Decline of The Native Speaker

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 156

Chapter 11

The Decline of the Native Speaker


DAVID GRADDOL The Demographic Evidence
Discussion of the place of English in the world often centres on numbers, particularly estimates of the growing numbers of people who speak English. This numerological tradition, like so much of our understanding of the history of English, was largely shaped in the nineteenth century a century obsessed by numbers and accounting practices required by the new industries, the formation of public companies and the administration of empire. At this time, there also emerged a rivalry between three European languages, particularly French and English but also to some extent German, as languages of international communication. French had been the undisputed international language of diplomacy during a period in which diplomacy formed the basis for international affairs. But in the second half of the nineteenth century, English challenged the status of French as an international language. The combination of industrial development and the British empire helped position English as the language of global trade, at a time when trade was taking over from diplomacy as the basis for international affairs. French was the catholic language of ideas and diplomacy. Its promoters made much of its beauty, literature and civilised cultural values. English was the protestant, working language of commerce. Its supporters promoted its virtues with frequent reference to numbers relating to its size and global reach. The industrial revolution had created a commercial culture which routinely dealt in numbers: new typographic styles and printing techniques were developed to handle new informational genres as railway timetables and shareholder accounts. Numbers became an important component of persuasive rhetoric and the larger the numbers were, the better. It is not surprising that contemporary commentators promoting the place of English employed such rhetoric. Bailey (1992) quotes, for example, the estimates of native speakers made by a Swiss scientist, Alphonse de Candolle in 1870:
156

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 157

The Decline of the Native Speaker

157

In England it doubles in 50 years; therefore, in a century (in 1970) it will be 124,000,000. In the United States, in Canada, in Australia, it doubles in twenty-five; therefore it will be 736,000,000. Probable total of English-speaking race in 1970, 860,000,000. (Bailey, 1992: 111) One can detect, even a century or so later, the sheer exhilaration in all those zeros. Figure 11.1 shows the logarithmic algorithm employed by Candolle, and projects the numbers of English speakers a century before and after the 100-year period he describes. It suggests that in 1770 there were only about seven million speakers of English, but by 2070 the projected number would be nearly 10 billion: rather more than the expected total population of the planet.

Figure 11.1 Candolles 1870 prediction


Candolle assumed a logarithmic growth of English speakers. Extrapolating a century before and after, using his calculations, shows that there should be over a billion native speakers of English today.

We may now smile at nineteenth century confidence in forecasts for English, but there undoubtedly remains a fascination with speaker numbers despite the fact that there are many other aspects of global English which might better capture its growing importance in the world. The issue of How many speakers? remains, for example, top of the list of frequently asked questions about global English, according to the British Councils FAQ.1 This paper examines what we know about

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 158

158

Translation in the New Millennium

trends in English language usage and provides a principled basis for estimating and forecasting the numbers of people speaking English in the world.

Long-term Growth of English


Although languages arise and evolve in fluid ways which prevent any straightforward location on a time-line, English provides an unusually clear starting point of origin in time and space because of a combination of factors: it arose from migration; that migration was to an island territory with well-defined borders; and the pre-existing languages with which the immigrants came into contact (Celtic, Latin) were not close linguistic relatives. Of course, the first boat-loads of immigrants to Britain in the fifth century did not arrive speaking English. They are likely to have spoken various Germanic varieties which may have been mutually intelligible. Here the official histories which tell of three tribes Angles, Saxons and Jutes are clearer than the evidence properly allows. But Old English somehow emerged from the mix, at least two or three generations after substantial settlement. This provides a credible starting point of around AD 700, at which time the numbers of speakers must have been in the low thousands. Before the European colonisation of North America in the seventeenth century, the number of first language English speakers could not have exceeded about seven million, almost all in the British Isles. During the nineteenth century, numbers of English speakers in North America overtook those in the British Isles, but numbers of native English speakers worldwide are unlikely to have passed the 100 million mark until the end of the nineteenth century. Figure 11.2 provides an overview of the long-term growth of English native-speakers. It is fairly easy to see where Candolle, and others with similarly extravagant forecasts about the numbers of native English speakers, went wrong. First, it was assumed that the growth of English speakers could be projected from the population trends in North America and Britain, although these were at a historic height because of largescale emigration from Europe to the US and rapid industrialisation and urbanisation of the late nineteenth century. In the event, population growth in the industrialised world has fallen sharply in the twentieth century in some European countries it has not only stabilised but is actually falling. Second, Candolles estimates did not allow for the way that English has spread to many more countries in particular those forming what Kachru (1984) calls the outer circle.

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 159

The Decline of the Native Speaker

159

Figure 11.2 Estimated growth of numbers of English native speakers since Anglo-Saxon times In The Future of English?, a research document published by the British Council (Graddol, 1977), I put forward a model for the growth of native speakers of English which provides a basis for projecting future trends. The model relies on two kinds of data: full demographic forecasts for populations of countries in which English is spoken as a first language and estimates of the proportion of those populations for whom English is a mother tongue. In principle, all the major world languages can be modelled in this way, but English is perhaps the most difficult. McArthur (1998: 42) lists 113 territories which currently use English on a widespread, sustained basis. The Ethnologue 2 database of world languages maintained by the Summer Institute of Linguistics lists 83 countries and territories in which English is spoken. Ideally, any model forecasting numbers of speakers requires data for each of these countries. Country by country population data is available from the United Nations Population Division. The figures reported in this article draw on the Sex and Age Quinquennial dataset for 19502050 (1998 revision). This dataset provides detailed population forecasts, including numbers for males and females in each of 21 age-bands, at five-year intervals through to 2050. Such detailed population forecasts are insufficient on their own. We also need to know the population of each countrys population who speak English as their mother tongue. This is a surprisingly difficult statistic to obtain. It raises some of the most problematical issues in linguistic surveys, such as What counts as a native speaker? How do you collect such information systematically in many countries? How do people interpret questions about language usage? Do people answer such questions honestly? Even in those countries such as Britain and the US where the

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 160

160

Translation in the New Millennium

linguistic culture is often described as monolingual, it is clear that a significant proportion of the population speak languages other than English as their first language. Furthermore, the assumption of low linguistic diversity in such countries means that language questions are rarely included in national census returns. We are left with an unsatisfactory rag-bag of sources from partial studies in different years, often based on very different criteria. The problem is demonstrated by the Ethnologue itself which estimates the global number of English first-language speakers at 322 million. They cite two sources for this estimate: a 1984 study of the UK, and the World Almanac (1995). The latter provides a half-page table of language statistics below a list of pen names of famous writers. The language data are compiled by a retired professor of psychology with an interest in Esperanto, but there appears to be no published account of how the figures were arrived at. Despite the problems associated with estimating native speaker numbers, approximate figures for present-day usage can be derived. David Crystal (1997), for example, provides estimates of L1 speakers in 56 countries, arriving at a total of over 337 million speakers worldwide. These figures can provide a starting point for modelling long-term trends. Since the size of population of each country in 1995 is known, we can estimate the proportion of the population who, according to David Crystals estimates, speak English as their first language. In practice, we can model only 24 of the 56 countries in this way these are the ones with large enough populations to be included in the UN dataset. The areas excluded from the model are mainly island territories, such as St Kitts and Nevis (with an estimated 39,000 speakers) and Guam (56,000). It is clearly important to include these areas in any comprehensive description of English in the World, but their exclusion from the statistical model makes almost no difference to global estimates. Figure 11.3 shows the projections provided by the model for native speaker numbers during the century 19502050. It shows that the growth of L1 speakers will slow in about 20 years time, leading to a likely total population of around 433 million in 2050. Despite the continued gradual growth in absolute numbers of native speakers, the proportion of the worlds population who speak English as their first language has, in fact, declined sharply. Figure 11.4 shows the same numbers as Figure 11.3, but expressed as a proportion of the worlds population calculated year-on-year. It shows the impact on English of the rapid population increase in parts of the world where English is not spoken as a first language. This represents a significant demographic shift away from English to other, non-European languages.

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 161

The Decline of the Native Speaker

161

Figure 11.3 Projections for native speaker numbers to 2050, and estimates 19501995

Figure 11.4 Decline of native English speakers as a proportion of the worlds population
In 1950, over 8% of the worlds population spoke English as their first language; by 2050 it will be less than 5%.

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 162

162

Translation in the New Millennium

Second Language Speakers


The apparent decline in the position of English native speakers does not necessarily herald a decline in the importance of the English language. The future status of English will be determined less by the number and economic power of its native speakers than by the trends in the use of English as a second language. During the twentieth century, the number of people using English as a second language has steadily grown. In his Encyclopedia of the English Language (Crystal, 1995) David Crystal estimates that only 98 million L2 speakers of English can be identified with confidence, though he suggests that making reasonable assumptions about L2 use in countries where there are also significant proportions of L1 users could bring this to about 180 million worldwide. This figure is broadly in line with the estimate provided by the World Almanac, which suggests 167 million (WA, 1999). Both these figures, however, suggest that the use of English in the world is much less than is generally assumed. Crystal (1997) provides a detailed table of estimated L2 usage in 66 countries, giving a grand total of 235 million L2 speakers worldwide. This seems a good starting point for modelling L2 usage over time. Line B on Figure 11.5 shows the projections of L2 numbers over the century 19502050, based on the assumption that Crystals figures provide an accurate snapshop of L2 demography in 1995, and using the proportions of national populations which are implied by these figures. Some 50 countries are included in this model. These projections suggest that, based solely on expected population changes, the number of people using English as their second language will grow from 235 million to around 462 million during the next 50 years. This indicates that the balance between L1 and L2 speakers will critically change, with L2 speakers eventually overtaking L1 speakers. One problem with these calculations is that 235 million L2 English speakers must also be an underestimate much lower than the figure of 350400 million speakers that regularly appears in the literature. Crystal himself draws attention to the gap between his tabulated figures and this consensus total. The problem is not that the number of L2 speakers of English in the world has been grossly over-stated. Rather, there are many countries in which English is used extensively, but for which there exist no statistics other than those provided by educated guesswork. In several African, Latin American and Asian countries, for example, undocumented numbers of people speak English as a second language. One way forward, in terms of modelling, is to make a notional allowance of 1% of populations in Sub-Saharan Africa, in the knowledge that this figure may be on the high side for some countries, but an underestimate for others. This brings the total population of second language speakers in

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 163

The Decline of the Native Speaker

163

1995 to 278 million. A similar allowance can be made for Latin America, particularly Argentina, Costa Rica, Honduras, Nicaragua and Panama. This addition brings the estimated total of L2 speakers worldwide in 1995 to 351 millions. By 2050 this may have grown to 668 millions (Figure 11.5, Line C).

Figure 11.5 Estimates of speakers of English as a second language worldwide


A B C D L1 speakers (see also Figure 11.3) L2 estimates based on Crystal (1997) figures for 1995 Further allowances made for Latin America and Africa Assumptions of higher usage in India, Pakistan (20%) and Bangladesh (10%)

English Speakers in India


India contains a significant proportion of the worlds speakers of English as a second language, but estimating the number of L2 speakers of English there is difficult. Agnihotri and Khanna (1997: 36), in a recent report on English usage in India, tell us: The overwhelming importance of English in several important domains of Indian society and the vested interests of the English language industry have understandably produced a considerable amount of confusion in the description and interpretation of data on English bilingualism. How many Indians know English? How many of these know it fluently and use it regularly in their day-do-day affairs? What percentage of Indian bilinguals are English-speaking? We do not have very reliable answers to these questions. Collecting and publishing such statistics in India is, as in many other countries, politically sensitive. The unreliability of census data is high-

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 164

164

Translation in the New Millennium

dramatically, according to census data, between 1971 and 1981 from 25 million to 3 million. This reflected a change in the census questions on language rather than actual patterns of English usage. Mahapatra (1990: 9) suggests that there has been a deliberate suppression of linguistic data on the extent of Indian multilingualism. Certainly, there seems to be a tradition of talking down the extent of Indian English usage. Traditionally, almost no Indians are regarded as using English as their first language (a notion which will undoubtedly come under further scrutiny in the future). Crystal (1997) suggests a figure of around 0.03% of the population. Srivastava (1990: 50) claims English is spoken and understood by only 2% of the population. Most linguists, however, seem to agree that around 4% of the Indian population speaks English as a second language. Kachru (1984), for example, suggests 4% as a conservative estimate; Crystal (1997) presents a similar figure. This suggests there were a little more than 37 million L2 users in 1995. This figure is represented in Line B of Figure 11.5. There is evidence, however, that the number of English speakers in India is higher than this. The magazine India Today, commissioned a survey in mid 1997 which claimed: contrary to the census myth that English is the language of a microscopic minority, the poll indicates that almost one in three Indians claims to understand English, although less than 20 per cent are confident of speaking it. (India Today, August 18, 1997). If the figure of 20% is accurate then there must be around 186 million English speakers in India. Of course, it is unlikely that India is the only country in which data is inaccurate. The global number of L2 speakers of English would be significantly affected if similar revisions were made to estimates in Bangladesh and Pakistan. Line D on Figure 11.5 shows the effect of upgrading the numbers for India and Pakistan to 20%, and Bangladesh to 10%. This trajectory assumes there were around 418 million L2 speakers worldwide in 1995 and indicates that by 2050 there may be 790 million L2 speakers. Although this projection is higher than Line B, the starting point (of 418 million speakers in 1995) is not much higher than the consensus figures. The model allows us to understand better the different projections which result from taking the lower or higher estimates.

The Place of English in Europe


Outside of the UK and the Irish Republic, English is traditionally regarded as a foreign, rather than second, language in Europe. This view may be out of date. Europe is rapidly integrating and reinventing itself as a multilingual area in which English plays an increasingly important role as a second language. In effect, it is becoming more like India as a

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 165

The Decline of the Native Speaker

165

geolinguistic space. Eurobarometer surveys of the populations of European Union (EU) countries have collected data about language usage for some years, and it is clear that in some countries English is now widely spoken: 77% of Danish adults and 75% of Swedish adults for example, say they can take part in a conversation in English. Eurobarometer 50, based on a survey made in late 1998, concludes that nearly one third of the citizens of the 13 non English-speaking countries in the EU can speak English well enough to take part in a conversation. Furthermore, it seems that the function of English is changing: European citizens learn and use English in order to communicate with European nationals from all countries and not just with native speakers. English, in other words, is fast becoming a second language in Europe. Eurobarometer 41, based on data collected at the end of 1994 and analysed by Labrie and Quell (1997), found large country variations in English usage, ranging from Spain (with 13% of adults able to hold a conversation in English) to the northern European countries such as Sweden (75%) and The Netherlands (71%). Figure 11.6, Line E shows the projections for European L2 use, based on the 13 relevant member countries, together with estimates for Norway and Switzerland. These suggest that in 1995 there were around 95 million English speakers in these 15 countries. The relatively static population figures for Europe mean that this number will soon peak, and then gradually reduce during the next 50 years.

Figure 11.6 Estimates of L2 English speakers in Europe


Line E shows projections based on Eurobarometer survey made at the end of 1994. Line F takes into account potential language shift, based on comparisons of Eurobarometer data from 1990, 1994, 1995 and 1998.

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 166

166

Translation in the New Millennium

Modelling Language Shift


The European case-study draws attention to another weakness of forecasts of language usage based on population figures alone. It is clear that the use of English in Europe has grown faster than can be accounted for by population growth. There has been a language shift towards English: a higher proportion of the populations of European countries now speak English than was true in previous generations. In modelling projections of second language use of English, we need to take into account such patterns of language shift. The Eurobarometer surveys provide some evidence of trends in Europe. For example, in 1990, 26% of the French population claimed they could easily engage in a conversation in English. By 1998 that figure had increased to 32%. An analysis of the survey data from 1994, the mid-point of this period, also showed a significant difference between the language proficiency of older and younger people (Labrie and Quell, 1997). There is evidence of language shift, therefore, both from trend data arising from surveys made at different points in time and from intergenerational data (Figure 11.7).

Figure 11.7 Shift towards English in Europe


Eurobarometer survey data from 1994 showed a significant intergenerational shift in language use in European countries. Here is shown data from France, as presented by Labrie and Quell (1997).

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 167

The Decline of the Native Speaker

167

The shift in Europe towards English which reflects increased biand multilingualism rather than abandonment of other languages undoubtedly has several causes, including significant changes to language curriculums in school; exchange programmes encouraging young people to move between EU member countries; the growing importance and utility of English as a second language in Europe; and greater exposure to English cultural products. The problem in modelling such shifts is that the rate of shift itself changes (Figure 11.8). Line F of Figure 11.6 shows the consequences of making conservative assumptions about the rate of shift in Europe. It assumes that ongoing shifts, as estimated from successive Eurobarometer surveys, are now at their most rapid point of development, and that in the future the rate of change will decline according to the curve shown in Figure 11.8. Line F should therefore safely underestimate the future trend towards L2 English usage in Europe. Throughout the EU as a whole, the increase in the number of people speaking English seems now to be in the region of just over 3% per year. (So, if 31% of the EU population spoke English as a second language in 1998, we would expect that to increase to 31.9% in 1999 and 32.9% in the year 2000.) It is clear, however, that the trend towards English is occurring

Figure 11.8 The trajectory of social changes


Most social changes do not occur at a linear rate over time. Their progress usually takes the form of S curve: a change may develop relatively slowly, gather speed, and then slow up again.

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 168

168

Translation in the New Millennium

more rapidly in some countries than others. Eurobarometer surveys suggest that of all EU countries, shift is slowest in Portugal and fastest in Spain. The projections in Line F suggest that the present one third of the EU population who speak English will grow to roughly two thirds to 2050. But the EU is likely to acquire new members during this time, such as Hungary and Poland. This will alter the relative positions of major European languages within the EU in less predictable ways. A similar modelling of language shift can be applied to all countries in the world where English is spoken as an L2. Figure 11.9, Line G shows the effect of adding the European numbers of L2 speakers to the global figure (i.e. Line D and E added together). This provides a grand total of 518 million L2 speakers in 1995, rising to 880 million by 2050. Line H includes conservative estimates of language shift in all countries with significant numbers of L2 speakers. This starts from the same point of 518 million speakers in 1995, but projects 1.2 billion speakers by 2050. Figure 11.10 compares the lowest estimates in this paper (Line B) and the highest (Line H). The future is likely to lie somewhere between these two extremes. The lowest variant (B) suggests that L2 speakers will not outnumber native speakers until the 2030s. The highest variant (H) suggests that native speakers lost their majority in the 1970s.

Figure 11.9 Global estimates of L2 usage, including Europe


Line G shows projections based on 1995 estimates of L2 usage. Line H includes estimates of language shift.

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 169

The Decline of the Native Speaker

169

Figure 11.10 High and low estimates for global use of English as a second language

Changing Discourses of English


Problems of definition There are two approaches to defining a second language user, both of which focus on the status of English in the speakers speech community. The first identifies L2 countries: countries where English serves a role in intranational communication the language may be officially recognised for such purposes although only a minority of the population actually understand and use English. The second approach defines an L2 speech community as one which is norm creating that is, developing its own institutionalised variety of English. These are the so-called New Englishes: mainly in former British colonial territories. Problems of definition are becoming acute, as English takes on a role as a global lingua franca, and as speech communities redefine themselves as cross-border affiliation groups rather than as geographic groups in national boundaries. Europe is a case in point, containing an increasing number of fluent speakers of English who do not conform to the traditional definition of L2 speaker and who are excluded from most estimates of L2 usage.

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 170

170

Translation in the New Millennium

The status of native speaker The decline of the native speaker in numerical terms is likely to be associated with changing ideas about the centrality of the native speaker to norms of usage. There has been a lively debate about the cult of the native speaker: do native speakers have privileged access to an understanding of the language, and are they therefore more reliable informants and teachers? The special status accorded to native speakers is long standing. In theoretical linguistics, it is associated with the rise of Chomskyan linguistics and the special role assigned to introspection and intuition in theory-building. The native speaker is claimed to have access to a much richer source of data (in judgements of grammaticality, for example) than a researcher could discover by studying a speakers actual utterances. This theoretical turn has had political repercussions. As the US was the centre of research and publishing the new paradigm, English became a privileged object of study. Theoretical linguistic studies of other languages lagged behind those of English. Although it was maintained that the project with Transformational Generative Grammar was a universalist one (more interested in universal properties of the human mind than the characteristics of particular languages), English remained the centre of attention. It would be wrong, however, to think that the importance of native speakers began with Chomsky. Traditional dialectologists, as well as anthropologists, drew on similar ideas of good speakers (often old, male, non-mobile). But the discourse of the native speaker is even older it is a product of modernity. Since the European Renaissance, identities have been constructed according to a particular model of perfection: unified, singular, well-ordered. Language has played a major role in the construction of modern European identities from the level of nation states and standard languages, to the subjectivities of individual speakers. Multilingualism, both in individual and social terms, does not fit well with concepts of modernity: multilingual speakers and societies have long been regarded with suspicion, and have caused problems for public policy. During the late twentieth century, much of the modernity project has unravelled. The construction of a new Europe has resulted in new formations of identity; greater mobility; and a new hierarchy of languages which places most European citizens in a plurilingual context. In many countries in which English is learned and spoken as a second or foreign language, the centrality of the native speaker is being challenged. At one time, the most important question regarding global English seemed to be will US English or British English provide the world model? Already

Chapter 11 24/6/02 12:21 pm Page 171

The Decline of the Native Speaker

171

that question is looking dated, with the emergence of New Englishes, and dictionaries and grammars that codify new norms. But the tantalising question still remains. Large numbers of people will learn English as a foreign language in the twenty-first century and they will need teachers, dictionaries and grammar books. But will they continue to look towards the native speaker for authoritative norms of usage?

Notes
1. http://www.britishcouncil.org/english/engfaqs.htm 2. http://www.sil.org/Ethnologue/

References
Agnihotri, R.K. and Khanna, A.L. (1977) Problematizing English in India. New Delhi: Sage. Crystal, D. (1995) Encyclopedia of the English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crystal, D. (1997) English as a Global Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Graddol, D. (1997) The Future of English? London: The British Council. Kachru, B.B. (1984) Standards, codification and sociolinguistic realism: The English language in the outer circle. In R. Quirk and H.G. Widdowson (eds) English in the World: Teaching and Learning the Language and Literatures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press for The British Council. Labrie, N. and Quell, C. (1997) Your language, my language or English? The potential language choice in communication among nationals of the European Union. World Englishes 16: 326. McArthur, T. (1998) The English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Mahapatra, B.P. (1990) Multilingualism in India: A demographic app raisal. In D.P. Pattanayak (ed.) Multilingualism in India. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. Srivastava, A.K. (1990) Multilingualism and school education in India: Special features, problems and prospects. In D.P. Pattanayak (ed.) Multilingualism in India. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy