Neurolinguistics
Neurolinguistics
Neurolinguistics
Soundararajan
Associate Professor in English
1
Neurolinguistics
How do humans learn to understand language?
What are the first steps in language acquisition?
How does language develop from a baby to a child and so on?
Are there certain preconditions in our brains that support language?
And, most importantly, why is language important?
Introduction
Neurolinguistics studies the relation of language and communication to different aspects of
brain function, i.e. it tries to explore how the brain understands and produces language and
communication.
This involves attempting to combine theory from neurology/neurophysiology (how the brain
is structured and how it functions) with linguistic theory (how language is structured and how
it functions).
Neurolinguistics is a branch of Cognitive Neuroscience, that, on its turn, together with many
other fields such as Systemic, Movement, Sensory, Cellular and others, is a branch of a
larger domain named the Neurosciences. Neurolinguistics can still be divided into two areas:
language acquisition and processing and language impairment.
As for as adults concerned, based on their knowledge of the world already represented in
their minds, they know or are able to make educated guesses about the contents of closed
containers. This system of world knowledge acquisition that maps objects to abstract, mental
representations gradually breaks children’s innocence in relation to the contents in the world
around them.
Since world concepts and objects are constantly updated along with collective history, the
break of a child’s innocence happens in accordance with the technology and the scientific
bias of a specific time.
The understanding of the mind-brain
Understanding the complex relationship between the mind and the brain means connecting
the subtle effects of cognition – speech, hearing, vision, motor coordination, memory and
others – to the brain, the material portion that gives rise to cognition.
The Central Nervous System consists of the spinal cord and the brain. (The peripheral
nervous system consists of neurons elsewhere in the body, motor and sensory, which are
connected to the spinal cord.) The brain is that portion of the central nervous system which
is located inside the skull. It is commonly divided into five main parts, listed here in order
from lowest (adjacent to the spinal cord) to highest (farthest from the spinal cord):
Certain parts of the brain are responsible for understanding words and sentences. These
brain areas are mainly located in two regions, in the left side of the brain, and are connected
by nerves. Together, these brain regions and their connections form a network that provides
the hardware for language in the brain. Without this brain network, we would not be able to
talk or to understand what’s being said.
Dr. R. Soundararajan
Associate Professor in English
3
Two brain regions are highlighted in red and orange. These regions are strongly involved in
processing speech and language. The blue and green lines illustrate connections that link
the two regions with one another and form a network of language areas. There is an upper
nerve connection (blue) and a lower nerve connection (green).
The above picture illustrates this talkative mesh in the brain. The connections within this
network are particularly important, because they allow the network nodes to exchange
information.
BROCA’S AREA , WERNICKE’S AREA, AND OTHER LANGUAGE-PROCESSING
AREAS IN THE BRAIN
Broca’s area is generally defined as comprising Brodmann areas 44 and 45, which lie
anterior to the premotor cortex in the inferior posterior portion of the frontal lobe. Though
both area 44 and area 45 contribute to verbal fluency, each seems to have a separate
function, so that Broca’s area can be divided into two functional units.
Area 44 (the posterior part of the inferior frontal gyrus) seems to be involved in phonological
processing and in language production as such; this role would be facilitated by its position
close to the motor centres for the mouth and the tongue. Area 45 (the anterior part of the
inferior frontal gyrus) seems more involved in the semantic aspects of language. Though not
directly involved in accessing meaning, Broca’s area therefore plays a role in verbal memory
(selecting and manipulating semantic elements).
Wernicke’s area lies in the left temporal lobe and, like Broca’s area, is no longer regarded as
a single, uniform anatomical/functional region of the brain. By analyzing data from numerous
brain-imaging experiments, researchers have now distinguished three sub-areas within
Wernicke’s area. The first responds to spoken words (including the individual’s own) and
other sounds. The second responds only to words spoken by someone else but is also
Dr. R. Soundararajan
Associate Professor in English
4
activated when the individual recalls a list of words. The third sub-area seems more closely
associated with producing speech than with perceiving it. All of these findings are still
compatible, however, with the general role of Wernicke’s area, which relates to the
representation of phonetic sequences, regardless of whether the individual hears them,
generates them himself or herself, or recalls them from memory.
Wernicke’s area, of which the temporal planum is a key anatomical component, is located on
the superior temporal gyrus, in the superior portion of Brodmann area 22. This is a strategic
location, given the language functions that Wernicke’s area performs. It lies between the
primary auditory cortex (Brodmann areas 41 and 42) and the inferior parietal lobule.
This lobule is composed mainly of two distinct regions: caudally, the angular gyrus (area 39),
which itself is bounded by the visual occipital areas (areas 17, 18, and 19), and dorsally, the
supramarginal gyrus (area 40) which arches over the end of the lateral sulcus, adjacent to
the inferior portion of the somatosensory cortex.
Research has identified two primary “language centers,” which are both located on the left
side of the brain. These are Broca's area, tasked with directing the processes that lead to
speech utterance, and Wernicke's area , whose main role is to “decode” speech.
What part of the brain is language?
Broca's area, located in the frontal lobe of the brain, is linked to speech production, and
recent studies have shown that it also plays a significant role in language comprehension.
Broca's area works in conjunction with working memory to allow a person to use verbal
expression and spoken words.
Why is the brain so well suited to learning language?
Language learning helps improve people's thinking skills and memory abilities. ... “Because
the language centers in the brain are so flexible, learning a second language can develop
new areas of your mind and strengthen your brain's natural ability to focus."
Issues in neurolinguistics and linguistic aphasiology
There are two types of language disorders: acquired language disorders and developmental
language disorders. Acquired language disorders result from brain damage, while
developmental language disorders do not.
The study of aphasia, an acquired language disorder, is a central part of neurolinguistics. It
is the loss of language ability due to brain damage. There are numerous types of aphasia,
including the following:
Broca's aphasia: Caused by damage to Broca's area, it results in declined speech
production, though comprehension remains relatively normal. Agrammatic speech is a
characteristic of Broca's aphasia.
Wernicke's aphasia: Caused by damage to Wernicke's area, it results in reduced
comprehension ability. People who suffer from Wernicke's aphasia can produce fluent
speech, but their speech typically makes no sense. Anomia, a chronic version of the TOT
phenomenon, is often observed in patients.
Conduction aphasia: Caused by damage to the arcuate fasciulus, it results in relatively
normal comprehension and speech but poor repetition. Paraphasia also occurs as they may
pronounce sounds they do not intend to pronounce.
Other language disorders include:
Dyslexia: It is a developmental disorder that results in a reduced ability to read.
Dementia: It is an acquired disorder that results in, among other things, reduced language
ability.
Specific Language Impairment: It is a developmental disorder that results in reduced
language ability with no obvious cause.
MODELS OF SPOKEN AND WRITTEN LANGUAGE FUNCTIONS IN THE BRAIN
A first model of the general organization of language functions in the brain was proposed by
American neurologist Norman Geschwind in the 1960s and 1970s. This “connectionist”
model drew on the lesion studies done by Wernicke and his successors and is now known
as the Geschwind-Wernicke model. According to this model, each of the various
Dr. R. Soundararajan
Associate Professor in English
6
First of all, their physical appearance, the way they dress, the way they carry themselves,
and their general attitude all form a context that lends a particular coloration to their verbal
messages. Next, the particular position of their bodies during conversation, the way their
eyes move, the gestures they make, and the ways they mimic each other will also impart a
certain emotional charge to what they say. There is also what is often called the music of
language—the variations in tone, rhythm, and inflection that alter the meanings of words.
When we are talking about language, it is therefore useful to distinguish between verbal
language—the literal meaning of the words—and everything that surrounds these words and
gives them a particular connotation. That is the big difference between denoting and
connoting: the message that is perceived never depends solely on what is said, but always
on how it is said as well.
Another good reason to distinguish between the denotative and connotative aspects of
language is that they call on different parts of the brain. In the great majority of people, it is
the left hemisphere that formulates and understands the meaning of words and sentences,
while the right hemisphere interprets the emotional connotation of these words.
For example, if you ask someone who has right
hemisphere damage to tell you which of the two
pictures here best portrays the expression “She
has a heavy heart”, that person will point to the
woman with the big heart on her sweater rather
than to the woman in tears. Similarly, if you
remarked in a sarcastic tone that someone was a
really nice guy, a person with right-hemisphere
damage would think you really meant it.
CLASSICAL CONNECTIONISTS MODELS
By the early 19th century, two clearly separate
schools of thought regarding the brain’s function in
language had developed.One school was
comprised of those investigators who believed
that specific (mental) functions were subserved by
specific areas of the brain. Investigators
supporting this view point became known as “localizationists”.
Opponents to localizationists viewpoint, known as “holists”, believed that the mental function
was the product of the entire brain working as a unit & that mental ability was a reflection of
total brain volume.
What is the connectionist theory?
Connectionism is a general theory of learning for animals and humans. ... If an animal
perceives that a particular stimulus goes with a particular response then the connection is
more readily established. For example, by opening the puzzle box (stimulus) the cat can get
at the food (response).
Dr. R. Soundararajan
Associate Professor in English
9
Localism tries to find locations or centers in the brain for different language functions.
Associationism places language functions in the connections between different areas
of the brain, making it possible to associate, for example, perceptions of different
senses with words and/or “concepts”
Dynamic localization of function assumes that functional systems of localized sub-
functions perform language functions. Such systems are dynamic, so that they can
be reorganized during language development or after a brain damage.
Holistic theories consider many language functions as handled by large parts of the
brain working together.
Evolution based theories stress the relation between how brain and language
evolved over time in different species, how they develop in children and how adults
perform language functions.
Acquired language disorders
• Aphasia is an acquired language disorder, often defined as a focal lesion (i.e. a lesion of
specific areas).
• Acquired disorders are also caused by progressive neurological diseases, e.g. dementias.
• Language and memory are closely connected and interdependent, especially in complex
higher cognitive functions.