory (1962). Washington, DC: Spar-
tan Books.
Ross, C.A. (1997). Intranuclear neuronal inclusions: a common pathogenic mechanism for
glutamine-repeat neurodegenerative diseases? Neuron 19: 1147–1150.
Rossi, F., and Strata, P. (1995). Reciprocal trophic interactions in the adult climbing fibre–
Purkinje cell system. Progr. Neurobiol. 47: 341–369.
Rothwell, J. (1994). Control of Human Voluntary Movement (2nd ed.). London: Chapman &
Hall.
Rubia, F.J. (1992). A possible connection between the mossy and climbing fiber systems at
precerebellar level. In: R. Llinás and C. Sotelo (eds.), The Cerebellum Revisited. New York:
Springer, pp. 226–254.
Ruigrok, T.J.H. (1997). Cerebellar nuclei: the olivary connection. Progr. Brain Res. 114: 168–
192.
326 Bibliography
Ruigrok, T.J.H., and Voogd, J. (1995). Cerebellar influence on olivary excitability in the cat.
Eur. J. Neurosci. 7: 679–693.
Ruigrok, T.J.H., and Voogd, J. (2000). Organization of projections from the inferior olive to
the cerebellar nuclei in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 426: 209–228.
Ruigrok, T.J.H., de Zeeuw, C.I., van der Burg, J., and Voogd, J. (1990). Intracellular labeling
of neurons in the medial accessory olive of the cat: I. Physiology and light microscopy.
J. Comp. Neurol. 300: 462–477.
Ruigrok, T.J.H., Osse, R.-J., and Voogd, J. (1992). Organization of inferior olivary projec-
tions to the flocculus and ventral paraflocculus of the rat cerebellum. J. Comp. Neurol. 316:
129–150.
Rumelhart, D.E. (1990). Series foreword. in: M.A. Gluck and D.E. Rumelhart (eds.), Neu-
roscience and Connectionist Theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp.
ix–x.
Rumelhart D.E., Hinton, G.E., and Williams, R.J. (1986a). Learning internal representations
by error propagation. In D.E. Rumelhart and J.L. McClelland (eds.), Parallel Distributed
Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, Vol. 1. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Rumelhart, D.E., Hinton, G.E., and Williams, R.J. (1986b). Learning representations by back-
propagating errors. Nature 323: 225–228.
Russell, C.J., and Bell, C.C. (1978). Neuronal responses to electrosensory input in momyrid
valvula cerebelli. J. Neurophysiol. 41: 1495–1510.
Rutherford, J.G., and Gwyn, D.G. (1980). A light and electron microscopic study of the
inferior olivary nucleus of the squirrel monkey, Saimiri sciureus. J. Comp. Neurol. 189:
127–155.
Sadato, N., Ibañez, V., Deiber, M.-P., Campbell, G., Leonardo, M., and Hallett, M. (1996).
Frequency-dependent changes of regional cerebral blood flow during finger movements.
J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 16: 23–33.
Saint-Cyr, J.A. (1983). The projection from the motor cortex to the inferior olive in the cat.
Neuroscience 10: 667–684.
Sakurai, M. (1987). Synaptic modification of parallel fibre–Purkinje cell transmission in in-vitro
guinea-pig cerebellar slices. J. Physiol. 394: 463–480.
Sakurai, M. (1989). Depression and potentiation of parallel fiber–Purkinje cell transmission
in in vitro cerebellar slices. In P. Strata (ed.), The Olivocerebellar System in Motor Control.
Berlin: Springer, pp. 221–230.
Salin, P.A., Malenka, R.C., and Nicoll, R.A. (1996). Cyclic AMP mediates a presynaptic form
of LTP at cerebellar parallel fiber synapses. Neuron 16: 797–803.
Sanner, R.M., and Slotine, J.-J. E. (1993). Stable adaptive control of root manipulators using
“neural” networks. Neural Comput. 7: 753–790.
Sasaki, K., Bower, J.M., and Llinás, R. (1989). Multiple Purkinje cell recording in rodent
cerebellar cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1: 572–586.
Scheibel, M.E., and Scheibel, A.B. (1955). The inferior olive: a Golgi study. J. Comp. Neurol.
102: 77–132.
Scheibel, M., Scheibel, A., Walberg, W., and Brodal, A. (1956). Areal distribution of axonal
and dendritic patterns in inferior olive. J. Comp. Neurol. 106: 21–49.
Schild, R.F. (1980). Length of the parallel fibers in rat cerebellar cortex. J. Physiol. (Lond.)
303: 25P
Schilling, K. (2000). Lineage, development and morphogenesis of cerebellar interneurons.
Progr. Brain Res. 124: 51–68.
Schlösser, R., Hutchinson, M., Joseffer, S., Rusinek, H., Saarimaki, A., Stevenson, J., Dewey,
S.L., and Brodie, J.D. (1998). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity
in a verbal fluency task. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 64: 492–498.
Bibliography 327
Schmahmann, J.D. (1991). An emerging concept. The cerebellar contribution to higher func-
tion. Arch. Neurol. 48: 1178–1187.
Schmahmann, J.D. (1992). In reply (letter to the editor). Arch. Neurol. 49: 1230.
Schmahmann, J.D. (1996). Dysmetria of thought: correlations and conundrums in the rela-
tionship between the cerebellum, learning, and cognitive processing. Behav. Brain Sci. 19:
472–473.
Schmahmann, J.D. (1997). (ed.). The Cerebellum and Cognition (Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 41).
San Diego: Academic Press.
Schmahmann, J.D. (1998). Dysmetria of thought: clinical consequences of cerebellar dysfunc-
tion on cognition and affect. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2: 362–371.
Schmahmann, J.D., and Pandya, D.N. (1989). Anatomical investigation of projections to the
basis pontis from posterior parietal association cortices in rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol.
289: 53–73.
Schmahmann, J.D., and Pandya, D.N. (1991). Projections to the basis pontis from the superior
temporal sulcus and superior temporal region in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 308:
224–248.
Schmahmann, J.D., and Pandya, D.N. (1997a). Anatomic organization of the basilar pontine
projections from prefrontal cortices in rhesus monkey. J. Neurosci. 17: 438–458.
Schmahmann, J., and Pandya, D.K. (1997b). The cerebrocerebellar system. In: J. Schmahmann
(ed.), The Cerebellum and Cognition. San Diego: Academic Press (Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 41):
31–60.
Schmahmann, J.D., and Sherman, J.C. (1998). The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome.
Brain 121: 561–579.
Schmahmann, J.D., Doyon, J., Toga, A.W., Petrides, M., and Evans, A.C. (2000). MRI Atlas of
the Human Cerebellum. San Diego: Academic Press.
Schnitzlein, B.N., and Faucette, J.R. (1969). General morphology of the fish cerebellum. In: R.
Llinás (ed.), Neurobiology of Cerebellar Evolution and Development, Chicago: American
Medical Association, pp. 77–106.
Schöner, G., and Kelso, J.A.S. (1988). Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural
systems. Science 239: 1513–1520.
Schreurs, B.G., and Alkon, D.L. (1993). Rabbit cerebellar slice analysis of long-term depression
and its role in classical conditioning. Brain Res. 631: 235–240.
Schreurs, B.G., Oh, M.M., and Alkon, D.L. (1996). Pairing-specific long-term depression of
Purkinje cell excitatory postsynaptic potentials results from a classical conditioning proce-
dure in the rabbit cerebellar slice. J. Neurophysiol. 75: 1051–1060.
Schreurs, B.G., Tomsic, D., Gusev, P.A., and Alkon, D.L. (1997). Dendritic excitability micro-
zones and occluded long-term depression after classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nicti-
tating membrane response. J. Neurophysiol. 77: 86–92.
Schweighofer, N., and Arbib, M.A. (1998). A model of cerebellar metaplasticity. Learn. Mem.
4: 421–428.
Schweighofer, N., Arbib, M.A., and Dominey, P.F. (1996a). A model of the cerebellum in
adaptive control of saccadic gain. I. The model and its biological substrate. Biol. Cybern.
75: 19–28.
Schweighofer, N., Arbib, M.A., and Dominey, P.F. (1996b). A model of the cerebellum in
adaptive control of saccadic gain. II. Simulation results. Biol. Cybern. 75: 29–36.
Schweighofer, N., Doya, K., and Kawato, M. (1999). Electrophysiological properties of inferior
olive neurons: a compartmental model. J. Neurophysiol. 82: 804–817.
Schweighofer, N., Spoelstra, J., Arbib, M.A., and Kawato, M. (1998a). Role of the cerebellum
in reaching movements in humans. I. Distributed inverse dynamics control. Eur. J. Neurosci.
10: 86–94.
Schweighofer, N., Spoelstra, J., Arbib, M.A., and Kawato, M. (1998b). Role of the cerebellum
328 Bibliography
in reaching movements in humans. II. A neural model of the intermediate cerebellum. Eur.
J. Neurosci. 10: 95–105.
Seeds, N.W., Williams, B.L., and Bickford, P.C. (1995). Tissue plasminogen activator induction
in Purkinje neurons after cerebellar motor learning. Science 270: 1992–1994.
Seijnowski, T.J. (1977). Storing covariance with nonlinearly interacting neurons. J. Math. Biol.
4: 303–321.
Seitz, R.J., Canavan, A.G.M., Yagüez, L., Herzog, H., Tellmann, L., Knorr, U., Huang,
Y.-X., and Hömberg, V. (1994). Successive roles of the cerebellum and premotor cortices in
trajectorial learning. Neuroreport 5: 2541–2544.
Shambes, G.M., Gibson, J.M., and Welker, W. (1978). Fractured somatotopy in granule cell
tactile areas of rat cerebellar hemispheres revealed by micromapping. Brain Behav. Evol.
15: 94–140.
Shibuki, C.A., and Okada, D. (1992). Cerebellar long-term potentiation under suppressed
postsynaptic Ca2+ activity. Neuroreport 3: 231–234.
Shidara, M., Kawano, K., Gomi, H., and Kawato, M. (1993). Inverse-dynamics model eye
movement control by Purkinje cells in the cerebellum. Nature 365: 50–52.
Shimansky, Y., Saling, M., Wunderlich, D.A., Bracha, V., Stelmach, G.F., and Bloedel, J.R.
(1997). Impaired capacity of cerebellar patients to perceive and learn two-dimensional
shapes based on kinesthetic cues. Learn. Mem. 4: 36–48.
Shinoda, Y., Futami, T., Sugiuchi, Y., Kakei, S., and Izawa, Y. (1993). Input–output organization
of the cerebellar nuclei. In: N. Mano, I. Hamada, and M.R. DeLong (eds.), Role of the
Cerebellum and Basal Ganglia in Voluntary Movement. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 133–145.
Shinoda, Y., Izawa, Y., Sugiuchi, Y., and Futami, T. (1997). Functional significance of excitatory
projections from the precerebellar nuclei to interpositus and dentate nucleus neurons for
mediating motor, premotor and parietal cortical inputs. Progr. Brain Res. 114: 193–207.
Shinoda, Y., Sugihara, E., Wu, H.-S., and Sugiuchi, Y. (2000). The entire trajectory of single
climbing and mossy fibers in the cerebellar nuclei and cortex. Progr. Brain Res. 124: 173–186.
Shmerling, D., Hegyi, I., Fischer, M., Blätter, T., Brandner, S., Götz, J., Rülicke, T., Flechsig,
E., Cozzio, A., von Mering, C., Hangartner, C., Aguzzi, A., and Weissmann, C. (1998).
Expression of amino-terminally truncated PrP [prion protein] in the mouse leading to ataxia
and specific cerebellar lesions. Cell 93: 203–214.
Shynk, J.J. (1995). Adaptive filtering. In: M.A. Arbib (ed.), The Handbook of Brain Theory
and Neural Networks. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 74–78.
Silkis, I. (2000). Interrelated modification of excitatory and inhibitory synapses in three-layer
olivary-cerebellar neural network. Biosystems 54: 141–149.
Silveri, M.C., Leggio, M.G., and Molinari, M. (1994). The cerebellum contributes to linguistic
production: a case of agrammatic speech following a right cerebellar lesion. Neurology 44:
2047–2050.
Simpson, J.I., and Alley, K.E. (1974). Visual climbing fiber input to rabbit vestibulo-cerebellum:
a source of direction-specific information. Brain Res. 82: 302–308.
Simpson, J.I., Graf, W., and Leonard, C.S. (1989). Three-dimensional representation of retinal
image movement by climbing fiber activity. In P. Strata (ed.), The Olivocerebellar System
in Motor Control. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 323–337.
Simpson, J.I., Van der Steen, J., and Tan, J. (1992). Eye movements and the zonal structure of
the rabbit flocculus. In R. Llinás and C. Sotelo (eds.), The Cerebellum Revisited. New York:
Springer Verlag, pp. 255–266.
Simpson, J.I., Wylie, D.R., and De Zeeuw, C.I. (1996). On climbing fiber signals and their
consequence(s). Behav. Brain Sci. 19: 384–398.
Singer, J.L. (1995). Mental processes and brain architecture: confronting the complex adaptive
systems of human thought (an overview). In: H. Morowitz and J.L. Singer (eds.), The Mind,
The Brain, and Complex Adaptive Systems. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley, pp. 1–9.
Bibliography 329
Sinkjær, T., Miller, L., Andersen, T., and Houk, J.C. (1995). Synaptic linkages between red
nucleus cells and limb muscles during a multi-joint motor task. Exp. Brain. Res. 102:
546–550.
Siouris, G.M. (1993). Aerospace Avionics Systems. New York: Academic Press.
Slater, N.T., Rossi, D.J., and Kinney, G.A. (1997). Physiology of transmission at a giant gluta-
matergic synapse in cerebellum. Progr. Brain Res. 114: 150–163.
Smith, A.M. (1996). Does the cerebellum learn strategies for the optimal time-varying control
of joint stiffness? Behav. Brain Sci. 19: 399–410.
Smolyaninov, V.V. (1971). Some special features of organization of the cerebellar cortex. In:
I.M. Gelfand, V.S. Gurfinkel, S.V. Fomin, and M.L. Tsetlin (eds.), Models of the Structural-
Functional Organization of Certain Biological Systems (translated from the Russian by C.R.
Beard, translation reviewed by J.S. Barlow). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 251–325.
Soechting, J.F., and Flanders, M. (1992). Moving in three-dimensional space: frames of refer-
ence, vectors, and coordinate systems. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 15: 167–191.
Sotelo, C., and Chédotal, A. (1997). Development of the olivocerebellar projection. Perspect.
Devel. Neurobiol. 5: 57–67.
Sotelo, C., Gotow, T., and Wassef, M. (1986). Localization of glutamic-acid-decarbolyxase-
immunoreactive axon terminals in the inferior olive of the rat, with special emphasis on
anatomical relations between GABAergic synapses and dendrodendritic gap junctions.
J. Comp. Neurol. 252: 32–50.
Sotelo, C., Llinás, R., and Baker, R. (1974). Structural study of inferior olivary nucleus of the
cat: morphological correlates of electrotonic coupling. J. Neurophysiol. 37: 541–559.
Stein, J.F. (1992). The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex. Behav.
Brain Sci. 15: 691–700.
Stein, J.F., and Glickstein, J.F. (1992). The role of the cerebellum in visual guidance of move-
ment. Physiol. Rev. 72: 967–1017.
Stone, L.S., and Lisberger, S.G. (1989). Synergistic action of complex and simple spikes in the
monkey flocculus in the control of smooth-pursuit eye movement. In P. Strata (ed.), The
Olivocerebellar System in Motor Control. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 299–312.
Stone, L.S., and Lisberger, S.G. (1990). Visual responses of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar floc-
culus during smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkeys. I. Simple spikes. J. Neurophysiol.
63: 1241–1261.
Strata, P., and Rossi, F. (1998). Plasticity of the olivocerebellar pathway. Trends Neurosci. 21:
407–413.
Strata, P., Rossi, F., and Tempia, F. (1995). Inferior olive and the saccadic neural integrator. In
W.R. Ferrell and U. Proskeu (eds.), Neural Control of Movement. New York: Plenum Press,
pp. 241–249.
Strata, P., Tempia, F., Zagrebelsky, M., and Rossi, F. (1997). Reciprocal trophic interactions
between climbing fibres and Purkinje cells in the rat cerebellum. Progr. Brain Res. 114:
263–282.
Sugawara, Y., Grant, K., Han, V., and Bell, C.C. (1999). Physiology of electrosensory lateral
line lobe neurons in Gnathonemus petersii. J. Exp. Biol. 202: 1301–1309.
Sugihara, I., Lang, E.J., and Llinás, R. (1993). Uniform olivocerebellar conduction time under-
lies Purkinje cell complex spike synchronicity in the rat cerebellum. J. Physiol. 470: 243–271.
Sugihara, I., Lang, E.J., and Llinás, R. (1995). Serotonin modulation of inferior olivary oscil-
lations and synchronicity: a multiple-electrode study in the rat cerebellum. Eur. J. Neurosci.
7: 521–534.
Sugihara, I., Wu, H.-S., and Shinoda, Y. (1999). Morphology of single olivocerebellar axons
labeled with biotinylated dextran. J. Comp. Neurol. 414: 131–148.
Sutton, R.S., and Barto, A.G. (1981). Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: expec-
tation and prediction. Psychol. Review 88: 135–170.
330 Bibliography
Svensson, P., Ivarsson, M., and Hesslow, G. (1997). Effect of varying the intensity and train
frequency of forelimb and cerebellar mossy fiber conditioned stimuli on the latency of
conditioned eye-blink responses in decerebrate ferrets. Learn. Mem. 3: 105–115.
Svensson, P., Ivarsson, M., and Hesslow, G. (2000). Involvement of the cerebellum in a new
temporal property of the conditioned eyeblink response. Progr. Brain Res. 124: 317–323.
Swenson, R.S., and Castro, A.J. (1983). The afferent connections of the inferior olivary complex
in rats. An anterograde study using autoradiographic and axonal degeneration techniques.
Neuroscience 8: 259–275.
Szentágothai, J. (1965). The use of degeneration methods in the investigation of short neuronal
connexions. Progr. Brain Res. 14: 1–32.
Szentágothai, J. (1968). Structuro-functional considerations of the cerebellar network. Proc.
IEEE 56: 960–968.
Takács, J., Gombos, G., Görcs, T., Becker, T., de Barry, J., and Hámori, J. (1997). Distribution
of metabotropic glutamate receptor type 1a in Purkinje cell dendritic spines is independent
of the presence of presynaptic parallel fibers. J. Neurosci. Res. 50: 433–442.
Tan, J., Epema, A.H., and Voogd, J. (1995). Zonal organization of the flocculovestibular nucleus
projection in the rabbit: a combined axonal tracing and acetylcholinesterase histochemical
study. J. Comp. Neurol. 356: 51–71.
Tan, J., Gerrits, N.M., Nanhoe, R., Simpson, J.I., and Voogd, J. (1995). Zonal organization of
the climbing fiber projection to the flocculus and nodulus of the rabbit: a combined axonal
tracing and acetylcholinesterase histochemical study. J. Comp. Neurol. 356: 23–50.
Tan, J., Simpson, J.I., and Voogd, J. (1995). Anatomical compartments in the white matter of
the rabbit flocculus. J. Comp. Neurol. 356: 1–22.
Tauer, U., Volk, B., and Heimrich, B. (1996). Differentiation of Purkinje cells in cerebellar
slice cultures: an immunochemical and Golgi EM study. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 22:
361–369.
Teune, T.M., van der Burg, J., De Zeeuw, C.I., Voogd, J., and Ruigrok, T.J.H. (1998). Single
Purkinje cell can innervate multiple classes of projection neurons in the cerebellar nuclei of
the rat: a light microscopic and ultrastructural triple-tracer study in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol.
392: 164–178.
Teune, T.M., van der Burg, J., and Ruigrok, T.J.H. (1995). Cerebellar projections to the red
nucleus and inferior olive originate from separate populations of neurons in the rat: a non-
fluorescent double labeling study. Brain Res. 673: 313–319.
Thach, W.T. (1967). Somatosensory receptive fields of single units in cat cerebellar cortex.
J. Neurophysiol. 30: 675–696.
Thach, W.T. (1972). Cerebellar output: properties, synthesis and uses. Brain Res. 40: 89–97.
Thach, W.T. (1980). Complex spikes, the inferior olive, and natural behavior. In: J. Courville, C.
de Montigny, and Y. Lamarre (eds.), The Inferior Olivary Nucleus: Anatomy and Physiology.
New York: Raven Press, pp. 349–360.
Thach, W.T. (1996a). A cerebellar role in acquisition of novel static and dynamic muscle
activities in holding, pointing, throwing, and reaching. In: J.R. Bloedel, T.J. Ebner, and S.P.
Wise (eds.), The Acquisition of Motor Behavior in Vertebrates. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press,
pp. 223–234.
Thach, W.T. (1996b). On the specific role of the cerebellum in motor learning and cognition:
clues from PET activation and lesion studies in man. Behav. Brain Sci. 19: 411–431.
Thach, W.T. (1996c). Q. Is the cerebellum an adaptive combiner of motor and mental/motor
activities? A. Yes, maybe, certainly not, who can say? Behav. Brain Sci. 19: 501–503.
Thach, W.T. (1997). Context–response linkage. In: J. Schmahmann (ed.), The Cerebellum and
Cognition. (Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 41: 599–611) San Diego: Academic Press.
Thach, W.T. (1998a). Combination, complementarity and automatic control: a role for the
cerebellum in learning movement coordination. Novartis Found. Symp. 218: 219–232.
Bibliography 331
Thach, W.T. (1998b). A role for the cerebellum in learning movement coordination. Neurobiol.
Learn. Mem. 70: 177–188.
Thach, W.T. (1998c). What is the role of the cerebellum in motor learning and cognition?
Trends Cogn. Sci. 2: 331–337.
Thach, W.T., Goodkin, H.P., and Keating, J.G. (1992). The cerebellum and the adaptive coor-
dination of movement. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 15: 403–442.
Thach, W.T., Kane, S.A., Mink, J.W., and Goodkin H.P. (1992). Cerebellar output: multiple
maps and modes of control in movement coordination. In: R. Llinás and C. Sotelo (eds.),
The Cerebellum Revisited. New York: Springer, pp. 283–300.
Thach, W.T., Mink, J.W., Goodkin, H.P., and Keating, J.G. (1993). Combining versus gating
motor programs: differential roles for cerebellum and basal ganglia? In N. Mano, I. Hamada,
and M.R. DeLong (eds.), Role of the Cerebellum and Basal Ganglia in Voluntary Movement.
Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 235–245.
Thach, W.T., Perry, J.G., Kane, S.A., and Goodkin, H.P. (1993). Cerebellar nuclei: rapid alter-
nating movement, motor somatotopy, and a mechanism for the control of muscle synergy.
Rev. Neurol. (Paris) 149: 607–628.
Their, P., Dicke, P.W., Haas, R., and Barash, S. (2000). Encoding of movement time by popu-
lations of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Nature 405: 72–76.
Thompson, R.F., Thompson, J.K., Kim, J.J., Krupa, D.J., and Shinkman, P.G. (1998). The nature
of reinforcement in cerebellar learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 70: 150–176.
Timmann, D., Shimansky, Y., Larson, P.S., Wunderlich, D.A., Stelmach, G.E., and Bloedel, J.R.
(1996). Visuomotor learning in cerebellar patients. Behav. Brain Res. 81: 99–113.
Tolbert, D.L., (1982). The cerebellar nucleocortical pathway. In S.L. Palay and V. Chan-Palay
(eds.), The Cerebellum – New Vistas. Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 296–319.
Tolbert, D.L., and Bantli, H. (1979). An HRP and autoradiographic study of cerebellar
corticonuclear-nucleocortical reciprocity in the monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 36: 563–571.
Tolbert, D.L., Bantli, H., and Bloedel, J.R. (1976). Anatomical and physiological evidence for
a cerebellar nucleo-cortical projection in the cat. Neuroscience 1: 205–217.
Tolbert, D.L., Bantli, H., and Bloedel, J.R. (1977). The intracerebellar nucleocortical projection
in a primate. Exp. Brain Res. 30: 425–434.
Tolbert, D.L., Bantli, H., and Bloedel, J.R. (1978). Organizational features of the cat and
monkey cerebellar nucleocortical projection. J. Comp. Neurol. 182: 39–56.
Tolbert, D.L., Kultas-Ilinsky, K., and Ilinsky, I. (1980). EM-autoradiography of cerebellar
nucleocortical terminals in the cat. Anat. Embryol. 161: 215–223.
Topka, H., Konczak, J., Schneider, K., Boose, A., and Dichgans, J. (1998). Multijoint arm
movements in cerebellar ataxia: abnormal control of movement dynamics. Exp. Brain Res.
119: 493–503.
Topka, H., Konczak, J., and Dichgans, J. (1998). Coordination of multi-joint arm move-
ments in cerebellar ataxia: analysis of hand and angular kinematics. Exp. Brain Res. 119:
483–492.
Trott, J.R., Apps, R., and Armstrong, D.M. (1990). Topographical organisation within the
cerebellar nucleocortical projection to the paravermal cortex of lobule Vb/c in the cat. Exp.
Brain Res. 80: 415–428.
Umetani, T. (1990). Topographic organization of the cerebellar nucleocortical projection in
the albino rat: an autographic orthograde study. Brain Res. 507: 216–224.
Van der Neut, R. (1997). Targeted gene disruption: applications in neurobiology. J. Neurosci.
Methods 71: 19–27.
van Kan, P.L.E., Gibson, A.R., and Houk, J.C. (1993). Movement-related inputs to interme-
diate cerebellum of the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 69: 74–94.
Vemuri, V.R. (1992). Artifical Neural Networks: Concepts and Control Applications. Los
Alamitos, CA: IEEE Computer Society Press.
332 Bibliography
Verhaagen, J., and Schrama, L.H. (1997). The application of gene transfer technology in neu-
robiology. J. Neurosci. Methods 71: vii.
Vetter, P., and Wolpert, D.M. (2000). Context estimation for sensorimotor control. J. Neuro-
physiol. 84: 1026–1034.
Victor, M., and Ropper, A.H. (2001). Adams and Victor’s Principles of Neurology (7th ed.).
New York: McGraw-Hill.
Viets, H.R., and Garrison, F.H. (1940). Purkinje’s original description of the pear-shaped cells
in the cerebellum. Bull. Hist. Med. 8: 1397–1398.
Vilis, T., and Hore, J. (1980). Central neural mechanisms contributing to cerebellar tremor
produced by limb perturbations. J. Neurophysiol. 43: 279–291.
Vincent, P., and Marty, A. (1996). Fluctuations of inhibitory postsynaptic currents in Purkinje
cells from rat cerebellar slices. J. Physiol. 494: 183–199.
von der Malsburg, C. (1997). The coherence definition of consciousness. In M. Ito, Y. Miyashita
and E.T. Rolls (eds.), Cognition, Computation, and Consciousness. Oxford, England: Oxford
University Press, pp. 193–204.
Voogd, J., and Bigaré, F. (1980). Topographical distribution of olivary and cortico nuclear
fibers in the cerebellum: a review. In J. Courville, C. de Montigny and Y. Lamarre (eds.),
The Inferior Olivary Nucleus: Anatomy and Physiology. New York: Raven Press, pp.
207–234.
Voogd, J., Gerrits, N.M., and Ruigrok, T.J. (1996). Organization of the vestibulocerebellum.
Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 781: 553–579.
Voogd, J., and Glickstein, M. (1998). The anatomy of the cerebellum. Trends Neurosci. 21:
370–375.
Voogd, J., and Ruigrok, T.J.H. (1997). Transverse and longitudinal patterns in the mammalian
cerebellum. Progr. Brain Res. 114: 21–37.
Vos, B.P., Volny-Luraghi, A., Maex, R., and De Schutter, E. (2000). Precise spike timing of
tactile-evoked cerebellar Golgi cell responses: a reflection of combined mossy fiber and
parallel fiber activation? Progr. Brain Res. 124: 96–106.
Waespe, W., Cohen, B., and Raphan, T. (1985). Dynamic modification of the vestibulo-ocular
reflex by the nodulus and uvula. Science 228: 199–202.
Wang, J., Zhou, T., Qiu, M., Du, A., Cai, K., Wang, Z., Zhou, C., Meng, M., Zhuo, Y., Fan,
S., and Chen, L. (1999). Relationship between ventral stream for object vision and dorsal
stream for spatial vision: an fMRI + ERP study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8: 170–181.
Wang, J.-J., Shimansky, Y., Bracha, V., and Bloedel, J.R. (1998). Effects of cerebellar nuclear
inactivation on the learning of a complex forelimb movement in cats. J. Neurophysiol.
79: 2447–2459.
Warr, W.B. (1982). Parallel ascending pathways from the cochlear nucleus: Neuroanatomical
evidence of functional specialization. In W.D. Neff (ed.), Contributions to Sensory Physiol-
ogy, Vol. 7. New York: Academic Press, pp. 1–38.
Webster, D.B. (1992). An overview of mammalian auditory pathways with an emphasis on
humans. In D.B. Webster, A.N. Popper, and R. R. Fay (eds.), The Mammalian Auditory
Pathway: Neuroanatomy. New York: Springer, pp. 1–22.
Weiss, C., Houk, J.C., and Gibson, A.R. (1990). Inhibition of sensory responses of cat inferior
olive neurons produced by stimulation of red nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 64: 1170–1185.
Welsh, J.P., and Harvey, J.A. (1989). Cerebellar lesions and the nictitating membrane reflex:
performance deficits of the conditioned and unconditioned response. J. Neurosci. 9: 299–311.
Welsh, J.P., and Llinás, R. (1997). Some organizing principles for the control of movement
based on olivocerebellar physiology. Progr. Brain Res. 114: 449–461.
Welsh, J.P., Lang, E.J., Sugihara, I., and Llinás, R. (1995). Dynamic organization of motor
control within the olivocerebellar system. Nature 374: 453–457.
Werbos, P.J. (1975). Beyond Regression: New Tools for Prediction and Analysis in the Behav-
ioral Sciences. Ph.D. Dissertation, Harvard University, Boston, MA.
Bibliography 333
Werbos, P.J. (1990). Backpropagation through time: what it does and how to do it. Proc. IEEE
78: 1550–1560.
White, S.A. (1975). An adaptive recursive digital filter. In S.-P. Chen (ed.), Conference Record:
Ninth Annual Asilomar Conference on Circuits, Systems, and Computers (Nov. 3–5 1975),
North Hollywood, CA: Western Periodicals Co. pp. 21–25.
Widrow, B. (1963). A statistical theory of adaptation. In: F.P. Caruthers and H. Levenstein
(eds.), Adaptive Control Systems. New York: Macmillan, pp. 97–121.
Widrow, B., and Hoff, M.E., Jr. (1960). Adaptive switching circuits. IRE WESCON Convention
Record, 96–104.
Widrow, B., and Lehr, M.A. (1990). Thirty years of adaptive neural networks: perceptron,
madaline, and backpropagation. Proc. IEEE 78: 1415–1442.
Widrow, B., and Stearns, S.D. (1985). Adaptive Signal Processing. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Widrow, B., and Stearns, S.D. (1985). Introduction to adaptive arrays and adaptive beamform-
ing. In: Adaptive Signal Processing. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, pp. 368–408.
Widrow, B., Glover, J.R., McCool, J.M., Kaunitz, J., Williams, C.S., Hearn, R.H., Zeidler, J.R.,
Dong, E., and Goodlin, R.C. (1975). Adaptive noise cancelling: principles and applications.
Proc. IEEE 63: 1692–1716.
Widrow, B., Mantey, P.E., Griffiths, L.J., and Goode, B.B. (1967). Adaptive antenna systems.
Proc. IEEE 55: 2143–2159.
Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics or Control and Communication in the Animal and the Machine.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wiener, N. (1950). Extrapolation, Interpolation and Smoothing of Stationary Time Series.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/New York: John Wiley.
Wiener, N. (1961). Cybernetics or Control and Communication in the Animal and the Machine
(2nd ed.) Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wiener, S., and Berthoz, A. (1993). Forebrain structures mediating the vestibular contribu-
tion during navigation. In A. Berthoz (ed.), Multisensory Control of Movement. Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press, pp. 427–456.
Williams, R.J., and Zipser, D. (1989). A learning algorithm for continually running fully recur-
rent neural networks. Neural Comput. 1: 270–280.
Wittmann, M. (1999). Time perception and temporal processing levels of the brain. Chrono-
biol. Int. 16: 17–32.
Wolpert, D.M., and Kawato, M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor
control. Neural Netw. 11: 1317–1329.
Wolpert, D.M., Goodbody, S.J., and Husain, M. (1998). Maintaining internal representations:
the role of the human superior parietal lobe. Nat. Neurosci. 1: 529–533.
Wolpert, D.M., Miall, R.C., and Kawato, M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends
Cogn. Sci. 2: 338–347.
Wolpert, M., and Kawato, M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor
control. Neural Netw. 11: 1317–1329.
Wouterlood, F.G., and Mugnaini, E. (1984). Cartwheel neurons of the dorsal cochlear nucleus:
a Golgi-electron microscope study in rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 227: 136–157.
Wouterlood, F.G., Mugnaini, E., Osen, K.K., and Dahl, A.L. (1984). Stellate neurons in
rat dorsal cochlear nucleus studies with combined Golgi impregnation and electron mi-
croscopy: synaptic connections and mutual coupling by gap junctions. J. Neurocytol. 13:
634–664.
Wullimann M.F., and Northcutt, R.G. (1990). Visual and electrosensory circuits of the dien-
cephalon in mormyrids: an evolutionary perspective. J. Comp. Neurol. 297: 537–552.
Wylie, D.R., De Zeeuw, C.I., and Simpson, J.L. (1995). Temporal relations of the complex
spike activity of Purkinje cell pairs in the vestibulocerebellum of rabbits. J. Neurosci. 15:
2875–2887.
334 Bibliography
Yamamoto, K., Kobayashi, Y., Takemura, A., Kawano, K., and Kawato, M. (1997). A math-
ematical model that reproduces vertical ocular following responses from visual stimuli by
reproducing the simple spike firing frequency of Purkinje cells in the cerebellum. Neurosci.
Res. 29: 161–169.
Yanagihara, D., and Kondo, I. (1996). Nitric oxide plays a key role in adaptive control of
locomotion in cat. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 93: 13292–13297.
Yeo, C.H., and Hardiman, M.J. (1992). Cerebellar cortex and eyeblink conditioning. Exp.
Brain Res. 88: 623–638.
Yeo, C.H., and Hesslow, G. (1998). Cerebellum and conditioned reflexes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2:
322–330.
Young, E.D., Nelken, I., and Conley, R.A. (1995). Somatosensory effects on neurons in the
dorsal cochlear nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 73: 743–765.
Young, E.D., Spirou, G.A., Rice, J.J., and Voigt, H.F. (1992). Neural organization and responses
to complex stimuli in the dorsal cochlear nucleus. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.
336: 407–413.
Yuen, G.L., Hockberger, J.C., and Houk, J.C. (1995). Bistability in cerebellar Purkinje cell
dendrites modelled with high-threshold calcium and delayed-rectified potassium channels.
Biol. Cybern. 73: 375–388.
Zagrebelsky, M., Rossi, F., Hawkes, R., and Strata, P. (1996). Topographically organized climb-
ing fibre sprouting in the adult rat cerebellum. Eur. J. Neurosci. 8: 1051–1054.
Zipser, D., and Andersen, R.A. (1988). A back-propagation programmed network that simu-
lates response properties of a subset of posterior parietal neurons. Nature 331: 679–684.
Index
335
336 Index
smooth (predictive) pursuit of eye movements, evaluation of subtle differences in electric field
113–6 strengths, 154
spinocerebellum, 14, 16 foliation (folding) of, 8
constituent parts of, 16 function of, 152–6
fastigial and interposed nuclei and, 16 idealized tapped delay line, as, 289–90
functions, 16 in mormyrid fish, 148–56
“spiny branchlets” of Purkinje cells, 19 non-bifurcating granule cells in, 149
superior colliculus, 274 precise evaluation of brief time differences and,
as distributed controller of eye movements, 154
274 ridges constituted only of a molecular layer, 149
synaptic plasticity ventrolateral nucleus of thalamus
absence of in non-true cerebellar-like terminology of, 75
structures, 166 vermis, 9, 16
vestibular nuclei, 16
tapped delay line, 293, 294, 295 control of eye movements and, 16
teleosts, 8, 9 vestibular organ, 7
theories and models of the cerebellum vestibular system and inertial navigation systems,
nonadaptive, 170–89 8
origins of, 169 vestibulocerebellum (flocculonodular lobe), 12,
reviews and critiques of, 202 14, 16, 273
theory vs. model (of the cerebellum), 169 control of eye movements and, 16
three major cerebellar divisions from perspective functions, 16
of adaptive controller, 283–5 vermis, phylogeny of, and, 9
cerebrocerebellum, 285–6 vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR), 49, 273, 274
spinocerebellum, 284–5 alternatives to the floccular hypothesis, 104–5
vestibulocerebellum, 283–4 effects of cerebellectomy, 102
timing functions, 129–30 flocculus hypothesis (of Ito), 102–3
trajectories of single fibers long-term depression as mechanism for, 103
climbing, 28 mechanisms of, 102
mossy, 28 modeling with an adaptive controller, 274
multiple sites and mechanisms of plasticity for,
unipolar brush cells, 282 105–8
urodeles, 7 plasticity, site(s), and mechanisms, 102
potential model for cerebellar function, as, 101
valvula roles of mossy and climbing fibers in plasticity
adaptive signal processor and, 156 of, 108–9
association with electrosensory system of velocity-storage integrator and, 111
certain fish, 8 vestibulo-ocular system
association with lateral line organs, 8 phylogeny of, 12
body of the cerebellum (corpus cerebelli) vs.,
148 Wiener filtering, 206