Benson GodImageFunction 1973
Benson GodImageFunction 1973
Benson GodImageFunction 1973
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
Wiley and Society for the Scientific Study of Religion are collaborating with JSTOR to
digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion
Department of Psychology
Eastern Michigan University
BERNARD SPILKA
Department of Psychology
University of Denver
A cognitive consistency framework was adopted to predict that a believer's level of self-
esteem and his location on the locus of control dimension influence his description and
definition of God. On a sample of 128 Catholic subjects with approximately identical reli-
gious backgrounds, self-esteem was positively related to loving-accepting God-images and
negatively to rejecting images. Locus of control was unrelated to controlling beliefs. Statis-
tical and methodological controls were utilized to offer an interpretation that self-esteem
may be a major determinant of God-images. It was proposed that these findings have
important implications for understanding the dynamics of personal religion.
297
METHOD
Subjects
Two hundred and five male Ss attending a Catholic high school were initially
tested. In order to obtain a highly homogeneous religious sample, Ss were selected
from this subject pool who met the following criteria: S considered himself Cath-
olic, both parents were Catholic, S had never been a member of a different denom-
ination, had never associated with a non-Catholic religious organization, and had
been a member of a local Catholic parish for at least the last ten years. This final
criterion was adopted as a control for geographical differences in theological
emphasis. In addition, only Ss who regarded religion as personally important
(above the midpoint on a nine-point semantic differential item) and who desig-
nated that they believed in God (above the midpoint on a seven-point item) were
used. These two items were utilized to eliminate Ss who might be religious
"detractors."
One hundred and twenty-eight Ss met these criteria. The final sample had the
following educational distribution: 44 Freshmen, 31 Sophomores, 19 Juniors,
and 34 Seniors. Mean S age was 15.4 years.
Tests
The first 23 items were selected from Coopersmith's (1967) original 50 items
to form a self-esteem scale. On a sample of 55 Catholic youth, this short form
correlated highly with the other 27 items (r = .78). Consistent with Coopersmith's
scoring procedures, each high self-esteem response was multiplied by four. Thus,
the maximum score on this scale was 92. The original scale has been widely used
with children and adolescents. It is concerned with self-attitudes in four areas
found to be important at these age levels: peers, parents, schools, and personal
interests. The short-form used in this study contains items related to each of
these areas. Rotter's (1966) 23-item scale was used as a measure of internal-
external control. One point accrued for each external option marked, so that the
maximum score on the scale was 23. External control was designated as a high
score on the scale, and internal control signified a low score.
Single items were used to obtain information regarding age, grade, father's
occupation, and religious behavior. These were included to measure possible
extraneous influences on the proposed personality and religion relationships.
Father's occupation was used as an indicant of socioeconomic status (SES), based
on procedures developed by Edwards (Miller, 1964). Occupations were categor-
ized into one of six SES levels. From high to low status, these levels were: pro-
fessional or technical, business managers or proprietors, clerical or sales workers,
craftsmen or foremen, operatives, and unskilled. On this variable, a score of one
was assigned to the highest status and a six was given to the lowest status. The
three religious behavior items were used to measure frequency of devotions at
home, frequency of religious discussions at home, and hours spent per month in
church or church-related activities. Each question had six options scored 0 to 5
with 5 corresponding to the highest level of activity.
RESULTS
TABLE I
7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16
4. Frequency of religious
discussions 19* -18* 23* -14 -02 -01 -05 04 06 01.
5. Church hours per month 09 -19* 19* -04 -06 -04 -07 -10 10 -07
7. Self-esteem -32** 51** -35** -49** -21* -23** -17* 31** -18*
to Controlling God (r = .30, p <.01), and to Stern Father (r = .45, p <.01). These
intercorrelations suggest that impersonality, rigid control, and vindictiveness may
have been to some degree perceived as dimensions or components of nonlove. Ac-
cordingly, they appeared antithetic to loving images. The negatively significant
correlations between Loving God and Impersonal Allness (r = -.32, p <.01), Im-
personal Distant (r = -.30, p <.01), Controlling God (r = -.30, p <.01), Stern Father
(r = -.21, p <.01), and Vindictive God (r = -.30, p <.01) support this contention.
Self-esteem was also correlated negatively with Impersonal Distant (r = -.17,
p <.05) and Impersonal Supreme Ruler (r = -.18, p <.05). Overall, self-esteem
was significantly related to all eight God measures. The pattern suggests that
self-esteem is related positively to loving images and negatively to rejecting-
impersonal-controlling views, thereby supporting both Hypotheses 1 and 2.
