Chapter 1
Chapter 1
Chapter 1
Neuroanatomy
The brain is divided into four main areas
Forebrain-
Ø Cerebrum divides into two hemispheres with four lobes (frontal, parietal,
temporal and occipital lobes)
Ø Internal capsule
Ø Basal ganglia (caudate nucleus, globus pallidus and putamen)
Ø Diencephalon (thalamus and hypothalamus)
Midbrain (brainstem)-
Ø Mesencephalon (midbrain).
Ø Pons.
Ø Medulla oblongata.
Hindbrain-
Ø Cerebellum
Ø Spinal medulla
Cerebrum-
Ø Conscious thought processes, intellectual functions
Ø Memory storage and processing
Ø Conscious and subconscious regulation of skeletal muscle contractions.
Thalamus-
Ø Relay and processing centers for sensory and motor information.
Hypothalamus-
Ø Centres controlling emotions, autonomic functions and hormone
production.
Ø Mesencephalo
Ø Processing of visual and auditory data
Ø Generation of reflexive somatic motor responses
Ø Maintenance of consciousness.
Pons-
Ø Relays sensory information to cerebellum and thalamus
Ø Subconscious somatic and visceral motor centers.
Medulla Oblongata-
Relays sensory information to thalamus and to other portions of the brain stem
Ø Autonomic centers for regulation of visceral function (cardiovascular,
respiratory and digestive system activities).
Cerebellum-
Ø Coordinates complex somatic motor patterns
Ø Adjusts output of other somatic motor centers in brain and spinal cord.
The arterial supply to the brain is from: The anterior circulation comprising two
internal carotid arteries which divide into two major arteries:
Ø Anterior cerebral artery.
Ø Middle cerebral artery.
The posterior circulation comprising two vertebral arteries which lead to:
Ø Posterior inferior cerebellar artery.
Ø Basilar artery.
Ø Posterior cerebral artery.
The anterior circulation can also be divided into right and left circulations, as there
is a carotid artery on each side. Because the vertebral arteries join quite low down
the brainstem, most of the posterior circulation is supplied by a single basilar
artery. If each of these circulations existed in isolation, then blockage of either
carotid or the basilar artery would result in extensive, life-threatening infarction.
This does not occur, however, because of anterior and posterior communicating
arteries which connect the brain arteries into an anatomical circle, known as the
circle of Willis. Thus when one vessel is blocked, an alternative (or collateral)
blood supply is available. There is considerable variation between individuals with
regard to how effective their communicating arteries are, and thus collateral
circulation, is. Thus carotid, or basilar, occlusion can result in life-threatening
stroke for some individuals and will pass unnoticed by others. In reality, most
patients exist on a spectrum somewhere between these extremes. This explains
why a given vascular abnormality, for example, carotid occlusion, will result in
different severities of stroke in different patients.
CHAPTER 2
ACUTE NEURO/NEUROSURGICAL CARE
-----
An accurate description of the patient’s neurological symptoms is an important aid
in establishing the diagnosis; but this must be taken in conjunction with
information from other systems, previous medical history, family and social history
and current medication. Often the patient’s history requires confirmation from a
relative or friend. The following outline indicates the relevant information to obtain
for each symptom, although some may require further clarification. Onset (sudden,
gradual) Precipitating factors (stooping, coughing) Relieving factors (analgesics)
Site, Severity , Character (aching, throbbing).
Neurological disease may produce systemic signs and systemic disease may affect
the nervous system. A complete general examination must therefore accompany
that of the central nervous system. In particular, note the following: Temperature,
Evidence of weight loss, Septic source, e.g. teeth, ears. Blood pressure Breast
lumps Skin marks, e.g. rashes , Neck stiffness. Lymphadenopathy, café-au-lait
spots, Pulse irregularity Hepatic and splenic angiomata, Carotid bruit enlargement,
Anterior fontanelle in baby Cardiac murmurs, Prostatic irregularity Head
circumference Cyanosis/respiratory insufficiency.
EXAMINATION – CONSCIOUS LEVEL ASSESSMENT
Orientated – Knows place, e.g. Southern General Hospital and time, e.g. day,
month and year
None
Obeys commands
Localizing to pain
Apply a painful stimulus to the supraorbital nerve, e.g. rub thumb nail in the
supraorbital groove, increasing pressure until a response is obtained. If the patient
responds by bringing the hand up beyond the chin = ‘localizing to pain’. (Pressure
to nail beds or sternum at this stage may not differentiate ‘localizing’ from
‘flexing’.)
Flexing to pain
Extending to pain
MEMORY TEST
REMOTE memory – Ask about events and circumstances occurring more than 5
years previously.
VERBAL memory – Ask a patient to remember a sentence or a short story and test
after 15 minutes.
VISUAL memory – Ask patients to remember objects on a tray and test after 15
minutes.
Test patient with two-step calculations, e.g. ‘I wish to buy 12 articles at 7 pence
each. How much change will I receive from £1?’
The examiner must compare patient’s present reasoning ability with expected
abilities base
Cranial Nerve Examination
OLFACTORY NERVE (I)
Test both perception and identification using aromatic non-irritant materials that
avoid stimulation of trigeminal nerve fibers in the nasal mucosa, e.g. soap, tobacco.
One nostril is closed while the patient.
Light? Severe deficit – Can a patient see movement? Visual acuity: Can a patient
count fingers? Mild deficit – Record reading acuity with wall or hand chart.
N.B. Refractive error (i.e. inadequate focusing on the retina, e.g. hyper-metropia,
myopia) can be overcome
by testing reading acuity through a pinhole. This concentrates a thin beam of vision
on the macula.
Visual fields
In the temporal portion of the visual field the physiological blind spot may be
detected. A 2 mm object should disappear here.
Test pain (pin prick) sensation temperature (cold object or hot/cold tubes) light
touch Compare each side. Map out the sensory deficit, testing from the abnormal to
the normal region.
Corneal reflex Test corneal sensation by touching with a wisp of wet cotton wool.
A blink response should occur bilaterally.
Jaw jerk
Ask the patient to open mouth and relax jaw. Place finger on the chin and tap with
hammer: Slight jerk – normal
Cochlear component
Test by whispering numbers into one ear while masking hearing in the other ear by
occluding and rubbing the external meatus. If hearing is impaired, examine
external meatus and the tympanic membrane with auroscope to exclude wax or
infection. Differentiate conductive (middle ear) deafness from perceptive (nerve)
deafness by:
1. Weber’s test: Hold base of tuning fork (512 Hz) against the vertex.
These nerves are considered jointly since they are examined together and their
actions are seldom individually impaired.
Note patient’s voice – if there is vocal cord paresis (X nerve palsy), voice may be
high pitched. (Vocal cord examination is best left to an ENT specialist.) Note any
swallowing difficulty or nasal regurgitation of fluids. Ask the patient to open their
mouth and say ‘Ah’. Note any asymmetry of palatal movements (X nerve palsy).
Uvula swings due to unopposed muscle action on one side. Gag reflex Depresses
the patient’s tongue and touches the palate, pharynx or tonsil on one side until the
patient ‘gags’. Compare sensitivity on each side (afferent route – IX nerve) and
observe symmetry of palatal contraction (efferent route– X nerve).
ACCESSORY NERVE (XI)
Sternomastoid
Ask the patient to rotate head against resistance. Compare power and muscle bulk
on each side. Also compare each side with the patient pulling head forward against
resistance. N.B. The left sternomastoid turns the head to the right and vice versa.
Trapezius
Ask patients to ‘shrug’ shoulders and to hold them in this position against
resistance. Compare power on each side. Patient should manage to resist any effort
to depress
Ask the patient to open mouth; inspect tongue. Look for – evidence of atrophy
(increased folds, wasting) fibrillation (small wriggling movements). Ask the
patient to protrude tongue. Note any difficulty or deviation. (N.B. apparent
deviation may occur with facial weakness if present; assess tongue in relation to
teeth.) Protruded tongue deviates towards the side of weakness. Non protruded
tongue cannot move to the opposite side. Dysarthria and dysphagia are minimal.
Appearance
Ensure that the patient is relaxed, and assess tone by alternately flexing and
extending the elbow or wrist.
Power
Muscle weakness. The degree of weakness is ‘scored’ using the MRC (Medical
Research Council) scale.
Score 0 – No contraction
Score 1 – Flicker
SENSATION
Pain
Pin prick with a sterile pin provides a simple method of testing this important
modality. Firstly, check that the patient detects the pin as ‘sharp’, i.e. painful, then
rapidly test each dermatome in turn. Memorizing the dermatome distribution is
simplified by noting that ‘C7’ extends down the middle finger.
If pin prick is impaired, then more carefully map out the extent of the abnormality,
moving from the abnormal to the normal areas.
Light touch
Temperature
Temperature testing
Hold the sides of the patient’s finger or thumb and demonstrate ‘up and down’
movements. Repeat with the patient’s eyes closed. Ask the patient to specify the
direction of movement. Ask the patient, with eyes closed, to touch his nose with
his forefinger or to bring forefingers together
Vibration
Place a vibrating tuning fork (usually 128 c/s) on a bony prominence, e.g. radius.
Ask the patient to indicate when the vibration, if felt, ceases. If impaired, move
more proximally and repeat. Vibration testing is of value in the early detection of
demyelinating disease and peripheral neuropathy, but otherwise is of limited
benefit. If the above sensory functions are normal and a cortical lesion is
suspected, it is useful to test for the following:
Ø Two point discrimination: the ability to discrimination.
Ø Two point discrimination: the ability to discriminate two blunt points
when simultaneously applied to the finger, 5 mm apart (cf, 4 cm in the legs).
Ø Sensory inattention (perceptual rivalry): the ability to detect stimuli (pin
prick or touch) in both limbs, when applied to both limbs simultaneously.
Ø Stereognosis: the ability to recognise objects placed in the hand.
Ø Graphesthesia: the ability to recognise numbers or letters traced out on the
patient.
REFLEXES
Hold the sides of the patient’s finger or thumb and demonstrate ‘up and down’
movements. Repeat with the patient’s eyes closed. Ask the patient to specify the
direction of movement. Ask the patient, with eyes closed, to touch his nose with
his forefinger or to bring forefingers together with the arms outstretched. Supinator
jerks C6, C7 roots. Radial nerve
CO-ORDINATION
CO-ORDINATION
Ask the patient to repeatedly run the heel from the opposite knee down the shin to
the big toe. Look for ATAXIA (in coordination). Ask the patient to repeatedly tap
the floor with the foot. Note: any DYSDIADOCHOKINESIA (difficulty with
rapidly alternating.
GAIT
Note:
Ø Length of step and width of base Normal
Ø Abnormal leg movements (e.g. excessively high step)
Ø Instability (gait ataxia)
Ø Associated postural movements (e.g. pelvic swinging.
CHAPTER 3
LESIONS OF PYRAMIDAL SYSTEM.
NEUROANATOMY, PYRAMIDAL TRACT LESIONS
Introduction
The central nervous system (CNS) is a complex network of components that allow
an organism to interact with its environment. It is made up of multiple different
parts, each of which plays a different role. Primarily, the CNS is formed by the
upper motor neurons (UMN) which carry signals for movement down to the lower
motor neurons (LMN) which signal the muscles to either contract or relax. The
UMN further subdivides into multiple tracts, each of which has specific functions
within the body. Specifically, the pyramidal tract is the main pathway that carries
signals for voluntary movement. Lesions to the pyramidal tract can lead to
devastating consequences such as spasticity, hyperactive reflexes, weakness, and a
Babinski sign (stroking the sole of the foot causes the big toe to move upward).
These symptoms are all characteristic of an upper motor neuron lesion. However,
certain symptoms are specific to a pyramidal tract lesion.
Structure and Function
The pyramidal tracts are part of the UMN system and are a system of different
nerve fibers that carry signals from the cerebral cortex to either the brainstem or
the spinal cord. It divides into two tracts: the corticospinal tract and the
corticobulbar tract. The corticospinal tract consists of neurons that synapse on the
spinal cord controlling movements in the limbs and trunk. It originates in multiple
areas of the brain, mainly in the primary motor cortex (Brodmann area 4) and in
premotor areas (Brodmann area 6). However, it can also originate from the
somatosensory cortex, cingulate gyrus, and the parietal lobe. From here, it will
descend through the corona radiata, internal capsule, cerebral peduncles, pons, and
upper medulla. Once it reaches the lower medulla, about 85 to 90% of the fibers
will cross over or “decussate” at the pyramidal decussation to form the lateral
corticospinal tract (LCST). They continue their descent in the lateral funiculus and
terminate at all levels of the spinal cord. A few of these fibers that are responsible
for fine motor function such as controlling finger and hand movement will synapse
directly on lower motor neurons. However, most will terminate in lower motor
neuron “pools” (groups of interneurons that process and integrate the information
before passing it on to the lower motor neurons). At the pyramidal decussation, the
10 to 15% of fibers that did not decussate will continue down uncrossed as the
anterior corticospinal tract (ACST). These fibers are involved in controlling
proximal muscles such as those in the trunk. Typically lesions of the ACST tend to
have a minimal clinical effect. The pyramidal decussation is a critical concept to
understand. Because of the crossing over of the fibers, the location of the lesion
will determine which side the symptoms will arise. Lesions above the decussation
will cause symptoms on the contralateral side of the body, whereas lesions below
the decussation (typically the spinal cord) will cause symptoms on the ipsilateral
side. The corticobulbar tract synapses on the cranial nerves controlling muscles of
the face, head, and neck. It originates in the frontal lobe’s primary motor cortex
and follows a similar path to the corticospinal tract. It descends through the corona
radiata and the internal capsule. They will then exit and synapse directly on the
lower motor neurons of cranial nerves. The fibers of the corticobulbar tract
bilateral innervation of almost every cranial nerve except for cranial nerves VII and
XII, which are innervated by the contralateral cortex. What this means is that a
corticobulbar tract lesion on the left side of the face will cause weakness of the
right side. However, since every other cranial nerve except for VII and XII are
innervated bilaterally (both the left and right hemispheres), lesions to both sides of
the corticobulbar tract will need to occur for symptoms to appear.
The pyramidal tract, specifically the corticospinal tract, spans an incredibly long
distance along the body. Damage to specific vascular structures can lead to damage
to the tract. The pyramidal tract originates in the primary motor cortex. The
primary motor cortex for the face and upper extremities receive blood from the
middle cerebral artery (MCA) while the primary motor cortex for the lower
extremities receives blood from the anterior cerebral artery (ACA). An occlusion
of either of these arteries can lead to weakness in the associated extremities. As the
corticospinal tract passes down, it will go through the corona radiata and internal
capsule, which are innervated by the lenticulostriate arteries (branches of the
MCA). The occlusion of these arteries will lead to the contralateral weakness of
both upper and lower extremities. As the corticospinal tract passes down into the
brainstem, it gets supplied by the basilar artery. The blockage of blood here can
result in a variety of symptoms ranging from isolated nerve palsies to tetraplegia or
death.
Surgical Considerations
Clinical Significance
Pyramidal tract lesions can occur from any type of damage to the brain or spinal
cord. They can result from a variety of injuries and diseases such as strokes,
abscesses, tumors, hemorrhage, meningitis, multiple sclerosis, or trauma. Damage
to the corticospinal tract will present similarly to upper motor lesion syndrome and
will present with symptoms such as spasticity, clonus, hyperreflexia, and Babinski
sign. Damage to the corticobulbar tract can present with pseudobulbar palsy or
damage to cranial nerves VII or X.
Pseudobulbar Palsy
The corticobulbar tract bilaterally innervates most of the cranial nerves, except VII
and XII, which means that for symptoms to arise from damage to these nerves,
both sides of the corticobulbar tract must be injured as is the case in pseudobulbar
palsy. Symptoms in this condition may include slow speech, dysphagia (difficulty
swallowing), dysarthria (difficulty speaking), spastic tongue, and pseudobulbar
affect (uncontrollable episodes of laughing or crying).
Unilateral lesions to either of these nerves will cause contralateral symptoms. Since
cranial nerve VII innervates muscles of the lower face, damage to this nerve will
cause deviation of angle of mouth towards the opposite side of the lesion due to the
over action of the muscles of the opposite side. Similarly, damage to cranial nerve
X will lead to the deviation of the uvula to the opposite side of the lesion.
Other Issues
There are a wide variety of pathologies associated with pyramidal tract lesions.
They can be the result of many diseases including stroke, amyotrophic lateral
sclerosis, multiple sclerosis, and central pontine myelinolysis.
Stroke
Multiple Sclerosis
Multiple sclerosis is a demyelinating autoimmune disease of the nervous system.
Its manifestations show a wide range of symptoms such as double vision, muscle
weakness, coordination trouble, or cognitive disturbance. It is the most common
CNS autoimmune disorder, and there currently is no cure. Management centers on
improving function after an attack, and preventing recurrent attacks.
CHAPTER 4
Strokes are usually classified by the mechanism and location of the vascular
damage. The two broad causes are ischemia and hemorrhage. Ischemic strokes
result from a blockage of a cerebral vessel and can further be categorized as caused
by thrombosis or embolism. Thrombosis is the stenosis or occlusion of a vessel,
usually as a result of atherosclerosis. This occlusion is typically a gradual process,
often with preceding warning signs, such as transient ischemic attack (TIA). An
embolism is dislodged platelets, cholesterol, or other material that forms at another
location, travels in the bloodstream, and blocks a cerebral vessel. Ischemic strokes
are the most common type, accounting for about 87% of strokes (American Stroke
Association, 2011). Hemorrhagic strokes result from a rupture of a weakened
cerebral blood vessel. In such strokes, blood accumulates outside of the vascular
space and compresses surrounding brain tissue. Hemorrhagic strokes are either
intracerebral (bleeding into the brain itself) or subarachnoid (bleeding into an area
surrounding the brain). Aneurysms and arteriovenous malformations are the most
common types of weakened blood vessels causing hemorrhagic strokes (American
Stroke Association, 2011). Hemorrhagic strokes are less common (an estimated
13% of strokes), but they result in a higher mortality rate than ischemic strokes
(Roger et al., 2011).
Location of Involvement
Most lesions are either anterior circulation strokes, which present signs and
symptoms of hemispheric dysfunction, or posterior circulation strokes, which
display signs and symptoms of brainstem involvement (Simon, Greenberg, &
Aminoff, 2009). Another distinction related to location of CVA is whether the
lesion results from large-vessel or small-vessel disease. Thrombosis occurs most
often in the large cerebral blood vessels. Small-vessel, or lacunar, strokes are very
small infarctions that occur only where small arterioles branch off the larger
vessels (National Institutes of Health, 2011).
INCIDENCE
Stroke is the third leading cause of death in the United States and a leading cause
of chronic disability among adults. Of the estimated 795,000 persons who have
first or recurrent CVAs in the United States each year, approximately two-thirds
survive, bringing the number of stroke survivors in the U.S. population at any one
time to over nine million (American Stroke Association, 2011). Stroke is the most
common neurological diagnostic category among patients seen by occupational
therapists (National Board for Certification in Occupational Therapy, 2008). The
projected aging of the U.S. population is expected to raise the incidence of stroke,
because about two-thirds of all strokes occur in those older than age 65 years
(National Institutes of Health, 2011).
MEDICAL MANAGEMENT
Acute stroke care focuses on determining the cause and site of the stroke,
preventing progression of the lesion, reducing cerebral edema, preventing
secondary medical complications, and treating acute neurological symptoms
(Bartels, 2011). Techniques of diagnostic imaging, including computed
tomography (CT) and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), can distinguish
ischemic from hemorrhagic lesions and define their location, size, and vascular
territory (National Institutes of Health, 2011). In acute ischemic stroke, treatment
concerns include restoration of blood flow and limitation of neuronal damage.
Medications, including antithrombotics and thrombolytics, are the most common
medical intervention for stroke (National Institutes of Health, 2011).
Antithrombotics include antiplatelet drugs (such as aspirin) and anticoagulation
drugs (such as heparin) and are used to prevent further clotting or thrombosis.
After stroke, physicians mainly prescribe antithrombotics for prevention, because
first time stroke survivors are at a high risk for recurrent strokes (National
Institutes of Health, 2011). Thrombolytic drugs, such as tissue plasmino-gen
activator (tPA), help reestablish blood flow to the brain by dissolving clots in
cerebral vessels. A 5-year trial conducted by the National Institute of Neurological
Disorders and Stroke found that appropriately selected subjects treated with TPA
within 3 hours of onset of stroke symptoms were at least 33% more likely than
patients given a placebo to recover from their stroke with little or no disability
(National Stroke Association, 2011). The use of thrombolytics is limited, however,
by the associated increased risk of hemorrhage and the fact that they must be
administered within a 3-hour window after stroke onset for effectiveness. It is
estimated that only 3%–5% of per-sons suffering a stroke will seek medical
attention in time for possible TPA treatment (American Stroke Association, 2011).
With hemorrhagic stroke, acute treatment includes control of intracranial pressure,
prevention of rebleeding, maintenance of cerebral perfusion, and control of
vasospasm. Surgery or endovascular procedures are often recommended to limit
damage (National Stroke Association, 2011).
The specific abilities lost or affected by stroke and the degree and time course of
recovery from stroke vary with the location, type, and extent of the initial injury,
and treatment provided (National Stroke Association, 2011). Early initial
improvement or spontaneous recovery occurs because pathologic processes in the
brain resolve, and neurotransmission resumes near and remote from an infarct or
hemorrhage (Dobkin, 2005). Later, ongoing improvement occurs with structural
and functional reorganization within the brain, or neuroplasticity; the term
neuroplasticity refers to transiently achieved functional changes in the context of
learning and recovery, as well as structural changes on the cellular level.
Neuroplasticity includes greater excitability and recruitment of intact neurons in
both hemispheres of the brain as a response to stimulation, participation, training,
and experience (Albert & Kesselring, 2011). Langton Hewer (1990) described a
model of stroke recovery that includes both intrinsic neurological and
compensatory adaptive recovery.
Neurological Impairment
Neurological impairments following stroke and describe the possible effects of
each on occupational functioning. The most typical manifestation of CVA is
hemiparesis or hemiplegia, ranging from mild weakness to complete paralysis on
the side of the body opposite the site of the CVA. About 80% of people who have
had a stroke have some degree of hemiparesis (National Stroke Association, 2011),
although the rate of physical impairment usually decreases after the first 3–6
months post stroke (Roger et al., 2011). Certain impairments are associated with
lesions in a particular hemisphere. For example, left CVA may cause right
hemiparesis, aphasia or other communication deficits, and/or apraxia or motor
planning deficits. Right CVA may result in left hemiparesis, visual field deficits
(VFDs) or spatial neglect, poor insight and judgment, and/or impulsive behavior
(American Stroke Association, 2011). Many patients do not regain full movement
or function of the upper extremity. Studies report that 65% of individuals after
stroke are unable to incorporate their hemiparetic upper extremity into daily
function (Lum et al., 2009). Historically, motor recovery in the patient with
hemiparesis was described as progressing from proximal to distal movement and
from mass, patterned, undifferentiated movement to selective, coordinated
movement (Brunnstrom, 1970; Fugl-Meyer et al., 1975). Therapists now rarely see
such a distinct progression of motor recovery in patients, partly because
thrombolytic agents limit brain damage and partly because insurance restrictions
limit long-term rehabilitation services.
ASSESSMENT
Care: A Clinical Practice Guideline (Duncan et al., 2005). The AOTA guideline
lists both standardized assessments and observationally based assessments
commonly used by occupational therapists with stroke survivors. The AHA/ASA
guideline lists recommended well-validated standardized measures for all
disciplines and stages of stroke care. The use of standardized evaluation tools helps
ensure reliable documentation, achieve consistency of treatment decisions,
facilitate team communication, and monitor progress for each survivor (Duncan et
al., 2005). Often, multiple assessment instruments must be used because of the
wide variation in individual stroke manifestations and the patient’s changing needs
over the course of recovery. Therapists should be familiar with commonly used
standardized stroke deficit scales because they are frequently used as an
interdisciplinary summary of baseline function and as indicators of recovery or
treatment outcomes. The National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale (NIHSS) (Brott
et al., 1989) is a brief, well-validated tool that can be administered by physicians,
nurses, or therapists. Items scored include consciousness, vision, extraocular
movements, facial palsy, limb strength, ataxia, sensation, speech, and language.
Although no single measure can fully describe or predict the diverse picture of
stroke disability and recovery, the NIHSS is widely used as an initial assessment
tool and for planning discharge disposition (Kasner, 2006).
Assessment of Areas of Occupation
The patient’s ability to perform the self-care, leisure, vocational, home, and
community tasks that he or she hopes to continue is evaluated by observation
rather than report because there can be a difference between what a patient can do
and actually does. Evaluation to determine a patient’s level of occupational
functioning is administered early to predict answers to the following questions
(Wade, 1992): (1) Where will the patient live, and what physical adaptations will
be necessary? (2) How much and what type of assistance will the patient need? (3)
What roles will the patient be able to fulfill, and how will he or she spend his or
her time? A patient’s ADL performance in a structured clinical setting may not
indicate performance at home. For example, patients who can put on and remove
clothing during therapy sessions may not be able to find and retrieve their clothes
in a cluttered closet, select clothing appropriate for the weather, or initiate the
dressing routine without prompting (Campbell et al., 1991). Conversely, a patient
may be unable to master simple meal preparation in the unfamiliar environment of
a clinic kitchen but may re-adapt easily to this task upon returning home. A home
evaluation can help determine what resources and means a patient has to achieve
independence in areas of occupation as well as assessing safety and accessibility
(see
CHAPTER 5
INTRODUCTION AND APPLICATION OF
APPROACHES/FORS USED IN NEUROLOGICAL
REHABILITATION SENSORIMOTOR TREATMENT
APPROACHES
INTRODUCTION
Sensory integration (SI) theory is a dynamic and ecological theory that specifies
the critical influence of sensory processing on human development and function.
This theory emphasizes a person’s ability to appropriately process sensory
information from the body and integrate it with information about what is going on
around the person so that he or she can effectively act on the environment. It
contributes to our understanding of how sensation affects learning,
social-emotional development, and neurophysiological processes such as motor
performance, attention, and arousal. Occupational therapists commonly use
sensory integration theory in practice as a frame of reference that can be used to
both assess and intervene with people who have sensory integrative or sensory
processing dysfunction that adversely affects function (Parham & Mailloux, 2005).
It is important to be clear what one is referring to when using the term sensory
integration, as this singular term has been used to refer to a theory, a neurological
process, a disorder, and an intervention approach (Bundy, 2002; Mulligan, 2003).
The theoretical principles that support the use of this frame of reference draw on
evidence from neuroscience as Dr. Ayres recognized that behavior and emotions
are regulated by brain mechanisms. Advances in neuroscience continue to expand
and support Dr. Ayres’ original hypotheses, contributing to evidence-based practice
(Bauman, 2005; Schneider, 2005). Dynamic systems theories have emerged that
provide an under- standing of the complexity of development (Thelen & Smith,
1994) and help to explain the complexity of the way in which this intervention
affects change in children (Spitzer, 1999). SI is now commonly applied in
educational settings with children who have learning and behavior disorders
(American Occupational Therapy Association, 2003), as well as in private settings.
While education in sensory integration theory and intervention principles is part of
the entry-level curricula for occupational therapists and occupational therapy
assistants, postgraduate training is recommended for those specializing in the use
of SI intervention in clinical practice. Occupational therapists with sensory
integration certification and mentorship with a master clinician are best prepared to
evaluate and provide SI intervention. Occupational therapy assistants may provide
intervention using SI principles with appropriate supervision by an occupational
therapist.
CHAPTER 6
Dr. Ayres developed a theory of sensory integration to explain the link between the
nervous system and behavior. Five basic assumptions formed the theoretical basis
for Dr. Ayres’ thinking:
1. The remarkable potential for change of the developing brain, or
neuroplasticity, which has recently been found to be true of the adult brain as
well (for reviews, see Buonomano & Merzenich, 1998; Cruikshank &
Weinberger, 1996; Gross, 2000)
2. Interactions between the “higher-order” (cortical) areas of the brain and
those in the “lower” subcortical areas as fundamental to adequate sensory
integration
3. Neurophysiological development of sensory integrative functions that
occur in a natural order and following basic sequence
4. An adaptive response, which is “the ability to adjust one’s action upon
environmental demand” and promotes a higher level of integration as a
consequence of the feedback to the central nervous system
5. Presence of an inner drive to meet and master a challenge, which fosters
the development of sensory integration
Neuroplasticity
Dr. Ayres’ appreciation of the importance of experience as a major determinant of
cortical organization and resulting function remains a bedrock principle in the field
of neuroscience today. This “experience-dependent plasticity in the cerebral cortex
reflects the importance of learning in our mental life and behaviors” (E. K. Miller,
2000, p. 1067). During the period when Dr. Ayres studied neuroplasticity,
researchers thought that the brain’s ability to be modified was robust during
childhood but, after a critical period of development, became quite limited. It is
now known that the brain has the capacity to be modified throughout the life
course (Bear, Connors, & Paradiso, 2006; Gilbert & Wiesel, 1992).
Viewing development as a process that the brain undergoes, Dr. Ayres noted that
“each child’s brain is designed to follow an orderly, predictable, interrelated
sequence of development that results in the capacity for learning” Given an
enriched, supportive environment, children will grow and develop sensory and
motor memories that help the children adapt to their own growth and interests in
the context of an ever-changing environment. Whereas Dr. Ayres focused on the
first seven years of life as the time frame in which this occurred, we now know that
the brain continues to develop throughout the life course (Bear et al., 2006; Gilbert
& Wiesel, 1992). A critical aspect of this process is that the child experiences
sensations, which places a demand on the brain to organize the incoming stimuli
into percepts. Depending on the context in which the stimuli are experienced, the
child might focus on and attend to the input (sensory detection as a central nervous
system process; sensory registration as inferred from the child’s behavior), or if
they are not relevant, the stimuli might be ignored (inhibition centrally, sensory
modulation behaviorally) (Lane, Miller, & Hanft, 2000; Miller & Lane, 2000). For
example, a child who is riding a bicycle typically attends to visual and auditory
inputs along the ride (sensory registration) while ignoring the feel of the shirt as it
is blown by the wind (inhibition). If the child’s brain is unable to organize
incoming sensation efficiently, these “filtering out” and “attending to” processes
might be inadequate. In this example, the child who is unable to tolerate the tactile
input from the shirt’s movement against the body will have difficulty attending to
the important visual and auditory information in the environment that are necessary
for both safety and skill. Until the tactile sensation is discriminated as “safe,” the
child is considered to remain in a more primitive “fight- or-flight” mode. In
general, this primitive state under- mines both the development of skills and
emotions as it is through the ability of the brain to organize sensations that “the
child gains control over his emotions” (Ayres,
Adaptive Response
Our ability to make adaptive responses to ever-changing environmental demands
and challenges allows us to learn something new and, in turn, change the
environment (Ayres, 1972, 1979, 2005). Spitzer (1999) discusses the congruence
between the concepts of self-organization in dynamic systems theory with Dr.
Ayres’ concept of the adaptive response in sensory integration theory. In both
theories, feedback from the individual’s spontaneous, active adjustments
contributes to self-organization of the brain (Smith & Thelen, 1993; Ayres, 1979,
2005). Dr. Ayres stressed the importance of “organizing adaptive responses to
increasing complexity” as a key component of intervention. She was guided by
neuroscience research suggesting that inefficiency in synaptic activity along
anatomical pathways might be contributing to poor integration of sensory
information and proposed that focusing on eliciting a response that was not yet
well developed might enhance synaptic function (Ayres, 1972; Katz & Shatz, 1996;
Schlaug, 2001). While an adaptive response occurs most typically during a motor
task, it is also apparent with demands arising in other domains (e.g., emotional
regulation, cognitive, and social interaction). “Therapy using a sensory integration
approach is a natural process”
Postural Adaptation
Postural adaptation, or postural control, refers to the individual’s ongoing ability to
achieve, maintain, or restore an upright position against gravity (balance) for
stability during activities or changes in body position (Pollock et al., 2000). The
recognition and treatment of deficits in postural adaptation constitute an important
aspect of therapy for stroke patients because so many daily living tasks (e.g.,
putting on socks, getting in and out of a bathtub, housework, and participating in
sports) depend on this skill. Evaluation and treatment limited to a patient securely
supported in bed or in a wheelchair fail to address most usual daily tasks that
require dealing with gravity. A person with hemiplegia typically has decreased
motor control, poor bilateral and sensory integration, and impaired automatic
postural responses (Oliveira et al., 2008). As a result, the patient must devote
increased effort to remaining upright, with decreased ability to focus on purposeful
tasks. When engaging in a challenging activity, the hemiplegic patient often resorts
to compensatory strategies to help maintain stability, such as using upper
extremities for support (Oliveira et al., 2008). Postural adaptation factors and skills
can best be observed during the performance of meaningful functional activities,
although the Berg Balance Scale.
Occupational therapists are the clinicians most often involved with the evaluation
and treatment of motor deficits in the hemiplegic or hemiparetic upper extremity.
Achievement of skilled arm and hand function is a complex, often difficult process
following stroke and involves interaction of several body functions and structures.
Evaluation of the involved upper extremity should address sensation; the
mechanical and physiological deterrents to movement; the presence and degree of
active or voluntary movement; the quality of this movement, including strength,
endurance, and coordination; and the extent of function resulting from movement.
