Candace Paradoxes Pitch
Candace Paradoxes Pitch
Candace Paradoxes Pitch
x
cOxford,
Music
MUSA
©
1468-2249
0262-5245
Original
XXX
p 2008Analysis
Blackwell
ANDACE
ARADOXESBlackwell
Article
bPublishing
UK OF PITCH
ROWER Publishing
sLtd
PACE Ltd.
CANDACE BROWER
Music, like visual art, evokes images of space. The succession of pitches,
harmonies and keys which make up a tonal composition may evoke movement
through a space within which these musical ‘objects’ relate to one another as
near or far, inside or outside, above or below. Given that we can measure
distances between pitches according to their position on a frequency spectrum,
it is tempting to imagine that we can map this space in geometric terms,
representing pitches as points and keys as regions. Yet when we attempt to
locate these musical objects precisely in relation to one another within this
imaginary space, we quickly confront its contradictions. C1 and C8 are at
opposite ends of the frequency spectrum, yet the ear, and the principle of
octave equivalence, also tell us that they are the ‘same’.1 C major and A natural
minor contain the same pitches, yet we perceive them as distinct tonal regions
with different emotional qualities, C major sounding ‘bright’ or ‘happy’ and A
minor ‘dark’ or ‘sad’.
Composers at one time attributed different qualities to enharmonically
equivalent keys as well: Beethoven, for example, described the key of CC
major as ‘hard’ and Dw major as ‘soft’.2 Evidence that the distinction between
enharmonically equivalent keys continued to be taken seriously long after it
was supposed to have been perceptually erased by equal temperament can be
found in Max Reger’s Beiträge zur Modulationslehre (1903; translated as On the
Theory of Modulation (1948)). At the end of a series of exercises showing how
to modulate from C major to all other keys, Reger presents his version of the
shortest route for modulation from C major to B C major (Ex. 1). It is clear
from the succession of keys which Reger advances that he intends this progres-
sion to be heard as ending a full twelve semitones higher along the circle of
fifths than the point where it began. Yet given that the progression is only two
bars long, we cannot help but notice that the sonority at the beginning is
identical to the one at the end. In this passage, we are confronted with one of
the most striking paradoxes of the twenty-four-key system: that keys identical
in sound can nevertheless appear to be very far apart.3
Douglas R. Hofstadter (1999) notes that contradictions such as these are to
be found not only in music, but also in visual art and mathematics. Illustrating
his point with examples drawn from the music of J. S. Bach, the visual art of
M. C. Escher and the mathematics of Kurt Gödel, Hofstadter proposes that
these three modes of thought – musical, visual and mathematical – exhibit
the number of distinct points within the grid from a potentially infinite number
to twelve. These are arranged in the shape of a three-by-four-unit period
parallelogram, which may be rolled up on itself in two directions, as shown by
the arrows, to form a doughnut-shaped torus.8
Their existence reveals how the human eye elaborates upon what is physically
seen, suggesting that while visual space has properties in common with physical
space, it also possesses a logic of its own.
In Mouch our attention is focused on the paradox of figure-ground reversal.
When the eye is presented with a boundary dividing the visual field into two
regions, it attempts to interpret each as lying either inside or outside – that is,
as either figure or ground. In Mouch, figure-ground ambiguity results from the
periodic division of the plane into black and white figures, which we perceive
alternately as black objects against a white background or white objects against
a black background. The paradoxical effect is heightened by Escher’s use of
another compositional device, that of metamorphosis. The black and white
parallelograms which emerge in frames 1–4 are first transformed into black and
white birds (frames 5–10), then into black and white fish (frame 11) and finally
into white fish swimming to the left interspersed with black birds flying to the
right (frame 12).
}
}
Fig. 8 Necker cube
rest, and departure from a centre with feelings of instability and an impulse to
return. The BALANCE schema reflects the ways in which we unconsciously
balance the forces acting upon the body so as to remain stable, upright and at
rest. Fig. 9c specifically reflects our somatosensory experience of the symmetry
of the human body in terms of the weight of the left side balancing that of
the right.
The schemas presented in Figs. 9d, e and f reflect ways in which we expe-
rience the body in motion. The PATH schema shows that we tend to move along
pre-established pathways leading to specific destinations. Paths are either
constructed or worn over time through repeated movement along the same
trajectory; thus they become associated with such properties as continuity,
regularity and predictability. The CONTAINER schema reflects our experience of
space as bounded – that is, as divided into insides and outsides. We experience
the body itself as a container and its boundaries as creating the division between
self and non-self. We also experience ourselves as moving within and between
containers, such as rooms, buildings, cities and regions. We may associate
containment either with stability and protection from external forces or,
conversely, with a lack of freedom, with constraint and inhibition of movement.
Finally, the CYCLE schema reflects the way in which we experience repeating
patterns in space and time. We learn about cycles most directly through such
repetitive actions of the body as breathing and walking, as well as from such
higher-level cycles as waking and sleeping. The CYCLE schema regulates our
experience of time, allowing us to anticipate future events, and is thus associated,
like the PATH schema, with continuity, regularity and predictability.
Certain general features of the image schemas shown in Fig. 9 should be
kept in mind when considering the roles they play in cross-domain mappings.
First, although they are typically presented as visual images, image schemas
combine input from all of the senses; each binds together visual, auditory,
motor and somatosensory information into a single experiential gestalt. For
example, the VERTICALITY schema includes not only the visual image of vertical
and horizontal axes, but also the somatosensory experience of our feet resting
on the ground and the kinaesthetic sensation of our muscles tensing to hold
the body upright.
Second, many image schemas are topological; that is, they can be fitted to
patterns of different shapes and sizes. Thus the CONTAINER schema, although
drawn as a circle in Fig. 9e, can be applied to any bounded region of space,
regardless of its shape or the number of dimensions in which it exists. The
CONTAINER schema, when mapped onto a bounded region of visual space,
causes us to perceive whatever falls on one side of the boundary as figure and
whatever falls on the other as ground. Thus the CONTAINER schema plays an
important role in determining the way in which we divide visual space into
objects, parts of objects and the empty space that surrounds them.
Finally, many of these image schemas have features or properties in common
that allow them to be combined, as illustrated in Figs. 10a–c. For example,
Fig. 11 From Rudolf Arnheim, Art and Visual Perception: A Psychology of the Creative
Eye, published by the University of California Press, p. 12. © 1997 by the Regents
of the University of California
Fig. 12 Arnheim’s structural skeleton of the square. From Rudolf Arnheim, Art
and Visual Perception: A Psychology of the Creative Eye, published by the University
of California Press, p. 13. © 1997 by the Regents of the University of California
Fig. 13 Effect of shape and spatial orientation on visual stability. Adapted in part
from Rudolf Arnheim, Art and Visual Perception: A Psychology of the Creative Eye,
published by the University of California Press, p. 102. © 1997 by the Regents of
the University of California
while the upside-down version in 13b seems precarious, ready to topple to one
side. Figs. 13c and d reveal even more clearly the combined effects of symmetry
and spatial orientation. Although the equilateral triangle of Fig. 13c seems
equally stable on each of its sides, the unequal triangle of Fig. 13d produces
three different percepts, each more unstable than the last, reflecting the
apparent rise in its centre of gravity and less evenly balanced distribution of
weight. Figs. 13a–d clearly illustrate the contribution of VERTICALITY and
BALANCE schemas to our embodied experience of visual ‘objects’, showing that
we expect them to exhibit properties analogous to those exhibited by physical
objects under the influence of the physical force of gravity.
That our embodied experience of visual ‘objects’ is determined in part by
their geometry and spatial orientation suggests that the same may be true of
the musical ‘objects’ represented in Fig. 1. At first glance, Fig. 1 might appear
to capture our intuition about the relative stability of major and minor triads
by representing the major triad as a right-side-up triangle resting on its stable
base and the minor triad as an upside-down triangle perched on its apex. Yet,
as Fig. 14 shows, this image presents a rather skewed picture of the triad’s
symmetry and spatial orientation. First, by representing the consonant triad as
an equilateral triangle, the neo-Riemannian model suggests that the triad’s
tones are equidistant and thus imposes upon this type of chord a symmetry
which it does not possess. Second, as the arrows in Fig. 14 indicate, the model
portrays ascending intervals within the space of the triad as moving in three
different directions: while the major third moves ‘upwards’, the minor third
moves ‘downwards’, and the perfect fifth moves ‘horizontally’ across the page.17
Fig. 15 presents a modified version of Fig. 14 which preserves the original
topology while changing its geometry and spatial orientation in order to reveal
more clearly its image-schematic organisation. It first rotates the figure by 90°
to place C at the bottom and G at the top, then flips it across the vertical axis
to position Ew on the left and EÖ on the right, locating these vertically so as to
divide the seven semitones of the perfect fifth into 3 + 4 on the left and 4 + 3
on the right. The positioning of C at the bottom and G at the top allows every
rising interval to ascend within the visual space of the page, thereby capturing
our intuition that the root of a triad corresponds to its lowest, most stable note.
By focusing attention on the vertical arrangement of notes within the triad,
Fig. 15 also highlights an important difference between the geometry of visual
‘objects’ and that of musical ‘objects’; that is, visual ‘objects’ have both height
and width, but musical ‘objects’ have only height. This can be seen clearly in
Fig. 15 in the way that the ‘sides’ of the C major ‘triangle’ fail to add up. To
form a triangle with sides three, four and seven units in length, we would have
to set the internal angles at 180° and 0°, which would cause the shorter sides to
collapse onto the longer one. This lack of a horizontal dimension also applies
to the space within which we imagine musical ‘objects’ to be in motion.
Although we often describe notes and triads as moving ‘up’ or ‘down’, it seems
nonsensical to describe them as moving to the ‘left’ or to the ‘right’.
What, then, is the meaning of the horizontal dimension in Fig. 15? What
determines the placement of pitches along the horizontal axis? These questions
are answered visually in Fig. 16. As the arrows in Fig. 16 show, we can con-
ceive of the triangles used to model major and minor triads as having been
formed by displacing the third of each triad away from the vertical fifth axis,
the third of the minor triad to the left and the third of the major triad to the
right. This allows the geometry of the space as a whole to be seen more clearly,
much as we might open out the gathered folds of a net in order to reveal the
geometric pattern of its construction. Not only does the horizontal displace-
ment of the thirds allow major and minor triads to be more easily differentiated
by eye, but it also allows the interval of a semitone (that is, Ew to EÖ) to be read
as ascending from left to right. Thus, Fig. 16 makes clear that the horizontal
dimension is nothing more than an aid to visualisation. Although the horizontal
displacements of Ew and EÖ make them look as if they are far apart, measurement
along the vertical axis reveals that they are in fact quite close.
Although Fig. 16 allows us to ‘see’ the vertical arrangement of tones within
major and minor triads, it offers no clue as to their relative stability. This
reflects the fact that whereas visual ‘objects’ map more or less directly onto
physical objects through their shared projection on the retina, the mapping of
visual ‘objects’ onto musical ‘objects’ involves not only different domains but
also different sense modalities – sight versus sound. Thus we should not expect
the visual model to portray directly the embodied qualities of musical ‘objects’,
but only those features of their geometry which contribute to their image-
schematic organisation.18
It is to psychoacoustics that we must turn for clues concerning our embod-
ied experience of musical sounds. Research in this field suggests that our
perception of tone combinations as stable or unstable is governed in part by
the way in which their intervallic patterns correlate with that of the overtone
series. It has been found that the overtone series, having been stored in mem-
ory through repeated exposure to the complex sounds of music and speech,
serves as a template for pattern matching carried out at an unconscious level.19
It is here that we find the most likely explanation for our perception of the
major triad as more stable than the minor: its intervallic pattern forms a better
match with the intervallic pattern of the overtone series.
We can gain further insight into the embodied nature of our experience of
musical sounds by examining the image-schematic organisation of the overtone
series itself. As Fig. 17 shows, the overtone series takes on an image-schematic
structure of its own when mapped onto a VERTICAL PATH schema. Fig. 17
portrays the fundamental of a pitch as its ‘ground’ – the lowest, most stable
location within the space of a complex pitch – and the upper partials as the rungs
each exhibiting properties of closure and/or symmetry which allows the space
to be mapped onto a CIRCULAR PATH and/or a CONTAINER⁄CENTRE-PERIPHERY⁄
BALANCE schema, thus giving rise to the embodied experience of containment,
centredness and balance. We will also witness the emergence of spatial para-
doxes analogous to those found in the artwork of M. C. Escher: figure-ground
reversal, circular staircases and the Möbius strip.
Because the present model is constructed using neo-Riemannian operators
(D, P, L and R) and its completed structure resembles that of the neo-Riemannian
Tonnetz, it is important to note the ways in which it differs from the neo-Riemannian
model. First, although neo-Riemannians disregard the acoustic origins of the
consonant triad, according equal weight to root, third and fifth, the present
model privileges the root, reflecting the contribution of these origins to our
sense of bodily orientation within triadic pitch space. Moreover, it views the root
as standing in some sense for the triad as a whole, much as the fundamental
does for a complex pitch. Second, although neo-Riemannian theory defines
harmonic distance in terms of voice leading, thus privileging L, P and R
transformations as maximally smooth, the present theory defines harmonic
distance in terms of root movement. This is because voice-leading motion is
presumed to take place within another dimension of musical space, one which
I have referred to elsewhere as melodic space (Brower 2000). In consequence,
whereas neo-Riemannians treat D as equivalent to L + R because these
operations yield identical voice leadings, the present model views the same
operations as distinct: D produces the harmonic motion of a single step along
a pathway of fifths, while L + R produces the motion of two steps along a
pathway of alternating major and minor thirds. Finally, although neo-Riemannians
treat P, L and R as a family of operators because each produces stepwise motion
within a single voice, the present model treats P as distinct from L and R
because it produces not root movement, but a change in the position of the
third of the triad with respect to its root and fifth.
The difference between these two interpretations of harmonic motion is
illustrated in Ex. 4. Ex. 4a depicts the progression C major–A minor as root
movement; thus, it maps root onto root while preserving internal relations
among pitches. By contrast, Ex. 4b represents the same progression as voice
leading; thus it preserves the common tones while mapping the fifth of the first
triad onto the root of the second so as to create movement along the shortest
melodic path. These two representations are combined in Ex. 4c, which shows
both the harmonic pathway followed by root movement in the bass and the
melodic pathways followed by voice leading in the upper voices.
The present model’s treatment of root movement and voice leading as
distinct components of harmonic motion helps to highlight the paradoxical
nature of the relationship between them. Harmonic and melodic distances tend
to be inversely related, so that what sounds melodically close often sounds
harmonically distant and vice versa.27 Furthermore, root movement and voice
leading tend to proceed in opposite directions, with root movement down by
fifth or third producing voice leading upwards by step and vice versa. As a
result, whether the perceived direction of harmonic motion will be governed
by root movement or voice leading cannot be determined independently of the
musical context. In passages predominantly made up of root-position triads,
our sense of direction is likely to be governed by root movement; in passages
formed primarily from inverted triads, it is likely to be governed by voice leading.
The model also shares certain features with the cognitively derived model of
pitch space advanced by Fred Lerdahl (2001). Using empirical data provided
by Diana Deutsch and John Feroe (1981), Carol Krumhansl (1990 and 1998)
and others, Lerdahl constructs three different models of tonal pitch space, which
he refers to as basic pitch space, chordal space and regional space. His basic pitch
space corresponds to what I am calling melodic space, while his chordal and
regional spaces combine in the present model to form what I am calling triadic
space. Lerdahl criticises Riemann and his followers for attempting to represent
distances within musical space using a single map, namely that of the Tonnetz,
in which, owing to a conflation of descriptive levels, points are sometimes taken
to represent pitch and on other occasions triad or key. The present model
avoids this problem by depicting pitches as points and triads and keys as
groups of points, each of which forms a closed region of musical space.
Because Lerdahl’s model represents triads and keys, like pitches, as points
rather than as regions, it cannot accommodate overlaps among these regions.
This is particularly problematic in the case of relative major and minor keys. I
will show here that the latter do not constitute different regions of musical
space, but rather alternative ways of perceiving the same region.28
In emphasising the embodied character of our experience of movement
through triadic pitch space, I do not claim that all listeners will hear changes
of harmony or key as rising or falling, or tensing or relaxing, movement. Nor
do I suggest that we can extrapolate from the experiences of Western subjects
to those of non-Western listeners. Because such metaphorical mappings require
active engagement of the bodily imagination, as well as familiarity with the
conventions of the major-minor system, individual listeners may differ
markedly in the degree to which they experience such sensations.29 Neverthe-
less, we find evidence of significant intersubjective agreement among trained
listeners concerning such experiences in the form of a strong consensus among
latter involves using the D operator to transform a triad into its dominant and
the S operator to transform a triad into its subdominant.31 From an embodied
perspective, these correspond to motion upwards or downwards, tensing or
relaxing, respectively, as shown by the arrows on the right.
Fig. 19b presents an alternative mapping of this constructed space onto a
CENTRE-PERIPHERY⁄BALANCE schema. It portrays the key of C major as a musical
‘object’ within which are nested three smaller objects, IV, I and V, with I at the
centre balanced by V above and IV below. This mapping reveals another dimen-
sion of our embodied experience of motion within diatonic space, that of the
tonic as centre and thus a maximally stable location. This in turn supports our
experience of the motion from tonic to either dominant or subdominant as
tensing and the motion of return as relaxing. On an embodied level, we can
understand this as reflecting the tensing and relaxing of our muscles as we
move with respect to our own centre of gravity.
The two mappings of Figs. 19a and b yield somewhat conflicting image-
schematic interpretations, with motion from tonic to subdominant connoting
relaxation in the mapping of the VERTICALITY schema and tension in the map-
ping of the CENTRE-PERIPHERY⁄BALANCE schema. This suggests that alternative
image-schematic mappings may at times yield contradictory meanings, and
that our embodied experience of musical sound may at times reflect those
contradictions.
In the second stage of expansion, shown in Fig. 20a, we begin by filling in
the spaces left open in Fig. 19 with E minor and A minor triads formed
through the operation of reflection, thus creating closure while preserving
symmetry. It is at this stage of construction that the minor triad first appears
as a musical ‘object’. The relationship between major and minor triads can be
viewed as analogous to that which emerges between the black and white areas
in Escher’s Mouch: areas initially perceived as ground take on the characteristics
of figures because of their similarity to the objects that they surround.
Having formed a circular pathway from the zigzag chain of thirds, we can
form another from the two chains of fifths. As shown in Fig. 21a, this involves
twisting the double chain of fifths and thirds by 180° while bringing the ends
together to align D at the top with D at the bottom, then inserting an ‘imper-
fect tile’, the B diminished triad, into the space remaining in order to link B
to F via a diminished fifth. The result of this operation, as shown in Figs. 21b
and c, is that one-sided yet two-sided geometric object the Möbius strip.36
With the insertion of the B diminished triad, we can trace a circular route
for harmonic motion along the diatonic chain of fifths which passes through
the three major triads on the front of the Möbius strip to the three minor triads
on the back via the diminished triad straddling the seam, as shown in Fig. 22.
Whereas the ‘two sides’ of the Möbius strip portrayed in Escher’s Möbius Band
II are indistinguishable, the two sides of the diatonic Möbius strip are easily
differentiated owing to the appearance of major triads on the ‘front’ and minor
triads on the ‘back’. Furthermore, whereas in Möbius Band II the dividing line
between ‘front’ and ‘back’ has been erased by the artist, yielding an apparently
seamless one-sided surface, the seam in the diatonic Möbius strip remains
marked by the presence of the diminished fifth, its imperfection serving to
orientate us within the circle of fifths.37
Fig. 23 (a) Construction of triadic pitch space, stage 4, part 1; (b) and (c) modulation
using D, S, L and R operators
diagonal line draws attention to the fact that modulation to the sharp side or
to a major key tends to be experienced somewhat similarly as ‘brightening’, and
that to the flat side or to a minor key likewise as ‘darkening.’ This effect is
attributable to the raising or lowering of the key container in the former case
and of the thirds of the primary triads in the latter.39
Fig. 23c also reveals the special nature of the transformation of a major key
into its relative minor and vice versa. Whereas application of D, S and L
operators changes the pitch content of the key, application of the R operator
changes only the mapping of the key container onto the CENTRE-PERIPHERY
schema. Using the R operator to transform the key of C major into that of A
minor causes the pitch D, which formerly appeared at the ‘top’ of the key
space, to now appear at the ‘bottom’. When D occupies the upper position, C
major appears in the centre of the key container, straddled by its major
dominant and subdominant; when D is in the lower position, A minor appears
in the centre, straddled by its minor dominant and subdominant.
The process of adding major and minor triads above and below the diatonic
space of C major/A minor is continued in Fig. 25 to produce a series of twelve
major triads on the left balanced by twelve minor triads on the right. At this
stage, closure can once again be achieved by aligning pitches at the bottom
with those at the top. Whereas in diatonic space we employed shearing to bring
D at the bottom into alignment with D at the top, in chromatic space we use
scaling to bring Gw into alignment with FC, and likewise Ew with DC, thus trans-
forming Pythagorean fifths and thirds into equal-tempered ones.40
As Fig. 25b shows, this chromatic chain of fifths and thirds can be doubled
back on itself to form not a Möbius strip, but a cylindrical loop. Within this
cylindrical loop, the imperfection of the diminished fifth and the ‘twist’ of the
diatonic Möbius strip have disappeared. Thus, repeated application of the D
or S operator will produce movement along either one chain of fifths or the
other. If we follow the pathway on the left we get a series of twelve major triads;
if we follow the pathway on the right, a series of twelve minor triads. Or, by
applying L and R operators in alternation, we encounter all twenty-four major
and minor triads in succession, yielding the equivalent of what Cohn (1997)
has dubbed the <LR> cycle.41
Within this space we once again confront the paradox of a pathway which
is both vertical, extending infinitely upwards and downwards, and circular,
closing in on itself to return to where it began. As a result, we may ascend from
C major to FC major or descend from C major to Gw major to reach the ‘same’
location. Yet we do not experience modulation to these two keys in the same
way. Rather, we experience movement along the sharp side of the circle of fifths
as ‘tensing’, ‘hardening’ or ‘brightening’ and movement along the flat side as
‘relaxing’, ‘softening’ or ‘darkening’. Thus FC major and Gw major take on
different meanings in the embodied imagination – ‘tense’ versus ‘relaxed’,
‘hard’ versus ‘soft’, ‘bright’ versus ‘dark’ – reflecting their positions at opposite
ends of a vertically oriented space centred around C major.
Having filled the musical plane completely in the vertical direction, we can
continue to add triads on both sides of our double chain of fifths, thus expanding
musical space in the ‘horizontal’ direction. We begin by reflecting our prototile
to the left, using the P operator to produce its parallel, C minor. From this
inverted prototile we can construct a series of interlocking major and minor
triads on the left which parallels the one on the right, thus producing a space
which is symmetrical around the fifth axis containing our starting pitch, C, as
shown in Fig. 26a.
This expansion increases the possibilities for modulation by adding the P
operator to those already in place. Using D, S, L, R and P operators we can
transform any major or minor key into any one of its five most closely related
keys: dominant, subdominant, mediant, submediant and parallel.42 Within this
expanded key space we may modulate from any major key to its parallel minor
(or vice versa) by following one of two different routes. As shown in Fig. 26a,
we can move from C major to C minor by following a pathway of descent along
the double chain of fifths and thirds, or we can move directly from C major to
C minor by using the P operator to reflect the key container across the fifth
axis.43 As in the case of enharmonically equivalent keys, these two modulations
are not the same, for although they both end in the key of C minor, each takes
us on a different journey through triadic pitch space. The appearance of these
two regions at different stages of mathematical construction is reflected in the
history of compositional practice as well: composers of the late Baroque
favoured descent along the circle of fifths, while Classical and Romantic
composers increasingly favoured the immediate contrast of mode made possible
by direct modulation.44
As a consequence of the previous expansion, two sets of diagonal pathways
have opened up, one made up of successive major thirds and the other of
successive minor thirds. We may once again expand musical space to a point
of closure by simply extending each diagonal chain of major thirds by one step,
as shown in Fig. 26b. We can then align the pitch at the top of each chain of
major thirds with the one at the bottom to form the set of circular pathways
shown by the arrows, marking off that closed region of triadic pitch space
constituting the hexatonic collection. Within hexatonic space we can move
from one major or minor triad to the next via translation, or we can apply P
and L operators in alternation to produce Cohn’s ‘<LP> cycle’. Or we can
move directly from one hexatonic pole to the other via glide reflection, producing
the maximally smooth voice leading shown in Ex. 3.
By adding one more link to the chain of consecutive minor thirds which
unfolds along the other diagonal, we achieve closure by yet another means, as
shown in Fig. 26c. This forms another set of circular pathways marking off that
region of triadic pitch space which constitutes the octatonic collection. Within
octatonic space we can again move from one major or minor triad to the next
via translation, or we can alternate between P and R operations to realise the
<PR> cycle.
Because hexatonic and octatonic collections constitute closed regions
contained within a larger chromatic space, we may conceive of them as we do
the diatonic collection – as containers or objects which may themselves be set
in motion. As Fig. 27 shows, it is within the space of the neo-Riemannian
Tonnetz that hexatonic and octatonic modulation takes place, the hexatonic
container moving through four possible locations within neo-Riemannian space
(H1–H4) and the octatonic container through three (O1–O3). Each of these
containers can be visualised as a cylindrical band which wraps around the
hexatonic or octatonic torus, respectively, sliding around it three or four times
before returning to its starting position.
Over the course of the expansions of Figs. 26a–c, a fourth, ‘hidden’ axis has
emerged consisting of a series of chromatic semitones … Cw–C–CC ... . We can
fill the musical plane completely by extending this chromatic axis by a full
twelve semitones in order to form the embodied Tonnetz of Fig. 28. Like the
three-by-four Tonnetz of Fig. 27, this twelve-by-twelve Tonnetz can be rolled up
in two directions, as shown by the arrows, to form a torus.45 This final
transformation produces a space which has neither a fixed centre nor fixed
boundaries, our starting pitch, C, having become the geometric equivalent of
every other. Yet the nested regions of musical space formed over the course of
its construction have not vanished, although their boundaries have become
invisible to the eye. Rather, their existence is made apparent through our
embodied experience of movement within and between these regions. Just as
the kinaesthetic and somatosensory sensations of the body serve to orientate
us within physical space, the bodily sensations associated with changes of
harmony and key allow us to intuit our location within the harmonic space
modelled by the embodied Tonnetz.
Fig. 29 shows that the nested and overlapping spaces which emerged in the
construction of the embodied Tonnetz combine to form a coherent geometric
pattern. At the centre of the figure appears that rhomboidal-shaped region of
triadic pitch space which constitutes the neo-Riemannian Tonnetz. Contained
within it are diatonic, hexatonic and octatonic spaces, each forming one of the
Tonnetz’s main constituent axes. Diatonic space unfolds vertically from D to D,
top to bottom, while hexatonic and octatonic spaces unfold diagonally along
each of its sides, along the major-third and minor-third axes, respectively. The
neo-Riemannian Tonnetz – the natural home of hexatonic and octatonic collec-
tions – overlaps with those regions of the embodied Tonnetz which I have
designated as key space and expanded key space. Together they make up the
natural home of the diatonic collection.
By representing diatonic, hexatonic and octatonic collections as intersecting
regions of a single space, Fig. 29 highlights how differently we experience
movement within them. Diatonic space offers a strong sense of vertical orien-
tation, giving rise to feelings of tension and relaxation as we move up and down
within its space by fifth or by alternating major and minor thirds. Its dual
centre-periphery organisation also makes it possible for us to orient ourselves
within its boundaries with respect to a triadic centre, either C major or A
minor, while its diminished fifth likewise provides orientation by locating us at
the ‘boundaries’ of diatonic space – the ‘seam’ in the Möbius strip. Finally,
when the key container itself is set in motion within key space and expanded
key space, we can attune to feelings of tensing and relaxing, brightening and
darkening, to sense whether we are moving from sharps to flats, or from major
to minor, or vice versa.
Hexatonic and octatonic spaces differ in that they provide few cues to spatial
orientation. Each unfolds diagonally along either the major-third or the minor-
third axis, thus prohibiting root movement upwards or downwards by fifth.
Each has an even rather than an odd number of triads and thus lacks the
centre-periphery organisation of the diatonic collection. And each confronts us
with the paradox of enharmonic equivalence more quickly than movement of
the diatonic container through key space, their smaller circles of major and
minor thirds bringing us from sharp to flat over the space of just an octave.
Furthermore, the maximally smooth voice leading and frequent changes of
inversion associated with movement within hexatonic and octatonic spaces
greatly undermines the effect of being grounded in the root of each triad.46 As
a result, movement within these spaces can seem uncanny, mysterious and
unsettling, as if the laws of nature have been transcended, causing us to lose
our sense of tonal centredness, stability, groundedness and balance.
Like the patterns of tilings which filled Escher’s notebooks, Figs. 19–29
constitute no more than an inventory of possible spaces; it is left to composers
to realise these in the context of particular musical works. The remainder of
this article will examine passages from four pieces in which the paradoxical
features of these spaces are brought to light in musically significant ways: the
fifteenth-century motet ‘Absalon fili mi’;47 the finale of Haydn’s String
Quartet in G major, Op. 76 No. 1; Brahms’s Intermezzo in B minor, Op. 119
No. 1; and Wagner’s Parsifal. These analyses suggest that at least some
composers have been aware of the paradoxes of triadic pitch space and have
intentionally exploited them, allowing listeners to experience sensations like
those associated with Escher’s Mouch, Ascending and Descending and Möbius
Band II.
Fig. 30 ‘Absalon fili mi’, bars 76–85: harmonic and tonal journeys
As the beaming indicates, the tenor voice initiates the descent by falling
through the space of a Bw major triad, a movement continued by the bassus in
bar 79. Movement by falling thirds passes back and forth between the two
voices to outline a succession of major triads, B w–Ew–A w–D w–Gw. Adding to the
overall feeling of descent is a melodic sequence, marked by the higher-level
beams, which forms the pattern F–Ew–D w in the tenor and Bw–A w–Gw in the
bassus.
The succession of major triads ending on Gw in bar 83 might lead us to
expect a cadence in Dw major. But when the voices break out of their sequence,
the Gw, rather than ascending to Aw, descends to F, leading to a V–i cadence in
Bw minor. The lack of a third in the final sonority lends a certain ambiguity of
mode, suggesting that the work may have ended after all in the key of Bw major.
But as Fig. 30 makes clear, the harmonic and tonal motion of the intervening
bars leads to a much lower region of triadic pitch space than the one in which
it began. The feeling of falling, softening and darkening as we descend along
the circle of fifths and shift from major to minor powerfully evokes a grieving
father’s descent into hell. This suggests that the paradoxical nature of triadic
pitch space – that it is open yet closed, vertical yet circular, one-sided yet
two-sided – can be intuited directly via its effect on both body and mind.
Ex. 7 Haydn, String Quartet in G major, Op. 76 No. 1/iv, bars 94–106
harmony and key at this instant – from seven flats to three sharps and from
minor to major – are represented in Fig. 31.53
By contrasting the minuteness of the melodic motion required to carry out
the transformation with the vastness of the key space traversed, Haydn draws
attention to the paradoxical nature of the relationship between melodic and
harmonic motion: that what we experience as near in one dimension may
appear to be quite distant in another. In the nineteenth century composers
increasingly exploited this inverse relation, using chromatic voice leading to
move easily yet mysteriously to seemingly distant tonal regions.
look for strangeness in what seems most familiar. Yet Brahms’s unusual
treatment of the diatonic collection in the opening bars of his Intermezzo in B
minor, Op. 119 No. 1 (Ex. 8), suggests that he was aware of its paradoxical
features and meant to bring them to light. The steady semiquaver rhythm and
continuously falling thirds of the opening present a nearly seamless musical
surface reminiscent of Escher’s Möbius Band II. Its continuous cycling through
the diatonic collection locates us firmly within the key space of D major/B
minor. Yet figure-ground ambiguity causes the ear to flip back and forth
between these two keys, which are counterposed in almost perfect balance.54
In the first three bars, the ambiguity of harmony and metre reinforces that
of key. Bar-length units are marked off by the repeating pattern of the right-
hand melody and the left-hand arpeggiation, as shown by the dotted lines
in Ex. 8. Yet these units resist segmentation into clearly defined beats and
harmonies. The division into beats is obscured by the uniform surface rhythm
and the 3/8 metre, which lends itself to alternative groupings of 2 + 2 + 2 or
3 + 3. Similarly, the unbroken succession of diatonic thirds and the sustaining
of every note to the end of the bar serves to blur triadic boundaries such that
each pitch may be heard as the root, third or fifth of the sounding harmony.
Some pitches can be heard as members of more than one harmony, as shown
by the overlapping circles in Ex. 8, a phenomenon which produces a smoothly
shifting, rather than a discretely changing, harmonic landscape. Thus, we are
encouraged by the seamlessness of the musical surface to process bars as
wholes rather than as sums of their constituent parts.
A seamless surface coupled with pattern repetition at the level of the
individual bar creates ideal conditions for the kind of figure-ground reversal
illustrated in Fig. 20d. In bar 1, emphasis on the pitches FC, B and E tilts the
ear towards hearing first i and then iv of B minor. This gives way in bar 2 to
an impression of V7 and then I of D major, an impression reinforced by the
4–3 suspension on the last beat of the bar. This is superseded in bar 3 by
emphasis on the pitches CC, FC and B, suggesting v and then i of B minor. Thus,
as shown in Fig. 32, each of these first three bars presents us with a different
view of the diatonic Möbius strip: first its ‘dark’ side in bar 1, followed by a
turn to its ‘bright’ side in bar 2, then another to its ‘dark’ side in bar 3. Only
in bar 4 do we experience a momentary lifting of the tonal haze as we hear the
clear outlines of a 4–3–2 descent in the melody supported by iiø7–i64 –V 42 in B
minor.
That Brahms intends the tonal ambiguity of the opening bars to come to
the listener’s attention is further suggested by the way it plays out over the
course of the movement as a whole. Its ABA′ form is set in the keys of B minor
and D major, thus reflecting on a formal level the figure-ground reversal of the first
three bars. Yet neither of the first two sections ends with a perfect cadence which
might serve to confirm its tonic.55 In fact, the identity of the tonic key, whether
D major or B minor, remains open to question until the final cadence. Bars 63
and 64 present the beginnings of a circle-of-fifths progression which seems to
prepare for a V7–I ending in D major, an expectation confirmed by the recall
of material from bar 2 (Ex. 9). Yet at the last moment Brahms undermines our
expectations of this resolution by substituting AC for AÖ (bar 65) while contin-
uing the pattern of falling thirds across the bar line. Above this we hear the G
once again function as a suspension, but in place of the expected 4–3, a 6–5
motion is introduced, all four upper voices sustaining the constituent pitches
of the diminished seventh chord across the bar line before resolving simultane-
ously as part of a V9–i cadence in B minor. In retrospect, the surrounding of
D major by its ‘alter ego’, B minor, in the first three bars foreshadows its
We can make sense of Lewin’s foray into the world of Escherian logic by
reinterpreting the passage in light of the model presented here. If we take
‘Stufen space’ to correspond to diatonic space and ‘Riemann space’ to correspond
to hexatonic space, we may proceed to rationalise the passage in question by
mapping it onto the model of triadic pitch space shown in Fig. 29.57 Fig. 33a
shows that the original version of the ‘Grail’ motive fits neatly within the key
the opera’s magical effects. As we pass through hexatonic space, our sense of
gravitational pull, vertical orientation and centredness is disrupted, giving rise
to a momentary sense of uncanniness, only to be restored, along with our sense
of tonal groundedness, at the final cadence. At the same time, chromatic trans-
formation of the ‘Grail’ motive, accomplished via the enharmonic relationship
between Cw and B, serves to change what was once heard as a dominant into
a subdominant. This in turn prepares us for the large-scale plagal cadence
which closes the entire opera. Lewin’s analysis reveals that the enharmony of
Cw/B, which might otherwise appear to be nothing more than a notational
inconvenience – a flaw in the logic of the diatonic system – takes on powerful
symbolic and affective meaning with respect to the drama as a whole.
Hofstadter points out that it was the presence of paradoxes such as these in
the field of mathematics which inspired Bertrand Russell and Alfred North
Whitehead to attempt to banish them from their great monument to the tenets
of logical empiricism, Principia Mathematica. This treatise, inspired by Russell’s
‘desperate quest for a way to circumvent paradoxes of self-reference’ (Hof-
stadter 1999, p. iv), was an attempt to construct a mathematical system which
would be both complete and free of internal contradiction. The futility of their
quest was revealed only thirty years later by Kurt Gödel’s incompleteness
theorem, which revealed the inevitability of the contradictions that appear in
even the most rigorously derived mathematical systems.
New light has been shed on the paradoxes of mathematics in a recent book
by George Lakoff and Rafael E. Núñez on the bodily basis of mathematical
thought (Lakoff and Núñez 2000). The authors show that mathematical
reasoning is largely spatial in nature, and that many of its basic concepts –
those which form its axiomatic foundations – have image-schematic origins.
Concepts such as equality, line, circle and set appear to originate in cross-
domain mappings of image schemas such as BALANCE, PATH, CYCLE and
CONTAINMENT. Lakoff and Núñez propose that mathematical systems such as
algebra, analytical geometry, number theory and set theory begin with axioms
which are image-schematic in nature, then build through successive layerings
of image-schematic mappings towards increasingly higher levels of complexity
and abstraction. As one metaphorical mapping is overlaid upon another,
contradictions arise within and between these systems, reflecting their different
image-schematic origins. For example, the axiom of Euclidean geometry which
asserts that nonparallel lines must intersect at a single point is contradicted by
axioms governing the geometry of elliptical and hyperbolic spaces, in which
lines are both ‘straight’ and ‘curved’. Thus mathematics, like music, reflects the
capacity of the embodied imagination to produce internally coherent systems
which are logical yet also paradoxical.
In the case of music, these paradoxes appear to be woven into the very fabric
of musical space itself – a residue of its cyclic nature. We may remain oblivious
to the fact that we are moving in circles; we may fail to notice when traversing
the chromatic circle of fifths that we have returned to the point where we
began; and we may be unaware of the twist in the diatonic Möbius strip as we
move from front to back. Because these paradoxes are not only pervasive but
inescapable, a composer must make special efforts to bring them to our
attention. The analyses presented here suggest that at least some composers
have shared with Escher the urge to create works of art which force us to
confront these contradictions.
Music transports us to a world in which the logic of everyday space, time
and movement gives way to impossible possibilities, leading us into contradic-
tion no matter which way we turn – pathways which are vertical yet circular,
locations which are the same yet different, near yet far, above yet below.
Far from constituting flaws in the logic of an otherwise perfect system, such
paradoxes help to make music meaningful for us, a source of never-ending
mystery and delight.
NOTES
M. C. Escher, Mouch, © 2008 The M. C. Escher Company – Holland. All
rights reserved. www.mcescher.com.
1. Octave equivalence appears to be one of the few truly universal principles of pitch
perception, applying not only to humans, but to other primates as well. See
Wright, Rivera, Hulse, Shyan and Neiworth (2000).
2. As reported by Schindler (1966, p. 368).
3. In his late essay ‘Ideas for a “Study on the Imagination of Tone”’, Hugo Riemann
(1914–15) identifies enharmonic equivalence as among the most important
theoretical puzzles remaining to be solved, predicting that ‘the study of enharmonic
identification … ultimately will solve and explain the contradictions between the results
of tone-psychological investigations and the practical experiences of musicians’ (Wason
and Marvin 1992, p. 110; italics as original).
4. As noted by Lakoff and Johnson (1980), the verb ‘to see’ is often used metaphoric-
ally to mean ‘to understand’, reflecting the workings of the conceptual metaphor
SEEING IS UNDERSTANDING. I use quotation marks here and elsewhere to draw
attention to the metaphorical connotations of a term wherever they might
otherwise be overlooked.
5. This article describes the origins of the Tonnetz, or ‘table of tonal relations’, in
the work of Hugo Riemann and other nineteenth-century German theorists and
explains how the Tonnetz came to be resurrected and recast in mathematical terms
largely through the efforts of Lewin (1982 and 1987) and Hyer (1989 and 1995).
6. For a useful introduction to the mathematics of tiling, see Grünbaum and
Shephard (1989).
7. Thurston (1997) notes that this property is shared by all periodic tilings.
8. It is precisely this property which distinguishes the neo-Riemannian Tonnetz from
its nineteenth-century predecessors. The numerical version of the Tonnetz was
first proposed by Hyer (1995).
9. Other artists who have incorporated tilings into their artwork include Koloman
Moser (1868–1918) and Victor Vasarely (1908–97).
10. The contents of Escher’s notebooks, unpublished during his lifetime, appear in
Schattschneider (1990).
11. This and some of Escher’s other artworks can be viewed online at http://
www.mcescher.net/ (accessed 6 June 2008).
12. For a simple explanation of local versus global properties, see Weeks (2002).
13. In this article Cohn offers examples of hexatonic polar progressions spanning
more than 300 years, from Carlo Gesualdo’s ‘Moro lasso’ to Schoenberg’s String
Trio, Op. 45.
14. The alternative layout of fifths along diagonal axes and major and minor thirds along
vertical and horizontal axes shown in Fig. 7 was introduced by Balzano (1980) and
is favoured by Cohn (1997). The geographic labelling of the four hexatonic systems
as Northern, Southern, Eastern and Western was introduced by Cohn (1996).
15. The term ‘de-souling’ is Cohn’s (2004, p. 297) rendering of the term Entseelung,
which translates literally as ‘removal of the soul’.
16. The concept of an image schema, first introduced by the linguist George Lakoff
and the philosopher Mark Johnson (Lakoff 1987; Johnson 1987; and Lakoff and
Johnson 1999), has since given birth to a thriving sub-discipline of cognitive
science which has engaged scholars in such fields as linguistics (Langacker 1987
and Talmy 2000), psychology (Gibbs and Colston 1995), literature (Turner 1991
and 1996), anthropology (Strauss and Quinn 1997), mathematics (Lakoff and
Núñez 2000), visual art (Esrock 2002) and music (Saslaw 1996 and 1997–8;
Mead 1997–8; Brower 1997–8 and 2000; Zbikowski 1997 and 2002; Larson
1997–8 and 2004; Cox 1999; Snyder 2000; Kemler 2001; Butterfield 2002; Iyer
2002; Eitan and Granot 2004; and Straus 2006). The following discussion is
particularly indebted to Johnson (1987).
17. Hyer (1995) notes the contradiction implied by the visual layout of Riemann’s original
Tonnetz: that whereas we descend in register when moving downwards along the major-
third axis, we ascend in register when moving downwards along the minor-third axis.
18. As Butterfield (2002) observes, the concept of a musical object is itself metaphorical.
He suggests that we identify as musical ‘objects’ those features of a musical work
– including the work itself – which share properties with objects in the physical world,
such as replicability, durability, containment and preservation of shape and size.
19. See especially Ernst Terhardt’s work on the perception of virtual pitch, a term
used by Terhardt to refer to our perception of the fundamental frequency of a
complex tone as its sounding pitch even when it is not actually sounded (Terhardt
1974 and 1979). The implications of this work have been extended to the percep-
tion of combinations of tones (Terhardt 1984; and Parncutt 1988 and 1989).
According to Terhardt (1984, p. 293), ‘the nature of the fundamental note (root) of
musical chords is identical with that of the virtual pitch of individual complex tones’.
20. The pitches shown in Figs. 15 and 16 constitute a type of pitch class because they
subsume those pitches that share the same letter name, regardless of register.
However, they differ from the pitch classes of atonal set theory in that they
exclude their enharmonic equivalents. Throughout this article, the use of a letter
name alone should be taken to imply this more limited notion of what we might
call letter-name pitch class.
21. The metaphor of root as ground also helps to explain why major and minor triads
sound more stable in root position than in first or second inversion. When the
root appears in the bass, it functions as the fundamental – as a stable foundation
for the sonority as a whole.
22. Although Western musicians tend to take for granted the correlation between
increasing frequency and ascending movement, cross-cultural studies suggest that
this mapping may not be universal (Zbikowski 2002 and Ashley 2004). For exam-
ple, Balinese and Javanese musicians refer to ‘high’ and ‘low’ pitches as ‘small’
and ‘large’, while the Suya of the Amazon basin refer to them as ‘young’ and ‘old’
(Zbikowski 2002). That changes of pitch tend to evoke feelings of rising and
falling movement in Western listeners may reflect the pervasively triadic nature of
Western tonal music, which supports the mapping of root (or tonic) as ground
and of melodic motion as rising and falling in relation to this ground.
23. Révész (1954) and Shepard (1964).
24. These are described in Shepard (1964).
25. For an auditory demonstration of Shepard’s tones, see http://www.cs.ubc.ca/nest/
imager/contributions/flinn/Illusions/ST/st.html (accessed 6 June 2008). A similar
effect can be experienced when one listens to certain organ works played on
mixture stops. For example, the bass line of Bach’s Fantasia in G minor, BWV
542, includes a sequential series of stepwise descents (bars 31–34) which seem to
create the illusion of a continuous descent in this respect.
26. Construction of the model in stages makes it possible to address certain
problematic features of the neo-Riemannian model noted by Lerdahl (2001):
first, that it appears to privilege root relations by third over those by fifth despite
the predominance of the latter even in the highly chromatic music which inspired
development of the model; and second, that it appears to posit the existence of
the chromatic collection before the diatonic even though the latter is both
syntactically central and historically prior to the former.
27. A particularly clear example of this inverse relation is to be found in Ex. 3.
28. Although the embodied features of the present model have yet to be empirically
tested, its representation of relative distances among chords and keys, similar to those
offered by Lerdahl (2001), fits well with the results of probe-tone studies (Krumhansl
1990 and 1998). They also correlate effectively with the results of a recent study
showing that key distances are represented in the form of a dynamic topography
in the brain ( Janata, Birk, Van Horn, Leman, Tillmann and Bharucha 2002).
29. Riemann (1914–15) also stresses the role played by the imagination in transform-
ing musical sounds. He notes that ‘the “Alpha and Omega” of musical artistry …
exists in the mental image of musical relationships that occurs in the creative
artist’s imagination – a mental image that lives before it is transformed into nota-
tion and re-emerges in the imagination of the hearer’ (Wason and Marvin 1992,
p. 82; italics as original). Later he observes that such images are at least partly
somatosensory in nature: ‘As a result of the valuation of pitch-level motion as
Göller and Müller (2002). Interestingly, the Möbius strip was discovered by a
procedure similar to that shown in Fig. 21. In experimenting with different ways
of forming closed loops from rectangular strips created by gluing triangles
together, A. F. Möbius discovered that any strip containing an even number of
triangles forms a cylindrical loop, while any strip containing an odd number of
triangles must be twisted by 180° to make the ends meet, forming what is now
known as the Möbius strip (Biggs 1993).
37. A precursor of the model shown in Figs. 19–21 appears in Hauptmann ([1893]
1991; first published in 1853 as Die Natur der Harmonik und der Metrik). Haupt-
mann portrays the diatonic pitches of C major as a chain of thirds, F–a–C–e–G–
b–D (where the capital and lowercase letters refer to the roots of major and of
minor triads respectively), made up of a ‘triad of triads’ (p. 9), I, IV and V, which
he describes as closing in on itself to form a circle. Because Hauptmann’s model
is one-dimensional, however, it does not show the ‘twist’ in the chain of interlock-
ing fifths and thirds. Furthermore, because Hauptmann, like many of his contem-
poraries, favoured just intonation, the third D–F is a syntonic comma smaller
than pure, thus producing not one but two ‘diminished’ triads. It appears that,
like many other German theorists of the nineteenth century, Hauptmann viewed
triadic pitch space as extending indefinitely towards increasingly chromatic
regions from the centrally located key of C major (Engebretsen 2002).
38. Although one finds varying descriptions of the qualities of individual keys in the
eighteenth and nineteenth centuries, there appears to have been a strong
consensus concerning the association of modulation to the sharp side of the circle
of fifths with tensing, hardening and brightening, and to the flat side with relax-
ing, softening and darkening (Steblin 2002). For example, according to Rameau
(1754), ‘the side of the dominant, that of the rising fifth, is rightly the side of
strength, so that the more fifths there are in going up, the more this strength
increases; the same reasoning holds conversely for softness, on the side of the
subdominant’ (quoted in Steblin 2002, p. 97). And according to Vogler (1778),
‘if we go up by fifths through G, D, A, and E, there is always an increase of
strength, effect, cutting quality and penetration. If we go down by fifths through
F, Bw, Ew, and Aw, all strength is reduced and the impression becomes duller and
darker’ (quoted in Steblin 2002, p. 121).
39. Steblin (2002) notes that some theoretical confusion arose in the eighteenth
century concerning the distinction between major mode and sharp keys on the
one hand and minor mode and flat keys on the other because movement from
sharp to flat keys and modulation from major to minor mode both require the
addition of flats and, likewise, movement from flat to sharp keys and modulation
from minor to major mode both require the addition of sharps. It seems likely
that this confusion was caused in part by the similar experiential qualities of these
changes in key and mode.
40. Scaling, a term used by graphic artists to refer to a uniform change in the size of
an object, here refers to a reduction in the size of each perfect fifth by one-twelfth
of a Pythagorean comma.
41. An early representation of the <LR> cycle appears in Vogler (1778). Vogler
portrays the cycle in circular score notation, the falling thirds in the bass being
filled in with passing notes in order to allow each triad to be approached by its
dominant. See Wason (1985), p. 17.
42. The results of a probe-tone study suggest that the modulations produced by
applying these five operators do in fact correspond to those keys heard as most
closely related (Krumhansl 1998).
43. Within this space, we can also chart a circular route around our starting pitch of
C by applying P, L and R operators in succession to produce what Cohn has
termed the <PLR> cycle. The resultant hexagon-shaped musical object, which binds
together all of the triads containing the pitch C, appears to be what Riemann
meant by Klangvertretung, translated by Wason and Marvin as ‘representation
of a tonal complex’ (1992, p. 86). According to Wason and Marvin, Riemann
used this term to refer to the psychological processing of a note in terms of its
six possible harmonic functions as root, third or fifth of a major or minor triad.
44. We find evidence of such preferences in the writings of composers and theorists
of the time. Georg Andreas Sorge, for example, notes the possibility of direct
modulation in his Vorgemach der musicalischen Composition (1745–7), only to reject
it as too abrupt, comparing the sounding of C major and C minor together to ‘a
knock on the head’ (p. 30).
45. Technically speaking, every horizontal row of pitches should consist of successive
sharpings and flattings of the same pitch, CÖ becoming Cwwwwww on the left and
CCCCCCC on the right. This permits every pitch in the grid to have its own letter
designation. For practical reasons, I have substituted the pitches of the chromatic
scale, thus yielding enharmonic spellings of triads which become more obvious as
we move away from the centre of the grid.
46. Cohn (1996) proposes treating the pitches of a triad within a hexatonic system as
equally weighted, regardless of their status as root, third or fifth. His proposal
underlines the degree to which the maximally smooth voice leading associated with
hexatonic progressions undermines the privileged position of the root within the triad.
47. We find evidence of such intentions in the score of Bach’s ‘Canon per tonos’ from
Das musikalische Opfer, a work which sequences upwards by major second six times
in order to conclude in the key in which it began. That Bach meant the paradox
of a circular staircase to come to the attention of its royal dedicatee, Frederick
the Great, can be inferred from his inscription: ‘As the modulation rises, so may
the King’s glory’. But although Hofstadter (1999, p. 10) describes the work as an
‘Endlessly Rising Canon’ and indicates by this that Bach intended the canon to
portray the king’s glory as ascending infinitely, Chafe (1984) argues that Bach
most likely intended just the opposite: not infinite ascent but merely the illusion of
ascent. Thus the dedication in fact reveals the earthly limitations of the king’s glory.
48. See Lowinsky (1946). Although this motet was long attributed to Josquin Desprez,
its authorship is now questioned, with many scholars arguing, largely on stylistic
grounds, that it was composed by Pierre de la Rue (Benthem 1989; Rifkin 1991; and
Meconi 1998). However, Davison (1996) maintains that the work cannot be
definitively assigned to either composer and that its authorship must remain uncertain.
49. Randel (1971) notes the ubiquity of the V–I cadence in fifteenth-century music
and refers to it as an example of ‘emergent triadic tonality’.
50. It is perhaps not surprising that the expansion of triadic space should have
proceeded far to the flat side before moving in the opposite direction. The
medieval gamut offered the ‘soft’ (flat) B as an alternative to the ‘hard’ (natural)
B, providing an extra link in the chain of perfect fifths. It seems only natural that
composers would have viewed the Bw at the bottom of the chain as the more
logical point for expansion. The closing bars of ‘Absalon fili mi’ reveal an inter-
mediate stage in the formation of key space, one which extends the chain of major
and minor thirds by seven steps, thus permitting a return to the initial tonic but
not to the initial mode.
51. That Haydn conceived of this motive as a ‘vehicle’ for the modulation is further
suggested by its only other appearance in the movement: in bar 143 of the
recapitulation, where it forms a prominent feature of the new theme which ushers
in an equally striking change of key from G minor to G major.
52. The remarkable effect of enharmonic modulation is noted by Anton Reicha in his
Traité de mélodie (1814); he observes that ‘when the key of FC is suddenly changed
into Gw, and CC into Dw (and vice versa), we fall from a very brilliant key into a
very sombre key, or from a very sombre into a very piercing key’ (quoted in
Steblin 2002, p. 126).
53. One could hear this modulation as simply a continuation of the descent along the
circle of fifths to the key of the Neapolitan, Bww major. However, assuming that
one has kept track of one’s position within the key space, this only delays the
arrival of the enharmonic modulation needed to return to the home key of G
minor. Given Haydn’s unusual treatment of this passage, his notation of the
modulation at precisely this point, and the fact that the key of Bww major falls far
outside the range of keys that in the late eighteenth century were judged accept-
able, it seems likely that the composer intended the modulation to be heard as it
is notated. Further support for this hearing is provided by the upwards resolution
of the Aw, strongly suggesting its reinterpretation as GC.
54. Many other ambiguous features of this piece have been pointed out by Dunsby
(1981).
55. Jordan and Kafalenos (1989) explore the tonal ambiguity of this composition in
terms of a ‘double trajectory’ of B minor and D major, showing how Brahms
maintains the ambiguity throughout.
56. The BÖ shown in parentheses in Ex. 10b reflects Wagner’s own notation, while the
Cw reflects our initial hearing of this triad as a flattened submediant in the key of
Ew major.
57. From the passages cited above, it would appear that Lewin conceived of Riemann
space as the Möbius strip and Stufen space as the cylindrical loop, precisely the
opposite of the interpretation offered here. This suggests that he had not fully
worked out the implications of his topological metaphor but was attempting
rather to capture the idea of two musical spaces that intersect, yet which remain
topologically distinct.
REFERENCES
Arnheim, Rudolf, 1984: ‘Perceptual Dynamics in Musical Expression’, Musical
Quarterly, 70/iii, pp. 295–309.
____, 1997: Art and Visual Perception: a Psychology of the Creative Eye, rev. edn
(Berkeley and Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press).
Ashley, Richard, 2004: ‘Musical Pitch Space across Modalities: Spatial and
Other Mappings through Language and Culture’, in S. D. Lipscomb,
R. Ashley, R. O. Gjerdingen and P. Webster (eds.), Proceedings of the 8th
International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition, Evanston, IL, 2004
(Adelaide: Causal Productions), pp. 64–71.
Balzano, Gerald, 1980: ‘The Group-Theoretic Description of 12-Fold and
Microtonal Pitch Systems’, Computer Music Journal, 4/iv, pp. 66–84.
Benthem, Jaap van, 1989: ‘Lazarus versus Absalom. About Fiction and Fact in
the Netherlands Motet. For Willem Elders’, Tijdschrift van de Koninklijke
Vereniging voor Nederlandse Muziekgeschiedenis, 39, pp. 54–82.
Biggs, Norman, 1993: ‘The Development of Topology’, in John Fauvel, Raymond
Flood and Robin Wilson (eds.), Möbius and His Band: Mathematics and
Astronomy in Nineteenth-Century Germany (Oxford and New York: Oxford
University Press), pp. 104–19.
Brower, Candace, 1997–8: ‘Pathway, Blockage, and Containment in Density
21.5’, Theory and Practice, 22–3, pp. 35–54.
____, 2000: ‘A Cognitive Theory of Musical Meaning’, Journal of Music Theory,
44/ii, pp. 323–79.
Brown, Howard Mayer, 1976: Music in the Renaissance (Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice Hall).
Butterfield, Matthew, 2002: ‘The Musical Object Revisited’, Music Analysis,
21/iii, pp. 327–80.
Chafe, Eric, 1984: ‘Allegorical Music: the “Symbolism” of Tonal Language in
the Bach Canons’, Journal of Musicology, 3/iv, pp. 340–62.
Cohn, Richard, 1996: ‘Maximally Smooth Cycles, Hexatonic Systems, and the
Analysis of Late-Romantic Triadic Progressions’, Music Analysis, 15/i, pp.
9–40.
____, 1997: ‘Neo-Riemannian Operations, Parsimonious Trichords, and Their
Tonnetz Representations’, Journal of Music Theory, 41/i, pp. 1–66.
____, 1998: ‘Introduction to Neo-Riemannian Theory: a Survey and a Historical
Perspective’, Journal of Music Theory, 42/ii, pp. 167–80.
____, 2004: ‘Uncanny Resemblances: Tonal Signification in the Freudian Age’,
Journal of the American Musicological Society, 57/ii, pp. 285–323.
Cox, Arnie W., 1999: ‘The Metaphoric Logic of Musical Motion and Space’
(PhD diss., University of Oregon).
Davison, Nigel, 1996: ‘Absalom fili mi Reconsidered’, Tijdschrift van de Koninkl-
ijke Vereniging voor Nederlandse Muziekgeschiedenis, 46/i, pp. 42–56.
Deutsch, Diana and Feroe, John, 1981: ‘The Internal Representation of Pitch
Sequences in Tonal Music’, Psychological Review, 88, pp. 503–22.
Dunsby, Jonathan, 1981: Structural Ambiguity in Brahms: Analytical Approaches
to Four Works (Ann Arbor, MI: UMI Research Press).
Eitan, Zohar and Granot, Roni, 2004: ‘Musical Parameters and Images of
Motion’, in Richard Parncutt, Annekatrin Kessler and Fränk Zimmer (eds.),
Proceedings of the Conference on Interdisciplinary Musicology, Graz/Austria,
____, 1999: Philosophy in the Flesh: the Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to
Western Thought (New York: Basic Books).
Lakoff, George and Núñez, Rafael E., 2000: Where Mathematics Comes From:
How the Embodied Mind Brings Mathematics into Being (New York: Basic
Books).
Langacker, Ronald, 1987: Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, vol. 1: Theoretical
Prerequisites (Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press).
Larson, Steve, 1997–8: ‘Musical Forces and Melodic Patterns’, Theory and
Practice, 22–3, pp. 55–71.
____, 2004: ‘Musical Forces and Melodic Expectations: Comparing Com-
puter Models and Experimental Results’, Music Perception, 21/iv, pp.
457–98.
Lerdahl, Fred, 2001: Tonal Pitch Space (New York: Oxford University Press).
Lewin, David, 1982: ‘A Formal Theory of Generalized Tonal Functions’,
Journal of Music Theory, 26/i, pp. 23–60.
____, 1984: ‘Amfortas’s Prayer to Titurel and the Role of D in Parsifal: the
Tonal Spaces of the Drama and the Enharmonic Cw/B’, 19th-Century Music,
7/iii, pp. 336–49.
____, 1987: Generalized Musical Intervals and Transformations (New Haven, CT
and London: Yale University Press).
Lowinsky, Edward E., 1946: Secret Chromatic Art in the Netherlands Motet,
trans. Carl Buchman (New York: Columbia University Press).
Mazzola, Guerino, 1990: Geometrie der Töne, Elemente der mathematischen
Musiktheorie (Basel: Birkhaüser).
Mazzola, Guerino, in collaboration with Göller, Stefan and Müller, Stefan, 2002:
The Topos of Music: Geometric Logic of Concepts, Theory, and Performance
(Basel: Birkhäuser Verlag).
Mead, Andrew, 1997–8: ‘Shedding Scales: Understanding Intervals in Different
Musical Contexts’, Theory and Practice, 22–3, pp. 73–94.
Meconi, Honey, 1998: ‘Another Look at Absalon’, Tijdschrift van de Koninklijke
Vereniging voor Nederlandse Muziekgeschiedenis, 48/i, pp. 3–29.
Parncutt, Richard, 1988: ‘Revision of Terhardt’s Psychoacoustical Model of the
Root(s) of a Musical Chord’, Music Perception, 6/i, pp. 65–94.
____, 1989: Harmony: a Psychoacoustical Approach (Berlin: Springer-Verlag).
Peretz, Isabelle; Gagnon, Lise; and Bouchard, Bernard, 1998: ‘Music and
Emotion: Perceptual Determinants, Immediacy, and Isolation after Brain
Damage’, Cognition, 68/ii, pp. 111–41.
Rameau, Jean-Philippe, 1754: Observations sur notre instinct pour la musique
(Paris: Prault fils).
Randel, Don M., 1971: ‘Emerging Triadic Tonality in the Fifteenth Century’,
Musical Quarterly, 57/i, pp. 73–86.
Reger, Max, 1948: On the Theory of Modulation, trans. John Bernhoff (New
York: Edwin F. Kalmus).
Reicha, Anton, 1814: Traité de mélodie (Paris).
Turner, Mark, 1991: Reading Minds: the Study of English in the Age of Cognitive
Science (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press).
____, 1996: The Literary Mind: the Origins of Thought and Language (Oxford:
Oxford University Press).
Vogler, Georg Joseph, 1778: Kuhrpfälzische Tonschule (Mannheim: C. F. Schwan
and M. Götz).
Wason, Robert W., 1985: Viennese Harmonic Theory from Albrechtsberger to
Schenker and Schoenberg (Rochester, NY: University of Rochester
Press).
Wason, Robert and Marvin, Elizabeth West, 1992: ‘Riemann’s “Ideen zu einer
Lehre von den Tonvorstellungen”: an Annotated Translation’, Journal of
Music Theory, 36/i, pp. 69–117.
Weeks, Jeffrey, 2002: The Shape of Space (New York: Marcel Dekker).
Wright, Anthony A.; Rivera, Jacquelyne J.; Hulse, Stewart H.; Shyan, Melissa;
and Neiworth, Julie J., 2000: ‘Music Perception and Octave Generalization
in Rhesus Monkeys’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129/iii,
pp. 291–307.
Zbikowski, Lawrence M., 1997: ‘Conceptual Models and Cross-Domain
Mapping: New Perspectives on Theories of Music and Hierarchy’, Journal
of Music Theory, 41/ii, pp. 193–225.
____, 2002: Conceptualizing Music: Cognitive Structure, Theory, and Analysis
(New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press).
ABSTRACT
Parallels between the mathematics of tiling, which describes geometries of visual
space, and neo-Riemannian theory, which describes geometries of musical space,
make it possible to show that certain paradoxes featured in the visual artworks
of M. C. Escher also appear in the pitch space modelled by the neo-Riemannian
Tonnetz. This article makes these paradoxes visually apparent by constructing
an embodied model of triadic pitch space in accordance with principles drawn
from the mathematics of tiling, on the one hand, and from cognitive science,
on the other – specifically, the notion that our experience of pitch relationships
is governed in part by the metaphorical projection of patterns abstracted from
embodied experience known as image schemas. These paradoxes are illustrated
with reference to passages drawn from four compositions to whose expressive
character such paradoxes contribute: the fifteenth-century motet ‘Absalon fili
mi’; the finale of Haydn’s String Quartet in G major, Op. 76 No. 1; Brahms’s
Intermezzo in B minor, Op. 119 No. 1; and Wagner’s Parsifal.