Eye Brain and Vision
Eye Brain and Vision
Second Edition
by
David H. Hubel
ISBN: 978-0716750208
This EPub version (v0.5) of Eye, Brian, and Vision is brought to you by Richard J. Cui at
Phoenix, USA, April 2017.
Preface
This book is mainly about the development of our ideas on how the
brain handles visual information; it covers roughly the period between 1950
and 1980. The book is unabashedly concerned largely with research that I
have been involved in or have taken a close interest in. I count myself lucky
to have been around in that era, a time of excitement and fun. Some of the
experiments have been arduous, or so it has often seemed at 4:00 A.M.,
especially when everything has gone wrong. But 98 percent of the time the
work is exhilarating. There is a special immediacy to neurophysiological
experiments: we can see and hear a cell respond to the stimuli we use and
often realize, right at the time, what the responses imply for brain function.
And in modern science, neurobiology is still an area in which one can work
alone or with one colleague, on a budget that is minuscule by the standards of
particle physics or astronomy.
The neurobiology group in the Department of Pharmacology at Harvard Medical School, 1963, the
group that later formed the Department of Neurobiology. Standing (left to right): Edwin Furshpan,
Stephen Kuffler, David Hubel. Seated (left to right): David Potter, Edward Kravitz, and Torsten Wiesel.
To have trained and worked on the North American continent has been
a special piece of good luck, given the combination of a wonderful university
system and a government that has consistently backed research in biology,
especially in vision. I can only hope that we have the sense to cherish and
preserve such blessings.
In writing the book I have had the astronomer in mind as my
prototypical reader—someone with scientific training but not an expert in
biology, let alone neurobiology. I have tried to give just enough background
to make the neurobiology comprehensible, without loading the text down
with material of interest only to experts. To steer a course between excessive
superficiality and excessive detail has not been easy, especially because the
very nature of the brain compels us to look at a wealth of articulated,
interrelated details in order to come away with some sense of what it is and
does.
All the research described here, in which I have played any part, has
been the outcome of joint efforts. From 1958 to the late 1970s my work was
in partner-ship with Torsten Wiesel. Had it not been for his ideas, energy,
enthusiasm, stamina, and willingness to put up with an exasperating
colleague, the out-come would have been very different. Both of us owe a
profound debt to Stephen Kuffler, who in the early years guided our work
with the lightest hand imaginable, encouraged us with his boundless
enthusiasm, and occasionally discouraged our duller efforts simply by
looking puzzled.
For help in writing one needs critics (I certainly do)—the harsher and
more unmerciful, the better. I owe a special debt to Eric Kandel for his help
with the emphasis in the opening three chapters, and to my colleague
Margaret Livingstone, who literally tore three of the chapters apart. One of
her comments began, “First you are vague, and then you are snide …”. She
also tolerated much irascibility and postponement of research. To the editors
of the Scientific American Library, notably Susan Moran, Linda Davis,
Gerard Piel, and Linda Chaput, and to the copyeditor, Cynthia Farden, I owe
a similar debt: I had not realized how much a book depends on able and
devoted editors. They corrected countless English solecisms, but their help
went far beyond that, to spotting duplications, improving clarity, and
tolerating my insistence on placing commas and periods after quotation
marks. Above all, they would not stop bugging me until I had written the
ideas in an easily understandable (I hope!) form. I want to thank Carol
Donner for her artwork, as well as Nancy Field, the designer, Melanie
Nielson, the illustration coordinator, and Susan Stetzer, the production
coordinator. I am also grateful for help in the form of critical reading from
Susan Abookire, David Cardozo, Whittemore Tingley, Deborah Gordon,
Richard Masland, and Laura Regan. As always, my secretary, Olivia Brum,
was helpful to the point of being indispensable and tolerant of my moods
beyond any reasonable call of duty. My wife, Ruth, contributed much advice
and put up with many lost weekends. It will be a relief not to have to hear my
children say, “Daddy, when are you going to finish that book?” It has, at
times, seemed as remote a quest as Sancho Panza’s island.
— David H. Hubel
The changes I have made for this paperback edition consist mainly of
minor corrections, the most embarrassing of which is the formula for
converting degrees to radians. My high school mathematics teachers must be
turning over in their graves. I have not made any attempt to incorporate
recent research on the visual cortex, which in the last ten years has mostly
focussed on areas beyond the striate cortex. To extend the coverage to
include these areas would have required another book. I did feel that it would
be unforgivable not to say something about two major advances: the work of
Jeremy Nathans on the genetics of visual pigments, and recent work on the
development of the visual system, by Caria Schatz, Michael Stryker, and
others.
— David H. Hubel, January 1995
CONTENTS
PREFACE
1. INTRODUCTION
3. THE EYE
3.1 THE EYEBALL
3.2 THE RETINA
3.3 THE RECEPTIVE FIELDS OF RETINAL GANGLION CELLS: THE OUTPUT OF
THE EYE
3.4 THE CONCEPT OF A RECEPTIVE FIELD
3.5 THE OVERLAP OF RECEPTIVE FIELDS
3.6 DIMENSIONS OF RECEPTIVE FIELDS
3.6 THE PHOTORECEPTORS
3.7 BIPOLAR CELLS AND HORIZONTAL CELLS
3.8 AMACRINE CELLS
3.9 CONNECTIONS BETWEEN BIPOLAR CELLS AND GANGLION CELLS
3.10 THE SIGNIFICANCE OF CENTER-SURROUND FIELDS
3.11 CONCLUSION
8. COLOR VISION
8.1 THE NATURE OF LIGHT
8.2 PIGMENTS
8.3 VISUAL RECEPTORS
8.4 GENERAL COMMENTS ON COLOR
8.5 THEORIES OF COLOR VISION
8.6 THE GENETICS OF VISUAL PIGMENTS
8.7 THE HERING THEORY
8.8 COLOR AND THE SPATIAL VARIABLE
8.9 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF COLOR VISION: EARLY RESULTS
8.10 THE NEURAL BASIS OF COLOR CONSTANCY
8.11 BLOBS
8.12 CONCLUSION
9. DEPRIVATION AND DEVELOPMENT
9.1 RECOVERY
9.2 THE NATURE OF THE DEFECT
9.3 STRABISMUS
9.4 THE ANATOMICAL CONSEQUENCES OF DEPRIVATION
9.5 NORMAL DEVELOPMENT OF EYE-DOMINANCE COLUMNS
9.6 FURTHER STUDIES IN NEURAL PLASTICITY
9.7 THE ROLE OF PATTERNED ACTIVITY IN NEURAL DEVELOPMENT
9.8 THE BROADER IMPLICATIONS OF DEPRIVATION RESULTS
FURTHER READING
SOURCES OF ILLUSTRATIONS
INDEX
1. INTRODUCTION
Santiago Ramon y Cajal playing chess (white) in 1898, at an age of about 46, while on vacation in
Miraflores de la Sierra. This picture was taken by one of his children. Most neuroanatomists would
agree that Ramon y Cajal stands out far before anyone else in their field and probably in the entire field
of central nervous neurobiology. His two major contributions were (1) establishing beyond reasonable
doubt that nerve cells act as independent units, and (2) using the Golgi method to map large parts of the
brain and spinal cord, so demonstrating both the extreme complexity and extreme orderliness of the
nervous system. For his work he, together with Golgi, received the Nobel Prize in 1906.
This view of a human brain seen from the left and slightly behind shows the cerebral cortex and
cerebellum. A small part of the brainstem can be seen just in front of the cerebellum.
How should we set about finding out? Our first thought might be that a
detailed understanding of the connections, from the eye to the brain and
within the brain, should be enough to allow us to deduce how it works.
Unfortunately, that is only true to a limited extent. The regions of cortex at
the back of the human brain were long known to be important for vision
partly because around the turn of the century the eyes were discovered to
make connections, through an intermediate way station, to this part of the
brain. But to deduce from the structure alone what the cells in the visual
cortex are doing when an animal or person looks at the sky or a tree would
require a knowledge of anatomy far exceeding what we possess even now.
And we would have trouble even if we did have a complete circuit diagram,
just as we would if we tried to understand a computer or radar set from their
circuit diagrams alone—especially if we did not know what the computer or
radar set was for.
Our increasing knowledge of the working of the visual cortex has come
from a combination of strategies. Even in the late 1950s, the physiological
method of recording from single cells was starting to tell us roughly what the
cells were doing in the daily life of an animal, at a time when little progress
was being made in the detailed wiring diagram. In the past few decades both
fields, physiology and anatomy, have gone ahead in parallel, each borrowing
techniques and using new information from the other.
I have sometimes heard it said that the nervous system consists of huge
numbers of random connections. Although its orderliness is indeed not
always obvious, I nevertheless suspect that those who speak of random
networks in the nervous system are not constrained by any previous exposure
to neuroanatomy. Even a glance at a book such as Cajal’s Histologie du
Systeme Nerveux should be enough to convince anyone that the enormous
complexity of the nervous system is almost always accompanied by a
compelling degree of orderliness. When we look at the orderly arrays of cells
in the brain, the impression is the same as when we look at a telephone
exchange, a printing press, or the inside of a TV set—that the orderliness
surely serves some purpose. When confronted with a human invention, we
have little doubt that the whole machine and its separate parts have
understandable functions. To understand them we need only read a set of
instructions. In biology, we develop a similar faith in the functional validity
and even ultimately in the understandability of structures that were not
invented, but were perfected through millions of years of evolution. The
problem of the neurobiologist (to be sure, not the only problem) is to learn
how the order and complexity relate to the function.
The principal parts of the nerve cell are the cell body containing the nucleus and other organelles; the
single axon, which conveys impulses from the cell; and the dendrites, which receive impulses from
other cells.
The connections within and between cells or groups of cells in the brain
are usually not obvious, and it has taken centuries to work out the most
prominent pathways. Because several bundles of fibers often streak through
each other in dense meshworks, we need special methods to reveal each
bundle separately. Any piece of brain we choose to examine can be packed to
an incredible degree with cell bodies, dendrites, and axons, with little space
between. As a result, methods of staining cells that can resolve and reveal the
organization of a more loosely packed structure, such as the liver or kidney,
produce only a dense black smear in the brain. But neuroanatomists have
devised powerful new ways of revealing both the separate cells in a single
structure and the connections between different structures.
As you might expect, neurons having similar or related functions are
often interconnected. Richly interconnected cells are often grouped together
in the nervous system, for the obvious reason that short axons are more
efficient: they are cheaper to make, take up less room, and get their messages
to their destinations faster. The brain therefore contains hundreds of
aggregations of cells, which may take the form of balls or of stacks of layered
plates. The cerebral cortex is an example of a single gigantic plate of cells,
two millimeters thick and a square foot or so in area. Short connections can
run between the neurons within a given structure, or large numbers of long
fibers that form cables, or tracts, can run from one structure to another. The
balls or plates are often connected in serial order as pathways (see the
illustration on the next page).
A good example of such a serially connected system is the visual
pathway. The retina of each eye consists of a plate having three layers of
cells, one of which contains the light-sensitive receptor cells, or rods and
cones. As we saw earlier, each eye contains over 125 million receptors. The
two retinas send their output to two peanut-size nests of cells deep within the
brain, the lateral geniculate bodies. These structures in turn send their fibers
to the visual part of the cerebral cortex. More specifically, they go to the
striate cortex, or primary visual cortex. From there, after being passed from
layer to layer through several sets of synaptically connected cells, the
information is sent to several neighboring higher visual areas; each of these
sends its output to several others (see the illustration on the next page). Each
of these cortical areas contains three or four synaptic stages, just as the retina
did. The lobe of the brain farthest to the rear, the occipital lobe, contains at
least a dozen of these visual areas (each about the size of a postage stamp),
and many more seem to be housed in the parietal and temporal lobes just in
front of that. Here, however, our knowledge of the path becomes vague.
Our main goal in this book will be to understand why all these chains
of neuronal structures exist, how they work, and what they do. We want to
know what kind of visual information travels along a trunk of fibers, and how
the information is modified in each region—retina, lateral geniculate body,
and the various levels of cortex. We attack the problem by using the
microelectrode, the single most important tool in the modern era of
neurophysiology. We insert the microelectrode (usually a fine insulated wire)
into whatever structure we wish to study—for example, the lateral geniculate
body—so that its tip comes close enough to a cell to pick up its electrical
signals. We attempt to influence those signals by shining spots or patterns of
light on the animal’s retina.
The visual pathway. Each structure, shown as a box, consists of millions of cells, aggregated into
sheets. Each receives inputs from one or more structures at lower levels in the path and each sends its
output to several structures at higher levels. The path has been traced only for four or five stages
beyond the primary visual cortex.
Because the lateral geniculate body receives its main input from the
retina, each cell in the geniculate will receive connections from rods and
cones—not directly but by way of intermediate retinal cells. As you will see
in Chapter 3, the population of rods and cones that feed into a given cell in
the visual pathway are not scattered about all over the retina but are clustered
into a small area. This area of the retina is called the receptive field of the
cell. So our first step, in shining the light here and there on the retina, is to
find the cell’s receptive field. Once we have defined the receptive field’s
boundaries, we can begin to vary the shape, size, color, and rate of movement
of the stimulus—to learn what kinds of visual stimuli cause the cell to
respond best.
We do not have to shine our light directly into the retina. It is usually
easier and more natural to project our stimuli onto a screen a few meters
away from the animal. The eye then produces on the retina a well-focused
image of the screen and the stimulus. We can now go ahead and determine
the position, on the screen, of the receptive field’s projection. If we wish, we
can think of the receptive field as the part of the animal’s visual world—in
this case, the screen— that is seen by the cell we are recording from. We
soon learn that cells can be choosy, and usually are. It may take some time
and groping before we succeed in finding a stimulus that produces a really
vigorous response from the cell. At first we may have difficulty even finding
the receptive field on the screen, although at early stages, such as in the
geniculate, we may locate it easily. Cells in the geniculate are choosy as to
the size of a spot they will respond to or as to whether it is black on a white
background or white on black. At higher levels in the brain, an edge (the line
produced by a light-dark boundary) may be required to evoke a response
from some cells, in which case the cells are likely to be fussy about the
orientation of the edge— whether it is vertical, horizontal, or oblique. It may
be important whether the stimulus is stationary or moves across the retina (or
screen), or whether it is colored or white. If both eyes are looking at the
screen, the exact screen distance may be crucial. Different cells, even within
the same structure, may differ greatly in the stimuli to which they respond.
We learn everything we can think to ask about a cell, and then move the
electrode forward a fraction of a millimeter to the next cell, where we start
testing all over again.
An experimental plan for recording from the visual pathway. The animal, usually a macaque monkey,
faces a screen onto which we project a stimulus. We record by inserting a microlectrode into some part
of the pathway, in this case, the primary visual cortex. (The brain in this diagram is from a human, but a
monkey brain is very similar.)
A large part of neuroscience concerns the nuts and bolts of the subject:
how single cells work and how information is conveyed from cell to cell
across synapses. It should be obvious that without such knowledge we are in
the position of someone who wants to understand the workings of a radio or
TV but does not know anything about resistors, condensers, or transistors. In
the last few decades, thanks to the ingenuity of several neurophysiologists, of
whom the best known are Andrew Huxley, Alan Hodgkin, Bernard Katz,
John Eccles, and Stephen Kuffler, the physicochemical mechanisms of nerve
and synaptic transmission have become well understood. It should be equally
obvious, however, that this kind of knowledge by itself cannot lead to an
understanding of the brain, just as knowledge about resistors, condensers, and
transistors alone will not make us understand a radio or TV, or knowledge of
the chemistry of ink equip us to understand a Shakespeare play. In this
chapter I begin by summing up part of what we know about nerve conduction
and synaptic transmission. To grasp the subject adequately, it is a great help
to know some physical chemistry and electricity, but I think that anyone can
get a reasonable feel for the subject without that. And in any case you only
need a very rudimentary understanding of these topics to follow the
subsequent chapters.
The job of a nerve cell is to take in information from the cells that feed
into it, to sum up, or integrate, that information, and to deliver the integrated
information to other cells. The information is usually conveyed in the form of
brief events called nerve impulses. In a given cell, one impulse is the same as
any other; they are stereotyped events. At any moment a cell's rate of firing
impulses is determined by information it has just received from the cells
feeding into it, and its firing rate conveys information to the cells that it in
turn feeds into. Impulse rates vary from one every few seconds or even
slower to about 1000 per second at the extreme upper limit.
This cross-sectional microscopic drawing of the nerve cells in the retina was made by Santiago Ramon
y Cajal, the greatest neuroanatomist of all time. From the top, where the slender rods and fatter cones
are shown, to the bottom, where optic nerve fibers lead off to the right, the retina measures one-quarter
millimeter.
First, how does the charge get there? Suppose you start with no charge
across the membrane and with the concentrations of all ions equal inside and
outside. Now you turn on a pump that ejects one kind of ion, say sodium, and
for each ion ejected brings in another kind, say potassium. The pump will not
in itself produce any charge across the membrane, because just as many
positively charged ions are pumped in as are pumped out (sodium and
potassium ions both having one positive charge). But now imagine that for
some reason a large number of pores of one type, say the potassium pores,
are opened. Potassium ions will start to flow, and the rate of flow through any
given open pore will depend on the potassium concentrations: the more ions
there are near a pore opening, the more will leak across, and because more
potassium ions are inside than outside, more will flow out than in. With more
charge leaving than entering, the outside will quickly become positive with
respect to the inside. This accumulation of charge across the membrane soon
tends to discourage further potassium ions from leaving the cell, because like
charges repel one another. Very quickly—before enough K+ ions cross to
produce a measurable change in the potassium concentration—the positive-
outside charge builds up to the point at which it just balances the tendency of
K+ ions to leave. (There are more potassium ions just inside the pore
opening, but they are repelled by the charge.) From then on, no net charge
transfer occurs, and we say the system is in equilibrium. In short, the opening
of potassium pores results in a charge across the membrane, positive outside.
Suppose, instead, we had opened the sodium pores. By repeating the
argument, substituting "inside" for "outside", you can easily see that the result
would be just the reverse, a negative charge outside. If we had opened both
types of pores at the same time, the result would be a compromise. To
calculate what the membrane potential is, we have to know the relative
concentrations of the two ions and the ratios of open to closed pores for each
ion—and then do some algebra.
It is analogous to any explosive event. How fast the bullet travels has
nothing to do with how hard you pull the trigger. For many brain functions
the speed of the impulse seems to be very important, and the nervous system
has evolved a special mechanism for increasing it. Glial cells wrap their
plasma membrane around and around the axon like a jelly roll, forming a
sheath that greatly increases the effective thickness of the nerve membrane.
This added thickness reduces the membrane's capacitance, and hence the
amount of charge required to depolarize the nerve. The layered substance,
rich in fatty material, is called myelin. The sheath is interrupted every few
millimeters, at nodes of Ranvier, to allow the currents associated with the
impulse to enter or leave the axon. The result is that the nerve impulse in
effect jumps from one node to the next rather than traveling continuously
along the membrane, which produces a great increase in conduction velocity.
The fibers making up most of the large, prominent cables in the brain are
myelinated, giving them a glistening white appearance on freshly cut
sections. White matter in the brain and spinal cord consists of myelinated
axons but no nerve cell bodies, dendrites, or synapses. Grey matter is made
up mainly of cell bodies, dendrites, axon terminals, and synapses, but may
contain myelinated axons. The main gaps remaining in our understanding of
the impulse, and also the main areas of present-day research on the subject,
have to do with the structure and function of the protein channels.
This scanning electron microscope picture shows a neuromuscular junction in a frog. The slender nerve
fiber curls down over two muscle fibers, with the synapse at the lower left of the picture.
At the other end of the nervous system we have the output: the motor
neurons, nerves that are exceptional in that their axons end not on other nerve
cells but on muscle cells. All the output of our nervous system takes the form
of muscle contractions, with the minor exception of nerves that end on gland
cells. This is the way, indeed the only way, we can exert an influence on our
environment. Eliminate an animal's muscles and you cut it off completely
from the rest of the world; equally, eliminate the input and you cut off all
outside influences, again virtually converting the animal into a vegetable. An
animal is, by one possible definition, an organism that reacts to outside
events and that influences the outside world by its actions. The central
nervous system, lying between input cells and output cells, is the machinery
that allows us to perceive, react, and remember—and it must be responsible,
in the end, for our consciousness, consciences, and souls. One of the main
goals in neurobiology is to learn what takes place along the way—how the
information arriving at a certain group of cells is transformed and then sent
on, and how the transformations make sense in terms of the successful
functioning of the animal.
Many parts of the central nervous system are organized in successive platelike stages. A cell in one
stage receives many excitatory and inhibitory inputs from the previous stage and sends outputs to many
cells at the next stage. The primary input to the nervous system is from receptors in the eyes, ears, skin,
and so on, which translate outside information such as light, heat, or sound into electrical nerve signals.
The output is contraction of muscles or secretions from gland cells.
Although the wiring diagrams for the many subdivisions of the central
nervous system vary greatly in detail, most tend to be based on the relatively
simple general plan schematized in the diagram on this page. The diagram is
a caricature, not to be taken literally, and subject to qualifications that I will
soon discuss. On the left of the figure I show the receptors, an array of
information-transducing nerves each subserving one kind of sensation such as
touch, vibration, or light. We can think of these receptors as the first stage in
some sensory pathway. Fibers from the receptors make synaptic contacts with
a second array of nerve cells, the second stage in our diagram; these in turn
make contact with a third stage, and so on. “Stage” is not a technical or
widely applied neuroanatomical term, but we will find it useful. Sometimes
three or four of these stages are assembled together in a larger unit, which I
will call a structure, for want of any better or widely accepted term. These
structures are the aggregations of cells, usually plates or globs, that I
mentioned in Chapter 1. When a structure is a plate, each of the stages
forming it may be a discrete layer of cells in the plate. A good example is the
retina, which has three layers of cells and, loosely speaking, three stages.
When several stages are grouped to form a larger structure, the nerve fibers
entering from the previous structure and those leaving to go to the next are
generally grouped together into bundles, called tracts. You will notice in the
diagram how common divergence and convergence are: how almost as a rule
the axon from a cell in a given stage splits on arriving at the next stage and
ends on several or many cells, and conversely, a cell at any stage except the
first receives synaptic inputs from a few or many cells in the previous stage.
We obviously need to amend and qualify this simplified diagram, but at least
we have a model to qualify. We must first recognize that at the input end we
have not just one but many sensory systems—vision, touch, taste, smell, and
hearing—and that each system has its own sets of stages in the brain. When
and where in the brain the various sets of stages are brought together, if
indeed they are brought together, is still not clear. In tracing one system such
as the visual or auditory from the receptors further into the brain, we may
find that it splits into separate subdivisions. In the case of vision, these
subsystems might deal separately with eye movements, pupillary
constriction, form, movement, depth, or color. Thus the whole system
diverges into separate subpathways. Moreover, the subpaths may be many,
and may differ widely in their lengths. On a gross scale, some paths have
many structures along the way and others few. At a finer level, an axon from
one stage may not go to the next stage in the series but instead may skip that
stage and even the next; it may go all the way to the motor neuron. (You can
think of the skipping of stages in neuroanatomy as analogous to what can
happen in genealogy. The present English sovereign is not related to William
the Conqueror by a unique number of generations: the number of “greats”
modifying the grandfather is indeterminate because of intermarriage between
nephews and aunts and even more questionable events.) When the path from
input to output is very short, we call it a reflex. In the visual system, the
constriction of the pupil in response to light is an example of a reflex, in
which the number of synapses is probably about six. In the most extreme
case, the axon from a receptor ends directly on a motor neuron, so that we
have, from input to output, only three cells: receptor, motor neuron, and
muscle fiber, and just two synapses, in what we call a monosynaptic reflex
arc. (Perhaps the person who coined the term did not consider the nerve-
muscle junction a real synapse, or could not count to two.) That short path is
activated when the doctor taps your knee with a hammer and your knee
jumps. John Nicholls used to tell his classes at Harvard Medical School that
there are two reasons for testing this reflex: to stall for time, and to see if you
have syphilis. At the output end, we find not only various sets of body
muscles that we can voluntarily control, in the trunk, limbs, eyes, and tongue,
but also sets that subserve the less voluntary or involuntary housekeeping
functions, such as making our stomachs churn, our water pass or bowels
move, and our sphincters (between these events) hold orifices closed. We
also need to qualify our model with respect to direction of information flow.
The prevailing direction in our diagram on page 10 is obviously from left to
right, from input to output, but in almost every case in which information is
transferred from one stage to the next, reciprocal connections feed
information back from the second stage to the first. (We can sometimes guess
what such feedback might be useful for, but in almost no case do we have
incisive understanding.) Finally, even within a given stage we often find a
rich network of connections between neighboring cells of the same order.
Thus to say that a structure contains a specific number of stages is almost
always an oversimplification. When I began working in neurology in the
early 1950s, this basic plan of the nervous system was well understood. But
in those days no one had any clear idea how to interpret this bucket-brigade-
like handing on of information from one stage to the next. Today we know
far more about the ways in which the information is transformed in some
parts of the brain; in other parts we still know almost nothing. The remaining
chapters of this book are devoted to the visual system, the one we understand
best today. I will next try to give a preview of a few of the things we know
about that system.
The initial stages of the mammalian visual system have the platelike organization often found in the
central nervous system. The first three stages are housed in the retina; the remainder are in the brain: in
the lateral geniculate bodies and the stages beyond in the cortex.
Illuminating only one such receptor may have hardly any detectable
effect, but if we could illuminate all the receptors with excitatory effects, we
could reasonably expect their summated influences to activate the cell— and
in fact they do. As we will see, for most retinal ganglion cells the best
stimulus turns out to be a small spot of light of just the right size, shining in
just the right place. Among other things, this tells you how important a role
inhibition plays in retinal function.
How these muscles all attach to the eye is not important to us here, but
we can easily see from the illustration that for one eye, say the right, to turn
inward toward the nose, a person must relax the external rectus and contract
the internal rectus muscles. If each muscle did not have some steady pull, or
tone, the eye would be loose in its socket; consequently any eye movement is
made by contracting one muscle and relaxing its opponent by just the same
amount. The same is true for almost all the body's muscle movements.
Furthermore, any movement of one eye is almost always part of a bigger
complex of movements. If we look at an object a short distance away, the two
eyes turn in; if we look to the left, the right eye turns in and the left eye turns
out; if we look up or down, both eyes turn up or down together.
When we flex our fingers by making a fist, the muscles responsible have to pass infront of the wrist and
so tend to contract that joint too. The extensors of the wrist have to contract to offset this tendency and
keep the wrist stiff.
All this movement is directed by the brain. Each eye muscle is made to
contract by the firing of motor neurons in a part of the brain called the
brainstem. To each of the twelve muscles there corresponds a small cluster of
a few hundred motor neurons in the brainstem. These clusters are called
oculomotor nuclei. Each motor neuron in an oculomotor nucleus supplies a
few muscle fibers in an eye muscle. These motor neurons in turn receive
inputs from other excitatory fibers. To obtain a movement such as
convergence of the eyes, we would like to have these antecedent nerves send
their axon branches to the appropriate motor neurons, those supplying the
two internal recti. A single such antecedent cell could have its axon split,
with one branch going to one oculomotor nucleus and the other to its
counterpart on the other side. At the same time we need to have another
antecedent nerve cell or cells, whose axons have inhibitory endings, supply
the motor neurons to the external recti to produce just the right amount of
relaxation. We would like both antecedent sets of cells to fire together, to
produce the contraction and relaxation simultaneously, and for that we could
have one master cell or group of cells, at still another stage back in the
nervous system, excite both groups. This is one way in which we can get
coordinated movements involving many muscles.
Practically every movement we make is the result of many muscles
contracting together and many others relaxing. If you make a fist, the muscles
in the front of your forearm (on the palm side of the hand) contract, as you
can feel if you put your other hand on your forearm. (Most people probably
think that the muscles that flex the fingers are in the hand. The hand does
contain some muscles, but they happen not to be finger flexors.) As the
diagram on the previous page shows, the forearm muscles that flex the
fingers attach to the three bones of each finger by long tendons that can be
seen threading their way along the front of the wrist. What may come as a
surprise is that in making a fist, you also contract muscles on the back of your
forearm. That might seem quite unnecessary until you realize that in making
a fist you want to keep your wrist stiff and in midposition: if you flexed only
the finger flexor muscles, their tendons, passing in front of the wrist, would
flex it too. You have to offset this tendency to unwanted wrist flexion by
contracting the muscles that cock back the wrist, and these are in the back of
the forearm. The point is that you do it but are unaware of it. Moreover, you
don't learn to do it by attending 9 A.M. lectures or paying a coach. A
newborn baby will grasp your finger and hold on tight, making a perfect fist,
with no coaching or lecturing. We presumably have some executive-type
cells in our spinal cords that send excitatory branches both to finger flexors
and to wrist extensors and whose function is to subserve fist making.
Presumably these cells are wired up completely before birth, as are the cells
that allow us to turn our eyes in to look at close objects, without thinking
about it or having to learn.
The eyeball and the muscles that control its position. The cornea and the lens focus the light rays onto
the back of the eye. The lens regulates the focusing for near and far objects by becoming more or less
globular.
Two other sets of muscles change the diameter of the pupil and thus
adjust the amount of light entering the eye, just as the iris diaphragm of a
camera determines the f-stop. One set, with radial fibers like the spokes of a
wheel, opens the pupil; the other, arranged in circles, closes it. Finally, the
self-cleaning of the front of the cornea is achieved by blinking the lids and
lubricating with tear glands. The cornea is richly supplied with nerves
subserving touch and pain, so that the slightest irritation by dust specks sets
up a reflex that leads to blinking and secreting of more tears.
Light enters the eye through the transparent cornea, where much of the bending of light takes place.
The white dot in the pupil is a reflection of light.
The enlarged retina at the right shows the relative positions of the three retinal layers. Surprisingly, the
light has to pass through the ganglion-cell and bipolar-cell layers before it gets to the rods and cones.
As it is, the layers in front of the receptors are fairly transparent and
probably do not blur the image much. In the central one millimeter, however,
where our vision is most acute, the consequences of even slight blurring
would be disastrous, and evolution seems to have gone to some pains to
alleviate it by having the other layers displaced to the side to form a ring of
thicker retina, exposing the central cones so that they lie at the very front. The
resulting shallow pit constitutes the fovea.
Moving from back to front, we come to the middle layer of the retina,
between the rods and cones and the retinal ganglion cells. This layer contains
three types of nerve cells: bipolar cells, horizontal cells, and amacrine cells.
Bipolar cells receive input from the receptors, as the diagram on this page
shows, and many of them feed directly into the retinal ganglion cells.
Horizontal cells link receptors and bipolar cells by relatively long
connections that run parallel to the retinal layers; similarly, amacrine cells
link bipolar cells and retinal ganglion cells. The layer of cells at the front of
the retina contains the retinal ganglion cells, whose axons pass across the
surface of the retina, collect in a bundle at the optic disc, and leave the eye to
form the optic nerve. Each eye contains about 125 million rods and cones but
only 1 million ganglion cells. In the face of this discrepancy, we need to ask
how detailed visual information can be preserved.
Examining the connection between cells in the retina can help resolve
this problem. You can think of the information flow through the retina as
following two paths: a direct path, from light receptors to bipolar cells to
ganglion cells, and an indirect path, in which horizontal cells may be
interposed between the receptors and bipolars, and amacrine cells between
bipolars and retinal ganglion cells. (See the drawing of these direct and
indirect connections on this page). These connections were already worked
out in much detail by Ramon y Cajal around 1900. The direct path is highly
specific or compact, in the sense that one receptor or only relatively few feed
into a bipolar cell, and only one or relatively few bipolars feed into a
ganglion cell. The indirect path is more diffuse, or extended, through wider
lateral connections. The total area occupied by the receptors in the back layer
that feed one ganglion cell in the front layer, directly and indirectly, is only
about one millimeter. That area, as you may remember from Chapter 1, is the
receptive field of the ganglion cell, the region of retina over which we can
influence the ganglion cell's firing by light simulation.
A cross section of the retina, about midway between the fovea and far periphery, where rods are more
numerous than cones. From top to bottom is about one-quarter millimeter.
This general plan holds for the entire retina, but the details of
connections vary markedly between the fovea, which corresponds to exactly
where we are looking—our center of gaze, where our ability to make out fine
detail is highest—and the far outer reaches, or periphery, where vision
becomes relatively crude. Between fovea and periphery, the direct part of the
path from receptor to ganglion cell changes dramatically. In and near the
fovea, the rule for the direct path is that a single cone feeds a single bipolar
cell, and a single bipolar in turn feeds into one ganglion cell. As we go
progressively farther out, however, more receptors converge on bipolars and
more bipolars converge on ganglion cells. This high degree of convergence,
which we find over much of the retina, together with the very compact
pathway in and near the very center, helps to explain how there can be a
125:1 ratio of receptors to optic nerve fibers without our having hopelessly
crude vision. The general scheme of the retinal path, with its direct and
indirect components, was known for many years and its correlation with
visual acuity long recognized before anyone understood the significance of
the indirect path. An understanding suddenly became possible when the
physiology of ganglion cells began to be studied.
As we go progressively farther out, however, more receptors converge
on bipolars and more bipolars converge on ganglion cells. This high degree
of convergence, which we find over much of the retina, together with the
very compact pathway in and near the very center, helps to explain how there
can be a 125:1 ratio of receptors to optic nerve fibers without our having
hopelessly crude vision.
The general scheme of the retinal path, with its direct and indirect
components, was known for many years and its correlation with visual acuity
long recognized before anyone understood the significance of the indirect
path. An understanding suddenly became possible when the physiology of
ganglion cells began to be studied.
Left: Four recordings from a typical on-center retinal ganglion cell. Each record is a single sweep of the
oscilloscope, whose duration is 2.5 seconds. For a sweep this slow, the rising and falling phases of the
circular spot, and maximal off responses to a ring of just the right dimensions impulse coalesce so that
each spike appears as a vertical line. To the left the stimuli are shown. In the resting state at the top,
there is no stimulus: firing is slow and more less random. The lower three records show responses to a
small (optimum size) spot, large spot covering the receptive-field center and surround, and a ring
covering surround only. Right: Responses of an off-center retinal ganglion cell to the same set of
stimuli shown at the left.
The more of a given region, on or off, the stimulus filled, the greater
was the response, so that maximal on responses were obtained to just the
right size circular spot, and maximal off responses to a ring of just the right
dimensions (inner and outer diameters). Typical recordings of responses to
such stimuli are shown on this page. The center and surround regions
interacted in an antagonistic way: the effect of a spot in the center was
diminished by shining a second spot in the surround – as if you were telling
the cell to fire faster and slower at the same time. The most impressive
demonstration of this interaction between center and surround occurred when
a large spot covered the entire receptive field of ganglion cell. This evoked a
response that was much weaker than the response to a spot just filling the
center; indeed, for some cells the effects of center to the same set stimulating
the two regions canceled each other completely.
An off-center cell had just the opposite behavior. Its receptive field
consisted of a small center from which off responses were obtained, and a
surround that gave on responses. The two kinds of cells were intermixed and
seemed to be equally common. An off-center cell discharges at its highest
rate in response to a black spot on a white background, because we are now
illuminating only the surround of its receptive field. In nature, dark objects
are probably just as common as light ones, which may help explain why
information from the retina is in the form of both on-center cells and off-
center cells. If you make a spot progressively larger, the response improves
until the receptive-field center is filled, then it starts to decline as more and
more of the surround is included, as you can see from the graph on the next
page. With a spot covering the entire field, the center either just barely wins
out over the surround, or the result is a draw. This effect explains why
neurophysiologists before Kuffler had such lack of success: they had
recorded from these cells but had generally used diffuse light – clearly from
the ideal stimulus.
You can imagine what a surprise it must have been to observe that
shining a flashlight directly into the eye of an animal evoked such feeble
responses or no response at all. Illuminating all the receptors, as a flashlight
surely does, might have been expected to be the most effective stimulus, not
the least. The mistake is to forget how important inhibitory synapses are in
the nervous system. With nothing more than a wiring diagram such as the one
on page 12 in Chapter 2, we cannot begin to predict the effects of a given
stimulus on any given cell if we do not know which synapses are excitatory
and which are inhibitory. In the early 1950s, when Kuffler was recording
from ganglion cells, the importance of inhibition in the nervous system was
just beginning to be realized.
Left: The two main types of retinal-ganglion-cell receptive fields are on center, with inhibitory
surround, and off center, with excitatory surround. "+" stands for regions giving on responses, "-" for
regions giving off responses. Right: As we stimulate a single on-center retinal ganglion cell with ever
larger spots, the response becomes more powerful, up to a spot size that depends on the cell—at most
about 1 degree. This is the center size. Further enlargement of the spot causes a decline, because now
the spot invades the antagonistic surround. Beyond about 3 degrees there is no further decline, so that 3
degrees represents the total receptive field, center plus surround.
The receptive fields of two neighboring retinal ganglion cells will usually overlap. The smallest spot of
light we can shine on the retina is likely to influence hundreds of ganglion cells, some off center and
some on center. The spot will fall on the centers of some receptive fields and on the surrounds of
others.
Two neighboring retinal ganglion cells receive input over the direct path from two overlapping groups
of receptors. The areas of retina occupied by these receptors make up their receptive-field centers,
shown face on by the large overlapping circles.
One millimeter on the retina corresponds to 3.5 degrees of visual angle. On a screen 1.5 meters away, 1
millimeter on the retina thus corresponds to about 3.5 inches, or 89 millimeters.
This electron-micrograph section through the peripheral retina of a monkey passes through the layers of
rods and cones. The tiny white circles are rods; the larger black regions with a white dot in the center
are cones.
It now appears that the pores of the receptor are kept open by molecules
of a chemical called cyclic guanosine monophosphate, or cGMP. When the
visual pigment molecule is bleached a cascade of events is let loose. The
protein part of the bleached pigment molecule activates a large number of
molecules of an enzyme called transducin; each of these in turn inactivates
hundreds of cGMP molecules, with consequent closing of the pores. Thus as
a result of a single pigment molecule being bleached, millions of pores close
off. All this makes it possible to explain several previously puzzling
phenomena. First, we have long known that a fully dark-adapted human can
see a brief flash of light so feeble that no single receptor can have received
more than 1 photon of light. Calculations show that about six closely spaced
rods must be stimulated, each by a photon of light, within a short time, to
produce a visible flash. It now becomes clear how a single photon can excite
a rod enough to make it emit a significant signal.
Second, we can now explain the inability of rods to respond to changes
in illumination if the light is already bright. It seems that rods are so sensitive
that at high illumination levels—for example, in sunlight—all the sodium
pores are closed, and that any further increase in light can have no further
effect. We say the rods are saturated. Perhaps a few years from now students
of biology will regard this entire story of the receptors as one more thing to
learn—I hope not. To appreciate fully its impact, it helps to have spent the
years wondering how the receptors could possibly work; then suddenly, in
the space of a decade or less of spectacular research, it all unfolds. The sense
of excitement still has not subsided.
In summary, horizontal cells get their input from receptors; their output
is still uncertain, but is either back to receptors, or to bipolar cells, or to both.
The relatively wide retinal area over which receptors supply horizontal cells
suggests that the receptive fields of horizontal cells should be large, and they
are. They are about the size of the entire receptive fields of bipolar cells or
ganglion cells, center plus surround. They are uniform, giving
hyperpolarization wherever you stimulate, and more hyperpolarization the
larger the spot. Much evidence points to the horizontal cells as being
responsible for the receptive-field surrounds of the bipolar cells—indeed they
are the only plausible candidates, being the only cells that connect to
receptors over so wide an expanse. When horizontal cells connect directly to
bipolars, the synapses to on-bipolars would have to be excitatory (for the
effect of light in the surround to be inhibitory) and those to off-bipolars,
inhibitory. If the influence is by way of the receptors, the synapses would
have to be inhibitory.
To sum this up: Bipolar cells have center-surround receptive fields. The
center is supplied by input from a small group of receptors; the surround
arises from an indirect path stemming from a wider expanse of receptors that
feed into horizontal cells, which probably feed into the bipolars. The indirect
path could also be the result of the horizontal cells feeding back and
inhibiting the receptors.
Outdoors Room
____________________________
3.12 CONCLUSION
The output of the eye, after two or three synapses, contains information
that is far more sophisticated than the punctate representation of the world
encoded in the rods and cones. What is especially interesting to me is the
unexpectedness of the results, as reflected in the failure of anyone before
Kuffler to guess that something like center-surround receptive fields could
exist or that the optic nerve would virtually ignore anything so boring as
diffuse-light levels. By the same token, no one made any guesses that even
closely approximated what was to come at the next levels along the path—in
the brain. It is this unpredictability that makes the brain fascinating—that
plus the ingenuity of its workings once we have uncovered them.
4. THE PRIMARY VISUAL CORTEX
The visual cortex in a monkey, stained by the Golgi method, shows a few pyramidal cells—a tiny
fraction of the total number in such a section. The entire height of the photograph represents about 1
millimeter. A tungsten microelectrode, typical of what is used for extra cellular recordings, has been
superimposed, to the same scale.
In the retina, the successive stages are in apposition, like playing cards
stacked one on top of the other, so that the fibers can take a very direct route
from one stage to the next. In the lateral geniculate body, the cells are
obviously separated from the retina, just as, equally obviously, the cortex is
in adifferent place from the geniculate. The style of connectivity nevertheless
remains the same, with one region projecting to the next as though the
successive plates were still superimposed. The optic-nerve fibers simply
gather into a bundle as they leave the eye, and when they reach the
geniculate, they fan out and end in a topographically orderly way. (Oddly,
between the retina and geniculate, in the optic nerve, they become almost
completely scrambled, but they sort out again as they reach the geniculate.)
Fibers leaving the geniculate similarly fan out into a broad band that extends
back through the interior of the brain and ends in an equally orderly way in
the primary visual cortex. After several synapses, when fibers leave the
primary visual cortex and project to several other cortical regions, the
topographic order is again preserved. Because convergence occurs at every
stage, receptive fields tend to become larger: the farther along the path we go,
the more fuzzy this representation-by-mapping of the outside world becomes.
An important, long-recognized piece of evidence that the pathway is
topographically organized comes from clinical observation. If you damage a
certain part of your primary visual cortex, you develop a local blindness, as
though you had destroyed the corresponding part of your retina. The visual
world is thus systematically mapped onto the geniculate and cortex. What
was not at all clear in the 1950s was what the mapping might mean. In those
days it was not obvious that the brain operates on the information it receives,
transforming it in such a way as to make it more useful. People had the
feeling that the visual scene had made it to the brain; now the problem for the
brain was to make sense of it—or perhaps it was not the brain's problem, but
the mind's. The message of the next chapters will be that a structure such as
the primary visual cortex does exert profound transformations on the
information it receives. We still know very little about what goes on beyond
this stage, and in that sense you might argue that we are not much better off.
But knowing that one part of the cortex works in a rational, easily understood
way gives grounds for optimism that other areas will too. Some day we may
not need the word mind at all.
A microscopic cross-sectional view of the optic nerve where it leaves the eye, interrupting the retinal
layers shown at the left and right. The full width of the picture is about 2 millimeters. The clear area at
the top is the inside of the eye. The retinal layers, from the top down, are optic-nerve fibers (clear), the
three stained layers of cells, and the black layer of melanin pigment.
The right visual field extends out to the right almost to 90 degrees, as you can easily verify by wiggling
a finger and slowly moving it around to your right. It extends up 60 degrees or so, down perhaps 75
degrees and to the left, by definition, to a vertical line passing through the point you are looking at.
Responses of one of the first orientation-specific cells. Torsten Wiesel and I recorded, from a cat striate
cortex in 1958. This cell not only responds exclusively to a moving slit in an eleven o'clock orientation
but also responds to movement right and up, but hardly at all to movement left and down.
The discovery was just the beginning, and for some time we were very
confused because, as luck would have it, the cell was of a type that we came
later to call complex, and it lay two stages beyond the initial, center-surround
cortical stage. Although complex cells are the commonest type in the striate
cortex, they are hard to comprehend if you haven't seen the intervening type.
Beyond the first, center-surround stage, cells in the monkey cortex indeed
respond in a radically different way. Small spots generally produce weak
responses or none. To evoke a response, we first have to find the appropriate
part of the visual field to stimulate, that is, the appropriate part of the screen
that the animal is facing: we have to find the receptive field of the cell. It then
turns out that the most effective way to influence a cell is to sweep some kind
of line across the receptive field, in a direction perpendicular to the line's
orientation. The line can be light on a dark background (a slit) or a dark bar
on a white background or an edge boundary between dark and light. Some
cells prefer one of these stimuli over the other two, often very strongly;
others respond about equally well to all three types of stimuli. What is critical
is the orientation of the line: a typical cell responds best to some optimum
stimulus orientation; the response, measured in the number of impulses as the
receptive field is crossed, falls off over about 10 to 20 degrees to either side
of the optimum, and outside that range it declines steeply to zero (see the
illustration on the previous page). A range of 10 to 20 degrees may seem
imprecise, until you remember that the difference between one o'clock and
two o'clock is 30 degrees. A typical orientation-selective cell does not
respond at all when the line is oriented 90 degrees to the optimal. Unlike cells
at earlier stages in the visual path, these orientation-specific cells respond far
better to a moving than to a stationary line. That is why, in the diagram on the
next page, we stimulate by sweeping the line over the receptive field.
Flashing a stationary line on and off often evokes weak responses, and when
it does, we find that the preferred orientation is always the same as when the
line is moved. In many cells, perhaps one-fifth of the population, moving the
stimulus brings out another kind of specific response. Instead of firing
equally well to both movements, back and forth, many cells will consistently
respond better to one of the two directions. One movement may even produce
a strong response and the reverse movement none or almost none, as
illustrated m the figure on the next page. In a single experiment we can test
the responses of 200 to 300 cells simply by learning all about one cell and
then pushing the electrode ahead to the next cell to study it. Because once
you have inserted the delicate electrode you obviously can't move it sideways
without destroying it or the even more delicate cortex, this technique limits
your examination to cells lying in a straight line. Fifty cells per millimeter of
penetration is about the maximum we can get with present methods. When
the orientation preferences of a few hundred or a thousand cells are
examined, all orientations turn out to be about equally rep-resented—vertical,
horizontal, and every possible oblique. Considering the nature of the world
we look at, containing as it does trees and horizons, the question arises
whether any particular orientations, such as vertical and horizontal, are better
represented than the others. Answers differ with different laboratory results,
but everyone agrees that if biases do exist, they must be small—small enough
to require statistics to discern them, which may mean they are negligible! In
the monkey striate cortex, about 70 to 80 percent of cells have this property
of orientation specificity. In the cat, all cortical cells seem to be orientation
selective, even those with direct geniculate input. We find striking differences
among orientation-specific cells, not just in optimum stimulus orientation or
in the position of the receptive field on the retina, but in the way cells behave.
The most useful distinction is between two classes of cells: simple and
complex. As their names suggest, the two types differ in the complexity of
their behavior, and we make the reasonable assumption that the cells with the
simpler behavior are closer in the circuit to the input of the cortex.
Various stimulus geometries evoke different responses in a cell with receptive field of the type in a
diagram of the previous figure. The stimulus line at the bottom indicates when the slit is turned on and,
i second later, turned off. The top record shows the response to a slit of optimum size, position, and
orientation. In the second record, the same slit covers only part of an inhibitory area. (Because this cell
has no spontaneous activity to suppress, only an off discharge is seen.) In the third record, the slit is
oriented so as to cover only a small part of the excitatory region and a proportionally small part of the
inhibitory region; the cell fails to respond. In the bottom record, the whole receptive field is
illuminated; again, there is no response.
Even after twenty years we still do not know how the inputs to cortical
cells are wired in order to bring about this behavior. Several plausible circuits
have been proposed, and it may well be that one of them, or several in
combination, will turn out to be correct. Simple cells must be built up from
the antecedent cells with circular fields; by far the simplest proposal is that a
simple cell receives direct excitatory input from many cells at the previous
stage, cells whose receptive-field centers are distributed along a line in the
visual field, as shown in the diagram on the next page.
It seems slightly more difficult to wire up a cell that is selectively
responsive to edges, as shown in the third drawing (c) on the previous page.
One workable scheme would be to have the cell receive inputs from two sets
of antecedent cells having their field centers arranged on opposite sides of a
line, on-center cells on one side off center cells on the other all making
excitatory connections cells on one side, off-center cells on the other, all
making excitatory connections. In all these proposed circuits, excitatory input
from an off-center cell is logically equivalent to inhibitory input from an on-
center cell, provided we assume that the off-center cell is spontaneously
active.
Working out the exact mechanism for building up simple cells will not
be easy. For any one cell we need to know what kinds of cells feed in
information—for example, the details of their receptive fields, including
position, orientation if any, and whether on or off center—and whether they
supply excitation or inhibition to the cell. Because methods of obtaining this
kind of knowledge don’t yet exist, we are forced to use less direct
approaches, with correspondingly higher chances of being wrong. The
mechanism summarized in the diagram on the next page seems to me the
most likely because it is the most simple.
This type of wiring could produce a simple-cell receptive field. On the right, four cells are shown
making excitatory synaptic connections with a cell of higher order. Each of the lower-order cells has a
radially symmetric receptive field with on- center and off-surround, illustrated by the left side of the
diagram. The centers of these fields lie along a line. If we suppose that many more than four center-
surround cells are connected with the simple cell, all with their field centers overlapped along this line,
the receptive field of the simple cell will consist of a long, narrow excitatory region with inhibitory
flanks. Avoiding receptive-field terminology, we can say that stimulating with a small spot anywhere in
this long, narrow rectangle will strongly activate one or a few of the center-surround cells and in turn
excite the simple cell, although only weakly. Stimulating with a long, narrow slit will activate all the
center-surround cells, producing a strong response in the simple cell.
Left: A long, narrow slit of light evokes a response wherever it is placed within the receptive field
(rectangle) of a complex cell, provided the orientation is correct (upper three records). A nonoptimal
orientation gives a weaker response or none at all (lower record). Right: The cortical cell from layer 5
in the strate cortex of a cat was recorded intracellularly by David Van Essen and James Kelly at
Harvard Medical School in 1973, and its complex receptive field was mapped. They then injected
procyon yellow dye and showed that the cell was piramidal.
The diagram on this page for the complex cell and the one on page 13
for the simple cell illustrate the essential difference between the two types:
for a simple cell, the extremely narrow range of positions over which an
optimally oriented line evokes a response; for a complex cell, the responses
to a properly oriented line wherever it is placed in the receptive field. This
behavior is related to the explicit on and off regions of a simple cell and to
the lack of such regions in a complex cell. The complex cell generalizes the
responsiveness to a line over a wider territory.
Complex cells tend to have larger receptive fields than simple cells, but
not very much larger. A typical complex receptive field in the fovea of the
macaque monkey would be about one-half degree by one-half degree. The
optimum stimulus width is about the same for simple cells and complex cells
—in the fovea, about 2 minutes of arc. The complex cell's resolving power,
or acuity, is thus the same as the simple cell's. As in the case of the simple
cell, we do not know exactly how complex cells are built up. But, again, it is
easy to propose plausible schemes, and the simplest one is to imagine that the
complex cell receives input from many simple cells, all of whose fields have
the same orientation but are spread out in overlapping fashion over the entire
field of the complex cell, as shown in the illustration on the next page. If the
connections from simple to complex cells are excitatory, then wherever a line
falls in the field, some simple cells are activated; the complex cell will
therefore be activated. If, on the other hand, a stimulus fills the entire
receptive field, none of the simple cells will be activated, and the complex
cell won't be activated.
This wiring diagram would account for the properties of a complex cell. As in the figure on page 15,
we suppose that a large number of simple cells (only three are shown here) make excitatory synapses
with a single complex cell. Each simple cell responds optimally to a vertically oriented edge with light
to the right, and the receptive fields are scattered in overlapping fashion throughout the rectangle. An
edge falling anywhere within the rectangle evokes a response from a few simple cells, and this in turn
evokes a response in the complex cell. Because there is adaptation at the synapses, only a moving
stimulus will keep up a steady bombardment of the complex cell.
The burst of impulses from a complex cell to turning on a stationary
line and not moving it is generally brief even if the light is kept on: we say
that the response adapts. When we move the line through the complex cell's
receptive field, the sustained response may be the result of overcoming the
adaptation, by bringing in new simple cells one after the next. You will have
noticed that the schemes for building simple cells from center-surround ones,
as in the illustration on page 15, and for building complex cells out of simple
ones, as in the illustration on this page, both involve excitatory processes. In
the two cases, however, the processes must be very different. The first
scheme requires simultaneous summed inputs from centersurround cells
whose field centers lie along a line. In the second scheme, activation of the
complex cell by a moving stimulus requires successive activation of many
simple cells. It would be interesting to know what, if any, morphological
differences underlie this difference in addition properties.
We don't know how such directionally selective cells are wired up. One
possibility is that they are built up from simple cells whose responses to
opposite directions of movement are asymmetric. Such simple cells have
asymmetric fields, such as the one shown in the third diagram of the
illustration on page 13. A second mechanism was proposed in 1965 by
Horace Barlow and William Levick to explain the directional selectivity of
certain cells in the rabbit retina cells that apparently are not present in the
monkey retina. If we apply their scheme to complex cells, we would suppose
that interposed between simple and complex cells are intermediate cells,
colored white in the diagram on the next page. We imagine that an
intermediate cell receives excitation from one simple cell and inhibition from
another (green) cell, whose receptive field is immediately adjacent and
always located to one side and not the other. We further suppose that the
inhibitory path involves a delay, perhaps produced by still another
intermediate cell. Then, if the stimulus moves in one direction, say, right to
left, as in the illustration of Barlow and Levick's model, the intermediate cell
is excited by one of its inputs just as the inhibition arrives from the other,
whose field has just been crossed. The two effects cancel, and the cell does
not fire. For left-to-right movement, the inhibition arrives too late to prevent
firing. If many such intermediate cells converge on a third cell, that cell will
have the properties of a directionally selective complex cell. We have little
direct evidence for any schemes that try to explain the behavior of cells in
terms of a hierarchy of complexity, in which cells at each successive level are
constructed of building blocks from the previous level. Nevertheless, we have
strong reasons for believing that the nervous system is organized in a
hierarchical series. The strongest evidence is anatomical: for example, in the
cat, simple cells are aggregated in the fourth layer of the striate cortex, the
same layer that receives geniculate input, whereas the complex cells are
located in the layers above and below, one or two synapses further along.
Thus although we may not know the exact circuit diagram at each stage, we
have good reasons to suppose the existence of some circuit.
The main reason for thinking that complex cells may be built up from
center-surround cells, with a step in between, is the seeming necessity of
doing the job in two logical steps. I should emphasize the word logical
because the whole transformation presumably could be accomplished in one
physical step by having center-surround inputs sum on separate dendritic
branches of complex cells, with each branch doing the job of a simple cell,
signaling electronically (by passive electrical spread) to the cell body, and
hence to the axon, whenever a line falls in some particular part of the
receptive field. The cell itself would then be complex. But the very existence
of simple cells suggests that we do not have to imagine anything as
complicated as this.
Horace Barlow and William Levick proposed this circuit to explain directional selectivity. Synapses
from purple to green are excitatory, and from green to white, inhibitory. We suppose the three white
cells at the bottom converge on a single master cell.
A picture is viewed by an observer while we monitor eye position and hence direction of gaze. The
eyes jump, come to rest momentarily (producing a small dot on the record), then jump to a new locus of
interest. It seems difficult to jump to a void—a place lacking abrupt luminance changes.
The visual system seems intent instead on keeping the image of a scene
anchored on our retinas, on preventing it from sliding around. If the whole
scene moves by, as occurs when we look out a train window, we follow it by
fixating on an object and maintaining fixation by moving our eyes until the
object gets out of range, whereupon we make a saccade to a new object. This
whole sequence— following with smooth pursuit, say, to the right, then
making a saccade to the left—is called nystagmus. You can observe the
sequence—perhaps next time you are in a moving train or streetcar—by
looking at your neighbor's eyes as he or she looks out a window at the
passing scene—taking care not to have your attentions misunderstood! The
process of making visual saccades to items of interest, in order to get their
images on the fovea, is carried out largely by the superior colliculus, as Peter
Schiller at MIT showed in an impressive series of papers in the 1970s. The
second set of facts about how we see is even more counterintuitive. When we
look at a stationary scene by fixating on some point of interest, our eyes lock
onto that point, as just described, but the locking is not absolute. Despite any
efforts we may make, the eyes do not hold perfectly still but make constant
tiny movements called microsaccades; these occur several times per second
and are more or less random in direction and about 1 to 2 minutes of arc in
amplitude. In 1952 Lorrin Riggs and Floyd Ratliff, at Brown University, and
R. W. Ditchburn and B. L. Ginsborg, at Reading University, simultaneously
and independently found that if an image is optically artificially stabilized on
the retina, eliminating any movement relative to the retina, vision fades away
after about a second and the scene becomes quite blank! (The simplest way of
stabilizing is to attach a tiny spotlight to a contact lens; as the eye moves, the
spot moves too, and quickly fades.) Artificially moving the image on the
retina, even by a tiny amount, causes the spot to reappear at once. Evidently,
microsaccades are necessary for us to continue to see stationary objects.
(Current evidence does not support this notion on microsaccade. Micro-
saccade is in fact samll saccade that shares the same function of large
saccade. Making stationary objects visible is probably just a epiphenomenon,
a byproduct that also comes with other types of eye movements. - Cui, J., M.
Wilke, N. K. Logothetis, D. A. Leopold and H. L. Liang (2009). Visibility
states modulate microsaccade rate and direction. Vision Research 49(2):
228-236.)
It is as if the visual system, after going to the trouble to make
movement a powerful stimulus—wiring up cells so as to be insensitive to
stationary objects—had then to invent microsaccades to make stationary
objects visible. We can guess that cortical complex cells, with their very high
sensitivity to movement, are involved in this process. Directional selectivity
is probably not involved, because microsaccadic movements are apparently
random in direction (Not random. Microsaccade direction is affected by
several factors, including attention. Moreover, the firing rate of cells in V1 is
modulated by the microsaccade direction - RJC). On the other hand,
directional selectivity would seem very useful for detecting movements of
objects against a stationary background, by telling us that a movement is
taking place and in what direction. To follow a moving object against a
stationary background, we have to lock onto the object and track it with our
eyes; the rest of the scene then slips across the retina, an event that otherwise
occurs only rarely. Such slippage, with every contour in the scene moving
across the retina, must produce a tremendous storm of activity in our cortex.
One scheme for explaining the behavior of a complex end-stopped cell. Three ordinary complex cells
converge on the end-stopped cell: one, whose receptive field is congruent with the end-stopped cell's
activating region (a), makes excitatory contacts; the other two, having fields in the outlying regions (b
and c), make inhibitory contacts.
This end-stopped simple cell is assumed to result from convergent input from three ordinary simple
cells. (One cell, with the middle on-center field, could excite the cell in question; the two others could
be off center and also excite or be on center and inhibit.) Alternatively, the input to this cell could come
directly from center-surround cells, by some more elaborate version of the process illustrated on page
15.
For an end-stopped cell such as the one shown on page 23, a curved border should be an effective
stimulus.
How arc cells in our brain likely to respond to some everyday stimulus, such as this kidney-shaped
uniform blob? In the visual cortex, only a select set of cells will show any interest.
In population studies of ocular dominance, we study hundreds of cells and categorize each one as
belonging to one of seven arbitrary groups. A group i cell is defined as a cell influenced only by the
contralateral eye—the eye opposite to the hemisphere in which it sits. A group 2 cell responds to both
eyes but strongly prefers the contralateral eye. And so on.
The recording electrode was close enough to three cells to pick up impulses from all of them.
Responses could be distinguished by size and shape of the impulses. This illustrates the responses to
stimuli to single eyes and to both eyes. Cells (1) and (2) both would be in group 4 since they responded
about equally to the two eyes. Cell (3) responded only when both eyes were stimulated; we can say
only that it was not a group 1 or a group 7 cell.
One of the three did not respond at all to either eye alone, and thus its
presence would have gone undetected had we not stimulated the two eyes
together. Many cells show little or no synergistic effect; they respond about
the same way to both eyes together as to either eye alone. A special class of
binocular cells, wired up so as to respond specifically to near or far objects,
will be taken up separately when we come to discuss stereopsis, in Chapter 7.
These hookups from single cells to the two eyes illustrate once more the high
degree of specificity of connections in the brain. As if it were not remarkable
enough that a cell can be so connected as to respond to only one line
orientation and one movement direction, we now learn that the connections
are laid down in duplicate copies, one from each eye. And as if that were not
remarkable enough, most of the connections, as we will see in Chapter 9,
seem to be wired up and ready to go at birth.
5. THE ARCHITECTURE OF THE VISUAL
CORTEX
Ocular-dominance columns are seen in this section through a macaque monkey's left striate cortex,
taken perpendicular to the surface in a left-to-right direction. As we follow the part of the cortex that is
exposed to the surface from left to right (top of photo), it bends around forming a buried fold that
extends from right to left. Radioactive amino acid injected into the left eye has been transported
through the lateral geniculate body to layer 4C, where it occupies a series of half-millimeter-wide
patches; these glow brightly in this dark-field picture. (The continuous leaflet in the middle is white
matter, containing the geniculo-cortical fibers.)
A large part of the cerebral cortex on the right side has been exposed under local anesthesia for the
neurosurgical treatment of seizures in this fully conscious human patient. The surgeon was Dr. William
Feindel at the Montreal Neurological Institute. The scalp has been opened and retracted and a large
piece of skull removed. (It is replaced at the end of the operation.) You can see gyri and suici, and the
large purplish veins and smaller, red, less conspicuous arteries. The overall pinkish appearance is
caused by the finer branches of these vessels. Filling the bottom third of the exposure is the temporal
lobe; above-the prominent, horizontally running veins are the parietal lobe, to the left, and frontal lobe,
to the right. At the extreme left we see part of the occipital lobe. This operation, for the treatment of a
particular type of epilepsy, consists of removing diseased brain, which is only permissible if it does not
result in impairment of voluntary movement or loss of speech. To avoid this, the neurosurgeon
identifies speech, motor, and sensory areas by electrical stimulation, looking for movements, sensations
related precisely to different parts of the body, or interference with speech. Such tests would obviously
not be possible if the patient were not conscious. Points that have been stimulated have been labeled by
the tiny numbered sterile patches of paper. For example, stimulation of these regions gave the following
results: (1) tingling sensation in the left thumb; (2) tingling in the left ring finger; (3) tingling in the left
middle and ring finger; (4) flexion of left fingers and wrist. The regions labeled 8 and 13 gave more
complex memory-like sensations typically produced on stimulation of the temporal lobe in certain types
of epileptic patients.
This view of a macaque monkey's brain, from behind, shows the occipital lobe and the part of the
striate cortex visible on the surface (below the dotted line).
Blood vessels normally form a conspicuous web over the surface, but
here they are collapsed and not visible. What we see in this rear view is
mostly the surface of the occipital lobe of the cortex, the area that is
concerned with vision and that comprises not only the striate cortex but also
one or two dozen or more prestriate areas. To get a half-millimeter-thick
plate of nervous tissue that is the area of a large index card into a box the size
of the monkey's skull necessitates some folding and crinkling, the way you
crinkle up a piece of paper before throwing it into the waste basket; this
produces fissures, or sulci, between which are ridges, or gyri. The area
behind (below, in this photograph) the dotted line is the exposed part of the
striate cortex. Although the striate cortex occupies most of the surface of the
occipital lobe, we can see only about one-third to one-half of itin the
photograph; the rest is hidden out of sight in a fissure. The striate cortex (area
17) sends much of its output to the next cortical region, visual area 2, also
called area 18 because it is next door to area 17. Area18 forms a band of
cortex about 6 to 8 millimeters wide, which almost completely surrounds area
17. We can just see part of area 18 in the photograph, above the dotted line,
the boundary between 17 and 18, but most of it extends down into the deep
sulcus just in front of that line. Area 17 projects to area 18 in a plate-to-plate,
orderly fashion. Area 18 in turn projects to at least three postage-stamp-size
occipital regions, called MT (for medial temporal), visual area 3, and visual
area 4 (often abbreviated V3 and V4). And so it goes, with each area
projecting forward to several other areas. In addition, each of these areas
projects back to the area or areas from which it receives input. As if that were
not complicated enough, each of the areas projects to structures deep in the
brain, for example to the superior colliculus and to various subdivisions of
the thalamus (a complex golfball-size mass of cells, of which the lateral
geniculate forms a small part). And each of these visual areas receives input
from a thalamic subdivision: just as the geniculate projects to the primary
visual cortex, so other parts project to the other areas. In the same
photograph, X indicates the part of area 17 that receives information from the
foveas, or centers of gaze, of the two retinas. As we move from X, in the left
hemisphere, toward the arrowhead, the corresponding point in the right half
of the visual field starts in the center of gaze and moves out, to the right,
along the horizon. Starting again from X, movement to the right along the
border between areas 17 and 18 corresponds to movement down in the visual
field; movement back corresponds to movement up. The arrowhead marks a
region about 6 degrees out along the horizon. The visual field farther out than
9 degrees is represented on the part of area 17 that is folded underneath the
surface and parallel to it.
To see what the cortex looks like in cross section, we have cut a chunk
from the visual cortex on the right side of the photograph on the previous
page. The resulting cross section, as in the photomicrograph on this page, is
stained with cresyl violet, a dye that colors the cell bodies dark blue but does
not stain axons or dendrites. With the photomicrograph taken at this low
power, we cannot distinguish individual cells, but we can see dark layers of
densely aggregated cells and lighter layers of more thinly scattered ones.
Beneath the exposed part of the cortex, we see a mushroom-shaped, buried
part that is folded under in a complicated way, but these two parts are
actually continuous. The lightly stained substance is white matter; it lies
under the part of the cortex that is exposed to the surface, separating it from
the buried fold of cortex, and consists mainly of myelinated nerve fibers,
which do not stain. The cortex, containing nerve-cell bodies, axons,
dendrites, and synapses, is an example of gray matter.
This cross section through the occipital lobe was made by cutting out a piece as shown in the
photograph on the previous page. It is what we would see if we were to walk into the groove and look
to the left. The letter a corresponds to a point halfway between X and the arrowhead. The Nissi stain
shows cell bodies only; these are too small to make out except as dots. The darker part of the top and
the mushroom-shaped part just below are striate cortex. The three letter d's mark the border between
areas 17 and 18.
A Golgi-stained section from the upper layers, 1, 2, and 3, of the visual cortex in a child several days
old. Black triangular dots are cell bodies, from which emanate an apical dendrite ascending and
dividing in layer 1, basal dendrites coming off laterally, and a single slender axon heading straight
down.
The main connections made by axons from the lateral geniculate body to the striate cortex and from the
striate cortex to other brain regions. To the right, the shading indicates the relative density of Nissl
staining, for comparison with the illustration on page 5.
The fibers coming to the cortex from the lateral geniculate body enter
from the white matter. Running diagonally, most make their way up to layer
4C, branching again and again, and finally terminate by making synapses
with the stellate cells that populate that layer. Axons originating from the two
ventral (magnocellular) geniculate layers end in the upper half of 4C, called
4C alpha; those from the four dorsal (parvocellular) geniculate layers end in
the lower half of 4C (4C Bata). As you can see from the diagram on this
page, these subdivisions of layer 4C have different projections to the upper
layers: 4C alpha sends its output to 4.B; 4Q Bata, to the deepest part of 3.
And those layers in turn differ in their projections. Seeing these differences in
the pathways stemming from the two sets of geniculate layers is one of many
reasons to think that they represent two different systems. Most pyramidal
cells in layers 2, 3, 4A, 5, and 6 send axons out of the cortex, but side-
branches, called "collaterals", of these same descending axons connect
locally and help to distribute the information through the full cortical
thickness.
The layers of the cortex differ not only in their inputs and their local
interconnections but also in the more distant structures to which they project.
All layers except 1, 4A, and 4C send fibers out of the cortex. Layers 2 and 3
and layer 4B project mainly to other cortical regions, whereas the deep layers
project down to subcortical structures: layer 5 projects to the superior
colliculus in the midbrain, and layer 6 projects mainly back to the lateral
geniculate body. Although we have known for almost a century that the
inputs from the geniculate go mostly to layer 4, we did not know the
differences in outputs of the different cortical layers until 1969, when
Japanese scientist Keisuke Toyama first discovered them with physiological
techniques; they have been confirmed anatomically many times since.
Ramon y Cajal was the first to realize how short the connections within
the cortex are. As already described, the richest connections run up and
down, intimately linking the different layers. Diagonal and side-to-side
connections generally run for 1 or 2 millimeters, although a few travel up to 4
or 5 millimeters. This limitation in lateral spread of information has profound
consequences. If the inputs are topographically organized—in the case of the
visual system, organized according to retinal or visual-field position—the
same must be true for the outputs. Whatever the cortex is doing, the analysis
must be local. Information concerning some small part of the visual world
comes in to a small piece of the cortex, is transformed, analyzed, digested—
whatever expression you find appropriate—and is sent on for further
processing somewhere else, without reference to what goes on next door. The
visual scene is thus analyzed piecemeal. The primary visual cortex cannot
therefore be the part of the brain where whole objects—boats, hats, faces—
are recognized, perceived, or otherwise handled; it cannot be where
"perception" resides. Of course, such a sweeping conclusion would hardly be
warranted from anatomy alone. It could be that information is transmitted
along the cortex for long distances in bucket-brigade fashion, spreading
laterally in steps of i millimeter or so. We can show chat this is not the case
by recording while stimulating the retina: all the cells in a given small
locality have small receptive fields, and any cell and its neighbor always have
their receptive fields in very nearly the same place in the retina. Nothing in
the physiology suggests that any cell in the monkey primary visual cortex
talks to any other cell more than 2 or 3 millimeters away.
For centuries, similar hints had come from clinical neurology. A small
stroke, tumor, or injury to part of the primary visual cortex can lead to
blindness in a small, precisely demarcated island in the visual field; we find
perfectly normal vision elsewhere, instead of the overall mild reduction in
vision that we might expect if each cell communicated in some measure with
all other cells. To digress slightly, we can note here that such a stroke patient
may be unaware of anything wrong, especially if the defect is not in the
foveal representation of the cortex and hence in the center of gaze—at least
he will not perceive in his visual field an island of blackness or greyness or
indeed anything at all. Even if the injury has destroyed one entire occipital
lobe, leaving the subject blind in the entire half visual field on the other side,
the result is not any active sensation of the world being blotted out on that
side. My occasional migraine attacks (luckily without the headache) produce
transient blindness, often in a large part of one visual field; if asked what I
see there, I can only say, literally, nothing—not white, grey, or black, but just
what I see directly be-hind—nothing.
Another curious feature of an island of localized blindness, or scotoma,
is known as "completion". When someone with a scotoma looks at a line that
passes through his blind region, he sees no interruption: the line is perfectly
continuous. You can demonstrate the same thing using your own eye and
blind spot, which you can find with no more apparatus than a cotton Q-tip.
The blind spot is the region where the optic nerve enters the eye, an oval
about 2 millimeters in diameter, with no rods and cones. The procedure for
mapping it is so childishly simple that anyone who hasn't should! You start
by closing one eye, say the left; keeping it closed, you fix your gaze with the
other eye on a small object across the room. Now hold the Q-tip at arm's
length directly in front of the object and slowly move it out to the right
exactly horizontally (a dark background helps). The white cotton will vanish
when it is about 18 degrees out. Now, if you place the stick so that it runs
through the blind spot, it will still appear as a single stick, without any gap.
The region of blindness constituting the blind spot is like any scotoma; you
are not aware of it and cannot be, unless you test for it. You don't see black or
white or anything there, you see nothing.
In an analogous way, if looking at a big patch of white paper activates
only cells whose fields are cut by the paper's borders (since a cortical cell
tends to ignore diffuse change in light), then the death of cells whose fields
are within the patch of paper should make no difference. The island of
blindness should not be seen—and it isn't. We don't see our blind spot as a
black hole when we look at a big patch of white. The completion
phenomenon, plus looking at a big white screen and verifying that there is no
black hole where the optic disc is, should convince anyone that the brain
works in ways that we cannot easily predict using intuition alone.
Stating that most cells above and below layer 4 are complex glosses
over major layer-to-layer differences, because complex cells are far from all
alike. They all have in common the defining characteristic of complex cells—
they respond throughout their receptive field to a properly oriented moving
line regardless of its exact position—but they differ in other ways. We can
distinguish four subtypes that tend to be housed in different layers. In layers 2
and 3, most complex cells respond progressively better the longer the slit
(they show length summation), and the response becomes weaker when the
line exceeds a critical length only if a cell is end stopped. For cells in layer 5,
short slits, covering only a small part of the length of a receptive field, work
about as well as long ones; the receptive fields are much larger than the fields
of cells in layers 2 and 3. For cells in layer 6, in contrast, the longer an
optimally oriented line is, the stronger are the responses, until the line spans
the entire length of the field, which is several times greater than the width
(the distance over which a moving line evokes responses). The field is thus
long and narrow. We can conclude that axons running from layers 5, 6, and 2
and 3 to different targets in the brain (the superior culliculus, geniculate, the
other visual cortical areas) must carry somewhat different kinds of visual
information.
In summary, from layer to layer we find differences in the way cells
behave that seem more fundamental than differences, say, in optimal
orientation or in ocular dominance. The most obvious of these layer-to-layer
differences is in response complexity, which reflects the simple anatomical
fact that some layers are closer than others to the input.
Ocular dominance remains constant in vertical microelectrode penetrations through the striate cortex.
Penetrations parallel to the surface show alternation from left eye to right eye and back, roughly one
cycle every millimeter.
The stripes of the pattern are most regular and striking some distance
away from the foveal representation. For reasons unknown, the pattern is
rather complex near the fovea, with very regular periodicity but many loops
and swirls, hardly the regular wallpaper-like stripes seen farther out. The
width of the stripes is everywhere constant at about 0.5 millimeter. The
amount of cortex devoted to left and right eyes is nearly exactly equal in the
cortex representing the fovea and out to about 20 degrees in all directions.
LeVay and David Van Essen have found that owing to the declining
contribution of the eye on the same side, the ipsilateral bands shrink to 0.25
millimeter out beyond 20 degrees from the fovea. Beyond 70 or 80 degrees,
of course, only the contralateral eye is represented. This makes sense,
because with your eyes facing the front, you can see with your right eye
farther to the right than to the left.
A second method for demonstrating the columns reveals the slabs in
their full thickness, not just the part in layer 4. This is the 2-deoxyglucose
method, invented by Louis Sokoloff at the National Institutes of Health,
Bethesda, in 1976. It too depends ultimately on the ability of radioactive
substances to darken photographic film. The method is based on the fact that
nerve cells, like most cells in the body, consume glucose as fuel, and the
harder they are made to work, the more glucose they eat. Accordingly, we
might imagine injecting radioactive glucose into an animal, stimulating one
eye, say the right, with patterns for some minutes—long enough for the
glucose to be taken up by the active cells in the brain—and then removing the
brain and slicing it, coating the slices with silver emulsion, and exposing and
developing, as before. This idea didn't work because glucose is consumed by
the cells and converted to energy and degradation products, which quickly
leak back out into the blood stream. To sidestep the leakage, Sokoloffs
ingenious trick was to use the substance deoxyglucose, which is close enough
chemically to glucose to fool the cells into taking it up: they even begin
metabolizing it. The process of breakdown goes only one step along the usual
chemical degradation path, coming to a halt after the deoxyglucose is
converted to a substance (2-deoxyglucose-6-phosphate) that can be degraded
no further. Luckily, this substance is fat insoluble and can't leak out of the
cell; so it accumulates to levels at which it can be detected in
autoradiographs. What we finally see on the film is a picture of the brain
regions that became most active during the stimulation period and took up
most of this fake food. Had the animal been moving its arm during that time,
the motor arm area in the cortex would also have lit up. In the case of
stimulating the right eye, what we see are the parts of the cortex most
strongly activated by that stimulus, namely, the set of right ocular-dominance
columns.
In this autoradiograph through the striate cortex, the white segments are the labeled patches in layer 4
representing the injected left eye; these patches are separated by unlabeled (dark) right-eye regions.
Top: A single section through the dome-shaped cortex is made parallel to the surface. It cuts through
layer 4 in a ring. Bottom: A reconstruction of many such rings from a series of sections—the deeper the
section, the bigger the ring—made by cutting out the rings and superimposing them. (Traces of the
rings can be seen because it was difficult to get all the sections exposed and photographed equally,
especially as I am strictly an amateur photographer.)
In three-dimensional view, the ocular- dominance columns are seen to be, not Greek pillars, but slabs
perpendicular to the surface, like slices of bread.
Seen here in LeVay's reconstruction are the ocular-dominance columns in the part of area 17 open to
the surface, right hemi- sphere. Foveal representation is to the right. (Compare right side of photograph
on page 3.) You see the result in the photographs on the next page.
A very oblique penetration through area 17 in a macaque monkey reveals the regular shift in orientation
preference of 23 neighboring cells.
The results of the experiment shown above are plotted in degrees, against the distance the electrode had
traveled. (Because the electrode was so slanted that it was almost parallel to the cortical surface, the
track distance is almost the same as the distance along the surface.) In this experiment 180 degrees, a
full rotation, corresponded to about 0.7 millimeter.
A tilted line segment shining in the visual field of the left eye (shown to the right) may cause this
hypothetical pattern of activation of a small area of striate cortex (shown to the left). The activation is
confined to a small cortical area, which is long and narrow to reflect the shape of the line; within this
area, it is confined to left oc-ular-dominance columns and to orientation columns representing a two
o'clock-eight o'clock tilt. Cortical representation is not simple! When we consider that the orientation
domains are not neat parallel lines, suggested here for simplicity, but far more complex, as shown in the
upper, deoxyglucose figure on page 26 and Blasdel's figure on same page, the representation becomes
even more intricate.
The group of center-surround layer 4 cells that is needed to build a simple cell that responds to an
oblique four o'clock—ten o'clock slit is likely to have cells in common with the group needed to build a
4:30-10:30 cell: a few inputs must be discarded and a few added.
6. MAGNIFICATION AND MODULES
A single module of the type discussed in this chapter occupies roughly the area shown in this
photograph of a Golgi-stained section through visual cortex. The Golgi method stains only a tiny
fraction of the nerve cells in any region, but the cells that it does reveal are stained fully or almost so;
thus one can see the cell body, dendrites, and axon.
These nine receptive fields were mapped in a cat striate cortex in a single microelectrode penetration
made perpendicular to the surface. As the electrode descends, we see random scatter in receptive-field
position and some variation in size but see no overall tendency for the positions to change.
The sizes of the fields remain fairly constant in any given layer but
differ markedly from one layer to another, from very small, in layer 4C, to
large, in layers 5 and 6. Within any one layer, the area of visual field
occupied by ten or twenty successively recorded receptive fields is, because
of this random scatter, about two to four times the area occupied by any
single field. We call the area occupied by a large number of superimposed
fields in some layer and under some point on the cortex the aggregate
receptive field of that point in that layer. In any given layer, the aggregate
field varies, for example in layer 3, from about 30 minutes of arc in the to
veal region to about 7 or 8 degrees in the far periphery.
Now suppose we insert the electrode so that it moves horizontally along
any one layer, say layer 3. Again, as cell after cell is recorded, we see in
successive receptive fields a chaotic variation in position, but superimposed
on this variation we now detect a steady drift in position. The direction of this
drift in the visual field is, of course, predictable from the systematic map of
visual fields onto cortex. What interests us here is the amount of drift we see
after 1 millimeter of horizontal movement along the cortex. From what I have
said about variation in magnification, it will be clear that the distance
traversed in the visual field will depend on where in the cortex we have been
recording— whether we are studying a region of cortex that represents the
foveal region, the far periphery of the visual field, or somewhere between.
The rate of movement through the visual field will be far from constant. But
the movement turns out to be very constant relative to the size of the
receptive fields themselves. One millimeter on the cortex everywhere
produces a movement through the visual field that is equal to about half the
territory occupied by the aggregate receptive field—the smear of fields that
would be found under a single point in the region. Thus about 2 millimeters
of movement is required to get entirely out of one part of the visual field and
into the next, as shown in the illustration below (top one). This turns out to be
the case wherever in area 17 we record. In the fovea, the receptive fields are
tiny, and so is the movement in the visual field produced by a 2-millimeter
movement along the cortex: in the periphery, both the receptive fields and the
movements are much larger, as illustrated in the lower figure on this page.
Upper: In the course of a long penetration parallel to the cortical surface in a cat, receptive fields drifted
through the visual field. The electrode traveled over 3 millimeters and recorded over sixty cells, far too
many to be shown in a figure like this. I show instead only the positions of four or five receptive fields
mapped in the first tenth of each millimeter, ignoring the other nine tenths. For the parts of the
penetration drawn with a thick pen in the lower half of the diagram (numbered 0, 1, 2, and 3), the
receptive fields of cells encountered are mapped in the upper part. Each group is detectably displaced to
the right in the visual field relative to the previous group. The fields in group 2 do not overlap with
those in group o, and group-3 fields do not overlap with group-1 fields; in each case the cortical
separation is 2 millimeters. Lower: In a macaque monkey, the upper-layer receptive fields grow larger
as eccentricity increases from the fovea (0 degrees). Also growing by an equal amount is the distance
the receptive fields move in the visual field when an electrode moves 2 millimeters along the cortex
parallel to the surface.
We call this our "ice cube model" of the cortex. It illustrates how the cortex is divided, at one and the
same time, into two kinds of slabs, one set for ocular dominance (left and right) and one set for
orientation. The model should not be taken literally: Neither set is as regular as this, and the orientation
slabs especially are far from parallel or straight. Moreover, they do not seem to intersect in any
particular angle— certainly they are not orthogonal, as shown here.
All this may help us to understand why the columns are not far more
coarse. Enough has to be packed into a 2 millimeter by 2 millimeter block to
include all the values of the variables it deals with, orientation and eye
preference being the ones we have talked about so far. What the cortex does
is map not just two but many variables on its two-dimensional surface. It
does this by selecting as the basic parameters the two variables that specify
the visual field coordinates (distance out and up or down from the fovea), and
on this map it engrafts other variables, such as orientation and eye preference,
by finer subdivisions. We call the 2 millimeter by 2 millimeter piece of cortex
a module. To me, the word seems not totally suitable, partly because it is too
concrete: it calls up an image of a rectangular tin box containing electronic
parts that can be plugged into a rack beside a hundred other such boxes. To
some extent that is indeed what we want the word to convey, but in a rather
loose sense. First, our units clearly can start and end anywhere we like, in the
orientation domain. They can go from vertical to vertical or -85 to +95
degrees, as long as we include all orientations at least once. The same applies
to eye preference: we can start at a left-eye, right-eye border or at the middle
of a column, as long as we include two columns, one for each eye. Second, as
mentioned earlier, the size of the module we are talking about will depend on
the layer we are considering. Nevertheless, the term does convey the
impression of some 500 to 1000 small machines, any of which can be
substituted for any other, provided we are ready to wire up 10,000 or so
incoming wires and perhaps 50,000 outgoing ones!
Let me quickly add that no one would suppose that the cortex is
completely uniform from fovea to far periphery. Vision varies with visual-
field position in several ways other than acuity. Our color abilities fall off
with distance, although perhaps not very steeply if we compensate for
magnification by making the object we are viewing bigger with increasing
distance from the fovea. Movement is probably better detected in the
periphery, as are very dim lights. Functions related to binocular vision must
obviously fall off because beyond 20 degrees and up to 80 degrees,
ipsilateral-eye columns get progressively narrower and contralateral ones get
broader; beyond 80 degrees the ipsilateral ones disappear entirely and the
cortex becomes monocular. There must be differences in circuits to reflect
these and doubtless other differences in our capabilities. So modules are
probably not all exactly alike.
How does this affect the overall shape of the striate cortex? Although I
have repeatedly called the cortex a plate, I have not necessarily meant to
imply that it is a plane. If there were no distortion at all in shape, the striate
cortex would be a sphere, just as the eyeball is and just as any map of the
earth, if undistorted, must be. (Strictly, of course, the striate cortex on one
side maps about half of the back halves of the two eyes, or about a quarter-
sphere.) In stretching, so as to keep thickness constant and yet manage many
more messages from the crowded layers of ganglion cells at the fovea, the
cortex becomes distorted relative to the spherical surface that it otherwise
would approximate. If we unfold and smooth out the creases in the cortex, we
discover that it is indeed neither spherical nor flat; it has the shape of a very
distorted quarter-sphere, rather like a pear or an egg. This result was
predicted in 1962 by Daniel and Whitteridge, who determined experimentally
the magnification in area 17 as a function of distance from the to veal
representation, as mentioned on page 1, and used the result to calculate the
three-dimensional shape. They then made a rubber model of the cortex from
serial histological sections and literally unfolded it, thus verifying the pear
shape they had predicted. We can see the shape in the illustration on the
previous page. Till then no one had reasoned out the question so as to predict
that the cortex would unfold into any reasonable shape, nor, to my
knowledge, had anyone realized that for any area of cortex, some shape or
other must exist whose configuration should follow logically from its
function. Presumably the folds, which must be smoothed out (without
stretching or tearing) to get at the essential shape, exist because this large,
distorted quarter-sphere must be crumpled to fit the compact box of the skull.
The foldings may not be entirely arbitrary: some of the details are probably
determined so as to minimize the lengths of cortico-cortical connections.
In the somatosensory cortex the problems of topography can become
extreme to the point of absurdity. The cortex corresponding to the skin
covering the hand, for example, should have basically a glove shape, with
distortions over and above that to allow for the much greater sensory
capacities of the finger tips, as compared with the palm or back of the hand.
Such a distortion is analogous to the distortion of the foveal projections
relative to the periphery, to allow for its greater acuity. Would the hand area
of the cortex—if we modeled it in rubber and then stood inside and blew
gently to get rid of the artificial creases—really resemble a glove? Probably
not. Determining the map of the somatosensory cortex has turned out to be a
daunting task. The results so far suggest that the predicted shape is just too
bizarre; instead, the surface is cut up into manageable pieces as if with
scissors, and pasted back together like a quilt so as to approximate a flat
surface.
7. THE CORPUS CALLOSUM AND
STEREOPSIS
Stereopair of the Cloisters, New College, Oxford. The right photograph was taken, the camera was
shifted about 3 inches to the left, and the left photograph was taken.
The corpus callosum is a thick, bent plate of axons near the center of this brain section, made by cutting
apart the human cerebral hemispheres and looking at the cut surface.
Here the brain is seen from above. On the right side an inch or so of the top has been lopped off. We
can see the band of the corpus callosum fanning out after crossing, and joining every part of the two
hemispheres. (The front of the brain is at the top of the picture.)
Until about 1950 the function of the corpus callosum was a complete
mystery. On rare occasions, the corpus callosum in humans is absent at birth,
in a condition called agenesis of the corpus callosum. Occasionally it may be
completely or partially cut by the neurosurgeon, either to treat epilepsy (thus
preventing epileptic discharges that begin in one hemisphere from spreading
to the other) or to make it possible to reach a very deep tumor, such as one in
the pituitary gland, from above. In none of these cases had neurologists and
psychiatrists found any deficiency; someone had even suggested (perhaps not
seriously) that the sole function of the corpus callosum was to hold the two
cerebral hemispheres together. Until the 1950s we knew little about the
detailed connections of the corpus callosum. It clearly connected the two
cerebral hemispheres, and on the basis of rather crude neurophysiology it was
thought to join precisely corresponding cortical areas on the two sides. Even
cells in the striate cortex were assumed to send axons into the corpus
callosum to terminate in the exactly corresponding part of the striate cortex
on the opposite side.
In 1955 Ronald Myers, a graduate student studying under psychologist
Roger Sperry at the University of Chicago, did the first experiment that
revealed a function for this immense bundle of fibers. Myers trained cats in a
box containing two side-by-side screens onto which he could project images,
for example a circle onto one screen and a square onto the other. He taught a
cat to press its nose against the screen with the circle, in preference to the one
with the square, by rewarding correct responses with food and punishing
mistakes mildly by sounding an unpleasantly loud buzzer and pulling the cat
back from the screen gently but firmly. By this method the cat could be
brought to a fairly consistent performance in a few thousand trials. (Cats
learn slowly; a pigeon will learn a similar task in tens to hundreds of trials,
and we humans can learn simply by being told. This seems a bit odd, given
that a cat's brain is many times the size of a pigeon's. So much for the sizes of
brains.)
Not surprisingly, Myers' cats could master such a task just as fast if one
eye was closed by a mask. Again not surprisingly, if a task such as choosing a
triangle or a square was learned with the left eye alone and then tested with
the right eye alone, performance was just as good. This seems not particularly
impressive, since we too can easily do such a task. The reason it is easy must
be related to the anatomy. Each hemisphere receives input from both eyes,
and as we saw in Chapter 4, a large proportion of cells in area 17 receive
input from both eyes. Myers now made things more interesting by surgically
cutting the optic chiasm in half, by a fore-and-aft cut in the midline, thus
severing the crossing fibers but leaving the uncrossed ones intact—a
procedure that takes some surgical skill. Thus the left eye was attached only
to the left hemisphere and the right eye to the right hemisphere. The idea now
was to teach the cat through the left eye and test it with the right eye: if it
performed correctly, the information necessarily would have crossed from the
left hemisphere to the right through the only route known, the corpus
callosum. Myers did the experiment: he cut the chiasm longitudinally, trained
the cat through one eye, and tested it through the other—and the cat still
succeeded. Finally, he repeated the experiment in an animal whose chiasm
and corpus callosum had both been surgically divided. The cat now failed.
Thus he established, at long last, that the callosum actually could do
something—although we would hardly suppose that its sole purpose was to
allow the few people or animals with divided optic chiasms to perform with
one eye after learning a task with the other.
7.2 STUDIES OF THE PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CALLOSUM
One of the first neurophysiological examinations of the corpus
callosum was made a few years after Myers' experiments by David
Whitteridge, then in Edinburgh. Whitteridge realized that for a band of nerve
fibers to join homologous, mirror-symmetric parts of area 17 made no sense.
No reason could possibly exist for wanting a cell in the left hemisphere,
concerned with points somewhere out in the right field of vision, to be
connected to a cell on the other side, concerned with points equally far out in
the left field. To check this further Whitteridge surgically severed the optic
tract on the right side, just behind the optic chiasm, thus detaching the right
occipital lobe from the outside world—except, of course, for any input that
area might receive from the left occipital lobe via the corpus callosum, as you
can see from the illustration on this page.
In his experiment, Whitteridge cut the right optic tract. For information to get from either eye to the
right visual cortex, it now has to go to the left visual cortex and cross in the corpus callosum. Cooling
either of these areas blocks the flow of nerve impulses.
He then looked for responses by shining light in the eyes and recording
from the right hemisphere with wire electrodes placed on the cortical surface.
He did record responses, but the electrical waves he observed appeared only
at the inner border of area 17, a region that gets its visual input from a long,
narrow, vertical strip bisecting the visual field: when he used smaller spots of
light, they produced responses only when they were flashed in parts of the
visual field at or near the vertical midline. Cooling the cortex on the opposite
side, thus temporarily putting it out of commission, abolished the responses,
as did cooling the corpus callosum. Clearly, the corpus callosum could not be
joining all of area 17 on the two sides, but just a small part subserving the
vertical midline of the visual field.
Anatomical experiments had already suggested such a result. Only the
parts of area 17 very close to the border between areas 17 and 18 sent axons
across to the other side, and these seemed to end, for the most part, in area
18, close to its border with area 17. If we assume that the input the cortex
gets from the geniculates is strictly from contralateral visual fields—left field
to right cortex and right field to left cortex—the presence of corpus-callosum
connections between hemispheres should result in one hemisphere's receiving
input from more than one-half the visual fields: the connections should
produce an overlap in the visual-field territories feeding into the two
hemispheres. That is, in fact, what we find. Two electrodes, one in each
hemisphere near the 17-18 borders, frequently record cells whose fields
overlap by several degrees. Torsten Wiesel and I soon made microelectrode
recordings directly from the part of the corpus callosum containing visual
fibers, the most posterior portion. We found that nearly all the fibers that we
could activate by visual stimuli responded exactly like ordinary cells of area
17, with simple or complex properties, selective for orientation and
responding usually to both eyes. They all had receptive fields lying very close
to the vertical midline, either below, above, or in the center of gaze, as shown
in the diagram on the privious page.
Perhaps the most esthetically pleasing neurophysiological
demonstration of corpus-callosum function came from the work of Giovanni
Berlucchi and Giacomo Rizzolatti in Pisa in 1968. Having cut the optic
chiasm along the midline, they made recordings from area 17, close to the 17-
18 border on the right side, and looked for cells that could be driven
binocularly. Obviously anybinocular cell in the visual cortex on the right side
must receive input from the right eye directly (via the geniculate) and from
the left eye by way of the left hemisphere and corpus callosum. Each
binocular receptive field spanned the vertical midline, with the part to the left
responding to the right eye and the part to the right responding to the left eye.
Other properties, including orientation selectivity, were identical, as shown in
the illustration on the next page.
This result showed clearly that one function of the corpus callosum is
to connect cells so that their fields can span the midline. It therefore cements
together the two halves of the visual world. To imagine this more vividly,
suppose that our cortex had originally been constructed out of one piece
instead of being subdivided into two hemispheres; area 17 would then be one
large plate, mapping the entire visual field. Neighboring cells would of
course be richly interconnected, so as to produce the various response
properties, including movement responses and orientation selectivity. Now
suppose a dicta-really happened, since the brain had two hemispheres long
before the cerebral cortex evolved.
This experiment of Berlucchi and Rizzolatti provides the most vivid
example I know of the remarkable specificity of neural connections. The cell
illustrated on this page, and presumably a million other callosally connected
cells like it, derives a single orientation selectivity both through local
connections to nearby cells and through connections coming from a region of
cortex in the other hemisphere, several inches away, from cells with the same
orientation selectivity and immediately adjacent receptive-field positions—to
say nothing of all the other matching attributes, such as direction selectivity,
end-stopping, and degree of complexity.
The receptive fields of fibers in the corpus callosum lie very close to the vertical midline. The receptive
fields here were found by recording from ten fibers in one cat.
Every callosally connected cell in the visual cortex must get its input
from cells in the opposite hemisphere with exactly matching properties. We
have all kinds of evidence for such selective connectivity in the nervous
system, but I can think of none that is so beautifully direct. Visual fibers such
as these make up only a small proportion of callosal fibers. In the
somatosensory system, anatomical axon-transport studies, similar to the
radioactive-amino-acid eye injections described in earlier chapters, show that
the corpus callosum similarly connects areas of cortex that are activated by
skin or joint receptors near the midline of the body, on the trunk, back, or
face, but does not connect regions concerned with the extremities, the feet
and hands.
This experiment by Berlucchi and Rizzolatti beautifully illustrates not only the function of the visual
part of the callosum but also the high specificity of its connections between cells of like orientation and
bordering receptive fields. Berlucchi and Rizzolatti cut the chiasm of a cat in the midline, so that the
left eye supplies only the left hemisphere, with information coming solely from the right field of vision.
Similarly, the right eye supplies only the right hemisphere, with information from the left visual field.
After making the incision, they recorded from a cell whose receptive field would normally overlap the
vertical midline. They found that such a cell's receptive field is split vertically, with the right part
supplied through the left eye and the left part through the right eye.
7.3 STEREOPSIS
The strategy of judging depth by comparing the images on our two
retinas works so well that many of us who are not psychologists or visual
physiologists are not aware of the ability. To satisfy yourself of its
importance, try driving a car or bicycle, playing tennis, or skiing for even a
few minutes with one eye closed. Stereoscopes are out of fashion, though you
can still find them in antique shops, but most of us know about 3-D movies,
where you have to wear special glasses. Both of these rely on stereopsis.
The image cast on our retinas is two-dimensional, but we look out on a
three-dimensional world. To humans and animals it is obviously important to
be able to tell how far away things are. Similarly, determining an object's
three-dimensional shape means estimating relative depths. To take a simple
example, circular objects unless viewed head-on produce elliptical images,
but we can generally recognize them as circular with no trouble; and to do
that requires a sense of depth.
We judge depth in many ways, some of which are so obvious that they
hardly require mention (but I will anyhow). When the size of something is
roughly known, as is so for a person, tree, or cat, we can judge its distance—
at the risk of being fooled by dwarves, bonsai, or lions. If one object is partly
in front of another and blocks its view, we judge the front object as closer.
The images of parallel lines like railroad tracks as they go off into the
distance draw closer together: this is an example of perspective, a powerful
indicator of depth. A bump on a wall that juts out is brighter on top if the
light source comes from above (as light sources generally do), and a pit in a
surface lit from above is darker in its upper part: if the light is made to come
from below, bumps look like pits and pits like bumps. A major clue to depth
is parallax, the relative motions of near and far objects that is produced when
we move our heads from side to side or up and down. Rotating a solid object
even through a small angle can make its shape immediately apparent. If we
use our lens to focus on a near object, a far one will be out of focus, and by
varying the shape of the lens—by changing accommodation (described in
Chapters 2 and 6)—we should be able to determine how far an object is.
Changing the relative directions of the eyes, adjusting the toeing in or toeing
out, will bring the two images of an object together over a narrow range of
convergence or divergence. Thus in principle the adjustment of either lens or
eye position could tell us an object's distance, and many range finders are
based on these principles.
Except for the convergence and divergence, all these depth cues need
involve only one eye. Stereopsis, perhaps the most important mechanism for
assessing depth, depends on the use of the two eyes together. In any scene
with depth, our two eyes receive slightly different images. You can satisfy
yourself of this simply by looking straight ahead and moving your head
quickly about 4 'inches to the right or left or by quickly alternating eyes by
opening one and closing the other. If you are facing a flat object, you won't
see much difference, but if the scene contains objects at different distances,
you will see marked changes. In stereopsis, the brain compares the images of
a scene on the two retinas and estimates relative depths with great accuracy.
Left: When an observer looks at a point P, directions of the eyes, adjusting the toeing in or toeing out,
will bring the two the two images of P fall on the foveas P. Qimages of an object together over a
narrow range of convergence or diveris a point that is judged by the observer to be the same distance
away as P. The two images of Q (QL and Qr) are then said to fall on corresponding points. (The surface
made up of all points Q, the same apparent distance away as P, is the horopter through P.) Right: If Q'
appears closer to the observer than Q, then the images of Q' (QL' and Qr') will be farther apart on the
retina in a horizontal direction than they would be if they were corresponding points. If Q' appears
farther away, QL' and Qr' will be horizontally displaced toward each other.
Above: Wheatstone's stereoscope, the original drawing of which is shown to the right.
Right: Wheatstone's diagram of his stereoscope. The observer faced two-45 degree mirrors (A and A')
and saw, superimposed, the two pictures, E through the right eye and E' through the left. In later,
simpler versions, the observer faces the two pictures placed side by side on a screen at a distance apart
roughly equal to the distance between the eyes. Two prisms deflect the directions of gaze so that with
the eyes aligned as if the observer were looking at the screen, the left eye sees the left picture and the
right eye the right picture. You can learn to dispense with the stereoscope by pretending you are
looking at a distant object, thus making the two directions of gaze parallel so that the left eye sees the
left picture and the right eye the right picture.
If the upper pair of circles is put in a stereoscope, the circle will stand out in front of the frame. For the
lower pair, it will seem to float behind the frame. (You may want to try to superimpose the frames by
crossing your eyes or uncrossing them. Most people find uncrossing easier. Placing a thin piece of
cardboard between the two images will help. You may at first find this a difficult and disturbing
exercise; don't persist very long the first try. With crossed eyes, the upper dot will be farther, the lower
one nearer.)
Another stereopair.
Not all people have the ability to perceive depth by stereoscopy. You
can easily check your stereopsis by using the illustrations on this page: each
of the diagrams shows two pictures that together would produce a stereogram
for use in an ordinary stereoscope. You can place a copy of these in a
stereoscope if you happen to have one, or you can try to look at one with each
eye by putting a thin piece of cardboard between them, perpendicular to the
plane of the page, and staring, as if you were looking at something far away;
you can even learn to cross your eyes, by holding a finger between you and
the pictures and adjusting its distance till the two fuse, and then (this is the
hard part) examining the fused image without having it break apart. If this
works for you, the depth will be reversed relative to the depth you get by the
first method. Even if you don't see the depth, because you have no
stereoscope or you can't cross or uncross your eyes, you will still be able to
follow the arguments— you just miss the fun.
In the uppermost example, we have a square containing a small circle a
little to the right of the center in one member and a little to the left in the
other. If you view the figure with both eyes, using the stereoscope or partition
method, you should see the circle no longer in the plane of the page but
standing out in front of it about an inch or so. Similarly, you should see the
second figure as a circle behind the plane of the page. You see the circle in
front or behind the page because your retinas are getting exactly the same
information they would get if the circle were in front or behind.
In 1960 Bela Julesz, at Bell Telephone Laboratories, invented an
ingenious, highly useful method for demonstrating stereopsis. The figure on
the previous page will at first glance seem like a uniformly random mass of
tiny triangles— and indeed it is except for the concealed larger triangle in the
center part. If you look at it through pieces of colored cellophane, red over
one eye and green over the other, you should see the center-triangle region
standing out in front of the page, just as the circle did. (You may, the first
time, have to keep looking for a minute or so.) Reversing the cellophane
windows will reverse the depth. The usefulness of these Julesz patterns is that
you cannot possibly see the triangle standing out in front or receding unless
you have intact stereopsis.
To sum up, our ability to see depth depends on five principles:
1. We have many cues to depth, such as occlusion, parallax, rotation of
objects, relative size, shadow casting, and perspective. Probably the
most important cue is stereopsis.
2. If we fixate on, or look at, a point in space, the images of the point on
our two retinas fall on the two foveas. Any point judged to be the same
distance away as the point fixated casts its two images on corresponding
retinal points.
3. Stereopsis depends on the simple geometric fact that as an object gets
closer to us, the two images it casts on the two retinas become outwardly
displaced, compared with corresponding points
4. The central fact ofstereopsis—a biological fact learned from testing
people— is this: an object whose images fall on corresponding points in
the two retinas is perceived as being the same distance away as the point
fixated. When the images are outwardly displaced relative to
corresponding points, the object is seen as nearer than the fixated point,
and when the displacement is inward, the object is seen as farther away.
5. Horizontal displacements greater than about 2 degrees or vertical
displacements of over a few minutes of arc lead to double vision.
To prepare this figure, called an anaglyph, Bela Julesz begins by constructing two arrays of randomly
placed tiny triangular dots, identical except that (1) one consists of red dots on a white background and
the other of green dots on a white background, and (2) over a large triangular region, near the center of
the array, all the dots in the grecn-and-whitc array are displaced slightly to the left, relative to the
corresponding red and white dots. The two arrays are now superimposed with a slight offset, so that the
dots themselves do not quite superimpose. If the figure is viewed through a green cellophane filter, only
the red dots are seen; through a red cellophane filter, only the green dots are seen. If you view the
figure with green cellophane over the left eye and red over the right, you will see the large triangle
standing out about 1 centimeter in front of the page. Reversing the filters (green over the right eye and
red over the left) causes the triangle to appear behind.
When both eyes are stimulated together by a vertical slit of light moving leftward, an ordinary
binocular cell in area 17 will have similar responses to three different relative alignments of the two
eyes. Zero disparity means that the two eyes are lined up as they would be if the monkey were looking
at the screen onto which the stimuli are being projected. The exact alignment makes little difference in
the response of the cell.
For this "tuned excitatory" cell, it makes a lot of difference whether the stimulus is at the distance the
animal is looking, or is nearer or farther away. The cell fires only if the slit is roughly at the distance the
animal is looking. In these experiments, the direction of gaze of one eye is varied horizontally with a
prism, but bodily moving the screen nearer or farther away would amount to the same thing.
For this cell, a "far" cell, objects closer than the screen evoke little or no response; at about zero
disparity (screen distance), a small shift of the screen has a very large influence on the effectiveness of
the stimulus. The response rises sharply to a plateau for distances farther away than where the animal is
looking. Beyond a certain point the two receptive fields no longer overlap; in effect, the eyes are being
stimulated separately. Response then falls to zero.
Gian Poggio at Johns Hopkins Medical School has recorded such cells
in area 17 of alert implanted monkeys trained to keep their eyes fixed on a
target. In anesthetized monkeys, such cells, although certainly present, seem
to be rare in area 17 but are very common in area 18. I would be surprised if
an animal or human could assess and compare the distances of objects in a
scene stereoscopically if the only cells involved were the three types—tuned
excitatory, near, and far—that I have just described. I would have guessed
that we would find a whole array of cells for all possible depths. In alert
monkeys, Poggio has also seen tuned excitatory cells with peak responses not
at zero but slightly away from zero, and it thus seems that the cortex may
contain cells with all degrees of disparity. Although we still do not know how
the brain reconstructs a scene full of objects at various distances (whatever
"reconstructs" means), cells such as these seem to represent an early stage in
the process.
If the figure on page 12 gave you the usual depth effects of a circle standing out in front or floating
back behind, the one which combines the two should give both a circle in front and one behind. It gives
neither, just a pair of circles at the same depth as the frame.
Now suppose we do both things at once, putting the two circles side by
side in each picture. Clearly that could give us two circles, one nearer and one
farther than the fixation plane. But we could also imagine that the result
might simply be two circles lying side by side in the plane of fixation: either
situation leads to the same retinal stimuli. In fact, such a pair of stimuli can
be seen only as two circles side by side, as you will see by fusing the two
squares on this page by any of the methods described. Similarly, we can
imagine that looking at two diagrams, each consisting of a row of x's, say six
of each, side by side, might result in any of a large number of sensations,
depending on which x in one eye you pair with which x in the other. In fact,
what you get when you view two such diagrams is always six fused x's in the
plane of fixation. We don't yet know how the brain resolves the ambiguities
and reaches the simplest of all the possible combinations. It is hard to
imagine, given the opportunities for ambiguity, how we ever make sense of a
scene consisting of a bunch of twigs and branches all at different depths. The
physiology, at any rate, tells us that the problem may not be so difficult, since
different twigs at different depths are likely to be in different orientations,
and as far as we know, stereoscopically tuned cells are always orientation
tuned. The second example of the unpredictability of binocular effects has
direct bearing on stereopsis but involves retinal rivalry, which we allude to in
our discussion of strabismus in Chapter 9.
You cannot fuse this pair in the way you can fuse other pairs, such as those on page 12 the previous.
Instead, you get "retinal rivalry" a patchwork quilt of vertical and horizontal areas whose borders fade
in and out and change position.
If two very different images are made to fall on the two retinas, very
often one will be, as it were, turned off. If you look at the left black-and-
white square on this page with the left eye and the right one with the right
eye, by crossing or uncrossing your eyes or with a stereoscope, you might
expect to see a grid, or mesh, like a window screen. Actually, it is virtually
impossible to see both sets of orthogonal stripes together. You may see all
vertical or all horizontal, with one set coming into view for a few seconds as
the other fades, or you may see a kind of patchwork mosaic of the two, in
which the patches move and blend in and out from one orientation to the
other, as shown by the figure on this page. For some reason the nervous
system will not put up with so different simultaneous stimuli in any one part
of the visual field—it suppresses one of them. But here we use the word
suppress as a short way ofredescribing the phenomenon: we don't really
know how the suppression is accomplished or at what level in the central
nervous system it takes place. To me, the patchy quality of the outcome of
the battle between the two eyes suggests that the decision takes place rather
early in visual processing, conceivably in area 17 or 18. (I am glad I do not
have to defend such a guess.) That we experience retinal rivalry implies that
in cases in which the visual system cannot get a sensible result out of the
combination of the two sets of inputs from the two eyes—either a single
fused flat scene if the images are identical or a scene with depth if the images
differ only in small horizontal disparities—it gives up and simply rejects one
of the two, either outright, as when you look through a monocular
microscope, keeping the other eye open, or in patchy or alternating fashion,
as in the example described here. In the case of the microscope, attention
surely plays a role, and the neural mechanisms of that role are likewise
unknown.
You can see another example of retinal rivalry if you attempt to fuse
two patches of different colors, say red and green, instead of vertical and
horizontal lines as just described. As I will show in the next chapter, simply
mixing red and green light produces the sensation of yellow. On the contrary,
when the two colors are presented to separate eyes the result is usually
intense rivalry, with red predominating one moment and green the next, and
again a tendency for red and green regions to break up into patches that come
and go. The rivalry however disappears and one sees yellow if the
brightnesses of the patches are carefully adjusted so as to be equal. It seems
that color rivalry is produced by differences in brightness rather than
differences in hue.
7.6 STEREOBLINDNESS
Anyone who is blind in one eye will obviously have no stereopsis.
But in the population of people with otherwise normal vision, a
surprisingly sizable minority seem to lack stereopsis. If I show stereopairs
like the ones on page 37 to a class of 100 students, using polaroids and
polarized light, four or five students generally fail to see depth, usually to
their surprise, because otherwise they seem'to have managed perfectly well.
This may seem strange if you have tried the experiment of driving with an
eye closed, but it seems that in the absence of stereopsis the other cues to
depth—parallax, perspective, depth from movement, occlusion—can in time
do very well at compensating. We will see in Chapter 9 that if strabismus, a
condition in which the two eyes point in different directions, occurs during
infancy, it can lead to the breakdown in connections responsible for binocular
interaction in the cortex and, with it, the loss of stereopsis. Since strabismus
is common, mild forms of it that were never noticed may account for some
cases of stereoblindness. In other cases, people may have a genetic defect in
stereopsis, just as they can be genetically color-blind. Having paired the two
topics, corpus callosum and stereopsis, I shouldn't miss the chance to
capitalize on what they have in common. You can set yourself the following
puzzle: What defect in stereopsis might you expect in someone whose corpus
callosum has been severed? The answer is revealed in the illustration on the
next page.
If you look at point P and consider a point Q, closer than P and falling
in the acute angle FPF, the retinal images QL and QR of Q will fall on
opposite sides of the two foveas: QL will project to your left hemisphere and
QR will project to your right hemisphere. This information in the two
hemispheres has to connect if the brain is to figure out that Q is closer than P
—in other words, if it is to perform stereopsis. The only way it can get
together is by the corpus callosum. If that path is destroyed, you will be
stereoblind in the shaded area. In 1970 Donald Mitchell and Colin
Blakemore, at the University of California, Berkeley, tested a subject who
had had his corpus callosum cut to relieve epilepsy, and indeed, they found
precisely this deficit.
Severing the corpus callosum leads to a loss of stercopsis in the shaded part of a subject's visual world.
Results of a longitudinal midline section ofthe optic chiasm: The subject is blind in the two darker
wedge-shaped areas at the extreme left and right. Between, in the more lightly shaded areas, stereopsis
will be absent, except in the wedge-shaped region beyond P, where there will be no vision at all, and in
front of P, where stereopsis will be intact.
The hundreds of dollars extra that consumers are willing to pay for
color TV in preference to black and white must mean that we take our color
sense seriously. A complex apparatus in the eye and brain can discriminate
the differences in wavelength content of the things we see, and the
advantages of this ability to our ancestors are easy to imagine. One advantage
must surely have been the ability to defeat the attempts of other animals to
camouflage themselves: it is much harder for a hunted animal to blend in
with the surroundings if its predator can discriminate the wavelength as well
as the intensity of light.
Color must also be important in finding plant food: a bright red berry
standing out against green foliage is easily found by a monkey, to his obvious
advantage and presumably to the plant's, since the seeds pass unharmed
through the monkey's digestive tract and are widely scattered. In some
animals color is important in reproduction; examples include the bright red
coloration of the perineal region of macaque monkeys and the marvelous
plumage of many male birds. Color serves many purposes in nature, some of
which are still mysterious. The blue spots on this Garibaldi fish fade as it
grows older, disappering when it matures. What significance they have for
older Garibaldi fish is not known.
In humans, evolutionary pressure to preserve or improve color vision
would seem to be relaxing, at least to judge from the 7 or 8 percent of human
males who are relatively or completely deficient in color vision but who seem
to get along quite well, with their deficit often undiagnosed for years, only to
be picked up when they run through red lights. Even those of us who have
normal color vision can fully enjoy black-and-white movies, some of which
are artistically the best ever made. As I will discuss later, we are all color-
blind in dim light.
Among vertebrates, color sense occurs sporadically, probably having
been downgraded or even lost and then reinvented many times in the course
of evolution. Mammals with poor color vision or none at all include mice,
rats, rabbits, cats, dogs, and a species of monkey, the nocturnal owl monkey.
Ground squirrels and primates, including humans, apes, and most old
world monkeys, all have well-developed color vision. Nocturnal animals
whose vision is specialized for dim light seldom have good color vision,
which suggests that color discrimination and capabilities for handling dim
light are somehow not compatible. Among lower vertebrates, color vision is
well developed in many species of fish and birds but is probably absent or
poorly developed in reptiles and amphibia. Many insects, including flies and
bees, have color vision. We do not know the exact color-handling capabilities
of the overwhelming majority of animal species, perhaps because behavioral
or physiological tests for color vision are not easy to do.
The subject of color vision, out of all proportion to its biologic
importance to man, has occupied an amazing array of brilliant minds,
including Newton, Goethe (whose strength seems not to have been science),
and Helmholtz. Nevertheless color is still often poorly understood even by
artists, physicists, and biologists. The problem starts in childhood, when we
are given our first box of paints and then told that yellow, blue, and red are
the primary colors and that yellow plus blue equals green. Most of us are then
surprised when, in apparent contradiction of that experience, we shine a
yellow spot and a blue spot on a screen with a pair of slide projectors, overlap
them, and see in the overlapping region a beautiful snow white. The result of
mixing paints is mainly a matter of physics; mixing light beams is mainly
biology.
In thinking about color, it is useful to keep separate in our minds these
different components: physics and biology. The physics that we need to know
is limited to a few facts about light waves. The biology consists of
psychophysics, a discipline concerned with examining our capabilities as
instruments for detecting information from the outside world, and
physiology, which examines the detecting instrument, our visual system, by
looking inside it to learn how it works. We know a lot about the physics and
psychophysics of color, but the physiology is still in a relatively primitive
state, largely because the necessary tools have been available for only a few
decades.
Left: The energy in a beam of light such as sunlight contains a broad distribution of wavelengths, from
400 or less to about 700 nanometers. The gentle peak is a function of the temperature of the source: the
hotter the source the more the peak is displaced towards the blue, or short-wave-length, end. Right:
Monochromatic light is light whose energy is mostly at or near one wavelength. It can be produced
with various kinds of filters, with a spectroscope containing a prism or a grating, or with a laser.
8.2 PIGMENTS
When light hits an object, one of three things can happen: the light can
be absorbed and the energy converted to heat, as when the sun warms
something; it can pass through the object, as when the sun's rays hit water or
glass; or it can be reflected, as in the case of a mirror or any light-colored
object, such as a piece of chalk. Often two or all three of these happen; for
example, some light may be absorbed and some reflected. For many objects,
the relative amount of light absorbed and reflected depends on the light's
wavelength. The green leaf of a plant absorbs long- and short-wavelength
light and reflects light of middle wavelengths, so that when the sun hits a
leaf, the light reflected back will have a pronounced broad peak at middle
wave-lengths (in the green). A red object will have its peak, likewise broad,
in the long wavelengths, as shown in the graph on the next page.
Most colored objects reflect light that is generally richer in some parts of the visible spectrum than in
others. The distribution of wavelengths is much broader than that for monochromatic light, however.
This graph shows the spectral content of light that would be reflected from a red object, using a broad-
band (white) light source.
An object that absorbs some of the light reaching it and reflects the rest
is called a pigment. If some wavelengths in the range of visible light are
absorbed more than others, the pigment appears to us to be colored. What
color we see, I should quickly add, is not simply a matter of wavelengths; it
depends on wavelength content and on the properties of our visual system. It
involves both physics and biology.
The pigments in the three cone types have their peak absorptions at
about 430, 530, and 560 nanometers, as shown in the graph below; the cones
are consequently loosely called "blue", "green", and "red", "loosely" because
(1) the names refer to peak sensitivities (which in turn are related to ability
toabsorb light) rather than to the way the pigments would appear if we were
to look at them; (2) monochromatic lights whose wavelengths are 430, 530,
and 560 nanometers are not blue, green, and red but violet, blue-green, and
yellow-green; and (3) if we were to stimulate cones ofjust one type, we
would see not blue, green, or red but probably violet, green, and yellowish-
red instead.
Absorption spectra (or sensitivity curves) differ for the three types of cones. (Spectral-energy curves
and absorption curves such as these have their y axes in log units because they operate over such a wide
range. The up-and-down position of the x-axis is therefore arbitrary and does not represent zero
absorption.)
The top graph, "cone sensitivities", repeats the graph on page 5. The rest of the figure suggests which
cones will be activated by various mixtures of colored light and what the resulting sensations will be.
With three slide projectors and three filters, three overlapping spots (red, green, and blue) are projected
onto a screen so that they overlap. Red and green give yellow, blue and green give turquoise, red and
blue give purple, and all three—red, blue, and green—give white.
The concept of primary colors does not even enter this scheme: if we
think of primaries in terms of the three receptor types, we have greenish-
yellow, green, and violet, shades hardly consistent with the idea of three pure,
basic colors. But if by primary we mean three colors from which any other
hues can be generated, these three will do, as will any other three that are far
enough apart. Thus nothing I have said so far gives any justification for the
idea of three unique primary colors.
A second kind of color results from adding white to any spectral color
or to purple; we say that the white "washes out" the color, or makes it paler—
the technical term is that it desaturates it. To match any two colors, we have
to make their hues and saturations the same (for example, by selecting the
appropriate position on the circle of colors and then adding the right amount
of white), and then we need to equate the intensities. Thus we can specify a
color by giving the wavelength of the color (or in the case of purple, its
complement), the relative content of white, and a single number specifying
intensity.
A mathematically equivalent option for specifying color is to give three
numbers representing the relative effects of the light on the three cone types.
Either way, it takes three numbers. A third kind of color these explanations
do not cover is typified by brown. I will come to it later.
Young's theory was adopted and championed by Hermann von
Helmholtz and came to be known as the Young-Helmholtz theory. It was
Helmholtz, incidentally, who finally explained the phenomenon mentioned at
the beginning of this chapter, that mixing yellow and blue paints gives green.
You can easily see how this differs from adding yellow and blue light by
doing the following experiment, for which you need only two slide projectors
and some yellow and blue cellophane. First, put the yellow cellophane over
the lens of one projector and the blue over the other and then overlap the
projected images. If you adjust the relative intensities, you will get a pure
white in the area of overlap. This is the kind of color mixing we have been
talking about, and we have said that the white arises because the combined
yellow and blue light manages to activate all three of our cones with the same
relative effectiveness that broad-band, or white, light does. Now turn off one
projector and put both filters in front of the other one, and you will get green.
To understand what is happening we need to know that the blue cellophane
absorbs long-wavelength light, the yellows and reds, from the white and lets
through the rest, which looks blue, and that the yellow filter absorbs mainly
blue and lets through the rest, which looks yellow. The diagram on this page
shows the spectral composition of the light each filter passes. Note that in
both cases the light that gets through is far from monochromatic, the yellow
light is not narrow-band spectral yellow but a mixture of spectral yellow and
shorter wavelengths, greens, and longer wavelengths, oranges and reds.
Similarly, the blue is spectral blue plus greens and violet. Why don't we see
more than just yellow or just blue? Yellow is the result of equal stimulation
of the red and the green cones, with no stimulation of the blue cone; this
stimulation can be accomplished with spectral yellow (monochromatic light
at 580 nanometers) or with a broader smear of wavelengths, such as we
typically get with pigments, as long as the breadth is not so great as to
include short wavelengths and thereby stimulate the blue cone. Similarly, as
far as our three cones are concerned, spectral blue light has about the same
impact as blue plus green plus violet. Now, when we use the two filters, one
in front of the other, what we get is what both filters let through, namely, just
the greens. This is where the graphs shown on this page, for broad-band blue
and yellow, overlap. The same thing happens with paints: yellow and blue
paints together absorb everything in the light except greens, which are
reflected. Note that if we used monochromatic yellow and blue filters in our
experiment, putting one in front of the other would result in nothing getting
through. The mixing works only because the colors produced by pigments
have a broad spectral content. Why discuss this phenomenon here? I do so
partly because it is gratifying to explain the dramatic and startling result of
mixing yellow and blue paint to get green, and the even more startling result
—because it is so unfamiliar to most people—of mixing yellow and blue
light to get white. (In a chapter on color theory in a book on weaving, I found
the statement that if you mix yellow and blue threads, as in warp and weft,
you get green. What you do get is gray— for biological reasons.) The
artificial results of mixing paints is doubtless what has led to the idea of
"primary colors," such as red, yellow, and blue. If any special set of colors
deserves to be called primary, it is the set of red, blue, yellow, and green. As
we will see in the section on Hering's color theory, what justification all four
have as candidates for primaries has little to do with the three cones and
much to do with the subsequent wiring in the retina and brain.
The blue filter passes a fairly broad band of wavelengths centered about 480 nanometers. The yellow
filter passes a fairly broad band of wavelengths centered about 580 nanometers. Both together pass only
wavelengths common to the two—light at a fairly broad band of wavelengths centered about 530,
which gives a green.
In many of his experiments Edwin Land used a Mondrian-like patchwork of colored papers. The
experiments were designed to prove that the colors remain remarkably constant despite marked
variations in the relative intensities of the red, green, and blue lights used to illuminate the display.
In David Ingle's experiment, a goldfish has been trained to swim to a patch of a given color for a
reward—a piece of liver. It swims to the green patch regardless of the exact setting of the three
projectors' intensities. The behavior is strikingly similar to the perceptual result in humans.
In 1958, Russell De Valois (rhymes with hoi polloi) and his colleagues
recorded responses strikingly similar to Svaetichin's from cells in the lateral
geniculate body of macaque monkeys. De Valois had previously shown by
behavioral testing that color vision in macaque monkeys is almost identical to
color vision in humans; for example, the amounts of two colored lights that
have to be combined to match a third light are almost identical in the two
species. It is therefore likely that macaques and humans have similar
machinery in the early stages of their visual pathways, and we are probably
justified in comparing human color psychophysics with macaque physiology.
De Valois
found many geniculate cells that were activated by diffuse
monochromatic light at wavelengths ranging from one end of the spectrum to
a crossover point, where there was no response, and were suppressed by light
over a second range of wavelengths from the crossover point to the other end.
Again the analogy to Hering's color processes was compelling: De Valois
tound op-ponent-color cells of two types, red-green and yellow-blue; for each
type, combining two lights whose wavelengths were on opposite sides of
some crossover point led to mutual cancellation of responses, just as,
perceptually, adding blue to yellow or adding green to red produced white.
De Valois' results were especially reminiscent of Hering's formulations in
that his two classes of color cells had response maxima and crossover points
in just the appropriate places along the spectrum for one group to be judging
the yellow-blueness of the light and the other, red-greenness.
The next step was to look at the receptive fields of these cells by using
small spots of colored light, as Torsten Wiesel and I did in 1966, instead of
diffuse light. For most of De Valois' opponent-color cells, the receptive fields
had a surprising organization, one that still puzzles us. The cells, like
Kufflcr's in the cat, had fields divided into antagonistic centers and
surrounds; the center could be "on" or "off". In a typical example, the field
center is fed exclusively by red cones and the inhibitory surround exclusively
by green cones. Consequently, with red light both a small spot and a large
spot give brisk responses, because the center is selectively sensitive to long-
wavelength light and the surround virtually insensitive; with short-
wavelength light, small spots give little or no response and large spots
produce strong inhibition with off responses. With white light, containing
short and long wavelengths, small spots evoke on responses and large spots
produce no response.
In a typical type 1 receptive field, the center receives excitatory input from red cones; the surround,
inhibitory input from green cones.
Although our first impression was that such a cell must be getting input
from red cones in the center region and green cones in the surround, it now
seems probable that the total receptive field is a combination of two
overlapping processes, as illustrated in the figure on the next page. Both the
red cones and the green cones feed in from a fairly wide circular area, in
numbers that are maximal in the center and fall off with distance from the
center. In the center, the red cones strongly predominate, and with distance
their effects fall off much more rapidly than those of the green cones. A long-
wavelength small spot shining in the center will consequently be a very
powerful stimulus to the red system; even if it also stimulates green cones,
the number, relative to the total number of green cones feeding in, will be too
small to give the red system any competition. different-shaped sensitivity-
versus-position curves.
These graphs plot the sensitivity of a cell (measured, for example, by the response to a constant very
small spot of light) against retinal position along a line AA' passing through the receptive-field center.
For an r+ center-g- surround cell, a small red spot gives a narrow curve and a small green spot, a much
broader one. The lower graph plots the responses to light such as white or yellow that stimulates both
of the opponent systems, so that the two systems subtract. Thus the red cones dominate in the center,
which gives on responses, whereas the green cones dominate in the surround, which yields off
responses.
8.11 BLOBS
By about 1978, the monkey's primary visual cortex, with its simple,
complex, and end-stopped cells and its ocular-dominance columns and
orientation columns, seemed reasonably well understood. But an unexpected
feature of the physiology was that so few of the cells seemed to be interested
in color. If we mapped a simple or complex cell's receptive field using white
light and then repeated the mapping with colored spots or slits, the results as
a rule were the same. A few cells, perhaps as many as a 10 percent of cortical
upper-layer cells, did show unmistakable color preferences—with excellent
responses to oriented slits of some color, most often red, and virtually no
response to other wavelengths or even to white light. The orientation
selectivity of these cells was just as high as that of cells lacking color
selectivity. But most cells in the visual cortex did not care about color. This
was all the more surprising because such a high proportion of cells in the
lateral geniculate body are color coded, and the geniculate forms the main
input to the visual cortex. It was hard to see what could have happened to this
color information in the cortex.
Suddenly, in 1978, all this changed. Margaret Wong-Riley, at the
University of California in San Francisco, discovered that when she stained
the cortex for the enzyme cytochrome oxidase, the upper layers showed an
unheard of inhomogeneity, with periodic dark-staining regions, pufflike in
transverse cross section, about one-quarter millimeter wide and one-half
millimeter apart. All cells contain cytochrome oxidase, an enzyme involved
in metabolism, and no one had ever imagined that a stain for such an enzyme
would show anything interesting in the cortex. When Wong-Riley sent us
pictures, Torsten Wiesel and I suspected that we were seeing ocular-
dominance slabs cut in cross section and that the most monocular cells were
for some reason metabolically more active than binocular cells. We put the
pictures in a drawer and tried to forget them.
This cross section through the striate cortex shows the layers stained for the enzyme cytochrome
oxidase. The darker zones in the upper third of the section are the blobs.
The dark areas are blobs seen tace on, about 50 of which form a polka-dot pattern. This section,
through layer 3 of area 17, is parallel to the cortical surface and about 0.5 millimeter beneath it. (The
yellow circles are blood vessels cut transversely.)
Top: Land's original formulation of the color-constancy problem seems to call for three kinds of cells,
which compare the activation of a given set of cones (red, green, or blue) in one region of retina with
the average activation of the same set in the surround. The result is three numbers, which specify the
color at the region. Thus yellow, brown, dark gray, and olive green each has a corresponding triplet of
numbers. We can therefore plot colors in a color space specified by three axes, for red, green, and blue.
Bottom: A mathematically equivalent system also gives three numbers, and is probably closer to the
way the brain specifies color. At any point on the retina, we can speak of red-greenness, the reading an
instrument would give if it were to record the relative stimulation of red and green cones (zero for
yellow or white). This value is determined for a particular region, and an average value is determined
for the surround; then the ratio is taken. The process is repeated for yellow-blueness and black-
whiteness. These three figures together are enough to specify any color.
A double-opponent cell could be built up from many geniculate type 2 cells. If the cell is r+'g- center, r-
g+ surround, then its inputs could be a large number of r g type 2 cells with fields scattered throughout
the cell's receptive field center, and r g type 2 cells with fields scattered throughout the cell's receptive
field surround, all making excitatory contacts with the double-opponent cell. Alternatively, the
surround could be formed from r+g- type 2 cells, making inhibitory contacts.
8.12 CONCLUSION
The subject of color vision illustrates so well the possibilities of
understanding otherwise quite mysterious phenomena—the results of color
mixing or the constancy of colors despite changes in the light source—by
using a combination of psychophysical and neurophysiological methods. For
all their complexity, the problems presented by color are probably simpler
than those presented by form. Despite all the orientation-specific and end-
stopped cells, we are still a long way from understanding our ability to
recognize shapes, to distinguish shapes from their background, or to interpret
three dimensions from the flat pictures presented to each of our eyes. To
compare the modalities of color and form at all may itself be misleading:
remember that differences in color at borders without any differences in
luminous intensity, can lead to perception of shapes. Thus color, like black
and white, is just one means by which shapes manifest themselves.
9. DEPRIVATION AND DEVELOPMENT
The slit shape of the pupil found in many nocturnal animals such as this cat presumably allows more
effective light reduction than a circular pupil.
Abnormal layers appear in the left and right lateral geniculate bodies (seen in cross section) of a
monkey whose right eye was closed at age two weeks for eighteen months. On both sides, the layers
receiving input from the eye that was closed (the right eye) are paler: layers 1, 4, and 6 on the left;
layers 2, 3, and 5 on the right, numbered from below. The cells in the affected layers are smaller, but
this cannot be seen at such low power. The width of the entire structure is about 5 millimeters.
Left: The left eye is almost completely dominant in a monkey whose right eye was sutured closed at an
age of five days, for six weeks. Middle: A closure of only a few days in a monkey a few weeks old is
enough to produce a marked shift in ocular dominance. Darker shading indicates the number of
abnormal cells. Right: If the closure of the monkey's eye is delayed until age four months, even a very
long closure (in this case five years) results in an eye-dominance shift that is far less marked than that
resulting from a brief closure at an age of a few weeks.
1: One eye was closed at birth for nine days in this monkey and then opened. The recordings were done
four years later, during which time the animal had had much testing of its vision. Even that long a
period with both eyes open produced little recovery in the physiology. 2: The right eye in this macaque
monkey was closed at birth. At five and a half weeks the right eyelids were opened and the left closed.
When at six months the recording was made from the right hemisphere, most of the cells strongly
favored the right, originally closed eye. 3: In this macaque monkey the right eye was closed at seven
days, for one year, at which time the right eye was opened and the left was closed. After another year,
the left eye was opened, and both remained open. When finally the recording was made at three and a
half years, most cells favored the eye that was originally open. Evidently one year is too late to do a
reverse suture.
9.1 RECOVERY
We next asked whether any recovery in physiology in a monkey could
be obtained by opening the eye that had been closed. The answer was that
after a week or more of eye closure, little or no physiological recovery ever
occurred if the closed eye was simply opened and nothing else done. Even a
few years later, the cortex was about as abnormal as it would have been at the
time of reopening the eye, as shown in the left graph on the previous page. If
at the time of reopening, the other, originally open eye was closed, in a
procedure called eye reversal, recovery did occur but only if the reversal was
done when the monkey was still in the critical period, as shown in the middle
and right-hand graphs above for early and late eye reversal. After the critical
period, even an eye reversal followed by several years with the second eye
closed failed to bring about anything more than slight recovery in the
anatomy or physiology.
The monkey's ability to see did not necessarily closely parallel the
cortical physiology. Without reversal, the originally closed eye never
recovered its sight. With reversal, sight did return and often approached
normal levels, but this was so even in late reversals, in which the physiology
remained very abnormal in the originally closed eye. We still do not
understand this disparity between the lack of substantial physiological or
anatomical recovery and what in some cases seemed to be considerable
restoration of vision. Perhaps the two sets of tests are measuring different
things. We tested the acuity of vision by measuring the ability to discriminate
such things as the smallest detectable gap in a line or circle. But testing this
type of acuity may yield an incomplete measure of visual function. It seems
hard to believe that such florid physiological and anatomical deficits in
function and structure would be reflected behaviorally by nothing more than
the minor fall in acuity we measured.
9.2 THE NATURE OF THE DEFECT
The results I have been describing made it clear that a lack of images
on the retina early in life led to profound long-lasting defects in cortical
function. These results nevertheless left open two major questions concerning
the nature of the underlying process. The first of these was a "nature versus
nurture" question: Were we depriving our animals of an experience that they
needed in order to build the right connections, or were we destroying or
disrupting connections that were already present, prewired and functional at
the time the animal was born? The dark-rearing experiments done in the
decades prior to our work had practically all been interpreted in the context of
learning—or failure to learn. The cerebral cortex, where most people thought
(and still think) that memory and mental activity resides, was looked upon in
roughly the same way as the 1-megabyte memory board for which we pay so
much when we buy our computers: these contain many elements and
connections, but no information, until we put it there. In short, people
regarded the cortex as a tabula rasa.
One obvious way to decide between these alternatives is to address the
question head on and simply record from a newborn cat or monkey. If
learning is necessary for the wiring up to occur, then we should fail to find
any of the rich specificity that we see in adult animals.
The Japanese macaque monkey, Macaca fiiscata, the largest of all macaques, lives on the ground and in
trees in northern Japan. It is protected by its thick grey-brown coat.
The day after its birth, a macaque monkey is looking about, fixating on objects, taking a keen interest in
his environment. Humans and cats show this degree of visual maturity only many weeks after birth.
We suppose a cortical cell receives input from two sources, one from each eye, and that covering one
eye has the effect of weakening the connections from that eye and strengthening the connections from
the other one.
9.3 STRABISMUS
The commonest cause of amblyopia in humans is strabismus, or squint,
terms that signify nonparallel eyes—cross-eye or wall-eye. (The term squint
as technically used is synonymous with strabismus and has nothing to do
with squinching up the eyes in bright light.) The cause of strabismus is
unknown, and indeed it probably has more than one cause. In some cases,
strabismus comes on shortly after birth, during the first few months when in
humans the eyes would just be starting to fixate and follow objects. The lack
of straightness could be the result of an abnormality in the eye muscles, or it
could be caused by a derangement in the circuits in the brainstem that
subserve eye movements.
In some children, strabismus seems to be the result of long-sightedness.
To focus properly at a distance, the lens in a long-sighted eye has to become
as globular as the lens of a normal eye becomes when it focuses on a near
object. To round up the lens for close work means contracting the ciliary
muscle inside the eye, which is called accommodation. When a normal
person accommodates to focus on something close, the eyes automatically
also turn in, or converge. The figure on this page shows the two processes.
The circuits in the brainstem for accommodation and convergence are
probably related and may overlap; in any case, it is hard to do one without
doing the other. When a long-sighted person accommodates, as he must to
focus even on a distant object, one or both eyes may turn in, even though the
convergence in this case is counterproductive. If a long-sighted child is not
fitted with glasses, turning in an eye may become habitual and eventually
permanent.
When we look at a near object two things happen: the lens rounds up because ciliary muscles contract,
and the eyes turn in.
This explanation for strabismus must surely be valid for some cases,
but not for all, since strabismus is not necessarily accompanied by long-
sightedness and since in some people with strabismus, one or other eye turns
out rather than in.
Strabismus can be treated surgically by detaching and reattaching the
extraocular muscles. The operation is usually successful in straightening the
eyes, but until the last decade or so it was not generally done until a child had
reached the age of four to ten, for the same reason that cataract removal was
delayed—the slight increase in risk. Strabismus that arises in an adult, say
from an injury to a nerve or eye muscle, is of course accompanied by double
vision. To see what that is like, you need only press (gently) on one eye from
below and one side. Double vision can be most annoying and incapacitating,
and if no better solution is available, a patch may have to be put over one eye,
as in the Hathaway shirt man. The double vision otherwise persists as long as
the strabismus is uncorrected. In a child with strabismus, however, the double
vision rarely persists; instead, either alternation or suppression of vision in
one eye occurs. When a child alternates, he fixes (directs his gaze) first with
one eye, while the nonfixating eye turns in or out, and then fixes with the
other while the first eye is diverted. (Alternating strabismus is very common,
and once you know about the condition, you can easily recognize it.) The
eyes take turns fixating, perhaps every second or so, and while one eye is
looking, the other seems not to see. At any instant, with one eye straight and
the other deviating, vision in the deviated eye is said to be suppressed.
Suppression is familiar to anyone who has trained himself to look through a
monocular microscope, sight a gun, or do any other strictly one-eye task, with
the other eye open. The scene simply disappears for the suppressed eye. A
child who alternates is always suppressing one or other eye, but if we test
vision separately in each eye, we generally find both eyes to be normal. Some
children with strabismus do not alternate but use one eye all the time,
suppressing the other eye. When one eye is habitually suppressed, vision
tends to deteriorate in the suppressed eye. Acuity falls, especially in or near
the central, or foveal part of the visual field, and if the situation continues, the
eye may become for practical purposes blind. This kind of blindness is what
the ophthalmologists call amblyopia ex anopsia. It is by far the commonest
kind of amblyopia, indeed of blindness in general. It was natural for us to
think of trying to induce strabismus, and hence amblyopia, in a kitten or
monkey by surgically cutting an eye muscle at birth, since we could then look
at the physiology and see what part of the path had failed. We did this in half
a dozen kittens and were discouraged to find that the kittens, like many
children, developed alternating strabismus; they looked first with one eye and
then the other. By testing each eye separately, we soon verified that they had
normal vision in both eyes. Evidently we had failed to induce an amblyopia,
and we debated what to do next. We decided to record from one of the
kittens, even though we had no idea what we could possibly learn. (Research
often consists of groping.) The results were completely unexpected. As we
recorded from cell after cell, we soon realized that something strange had
happened to the brain: each cell responded completely normally, but only
through one eye. As the electrode advanced through the cortex, cell after cell
would respond from the left eye, then suddenly the sequence would be
broken and the other eye would take over. Unlike what we had seen after eye
closure, neither eye seemed to have suffered relative to the other eye in terms
of its overall hegemony.
After we cut one eye muscle in a kitten at birth and then recorded after three months, the great majority
of cells were monocular, falling into groups 1 and 7.
Below: We obtained these sections from a macaque monkey that had an eye sutured closed from birth
for eighteen months. The left (open) eye was then injected with radioactive amino acid, and after a
week the brain was sectioned parallel to the surface of the visual cortex. (The cortex is dome shaped, so
that cuts parallel to the surface are initially tangential, but then produce rings like onion rings, of
progressively larger diameter. In the picture on the right, these have been cut from photographs and
pasted together. We have since learned to flatten the cortex before freezing it, avoiding the cutting and
pasting of serial sections.) In an ordinary photograph of a microscopic section the silver grains are
black on a white background. Here we used dark-field microscopy, in which the silver grains scatter
light and show as bright regions. The bright stripes, representing label in layer 4C from the open,
injected eye, are widened, the dark ones (closed eye), are greatly narrowed.
Here, in a different monkey, the closed eye was injected. The section is transverse rather than
tangential. The stripes in layer 4C, seen end on and appearing bright in this dark-field picture, are much
shrunken.
This competition model explains the segre- gation of fourth-layer fibers into eye- dominance columns.
At birth the columns have already begun to form. Normally at any given point if one eye dominates
even slightly, it ends up with a complete monopoly. If an eye is closed at birth, the fibers from the open
eye still surviving at any given point in layer 4 take over completely. The only regions with persisting
fibers from the closed eye are those where that eye had no competition when it was closed.
The various parts of the two retinas project onto their own areas of the right lateral geniculate body of
the cat (seen in cross section). The upper geniculate layer, which receives input from the opposite (left)
eye, overhangs the next layer. The overhanging part receives its input from the temporal crescent, the
part of the contralateral nasal retina subserving the outer (temporal) part of the visual field, which has
no counterpart in the other eye. (The temporal part of the visual field extends out farther because the
nasal retina extends in farther). In monocular closure (here, for example, a closure of the left eye), the
overhanging part doesn't atrophy, presumably because it has no competition from the right eye.
The discovery that at birth layer 4 is occupied without interruption
along its extent by fibers from both eyes was welcome because it explained
how competition could occur at a synaptic level in a structure that had
seemed to lack any opportunity for eye interaction. Yet the matter may not be
quite as simple as this. If the reason for the changes in layer 4 was simply the
opportunity for competition in the weeks after birth afforded by the mixture
of eye inputs in that layer, then closing an eye at an age when the system is
still plastic but the columns have separated should fail to produce the
changes. We closed an eye at five-and-a-half weeks and injected the other
eye after over a year of closure. The result was unequivocal shrinkage-
expansion. This would seem to indicate that in addition to differential
retraction of terminals, the result can be produced by sprouting of axon
terminals into new territory.
To throw a strike, a pitcher must project the ball over a plate about 1 foot wide, 60.5 feet away—a
target that subtends an angle of about 1 degree, about twice the apparent size of the moon. To
accomplish such a feat, with velocity and spin, requires excellent vision plus the ability to regulate the
force and timing of over a hundred muscles. A batter to connect with the ball must judge its exact
position less than a second after its release. The success or failure of either feat depends on visual
circuits—all those discussed in this book and many at higher visual levels—and motor circuits
involving motor cortex, cerebellum, brainstem, and spinal cord.
This surprising tendency for attributes such as form, color, and
movement to be handled by separate structures in the brain immediately
raises the question of how all the information is finally assembled, say for
perceiving a bouncing red ball. It obviously must be assembled somewhere, if
only at the motor nerves that subserve the action of catching. Where it's
assembled, and how, we have no idea. This is where we are, in 1995, in the
step-by-step analysis of the visual path. In terms of numbers of synapses
(perhaps eight or ten) and complexity of transformations, it may seem a long
way from the rods and cones in the retina to areas MT or visual area 2 in the
cortex, but it is surely a far longer way from such processes as orientation
tuning, end-stopping, disparity tuning, or color opponency to the recognition
of any of the shapes that we perceive in our everyday life. We are far from
understanding the perception of objects, even such comparatively simple
ones as a circle, a triangle, or the letter A—indeed, we are far from even
being able to come up with plausible hypotheses.
We should not be particularly surprised or disconcerted over our
relative ignorance in the face of such mysteries. Those who work in the field
of artificial intelligence (AI) cannot design a machine that begins to rival the
brain at carrying out such special tasks as processing the written word,
driving a car along a road, or distinguishing faces. They have, however,
shown that the theoretical difficulties in accomplishing any of these tasks are
formidable. It is not that the difficulties cannot be solved—the brain clearly
has solved them— but rather that the methods the brain applies cannot be
simple: in the lingo of AI, the problems are "nontrivial". So the brain solves
nontrivial problems. The remarkable thing is that it solves not just two or
three but thousands of them.
In the question period following a lecture, a sensory physiologist or
psychologist soon gets used to being asked what the best guess is as to how
objects are recognized. Do cells continue to become more and more
specialized at more and more central levels, so that at some stage we can
expect to find cells so specialized that they respond to one single person's
face—say, one's grand-mother's? This notion, called the grandmother cell
theory, is hard to entertain seriously. Would we expect to find separate cells
for grandmother smiling, grandmother weeping, or grandmother sewing?
Separate cells for the concept or definition of grandmother: one's mother's or
father's mother? And if we did have grandmother cells, then what? Where
would they project? The alternative is to suppose that a given object leads to
the firing of a particular constellation of cells, any member of which could
also belong to other constellations. Knowing as we do that destroying a small
region of brain does not generally destroy specific memories, we have to
suppose that the cells in a constellation are not localized to a single cortical
area, but extend over many areas. Grandmother sewing then becomes a
bigger constellation comprising grandmother-by-definition, grandmother's
face, and sewing. It is admittedly not easy to think of a way to get at such
ideas experimentally. To record from one cell alone and make sense of the
results even in the striate cortex is not easy: it is hard even to imagine coming
to terms with a cell that may be a member of a hundred constellations, each
consisting of a thousand cells. Having tried to record from three cells
simultaneously and understand what they all are doing in the animal's daily
life, I can only admire the efforts of those who hope to build electrode arrays
to record simultaneously from hundreds. But by now we should be used to
seeing problems solved that only yesterday seemed insuperable.
Running counter to wooly ideas about constellations of cells is long-
standing and still accumulating evidence for the existence of cortical regions
specialized for face perception. Charles Gross's group at'Princeton has
recorded from cells in the monkey, in a visual area of the temporal lobe, that
seem to respond selectively to faces. And humans with strokes in one
particular part of the inferior occipital lobe often lose the ability to recognize
faces, even those of close relatives. Antonio Damasio, at the University of
Iowa, has suggested that these patients have lost the ability to distinguish not
just faces but a broader class of objects that includes faces. He describes a
woman who could recognize neither faces nor individual cars. She could tell
a car from a truck, but to find her own car in a parking lot she had to walk
along reading off the license plate numbers, which suggests that her vision
and her ability to read numbers were both intact.
Speculating can be fun, but when can we hope to have answers to some
of these questions about perception? Some thirty-seven years have passed
since Kuffler worked out the properties of retinal ganglion cells. In the
interval the way we view both the complexity of the visual pathway and the
range of problems posed by perception has radically changed. We realize that
discoveries such as center-surround receptive fields and orientation
selectivity represent merely two steps in unraveling a puzzle that contains
hundreds of such steps. The brain has many tasks to perform, even in vision,
and millions of years of evolution have produced solutions of great ingenuity.
With hard work we may come to understand any small subset of these, but it
seems unlikely that we will be able to tackle them all. It would be just as
unrealistic to suppose that we could ever understand the intricate workings of
each of the millions of proteins floating around in our bodies.
Philosophically, however, it is important to have at least a few examples—of
neural circuits or proteins—that we do understand well: our ability to unravel
even a few of the processes responsible for life—or for perception, thought,
or emotions—tells us that total understanding is in principle possible, that we
do not need to appeal to mystical life forces—or to the mind.
Some may fear that such a materialistic outlook, which regards the
brain as a kind of super machine, will take the magic out of life and deprive
us of all spiritual values. This is about the same as fearing that a knowledge
of human anatomy will prevent us from admiring the human form. Art
students and medical students know that the opposite is true. The problem is
with the words: if machine implies something with rivets and ratchets and
gears, that does sound unromantic. But by machine I mean any object that
does tasks in a way that is consonant with the laws of physics, an object that
we can ultimately understand in the same way we understand a printing press.
I believe the brain is such an object.
Do we need to worry about possible dire consequences of
understanding the brain, analogous to the consequences of understanding the
atom? Do we have to worry about the CIA reading or controlling our
thoughts? I see no cause for loss of sleep, at least not for the next century or
so. It should be obvious from all the preceding chapters of this book that
reading or directing thoughts by neurophysiological means is about as
feasible as a weekend trip to the Andromeda galaxy and back. But even if
thought control turns out to be possible in principle, the prevention or cure of
millions of schizophrenics should be easy by comparison. I would prefer to
take the gamble, and continue to do research.
We may soon have to face a different kind of problem: that of
reconciling some of our most cherished and deep-seated beliefs with new
knowledge of the brain. In 1983, the Church of Rome formally indicated its
acceptance of the physics and cosmology Gallileo had promulgated 350 years
earlier. Today our courts, politicians, and publishers are struggling with the
same problem in teaching school children the, facts about evolution and
molecular biology. If mind and soul are to neurobiology what sky and heaven
are to astronomy and The Creation is to biology, then a third revolution in
thought may be in the offing. We should not, however, smugly regard these
as struggles between scientific wisdom and religious ignorance. If humans
tend to cherish certain beliefs, it is only reasonable to suppose that our brains
have evolved so as to favor that tendency—for reasons concerned with
survival. To dismantle old beliefs or myths and replace them with scientific
modes of thought should not and probably cannot be done hastily or by
decree. But it seems to me that we will, in the end, have to modify our beliefs
to make room for facts that our brains have enabled us to establish by
experiment and deduction: the world is round; it goes around the sun; living
things evolve; life can be explained in terms of fantastically complex
molecules; and thought may some day be explained in terms of fantastically
complex sets of neural connections.
The potential gains in understanding the brain include more than the
cure and prevention of neurologic and psychiatric diseases. They go well
beyond that, to fields like education. In educating, we are trying to influence
the brain: how could we fail to teach better, if we understood the thing we are
trying to influence? Possible gains extend even to art, music, athletics, and
social relationships. Everything we do depends on our brains.
Having said all this, I must admit that what most strongly motivates
me, and I think most of my colleagues, is sheer curiosity over the workings of
the most complicated structure known.
FURTHER READING
Chapter 1
Scientific American issue on the brain, 241(3), September 1979.
Reprinted as The Brain, A Scientific American Book, W. H. Freeman, New
York, 1979.
Nauta, W. J. H., and M. Feirtag: Fundamental Neuroanatomy, W. H.
Freeman, New York, 1986.
Ramon y Cajal, Santiago: Histologie du Systeme Nerveux de I'Homme
et des Vertebres, vols. 1 and 2 (translated by L. Azoulay from the Spanish),
Madrid, 1952.
Chapter 2
Kandel, E. R., J. H. Schwartz, and T. M. Jessell: Principles of Neural
Science, 3d ed., Elsevier North-Holland, New York, 1991.
Nicholls, J. G., A. R. Martin, and B. G. Wallace: From Neuron to
Brain, 3d ed., Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Mass., 1992.
Chapter 3
Dowling, J. E.: The Retina—An Approachable Part of the Brain,
Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass., 1987.
Kuffler, S. W.: Neurons in the retina: Organization, inhibition and
excitatory problems. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology
17: 281-292 (1952).
Nicholls, J. G., A. R. Martin, and B. G. Wallace: From Neuron to
Brain, 3d ed., Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Mass., 1992.
Schnapf, J. L., and D. A. Baylor: How photoreceptor cells respond to
light. Sri. Am. 256: 40-47 (1987).
Nicholls, J. G., A. R. Martin, and B. G. Wallace: From Neuron to
Brain, 3d ed., Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Mass., 1992.
Schnapf, J. L., and D. A. Baylor: How photoreceptor cells respond to
light. Sri. Am. 256: 40-47 (1987).
Chapter 4
Hubcl, D. H., and T. N. Wiesel: Receptive fields of single neurones in
the cat's striate cortex. J. Physiol. 148: 574-591 (1959).
—— and —— : Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional
architecture in the cat's visual cortex. J. Physiol. 160: 106-154 (1962).
—— and ——: Receptive fields and functional architecture in two non-
striate visual areas (18 and 19) of the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 28: 229-289
(1965).
—— and ——: Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey
striate cortex. J. Physiology. 195: 215-243 (1968).
—— and ——: Brain mechanisms of vision. Sci. Am. 241: 130-144
(1979).
Hubel, D. H.: Exploration of the primary visual cortex, 1955-78 (Nobel
Lecture). Nature 299: 515-524 (1982).
Chapters 5 and 6
Hubel, D. H., and T. N. Wiesel: Functional architecture of macaque
monkey visual cortex (Ferrier Lecture). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 198: 1-59
(1977).
Hubel, D. H.: Exploration of the primary visual cortex, 1955-78 (Nobel
Lecture). Nature 299: 515-524 (1982).
Chapter 7
Sperry, Roger: "Some effects of disconnecting the cerebral
hemispheres" (Nobel Lecture, 8 Dec. 1981), in Les Prix Nobel, Almqvist &
Wiksell International, Stockholm, 1982.
Gazzaniga, M. S., J. E. Bogen, and R. W. Sperry: Observations on
visual perception after disconnexion of the cerebral hemispheres in man.
Brain 88: 221-236 (1965).
Gazzaniga, M. S., and R. W. Sperry: Language after section of the
cerebral commissures. Brain 90: 131-148 (1967).
Lepore, F., M. Ptito, and H. H. Jasper: Two Hemispheres—One Brain:
Functions of the Corpus Callosum, Alan R. Liss, New York, 1984.
Julesz, Bela: Foundations of Cyclopean Perception, University of
Chicago Press, Chicago, 1971.
Poggio, G. P., and B. Fischer: Binocular interaction and depth
sensitivity of striate and prestriate cortical neurons of the behaving rhesus
monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 40: 1392— 1405 (1977).
Chapter 8
Daw, N. W.: The psychology and physiology of colour vision. Trends
in Neurosci. 1: 330-335 (1984).
Hering, Ewald:. Outlines of a Theory of the Light Sense (translated by
Leo M. Hurvich and Dorothea Jameson), Harvard University Press,
Cambridge, Mass., 1964.
Ingle, D.: The goldfish as a Retinex animal. Science 227: 651-654
(1985).
Land, E. H.: An alternative technique for the computation of the
designator in the Retinex theory of color vision. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
83: 3078-3080 (1986).
Livingstonc, M. S., and D. H. Hubel: Anatomy and physiology of a
color system in the primate visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 4: 309-356 (1984).
Nathans, J.: Genes for color vision. Sci. Am. 260: 42-49 (1989).
Schnapf, J. L., and D. A. Baylor: How photoreceptor cells respond to
light. Sd. Am. 256: 40-47 (1987).
Southall, J. P. C. (ed.): Helmholtz's Treatise on Physiological Optics
(translated from 3d German edition), 3 vols. bound as 2, Dover Publishers,
New York, 1962.
Chapter 9
Hubel, D. H.: Effects of deprivation on the visual cortex of cat and
monkey, Harvey Lectures, Series 72, Academic Press, New York, 1978, pp.
1-51.
Wicsel, T. N.: Postnatal development of the visual cortex and the
influence of environment (Nobel Lecture). Nature 299: 583-591 (1982),
Wiesel, T. N., and Hubel, D. H.: Effects of visual deprivation on
morphology and physiology of cells in the cat's lateral geniculate body. J.
Neurophysiol. 26: 978-993 (1963).
—— and ——: Receptive fields of cells in striate cortex of very young,
visually inexperienced kittens. J. Neurophysiol. 26: 994-1002 (1963).
—— and ——: Single-cell responses in striate cortex of kittens
deprived of vision in one eye. J. Neurophysiol. 26: 1003-1017 (1963).
—— and ——: Comparison of the effects of unilateral and bilateral eye
closure on cortical unit responses in kittens. J. Neurophysiol. 28: 1029-1040
(1965).
—— and ——: Binocular interaction in striate cortex of kittens reared
with artificial squint. J. Neurophysiol. 28: 1041-1059 (1965).
—— and ——: Extent of recovery from the effects of visual
deprivation in kittens. J. Neurophysiol. 28: 1060-1072 (1965).
Hubel, D. H., T. N. Wiesel, and S. LeVay: Plasticity of ocular
dominance columns in monkey striate cortex. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B.
278: 377-409 (1977).
Shatz, C. J.: The developing brain. Sci. Am. 267: 60-67 (1992).
Chapter 10
Crick, F. H. C.: Thinking about the brain. Sci. Am. 241: 219-233
(1979).
Hubel, D. H.: "Neurobiology: A science in need of a Copernicus," inj.
Neyman (ed.), The Heritage of Copernicus, Part II, M.I.T. Press, Cambridge,
Mass., pp. 243-260.
Van Essen, D. C., and J. H. R. Maunsell: Hierarchical organization and
functional streams in the visual cortex. Trends in Neurosci. 6: 370-375
(1983).
SOURCE OF ILLUSTRATION
Illustrations by Carol Dormer, Tom Cardamone Associates, Frontispiece, and Joseph Gagliardi.
Page 6 (left, middle), page 7 Drawings by Santiago Ramon y Cajal. From Histologie du Systeme
Nerveux de I'Homme et des Vertebres, Madrid, 1952.
Page 35 © Lennart Nilsson. From his book Behold Man, Little, Brown, and Company, Boston.
Page 48 S Polyak. From The Retina, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1941.
Page 74 Adapted from David H. Hubel and Torstcn N. Wicsel, j. Physiol. 160: 106-154 (1962), Fig.
19.
Page 75 (top) James P. Kelly, Columbia University Pages 75 (bottom), 76, and 77 Adapted from David
H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, J. Physiol. 160: 106-154 (1962), Figs. 7, 20, and 8.
Page 80 From A. L. Yarbus, Eye Movements and Vision, Plenum, New York, 1967.
Page 89 (left) From David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, J. Physiol. 160: 106-154 (1962), Fig. 17.
Page 89 (right) From David H. Hubel, Harvey Lectures, series 72, Academic Press, New York, 1978,
Fig. 6.
Page 90 From David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, J. Physiol. 160: 106-154 (1962), Fig. 10
Page 92 From David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, Ferrier Lecture, Proc. R. Soc. 198: 1-59. 231
Page 94 Montreal Neurological Institute
Pages 95, 96, and 97 From David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, Ferrier Lecture, Proc. R. Soc. 198:
1-59, Figs. 6a, 6b, and 10.
Pages 109, 110, and 111 From David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, Ferrier Lecture, Proc. R. 198: 1-
59, Figs. 21, 22, and 23.
Page 112 (bottom) From Simon LeVay, David H. Hubel, and Torsten N. Wiesel, j. Comp. Neural. 159:
559-576.
Page 113 Adapted from C. Kennedy ct al., Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 73:
'4230-4234 (1976), Fig. 2.
Page 114 From Roger Tootcll ct al., Deoxyglucose Analysis of Retinotopic Organization in Primate
Streate Cortex, Science 218: 902-904 (1982), Fig. 1.
Pages 116 and 117 From David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, J. Comp. Neural. 158: 267-294, Fig.
1.
Page 118 From David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, J. Physiol. 195: 215-243 (1968), Fig. 9.
Page 119 (top) From David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, j. Comp. Neurol. 158: 267-294 (1974),
Fig. 8c.
Page 121 (top and bottom) From David H. Hubel, Torsten N. Wiesel, and M. P. Stryker, j. Comp.
Neural. 177: 361-379 (1978), Fig. 7c.
Page 124 From David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, Ferrier Lecture, Proc. R. Soc. 198: 1-59
(1977), Fig. 28.
From David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, Uniformity of Monkey Striate Cortex: A Parallel
Relationship between Field Size, Scatter, and Magnification Factor, J. Camp. Neurol. 158:295-306
(1974), Figs. 1 and 2.
Page 129 From David H. Hubel, Nobel Lecture, Nature 299: 515-524 (1982), Fig. 14.
Page 133 From David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, Ferrier Lecture, Proc. R. Soc. 198: 1-59
(1977), Fig. 14.
Page 134 Adapted from P. M. Daniel and David Whitteridgc, J. Physiol. 159:203-221 (1961), Fig. 6.
Page 136 David H. Hubel Pages 138 and 139 Art by Carol Donner
Page 142 From David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, J. Neurophysiol. 30: 1561-1573 (1967), Fig. 4.
Page 148 (left) ET Archive Ltd. Page 148 (right) Sir Charles Wheatstone, Contribution to the
Physiology of Vision, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. (1838).
Page 150 From Bela Julesz, Foundations of Cyclopean Perception, University of Chicago Press,
Chicago and London, 1971, p. 319.
Pages 152 and 153 From Margaret S. Livingstone and David H. Hubel, j. Neurosci., in press.
Page 177 Photo from Edwin Land. From D. J. Ingle, The Goldfish as a Retinex Animal, Science 227:
651-653 (1985).
Page 179 From Gunnar Svaetichin and Edward F. MacNichol, Jr., Annals of the Neiv York Academy
a/Sciences, 74: 385-404 (1958).
Pages 184 and 185 From Margaret S. Livingstone and David H. Hubel, Anatomy and Physiology of a
Color System in the Primate Visual Cortex, J. Neurosci. 4: 309-356 (1984), Figs. 32 and 31.
Page 194 (top left) From Torsten N. Wiesel and David H. Hubel, J. of Neurophysiol. 26:1003-1017
(1963), Fig. 3.
Page 194 (top right) From David H. Hubel and Torsten N. Wiesel, J. of Physiol. 160: 106-154 (1962),
Fig.' 12.
Page 194 (bottom left) From David H. Hubel, Torsten N. Wiesel, and Simon LcVay, Phil. Trans. 278:
377-409 (1977), Fig. 1.
Page 194 (bottom right) From David H. Hubel, Harvey Lec tures, series 73, Academic Press, 1978,
Fig. 6.
Page 196 From Torstcn N. Wicsel and David H. Hubel,J. ofNeurophysiol. 26: 978-993 (1963), Fig. 1.
Page 199 (left) From Torstcn N. Wicsel, Nobel Lecture, Nature 299: 583-591 (1982), Fig.2.
Page 199 (middle) From David H. Hubcl, Harvey Lecture, series 72, Academic Press, 1978, Fig.18.
Page 200 (left and right) From David H. Hubel, Harvey Lecture, series 72, Academic Press, 1978,
Figs. 19 and 20.
Page 202 (top) June E. Armstrong/New England Regional Primate Research Center
Page 202 (bottom) From Torsten N. Wie^el and David H. Hubcl,7. Comp. Neurol. 158: 307-318
(1974), Fig. 2.
Page 206 From David H. Hubcl and Torsten N. Wiesel.J. Neurophysiol. 28: 1041-1059 (1965), Fig. 5.
Pages 209, 210, 211, and 212 From David H. Hubel, Torstcn N. Wiesel, and Simon LeVay, Phil.
Trans. R. Soc. 278: 377-409 (1977), Figs. 15, 22, 25, and 28.