Somatosensory Assessment
Factors that can interfere with movement and function of the hemiplegic upper
extremity include limitations in PROM, joint mal alignment, abnormal muscle
tone, and pain. Interview and medical records can help determine whether these
conditions resulted from the stroke or were present prior to onset. Passive
movement restrictions in the joints and soft tissues of the extremity may result
from an individual’s anatomy and lifestyle or from premorbid conditions such as
arthritis or injury. Limitations may result more directly from the stroke, with
sudden and prolonged immobilization of joints caused by weakness or spasticity in
muscles. Persistent stereotyped positioning of joints without counteracting
movement results in the shortening and eventual contracture of muscles, tendons,
and ligaments. Edema secondary to reduced circulation and loss of muscle action
can further limit passive joint motion, particularly in the hand. Goniometric
measurement of passive range of motion (PROM) is usually not indicated unless
treatment is specifically aimed at increasing passive motion, such as when trying to
eliminate an elbow flexion contracture. More useful in assessing patients with
stroke is a comparison of the involved to the uninvolved arm to determine probable
baseline joint motion. Shoulder subluxation, or mal alignment of the glenohumeral
joint, occurs in approximately 50% of stroke patients (Yu, 2009). This condition is
probably caused by the weight of the arm pulling down the humerus when the
supraspinatus and deltoid muscles are weak and by weakness of scapular muscles
that allows the glenoid cavity to rotate downward (Stein & Brandstater, 2010).
Shoulder subluxation can be identified by palpation: the seated patient’s arm hangs
freely with trunk stabilized while the examiner palpates the sub acromial space for
the separation between the acromion and the head of the humerus. The distance
separating the two is measured in finger widths, that is, the number of fingers that
can be inserted in the space (Yu, 2009).The role of subluxation in the painful
shoulder is controversial; in the shoulder, adhesive capsulitis, tendonitis, bursitis,
rotator cuff tear, traction/compression neuropathy, and complex regional pain
syndrome (CRPS) are common complications of hemiparesis, and all can result in
pain and limited range of motion (ROM) (Duncan et al., 2005). Spasticity, defined
as velocity-dependent hyperactivity of tonic stretch reflexes, can result in ROM
limitations and pain, often leading to contractures and functional impairments. The
Ashworth Scale/Modified Ashworth Scale (Bohannon & Smith, 1987) is included
in the AOTA practice guideline as a recommended assessment of spasticity (see
Chapter 8; see Chapter 3 for descriptions of pain evaluation).
Voluntary Movement
Determining the amount and quality of voluntary movement a patient can produce
is one of the first steps in assessing movement potential (Warren, 1991). The
patterns of motion available are different for each stroke patient. Movement can
change dramatically or subtly with time; hence, it requires careful reassessment
throughout recovery. Factors to consider when evaluating motor control of the
involved upper extremity include the following:
Ø Can the patient perform reflexive but not voluntary movement? Example:
Patient demonstrates active el-bow extension in the involved arm when
balance is disturbed (equilibrium reaction) or flexes the hemi paretic elbow
while yawning (associated reaction) but can perform these movement on
request Do proximal segments (neck, trunk, shoulder, hip) stabilize as
needed to provide firm support for movement of the distal parts, or do they
substitute for distal movement? Example: A patient can raise his hemi
paretic arm only with pronounced lateral bending of the trunk and excessive
elevation of the shoulder girdle.
Ø Can voluntary movement be performed unassisted against gravity, or is it
possible only with assistance in the form of positioning, support, or
facilitation? Example: A patient can bring her hand to her mouth only by
flexing her elbow in a horizontal plane with gravity eliminated.
Ø Can voluntary movement be performed in an isolated fashion or only in a
synergistic pattern? Example: A patient can reach for an object on a table
only with a pat-tern of shoulder abduction, elbow flexion, and trunk flexion
rather than with the more efficient pattern of shoulder flexion and elbow
extension.
Ø Can reciprocal movement (the ability to perform agonist/antagonist
motion in succession in an individual joint) be performed with practical
speed and precision? Examples: A patient cannot produce a smooth pattern
of elbow extension-flexion-extension needed to grasp a glass, take a drink,
and set it back on the table but can perform each movement separately. A
patient cannot perform the rapid alternating movements necessary to brush
teeth.
One of the major movement difficulties following stroke is attaining the capacity
and ability to isolate and control single muscle actions and combine them in a
pat-tern appropriate for the task at hand. In motor patterns typical in hemiplegia,
movement initiated in one joint results in automatic contraction of other muscles
linked in synergy with that movement. This results in limited, stereotyped
movement patterns rather than adaptive, selective motions. In Brunnstrom’s theory
of hemiplegic limb synergies (Brunnstrom, 1970), typical stereotyped patterns are
described as flexor or extensor synergy patterns according to the motion at the
elbow. The flexor synergy presents with scapular retraction and/or elevation,
shoulder abduction and external rotation, elbow flexion, and forearm pronation.
The extensor synergy presents with scapular protraction, shoulder horizontal
adduction and internal rotation, elbow extension, and forearm pronation. Wrist and
hand position varies (Brunnstrom, 1970). There is considerable variation in
synergistic patterning, and other causes of abnormal stereotyped patterns include
compensatory movements, unnecessary movement, muscle tension resulting from
exertion or stress, and movement in response to gravity (e.g., pronation).
According to Brunnstrom (1970), movement recovery after stroke is determined by
an individual’s ability to move independently of synergies. More contemporary
clinical studies suggest that in addition to pathological limb synergies, loss of
strength or centrally mediated impaired interjoint coordination can contribute to
movement disturbances and impaired function (Welmer, Holmqvist, &
Sommerfeld, 2006). A study designed to look at the extent abnormal limb
synergies influence voluntary movements of hemi paretic patients with first-time
stroke found that 3 months after stroke, 13% of all subjects moved within synergies
(Welmer, Holmqvist, & Sommerfeld, 2006). Several methods for evaluating
voluntary movement post stroke are described in Chapter 8 and online Chapters A,
B, and C. Valid and reliable tools for evaluating voluntary movement post stroke
recommended by both the AHA/ASA clinical guideline and the AOTA practice
guideline are the Fugl-Meyer Assessment of Motor Function (Fugl-Meyer et al.,
1975) and the Motor Assessment Scale (Carr et al.,1985). The Fugl-Meyer
Assessment is an adaptation of Brunnstrom’s original Hemiplegia Classification
and Progress Record (Brunnstrom, 1970) and incorporates Brunnstrom’s six stages
of motor recovery with an underlying predicted sequence of recovery. The
Fugl-Meyer Assessment of the Upper Extremity (FMA-UE) is the most commonly
used research assessment to describe upper extremity motor impairment and
evaluate the success of new interventions, but its focus on synergy patterns no
longer forms the basis for newer function-oriented treatments (Woodbury et al.,
2007). After investigating the dimensionality and construct validity of the
FMA-UE, researchers suggest that assessment of reflexes in the FMA-UE gives
little information about volitional movement. They also challenge the stepwise
orderly sequence of motor recovery described by Brunnstrom and Fugl-Meyer and
suggest instead that “UE motor behavior during recovery may be a dynamic
interaction of neural factors with the task-specific difficulty of a movement”
(Woodbury et al., 2007, p. 720).
TREATMENT
Intervention will vary with the patient’s stage of recovery, intervention setting,
living environment, extent of impairment, and personal goals and preferences
(Sabari & Lieberman, 2008).
Acute Phase
Stroke rehabilitation begins “as soon as the diagnosis of stroke is established and
life-threatening problems are under control” (Duncan et al., 2005, p. e104). Length
of stay in acute hospital beds is typically just long enough for necessary diagnostic
tests, for initiation of appropriate control over the environment and to begin to
improve occupational functioning and component abilities and capacities (Gresham
et al., 1995). Even at this early stage, the occupational therapist’s assessment of a
patient can help determine the most appropriate setting for rehabilitation and
discharge. The goals of discharge planning during the acute phase are to determine
the need for postacute rehabilitation, arrange the best possible living environment,
and ensure continuity of care after discharge (Gresham et al., 1995).
As part of the stroke care team, the occupational therapist should practice methods
to prevent or lessen complications resulting from stroke.
Skin Care
Early in recovery, support for patients who have had strokes and their families may
best be provided in the form of education to promote a realistic understanding of
the causes and consequences of stroke and the process, goals, and prognosis of
rehabilitation (Duncan et al., 2005). In a systematic review, Forster et al. (2001)
found evidence that passive education alone, such as providing written handouts, is
not as effective as a combination of passive and interactive education, such as
classes or training sessions. All aspects of OT assessment and treatment for
survivors of stroke should be considered opportunities for education: to engage
cooperation and participation in the identification of meaningful treatment goals, to
highlight residual abilities as well as disabilities, and to promote carryover of
treatment gains. Because the period after stroke is stressful, emotional, and tiring
for both the patient and family, education sessions provided during the acute phase
should be brief, simple, and reinforced as needed with repetition or appropriate
learning aids (see Chapter 13).
Rehabilitation Phase
Part of discharge planning during the acute phase of stroke is screening for
rehabilitation services. The AHA/ASA guideline recommends that “patients who
have sustained an acute stroke should receive rehabilitation services if their
poststroke functional status is below their prestroke status, and if there is a
potential for improvement” (Duncan et al., 2005, p. e118). Rehabilitation choices
depend on a patient’s condition, the social support system, and the resources
available in a community. To qualify for further treatment in an inpatient
rehabilitation facility, a patient must require active and ongoing intervention of
multiple therapy disciplines, require an intensive rehabilitation therapy program
(generally consisting of at least 3 hours of therapy per day at least 5 days per
week), and reasonably be expected to actively participate in and significantly
benefit from the rehabilitation program (U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services, 2011). Patients who do not qualify for this level of rehabilitation may
receive multidisciplinary rehabilitation services at a skilled nursing facility or
long-term care facility or treatment by one or more disciplines in home care or in
an outpatient clinic. During this phase of recovery, the patient and family are
focused on getting better and are usually more concerned with recovering lost
function than on adapting to a life of chronic disability (Sabari, 1998). Successful
OT intervention coordinates a patient’s striving for restoration of function with the
potential for compensation and alternative occupational roles.
Postural Adaptation
The ability to make automatic postural adjustments, including trunk control and the
maintenance of balance, is a prerequisite for successful performance of
occupational tasks. A systematic review of recent evidence related to the effect of
balance training on balance performance among individuals poststroke supports the
use of balance training for individuals with moderately severe stroke
(Lubetzky-Vilnai & Kartin, 2010). Part of the occupational therapist’s role in
training a patient with stroke in ADL independence poststroke is in understanding
each patient’s particular strengths and weaknesses regarding stability and mobility.
For example, some clients may be challenged in maintaining sitting balance on the
side of a bed for the duration of eating an entire meal, whereas others may be ready
to im-prove dynamic standing balance in order to cast a fishing line or play golf.
Therapists should teach patients the safest, most effective and efficient “ready”
position for engaging in activities.
CHAPTER 7
BRUNNSTROM APPROACH
Stage III Beginning voluntary movement but only in synergy; spasticity may be
significant
CHAPTER 8
ROOD AND PROPRIOCEPTIVE NEUROMUSCULAR
FACILITATION APPROACHES
CHAPTER 9
Berta Bobath, a physical therapist, and her husband, Karel Bobath, a psychiatrist
and neurophysiologist, began advancing a different set of ideas for enhancing
motor recovery in children and adults as early as the 1940s. Their first major
publication about stroke rehabilitation (Bobath, 1970) was, in several ways, a
direct challenge to primary tenets espoused by Brunnstrom. Where Brunnstrom’s
approach capitalized on spasticity and hyperactive reflexes as opportunities to elicit
early movement, the Bobaths viewed hypertonicity as an obstacle to a person’s
ability to move freely. In addition, they recognized that once a person begins to
move his paretic limbs in abnormal patterns, it is extremely difficult to break away
from these obligatory patterns for flexible movement sequences that meet the
contextual demands of activity performance. Finally, the Bobath approach
conceptualized the underlying cause of limb synergies quite differently than
Twitchell and Brunnstrom. Instead of viewing the synergies as primary
impairments that are directly associated with cerebral damage, they conceptualized
these abnormal movement patterns as secondary maladaptive strategies. Carr and
Shepherd (2003) later coined the term “adaptive features'' to describe disorders of
movement that develop in response to a person’s attempt to move within the
constraints of limited mobility, specific weakness, or hypertonicity in specific
muscles. For example, Figures 21-1A and 21-1B illustrate the marked difference in
scapular mobility in two people. Notice the extent to which the scapula protracted
and upward rotates when the young man on the left flexes his shoulder. As
demonstrated by the photo on the right, many stroke survivors experience limited
scapula mobility (also described as impaired dissociation between the scapula and
the thorax). This is a secondary impairment, usually caused by loss of muscle
length in the middle trapezius muscle. As the man in the photo tries to raise his arm
into flexion, his lack of scapulohumeral rhythm interferes with efficient
kinematics. To achieve the movement, he uses what motor capacities are available
to him. He laterally flexes his trunk, elevates his scapula, abducts his arm away
from the body, and ultimately, to achieve greater active range of motion (AROM),
externally rotates at the glenohumeral joint. Compare this photo to Figure A-1 in
online Chapter A, which illustrates a flexor synergy pattern. Neurodevelopmental
therapy (NDT) introduced the concept that therapy could “prevent” the
development of abnormal limb synergy patterns by removing mechanical obstacles
to movement. NDT training, which continues today, prepares therapists to have
keen observational skills and to employ kinesiological concepts when determining
motor interventions. In particular, NDT provides specific guidelines for observing
a patient’s postural alignment, predicting how misalignment might impact the
kinematics of limb movement, and improving the patient’s postural symmetry and
balance. The Bobaths were the first to articulate that motor performance problems
in stroke survivors are due to factors beyond the activation of individual muscles.
They stressed that many stroke survivors have “lost the feeling of normal
movement” (Bobath, 1970). In today’s lexicon, this concept of “normal
movement” is described as motor programs or flexible attractor states (to be
discussed later in this chapter). In addition, they recognized that postural alignment
and postural control provide a critical foundation for functional movement. Thanks
to the foresight of Berta and Karel Bobath, the NDT approach has continued to
evolve over time, in response to newer ideas about motor control and relearning.
Numerous textbooks by the Bobaths and others who have studied with them
provide helpful guidelines for structuring motor interventions with stroke survivors
(Davies, 2000; Howle, 2002; Ryerson & Levit, 1997). However, outcome studies
have not supported the efficacy of the total package of NDT intervention, as
compared with other approaches to improving motor control in stroke survivors
(Kollen et al., 2009). In addition, some have suggested that NDT therapists put too
much emphasis on monitoring the kinematics of movement and using hands-on
techniques in an attempt to prevent the person from developing “abnormal
movement patterns.” Be-cause of this, NDT interventions may fail to provide
patients with sufficient opportunities to actually practice using their emerging
motor skills (French et al., 2010). Influences from NeuroscienceEvidence about
Brain Plasticity. There has been an explosion of evidence in the past 30 years that
confirms the human brain’s capacity to reorganize after injury has occurred. The
American Occupational Therapy Association (AOTA) Practice Guidelines for
Adults with Stroke present an extensive review of research findings that support
the importance of activity-based environmental challenges and repetitive practice
opportunities in facilitating this neural reorganization (Sabari, 2008). Combined
with advances in medical treatment of acute cerebrovascular accident (Wardlaw et
al., 2003) and stroke rehabilitation (Stroke Unit Trialists’ Collaboration, 2007),
emerging concepts related to neuroplasticity have contributed to a heightened sense
of optimism that stroke survivors have the potential to recover motor function
(Gauthier et al., 2008). Depending on the extent of neuropathology, all stroke
survivors have varying potential for spontaneous recovery and reorganization of
neural mechanisms. Studies of humans and other mammals have provided
significant evidence that recovery of function after brain lesions is associated with
recruitment of brain regions not typically activated for a specific function
(Butefish, 2004; Murphy & Corbett, 2009). These studies consistently find that
brain plasticity is a dynamic process that is influenced by the individual’s active
efforts to meet environmental and task demands (Hoffman et al., 2008). This
finding has contributed significantly to changing views about neurorehabilitation.
The philosophy, long held within the OT profession, that engagement in specially
designed therapeutic activities can improve motor function is now supported by the
scientific community. In addition, neurorehabilitation goals have been reframed
from a focus on motor execution to an emphasis on helping patients improve motor
control and, with regard to paretic limbs, amount of use. Several streams of
research, in a variety of disciplines, have added support to incorporate these ideas
into rehabilitation interventions to improve motor performance in survivors of
stroke and brain injury.
Although kinesiology has long been a core subject of study in OT and physical
therapy curricula, it was not until the 1970s and 1980s those rehabilitation
professionals began to seriously consider how principles from the academic
disciplines of movement science and motor learning could be applicable to
therapeutic interventions. Many of these principles related to stages of learning,
types of feedback, and therapeutic use of practice are described in Chapter 13,
“Learning.” We recommend that you review this chapter, with particular emphasis
on Definition 13-2 (implicit learning and explicit learning) and Procedures for
Practice 13-2 (Therapeutic Use of Context, Feedback, and Practice to Promote
Transfer and Generalization). At the same time, a major paradigm shift was
occurring in neuroscience. Previously, neural control of movement was explained
by tracking the descent of neural signals through corticospinal pathways,
delineating the direct influences from brainstem nuclei on motor neurons in the
spinal cord, and describing communication between alpha motoneurons and
muscle fibers (the “final common pathway”). In essence, motor performance was
viewed as the collection of neural stimuli to individual muscles. Furthermore,
regions of the CNS were viewed as relatively static structures, with assigned
functions that did not change in response to experience or injury. A proliferation of
neuroscience research provides significant evidence that neural control of
movement can best be conceptualized as a fluid, dynamic system. Through
repetitive but varied practice, people develop motor programs that govern the
production of generalized categories of movement. These motor programs are then
adapted to suit particular features of task performance. Therefore, a unique pattern
of activity with core foundational characteristics emerges whenever the motor
program is executed. For example, a tennis player develops an underlying motor
program that structures her posture and sequence of muscle recruitment for
producing a forehand swing. This set of kinesiological relationships forms a core
foundation, but the athlete alters the force characteristics, timing, and spatial details
each time she returns the ball. The tennis player chooses these alterations (or
variant characteristics of the motor program) based on the speed, force, and
direction of the tennis ball, as well as the player’s intentions regarding how to
return the ball to her opponent. The confluence between empirical findings in
movement science and neuroscience research has led to increasing overlap between
these two fields of study. These findings have had significant influence on
rehabilitation interventions to improve motor performance in survivors of stroke
and TBI. Schmidt and Lee (2011) provide useful background information about
core concepts and research evidence related to human motor control and motor
learning. Shumway-Cook and Woollacott (2012) provide more specific
applications to OT and physical therapy practice. Winstein and Knecht (1990),
Carr and Shepherd (1998, 2003), and Mathiowetz and Bass- Haugen (1994)
developed practical guidelines for therapeutic intervention based on dynamic
systems, ecological systems, and motor program theories.
Cognition refers to the integrated functions of the human mind that together result
in thought and goal-directed action. Cognition underlies being and doing and is
evidenced in how people interact with others and perform both simple and complex
activities of daily life. Perhaps the central role of cognition in occupational
performance is best illustrated as one imagines what it must be like to suddenly
lose the ability to concentrate, remember, and problem solve. Survivors of
traumatic brain injury (TBI) describe how their once-familiar routines and
environments become chaotic, confusing, and frightening, which devastates their
sense of identity and competence (Erikson et al., 2007). Occupational therapists
assess cognition because many people seeking occupational therapy services are
likely to have some degree of cognitive impairment that influences their ability to
participate in rehabilitation and achieve rehabilitation outcomes (Skidmore et al.,
2010). Cognitive changes can be temporary, relatively static, or progressive. As
above, many survivors of TBI experience deficits in information processing speed,
attention, memory, and executive functions that persist for months or years
postinjury (Skandsen et al., 2010). A significant number of persons who sustain a
spinal cord injury also have a concurrent TBI with similar implications for
cognition (Macciocchi et al., 2008). Individuals with chronic conditions may also
experience cognitive changes including those with multiple sclerosis (Rogers &
Panegyres, 2007), Parkinson’s disease (Caviness et al., 2007), cancer (Boykoff,
Moieni, & Subramanian, 2009), epilepsy ( Helmstaedter et al., 2003), systemic
lupus erythematosus (McLaurin et al., 2005), and human immunodeficiency
virus/acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (Heaton et al., 2004). Even
individuals with mild stroke who are independent in activities of daily living
(ADL) may have executive dysfunction that impacts their ability to work, drive,
and engage in recreational activities (Edwards et al., 2006; Wolf, Barbee, & White,
2011). This chapter begins with descriptions of specific cognitive domains and
processes. We then review clinical reasoning considerations pertinent to cognitive
assessment and describe specific methods and tools based on three complementary
approaches to cognitive assessment. We conclude with considerations for
interpreting the results of cognitive assessment.
The term cognition generally refers to the product of many integrated processes
carried out by the brain that allow humans to be aware, think, learn, judge, plan,
and execute behavior (National Institutes of Health [NIH], n.d.). These domains
and processes include orientation, perception, attention , memory and learning,
judgment, reasoning, language, and executive functions (NIH, n.d.). How these
processes and domains precisely interact to enable being, thinking, and doing is
unclear, but it appears that executive functions may control and coordinate the
other cognitive operations in the service of goal-directed action (Salthouse, 2005).
Consider the working definitions that follow and how dysfunction might be
observed in every-day activities (see Table 6-1).
Orientation
Orientation refers to the awareness of self in relation to person, place, time, and
circumstance (Sohlberg & Mateer, 1989). Orientation deficits are typically
symptoms of brain dysfunction, with disorientation to time and place being most
common (Lezak, 1995).
Attention
The term attention was famously defined by William James as “the taking
possession by the mind, in a clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several
simultaneously present objects or trains of thought” (James, 1890, pp. 403–404).
Attentional abilities are dependent on multiple brain regions including the
cingulate cortex, limbic system, prefrontal cortices, and sensorimotor regions
(Posner, 1980). Each person is thought to have a limited capacity for consciously
attending to information, a hard-wired upper limit that dictates how many inputs
can be simultaneously processed (Lezak, 1995). Deficits in attention are common
following brain injury as numerous and diffuse neural regions work to support the
individual’s attentional abilities. Deficits can also be experienced because of a lack
of sleep or decreased nutrition (Groeger et al., 2011). Learning is dependent on
attention as individuals can-not encode into memory stimuli or content they do not
attend to. Definition 6-1 describes five components of attention (Sohlberg &
Mateer, 2001) and their implications for task performance.
Memory
Memory broadly refers to information storage and retrieval (Lezak, 1995). Rather
than a unitary process or construct, there are many types of neural processes that
support an individual’s memory capacity (Sohlberg & Turkstra, 2011). Experts still
debate about what transpires during the process of remembering (Cowan, 2008).
Atkinson and Shiffrin’s (1971) Information-Processing Model, which highlights
stages of acquiring and employing new knowledge and skills, is one of many
conceptions as to how this process occurs. We use this model to introduce
memory-related concepts, acknowledging the continued debate regarding precise
terms and their meanings in this realm. (Also see Dubuc [2011] and Levy [2011]
for more in-depth information on the following discussion.)
Sensory Registers
Information from the environment is briefly (milliseconds) held in registers (or
stores) specific to the human senses (Lezak, 1995). This registration stage has been
called the intake valve for determining what data from the environment are
ultimately stored. This phase is influenced by acuity of the senses (such as hearing
and vision), affective set, and perception.
Whereas data in working memory have a short shelf life, information in long-term
memory can be stored for minutes to a lifetime (Lezak, 1995). When we remember
information (an event that occurred an hour ago or a year ago), we have located
and retrieved data from long-term memory and are holding it for conscious
attention and thought in limited-capacity working memory. Storage in long-term
memory is based on relatively permanent changes in brain cell structure (Glover,
Ronning, & Bruning, 1990), although there does not appear to be a single local
storage site for stored memories (Lezak, 1995). Long-term memory is thought to
consist of two subsystems, explicit (or declarative) memory and implicit (or non
declarative) memory (Fig. 6-2). Declarative memory pertains to factual
information and includes episodic memory (knowledge of personal information
and events such as what you ate for breakfast) and semantic memory (knowledge
of facts about the world such as that horses are big and ants are small) (Eysenck &
Keane, 1990). Prospective memory is another form of declarative memory, having
to do with remembering to do tasks that one intends (Fish, Wilson, & Manly,
2010). Implicit or nondeclarative memory differs from explicit memory because it
does not involve conscious awareness of learning (Sohlberg & Turkstra, 2011).
This includes procedural memory, which pertains to knowing how to do things; it
allows us to learn and perform skilled motor actions (Eysenck & Keane, 1990).
Memory impairments are typically characterized as mild, moderate, or severe
based on the results of cognitive assessment. The term neurologic amnesia is
reserved to describe losses of broad categories or segments of memory resulting
from brain trauma, stroke, or tumor
Informal Observation
MOTOR LEARNING
Consider the attentional resources required of you the first time you drove a car
compared with the attention required for the same task now. After years of
experience, the car seems to drive itself, freeing the driver to concentrate on plans
for the day or talk to a passenger. This example typifies the difference between
controlled and automatic processing.
Controlled Processing
Processing
Thus, patients learning new skills or behaviors can process only a limited number
of inputs (instructions, cues, and environmental distractions) at a time.
Automatic Processing
With enough controlled processing, a task requires less and less concentration to
carry out (Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977). That is, the task becomes increasingly
automatic, as in the example of the proficient driver. Automatic processing occurs
when specific contextual stimuli internal or external to the person trigger the
activation of a specific learned sequence in long-term memory (Schneider &
Shiffrin, 1977). Given enough repetition, the individual performs the skill or task
in a consistent manner with little or no attention. Sternberg (1986) suggested that
development of a full level of automatization requires at least 200 trials of a task
but that automatization begins in as few as 10 trials, so long as those trials are
consistent. With fully automatic skills, people cannot stop themselves from
per-forming the overlearned sequence unless control processes are employed to
override it. Definition 13-1 discusses implications related to helping patients with
memory impairment to reacquire skills. According to Giles (2005), overlearning is
practice of a skill or strategy well beyond demonstration of learning or proficiency;
it increases the likelihood that the skill or strategy will become automatic. These
automatic skills and strategies become the easiest behaviors to initiate from an
array of possible behaviors (Giles, 2005), minimizing demands on attention and
decision making. Habits and routines are examples of automatic motor sequences
that, according to Kielhofner, Barris, and Watts (1982), organize a person’s tasks,
space and time. Characteristics of automatic skills and strategies, habits and
routines are responsive to the environmental conditions under which they are
learned and develop with repetition (Kielhofner, Barris, & Watts, 1982; Wood,
Quinn, & Kashy, 2002). Habit learning is a form of implicit learning, which is
described in Definition 13-2. Occupational therapists often help patients resume or
relearn self-maintenance tasks so that they are once again automatic.
Learner Characteristics
Interestingly, when a child is newly born, the number of neurons within the CNS is
approximately 100 billion (Nolte, 2002). After birth, these neurons are believed to
be unable to divide and reproduce themselves. Additionally, as a person traverses
the decades of his or her life starting in the second decade, the CNS tends to lose
neurons. This is considered to be a part of the normal aging process associated with
the CNS. The point here, though, is that when a baby is born, it essentially has all
of its neurons at the first day of its life. The question then becomes: If the infant
has all of its neurons, then why does the child need to learn? Clearly, there are
many processes involved in the development of the child. This is particularly true
in the child’s motor development. Some of the earliest patterns of movement or
positioning are in the form of primary reflexes. These primary reflexes are often
conspicuous when one views the positioning of a child younger than six months of
age. Two common primitive reflexes are known as the asymmetrical tonic neck
reflex and the symmetrical tonic neck reflex. These two reflexes affect the position
of the upper and lower extremities and are elicited or determined by the position of
the head. Although these reflexes remain with us throughout life, their influence on
the positioning of the body is not as apparent after the age of approximately six
months. Sometimes these reflexes can emerge in older children or adults after an
injury to the brain occurs. There are several theories as to why these primitive
reflexes become integrated. One of the most prevalent theories has to do with the
rapid myelination of the brain within the first few years of life. The average mass
of a human brain at birth is about 400 grams, and by the end of the third year, the
mass has increased to approximately 1,200 grams (Nolte, 2002). Much of this
increase is thought to come from the myelination of the CNS neurons. Myelination
is thought to continue at least into the second decade of life. Further, it is believed
that as the individual ages, the rapidity of myelination decreases. Why is
myelination so important? Once myelin is established, the ease of neuronal
depolarization and the rate of the neuronal propagation increase dramatically. This
results in the ability of the neurons to communicate with each other more
efficiently. Simultaneously the establishment and proliferation of neuronal
dendrites occur at a relatively intense rate during the early years of life. The more
dendrites there are, the more connections there are to other neurons. Whereas the
myelin facilitates the speed at which depolarization travels down the neuronal
axon, the dendrites act as connections between other neurons. The increase in
myelin and dendrites results in more efficient communication to many more
neurons, which in turn means that the brain can process much more information
much more efficiently. Pragmatically speaking, this results in being able to process
more information, allows for greater coordination of movement, and facilitates the
ability to learn new skills at a quicker rate. As these neuronal changes are rapidly
developing in typically developing children, growth charts provide some insight
into the physical development in terms of height and weight. From birth to
approximately age 20, there are two large growth spurts, which can be readily seen
How Body Systems Change with Motor Development
Once the person has successfully navigated puberty and enters the twenties, many
of the body’s systems (i.e., muscle mass, skeletal size, CNS myelination) remain
relatively stable for the next several decades. However, detrimental changes in
muscle mass can be seen as early as age thirty, when loss of muscle fibers begins,
continuing to decrease at least through age 75 years (Abrams, Beers, Berkow,
Fletcher, & Besdine, 1995). Bone mass is another area that changes, especially for
menopausal women, but both sexes normally experience slow progressive loss of
bone mass beginning at about age 50 years. Brain mass tends to decline by
approximately 10% between the ages of 25 and 90 years (Abrams et al., 1995).
This reduction in brain mass is not necessarily associated with decreased cognitive
function, though some cognitive changes do occur with the normal aging process.
As an example, people with large ventricles can still function normally, while those
with “normal” sized ventricles have been known to have severe dementia.
Therefore, normal age-related loss of brain mass does not necessarily mean loss of
cognitive function. Although we just touched on three systems that change across
the life course (bone mass, muscle mass, and brain mass), changes occur in every
organ and system through- out the life course, including, but not limited to, the
cardio- vascular system, the kidneys, the gastrointestinal system, the skin, and the
immune system. Just as when the development of the child’s subsystems provides
the potential to acquire skills, the opposite can be true as the person traverses the
fifth and sixth decades (and beyond). For instance, as the individual ages and
experiences age-related organ and system changes, the potential for either learning
new skills or maintaining the ability to perform skills learned earlier in life may
decline. Having discussed some developmental, growth, and maturational issues
that occur throughout life, we turn in the following section to motor control
concepts that are specific to functional movement and skills.
Most motor control theories propose several roles for sensory input and feedback
in movement regulation. The sensory channels provide information from the
environment and the body that is useful in initiating and maintaining movement.
Sensory input not only has a role in initiating movement, but also has a role as
sensory feedback in con- trolling the movement as the movement occurs.
Perception is the integration of sensory impressions from the different sensory
sources into psychologically meaningful information. Both the perceptual and
sensory systems provide information about the state of the body and features of the
environment that influence movement control. Sensation and perceptual
information are important to action just as action is essential to perceptual
information (Rosenbaum, 1991). We act on what we perceive, and we perceive and
act on our perceptions. Therefore, understanding movement requires an
understanding of the systems that control sensation and perception and how these
systems influence and form action.
Movements are housed within actions, and for the occupational therapy
practitioner, the focus on movement control is to understand how the individual
organizes movements into meaningful and purposeful actions, activities, and
routines within different occupations. Also, movements are described and best
understood within the context of accomplishing goal-directed actions. Movement
control is studied within the context of an action, such as reaching, with the
assumption that the control processes that are involved in this function will provide
insight into principles related to how similar movements are controlled.
Understanding the control of action implies under- standing how the motor output
is based on musculoskeletal and neuromuscular components, including movement
synergies and the motor programming systems. The musculoskeletal system
includes joint and muscle properties, such as joint and muscle lengthening
flexibility, contributing to the organization of the movement. There are numerous
joints and muscles in the body, and these must be controlled during the execution
of coordinated, functional movement. The problem of coordinating many muscles
and joints into a coordinate structure, called the degrees of freedom problem, is a
major research issue for motor control researchers. Another movement structure
that is relevant to action are synergies, which are categories of movement that
require the action of more than one muscle. As defined by Shumway-Cook and
Woollacott (2007), synergies are functional couplings of groups of muscles
constrained to act as a unit and reflect preferred strategies for moving. Examples
include grasping an object and throwing a ball. Synergies are characterized by
spatial and temporal characteristics that allow for both stability and flexibility in
movement. Flexibility in synergistic patterns allows the coordinated structure to
adapt to the environmental demands, while stability is evidenced by the reliability
of the spatial and temporal ordering of the specific movement. In the standing
position, lower extremity stability is provided by the distal-to-proximal muscle
sequence activation, while flexibility allows for walking on an uneven surface
(Shumway-Cook & Woollacott, 2007). In occupational therapy literature, atypical
synergies associated with impaired movement control have frequently been
described, particularly in populations with stroke and cerebral palsy. These atypical
flexion- and extension- dominated synergies are groups of muscles acting as mass
movement patterns that reflect the individual’s response to reorganization of
movement control subsequent to brain injury. Such synergies are marked by lack of
flexibility and stability and can result in muscle shortening and musculoskeletal
deformity as set postures emerge and normal reciprocal movements and sequences
do not occur as part of movement control.
Actions can occur as a result of external prompts, such as seeing a toothbrush and
reaching for it, or reflexively in response to an external stimulus, or as a result of
an internal intent or motivation, such as wanting to brush one’s teeth in the
morning and looking for and reaching for a toothbrush. Cognitive processes that
are important to action include attention, memory, motivation, and the emotional
aspects of motor control underlying the establishment of the person’s intent or
goals (Shumway-Cook & Woollacott, 2001). Also, the ability to attend to the
relevant cues of a task and the performance environment and the ability to make
task comparisons, to evaluate one’s own performance, and to identify errors in
movement planning are cognitive strategies that are useful in skill learning and
relearning.
Reaction Time
One example of cognition and action that has been relatively well researched as a
measure of cognitive processing is reaction time. Reaction time is considered to be
a measure of how efficiently the CNS is working. Reaction time is defined as the
time from when a stimulus is given to the time when a movement begins in
reaction to the stimulus. There are different types of reaction time. For instance,
simple reaction time is the reaction time when there is a single stimulus associated
with a solitary specific response. For example, suppose that when a person sees a
light turn on, he or she is required to reach forward; in this case, there is a
one-to-one relationship between the stimulus and the response. Choice reaction
time is somewhat more complex, occurring when a number of potential stimuli are
present and the motor response is unique for each of the different stimuli. For
example, there might be three potential stimuli involving a red light, a green light,
and a blue light. The directions might be that when he or she sees the red light, the
participant is required to lift his or her arm straight up; when he or she sees the
green light, the participant is supposed to move his or her arm to the left; and when
he or she sees the blue light, the participant is supposed to move his or her hand to
the right. It has been shown that a person’s choice reaction time performance is
typically longer than his or her simple reaction time performance. It has also been
shown that the best reaction times occur during the late teens and early twenties
and that the reaction time increases each decade thereafter. Men tend to have faster
reaction times than women. It also appears that the more education a person has,
the better is his or her reaction time (Fozard, Vercruyssen, Reynolds, Hancock, &
Quilter, 1994; Houx & Jolles 1993) Within the individual movement is the product
of many systems that need to be examined as they interact and contribute to
movement organization and control. Deficits in one system affect the function of
other systems. To understand motor control and organization, the perceptual,
cognitive, and action components of motor control must be considered and
synthesized as part of the full picture of motor control. In addition to individual
body factors affecting control, different tasks as well as the environment impose
constraints on motor control. The Person, the Task, and the Environment Skill
acquisition is dependent on several factors. Practitioners help patients to learn
skills, and these skills have multiple dimensions that influence reacquisition.
Understanding the types and requirements of tasks as well as the regulatory
features of the environment that affect task performance allows the practitioner to
plan with the patient the optimal environment for skill learning. Also, under-
standing how movement control is affected in the client with a neurological
disorder and what body systems have been affected helps the practitioner to plan
effective multi systems interventions for skill learning. Along with using task
analysis, a task taxonomy can be useful for retraining functional movements in the
patient with a neurological disorder. Using the three continuums of closed to open
skills, stability to mobility, and no manipulation versus manipulation, the
practitioner could assess a client’s capability and skill level across many basic and
instrumental activities of daily living. By systematically varying the regulatory
features of the task and the environment, the practitioner can help the patient to
develop the motor program and plans that are necessary for rebuilding task
routines. Now that we have reviewed some fundamental aspects of motor learning
and control and its development from a theoretical perspective, the next section
focuses on defining and assessing motor skills and motor performance.
Bernstein, a Russian scientist, was among the first to look at internal and external
forces acting on the body to under- stand the characteristics of the system being
moved. The body was regarded as a mechanical system with mass and subject to
external forces, such as gravity, as well as inertial and movement-dependent forces.
Bernstein asked questions related to (1) the function of the system in a continually
changing environment, (2) the properties of the initial conditions affecting
movement, and (3) the body as a mechanical system influencing the control
process (Shumway-Cook & Woollacott, 2001). Bernstein (1967) was also
responsible for identifying what is known as the degrees of freedom problem. In
describing the mechanics of the system, Bernstein noted that many degrees of
freedom need to be controlled for coordinated movement to occur. For example,
there are many joints that can flex, extend, and/or rotate, and these multiple options
complicate the control of movement. Control therefore involves converting the
body into a “controllable” system (Schmidt, 1988). Bernstein’s solution to this
problem was proposing that hierarchical control exists to simplify the body’s
multiple degrees of freedom. He proposed that
1. groups of muscle are constrained to act together as a unit, and
2. These units are activated at lower levels in the system.
CHAPTER 12
The Motor Re-Learning Program (Carr & Shepherd, 1987) is a synthesis of the
prevalent contemporary models of motor control and the motor learning process
(Sabari, 1995). It is specific to the rehabilitation of patients following stroke. The
program is based on four factors that are thought to be essential for the learning of
motor skill and assumed to be essential for the relearning of motor control:
1. Elimination of unnecessary muscle activity
2. feed- back
3. practice,
4. The interrelationship of postural adjustment and movement. In this
program, treatment is directed toward relearning of control rather than to
activities incorporating exercise or to facilitation or inhibition techniques.
Implications
Specific strategies are espoused for the remediation of limiting factors within the
person. Many of these strategies are founded in the motor learning body of
knowledge. Some of these strategies involve how feedback is given. For instance,
it has been shown (particularly with nondisabled populations) that reduced
feedback actually results in better retention and transfer of the motor skill being
learned (Rice, 2003; Rice & Hernandez, 2006; Winstein & Schmidt, 1990; Wulf,
Schmidt, & Deubel, 1993). See Shea and Wulf (2005) for a review. Another motor
learning strategy involves presenting tasks to be learned or practiced in a random
fashion (e.g., moving from task to task without providing repeated trials on any
given task) rather than giving multiple trials of the same task before moving on to
another task. Much of the motor learning research has been performed on healthy
college students, and the tasks have usually involved simple rote motor skills that
do not represent normal occupationally oriented activities. Therefore, the
generalization of these strategies to special populations should be done with
caution until a greater body of evidence supports their use with special populations.
Thinking, remembering, reasoning, and making sense of the world around us are
fundamental to carrying out everyday living activities” (Unsworth, 1999, p. 3).
Cognition consists of interrelated processes including the ability to perceive,
organize, assimilate, and manipulate information to enable the person to process
information, learn, and generalize (Abreu & Toglia, 1987). Because so much of
rehabilitation in general requires learning and generalization, the principles of
intervention that are discussed in this chapter are important to consider with a wide
spectrum of clients and are not limited to those who are typically identified with
cognitive impairments. Cognitive impairments may be seen as a result of
developmental or learning problems, brain injury or disease, psychiatric
dysfunction, or sociocultural conditions (American Occupational Therapy
Association, 1999). Cognitive impairments can result in significant activity
limitations and participation restrictions in all aspects of the client’s life, potentially
compromising safety, health, and well-being. For example, decreased abilities to
recognize potential hazards, anticipate consequences of actions and behaviors,
follow safety precautions, and respond to emergencies are often major factors that
interfere with independence. Cognitive limitations can also diminish one’s sense of
competence, self-efficacy, and self–esteem, further compounding difficulties in
adapting to the demands of everyday living. The influence of cognitive symptoms
can be observed across all aspects of the domain of occupational therapy practice.
The aim of occupational therapy intervention for people with cognitive-perceptual
impairments is to decrease activity limitations, enhance participation in everyday
activities, and assist individuals to gain the abilities they need to take control over
their lives and develop healthy and satisfying ways of living. Although the ultimate
goal of intervention with this population is clear, there are different perspectives
and rehabilitation approaches to accomplish the goal.
CHAPTER 13
In this section, the main constructs involved in cognition will be discussed in terms
of their definitions, evaluation, and treatment. Self-awareness will be discussed
first because lack of awareness can affect the motivation, effort, and sustained
participation that are needed for intervention. Following discussion of
self-awareness, the areas of orientation, attention, memory, executive functions,
motor planning, unilateral neglect, and visual processing will be reviewed.
Evidence-based reviews in cognitive rehabilitation have found that training in
strategies, and self-monitoring or self-regulatory skills are key characteristics of
studies demonstrating the effectiveness of cognitive rehabilitation (Cicerone et al.,
2000, 2005). These interventions will therefore be emphasized. It should be kept in
mind that the context of the person’s life needs to be considered in planning and
choosing intervention activities (Johnston, Goverover, & Dijkers, 2005). This
includes the person’s occupations, personality, interests, and premorbid level of
functioning, culture, values, external support, and resources. Interventions that
address cognitive impairments need to be blended with those that address
interpersonal skills, social participation, and everyday activities, routines, and roles
(Abreu & Peloquin, 2005).
Self-Awareness
Orientation
Orientation is the ability to understand the self and the relationship between the
self and the past and present environment. Orientation depends on the integration
of several mental activities that are represented in different areas of the brain.
Disorientation is indicative of significant impairments in attention and memory
(Lezak, Howieson, & Loring, 2004). For example, disoriented clients might think
they are home rather than in a hospital, might confuse the hospital staff with
relatives, or might believe that it is a new day each time they wake up from a short
nap.
Evaluation
INTERVENTION
STRATEGY TRAINING AND/OR ADAPTATIONS OF TASK OR
ENVIRONMENT
Strategy training for disorientation involves teaching the person to look for
external cues when he or she is feeling confused or is having difficulty recalling
orientation information. For example, an information poster that contains
orientation facts can be placed on a wall, in a closet, or eventually inside a
notebook. When the client is asked for orientation information, he or she is
expected to locate the information poster to verify responses or to find the correct
answers. A memory book, containing pictures and names of familiar people or
important life events, can also be placed in a key location within the room. As an
alternative, an audiotape or videotape can be created by a family member to review
orientation information at set times during the day or used whenever the person
feels confused. An alarm that is preprogrammed to ring several times a day can be
used to cue the person to read his or her orientation fact book or listen to the
audiotape. Orientation questions with use of cueing strategies can also be
incorporated into a bean bag toss game, a board game, or a “Family Feud” style
game within a group format (Toglia & Golisz, 1990). A calendar posted on the wall
or closet may be helpful in orienting the person to time. If the client has poor
selective attention, a single piece of paper with the day and date written daily,
rather than a monthly calendar, might be needed. To assist the client in finding his
or her room, directional arrows can be placed in the hallway, and tape indicating
the route to his or her room can be placed on the floor. Key landmarks can be
pointed out and made more salient with arrows or colored tape. The therapist needs
to immediately reinforce initiation or use of any of these external cues by praising
the client (or rewarding points), and each time the client initiates the use of an
external cue, the therapist should keep track of it by recording it on a chart or
visual graph. The use of external cues should be gradually faded until the
orientation information is internalized. In addition, the person should be trained to
look for orientation cues (e.g., clocks, calendars) in different environments. Spaced
retrieval techniques can be used to train use of strategies and external aids, such as
using a daily calendar. Spaced retrieval involves systematically lengthening the
period of retention and recall. There is evidence that this technique is more
effective than cueing hierarchies in treating people with dementia (Bourgeois et al.,
2003).
Attention
Unilateral Neglect
Unilateral neglect is a failure to orient to, respond to, or report stimuli that are
presented on the side contralateral to the cerebral lesion in clients who do not have
primary sensory or motor impairments (Heilman, Watson, & Valenstein, 2003).
The term neglect connotes a volitional component to the disorder, but this is a
misnomer. The client with unilateral neglect is unaware of the incompleteness of
his or her perception of, and responses to, the environment. He or she often
behaves as though one half of the world does not exist (Corben & Unsworth,
1999). For example, following right-hemisphere strokes, clients often begin
scanning on the right side and miss or fail to explore most of the stimuli on the left.
Asymmetry may be observed in functional activities, drawing tasks, reading, or
writing. In severe cases, clients may eat food on one side of their plate, shave half
their face, or dress half of their body without recognizing that anything is wrong. In
milder cases, they may misread the first letter of a particular word or fail to attend
to information while crossing a street, shopping, or driving (see the Ethical
Dilemma). Many clients with unilateral neglect also exhibit anxiety or flattened
affect. Unilateral neglect has been identified as a major factor impeding functional
recovery in clients who have sustained strokes (Chen Sea, Henderson, & Cermack,
1993; Cherney, Halper, Kwasnica, Harvey, & Zhang, 2001). Those with unilateral
neglect have more difficulty resuming activities of daily living, have longer
hospital stays (Gillen, Tennen, & McGee, 2005; Katz et al., 1999), and are at
increased risk for accidents (Webster et al., 1995). Unilateral neglect has been
described as a heterogeneous disorder that includes different clinical subtypes and
behavioral components (Mesulam, 1994; Pierce & Buxbaum, 2002; Stone,
Halligan, Marshall, & Greenwood, 1998). Unilateral neglect can involve one or
more modalities, may vary with the nature of the stimuli (e.g., verbal versus
nonverbal), and can encompass single objects or different spatial frames of space:
extra personal or large space, peri personal or space within reach, and personal or
body space (Mesulam, 2000; Plummer, Morris, & Dunai, 2003). For example,
some clients demonstrate neglect symptoms in large spaces, such as a room
(extrapersonal neglect), but do not have reduced awareness of their body (personal
neglect) or difficulty on paper-and-pencil tasks (peripersonal neglect). Neglect
subtypes have also been proposed that involve internal mental images
(representational neglect), decreased movement into or toward the contralesional
space (motor neglect), or decreased ability to perceive sensory stimuli in
contralesional space (sensory neglect) (Mesulam, 1994).
Evaluation
Intervention
STRATEGY TRAINING
Strategies for unilateral neglect can be practiced within everyday tasks such as
setting a table for several people, dealing a deck of cards to six people, identifying
appointments on a wall calendar, reading a newspaper, addressing envelopes of
different sizes, or identifying all the pictures or chairs in the room. Because
unilateral neglect symptoms vary with the size of space, arrangement of space, and
amount and density of information presented, these activity parameters need to be
matched with the neglect symptoms and systematically varied and graded in
treatment. In some cases, treatment activities should emphasize large-space
activities; in other situations, activities should focus on tabletop tasks that involve
visual detail. In general, activities that are unpredictable or involve stimuli
randomly scattered on a table or page are more sensitive to the symptoms of
unilateral neglect than are activities that are arranged in a predictable, structured, or
horizontal array (Ferber & Karnath, 2001). Intervention should include practice in
identifying situations in which neglect symptoms are most likely to occur, such as
filling multiple bowls with salad, placing cookie dough on a baking sheet, or
arranging photographs in a picture album. Individuals with unilateral neglect do
not always know when they are attending to the left side. Intervention needs to
assist clients in finding external cues that will provide feedback about when they
are indeed attending to the left. An emphasis in intervention should be teaching the
client to find the edges of a page or a table or the periphery of stimuli before
beginning a task and to mark it with spatial point of reference, such as colored
tape, a colored highlighter, a bright object, or placement of his or her arm on the
left border. Auditory cueing, utilizing a beeper or alarm device, can be combined
with strategy training to remind the person to use a strategy or visual cue. The
alarm device can require the client to scan space and attend to the left to turn off
the sound (Seron, Deloche, & Coyette 1989). Other intervention strategies for
unilateral neglect include tactile search, use of mental imagery, and general alerting
techniques. Tactile search includes teaching the client to feel the left side of space
with eyes closed or to feel the left edges of objects before visual search. Visual
imagery teaches imagining and describing familiar scenes or routes and using
mental images during movement of limbs or visual scanning (Niemeier, 1998;
Smania, Bazoli, Piva, & Guidetti, 1997). For example, reduction in neglect
symptoms and increased performance on functional tasks were reported after a
mental imagery program that involved teaching people with neglect to imagine
their eyes as sweeping beams of a lighthouse from left to right across the visual
field. Clients were cued to use this mental image during functional and therapy
training tasks (Niemeier, 1998; Niemeier, Cifu, & Kishore, 2001). In addition to
strategies specifically aimed at facilitating attention to the left side, strategies that
focus on the general ability to sustain attention have also been found to reduce
unilateral neglect. For example, Robertson, Tegner, Tham, Lo, and Smith (1995)
taught clients with chronic unilateral neglect to mentally tell themselves to “pay
attention” and to tap loudly on a table. It has been observed that response to
strategy training depends on whether people with unilateral neglect show
improvements in their awareness (Tham et al., 2001; Robertson & Halligan, 1999).
This underscores the importance of deeply embedding awareness training
techniques, such as those described earlier, into all intervention activities.
To minimize the need to attend to the left, it has been suggested that the
environment be rearranged so that key items (e.g., the telephone, the nurse call
button) are on the unaffected side. However, a study by Kelly and Ostreicher
(1985) found no significant difference in functional outcome in clients whose
hospital rooms were rearranged in this way. Lennon (1994) described the
successful use of large colored paper markings on the edges of tables, corners, and
elsewhere to prevent collision for clients with unilateral neglect. The client was
trained to look for these markers. Markers gradually faded. Performance improved
and was maintained with removal of markers; however, effects did not generalize
to other environments. Calvanio, Levine, and Petrone (1993) described the use of
an adapted plate to increase feeding skills in a client with a severe case of left
inattention and a dense left hemianopsia. The plate was mounted on a lazy Susan
so that it could be rotated. As the client pushed at the food with a fork, the plate
rotated so that all the food eventually came into view, thus eliminating the need for
scanning to the left. Other environmental adaptations include placing red tape on
the client’s wheelchair brakes or placing brightly colored objects such as a napkin
or cup on the left side (Golisz, 1998).
