AJCEH Vol25 No1 MAY97
AJCEH Vol25 No1 MAY97
AJCEH Vol25 No1 MAY97
Published by the
EDITORIAL . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iii
INVITED PAPER FROM STEVEN JAY LYNN — KEYNOTE SPEAKER
AT THE ASH ANNUAL CONGRESS, PERTH, 7–12 SEPTEMBER 1997
EDITORIAL BOARD
Editor
Barry J. Evans, PhD, Monash University, Victoria
Associate Editors
Greg J. Coman, MSc, Addiction Research Institute, Melbourne
Kevin McConkey, PhD, University of New South Wales
Graham Scott, BDSc, BBehSc, Melbourne
Robb O. Stanley, DClinPsych, University of Melbourne
Wendy-Louise Walker, PhD, Sydney
Editorial Consultants
Peter B. Bloom, MD, Institute of Pennsylvania Hospital and University of Pennsylvania
Graham D. Burrows, AO, KSJ, MD, ChB, BSc, DPM, FRANZCP, FRCPsych, MRACMA
University of Melbourne
Harold B. Crasilneck, PhD, PC, University of Texas Health Science Center,
Southwestern Medical School, Dallas, Texas
Frederick J. Evans, PhD, Carrier Foundation and UMDNJ
Rutgers Medical School
Fred H. Frankel, MD, ChB, DPM, Beth Israel Hospital and Harvard Medical School
Ernest R. Hilgard, PhD, Stanford University
Martin T. Orne, PhD,
Institute of Pennsylvania Hospital and University of Pennsylvania
Campbell Perry, PhD, Concordia University, Montreal
Peter W. Sheehan, PhD, University of Queensland
Editorial Assistant
June Simmons
FEDERAL EXECUTIVE OF
THE AUSTRALIAN SOCIETY OF HYPNOSIS LIMITED
President: Dr Wendy-Louise Walker (New South Wales)
President-Elect: Dr Len Rose (Victoria)
Past President: Mr Robb Stanley (Victoria)
Federal Secretary: Dr Mark Earl (South Australia)
Federal Treasurer: Mr Greg Coman (Victoria)
Chairman — Publications: Dr Barry Evans (Victoria)
Chairman — Board of Education: Mr Robb Stanley (Victoria)
ISH Representatives: Dr Barry Evans (Victoria)
Dr Graham Wicks (South Australia)
ASH Federal Secretariat, Clinical School Suites, NEMPS Campus,
Austin & Repatriation Medical Centre, Private Bag 1, Rosanna, Victoria 3084
Fax: (03) 9243 1158 Tel: (03) 9243 1159
Manuscripts and editorial matter should be addressed to the Editor, Dr Barry J. Evans, at
the above address. All journal business communications and subscriptions should be
addressed to the Editor.
EDITORIAL
iii
iv Editorial
Binghamton University
Judith Pintar
University of Illinois
This paper is based on a commentary on a talk that was delivered by Sherrill Mulhern at
the Nato Symposium on Recollections of Childhood Trauma, Port de Bourgenay, France,
1996. Her talk presented a sociological and anthropological perspective on the history of
multiple personality disorder (MPD, now dissociative identity disorder) and its treatment.
In this paper, which touches on a number of themes introduced by Dr Mulhern, we
advance a narrative model or framework for understanding certain aspects of serious
dissociative disorders. An expanded version of this paper will be published in the
forthcoming book, Recollections of Childhood Trauma: Scientific, Research, and Clinical
Practice, Don Read and D. Stephen Lindsay (Eds.).
Requests for reprints should be sent to Steven Jay Lynn, 45 West End Avenue, Binghamton,
New York 13905, U.S.A.
1
2 Lynn and Pintar
An interesting aspect of our social identities is that we can have many such
roles, which can exist together without discontinuity. A woman can be a mother,
a daughter, a doctor, and a wife, a complex set of roles which may wear her out,
but will probably not confuse her sense of herself. A child who is sexually
abused, on the other hand, finds herself in the position of having experiences
which not only exist outside of shared social narratives, but which blatantly
contradict them. Elsewhere (Lynn, Pintar, & Rhue, in press; Rhue, Lynn, &
Pintar, 1996) we have suggested that the phenomenon of dissociated identity
may be a straightforward reflection of traumatic and contradictory social
conditions; in other words, if personal identity is constructed through social
relations, and if those social relations are ruptured in traumatic fashion, then the
construction of identity is accordingly disrupted. In this view, the dissociation
itself is not a pathology, but the particular manifestation of a normal cognitive
capacity under abnormal circumstances.
A narrative view of dissociation — in contrast to mechanistic concep
tualisations which locate the phenomenon within an individual as a symptom of
a disorder, or as a defence mechanism outliving its usefulness — locates the
phenomenon in the social relations of the victim, both at the moment of the
trauma, and as a continuing or recurring process. We might ask the question, if
identity is constructed within social relations, what type of social relations
would lead to the subjective experience of multiple or partial selves? In
attempting to answer this question, it is necessary to distinguish in greater detail
between dissociated multiple identities, and the multiple identities we all
experience that are merely complex.
A child who lives in separate households may develop a complex sense of
self that encompasses two distinct narratives: “I am a responsible big sister” in
one household and “I am just a little kid” in the other. This is a multiple personal
narrative, but it is occurring within a larger social narrative which may be
stressful, but is not traumatic. The child has toys and clothes at Mum’s and
different toys and clothes at Dad’s, and she can bring the toys from Dad’s house
to Mum’s house if she wants to. But what if she is being abused at Dad’s house?
There are still two personal narratives, but something is different: what Dad is
doing is no longer reflected in a shared social narrative. What does the child do
now? Who is she when she’s with her Dad? Not the same person she is when
she is with her Mum where personal and social narratives match. She cannot
bring Dad’s toys with her to Mum’s house. She may say that she “split” at that
point, but the crucial thing to note is that the world split first.
The social narrative upon and through which she constructed her identity
ruptured. It lost coherence and consistency. We would suggest that rather than
being a way to separate from reality, dissociation may well be, under some
circumstances, a way to bring identity in line with those circumstances. Trapped
between a broad social narrative that says, “fathers care for their daughters and
don’t hurt them” and a personal narrative based on the experience of being
repeatedly raped by her father, the child’s construction of a multiple identity is
Dissociative Identity Disorder 3
a realistic reflection of her actual social condition, and her subjective experience
of having many selves which appears to be a distortion of reality, may be an
accurate reflection of lived experience.
Multiplicity is perhaps the only way that both narratives can be true at the
same time. She can be both a nine-year-old school girl and her father’s lover. It
may be true, as many have postulated, that pathology arises because she cannot
“integrate” the two identities within one coherent self. But the reason the
identities cannot co-exist is that the social world does not acknowledge that they
might both be true. It may be accurate to suggest that the world and she are
mutually dissociated from one another.
The important point to note is that what the experience of trauma disrupts is
not identity itself but the social process through which identity is constructed.
Correspondingly, in this view, the phenomenon of multiplicity is not so much a
disorder of identity, but a disorder of identity construction. The suggestion that
multiplicity is in part, or whole, iatrogenic becomes less surprising (though no
less problematic) in this light. Multiple identity, like unitary identity, is
performed, that is to say it is dynamically constructed through social
relationships.
Therapists treating multiples share with their clients a narrative about identity
that includes a belief in the existence of MPD/DID (dissociative identity disorder,
the current nomenclature for what was once multiple personality disorder). They
engage in discussions with alters, developing relationships with them within
which the alters come alive. The therapist–client relationship may not be the only
one in which the alters perform, but is often the most important one.
The meaning of whether therapists convince their clients that they are
multiples varies as a function of what the client believes in advance. If the client
believes “Oh, I’m not a multiple; that’s just a mood that I’m in sometimes,”
that’s one thing. It’s another if the client’s belief is “Oh, I’m not a multiple; Jane
is a physically distinct other person, rather than another personality.” In the first
case, the therapist’s effort can legitimately be construed as iatrogenic creation
of MPD. In the second, it can be construed as countering a delusion and not at
all iatrogenic, but a reflection of the subjective conviction of multiplicity.
The process in which multiple identities are constructed or maintained within
the discourse of a therapeutic relationship will be even more powerful in a
relationship between therapists and their highly suggestible and fantasy-prone
clients. Research in our laboratory (Lynn, Rhue, & Green, 1988) has shown
there are many parallels between persons with a profound history of fantasy
involvements that date to early childhood. Indeed, when we look at what
measures of dissociation index, one prominent component is fantasy and
imaginative proclivities. Fantasy-prone persons report that they play with
imaginary companions during childhood, often pretend to be other people
during times of stress, report out-of-body experiences to deal with traumatic and
non-traumatic experiences, and create absorbing imaginative narratives during
which time stands in abeyance. In short, we might say they dissociate.
4 Lynn and Pintar
REFERENCES
Lynn, S. J., Pintar, J., & Rhue, J. W. (in press). Fantasy proneness, dissociation, and
narrative construction. In S. Powers & S. Krippner (Eds.). Broken selves:
Dissociative narratives and phenomena. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Lynn, S. J., Rhue, J., & Green, J. (1988). Multiple personality and fantasy proneness:
Is there an association or dissociation? British Journal of Experimental and Clinical
Hypnosis, 5, 138–142.
Mare, C., Lynn, S. J., Kvaal, S., Segal, D., & Sivec, H. (1994). Hypnosis and the dream
hidden observer: Primary process and demand characteristics. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 103, 316–327.
Meichenbaum, D., & Fong, G. T. (1993). How individuals control their own minds: A
constructive narrative perspective. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.),
Handbook of mental control (pp. 473–490). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.,
Mulhern, S. (in press). Commentary on a paper by Willem Waagenar, D. Read, &
D. S. Lindsay (Eds.). Recollections of childhood trauma: Scientific, research, and
clinical practice. New York: Plenum Press.
Rhue, J., Lynn, S., & Pintar, J. (1996). Narrative and imaginative storytelling:
Treatment of a sexually abused child. In S. J. Lynn, I. Kirsch, & J. W. Rhue (Eds.),
Casebook of clinical hypnosis (pp. 251–270). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Sarbin, T. (1986). Narrative psychology: The storied nature of human contact. New
York: Praeger.
Spence, D. (1984). Narrative truth and historical truth: Meaning and interpretation in
psychoanalysis. New York: W. W. Norton.
Australian Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis
Vol. 25, No. 1, 1997, pp. 8–17
The following document is the Final Report of the Working Party established by the
American Psychological Association [APA] to investigate memories of childhood abuse
and how these, if they exist, can be validly accessed in therapy.
The APA Working Party commenced its deliberations in 1993, to review the current
scientific literature and identify future research and training needs regarding the evaluation
of memories of childhood abuse. Members of the Working Party were Drs Judith Alpert,
Laura Brown, Stephen Ceci, Christine Courtois, Elizabeth Loftus, and Peter Ornstein.
The Final Report of the Working Party, reproduced below, was presented to the APA
Board of Directors on 14 February 1996. The preface to the report notes that the report
does not offer any “solution” to the problem of adult memories of childhood abuse. It
does, however, offer a clear and concrete delineation of the issues, what research we need,
and the differences that must be reconciled for the science and profession of psychology
to attend responsibly to the dilemmas posed by adult memories of childhood abuse.
It is clear from the Working Party’s Final Report that consensus on many issues could
not be reached. At the same time, it provides a pragmatic and immediately applicable
document that the Working Party hopes will contribute to a widespread questioning of the
divisive discourse around this issue, to the ultimate benefit of the public the APA is
committed to serve.
The Final Report is reproduced, with permission of the APA, to facilitate a similar
debate among members of the Australian Society of Hypnosis.
Published with permission of the American Psychological Association. Requests for reprints
should be sent to Barry J. Evans, Editor, AJCEH, Edward Wilson Building, Austin Campus,
Austin and Repatriation Medical Centre, Heidelberg, Victoria 3084.
For a copy of the Final Report, contact Paul J. Donnelly Jr, Director, Public Interest
Administration, Board Operations & Governance Communication, American Psychological
Association, 750 First Street, NE, Washington DC 20002-4242 USA. Fax: 202 336 5723;
Email: pzd:apa@email.apa.org
8
Memories of Abuse 9
In this section (of the Final Report), we seek to set a context for the following
documents by summarising our points of agreement and disagreement. We also
articulate some of the implications of our deliberations for clinical practice,
forensic practice, research, and training. Finally, we conclude with a plea for
unity within our discipline.
WHERE DO WE STAND?
Inspection of the reviews and commentaries in the Final Report will indicate
that we are in agreement concerning a number of key points. Indeed, as indicated
in the Working Group’s Interim Report, we agree on the following:
1. Controversies regarding adult recollections should not be allowed to obscure
the fact that child sexual abuse is a complex and pervasive problem in
America that has historically gone unacknowledged.
2. Most people who were sexually abused as children remember all or part of
what happened to them.
3. It is possible for memories of abuse that have been forgotten for a long time
to be remembered.
4. It is also possible to construct convincing pseudomemories for events that
never occurred.
5. There are gaps in our knowledge about the processes that lead to accurate and
inaccurate recollections of childhood abuse.
As important as these areas of agreement are, it is equally, if not more important,
to acknowledge frankly that we differ markedly on a wide range of issues. At
the core, the clinical and research subgroups have fundamentally differing views
of the nature of memory. These contrasting conceptions of memory have led to
debate concerning: (a) the constructive nature of memory and the accuracy with
which any events can be remembered over extended delays; (b) the tentative
mechanisms that may underlie delayed remembering; (c) the presumed “special”
status of memories of traumatic events; (d) the relevance of the basic memory
and developmental literatures for understanding the recall of stressful events; (e)
the rules of evidence by which we can test hypotheses about the consequences
of trauma and the nature of remembering, (f) the frequency with which
pseudomemories may be created by suggestion, both within and outside of
therapy; and (g) the ease with which, in the absence of external corroborative
evidence, “real” and pseudomemories may be distinguished.
WHERE DO WE GO?
Because the entire Working Group converges on the belief that a science-
informed practice will be the most effective strategy for treatment, we believe
that practice-informed research will enhance the integration of knowledge about
memory into the overall field of trauma treatment. This direction in research
may also help us to answer the still unclear questions about the nature of various
observed human behaviours in response to trauma.
leading manner, and in the most open-ended way possible, in order to promote
a more behaviourally descriptive and less affectively laden introduction of this
difficult topic into the history-taking process.
When clients report what they phenomenologically experience as memories
of previously unrecollected trauma, therapists should take a number of steps to
avoid imposing a particular version of reality on these experiences and to reduce
risks of the creation of pseudomemories. If these materials are intrusive and
create problems for the client’s functioning, the first goal of treatment should be
stabilisation and containment following the recommendations of many experts
in the field of trauma treatment. It is important to remember that the goal of
therapy is not archaeology; recollection of trauma is only helpful insofar as it is
integrated into a therapy emphasising improvement of functioning. Therapists
should avoid endorsing such retrievals as either clearly truthful or clearly
confabulated. Instead, the focus should be on aiding the client in developing his
or her own sense of what is real and truthful. Clients can be encouraged to
search for information that would add to their ability to find themselves credible
(e.g., contemporaneous writings, reports of third parties), and to carefully weigh
the evidence. Therapists should carefully consider all alternative hypotheses,
including: (a) that the retrieved material is a reasonably accurate memory of real
events; (b) that it is a distorted memory of real events, with distortions due to
developmental factors or source contaminations; (c) that it is a confabulation
emerging from underlying psychopathology or difficulties with reality testing;
(d) that it is a pseudomemory emerging from exposure to suggestions; or (e) that
it is a form of self-suggestion emerging from the client’s internal suggestive
mechanisms.
Clients who seek hypnosis as a means of retrieving or confirming their
recollections should be advised that it is not an appropriate procedure for this
goal because of the serious risk that pseudomemories may be created in trance
states and of the related risk due to increased confidence in those memories.
Clients should also be informed that the use of hypnosis could jeopardise any
future legal actions they might wish to take. Moreover, in those situations in
which hypnosis is employed, it is necessary to interpret the client’s responses in
light of parallel measures of suggestibility and proneness to fantasy.
As indicated above, it can be helpful to seek corroborative evidence for
claims of sexual abuse. Nonetheless, denials by alleged perpetrators should also
not be taken as evidence that the client is experiencing other than an accurate
recollection. Indeed, known perpetrators of child sexual abuse can also deny and
lie about their behaviours, even in the presence of physical evidence that
incontrovertibly links them to the abuse, and sometimes tell their victims that
the abuse “never happened” or should be forgotten. Finally, while there are no
statistics available on its prevalence, it is known that on occasion, adults who
report recovering memories will lie, particularly when the constellation of
motives (fear, embarrassment, desire to protect loved ones, desire for revenge)
outweighs the incentives to tell the truth.
Memories of Abuse 13
The role of the forensic psychologist is that of an educator to the triers of fact,
the judge and jury. In that capacity, forensic psychologists, in general, should
avoid attempting to speak to the ultimate issue (i.e., guilt or innocence) in a
case, because they usually are not in a position to know the truth. Forensic
psychologists should always exercise caution, temper the degree of certainty
with which they offer their testimony, and be aware of both the problems and
the strengths of their methodologies. How, then can forensic experts practise
responsibly in cases where questions of recovered memories of childhood abuse
are involved?
First, whenever possible, therapists should avoid serving as expert forensic
witnesses in the cases involving clients whom they are treating. This is consistent
with APA ethical standards and with guidelines published by the Division 41 of
APA (Psychology and Law — AP/LS) for forensic practice. Experts, moreover,
should confine their testimony to their specific areas of expertise and knowledge.
For example, questions of the appropriate standard of care would ordinarily be
the purview of those trained in the fields of professional psychology; this might
be distinguished from expert testimony about risks of a specific therapeutic
14 APA Working Group
Just as the Working Group has endorsed the value of a scientifically informed
approach to practice, its members also endorse the value of a practice-informed
approach to research. There is much to be gained by both researchers and
practitioners when their respective insights cross-fertilise each other’s professional
activities.
For researchers, this means incorporating into their designs as many of the
ingredients of real-world trauma as is ethically and practically permissible, and
learning from clinicians about those phenomena that require further study. In this
regard, in recent years there has developed a large body of naturalistic empirical
research that stands at the interface between the domains of memory and trauma.
Examples include Wagenaar and Groeneweg’s (1990) study of Dutch concentration
camp survivors’ memories of victimisation; Parker, Bahrick, Lundy, Fivush, and
Levitt’s (1995) study of child survivors of Hurricane Andrew; both Merritt,
Ornstein, and Spicker’s (1994) and Goodman, Quas, Batterman-Faunce,
Riddlesberger, and Kuhn’s (1994) explorations of children’s bladder
catheterisation; and Eisen, Goodman, and Qin’s (1995) study of the susceptibility
of sexually abused children to sexual suggestions. More recently there have also
been studies of the neurobiological substrates of memory for traumatic events
(Bremer, Randall, Scott, Bronen, Seibyl, et al., 1995; Yehuda, Kahana, Binder-
Byrnes, Southwick, Mason, et al., 1995). Nonetheless, there is a clear need for
more of this sort of research, particularly as it applies to the phenomenon of
repetitive boundary violations within the family setting, in which a number of
complex person–situation variables are at play.
Memories of Abuse 15
A FINAL STATEMENT
We wish to end on a note with which we can all agree. The members of the
Working Group, individually and collectively, bemoan the increasing
“Balkanisation” of psychology, a development that has surely made our tasks
more difficult as we have attempted to bridge across powerful gaps of
understanding. Our discipline has spawned many psychologies, often
disconnected from each other, and both the cortical and ethical glues that ought
to have connected them seem to have been neglected. We are fast becoming a
collection of psychologies, each uninformed by the data and epistemologies of
the others; in short, we are pluribus, but not unum. And, most critically, we need
to change dramatically if psychology as a discipline seeks to lead the way in
avoiding harm to all those who are affected by the consequences of both
accurate and false recollections of abuse.
REFERENCES
Bremer, J. D., Randall, P., Scott, T. M., Bronen, R. A., Seibyl, J. P., Southwick, S. M.,
Delaney, R. C., McCarthy, G., Charney, D. S., & Innis, R. B. (1995). MRI-based
measurement of hippocampal volume in patients with combat-related posttraumatic
stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 973–981.
Caddy, G. R. (1981). The development and current status of professional psychology.
Professional Psychology, 12, 377–384.
Dawes, R. M. (1994). House of cards: Psychology and psychotherapy built on myth.
New York: Free Press.
Eisen, M. L., Goodman, G. S., & Qin, J. (1995, May). The impact of dissociation and
stress arousal on the suggestibility and memory of abused or neglected children. In
P. J. Morrison (Moderator). Children’s memory: Implications for testimony. Invited
symposium at the meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago.
Goodman, G. S., Quas, J. A., Batterman-Faunce, J. M., Riddlesberger, M. M., & Kuhn,
J. (1994). Predictors of accurate and inaccurate memories of traumatic events
experienced in childhood. Consciousness and Cognition, 3, 269–294.
Jacobson, A., & Richardson, B. C. (1987). Assault experiences of 100 psychiatric
inpatients: Evidence of the need for routine inquiry. American Journal of Psychiatry,
144, 434–440.
Kalinkowitz, B. (1978). Scientist–practitioner: The widening schism. Clinical
Psychologist, 32, 4–5.
Merritt, K. A., Ornstein, P. A., & Spicker, B. (1994). Children’s memory of a salient
medical procedure: Implications for testimony. Pediatrics, 94, 17–23.
Memories of Abuse 17
Parker, J. F., Bahrick, L. E., Lundy, B., Fivush, R., & Levitt, M. J. (1995). Children’s
memory for a natural disaster: Effects of stress. In J. Parker (Moderator). Eyewitness
memory: Effects of stress and arousal on children’s memories. Symposium at the
meetings of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, Vancouver,
BC.
Poole, D. A., Lindsay, D. S., Memon, A., & Bull, R. (1995). Psychotherapy and the
recovery of memories of childhood sexual abuse: U.S. and British practitioners’
opinions, practices, and experiences. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
63, 426–437.
Wagenaar, V. A., & Groeneweg, J. (1990). The memory of concentration camp
survivors. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 4, 77–87.
Yapko, M. D. (1994). Suggestibility and repressed memories of abuse: A survey of
psychotherapists’ beliefs. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 36, 163–171.
Yehuda, R., Kahana, B., Binder-Byrnes, K., Southwick, S., Mason, J. W., & Giller,
E. L. (1995). Low urinary cortisol excretion in Holocaust survivors with posttraumatic
stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 982–986.
Australian Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis
Vol. 25, No. 1, 1997, pp. 18–39
Peter Sheehan
University of Queensland
This article examines memory distortion in hypnosis and imbeds its significance and that
of the relevant field (forensic hypnosis) into the context of international comparative data
that have been collected from an analysis of the hypnotic literature in the last decade. One
issue, in particular, stands out from this analysis — recovered memories. This finding
emphasises the growing importance of the need to reconcile clinical concerns and
experimental findings from the laboratory setting. The recovered memory debate has
profound theoretical, empirical and professional importance and shows up the tensions in
the clinical–laboratory interface. Preliminary data are discussed which attempt an analysis
of the phenomenon in a relatively neutral fashion so to provide limited base-rate data to
facilitate that interface.
This article attempts first to place its concerns within a brief review of
contemporary trends in hypnosis research. I choose to define the term
“contemporary” in terms of major themes in research occurring since the 1985
review of the field of hypnosis by John Kihlstrom in the Annual Review of
Psychology. Then I shall move to the issue of recovered memories, pointing out
some of the major experimental, conceptual, and clinical challenges.
In introducing my topic in this way, I want to comment comparatively on
research in hypnosis viewed internationally over the last decade. I will talk to
the nature of hypnosis and memory, then focus on recovered memories in
particular, to present some suggestive and controversial data.
Paper presented at Workshop, Memories of Abuse: True Memories and the False Memory
Syndrome, The Australian Society of Hypnosis, Melbourne, 12 October 1996.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Peter Sheehan, Deputy-Vice-Chancellor
(Research and Postgraduate Studies), University of Queensland, Cumbrae-Stewart Building,
Research Road, Brisbane, Queensland 4072.
18
Recovered Memories 19
and controversy over time. The original review (Kihlstrom, 1985) was divided
into many sections.
Table 1 gives the major content areas in hypnosis journals for the period
1986–1995. The journals analysed are: the American Journal of Clinical
Hypnosis, the Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, the
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis (IJCEH), and
Contemporary Hypnosis (previously known as the British Journal of Experimental
and Clinical Hypnosis). An overwhelming number of publications classified
under health and mental health treatment have been excluded. The classification
shown in Table 1 is quite similar to the content area covered in Kihlstrom’s
review; only 11 of the categories are actually distinct. Figure 1 shows the
distribution of content areas in the IJCEH (the most international of the
journals). The modal themes illustrated in this figure are physiological linkages
Table 1 Content Categories Drawn From the Literature in the Last Decade
1 Absorption
2 Amnesia
3 Analgesia
4 Brain mechanisms*
5 Development of hypnotisability
6 Ethical/professional issues*
7 Expectancy effects*
8 Forensic issues*
9 Historical aspects
10 Hypnotic communications*
11 Hypnotic relationship*
12 Hypnotic types
13 Hypermnesia
14 Imagery
15 Immune functioning*
16 MPD
17 Memory
18 Modifying hypnotisability*
19 Other hypnotic phenomena
20 Personality correlates
21 Phenomenology/EAT*
22 Physiological links*
23 PTSD
24 Scales of hypnotisability
25 Self-hypnosis
26 Sports hypnosis*
27 Statistics/psychometrics
* Rapidly expanding/new categories in the decade since Kihlstrom’s review article,
20 Sheehan
THEORY
SELF HYP.
SCALES OF HYP.
R.E.S.T.
PTSD
PHYSIOLOGICAL LINKAGES
PHYSIOLOGY
PHENOMENOLOGY
PERS. CORREL.
OTHER HYPNOTIC PHENOMENA
OTHER HYP. PH.
MODIFYING HYP.
MEMORY
MEMORY
MPD
LEARNING
IMAGERY
HYPERMNESIA
HYP. TYPES
HYP. RELTSHP
HYPNOTIC COMMUNICATIONS
HYP. COMMS
HISTORICAL
HISTORICAL
FORENSIC FORENSIC
EXPECTANCY
ETHICAL-PRO.
EAT
DEVELOPMENT
DENTISTRY
BRAIN MECHS
ANALGESIA
AMNESIA
ABSORPTION
0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18
Numbers of publications (papers)
Recovered Memories 21
(17 articles); historical aspects and memory (15 in each category); and hypnotic
communications, other hypnotic phenomena and forensic issues (12 in each
category).
Table 2 takes the Psych LIT Classification Scheme and applies it to the field
of hypnosis. Immediately it is obvious how much research in hypnosis really
interrelates with major domains in the study of psychology as a whole. Figure 2
shows the content of our own journal and Figure 3 contrasts the number of
articles in IJCEH and AJCEH. Finally, Figure 4 shows the numbers of
publications for Australia as a percentage of the world.
I have analysed the literature in three of our major journals. There are
differences in the reporting patterns of journals and some obvious arbitrariness
Table 2 The Psych LIT Classification Scheme* as Applied to the Major Areas of
Hypnosis Literature
21 General Psychology
Historical aspects and debate on underlying nature of hypnosis
22 Psychometrics, Statistics, Methodology
Scales of hypnotisability, absorption, imagery; experimental methods
23 Human Experimental Psychology
Experimental work on hypnotic phenomena: analgesia, memory effects,
perceptual distortions
25 Physiological Psychology, Neuroscience
Brain processes — EEG rhythms and diurnal rhythms
26 Communication Systems
Hypnotic communications; induction types
28 Developmental Psychology
Development of “high” hypnotisability; ageing and hypnotisability
29 Social Processes, Social Issues
The hypnotic relationship
30 Social Psychology
Beliefs, attitudes and expectancy effects; experimental demand effects
31 Personality Psychology
Individual differences in hypnotisability; hypnotisability correlates
32 Psychological/Physical Disorders
Mental/physical disorders and hypnosis
33 Health/Mental Health Treatment and Prevention
Medical, dental hypnosis; hypnotherapy; hypnotic interventions
34 Professional Psychology/Health Personnel Issues
Professional and ethical issues in use of hypnosis by professionals
35 Educational Psychology
Modifications of hypnotisability; learning enhancement
37 Sport Psychology and Leisure
Hypnosis in sport
42 Forensic Psychology and Legal Issues
Forensic hypnosis, hypnosis and the law
0
5
10
15
22 Sheehan
GEN.-HISTORICAL
PSYCHOMETRICS
EXPERIMENTAL PSYCH.
PHYSIOLOGICAL
COMMUNICATION
DEVELOPMENTAL
Figure 2: Contents of AJCEH 1986–1995
SOCIAL CONTEXT
INDIVIDUAL DIFFS
DISORDERS
PROFESSIONAL
EDUCATION
SPORTS
Other
Recovered Memories 23
FORENSIC-LEGAL
SPORTS
EDUCATION
PROFESSIONAL
Categories based on Psych LIT classification scheme
DISORDERS
INDIVIDUAL DIFFS
SOCIAL CONTEXT
DEVELOPMENTAL
COMMUNICATION
PHYSIOLOGICAL
EXPERIMENTAL PSYCH.
PSYCHOMETRICS
GEN.-HISTORICAL
0
10
20
30
40
Numbers of articles
International Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis
Australian Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis
24 Sheehan
1974–76
4 1983–85
1992–94
3.5
3
Percentage
2.5
1.5
0.5
198 203 95 168 219 130 41 80 661 580 201 35 41
0
22: Psychometrics
25: Physiological
28: Developmental
30: Social
31: Personality
32: Disorders
35: Educational
37: Sports
42: Forensic
Classification Code
Recovered Memories 25
In the past, work in hypnosis has highlighted errors in memory that clearly
depend upon a combination of social psychological variables, such as beliefs,
attitudes, sets/expectations, and person-attributes (like imagery, imagination,
Table 3 Issues Arising From the Comparison of Kihlstrom’s 1985 Review with the
Content of 3 Main Hypnosis Journals (1986-1995)
Table 4 Major Inferences from Experimental Data Drawn on the Association between
Memory and Hypnosis
The literature warns that the variables involved in pseudomemory creation are
complex, involving some combination of what is measured by inventories
assessing “absorption,” those assessing some preference for imagining, and
some interaction between the two (Labelle et al., 1990) as well as other
contextual factors in relation to pseudomemory suggestion itself (Sheehan,
Statham, & Jamieson, 1991). Table 4 sets out what I consider are the major
inferences from the experimental data.
Let me turn now to explain my view of the nature of memory, and of
hypnosis, before commenting more specifically on recovered memories.
The nature of hypnosis has been much debated in the literature (Kihlstrom,
1985; Lynn & Rhue, 1991; Orne, 1959). Nevertheless, there seems reasonable
consensus about some of its defining properties. Although distortions typically
occur, hypnosis can be said to occur when one person (the subject) experiences
alterations in perception, memory, or mood in response to suggestions given by
another person (the hypnotist). Although distortions can and do occur, hypnosis
is essentially an experiential phenomenon wherein the hypnotist typically
guides the subject to create a favourable situation for the display of his or her
special capacities and skills. Substantial reliance has to be placed therefore on
the subject’s self-report as to the nature of the experience.
Typically, however, susceptible subjects use their own capacities to respond
appropriately (though at times constructively) to what the hypnotist is suggesting
even though the content of these suggestions is false. This is not a view that is
compatible with hypnosis recovering traces of original perception, and sits most
comfortably with the perspective that memories retrieved in hypnosis are
28 Sheehan
Recovered Memories
1 We know that child abuse and sexual trauma happen too frequently no matter
what the frequency is.
2 We know that traumatic memories are formed in some ways that are different
from normal memories.
3 We know that dissociation is a capacity that can be used defensively, giving
rise to amnesia which may lift at a later time.
4 We know that memory can be highly reliable, we know that memory can be
highly unreliable, and we know that memory can be influenced by a variety
of factors, including suggestion and misinformation.
5 We know that people can be influenced by others even to the extreme. We
know that misinformation provided by credible authorities with no apparent
motive to deceive can be absorbed and responded to as though it were true.
reports in therapy that can be trusted and reports which cannot. The problem is
to know which is which and we have no precise way of making that
discrimination — a problem that is exaggerated in its significance when many
of us accept routinely that the important issue for therapy is the client’s personal
account of what has happened and what he or she is feeling about that now.
Therapy certainly can facilitate access to memories and produces memory
reports, but it is also a mode of influence that inevitably transmits specific cues
and suggestions about what is fitting, wanted, or appropriate to report. Therapy
(like other influence techniques) can produce significant memory distortion
while at the same time fresh memories can be retrieved that are true and
reported. The essential problem is that we have no easy guarantee of the veracity
of verbal reports offered in therapy, and past emotional events can rarely be
corroborated independently to establish their truth value.
Because of the risks of suggestion and the many possibilities of distortion,
special professional obligations exist when the memories being explored in
therapy are associated with possible past abuse. This leads one to a major matter
for consideration — the need for guidelines for practice.
Summary Comment
be expected between the results for accuracy and confidence, the experimental
literature indicates the value of separately examining the pattern of differences
among subjects with respect to both these measures.
Tables 6 and 7 set out the experimental data for the accuracy and confidence
measures.
Data in Table 6 show that the first of the questions produced no confirmatory
response whatsoever but by the fourth (repeated) question, four out of 25
subjects acquiesced — all of them highly susceptible to hypnosis. By the third
question, one of these subjects had already shifted response to acquiesce (in
hypnosis). The conflicting nature of subjects’ responses, however, is heavily
evidenced in the data. To the third question (“Do you know who came in?”),
four subjects said they were perfectly confident that a person entered the room,
but at least three-quarters of this same group denied this. And the same variable
pattern of response is evident for the confidence data. My point is that what
simply looks inconsistent in an experimental context could be interpreted as
dynamically meaningful in a therapeutic one.
The overall pattern of the data tells us clinically relevant responses in a
neutral context are possible (for susceptible subjects particularly) when
suggestive cues are present and the repetition of the questions reinforces the
subject’s acquiescence. It is not too hazardous to conclude that in a clinical —
more relevant — context where that same repetition exists, the rate of acquisition
and confusion would be much higher — enough, one might say, to highlight a
genuine forensic problem.
Note: For each of these four questions, a 5-point confidence scale was used where “1”
meant no confidence that a person had entered the room, and “5” meant perfectly
confident that a person had entered the room.
There are some important practical implications from the data presented
here. The development of tools which can differentiate between real and
constructed memory may not be possible if it is proven that original memories
are overlaid by recent imaginings and remembering. If, however, original
memory is still intact after being recalled as suggested in some research
(McCann & Sheehan, 1987), there may be a way in some cases (e.g., Johnson,
Foley, Suengas, & Raye, 1988; Spence, 1994; Steller & Koehnken, 1989) of
making the important distinction between fact and fantasy or differentiating “the
signal of true repressed memories from the noise of false ones” (Loftus, 1993,
p. 534). Research into memory systems and how they function is of crucial
importance in establishing how best to investigate the extremely controversial
and emotive area of truth in recovered memory.
Recovered Memories 35
Clinical researchers emphasise that the historical truth is not always the issue
in therapy. Information may be clinically useful though not necessarily accurate
(Bowers & Farvolden, 1996; Spence, 1982). However, in the legal setting it has
been said there is another reality (Loftus, 1993) echoing also others’ claims that
the nature of recovered memories has profoundly different meanings for legal as
opposed to therapeutic settings (Pennebaker & Memon, 1996).
The limitations imposed by the reported content of memories mean that the
conclusions of pseudomemory studies can only be extrapolated with difficulty
into the clinic or courtroom setting (Greene, Wilson, & Loftus, 1989).
Laboratory experiments do not relate well to situations such as cases of
suspected child abuse, or criminal events where the memories may have strongly
emotional overtones. As Yuille and Cutshall (1986) comment in relation to
eyewitness reports of real-life traumatic events, subjects can behave and
remember very differently when they are true participants, and not passive
observers. The issues are complex and both the clinical and experimental
settings can help unravel the dilemmas.
CONCLUSION
The data just presented returns me to the relevance of what I signalled earlier.
REFERENCES
Annonn, J. S. (1988). Detection of deception and search for truth: A proposed model
with particular reference to the witness, the victim, and the defendant. Forensic
Reports, 1, 303–360.
Bányai, É. I., (1991). Toward a social-psychobiological model of hypnosis. In S. J.
Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Theories of hypnosis: Current models and perspectives
(pp. 564–598). New York: Guilford Press.
Recovered Memories 37
Bányai, É. I., Gosi-Gerguss, A. C., Vágó, P., Varga, K., & Horváth, R. (1990).
Interactional approach to the understanding of hypnosis: Theoretical background
and main findings. In R. Van Dyck, P. H. Spinhoven, A. J. W. Vander Does, Y. R.
Van Rood, & W. De Moor (Eds.), Hypnosis: Current theory, research and practice
(pp. 53–69). Amsterdam: VU University Press.
Bowers, K. S., & Farvolden, P. (1996). Revisiting a century-old Freudian slip — from
suggestion disavowed to the truth repressed. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 335–
380.
Ceci, S. J., Ross, D. F., & Toglia, M. P. (1987). Suggestibility of children’s memory:
Psycholegal implications. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116,
38–49.
Dinges, D. F., Whitehouse, W. G., Orne, E. C., Powell, J. W., Orne, M. T., & Erdelyi,
M. H. (1992). Evaluating hypnotic memory enhancement (hypermnesia and
reminiscence) using multitrial forced recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 1139–1147.
Echabe, A. E., & Rovira, D. P. (1989). Social representations and memory: The case
of AIDS. European Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 543–551.
Erdelyi, M. H. (1994). Hypnotic hypermnesia: The empty set of hypermnesia.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 379–390.
Greene, E., Wilson, L., & Loftus, E. F. (1989). Impact of hypnotic testimony on the
jury. Law and Human Behavior, 13, 61–78.
Hilgard, E. R. (1987). Research advances in hypnosis: Issues and methods. International
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 35, 248–264.
Johnson, M., Foley, M. A., Suengas, A. G., & Raye, C. L. (1988). Phenomenal
characteristics for perceived and imagined autobiographical events. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 371–376.
Kihlstrom, J. F. (1985). Hypnosis. Annual Review of Psychology, 36, 385–418.
Kihlstrom, J. F. (1994). Hypnosis, delayed recall, and the principles of memory.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 337–345.
Kinnunen, T., Zamansky, H. S., & Block, M. L. (1994). Is the hypnotised subject
lying? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 184–191
Labelle, L., Laurence, J.-R., Nadon, R., & Perry, C. (1990). Hypnotisability, preference
for an imagic cognitive style and memory creation in hypnosis. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 99, 222–228.
Lindsay, D. S., & Read, J. D. (1994). Psychotherapy and memories of childhood sexual
abuse: A cognitive perspective. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 281–338.
Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48,
518–537.
Loftus, E. F., & Ketcham, K. (1994). The myth of repressed memory: False memories
and allegations of sexual abuse. New York: St Martin’s Press.
Lynn, S. J., & Rhue, J. W. (Eds.). (1991). Theories of hypnosis: Current models and
perspectives. New York: Guilford.
38 Sheehan
McCann, T., & Sheehan, P. W. (1987). The breaching of pseudomemory under hypnotic
instruction: Implications for original memory retrieval. British Journal of
Experimental and Clinical Hypnosis, 4, 101–108.
McConkey, K. M. (1992). The effects of hypnotic procedures on remembering: The
experimental findings and their implications for forensic hypnosis. In E. Fromm &
M. Nash, (Eds.), Contemporary hypnosis research (pp. 405–426). New York:
Guilford Press.
McConkey, K. M. (1995). Hypnosis, memory, and the ethics of uncertainty. Australian
Psychologist, 30, 1–10.
McConkey, K. M., & Sheehan, P. W. (1995). Hypnosis, memory, and behavior in
criminal investigation. New York: Guilford.
Neisser, U. (1993). Memory with a grain of salt. Paper presented at the False Memory
Syndrome Foundation Conference (Memory and Reality: Emerging Crisis), Valley
Forge, PA, U.S.A.
Neisser, U., & Harsch, N. (1992). Phantom flash bulbs: False recollections of hearing
the news about Challenger. In E. Winograd, & U. Neisser (Eds.), Affect and
Accuracy in Recall: Studies of “Flash Bulb” Memories (pp. 9–31). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Ofshe, R. J. (1992). Inadvertent hypnosis during interrogation: False confession due to
dissociative state misidentified multiple personality and the satanic cult hypothesis.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 40, 125–156.
Orne, M. T. (1959). The nature of hypnosis: Artifact and essence. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 58, 277–299.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Memon, A. (1996). Recovered memories in context: Thoughts
and elaborations on Bowers and Farvolden (1996). Psychological Bulletin, 119,
381–385.
Perry, C. (1992). Theorizing about hypnosis in either/or terms. International Journal
of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 40, 238–252.
Pettinati, H. M. (Ed.), (1988). Hypnosis and memory. New York: Guilford Press.
Sheehan, P. W. (1979). Clinical and research hypnosis: Toward rapprochement.
Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 7, 135–146.
Sheehan, P. W. (1994, August). The effects of asking leading questions in hypnosis.
Paper presented at the 13th International Congress of Hypnosis, Melbourne,
Australia.
Sheehan, P. W. (1996, August). Contemporary trends in hypnosis research. Paper
presented at 26th International Congress of Psychology, Montreal, Canada, (State-
of-the-art address).
Sheehan, P. W. (in press). Memory and hypnosis — general considerations. In G. D.
Burrows, R. O. Stanley, & P. B. Bloom (Eds.), Advances in clinical hypnosis. New
York: Wiley.
Sheehan, P. W., & McConkey, K. M. (1982). Hypnosis and experience: The exploration
of phenomena and process. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sheehan, P., Statham, D., & Jamieson, G. (1991). Pseudomemory effects occurring
over time in the hypnotic setting. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 39–44.
Recovered Memories 39
Spence, D. P. (1982). Narrative truth and historical truth: Meaning and interpretation
in psychoanalysis. New York: Norton.
Spence, D. P. (1994). Narrative truth and putative child abuse. International Journal of
Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 289–303.
Steller, M., & Koehnken, G. (1989). Criteria-based statement analysis. In D. C. Raskin
(Ed.), Psychological methods in criminal investigation and evidence (pp. 217–245).
New York: Springer.
Weekes, J. R., Lynn, S. J., Green, J. P., & Brentar, J. T. (1992). Pseudomemory in
hypnosis and task-motivated subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 356–
360.
Whitehouse, W. G., Dinges, D. F., Orne, E. C. & Orne, M. T. (1988). Hypnotic
hyperamnesia: Enhanced memory accessibility or report bias? Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 97, 289– 295.
Yapko, M. (in press). The troublesome unknowns about trauma and recovered
memories. In M. Conway (Ed.), Recovered memories and false memories. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Yuille, J. C., & Cutshall, J. L. (1986). A case study of eyewitness memory of a crime.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 291–301
Burkhard Peter
This article reviews a number of issues relating to the pain of cancer and its treatment, and
the role of hypnosis in pain management. The author concludes with a number of case
vignettes which illustrate the efficacy of hypnotic techniques for these patients.
Cancer still remains one of the most feared diseases, despite significant advances
in diagnosis and treatment in recent years. One of the reasons is the associated
pain. That this fear is not unwarranted has been demonstrated by epidemiological
studies which have shown that 60% to 90% of adult tumour patients develop
pain during the later stages of cancer (Bonica, 1980; Cleeland, 1984).
Cancer pain is defined here as acute and chronic pain which occurs directly
or indirectly from tumour lesions and/or the accompanying medical treatment.
For example, skeletal and neuropathic pain is caused by tumour infiltration.
Classical medical treatment (surgery, radiotherapy, and chemotherapy) can
curtail pain symptomatology in many cases by reducing the malignant tissues,
but quite often patients experience painful side effects (secondary pain) like
phantom pain after amputation, polyneuropathies after chemotherapy, or
myelopathies after radiotherapy. Moreover, some medical interventions are
quite painful themselves, such as bone marrow aspirations or lumbar punctures.
These again can lead to conditioned pain, fears, and other unpleasant and
anticipatory reactions, for example, conditioned vomiting (Noeker & Petermann,
1990; Rosen, 1984).
Studies have shown that the variance of the experience of cancer pain is only
explained in part by such organic factors as type of cancer, illness stage, number
and location of metastasis, or terminal phase. Therefore, part of the pain
experience of cancer patients should also be attributed to psychological factors
(Bond & Pilowsky, 1966; Bond & Pearson, 1969; Spiegel & Bloom, 1983a,
Reprinted with permission from Hypnos: Swedish Journal of Hypnosis in Psychotherapy
and Psychosomatic Medicine (1996, XXIII, 99–108)
Requests for reprints should be sent to Burkhard Peter, MEG, Konradstr. 16, D-80801
Munich, Germany.
40
Hypnosis in the Treatment of Cancer Pain 41
Hypnosis was one of the few effective analgesics until the introduction of ether
in 1846 and chloroform in 1847. According to Gravitz (1988), the first
documented anaesthesia with hypnosis was performed 1829 by a Parisian
surgeon during a mastectomy. The British surgeon John Elliotson (1843)
reported several pain-free operations with hypnosis (mesmerism). The Scottish
surgeon James Esdaile (1846) performed several hundred minor operations as
well as over 300 major surgeries in India with hypnosis (mesmerism) during
which 80% of the patients felt no pain. Numerous experimental and clinical
studies have since demonstrated that hypnosis is a very effective method in pain
control (Crasilneck & Hall, 1973; Elton, Stanley, & Burrows, 1983; Evans,
1987; Hilgard & Hilgard, 1975; Peter, 1986, 1990).
Theoretical controversies concerning hypnosis traditionally revolved around
two competing paradigms, the so-called special process and the sociocognitive
perspective. Special process theorists (cf. Hilgard, 1989, 1991) hypothesise
non-ordinary states of consciousness (e.g., trance), and special psychological or
psychophysiological mechanisms (e.g., dissociation), and maintain that hypnotic
behaviour, like hypnotic pain control, differs fundamentally from ordinary
behaviour. Sociocognitive proponents, such as Spanos (1991), on the contrary,
insist there is nothing special about the hypnotic situation because it simply
represents a salient example of social interaction variables which social and
cognitive psychologists regularly employ to explain other forms of social
behaviour (like demand characteristics of the situation, attitudes, expectation,
role enactment, etc.). According to sociocognitive theorists, the subjects in pain
studies simply follow instructions and show the desired behaviour (Spanos,
Carmanico & Ellis, 1994).
In a recent study, Kiernan, Dane, Phillips, and Price (1995) have shown,
however, that hypnosis significantly attenuates even a physiological variable,
the R-III, a nociceptive spinal reflex. This demonstrates that hypnotic pain
control can be beyond the mere reduction of unpleasantness. The reports of
reduced pain may truly represent not only the attenuation of sensory pain but
42 Peter
also that of a spinally mediated, that is, peripheral, process. Even though this
kind of research was pioneered by Hagbarth and Finer, who already showed an
altered spinal nociceptive withdrawal reflex by hypnotic analgesia, it calls for
replication for it seems to show a truly descendent (“top down”) inhibition. This
would add a third component to the already known two factors of pain reduction,
the sensory and the affective.
Erickson (1959, 1966, 1967) and Sacerdote (1970, 1982) provided some of the
best known clinical contributions to hypnosis in the control of cancer pain. Less
well known are the early reports by Butler (1954), Lea, Ware, and Monroe
(1960), and Cangello (1961). Butler’s hypnotic analgesia was successful in 5 out
of 12 cancer patients. However, successful hypnotic analgesia correlated with
the ability of the patient to be hypnotised, that is, suggestibility. Spiegel and
Bloom (1983a, 1983b) reported significant differences in control of cancer pain
(and other symptoms like depression and anxiety) with hypnotic analgesia in 34
patients with metastasised breast cancer in comparison to a control group which
only received medication. Reeves, Redd, Storm, and Minagawa (1983) compared
a group of cancer patients receiving two sessions of hypnotic training for the
reduction of pain induced by hyperthermia with an untreated control group. The
treated patients reported significantly less pain than the patients in the control
group. More recent studies have also demonstrated the efficacy of hypnotic
analgesia. For example, Kaye (1987) was successful with 8 out of 12, Kraft
(1992) with 5, and Campanella (1993) with 8 cancer patients. In a controlled
clinical trial, Syriala, Cummings and Donaldson (1992) were able to show that
hypnosis was effective in reducing reported oral pain in 45 haematological
cancer patients undergoing bone marrow transplantations. These studies, as well
as my own clinical experiences (Peter & Gerl, 1985), demonstrate the efficacy
of hypnosis in pain control in adult patients.
Hilgard and LeBaron (1982, 1984) treated leukaemic children and juveniles
who had to continuously undergo bone marrow aspirations. The pain baseline
was assessed on 63 patients and they were then offered an hypnotic analgesia
pain management programme. Of the 24 patients who participated, 19 were
highly suggestible and 10 out of 19 were able to reduce their pain during the
bone marrow aspiration after only one hypnotic session and 5 after two sessions.
The five patients who were less suggestible were unable to reduce their pain
with hypnosis, but were able to reduce their anxiety before and during the bone
marrow aspiration. Zeltzer and LeBaron (1982) compared the efficacy of
hypnotic (guided imagery) and non-hypnotic behavioural intervention (deep
breathing exercises and distraction procedures) in children and juveniles during
Hypnosis in the Treatment of Cancer Pain 43
bone marrow aspirations and lumbar punctures. The pain and anxiety baseline
was obtained from the patients themselves as well as independent observers
during one to three medical diagnostic treatments without any psychological
interventions. The baseline data showed that experienced pain during the bone
marrow aspiration was greater than during the lumbar puncture. Results
indicated that the two psychological interventions reduced the experienced pain
during bone marrow aspiration significantly, with hypnosis being more effective
than the behavioural intervention.
Hypnosis was also effective during lumbar puncture. Anxiety was reduced by
the behavioural intervention, but significantly less so than with the hypnotic
intervention. Kuttner, Bowman, and Teasdale (1988) compared imaginative
involvement, behavioural distraction, and standard medical procedures on a
sample of 59 children with leukaemia. After two intervention sessions the
authors were able to show that the hypnotic method had an all-or-none effect
during bone marrow aspiration, while distraction appeared to require coping
skills which had to be learned over more than one session.
I would like to discuss rapport with cancer patients in more detail, since I am
frequently painfully aware of the problem of therapeutic relationship and
successful outcome. With some of my patients I have been unable to establish a
good therapeutic relationship. In such cases hypnotisability is relatively
unimportant, since I cannot use it. Therefore it would be helpful for me if I
could assume that suggestibility is considered a stable trait and that I could
attribute my failure to the low or missing suggestibility of the patient. On the
other hand I also know AIDS and cancer patients whose established low
suggestibility could be improved due to our good rapport (Peter, 1994a).
Sometimes I reach my limits with cancer patients, in comparison to neurotic and
psychosomatic patients, because I tend to get too involved; that is, I lose the
balance of distance and closeness. Too much closeness and empathy results in
my suffering along with the patient and I become helpless. Maintaining too
much distance, based on the need to protect myself from this kind of suffering,
also inhibits rapport and prevents effective hypnotherapeutic therapy. Sometimes
supervision is helpful, but not always.
Unrealistic and very high expectations on the part of the patient can also
prevent therapeutic cooperation because any correction of these expectations
might be taken as a rejection by the patient. Such expectations might be the
convictions that even hopeless cases can be cured and that pain is completely
eliminated with hypnosis. Such expectations and similar ones suggested by
some dubious publications make it impossible to offer some patients that
measure of hypnotic analgesia which might still be possible under the given
circumstances, or even to talk to them about death or dying (Peter, 1994b).
1993).
Associative techniques require that the patient pay attention to the pain,
which at first most probably will have a pain reinforcing effect and, for this
reason, should not be used with all patients, since it requires high motivation
and a great deal of trust in the therapist. It is especially helpful in cases of
radiating pain to determine the exact pain boundaries, that is, to find out where
the pain-free areas start. Just the determination of pain boundaries can facilitate
pain relief, in that the patient is told indirectly they can control pain. Once the
boundaries have been established they can be traced with an imaginary pencil
or pen suggesting to the patient that these boundaries can be changed, meaning
they can be shifted more to one side or the other. It is better, however, to ask the
patient to first perform such a shift by enlarging the pain area since this is easier
than reducing the boundaries right from the beginning, or to simply ask her/him
to change the location in terms of symptom shifting. Both instructions contain
the message that pain is not something permanent or unmovable, but changeable
and therefore reducible and relievable.
As soon as the pain boundaries have become variable, other pain modalities
can be changed too. At this point it is important to know how the patient
represents her/his pain in order to get to the so-called submodalities of
perception. For example, if the pain sensation is experienced as sharp and
stabbing, then the attempt is to dull it; if the pain is rather a pulling sensation,
reduce the muscle tension or, for a burning pain, suggest a cooling sensation.
Most important, however, is to determine which submodality is responsible
for most of the suffering. If it becomes possible to completely separate the
suffering from the total pain experience then the rest pain, maybe in form of a
so-called “white pain,” may become much easier to bear. The most extreme
form of such pain awareness would be to imagine the pain process as a purely
physical process occurring within nerve cells and certain areas of the brain.
The step towards symbolic information processing is no longer difficult, once
certain pain aspects have been changed. By symbolic processing I mean that the
patient changes the total pain or certain aspects of it into symbolic or synesthetic
representation, that is, into an adequate acoustic and/or visual hallucination.
Within this new synaesthetic frame, changes can be executed much more easily
than at the originally kinaesthetically experienced pain site, by imaging, for
example, extinguishing a glowing fire, letting a storm die down, or calming
down a stormy ocean.
The utilisation of associative or symbolic techniques by the therapist,
however, should not imply to the patient they should view pain as a partner with
whom one can converse or argue. Most cancer patients with unbearable pain
would consider this an impertinence, if not a ridicule of their suffering (whereas
pain as a partner would be appropriate for patients who are suffering from
psychosomatic pain). Cancer pain is one of the most unnecessary pain
experiences which really does not make any sense and which should be relieved
with anything that has proved to be effective.
Hypnosis in the Treatment of Cancer Pain 47
CASE VIGNETTES
The following case illustrates the use of dissociative techniques. This patient
had cancer of the mouth and came to me about one year after being diagnosed.
The first pain episodes could easily be controlled with relaxation, based on
progressive muscle relaxation. At that stage it was difficult to determine whether
pain control was achieved by the relaxed body state, or because of the patient’s
mental concentration while doing these exercises, or because of the mind–body
interaction.
His pain became a lot worse when he had to have all his teeth pulled, two or
three at a time, because the medical analgesia no longer seemed to work. At the
beginning I taught him to remember how it used to be when he visited his dentist
and when the injections brought about feelings of numbness. During this age
regression, I suggested that the patient could leave the treatment room in his
mind and travel to his favourite vacation place. This was a picturesque island
located in the middle of the Mediterranean Sea, warm, sunny, and surrounded
by cool waters. When the patient arrived there, he told me exactly what he saw,
heard, felt, smelt, and did. By asking detailed questions about all the different
sensory experiences, I tried to create experiential hallucinations in order to
divert his attention from the actual experience in the dentist’s office. At the next
tooth extractions the patient was able to apply these dissociative techniques and
he hardly felt any pain, except for some itching and pressure.
After this incident he did not discuss pain control for a long time, although
tumour progression was evident. When I asked him he said that his “vacation
technique” was still working very well.
Only a few months before his death, when the skin of the upper and lower
jaw had already become necrotic, did he begin to complain once again about
pain which no longer could be controlled by hypnosis alone, but only in
conjunction with time-contingent opioid analgesia. At this stage he was only
able to communicate by moving his head since he was unable to talk. Every time
I visited him I told him stories about his special vacation place and kept
reiterating that in one’s mind one could leave the body behind and go to a place
where one could think and feel differently. From his breathing and from the
expression in his eyes it seemed as if my stories helped him, although it was
48 Peter
impossible to ascertain this from his facial gestures as his face was almost
entirely destroyed by the cancer. Shortly thereafter, the patient went into a coma
and died.
The next case illustrates the use of associative techniques. The patient had
breast cancer and the cancer had already metastasised throughout her body, with
the metastasis in the spine, in particular, being extremely painful. My idea was
to increase the muscle tonus of her back muscles in order that those muscles
would form a so-called corset around her spine. At first I used arm levitation
with the right, then with the left arm, and then with both arms simultaneously.
Then I instructed her “unconscious mind” to transfer the exact feeling of “light
tension,” that is the “tight lightness” of the arms, across her shoulders down to
her back and to continue doing this until this tightness formed a complete
enclosure around the painful areas in her back, so that the pain would be locked
in. At first the radiating pain was reduced considerably and with more and more
practice the patient was able to change her pain experience. The hot and stabbing
pain changed into a dull and cool sensation which felt uncomfortable but not
painful. She was able to go without pain medication for about two months. After
that she was admitted to the hospital because of pathological fractures of the
spinal vertebrae and had to wear a real corset. During the last two weeks of her
life, only very high doses of opioid analgesia were able to keep the pain under
some control.
Symbolic change of pain experience can be illustrated with the following
case. A patient with small-cell lung cancer came to me with extreme pain in the
chest area following radiotherapy. I asked this patient if she would like to learn
something about imagery. For example, she could imagine that she was standing
in the middle of a meadow and in addition to seeing and feeling the different
grasses and flowers she could also taste and smell them if she only got close
enough. Furthermore, she would be able to hear the singing of the birds and the
humming of the insects and their music filled the air. Just before she had seen
and heard enough, and just before she would become bored, she could remember
her chest pains out there in the normal world. She could then put this pain into
the container which was lying at her feet but at the same time she could take
great care that each and every corner of the pain would be in the box so that
none would be sticking out when she closed it and picked it up.
After she had done this she was to take a walk into the nearby forest carrying
her box under her arm or on her shoulders until she found a bubbling spring. At
that spring she should take a rest, have a drink of water, and continue to go
deeper and deeper into the forest until she came to a cave. She enters the cave
still carrying her pain box. As she explores the cave it gets larger and larger until
she comes to a very big and light room. In this room she finds a very old and
wise woman. She goes to the woman, holds up her box, and asks her what she
is supposed to do with it. The old woman first looks at her and then at the box
for a long time and finally asks how much this box weighs. The patient is very
surprised when she realises that the box has become much lighter than before.
Hypnosis in the Treatment of Cancer Pain 49
Actually it had become so light and small that she was able to carry it in her
hand, whereas before she had to frequently carry it on her back or change sides
because it had been so heavy. Now she was happy. She thanks the old woman,
leaves the cave and goes back to the meadow, with the box remaining small and
light.
When the patient came out of the trance, and after the discussion of various
unimportant matters, the patient said, after being asked, that her chest pains
were a lot less — about half as bad as before. I repeated this story every session
— the old woman in the cave kept changing important pain aspects — until the
patient no longer talked about chest pains. The pain-free period that followed
might, however, also be attributed to the fact that the lungs were healing. This
patient is really an unusual case. According to medical statistics she should be
dead, since small-cell lung cancer has a very poor prognosis. However, she is
still alive three years after diagnosis and doing well.
CONCLUSION
Hypnotic pain control can be learned and applied relatively easily. It provides
the patient with an additional measure of controlling the illness and pain. This
again gives the patient back their feeling of independence and leads to an
improvement in quality of life experience. The expectations of the patient about
its effectiveness should be realistic and should be realistically conveyed as such
by the therapist. Hypnotic pain control should not be considered a substitute for
effective opioid pain management. However, in many cases hypnotic analgesics
can reduce the amount of otherwise necessary analgesia and in some cases even
replace them for a longer period of time.
REFERENCES
Bányai, É. I. (1991). Toward a social-psychobiological model of hypnosis. In S. J.
Lynn, & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Theories of hypnosis: Current models and perspectives
(pp. 564–600). New York: Guilford.
Bond, M. R., & Pearson, I. B. (1969). Psychological aspects of pain in women with
advanced cancer of the cervix. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 13, 13–19.
Bond, M. R., & Pilowsky, I. (1966). Subjective assessment of pain and its relationship
to the administration of analgesics in patients with advanced cancer. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 10, 203–208.
Bonica, J. J. (1980). Cancer pain. In J. J. Bonica (Ed.), Pain (pp. 335–362). New York:
Raven Press.
Butler, B. (1954). The use of hypnosis in the care of the cancer patient. Cancer, 7,
1–14.
Campanella, G. (1993). Neurophysiologische überlegungen zur Hypnose und deren
mögliche Wirkungsweise bei chronischem Krebsschmerz. Hypnose und Kognition,
10, 35–45.
50 Peter
Cangello, V. W. (1961). The use of hypnotic suggestion for pain relief in malignant
disease. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 9, 17–23.
Cleeland, C. S. (1984). The impact of pain on the patient with cancer. Cancer, 54,
2635–2641.
Crasilneck, H. B., & Hall, J. A. (1973). Clinical hypnosis in problems of pain.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 15, 153–161.
Elliotson, J. (1843). Numerous cases of surgical operations without pain in the
Mesmeric state: With remarks upon the opposition of many members of the Royal
Medical and Chirurgical Society and others to the reception of the inestimable
blessings of Mesmerism. London: Cantab., F.R.S.
Elton, D., Stanley, G. V., & Burrows, G. D. (1983). Psychological control of pain.
Sydney: Grune & Stratton.
Erickson, M. H. (1959). Hypnosis in painful terminal illness. American Journal of
Clinical Hypnosis, 1, 117–121
Erickson, M. H. (1966). The interspersal hypnotic technique for symptom correction
and pain control. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 8, 198–209
Erickson, M. H. (1967). An introduction to the study and application of hypnosis for
pain control. In J. Lassner (Ed.), Handbook of hypnosis and psychosomatic
medicine. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Esdaile, J., (1902). Mesmerism in India and its practical application in surgery
medicine. Chicago: The Psychic Research Company (Original published in London,
1846).
Evans, F. J. (1987). Hypnosis and chronic pain management. In G. D. Burrows, D.
Elton, & G. V. Stanley (Eds.), Handbook of pain management (pp. 285–299).
Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Gorassini, D. R., & Spanos, N. P. (1986). A cognitive skills approach to the successful
modification of hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 50, 1004–1012.
Gracely, R. H. (1995). Editorial comment: Hypnosis and hierarchical pain control
systems. Pain, 60, 1–2.
Gravitz, M. A. (1988). Early uses of hypnosis as surgical anesthesia. American Journal
of Clinical Hypnosis, 30, 301–308
Hilgard, E. R., (1965). Hypnotic susceptibility. New York: Harcourt.
Hilgard, E. R. (1989). Eine Neo-Dissoziationstheorie des geteilten Bewultseins.
Hypnose und Kognition, 6, 3–22.
Hilgard, E. R. (1991). A neodissociation interpretation of hypnosis. In S. J. Lynn, &
J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Theories of hypnosis: Current models and perspectives (pp.
83–104), New York: Guilford.
Hilgard, E. R., & Hilgard, J. R. (1975). Hypnosis in the relief of pain. Los Altos, CA:
William Kaufmann.
Hilgard, J. R. (1965). Personality and hypnotisability, In E. R. Hilgard (Ed.), Hypnotic
susceptibility (pp. 343–374). New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.
Hypnosis in the Treatment of Cancer Pain 51
Hilgard, J. R. & LeBaron, S. (1982). Relief of anxiety and pain in children and
adolescents with cancer: Quantitative measures and clinical observations.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 30, 417–442.
Hilgard, J. R., & LeBaron, S. (1984). Hypnotherapy of pain in children with cancer.
Los Altos, CA: William Kaufmann.
Hilgard, J. R., & LeBaron, S. (1990). Linderung von Angst und Schmerz bei
krebskranken Kindern und Jugendlichen. Hypnose und Kognition, 7, 7–29.
Kaye, J. M. (1987). Use of hypnosis in the treatment of cancer patients. Journal of
Psychosocial Oncology, 5, 11–22.
Kiernan, B. D., Dane, J. R., Phillips, L. H., & Price, D. D. (1995). Hypnotic analgesia
reduces R-III nociceptive reflex: Further evidence concerning the multifactorial
nature of hypnotic analgesia. Pain, 60, 39–47.
Kraft, T. (1992). Counteracting pain in malignant disease by hypnotic techniques. Five
case studies. Contemporary Hypnosis, 9, 123–129.
Kuttner, L., Bowman, M., & Teasdale, M. (1988). Psychological treatment of distress,
pain, and anxiety for young children with cancer. Journal of Developmental and
Behavioral Pediatrics, 9, 374–381.
Lea, P., Ware, P., & Monroe, J. R. (1960). The hypnotic control of intractable pain.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 3, 3–8.
Lynn, S. J., Neufeld, V., & Mare, C. (1993). Direct versus indirect suggestions: A
conceptual and methodological review. International Journal of Clinical &
Experimental Hypnosis, 41, 124–152.
Melzack, R., & Wall, P. D. (1965). Pain mechanism: A new theory. Science, 150,
971–979.
Noeker, M., & Petermann, F. (1990). Treatment-related anxieties in children and
adolescents with cancer. Anxiety Research, 3, 101–111.
Peter. B. (1986). Hypnotherapeutische Schmerzkontrolle: Ein überlick. Hypnose und
Kognition, 3, 27–41.
Peter, B. (1990). Hypnose. In H. D. Basler, C. Franz, B. Kroner-Herwig, H. P.
Rehfisch, & H. Seemann (Eds.), Psychologische Schmerztherapie (pp. 482–500).
Berlin: Springer.
Peter, B. (1994a). Hypnose und Psychotherapie bei HIV-, ARC- und AIDS-Patienten.
Hypnose und Kognition, 11, 65–93.
Peter, B. (1994b). Hypnotherapy with cancer patients: On speaking about death and
dying. Hypnos, 21, 246–251.
Peter, B. (1994c). Zur Relevanz hypnotischer Trance und hypnotischer Phanomene in
Psychotherapie und Psychosomatik. Verhaltenstherapie, 4, 276–284.
Peter, B. (1996, October). Normal instruction or hypnotic suggestion: What makes the
difference? Paper presented at the 2nd European Congress on Ericksonian hypnosis
and Psychotherapy, Munich.
Peter, B., & Gerl, W. (1984). Hypnotherapie bei Krebspatienten. Hypnose und
Kognition, EH, 56–69.
52 Peter
Peter, B., & Gerl, W. (1985). Clinical hypnosis in psychological cancer treatment. In
J. K. Zeig (Ed.), Ericksonian psychotherapy: Vol. II. Clinical applications, (pp.
389–397). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Reeves, J. L., Redd, W. H., Storm, F. K., & Minagawa, R. Y. (1983). Hypnosis in the
control of pain during hyperthermia treatment of cancer. In J. J. Bonica, U.
Lindblom, & A. Iggo (Eds.), Advances in pain research and therapy: Vol. 4 (pp.
857–861). New York: Raven.
Rosen, S. (1984). Hypnose als begleitende Malßnahme bei der chemotherapeutischen
Behandlung von krebs. Hypnose und Kognition, EH, 17–26.
Rosen, S. (1985). Hypnosis as an adjunct to chemotherapy in cancer. In J. K. Zeig
(Ed.), Ericksonian Psychotherapy: Vol. II. Clinical Applications (pp. 387–397).
New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Sacerdote, P. (1970). Theory and practice of pain control in malignancy and other
protracted or recurring painful illnesses. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 18, 160–180.
Sacerdote, P. (1982). Techniques of hypnotic intervention with pain patients. In J.
Barber, & C. Adrian (Eds.), Psychological approaches to the management of pain
(pp. 60–83). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Spanos, N. P. (1991). A sociocognitive approach to hypnosis. In S. J. Lynn, & J. W.
Rhue (Eds.), Theories of hypnosis: Current models and perspectives (pp. 324–361).
New York: Guilford.
Spanos, N. P., Carmanico, S. J., & Ellis, J. A. (1994). Hypnotic analgesia. In P. D. Wall,
& R. Melzack (Eds.), Textbook of pain (pp. 1349–1366). Edinburgh: Churchill
Livingstone.
Spanos, N. P., & Coe, W. C. (1992). A social-psychological approach to hypnosis. In
E. Fromm, & M. R. Nash (Eds.), Contemporary hypnosis research (pp. 102–130).
New York: Guilford.
Spiegel, D. (1986). The use of hypnosis in controlling cancer pain. Australian Journal
of Clinical Hypnotherapy and Hypnosis, 7, 82–99.
Spiegel, D., & Bloom, J. (1983a). Group therapy and hypnosis reduce metastatic breast
carcinoma pain. Psychosomatic Medicine, 45, 333–339.
Spiegel, D., & Bloom, J. R. (1983b). Pain in metastatic breast cancer. Cancer, 52,
341–345.
Syriala, K. L., Cummings, C., & Donaldson, G. W. (1992). Hypnosis or cognitive
behavioral training for the reduction of pain and nausea during cancer treatment: A
controlled clinical trial. Pain, 48, 137–146.
Turk, D. C. & Fernandez, E. (1990). On the putative uniqueness of cancer pain: Do
psychological principles apply? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 1–13.
Zeltzer, L., & LeBaron, S. (1982). Hypnosis and nonhypnotic techniques for reduction
of pain and anxiety during painful procedures in children and adolescents with
cancer. Journal of Pediatrics, 101, 1032–1035.
Australian Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis
Vol. 25, No. 1, 1997, pp. 53–60
Patricia Burgess
General Practitioner
This case illustrates the use of hypnosis to alleviate chronic headaches in an 8-year-old
girl. It underlines the importance of a child’s need for mastery and how hypnosis can help
a child regain that sense of mastery and so make the necessary responses for change. It
also demonstrates how much therapeutic work can be achieved at an unconscious level
without the therapist knowing the content.
HISTORY
Presenting Problem
Jane was referred by her general practitioner in March 1995 and came
accompanied by her mother. She complained of increasing frequency and
severity of frontal headaches over a four-month period.
The onset coincided with a febrile illness with accompanying headache and
fatigue and was thought to have been viral in nature. The illness lasted about
two weeks, the symptoms being relieved by the use of paracetamol.
However, the headaches continued to occur at a frequency of about three per
week, until the last two months when they began to occur daily. Jane would
often wake up in the morning with a headache or the headache could begin any
time of the day, at school or on weekends. There had been no nausea, vomiting
or aura, but Jane’s mother reported that Jane would often appear very pale. Jane
was experiencing difficulty going to sleep and would often lie awake for a
couple of hours because of the headache. The symptoms were interfering with
daily activities. She had begun to miss days of school and not infrequently
needed to lie down in the sick room. Jane had previously been very active in
basketball but there had been a reduction in performance and she often needed
to leave the court during the match, because of fatigue or headache.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Patricia Burgess, 27 Nunyah Drive, Banksia Park,
S.A. 5091.
53
54 Burgess
There had been changes in mood and behaviour and her mother reported that
she had become more “clingy” and seemed insecure. She appeared easily upset
and at times very teary.
Jane’s own assessment was that she did not know why she had headaches.
She told me sometimes she “felt sad” and even though she liked school, had lots
of friends, was happy in her family and loved her sport, she sometimes felt she
could not “cope.”
Family History
Jane is the youngest of five children in a close and supportive family
environment. She has two brothers and two sisters.
Jane’s mother suffered several migraine headaches when younger. There was
no history in the family of depressive illness. Jane liked school and was a good
achiever under her own efforts. She enjoyed reading, while her main sport was
basketball, in which other members of the family were involved. Jane was a
good player in a team which was achieving considerable success.
The family had a pool at home and Jane often spent time swimming, and just
enjoying being in the pool. She loved birds and she had many budgies of her
own, many different colours, but her favourites were the yellow ones.
Treatment of Headaches 55
GOALS OF MANAGEMENT
1. To explain the nature and cause of the problem to Jane and her mother.
2. To actively increase Jane’s motivation by my enthusiasm for success and so
56 Burgess
SPECIFIC TECHNIQUES
SESSION ONE
With the knowledge of the positive Epstein-Barr study, I was able to give a name
to the origin of Jane’s problem. I explained the nature of post-viral headaches
and time needed for their cure. I also explained how tension and worry can make
headaches worse. The use of medication was discussed, particularly possible
dependence and side-effects.
I believe that therapy actually commenced when Jane first began to explain
her problem to me, and together we were able to create favourable conditions
for a positive outcome. When Jane and her mother first presented, the situation
to me seemed rather urgent, as she had been in discomfort a long time, and she
had this feeling of “sadness.” Her headache was usually about level 5 on the
scale but with Paradex at times it was 3.
I told Jane that I could show her ways of curing her headache if she was
willing to learn. She could do it where no-one else had been able to. My
explanation of hypnosis was that it would involve imagining doing things that
she liked and going on imaginary trips to find interesting and lovely places and
Treatment of Headaches 57
SESSION TWO
When I saw Jane one week later her headache was on number 2. She had been
without Paradex for three days. She was feeling better and was back playing
basketball although she had not played in a competition. Getting to sleep was no
longer a problem. Jane had practised floating her headache away twice a day
(always at bedtime) and at school when needed. She looked better, and was keen
to learn more.
I planned at this session to:
1. Reinforce Jane’s achievements so far.
2. Allow her to develop an increasing sense of control through imaginary
involvement, and enhance her ability to solve her problem when no-one else
could.
3. To build on previous post-hypnotic suggestion for achieving success.
58 Burgess
SESSION THREE
One week later Jane’s headache was at a number 1 and there had been no need
for Paradex. She continued to practise every night, going through the rainbow
to speak to her new friend. Jane’s mother noticed that Jane rarely mentioned her
headache, she was enjoying life again, basketball was in full swing and Jane did
not need to practise at school any more. Jane’s mother commented that she “had
her child back again.”
The family had kept an appointment previously arranged by their general
practitioner with a second paediatrician. He had concurred with the diagnosis
and was supportive of hypnosis. Inderal was to be withdrawn on a sliding
scale.
Despite good progress, I felt that Jane still needed to experience one further
dimension important for childhood growth, that of rewards. At this session I
planned to extend the metaphor used previously. This time the “child like you”
was better and wanted to thank Jane for her help. She shared a special secret
with Jane, taking her to an old palace that she had discovered. The two girls
spent time dressing up in princesses’ clothes, with lots of jewellery and crowns,
all the while feeling how good it was to be happy and free.
OUTCOME
Just over a week later Jane was headache free. She continued to practise and had
been floating the headache away in cool sand at the beach, telling me that she
had made this up herself.
Later, Jane’s mother told me that Jane had helped her older brother deal with
an itchy rash, telling him how easy it was to “imagine it away.”
Up to this time Jane remains well. She chose to use her hypnotic talents,
instead of taking a Panadol recently, after she had had sutures to a chin
laceration.
DISCUSSION
The goals of management were achieved in three sessions of hypnosis. Once
Jane had a taste of success, her mastery increased. The final step was to use her
natural childhood tendency for sympathy and the innate impulse to help “a child
like you” who was really herself. She allowed nothing to be final or fixed about
what she had learnt, being able to use her skills with her family.
Very often hypnosis is thought of as a last resort, and I must say that I found
this to be a huge advantage. I had the benefit of information about failed
treatments: I had a child who felt oppressed and a failure because she had not
met medical expectations: I had supportive parents who needed answers and
help: but best of all I had a child who wanted to get better.
It makes sense to postulate that Jane’s headache had many facets to its
chronicity. Her lack of understanding and information about its cause created
anxiety, which probably decreased pain tolerance, and side-effects from one or
60 Burgess
REFERENCES
Gardner, G. G. (1976). Attitudes of health professionals towards hypnosis: Implications
for training. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 14,
63–73.
Gardner, C. G., & Hinton, R. M. (1980). Hypnosis with children. In G. D. Burrows &
L. Dennerstein (Eds.), Handbook of hypnosis and psychosomatic medicine (pp.
205–231). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Australian Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis
Vol. 25, No. 1, 1997, pp. 61–73
Malcolm Desland
Psychologist
A 40-year-old woman presented with a complex chronic PTSD, including a rape (18
months before admission), a rape at age 15, and a childhood sexual abuse history. Coupled
with this was a previous negative therapy experience. At presentation she was severely
symptomatic, excessively anxious and avoidant. This case study discusses her history and
presentation information, assessment of hypnotisability, case formulation in light of a
previous negative therapy experience, counter-transferential issues, and initial interventions
utilising hypnosis and cognitive behaviour therapy. Therapy is still continuing, so follow-
up and outcome data are not available.
ASSESSMENT INFORMATION
Presenting Problem
Background Information
Ms V was the second youngest of six children (five boys, one girl). Her early
family childhood was described as “idyllic.” She was closest to her father, but,
at about age eight years, this changed when her father developed a drinking
problem. The closeness turned to sexual abuse. This lasted from age 8 to age 16,
when she left the family to marry her present husband. This still exerted effects
upon Ms V. In a flat detached manner she spoke of her anger and disappointment
at him “ruining” the relationship. The sexual abuse has remained a secret to
other family members. Her father died from cancer when Ms V was 21. She
reported nightmares of her father and stated her sexual relationship was
negatively affected by what had happened to her. At age 15, Ms V was raped by
a masked man while dating her present husband. The couple were parked at a
secluded spot when a torch light shone in their faces. A man claiming to be a
police officer told her husband to leave the car. He was tied up and Ms V was
raped.
In the late 1980s, she was contacted to give evidence in a legal case where a
police officer was charged with multiple rapes. Ms V gave evidence about her
own rape. This was disturbing, as the accused was the chief investigating officer
on her case. The investigation against the accused was inconclusive, he was
PTSD 63
Medical Status
Previous Treatment
Ms V’s first counselling was with the sexual assault centre. She viewed this
counselling as negative, “going around and around in circles. I couldn’t see the
point of going over and over it again.” She perceived the counselling as
bewildering and focused unnecessarily on her husband’s former drinking. She
was encouraged to attend AA, even though she did not feel alcohol was
relevant.
The “final straw” came when the therapist conducted an eye movement
desensitisation and reprocessing (EMDR) technique (Shapiro, 1989a, 1989b).
Prior to the EMDR usage, a progressive muscle relaxation was conducted which
resulted in a dissociative experience for Ms V. She visualised Freddie Kruegger
(the main character in the Nightmare on Elm Street films) at her feet. She
reported the session was conducted as the therapist read a journal article on
EMDR. The session ended with Ms V leaving extremely traumatised, and she
had a car accident while driving home. Contact with the therapist was
terminated.
64 Desland
The psychiatrist’s sessions were supportive but had not decreased her distress
greatly. Commenting she could “often see the point of the advice, but [I] can’t
do it.”
Current Symptomatology
Psychometric Testing
DIAGNOSIS
Following hospital policy, DSM-IV (APA, 1994) criteria were used. The
following diagnosis and classifications were applied:
Axis 1: Post-traumatic Stress Disorder (Immediate Onset/Chronic type).
Axis 2: No evidence of personality disorder.
PTSD 65
CASE FORMULATION
ASSESSMENT OF HYPNOTISABILITY
THERAPY
This description begins with session 5, in which the hypnotic procedures were
commenced.
66 Desland
Session 5
Session 6
Ms V felt better since the last discussion and hypnosis was commenced with a
modified eye fixation induction with suggested eye-closure. Suggestions to
relax with each exhalation occurred. Deepening constituted a 1–20 count. The
PTSD 67
Session 7
Hypnosis was not conducted in this session. Ms V reported she had experienced
intrusive stimuli during the day and nightmares at night since her last session.
Despite this, she felt more relaxed, but “hassled” and intrigued by Freddie
Kruegger’s image, and expressed the desire to explore methods of alleviating
these images. Explanations regarding CBT interventions were given including
exposure, desensitisation, or modifying the image (Spiegel, 1994). Ms V
became eager to “get” Freddie Kruegger. This idea evoked embarrassment, glee
and anxiety in her, and she was cautioned to maintain realistic expectations. She
was encouraged to use her absorption capacity by attending to positive stimuli.
Absorption as a concept was elaborated, indicating it would work both positively
and negatively. Ms V was asked to experiment by focusing on flowers and a
68 Desland
fountain in the hospital grounds. She often found herself “looking at things and
not knowing where the time went.” This was exemplified as positive absorption
and she was encouraged to consciously use this to lift her moods, rather than
using it as an avoidance strategy. The next session was scheduled in two days.
Session 8
Session 9
This session took place three days later. No hypnosis was used. Ms V reported
feeling good about the last session. Since then, she supported herself by positive
visualisation. Although Ms V felt positive and motivated, her symptoms
remained unchanged. She introduced new therapeutic material in this session by
speaking of her relationship with her father, specifically his tacit coercion. The
striking difference was the increase in expressed affect. She shared with me her
use of dissociative coping strategies as a child. I highlighted the parallel process
with the previous hypnosis session, specifically how her strategies promoted
safety as a child, but, being safer now, she was able to express her emotions.
These different coping behaviours empowered her to face her perpetrators in
different ways as a child and as an adult. She became animated on awareness of
the connection. The next session was scheduled for three days later.
Session 10
Since the last session, Ms V had thought things over, perceived pressure from
nursing staff, and felt more uncomfortable with Freddie Kruegger. She said it
was new for her to feel sad about things in the way she now was. Her sleep was
less disturbed. We talked about feeling emotions and grieving losses and she
lamented the nurses felt she was doing nothing. The vague and derogatory
PTSD 69
Session 11
Session 12
Since the last session, Ms V’s use of absorption was continuing well. Intrusive
stimuli during the day were fewer, and more manageable. Her sleep remained
erratic and only slightly improved. Ms V requested the same trance procedure
with the split screen, evoking the affect with improved management, then the
affect bridge. The experiences generated — in particular, the rapist’s actions
which were unpredictable, at times conversant and apologetic, silent, highly
agitated or impulsive — still distressed her.
Material from the earlier, more threatening rape also emerged. He had
threatened to kill her or her boyfriend if anything was said and stated she “had
it coming” to her. I explored the effects of this on her, at physiological,
emotional, and cognitive levels. I tried to subtly dispute her beliefs about blame
and responsibility. Her concern about the possibility of the rape happening again
were reframed as self-protective.
Ms V was asked to consider new thoughts about safety and the possibility of
another rape. This prompted silent vexation. Encouragement was given to
consider the belief that rape would not happen again as she was safe, but she felt
dissonant about this. This was resolved by helping her adopt the belief that she
was safe at this time. She repeated this to herself several times, paired with
concentration on breathing to promote comfort. The session ended with the safe
place screen to stabilise affect.
In debriefing, Ms V spoke about how clear her memories were now, compared
with the “fuzzy” ones she had before. She was exhausted but enthused by the
session. Next session was in two days.
Session 13
In the time since the last session Ms V felt less agitated. Daytime intrusive
symptoms had decreased, but her sleep pattern had worsened. She was aware of
nightmares, but had no memory of them. She requested the same hypnotic
procedure with the intention of getting in touch with the past and “getting on
with it.”
The same procedure swiftly moved from split screens to affect bridge. Ms V
detailed family experiences, with affect ventilated towards her father for his
behaviour. She expressed anger and sadness at the mixed messages from her
father: How she was labelled as “special” and how her father “couldn’t help
it.”
She then expressed anger and bewilderment to her mother for not knowing
the obvious. Ms V was asked to express her new awareness and views to each
parent, and she demonstrated tentative changes in her beliefs. The new beliefs
were coupled with breathing to reinforce the changes and empower her.
The safe-place screen diverted Ms V from this material and she was asked to
recall positive experiences with her parents. This was to promote a complete
PTSD 71
Session 14
In the two days since the last session, Ms V’s interrupted sleep continued.
Although accepting this, she sought support from the night nursing staff. She
was fatigued and confused, but okay about the last session. The split-screen
technique was again used. Suggestions were made to investigate and tolerate the
feelings evoked by the positive and negative images. When comfortable
toleration was gained, the images were modified to later events. Using feedback,
this shift occurred twice. Images alternated between each rape and the positive
images of support from others and herself were intensified by counting 1–5 and
coupling breathing.
Sessions 15–18
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Bernstein, E. M., & Putnam, F. W. (1986). Development, reliability and validity of a
dissociation scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174, 727–735.
Branscomb, L. (1991). Dissociation in combat related post-traumatic stress disorder.
Dissociation, 4, 13–20.
Bremner, J. D., Southwick, S., Brett, E., Fontana, A., Rosenheck, R., & Charney, D. S.
(1992). Dissociation and post-traumatic stress disorder in Vietnam combat veterans.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 328–333.
Brown, D. P., & Fromm, E. (1987). Hypnosis and behavioural medicine. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bryant, R. A. (1994, August). Visual imagery in post-traumatic stress disorder. Paper
presented at the 13th International Congress of Hypnosis, Melbourne.
Coons, P. M., Bowman, E., Pellow, T. A., & Schneider, P. (1989). Post-traumatic
aspects of the treatment of victims of sexual abuse and incest. Psychiatric Clinics
of North America, 12, 325–335.
Evans, B. J. (1991). Hypnotisability in post-traumatic stress disorders: Implications for
hypnotic interventions in treatment. Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Hypnosis, 19, 49–58.
Evans, B. J. (1994, May). Hypnosis in the treatment of trauma and post-traumatic
stress disorders. Presented at an Australian Society of Hypnosis workshop,
Melbourne.
Gibson, H. B., & Heap, M. (1991). Hypnosis in therapy. London: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Golden, W. L. (1989). Resistance and change in cognitive behaviour therapy. In W.
Dryden & P. Trower (Eds.), Cognitive psychotherapy: Status and change (pp.
3–13). London: Cassell.
Horowitz, J. M., Wilner, N., & Alvarez, W. (1979). Impact of event scale: A measure
of subjective stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 41, 209–218.
Mackay, C. (1994, August). Hypnotisability and PTSD symptoms in Vietnam veterans.
Paper presented at the 13th International Congress of Hypnosis, Melbourne.
Morgan, A. H., & Hilgard, J. R. (1979). The Stanford Hypnotic Clinical Scale for
Adults. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 21, 134–147.
Shapiro, F. (1989a). Eye movement desensitisation: A new treatment for post-traumatic
memories. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 20, 211–
217.
Shapiro, F. (1989b). Efficacy of the eye movement desensitisation procedure in the
treatment of traumatic memories. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 2, 199–223.
Spiegel, D. (1994, August). Hypnosis in the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder.
Presented at the 13th International Congress of Hypnosis, Melbourne.
Spiegel, D., Hunt, T., & Dondershine, H. E. (1988). Dissociation and hypnotisability
in post-traumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 301–305.
PTSD 73
Stanley, R. O., Burrows, G. D., & Judd, F. K. (1988). Hypnosis in the management of
anxiety disorders. In R. Noves, M. Roth, & G. D. Burrows (Eds.), Handbook of
anxiety: Vol. 4. The treatment of anxiety (pp. 537–548). Amsterdam: Elsevier/
North-Holland Biomedical Press.
Tellegen, A., & Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-altering
experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 83, 268–277.
Turner, S. W. (1991, August). Post-traumatic stress disorder. Hospital Update, pp.
644–648.
Walker, W.-L. (1994, August). Hypnotisability and dissociation. Paper presented at the
13th International Congress of Hypnosis, Melbourne.
Australian Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis
Vol. 25, No. 1, 1997, pp. 74–79
Carolyn Sauer
Psychologist
Marc I. Oster
Psychologist
This article examines the use of hypnosis in preparation for childbirth. Two cases are
described in which a model of obstetric hypnosis was used in psychological preparation
for labour, delivery, and postpartum issues. The techniques were utilised over a relatively
short period of time and offered significant benefits to the mothers. One benefit was
flexibility in terms of helping mothers achieve their individual goals. Other benefits
included an increased sense of self-confidence, ability to remain calm during labour, and
ease of transition into breast feeding. The role of practice on treatment outcome is also
addressed.
THE APPROACH
SESSION 1
History was obtained regarding the expectant mothers’ previous birth experiences
and their attitudes to childbirth. In both cases this was a second pregnancy and
delivery. Information regarding their current expectations about labour, delivery,
and postpartum experience was also obtained. The patients were then introduced
to hypnosis.
Initially a technique was used to help relieve the patients of previous negative
experiences associated with childbirth and give them some positive expectations
and affirmations (Preston, 1995). This technique involved guided imagery, in
which the patients were asked to picture themselves at a blackboard in a
classroom writing the sentence, “I can manage my discomfort during labour and
delivery.” It was then suggested to the patients they picture themselves in a
library throwing out all books containing negative thoughts and experiences
related to pregnancies and deliveries. They were also given the suggestion to
unwrap three books titled: Success, Desires, and Health and Happiness. These
books were placed in a prominent spot on the shelf. The patients were then
asked to find a comfortable spot in the library and go there for the treatment
phase of the session. Hilgard and Hilgard (1994) have suggested testing for
depth in hypnosis research. These patients were tested for depth of hypnosis at
various times during the treatment using the heavy hand and hand levitation
tests. The results of the tests were consistently positive. Both patients appeared
to achieve a very deep state of hypnosis.
Once in a comfortable place in the library, the patients were given several
techniques for pain management (Longacre, 1995). A direct suggestion was
made that touching the forehead by either the patients or their husbands would
cause numbness in the midsection and total relaxation, and that a touch by a
gloved hand would cause numbing and relaxation to areas such as the vagina
(Preston, 1995). The patients were also asked to picture a pressure gauge on the
wall that measured discomfort related to pressure on the pelvic region. They
could control their level of discomfort by decreasing the numbers on the gauge.
Patients were then asked to picture themselves going through labour and
delivery in great detail, starting with the onset of labour, the trip to the hospital,
arriving on the hospital unit, stage I and II of labour, pushing, and finally ending
with breast-feeding. Each was given additional suggestions related to diminished
bleeding, numbness in the vagina, peritoneum and abdomen, and rapid
postpartum recovery.
76 Sauer and Oster
SESSION 2
The patients were instructed in the use of self-hypnosis which they practised in
the office. They were also taken through the same guided imagery exercise from
the previous session.
CASE PRESENTATIONS
Case One
The patient, aged 32 years, was self-referred for obstetric hypnosis. She reported
this was her second pregnancy in three years. She had attended Lamaze class
prior to her first childbirth. During our initial hypnosis session she recalled
several negative experiences associated with her previous pregnancy. Her labour
was long, approximately 20 hours, and she had complications during the final
phase of her labour, the transition. She received an injection of fentanyl in the
lumbar region of her spine (intrathecal) for pain when she was at approximately
5 cm dilation. She reported that the baby got stuck on her pelvic bone, causing
extreme pain, affecting her ability to push successfully. After being faced with
the possibility of a caesarean she was able to finally deliver vaginally. She
indicated that her difficulty was concern about modesty. Several friends were
present at this birth watching her cope with labour and transition. She was
worried about their impressions of her. The patient reported she had difficulty
recovering from this pregnancy and difficulty nursing. The baby would not latch
on to the breast, reportedly due to inadequate milk production. The patient’s
concerns regarding this pregnancy were: being able to push successfully during
transition, being able to breastfeed comfortably, and whether or not her 2-year-
old daughter would adjust to the new baby. She was also concerned about
embarrassing herself in front of her friends by appearing unable to control her
behaviour during labour and transition. She exhibited some apprehension which
she described as performance anxiety.
The patient reported her labour began at home the day following her second
hypnosis session. She reported she used the hypnosis successfully to control her
discomfort at home. She was unaware of using hypnosis after she arrived in the
hospital at 2 a.m., despite her report of being in a trance-like state for the next
two hours while her husband touched her forehead. She received her first
intrathecal when she was dilated to 4 cm. She described her contractions as very
intense compared to her previous labour. She received a second intrathecal at
6.30 a.m. when she was dilated to 8 cm. Subsequently the obstetrician told her
to push. Her initial response was to cry and request a caesarean. However, she
found a comfortable position and began to push effectively, despite the fact that
Obstetric Hypnosis 77
the baby became caught on her pelvic bone. In spite of the baby being caught,
the patient reported her efforts at pushing were more efficient compared to her
previous delivery experience.
The patient reported several experiences that gave her comfort. She said that,
while pushing, she felt an increased sense of comfort when the obstetrician put
his hand in her vagina and applied pressure posteriorly. She reported achieving
comfort when her husband, watching the foetal monitor, made the comment,
“The numbers are going down.” The patient also reported that the affirmation,
“I can manage my discomfort,” was helpful in coping with the difficulty of
transition.
The patient’s husband was present for the labour and delivery. He reported
this delivery was markedly smoother than the first and he indicated his wife was
calmer and more focused during labour and was more effective when required
to push. According to him, the patient seemed more confident throughout the
entire process and he was impressed with the results.
The patient reported she had heavy bleeding for one day following delivery
and then noticed the bleeding had greatly diminished. Her nipples were very
comfortable despite 24 hours of nursing. She received one dose of Tylenol with
codeine for the afterpains and used hypnosis to control subsequent cramping.
She was surprised to find that she was up and about sooner after this delivery,
compared to her previous experience. Her baby boy, she reported, was easy to
manage and seemed very calm. The Apgar scores were 7 and 9.
At two week follow-up, the patient reported her baby continued to be very
calm and easy to manage. Her milk supply was abundant and her nursing was
going exceptionally well. Her 2-year-old daughter was adjusting nicely to the
presence of a new sibling.
Case Two
The second patient, a 29-year-old, was also self-referred for obstetric hypnosis.
She reported this was her second pregnancy in four years. She attended
childbirth education classes and learned the Lamaze technique with her first
pregnancy, but reported limited success using this technique. She indicated that
once her water broke the contractions began slowly, with low intensity. After six
to seven hours of labour she felt intense discomfort and received an injection of
Nubain. She pushed for one and a half hours.
Her reason for using obstetric hypnosis with this pregnancy was to avoid the
use of medication during and after labour. Her other goal was to have the baby
one hour after she checked into the hospital. She used the Bradley method for
childbirth preparation (Eisenberg, Murkoff, & Hathaway, 1984). The patient had
one complicating factor, which was some abnormal cells on her pap smear. She
was given a suggestion to remove the abnormal cells from her cervix. Two
weeks later a coloscopy showed no abnormal cells.
78 Sauer and Oster
During hypnosis the patient accurately predicted the date her son would be born.
Labour began at home where her water broke. She checked into the hospital at
11.30 p.m. and the baby was born at 1 a.m. The patient reported she was very
comfortable until transition, when the pressure became more intense. She felt
calm and in control during the entire process and received no medication despite
the hospital staff suggesting she might want some. She was pleased that she felt
very alert and reported the birth was very easy. During the pregnancy, the patient
had expressed concerns that the baby might be resistive to coming out as she had
this sense with her first child during transition. This baby seemed to come out
on his own as the obstetrician told her to stop pushing toward the end of the
delivery. She indicated that two friends who were present during the delivery
commented on how she made the labour and delivery look easy. The patient’s
husband commented on how he had never seen anything like this. He said, “I
don’t know what you did, but it was incredible.”
According to the patient, the baby was calm and relaxed at birth. The Apgar
score was 9. She reported that she had very little discomfort postpartum. There
were no reported complications associated with breast-feeding. At one-week
follow-up, the baby was still calm and easy to manage. The mother’s bleeding
had stopped and she was feeling comfortable. She felt the hypnosis was useful
for both herself and the baby. She had practised self-hypnosis once a day for
four weeks prior to her delivery.
DISCUSSION
REFERENCES
Craker, R., (1992). The use of hypnosis in psychological preparation for an elective
caesarean section. Paper presented at the Annual Scientific Meeting of the
American Society of Clinical Hypnosis, Las Vegas, NV.
Eisenberg, A., Murkoff, H. E., & Hathaway, S. E. (1984). What to expect when you’re
expecting (2nd ed.). New York: Workman Publishing.
Flatt, J. R. (1984). Hypnosis in the treatment of smoking: A single session success
story? Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 12, 58–61.
Harmon, T. M., Hynan, M., & Tyre, T. E. (1990). Improved obstetric outcomes using
hypnotic analgesia and skill mastery combined with childbirth education. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 525–530.
Hilgard, E. R., & Hilgard, J. R. (1994). Hypnosis in the relief of pain. New York:
Brunner/Mazel.
Longacre, R. D. (1995). Visualisation and guided imagery for pain management.
Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt.
Oster, M. I. (1994). Psychological preparation for labor and delivery using hypnosis.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 37, 12–21.
Preston, M. D. (1995). Seminar on medical hypnosis. Redding, CA.
Australian Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis
Vol. 25, No. 1, 1997, pp. 80–84
Brian Alman
Psychologist
Requests for reprints should be sent to Brian M. Alman PhD, PO Box 746, Del Mar, USA.
80
Theory of Self-Hypnosis 81
inner evolution.
The meditation teachers in India were open to learning self-hypnosis but
wanted a better definition. I told them what I knew, based on my years of
experience.
THEORETICAL IDEAS
The conscious mind wants to stay in control. It knows what it’s doing and wants
to keep things as they are, very familiar. The conscious mind wants to keep the
unconscious mind from taking over (which it thinks of as the unknown). The
conscious mind will frequently try to dominate the unconscious mind when it
thinks it is in danger of the unconscious mind manifesting itself in an
uncontrolled way. The conscious mind represents the known and is usually
under control. When we utilise self-hypnosis we are tapping into our unconscious
mind, too. Most people believe that they can manage the conscious mind, but
opening of the unconscious mind could lead to insecurity and this perpetuates
the fear of the unconscious.
This is the conflict: the conflict between the conscious and the unconscious;
between the energy that has become manifest and the energy that wants to
manifest.
Each individual is unique and has the choice whether or not to evolve. Self-
hypnosis is a risk, with the possibility of disappointments, and there are no
guarantees on this journey. One person’s understanding cannot be given to
another. Individuals need to find their own sense of understanding. There are
trials, errors, failures, frustrations, and real living that occurs. With self-hypnosis
there is growth. Clearly, an individual may simply continue their biological/
mechanistic existence and never pursue any kind of individual evolving process.
That is one’s choice.
Every person develops their own unique wall between their conscious mind
and their unconscious mind. As a child, this may have been protective, self-
sustaining, and intelligent at the time. But as the person becomes an adolescent
(medium-sized kid) or an adult (grown-up-sized kid), the protective wall
becomes a barrier and very likely keeps positive potential away more than it
keeps problems at bay. The conscious mind, in trying to protect itself and keep
itself in control, actually ends up keeping the creative potential of the
unconscious mind at a distance. Unfortunately, this means that people try to
resolve problems either with their conscious mind (most waking hours) or with
their unconscious mind (much of sleeping time in dreams which are often
forgotten or discounted). Perhaps, you’re beginning to see where I’m going with
this self-hypnosis theory: self-hypnosis helps remove the wall/barrier and builds
a bridge connecting the conscious mind with the unconscious mind. For the first
time in a long time, an individual can benefit from all of their inner resources
and their conscious and unconscious, simultaneously and together again.
A harmonious relationship between the conscious and the unconscious can
82 Alman
begin with self-hypnosis. For example, imagine a river that has a wall in the
middle of it that is intended to keep two communities apart (which represent the
conscious and unconscious). In order to build a bridge that connects these two
sides of the river, the wall is gradually removed. Once a longstanding wall is
removed, including the Berlin Wall, which surprised the whole world when it
came down, a bridge for new communication develops automatically.
As a demonstration of this, I had each of the meditation practitioners go
through a six step-process:
1. With eyes open, write down a challenge that you are facing in your life at the
present time
2. With your dominant hand, write down everything you can think about while
focusing on this problem . . . thoughts, ideas, doubts, etc.
3. With your eyes closed and with your non-dominant hand, communicate
everything you feel, and imagine while focusing on this challenge . . . and
this may be with shapes, designs, doodling or words.
4. Still with your eyes closed, imagine that you are up on a hill looking down
on a bridge that is connecting two sides of a river (like East and West) and
it’s replacing a wall that has blocked the two sides of the river for years. Now,
from this observer perspective without judgment or criticism, and with your
non-dominant hand (or with your dominant hand or both hands together),
allow your writing utensil to move across the paper as you allow for a
dialogue between the conscious mind and the unconscious mind . . . the East
side of the river and the West side of the river . . . and everything else that
you are able to observe about this transformation from this detached and yet
absorbing point of view. Be aware that at any time you need to feel safe,
secure, with just enough detachment and perspective, you can always feel
free in the observer perspective . . . during this experience and during any
experience, as need be. Simply, remember the bridge that you are creating
and allow your breathing to re-connect you with this any time. Naturally,
your breathing is a bridge between your outermost self, your body, and your
innermost self, your awareness. Perhaps, you can notice that your conscious
mind and your unconscious mind are thanking one another for their concern
and even saying, “I’m sorry for ignoring you in the past.” It’s possible now
for them to tell each other what they believe is important for the other to
know. Let them connect closer on the bridge and in your own worlds, saying
something like, “I want to come closer and be a friend . . . I never thought
about you working for me all those years and I have never thanked you and
is there anything that I can do better for you in our future?” Just allow the
writing utensil to move across the paper as they dialogue.
5. Next, with your eyes closed, imagine that you are up on what may be the
highest hill around and you’re looking down at the observer on the hill below
that has been observing the bridge that’s connecting the two sides of the river.
So, you’re observing the observer and you’re observing the bridge that’s
Theory of Self-Hypnosis 83
connecting your conscious side with your unconscious side. Now, with both
hands together or your non-dominant hand or dominant hand . . . it really
doesn’t matter . . . your choice . . . and without any judgment, criticism or
filtering . . . allow your writing utensil to move across the paper as you
describe what you’re observing about the observer who is observing the
bridge and both sides of the river. Allow the writing utensil to move across
the paper as you, from the highest hill point of view, observe what you can
about what’s going on down there on the bridge as the two sides of the river,
(the conscious mind’s ideas and the unconscious mind’s creativity), connect
with one another harmoniously.
6. Finally, as you are getting ready to finish with all this in the next few minutes
(and you know you can come back to this experience any time you choose
to), re-focus on the rise and fall of your breath. Also be aware that the bridge
that connects your conscious mind with your unconscious mind is the key
that opens up the wall, removes the barrier and develops the new internal
structure for your whole self. You may want to acknowledge that from any
observer perspective, this is the master key that can open up all the doors.
While you’re finishing up and relaxing inside about letting go and going with
the flow, keep in mind that the pilgrimage itself is the goal.
The unconscious mind has almost always been in conflict with the conscious
mind. The conscious mind has tried to dominate the unconscious mind because
it believed it was in danger of the unconscious manifesting itself. The conscious
mind is controlled and the unconscious is not. You can manage the conscious but
with a burst from the unconscious an insecurity may develop which is the fear
of the conscious mind. This is the conflict. The conflict between the conscious
and the unconscious; between the energy that has become manifest and the
energy that wants to.
Self-hypnosis and hetero-hypnosis open up the whole issue of vulnerability
and what is possible. As you approach the bridging the conscious mind and the
unconscious mind in a way that is respectful, maintains each individual’s
dignity and allows for a whole outcome, making friends with yourself like this
expands the communicating, caring and relaxation better than perhaps ever
before. The building of the bridge can take time and a commitment to the
construction.
As an example, on the first day of my five-day workshop in India, a doctor
asked me if self-hypnosis might be helpful in helping him manage his anger. He
went on to tell me that his younger brother had just died of leukemia and how
quickly it seemed that his brother, who was only 19, had gone from healthy to
dying. This doctor had always benefited from his meditations when he was
unhappy, going through a difficult time, or even been sick himself. This time he
was really troubled because he was getting angry with his wife and co-workers,
and felt like these feelings were underneath the surface at all times. When I
demonstrated the approach described above, he was the volunteer subject. As
84 Alman
one might expect, his meditation experience allowed him to get into a calm,
peaceful state very easily. Once he allowed his conscious mind to communicate
what it was thinking about his present life situation, he expressed a lot of self-
doubt, self-criticism regarding not being a good enough brother, and a variety
of negative expectations about his future. His unconscious communication
allowed for sadness, tears, anger, and longing for his brother. He also expressed
a comfort as if his brother were with him, telling him not to worry, that he was
okay now, and that he loved him for all that he had been. The bridge and
observer perspective opened up a whole new way of looking at the whole thing.
The doctor felt like his own inner therapist was reminding him that he could
choose to be afraid of living now or he could be free and evolve with all that was
happening to him and his family. He was able to realise that if he stayed stuck
in the fear of living he would be a slave. He would not have to be responsible
and this could even become comfortable. This could be less of a burden. Then,
he made the choice to be responsible and it was time now to deal with the
struggle. He could see that he alone was responsible and there was no escaping
this. He did not want to postpone the problem or deny the responsibility. In all
of this he felt courageous for the first time in a long time. He felt more
comfortable being expressive and it was described as a “positive force.” He
finished the process with a sentence about his choice as being totally his
responsibility.
Self-hypnosis can open up the doors of inner evolution including: listening to
your inner wisdom (both logical and poetic), learning to deal with all sides of
oneself (female, male, aggressive, receiving, open and waiting), reorganising
the way you experience yourself and situations (in friendship, work, love, or
problems . . . when in doubt, go deeper), living in the present and stopping the
past from controlling you now and in the future (with awareness and courage
dealing with the predetermined and the undetermined ways of life), and
accessing personal resources to overcome denial, boredom, imbalance, or any
vicious cycle (each of us must grow like a tree – in all directions – if we grow
in only one direction we are bound to be in deep difficulty). Naturally, there are
unlimited applications for self-hypnosis, especially as we learn to accept
ourselves as a whole human being. And we remember that the pilgrimage is the
goal.
Thank you for your openness in accepting your conscious mind’s ideas and
your unconscious mind’s creativity. Together, you are able to utilise all of your
resources with any issue, any time you choose.
Australian Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis
Vol. 25, No. 1, 1997, pp. 85–88
CASE NOTES
The aim of Case Notes is to enable readers to contribute brief items and case material
drawn from their own experience. These may be case situations in which hypnosis has
been used in treatment or a description of specific hypnotherapeutic techniques used
within treatment contexts. The contributor is asked to supply as much information as is
needed to ensure the reader has an understanding of the situation, the therapeutic aims of
the hypnosis, and outcomes. It may also be appropriate for the contributor to research the
relevant research and clinical literature to justify and explain their use of hypnosis. While
the standard criteria for publication in the journal will not necessarily apply to Case Notes,
a clear exposition of the ethical professional practice of hypnosis will be required if the
material is to be published.
Kathryn Gow
Psychologist
Kathryn Gow, Queensland University of Technology, PO Box 268, Red Hill, Queensland
4059
85
86 Case Note
The underlined words indicate the main NLP techniques or messages that are
incorporated within the general theme of positive thinking and ego-strengthening.
These underlined words are commands to be emphasised. The names of the
techniques are listed at the beginning of the scripts for readers’ information.
Those of you who are familiar with NLP terminology will recognise the subtlety
of the suggestions.
You will note that two dots (..) are used to indicate the blurring of one
message into the other.
Script 1 contains references to NLP and Ericksonian concepts and is the
simpler of the scripts. The concepts, heavily embedded in Ericksonian language,
covered in Script 1 are: the swish technique, transformational search, the self-
edit, Bateson’s logical types and general Ericksonian language.
Script 2 contains more NLP concepts to be reviewed: parts integration,
Bateson’s logical types, Bateson’s distinctions, Rossi’s and others’ neurological
connections, Dilt’s belief changes, calibrations, physiological shifts,
submodalities, drill, the swish technique, Bateson’s ecology, the magic of
Erickson, Tad James’ time line therapy, secondary gains, Clare Graves values,
identity change, metaphors, principles and rules, the circle of excellence and the
Milton model.
Outcome: For trainees to review the day with insight and understanding as they
build their self-esteem.
As you prepare to review your learnings today with insight and understanding,
you may swish to close your eyes and relax or you may prefer to sit in your
chair very comfortably with your eyes open and fixed on one spot and go into
a trance whichever way you prefer to do it .. your way (that’s right) and if you
wish to transform your search, you can go in and out of trance during your
edit, then you can allow your conscious and unconscious mind to come
together to produce the most useful insights for you to feel good .. that you
have now come a different way along the path that you have chosen to follow
.. your inner voice which may have one special message about your learnings
which is highly significant for you..r integration is occurring now at a deeper
level than before you leave soon with the special gift that your unconscious
mind is now giving you, because you are special, as there is only one of you,
and if you are a logical type, then you know that it will depend on the quality
of accessing which distinctions you can now make about how brilliant you
really are.
And it’s a good thing to value the possibilities of making the changes
that you have, or have not, allowed to occur, at a neurological level, to a time
Case Notes 87
in the future when you are standing up in front of people who need to change
beliefs about learning, and you can calibrate the shifts in submodalities
because you have let go the blocks and refined your drill to such a special
tune that you can almost hear the swish of your hands as those powerful parts
of you come together to make changes so the ecology of your being is in time
with the cosmic play that you can not not prevent yourself from finding
joyful as you open your eyes now and anticipate with clarity new ways of
bringing more magic into your life.
Outcome: For participants to confirm that they are more confident and
comfortable in changing their identity to that of an NLP trainer.
As you now prepare to model Milton and conduct a self-edit on your
insightful strategies employed today, notice that you can not resist making
your chair feel comfortable by being totally entranced by what you have
learned .. a lot and changed your identity to that of an NLP trainer and you
already know what to do, didn’t you.
A part of me is wondering if you are completely comfortable now
because that means that you can make more and more new neurological
connect .. your mind and body in such a way from old patterns that resisted
you and violated your integrity is of value and I know that a part of you wants
to ensure you preserve the learnings from all of these experiences and let go
of the emotions, so that you block out the old picture and swish into your new
identity with distinction.
As you turn up the volume on the voice of your inner wisdom, you can
know just how logical it is to type the Clare Graves levels of value .. your
own progress because that means that you can calibrate towards success and
you may negotiate your gains with a secondary part of you that is critical to
finer distinctions, didn’t you.
As I spoke to Judy yesterday about this feeling of taking a flying leap,
she said that apart from experiencing how to fly, you can enjoy landing safely
on the other side and notice you have survived this metaphor of let .. ting go
your old identity, didn’t you. As you say “me too!!”, you can observe the
playful ecology of your principles as the creative design of the universe
empowers you to process the content and structure your play in alignment
with your circle of excellence and you can not not know your outcomes and
when you are ready, open your eyes to the models surround you.
RECOMMENDED READING
Bandler, R., & Grinder, J. (1979). Frogs into Princes: Neuro-linguistic programming.
Moab, UT: Real People Press.
88 Case Note
Bandler, R., & Grinder, J. (1982). Reframing: Neuro-linguistic programming and the
transformation of meaning. Moab, UT: Real People Press.
Dilts, R. (1983). Roots of neuro-linguistic programming. Cupertino, CA: Meta
Publications.
Dilts, R., Hallbom, T., & Smith, S. (1991). Beliefs: Pathways to health and well-being.
Portland, OR: Metamorphous Press.
Grinder, J., & Bandler, R. (1981). Trance-formations: Neuro-linguistic programming
and the structure of hypnosis. Moab, UT: Real People Press.
Grinder, J., DeLozier, J., & Bandler, R. (1977). Patterns of the hypnotic techniques of
Milton H. Erickson: Vol 2. Cupertino, CA: Meta Publications.
Laborder, G. Z. (1983). Influencing with integrity: Management skills for communication
and negotiation. Palo Alto: Syntony Publishing.
Lewis, B., & Pucelik, F. (1992). Magic of NLP demystified. Portland, OR: Metamorphous
Press.
Rosen, S. (1982). My voice will go with you: The teaching tales of Milton H. Erickson.
New York: W.W. Norton & Company.
Australian Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis
Vol. 25, No. 1, 1997, pp. 89–90
BOOK REVIEWS
The history of hypnosis, with its roots in the practice of Franz Anton Mesmer
and the French Mesmerists, is a source of deep fascination for me. So, given the
opportunity to review a book on the history of hypnosis has been a great
opportunity to indulge.
The main theme of From Mesmer to Freud is obviously not a new one.
Crabtree basically argues, along the lines of Ellenberger (1970), that psychiatry
and psychology owe an unacknowledged debt to hypnosis and mesmerism as the
fundamental sources of subsequent psychological theorising and research.
However, Crabtree goes on to introduce masses of original source material that
really seems to capture the concepts and questions that were current for the
historical periods he covers. In many ways it would appear that Crabtree is
trying to cover ground that has been left a bit bare by earlier historical
references. In doing so he has taken on a massive task.
From Mesmer to Freud is organised in four parts that appear to loosely follow
the historical development of hypnosis from magnetic healing to mesmerism to
magnetic sleep. Crabtree intends to trace the basis of many modern psychological
concepts in the earlier work of the magnetisers. Indeed, he seems to be deeply
enamoured of mesmeric history and it is easy to become overwhelmed by the
amount of detail he provides on the early mesmerists and their cases. Overall,
Crabtree spends considerably less time on the practitioners of hypnosis than the
mesmerists. This is not such a bad thing, insofar as the detailed history of
Mesmer and his followers seems to have become increasingly marginalised and
mythologised in the history of hypnosis. But some of the book becomes a
repetitious presentation of case material as Crabtree minutely traces the
contribution of mesmerism to psychological concepts, mind–body issues,
spiritualism, and several other topics.
Crabtree explains that his approach to historical research is to avoid the
“error” of presentism. Presentism is the practice of interpreting historical
documents and events within the sociopolitical context of the present day.
Crabtree sees this as an error in that it does not take into account the context
within which events occur. To my mind, however, this suggests that Crabtree
should endorse a position of analysing documentation within its historical
89
90 Book Reviews
sociopolitical context. But the major weakness of his book is its almost total
lack of analysis. There were very powerful societal and professional forces
arraigned against the practitioners of mesmerism and hypnosis, yet Crabtree
fails to analyse the factors and issues that led to the various twists and turns in
the fortunes of hypnotic practice. Rather, he presents the material that tells the
story without trying to set the scene. I found this very unsatisfying as I think
Crabtree has taken an erroneous position in trying to be objective by presenting
history as the source material by itself. I doubt that it is possible to be objective
in relation to anything. Further, I feel that unless we analyse historical records
from an understanding of the broader context of that period, it is not possible to
tease out the whys and wherefores of the production of information. This is very
important in the history of science, which is especially clouded by its mantle of
scientific “objectivity,” “rationalism,” and “truth.”
In relation to this Crabtree highlights an interesting question that nobody
appears to have properly explored. Whatever happened to the practice of
mesmerism? From Crabtree’s presentation it would appear that it was transformed
into spiritualism and largely disappeared in medical and psychological circles.
This is a fascinating example of the political avoidance of a therapeutic practice
due to its association with so-called “unscientific” superstition. Even today
there are only two references to using mesmeric “technology” and I could only
find one of these (Pulos, 1980). But Crabtree really fails to explore any of this
in detail.
In general, From Mesmer to Freud is a well-written book with a fascinating
tale to tell. There is probably too much detail and repetition to make it a really
great book but it is stimulating in its own right. Crabtree could certainly have
got away with producing several volumes of information. I would be inclined to
read any subsequent volumes he produces because he is an apparently meticulous
and careful historian, despite my disagreement about his philosophical approach
to his task.
REFERENCES
Ellenberger, H. F. (1970). The discovery of the unconscious: The history and evolution
of dynamic psychiatry. New York: Basic Books.
Pulos, L. (1980). Mesmerism revisited: The effectiveness of Esdaile’s techniques in the
production of deep hypnosis and total body hypnoanaesthesia. American Journal of
Clinical Hypnosis, 22, pp. 206–211.
91
The Australian Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis
Contents
Introduction
Eating attitudes and behaviours in the normal population. . . . Nicole Lefkovitz and Barry J. Evans
Psychotherapy and hypnotherapy for overweight and obesity counselling. . . . Diane J. McGreal
Bulimia Nervosa
Hypnosis in the treatment of bulimia nervosa: A review of the literature . . . . . . . Greg J. Coman
Group therapy and hypnosis for the treatment of bulimia nervosa. . . . . . . Marcia Degun-Mather
Anorexia Nervosa
Hypnotic and strategic interventions in the treatment of anorexia nervosa. . . . Michael D. Yapko
No. of copies
❏ Mastercard ❏ Visa
Signature__________________________________________________
Published by the