Keyreference
Keyreference
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/
info/about/policies/terms.jsp
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content
in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship.
For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Financial Education Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of Financial
Education.
http://www.jstor.org
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Personal Financial Literacy: Perceptions of
Knowledge, Actual Knowledge and
Behavior of College Students
INTRODUCTION
Financial literacy has been studied and discussed in both the academic world
and the popular press. Indeed, the lack of financial literacy has been linked to the
subprime mortgage problems in the United States (Gerardi, Goette and Meier,
2010), retirement planning (Lusardi and Mitchell, 2007), lack of estate
inadequate
planning (Volpe, Chen and Liu, 2006), student debt and subsequent bankruptcy
(Mandell, 1999), and basic household financial mismanagement (Hilgert, Hogarth
and Beverly, 2003). Education is seen as the means to change the levels of low
financial literacy, although the effectiveness of the education has not been very
strong to date (Lyons, Palmer, Jayaratne and Scherpf, 2006).
While personal financial knowledge and personal financial education are
discussed in some K-12 programs (Bernheim, Garrett, and Maki, 2001; Mandell,
1999) and in some adult education venues, such as those found in the workplace
(Lusardi and Mitchell, 2007), the focus of this research is on the financial education
of undergraduate college students. Chen and Volpe (1998) found low levels of
Fall/Winter 2013 1
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
personal financial literacy at the college level with lower scores for younger college
students, females, non-Caucasian, no or little work experience, and lower parental
income. They also demonstrated that college students had more knowledge about
things with which they were currently familiar, such as auto insurance, as opposed
to topics they might have to grapple with in the future such as retirement decisions.
One of the goals of this paper is to update that study.
This study also augments previous research by examining a broader range of
financial topics. More importantly, it explores the linkages between knowledge,
perception of knowledge, and where feasible, behaviors. Of primary interest is
gaining a better understanding of whether differences between actual knowledge,
perceived knowledge and behaviors varies between different student groups. If, for
example, certain students perceive that they possess greater financial acumen than
they actually possess, this would indicate the need to better inform such students
about why taking a personal finance course has relevance for them. Further, there
are implications with respect to the types of pedagogical tools that may be
needed to convince students that they have more to learn, and that this knowledge
will be important to them both today and in the future. A literature review will
cover the concepts of financial literacy (or financial knowledge), perceived
knowledge, and actual behavior and their respective connections around six main
financial topic areas. Variables that have been examined in the literature that have
been shown to affect any of these areas will also be reviewed. The methodology and
results of a survey that was fielded to students at one mid-sized traditional public
university will be outlined and the implications for that university and others will
be offered.
LITERATURE REVIEW
Financial Literacy—Knowledge
Financial literacy, according to the U.S. Department of Treasury (2006) is, "the
ability to make informed judgments and to take effective actions regarding the
current and future use and management of money." Chen and Volpe (1998)
demonstrated that college students in several universities have low levels of
financial literacy across topic categories including general knowledge, savings and
borrowing, insurance, and investments. While study participants generally were not
very literate about personal finance they did demonstrate higher levels of literacy in
the areas of "general knowledge" and "insurance" (particularly auto insurance) with
which they probably have experience. The study found variables that demonstrated
significant differences between group means indicating possible causal relationships
between these variables and personal financial literacy. These variables include:
(1) business or non-business major, (2) class rank (freshman through graduate
student), (3) gender, (4) race, (5) nationality, (6) years of work experience, (7) age,
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
and (8) income. There were statistically significant differences between groups
within almost all variables with respect to personal financial knowledge in each
category of financial literacy.
Fall/Winter 2013
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Actual Financial Decisions and Behaviors
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
extended in this study from Chen and Volpe's (1998) division of business versus
non-business college major to include a wide variety of college majors in the sample
so that patterns within discipline groups can be explored.
METHODOLOGY
Fall/Winter 2013 5
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
administered to a group of 51 students as a pre-test. Minor adjustments were made
before it was fielded to the larger group.
RESULTS
Sample
Of the 13,012 e-mails sent with a link to the online survey, 1,783 students
responded after two separate e-mailings. After removing 580 incomplete surveys,
a final sample of 1,203 students remained which yielded a 9.23% response rate that
meets the criteria set by Hair, Bush and Otinau (2000) for the population size,
number of questions and type of study. The responses to the first e-mail and the
second e-mail were compared and no statistically significant differences were found.
Table 1 contains selected demographics of the survey respondents. The sample
profile is similar to the university student population in most respects with a few
specific differences that make it less than perfectly representative of the population.
In particular, the female response rate was higher than the population of female
students compared with male students. This response rate is in contrast to prior
research findings (Orr, Sherony & Steinhaus, 2007) which found male response
rates to online surveys to skew from females in comparisons of 2000 and 2005
responses.
A group of four male undergraduate students undertook the fielding of the
online survey as part of a marketing class. They were responsible for a number of
items including developing the cover letter for the e-mail. The e-mail that was sent
to the campus community was e-signed by the students and contained their pictures.
Dommeyer (2008) studied the effect of physical attractiveness of survey requests
upon response rates. The research demonstrated a strong link between attractive
photos and survey responses. The perceived attractiveness of the four senior-level
male students may have influenced the high percentage of female responses.
Additional descriptive data was collected with respect to the percentage of
students working versus those not working, parental income levels, ethnicity, age,
college (major) affiliation and class standing (freshman, sophomore, junior or
senior). Overall, with the exception of gender and possibly the number of students
working, the sample is a reasonably reliable indicator of the population of this study.
Table 2 contains the results for each question topic in the "Overall" column. For
all respondents the results ranged from a low of 15% correct on questions about
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Table 1. Demographic Information
Population of Total
(8.5%)**
Education (6.39%) Education (7.1%)**
Science & Technology (13.62%) Science & Technology
(10.2%)**
Environmental Studies (5.05%) Environmental Studies
(2.4%)**
Fine & Performing Arts (2.86%) Fine & Performing Arts
(4.1%)**
Interdisciplinary (2.44%) Interdisciplinary (1.0%)**
Ethnic White/Caucasian (84.7%) White/Caucasian (76.5%)**
Background Black/African-American (1.0%) Black/African-American
(2.9%)**
Hispanic American (3.0%) Hispanic American (4.4%)**
Asian-American (4.1%) Asian-American (8.7%)**
American Indian/Alaska Native American Indian/Alaska
(1.0%) Native (2.5%)**
Other (6.24%) Other (5.2%)**
*National statistics from the U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics (2009), specific
university statistics were unreliable.
students at the university from which the
**Population data is for the undergraduate
sample was derived and is from the same time period as the survey data collection.
Fall/Winter 2013
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Table 2. Overall Gender Results and Topic -
by Category Knowledge
Gender**
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Table 3. Gender Comparison of Knowledge (K) and Perception (P)
[Values represent average overall score (with 5.0 being a perfect score
for a Knowledge category and 5.0 being the highest perception
of their own knowledge)]
All
Respondents Male Female
n=1203 n=364 II 00<N
ca II Tt"
oo1
(NI
Category K P K p
P K P
Understanding Debt 2.4 3.0 2.T
2.7a 3.3b 2.2a 2.8b
Insurance 2.9 2.6 3.2a 3.1" 2.8a 2.4b
2.4»
Fall/Winter 2013 9
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
• have an average of 0.83 credit cards,
• have "maxed out" about 0.21 of those at some point in their credit history, and
• tend to pay more than the minimum amount due, but less than the entire credit
card bill each month.
Gender
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
cards for females,
• are more inclined than females to order a copy of their credit report, and
• are more aware of what coverage is included with their car insurance than
females, male respondents:
• are less likely than females to record a debit or ATM transaction, and
• plan to begin financial planning for retirement later than females after
graduation.
Age
The average age of the respondents was 20.93 years. Distribution by age group
was fairly even as shown in Table 1. Table 4 demonstrates the differences between
knowledge and perceptions with respect to age.
Not surprisingly, the older the respondent, the better the performance. On
average, 18-year-olds correctly answered 54% of the questions while respondents
aged 22 or older correctly answered 68% of the questions. This result held across
all six Knowledge groups and is most pronounced for the Understanding Debt
group, followed by Taxes.
Respondents over age 22 perceived themselves as more knowledgeable in 13 of
the 16 questions, which is justified by the data. The main exception is in the area
of Investing where older respondents did not rate their perceptions higher, although
those over 22 scored higher in Knowledge than 18- and 19-year-olds. Thus, while
older respondents did not perceive their comfort level with this topic to be higher
than younger respondents, older respondents did know more than certain other age
groups. Older respondents are more likely to perceive that there is more that they
need to know.
With respect to financial behavior, younger respondents:
respondents were more likely to pay something less than the full amount
owed, but more than the minimum payment,
• are far less likely (3%) to have transferred a balance from one credit card to
another than the average respondent over 22 (33%),
• are far less likely to have ordered copies of their credit report within the past
Fall/Winter 2013 U
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Table 4. Age Comparison of Knowledge (K) and Perception (P)
19 20 21 22 Over 22
(n=230) (n=239) (n=236) (n=142) (n=178)
Understanding
Debt K P K P K P K P K P
(n=178) 18 -.11 -.01 -.36* -.06 -.47* -.04 -.65* -.23 -.96* -.53*
19 -.25 -.05 -.36* -.03 -.54* -.22 -.84* -.52*
20 -.11 .02 -.29 -.17 -.59* -.47*
21 -.18 -.19 -.48* -.49*
22 -.30 -.30*
Insurance K p K P K P K P K P
(n=178) 18 -.21 -.01 -.28 .04 -.39* .01 -.52* -.21 -.70* -.51*
19 -.07 .05 -.17 .02 -.31 -.20 -.48* -.50*
20 -.11 -.03 -.24 -.26 -.42* -.55*
21 -.13 -.23 -.31* -.52*
22 -.17 -.29
Investing K p K P K P K P K P
(n=178) 18 -.19 -.05 -.36 -.02 -.47* -.10 -.72* -.20 -.71* -.28
19 .03 -.27 -.05 -.53* -.16 -.52* -.23
20 -.11 -.08 -.37 -.18 -.35 -.26
21 -.26 -.11 -.24 -.18
22 .01 -.07
Money
Management K P K P K P K P K P
(n=178) 18 -.11 -.13 -.27* -.21 -.24 -.23 -.42* .37* -.53* -.46*
19 -.16 -.08 -.12 -.10 -.31* -.24 -.42* -.33*
20 .03 -.01 -.15 -.16 -.26 -.24*
21 -.18 -.14 -.29* -.23
22 -.11 -.09
Retirement K P K P K P K P K P
(n=178) 18 -.02 -.01 -.17 -.05 -.11 -.13 -.38* -.27 -.51* -.34*
19 -.15 -.04 -.09 -.11 -.36* -.26 -.49* -.33*
20 .06 -.08 -.21 -.22 -.34* -.29
21 -.27 -.15 -.40* -.21
22 -.13 -.07
Taxes K P K P K P K P K P
(n=178) 18 -.10 -.03 -.21 -.11 -.37* -.07 -.58* -.26 -.83* -.76*
19 -.11 -.08 -.27 -.04 -.48* -.23 -.72* -.72*
20 -.15 .04 -.37* -.15 -.61* -.64*
21 -.22 -.19 -.46* -.68*
22 -.24 -.49*
*
statistically significant difference between age group at the 5% level or better
a
For example, when comparing the Understanding Debt category,
18-year-olds
scored lower, on average, than those Over 22 by -0.96.
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Table 5. Employment Status Comparison of Knowledge (K) and Perception (P)
[Values represent average overall score (with 5.0 being a perfect score
for a Knowledge category and 5.0 being the highest perception
of their own knowledge)]
Employment Status
Fall/Winter 2013 13
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Table 6. Ethnic Group Comparison of Knowledge (K) and Perception (P)
(Values represent average overall score (with 5.0 being a perfect score for a
Knowledge category and 5.0 being the highest perception of their own knowledge)]
Caucasian Non-Caucasian
(n=1004) (n= =181)
Category K P K P
Understanding Debt 2.4"
2.4a 3.0
3.0 2.1"
2.1a 3.0
3.0
Insurance 3.0"
3.0a 2.6 2.6a 2.6
O"
Investing 3.3"
3.3a u>
3.0b
o 2.9a 2.8b
Money Management 4.3"
4.3a 3.6 4.0a 3.4
Retirement 2.4"
2.4a 2.5
2.5 2.2"
2.2a 2.5
Taxes 3.0 2.7 2.8 2.7
a
statistically significant difference between ethnic groups' Knowledge at the 5%
level or better
b
statistically significant difference between ethnic groups' Perception at the 5%
level or better
cards. Employed respondents have one credit card on average, compared to 0.66
credit cards held by unemployed respondents.
Ethnicity
Knowledge, perceptions and behaviors were evaluated across ethnic groups (see
Table 6). Because the sample (and population) was predominately Caucasian (85%)
and the remaining ethnic groups each had only a small number of respondents the
sample was divided into two groups: Caucasian and Non-Caucasian (Table 1 gives
a full breakdown of ethnicities).
Caucasians statistically outperformed Non-Caucasians on every Knowledge
group except Taxes. However, while Caucasians outperformed Non-Caucasians in
nearly every Knowledge group, there were no statistically significant differences in
Perception of knowledge between the two groups except in the area of Investing,
where Caucasians had a higher perception of their own knowledge. The lack of a
statistically significant difference with respect to the Perception of Knowledge is
alarming when compared with the finding of a significant difference as to actual
knowledge between Caucasians and non-Caucasians.
Caucasian respondents were generally more prudent than non-Caucasians with
respect to their financial behaviors, although the results are mixed. For example:
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
non-Caucasians,
• Caucasians have fewer credit cards (0.80) compared to non-Caucasians (0.95),
on average,
• more Caucasians (63%) than non-Caucasians (51%) pay the full amount on
their credit card each month, and
• non-Caucasians tend to transfer balances more often across credit cards (19%
vs. 10% for Caucasians);
however,
• non-Caucasians are more likely to record ATM transactions (46% vs. 40% for
Caucasians), and
• more non-Caucasians (23%) have ordered a copy of their credit report in the
past year than Caucasians (16%).
Respondents were asked where they learned about personal finance. The
choices included non-academic sources, such as family, talking with friends, media,
and managing one's own funds; as well as academic sources, such as a bank or
credit union class, high school personal finance or economics class, college personal
finance or economics class, other courses, or other sources. The respondents could
choose all that applied. The respondents were then coded as to whether they
obtained their knowledge from academic, non-academic or "both"
(See sources
Table 7).
Not surprisingly, respondents who learned about personal finance from both
Academic and Non-Academic sources (category Both) scored significantly higher
in financial knowledge than those learning from purely Non-Academic sources on
Understanding Debt, Insurance and Investing. There were no statistically significant
differences in Knowledge results for Academic vs. Non-Academic groups or the
Academic vs. Both groups.
Respondents in the Both category have a statistically higher perception of their
comfort level in all six areas compared to the Non-Academic group. This higher
perceived comfort level appears to be justified for the Understanding Debt,
Insurance and Investing areas, but not for Money Management, Retirement or
Taxes. Those in the Both group have statistically higher perceptions of knowledge
of Taxes than the Academic group. No statistically significant difference exists
between any of the six Perception areas for the Academic vs. Non-Academic groups.
who learned about personal finance from both academic and non
Respondents
academic sources:
• have 0.84 credit cards on average compared to 0.82 for the Non-Academic
Fall/Winter 2013 IS
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Table 7. Source of Literacy Comparison of Knowledge (K) and Perception
Non-academic Both
Category (n=533) (n=629)
Understanding Debt K P K P
Investing K p K P
Money Management K P K P
• tend to use a budget to manage their spending (50%) as compared to the Non
Academic (44%) and Academic (37%) groups,
• were slightly more likely to have a savings account (92%) compared to Non
Academic and Academic groups (each at 89%), and
• were more likely to pay the full balance on their credit card each month (66%
However, only 43% of the Academic group regularly balanced their checking
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
account compared to 50% of the Non-Academic and 48% of the Both groups.
Other Variables
IMPLICATIONS
This reearch supports several crucial previous findings: (1) financial literacy is
at a very low level among college students, (2) females, non-Caucasians, and non
business majors underperform relative to males, Caucasians and Business majors,
respectively, and (3) younger students know less than older students. Additionally,
the findings imply that the closer the "need to know" is perceived, the more the
students actually realize the relevance of the knowledge acquisition and the greater
the actual knowledge Employment did not seem to have much effect on
becomes.
knowledge, perceptions or behaviors, unlike some earlier studies. Behaviors seem
to trend with knowledge levels; however, there are notable exceptions.
The finding that women continue to know significantly less than men runs
counter to all the advances women have achieved in terms of employment
in the last few decades. Either through continued socialization that
opportunities
tends to channel women away from the mathematical and scientific fields, or for
Fall/Winter 2013 17
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
other reasons, women either do not realize how much they do not know or, as this
study demonstrates, they realize how much they do not know and either haven't
received the training, or haven't learned from that training to the degree that males
have learned. It is likely that the same training has been offered to women as to men
in the school setting. Therefore, the lack of knowledge could be coming from the
home or from peers. Since personal financial knowledge is critical for both men and
women in today's world and in the future, educational offerings at both the college
level and pre-college level is imperative.
The need for pre-college personal finance education implies the probable need
for training K-12 educators on what and how to teach personal finance topics. Little
research has been done in the area of teacher preparation in the area of financial
education. The No Child Left Behind Act requires that for teachers to be considered
"highly qualified" to teach a topic they must have earned a subject-specific degree
or have taken 24 credit-hours of college level coursework in the area, or they must
pass a rigorous state competency exam. A national survey of K-12 educators (Way
and Holden, 2009) indicates that by this standard, there are few K-12 educators
nationwide who are "highly qualified" to teach personal finance. Introductory
micro- and macro-economics courses, which have historically devoted minimal class
time to personal financial education, are the most commonly cited sources of
collegiate level financial education for the 37% of teachers who reported having any
academic background in this area. Given that the study found that educators who
had taken college-level courses in personal finance were more likely to identify
themselves as being competent to teach personal finance, it is not surprising that the
majority of educators reported that they did not feel competent to teach personal
finance. Believing that one has adequate mastery of the content matter is a
necessary, but not a sufficient condition for educators to integrate a subject area into
their curriculum. They also must have knowledge of and be comfortable with the
pedagogy, and here pre-service teacher preparation courses appear to fall even
shorter of the mark. Fewer than 3% of teachers indicated receiving any collegiate
level preparation in personal financial education methods.
The absence of such training at the pre-service level could potentially be
compensated for by post-collegiate training in the form of non-credit workshops.
Unfortunately, fewer than 20% of educators report having attended a workshop on
a topic related to personal finance in the last three years (Way and Holden, 2009).
The most frequently attended workshops were those presented by financial planners
or financial institutions. Such workshops are often focused on topics such as
retirement planning or budgeting as they relate to the personal needs of educators.
Only 12% attended a workshop that focused on teaching personal finance at the K
12 level. This highlights the need for non-credit workshops that integrate both
content and methods to meet the needs of practicing teachers.
The findings with respect to knowledge, perceptions and actions of education
majors, particularly as compared to other discipline majors, indicates that more
training of education majors is needed. Having education majors instructed in
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
personal finance would be a minimum requirement under this scenario, and training
on how to teach different aspects of financial literacy, depending on pre-college
teaching field, would be optimal.
The findings with respect to Knowledge have implications for curriculum
content. Since Money Management is basically understood, topics such as basic
budgeting and balancing a checkbook probably need not be covered at the college
level. However, in order to fund retirement spending (which is poorly understood),
saving must occur, particularly if one follows the longer term economic models of
Ando and Modigliani (1963). Since saving should be a component of budgeting,
money management issues can form a basis for later lessons on interest and
retirement where knowledge is deficient. Teaching students about the time value of
money would help them understand the opportunity cost of current consumption and
the importance of beginning to save for retirement while young. Tools such as
calculating the value of not buying a daily latte (Shaw, 2006) can help students
realize the future value of managing their money.
Improved understanding of money management can also make the topic of
Retirement more relevant to college students. Retirement knowledge has become
increasingly important as employers transfer more responsibilities to employees in
this area. Students will commonly be asked to make retirement and investment
decisions or days of beginning their first jobs. Care should also be
within weeks
taken to develop gender- and ethnic-specific examples, particularly those geared to
females and non-Caucasians to increase the incentive for these groups to enhance
their knowledge in this area.
The results indicate that learning about personal finance in non-academic
settings is not the best option. Coupling academic learning with non-academic
resources appears to improve one's knowledge in key areas. Thus, the inclusion of
a personal financial course at the college level should be of significant value to most
students. Respondents to this survey indicated their strong interest in taking a
personal financial literacy course, especially if it satisfies a university requirement
(73% stating they would be Very Likely or Likely to take a course under this
scenario).
The college students whose parents have lower levels of earnings and who also
have lower levels of education need more help in many areas than some other
students. Identification of these students and course advising could help them find
their way.
Fall/Winter 2013 19
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
knowledge. This finding indicates that older students may not perceive they know
as much as they seem to, or, at the very least, they are uncomfortable with their
knowledge set. Conversely, younger students do not perceive they know less than
they think they do. It may be difficult to encourage people to take a course on
personal finance if they perceive they know more than they actually do. This could
be partially remedied by the K-12 system. If the need for an understanding of
personal finance is broached in the lower grade levels and more in-depth courses are
offered at the college level, the university student is more likely to recognize the
relevance of the material. Overcoming perceptions in order to help students decide
to seek out personal financial education indicates the need for strategic plans to be
implemented that would make taking a personal finance course required or
perceived as beneficial in some way to the student. At the university used in this
research, the Personal Finance course was made an option in the General University
Requirement set of courses. The course has filled to capacity every time since that
change was made. An information campaign including articles in the student
newspaper also helped to catch the attention and interest of students. However,
more "marketing" of the course to the lower-performing groups, such as women, is
needed. Finding ways to help students understand the future benefits that will
accrue to them personally from additional knowledge and the impact upon their
future that their current lack of knowledge can have is needed.
One of the more disturbing findings was that only 15% of the respondents knew
the factors determining credit-worthiness although their actions clearly show that
they are borrowing money. For 18- to 22-year-olds to have a low understanding
regarding retirement issues is perhaps to be expected as it is doubtful few, if any,
have yet begun to save for retirement. However, the majority of the respondents are
engaged in borrowing and they are equally illiterate on this subject as they are on
retirement. Respondents tend to perceive that they understand debt, but they do not
exhibit a great deal of knowledge or prudent behaviors concerning debt-related
issues.
Older respondents exhibited interesting characteristics in terms of their
behavior. Compared to younger respondents, they had more credit cards, fewer paid
their credit card bill in full monthly (implying a buildup of a "permanent" credit card
debt level), they were less likely to have a savings account, and less likely to record
ATM/debit card transactions. The reason for this is uncertain. It appears as though
the younger students started off with good intentions, and then went astray as they
aged, incurred student loans, and/or mismanaged their money. Peer pressure or
other pressure for acquisition of material objects, easy credit card acquisition or bad
habits learned from other students could be at fault. If they are better educated by
the time they are age 18 or 19 other pressures may have less effect. However,
perception of personal knowledge about debt was high, and actual knowledge about
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
debt was greater with the older age groups, but behavior didn't match knowledge.
Hence, other pressures seem to be affecting debt-related behavior causing the
incidence of "maxed-out" credit cards, balance transfers, not checking their credit
reports and paying less than the total amount due. Consequently, it is imperative
that further research be undertaken to gain a better understanding of the pressures
affecting debt-related behavior. Lessons and case studies showing the impact of
high debt levels need to be part of the education offered.
Respondents, especially females, perceive the need to start saving for retirement
and many plan to do so within a decade or so after graduation. Given females'
longer life spans, their propensity to begin financial planning for retirement sooner
than males is prudent. If they graduate at age 22 and begin to save around age 30,
this seems reasonable even though the opportunity cost of delaying savings for eight
years is high. Along with the survey data, this demonstrates that overall, the
students' intentions (with respect to retirement planning) are good, but their
knowledge is inadequate. Further, the idea of retirement is not part of the student's
current world and therefore interest and motivation to learn about retirement
as a 401 (k) plan allocation
planning, or even rudimentary retirement decisions such
is not on
(which will most likely be facing them upon employment after college),
their agenda.
How do educators motivate students to learn things they don't think they need
to know yet? Communication to students about what they need to know as part of
and resources at a
getting their first job upon graduation could help. People
from placement
university, beyond the personal finance faculty, can help. Advising
offices on campus would be a good resource in many cases. Students' college years
offer a prime time to introduce the concept of time value of money and the benefits
of beginning to save for retirement when one is still young. Additional emphasis on
the fact that females tend to outlive males and will have to manage their own
financial assets at some point in their life needs to be communicated to the entire
to be broached in a wide spectrum
population of college students. These topic need
of college courses and professors across a campus should be recommending that
students take a course in personal finance. The finding that women know
finance should serve as a "wake-up call" for many
significantly less about personal
college women. Finding ways to train other college professors to incorporate the
social and economic impact of financial illiteracy would also be helpful.
This study would be enhanced by using multiple colleges and universities with
a variety of student bodies in order to see if the results hold under differing
to
conditions or if they vary by individual school or school type. This would help
a broader with more
make the results more meaningful to population. Samples
and racially diverse populations would also enhance understanding of
ethnically
what appears to be a problematic area.
Fall/Winter 2013 21
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
An essential component of a successful pedagogical or curriculum change is the
measurement of the knowledge acquired and potentially the application of this
knowledge. Lyons, Palmer, Jayaratne and Scherpf (2006) make a good case for
individual and national evaluations of the effectiveness of financial education
programs. Evaluating the effectiveness of financial literacy programs at the college
level is clearly needed. Pre- and post-educational experience testing in a personal
finance course, as well as college programs in general, would help to verify the
assumption is the primary answer to the lack of financial knowledge
that education
among college students as well as other populations.
A population besides college students is critical as well. The financial literacy
of high school students has been examined in surveys, the best known of which have
been conducted by JumpStart. Jump$tart has been conducting national surveys
every two years to measure the financial knowledge of high school seniors. The
average score on the baseline 1997-1998 survey was 57.3%. The average score in
the most recent survey (Mandell, 2008) was 48.3%. Effective remedies for the
generally low levels of knowledge need to be researched and determined. The
success rate of employer-sponsored financial literacy efforts would be helpful to
know. Also, it would be beneficial to know more about programs offered through
nonprofit groups and support of on-campus efforts that are not part of the
curriculum, possibly with finance or accounting societies participating. In general,
the alarming extent of the financial literacy problem is becoming better understood,
but the solutions have been under-researched and not adequately adopted.
ENDNOTES
1
Acknowledgement: The authors wish to thank the WWU graduates Spencer
Covich, Thomas Evans, Jordan Maughan and Simon Trigg without whom this
research would not have been possible.
REFERENCES
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
47-70.
Gerardi, K., L Goette and S. Meier. "Financial Literacy and Subprime Mortgage
Delinquency: Evidence from a Survey Matched to Administrative Data,"
Working Paper Series (Federal Reserve Bank of Atlanta), (April, 2010), 1-53.
Goldsmith, E. and R. E. Goldsmith. "Gender Differences in Perceived and Real
Knowledge of Financial Investments," Psychological Reports, 80 (February,
1997), 236-238.
Hair, J. F., Jr., R. P. Bush and D. J. Ortinau. Marketing Research: A Practical
Approach for the New Millennium, (Boston, 2000), Irwin McGraw-Hill.
Hilgert, M., J.M. Hogarth and S. Beverly. "Household Financial Management: The
Connection Between Knowledge and Behavior," Federal Reserve Bulletin,
(July, 2003), 309-322.
Lusardi, A. and O. S. Mitchell. "Financial Literacy and Retirement Preparedness:
Evidence and Implications for Financial Education," Business Economics, 42
(January, 2007), 35-43.
Lyons, A. C., L. Palmer, K. S. U. Jayaratne, and E. Scherpf. "Are We Making the
Grade? A National Overview of Financial Education and Program Evaluation,"
The Journal of Consumer Affairs, 40 (Winter, 2006), 208-235.
Mandell, L. "Our Vulnerable Youth: The Financial Literacy of American 12th
Graders - A Failure by Any Measurement," Credit Union Magazine, 65 (Jan
uary, 1999), 4a-6a.
. "The Financial Literacy of Young American Adults: The Results of the
2008 National JumpStart Coalition Survey of High School Seniors and College
Students," accessed at http://www.jumpstart.org/assets/files/ 2008Survey Book,
pdf (2010).
Orr, C., B. Sherony, and C. Steinhaus. "Drifting Response Rate Patterns Resulting
From Electronic Returns: A Research Note," International Journal of Business
Research, 7 (6, 2007), 116-120.
Palmer T. S., M. B. Pinto and D.H. Párente, 2001. "College Students' Credit Card
Debt and the Role of Parental Involvement: Implications for Public Policy,"
Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 20 (Spring, 2001), 105-113.
Radecki, C.M. and J. Jaccard. "Perceptions of Knowledge, Actual Knowledge, and
Information Search Behavior," Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31
(March, 1995), 107-138.
Rebeck, K. and W. Walstad. Financial Fitness for Life (New York, 2005), National
Council on Economic Education.
Rosacker, K. M., S. Ragothaman, M. Gillispie. "Financial Literacy of Freshmen
Business School Students," College Student Journal, 43 (June, 2009), 391-399.
16.
Shaw, S. "Save a Latte a Day to Retire Early," Money Marketing, (July, 2006),
Stipek, D. and D.M. Maclver. "Developmental Change in Children's Assessment
of Intellectual Competence," Child Development, 60 (June, 1989), 521-538.
Stuhldreher, W.L., T.J. Stuhldreher, K.Y-Z. Forrest, and J. Eicher. "The
Association of'BMOC'-Big Money on Campus and Health Behaviors Among
Fall/Winter 2013 23
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Students," Advances in Financial Education, (Fall, 2007), 77-94.
Supiano, B. "To Get This Grant, Students Have to Take 'Personal Finances 101',"
The Chronicle of Higher Education, 39 (December, 2009), A17.
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, "Economic News Release: College Enrollment and
Work Activity of 2009 High School Graduates" http://data.bls.gov/cgi
bin/print.pl/ news.release/hsgec.nrO.htm, accessed
(2010).
U. S. Department of Treasury, Financial Literacy Education Commission. The
National Strategy for Financial Literacy, (2006), accessed September 2010.
Volpe, R. P., H. Chen, and S. Liu. "An Analysis of the Importance of Personal
Finance Topics and the Level of Knowledge Possessed by Working Adults,"
Financial Services Review, 15 (Spring, 2006), 81-98.
Way, W. L., and K. C. Holden. "Teachers' Background and Capacity to Teach
Personal Finance: Results of a National Study," Journal of Financial Coun
seling and Planning, 20 (2009), 64-78.
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
APPENDIX A
Fall/Winter 2013 25
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Investments Q: Charlie opens a savings account and deposits $500.
If the savings account has a fixed annual interest rate of
5%, offers compound interest, and Charlie makes no
additional deposits or withdrawals, what amount will
he have in his savings account at the end of two
years?*
A: More than $550
Q: Beginning to save while you're young is
recommended by financial experts because it:*
A: Lets compound interest work in your favor by
earning interest on interest.
5/14/08
Beginning balance
$500
500
5/15/08
Century Auto Parts
$100
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
5/31/08
Paycheck
$200
501
6/2/08
Best Clothes
Fall/Winter 2013 27
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Taxes Q: Which of the following represents a progressive tax
system (i.e., those who earn more pay a higher
percentage of their income toward this tax)?
A: Federal income tax
Q: Of the following taxes which one is paid for partly
by the employer and partly by the employee?
A: Social Security tax
Q: Which of the following statements is true regarding
estate taxes?
A: If the value of the estate is less than a certain
amount, no federal estate taxes need to be paid.
Q: Which of the following is not generally an
allowable deduction for federal income taxes regardless
of the amount?
A: Neither interest paid on credit card debt nor interest
on a car loan are generally allowable deductions
Q: You are single with no dependents and you claim
yourself as an exemption for federal income tax
purposes. The standard deduction is $5,350. The
deductible interest on your mortgage totals $10,700 this
year. You should:
A: File Form 1040 and itemize deductions.
*Rebeck and Walstad (2005], Questions used with permission from the Nationa
Council on Economic Education.
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Appendix B
Category Statement
Understanding Debt I know what makes me a good or bad credit risk.
I understand what affects the credit terms I am offered
by different lending institutions.
Insurance I feel comfortable with my ability to make decisions
about what kind of life insurance (if any) to purchase
in the future.
I understand the difference between various types of
insurance.
I understand the difference between the different
types of automobile insurance.
Investing I am comfortable with my ability to make decisions
about savings instruments based on their fixed and
compounded interest rates.
I understand the general relationship between risk and
reward in investing.
I feel confident in my understanding of the
differences between bonds, stocks, U.S. Treasury bills
and mutual funds.
I feel comfortable with my understanding of the
various financial terms that go along with buying a
home someday.
Money Management I understand what personal net worth means.
I am confident in my ability to write a monthly
budget.
I am proficient at balancing my checkbook.
Retirement I feel comfortable with my ability to financially plan
for my retirement.
I feel comfortable with my ability to make decisions
about pension plans, 401(k)s, and IRAs when
required to do so.
Taxes I understand what an employer's responsibility is
with respect to social security taxes.
I feel comfortable filling out and filing my own
income taxes.
Fall/Winter 2013 29
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Appendix C
Category Question
Understanding How many credit cards do you use?
Debt How many of your credit cards have ever been "maxed
out"?
What amount of your credit card bill do you normally pay
each month?
What is the primary reason that you use a credit card?
Have you ever transferred a balance from one credit card to
another?
Have you ordered a copy of your credit report in the past
year?
Insurance Do you know what type(s) of coverage are included with
your car insurance?
Money Do you use a budget to manage your spending?
Management Do you have a savings account?
Do you balance your checking account regularly?
When you use your ATM or debit card, do you record the
amount somewhere?
Retirement When I graduate and get my first job, I know that I will have
to make decisions about 401(k)s, IRAs, etc.
When will you begin financial planning for your retirement?
Taxes Do you fully understand how to fill out a W-2 form?
This content downloaded from 128.184.220.23 on Fri, 01 Jan 2016 08:06:12 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions