The Structural Status of Bora Classifiers
The Structural Status of Bora Classifiers
Volume 46 Article 2
January 2002
Recommended Citation
Weber, David J. (2002) "The structural status of Bora classifiers," Work Papers of the Summer Institute of
Linguistics, University of North Dakota Session: Vol. 46, Article 2.
DOI: 10.31356/silwp.vol46.02
Available at: https://commons.und.edu/sil-work-papers/vol46/iss1/2
This Article is brought to you for free and open access by UND Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for
inclusion in Work Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota Session by an
authorized editor of UND Scholarly Commons. For more information, please contact
und.commons@library.und.edu.
The structural status of Bora classifiers1
David Weber
I claim that Bora classifiers have the structural status of (bound) nouns, based on facts like the
following:
1. Some classifiers also occur as independent nouns (possibly with minor phonological differ-
ences).
2. Classifiers have the referential properties typical of nouns. Like typical nominals, they denote
classes of objects and may refer to a member of the class they denote. They are never used
to attribute properties to another referring expresssion.
3. Classifiers have the distribution typical of nouns: they may be a clausal subject, they may
be modified by a relative clause, they may have a prepositional complement, and so forth.
And classifiers head noun phrases, a claim for which various arguments are given, among them
one based on the remarkable similarity between the host-classifier and possessor-possessed con-
structions.
Introduction
Following Thiesen and Weber [4], I claim here that Bora classifiers are bound nouns, ones with rather general
denotations, and that they head noun phrases. Phrases like ath EREE–kpa (worthless-hslabi) ‘worthless plank
(table, bench, machete, etc.)’, I claim, have structures like the following:
[NP [Adj áth ÉRÉÈ– ] [N –kpà ] ]
worthless hslabi
This paper contributes to the study of the typology of classifier systems (Senft [3], Aikhenvald [1]) by
providing another example of a system in which the classifiers are very noun-like.2
Section 1 gives reasons for believing that classifiers are nouns. Section 2 gives reasons to believe that
classifiers head their phrases.
may be epiphenomenal, as suggested by Croft [2]—but that classifiers have many of the formal, distributional
and semantic properties typical of nouns.
Weber: Bora classifiers 2
2. Some classifiers denote large classes of objects that share one or more properties; e.g., –i:Pj o hsticki
denotes the class of things that are relatively long and slender, roughly cylindrical, and have an
orientation (toward one end); –hi hdiski denotes the class of things that are disk-like, which includes
pills, fields,. . . and even nations.
3. Some classifiers denote classes of objects defined rather narrowly; e.g., –tsh i hchildi, –PE htreei, and
–pa hboxi.
No matter whether broad or narrow, whether deictically-determined or not, classifiers are never used to
attribute their properties to some other referring expresssion (like adjectives do); they are only used to refer
to an object that has these properties.
Classifiers may also bind anaphors. For example, in 3 –mE hAnPli binds the anaphor i ‘self’:
(3) ı̀mı́tsj É–mÉ ı̀ máx tsjh ò–nÈ ‘They want to eat.’
want-hAnPli self eat-høi
Weber: Bora classifiers 3
Second, classifiers are like nouns in that they may be the subject of a sentence, as illustrated with –:ph E
hSgMi, –PE htreei and –ha hshelteri in 10:
(10) th á:pò–:pÈ ‘He treats (medically).’
áı́:Bj È–PÈ ‘The tree is burning.’
ı́mı́:Bj È–hà ‘The house (clothes, etc.) is finished.’
Third and quite significantly, preverbal overt subjects do not co-occur with such classifier subjects. This is
because the classifier is the subject; it is not simply an agreement marker. (A subject indicated by a classifier
following the verb may be followed by an overt subject noun phrase, but the overt subject is appositive to
the classifier subject.)
Fourth, like nouns, classifiers may head relative clauses (either restrictive or nonrestrictive). The initial
syllable of the relative clause’s verb bears high tone, as characteristic of subordinate clauses. (This high tone
is represented with S.) Examples follow:
S
(11) ó áx tjh Ẁmı́–ij th È:–mı̀ [tı̀–pj È ımı́páx tsjh ò ]–mı̀
I see-hti that-htransporti that-hSgMi fix -htransporti
‘I saw the canoe (launch, car, etc.) that he fixed.’
S
(12) [ó–ph ÉÈ ò ts ı x kh ò ]–hà tsh Ẃ:kh à nóx kh ànẂ–Pı̀
I-rem I sew -hshelteri already deteriorate-hti
‘The clothes that I sewed are now deteriorated.’
And fifth, like nouns, classifiers may have “prepositional” complements as, for example, in English (a)
table like this one. In Bora, this is ı́–kpá–ÈmÉ–kpà ‘a plank (table, machete, etc.) like this one’, in which
–EmE– ‘similar to’ is a postposition. Compare the structures in Figure 1:
a. NP b. NP
HH
H
HH
H H
N PP PP N
H
table
H HH
P NP H –kpà
NP P
H
H -hslabi
like Det N HH
DET N –ÈmÉ
this one -like
ı́– –kpá
this -hslabi
In both cases there are two noun phrases. The lower refers to the object of comparison while the higher
refers to the object being likened to it. In Bora both of these noun phrases are headed by an instance of
–kpa hslabi.
used as post-verbal subjects3 ). Given that classifiers are nouns, what is projected from them are noun
phrases. What precedes (nonfinite verb, subordinate verb, demonstrative, numeral, quantifier, adjective,
etc.) is a modifier.
In the following sections various arguments are presented.
The possessor and head form a single tonal phrase within which the *llx constraint must be respected.
When the head is mono- or bisyllabic, the genitive tone docks on the possessor’s final syllable. When the
head has more than two syllables, the genitive tone docks on the head’s initial syllable.
The possibilities are charted in Table 1. (The genitive low tone is indicated by a G over the vowel.
The possessor and head are separated by .) These are the basic patterns; see Thiesen and Weber [4] for
discussion of how lexically-marked tones may perturb these.
Weber: Bora classifiers 7
head (possessed)
σ# σσ# σσσ(. . . )
G G G
#σ σ σ σ σ́ σ σ́ σ σ́ σ(. . . )
G G G
possessor #σσ σ́ σ σ σ́ σ σ́ σ σ́ σ́ σ σ́ σ(. . . )
G G G
(. . . )σσσ (. . . σ) σ́ σ σ (. . . σ) σ́ σ σ́ σ (. . . )σσ́ σ́ σ σ́ σ(. . . )
Now consider the tone of a host followed by a classifier. A low tone C occurs at the boundary between
a classifier and what precedes it. It is docked (with a few exceptions) as in Table 2:
Monosyllabic classifiers place the low tone on their host’s final syllable, as in 16:
C
(16) a. tsh ı :–kpà ‘other plank’
other-hslabi
C
b. tsh Ẃ:kh á–a–kpà ‘old plank’
old-hslabi
After a monosyllabic host, a bisyllabic classifier bears the low tone on its initial syllable, as in 17:
C
(17) tsh ı́–ija:mı̀ ‘other leaf’
other-hleafi
After a polysyllabic host, a bisyllabic classifier places the low tone on the host’s final syllable, as in 18:
C
(18) tsh Ẃ:kh áa–Pá:mı̀ ‘old leaf’
old-hleafi
Longer classifiers (three or more syllables) bear the low tone on their initial syllable, as in 19:
C
(19) tsh Ẃ:kh áá–a:máı̀ ‘old row’
old-hrowi
A comparison of the docking of the genitive tone G and the classifier tone C shows a remarkable
similarity; they differ only in one cell, the top middle one. This suggests a tight formal (and possibly
historic) relationship between the two constructions.
Functional similarities. The genitive construction is used to express various relationships between
(the referents of) the possessor and head, ones typically expressed with genitives, such as ownership (my
canoe, his food), kinship and other social relations (my mother, that town’s chief), properties (my goodness,
my height, his size), and so forth. Among these, two are shared with the host+classifier construction.
First, the possessor may indicate the subject of the head, when the head is a nonfinite verb. The
possessor’s thematic relation to the head may be—among other possibilities—agent. This is illustrated in
20, where ‘he’ is the agent of ‘teaching’. (In this example the genitive low tone, indicated by G, and the
nonfinite low tone, indicated by N, coincide on the head’s first syllable.)
G
N
(20) tı́:pj É Wkpá:pò ‘his teaching’
his teach[−finite]
Weber: Bora classifiers 8
See Thiesen and Weber [4] for further discussion and examples.
Turning to the host+classifier construction, one of the most common uses is for the host to be a finite
verb, with the classifier indicating the subject; see, for example, 20 above.
Second, the possessor may be a subordinate clause modifying the head. This is like a relative clause in
which the modifying clause is the possessor of a genitive construction, as evidenced by the genitive tonal
pattern. Examples follow:
G
(21) [tı̀–tsj È tsh ı́:má–Bá–th Ẁ–nÉ] ijax tsjh óth à
that-hSgFi child-have-neg-høi length.of.time
‘during the time she had not given birth’
G
(22) [kpáx ph ı̀:–kh È Ẃ:hÉ–th Ẃ–nÉ] patsı́ x kh à–hà
man-objAn arrive-neg-høi adolescent-hSgFi
‘young woman who has not been with a man’
Turning to the host+classifier construction, when the host is an adjective (demonstrative, quantifier,. . . )
or a noun, the relation is one of simple modification.
These formal and functional similarities between the genitive and classifier constructions strongly sug-
gest that the order of the modifier and head should be the same in both constructions. For the genitive
construction the order is clearly modifier+head, (just as expected for a head-final language). From this we
can conclude that the order for the classifier construction is also modifier+head, so the classifier is the head,
and thus heads the noun phrase.
Conclusion
Based on a variety of evidence we conclude that (1) Bora classifiers are nouns, and (2) except for when they
follow a finite verb, the host+classifier construction is a noun phrase headed by the classifier.
References
[1] Aikhenvald, Alexandra. 2000. Classifiers: A typology of noun categorization devices. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
[2] Croft, William. 2001. Radical construction grammar: Syntactic theory in typological perspective. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
[3] Senft, Gunter (editor). 2000 Systems of Nominal Classification. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
[4] Thiesen, Wesley and David Weber (forthcoming). A grammar of Bora. Publications in Linguistics.
Dallas: SIL International and the University of Texas at Arlington.
David Weber
7264 W Main St
Westmoreland, NY 13490
david weber@sil.org