Visual Processing
Visual Motor
Visual motor skills include drawing tasks (e.g., drawing a map, copying a design)
or construction of three- dimensional figures (e.g., assembling a coffee pot).
Clients may demonstrate difficulty on visual motor tasks for many reasons. For
example, a client might have difficulty constructing a block design because of a
poor ability to scan the complete design, decreased planning and organization,
unilateral neglect, or impaired discrimination of size, angles, and rotations. The
term constructional apraxia is used to refer to difficulty with drawing or assembly
tasks that cannot be attributed to primary motor or sensory impairment, ideomotor
apraxia, or general cognitive impairments (Farah, 2003). Constructional abilities
are closely related to ADL performance (Neistadt, 1992a; Warren, 1981). Clients
may have difficulty dressing (dressing apraxia), orienting clothes correctly on a
hanger, or assembling a sandwich or coffee pot. People with left-hemisphere
parietal lesions tend to omit individual pieces or details in constructional tasks,
whereas those with right-hemisphere lesions demonstrate spatial disorganization of
the pieces and lose the overall gestalt (Kramer, Kaplan, & Blusewicz, 1991).
Constructional apraxia is not a unitary syndrome. Impairments in different types of
perceptual processing or spatial relations are thought to underlie constructional
apraxia in both right and left-hemisphere lesions (Laeng, 2006).
Evaluation
Evaluation for people with visual perceptual impairments should examine visual
foundations skills, visual abilities without a motor response, and visual motor
skills. Visual foundation skills, including visual acuity, oculomotor skills, and
visual fields, should be evaluated prior to a visual processing evaluation to screen
out visual problems that will interfere with the accuracy of perceptual testing (Cate
& Richards, 2000). Several clinical observations during functional tasks can alert
occupational therapists to the need for a formal visual assessment: compensatory
head movements and tilting, squinting, shutting of one eye, or a tendency to lose
one’s place while reading. A basic screening can be performed by the occupational
therapist (i.e., visual acuity, range of motion of the eyes, ocular alignment, visual
pursuits or smooth tracking of moving objects, saccades or quick eye movements
to place an object of interest in view, and visual-scanning functions). Any
disruptions of these foundational skills will affect interpretations of higher level
visual-processing assessments (Warren, 1993). Standardized nonmotor assessments
of visual perception (see Table 57.1) categorize visual perception into specific
skills such as figure-ground, position in space, form constancy, spatial relations,
and visual recognition. Adults with neurological lesions may have difficulty
performing various types of visual processing tasks for similar reasons (e.g., a
tendency to over focus on parts, a tendency to miss visual details, failure to
simultaneously attend to the details as well as the whole). Therefore, Toglia (1989)
recommends an approach that conceptualizes visual processing on a continuum and
evaluates both conventional and unconventional objects under a variety of different
activity conditions. In a dynamic approach to visual perception, the therapist
systematically manipulates activity parameters and analyzes responses to cues to
understand why a client is having difficulty accurately discriminating objects or
visual stimuli (Kline, 2000; Toglia, 1989; Toglia & Finkelstein, 1991). Visual
perceptual assessment should examine responses to activities with and without a
motor response to examine differences in performance. Visual motor skills are
typically evaluated with block designs, puzzles, or copying designs. The therapist
needs to observe how the person begins and how he or she proceeds. For example,
does the client begin by drawing the details rather than attending to the overall
shape of the figure? Informal observations in tasks such as copying a map route,
assembling a coffeepot or woodworking project, wrapping a package, packing a
lunch box, or folding clothes can provide additional information on visual motor
abilities. Symptoms may include angular deviations; improper position, location,
spacing, or alignment of parts; and spatial distortions. The client’s ability to
recognize and correct errors in alignment or position should be investigated. For
example, some clients do not recognize visual spatial errors even when attention is
directed to the problem area, whereas other clients recognize errors but are unable
to correct them.
Intervention
Interventions may address visual foundations skills or visual processing skills with
or without a motor response.
Treatment of visual foundation skills such as visual acuity and contrast sensitivity,
oculomotor skills, and visual fields generally involves adaptations such as
large-print reading materials; magnifiers; talking devices; increasing contrast of
edges, borders, or backgrounds; and changes in lighting. However, remedial
exercises may be recommended for individuals with oculomotor or visual field
deficits. For example, range-of-motion eye exercises to the involved muscle have
been advocated for individuals with eye muscle pare- sis. Occlusion of the intact
visual field with eye patching has been used to force use of the impaired visual
field (Warren, 1993).
STRATEGY TRAINING
Strategies that maximize the client’s ability to process visual information can be
trained within everyday activities that involve choosing among objects that are
similar in shape and size (e.g., matching socks, sorting teaspoons and soup
spoons); locating information within supermarket circulars, calendars, maps, or
schedules; arranging information within grids or spread- sheets; copying patterns in
arts and craft activities; or finding information in crowded draws, shelves, tables,
or bulletin boards. Strategies can include getting a sense of the whole before
looking at the parts; teaching the person to partition space before localizing details;
using one’s finger to scan, trace visual stimuli, or focus on details; covering or
blocking visual stimuli when too much information is presented at once;
verbalizing salient visual features or subtle differences; and mentally visualizing a
particular item before looking for it (Toglia, 1989, 1998). Intervention involves
careful manipulation of activity parameters. Activities that involve familiar items
or contexts, high contrast (e.g., red socks and white socks), distinctive features,
little detail, and solid colors or backgrounds require less attention, effort, and visual
analysis than do activities that involve choosing among items that have low
contrast (e.g., light beige and white socks), are in unusual positions, are embedded
within crowded or distracting visual backgrounds, or are partially obscured.
Changes in the familiarity, number of items, and degree of detail can place greater
demands on visual processing. In addition, verbal mediation, including repeating a
list of step-by step instructions during a functional activity such as dressing,
capitalizes on strengths in verbal abilities and can be effective in facilitating
functional performance (Sunderland, Walker, & Walker, 2006).
Evaluation
Intervention
STRATEGY TRAINING
Clients may be taught to use verbal, visual, or tactile cues to enhance movement.
For example, before performing an activity, the client might mentally practice or
imagine the task performance; or the client might imagine how an object should
look in his or her hand before picking it up. Incorrect patterns of movement, such
as holding an object the wrong way, can also be visualized, with an emphasis on
having the client mentally practice correcting the movement. Talking a client
through action sequences or use of step-by-step written lists or illustrations can be
useful in facilitating functional performance in tasks such as drinking from a cup
(Butler, 1999). The person can be taught to verbally rehearse an action sequence or
associate the movement with a rhyme, rhythm, or musical tune with a gradual
fading of the verbalization. Self-monitoring strategies can be used to teach a client
to monitor unnecessary cocontraction, incomplete actions, or difficulty in
switching direction of movements. Preliminary studies indicate that strategy
training is effective in improving everyday function (Donkervoort, Dekker,
Stehmann-Saris, & Deelman, 2001; Geusgens et al., 2006). For example, in a
randomized study design, changes in non-trained ADL activities were greater in a
group of people with stroke who had received strategy training as compared with
those receiving usual occupational therapy. This suggests that the strategies
generalized to everyday activities (Geusgens et al., 2006).
Simple adaptations to objects that draw attention to the critical features of the
object or activity can facilitate action and motor planning (e.g., colored tape on the
knife handle or toothbrush handle). Patterns and designs on utensils or clothing
might draw attention to the wrong detail and result in an inappropriate motor
response. Tool use should be minimized (Poole, 2000), and adaptive equipment
should be selected with caution for the apraxic client. For example, some
adaptations, such as a button hook, one-handed shoe tying, or a one-arm drive
wheelchair, might be confusing for clients with apraxia and place greater demands
on motor-planning abilities. Other adaptations, such as adaptive clothing closures,
may simplify the task or motor pattern required to manipulate or hold objects,
reduce the number of steps, and facilitate function in the client with apraxia. Other
adaptations include training the caregiver to modify instructions so that the activity
is broken into one command at a time (Unsworth, 2007). Simple whole commands
(e.g., “Get up”) can put the activity on an automatic level and effectively enhance
motor planning (Zoltan, 1996).
Memory
Memory gives us the ability to draw on past experiences and learn new information
(Toglia, 1993a). This provides us with a sense of continuity in the environment and
frees us from dependency in here-and-now situations. Memory is conceptualized as
a multistep process involving encoding (i.e., input of information), storage (i.e.,
hold- ing information), and retrieval (i.e., getting information) (Levy, 2005b).
There are different types of memory. Working memory is the temporary storage of
information while one is working with it or attending to it. It includes the ability to
recall information immediately after exposure. It allows one to focus conscious
attention and keep track of information as one is performing an activity.
Declarative memory is one aspect of long-term memory and includes conscious
memory for events, knowledge, or facts. Procedural (nondeclarative) memory
involves the ability to remember how to perform an activity or procedure without
conscious awareness. Prospective memory involves the ability to remember
intentions or activities that will be required in the future (Levy, 2005b).
Evaluation
Intervention
Training of internal memory strategies is most appropriate for people with mild
memory deficits or those in whom other areas of cognition are intact (Cicerone et
al., 2000, 2005). The client practices one or two targeted memory strategies in a
variety of different tasks, such as remembering telephone numbers, news headlines,
a sequence of errands, items that need to be bought in a store, or instructions to an
activity. During practice on different memory tasks, a variety of awareness training
techniques may also be used. Memory strategies may be directed primarily at
encoding operations (i.e.,getting information in) or the retrieval phase of memory
(i.e., getting information out). Encoding strategies include the following:
Ø Chunking or grouping similar items
Ø The story method, or linking a series of facts or events into a story
Ø Rehearsal, or repeating information over and over silently
Ø Rhymes, or recalling a fact by changing the fact into a rhyme
Ø Visual imagery
Tasks and environments can be rearranged so that they place fewer demands on
memory:
Ø Cue cards or signs in key places (e.g., a sign on door where it will be seen
before leaving: “Take keys and . . .”)
Ø Labeling the outside of drawers or closets to minimize the need to recall
the location of items
Ø Providing step-by-step directions to reduce memory demands
Ø Providing checklists to assist in keeping track of task steps
Significant others can be trained to use methods that increase the likelihood that
the client will remember material, such as asking the client to repeat any
instructions or important information in his or her own words; encour- aging the
client to ask questions; and presenting material in small groups, clusters, or
categories (Levy, 2005a.
Executive functions are a broad band of performance skills that allow a person to
engage in independent, purposeful, and self-directed behavior. Higher-level
cognitive skills, including planning, cognitive flexibility, organization,
problem-solving, and self–regulation, are fundamental components of executive
function (Katz & Hartman-Maeir, 2005). Lezak and colleagues (2004) identifies
four primary components of executive functions: volition, planning, purposeful
action, and self-awareness and self monitoring. Impairments are associated with
prefrontal lesions and may be seen in all of these components, with one or two
areas of impairment especially prominent (Lezak et al., 2004). Volition is the
capacity to formulate an intention or goal and to initiate action. Planning involves
the ability to efficiently organize the steps or elements of a behavior or activity and
includes the ability to look ahead, anticipate consequences, weigh and make
choices, conceive of alternatives, sustain attention, and sequence the activity.
Purposeful action is the translation of an intention into an activity, requiring the
ability to initiate, switch, and stop sequences (flexibility), as well as
self-regulation. Self Regulation involves the ability to monitor, self-correct, and
evaluate performance. Executive function impairments significantly influence
social participation, daily activity, and functional outcome (Goverover, 2002, 2004;
Reeder, Newton, Frangou, & Wykes, 2004). Clients who display executive
dysfunction may be able to verbalize plans but have difficulty carrying them out.
There is often a disassociation between stated intentions and actions. This creates
gaps between what a person needs to do or wants to do and what the person
actually does (Eriksson, Tham, & Borg, 2006). Decreased initiation, flexibility,
impulsivity, or perseveration may be observed during performance. Often, the
client’s approach is haphazard or consists of trial and error, and there is decreased
ability to maintain goal-directed actions and to monitor or modify behaviors. For
example, when grocery shopping, the client might proceed in an unorganized
manner, not using a list or the aisle headings and reentering the same aisle multiple
times. The client might have difficulty deciding on appropriate substitute items,
buy items that are not needed, and forget items that were needed (Sohlberg &
Mateer, 2001). Rempfer, Hamera, Brown, and Cromwell (2003) found that grocery
shopping accuracy and efficiency were significantly associated with measures of
executive functions in people with chronic schizophrenia. In addition, limitations
in the ability to view information from different perspectives, generate alternative
solutions, and respond flexibly can reduce the ability to cope, adapt to everyday
demands, and relate to others. Executive functions impairments represent a distinct
challenge because they can be masked within familiar ADLs or routines but are
most apparent when the client is required to function in situations that are less
structured, require multitasking, or require dealing with novelty and unexpected
situations (Burgess et al., 2006; Katz & Hartman-Maeir, 2005). Examples of
activities that might present difficulty include following directions to a new
location; selecting and ordering a gift from a catalog; organizing a day’s activities;
planning a menu, lunch, picnic, vacation, or social gathering; investigating and
comparing prices for delivery of flowers; mailing a package; or purchasing an
electronic device.
Evaluation
Most standardized cognitive assessments are structured and do not adequately
examine the area of executive functions (Sohlberg & Mateer, 2001) (see Table
57.1). Several assessments for executive functions have recently been developed
(Bamdad, Ryan, & Warden, 2003; Birnboim & Miller, 2004; Wilson, Alderman,
Burgess, Emslie, & Evans, 1996). Although these assessments appear more
“ecologically valid” (i.e., able to predict behavior in everyday situations) than
previous assessments were, further research data on the reliability and validity of
these assessment tools are needed.
INTERVENTION
STRATEGY TRAINING
GROUP INTERVENTIONS
SUMMARY
Recently, there has been a move away from intervention programs that focus
exclusively on remediation of cognitive impairments. There is increasing evidence
that supports the use of comprehensive and holistic cognitive rehabilitation
programs that address a combination of cognitive, emotional, functional, and social
participation skills in people with brain injury (Cappa et al., 2005; Cicerone et al.,
2005, Cicerone, Mott, Azulay, & Friel, 2004; Sarajuuri et al., 2005; Tiersky et al.,
2005). The need to blend cognitive interventions with those that address
interpersonal and real-world functioning has been emphasized in recent literature;
however, the outcome of cognitive rehabilitation is most commonly measured at
the impairment level. As occupational therapists return to more community
focused intervention, we need to widen our perspective on the influence that
cognitive perceptual impairments have on our clients’ ability to engage in the
occupations they need or want to do within the contexts of their lives. We need to
explore the effect of cognitive rehabilitation on occupational engagement and
social participation, for even subtle cognitive impairments can decrease
satisfaction, participation, and quality of life, preventing our clients from leading
enriching lives (McDowd, Filion, Pohl, Richards, & Stiers, 2003). The outcome or
benefit of cognitive rehabilitation needs to be examined broadly across different
populations, including effects on changing existing habits; routines or increasing
productive activity patterns; increasing the frequency and quality of social
participation; decreasing caregiver assistance, stress, or burden; improving
subjective well-being, including self-efficacy, self-esteem, satisfaction and quality
of life; and preventing functional decline.
CHAPTER 14
MOTOR NEURON DISEASE
TYPICAL COURSE
The onset of the disease varies from one individual to another. Some people
experience weakness in the arms with difficulty lifting or doing fine motor tasks or
in the legs with difficulty walking or in the muscles that control speech and
swallowing. Some experience generalized muscle fatigue. If the weakening begins
in the hand, it will progress through the affected limb before becoming more
generalized. Problems with speech or swallowing occur when motor neurons die in
the brain stem. The disease progresses and eventually affects the person’s ability to
walk and continue performing ADL. The disease affects only the motor pathways;
therefore, eye movement, bowel and bladder functions, cognition, personality, and
skin sensation remain intact. Respiratory weakness ultimately is affected as the
muscles deteriorate, and death usually follows unless the individual is put on a
ventilator.
POTENTIAL SYMPTOMS
Ø Damage to lower motor neurons (in the spinal cord) leading to flaccid
paralysis, decreased muscle tone, and decreased reflexes
Ø Damage to upper motor neurons (in the brain) and to the corticospinal
tract leading to spasticity and hyper- reflexia (exaggerated reflexes)
Ø Muscle weakness
Ø Muscle atrophy (distal to proximal)—a symptom that is unique to ALS
Ø Fatigue
Ø Stumbling and falling due to lower extremity weakness
Ø Fasciculation (muscle twitching)
Ø Decreased ability to regulate body temperature
Ø Loss of emotional control/depression
Ø Dysphagia (difficulty swallowing)
Ø Dysarthria (difficulty speaking) due to impaired cranial nerves controlling
speech
Ø Impaired respiration due to muscle weakness
Ø Sialorrhea (excess drooling)
Ø Night cramps
Ø Weight loss
Ø Loss of endurance
Ø Loss of dexterity
MEDICAL MANAGEMENT
PROGNOSIS
Ø Most people with ALS die secondary to respiratory failure
Ø People who go on a ventilator generally live longer than people who do
not
Ø 90% of people with ALS live 3–5 years after the onset of the disease
Ø 10% of people with ALS survive for 10 or more years after the onset
PRECAUTIONS
Ø Pneumonia and pulmonary emboli
Ø Ventilators: intermittent positive pressure ventilation (IPPV) or bilevel
positive airway pressure (BiPap)
Ø Inability to cough to clear normal amount of mucus from airway
Ø Pressure sores due to decreased mobility
Ø Swallowing problems could lead to choking
Ø Posture and balance could lead to falls
Ø Emotional lability (outbursts of laughing or crying)
Ø Difficulty maintaining weight
Ø Shoulder subluxation
Ø Joint contractures
IADL/Leisure Evaluations
Ø Activity Card Sort
Balance Evaluations
Ø Berg Balance Scale
CAREGIVER CONCERNS
People who develop ALS experience a sudden change in life roles that can be
overwhelming. Because of the rapid progression of ALS, it can be difficult for
caregivers to adapt to a loved one’s inability to continue life roles. Discontinuation
of work after the diagnosis of ALS is common and can lead to financial strain. The
sudden decrease in ADL/IADL capabilities can be difficult for the family. Caring
for a family member with ALS can be physically and psychologically demanding
and draining. It is important to address the issue of coping not only with the client
but also with the family members. Because so much is unknown about ALS,
caregivers often have many unanswered questions. Caregiver support groups can
be beneficial in providing the family with necessary support and information.
CHAPTER 15
BELL'S PALSY
Aetiology
Uncertain, but may be associated with viral infections, e.g. herpes simplex and
varicella- zoster; epidemics of Bell’s palsy occur sporadically.
Symptoms
Pain of variable intensity over the ipsilateral mastoid precedes weakness, which
develops over a 48 hour period. Impairment of taste, hyperacusis and salivation
depend on the extent of inflammation and will be lost in more severe cases.
Lacrimation is seldom affected.
Treatment During the acute stage protects the exposed eye during sleep. There is
good evidence prednisolone given in high dosage in the acute stage (50 mg per day
for 10 days) improves recovery. The role of antiviral therapy is less clear as
conflicting results have been found in recent large trials. Eye care (shielding and
artificial tears) is important in preventing corneal abrasion.
Prognosis
Most patients (70%) recover in 4–8 weeks without treatment. In the remainder,
residual facial asymmetry may require corrective surgery. Incomplete paralysis
indicates a good prognosis. In patients with complete paralysis, electrical absence
of denervation on electromyography is an optimistic sign. Occasionally aberrant
reinnervation occurs movement of the angle of the mouth on closing the eyes (jaw
winking) or lacrimation when facial muscles contract (crocodile tears). On
attempting to close the eyes and show the teeth, the one eye does not close and the
eyeball rotates upwards and outwards Bell’s phenomenon (normal eyeball
movement on eye closure).
Diagnosis
CHAPTER 16
MOVEMENT DISORDERS
Cerebellar Dysfunction
Three major phylogenetic subdivisions of the cerebellum are recognised.
1. The anterior lobe (paleocerebellum)
2. The posterior lobe (neocerebellum)
3. The flocculonodular lobe (archicerebellum)
CEREBELLAR DYSFUNCTION
DIAGNOSTIC APPROACH
180 Anatomy
The cerebellum lies in the posterior fossa, posterior to the brain stem, separated
from the cerebrum above by the tentorium cerebelli. The cerebellum consists of
two laterally placed hemispheres and the midline structure the vermis. Receives
afferent fibers from (spinocerebellar pathways) in the spinal cord.
The Purkinje cells give rise to all different axons. These pass either to the deep
nuclei of the cerebellum and then to the brain stem, or to the vestibular nuclei of
the brain stem. From there fibers relay back to the cerebral cortex and thalamus, or
project into the spinal cord, The afferent systemConnections between the vestibular
system and the cerebellum are described on page 173. The spinocerebellar
pathways form a major afferent input. These transmit ‘subconscious’
proprioception from muscles, joints and skin especially of the lower limbs.
The close relationship of structures within the posterior fossa makes the
identification of exclusively cerebellar symptoms and signs difficult. Disease of the
brainstem and its connections may produce identical results.Damage to midline
structures vermis (and flocculonodular lobe) Results in: disturbance of equilibrium
with unsteadiness on standing, walking and even sitting (truncal ataxia). The
patient’s gait is broad based and reeling. Eye closure does not affect balance (see
Romberg’s test). Tests of vestibular function, e.g. calorics, may be impaired.
Damage to hemisphere structures always produces signs ipsilateral to the side of
the lesion. Results in: a loss of the normal capacity to modulate fine voluntary
movements. Errors or inaccuracies cannot be corrected. The patient complains of
impaired limb coordination and certain signs are recognised:Ataxia of extremities
with unsteadiness of gait towards the side of the lesion.
Intention tremor: a tremor which increases as the limb approaches its target
Eye movements
Disturbance of speech
Scanning dysarthria (where the same emphasis is put on each syllable like scanning
a poem) may occur with speech occasionally delivered with sudden unexpected
force explosive speech. Whether dysarthria results from hemisphere or midline
vermis disease remains debatable. Dysarthria, like nystagmus, is an inconsistent
finding in cerebellar disease.
Titubation
Titubation is a rhythmic ‘nodding’ tremor of the head from side to side or to and
fro, usually associated with distal limb tremor. It appears to be of little localizing
value.
Head tilt
Abnormal head tilt suggests a lesion of the anterior vermis. Note that a IV
(trochlear) cranial nerve palsy and tonsillar herniation also produce this abnormal
posture.
Involuntary movements
The following disorders are dealt with in their specific sections. Developmental
Infectious
Ø agenesis – abscess formation
Ø Dandy-Walker malformation – acute cerebellitis (viral)
Ø Arnold-Chiari malformations – Creutzfeldt–Jakob disease
Ø Von Hippel Lindau disease. Metabolic
Demyelinative – myxedema
Ø multiple sclerosis. – hypoxia, hypoglycaemia.
Ø acute disseminated – alcohol (vitamin B1 deficiency) encephalomyelitis
(ADEM) – inborn disorders of metabolism.
Neoplastic Drugs/toxins
Ø Astrocytoma, medulloblastoma, – alcohol hemangioblastoma, metastasis –
phenytoin.
Ø Paraneoplastic – carbamazepine.
Nystagmus
Nystagmus is defined as an involuntary ‘to and fro’ movement of the eyes in a
horizontal, vertical, rotatory or mixed direction. The presence and characteristics of
such movements help localize to the site of neurological disease.Nystagmus may
be pendular – equal velocity and amplitude in all directions, or jerk – with a fast
phase (specifying the direction) and a slow phase. The normal maintenance of
ocular posture and alignment of the eyes with the environment
If nystagmus is detected, note the type (jerk or pendular), direction (of fast phase)
and degree. Nystagmus suppressed by visual fixation may appear in darkness, but
this requires specialized techniques (electronystagmography – see page 65) to
demonstrate.
VESTIBULAR NYSTAGMUS
Physiological
1. Rotational acceleration produces nystagmus in the plane of rotation.
Creates an imbalance between each side resulting in a slow drift of the eyes
towards the damaged side (or side with the reduction in stimulus) followed
by a fast compensatory movement to the opposite side. Slow phase in a
direction tending to maintain the visual image. Fast phase in the opposite
direction. Slow Fast Slow Fast
2. Caloric testing sets up convection currents in the lateral semicircular canal
producing a horizontal nystagmus (see page 65).
Pathological
Damage to labyrinth or vestibular nerve. Slow Fast Slow phase to side of lesion.
Quick or fast phase to normal side. Rotatory components are often present.Turning
eyes away from the side of the lesion increases amplitude but does not change
direction of nystagmus. In severe cases, the nystagmus is 3rd degree and gradually
settles to 1st degree with recovery. Enhanced by loss of ocular fixation. Vertigo
accompanies nystagmus. After a delay of several seconds, nystagmus develops
often with a rotatory component. With repeated testing, the nystagmus fatigues. To
elicit, suddenly reposition the patient: Often associated with tinnitus and hearing
loss. Vertigo and nystagmus settle simultaneously. Occurs in acute labyrinthine
disease Menière’s disease, vestibular neuronitis, vascular disease.
POSITIONAL
AETIOLOGY/INCIDENCE
AETIOLOGY (cont’d)
a. Inactivation of expression of tumor suppressor genes (e.g. mutation of the
p53 gene with loss of heterozygosity on the 17p chromosome in many
patients with low grade astrocytoma).
b. Overexpression of genes controlling growth factor (e.g. amplification of
EGFR in primary glioblastoma).
Clearly defined inherited factors play a minor role. Only 5% of patients have a
family history of brain tumor and with the exception of tuberous sclerosis (related
to the formation of subependymal astrocytomas) and neurofibromatosis (linked to
an increased incidence of schwannoma, optic nerve glioma and meningioma) do
not fall into an obvious autosomal recessive or dominant pattern. Others include
von Hippel-Lindau disease, Cowden’s disease and Li-Fraumeni syndrome.
INCIDENCE
The table below shows the approximate incidence of intracranial tumors extracted
from large series.
Adults Children
TEMPORAL LOBE
HYPOTHALAMUS/PITUITARY
Ø Endocrine dysfunction.
Ø Right/left confusion
Ø Finger agnosia dominant
Ø Acalculia hemisphere
Ø Agraphia
PARIETAL LOBE
Disturbed sensation
Ø localisation of touch
Ø two point discrimination
Ø passive movement
Ø astereognosis
Ø sensory inattention
CEREBELLUM
Ø Ataxic gait
Ø Intention tremor
Ø Dysmetria
Ø Dysarthria
Ø Nystagmus
Ø Infratentorial
MIDBRAIN/BRAIN STEM
Ø Cranial nerve lesions III–XII
Ø Long tract signs – motor and sensory
Ø Deterioration of conscious level
Ø Tremor (red nucleus)
Ø Impaired eye movements
Ø Pupillary abnormalities
Ø Vomiting, hiccough
Chest X-ray, The high incidence of metastatic tumor makes these tests mandatory
in ESR, CRP patients with suspected intracranial tumors. Skull X-ray (if
performed) Note:Pineal shift–if gland is calcified (ensure ‘shift’ is not Towne’s
view due to film rotation). Single or multiple lesions if multiple → metastasis
Effect of contrast enhancement e.g. none – low grade astrocytoma irregular –
malignant astrocytoma homogeneous – meningioma Effect on adjacent bone i.e. if
meningioma → hyperostosis
MASS EFFECT
Ø midline shift.
Ø ventricular compression.
Ø hydrocephalus (secondary to 3rd ventricular or posterior fossa lesion).
Ø obliteration of basal cisterns
Lateral view
OPERATIVE MANAGEMENT
Most patients with intracranial tumors require one or more of the following
approaches:
If necessary, combined with image guidance to aid positioning the flap and to give
accurate lesion localisation
guided biopsy
and clivus provides access to the anterior aspect of the brain stem and upper
cervical cord. Rarely required for anteriorly situated tumors, e.g. neurofibromas,
chordoma.
posterior fossa
OPERATIVE MANAGEMENT (cont’d)
Stereotactic surgery: by rigidly attaching the frame to the patient’s head and using
a CT or MR to identify the position of the locating rods, coordinates are
determined for a selected target allowing accurate placement of a biopsy needle to
within 1 mm (see page 384). This technique is routinely used to biopsy selected
points within the tumor. It is possible to perform a craniotomy and tumor resection
with the frame in place, but the frame tends to impede access and after opening the
bone flap, the brain may shift, introducing errors of localisation. When a
craniotomy is planned, most now use neuronavigation (‘frameless’ stereotaxy) if
available.
When intrinsic tumors lie adjacent to or within eloquent areas within the brain, i.e.
speech area, motor strip, basal ganglia and internal capsule, resection is potentially
hazardous. Various techniques have been developed to try to minimize this risk
fMRI/Diffusion Tensor Imaging (Tractography): Superimposing speech and/or
motor strip areas seen on fMRI and white matter tracts seen on tractography on to
the standard MR image, demonstrates the relationship of the tumor to these crucial
structures. When these images are incorporated into the neuro-navigation system it
enables the surgeon to avoid extending the tumor resection into these areas and
causing irreversible neurological deficits. The reliability of each technique,
however, is still in question and benefits remain uncertain. ‘Awake’ craniotomy: by
either performing the surgery wholly under sedation with local anesthetic, or by
giving an anesthetic for opening and closing the craniotomy and waking the patient
up in between, gives the surgeon the opportunity to identify eloquent areas by
applying electrical stimulation direct to the cortical surface and observing the
functional effect. Studies show that patients tolerate the technique well and
maximal tumor resection is possible with a low risk of deficits.
RADIOTHERAPY
of the following:
Ø megavoltage X-rays (by far the most common method)
Ø electron beam from a linear accelerator (which can also produce
megavoltage X-rays)
Ø accelerated particles from a cyclotron, e.g. nuclei of helium, protons
(awaits full evaluation)
Ø γ rays from cobalt 60.
Oedema, demyelination and radionecrosis may involve the spinal cord after
irradiation of spinal tumors. Other harmful effects include hair loss, skin reactions
and endocrine reaction.
CHEMOTHERAPY
Chemotherapeutic agents have been used for many years in the management of
malignant brain tumors, but their benefits remain limited. Historically drugs most
commonly used include nitrosoureas (e.g. BCNU, CCNU), procarbazine,
vincristine and methotrexate (for lymphoma). Temozolomide, an oral alkylating
agent with excellent blood brain barrier penetration and modest toxicity is
established as an alternative treatment for patients with recurrent high grade
gliomas. It has also been shown to improve survival for patients with newly
diagnosed glioblastoma when given concomitantly with radiotherapy. A
combination of maximal safe surgery followed by combined chemoradiotherapy is
now the standard of care for good performance patients with glioblastoma. Patients
with methylation of the MGMT gene in the tumor appear particularly to benefit.
Carmustine impregnated wafers (Gliadel) may also be considered both as a
primary treatment or for tumor recurrence (see below). Patients with anaplastic
oligodendrogliomas and oligoastrocytoma with loss of heterozygosity on
chromosomes 1p and 19q have a good prognosis and respond well to both radiation
and to alkylating agent based chemotherapy (nitrosoureas, Temozolomide).
Chemotherapy may be used either at initial diagnosis or at relapse in these patients.
Other tumors where chemotherapy plays an important role include
medullo-blastomas, primary CNS lymphomas and germ cell tumors. Traditionally,
chemotherapy has had a lesser role in low grade glial tumors but current studies are
examining its use in both astrocytomas and oligodendrogliomas as an alternative to
radiation in newly diagnosed patients. Problems of drug administration
DEFINITION
CSF forms at a rate of 500 ml/day (0.35 ml/min), secreted predominantly by the
choroid plexus of the lateral, third and fourth ventricles. CSF flows in a caudal
direction through the ventricular system and exits through the foramina of Luschka
and Magendie into the subarachnoid space. After passing through the tentorial
hiatus and over the hemispheric convexity, absorption occurs through the
arachnoid granulations into the venous system.
CLASSIFICATION
PATHOLOGICAL EFFECTS
In the infant, prior to suture fusion, head expansion and massive ventricular
dilatation may occur, often leaving only a thin rim of cerebral ‘mantle’. Untreated,
death may result, but in many cases the hydrocephalus ‘arrests’; although the
ventricles remain dilated, intracranial pressure (ICP) returns to normal and CSF
absorption appears to balance production. When hydrocephalus arrests, normal
developmental patterns resume, although pre-existing mental or physical damage
may leave a permanent handicap. In these patients, the rapid return of further
pressure symptoms following a minor injury or infection suggests that the CSF
INVESTIGATIONS
Skull X-ray
Note: – skull size and suture width.
CT scan
Dilatation
MANAGEMENT
Shunt techniques
A valve is incorporated in the system, with either fixed opening pressure e.g.
Heyer-Schulte, Hakim variable opening pressure (flow regulated) e.g. Orbis sigma,
Delta programmable e.g. Medos, Sophy. Valve opening pressures range from
5–150 mmH2O [Lumboperitoneal shunt catheter inserted into the lumbar theca
either directly at open operation or percutaneously through a Tuohy needle. The
distal end is sited in the peritoneal cavity.]
Complications of shunting
Subdural haematoma: ventricular collapse pulls the cortical surface from the dura
and leaves a subdural CSF collection or tears bridging veins causing subdural
hemorrhage. The risk may be reduced with a variable pressure or programmable
valve.
Shunt obstruction: blockage of the shunt system with choroid plexus, debris,
omentum or blood clot results in intermittent or persistent recurrence of symptoms.
Demonstration of an increase in ventricular size compared to a previous baseline
CT scan confirms shunt malfunction. Over a third require revision within 1 year
and 80% within 10 years.
Low pressure state: following shunting, some patients develop headache and
vomiting on sitting or standing. This low pressure state usually resolves with a high
fluid intake and gradual mobilization. If not, insertion of an antisyphon device or
conversion to a high pressure valve is required.
CHAPTER 18
DEMYELINATING POLYNEUROPATHY)
Incidence: 2 per 100 000 population per year. Characteristically it occurs 1–3
weeks after a viral or other infection or immunization.
Aetiology/pathology
The condition may follow viral infection, e.g. varicella-zoster, mumps and
cytomegalovirus. It is also associated with Mycoplasma, Campylobacter,
infections, immunisations with both live and dead vaccines, antitoxins, trauma,
surgery and, rarely, malignant disease and immunodeficiency. Both antibody and
cell-mediated reactions to peripheral nerve myelin are involved. Some patients
produce antibodies to myelin glycoproteins or gangliosides, others develop a T cell
mediated assault on myelin basic protein. Segmental demyelination results with
secondary axonal damage if the process is severe. Perivascular infiltration with
lymphocytes occurs within peripheral nerves and nerve roots. Lymphocytes and
macrophages release cytotoxic substances (cytokines) which damage Schwann
cell/myelin. When axon damage and nerve cell death occur, regeneration cannot
take place.
Clinical features
Sensory symptoms predominate at the beginning with paraesthesia of the feet, then
hands. Pain, especially back pain, is an occasional initial symptom. Weakness next
develops this
Investigations
CSF protein is elevated in most patients but often not until the second or third
week of illness. Cells are usually absent but in 20% up to 50 cells/mm3 may be
found.
When carried out early in the illness, these may be normal. Findings of multifocal
demyelination soon develop with slowing of motor conduction, conduction block
and prolonged distal motor latencies.
Ancillary investigations
Performed to identify any precipitating infection: e.g. viral and bacterial studies.
Electrolytes are checked for inappropriate secretion of antidiuretic hormone and
immune complex Diagnosis is based on clinical history supported by CSF and
neurophysiological investigation and exclusion of acute spinal cord disease,
porphyria and myasthenia gravis. Some antibodies have been identified as being
associated with some subtypes including:
Treatment
Outcome
Mortality – 2%. Of those progressing to respiratory failure, 20% are left severely
disabled and 10% moderately disabled. In milder cases the outcome is excellent.
Recurrence – 3%. Miller Fisher variant of Guillain Barré The Miller Fisher
syndrome consists of ophthalmoplegia, areflexia and ataxia without significant
limb weakness. Serum IgG antibodies to a specific ganglioside are characteristic
(anti-GQ1B antibodies). Management is that of Guillain Barré.
Dr. Ayres analyzed test scores and clinical observations of children with learning
disabilities to empirically identify patterns of dysfunction (Ayres, 1974, 1989).
Later, Mulligan (1998a, 1998b, 2000) conducted confirmatory factor and cluster
analyses that supported Dr. Ayres’ original patterns of dysfunction. Consequently,
the strongest research in sensory integration is in the identification of the types and
patterns of sensory integration dysfunction using the Sensory Integration and
Praxis Tests (Ayres, 1989; Mulligan, 1998a, 1998b, 2000). Various types and
patterns of sensory processing dysfunction continue to be examined (Ayres &
Tickle, 1980; Brown & Dunn, 2002; Dunn, 1999, 2002a, 2002b; Miller, McIntosh,
McGrath, Shyu, Lampe, & Taylor, 1999; Miller & Summers, 2001). These studies
suggest that sensory integrative dysfunction is not a single disorder but a spectrum
of disorders (Parham & Mailloux, 2005) related to the following processes.
Sensory Modulation
Dr. Ayres (1979, 2005) proposed that the “combination of facilitatory and
inhibitory messages produces modulation, which is the nervous system’s process of
self-organization” Sensory modulation disorder is one type of SI dysfunction
characterized by fluctuating or extremes in the responsiveness to the intensity of
one or more sensations. Difficulties with sensory modulation are often observed
during everyday activities such as grooming, social events, or transitions. Specific
behaviors may include auditory hypersensitivity or tactile defensiveness,
gravitational insecurity, aversion to movement, or feeling over- whelmed in
high-stimulus environments such as shopping malls (Koomar, 1995; Weisberg,
1984). Sensory modulation disorders are seen in conjunction with arousal, external
regulation, or self-regulation issues, including colic and poor rhythmic respiration,
digestion, and elimination; arousal and attention deficits; hyperactivity; anxiety and
other signs of emotional instability; and social problems.
Sensory Discrimination
Praxis
Praxis is the ability to conceptualize, plan, and execute skilled tasks. It underlies
engagement in purposeful activity. Any non-habitual motor task that the child
needs to think through requires praxis. The child who is learning how to dive needs
to stand at the water’s edge, get the arms in position over the head, shift the weight
forward, tuck the chin against the chest, and then maintain this position as he or
she falls head first into the water. All of this requires cognitive effort as well as
physical coordination. Dr. Ayres (1972) hypothesized that sensory discrimination,
particularly of tactile, vestibular, and proprioceptive sensations along with visual
input, was the foundation for praxis. The resulting sensation from the activity is
processed by the brain, and this further informs individuals about their body in
relation to itself as well as to other people and objects in the environment (Ayres,
1989, 2004). Through her factor analyses, she later found a consistent relationship,
particularly between the tactile system and praxis. There are a variety of different
types of praxis dis- orders, including poor ideation of creative or novel activities
(e.g., the child in the sandbox who shovels sand into the pail and then pours it out
and repeats the process but does not come up with the idea that she can “make a
cake” by filling the pail with sand and then turning the pail over to form a mound
that she can then decorate); somatodyspraxia, or poor use of the body to motor plan
action sequences; poor use of language for sequencing and planning; poor ability to
modify an action while in motion to enhance skill and precision; poor visual
construction (e.g., difficulty replicating a block design); and poor ability to
organize behavior in future time and space. Conceptualizing an action, or forming
the idea about what one wants to do, is a critical aspect of praxis. This is ideation,
and it is a cognitive function (May-Benson, 2001). Planning how one intends to
engage in the task is also cognitive. A child observing a newly constructed
playground with swings, slides, tree house, poles, and sand will have 101 ideas
about what to do and will want to do them all. The actual execution of the activity
is the part of praxis that we can observe, and this is the part that is assessed to
determine a child’s skill for motor tasks. The child then sequences these ideas into
swinging, sliding, and digging activities and modifies the sequence or the
challenge so that it is more fun and successful.
ASSESSMENT
The process of sensory integration cannot be observed directly, which is why Dr.
Ayres used the term “hidden disabilities” when referring to SI dysfunction
INTERVENTION PLANNING
INTERVENTION
Currently, there is no single discipline that is responsible for the evaluation and
intervention of cognitive-perceptual impairments. Multiple team members have the
potential to make valuable contributions to understanding the client who has a
cognitive-perceptual impairment. A strong interdisciplinary approach is needed to
address the complex of issues that arise from cognitive-perceptual problems. Team
goals should be identified, as well as specific discipline goals. The family and
client are also members of the team and should be involved in team discussions
and provide input into the overall intervention plan. Occupational therapists
provide a unique contribution to the evaluation and rehabilitation of cognitive
perceptual process skills because of their educational background, knowledge of
occupation, training in activity analysis, and ability to analyze how
cognitive-perceptual symptoms are affected by changes in activity demands and
context. The role of the occupational therapist in evaluating cognition and
perception is to provide clear, comprehensive information on the effect of
cognitive-perceptual impairments on activities of daily living (ADLs), instrumental
activities of daily living (IADLs), education, work, play and leisure, and social
participation. The work environment in which the therapist practices may
determine the depth of the occupational therapist’s involvement because of the
nature of the practice setting and the client’s length of stay. An interdisciplinary
intervention program should emphasize the same major goals during intervention
rather than working on separate skills. For example, the speech language
pathologist might address attention problems within the context of language
material, such as listening to tapes or conversations; the neuropsychologist might
use remedial attentional exercises; the physical therapist might reinforce attention
through motor tasks; and the occupational therapy practitioner might address
attentional strategies within the context of self-care, leisure, community, or work
activities. An integrated approach that assists the per- son in seeing patterns of
behaviors across different activities is strongly advocated, rather than one that
reinforces the fragmentation that the client already perceives. The occupational
therapy assistant works in cooperation with the occupational therapist to contribute
to the evaluation process and implement aspects of the occupational therapy
intervention plan (AOTA, 1999). If state licensure permits and service competency
has been demonstrated, an occupational therapy assistant can administer selected
portions of cognitive assessments as directed by the occupational therapist, provide
clinical observations, and complete behavioral checklists. Once the targeted
behaviors for intervention have been clearly identified, the occupational therapy
assistant and occupational therapist collaborate to choose a variety of different
activities that can be used to reinforce the desired behaviors.
CHAPTER 20
This section provides detailed information about cognitive evaluation issues that
are necessary for practitioners to understand before choosing and performing
appropriate evaluations and assessments. We present the importance of the
evaluation process, its goals, and important considerations in choosing an
assessment. The evaluation process begins with an occupational profile that
considers the client’s typical routines and occupations (AOTA, 2002). The client is
usually asked to identify everyday activities that he or she is most concerned about
or would like to be able to do with greater ease. However, people with cognitive
impairments often have limited awareness of their impairments and limited
understanding of the implications of these impairments (Goverover, Chiaravalloti,
& DeLuca, 2005); therefore, a close relative or friend should participate in
identifying concerns and priorities for intervention. It should be kept in mind that
in inpatient settings, clients and their relatives might be unaware of the presence of
mild cognitive impairments. Subtle cognitive symptoms tend to be apparent only in
higher-level activities such as driving, social participation, shopping, or using
public transportation. A client in an acute or rehabilitation inpatient setting has not
yet had the opportunity to resume these higher-level activities. Comprehensive
cognitive evaluations are needed for two primary reasons. First, evaluations
provide evidence and information about the presence of impairments and
competencies. Such information can be used to establish baselines, to plan
discharge, and to measure intervention effectiveness (e.g., rehabilitation outcomes).
Second, evaluations are needed to gather information for intervention planning.
Models for cognitive intervention in occupational therapy often guide the focus of
evaluation. For example, the cognitive disability model (Allen, 1985) and the neuro
functional model (Giles, 2005) focus on occupational performance and are not
concerned with identifying specific cognitive impairments. The cognitive disability
approach (Allen, 1985) describes hierarchical levels of cognitive function.
Evaluation focuses on identifying the cognitive level at which the person is
functioning. The neuro functional approach (Giles, 2005) emphasizes training
functional skills and habits within naturalistic settings; therefore, evaluation
emphasizes observation of real-life functioning. The quadraphonic approach
(Abreu & Peloquin, 2005) and the cognitive retraining model (Averbuch & Katz,
2005) are concerned with identifying and understanding the cognitive impairments
that are influencing occupational performance. Measures of cognitive impairment
are examined in combination with broader measures of occupation to guide
intervention. The multi context approach (Toglia, 2005) is concerned with
facilitating transfer of learning, so evaluation emphasizes evaluation of learning
potential or dynamic assessment. Each of these perspectives is described in greater
detail later in the chapter.
Interventions for people with cognitive dysfunction differ in the areas that are
targeted for intervention and in the underlying assumptions about individuals’
abilities to learn and generalize information. The characteristics and underlying
assumptions of the different intervention approaches are explored in this section.
Factors that are critical in the selection of these intervention approaches as well as
methods for systematically integrating them are also discussed. As you read this
section, review the case study on cognition and performance context.
Adaptation
Compensation
CHAPTER 21
MULTIPLE SCLEROSIS
Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a progressive neurological condition that is
characterized by patches of demyelination of nerves in areas of the brain and the
spinal cord, which result in distorted or interrupted transmission of nerve impulses
to and from the brain (Beers & Berkow, 1999). MS is considered to be an
autoimmune disease. The body’s own defense system attacks the myelin sheath
that surrounds and protects the nerve fibers of the CNS. The sites where myelin is
lost appear as hardened sclerotic (scarred) areas in the CNS and cause a variety of
physical and neurological symptoms (Reed, 2001).
PREVALENCE/INCIDENCE
Approximately 400,000 Americans have MS, and every week, about 200 people
are diagnosed. Worldwide, MS may affect 2.5 million individuals. Most people
with MS are diagnosed between the ages of 20 and 50 years. It affects twice as
many women as men.
COURSE
People with MS can expect one of four clinical courses of disease, each of which
might be mild, moderate, or severe (National Multiple Sclerosis Society, 2004).
Relapsing-Remitting
Primary Progressive
In the primary-progressive course, the person experiences a slow but nearly
continuous worsening of the disease from the onset, with no distinct relapses or
remissions. However, there are variations in rates of progression over time,
occasional plateaus, and temporary minor improvements. This course is relatively
rare (∼10%).
Secondary-Progressive
Progressive-Relapsing
PROGNOSIS
SYMPTOMS
Secondary Signs
Ø Pain
Ø Staggering gait and/or loss of balance
Ø Poor coordination
Ø Loss of bladder or bowel control; frequent urinary tract infections
Ø Anxiety and sleep disturbances
Ø Contractures and pressure sores
Ø Decreased ADL skills
Tertiary Signs
Ø Shift in roles, changing responsibilities
Ø Social isolation
Ø Divorce
Ø Loss of independence
Ø Stigma of disability (Shapiro, 2003)
INTERDISCIPLINARY TREATMENT
Medications
Ø Corticosteroids are used to shorten acute attacks, reduce inflammation,
and ease symptoms.
Ø Disease-modifying treatment with injections of the protein interferon-beta
reduces the frequency of re- lapses in MS and might help to delay eventual
disability. These medications have shown effectiveness in modifying the
natural course of relapsing MS by altering the rate and/or extent of disease
progression. Medication management with these “disease modifiers” is
recommended as early as possible for individuals with a relapsing course.
Ø More potent medications are being developed that are effective in slowing
down MS that is rapidly worsening or becoming progressive (Beers &
Berkow, 1999).
Rehabilitation
Ø Physical therapy: Physical therapy focuses primarily on mobility and the
use of mobility aids, spasticity of the muscles, and physical fitness.
Personalized exercise programs may help people recover muscle control and
strength after an exacerbation. There is significant evidence associating
aerobic exercise with improved quality of life, mobility, endurance, and
reduction in fatigue (Mostert & Kesselring, 2002). Hydrotherapy is a
popular form of aerobic exercise that addresses the fatigue and weakness
experienced by individuals with MS.
Ø Occupational therapy: Occupational therapy focuses on strength,
coordination, and fine motor control of the upper extremities. Fatigue and
pain management techniques have been well supported as a means to
improve occupational performance and satisfaction. Psycho- social
intervention is embedded in the treatment plan to address cognitive,
emotional, and self-concept issues.
Ø Speech therapy: In progressive forms of MS, problems with speech or
swallowing due to muscle weakness or a lack of coordination may need to
be addressed.
Ø Psychosocial support/counseling: Individual or group therapy can help
individuals with MS and their families to deal with depression, anxiety, and
the unpredictability of the disease process. Evidence suggests that a
personalized psychosocial rehabilitation program encourages active
participation, increased autonomy, and improved quality of life (Ferriani et
al., 2002).
Comprehensive Evaluations
Ø The Functional Assessment of Multiple Sclerosis (FAMS)
Ø Functional Independence Measure (FIM)
Ø Multiple Sclerosis Impact Scale (MSIS-29)
ADL Evaluations
Ø Assessment of Motor and Process Skills (AMPS)
Ø Barthel Index (BI)
Fatigue Evaluations
Ø Fatigue Severity Scale (FSS)
Ø Modified Fatigue Impact Scale (MFIS)
Psychosocial Evaluations
Ø Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)
Ø Self-Perceived Burden Scale (SPBS)
Ø Ways of Coping Checklist (WCC)
OT INTERVENTIONS
Ø ADL training
Ø Assistive technology
Ø Cognitive retraining
Ø Employment modifications
Ø Energy conservation
Ø Environmental modifications
Ø Home management
Ø Pain treatment
Ø ROM/endurance/strengthening for functional activity
Ø Safety awareness
Ø Splinting
Ø Stress management
OT AND THE EVIDENCE
CAREGIVER CONCERNS
Approximately 100,000 people who have MS require help with daily activities or
personal care. They receive most of their help from spouses, who typically have
major additional responsibilities, including employment and child care. Caregiver
burnout has many different causes, including physical strain and emotional stress.
Some ways to address these issues include the following:
Ø Effective communication
Ø Relieving pressures of caregiving
Ø Planning and decision making for the future (Kalb.