The data do not support Hypothesis 3. Locus of control was unrelated to the
Controlling God scale (r = .02). It was however, significantly related to three
other measures: with Loving God (r = -.28, p <.01), Vindictive God (r = .23,
p <.01), and Impersonal Allness (r = .18, p< .05). In all three instances, the direc-
tion of the relationship is opposite that obtained with self-esteem. One possible
explanation is that a high score on the locus of control measure, which is an exter-
nal orientation, is a measure of low self-esteem. The negative and significant
relationship between self-esteem and locus of control (r = -.32, p <.01) tentatively
supports this interpretation. It is also possible that the Controlling God scale is
more sensitive to a loving-nonloving dimension than to a controlling-noncontrol-
ling one. The intercorrelations among the scales support this contention. For
example, the Controlling God index correlated negatively with both Loving God
(r = -.30, p <.01) and Kindly Father (r = -.22, p <.05), and positively with
Vindictive God (r = .30, p <.01) and Stern Father (r = .31, p <.0 1).
The three religious behavior items were related to internal control and one was
associated positively with self-esteem. Frequency of religious discussions corre-
lated with both self-esteem (r = .19, p <.05) and internal control (r = -18, p <.05).
Frequency of devotions (r = -.30, p <.01) and hours spent in church activities
(r = .-19, p <.05) were both significantly related to internal control.
for questioning explanations that the personality and religion relationships are
spurious. However, the removal of these four correlates from self-esteem in those
cases where their relationship to religious measures is minimal is quite useful. In
these situations, partial correlation techniques clarify the amount of variance that
is uniquely shared by self-esteem and the religious variables. In essence, a partial
correlation in such cases reduces to a part correlation. Age, grade, and SES did not
correlate significantly with self-esteem. While it is doubtful that extracting these
variables will lead to noteworthy changes in the self-esteem-religious variable
correlations, doing so reveals whether these measures function as "suppressors."
Since in a few cases grade and SES correlate negatively (though nonsignificantly)
with the religious variables, they may possibly suppress the self-esteem-religion
relationships. Age and grade are highly correlated in this sample (.95); thus
controlling for grade level essentially controls for age.
Table 1 also reveals that the locus of control and religious variable relation-
ships could be influenced by six variables. Four that correlated with both locus of
control and with some of the religious variables are frequency of devotions, fre-
quency of discussions, hours spent in church, and self-esteem. These were also
submitted to a partial correlation analysis. A fifth variable, SES, correlated signifi-
cantly with locus of control, but with none of the religious measures. A partial
analysis of this variable functions primarily to remove SES from locus of control.
Grade level was also partialled out for reasons similar to the case with self-esteem
discussed above.
In summary, six variables were partialled from self-esteem and the religious
measures, and from internal-external control and the same indices. If relationships
between the self-regarding attitudes and the religious measures exist after the
partial correlation analysis, it can be argued that the explanation that the associa-
tions are spurious has been seriously questioned. However, a note of caution is in
order. Two factors make it invalid to dismiss the "spurious" explanation in any
absolute sense. First, the partial correlation does not simply represent the corre-
lation between two variables when a third variable has been held constant. Rather,
it can be interpreted "as a weighted average of the correlation coefficients that
would have been obtained had the control variable been divided into very small
intervals and separate correlations computed within each of the categories"
(Blalock, 1972: 436). Therefore, the resulting partial correlation is affected by the
ranges of the scores obtained on the controlled variables. In the present study,
these ranges may have been restricted by the characteristics of the subjects sam-
pled. Therefore, if relationships exist after the partials, it can only be said that
the variables were controlled within certain restricted boundaries. Second, since
only six variables were partialled in each analysis, other uninvestigated variables
could possibly affect the self-regarding attitude and religion associations. Ob-
viously, it is impossible to absolutely dismiss a "spurious" explanation of the
data, since it is impossible to control for all correlates. At best, it can be argued
that the variables actually controlled in a study were the ones that could have the
greatest influence, based on theoretical or empirical information.
Tables 2 and 3 contain the results that emerge from these statistical controls.
Before the partial correlation analysis, self-esteem was related significantly to all
eight God measures. After all six variables (internal-external control, frequency
of discussions, frequency of devotions, church participation rates, SES and grade)
were removed, self-esteem remained signiflcantly related to six measures. Rela-
tionships to Impersonal Distant and Impersonal Supreme Ruler did not attain the
.05 level of signiflcance. In both instances, however, the amount of shared variance
declined negligibly. This suggests that these relationships were in fact minimal
before the partial analysis was undertaken. The Loving God and self-esteem
relationship changed from .51 to .47, indicating that the amount of shared variance
fell from 26 percent to 22 percent. The correlation remains significant at the .01
level.
