Chapter 51

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 23

Chapter 51 Behavioral Ecology

Lecture Outline

Overview: Studying Behavior

 Humans have studied animal behavior for as long as we have


lived on Earth.
 As hunter and hunted, knowledge of animal behavior was
essential to human behavior.
 The modern scientific discipline of behavioral ecology studies
how behavior develops, evolves, and contributes to survival and
reproductive success.

Concept 51.1 Behavioral ecologists distinguish between


proximate and ultimate causes of behavior
 Scientific questions that can be posed about any behavior can
be divided into two classes: those that focus on the immediate
stimulus and mechanism for the behavior and those that
explore how the behavior contributes to survival and
reproduction.
 What is behavior?
 Behavioral traits are an important part of an animal’s
phenotype.
 Many behaviors result from an animal’s muscular activity,
such as a predator chasing a prey.
 In some behaviors, muscular activity is less obvious, as in
bird song.
 Some nonmuscular activities are also behaviors, as when an
animal secretes a pheromone to attract a member of the
opposite sex.
 Learning is also a behavioral process.
 Put simply, behavior is everything an animal does and how it
does it.
 Proximate questions are mechanistic, concerned with the
environmental stimuli that trigger a behavior, as well as the
genetic, physiological, and anatomical mechanisms underlying a
behavioral act.
 Proximate questions are referred to as “how?” questions.
 Ultimate questions address the evolutionary significance of a
behavior and why natural selection favors this behavior.
 Ultimate questions are referred to as “why?” questions.
 Red-crowned cranes breed in spring and early summer.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-1
 A proximate question about the timing of breeding by this
species might ask, “How does day length influence breeding
by red-crowned cranes?”
 A reasonable hypothesis for the proximate cause of this
behavior is that breeding is triggered by the effect of
increased day length on the crane’s production of and
response to particular hormones.
 An ultimate hypothesis might be that red-crowned cranes
reproduce in spring and early summer because that is when
breeding is most productive.
 At that time of year, parent birds can find an ample
supply of food for rapidly growing offspring, providing an
advantage in reproductive success compared to birds
that breed in other seasons.
 These two levels of causation are related.
 Proximate mechanisms produce behaviors that evolved
because they increase fitness in some way.
 For example, increased day length has little adaptive
significance for red-crowned cranes, but because it
corresponds to seasonal conditions that increase
reproductive success, such as the availability of food for
feeding young birds, breeding when days are long is a
proximate mechanism that has evolved in cranes.
Classical ethology presaged an evolutionary approach to
behavioral biology.
 In the mid-20th century, a number of pioneering behavioral
biologists developed the discipline of ethology, the scientific
study of animal behavior.
 In 1963, Niko Tinbergen suggested four questions that must
be answered to fully understand any behavior.
1. What is the mechanistic basis of the behavior, including
chemical, anatomical, and physiological mechanisms?
2. How does development of the animal, from zygote to mature
individual, influence the behavior?
3. What is the evolutionary history of the behavior?
4. How does the behavior contribute to survival and
reproduction (fitness)?
 Tinbergen’s list includes both proximate and ultimate
questions.
 The first two, which concern mechanism and development,
are proximate questions, while the second two are ultimate,
or evolutionary, questions.
 A fixed action pattern (FAP) is a sequence of unlearned
behavioral acts that is essentially unchangeable and, once
initiated, is usually carried to completion.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-2
 A FAP is triggered by an external sensory stimulus called a
sign stimulus.
 In the red-spined stickleback, the male attacks other males
that invade his nesting territory.
 The stimulus for the attack is the red underside of the
intruder.
 A male stickleback will attack any model that has some red
visible on it.
 A proximate explanation for this aggressive behavior is that
the red belly of the intruding male acts as a sign stimulus that
releases aggression in a male stickleback.
 An ultimate explanation is that by chasing away other male
sticklebacks, a male decreases the chance that eggs laid in his
nesting territory will be fertilized by another male.
 Imprinting is a type of behavior that includes learning and
innate components and is generally irreversible.
 Imprinting has a sensitive period, a limited phase in an
animal’s behavior that is the only time that certain
behaviors can be learned.
 An example of imprinting is young geese following their
mother.
 In species that provide parental care, parent-offspring
bonding is a critical time in the life cycle.
 During the period of bonding, the young imprint on their
parent and learn the basic behavior of the species, while
the parent learns to recognize its offspring.
 Among gulls, the sensitive period for parental bonding on
young lasts one or two days.
 If bonding does not occur, the parent will not initiate
care of the infant, leading to certain death of the
offspring and decreasing the parent’s reproductive
success.
 How do young gulls know on whom—or what—to imprint?
 The tendency to respond is innate in birds.
 The world provides the imprinting stimulus, and young gulls
respond to and identify with the first object they
encounter that has certain key characteristics.
 In greylag geese, the key stimulus is movement of the
object away from the young.
 A proximate explanation for young geese following and
imprinting on their mother is that during an early, critical
developmental stage, the young geese observe their mother
moving away from them and calling.
 An ultimate explanation is that, on average, geese that follow
and imprint on their mother receive more care and learn

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-3
necessary skills, and thus have a greater chance of surviving,
than those that do not follow.
 Early study of imprinting and fixed action patterns helped
make the distinction between proximate and ultimate causes of
behavior.
 They also helped to establish a strong tradition of
experimental approaches in behavioral ecology.

Concept 51.2 Many behaviors have a strong genetic component


Behavior results from both genes and environmental factors.
 Behavioral traits, like other aspects of a phenotype, are the
result of complex interactions between genetic and
environmental factors.
 In biology, the nature-versus-nurture issue is not about
whether genes or environment influence behavior, but about
how both are involved.
 All behaviors are affected by both genes and environment.
 Behavior can be viewed in terms of the norm of reaction.
 We can measure the behavioral phenotypes for a particular
genotype that develop in a range of environments.
 In some cases, the behavior is variable, depending on
environmental experience.
 In other cases, nearly all individuals in the population
exhibit identical behavior, despite internal and external
environmental differences during development and
throughout life.
 Behavior that is developmentally fixed is called innate
behavior.
 Such behaviors are under strong genetic influence.
 The range of environmental differences among individuals
does not appear to alter innate behavior.
Many animal movements are under substantial genetic
influence.
 A kinesis is a simple change in activity or turning rate in
response to a stimulus.
 For example, sowbugs are more active in dry areas and less
active in humid areas.
 This increases the chance that they will leave a dry area and
encounter a moist area.
 A taxis is an automatic, oriented movement toward or away
from a stimulus.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-4
 For example, many stream fishes exhibit positive rheotaxis,
automatically swimming or orienting themselves in an
upstream direction (toward the current).
 This keeps them from being swept away and keeps them
facing in the direction in which food is coming.
 Ornithologists have found that many features of migratory
behavior in birds are genetically programmed.
 Migration is the regular movement of animals over relatively
long distances.
 One of the best-studied migratory birds is the blackcap
(Sylvia atricapilla), a small warbler that ranges from the Cape
Verde Islands off the coast of West Africa to northern
Europe.
 Migratory behaviors of blackcaps vary greatly among
populations.
 During the migration season, captive migratory blackcaps
hop restlessly about their cages all night or rapidly flap
their wings while sitting on a perch.
 Peter Berthold studied the genetic basis of this behavior,
known as “migratory restlessness,” in several populations of
blackcaps.
 In one study, the researchers crossed migratory blackcaps
with nonmigratory ones and subjected their offspring to
environments simulating one location or the other.
 Forty percent of offspring raised in both conditions showed
migratory restlessness, leading Berthold to conclude that
migration is under genetic control and follows a polygenic
inheritance pattern.
Animal communication is an essential component of
interactions between individuals.
 Much of the social interaction between animals involves
transmitting information through specialized behaviors called
signals.
 In behavioral ecology, a signal is a behavior that causes a
change in another animal’s behavior.
 The transmission, reception, and response to signals constitute
animal communication.
 Some features of animal communication are under strong
genetic control, although the environment makes a significant
contribution to all communication systems.
 Many signals are efficient in energy costs.
 For example, a territorial fish erects its fins when
aggressively approaching an intruder.
 It takes less energy to erect fins that to attack an invading
fish.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-5
 Animals communicate using visual, auditory, chemical, tactile,
and electrical signals.
 The type of signal is closely related to an animal’s lifestyle and
environment.
 For example, nocturnal species use olfactory and auditory
signals.
 Birds are diurnal and have a poor olfactory sense.
 They communicate primarily by visual and auditory
signals.
 Humans are more attentive to the colors and songs of birds
than the rich olfactory signals of many other animals
because of our own senses.
 Many animals secrete chemical substances called pheromones.
 These chemicals are especially common in mammals and
insects and often relate to reproductive behaviors.
 In honeybees, pheromones produced by the queens and her
daughters (workers) maintain the hive’s very complex social
order.
 Male drones are attracted to the queen’s pheromone
when they are outside the hive.
 Pheromones can also function in nonreproductive behavior.
 When a minnow is injured, an alarm substance is released
from glands in the fish’s skin, inducing a fright response
among other fish in the area.
 They become more vigilant and group in tightly packed
schools.
 Pheromones are effective at very low concentrations.
 The songs of most birds are at least partly learned.
 In contrast, in many species of insect, mating rituals include
characteristic songs that are under direct genetic control.
 In Drosophila, males produce a song by wing vibration.
 A variety of evidence suggests that song structure in
Drosophila is controlled genetically and is under strong
selective pressure.
 Females can recognize the songs of males of their own
species.
 Males raised in isolation produce a characteristic song
with no exposure to other singing males.
 The male song shows little variation among individuals.
 Some insect species are morphologically identical and can be
identified only through courtship songs or behaviors.
 For example, morphologically identical green lacewings were
once thought to belong to a single species.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-6
 However, studies of their courtship songs revealed the
presence of at least 15 different species, each with a
different song.
 Hybrid offspring sing songs that contain elements of the
songs of both parental species, leading researchers to
conclude that the songs are genetically controlled.
Prairie vole mating and parental behaviors are under strong
genetic influence.
 Mating and parental behavior by male prairie voles (Microtus
ochrogaster) are under strong genetic control.
 Prairie voles and a few other vole species are monogamous, a
social trait found in only 3% of mammalian species.
 Male prairie voles help their mates care for young, a
relatively uncommon trait among male mammals.
 Male prairie voles form a strong pair-bond with a single
female after they mate, engaging in grooming and huddling
behaviors.
 Mated males are intensely aggressive to strange males or
females, while remaining nonaggressive to their mate and
pups.
 Research by Thomas Insel at Emory University suggests that
arginine-vasopressin (AVP), a nine-amino-acid neurotransmitter
released in mating, mediates both pair-bond formation and
aggression in male prairie voles.
 In the CNS, AVP binds to a receptor called the V1a receptor.
 The researcher found significant differences in the
distribution of V1a receptors between the brains of
monogamous prairie voles and related promiscuous montane
voles.
 Insel inserted the prairie vole V1a receptor gene into
laboratory mice.
 The mice developed the same distribution of V 1a receptors
as the prairie voles and also showed many of the mating
behaviors of the voles.
 Thus, a single gene appears to mediate much of the complex
mating and parental behavior of the prairie vole.

Concept 51.3 Environment, interacting with an animal’s genetic


makeup, influences the development of behaviors
 Environmental factors modify many behaviors.
 Diet plays an important role in mate selection by Drosophila
mojavensis, which mates and lays its eggs in rotting cactus
tissues.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-7
 Two populations of this fruit fly species use different species
of cactus for their eggs.
 Flies from each population were raised on artificial media in
the lab.
 Females would mate only with males from their own
population.
 The food eaten by male flies as larvae strongly influenced
mate selection by female flies.
 The proximate cause in the female mate choices was in
the exoskeletons of the flies, assessed by the sense of
taste in female flies.
 When males from the other population were “perfumed”
with hydrocarbons extracted from males of the same
population, they were accepted by female flies.
 The California mouse (Peromyscus californicus) is monogamous.
 Like male prairie voles, male California mice are highly
aggressive to other mice and provide considerable parental
care.
 Unlike prairie voles, even unmated California mice are
aggressive.
 Researchers placed newborn California mice in the nests of
white-footed mice (and vice versa).
 White-footed mice are not monogamous and provide little
parental care.
 This cross-fostering changed the behavior of both species.
 Cross-fostered California mice provided less parental care
and were less aggressive toward intruders when they grew
up and reared their own young.
 Their brains had reduced levels of AVP, compared with
California mice raised by their own parents.
 White-footed mice reared by California mice were more
aggressive as parents than those raised by their own
parents.
 One of the most powerful ways that environmental conditions
can influence behavior is through learning, the modification of
behavior based on specific experiences.
 Learned behaviors can be very simple, such as imprinting, or
highly complex.
 Habituation involves a loss of responsiveness to unimportant
stimuli or stimuli that do not provide appropriate feedback.
 For example, some animals stop responding to warning
signals if signals are not followed by a predator attack (the
“cry wolf” effect).
 In terms of ultimate causation, habituation may increase
fitness by allowing an animal’s nervous system to focus on

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-8
meaningful stimuli, rather than wasting time on irrelevant
stimuli.
The fitness of an organism may be enhanced by the capacity
for spatial learning.
 Every natural environment shows spatial variation.
 As a consequence, it may be advantageous for animals to
modify their behavior based on experience with the spatial
structure of their environment, including the locations of nest
sites, hazards, food, and prospective mates.
 The fitness of an animal may be enhanced by the capacity
for spatial learning.
 Niko Tinbergen found that digger wasps found their nest
entrances by using landmarks, or location indicators, in their
environment.
 Landmarks must be stable within the time frame of the
activity.
 Because some environments are more stable than others,
animals may use different kinds of information for spatial
learning in different environments.
 Sticklebacks from a river learned a maze by learning a
pattern of movements.
 Sticklebacks from a more stable pond environment used a
combination of movements and landmarks to learn the
maze.
 The degree of environmental variability influences the
spatial learning strategies of animals.
 Some animals form cognitive maps, internal codes of spatial
relationships of objects in their environment.
 It is difficult to distinguish experimentally between the
use of landmarks and the development of a true cognitive
map.
 Researchers have obtained good evidence that corvids
(a bird family including ravens, crows, and jays) use
cognitive maps.
 Many corvids store food in caches and retrieve it
later.
 Pinyon jays may store nuts in as many as a thousand
widely dispersed caches, keeping track of location and
food quality (based on time since the food was stored).
 Birds can learn that caches are halfway between
two landmarks.
Many animals can learn to associate one stimulus with
another.
 Associative learning is the ability of animals to learn to
associate one stimulus with another.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-9
 For example, a mouse may have an unpleasant experience
with a colorful, poisonous caterpillar and learn to avoid all
caterpillars with that coloration.
 Classical conditioning is a type of associative learning.
 Researchers trained Drosophila melanogaster to avoid air
carrying a particular scent by coupling exposure to the odor
with an electrical shock.
 Drosophila has a surprising capacity for learning.
 Associative learning may play an important role in helping
animals to avoid predators.
 Zebra fish, an Asian minnow, and pike, an American
predatory fish, do not occur together in the wild.
 Researchers exposed zebra fish in an experimental group to
an influx of 20 mL of water containing an alarm substance
and then, 5 minutes later, to 20 mL of water with pike odor.
 Zebra fish had no innate negative reaction to pike odor, but
learned to associate pike odor with the alarm substance.
 The zebra fish were conditioned to associate pike odor with
the alarm substance.
 Operant conditioning is also called trial-and-error learning.
 An animal learns to associate one of its own behaviors with a
reward or a punishment.
 An example is the mouse eating the poisonous caterpillar
and learning to avoid such caterpillars in the future.
The study of cognition connects behavior with nervous system
function.
 The term cognition is variously defined.
 In a narrow sense, it is synonymous with consciousness or
awareness.
 In a broad sense, animal cognition is the ability of an
animal’s nervous system to perceive, store, process, and use
information gathered by sensory receptors.
 The study of animal cognition, called cognitive ethology,
examines the connection between an animal’s nervous system
and its behavior.
 One area of research investigates how an animal’s brain
represents physical objects in the environment.
 Cognitive ethnologists have discovered that many animals,
including insects, categorize objects in their environment as
“same” or “different.”
 Primates, dolphins, and corvids (crow, ravens, and jays) are
capable of novel problem-solving behavior.
 Individual animals may show great individual variation in the
way they attempt to solve a problem.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-10
 Many animals solve problems by observing the behavior of
other individuals.
 Chimpanzees learn to solve problems by copying the
behavior of other chimpanzees.
Varying degrees of genetic and environmental factors
contribute to the learning of complex behavior.
 Considerable research on the development of songs by birds
has revealed varying degrees of genetic and environmental
influence on the learning of complex behavior.
 In some species, learning plays only a small part in the
development of song.
 For instance, New World flycatchers that are reared away
from adults of their own species will sing the song
characteristic of their own species without every having
heard it.
 Some songbirds have a sensitive period for developing their
songs.
 Individual white-crowned sparrows reared in silence
perform abnormal songs, but if recordings of the proper
songs are played early in the life of the bird, normal songs
develop.
 Although the young bird does not sing during the sensitive
period, it memorizes the song of its own species by listening
to other white-crowned sparrows sing.
 During the sensitive period, white-crowned sparrows
fledging seem to be stimulated more by songs of their own
species than songs of other species, chirping more in
response.
 The young birds learn the songs, but the learning appears to
be bounded by genetically controlled preferences.
 The sensitive period in a white-crowned sparrow’s learning of
his song is followed by a second learning phase, when the
juvenile bird sings some tentative notes that researchers call a
subsong.
 The juvenile bird hears its own song and compares it with
the song that it memorized in the sensitive period.
 Once they match, the bird sings that song for the rest of
his life.
 Canaries may learn new song “syllables” each year, adding to
their song during a yearly plastic song stage.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-11
Concept 51.4 Behavioral traits can evolve by natural selection
 Because of the influence of genes on behavior, natural
selection can result in the evolution of behavioral traits in
populations.
Behavior varies in natural populations.
 Behavioral differences between closely related species are
common.
 Males of different Drosophila species sing different
courtship songs.
 Species of voles differ in paternal care.
 Although less obvious, significant differences in behavior can
be found within animal species.
 When behavioral variation within a species corresponds to
variation in environmental conditions, it may be evidence of
past evolution.
 One of the best-known examples of genetically based variation
in behavior within a species is prey selection by the garter
snake Thamnophis elegans.
 Coastal garter snakes feed on salamanders, frogs, and
toads, but mainly on slugs.
 Inland snakes eat frogs, leeches, and fish, but not slugs.
 Stevan Arnold investigated this variation.
 He offered slugs to snakes from both populations, but only
coastal snakes readily accepted the slugs.
 He tested newborn snakes born in the laboratory and found
that 73% of young snakes from coastal mothers attacked
slugs they were offered.
 Only 35% of naïve snakes from inland mothers attacked
the slugs.
 Arnold proposed that when inland snakes colonized coastal
environments 10,000 years ago, a small fraction of the
population had the ability to recognize slugs by
chemoreception.
 These snakes took advantage of the abundant food source
that slugs represented and had higher fitness than snakes
that ignored slugs.
 The capacity to recognize slugs as prey increased in
frequency in coastal populations.
 The funnel web spider Agelenopsis aperta lives in riparian
zones and the surrounding arid environment in the western
United States.
 The spider’s web is a silken sheet ending in a hidden funnel,
where the spider sits and watches for food while foraging.
 When prey strikes the web, the spider runs out across the
web to make its capture.
Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-12
 Riechert and her colleagues found a striking contrast in the
behavior of spiders in riparian forests and those in arid
habitats.
 In arid, food-poor habitats, A. aperta is more aggressive to
potential prey and to other spiders in defense of its web,
and it returns to foraging more quickly following
disturbance.
 Hedrick and Riechert reared spiders in the lab and found that
the differences in aggressiveness between desert and riparian
spiders are genetic.
 Highly productive riparian sites are rich in prey for spiders,
but the density of bird predators is also high.
 The timid behavior of A. aperta in riparian habitats was
selected for by predation risk.
Experiments provide evidence for behavioral evolution.
 Researchers are carrying out experiments on organisms with
short life spans, looking for evidence of evolution in laboratory
populations.
 Marla Sokolowski studied a polymorphism in a gene for
foraging in Drosophila melanogaster.
 The gene is called for, and it has two alleles.
 One allele, forR, results in a “rover” phenotype in which the
fly larva moves more than usual.
 The other allele, forS, results in a “sitter” phenotype in
which the fly larva moves less than usual.
 Sokolowski reared Drosophila at high and low population
densities for 74 generations.
 The forS allele increased in low-density populations, while
forR increased in high-density populations.
 At low densities, short-distance foraging yielded sufficient
food.
 At high densities, long-distance foraging helped the larvae
to move beyond areas of food depletion.
 Peter Berthold and his colleagues captured 20 male and 20
female blackcaps wintering in Britain and transported them to
southwest Germany.
 The birds were caged in glass-covered funnel cages lined
with carbon paper.
 As the birds moved around the funnels, the marks they
made on the paper showed the direction they were trying to
migrate.
 The migratory orientation of wintering adult birds
captured in Britain was similar to their laboratory-reared
offspring.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-13
 Young birds originally from Germany had a very different
migratory orientation.
 This study indicates a genetic basis for migratory
orientation of the young birds.
 Has the behavior evolved over time?
 Berthold’s study suggests that the change in
migratory behavior of the blackcaps is recent and
rapid, having taken place over the past 50 years.
 Before 1960, there were no westward-migrating
blackcaps in Germany.
 By the 1990s, westward migrants made up 7–11% of
the blackcap populations of Germany.
 Berthold suggested that westward migrants benefited
from their new behavior, due to the milder winter
climate and greater abundance of bird feeders in
Britain.

Concept 51.5 Natural selection favors behaviors that increase


survival and reproductive success
 The genetic components of behavior evolve through natural
selection favoring traits that enhance survival and
reproductive success in a population.
 Two of the most direct ways that behavior can affect fitness
are through influences on foraging and mate choice.
 Foraging includes not only eating, but also any mechanisms that
an animal uses to recognize, search for, and capture food
items.
 Optimal foraging theory views foraging behavior as a
compromise between the benefits of nutrition and the costs of
obtaining food, such as the energy expenditure and risk of
predation while foraging.
 Natural selection should favor foraging behavior that
minimizes the costs of foraging and maximizes the benefits.
 Behavioral ecologists apply cost-benefit analysis to study the
proximate and ultimate causes of diverse foraging strategies.
 Reta Zach of the University of British Columbia carried out a
cost-benefit analysis of feeding behavior in crows.
 Crows search the tide pools of Mandarte Island, B.C., for
snails called whelks.
 A crow flies up and drops the whelk onto the rocks to break
its shell.
 If the drop is successful, the crow eats the snail’s soft
body.
 If it is not successful, the crow flies higher and tries again.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-14
 Zach predicted—and found—that crows would, on average,
fly to a height that would provide the most food relative to
the total amount of energy required to break the whelk
shells.
 Bluegill sunfish feed on small crustaceans called Daphnia,
selecting larger individuals that supply the most energy per
unit time.
 Smaller individuals will be selected if larger prey are too far
away.
 Optimal foraging theory predicts that the proportion of small
to large prey captured will vary with prey density.
 At high densities, it is efficient for bluegill sunfish to feed
only on large crustaceans.
 At low densities, bluegill sunfish should exhibit little size
selectivity because all prey are needed to meet energy
requirements.
 In experiments, young bluegill sunfish forage efficiently but
not as close to optimum as older individuals.
 Perhaps younger fish do not judge size and distance as
accurately because their vision is not yet completely
developed.
 Learning may also improve the foraging efficiency of bluegill
sunfish as they age.
 Risk of predation is one of the most significant potential costs
to a forager.
 Mule deer are preyed on by mountain lions throughout their
range.
 Researchers studied mule deer populations in Idaho to
determine if they forage in a way that reduces their risk of
falling prey to mountain lions.
 The researchers found that food available to mule deer was
fairly uniform across the potential foraging area.
 Risk of predation varied greatly, however.
 Mountain lions killed most mule deer at forest edges.
 Few were killed in open areas and forest interiors.

 How does mule deer feeding behavior respond to the


differences in feeding risk?
 Mule deer feed predominantly in open areas, avoiding forest
edges and forest interiors.
 When deer are at the forest edge, they spend significantly
more time scanning their surroundings than when they are in
other areas.
 Mating behavior, which includes seeking and attracting mates,
choosing among potential mates, and competing for mates, is
the product of a form of natural selection called sexual
selection.
Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-15
 The mating relationship between males and females varies a
great deal from species to species.
 In many species, mating is promiscuous, with no strong pair-
bond or lasting relationships.
 In species where the mates remain together for a longer
period, the relationship may be monogamous (one male
mating with one female) or polygamous (one individual
mating with several partners).
 Polygamous relationships may involve a single male and many
females (polygyny) or a single female and many males
(polyandry).
 Among monogamous species, males and females are often so
much alike morphologically that they are impossible to
distinguish based on external characteristics.
 Polygynous species are generally dimorphic, with males being
larger and more showy.
 In polyandrous species, females are ornamented and larger
than males.
 The needs of young are an important factor constraining the
evolution of mating systems.
 Parental investment refers to the time and resources
expended for the raising of offspring.
 Most newly hatched birds cannot care for themselves and
require a large, continuous food supply that a single parent
cannot provide.
 In such cases, a male will have more successful offspring if
he helps his partner to rear their chicks than if he goes off
to seek more mates.
 This is why most birds are monogamous.
 Birds with young that can feed and care for themselves from
birth, such as pheasant and quail, have less need for parents to
stay together.
 Males of these species can maximize their reproductive
success by seeking other mates.
 In mammals, the lactating female is often the only food source
for the young, and males play no role in caring for them in most
mammal species.
 In some mammal species, males protect many females and
their young.
 Certainty of paternity can influence mating systems and
parental care.
 If the male is unsure if offspring are his, parental
investment is likely to be lower.
 Females can be sure that they contributed to an offspring
when they give birth or lay eggs.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-16
 Males do not have that assurance because the acts of
mating and birth are separated over time.
 Males in many species with internal fertilization engage in
behaviors that appear to increase their certainty of
paternity, including guarding females, removing sperm from
the female’s reproductive tract before copulation, and
introducing large numbers of sperm to displace the sperm of
other males.
 Certainty of paternity is much higher when egg laying and
mating occur together, in external fertilization.
 Parental care in aquatic invertebrates, fishes, and
amphibians, when it occurs, is as likely to be by males as
females.
 Male parental care occurs in only 7% of fish and
amphibian families with internal fertilization and in 69%
of families with external fertilization.
 The expression certainty of paternity does not imply
conscious awareness of paternity by the father.
Sexual selection is a form of natural selection.
 Sexual dimorphism within a species results from sexual
selection, a form of natural selection in which differences in
reproductive success among individuals are a consequence of
differences in mating success.
 Sexual selection can take the form of intersexual selection,
in which members of one sex choose mates on the basis of
particular characteristics of the other sex—such as
courtship songs, or intrasexual selection, which involves
competition among members of one sex for mates.
 Mate preferences by females may play a central role in the
evolution of male behavior and anatomy through intersexual
selection.
 Witte and Sawka experimented to see whether imprinting by
young zebra finches on their parents influenced their choice of
mates when they matured.
 They taped a red feather to the heads of both parents,
male parent only, or female parent only, before the young
chicks opened their eyes.
 Control zebra finches were reared by unadorned parents.
 When the chicks matured, they were given a choice of
ornamented or unornamented mate finches.
 Males showed no preference, but females reared by
ornamented fathers preferred ornamented mates.
 These results suggest that females imprint on their fathers
and that mate choice by female zebra finches has played a key
role in evolution of ornamentation in male zebra finches.
 Courtship behaviors of stalk-eyed flies are fascinating.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-17
 Males have elongated eyestalks, which they display to
females during courtship.
 Females prefer to mate with males with relatively long
eyestalks.
 How is this preference adaptive for females?
 Researchers have correlated certain genetic disorders in
male flies with an inability to develop long eyestalks.
 Males with long eyestalks may be demonstrating their
genetic quality to females.
 In general, ornaments such as long eyestalks and brightly
colored feathers correlate with a male’s health and vitality.
 A female that chooses a healthy male increases the chance
that her offspring will be healthy.
 Males compete with each other by (often ritualized) agonistic
behaviors that determine which competitors gain access to
resources.
 The outcome of such contests may be determined by
strength or size.
 In some species, more than one mating behavior can result in
successful reproduction.
 In such cases, intrasexual selection has led to the evolution
of alternative male mating behavior and morphology.
 Alternative male mating behaviors have been documented in
the marine intertidal isopod Paracerceis sculpta, which lives in
sponges in the Gulf of California.
 This species includes three genetically distinct male types—
alpha, beta, and gamma.
 Large alpha males defend harems of females within
intertidal sponges, largely against other alpha males.
 Beta males mimic female morphology and behavior and gain
access to guarded harems.
 Tiny gamma males invade and live within large harems.
 The mating success of each type of isopod depends on the
relative density of males and females in the sponges.
 The alpha males sire the majority of young when defending
a single female.
 If more than one female is present, beta males father 60%
of the offspring.
 The reproductive rate of gamma males increases linearly
with harem size.
 Overall, all three types of males have approximately equal
mating success, and variation among males in this species is
sustained by natural selection.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-18
Game theory can model behavioral strategies.
 Game theory evaluates alternative strategies in situations
where the outcome depends on each individual’s strategies and
the strategies of other individuals.
 Barry Sinervo and Curt Lively used game theory to account for
the existence of three different male phenotypes in
populations of side-blotched lizards (Uta stansburiana).
 Males have three genetically controlled colors: orange throats,
blue throats, and yellow throats.
 Orange-throat males are the most aggressive and defend
large territories with many females.
 Blue-throat males are also aggressive but defend smaller
territories with fewer females.
 Yellow-throat males are nonterritorial and use sneaky
tactics to mimic females and sneak copulations.
 Frequency of the three types of males varies from year to
year.
 Modeling showed that the relative success of different
males varies with the abundance of other types of males.
 When blue-throat males are abundant, they can defend
their few females from the sneaky yellow-throat males.
 However, they cannot defend their territories against
the aggressive orange-throat males.
 Orange-throat males take over large territories but
cannot defend large numbers of females against the
sneaky yellow-throat males.
 Yellow-throat males then increase in numbers but are
defeated by the blue-throat males.
 The cycle continues.

Concept 51.6 The concept of inclusive fitness can account for


most altruistic social behavior
 Most social behaviors are selfish, meaning that they benefit
the individual at the expense of others, especially competitors.
 Behavior that maximizes an individual’s survival and
reproductive success is favored by selection, regardless of its
effect on other individuals.
 How do we account for behaviors that help others?
 Altruism is defined as behavior that appears to decrease
individual fitness but increases the fitness of others.
 Belding’s ground squirrel lives in some mountainous regions of
the western United States.
 The squirrel is vulnerable to predators such as coyotes and
hawks.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-19
 If a squirrel sees a predator approach, it often gives a high-
pitched alarm call, which alerts unaware individuals.
 The alerted squirrels then retreat to their burrows.
 This conspicuous alarm behavior calls attention to the caller,
who has a greater risk of being killed.
 In honeybees, workers are sterile but labor on behalf of a
single fertile queen.
 Workers will sacrifice themselves to sting intruders in
defense of the hive.
 Naked mole rats are highly social rodents that live in
underground chambers and tunnels in Africa.
 These rodents are hairless and nearly blind and live in
colonies of 75–250 individuals.
 Each colony has only one reproducing female, the queen, who
mates with one to three males, called kings.
 The rest of the colony consists of nonreproductive females
and males who forage for underground roots and tubers and
care for the kings, queen, and young rats.
 How can a naked mole rat (or a honeybee or a ground squirrel)
enhance its fitness by helping other members of the
population?
 How is altruistic behavior maintained by evolution?
 If related individuals help each other, they are, in effect,
helping keep their own genes in the population.
 Inclusive fitness is defined as the effect an individual has on
proliferating its own genes by reproducing and by helping
relatives raise offspring.
 William Hamilton proposed a quantitative measure for
predicting when natural selection should favor altruistic acts.
 Hamilton’s rule states the conditions under which altruistic
acts will be favored by natural selection.
 The three key variables are as follows:
1. The benefit to the recipient is B.
2. The cost to the altruist is C.
3. The coefficient of relatedness is r, which equals the
probability that a particular gene present in one individual
will also be inherited from a common parent or ancestor in a
second individual.
 The rule is as follows:
 rB > C
 The more closely related two individuals are, the greater
the value of altruism.
 Kin selection is the mechanism of inclusive fitness, where
individuals help relatives raise young.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-20
 Some animals behave altruistically toward others who are not
close relatives.
 Such behavior can be adaptive if the aided individual can be
counted on to return the favor in the future.
 This exchange of aid is called reciprocal altruism and is
commonly used to explain altruism between unrelated humans.
 Reciprocal altruism is limited to species with stable social
groups in which individuals have many opportunities to
exchange aid and where there would be negative social
consequences for those who “cheat” and refuse to return
favors to those who have helped them in the past.
 However, because cheating may provide a large benefit to
cheaters, behavioral ecologists have questioned how reciprocal
altruism could arise.
 To answer this question, behavioral ecologists have turned to
game theory.
 Axelrod and Hamilton found that reciprocal altruism can
evolve and persist in a population where individuals adopt a
behavioral strategy called tit for tat.
 In this strategy, an individual treats another individual the
same way it was treated the last time they met.
 Individuals are always altruistic, or cooperative, on the first
encounter, and will remain so as long as their altruism is
reciprocated.
 When it is not, they will retaliate immediately but will
return to cooperative behavior as soon as the other
individual becomes cooperative.
Animals learn by observing others.
 Social learning is learning through observing others.
 Social learning forms the roots of culture, which can be
defined as a system of information transfer through social
learning or teaching.
 Cultural transfer of information has the potential to
alter behavioral phenotypes and influence the fitness of
individuals.
 Social learning is not restricted to humans.
 In many species, mate choice is strongly influenced by social
learning.
 Mate choice copying, a behavior in which individuals in a
population copy the mate choices of others, has been
extensively studied in the guppy Poecilia reticulata.
 Female guppies prefer to mate with males having a high
percentage of orange coloration.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-21
 However, if a female sees another female engaging in courtship
with a male with relatively little orange, she will choose a male
with little orange herself.
 Below a certain threshold of difference in mate color, mate
choice copying by female guppies can mask genetically
controlled female preference for orange males.
 What is the advantage for females?
 A female that mates with males that are attractive to other
females may increase the probability that her male
offspring will also be attractive and have high reproductive
success.
 In their studies of vervet monkeys in Amboseli National Park,
Kenya, Dorothy Cheny and Richard Seyfarth found that
performance of a behavior can improve through learning.
 Vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) produce a complex
set of alarm calls.
 Distinct alarm calls warn of leopards, eagles, or snakes, all
of which prey on the small vervets.
 Vervets react to each alarm differently, depending on the
threat.
 Infant vervets give alarm calls but in an undiscriminating
way.
 For example, they call “eagle” for any bird.
 With age, they improve their accuracy.
 Vervets learn how to give the right call by observing
other members of the group and by receiving social
confirmation for accurate calls.
Sociobiology places social behavior in an evolutionary context.
 Human culture is related to evolutionary theory in the
discipline of sociobiology, whose main premise is that certain
behavioral characteristics exist because they are expressions
of genes that have been perpetuated by natural selection.
 In his seminal 1975 book Sociobiology: The New Synthesis, E.
O. Wilson speculated about the evolutionary basis of certain
kinds of social behavior in nonhuman animals, but he also
included human culture, sparking a heated debate.
 The spectrum of possible human social behaviors may be
influenced by our genetic makeup, but that is very different
from saying that genes are rigid determinants of behavior.
 This distinction is at the core of the debate about evolutionary
perspectives on human behavior.
 Evolutionary explanations of human behavior do not reduce
us to robots stamped out of rigid genetic molds.
 Just as individuals vary extensively in anatomy, so we should
expect variation in behavior.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-22
 Because of our capacity for learning, human behavior is
probably more plastic than that of any other animal.
 Over our recent evolutionary history, we have built up a
diversity of structured societies with governments, laws,
religions, and cultural values that define acceptable and
unacceptable behavior, even when unacceptable behavior might
enhance an individual’s Darwinian fitness.
 In human behavior, as in other animals, genes and
environmental factors build on each other.
 What is unique about our species?
 Perhaps it is our social and cultural institutions that provide
us with the only uniquely human feature.

Lecture Outline for Campbell/Reece Biology, 7th Edition, © Pearson Education, Inc. 51-23

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy