Thesis 2000D S592e
Thesis 2000D S592e
THE POSITIVE
By
BRETL. SIMMONS
Bachelor of Science
Park College
Parkville, Missouri
1992
By
Bret L. Simmons
December, 2000
EUSTRESS AT WORK: ACCENTUATING
THE POSITIVE
Thesis Approved:
Thesis Adviser
ii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I am compelled to offer praise to God and thanks to many people for the completion of this
study. First and foremost, my heartfelt love and appreciation are due to my wife, Dalsun, and
daughter, Sarah, for their unconditional love, support, and patience, without which this effort
would not have been possible. Similar appreciation is due to Dr. Mary T. Newman, who first
encouraged me to seek a doctoral degree and provided steadfast friendship and professional
I wish to express my sincere appreciation to my major advisor, Dr. Debra L. Nelson for her
intelligent supervision, constructive guidance, creative inspiration, enduring patience, and valued
friendship. Dr. Debra Nelson's consummate professionalism was the primary enabler for the
quality of this study. My sincere appreciation also extends to my other committee members, Dr.
Margaret A. White, Dr. Mark B. Gavin, Dr. Robert S. Dooley, Dr. Ken E. Case, and Dr. James C.
Quick, whose guidance, assistance, encouragement, support, and friendship were also invaluable.
I am especially indebted to Dr. Mark B. Gavin for his expertise in analyzing and interpreting the
results of this study and to Dr. James C. Quick not only for his incredible mastery of the field of
Occupational Stress and Health, but also for the time, effort, and financial resources that he
Finally, I want to thank Dr. Wayne A. Meinhart and the Department of Management at
Oklahoma State University, as well as Dr. James M. Collins and Dr. Mary R. Lindahl of the
University of Alaska Fairbanks School of Management for their support as I conducted this
study, and Beverlee Dunham for her efforts to make this document look professional.
iii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Chapter Page
1: Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
The Research Problem . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
Theoretical Background ..................................................... 3
Dissertation Objectives . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
Theoretical and Practical Implications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
iv
Chapter Page
4: Results .................................................................. 69
Survey Response . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69
Item Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69
Combining Two Samples Into One . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73
Data Analysis Strategy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74
Assessing Model Fit .................................................... 74
Order of Procedures . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
Second-Order CFA ...................................... · . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78
Structural Analyses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 114
Appendix . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124
V
LIST OF TABLES
Table Page
1. Summary of Physiological Changes That Can be Elicited by
Cannon's Fight or Flight Response ........................................... 12
10. Correlations Among Latent Variables Included in the Structural Model .............. 87
12. Factors, Associated Items, and Pattern Coefficients for the Structural Model .......... 91
vi
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure Page
1. A Holistic Model of Stress . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
. vii
CHAPTERl
INTRODUCTION
This dissertation proposes a new way to conceptualize and measure eustress. This introduc-
tory chapter begins with a presentation of the research problem. The theoretical background for
the dissertation will then be presented, followed by an outline of the dissertation objectives.
What is stress? The answer to this question is surprisingly complex because more than 40
definitions of stress can be found in the literature (Hoes, 1986). In the literature, the term "stress"
has been popularly used to refer to a wide range of physiological changes, psychological states,
and environmental pressures (Bieliauskas, 1982). Quick, Horn, and Quick (1987) offer the
following definition: "Stress is the naturally occurring mind-body response to demanding and/or
emergency situations, either of a chronic or episodic nature" (p. 19, emphasis added). The
Leaving out the descriptive clauses, stress can be thought of as a process involving response to a
situation. In its most basic terms, both the stimulus (i.e., stressor) and the response (i.e., the
Why should we care about confusion over the conceptualization of stress? Probably the most
important outcome variables associated with the study of stress are health and well-being (Quick
et al., 1987; Ganster & Schaubroeck, 1991; Quick, Quick, Nelson, & Hurrell, 1997). Despite the
their health. These physical or psychological stimuli to which the individual responds are
commonly termed distress, and it is distress that is commonly studied for its relationship to
adverse health outcomes (Quick et al. 1997). Distress, as such, is negative and dysfunctional
Interestingly, some have suggested that there is also good stress, which Selye (1976a, 1976b)
termed eustress. Quick et al. (1997) associate eustress with healthy, positive outcomes.
Unfortunately, the positive response to stressors and the associated health benefits of these
responses has received little attention in the literature. For example, Quick et al. (1997) define
preventive stress management as "an organizational philosophy and set of principles that employs
specific methods for promoting individual and organizational health while preventing individual
and organizational distress" (p. 149). Like that of other stress researchers, the overwhelming
focus of their approach is the prevention of the negative, that being distress. While they
acknowledge the importance of promoting the positive (i.e., eustress), they offer little research or
practical support for this position. Although their model incorporates eustress, they present a
discussion of stress in which eustress, although conceptually distinct, is operationally merely the
absence of distress.
Is this simplistic model of stress accurate, or is a more complex model of stress possible? In
more than just the absence of distress. A more complex model suggests that the presence/
absence of the positive as well as the presence/absence of the negative is necessary to fully
Consider the metaphor of a bathtub to illustrate this point. As a minimum, we are concerned
about two things when we settle in for a bath - the level of water in the tub and the temperature
of water in the tub. Essentially two things determine the level of water in the bathtub - the flow
of water into the bathtub and the flow of water out of the bathtub over time. Likewise, the
simultaneous flow of both hot and cold water into the bathtub determine the temperature of the
water in the tub. Ifwe liken the study of stress to the study of water in the bathtub, our current
2
approach is like studying a bathtub with a single water faucet - cold water - representing distress.
We know a lot about the sources of cold water, and we can tell individuals how to either decrease
the flow of cold water into or increase the flow of cold water out of their bathtubs. We also know
quite a bit about the physiological, behavioral, and psychological consequences of sitting in a tub
of cold water for a prolonged period of time. Our knowledge of cold water (distress) is important
but does not present a complete understanding of the water (stress) in the bathtub. A more
complete model of stress would acknowledge that the bathtub does indeed have two faucets - hot
and cold - and both are necessary to get the level and temperature of the water just right for a
comfortable bath.
stress. The challenge then becomes conceptualizing and operationalizing eustress in terms that
distinguish it from distress; examining how eustress and distress are related; and exploring their
differential contributions to the health of individuals, in particular, their health related to work.
Theoretical Background
Edwards and Cooper (1988), in the most extensive review of the subject of eustress, suggest
that eustress may improve health either directly through hormonal and biochemical changes or
indirectly by facilitating effort and abilities directed toward coping with existing distress. They
reviewed findings from a variety of sources, including anecdotal evidence, laboratory experi-
ments, and studies of positive life events and job satisfaction. They found suggestive, but not
conclusive, evidence for the direct effect of eustress on health. Little research has focused on the
effects of eustress on coping with existing distress, so evidence of the indirect benefits of eustress
is scant. They identified only a single study (Sales, 1969) demonstrating that eustress is
physiological damage. Edwards and Cooper (1988) assert that a conclusive demonstration of the
3
effects of eustress on health requires attention to the methodological issues of measurement,
Edwards and Cooper (1988) further suggest that the most general approach to the measure-
ment of eustress involves the assessment of positive psychological states. They assert that a
major issue in this approach is establishing the presence of positive psychological states, rather
than merely the absence of negative states. They state that rather than representing opposite ends
of a single continuum, positive (eustress) and negative (distress) states may represent two distinct
constructs, which would require separate multivariate indices for their measurement. This more
The challenge of the model presented in Figure l is explaining what starts the process. If
there are separate and distinct patterns of response to a given stressor, what produces them? This
study is grounded in the cognitive appraisal approach to stress in which the interpretation of
stressors, rather than stressors per se, determine how individuals respond. According to this
approach, two individuals with significantly different perceptions of the same stressor (or a single
individual with differing interpretations at different times) would respond differently. Likewise,
,two individuals with similar perceptions of the same (or different) stressors would experience
4
The cognitive appraisal approach to the study of stress is most commonly associated with the
work of Richard Lazarus (l 966). The essence of this approach to understanding stress is that
people can have different responses to stressors they encounter depending on whether they
existence of positive responses, he, like the majority of stress researchers, focused almost
When a person encounters a stressor, she or he evaluates the encounter with respect to its
significance for well-being. This evaluative process is the essence of cognitive appraisal. If a
stressor is not appraised as irrelevant, Lazarus .and Folkman ( 1984) assert that appraisals can be
complex and mixed, depending on person factors and the situational context. They essentially
describe two types of appraisals and associated response patterns: positive and stressful.
Positive appraisals "occur if the outcome of an encounter is construed as positive, that is, if it
preserves or enhances well-being or promises to do so" (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984, p. 32). As
indicators of positive appraisals, they suggest looking for the presence of positive or pleasurable
psychological states.
include harm/loss, threat, and challenge. In harm/loss, some damage to the person has already
occurred (e.g., injury, illness, loss of a loved one, damage to self-esteem). Threat involves harms
or losses that have not yet occurred but are anticipated. Challenge appraisals occur if the
outcome of an encounter holds the potential for gain or growth. ·As indicators of challenge
appraisals, they suggest looking for some of the same positive or pleasurable psychological states
Lazarus and Folkman ( 1984) do not view challenge and threat as poles of a single continuum.
They believe that challenge and threat responses can occur simultaneously, as the result of the
same stressor, and should be considered as separate but related constructs. While threat is clearly
5
a negative appraisal, challenge is better thought of as a positive appraisal (they share the same
indicators).
As such, the reasoning they apply to the distinction between challenge and threat to the
higher levels of positive and negative response can be extended. Accordingly, positive and
negative responses can occur simultaneously, as a result of the same stressor, and should be
considered separate but related constructs. Thus, for any given stressor, an individual can have
both a degree of positive and a degree of negative response. This is consistent with Lazarus and
Folkman's (1984) view that any psychophysiological theory of stress or emotion that views the
They support this with research of emotions and autonomic nervous system activity (Elkman,
Levenson, & Friesen, 1983) as well as research of hormonal response to arousing conditions
It is important to note that the focus of this research is limited to the stress response, not the
process of cognitive appraisal. As others have done (Folkins, 1970; Folkman & Lazarus, 1985;
Nomikos, Opton, Averill, & Lazarus, 1968), this study will not attempt to either manipulate or
assess appraisal directly. Cognitive appraisal is inferred and employed as the theoretical
explanation for the separate positive and negative responses hypothesized in this holistic model
of stress.
As shown in Figure 2, each separate response wilLhave its associated indicators, will inform
the other response, and will produce a differential effect on the ultimate outcome variable (e.g.,
health). As suggested by Edwards and Cooper (1988), the indicators of the positive response will
be positive psychological states (e.g., positive affect and hope) and the indicators ofthe·negative
response will be negative psychological states (e.g., anxiety and hostility). Consistent with this
holistic representation of stress, for the purposes of this study eustress and distress will be
defined as follows.
6
Indicators
t
Stressor < Positive Response ~
t
Negative Response
/'. Outcomes
t
Indicators
Eustress: A positive response resulting from the evaluation of a particular entity or event
(stressor) with a degree of favor.
Distress: A negative response resulting from the evaluation of a particular entity or event
(stressor) with a degree of disfavor.
Dissertation Objectives
The purpose of this dissertation is to test the model presented in Figure 1. The objective of
Research Question 1: For any given stressor, can an individual simultaneously have both
positive and negative responses that are separate and distinct?
Theoretically, this dissertation will show that as some ~ave suggested (Edwards & Cooper,
1988; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), eustress and distress represent separate and distinct constructs,
not merely opposite ends of a single continuum. The study will propose multivariate indicators
of both the positive psychological response (eustress) and the negative psychological response
(distress) to any given stressor. The study will demonstrate both the discriminant validity among
the positive/negative indicators as well as the discriminant validity between the higher order
7
constructs, eustress and distress, that they represent. The study will also demonstrate that an
individual can experience eustress and distress simultaneously as the result of any given stressor
and that eustress and distress make separate and distinct contributions to an individual's
As a result of making the first step toward demonstrating this holistic model of stress,
researchers interested in understanding the full complexity of the stress response should be
encouraged to conceptualize, measure, and account for both eustress and distress in future stress
research. While this study focuses on psychological states as the sole indicators of eustress and
distress, future research. can explore multivariate behavioral and physiological indicators of each
construct as well. This model will also benefit from future research that identifies specific
stressors and modifying conditions that seem to enhance the eustress response.
This model of stress has implications for both individuals and organizations. For individuals,
it suggests a focus on generating eustress in our lives, not just preventing distress. In the context
of work, this may mean that in addition to established preventive distress management
techniques, exposure to work stressors that contribute more strongly to the experience of eustress
would be beneficial. While it is not presented in this research model, an individual's health may
have a reciprocal effect on their experience of both eustress and distress (Keesler, Magee, &
Nelson, 1996). This would suggest that individuals should pay increased attention to their health
as a way to regulate ( e.g., the bathtub metaphor) their experience of stress. Healthy individuals
may be more prone to appreciate the positive aspects of the full range of stressors to which they
The organizational implication of this holistic model of stress is the suggestion that as
managers interested in stress in our organizations, we should focus not just on the prevention of
distress, but also the generation of eustress among individuals in our organizations. As suggested
above, we could do this by focusing organizational interventions both on stressors and individual
8
responses. Returning to the bathtub metaphor, if the hot water faucet represents the flow of
positive experiences and eustressed individuals in organizations and the cold water faucet
represents the flow of negative experiences and distressed individuals, this is informative about
both the temperature and level of water (stress) in the bathtub (organization). To have a healthy
organization (a tub full of comfortably warm water), managers must attempt to regulate the
complex flow and level over time of both eustressed and distressed individuals in their
organizations.
9
CHAPTER2
LITERATURE REVIEW
The purpose of this chapter is to review the literature relevant to the research topic of
ignored the concept of eustress in favor of the concept of distress, a generalized review of the
field will not be presented. As such, this chapter traces the development of the concept of
eustress from its origins to its current state. At that point, the Holistic Stress Response model will
consequence of eustress and distress. Accordingly, the rational for the selection of the constructs
in the model will be presented and the relationships between trust, eustress, distress, and health
will be explored. This chapter concludes with a presentation of the research hypotheses.
The concept of eustress shares its origin with the concept of human stress. It is rare to find a
stress study that even considers the concept of eustress; but when eustress is considered, it is
almost always presented in its original conceptual form. Because this concept has received very
little attention, it is underdeveloped. The literature review will present evidence, however, that
represents advancement of the concept of eustress, even though the advancements were rarely
identified with eustress by the researchers. Based on this evidence, a new definition of the
constructs of eustress and distress will be suggested. These new definitions will facilitate a
strategy for the assessment of eustress that has been suggested in the literature but never pursued.
Hans Se lye ( 1976b), a medical doctor, incorporated the term eustress as an element of his
theory of human stress. While Se lye was the first to write extensively about stress in humans, the
concept of stress can be traced back much further. The term stress has its origins in Latin as a
10
verb meaning "to injure, molest, or constrain" and has been in the English language a long time
(Kahn & Byosiere, 1992). As long ago as the eighteenth century, the term stress was associated
with a force or pressure exerted upon a material object or person. In physics, the term stress is
used to describe an internal resisting force of a solid body in response to the application of an
external force.
A few individuals made noteworthy contributions to our understanding of human stress prior
to Hans Selye. Lovallo (1997) credits the physiologist Claude Bernard (1961/1865) with
establishing the foundation of the modern human stress concept. Bernard (1961/1865) asserted
that both the external environment and the internal environment determine the functions of
complex living organisms. In his view, the maintenance of life is critically dependent on keeping
the internal environment constant in the face of a changing external environment. Bernard was
the first to suggest that physical challenges to the integrity ofan organism provoke responses to
Quick et al. (1997) begin their historical view of the stress concept with the Yerkes-Dodson
Law (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908), which attempts to describe the relationship between stress and
performance. This law can be illustrated graphically by visualizing stress on a horizontal axis
and performance on a vertical axis. Stress in this sense is quantitative and represents a force or
demand with which an individual is confronted. The relationship between stress and perfor-
mance takes the shape of an inverted U. This law holds that we seem to do our best under some
pressure or when there is a challenge to be met. For each performance activity and each person,
a given amount of pressure will produce optimal performance. If individuals are pushed beyond
the optimal point, performance declines and they gradually shut down.
The next major figure in the development of modern conceptualizations of stress is Walter
Cannon. Cannon was concerned with the specific mechanisms of response to changes in an
individual's external environment that provided optimal bodily function (Cannon, 1929). His
11
research focused on the sympathetic-adrenal system of the body, which is responsible for the
production of the catecholamines adrenaline and noradrenaline. Cannon used the term homeo-
stasis to describe the body's process of attempting to maintain internal stability in the face of
environmental change. He posited that failure to meet challenges .to the homeostasis of the body
could result in tissue damage or death if not countered with adequate responses to return the
internal environment to normal. Cannon's work looked at the effect of physical challenges as
well as the effect on the person of psychologically meaningful stimuli (Cannon, 1935). He also
identified the fight or flight response as the initial phase of the response to an external threat.
This essentially states that when faced with a threat to survival, we must either fend off the threat
or flee the dangerous situation in order to survive. A summary of the physiological changes that
12
Hans Selye is widely credited as the first to note the existence of human stress, describe its
qualities, define the concept, and give the phenomenon a name. While Walter Cannon focused
his research on the sympathetic-adrenal system, Selye's research focused on the pituitary-adrenal
system and its production of cortisol. Selye clearly viewed stress as a physiological response
that is associated with the process of adaption. Selye's (1976) most widely accepted definition of
stress is: "Stress is the nonspecific response of the body to any demand, whether it is caused by,
or results in, pleasant or unpleasant conditions" (p. 76b). That stress can be associated with
pleasant and unpleasant experiences is illustrated in Figure 3. ''Note that the physiological stress
level is lowest during indifference but never goes down to zero (that would be death). Pleasant as
not necessarily distress) (Selye, 1974, p. 32). Selye's use of the term distress will be explained
shortly.
Stress
· Extremely Extremely
Unpleasant Pleasant
Experience Continuum
Figure 3: Relation Between Stress and Various Types of Life Experiences (Selye, 1974)
He clearly intended stress to refer to those altered physiological states, and he proposed
stressor as the term designating the variety of demands capable of evoking the stress response.
"All endogenous or exogenous·agents that make such demands are called stressors. Distinguish-
ing between their widely differing specific effects and the common biological response that they
elicit is the key to a proper understanding of biologic stress" (Selye, 1976a, p. 14).
13
The use of the term "nonspecific" in his definition is important. From this perspective, the
stress response is nonspecific both in cau_se and effect. Anything that changes conditions for the
body, to which it must then adapt, produces stress. Inherent in this concept is the notion that the
human body does not recognize the distinction between pleasant and unpleasant circumstances
(i.e., pleasure and pain). Both change the circumstances under which the body must operate;
therefore, both positive and negative stimuli produce an undifferentiated stress response in the
body. Table 2 presents the range of positive and negative situations that might constitute a
stressor. According to Selye's conceptualization of stress, all of these situations would produce
nonspecific response. This stress response, in tum, has the potential to effect nearly every system
Table 2: Situations That Might Constitute a Stressor (adapted from Toates, 1995)
While "stress" is the term Selye uses for the biologic response to a stressor; he uses the terms
distress and eustress to denote the effects of the nonspecific response. The relationship between
stressors, stress, eustress; and distress is shown in Figure 4. Selye (1976a) writes:
In everyday life we must distinguish two types ofstress effects, namely, eustress (from the
Greek eu or good - as in euphony, euphoria, eulogy) and distress (from the Lqtin dis or
bad - as in dissonance, disease, dissatisfaction). Depending upon conditions, stress is
associated with desirable or undesirable effects. In view of these conditions it is also
quite obvious that there cannot be different types of stress, although the effects of
stressors are almost invariably different. (p. 15)
14
<
Eustress
(positive effects)
Stressor Stress
(cause) ----•
> (nonspecific response)
Distress
(negative effects)
Yet Selye's own writings have been a source of confusion in the field. In the following
passage representing his earlier thoughts on the issue, he intends to separate the response from its
effects, but the terminology is much less clear. Selye ( 1976b) writes:
We must, however, differentiate within the general concept of stress between the unpleas-
ant or harmful variety, called distress ... and eustress. During both eustress and distress
the body undergoes virtually the same nonspecific responses to the various positive or
negative stimuli acting upon it. However, the fact that eustress causes much less damage
than distress graphically demonstrates that it is "how you take it" that determines,
ultimately, whether one can adapt successfully to change. (p. 74)
In the above passage, he does not explicitly identify distress and eustress as stress effects. In
fact, in one part of this passage he seems to use the terms eustress and distress to distinguish
different types of stress; in another they are identified as causal agents. Along this same line
Selye (1976c) has written: "When applied to everyday problems, this understanding should lead
to choices most likely to provide us the pleasant eustress ... involved in achieving fulfillment
and victory, thereby avoiding the self-destructive distress of frustration and failure" (p. 13).
In a latter writing, Selye (1983)seemsto add yet another twist to his concept by suggesting
that there are four different varieties of stress (see Figure 5). Selye (1983) explains:
The stress of life has four basic variations, although in their most characteristic non-
specific manifestations they all depend on the same central phenomenon. Our goal
should be to strike a balance between the equally destructive forces of hypo- and hyper-
s tress, to find as much eustress as possible, and to minimize distress. Clearly,. we cannot
run away timidly from every unpleasant experience; in order to achieve our purposes, we
must often put up with unhappiness, at least for a time. Here faintheartedness would in
the long run prove even more distressing by depriving us of the joy of ultimate success.
Unnecessary or too much distress - all distress, in general, that does not hold promise of
eustress - is what is to be avoided. (p. 18)
15
Overstress
(hyperstress)
Understress
(hypostress)
has been, Quick et al. (1997) offer the following definition of the stress response: "The stress
resources when confronted with a demand, or stressor" (p. 3). They define eustress as the
"healthy, positive, constructive outcome of stressful events and the stress response" (p. 4). When
The diverse organizational demands and stressors ... lead to one common result: the
stereotypical psychophysiological reaction known as the stress response. Each individual
exhibits the same basic response, although the immediate and long-term consequences of
the stress response vary greatly among individuals. The variance among individuals is
influenced by a number ofmodi.fiers of the stress response. These modifiers affect
whether the stress response is channeled into positive and constructive outcomes
(eustress) or negative and destructive outcome (distress). (p. 41)
Although Quick et al. (1997) identify eustress, their workrepresents little theoretical or
empirical advancement of the concept presented by Selye. For Quick et al. (1997) eustress is
essentially good health and high performance. In their model, the path to good health and high
performance is the application of preventive stress management principles and methods. When
they invoke the term "preventive stress," it is clear that they mean the prevention of distress.
They offer no recommendations for the promotion of eustress beyond the prevention of distress.
While their model incorporates eustress as a separate and distinct effect, eustress is essentially the
absence of distress. As distress is commonly associated with disease, this is consistent with their
16
conceptualization of health, which is "freedom from disease 11 (Quick et al., 1987, p. 20). In
contrast, others believe that 11 health is more than the absence of disease 11 (Sutherland and Cooper,
1990, p. 2).
While Quick et al. (1997) define eustress as the effect of the stress response being "channeled
into positive and constructive outcomes 11 (p. 41 ), they offer little specific guidance on how this is
supposed to occur. They seem to confuse the matter by suggesting that eustress results from the
regulation of the intensity and frequency of the stress response. Expanding on the Yerkes-
Dodson law presented earlier, they graphically represent eustress. as the optimum level of
exposure to a stressor (stimulation) for an individual (see Figure 6). According to this concept,
maximum performance results when individuals are stressed at this optimum level. While
eustress was defined as an effect, it is represented in this case as a quantity of stressor. This
relationship between degree of demand and degree of effective performance has been found in
professors (Wilke, Gmelch, &Lovrich, 1985) as well as little league baseball players (McGrath,
1976). Milsum (1985) proposes a very similar concept in his model of the eustress system for
health and illness. Milsum (1985) states that 11 eustress implies a correct, right, or optimal level,
and therefore implicitly recognizes that this is a regulatory system in the same way as all the
The quantitative approach described above is one way to distinguish eustress from distress.
Allen (1983) asserts that eustress and distress can also be differentiated by qualitative characteri-
sties. From this perspective, the same quantity of stress may serve as eustress or distress,
depending on how one interprets events triggering it. Interpreting Selye's work, he writes:
According to Se/ye, the critical difference is whether one interprets stressors as challenge
or threat. He believes that if we consider life 's stressors to be threats, then damaging
results will occur. If, however, we look at the same events as challenges, they foster
growth. Se/ye 's personal strategy for stress management does not involve a reduction of
stress. Rather, he views stressors as challenges - healthy, growth promoting
opportunities. (p. 14)
17
High
Health
and
Performance Optimum Overstimulation
.(Eustress) of the stress
response
Low
Low High
(Distress (Distress)
Stress Level
Figure 6: An Expanded Yerkes-Dodson Curve (adapted from Quick et al., 1997, p. 156)
This is indeed a compelling account, but it too is confusing. Recall that in Selye's concept of
stress, stressors are directly associated with the stress response, which is nonspecific with respect
to its cause and physiological manifestations. Selye seems to suggest that the mind differentiates
between pleasant and unpleasant stressors but the body does not. If the individual can differen-
· tiate stressors as either threatening (negative) or challenging (positive), and eustress/ distress is
determined by this evaluation of the stressor, then the relationship between stressors and eustress
and distress cannot be mediated by a nonspecific physiological response. The stress response
As evidenced by the work of Quick et al. ( 1997), little has been done to advance the concept
of eustress beyond how it was originally presented by Hans Selye. Although Selye described
eustress as the positive effect of the nonspecific physiological stress response, neither Se lye nor
anyone since who uses his framework has been able to adequately describe how this positive
effect of the stress response occurs. Few will argue that Selye accurately described an aspect of
howthe endocrine system responds when confronted with a demand. Based on his research of
18
. one biological subsystem, the definition of stress as a nonspecific response, with eustress and
distress as effects of this response, was born and remains influential still today. This is
problematic because it is difficult to explain how a nonspecific response fully mediates the
A few researchers have noticed this problem and taken issue with Selye's definition of stress,
suggesting that the stress response is more complex and more specific than Selye asserts. If this
is true, then the concepts of eustress and distress as Selye presents them are also subject to
reevaluation. A more holistic conceptualization of the stress response would expand to encom-
pass knowledge of the human system beyond the endocrine subsystem. With a holistic represen-
tation of the stress response, it is possible to make qualitative assessments {i.e., good/bad,
positive/negative, healthy/damaging) of the response that were previously reserved for the effects
of the response. This would suggest the possibility of conceptualizing eustress and distress as
positive and negative aspects of the stress response itself instead of positive and negative aspects
of the effects of the stress response; consequently, this could resolve the conflict presented by
Selye's definitions.
The literature provides both psychological and physiological support for a more holistic
model of the stress response and subsequent redefinition of the concepts of eustress and distress.
The psychological support will be presented first because it forms the basis for the research
model that will ultimately be presented in this chapter. The physiological support will then be
presented in considerable detail. While the level of detail presented for the physiological support
may be more than some readers may desire, it is critical because the study of stress originated
Psychological Support
Much of the early work that established the foundation for the medical perspective of stress
was done through experimentation with animals. From this perspective, the stress response is
19
fundamentally similar between as well as within species. Lazarus ( 1966) took issue with the
medical perspective when establishing the basis for his approach to psychological stress. He
noted that the psychological structure of an animal that behaves primarily in accordance with
instinctual mechanisms must be thought of differently than that of humans, whose behavior
depends more on learning or high-level cognitive processes. While there are areas of overlap in
the kinds of motives that are assumed to be shared by all animals, some categories of motivation
and cognitive processes are unique to humans. Humans can distinguish among experiences that
harm, threaten, challenge, or nurture; and our sense of well-being is based on our ability to make
such evaluative perceptions. Humans reactions are produced by more varied stimuli than in
lower animals, and the reactions themselves are more variable than in lower animals. Because
humans are more cognitively complex than most animals, it is reasonable to expect the stress
Lazarus (1966) posited thatin humans, psychological processes intervene between the stress
stimuli and stress response. He utilized the term threat to express the condition of the person
when confronted with a stimulus that the person appraises as endangering important values and
goals. Lazarus (1966) posited the response to appraised threat can be psychological (e.g.,
For threat to occur, an evaluation must be made of the situation, to the effect that a harm
is signified The individual's knowledge and beliefs contribute to this. The appraisal of
threat is not a simple perception of the elements of the situation, but a judgment, an
inference in which the data are assimilated to a constellation of ideas and expectations.
. . , The mechanism by which the interplay between the properties of the individual and
those of the situation can be understood is the cognitive process of appraisal, a judgment
about the meaning offuture significance of a situation based not merely on the stimulus,
but on the psychological makeup. (p. 44)
In contrast to the medical perspective, Lazarus ( 1966) suggested that whether and how an
individual responds to the presence of a stimulus depends upon what the individual thinks and
believes about the stimulus and the situation in which it is presented. While Lazarus's ( 1966)
20
initial formulation ofthe concept of appraisal explicitly concerned threat and the resulting
Although the concept of appraisal was developed to explain the negative response of
psychological distress, the above passage suggests that individuals may also have positive
responses to situations or events. Lazarus (1966) did not elaborate on the positive response, and
he did not use the term "eustress" to identify this response; however, this positive psychological
response is consistent with the notion that eustress is "good stress" (Quick et al., 1997; Selye,
1976a). The fundamental difference is that the stress response is presented as multidimensional,
positive and negative responses, as opposed to positive and negative effects of a stereotypical
physiological response.
Lazarus and Folkman (1984) extended the concept of appraisal and observe that appraisals
and their resulting responses can be complex and mixed. They presented what can be interpreted
as two basic categories of appraisal: positive and negative. Positive appraisals can occur if the
positive appraisal. They noted, however, that totally positive appraisals should be rare. For most
situations and events, most people can always conceive that things are not totally perfect or that
something could change and the desirable state could be adversely affected.
The other category of appraisals is labeled stress appraisals, which can be interpreted as
negative appraisals (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984). They subcategorized negative appraisals as
21
those involving harm/loss, threat, and challenge. In harm/loss, some damage to the person has
already occurred (e.g., injury, illness, loss of a loved one, damage to self-esteem). Threat
involves harms or losses that have not yet occurred but are anticipated. Challenge appraisals
occur if the outcome of an encounter holds the potential for gain or growth. As indicators of
challenge appraisals, they suggested looking for some of the same positive or pleasurable
psychological states they identify as indicators of the positive response. They suggested that "to
be challenged means feeling positive about demanding encounters" (p. 34). Discussing the
difference between challenge and threat appraisals, Lazarus and Folkman (1984) stated: "The
main difference is that challenge appraisals focus on the potential for gain or growth inherent in
an encounter and they are characterized by pleasurable emotions such as eagerness, excitement,
and exhilaration, whereas threat centers on the potential harms and is characterized by negative
emotions such as fear, anxiety, and anger" (p. 33, emphasis added).
Lazarus and Folkman (1984) did not view challenge and threat as poles of a single contin-
uum. They believed that challenge and threat responses are not mutually exclusive and can occur
simultaneously, as the result of the same stressor, and should be considered as separate but
related constructs. Importantly, they stated that "it is possible that the psychological stress
response to challenge is different from that in threat" (p. 34). While threat is clearly a negative
appraisal, I think challenge is better thought of as a positive appraisal (they share the same
indicators). Lazarus and Folkman (1984) as much as said the same thing by showing that the
immediate psychological effects of appraisals are positive or negative feelings (see figure 10.4, p.
308).
When discussing separate components of appraisal, Lazarus ( 1993) again noted that the
feelings or emotions that result from appraisal are essentially positive and negative. Lazarus
(1993) contended that "stress cannot be considered in terms of a single dimension such as
activation ... such a recognition involves considering diverse emotional states, some negative,
22
some positive" (p. 5). He reminded readers that eustress is the good kind of stress, associated
with positive feelings and healthy bodily states, while distress is bad stress, associated with
As such, the reasoning Lazarus and Folkman (1984) apply to the distinction between
challenge and threat can be extended to the higher levels of positive and negative appraisal.
Following the reasoning on threat and challenge, positive and negative appraisals result in
separate but related stress responses. Accordingly, positive and negative responses can occur
simultaneously, as a result of the same stressor, and should be considered separate but related
constructs. Thus, for any given stressor, an individual can have both a degree of positive and a
supports the conclusion stated above. They examined the students' emotions at three stages of a
midterm exam: before the exam, before the grades were announced, and after the grades were
announced,, They inferred appraisals of threat or challenge and outcomes of benefit and harm by
assessing emotions. They found clear support for the existence of both positive and negative
emotions as a result of the encounter. They also found that the greater the ambiguity of the
situation, the greater the probability that people would experience both positive and negative
emotions became increasingly negatively correlated as the encounter unfolded to its conclusion.
Folkman' s ( 1997) longitudinal study of caregivers of men with AIDS provides strong
additional support for the contention that positive and negative responses can occur siinulta-
neously and as a result of the same stressor. She found that in addition to negative psychological
23
states, the caregivers also experienced positive psychological states in the midst of caregiving and
bereavement. Her study found covariation around high mean levels of both negative and positive
states; and with the exception of the weeks surrounding the death of the patient, the frequency of
the caregivers' positive affect was never significantly lower than the frequency of their negative
affect. If positive and negative psychological states can co-occur in the midst of enduring and
profoundly demanding circumstances, then it is reasonable to expect them exhibit a degree of co-
Please recall that the focus of this research is eustress, which was defined in Chapter 1 as a
positive stress response. The concept of cognitive appraisal provides theoretical support for this
definition, but cognitive appraisal will be neither manipulated nor directly assessed within the
scope of this research. Appraisal has been operationalized as an evaluation of what was at stake
in a stressful encounter (Folkman, Lazarus, Dunkel-Schetter, Delongis, & Gruen, 1986). Their
scale revealed two factors that they labeled threats to self-esteem (e.g., losing the affection of
someone important to you) and threats to a loved one's well-being (e.g., harm to a loved one's
health or safety). This approach to appraisal was deemed inappropriate for the occupational
psychology focus of my study, and development of a new appraisal scale was considered beyond
the scope of this study. However, the independent variable selected for this research subsumes
appraisal. In the next chapter, it will be shown that trust in the supervisor forms as the result of
Additional research on motivation, emotion, and attitudes provides support for representing
appraisal and response as characteristically positive or negative. Roseman, Spindel, and Jose
(1990) categorized 16 different emotions as positive or negative and showed that subjects'
appraisals of situational states differentiated among the emotions. Weiner ( 1986) employed the
concept of cognitive appraisal in his attribution theory of motivation and emotion. He embraced
the presumption that how we think (i.e., appraise a situation) influences how we feel. He defined
24
an emotion simply as a "complex syndrome or composite of many interacting factors. Emotions
are presumed "to 1) have positive or negative qualities of, 2) have a certain intensity that,
3) frequently are preceded by an appraisal of a situation, and 4) give rise to a variety of actions"
(p. 119). Weiner's (1986) attributional model of motivation and emotion (p. 162) indicates that a
motivational sequence is initiated by an outcome that the person interprets as positive or negative.
This is consistent with the finding that appraisals are more strongly related to emotional expe-
rience than are causal attributions (Roseman et al., 1990; Smith, Haynes, Lazarus, & Pope, 1993).
The psychology of att.itudes also views the appraisal process in positive and negative terms.
The important point to take away from this definition is that an evaluative process produces
results that are characterized as positive and negative. It can also be observed that emotions from
this perspective are a type of affective evaluative response. Whether it is labeled as an emotion
or an attitude, researchers from several different perspectives seem to agree that a response
Lazarus and Folkman (1984) supported their view that appraisals and their resulting stress
responses are complex and mixed with evidence from research that challenged Selye's concept of
stress as nonspecific physiological response. They concluded that any psychophysical theory of
stress that views the response as unidimensional disequilibrium or arousal is untenable or at least
grossly incomplete. Aspects of the research they examined as well as additional research along
25
appraisal is established and accepted as a valid explanation of degrees of negative responses,
which have commonly been labeled distress. The Holistic Stress Response model in this study
extends Lazarus and Folkman's (1984) thinking on the positive response to appraisal and
provides it the label "eustress." This extension reflects the view that the stress response itself is
multidimensional and can be categorized along the higher order dimensions of positive and
negative. Cognitive appraisal explains why the response itself is not unidimensional and how the
qualitative characteristics of the separate and specific responses arise. Selye's original labels
"eustress" and "distress" are retained and applied as descriptors of the higher order dimensions of
the stress response. In other words, eustress and distress are recast as descriptors of the stress
response instead of the effect to a unidimensional response .. The next section will provide
Physiological Support
Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), it is necessary to examine how researchers from the medical perspec-
tive of stress challenged the assumptions of its founder, Hans Selye (1976a). This section will
present clear evidence that the physiological response to stress is more complex and specific than
it has been presented by some researchers. All of the research presented in this section attributes
the complexity of the stress response to cognitive appraisal of the stressor. Recall that cognitive
appraisal was shown in the previous section to be primarily either positive or negative. Accord-
ingly, the physiological research suggests that a more complex stress response can be qualita-
tively characterized along the primary dimensions of positive and negative. This sets the stage
for identifying eustress and distress with the positive and negative affective, behavioral, and
physiological manifestations of the stress response, not as effects of the stress response. This
section is complex due to the terminology involved and is presented in detail in order to attempt
26
It will be useful at this point to review some common terms and concepts used in neuroendo-
crine (i.e., medical) stress research. Most research on the endocrine (hormonal) effects of work
stress has focused primarily on two major systems. Figure 7 provides a schematic representation
of pathways from the brain to the adrenal systems involved.in stress (Frankenhauser, 1986).
The first system is the sympathetic-adrenal system, studied by Walter Cannon, and relates to
changes in the catecholamines or epinephrine and norepinephrine, also known as adrenaline and
heart rate and blood pressure. A large number ofenvironmental factors seem to be capable of
provoking changes in catecholamines. They are easily perturbed and can change rapidly even
involvement. When individuals are interacting more intensely with their environment, either
Environmental Demands
t
Higher Brain Centers
!
q
. . . - - - Hypothalamus
Other Body
Organs
Sympathetic
Nervous
System Pituitary Gland
1! Cortisol
Adrenal Cortex _ _ _...,
Adrenaline
.___. Adrenal Medulla -N,-o-ra_dr_e_n_a-lin_e_ _.
27
The second system involved in stress is the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal system studied by
Hans Selye, which produces cortisol. While the release of cortisol is not as easily stimulated as
challenges (Rose, 1987). While cortisol acts on a variety of the body's organs, its primary effect
is to increase the supply of glucose and fatty acids in the bloodstream. Cortisol can also have
harmful effects on the body's digestion, immune response, and muscular-skeletal system (Quick
et al., 1997).
Mason ( 1971) was among the first to take issue with Selye's concept of stress with his
that the pituitary-adrenal cortical system was actually very sensitive to psychological influences.
He asserted that there are extensive anatomical linkages that provide points of contact for neural
and psychological influences upon a variety of endocrine systems (Mason, 1968a). He proposed
that higher level psychological integrative mechanisms rather than lower level physiological or
biochemical mechanisms, as proposed by Se lye, acted as the first mediator of the relationship
between environmental stimuli and the individual's response to stress (Mason, 1975).
Mason (1975) stated, "Of all the known responses of higher organisms, emotional arousal is
certainly one of the most ubiquitous or relatively "nonspecific" reactions common to a great
diversity ofsituations" (p. 25). Selye'(l 976a, p. 13) seemed to interpret this statement by Mason
Mason also believed that the stress response was complex and could not be adequately
represented by lines of research that focused exclusively on one physiological system as Se lye
... different hormones are closely interdependent, often bearing antagonistic, synergistic,
additive, or permissive relationships to each other, which suggests that the activity of any
given metabolic process at any given moment must be a resultant of the over-all balance
between cooperating and opposing hormonal influences playing upon it." (p; 571)
28
Mason (1975) states:
We have not found evidence that any single hormone responds to all stimuli- in absolutely
non-specific fashion, as implied by Se/ye 's present definition of "stress" as occurring in
response "to any demand " The picture emerging so far from our study of multihormonal
patterns, in fact, is one suggesting that such patterns are organized in a rather specific or
selective manner, depending upon the particular stimulus under study, and probably in
relation to the complex interdependencies in hormonal actions at the metabolic level. (p.
27)
In his review of Mason's (1975) work, Selye (1975) stood firm on his original conceptualiza-
tion of stress. He reasserted that he believes the stress syndrome is nonspecific in its causation,
but concedes that the same stressor can elicit different manifestations in different individuals.
Whereas Mason (1975) explained these differences as being due to psychological mechanisms,
Selye (1975) did not concur. For Selye (1975) these "conditioning factors" that can selectively
enhance or inhibit the stress effect (not the response) are either biological (e.g., age, sex, genetic
predisposition) or external (e.g., drugs, diet, treatment with certain hormones). According to
Selye, these conditioning factors selectively influence the reactivity of certain organs, resulting in
the possibility that the same stressor can elicit different manifestations in different individuals.
typed response as a result of these selective conditioning factors. Nevertheless, "it seems
increasingly likely that the response is not so universal as concluded by Selye" (Mason, 1968b, p.
800).
Frankenhauser (1979, 1983, 1986) provided additional .support for the concept that different
psychological processes affect the physiologica,l response pattern in different ways. Through a
series of experiments, she an~ her colleagues consistently found that two components of
psychological arousal determined cortisol and catecholamine responses. The psychological state
characterized by positive emotions was labeled "effort," and the psychological state characterized
by negative emotions was labeled "distress." Indicators of effort were variables labeled effort,
tenseness, and concentration, while indicators of distress were variables labeled boredom,
29
impatience, tiredness, and lack of interest (Lundberg & Frankenhauser, 1980). Frankenhauser
(1979) notes the general resemblance of these two factors and the "cortisol factor" and "catechol-
amine factor" reported by Ursin, Baade, and Levine ( 1978) in their study of parachute trainees.
Effort (positive emotions) and distress (negative emotions) may be experienced either singularly
or in combination, and they seem to be differentially associated with catecholamine and cortisol
secretion. Effort without distress is a positive, joyous state that results froin high job involvement
and the suppression of cortisol. Effort with distress is the state typical of the hassles of daily
work life and is accompanied by an increase in both catecholamine and cortisol secretion.
Distress without effort is a very negative psychological state that implies feeling helpless or
may be e,evated also. Elsewhere, Frankenhauser (1991) described these three states as activity
paired with positive affect, activity paired with negative affect, and passivity paired with negative
affect, respectively.
30
Frankenhauser ( 1983) asserted that these findings support the notion that psychologically
different conditions produce a selective response. "In short, pituitary-adrenal activation was
associated with the negative feelings of distress in the low control situation, and sympathetic-
adrenal activation was associated with the positive feeling of effort in the high-control situation"
(p. 94, emphasis added). Frankenhauser (1981) emphasized that "neurendocrine responses to the
psychosocial environment are determined by the individual's cognitive appraisal of the situation
and the emotional impact of the stimuli rather than by their objective characteristics" (p. 493).
Thus, cognitive appraisal of the situation produces positive and negative emotions, as well as a
Rose's ( 1987) longitudinal study of air traffic controllers (ATCs) provides a slightly different
picture of how cognitive appraisals affect the stress response. Over a three-year period, the
cortisol values of201 men were measured every 20 minutes for five hours on three or more days
and compared to both objective and subjective assessments of workload. While the increases in
cortisol for all levels of workload were slight, the men who showed the highest increase in
cortisol to increased work reported themselves as more satisfied and were regarded by peers as
more competent. These high cortisol responders also showed less frequent illness than those with
lower cortisol levels, who for any given level of work tended to have more minor health
problems. Rose described the men whose cortisol increased in response to challenging work as
engaged rather than stressed. Elsewhere, the happiness derived from engagement in mindful
challenge has been termed "flow" (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). In their review of Rose's study,
Ganster and Schaubroek (1991) described the healthy state of physiological arousal experienced
Lovallo ( 1997) concurred that the effect of a stressor depends heavily on the way the person
interprets the situation at hand. He contended that two situations that are equally physically
demanding can have different consequences for the individual based on the interpretation of the
31
situation and the accompanying emotions. To illustrate this point, two studies were conducted
that called on human volunteers to perform tasks with nearly identical physical requirements but
having substantial differences in their psychological components (Lovallo, Pincomb, & Wilson,
In one study, young adult men were asked to perform a simple psychomotor task to avoid two
adverse stimuli, noise and electric shock (Lovallo et al., 1985). The task was a simple, variable-
interval reaction-time task in which subjects pressed a response key whenever a red light in front
of them came on. The shocks were brief and mild (harmless), yet enough to be unpleasant. The
noise was brief and safe, but intense enough to produce a strong degree of startle. From the
subject's point of view, the task involved anxiety, fear, and uncertainty about the timing and
source of an aversive event. The subject also had to maintain continuous attention for the
The subjects reported the task as being activating (increased concentration, effort, interest,
and tenseness) as well as unpleasant (less control and more impatience and irritation). The
cardiovascular patterns of the subjects showed increased systolic and diastolic blood pressure.
The subjective and cardiovascular changes were also accompanied by alterations in the
Because the subjects were.seated in a relaxed position for the entire study, their findings suggest
that the rises in norepinephrine and cortisol resulted from responses related to negative emotions
In the second study, subject were presented with a rewarding, nonaversive version of the
same reaction time task (Lovallo et al., 1986). The major difference was that instead of noise or
shock, subjects received a $.50 bonus for each rapid response they could perform. Subjects self-
reported that the task was primarily activating and nonaversive. The subjects experienced the
same pattern of cardiovascular and norepinephrine increases as with the aversive task; however,
32
the cortisol patterns were different. In this situation, there was no change in cortisol secretion
from baseline. Figure 9 presents the differences in the hormonal responses between the two
experiments. The lack of increase in cortisol was attributed to the nonaversive nature of the task.
They interpreted these results as showing that activation of attentional and motor response centers
in the brain can lead to substantial activation of cardiovascular function, even in the absence of
Both tasks prompted self-reports suggesting that the subjects were actively engaged in the
effort of performing as well as possible regardless of the nature of the incentives. The cardiovas-
cular changes, therefore, seem most closely related to the effortful aspect of the task. In contrast,
the pattern of endocrine changes, especially the cortisol response, appears to be determined by
Q,I 18
:§
Q,I
w.i
16
="e 14
.:::= 12
i 10
",,.,., :,,.,
-="
u
8 1
.;.'•i/{c
;';:"' J')
Q,I 6
1:11)
...."
1
ti:]iH
5 4
~
I::Q,I 2 l{}h:,[1
:·.
···51
0 Norepinephrine Cortisol
I D Shock D Reward I
appraisal of events (see Figure 10). The purpose of presenting this somewhat complex explana-
tion is to provide further support for the notion that the stress response is complex as opposed to
nonspecific. Although these mechanisms will not be explained with the same detail presented by
33
Lovallo (1997), the details that illustrate the body's potential to respond differentially based upon
Information gathered by our senses is relayed through the thalamus, which acts as the initial
gathering point for most incoming information. Raw sensory information is elaborated with
stored information related to that sensory modality. In the prefrontal cortex, meaning or
significance is attached to the information we receive. At this point, information from the various
senses is integrated into a unified whole to provide an accurate picture of the external environ-
ment. Events receive meaning and significance based upon how they are appraised. The frontal
cortex is at the beginning of the chain of events resulting in normal emotional responses and
• Prefrontal Cortex
• Hippocampus
t
Generation of Emotions Based on Appraisals
• Prefrontal Amygdala Activity ~
• Insula-Hiooocampal Connection
I
Initiation of Autonomic
~
Feedback to Cortex & Limbic System
& Endocrine Responses (Brainstem)
• RACER (Hypothalamic Area
Controlling Emotional Responses)
-
~
• Locus Ceruleus
• Raphe Nuclei
• Paraventricular Nucleus • Ventral Tegmental
~
• Nucleus
34
The next step involves the physiological formulation of emotions. Emotions formed in
response to environmental events arise from a transaction between the prefrontal areas and the
hippocampus and amygdala located in the temporal lobe of the brain. The hippocampus is
involved in memory storage and in associating new information with prior experience. The
amygdala enables us to have emotions in connection with present knowledge and to modify our
actions based on those emotions and knowledge of the past. The amygdala is important for
matching environmental appraisals with negative emotions such as disgust, fear, or anger.
The amygdala then sends signals to both the hypothalamus and the brainstem. The amygdala,
therefore, is the focal point of transition between sensory input and appraisals of our environment
on the one hand and our formulation of autonomic and endocrine responses on the other.
reflexes and skeletal motor functions. Additionally, the brainstem feedback system serves to
coordinate the level of arousal and behavioral state of the entire central nervous system in
response to the commands of the amygdala or other lymbic systems. These subsystems coordi-
nate the entire system whether the situation appraised calls for fight-flight, approach, avoidance,
or sleep.
The hypothalamic area controlling emotional responses (HACER) coordinates the autonomic
nervous system and endocrine aspect of the stress response. The HACER causes the pituitary to
secrete corticotropin releasing factor (CRF). CRF then causes secretion of Pendorphin and
adrenocorticotropin (ACTH): ACTH in tum causes the adrenal cortex to secrete cortisol.
Cortisol is one of the most commonly studied physiological indicators of distress. p endorphins
may serve as transmitters or modulators in neuronal systems for the mediation of satisfaction or
reward (Stein and Belluzzi, 1978). They have been associated with pleasurable or euphoric
states, such as the "runner's high." As such, p endorphins may be a chemical indicator of
eustress.
35
Lovallo (1987) presented two explanations for how appraisals and their associated emotions
come to differ among persons. The first explanation arises from the increasing awareness that
positive and negative emotions are served by different subsytems in the frontal and temporal
lobes of the brain. Gray (1987, 1991) proposed two distinct frontal lobe systems responsible for
the interpretation of incoming events and formulation of responses. One is associated with the
septa! area of the limbic system, often referred to as the pleasure center of the brain, which has
connections to and from the amygdala and hippocampus. He posited that this pleasure center
may assist in associating material in working memory with a positive or negative emotional
valence. The second he termed the behavioral inhibition (withdrawal) system, which he believes
plays a central role in anxiety. This system seems to organize autonomic and behavioral
responses to stimuli appraised as aversive. Lane et al. (1997) provided support for this view in a
study that confirmed that there are both common and unique components of the neural networks
mediating pleasant and unpleasant emotion in healthy women. This study demonstrated
physiologically that positive and negative emotions are separate and not merely opposite ends of
The second explanation for individual differences in appraisal is the notion that the frontal
lobes may be asymmetric for the generation of emotions and that persons may differ in the degree
to which they characteristically activate structures on one side or the other. Gray ( 1987, 1991)
also suggested a behavioral approach system that responds to appetitive stimulation and is linked
to positive emotions and motivates exploration and desirous behavior. Cacioppo and Berntson
(1994) suggested that Gray's approach system originates primarily in the left hemisphere of the
brain and that the withdrawal system originates primarily in the right hemisphere. As such, the
right hemisphere of the brain differentially activates negative emotions associated with adverse
events. The behavioral approach and inhibition systems can motivate behaviors and
36
physiological responses in conjunction with appraisal of events and their associated positive and
negative emotions.
The research presented in this section provided evidence that the stress response is more
complex and less specific than Selye (1976a) originally hypothesized. In general, researchers
consistently found at least two differential physiological response patterns, which they explained
resulted from a cognitive appraisal of the positive and negative aspects of a situation. The end of
this section provided an explanation of the physiological mechanisms underlying the psychologi-
Although the concept of eustress was rarely dealt with explicitly in either of the preceding
sections, the research made it clear that a definition of eustress as a good or positive stress effect
is subject to revision. The research evidence also supports the possibility of describing the
complex stress response as qualitatively in terms of eustress (positive) and distress (negative), as
well as providing a solid explanation of how the eustress response and distress response result
from cognitive appraisal of the stressor. The following section presents the current state of the
concept of eustress.
The concept of eustress remains underdeveloped. Although the opportunity to improve the
current notion of eustress is evident, few efforts have been made to do so. There are a few
empirical studies of bungee jumpers, rock climbers, and outdoor adventure participants that
explicitly attempt to incorporate the concept of eustress (Bunting, Little, Tolson, & Jessup, 1986;
Henning, Laschefski, & Opper, 1994; Priest, 1992). While it is encouraging to see empirical
studies incorporate the term eustress, the conceptual development of eustress is limited.
Although eustress has not been studied empirically in occupational health psychology, one
important paper provided a theoretical review of the eustress that represents an advancement of
the concept (Edwards and Cooper, 1988). This section will briefly review empirical findings
37
from three studies outside of the field of occupational psychology, as well as the theoretical
advancement within the field. This section will conclude with the presentation of the research
Experience (DAE) survey on the responses of first- and second-year students engaged in a ropes
course program. The DAE is a semantic differential of twenty-four bipolar adjectives concerned
with perceptual changes in risk and competence that may occur from participating in adventure
experiences. Adventures are defined as having uncertain outcomes, being a state of mind,
involving intrinsic motivation, and a perception of free choice. Results showed that the DAE
scale measuring risk formed three factors, which were labeled eustress, distress, and fear. The
adjectives from the DAE that formed the eustress factor were excitement, challenge, stimulation,
tension, and positivity; for distress, uncertainty, threat, and difficulty; and for fear, potential
harm, exposure, danger, and hazard. The incremental value added by this study is that eustress
was associated with positive states of mind and distress was associated with negative states of
mind.
Another study of 12 novice bungee jumpers investigated both emotional and physiological
changes induced by this acute psychological stressor (Hennig et al., 1994). They found that
subjective ratings of anxiety increased prior to the jump and dropped significantly after the jump.
Cortisol also increased after the jump and declined to baseline within the next hour. In contrast,
significantly after performing the jump and remained highly elevated for the next thirty minutes,
while ratings of anxiety decreased. An increase of more than 200 percent in P-endorphin was
observed after the jump. After the jump, positive feelings were significantly correlated with P-
endorphin levels but not with cortisol levels. The researchers termed the positive feelings and
38
Bunting et al. (1986) used the term eustress liberally, sometimes associating it with the
stressor and other times associating it with the stress response. They studied twelve male
students engaged in a series of three-hour rock climbing/rappelling session, which they defined as
the eustress treatment. This is problematic because the stressor is physical as well as psychologi-
cal, and the psychological perception of the stressor was not assessed. They administered the
Spielberger Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Luskene, 1970) and also
measured epinephrine, norepinephrine, and heart rate responses before and after two adventure
sessions. Results showed that STAI did not correlate with changes in any of the physiological
measures. They did find that perceived anxiety as measured by the STAI dropped during the
adventure sessions, which they attributed "to the eustress experienced during these activities,
rather than the subjects being aware of the unpleasant feelings that are normally associated with
anxiety" (p. 19). They seem to suggest that eustress is a positive response, operationalized as
These three studies highlight the paucity of empirical research that incorporates the concept
of eustress. None of the studies attempted to assess eustress directly, yet all of the studies labeled
as eustress positive aspects of the psychological response in subjects. Also, none of the studies
were grounded in occupational psychology. As such, the opportunity exists to develop the theory
Edwards and Cooper (1988) correctly pointed out that while much research has focused on
the impacts of negative psychological states, relatively little research has examined the effects of
positive psychological states. They attributed this to the inadequate theoretical and methodologi-
cal development of positive psychological states and their impact on important outcomes such as
health. They associated eustress with positive psychological states and distress with negative
psychological states. They concurred with the evidence that positive assessment of stressors
produces a differential physiological response. They also suggested that positive and negative
39
psychological states may represent two distinct constructs, which would require separate indices
for measurement. The basis for this assumption is the research into self-reported mood, which
suggests that measures of positive and negative affect are often uncorrelated (Diener & Emmons,
1984; Watson & Tellegen, 1985). Watson, Pennebaker, and Folger (1987) suggested a two-
factor model of stress (negative feelings) and satisfaction (positive feelings) based upon similar
Accordingly, Edwards and Cooper (1988) suggested that the measurement of eustress could
involve the assessment of positive psychological states. Positive psychological states would
include but not be limited to satisfaction, happiness, and positive affect. They also suggested that
it is imperative to establish the presence of the positive psychological states, rather than merely
states are more numerous and include but are not limited to anxiety, hostility, and negative affect.
Folkman (1997) operationalized positive psychological states of mind with the six-item
Positive States of Mind (PSOM) scale developed by Horowitz, Adler, and Kegeles (1988). The
questions on this scale represent positive emotions by asking respondents how much trouble they
'
have, if any, in having several states of mind (e.g., "Feeling able to attend to a task you want or
need to do, without many distractions from within yourself," "Feeling of being able to stay at
work until a task is finished," "do something new to solve a problem or express yourself
assessed with the 20-item Centers for Epidemiological Studies - Depression measure (CES-D)
(Radloff, 1977). She also assessed positive and negative affect in her study. She found that both
positive and negative psychological states occurred as a result of caregiving and bereavement of
terminally ill patients. Folkman (1997) theoretically framed her study as one of distress and did
not associate positive psychological states with eustress; consequently, the concept of eustress
was not advanced theoretically in her study. Her study is important but could be extended by
40
expanding the operationalization of psychological states to include a multivariate assessment of
work attitudes and not just emotions, as well as applying a theoretic framework that associates
positive psychological states with eustress and negative states with distress.
While Edwards and Cooper (1988) suggested that eustress cannot be operationalized as
simply the absence of distress, this remains a question that requires empirical justification.
Accordingly, this review of the literature has reached a stage where the formal definitions of
eustress and distress can be reintroduced and refined, the research questions can be reiterated,
This review of the literature has provided support for the proposed definitions for eustress
and distress presented in Chapter 1. Recall that eustress was defined as a positive response and
distress as a negative response, both resulting from the individual's evaluation of a stressor. The
first objective of this research, as previously stated in the research questions, is to show that for
any given stressor, an individual can have both positive and negative responses that are separate
and distinct. The second objective, after establishi~g the presence of separate responses, is to
examine the differential effects of the responses on an outcome variable, the individual's
perception of health.
behavioral. A full model of eustress and distress should incorporate indicators of all of these
health (e.g., performance), and moderators of the response and the effects of the response. Such
a model is beyond the scope of this research. Consistent with Edwards and Cooper (1988), this
research will be limited to psychological states as manifestations of eustress and distress and for
logistical reasons will examine a single stressor and a single outcome variable, Accordingly, the
operational definitions of eustress and distress proposed by this study are as follows.
41
Eustress: A positive psychological response to a stressor, as indicated by the presence of
positive psychological states~
Figure 2 presented the basic research model without identifying constructs specifically. The
Holistic Stress Response model proposed by this study is now presented in Figure 11. The
aspects of the model that require discussion in this chapter are those related to the hypothesized
relationships .between eustress, distress, the stressor (interpersonal trust), and the outcome
perception of health. Discussion of the specific indicators of eustress and distress as well as
demographic and instrumental variables will be presented in the next chapter. Eustress and
distress are modeled as second-order latent variables, indicated by established positive and
Control Variables:
• Role Ambiguity
• Work Overload
• Death/Dying
Demographics:
• Age
• Gender
• Experience
• Education
• Hours worked
• Type of nurse
• Area of practice
42
The first hypothesis concerns the relationship between eustress and distress. Extending
Lazarus and Folkman (1984), this study proposes that eustress and distress are separate
constructs that can occur simultaneously in response to the same stressor. They also suggested
that the response was complex and mixed, and this point was supported empirically with data
from studies of the physiology of stress. Recall that the bathtub metaphor introduced in the first
chapter of this study suggested that the simultaneous flow of both hot and cold water determine
the level and temperature of the water in the bathtub. This study has presented a model in which
the stress response (water in the bathtub) is determined by the simultaneous presence of both
eustress and distress (hot and cold flows of water). The relationship between eustress and
distress can be expected to be non-recursive, meaning that the separate responses reciprocally
inform each other. Although this study will test a competing model in which the relationship
between eustress and distress is recursive (not reciprocal), the literature presented in this chapter
Interpersonal Trust
Although interpersonal trust has not been studied within the context of stress, it is an ideal
stressor for this study for several reasons. First, because trust has not previously been identified
simply a stressor, with no preconceived qualitative attributions. Trnst enters this study neutral,
with no previous empirical association with either eustress or distress. Second, trust results from
appraisal, and there are aspects of this appraisal that individuals can perceive both with a degree
of favor and a degree of disfavor or apprehension. As the aim of this study is to examine whether
degrees of eustress and distress can results from the same stressor, trust is again a unique yet
ideal stressor.
43
Quick et al. (1997) identified four major categories of stressors: physical stressors, tasl<.
demands, role demands, and interpersonal demands. An interpersonal demand was chosen for
this study, interpersonal trust, which has not previously been identified and studied as an
to the actions of another party (Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995, p. 712.) This is consistent
with the description of interpersonal stressors at work as "concerned with the demands of the
normal course of social, personal, and working relationships in the organization" (Quick et al.,
1997, p. 34). Interpersonal stressors come from the demand of relationships at work, and
Figure 12 graphically presents the Mayer et al. (1995) conceptualization of trust. The
psychological state of trust forms in the individual as a result of the individual's appraisal of the
the trustee, while trust is a characteristic of the trustor. Although others have suggested as many
as 10 characteristics of trustworthiness (e.g., Butler, 1991), Mayer et al. (1995) contend that the
three mostcommon characteristics of trustworthiness are the trustee's ability, benevolence, and
integrity. Ability is the domain-specific skills; competencies, and characteristics that enable the
trustee to have influence with the trustor. Benevolence is the extent to which the trustee is
believed to want to do good to the trustor, asiqe from an egocentric profit motive. Integrity is the
extent to which the trustee adheres to a,set of principles that the trustor finds acceptable.
Propensity
to Trust
Figure 12: A Model oflnterpersonal Trust (adapted from Mayer et al., 1995)
44
I
The psychological state of trust as a willingness to be vulnerable forms in the trustor as a
result of the appraisal of trustworthiness in the trustee. This study focuses on the individual's
trust in his/her supervisor. Trust in this relationship is salient because supervisors are in a unique
position to be a source of stress for their employees (Quick et al., 1997). While it was noted
previously that this study does not directly assess cognitive appraisal, the psychological state trust
represents a subjective stressor that was formed through appraisal. In appraisal, "the person
evaluates whether her or she has anything at stake in this encounter" (Folkman et al., 1986).
Using this definition of trust as a stressor is entirely consistent with cognitive appraisal's
relational approach to stress, where stress "cannot be defined as an environmental agent but
represents a particular kind ofrelationship between a stimulus and a vulnerable person" (Lazarus
& Folkman, 1986, p. 70). In fact, interpersonal trust is listed as one potential causal antecedent in
illustrations of the system variables for the stress rubric (Lazarus, Delongis, Folkman, & Gruen,
The Mayer et al. (1995) model limits its focus of the effect of trust to behavior. Within the
stress theoretic framework presented in this chapter, behavior is a legitimate indicator of the
degree of positive and negative response to a stressor. The stress theoretic framework incorpo-
rates interpersonal trust as a psychological stressor and extends the trust model to include the
possibility of psychological, behavioral, and physiological indicators of the response. For the
purposes of this study, hypothesis development should be guided by empirical studies that
examined the relationship between trust operationalized as a psychological state within the trustor
Intuitively, a positive relationship between trust and eustress and a negative relationship
between trust and distress should be expected. Trust increases as the trustor perceives that the
trustee increasingly exhibits ability, benevolence, and integrity. The increased willingness to be
45
vulnerable reflects the positive assessment of the trustee and should be accompanied by both an
· increase in eustress and a decrease in distress. Lack of trust reflects a negative appraisal of the
relationship with the trustee and should be accompanied by both a decrease in eustress and an
increase in distress.
Support for this rationale comes from the study of relational schemas. A schema is a mental
knowledge structure in which people organize and represent information about themselves and
about others (Berscheid, 1994). A relational schema is based on the notion that people develop
(Baldwin, 1992). Thus, relational schemas are the knowledge structures that organize the
Berscheid (1994) reviewed an array of theories and evidence on security and trust (e.g.,
Bowlby, 1982; Hazen & Shaver, 1987; Holmes, 1991) that suggests that expectations concerning
whether care will be received from a relationship partner in response to a need may be an
important component of most relational schemas. These theories share an assumption that
attitudes of trust reflect people's abstract positive expectations that they can count on the other
person in the relationship to care for them and be responsive to their needs. By contrast,
insecure, avoidant~ or ambivalent attitudes result from negative expectations about close
relationships. Because relationship schemas are cognitive structures, they can evolve as
appraisals evolve. While most of these theories recognize that an individual's early relationship
schemas are important (e.g. an infant's inner working model regarding expectations of the
caregiver's responsiveness to satisfying the infant's needs), Bersheid (1994) contends that "in
healthy individuals such schemas are continuously revised and modified to reflect experience" (p.
102).
46
Trust in relational schemas can be represented as both positive and negative. Baldwin (1992)
reviewed evidence that showed that the concept of vulnerability could mean different things to ·
different people. When individuals who anticipated that interpersonal vulnerability leads to being
hurt or abused by others were primed with the words trust and openness, they associated them
with words and phrases such as hurt and used. They also found that individuals who linked
interpersonal vulnerability with intimacy with others associated trust with closeness and warmth.
A study of the relationship between trustworthiness and breach of the psychological contract
provides some confirmation of the positive and negative influences of trust (Robinson, 1996).
Robinson (1996) found that employees with a low initial assessment of the employer's trust-
worthiness experienced a greater d~cline in this perception following a breach of the psychologi-
cal contract than did employees with a high initial assessment of their employer's trustworthiness.
She concluded that high trustworthiness leads people to experience other aspects of the relation-
ship in a positive light, while low trustworthiness leads individuals to interpret other events in the
relationship in the most unfavorable light and thus confirm their prior appraisal.
The intuitive nature of the relationship between trust in the supervisor and eustress/distress is
credible and supported by some evidence. Accordingly, these relationships are as follows.
H2: There is a positive relationship between trust in the supervisor and eustress.
ID: There is a negative relationship between trust in the supervisor and distress.
The next hypothesis for this study concerns the relationship between eustress and distress and
health.
Perception of Health
Not all aspects of the stress process reflect actual response to the stressor. Some responses
are clearly linked to the stressor and therefore constitute true responses. In this model, eustress
and distress are the true responses directly linked to the stressor interpersonal trust. Other states
associated with stress, such as well-being, illness, or fatigue, are more appropriately viewed as
47
products or effects of the stress response (Baum & Singer, 1987). Lazarus and Folkman (1986,
see figure 2, p. 73) provided an illustration of the variables for the stress rubric in which interper-
sonal trust is a causal antecedent, affect (positive/negative response) is an immediate effect, and
health and well-being are a long-term effects. Accordingly, an individual's perception of health
was selected as an appropriate outcome variable of the stress response for this study.
The fact that distress is not healthy is well established: "Heart attack, stroke, cancer, peptic
ulcer, asthma, diabetes, hypertension, headache, back pain, and arthritis are among the many
diseases and symptoms that have been found to be caused or worsened by stressful events"
(Quick et al., 1997, p. 77). Ganster and Schaubroeck (1991) reviewed the literature on work
stress and found that although there is no convincing evidence that stressors associated with the
job cause health effects, the indirect evidence strongly suggests a work stress effect. Recent
evidence has confirmed that job strain (distress) is associated with increased report of medical
symptoms and health damaging behavior in men (Weidner, Boughal, Connor, Peiper, & Mendell,
1997). The was no evidence to suggest that distress was associated with an improvement in
health.
Not surprisingly, there is less evidence concerning the relationship between eustress and
health. Edwards and Cooper (1988) speculated that eustress may improve health directly through
physiological changes or indirectly by reducing existing distress. They reviewed findings from a
variety of sources and found that the bulk of the evidence suggests a direct effect of eustress on
health. They noted that this evidence is merely suggestive rather than conclusive and that only
one study was able to demonstrate that eustress is associated with an improvement in physiologi-
cal functioning rather than just a reduction in damage. There was no evidence to suggest that
eustress was associated with a deterioration in health. Accordingly, the relationships between
48
HS: There is a negative relationship between distress and an individual's
perception of health.
The final hypothesis is proposed in order to satisfy the rigor of the approach to modeling
relationships between constructs employed in this study. Kelloway (1995) advises researches
using structural equation models to theoretically develop every relationship in the model,
including those thought to be zero; This satisfies James, Muliak, and Brett's ( 1982) condition
number 10, which requires that unestimated parameters are confirmed to be zero. The one
possible remaining relationship in this model would be a direct relationship between the stressor
trust and the outcome variable health. In the research on stress and health, the relationship
between stressors and long-term health outcomes is always represented as being fully mediated
by the stress response. There is no theoretical support suggesting that the relationship between a
stressor and a long-term health outcome is only partially mediated by the stress response.
Accordingly, the relationship between trust in the supervisor and perception of health is as
follows.
This chapter has presented the theoretical basis for a more holistic model of stress that
includes both eustress and distress as separate responses to any given stressor. This model also
provides the advantage of examining the differential affects of eustress and distress on an
outcome variable, in this case an individual's perception of his/her health. The next chapter will
49
CHAPTER3
RESEARCH METHODS
This purpose of this chapter is to discuss the research methods used to test the hypotheses
concerning the structure and hypothesized relationships among variables in the research model.
Following a brief discussion of the research settings and sample, details will be presented about
how each of the constructs in the model are operationalized. The results of a pilot study of the
survey instrument will then be presented. This chapter will conclude with a discussion of the
procedures to collect and analyze data in the main study. (Note: This chapter reflects the
anticipated plan, but will be revised to reflect what takes place in the main study.)
Research Settings
The research will be conducted at two separate hospitals in two cities in Oklahoma. A
hospital in Tulsa will be referred to in this study as Site A, and a hospital in Enid will be referred
to as Site B. Site A is an accredited General Medical Surgical Hospital with 550 beds. Site Bis
an accredited General Medical Surgical Hospital with 303 beds. Site A is managed by a non-
profit corporation, and Site B is managed by a for-profit corporation. A third site, C, was
included as a research site in the pilot study. Although discussions with the contact person at Site
C were instrumental in the development of the researcher's knowledge of the research setting,
this hospital will not be included in the main survey. Site C is an accredited General Medical
Surgical Hospital with 897 beds and is operated on a non-profit basis by a religious organization.
The positions held by the points of contact at the three sites were Chief Executive Officer, Vice-
Telephone conversations and meetings with the points of contact (POC) at these hospitals had
a major influence on the research design and survey construction of this research. All three
hospitals had workforces that were considering unionization, which made the leadership sensitive
50
to the content of a survey of their workforce. They placed constraints on the types informants
they would allow access to, which impacted the design of the study. All sites were only willing
to allow nurses as informants but not the nurses' supervisors. An issue that consistently arose as
their main concern was the length of the survey. All sites expressed a strong desire for a survey
instrument that could be completed in around 15 minutes. This constraint heavily influenced
decisions about selection of and alterations made to several of the scales in the study.
In the time period between the pretest and the main study, the sample workforce at Site A
voted on unionization. Unionization was rejected by one vote, but the union planned to protest.
Not long after this event, the contact person at Site A was removed from her position and the
Sample
The proposed sample for this study are registered (RN) or licensed practical (LPN) hospital
nurses. These two types of nurses were selected based upon the recommendation of a POC, who
suggested that nurses recognize the term "nurse" as referring to either an RN or an LPN. This
The nursing profession has been recognized as being possibly the most stressful of all the
health professions (Phillips, 1982). Hospital nursing staff are subject to stress that arises from the
physical, psychological, and social aspects of the work environment (Gray-Toft & Anderson,
1981 a). Some of the primary sources of stress for hospital nurses are work overload, dealing
with death and dying patients, poor communication with colleagues, shift work, inadequate
preparation, conflict with doctors or other supervisory personnel, uncertainty over authority,
political and union issues, financial resources, and increasing bureaucracy (Tyler & Ellison,
1994). The effects of stress on nurses is not always presented as negative, with some maintaining
that stress maintains alertness and ability to respond to pressure (Hay & Oken, 1972; Tyler &
Cushway, 1992).
51
Several models of stress for hospital nurses have incorporated the relationship between a
nurse and her supervisor as an important source of stress (Gray-Toft & Anderson, 1985; Revicki
& May, 1989). Nurses must exercise judgment and make critical decisions, sometimes under
life-threatening circumstances. Relationships between nurses and their supervisors that are open
and supportive can reduce the role ambiguity and increase the satisfaction experienced by nurses
(Gray-Toft & Anderson, 1985). ·Although trust between a nurse and her supervisor has not
When considering health and nursing, the focus is usually on the service the nurse provides to
the patient. Yet in order for the nurse to deliver the optimum service, the nurse must herself be
healthy. The health of the health care provider can affect the quality of health care delivery (Cox
& Leitter, 1992). Accordingly, the nurses' perception of their own health is a salient outcome
variable.
Design
The design selected for this exploratory research is cross-sectional field research. The
original design included the nurse supervisor as an informant for the stressor and other informa-
tion about the work environment. None of the research sites agreed to allow access to the nurse
supervisor; consequently, the subject is the single source of all information in this study. This
increases the potential for problems associated with common method variance. While there is
disagreement as to the prevalence of problems associated with common methods variance (e.g.,
Spector, 1987; Williams, Cote, & Buckley, 1989), an important issue is the potential for
divergence between observed and true relationships among constructs. A meta-analysis found
that while this common method bias was a cause for concern, it did not invalidate many research
findings (Doty & Glick, 1998). Also, self-report data are less prone to problems with inflation
when the data are factual, well known by the respondent, and verifiable (Crampton & Wagner,
52
1994; Podaskoff & Organ, 1986). The outcome variable, perception of health, is representative
of this kind of objective variable, especially as evaluated by the nurses in this sample.
It was also not possible to incorporate true random sampling into the design. Due to concerns
over unionization of its employees, none of the hospitals were willing to provide rosters of
employees that would have facilitated random selection for inclusion in the study. The main sites
were willing to allow distribution of surveys to all nurses present the day of the study - an
acceptable compromise. It will be important to keep these limitations in mind when discussing
The following sections will present the indicators selected to operationalize the constructs
presented in the model. All scales are non-proprietary, which eliminated from consideration
many of the most popular measures used in stress research. With the exception of perception of
health, measures were selected that did not contain the name of the construct being measured
(e.g., trust, distress). All items are measured on a five-point Likert. scale (at the request the
contact person at Site C) unless noted otherwise. Scales were adapted to reflect the hospital
setting. All measures, with the exception of the instrumental variables, are state measures. All
attitudinal scales asked the respondent to consider the present time (right now) when responding.
When individual scales were shortened to make the survey shorter, items with the strongest factor
loadings on the original scale were generally retained. A summary of the survey items including
their definitions, sources, item numbers in the attached survey, and reliabilities can be found in
Table 3.
53
Table 3: Summary of Work and Health Survey
54
Survey Item Reported
Construct Definition Numbers Source Reliability
Job Alienation One's generalized cognitive Section G, Kanungo, 1982 .70
state of psychological separa- Items 1-2
tion from one's job, insofar
as the job is perceived to lack
the potential for satisfying
one's salient needs and
expectations of the job
Anxiety Transitory sense of danger Section E, Beck et al., 1987; .90
and threat of loss Items 1-6 6 of 12 items from
the Anxiety Subscale
of CCL
Anger/Hostility Dimension of neuroticism Section B, Derogatis et al., Not
Items 3, 5, 9 1970; Subscale of reported
Symptom of Distress
Checklist (SCL)
Other Variables
Control Defined in terms of the pre- Section D, Rizzo, House, & .79
Variable: Role dictability ofthe outcomes of Items2, 10, Lirtzman, 1970
Ambiguity one's behavior and.the exist- 12, 15, 17
ence of environmental guide-
lines to provide knowledge
that one is behaving
appropriately
Control Two of the most common Section B, Gray-Toft& .70
Variables: stressors for nurses Items I, 2, 6, Anderson, 1981b;
Workload, Death 8 (workload); Subscales of Nurse
and Dying 4, 7, 10 Stress Scale
(death/ dying)
Instrumental A dispositional tendency to Section A, Scheier & Carver, .76
Variables: believe that one will gener- Items 1, 3, 4, 1985; LOT
Optimism/ ally experience good versus 6, 7,9
Pessimism bad outcomes in life
Other variables included in the surv~ but not 12.art o[.this dissertation. ·
Frustration Indication thafan individual's Section F, Adapter from .88
(supervisor) efforts are Items 2, 4, 6, Spector, 1987
ineffective 8, 9, 11, 12
Job Satisfaction Overall measure of degree to · Section A, Hackman& .76
which an employee is happy Items 2, 5, 8 Oldman, 1975
with the job
Social Esteem Self-concept in a social Section D, Adapted from .92
setting Items 4, 7, 11, . Heatherton &
16, 19 Polivy, 1991
55
Positive Psychological States: Indicators of Eustress
Eustress reflects the extent to which cognitive appraisal of a situation or event is seen to
either benefit an individual or enhance his/her well-being. The indicators of eustress should be
positive psychological states, for example attitudes or emotions. Stable dispositional variables
are not acceptable indicators of eustress, which must be subject to change according to changes in
cognitive appraisal of stressors. Work attitudes are preferable for this study, and the measures
should not overlap conceptually. The constructs selected for this study are positive affect (PA),
hope, meaningfulness, and manageability. Other items could also be indicators of eustress ( e.g.,
Hope. Hope has been identified as a positive emotion reflecting a degree of expected benefit
resulting from an evaluation of a particular situation (Lazarus, 1993; Smith et al., 1993 ). Hope
was defined as a cognitive set that is based on a sense of successful goal-directed determination
and planing to meet goals (Snyder et al., 1996). The state hope scale thus provides a snapshot of
a person's goal-directed thinking. All six items of this new scale were retained for use in this
study. This represents the first use of this measure in occupational psychology.
Meaningfulness and Manageability. These two constructs are part of a new scale devel-
oped by a nurse to measure situational sense of coherence (Artinian, 1997). Sense of coherence
(SOC) was a term developed to denote factors that promote a healthy response to stressful
situations (Antonovsky, 1987). It has traditionally been measured as a trait variable but was
adapted by Artinian (1997) as a situational or state measure. Two of three subscales are included
in this study. Meaningfulness is the extent to which one feels that life makes sense emotionally,
that problems and demands are worth investing energy in, are worthy of commitment and
engagement, and are challenges that are welcome. Manageability is the extent to which one
perceives that resources at one's disposal are adequate to meet the demands posed by the
56
situation. Both meaningfulness and manageability are measured with seven-point scales and
Positive Affect. Positive affect (PA) is a state of pleasurable engagement and reflects the
extent to which a person feels enthusiastic, active, and alert (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988).
PA can be measured as a state or trait, with state PA capturing how one feels at given points in
time, whereas the trait represents stable individual differences in the level of affect generally
experienced (George & Brief, 1992; Watson & Pennbaker, 1989). State and trait PA are both
conceptually and empirically distinct, and state PA is also a separate factor from negative affect
(George & Brief, 1992). The ten items from the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
(PANAS) are included in this study to measure state PA (Watson et al., 1988).
In contrast to eustress, distress reflects the degree to which CO$flitive appraisal of a situation
or event identifies the possibility for undesirable or harmful consequences to result. The
indicators of distress are negative psychological states. The state measures employed in this
study as indictors of distress are common in stress research. It was surprisingly difficult to
identify measures of distress that were non-proprietary. The indicators selected for this study are
negative affect (NA}, job alienation, anger/hostility, and anxiety. As with the indicators of
eustress, this set is not exhaustive of the types of possible indicators of distress.
and unpleasurable engagement (Watson et al., 1988). NA is a common variable in studies not
just as an indicator of distress, but also because of the possibility that NA may affect the
measurement of and substantive relationships between stressors and strains in general (Hurrell,
Nelson, & Simmons, 1998). State NA was measured with the ten items from the PANAS scale
57
Alienation. Job alienation reflects one's separation from one's job, insofar as the job is
perceived to lack the potential for satisfying one's salient needs and expectations of the job
(Kanungo, 1982). Job alienation is a specific belief about the present job in contrast to work
alienation, which refers to a general belief about work. Job alienation is a cognitive belief that is
descriptive of a worker's relations to his/her present job and is determined by stimuli in his/her
present work environment. The separation from the job reflected in job alienation is a psycholog-
ical state indicative of a degree of negative response to stimuli in the work environment.
Alienation is measured with a two-item graphic scale (Kanungo, 1982). While a two-item scale
should be expected to be less reliable, this scale met with significant approval from hospital
personnel from the research sites. They found that the graphical items provided some relief from
Anxiety. Anxiety is a transitory state that reflects feelings of danger or threat of loss as a
result of cognitive appraisal. Beck, Brown, Steer, Eidelson, & Riskind (1987) developed 12
items for an anxiety subscale as part of their Cognition Checklist (CCL). Only six of the items
were retained for use in this study in an attempt to address concerns about the length of the
survey. The items retained were ones with the greatest factor loadings from the original subscale.
Checklist (SCL) that was developed to assess self-reported neurotic symptoms. Three items from
the four-item subscale were retained for this study. The item "I have impulses to beat, injure, or
hurt someone" was determined inappropriate for a study of nurses. This scale was inadvertently
omitted from the version of the survey that was prepared for the pilot study. The items are ( 1) I
feel critical of others, (2) I feel easily annoyed or irritated, and (3) l have temper outbursts I could
not control.
58
Trust
Trust in the supervisor was measured with Mayer and Davis' (1999) four-item trust scale.
The scale was adjusted to reflect the relationship with the supervisor and the hospital context.
The item "I would be comfortable giving top management (supervisor) a task or problem which
was critical to me, even if I could not monitor their actions," was identified as confusing by
hospital personnel and graduate students that reviewed the survey. The item was changed to read
"I am comfortable discussing with my supervisor concerns I have about my ability to do my job."
In an attempt to improve the scale's marginal reliability (.60), two items were added to the scale.
These items were intended to reflect the willingness to be vulnerable when discussing the
working relationship and suggestions for workplace improvements with the supervisor. Both of
these issues would be reflective of a degree of trust between the nurse and the supervisor.
Perception of Health·
An individual's current perception of his/her health was measured with four items from a ten-
item subscale of the Health Perceptions Questionnaire (Ware et al., 1978). They report that
general health ratings are valid measures of health status as they significantly correlated with a
variety of other health measures (e.g., physician's assessment). Others have used a single-item
version of this scale with nurses (How would you rate your overall health at the present time?)
and found it acceptable (Pender, Walker, Sechrist, & Frank-Stromborg, 1990). Nurses are
knowledgeable of health issues and tend to see their physicians more often, which enhances the
validity of health perception. All ten items were included in initial versions of the survey, but a
POC found the redundancy of the items annoying and asked for the .scale to be shortened. The
59
Instrumental Variables
Instrumental variables are needed in order to identify nonrecursive models (Bentler and
Chou, 1987). Instrumental variables are variables that are theoretically and statistically related to
only one of variables in the reciprocal relationship. Dispositional optimism and pessimism
(Scheier & Carver, 1985) were theorized to be uniquely related to eustress and distress, respec-
tively. Optimists expect things to go their way and generally believe that good rather than bad
things will happen to them. Pessimists, on the other hand, anticipate bad outcomes. Six items
from the Life Orientation Test (LOT) are used to assess optimism and pessimism (Scheier &
Carver, 1985).
Control Variables
The most common causes of stress.among nurses are included as control variables. Gray-
Toft and Anderson (1981 b) dev;eloped a 34-item nursing stress scale to measure death and dying,
conflict with physicians, lack of support, conflict with other nurses, and workload. Workload and
death and dying were foundto be the most significant sources of stress (Gray-Toft & Anderson,
1981a). Three items from the death and dying subscale and four items from the workload
subscale that had the best factor loadings (greater than .59) were included in this study.
Role ambiguity has also been found to be a significant source of stress for nurses (Revicki &
May, 1989). Conversations with a hospital POC confirmed that confusion over expectations is a
source of stress for.nurses. Following Schaubroeck, Ganster, Sime, and Ditman (1993), items
Demographic Variables
The demographic variables selected for this study are age, gender, experience, education,
hours worked, type of nurse (RN or LPN), and area of practice (intensive care or other). While
not an exhaustive list of possible relevant demographics, theses are the some of the most common
60
used in studies of nurse stress (Gray-Toft & Anderson, 1981a, 1981b, 1985; Revicki & May,
1989). A test for significant difference between the two hospitals on the independent and
Pilot Study
A pilot study was conducted for the purpose of assessing the psychometric properties of the
research instrument. All of the scales used in the survey were previously established and found
to have acceptable psychometric properties, but some of the scales were altered for the purpose
of this study. Most of the changes involved incorporating the hospital as the research setting. As
such, the rigorous pretest procedures required for new scale development were not necessary and
Because the hospitals were concerned about the length of the test, the survey was admin-
Oklahoma State University in order to get an initial determination of the completion time of the
survey. The completion time for these five individuals ranged from a low of 11 minutes to a high
of 18 minutes. As the points of contact at the sample hospitals felt that a completion time of 15
minutes was acceptable, the instrument was detertnined to be ready for pretest.
The sample size for the pretest was 102 hospital .nurses from two separate hospitals. At one
hospital, 45 supervisory-level n~rses that had gathered for a day of training were administered the
survey, and all responded. They were given time during the meeting to complete the survey, and
completed surveys were dropped in a collection box provided by the researcher. This hospital
will also participate in the main study. The other 57 nurses were employees of a separate hospital
that will not be participating in the main survey. These nurses were administered surveys by the
hospital contact personnel during a training session held at the hospital. It is unclear how many
surveys were actually distributed. Forty-three of the nurses deposited their completed surveys in
61
a sealed box provided by the researcher, and the remaining 14 returned surveys with a self-
As a general rule in factor analysis, the minimum sample size would be five times as many
observations as there are variables to be analyzed, and the more acceptable range would be a ten-
to-one ratio. This study uses 14 variables with a total of 72 indicators; consequently, the sample
size is not large enough to submit all variables to a single factor analysis. It was not logistically
possible to obtain the recommended sample size of 3 50 for this pretest. In order to meet the
minimum ratio of five-to-one, constructs were organized into groups of20 or fewer variables
before being factor analyzed. The heuristic used in forming these groups was an attempt to
subject related constructs to a single factor analysis. Thus, the control variables, negative
psychological states, and positive psychological states excluding positive affect formed three of
the factor analysis groups (the 10 items from the PA scale would put this group over the 20
variable guideline). The 10 items for positive affect were grouped with the independent variable
trust and the dependent variable health perception to form the fourth group.
All groups of variables were subject to principal component factor analysis. Principal
component factor analysis is the only factor analytic method available in SPSS 9.0; consequently,
the decision to use this method instead of common factor analysis was a practical one. While
there remains considerable debate over which factor model is the more appropriate, empirical
research has demonstrated that in many instances the methods produce similar results (Hair,
Anderson, Tatham, & Black, 1995). Two criteria were used to determine the number of factors
to be extracted. The first method considered only factors having latent roots or eigenvalues
greater than 1 as significant. The second method involved analysis of the scree test plots as well
When interpreting the factors, a loading of .40 could be considered important and a loading of
.50 or greater considered practically significant. With a sample size of 100, a factor loading of
62
.55 is statistically significant at the .05 significance level with a power of 80 percent (Hair et al.,
1995). When the initial factor solution was not interpretable, the factors were rotated using the
oblique method PROMAX. The oblique rotation method is appropriate when the goal of the
factor analysis is to obtain several theoretically meaningful factors or constructs (Hair et al.,
1995). Reliability analysis using Cronbach's alpha will be conducted after meaningful factors
The first factor analysis included the variables for trust, PA, and health perception. Examina-
tion of latent roots and scree plots revealed three factors, and a PROMAX rotation produced
interpretable factors. All the variables for PA and health perception produced significant factor
loadings for their factor in the range from .64 to .86. Reliability analysis of these two scales
revealed a Chronbach's alpha (a) of .92 for PA and .86 for health perception. Item loadings from
.48 to .86 were produced for items on the trust scale (see Table 4). While three of the items had
factor loadings below the preferred significance level, all six items of the scale produced an
a= .75. Alpha would only improve to .77 if the lowest item was deleted from the scale; conse-
quently, all six items will be retained in the scale for use in the main study.
Rotated Factor
Items of Trust Scale Loading
I am comfortable discussing with my supervisor concerns I have about our
.86
working relationship.
I am comfortable discussing with my supervisor concerns I have about my
.85
ability to do my job.
I am comfortable discussing with my supervisor my ideas for improve-
.77
ments in the workplace.
Ifl had my way, I wouldn't let my supervisor have any influence over
.52
issues that are important to me.
I really wish I had a good way to keep an eye on my supervisor. .51
I would be willing to let my supervisor have complete control over my
.48
future in this hospital.
63
The next factor analysis involved the variables negative affect (NA), anxiety, and alienation.
Examination of the test criteria revealed five distinct factors, with items from the NA scale
forming three distinct factors. Items from the anxiety scale loaded together on one factor with
rotated factor loadings ranging from .61 to .85. Items from the alienation scale loaded together
on another factor with loadings ranging from .83 to .85. Alpha for the resulting anxiety and
alienation scales were .83 and .61, respectively. The low reliability estimate for the alienation
scale may be attributed to the fact that it contains only two items.
The first factor produced by the NA scale contained the items afraid, scared, nervous,
distressed, and jittery, with rotated factor loadings ranging from .57 to .87. The second factor
produced by the NA scales contained the ~terns hostile, irritable, and upset, with factor loadings
from .57 to .89. The third factor produced by the NA scale contained the items ashamed and
guilty, with factor loadings from .77 to .78. It is encouraging to note that none of the NA factors
Reliability analysis for all ten items of the NA scale reveals an alpha of .78. The three
subscales produce alphas of .81, .70, and .58, respectively. Because even the best subscale
produces only a small increase in reliability, all ten items will be retained as the NA factor in the
main study. If the larger sample size of the main study continues to produce this factor structure,
the first factor minus the term distressed may be a sufficient indicator of NA.
The third factor analysis involved the variables meaningfulness, manageability, and hope.
Examination of the test criteria reveals a three factor solution, with all of the items loading on
their intended construct. The alphas were as follows: hope, .80; meaningfulness, .79; and
manageability, .60. When the ten items from the PA scale were added to this group and subject
to a separate factor analysis (25 variables total), the PA scale held together and formed the first
factor extracted. In this analysis, the first item of the hope scale formed a separate factor and the
manageability scale produced two factors. When the meaningfulness and manageability scales
64
were subjected to a separate factor analysis together, the manageability scale produced two
subscales. One of these subscales was associated with the ability to find solutions to challenges
at work; the other was more associated with a feeling of being able to count on others at work.
As both of the subscales were less reliable than the original scale, all items will be retained for
the main study. Likewise, deletion of the first item of the hope scale would only improve the
reliability from .798 to .803, so all the items of the hope scale will be also be retained for the
main study.
The final factor analysis included the variables role ambiguity, death/dying, workload,
pessimism, and optimism. Examination of the test criteria reveals the expected five factor
solution, with one exception. The item for role ~biguity that states "I have clear, planned goals
and objectives for my job" did not load with the other items on the role ambiguity factor. This
item loaded on the optimism factor. In the survey, this item immediately follows an item for
hope that reads ''at the present time, I am energetically pursuing my goals." Because these two
items overlap, the role ambiguity item will be eliminated from the main survey. Elimination of
this one item from the role ambiguity scales increases its alpha nominally from .76 to .78. The
-
reliabilities of the scales death/dying, workload, pessimism, and optimism are .76, .73, .85, and
.70, respectively. The reliabilities for all the scales are reported in Table 5.
The point of contact at the main research site (the one with the largest potential sample size)
also expressed a desire to change the name of the survey. The nurses at her hospital are
considering unionization, so she was concerned about the impression that a "Nurse Attitude
Survey" would convey. It was decided to change the name of the survey in the main study to
"Work and Health Survey." A reviewer of the pretest results suggested changing from a five-
point to a seven-point Likert response scale. The revised survey is included as Appendix 1.
65
Table 5: Summary of Pretest Reliabilities
Item Means
Scale Alpha N of items Mean Min. Max. Variance
Trust .75 6 3.59 2.18 4.17 .50
Health Perception .85 4 3.69 3.35 4.00 .08
Meaningfulness .79 4 5.60 5.34 6.11 .13
Manageability .60 5 4.90 4.25 5.59 .27
Hope .80 6 3.90 3.68 4.15 .04
PA .92 10 3.47 2.72 3.85 .11
NA .78 10 1.46 1.11 1.94 .06
Anxiety .83 6 1.56 1.37 2.03 .05
Alienation .61 2 3.56 3.21 3.90 .24
Role Ambiguity .78 5 3.89 3.68 4.14 .04
Death/Dying .76 3 3.14 2.84 3.56 .14
Workload .73 4 3.45 3.16 3.59 .04
Pessimism .85 3 2.01 1.96 2.11 .01
OJ:!timism .70 3 3.82 3.77 3.86 .01
Two separate hospitals, Site A and Site B, have agreed to participate in the main research
study to this point. Questionnaires will be distributed to nurses at work. Each of the sites has
indicated that it will provide the nurses 15-20 minutes of work time to complete the surveys. The
nurses who complete the surveys will return them to a collection box that will be provided to ·
each of their departments. The questionnaires will be collected from these boxes, thereby
eliminating handling of the surveys by hospital personnel. This procedure was employed
successfully by Fox, Dwyer, and Ganster (1993). A true random sample was not possible due to
unionization concerns; neither of the research sites was willing to provide a list of employees.
Because surveys will be distributed to all nurses present at the time of the survey, selection biases
should be minimized.
Site A as indicated that access to between 300 and 400 nurses is possible. Site B has
indicated that around 200 nurses should be available to complete surveys at its hospital. Because
the sites are allowing time for the nurses to complete the surveys as work, an 80 percent
66
completion rate is expected. Accordingly, a combined sample size of around 480 hospital nurses
from the two hospitals is anticipated. This sample size would satisfy the five-to-one observation
to variable recommended ratio for both exploratory factor analysis and structural equation
modeling, the data analytic techniques that will be discussed in the next section.
Once the data are collected, they will be input into SPSS for univariate analysis of the
variables. The most important assumptions of the data analytic technique employed in this study,
structural equation modeling (LISREL), are primarily conceptual. The statistical requirements
linearity of all relationships (Bentler & Chou, 1987; Hair et al., 1995). LISREL is also sensitive
to departures from multivariate normality in the data. Multivariate normality will be assessed
through the use of univariate statistics, particularly the skewness and kurtosis of the data. The
data will also be examined for missing data and outliers, and available strategies for these
Identification of the model was also addressed. Eustress, distress, trust, and health each have
at least four indicators, which helps to ensure identification of the model. Instrumental variables
(optimism and pessimism) are included to facilitate identification of the nonrecursive portion of
The specified measurement model will be examined to confirm that it is congeneric before
any structural relationships proposed by the study are examined. Confirmation of the con generic
measurement model will allow for assessment of convergent and discriminant validity, whereas
confirmation of the structural model will allow assessment of the nomological validity of the
model (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988; James et al., 1982). While assessing the measurement
model, the discriminant validity of the indicators for eustress and distress must be demonstrated
67
Several absolute fit measures will be assessed: Chi-square, Goodness-of-fit index (GFI), and
root mean square residual error of approximation (RMSEA). An incremental fit measure, the
comparative fit index (CFI), will also be assessed to evaluate the fit of the measurement model.
When the overall model is accepted, each of the constructs will be evaluated separately by
(I) examining each indicator's loading for statistical significance and (2) assessing the
The structural model will provide for hypothesis testing. The control variables will be
included in the model with direct paths to the dependent variable. Hypotheses two through six
will be tested by examining the significance and direction of the estimated coefficients produced
by the structural model. Hypothesis one will be tested through a series of nested models in which
zero, one, and two paths between eustress and distress are examined for their statistical signifi-
cance. A series of chi square difference tests of these competing models will determine the most
Summary
This chapter detailed the research methods that will be employed to test the proposed research
model. The sample of nurses should provide an ideal setting to examine the potential for trust in
the supervisor to generate degrees of both eustress and distress. The pilot study was limited in its
ability to assess the survey instrument; nevertheless, potential improvements were identified, and
the instrument has sufficient reliability to justify its use in the main study.
68
CHAPTER4
RESULTS
This chapter will describe the steps taken to test the proposed model and the series of
relationships contained in the model presented in Hypotheses 1-6. The final model that is
confirmed will provide support for the hypothesized relationships between trust, eustress, and
health. The results will also provide valuable. information about the efficacy of the positive and
negative psychological states selected as indicators of eustress and distress. Finally, the results
will provide suggestions for improvement of the current model and method of study.
Survey Response
Surveys were administered to a total of450 RNs and LPNs in two separate hospitals. The
response rates for each of the hospitals were 4 7 percent (4 7 out of 100) and 32 percent (111 out
of 350), with an overall response rate of 35 percent (158 out of 450). Most of the respondents
were female (92 percent) registered nurses (91 percent). Exactly half of the nurses practiced in
intensive/critical care areas, while the other 50 percent practiced in other areas of the hospital. A
total of 79 percent of respondents had practiced nursing for at least 6 years, and 39 percent had
more than 15 years of nursing experience. Eight percent reported working 51 or more hours per
week, 7 percent less than 20 hours per week, 44 percent between 31 to 40 hours per week, and
41 percent reported working between 41- to 50 hours per week. Due to the complexity of the
model, the demographic data are presented for descriptive purposes only and will not enter into
the current data analysis. A summary of the demographic data is presented in Table 6.
Item Analysis
Descriptive statistics for the manifest indicators in the model are presented in Table 7.
Because the items would be combined into summated scales only for the purpose of assessing
differences in responses between the two hospitals, analysis of data analytic assumptions was
69
Table 6: Demographic Questions and Responses (Expressed
as a Percentage of Total Responses)
70
Table 7: Descriptive Statistics for Manifest Indicators
Item
Construct (R = reverse scored) Min Max Mean Std.
Health (a= .87) healthl 7 5.31 1.41
health2 1 7 4.34 1.55
health3 1 7 4.82 1.64
health4 1 7 5.04 1.63
Trust (a= .79) trustl (R) 1 7 4.78 1.85
trust2 1 7 5.08 1.95
trust3 1 7 2.61 1.67
trust4 (R) 1 7 5.56 1.7
trusts 1 7 4.96 1.91
trust6 1 7 4.89 1.8
Hope (a= .84) hopel 1 7 5.58 1.2
hope2 1 7 4.73 1.56
hope3 1 7 4.9 1.36
hope4 1 7 5.41 1.25
hope5 2 7 5.43 1.17
hope6 7 5.16 1.36
Meaningfulness (a = .67) meaningl (R) 2 7 5.96 1.12
meaning2 2 7 5.24 1.13
meaning3 (R) 1 7 5.52 1.33
meaning4 7 5.05 1.31
Manageability (a= .60) manageability 1 2 7 5.22 1.41
manageability2 7 4.69 1.57
· manageability3 7 4.89 1.25
. manageability4 7 4.82 1.27
manageability5 7 4.48 1.19
Positive Affect (a= .90) PAI 1 5 3.85 0.85
PA2 5 2.57 1.21
PA3 5 3.52 1.00
PA4 5 3.19 1.02
PAS 1 5 3.57 1.12
PA6 2 5 4.14 0.77
PA7 1 5 2.87 1.17
PAS 1 5 3.82 1.00
PA9 5 4.03 0.83
PAlO 5 4.00 0.94
71
Item
Construct (R = reverse scored) Min Max Mean Std.
Anxiety (a= .82) anxiety! 1 7 2.08 1.31
anxiety2 1 7 1.82 1.21
anxiety3 1 7 1.73 1.01
anxiety4 I 6 1.85 1.10
anxiety5 I 6 1.66 1.03
anxiety6 1 6 2.37 1.36
Anger (a = .77) anger! 7 3.61 1.43
anger2 7 3.55 1.33
anger3 I 7 1.85 1.12
Job Alienation (a = .71) joball 1 7 4.04 1.15
jobal2 1 7 3.42 1.31
Negative Affect (a = .80) NAl 1 5 2.22 1.06
NA2 1 5 1.79 0.96
NA3 1 5 1.29 0.69
NA4 1 5 1.45 0.80
NA5 I 5 1.35 0.73
NA6. I 5 1.78 0.92
NA7 1 4 1.11 0.40
. NA8 1 5 1.58 0.86
NA9 1 5 1.33 0.71
NAIO I 4 1.32 0.66
Workload (a= .68) work! 7 5.21 1.38
.work2 7 5.05 1.34
work3 7 4.37 1.67
work4 7 4.76 1.63
Death/Dying (a= .67) death! 7 4.03 1.73
death2 I 7 4.65 1.51
death3· I 7 3.48 1.79
Role Ambiguity (a= .80) RAI I 7 3.33 1.48
RA2 I 6 2.61 1.22
RA3 6 2.08 1.05
RA4 7 2.55 1.34
RAS 7 2.15 1.10
performed at the item level. The exception to this is that coefficient alpha reliabilities (a) were
computed in SPSS 9.0 for each scale. When the results of the subsequent confirmatory factor
analysis (CFA) are presented, it will be important to remember that coefficient alpha under-
72
estimates reliability unless all of the factor loadings of a manifest indicator on its latent variable
are equal (Kenny, 1979; McDonald, 1985). The coefficient alpha for the variable manageability·
was under desired standards at .60. Because the subsequent analysis will assess all the items for
their factor loadings on the latent variable and the items will not be combined to form a scale, no
attempts will be made to improve reliability by eliminating items. Examination of the individual
items for manageability revealed that there was no single item that if eliminated would substan-
tially improve alpha. The rest of the reliabilities were acceptable (a= .67 to a =.90) (Nunnally &
Bernstein, 1994).
The 74 items employed as manifest indicators in the model were assessed for univariate
normality. Most of the items exhibited acceptable levels of skewness and kurtosis; however, all
of the indicators for NA, anxiety, and pessimism were skewed to the low end of the scale. The
explanation for this result is that people may tend to underreport negative things about them-
-selves. Although some of the items were skewed, all 74 items passed the Kologorov-Smirnov
test (modified with the Lilliefors significance correction) for normality (Hair et al., 1995).
When missing values were encountered in the data, they were replaced with the mean of the item
in the sample using an option provided by SPSS 9.0. This strategy for replacing missing
observations should have minimal effect in the structural equation modeling analysis. While
some variables had no missing observations, several hems in the NA and PA scale had as many
as seven missing observations. Observations replaced with missing values represent 2.4 percent
(276 of 11,692) of the total observations in the analysis. This was considered an acceptable
In order to ensure that the 158 responses between the two hospitals could be combined into a
single sample for analysis, the major variables in the model were subjected to a MAN OVA
analysis with hospitals as the grouping variable. None of the MANOVA test statistics were
73
significant (e.g., Pillai's Trace= .034, F= .511,p = .881), which supports the conclusion that
there is no significant difference between the two hospitals in the vector of means formed by the
"anger/hostility," and "job alienation." Although the unequal cell sizes limit the power of the
MANOVA, all 158 responses will be combined for the LISREL 8.30 analysis.
A separate MANOVA was run to check for significant differences between ICU nurses and
non-ICU nurses. There was no significant difference between the ICU and non-ICU nurses in the
vector of means formed by the variables "health," "trust," "hope," "meaningfulness," "manage-
ability," "PA," "NA," "anxiety," "anger/hostility," and "job alienation" (e.g., Pillai's Trace= .07,
F= 1.25,p = .27).
Both estimation methods and the tests of model fit in structural equation modeling (SEM) are
based on the assumption oflarge samples. Because the overall response rate was smaller than
When assessing the fit of a model, the recommended strategy is to use more than one
perspective (Kellaway, 1998; Loehlin, 1998). Among the many alternatives available, it is
worthwhile to remember that "the quality of model fit should be assessed in the context of the
substantive concerns motivating model construction"(Hayduk, 1987, p. 169). Because the most
important objective of the present study was to examine the factor structure of eustress and
distress, this objective could be accomplished through a comparison of competing nested models
(e.g., one factor versus two factors). This is fortunate because the available techniques for
assessing model fit do a better job of determining the model with the best fit, as opposed to
74
evaluating an isolated model in some absolute sense (Bollen & Long, 1993; Loehlin, 1998, p.
39).
A useful fit index for this study, the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), is
one that is gaining recognition as one of the most informative criteria in SEM (Byrne, 1998;
tive to sample size. It also has an explicit adjustment for the complexity of the model. The
developerofRMSEA considers values below .10 "good" and below .05 "very good" (Steiger,
1989, p. 81). LISREL 8.30 also reports a test of the hypothesis that RMSEA in the population is
less than .05. If the acceptance of a model is to be meaningful, there must be a reasonable chance
of rejecting it if it is false. Loehlin ( 1998) provides a power table for RMSEA that indicates that
for a sample size of 144, the power to reject the hypothesis of poor fit (RMSEA > . I 0) given
population RMSEA of .05 at the .05 significance level is .90 if the model has at least 40 degrees
of freedom. The present model has a sample size of 158 and a minimum of 880 degrees of
freedom.
A 90 percent confidence interval (CI) around the RMSEA value will also be reported. In
contrast to point estimates of model fit, confidence intervals allow the researcher to assess the
imprecision of the estimate. An acceptable value of RMSEA and a narrow confidence interval
represents good precision of the RMSEA value reflecting model fit in the population. With a
sma:ll sample size and a complex model, larger confidence intervals would be expected; conse-
quently, small confidence intervals in the present model will be considered favorably (Byrne,
1998). Furthermore, if the upper limit of the 90 percent confidence interval for RM SEA lies
below .10 (unacceptable fit), one can reject the hypothesis that the fit of the model in the
population is that or worse and conclude that the present model fits acceptably in the population
(Loehlin, 1998).
75
The Expected Cross Validation Index (ECVI) was used as a measure of comparative fit
(Kelloway, 1998). ECVI represents an estimate of the F needed to compare the implied
covariance matrix of the present solution with a new sample drawn from the population. The
smaller the ECVI, the better the model is expected to cross-validate in a new sample. Although
there are no standards to evaluate the value ofECVI, a 90 percent confidence interval is
computed for this statistic. Thus, the researcher knows not only what kind of fit to expect on
average in a new sample, but also has some idea of how precise that estimate is (Loehlin, 1998).
Another measure of comparative fit employed in this study was the comparative fit index (CFI).
The CFI compares the model to the worst fitting model, one that specifies no relationships
between the variables composing the model. The values of CFI will range between O and l, with
values exceeding .90 indicating a good fit to the data. Because of the relatively small sample size
and complexity of the present model, the CFI·values obtained were not expected to indicate good
A final measure of fit used in this study is a new measure developed specifically for higher-
order models (Marsh & Hocevar, 1985). The index, called the target coefficient (1), is the ratio
of the chi-square (i) of the first-order model to the t of the more restrictive higher-order model.
The idea is that the basic first-order model provides a target or optimum fit for the higher-order
model because the fit of the higher-order model will never be better than the first-order measure-
ment model. The target coefficient has an upper limit of 1, which would be possible if the
relations among the first-order factors could be totally accounted for in terms of the higher-order
factors. This index has the advantage of separating the lack of fit due to the second-order
structure from lack of fit in the definition of the first-order factors. One limitation of this index is
that Twill generally be higher as the number of parameters in the higher-order model increases.
Although the distributional properties of Tare unknown, the highest T obtained by Marsh and
Hocevar (1985) was .95 in a model with 1,379 degrees of freedom. A high of T= .93 was
76
obtained in a study with a model that contained two correlated higher-order factors (Avolio, Bass,
& Jung, 1999) and a high of T= .89 in a study with three higher-order factors (Farmer, Maslyn,
Order of Procedures
Covariance matrices were used for all LISREL analyses in this study, and the method of
analysis (CFA) of the proposed model, LISREL would have to estimate 214 free parameters.
Because the total sample· size was smaller than the number of estimated parameters, the
parameter estimates may not be reliable. Although it was a departure from standard practice,
preliminary attempts to make the model more parsimonious by ensuring that only the necessary
variables are included in the final structural analysis were warranted. Complex models with
several levels of analysis and a large number of measures should be constructed in layers, with
the final model being an integration of theory and data (Gerbing, Hamilton, & Freeman, 1994).
One way to facilitate parsimony was to perform the second-order CFA first. Examination of
the factor structure of eustress and distress will reveal .whether higher-order factors are in~eed
present and, if so, how many. If the higher-order factors were not present, or if a model with a
single higher-order factor was confirmed, the instrumental variables would not be necessary in
the structural analysis. Additionally, if one or more second-order factors were confirmed, only
the latent variables that loaded significantly would be included in the structural model as
indicators of the higher-order factor(s). Because the first-order factors were only used as
indicators of the second-order factors and all hypotheses in the model relate to the second-order
factors, this was a reasonable approach to "theory trimming" (Kelloway, 1998). This initial
second-order CFA would provide a direct test of the first and most important hypothesis in the
study.
77
Following the second-order CFA, relevant indicators of eustress and distress would be
combined with the stressors and outcome variable and subjected to a first-order CF A to establish
the measurement model before proceeding to the structural model. It is possible at this stage that
the number of estimated parameters may still exceed the total sample size, but any improvement
would be helpful.
Second-Order CFA
LISREL 8.30 was used to analyze a series of four models to determine the factor structure of
the four positive and four negative psychological states used as indicators of eustress and distress.
This provided an initial test of Hypothesis 1, which stated eustress and distress are separate con-
structs in a nonrecursive relationship. A description of the models as well as the fit statistics and
The first model was the measurement model for the eight latent variables of positive and
negative psychological states and their 46 manifest indicators. All of the indicators loaded
positively and significantly on their intended latent variables. Examination of both the RM SEA
and ECVI statistics indicate that the model represented a good fit to the data. The CFI is
relatively low at .74, but that was not unexpected with such a complex model.
78
Table 8: Results of the Second Order CFA
Model Description
Measurement model with 8 latent variables
2 Two correlated higher-order factors with 8 latent variable indicators
3 Measurement model with 7 latent variables
4 Two correlated higher-order factors with 7 latent variable indicators
5 Unidimensional higher-order factor with 7 latent variable indicators
RMSEA ECVI Significant
Model 1..2 Df (90% Cl) (90% Cl) CFI T Compare Difference Favor
1 1710.35 961 .076"' 13.16 .74
(.07, .081) (12.41, 13.95)
2 1766.34 982 .078"' 13.44 .73 .968
(.072, .083) (12.67, 14.25)
3 1589.63 881 .077"' 12.24 .75
(.072, .083) (11.52, 13.01)
4 1621.73 894 .078"' 12.42 .74 .980
(.073, .084) (11.69, 13.21)
5 1622.06 895 .078"' 12.39 .74 .980 5 to4 No""" 5
(.073, .084) (11.65, 13.17)
• p < .05 (test of close fit - RMSEA < .05)
0 The i e.
difference between these two models is .33 2 with 3.84 rel!resenting a significant difference of < .05.
The correlations among the eight latent variables, taken from the PHI matrix, are presented in
Table 9. Examination of the correlations shows that the positive and negative psychological
states related to each other as expected, with the exception of ''.job alienation," which did not
perform at all as expected except for its relationship with PA. Part of the problem with "job
alienation" may be that it was the only variable in the study with only two indicators, which
would tend to reduce its reliability. The four positive indicators "hung together" relatively well,
while the four negative indicators did not "hang together" as well.
The second model run was the hypothesized second-order factor structure of eustress and
distress. The fit indices for this model (Table 8) were similar to the first model and indicated a
good fit to the data. Comparing the x2 of model 2 to the i of model 1 (T = .968) also indicated
very good model fit. The factor loadings of the latent variable indicators on their hypothesized
factors were confirmed, with most of the indicators loading significant and in the expected
79
direction. Accordingly, the two second-order factors could be labeled "eustress" and "distress."
The one exception to indicator performance was ''.job alienation," which did not load significantly
on the second-order factor distress. Due to its poor performance, "job alienation" was dropped as
an indicator in all subsequent analyses. Finally, examination of the Plil matrix indicated a
significant correlation between the two second-order factors (-0.91), which suggested that a
It must be noted at this point that the measurement model fit the data better than the model
with higher-order factors. This was expected because even when a higher-order model is able to
theoretically explain effectively the factor covariations, the empirical goodness-of-fit of the
higher-order model can never be better than the corresponding first-order measurement model
(March & Hocevar, 1985). Because the purpose of the study was to investigate the stress
response as a second-order factor, the higher-order model was favored over the measurement
model.
The third model represented a new measurement model with "job alienation" removed and
seven latent variables remaining. This model was necessary to allow for computation of the T
statistic. All of the indicators loaded positively and significantly on their intended latent
variables. Examination of both the RMSEA and ECVI statistics indicated that the model
80
Table 9: Second-Order CFA Correlations Among Latent Variables and
Factor Loadings on Hypothesized Second-Order Factors
-
00 8. Job Alienation
* p < .05
-0.02 -0.02 -0.10 -0.34* -0.04 -0.03 -0.05 -0.05
Note: The correlations were obtained from the PHI matrix in Model 1, and the factor loadings of the latent variables indicators
on their h_yp_othesized second-order factor were obtained from the standardized GAMA matrix in Models 2 - 4.
It must be noted at this point that the first-order measurement model fits the data better than
the model with higher-order factors. Examination of the fit statistics and factor loadings for this
model again supported the hypothesized two-factor structure of eustress and distress. Comparing
the i of Model 4 to the i of Model 3 produced a T= .980, another indication of good fit.
Examination of the PIIl matrix revealed that eustress and distress remained significantly
warranted.
The fifth model represented a unidimensional second-order factor with all seven positive and
negative latent variables as indicators. For this model, RMSEA, CFI, and T remained the same,
while ECVI was slightly worse. Because mode is 4 and 5 were nested, a difference test was
performed as a basis for comparison. This test revealed a difference of .33, which is nonsignifi-
cant (3.84 atp < .05 with 1 degree of freedom). Because there was no significant difference
between the models, the more parsimonious unidimensional second-order factor model (more
degrees of freedom) was favored. As a result, Hypothesis 1, that eustress and distress are
separate constructs in a nonrecursive relationship, could not be properly tested in the structural
Further inspection of this single second-:-order factor showed that not surprisingly, the positive
indicators loaded positively and the negative indicators changed direction and loaded negatively
on this factor. The factor loadings of the positive indicators were also stronger than the factor
loadings of the negative indicators. A reasonable conclusion is that this factor behaved as a
positive factor because of the strength of the positive indicators relative to the negative indicators.
In support of this conclusion, if the positive indicators were removed from the model one at a
time, the factor loadings on both the positive and negative indicators changed direction when
only one (any one) positive factor was left with the three negative indicators. Consequently, the
82
Because of the presence of the negative states as indicators, labeling this factor "eustress" is
inconsistent with the proposed theory. A neutral labeling for this new factor (e.g., stress
response) was not supported by the proposed theory,· and the performance of this factor in any
subsequent structural analyses would be difficult to interpret. Accordingly, this single second-
order factor with both positive and negative indicators was not subjected to any further analysis.
One alternative at this point was to abandon the investigation of second-order factors of the
stress response and to consider the seven latent variable indicators at the first-order level. This
was undesirable for two reasons. A reasonable conclusion is that this factor behaves as a positive
factor because of the strength of the positive indicators relative to the negative indicators,
implying that this factor should be labeled "eustress." Unfortunately, if the positive indicators are
removed from the model one at a time, the factor loadings on both the positive and negative
indicators change direction when only one (any one) positive factor is left with the three negative
problematic.
Since the second-order CFA failed to support two distinct second-order factors of eustress
and distress but instead favored a single second-order factor that behaved positively due to the
relatively poor performance of the negative indicators, a reasonable compromise at this point was
to simplify this second-order factor. If the negative indicators were removed from this factor,
leaving only positive indicators, this factor could be interpreted - consistent with theory - as
eustress.
Still, to remain consistent with Edwards and Cooper's (1988) recommendation that eustress
should not be considered in isolation of distress, at least one first-order level indicator of distress
must be retained for the structural analyses. Because NA has ten manifest indicators, if it was
selected as this single indicator of distress the number of estimated parameters in the structural
model would still exceed the sample size. If, however, either "anxiety" (six indicators) or "anger"
83
(three indicators) were selected to represent distress, then the number of estimated parameters
would be less than the sample size. "Anger" was selected because it had the smallest number of
indicators.
Although the decision to select "anger" instead of either "anxiety" or NA as a proxy for
distress was a practical one, the selection of "anger" as a proxy remains consistent with the
theoretical foundations of the model. Lazarus ( 1999) considers anger one of the core "nasty"
emotions. Because high levels of experienced anger have been found to interfere with inter-
behaviors (Martin et al., 1999), the variable "anger'' may have particular efficacy in a model that
Figure 13 presents the revised model that was subjected to structural analyses. Although
Hypothesis 1 was not fully supported, the remaining hypotheses proposed by the theory devel-
oped in this study were tested. In this model; "anger" acted as .a proxy for, rather than an
indicator of; distress. This measurement model had 145 estimated parameters, thereby stabilizing
84
Structural Analyses
A first-order CFA was conducted with the ten first-order latent variables of the revised model
and their 44 manifest indicators. All of the indicators loaded positively and significantly on their
intended latent variables. Examination of both the RMSEA (.058) and ECVI (12.87) statistics
indicated that the model represented a good fit to the data. The CFI was again relatively low at
.80, but that continued to be expected with such a complex model. As a basis for calculation of
10. The four latent variables indicators of eustress continued to "hang together" well. The
strongest correlation occurred between the variables "meaningfulness" and "manageability" (.90),
and the next highest correlation was between "meaningfulness" and "hope" (.76); consequently,
multicollinearity concerns were not warranted. The strongest correlation with the outcome
variable "health" occurred with the variable "hope" (.52); and the stressor of interest, "trust," was
most strongly correlated with "manageability" (.47). Two other stressors also found their
strongest correlations with "manageability," "workload" (-.57) and "role ambiguity" (-.64). The
directions of all the relationships were consistent with the hypotheses except for the variable
"death." The direction of the relationship between "death" and the positive indicators was
positive, and negative between "death" and "role ambiguity." The relationship between "death"
Before analyzing the complete structural model, a second baseline model was run that
incorporated the second-order factor "eustress" in order to obtain the zero-order correlations
between "eustress" and the other first-order variables in the model. These correlations are also
reported in Table 10. All four of the first-order indicators of "eustress" loaded positively and
significantly on the second-order factor. "Eustress" was significantly correlated with all of the
first-order variables, and all of the relationships were in the expected direction except the variable
85
"death," which is positive. "Eustress" was most strongly correlated with "role ambiguity" (-.70),
The complete structural model was submitted to LISREL 8.20 for analysis. In order to
identify the variable "eustress," the latent variable indicator "manageability" was used as a
reference variable (equation: manageability= 1*eustress). The effect of this was to fix the
pattern coefficient of the variable "manageability" for its loading on the second-order factor
"eustress" to 1.00. For identification purposes, the error variance of each of the exogenous
variables ("role ambiguity," "trust," "workload," "death/dying") was set to 1.0. Fixing the loading
of one of its manifest indicators to 1.0 identified each of the endogenous variables.
86
Table 10: Correlations Among Latent Variables Included in the Structural Model•
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Eustressh
1. Health 0.54*
2. Trust 0.07 0.60*
3. Hope 0.52* 0.23*
4. PA 0.20* 0.24* 0.45*
5. Meaningfulness 0.41 * 0.33* 0.76* 0.57*
6. Manageability 0.34* 0.47* 0.65* 0.45* 0.90*
7. Anger -0.06 -0.12 -0.21 * -0.30* -0.48* -0.49 -0.32*
8. Workload 0.03 -0.08 -0.09 -0.09 -0.23* -0.57* 0.47* -0.33*
00 9. Death 0.09 0.13 0.31 * 0.06 0.13 0.10 0.05 0.21* 0.22*
-...J
10. Role Ambiguity -0.28* -0.26* -0.60* -0.36* -0.56* -0.64* 0.26* 0.34 -0.05 -0.70*
* p < .05
• Correlations from the ten first-order latent variables were obtained from the measurement model.
b Correlations with eustress were obtained from a second baseline model.
Three additional models were run that in turn used "hope," "meaningfulness," and "PA" as the
reference variables for eustress. The results were the same regardless of which variable was used
as a reference, establishing the stability of the results. Subsequent structural models used
"manageability" as the reference variable. The SIMPLIS code for the structural model with
"manageability" as the reference for eustress is presented in Table 11. The factors, associated
items, and pattern coefficients for this model are presented in Table 12.
The analyses to this point confirmed the effectiveness of the measures used for the one
second-order and ten first-order factors included in the final structural model. Evaluation of the
subsequent structural models involved specifying the order and direction of hypothesized
relationships between these variables inan attempt to confirm whether the significance and
A series of competing structural models was run to test Hypotheses 2-:-6, and the results are
presented in Table 13. Model I was the hypothesized structural model. Model 2 allowed the
residual errors of "eustress" and "anger" to correlate, which tested the assumption that there are
other common predictors of eustress and anger that were not included in the model. Model 3
added a direct path from "trust" to "health" in Model 2 in order to test Hypothesis 6 and confirm
88
Table 11: SIMPLIS Code for Structural Model
ooserved variables:
HEALTHI HEALTH2 HEALTH3 HEALTH4
TRUSTl TRUST2 TRUST3 TRUST4 TRUSTS TRUST6
WORKl WORK2 WORK3 WORK4
DEATHl DEATH2 DEATH3
RAl RA2 RA3 RA4 RAS
MEAN1MEAN2MEAN3MEAN4
MANAGIMANAG2MANAG3MANAG4MANAG5
HOPE I HOPE2 HOPE3 HOPE4 HOPES HOPE6
ANGER! ANGER2 ANGER3
POSAIPOSA2POSA3POSA4POSA5POSA6POSA7POSA8POSA9POSA10
SPSS-DATA from file c:\spssdata\discov8.sav
sample size: 158
latent variables: Eustress Trust Health Mean Manag Hope Pa
Anger Roleam Work Death
equation: RAI = Roleam
equation: RA2 = Roleam
equation: RA3 = Roleam
equation: RA4 = Roleam
equation: RAS = Roleam
set the error variance ofRoleam to 1.0
equation: WORKI = Work
equation: WORK2 = Work
equation: WORK3 = Work
equation: WORK4 = Work
set the error variance of Work to 1.0
equation: DEATHI = Death
equation: DEATH2 = Death
equation: DEATH3 = Death
set the error variance of Death to 1.0
equation: TRUST6 = Trust
equation: TRUSTS = Trust
equation: TRUST4 = Trust
equation: TRUST3 = Trust
equation: TRUST2 = Trust
equation: TRUSTl = Trust
set the error variance of Trust to 1.0
equation: HEALTH4 = Health
equation: HEALTH3 = Health
equation: HEALTH2 = Health
equation: HEALTH!= I *Health
equation: MEAN I = Mean
equation: MEAN2 = 1*Mean
equation: MEAN3 = Mean
equation: MEAN4 = Mean
equation: MANAG 1 = Manag
equation: MANAG2 = I *Manag
equation: MANAG3 = Manag
89
equation: MANAG4 = Manag
equation: MANAGS = Manag
equation: HOPEl = 1*Hope
equation: HOPE2 = Hope
equation: HOPE3 = Hope
equation: HOPE4 = Hope
equation: HOPES = Hope
equation: HOPE6 = Hope
equation: POSAl = 1*Pa
equation: POSA2 = Pa
equation: POSA3 = Pa
equation: POSA4 = Pa
equation: POSAS = Pa
equation: POSA6 = Pa
equation: POSA7 = Pa
equation: POSAS = Pa
equation: POSA9 = Pa
equation: POSA 10 = Pa
equation: ANGER3 = 1*Anger
equation: ANGER2 = Anger
equation: ANGERl = Anger
equation: Health = Eustress Anger
equation: Eustress Anger = Trust Work Roleam Death
equation: Manag = 1*Eustress ·
equation: Hope = Eustress
equation: Pa = Eustress
equation: Mean = Eustress
iterations = 2000
admissibility check = off
lisrel output: sc ss
path diagram
end of problem
90
Table 12: Factors, Associated Items, and Pattern Coefficients for the Structural Model
Completely Completely
Standardized Standardized
Pattern Residuals 1
Coefficients
First Second
Factor/Item order order TE PS
Eustress 0.47**
Hope (a= .84) 0.82* 0.33*
definitely false (1} - definitely true (7}
If I should find myself in a jam, I could
HI 0.58 0.66
think of many ways to get out of it.
At the present time, I am energetically
H2 0.62 0.62
pursuing my goals.
There are lots of ways around any
H3 0.62 0.62
problem that I am facing now.
Right now, I see myself as being pretty
H4 0.69 0.52
successful.
I can think of many ways to reach my
HS 0.81 0.34
current goals.
At this time, I am meeting the goals I
H6 0.76 0.42
have set for myself.
Meaningfulness (a= .67) 0.94* 0.12
Do you have the feeling that you don't
MEI (R) really care about what goes on around 0.53 0.72
you: ((very seldom (1) - very often (7))
At work you have:
ME2 ((no clear goals or purpose(!)- very 0.58 0.67
clear goals and purpose (7))
When you think about your present situa-
tion at work, you very often: ((feel how
ME3 (R) 0.65 0.57
good it is to be alive ( 1) - ask yourself
why you exist at all (7))
How often do you have feelings that
there's little meaning in the things you do
ME4 in your daily work activities: ((always 0.54 0.71
have this feeling (1) - never have this
feeling (7))
91
Completely Completely
Standardized Standardized
Pattern Residuals 1
Coefficients -------------
First Second
Factor/Item order2 order TE PS
Eustress
Manageability (a= .60) 0.92* 0.15
When you think of the challenges you are
facing at work, do you feel that: ((you can
MAI 0.53 0.71
find a solution (1) - there is no solution
(7))
At work, do you have the feeling that you
MA2 are being treated unfairly: ((very often (1) 0.46 0.79
- very seldom ornever (7))
People you count on at work often disap-
MA3 point you: ((never happens (1) - always 0.36 0.87
happens (7))
At work, if you have to do something that
depends upon the cooperation of others,
MA4 do you have the feeling that it: ((surely 0.57 0.67
won't get done (1) - surely will get done
(7))
When you think about problems you are
currently having at work, do you tend to:
MAS 0.41 0.83
((keep worrying about it (1) - say "ok",
that's that, I have to live with it (7))
Positive Affect (PA) (a= .90) 0.55* 0.70*
Vea slightly or not at all (1) - extremely
ill
PAI Interested 0.74 0.45
PA2 Excited 0.69 0.53
PA3 Strong 0.56 0.68
PA4 Enthusiastic 0.81 0.35
PAS Proud 0.74 0.45
PA6 Alert 0.62 0.62
PA7 Inspired 0.79 0.38
PA8 Determined 0.61 0.63
PA9 Attentive 0.66 0.57
PAIO Active 0.62 0.61
Health (a= .87)
0.50* · 0.76*
definitely false (1) - definitely true (7)
According to the doctors I've seen, my
HEALTH I 0.78 0.39
health is now excellent.
HEALTH2 I feel better now than I ever have before. 0.63 0.61
HEALTH3 I'm as healthy as anybody I know. 0.86 0.26
HEALTH4 My health is excellent. 0.91 0.17
92
Completely
Standardized Completely
Pattern Standardized
Coefficients Residuals 1
First Second
Factor/Item order order TE T
Trust (a= .79)
strongly disagree (1) - strongly agree (7)
Ifl had my way, I wouldn't let my super-
TRUST1 (R) visor have any influence over issues that 0.48 0.77
are important to me.
I am comfortable discussing with my
TRUST2 supervisor my ideas for improvement in 0.72 0.48
the workplace.
I would be willing to let my supervisor
TRUST3 have complete control over my future in 0.29 0.92
this hospital.
I really wish I had a good way to keep an
TRUST4 (R) 0.48 0.77
eye on my supervisor.
I am comfortable discussing with my
TRUSTS supervisor concerns I have about our 0.82 0.32
working relationship.
I am comfortable discussing with my
TRUST6 supervisor concerns I have about my 0.81 0.35
ability to do my job.
Role Ambiguity (a= .80)
definitely false (I) - definitely true (7)
I feel certain about how much authority I
RAI 0.43 0.81
have.
I know I have allocated my work time
RA2 0.52 0.73
properly.
RA3 I know what my responsibilities are. 0.77 0.40
I know exactly what is expected of me at
RA4 0.92 0.15
work.
I understand what needs to be done at
RAS 0.8 0.36
work.
Workload (a= .68)
never (1) - veD:'. freguently (7)
Too many non-nursing tasks are required,
WORK.I 0.34 0.88
such as clerical work.
I don't have enough time to provide emo-
WORK.2 0.51 0.74
tional support to a patient.
I don't have enough time to complete all
WORK.3 0.7 0.51
of my nursing tasks.
There is not enough staff to adequately
WORK.4 0.77 0.39
cover the unit.
93
Completely Completely
Standardized Standardized
Pattern Residuals'
Coefficients
First Second
Factor/Item order2 order PS TE TD
Death (a= .67)
never (I) - vea freguently (7)
DEATHl I witness the death of a patient. 0.77 0.41
DEATH2 I observe a patient suffering. 0.56 0.69
I listen to a patient talking about his/her
DEATH3 0.59 0.65
approaching death.
Anger (a= .77) 0.13 0.78**
never (I) - vea freguently (7)
ANGERl I feel critical of others 0.61 0.62
ANGER2 I feel easily annoyed or irritated 1.03 -0.07
ANGER3 I have tem12er outbursts I cannot control 0.57 0.67
94
Table 13: Results of the Structural Analysis
df= 1,153
Hope 0.80*
PA 0.56* RMSEA = .060*
Eustress 0.19* 0.60* ECVI = 13.01
Anger -0.06 0.19 CFI = .79
Trust -0.13 T= .971
Workload -0.10 0.44*
Role Ambiguity -0.57* 0.09
Death 0.19* -0.03
95
Inspection of the fit indices for Model 1 indicated that the Model provided a reasonably good
fit to the data. The value RMSEA of .06 was significant and fell within a relatively tight 90
percent confidence interval ( .055 - .065). The ECVI value of 13 .07 was good and fell within its
90 percent confidence interval, and the CFI value of .79 was as good as any of the models
evaluated so far. The ratio ofx2 degrees of freedom was an acceptable 1.62, and T= .964.
All of the latent variable indicators of eustress loaded positively and significantly on the
second-order factor. Hypothesis 2 stated that the relationship between "trust" and "eustress"
would be positive and significant, and Hypothesis 3 stated that the relationship between "trust"
and "anger" (distress) would be negative and significant. Inspection of the factor loadings
showed that the relationship between "trust" and "eustress" was positive and significant,
providing support for Hypothesis 2. The relationship between "trust" and "anger" was negative
and nonsignificant, so Hypothesis 3 is not supported. Hypothesis 4 stated that the relationship
between "eustress" and "health" would be positive and significant, while Hypothesis 5 stated that
the relationship between "distress" and "health" would be negative and significant. The
relationship between "eustress" and "health" was positive and significant, providing support for
Hypothesis 4. Hypothesis 5 was not supported because the relationship between "anger" and
"health" was not significant and was unexpectedly positive. It is unclear why the direction of the
relationship between "health" and "anger" changed in the structural model, because the zero-
order correlation between these two variables, while still non-significant, was negative as
expected.
Model 2 allowed the residual errors of "eustress" and "anger" to correlate to test the assump-
tion that there might be other common predictors of eustress and anger not included in the model.
The correlation between the residuals of "eustress" and "anger" was significant (-.23). All of the
unchanged in this model. The fit statistics were nearly identical with Model 1, with a slight
96
improvement of Tto .97. A x2 difference test between Models 2 and l (10.82, ldf) favors the
more complex Model 2 that allows the residual errors of eustress and anger to correlate.
The presence of correlated residuals between "eustress" and "anger" suggested that there are
other significant common predictors of these variables (i.e., stressors) that were not included in
the present model. Because of the strength of the relationship between eustress and health, it is
unlikely that the inclusion of additional stressors as control variables would affect the
significance of this relationship. Yet because the relationship between "trust" and "eustress" was
marginally significant (t = L96), the inclusion of additional stressors might affect the significance
of this relationship.
The final hypothesis is a test that satisfies James et al. 's (1982) condition number 10, which
requires that constrained parameters are confirmed to be zero. In this study, the only
theoretically significant path hypothesized to be zero was between "trust" and "health." To test
Hypothesis 6, Model 3 was evaluated including an extra path between "trust" and "health." The
results of this model are also found in Table 13. The path between "trust" and "health" was
negative and not significant, and there was no change in .the significant paths from Model 1.
Although it was not hypothesized, the direction of the relationship between "trust" and "health"
would have been reasonably expected to be positive. Interestingly, the strength of the positive
relationship between eustress and health actually increased in this model. While the fit statistics
between Models 2 and 3 were nearly identical, a "i difference test between the two models ( 1.91,
1df) favored the more parsimonious Model 2, thereby providing additional support for
Hypothesis 6, which confirms a nonsignificant direct relationship between trust and health. A
97
Table 14: Summary of Hypotheses Tests
HI: The relationship between the separate Partially Supported: A two-factor second-
constructs of eustress and distress is order model was confirmed but did not fit the
nonrecursive and negative in both data as well as a one-factor model. The
directions. hypothesized nonrecursive relationship could
not be tested
H3: There is a negative relationship Not Supported: The single variable "anger"
between trust in the supervisor and served as a proxy for distress.
distress.
98
CHAPTERS
This dissertation demonstrated that the positive stress response may best be represented by a
higher-order construct, eustress, and also demonstrated the effectiveness of several established
justification for a similar higher-order construct for distress was provided, the empirical
justification for this construct was not established in this study. The positive psychological states
used as indicators of eustress in this study were much more effective in establishing this construct
than the negative psychological states were at establishing distress. As a result, the full two-
factor model of eustress and distress could not be tested. Although the hypothesized relationship
between eustress and distress could not be tested, the other substantive relationships in the model
were tested by using the first-order variable "anger" as a proxy for distress while retaining the
It was demonstrated that trust in the supervisor was a legitimate interpersonal demand in this
sample of hospital nurses in that it was sufficient to elicit a significant response even in the
presence of other significant stressors. Trust in the supervisor was positively related to eustress,
but the negative relationship between trust and the single indicator for distress was not signifi-
work represented by the positive psychological states used to represent eustress. The willingness
to be vulnerable related a negative but nonsignificant level of anger and hostility in this sample of
nurses. Because this study was unable to demonstrate that the interpersonal demand represented
by trust produces a significant negative stress response while simultaneously producing a positive
99
Another important finding of this study was the strong positive relationship between eustress
and the perception of health in this sample of nurses. The only indicator other than the four
positive psychological states and eustress that had a significant zero-order correlation with health
was "role ambiguity" (-.28). The variable "anger" had significant zero-order correlations with
eustress (-.32) and the stressors "workload" (.47) and "role ambiguity" (.26). Contrary to
expectations, the relationship between anger (distress) and health in the structural model was not
significant. Although a strong positive relationship of .43 between "workload" and "anger" was
confirmed in the structural model, this distress response was not strong enough in this sample of
nurses to elicit a significant effect in their perception of health. Finally, the direct relationship
While all the zero-order correlations between health and the four indicators of eustress were
significant, none was high enough to elicit concerns about multicollinearity. The strongest zero-
order correlation with health occurred with the variable "eustress" (.54) and its indicator "hope"
(.52), and the weakest occurred with the variable "positive affect" (.20). "Role ambiguity" had a
strong zero-order correlation with both eustress (-.70) and its indicator "hope" (-.60), and this
stressor was significantly correlated with every other variable in the study except the stressors
"death" and "workload." This finding suggests a potential specific link between role ambiguity
and health through the eustress of hope. While the focus of this study was on the relationship
between trust and eustress, these findings suggest that to promote eustress, supervisory efforts
may be more effectively directed at clarifying roles for employees than in building trust.
Although not part of the formal hypotheses, the relationships between the stressors used as
controls and eustress and anger are also interesting. The expected direction of the relationship
between these stressors should be negative for "eustress" and positive for "anger." The variable
"workload" performed in the directions expected but was only significant with "anger." This
variable was more a measure of how effectively the individual could focus on the essential tasks
100
at work than a measure of the amount of workload. "Role ambiguity" also performed in the
expected directions, but was only significant with "eustress." Contrary to expectations, the
variable "death/dying" had a significant, positive relationship with "eustress" and a nonsignifi-
cant, negative relationship with "anger." When the nurses in this study were faced with the
demand of dealing with death/dying in their patients, they apparently became significantly more
Contributions to Research
The modeling of eustress and distress as second-order variables using established positive
and negative psychological states as indicators had not been previously attempted. The primary
explanation for this is the state of theory development concerning the concept of eustress and its
relationship to distress. This study advanced the theory of both eustress and distress and
Cooper (1988).
The theory of eustress was advanced by developing support for the hypothesis that eustress is
a psychological state that represents a degree of positive response to any given demand. This is a
break from the common approach in the literature to view eustress and distress as opposite ends
of a continuum and to treat eustress as simply the absence of distress. The theory presented here
supports a view that eustress and distress are separate positive and negative constructs repre-
senting the response to a demand, and exposure to any work demand should simultaneously
The contrast between the view presented in this dissertation and the more common view is
captured in a very recent article by Folkman and Moskowitz (2000). As this study does, they
accept the fact that positive and negative affect can co-occur during a stressful period of time. In
contrast to the theory presented here, they suggest that positive and negative responses are
produced by different events (stressors). In contrast to this study, the effects of the positive
101
response are viewed as a coping strategy, a way to adapt to distress and its negative effects.
While it is encouraging to see attention given to the effects of positive psychological states, this
approach simply reinforces the prevailing primacy of distress and the associated psychology of
pathology. A legitimate positive psychology will shift from an exclusive focus on pathology to
the independent and direct effects ofpositive psychological states on important indicators of
well-being as well as disease (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). The theory of eustress
positive psychology.
The importance of studying eustress lies in its relationship to health. Similar to stress, health
has also been viewed as a unidimensional construct representing the absence of the negative, that
being disease. Separating the positive and negative stress responses and examining their
independent effects on health presented a more holistic model of stress in this study.
The relationship between eustress and health had been suggested but not often assessed in the
literature. For example, a recent review of the literature stated that "positive emotional states may
promote healthy perceptions, beliefs, and physical well-being itself'' (Salovey, Rothman,
Detweiler, & Steward, 2000, p. 110, emphasis added). This study provided a much-needed
health.
This study limited its definition of the stress response to psychological states. Theoretically,
any positive psychological state could serve as an indicator of a degree of positive response. The
positive psychological states used in this study as indicators of eustress were selected for their
ability to represent an aspect of engagement in work, which the literature suggested was an
102
The positive psychological states examined in this study appear to be good indicators of
affect" were all significantly correlated with each other, but not so much as to warrant concerns
about multicollinearity. Each of theses indicators also loaded significantly on the second-order
factor eustress. Accordingly, they may be good indicators of the state of active engagement in
work commonly associated with eustress. With the exception of positive affect, each of these
indicators makes its debut inthe work stress literature in this study. The indicator with the
strongest factor loading on eustress was "meaningfulness," which was defined as the extent to
which one feels that work makes sense emotionally, that problems and demands are worth
investing energy in, are worthy of commitment and engagement, and are challenges that are
welcome.
The state of active engagement represented by eustress was most negatively associated with
"role ambiguity." This suggests that future research on eustress in hospital nurses should
continue to account for the effect of role ambiguity, especially if indicators of engagement are
used as in this study. Even if the effect of role ambiguity is not of interest, it should be included
An extension to our knowledge of trust also emerged from this study. This was the first study
to suggest that trust in the supervisor was a significant interpersonal demand that could elicit a
stress response in an individual. The significant relationship between trust and the positive stress
studying trust in the workplace. Trust in the supervisor, senior management, and coworkers
especially in studies of eustress. Because the findings also suggested that significant predictors
of eustress and anger were not included in the model, the efficacy of trust as a stressor requires
verification.
103
This study also provided an additional test of the Mayer and Davis.(1999) trust scale
developed to measure trust as an attitude rather than as trustworthiness (an attribution). This is an
important new scale that has yet to gain widespread acceptance. With slight modifications to
reflect the sample of nurses in this study, the scale performed adequately. "Trust" exhibited a
significant positive zero-order correlation with all of the positive indicators. The strongest
correlation was with the variable "manageability" ( .4 7), which operationalized the extent to
which the nurses in the sample perceived that resources at their disposal were adequate to meet
the demands of their work. "Trust" was significantly correlated with only one stressor, "role
ambiguity" (-.26). Because of the strength of the relationship between "role ambiguity" and
The holistic model of stress presented in this study is an important departure from established
models of stress. The development of a model of stress that incorporates eustress is important
because there may be little incremental understanding we can gain from the simplistic model of
stress, distress, and disease. The comprehensive hypothesized holistic model of eustress and
distress as second-order constructs in a reciprocal relationship was strongly suggested by the data
but not confirmed. Two separate higher-order factors were confirmed as hypothesized, but the
two-factor model failed a significance test against a unidimensional model. The higher-order
factor "eustress" was incorporated in the structural model, and it behaved exactly as
hypothesized.
There is every reason to believe that the holistic model of stress proposed by this study will
be confirmed in future studies that are able to incorporate better indicators of distress (e.g., burn-
out, psychosomatic symptoms) and overcome the sample size limitations of the present study.
When these limitations are overcome and eustress and distress can be modeled in a reciprocal
relationship as hypothesized, it is possible that the correlation of the residual errors between these
factors that was found in this model will become nonsignificant. This is another way to say that
104
"missing stressors" suggested by the correlated error terms may be the effect that eustress and
There are several benefits to modeling the stress response as two separate higher-order
factors. First, it is the most convincing way to demonstrate the hypothesized existence of two
separate but correlated stress responses. A theoretical case for the existence of eustress could be
logical states and health, but the empirical case can only begin to be established with the use of
higher-order factor models. A strong case for the eustress and distress will be established if
future studies continue to establish the existence of underlying factars that cause a variety of
Second, since there is no established scale to measure eustress, the use of established positive
empirical validity of eustress and examine its behavior in relation to other variables. Any future
study that incorporates this approach will be faced with the same issues of sample size and model
complexity that proved challenging in this study, and structural equation modeling will continue
to be the most effective data analytic approach. Yet once a set of positive psychological states
has been consistently demonstrated to be the result of a higher-order factor, these states can be
examined individually as proxies of eustress. At that point, the more common data analytic
strategies should attract more researchers to the concept of eustress, thereby expanding knowl-
Contributions to Practice
For health care administrators recognizing that healthy workers are more productive workers,
this study suggested that workers can have a positive response to even the most demanding work
environments, and this positive response is significantly related to employee health. The nursing
profession is a demanding profession by nature, but there were characteristics about this sample
105
of nurses that suggested possibly greater than normal demands. All of the nurses in this sample
worked in hospitals that faced the threat of unionization by nurses. This suggests the possibility
of a strong degree of dissatisfaction with and hostility toward the working conditions and
management relations from the nurses in these hospitals. Approximately half of the nurses in this
sample worked more than 40 hours per week. Half of the sample of nurses in this study worked
in critical or intensive care units - environments more demanding than most workers will ever
encounter.
Despite the demands of their work situation, the nurses reported a high degree of eustress as
indicated by the positive psychological states hope, meaningfulness, manageability, and positive
affect. The nurses in this study remained actively engaged in their work, and this positive·
response to the demands they faced showed a significantrelationship to their own well-being.
The stressor "workload," which indicated the individual's ability focus on the essential aspects of
their work, found its only positive association with "anger" (.43). This suggests that the inability
to focus on the essential tasks of the job made the nurses angry about their situation, but it did not
detract from their active and pleasurable engagement in this most demanding work.
In this sample of nurses, the stressor with the strongest negative relationship to eustress was
"role ambiguity" (-.57). While this study did not present a case for eustressor management,
hospital administrators and other managers should not overlook the negative effect of role
ambiguity. Consideration of the indicators of eustress used in this study results in more specific
For example, consider the variable "hope." The ability to generate hope among an organiza-
tion's members may be particularly important during radical change efforts, which the nurse in
this sample were probably facing. When people believe that their actions will lead to positive
results, they may be more willing to accept difficult and uncertain challenges. Health care
administrators can generate hope by establishing goals that are meaningful to all members,
106
allocating the organizational resources necessary for individuals to excel at their jobs and
maintaining a frequent and inspirational dialogue with their constituents (Huy, 1999).
. .
An example of an organizational resource that may be important for generating hope is
information. In this study, "role ambiguity" had a significant negative zero-order correlation with
"hope" (-.26), which suggests that efforts to decrease role ambiguity should have a positive effect
on hope. Role ambiguity is the confusion a person experiences related to not understanding what
is expected, not knowing how to perform or change to meet new expectations, or not knowing the
consequences of failing to meet expectations (Nelson & Quick, 2000). Relationships between
nurses and their supervisors that are open and supportive can reduce role ambiguity and increase
the satisfaction experienced by nurses (Gray-Toft & Anderson, 1985). Health care administrators
who are easily accessible, actively share information regarding current as well as evolving
expectations with their constituents, and encourage their management staff to do the same should
establish a solid foundation for the generation of hope by lessening role ambiguity.
In addition to lessening role ambiguity, this type of open and supportive relationship with
subordinates would also facilitate the development of trust. Supervisors who listen to and share
information with subordinates may be more likely than those who don't to have their subordi-
nates make a favorable attribution concerning their benevolence toward them. This attribution is
essential in order for an attitude of trust to develop within a subordinate toward a supervisor. As
demonstrated in this study, trust in the supervisor appears to play a significant role in the
development of eustress.
While this may be one of the first studies to examine the role of eustress at work, the state of
active engagement in work represented by this construct merits additional consideration from
both health care administrators and other managers. Health care administrators should recognize
that the interpretation of and response to work demands could be positive as well as negative.
Accordingly, administrators and employees alike would benefit from programs designed to
107
identify and encourage engagement, or eustress, at work. In this sense, we would be moving
An initial step in this effort might be to identify which aspects of the work employees find
most engaging, and then more importantly identify why individuals find the work pleasurable and
what administrators could do to enhance the positive aspects of the work experience. As a
specific example with respect to meaningfulness, why do they feel that work makes sense
emotionally, that problems and demands are worth investing energy in, are worthy of commit-
ment and engagement, and are challenges that are welcome? By doing so, administrators would
be demonstrating that their employees are worth investing energy in, are worthy of commitment
and engagement from managers, and that the challenges of improving the employee's work
experience are welcome. Administrators should also explore opportunities to improve policies,
procedures, and the physical work environment in an effort to enhance the employee's exposure
to work that they find engaging and to eliminate potential impediments to eustress.
Managers should not overlook the strong positive relationship between eustress and health
demonstrated by this study. This may be particularly important in a sample of health care
.
workers because the health of the health care worker can directly impact the delivery of care. In
order for the nurse to deliver the optimum service, she must herself be healthy. Because nurses
provide the majority of the care for hospital patients and the perception of how they are treated
while in the hospital is as important to patients as the technical quality of the care, nurses that
respond positively to the demands of the job can have a substantial impact on the image of a
Additionally, the health care worker plays a particularly important role in inspiring hope in
others. By raising a patient's level of hope, the health care professional's positive expectations
can have a concrete impact on the health of the patient. "The positive mood experience that
108
comes from a renewal of hope, particularly among those who are struggling with illness, reaps
Limitations
The most important limitations of this study were sample size, fit of the model, performance
of the indicators of distress, and design of the study. While adequate for simpler models
involving more standard data analysis techniques, the resulting sample size of 158 imposed
several restrictions on the analysis of the data and must be strongly considered in any interpreta-
tion of the results. The comprehensive hypothesized model prior to theory trimming was very
complex, containing more estimated parameters than observations, which tends to make LISREL
models unstable. A sample size of at least 225 was required to conduct a CF A containing all the
variables in the comprehensive hypothesized model. The "theory trimming" accomplished by the
first CFA conducted in this study represented a necessary compromise in order to obtain a model
Although the model subjected to LISREL structural analysis estimated fewer parameters than
the sample size, the resulting fit indices suggest that sample size also had a significant impact on
model fit. Although RMS EA, ECVI, and Tall indicated acceptable fit of the final structural
model, the CFI indicated a substantial lack of fit. This measure of fit is sensitive to both small
sample size and model complexity, and both of these were significant factors in this study.
Examination of the modification indices for the final model also indicated that some of the
manifest indicators might have been significantly correlated, which would also contribute to
problems with fit. These conditions are not entirely unexpected in a complex model with many
variables, even using established measures. The lack of parsimonious model fit is the primary
reason why the results of this study must be interpreted with caution.
The weak performance of the indicators of distress employed in this study may be an
important reason why the hypothesized second-order factor of distress that was confirmed along
109
with eustress in the first CFA lost its significance in a test against a unidimensional model.
Although significantly correlated with other variables in the model, the four negative psycho-
logical states did not correlate well with each other; and job alienation was only significantly
correlated with PA. Because most of the best indicators of distress are proprietary, which is why
they were not selected for use in this study, with the exception of NA, these indicators all
represented something of a compromise in the initial design of the study. While the use of
multiple proprietary indicators of negative psychological states in studies with large sample sizes
may discourage replication, it may be the only way to persuasively establish the second-order
In addition to the indicators of distress, the design of the study had several other shortcomings
that have been previously acknowledged but merit further consideration. Care has been taken
when discussing the results of the study not to imply the demonstration of causal links among the
significant constructs. Because of the lack of true random sampling, cross-sectional design, and
the use of a single informant, the best that can be said is that in this sample of nurses, there
limitation, because the establishment of causal inferences was never a goal of this somewhat
exploratory study.
Although the stressors employed in this study were spe.cific to nurses, the theory of eustress,
distress, and their relationship to health are legitimate in all populations of workers. Consider-
ations of sample size and design not withstanding, the generic nature of the theory of stress
response and the strong positive relationship between eustress and health suggest that this
Finally, the significance of the correlated residual error terms of eustress and anger suggests
that important common predictors of eustress and anger were not hypothesized and included in
the model. The most significant effect of this limitation is that the addition of an additional
110
significant stressor in this model might have had an effect on the marginally significant relation-
ship between trust and eustress. This "missing stressor'' may have been the reciprocal relation-
ship between eustress and distress, which was hypothesized but could not be tested.
Future studies of eustress would benefit by developing the theory for additional psychological
indicators of eustress and including additional indicators of distress (e.g., burnout) and significant
outcome variables (e.g., performance). Once the hypothesized two-factor structure of eustress
and distress is demonstrated, additional behavioral and physiological in.dicators of eustress and
The differences and similarities between the engagement of eustress and familiar concepts
such as satisfaction, involvement, and self-efficacy will also require empirical examination.
should be anticipated that there might be some that are skeptical that eustress does not differ
significantly from job satisfaction. If the variable "hope: is retained as an indicator of eustress, it
would be beneficial to include "self-efficacy" in the design of the study in order to differentiate
these two goal-directed attitudes. The state of active engagement represented by eustress will
also require differentiation from the concept ofinvolvement from the literature on job design.
Because initial studies should continue to focus on the stress response, this stimulus-centered
research should be reserved to later studies. Before moving forward to identify the antecedents
of eustress, it must first be persuasively established that this construct is indeed separate and
distinct from distress. Better data collection methods (e.g., multiple informants) and additional
dependent variables (e.g., performance) would also be necessary to begin to demonstrate that
Conclusions about the relationship between the stress response and health may be best estab-
lished through studies using a longitudinal design and exploring the potential nonrecursive
111
relationships that may be present (e.g.,· between stress and health; between eustress and distress).
In addition to modeling the positive and negative aspects of the stress response, future studies
should also attempt to incorporate models of health that simultaneously include the presence of
well-being as well as the absence of disease. The truly holistic model of the stress response will
include multiple psychological, behavioral, and physiological indicators of eustress and distress,
indicators of well-being and disease, additional outcome variables that capture the results of
Finally, to complement the well-developed modelof distress prevention (Quick et al., 2000),
a model of eustress generation guided by research results will need to be developed to inform
practicing managers how to recognize and facilitate the positive stress response in their
employees. The wealth of accumulated knowledge on distress will shorten the time required to
develop the understanding of eustress required to develop such a model. The temptation will be
to take what we know about distress and assume that the eustress response will be the exact
opposite. This would reduce eustress to the polar opposite, or absence of distress, and would be
Several recent studies of the happy-productive worker hypothesis provide additional support
for the need to develop a model of eustress generation. One study found that a pleasantness-
based measure of dispositional affect predicted rated job performance, although the same was not
true of positive affect in this study (Wright & Staw, 1999). A second set of studies found that
psychological well-being was predictive of job performance for 47 human service workers
the absence of the negative (e.g., how often have you felt depressed or very unhappy), again
supporting the prevailing primacy of distress as other recent studies attempting to shift to a
112
Conclusion
This study provided a much-needed investigation of the positive stress response, eustress, and
its relationship to distress, trust, and health. Exploring the concept of eustress presents a unique
opportunity to bring fresh insight to both stress research and management practice. The nature,
causes and consequences of eustress at work, as well as the complex interactions potentially
present in this holistic model, should keep both researchers and managers engaged in pleasurable
113
REFERENCES
Allen, R.J. (1983). Human stress: Its nature and control. Minneapolis, MN: Burgess Publishing
Company.
Anderson, J.C., & Gerbing, D.W. (1988). Structural equationmodeling in practice: A review
and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103: 411-423.
Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unraveling the mystery of health: How people manage stress and stay
well. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Artinian, B.M. (1997). Situational sense of coherence: Development and measurement of the
construct. In B.M. Artinian and M.M. Conger (Eds.), The intersystem model: Integrating
theory and practice. (pp. 18-30). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Avolio, B.J., Bass, B.M., & Jung, D.I. (1999). Re-examining the components of transforma-
tional and transactional leadership using the Multifactor Leadership Questionnaire. Journal
of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 72: 441-462.
Baldwin, M.W. (1992). Relational schemas and the processing of social information. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 112: 461-484.
Baum, A. & Singer, J.E. (1987). Handbook of Psychology and Health. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Beck, A.T., Brown, G., Steer, R.A., Eidelson, J.I., & Riskind, J.H. (1987). Differentiating
anxiety and depression: A test of the cognitive content-specificity hypothesis. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 96: 179-183.
Bentler, P.M., & Chou, C.P. (1987). Practical issues in structural equation modeling. Sociologi-
cal Methods and Research, 16: 78-117.
Bieliauskas, L.A. ( 1982). Stress and its relationship to health and illness. Boulder, Colorado:
Westview Press.
Bollen, K.A. & Long, S. (1993). Introduction. In K.A. Bollen and S. Long (Eds.) Testing
Structural Equation Models. (pp. 1-9 ). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss volume 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books. 2nd
ed.
114
Bunting, C.J., Little, M.J., Tolson, H., & Jessup, G. (1986). Physical fitness and eustress in the
adventure activities of rock climbing and rappelling. Journal of Sports Medicine, 26: 11-29.
Butler, J.K. (1991). Toward understanding and measuring conditions of trust: Evolution of a
conditions of trust inventory. Journal of Management, 17: 643-664.
Byrne, B.M. (1998). Structural equation modeling with LISREL, PREUS, and SIMPLIS: Basic
concepts, applications, and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
Cacioppo, J.T., & Berntson, G.G. (1994). Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space:
A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates.
Psychological Bulletin, 115: 401-423.
Cannon, W.B. (1929) .. Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear, and rage (2°d ed.}. New York:
Appleton.
Cannon, W.D. (1935). Stresses and strains of homeostasis. American Journal of Medical
Sciences, 189: 1-14.
Cacioppo, J.T. & Berntson, G.G. (1994). Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A
critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates.
Psychological Bulletin, 115: 401-423.
Cox, T., & Leitter, M. (1992). The health of health care organizations. Work & Stress, 6: 219-
227.
Crampton, S.M., & Wagner, J.A., III. (1994). Percept-percept inflation in microorganizational
research: An investigation of prevalence and effect. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79: 67-
76.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper
&Row.
Derogatis, L.R., Lipman, R.S., Covi, L., Rickels, K., & Uhlenhuth, E.H. (1970). Dimensions of
outpatient neurotic pathology: Comparision of a clinical versus an empirical assessment.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54: 164-17.1.
Diener, E. & Emmons, R.A. (1984). The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal
of Personality and 'Social Psychology, 4 7: 1105-1117.
Doty, D.H., & Glick, W.H. (1998). Common methods bias: Does common methods variance
really bias results? Organizational Research Methods, 1: 374-406.
Eagly, A.H. & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth, Texas: Harcourt
Brace Jovanovich College Publishers.
Edwards, J.R. & Cooper, C.L. (1988). The impacts of positive psychological states on physical
health: A review and theoretical framework. Social Science Medicine, 27, (12): 1147-1459.
115 J
Elkman, P., Levenson, R.W., & Friesen, W.V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity
distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221: 1208-1210.
Farmer, S.M., Maslyn, J.M., Fedor, D.B., & Goodman, J.S. (1997). Putting upward influence
strategies in context. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18: 17-42.
Folkins, C.H. (1970). Temporal factors and the cognitive mediators of stress reaction. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 14: 173-184.
Folkman, (1997). Positive psychological states and coping with severe stress. Social Science
Medicine, 45: 1207-1221.
Folkman, S. & Lazarus, R.S. (1985). If it changes it must be a process: Study of emotion and
coping during three stages of a college examination. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 48: 150-170.
Folkman, S., Lazarus, R.S., Dunkel-Schetter, C., DeLongis, A., & Gruen, R.J. (1986). Dynamics
of stressful encounter: Cognitive appraisal, coping, and encounter outcomes. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 50: 992-1003.
Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J.T. (2000). Positive affect and the other side of coping. American
Psychologist, 55: 647-654.
Fox, M.L., Dwyer, D.J., & Ganster, D.C. (1993). Effects of stressful job demands and control on
physiological and attitudinal outcomes in a hospital setting. Academy of Management
Journal, 36: 289-318. ·
Frankenhauser, M. (1981 ). Coping with stress at work. International Journal of Health Services,
l.l.;. 491-510.
Frankenhauser, M., Von Wright, M.R., Collins, A., Von Wright, J., Sedvall, G., and Swahn, C.G.
(1978). Sex differences in psychoendocrine reactions to examination stress. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 40: 334-343.
116
Ganster, D.C. & Schaubroeck, J. (1991). Work stress and employee health. Journal of Manage-
ment 17, 235-271.
George, J.M., & Brief, AP. (1992). Feeling good-doing good: A conceptual analysis of mood at
work-organizational spontaneity relationship. Psychological Bulletin, 112: 310-329.
Gerbing, D.W.,Hamilton, J.G., & Freeman, E.B. (1994). A large-scale second-order structural
equation model of the influence of management participation on organizational planning
benefits. Journal of Management 20: 859-885.
Gray, J.A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress (2nd edition). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Gray, J.A. (1991). Neural systems, emotion, and personality. In J. Madden (Ed.) Neurobiology
of learning, emotion, and affect. (pp. 273-306). New York: Raven.
Gray-Toft, P., & Anderson, J.G. (1981a). Stress among hospital nursing staff: Its causes and
effects~ Social Science Medicine, 15: 639-64 7.
Gray-Toft, P., & Anderson, J.G. (1981b). The nursing stress scale: Development of an instru-
ment. Journal of Behavioral Assessment, 3: 11-23.
Gray-Toft, P.A., & Anderson, J.G. (1985). Organizational stress in the hospital: Development of
a model for diagnosis and prediction. Health Services Research, 19: 753-774.
Hackman, J.R., & Oldman, G.R. (1975). Development of the job diagnostic survey. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 60: 159-170.
Hair, J.F., Jr., Anderson, R.E., Tatham, R.L., & Black, W.C. (1995). Multivariate data analysis:
Fourth Edition, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Hay, D., & Oken, D. (1972). The psychological stresses of intensive care unit nursing. Psycho-
somatic Medicine, 34: 109-118. ·
Hayduk, L.E. ( 1987). Structural eguation modeling with LIS REL: Essentials and advances.
Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Hazen, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52: 511-524.
Heatherton, T.F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a scale for measuring self-
esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology: 895-910.
Henning, J., Laschefski, U., & Opper, C. (1994). Biopsychological changes after bungee
jumping: p-endorphine immunoreactivity as a mediator of euphoria? Neuropsychobiology,
29: 28-32.
Hoes, M.J. (1986). Stress and strain: Their definition, psychobiology and relationship to
psychosomatic medicine. Journal of Orthomolecular Medicine, 1, 30-38.
117 ,
Holmes, J.G. (1991). Trust and the appraisal process in close relationships. In W.H. Jones & D.
Perlman (Eds.). Advances in personal relationships: Volume 2. London: Kingsley.
Horowitz, M., Adler, N, & Kegeles, S. (1988). A scale for measuring the occurrence of positive
states of mind: a preliminary report. Psychosomatic Medicine, 50: 477-483.
Hurrell, J,J., Jr., Nelson, D.L., & Simmons, B.L. (1998). Measuring job stressors and strains:
Where we have been, where we are, and where we need to go. Journal of Occupational
Health Psychology, 3: 368-389. ·
Huy, Q.N. (1999). Emotional capability, emotional intelligence, and radical change. Academy of
Management Review, 24: 325-345.
James, L.R., Muliak, S., & Brett, J. (1982). Causal analysis: Assumptions, models, and data.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. ·
Joreskog, K. & Sorbom, D. (1993}. LISREL 8: Structural equation modeling with the SIMPLIS
command language. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
Kahn, R.L. & Byosiere, P. (1992). Stress in organizations. In M.D. Dunnette and L.M. Hough
(Eds.) Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology, Volume 3, (pp. 571-650).
Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.
Keesler, R.C., Magee, W.J., & Nelson, C.B. (1996). Analysis of psychosocial stress. In H.B.
Kaplan (Ed.), Psychosocial stress: Perspectives on structure, theoi:y, life-course, and
methods, (pp. 333-366). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Kelloway, E.K. (1998). Using LISREL for structural equation modeling: A researcher's guide.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Lane, R.D., Reiman, E.M,, Bradley, M.M., Lang, P.J., Ahem, G.L., Davidson, R.J., & Schwartz,
G.E. (1997). Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion. Neuro-
psycholgia, 35Neuropsycholgia, 35: 1437-1444.
Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Lazarus, R.S. (1993). From psychological stress to the emotions: A history of changing
outlooks. In L.W. Porter & M.R. Rosenzweig (Eds.). Annual Review of Psychology,
Volume 44. (pp. 1-21). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews Inc.
Lazarus, R.S. (1999). Stress and emotion: A new synthesis. New York: Springer Publishing
Company.
118
Lazarus, R.C., DeLongis, A., Folkman, S., & Gruen, R. (1985). Stress and adaptational
outcomes: The problem of confounded measures. American Psychologist. 40: 770-779.
Lazarus, R.S. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress. appraisal. and coping. New York: Springer
Publishing Company.
Lazarus, R.S. & Folkman, S. (1986). Cognitive theories of stress and the issue of circularity. In
M.H. Appley and R. Trumbull (Eds.). Dynamics of stress: physiological, psychological, and
social perspectives. (pp. 63-80). New York: Plenum Press.
Loehlin, J.C. (1998). Latent variable models: An introduction to factor. path. and structural
analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
Lovallo, W.R. (1997). Stress & Health: Biological and psychological interactions. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Lovallo, W.R., Pincomb, G.A., Wilson, M.F. (1986). Predicting response to a reaction time task:
Heart rate reactivity compared with Type A behavior. Psychophysiology, 23: 648-656.
Lovallo, W.R., Wilson, M.F., Pincomb, G.A., Edwards, G.L., Tompkins, P., & Brackett, D.
(1985). Activation patterns to aversive stimulation in man: Passive exposure versus effort to
control. Psychophysiology. 22: 283-291.
Marsh, H.W., & Hocevar, D. (1985). Application of confirmatory factor analysis to the study of
self-concept: First- and higher order factor models and their invariance across groups.
Psychological Bulletin. 97: 562-582.
Martin, R., Wan, C.K., David, J.P., Wegner, E.L., Olson, B.D., & Watson, D. (1999). Style of
anger expression: Relation to expressitivy, personality, and health. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin. 25: 1196-1207.
Mason, J.W. (1968a). The scope of psychoendocrine research. Psychosomatic Medicine, 5: 565-
575.
Mason, J.W. (1974). Specificity in the organization response profiles. In P. Seeman & G.
Brown (Eds.), Frontiers in neurology and neuroscience research. Toronto: University of
Toronto.
Mason, J.W. (1975). A historical view of the field: Part II. Journal of Human Stress, 2: 22-36.
119
Mayer, R.C., Davis, J.H., & Schoorman, F.D. (1995). An integrative model of organizational
trust. Academy of Management Review, 3: 709-734.
Mayer, R.C., & Davis, J.H. (1999). The effect of the performance appraisal system on trust for
management: A field quasi-experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84: 123-136.
McDonald, R.P. (1985). Factor analysis and related methods. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
McGrath, J.E. (1976). Stress and behavior in organizations. In M. Dunnette (Ed.) Handbook of
Industrial and Organizational Psychology. (pp. 1351-1395). Chicago: Rand McNally.
Miller, N.E. (1948). Theory and experiment relating psychoanalytic displacement to stimulus-
response generalization. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 43: 155-178.
Miller, N.E. (1961). Some recent studies on conflict behavior and drugs. American Psycholo-
gist, 16: 12-24.
Milsum, J.H. (1985). A model of the eustress system for health/illness. Behavioral Science, 30:
179-186.
Nelson, D.L., & Quick, J.C. (2000). Organizational behavior: foundations, realities, and
challenges. 3rd edition. Cincinnati, OH: South-Western College Publishing.
Nomikos, M.S., Opton, E.M., Jr., AveriH, J.R., & Lazarus, RS. (1968). Surprise versus
suspense in the production of stress reaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
8: 204-208.
Nunnally, J.C., & Bernstein, I.H. (1994). Psychometric Theory, 3rd Edition. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Patchen, M., Hofman, G., & Davidson, J.D. (1976). Interracial perception among high school
students. Sociometry, 39: 341-354.
Pender, N.J., Walker, S.N., Sechrist, K.R., & Frank-Stromborg, M. (1990). Predicting health-
promoting lifestyles in the workplace. Nursing Research, 39: 326-332.
Phillips, E.L. ( 1982). Stress, health, and psychological problems in the major medical profes-
sions. Washington, DC: University Press of America.
Podsakoff, P.M., & Organ, D.W. (1986). Self-reports in organizational research: Problems and
prospects. Journal of Management, 12: 531-544.
Priest, S. (1992). Factor exploration and confirmation for the dimensions of an adventure
experience. Journal of Leisure Research, 24: 127-139.
Quick, J.C., Quick, J.D., Nelson, D.L., & Hurrell, J.J. (1997). Preventive stress management in
organizations. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.
Quick, J.D., Horn, R.S., & Quick, J.C. (1987). Health consequences of stress. Journal of
Organizational Behavior Management, 8, 19-36.
120
Radloff, L.S. (1977). The CES-D: A self-report depression scale for research in the general
population. Ap,plied Psychological Measurement. 1: 385-401.
Revicki, D.A., & May, H.J. (1989). Organizational characteristics, occupational stress, and
mental health in nurses. Behavioral Medicine, 30-36.
Rizzo, J.R., House, R.J., & Lirtzman, S.I. (1970). Role conflict and ambiguity in complex
organizations. Administrative Science Quarterly. 15: 150-163.
Robinson, S.L. (1996). Trust and breach of the psychological contract. Administrative Science
Quarterly.41: 574-599.
Rose, R.M. (1987). Neuroendocrine effects of work stress. In J.C. Quick, R.S. Bhagal, J.E.
Dalton, and J.D. Quick (Eds.) Work stress: Health care systems in the workplace. (pp. 130-
147). New York: Praeger.
Roseman, I.J., Spindel, M.S., & Jose, P.E. (1990). Appraisals of emotion-eliciting events:
Testing a theory of discrete emotions, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 59:
899-915. .
Sales, S.M. (1969). Organizational role as a risk factor in coronary disease. Administrative
Science Quarterly, 14: '325-336.
Salovey, P., Rothman, A.J., Detweiler, J.B., & Steward, W.T. (2000). Emotional states and
physical health. American Psychologist. 55: 110-121.
Schaubroeck; J., Ganster, D.C., Sime, W.E., & Ditman, D. (1993). A field experiment testing
supervisory role clarification. Personnel Psychology. 46: 1-25.
Scheier, M.F., & Carver, C.S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implica-
tions of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology. 4: 219-24 7.
Selye, H. (1975). Confusion and controversy in the stress field. Journal of Human stress, 2: 37-
44.
Selye, H. (1976b). The stress of life: Revised Edition. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Selye, H. (1976c). Stress and distress. Frontiers in Medicine. special issue, 9-13.
121
Selye, H. (1983). The stress concept: Past, present, and future. In Cary L. Cooper (Ed.) Stress ·
research: Issues for the eighties. pp. 1-20. Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons.
Smith, C.A., Haynes, K.N., Lazarus, R.S., & Pope, L.K. (1993). In search of the "hot" cogni-
tions: Attributions, appraisals, and their relation to emotion. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 65: 916-929.
Snyder, C.R., Sympson, S.C., Ybasco, F.C., Borders, T.F., Babyak, M.A., & Higgins, R.L.
( 1996). Development and validation of the state hope scale. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 70: 321-335.
Spector, P.E. (1987). Method variance as an artifact in self-reported affect and perceptions at
work: Myth or significant problem? Journal of Applied Psychology, 72: 438-443.
Spiegel, D. (1999). Healing words: Emotional expression and disease outcome. JAMA, 281:
1328-1329.
Spielberger, C.D. (1991). State-trait anger expression inventory: Revised research edition.
Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L., & Luskene, R.E. (1970). Manual for the state-trait anxiety
inventory. Palo Alto, CA: · Consulting Psychologists Press.
Steiger, J.H. (1989). EzPath: Causal modeling. Evanston, IL: SYSTAT Inc.
Stein, L. & Belluzzi, J.D. (1978). Brain endorphins and tlie sense of well-being: A psycho-
biological hypothesis. In E. Costa and M. Trabucchi (eds.) Advances in Biochemical
Psychopharmacology, Vol. 18. (pp. 299-311 ). New York: Raven Press.
Sutherland, V.J. & Cooper, C.L. (1990). Understanding Stress. London: Chapman and Ji:all.
Toates, F. (1995). Stress: Conceptual and biological aspects. John Wiley & Sons: Chichester,
England.
Tyler, P.A., & Cushway, D. (1992). Stress, coping, and mental well-being in hospital nurses.
Stress Medicine, 8: 91-98.
Tyler, P.A., & Ellision, R.N. (1994). Sources of stress and psychological well-being in high-
dependency nursing. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 19: 469-476.
Ursin, H., Baade, E., and Levine, S. (1978). Psychobiology of Stress. New York: Academic
Press.
Ware, J.E., Davies-Avery, A., & Donald, C.A. (1978). Conceptualization and measurement of
health for adults in the health insurance study: Vol. V, General Health Perceptions. Santa
Monica, CA: Rand Corporation.
Watson, D., Clark, L.A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures
of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scale. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 54: 1063-1070.
122
Watson, D. & Pennebaker, J.W. (1989). Health complaints, stress and distress: Exploring the
central role of negative affectivity. Psychological Review, 96: 234-254.
Watson, D., Pennebaker, J.W., & Folger, R. (1987). Beyond negative affectivity: Measuring
stress and satisfaction in the workplace. Journal of Organizational Behavior Management: 8:
141-157.
Weidner, G., Boughal, T., Connor, S.L., Peiper, C., & Mendell, N.R. (1997). Relationship of job
strain to standard coronary risk factors and psychological characteristics in women and men
of the family heart study. Health Psychology, 16: 239-247.
Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer-
Verlag.
Wilke, P.K., Gmelch, W.H., &Lovrich, N.P. (1985). Stress and productivity: Evidence of the
inverted U-function. Public Productivity Review, 9: 342-356
Williams, L.J., Cote, J.A., & Buckley, M.R. (1989). Lack of method variance in self-reported
affect and perception. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74: 462-468.
Wothke, W. (1993). Nonpositive definite matrices. In K.A. Bollen and S. Long (Eds.) Testing
Structural Equation Models. (pp. 256-293). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Wright, T.A., & Cropanzano, R. (2000). Psychological well-being and job satisfaction as
predictors of job performance. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 5: 84-94.
Wright, T.A., & Staw, B.M. (1999). Affect and favorable work outcomes: two longitudinal tests
of the happy-productive worker thesis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20: 1-23.
Yerkes, R.M., & Dodson, J.D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit
formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18: 459-482.
123
APPENDIX
124
0SU
Department of Management
Dear Nursing Staff Member:
While I am earnestly hoping for your help, you are under no obligation to
participate in this study. You will find a statement of informed consent at
the back of this booklet for your review.
Sincerely,
Bret L. Simmons
Department of Management
College of Business Administration
125
There are no correct or incorrect answers. Be sure to answer all the
questions, but don't spend too much time on any one item.
SECTION A. Using the scale shown below, please indicate the extent to
which you agree with each of the items.
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree
1. I'm always optimistic about my future. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
2. Generally speaking, I am very satisfied with 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
this job.
3. If something can go wrong for me, it will. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
4. I always look on the bright side of things. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
5. I frequently think of quitting this job. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
6. I hardly ever expect things to go my way. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
7. Things never work out the way I want them to. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
8. I am generally satisfied with the kind of work I 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
do in this job.
9. I'm a believer in the idea that "every cloud has 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
a silver lining."
SECTION B. Using the scale shown below, please indicate how often you
experience these situations in your present work unit.
Very
Never Frequently
1. Too many non-nursing tasks are required, I 2 3 4 5 6 7
such as clerical work.
2. I don't have enough time to provide I 2 3 4 5 6 7
emotional support to a patient.
3. I feel critical of others. I 2 3 4 5 6 7
4. I witness the death of a patient. I 2 3 4 5 6 7
5. I feel easily annoyed or irritated. I 2 3 4 5 6 7
6. I don't have enough time to complete all I 2 3 4 5 6 7
of my nursing tasks.
7. I observe a patient suffering. I 2 3 4 5 6 7
8. There is not enough staff to adequately I 2 3 4 5 6 7
cover the unit.
9. I have temper outbursts I cannot control. I 2 3 4 5 6 7
10. I listen to a patient talking about his/her I 2 3 4 5 6 7
approaching death.
126
SECTION C. The following are nine questions relating to various aspects of your
situation at work. Please mark the number that expresses your answer, with
numbers 1 and 7 being the extreme answers.
1. Do you have the feeling that you don't really care about what goes on around
you:
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Very seldom or never Very often
2. When you think of the challenges you are facing at work, do you feel that:
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
You can find a solution There is no solution
4. At work, do you have the feeling that you are being treated unfairly:
I 2 3 4 5 6 7
Very often Very seldom or never
5. When you think about your present situation at work, you very often:
I 2 3 4 5 6 7
Feel how good it is to be alive Ask yourself why you exist at all
7. How often do you have feelings that there's little meaning in the things you
do in your daily work activities:
I 2 3 4 5 6 7
Always have this feeling Never have this feeling
9. When you think about problems you are currently having at work, do you
tend to:
I 2 3 4 5 6 7
Keep worrying about it Say "ok", that's that,
1 have to live with it. 1'
127
SECTION D. Using the scale shown below, please select the number that best
describes how you think about yourself right now. Please take a few moments to
focus on yourself and what is going on in your life at this moment. Once you have
this "here and now" mindset, go ahead and answer each item according to the
following scale.
Definitely Definitely
False True
1. If I should find myself in a jam, I could think of I 2 3 4 5 6 7
many ways to get out of it.
2. I feel certain about how much authority I have. I 2 3 4 5 6 7
3. According to the doctors I've seen, my health is now I 2 3 4 5 6 7
excellent.
4. I am worried about whether I am regarded as a I 2 3 4 5 6 7
success or failure.
5. I feel better now than I ever have before. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
6. At the present time, I am energetically pursuing my 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
goals.
7. I feel self-conscious. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
8. There are lots of ways around any problem that I am 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
facing now.
9. I'm as healthy as anybody I know. J 2 3 4 5 6 7
10. I know I have allocated my work time properly. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
11. I am worried about what other people think of me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
12. I know what my responsibilities are. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
13. My health is excellent. l 2 3 4 5 6 7
14. Right now, I see myself as being pretty successful. l 2 3 4 5 6 7
15. I know exactly what is expected of me at work. - 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
16. I am worried about looking foolish. l 2 3 4 5 6 7
17. I understand what needs to be done at work. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
18. I can think of many ways to reach my current goals. l 2 3 4 5 6 7
19. I feel concerned about the impression I am making. l 2 3 4 5 6 7
20. At this time, I am meeting the goals I have set for l 2 3 4 5 6 7
mvself.
128
SECTION E. Using the scale shown below, please select the number that best
describes how often each thought typically occurs to you while you are working.
Never Always
I. I am going to be injured 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
2. What ifno one reaches me in time to help? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
3. I'm going to have an accident. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
4. There's something wrong with me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
5. Something awful is going to happen to me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
6. Something will happen to someone I care about. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
SECTIONF. Think about your supervisor in your present job. For each
statement, circle the number that best describes how much you agree or disagree
with each statement.
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree
I. Ifl had my way, I wouldn't let my supervisor
have any influence over issues that are important 2 3 4 5 6 7
to me.
2. My supervisor can't get the approvals she/he 2 3 4 5 6 7
needs in efforts to be creative.
3. I am comfortable discussing with my supervisor 2 3 4 5 6 7
my ideas for improvement in the workplace.
4. My supervisor is held accountable for things that 2 3 4 5 6 7
are not her/his fault.
5. 1 would be willing to let my supervisor have 2 3 4 5 6 7
complete control over my future in this hospital.
6. My supervisor does not have the authority 2 3 4 5 6 7
she/he needs to make needed improvements
7. I really wish I had a good way to keep an eye on 2 3 4 5 6 7
my supervisor.
8. My supervisor does not trust me. 2 3 4 5 6 7
9. My supervisor feels my performance is worse 2 3 4 5 6 7
than it is.
10. I am comfortable discussing with my supervisor 2 3 4 5 6 7
concerns I have about our working relationship.
11. My supervisor does not have the resources 2 3 4 5 6 7
she/he requires to make needed improvements.
12. My supervisor always seems to be looking over 2 3 4 5 6 7
my shoulder.
13. I am comfortable discussing with my supervisor 2 3 4 5 6 7
concerns I have about my ability to do my job.
129
SECTIONG
l. Below are seven boxes, each containing two circles. One circle represents your
present job and the other circle represents yourself. The circles overlap in various
degrees. At one extreme, (box 7) the two circles are separate, representing you
being separate from your present job. At the other extreme (box 1) the two circles
are totally overlapping, representing you being totally immersed in your present
jQQ. Select the box which most accurately depicts your relationship to your present
jQQ and circle the appropriate number
7 0
6
130
2. Below are seven diagrams. In each diagram, there is a person, representing
yourself and a desk, representing your present job. The figures are placed at
different distances from each other, depicting how close or far one is from one's
,iQQ. Distance in the diagram does not represent physical distance from the job.
Instead, being close to one's job implies that the job is central to one's life and
being far from one's job implies that the job is not central to one's life. In your
opinion, how far are you from your present job? Circle the appropriate number.
t - 7
t - 6
t
t -
- 5
t - 3
t- 2
t.
131
SECTION H. This scale consists of a number of words that describe different
feelings and emotions you may have at work. Read each item and then mark the
appropriate answer in the space next to the word. Indicate to what extent you feel
this way right now, that is, at the present moment.
1 2 3 4 5
very slightly a little moderately quite a bit extremely
or not at all
SECTION I. Please answer the following questions by placing a check (.../) on the
line in front of the answer chosen..
132
5. What is the highest educational 6. How long have you been a nurse?
degree you have earned?
Less than 1 year
Diploma 1 to 2 years
Associate Degree 3 to 5 years
BS/BSN 6 to IO years
MS/MSN 11 to 15 years
Ed.D more than 15 years
Ph.D.
Other (please specify_ _...,)
7. How long have you been employed 8. How long have you been in your
at this hospital? current nursing position?
9. Average number of hours worked 10. In the past six months, how many
per week as a nurse at this hospital: days have you been .absent from
work for reasons related to your
Less than 20 health (e.g you were sick or
21 to 30 needed to see a doctor)?
31.to 40
41 to 50
51 or more Number of days absent: _ _ __
133
STATEMENT OF INFORMED CONSENT
134
OKLAHOMA STATE UNIVERSITY
INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD
The Principal Investigator(s) may wish to use the term "anonymous" rather than
confidential since there are no definite identifiers collected. The inclusion of the informed
consent statement at the end of the questionnaire is very good.
Approvals arc \'alid for one c:ilcndar year, after which time a request for continuation must be submitted.
Any modification to the rcsc.irch project approvai by the lRB must be submitted for approval. Appro\·ai
projects arc subject to monitoring by the IRB. fapcditcd and c:x.cmpt projects may be micwcd by the full
Institutional Rc\icw Board.
135
VITA~
Bret L. Simmons
Doctor of Philosophy
Biographical:
Personal Data: Born in Wichita, Kansas, on June 24, 1961, the son of Henry L. and Alyce
Rae Simmons.
Education: Graduated from East Central High School, Tulsa, Oklahoma in May 1979.
Received an Associate of Applied Science degree in Logistics from the Community
College of the Air Force in 1988; a Bachelor of Science degree in Management from
Park College, Parkville, Missouri, in 1992; and a Masters in International Management
degree from Whitworth College, Spokane, Washington, in 1994. Completed the
requirements for the Doctor of Philosophy degree in Business Administration at
Oklahoma State University in December, 2000.
Experience: Began work for McDonald's Corporation at the age of 16 and worked a variety
of jobs for the corporation for the next 9Yi years, holding the position of unit manager
for the last three of those years. Joined the United States Air Force (USAF) in 1987 and
served as a Communications Systems Program Manager. Left the USAF in 1994 as a
Staff Sergeant, having served tours of duty in South Korea and Spokane, Washington.
Created and served in the position of Federal Sales Manager for Telect, Inc. in Spokane,
Washington from December 1994 until July of 1995. Began work on a doctoral degree
at Oklahoma State University in August of 1995, working part time teaching classes in
management until July of 1999. Currently hold the position of Assistant Professor of
Business Administration in the School of Management, University of Alaska Fairbanks,
since August of 1999.