Deniz 2020
Deniz 2020
Deniz 2020
https://www.emerald.com/insight/1741-0398.htm
Abstract
Purpose – Expert evaluation is the backbone of the multi-criteria decision-making (MCDM) techniques. The
experts make pairwise comparisons between criteria or alternatives in this evaluation. The mainstream
research focus on the ambiguity in this process and use fuzzy logic. On the other hand, cognitive biases are the
other but scarcely studied challenges to make accurate decisions. The purpose of this paper is to propose pilot
filters – as a debiasing strategy – embedded in the MCDM techniques to reduce the effects of framing effect, loss
aversion and status quo-type cognitive biases. The applicability of the proposed methodology is shown with
analytic hierarchy process-based Technique for Order-Preference by Similarity to Ideal Solution method
through a sustainable supplier selection problem.
Design/methodology/approach – The first filter’s aim is to reduce framing bias with restructuring the
questions. To manipulate the weights of criteria according to the degree of expected status quo and loss aversion
biases is the second filter’s aim. The second filter is implemented to a sustainable supplier selection problem.
Findings – The comparison of the results of biased and debiased ranking indicates that the best and worst
suppliers did not change, but the ranking of suppliers changed. As a result, it is shown that, to obtain more
accurate results, employing debiasing strategies is beneficial.
Originality/value – To the best of the author’s knowledge, this approach is a novel way to cope with the
cognitive biases. Applying this methodology easily to other MCDM techniques will help the decision makers to
take more accurate decisions.
Keywords Cognitive biases, Behavioral operations management, Heuristics, Debiasing, Decision-making,
Supplier selection, Sustainability
Paper type Research paper
1. Introduction
The quality of choice is a vital issue in which decision makers choose an alternative from a set
of options in multiple-criteria decision-making (MCDM) research. However the decision
maker’s relative preferences between pairwise alternatives or on elicited tradeoffs between
the criteria determine the result (Korhonen et al., 2018). As a real-life landfill location selection
problem, if the expert is a protector of wildlife, the expert cannot assign precise importance
degrees to some criteria (Ma et al., 2017). Contrary to the classic economic models which
assume that individuals act to maximize self-interest according to the “economic rationality,”
Simon’s studies proved that people make decisions that are inconsistent, that are not Pareto
efficient or that are based on normatively irrelevant factors. Therefore, it is advised to focus
on perceptual, psychological and cognitive factors that cause deviations from the “rational
man.” A human’s inability to evaluate decision alternatives simultaneously rather than
sequentially, tendency to select an alternative that is good enough rather than the optimal one
and use of simplifying rules or heuristics to reduce the cognitive demand of decision-making
are defined as decision maker’s weaknesses and limitations in the “bounded rationality”
Journal of Enterprise Information
Management
This study is presented in MCDM 2019 “25th International Conference on Multiple Criteria Decision © Emerald Publishing Limited
4.1 Filter 1
According to the “prospect theory” (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979), the wording of decision
alternatives affects choices by changing the reference point. When the same decision problem
is framed in different ways, it is shown that the relative attractiveness of options varies
according to the framing effect (Cheng and Wu, 2010). Buchanan and Corner (1997) suggested
that the choice of the starting point is important and influential. Korhonen and Wallenius
(1996) stated if allowed, decision makers may wish to reconsider alternatives once discarded.
The first reason of this cyclic behavior is the subjects’ comparison of alternatives in a relative
sense, rather than absolute sense. One of the other reason is subjects’ mistakes due to the
complexity of the decision task. Korhonen and Wallenius (1996) also indicate that the most
preferred solution may depend on the starting point and/or the path leading to the most
preferred solution. It is proved that the final choice is affected by “path” or sequence in which
solutions or settlements are presented to decision maker. According to the suggestions of
Korhonen and Wallenius (1996), to pay attention to the path and “framing” a problem are very
important. Taking into consideration this information and evidence, the first filter is
proposed to restructure the questions. Einstein’s “If I were given an hour in which to do a
problem upon which my life depended, I would spend 40 min studying it, 15 min reviewing it
and 5 min solving it” quote shows that the difficult part of decision-making is often not about
the right answer, but rather asking the right question (Ryan et al., 2018). To implement this
idea, it is proposed to rank the criteria in an alphabetical order in the first step. After this, it is
proposed to start not always with the starting point, but to change the starting point
sequentially to make the comparison pairs more balanced. The implementation of this two-
step approach is explained in the next section in detail.
4.2 Filter 2
MCDM problem’s solution involves two aspects: the evaluation of each decision alternative
regarding each criterion and the importance degree of each criterion. These two aspects need
to be investigated to model an MCDM problem with cognitive limitations (Ma et al., 2017). As
a result, the second filter depends on manipulating the weights according to the degree of
expected cognitive biases. Weights are defined as “scaling constants that represent value
tradeoffs and aggregate the partial values.” It is displayed that many common mistakes are
made in defining weights and several elicitation protocols for eliciting weights in an
appropriate way (Montibeller and Winterfeldt, 2015). Korhonen and Wallenius (1996) made
experimental research on individuals’ cognitive models. As a result of this study, it was found
that the relationship between weights and importance of criteria cannot be explained
exhaustively. In addition, Kim et al. (2014) showed that the weight of the framed attribute
becomes more important in the positive goal framing condition than in the negative. As an
example, it is proved that women decisions affected more than men decisions from cognitive
bias. Also, from the point of view that deciding faster will increase the bias risk, the response
time will be measured and will be used to manipulate the weights (Guo et al., 2017). Expertise
JEIM level and SQ effects are also quantified to manipulate the weights. The objective of the criteria
whether minimizing or maximizing is the other important factor to detect the gain–loss bias.
There is evidence about the dominance of cost which is a minimizing criterion (Buchanan and
Corner, 1997). After the extensive investigations on framing effects, it is shown that losses
loom larger than gains. This means, when alternatives are framed as gains, people display
risk aversion. On the other hand, people are risk-seeking when objectively equivalent
alternatives are framed as losses (Miu and Crişan, 2011).
Criteria number Criteria order (Haeri and Razei, 2019) Criteria order (alphabetical order)
1 Quality Cost
2 Cost Delivery speed
3 Delivery speed Green image
Table 1. 4 Green Image Pollution control
Criteria chosen and 5 Pollution control Quality
their alphabetical order 6 Worker safety
Comparison number Comparison pairs (actual) Comparison pairs (proposed)
Cognitive
biases in
1 1–2 1–2 MCDM
2 1–3 2–3
3 1–4 3–4 methods
4 1–5 4–5
5 1–6 5–6
6 2–3 6–1
7 2–4 5–2
8 2–5 4–6
9 2–6 3–5
10 3–4 2–4
11 3–5 1–3
12 3–6 6–2
13 4–5 3–6
14 4–6 4–1 Table 2.
15 5–6 5–1 Comparison pairs
the first criterion, and it is fixed. All the other criteria are compared with the first one. To make
the comparison pairs more balanced and reduce the framing effect, it is proposed to make
comparisons like in Column 3 of Table 2. The first criterion in the pair is always changing
sequentially in these alternative pairs.
As a result of changing the comparison pairs, the first and the second rank of the pairs
become more balanced. Table 3 shows this effect quantitatively. For example, the first
criterion is placed five times in the first rank in actual comparison, and this number decreases
with the growing number of criterion. Also, the first criterion is never placed in the second
rank in actual comparison, and this number increases with the growing number of criterion.
In accordance with the framing effect, this can result bias in decision-making. By the way,
with the proposed pairs, each criterion is placed in both the first and second rank two and
three times, respectively. These results show the proposed filter’s objectivity.
Actual Proposed
1st rank 2nd rank 1st rank 2nd rank
1 5 – 2 3
2 4 1 2 3
3 3 2 3 2
4 2 3 3 2
5 1 4 3 2 Table 3.
6 – 5 2 3 Effect of proposed
TOTAL 15 15 15 15 comparison pairs
JEIM obtained with the same MCDM techniques. The workflow diagram for Filter 2 is visualized in
Figure 1. The details are given below.
5.2.1 Biased ranking.
First step: determining the criteria weights with analytic hierarchy process
The weights of six criteria are determined with the AHP technique. As a result, 15 binary
comparisons are made by the author, and the weights of the six criteria are determined,
respectively, as 0.204, 0.323, 0.076, 0.179, 0.142 and 0.077.
Second step: obtaining supplier evaluations
Expert opinions are derived from the study of Haeri and Rezaei (2019) article in which five
experts are ranking five suppliers. Each expert’s evaluation expressed with linguistic terms
are transformed to numbers. For example “very good” statement transformed to 5, and
“good” statement transformed to 4. The other transformations are made as “medium” 5 3,
“poor” 5 2 and “very poor” 5 1. As an example, the second expert’s evaluations in terms of
numbers can be seen in Table 4.
Third step: generating group decision
The next step is getting the average value of each expert’s evaluation as a group decision.
This means all experts’ decisions are equally weighted. Because there are five experts, each
1. Determining criteria
1. Manipulating criteria 2. Determining experts’
weights (AHP)
weights (loss-aversion) expected bias degree
Second expert S1 S2 S3 S4 S5
1 – Quality 3 4 2 5 1
2 – Cost 2 2 2 4 3
3 – Delivery speed 3 4 2 3 3
Table 4. 4 – Green image 3 2 5 3 3
Second expert’s 5 – Pollution control 3 3 4 2 3
evaluations 6 – Worker safety 2 3 3 2 3
expert’s weight is determined as 0.2 (1/5). As a result, Table 5 is generated as the input for Cognitive
TOPSIS. biases in
Fourth step: ranking suppliers with Technique for Order-Preference by Similarity to Ideal MCDM
Solution methods
TOPSIS is a common MCDM technique used for ranking the alternatives. Its simplicity and
taking into consideration both ideal and anti-ideal solutions are the reasons of its popularity
(Behzadian et al., 2012). The inputs of TOPSIS are the weights obtained in the first step and
the expert group average generated in the third step. After the weighted normalization, both
ideal and negative ideal distances are calculated. The final ratios of each supplier are
obtained, respectively, 0.711, 1.096, 2.488, 0.041 and 0.707. According to the ranking of final
ratios (from biggest to smallest), the ranking of the suppliers is obtained as Supplier 3 (2.488)
> Supplier 2 (1.096) > Supplier 1 (0.711) > Supplier 5 (0.707) > Supplier 4 (0.041).
5.2.2 Debiased ranking.
First step: manipulating criteria weights
It was shown that decision makers’ choices are affected from the “loss aversion effect.” The
criteria weights verify this finding as the highest weight is the cost’s weight. As a debiasing
technique, it is proposed to distribute the difference of the first (w1 5 wmax) and the second
(w2) highest weight to other criteria proportionally to the initial weights. Each criterion’s
debiased weights are calculated via equation (1). wdi represents the debiased weight of each
criterion and wi represents the biased weight of each criterion in this equation:
Table 6 shows the result of this process and criteria’s debiased weights.
Second step: determining experts’ bias degrees
This step is added to the previous one to determine the expert’s bias degrees. In this step, two
factors are used to determine the degree of the experts: “expertise level” and “response time.”
As Korhonen et al. (2018) generated their problem with random integers, aforementioned
factors are randomly created with discrete simulation in MS Excel via equation (2):
C1 0.204 0.239
C2 0.323 0.204
C3 0.076 0.089
C4 0.179 0.210 Table 6.
C5 0.142 0.167 Initial and debiased
C6 0.077 0.091 weights
JEIM ¼ IFðC2 < 0:2; 1; IFðC2 < 0:4; 2; IFðC2 < 0:6; 3; IFðC2 < 0:8; 4; 5ÞÞÞÞ (2)
The decision rules about weighting according to the “expertise level” and “response time” are
as follows:
(1) The bigger expertise level, the bias will be lower. The expert’s weight will be higher.
(2) The lower response time, the bias will be higher. The expert’s weight will be lower.
Then, expertise level and response time degrees are normalized with dividing the degrees to
total degree. In the last step, as it can be seen in the last column of Table 7, total bias is
determined according to the average of both normalized levels.
Third step: obtaining supplier evaluations
It is aimed to take the SQ effect into consideration in this step. In accordance with the SQ
effect, there is an assumption that the decision makers have a tendency to stick to a given
default option (Geng, 2016). Due to the lack of information about the previous decisions of the
experts, the SQ effect is derived from expert opinions indirectly. An expert’s total score given
for each supplier is calculated and assumed that the biggest one is the previous supplier. To
add the SQ effect, it is experienced to reduce this supplier’s degrees one less. Because of the
highest total score of the second expert is 19 (fourth supplier), the degrees of the fourth
supplier’s column reduced one less and transformed to next column called “4 (debiased)”
(Table 8).
Fourth step: generating group decision
To generate group decision, experts’ decisions are weighted according to the bias degree
shown in the last column of Table 7. As a result, Table 9 is generated to be the input for
TOPSIS.
Fifth step: ranking suppliers with Technique for Order-Preference by Similarity to Ideal
Solution
Equally
Expert Expertise Response Response time Normalized Normalized weighted
No level time transformed expertise level response time Total bias
1 – Quality 3 4 2 5 4 1
2 – Cost 2 2 2 4 3 3
3 – Delivery speed 3 4 2 3 2 3
4 – Green image 3 2 5 3 2 3
5 – Pollution control 3 3 4 2 1 3
Table 8. 6 – Worker safety 2 3 3 2 1 3
SQ effect reduction TOTAL 16 18 18 19 13 16
Table 9 related to the expert group is used for TOPSIS input. The ratios obtained from the Cognitive
TOPSIS result of each supplier’s are shown in Table 10 compared with the biased version. biases in
According to the final ratios (from biggest to smallest), the ranking of the suppliers is
obtained as Supplier 3 > Supplier 1 > Supplier 2 > Supplier 5 > Supplier 4.
MCDM
Haeri and Rezaei (2019) obtained Supplier 3 > Supplier 5 > Supplier 2 > Supplier methods
1 > Supplier 4 ranking using an integrated gray relational degrees using an interval analysis
approach. As it can be seen in Table 10, for the biased and debiased versions, the best and the
worst suppliers did not change, but the ranking is changed. Supplier 5’s rank decreased from
second to fourth, and vice versa. MCDM techniques are used for not only choosing the best
alternative, but also ranking and classifying the alternatives. The bias effect gains more
important in such cases. Additionally, in another implementation, the best choice can be
changed according to the bias degree.
S1 0.711 1.221
S2 1.096 1.178
S3 2.488 1.436
S4 0.041 0.011 Table 10.
S5 0.707 0.690 AHP-based TOPSIS
Ranking S3 > S2 > S1 > S5 > S4 S3 > S1 > S2 > S5 > S4 results
JEIM education and law (Morewedge et al., 2015). Debiasing studies are crucial to improve the
decision-making quality and performance (Cheng et al., 2014).
Unfortunately, there are limitations of this study. First of all, this study is an extended
version of a proceeding, and because of the time and resource limitations, data are obtained
randomly. To make experiments to verify the proposed methodology is one of the future
direction of this study. It is seen that the best practices are employed by decision and risk
analysts in debiasing, but few of them have been tested experimentally (Montibeller and
Winterfeldt, 2015). Borrero and Henao (2017) suggest that a totally rational decision-making
process – and therefore, a completely objective, analytical, tool-supported and evidence-based
management might be a utopic endeavor. In this experiments to reduce the SQ effect, to take
the last ten choices of each experts will be a more objective way. Also expertise level and
response time can be monitored quantitatively. As Morton and Fasolo (2009), sensitivity
analysis is omitted in this study, but the next step of this research is making sensitivity
analysis.
As a future direction, this novel debiasing strategy can be developed by adding other
types of biases that affect decisions. Indeed, not only the effects of some biases but also the
joint effect of the bias groups need to be studied. It is also interesting to study other MCDM
methods such as ELECTRE, PROMETHEE, ARAS, etc. By this way, the ultimate goal is to
find the most resistant technique to biases can be found. The results of these kind of studies
will show us which MCDM technique is more resistant to which cognitive bias. As a
consequence, before evaluation, we can test ourselves according to inherent biases and
choose the MCDM techniques according to this result and hybridize them. Also, this
technique can be applied to other decision problems. Especially in health-related decisions, it
is highly important. The other implementation area is ranking the nominees who applied a
job. In the literature, it was shown that there are some moderators such as decision makers’
self-esteem, neuroticism, mode of processing and gender. As an example, the framing effect
regarding risky products was found larger for those in a negative mood, and women exhibit
greater framing effects than men (Fagley et al., 2010). Location selection is another kind of
decision problem. Another avenue for future research relates to the gaps in the literature
about the antecedents of cognitive biases like individual differences (such as gender) and
emotions (such as such as apprehension, fear or anxiety) (Fagley et al., 2010). To reduce the
bias, understanding the underlying psychological process is crucial (Lee, 2019). As decision-
making is a complex activity that is influenced by many factors (Korhonen et al., 2018),
cognitive biases’ antecedents need to be analyzed. Considerable empirical and experimental
evidence in the behavioral psychology literature testifies to the existence of the bias in human
decision-making. As the BDT is a sub-discipline of psychology (Morton and Fasolo, 2009) and
according to the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, activities of emotion-
related brain regions, such as the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex (Osmont et al., 2015),
biases in decision-making need to be handled in an interdisciplinary perspective.
References
Almashat, S., Ayotte, B., Edelstein, B. and Margrett, J. (2008), “Framing effect debiasing in medical
decision making”, Patient Education and Counseling, Vol. 71, pp. 102-107.
Arabsheybani, A., Paydar, M.M. and Safaei, A.S. (2018), “An integrated fuzzy MOORA method and
FMEA technique for sustainable supplier selection considering quantity discounts and
supplier’s risk”, Journal of Cleaner Production, Vol. 190, pp. 577-591.
Bazerman, M.H. and Tenbrunsel, A. (1998), “The role of social context on decisions: integrating social
cognition and behavioral decision research”, Basic and Applied Social Psychology, Vol. 20 No. 1,
pp. 87-91.
Behzadian, M., Otaghsara, S.K., Yazdani, M. and Ignatius, J. (2012), “A state-of the-art survey of Cognitive
TOPSIS applications”, Expert Systems with Applications, Vol. 39, pp. 13051-13069.
biases in
Borrero, S. and Henao, F. (2017), “Can managers be really objective? Bias in multicriteria decision
analysis”, Academy of Strategic Management Journal, Vol. 16 No. 1, pp. 244-259.
MCDM
Braverman, J.A. and Blumenthal-Barby, J.S. (2012), “Assessment of the sunk-cost effect in clinical
methods
decision-making”, Social Science and Medicine, Vol. 75, pp. 186-192.
Buchanan, J.T. and Corner, J.L. (1997), “The effects of anchoring in interactive MCDM solution
methods”, Computers Ops Res, Vol. 24 No. 10, pp. 907-918.
Cheng, F. and Wu, C. (2010), “Debiasing the framing effect: the effect of warning and involvement”,
Decision Support Systems, Vol. 49, pp. 328-334.
Cheng, F.F., Wu, C.S. and Lin, H.H. (2014), “Reducing the influence of framing on internet consumers’
decisions: the role of elaboration”, Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 37, pp. 56-63.
Croskerry, P., Singha, G. and Mamede, S. (2013a), “Cognitive debiasing 1: origins of bias and theory of
debiasing”, BMJ Qual Saf, Vol. 22, pp. 58-64.
Croskerry, P., Singha, G. and Mamede, S. (2013b), “Cognitive debiasing 2: impediments to and
strategies for change”, BMJ Qual Saf, Vol. 22, pp. 65-72.
Deniz, N. (2018), “Supplier selection problem in the behavioral operations management context”, Proceedings
of XIII Balkan Conference on Operational Research, held at Belgrade, Serbia during May 25-28, p. 47,
available at: http://balcor2018.fon.bg.ac.rs/download/BALCOR2018BookOfAbstracts.pdf.
Fagley, N.S., Coleman, J.G. and Simon, A.F. (2010), “Effects of framing, perspective taking, and
perspective (affective focus) on choice”, Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 48,
pp. 264-269.
Frydman, C. and Rangel, A. (2014), “Debiasing the disposition effect by reducing the saliency
ofinformation about a stock’s purchase price”, Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization,
Vol. 107, pp. 541-552.
Geng, S. (2016), “Decision time, consideration time, and status quo bias”, Economic Inquiry, Vol. 54
No. 1, pp. 433-449.
Guo, L., Trueblood, J.S. and Diederich, A. (2017), “Thinking Fast increases framing effects in risky
decision making”, Psychological Science, Vol. 28 No. 4, pp. 530-543.
Haeri, S. and Rezaei, J. (2019), “A grey-based green supplier selection model for uncertain
environments”, Journal of Cleaner Production, Vol. 221, pp. 768-784.
Haita-Falah, C. (2017), “Sunk-cost fallacy and cognitive ability in individual decision-making”, Journal
of Economic Psychology, Vol. 58, pp. 44-59.
Huang, H.H., Su, H.J. and Chang, C.J. (2015), “The moderating effect of a no-choice option on risky
framing effect”, Asia Pacific Management Review, Vol. 20, pp. 18-23.
Hwang, C.L. and Yoon, K. (1981), Multiple Attribute Decision Making: Methods and Applications,
Springer-Verlag, New York.
Kahneman, D. (2013), Thinking Fast and Slow, Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1979), “Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk”,
Econometrica, Vol. 47 No. 2, pp. 263-292.
Kim, J., Kim, J. and Marshall, R. (2014), “Search for the underlying mechanism of framing effects in
multi-alternative and multi-attribute decision situations”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 67,
pp. 378-385.
Korhonen, P. and Wallenius, J. (1996), “Letter to the editor behavioural issues in MCDM: neglected
research questions”, Journal of Multi-Criteria Decision Analysis, Vol. 5, pp. 178-182.
Korhonen, P.J., Malo, P., Pajala, T., Ravaja, N., Somervuori, O. and Wallenius, J. (2018), “Decision
Support Context matters: the impact of product type, emotional attachment and information
overload on choice quality”, European Journal of Operational Research, Vol. 264, pp. 270-279.
JEIM Lee, K.K. (2019) “An indirect debiasing method: priming a target attribute reduces judgmental biases
in likelihood estimations”, PloS One, Vol. 14 No. 3, pp. 1-13.
Ma, W., Luo, X. and Jiang, Y. (2017), “Multicriteria decision making with cognitive limitations: a DS/
AHP-based approach”, International Journal of Intelligent Systems, Vol. 32, pp. 686-721.
Miu, A.C. and Crişan, L.G. (2011), “Cognitive reappraisal reduces the susceptibility to the framing
effect in economic decision making”, Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 51,
pp. 478-482.
Montibeller, G. and Winterfeldt, D. (2015), “Cognitive and motivational biases in decision and risk
analysis”, Risk Analysis, Vol. 35 No. 7, pp. 1230-1251.
Morewedge, C.K., Yoon, H., Scopelliti, I., Symborski, C.W., Korris, J.H. and Kassam, K.S. (2015),
“Debiasing decisions: improved decision making with a single training intervention”,
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Vol. 2 No. 1, pp. 129-140.
Morton, A. and Fasolo, B. (2009). “Behavioural decision theory for multi-criteria decision analysis: a
guided tour”, Journal of the Operational Research Society, Vol. 60 No. 2, pp. 268-275.
Osmont, A., Cassotti, M., Agogue, M., Houde, O. and Moutier, S. (2015), “Does ambiguity aversion
influence the framing effect during decision making?”, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,
Vol. 22, pp. 572-577.
Pandey, R. and Jessica, V.M. (2018), “Measuring behavioral biases affecting real estate investment
decisions in India: using IRT”, International Journal of Housing Markets and Analysis, Vol. 11
No. 4, pp. 648-668.
Perfecto, H., Nelson, L.D. and Moore, D.A. (2018), “The category size bias: a mere misunderstanding”,
Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 13 No. 2, pp. 170-184.
Qian, X., Fang, S.C., Huang, M. and Wang, X. (2019), “Winner determination of loss-averse buyers with
incomplete information in multiattribute reverse auctions for clean energy device procurement”,
Energy, Vol. 177, pp. 276-292.
Ryan, A., Duignan, S., Kenny, D. and McMahon, C.J. (2018), “Decision making in paediatric
cardiology. Are we prone to heuristics, biases and traps?”, Pediatric Cardiology, Vol. 39,
pp. 160-167.
Saaty, R.W. (2016), Decision Making in Complex Environments- the Analytic Network Process (ANP)
for Dependence and Feedback, Super Decisions, Pittsburgh.
Sellier, A.L., Scopelliti, I. and Morewedge, C.K. (2019), “Debiasing training improves decision making
in the field”, Psychological Science, Vol. 30 No. 9, pp. 1371-1379.
Shu, S. (2018), “Debias human judgment with a common decision bias: an experimental study of the
anchoring effect in hiring decision-making”, A Dissertation Thesis of PhD, University of
Minnesota Digital Conservancy, available at: http://hdl.handle.net/11299/199016.
Soman, D. and Liu, M.W. (2011), “Debiasing or rebiasing? Moderating the illusion of delayed
incentives”, Journal of Economic Psychology, Vol. 32, pp. 307-316.
Pamucar, D., Puska, A. and Chatterjee, P. (2020), “Sustainable supplier selection in
Stevic, Z.,
healthcare industries using a new MCDM method: measurement of alternatives and ranking
according to COmpromise solution (MARCOS)”, Computers and Industrial Engineering,
Vol. 140, pp. 1-15.
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1974), “Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases”, Science
New Series, Vol. 185 No. 4, pp. 1124-1131.
Walmsley, S. and Gilbey, A. (2016), “Cognitive biases in visual pilots’ weather-related decision
making”, Applied Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 30, pp. 532-543.
Zimmer, K., Fr€ohling, M. and Schultmann, F. (2016), “Sustainable supplier management – a review of
models supporting sustainable supplier selection, monitoring and development”, International
Journal of Production Research, Vol. 54 No. 5, pp. 1412-1442.
About the author Cognitive
Nurcan Deniz is an Assistant Professor Doctor of Operations Management and Marketing at Eskisehir
Osmangazi University, Turkey. She received her BS degrees from the Industrial Engineering and biases in
Chemical Engineering (Double Major) Department of the Eskisehir Osmangazi University, in 2005 and MCDM
2006. Also, she graduated from Business Administration Open Faculty in Anadolu University (2005). methods
Her MSc degrees are from Statistics (2009) and Industrial Engineering (2015). Her first PhD degree is
from Business Administration (2016). Her main research interests are in the areas of supply chain
management, service quality and lean production. She also continues her second PhD in Industrial
Engineering Department. Nurcan Deniz is the corresponding author and can be contacted at: ndeniz@
ogu.edu.tr
For instructions on how to order reprints of this article, please visit our website:
www.emeraldgrouppublishing.com/licensing/reprints.htm
Or contact us for further details: permissions@emeraldinsight.com