In all other cases the reduction in shared variance was 3 percent or less. Over-
all, it appears that the partialling of these six variables did not appreciably affect
TABLE 2
After After
After Devotions Devotions
After Devotions Discussions Discussions
Partials Discussions Attendance Attendance
Before on all and Atten- and I-E I-E and SES
Religious Variable Partials Variables dance Removed Removed Removed
the original relationships. This could have been expected for all associations other
than that of selfesteem and Loving God. In these other situations, the original
correlations between the God measure and the partialled variables were small.
Potentially, the greatest change could have involved Loving God since four of the
six partialled variables correlated with this variable. Nevertheless this relationship
remained intact.
Self-esteem remains positively correlated with Loving God (r = .47, p <.01)
and Kindly Father (r = .29, p <.01) and negatively with Controlling God (r = -.37,
p <.01), Vindictive God (r = -.46, p <.01), Stern Father (r = -.19, p <.05), and
Impersonal Ailness (r = -.20, p <.05). These data, then, are consistent with the
hypothesis that self-esteem influences these God-images. The influence can be
understood in terms of both loving and nonloving images. It can be argued that this
is a reasonable explanation since the data were obtained on subjects with highly
similar backgrounds after a number of correlates were controlled.
TABLE 3
After After
Devotions, Devotions,
After Discussions, Discussions,
After Devotions, Attendance Attendance,
Partials Discussions, and Self-esteem,
Before on all and Atten- Self-esteem and SES
Religious Variable Partials Variables dance Removed Removed Removed
The partial correlation analysis was more influential in the case of locus of
control and God-belief relationships. Before the analysis, locus of control was
significantly correlated with three of the measures, although not with Controlling
God as predicted. After the analysis, all three relationships failed to reach the .05
level of significance. The original associations with Loving God, Vindictive God,
and Impersonal Allness reduce to .10 or less. Table 3 shows that the removal of
self-esteem after the initial removal of the three religious behavior items was most
responsible for these changes. This step accounted for a 3 percent reduction in the
variance originally shared by locus of control and Loving God, and a 4 percent
reduction in variance shared with Vindictive God. Self-esteem may therefore be
an important component of locus of control. In summary, it does not appear that
locus of control influences God-images, at least as they are measured in this study.
DISCUSSION
The major finding of this study is that self-esteem is related positively to loving
God-images and negatively to rejecting-impersonal-controlling definitions of God.
Furthermore, it is argued that the data provide some support for the explanation
that self-esteem is a major determinant of God-images. This interpretation was
made after a consideration of the alternatives. By obtaining the results on Ss with
nearly identical religious training, it was posited that a "belief influencing person-
ality" position was seriously questioned as the primary explanation for the find-
ings. And by partialling variables dealing with internal-external control, SES,
grade, and frequency of participation in religious activities, it was further sug-
gested that explanations based on the premise that both self-esteem and God-
images were the common effects of other variables were considerably weakened.
By the processes of elimination, then, it seems appropriate to argue that self-
esteem influences God-images. Several other explanations of the data need to be
investigated, and refuted, in order to maximize the credibility of this position.
Other religious training factors need to be controlled. Differences in parish empha-
ses, teachers, clergy, and reading material could possibly affect beliefs, which in
turn influence self-esteem. Parents and peers may influence both God-images and
self-esteem. For example, rejecting parents might induce low self-esteem, and as
previous research suggests (Siegman, 1961; Strunk, 1959), Ss may define God in
terms similar to the way they view their parents (which could include a rejecting
image in this case). It may also be true that those with low self-esteem engage in
problem behaviors which elicit from parents and other authorities fear-provoking
religious concepts used in an attempt to control the behavior. Accordingly, these
Ss could receive a different religious "treatment" than those with high self-esteem.
Thus, low self-esteem could be paired with rejecting God concepts.
While such processes may influence God-images and self-esteem in certain
individual cases, it is proposed that they are not of sufficient general strength to
account for any large portion of shared variance between self-esteem and God-
images in a relatively large sample.
Other issues need to be raised. While the reported data suggest a broad inter-
pretation of self-esteem influencing God-images, the results do not provide many
details about the relationship. Table 4 presents frequencies and percentages of
Loving God scale scores at three levels of self-esteem. The average scale score
among all 128 Ss was 25.3. Ss were divided into low, medium, and high levels of
self-esteem. The first row shows that 79 percent of the "highs" scored above the
mean (scale score of 26-30) while only 30 percent of the "lows" scored similarly.
A chi square performed on scores above and below the mean suggests that "highs"
tend to perceive God as significantly more loving than "lows" (X 2 = 21.45, p<
.001). Additionally, scores below 20 occurred only in the "low" group. Twenty-
eight percent of the "lows" scored between nine and 20 points. Twenty-three per-
cent of the "highs" and only 5 percent of the "lows" scored the maximum 30
points.
TABLE 4
Level of Self-esteem
Above 25.3 13 26 34
(30%)' (62%) (79%)
Below 25.3 30 16 9
(70%) (38%) (21%)
30 (Maximum scale score) 2 5 10
5%) (12%) (23%)
Below 20 12 0 0
(28%)
Mean Scale Score 22.40 26.21 27.23
The combination of these figures and the previously cited correlations suggests
that a majority of Ss tend to perceive God in a way that is congruent with self-
image. Some God-images, however, do not fit this consistency pattern. Twenty-
three percent of the "lows" score above 26 points. This may indicate that some
Ss are motivated to attain approval rather than consistency. Persons with high
self-esteem can meet consistency and approval needs simultaneously, while per-
sons with low self-esteem find these needs in conflict. A highly accepting God
communicates approval, while a more rejecting God preserves consistency. In this
context, it would be important to explicate what factors determine which option
is chosen, and whether or not these needs operate for the same S in different
situations.
Interpreting how individuals actually develop these God-images is difficult.
Several possibilities exist. "Highs" may internalize the preached axioms of a given
theology. Accordingly, "lows" then tend to change or modify this theology to con-
form more consistently with their self-images. It could also be, however, that
individuals are exposed to a variety of religious teachings. These teachings could
variously present God as loving and punishing, supernatural and immanent, warm
and impersonal. Thus, the believer may select the set of preached God-images
which is most consistent.
The nonsignificant correlations between locus of control and God beliefs
merit discussion. Rotters (1966) scale defines external control in terms of luck,
fate, and chance. While it seems reasonable to argue that one who places his fate
in God's hands is externally controlled, he may find that options phrased in luck
and chance terminology are irrelevant. Relationships between locus of control
and God-images could have been masked by this measurement problem.
The consequences of holding a controlling God-image warrants further
research. Belief in a God who controls the destiny of human events may lead an
individual to view his own behavior as inconsequential for social change. Seeman's
(1959) work on alienation implies that external control and powerlessness are
conceptually similar. This may partially explain why some church members refuse
to engage in social action and conform to approved expectations in order to gain
some feeling of control.
SOME IMPLICATIONS
It has often been charged that religious individuals use religion for purposes
of comfort-at the expense of challenging and changing the secular world (Glock,
Ringer & Babbie, 1967). If this criticism is valid, where should the blame be
placed?
One possible explanation lies in the conservative emphasis of religious institu-
tions. As O'Dea (1966) has indicated, organized institutions tend to be concerned
with stability and continuity. In the service of self-maintenance, such institutions
attempt to avoid conflict. Consequently, members may learn that a life of faith is
personal and not social. Theology can be presented to justify such a nonsocial
orientation.
The data from the present study, however, suggest that the problem may also
have its source in the psychological processes of the individual. Low self-esteem
may lead one to resist a loving God and a compassionate ethical orientation, even
when both are emphasized in a particular institutional setting. Assuming that
self-esteem influences the content of religious beliefs, we can hypothesize that
religious belief functions in part to maintain or preserve one's level of self-esteem.
Belief justifies good or bad self-images, providing relief from the struggle to find
meaningful explanations for one's identity. One's self-esteem level affects what
can and cannot be accepted as a tenet of faith. Church members with high self-
esteem can understand and appreciate a loving, accepting God-and may also
apply principles of love and acceptance to various social settings. Persons with
low self-esteem, however, find both God and ethics based on love and acceptance
as uncomfortably inconsistent. Consequently, their needs lead them to replace
these images with a more rejecting God who does not command a loving orienta-
tion to the world. Thus, some of the social complacency attributed to church
members may reflect personal inhibitions rather than church policy. This problem
may be immense, if as previous research indicates, self-concept variables corre-
late negatively with religious attitudes and involvement (Cowen, 1954; Strunk,
1958).
While such an inference implies that level of self-esteem is an antecedent to
specific belief content, it should be recognized that theology might (if known by
the member) also influence self-concept. Perhaps self-concept and belief are an-
tinomously related, where there is continual interaction between these variables.
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES