Neurocognitive Functioning in Alcohol Use Disorder
Neurocognitive Functioning in Alcohol Use Disorder
Background characteristics
Gender, female/male 4/11 27/73 10/10 50/50 1.94 1 0.163 –
MIAMI, Momentary Influences, Attitudes and Motivation Impact on Cognitive Performance Scale (higher scores designate greater
impairment).
quickly as possible. Part B assesses set-shifting ability; the partici- as fast as possible. Norm values derived from a large sample of par-
pant has to alternate between numbers and letters in ascending ticipants were applied for median reaction times and number of
order (i.e., 1-A-2-B, etc.). Age-adapted norm scores were applied omissions [32].
[29].
Selective Attention Subtest from the Test for
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [30] Attentional Performance [32]
The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [31] was used in a com- For this selective attention test, participants were consecutively
puterized version to assess executive functioning. Partici- shown either a “+” (distractor) or “×” (target). If the target stimu-
pants were presented a maximum of 128 cards with colored (i.e., lus appeared, the participant had to press a button as fast as pos-
red, blue, yellow, or green) symbols (i.e., stars, rectangles, tri- sible. Norm scores for median reaction times and errors from a
angles, or crosses) in different frequencies (i.e., 1–4). Cards had large German population were applied [32].
to be matched according to 3 principles that–unbeknownst to
the participant–changed during the course of the assessment Block Design from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
(i.e., color, symbols/shape, number of items). A tone (high vs. Scale–4th Edition [33, 34]
low) and a corresponding verbal cue indicated whether a selec- Individuals were asked to match colored cubes to a two-dimen-
tion was correct or incorrect. The dependent variables were the sional pattern as quickly as possible. Scaled scores from a large
number of categories completed (i.e., 0–6) and perseverative er- German population were applied [34].
rors.
Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) [35]
Divided Attention Subtest from the Test for We measured learning capacity and retention with the Ger-
Attentional Performance [32] man version of the AVLT [36]. A list of 15 words was read to the
For this test, participants needed to perform 2 tasks concur- participants 5 times. At the end of each trial, the individuals had
rently. Whenever asterisks formed a rectangle on a 4 × 4 dot matrix to repeat as many words as they could recall. Then, a new (inter-
(optical target) or 2 tones of the same frequency (high or low) were ference) list was read and had to be remembered. Afterward,
heard (acoustical target), the participant had to press a space bar items from the first list had to be recalled, immediately afterwards
130.237.122.245 - 8/29/2018 1:19:10 PM
Karolinska Institutet, University Library
Speed
Trail-making test A, s 44.00 18.04 31.33P 28.95 8.98 53.95P 47/10 3.00 19.20 0.008 1.11 0.149 0.52
Divided attention RT auditory, ms 726.40 292.71 39.47T 618.00 90.65 41.60T 60/40 1.38 16.03 0.185 0.53 0.469 0.26
Divided attention RT visual, ms 944.53 193.03 43.00T 808.25 116.14 51.10T 40/15 2.69 33 0.011 0.89 0.531 0.22
Go/no go RT, ms 484.07 103.02 42.60T 427.85 99.95 49.00T 53/25 1.65 33 0.108 0.56 0.579 0.20
Attention
Go/no go errors 2.33 2.32 44.20T 1.32 2.14 49.32T 40/19 1.33 33 0.194 0.46 0.211 0.46
DOI: 10.1159/000492160
Memory
AVLT learning 43.13 12.28 45.40T 56.95 9.10 57.00T 33/0 3.83 33 0.001 1.31 0.035 0.78
AVLT retention 8.73 3.88 44.13T 12.10 2.90 53.45T 32/10 2.94 33 0.006 1.01 0.106 0.59
Logical memory immediate 22.93 8.63 34.80P 30.05 5.62 62.85P 33/0 2.95 33 0.006 1.01 0.224 0.44
Logical memory delayed 17.93 10.30 34.13P 27.00 5.76 64.90P 40/10 3.07 20.50 0.006 1.13 0.134 0.54
Reasoning
Similarities 19.87 8.55 7.67S 26.70 2.72 10.15S 47/5 2.98 16.13 0.009 1.15 0.029 0.81
Matrix reasoning 13.47 6.02 8.60S 20.45 2.91 12.85S 47/0 4.14 18.91 0.001 1.55 0.010 0.97
Spatial Performance
Block design 30.87 12.45 7.07S 50.85 9.10 12.20S 60/0 5.25 24.55 <0.001 1.88 <0.001 1.51
Executive functioning
Trail-making test B, s 170.53 98.82 19.93P 61.45 15.41 52.80P 73/20 4.27 14.51 0.001 1.67 0.002 1.18
WCST categories 2.43 2.10 – 5.00 1.75 – – 3.88 33 <0.001 1.35 0.001 1.27
WCST perseveration 24.79 13.41 – 13.27 7.98 – – 3.14 32 0.004 1.08 0.034 0.79
Zoo test total 12.43 4.07 – 15.90 0.45 – – 3.18 13.22 0.007 1.30 0.022 0.87
Aggregated scores (z-transformed)
Speed (slowing) 0.48 0.88 – −0.36 0.45 – – 3.39 19.52 0.001 1.26 0.076 0.65
Accuracy (malperformance) 0.55 0.62 – −0.39 0.31 – – 5.37 19.29 <0.001 2.01 <0.001 1.39
Hauschildt/Lipp
Moritz/Irshaid/Lüdtke/Schäfer/
Downloaded by:
Karolinska Institutet, University Library
130.237.122.245 - 8/29/2018 1:19:10 PM
Fig. 1. Results from the mediation analyses
using Hayes’ process macro. The indirect MIAMI MIAMI
effects for speed (a) and accuracy (b) were
significant, indicating that momentary in-
fluences, concerns, and fears as well as mo- a = 0.052 b = 1.265
tivation (Momentary Influences, Attitudes (0.014)**** (0.412)*** a = 0.052 b = 0.821
(0.014)**** (0.300)**
and Motivation Impact on Cognitive Per-
formance Scale [MIAMI] total for pre and
post) largely contributed to group differ- c = 0.140 (0.038)**** c = 0.156 (0.267)****
ences (0 = healthy, 1 = alcohol use disorder) Group Speed Group Accuracy
on test results. For speed, the direct effect c’ = 0.073 (0.040)+ c’ = 0.113 (0.029)****
was reduced to a statistical trend. + p < 0.1; (0.020–0.133; sobel p = 0.021) (0.017–0.128; sobel p = 0.031)
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.005; **** p < a b
0.001.
with addiction. Yet, we question the validity of bold Table 3. Correlations between MIAMI scores and aggregated neu-
claims that monocausally attribute these deficits to brain ropsychological parameters for speed (slowness) and accuracy
(malperformance)
regions hosting neurocognitive faculties.
We found a very large group difference for memory. Slowness Malperformance
However, the patients as a group achieved better scores
than might be expected from this result (T-score > 40; Total score pre 0.594* 0.410
percentile > 30), and no more than one third showed Total score post 0.314 0.569*
malperformance on one of the 4 parameters as defined Total score pre and post 0.529* 0.591*
by 1 standard deviation below the mean. This result is * p < 0.05.
partly owing to above-normal performance in controls,
so we advise researchers to calculate norm scores in their
samples; some control groups might not be representa-
tive because they include, for example, students or vol- of potential metacognitive deficits in patients with psy-
unteers from data bases who are assessed repeatedly for chological problems, interview scales are needed to docu-
different studies and thus have an advantage due to pri- ment signs of fatigue, test anxiety, and distractibility (e.g.,
or experience with such assessment situations (even if number of requests to terminate the assessment; signs of
they are not familiar with the exact tests). As stated be- poor motivation). Recently, we found evidence of an ef-
fore [21], poor neurocognitive functioning may not be a fect of symptoms and motivation on neurocognitive per-
consequence but is perhaps a risk factor for addiction formance by both objective and subjective assessments
since poor IQ and neuropsychological functioning [19]. Fourth, no alcoholism scale was administered; such
clearly compromise academic achievement [44]. Indi- a scale might elucidate which factors are especially tied to
viduals with lower cognitive performance are known to primary cognitive or secondary malperformance. For ex-
have worse vocational functioning [45] and are less like- ample, craving may lead to distraction from the assess-
ly to have well-paid, permanent jobs, which may foster ment and likely represents an important mediator (see
psychological problems, including depression and ad- also our second point); tentative evidence in this direc-
diction [46, 47]. tion has already been obtained [17, 25]. Finally, and as
We would like to acknowledge a number of limita- stated in the preceding paragraph, there is a chicken-or-
tions. First, the sample size was small, so independent the-egg problem; due to the cross-sectional design, we
replication in other institutions is warranted. Second, cannot make clear causal inferences as to whether deficits
other contextual factors or mediators need to be exam- already existed at a premorbid stage or even whether pa-
ined for their role in functioning. For example, it was re- tients with normal scores showed a neurodegenerative
cently found that stereotype threat may also lead to sec- decline from a higher-than-normal baseline level. We
ondary impairment [48]. In AUD, motor impairment cannot rule out that unmeasured confounding variables
(e.g., ataxia) may also play a role in timed tests with a have further attenuated the observed indirect effect. It
strong motor component (e.g., the TMTs). Third, in view also deserves to be tested whether some of the contextual
130.237.122.245 - 8/29/2018 1:19:10 PM
Karolinska Institutet, University Library
References
1 Stavro K, Pelletier J, Potvin S: Widespread and 7 Vetreno RP, Hall JM, Savage LM: Alcohol- A, Solfrizzi V: Alcohol consumption in mild
sustained cognitive deficits in alcoholism: a related amnesia and dementia: Animal mod- cognitive impairment and dementia: harmful
meta-analysis. Addict Biol 2013;18:203–213. els have revealed the contributions of differ- or neuroprotective? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
2 Bates ME, Bowden SC, Barry D: Neurocogni- ent etiological factors on neuropathology, 2012;27:1218–1238.
tive impairment associated with alcohol use neurochemical dysfunction and cognitive im- 12 Ganguli M, Vander Bilt J, Saxton JA, Shen C,
disorders: Implications for treatment. Exp pairment. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2011; 96: Dodge HH: Alcohol consumption and cogni-
Clin Psychopharmacol 2002;10:193–212. 596–608. tive function in late life: a longitudinal com-
3 Stephan RA, Alhassoon OM, Allen KE, et al: 8 Fama R, Le Berre AP, Hardcastle C, Sassoon munity study. Neurology 2005; 65: 1210–
Meta-analyses of clinical neuropsychological SA, Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV, Zahr NM: 1217.
tests of executive dysfunction and impulsivity Neurological, nutritional and alcohol con- 13 Squeglia LM, Spadoni AD, Infante MA, My-
in alcohol use disorder. Am J Drug Alcohol sumption factors underlie cognitive and mo- ers MG, Tapert SF: Initiating moderate to
Abuse 2017;43:24–43. tor deficits in chronic alcoholism. Addict Biol heavy alcohol use predicts changes in neuro-
4 Scheurich A: Neuropsychological function- 2017. Epub ahead of print. psychological functioning for adolescent girls
ing and alcohol dependence. Curr Opin Psy- 9 Anderson ML, Nokia MS, Govindaraju KP, and boys. Psychol Addict Behav 2009;23:715–
chiatry 2005;18:319–323. Shors TJ: Moderate drinking? Alcohol con- 722.
5 Xiao P, Dai Z, Zhong J, Zhu Y, Shi H, Pan P: sumption significantly decreases neurogene- 14 Rourke SB, Grant I: The interactive effects of
Regional gray matter deficits in alcohol sis in the adult hippocampus. Neuroscience age and length of abstinence on the recovery
dependence: a meta-analysis of voxel-based 2012;224:202–209. of neuropsychological functioning in chronic
morphometry studies. Drug Alcohol Depend 10 Neafsey EJ, Collins MA: Moderate alcohol male alcoholics: a 2-year follow-up study. J Int
2015;153:22–22. consumption and cognitive risk. Neuropsy- Neuropsychol Soc 1999;5:234–246.
6 Zakhari S: Overview: how is alcohol metabo- chiatr Dis Treat 2011;7:465–484. 15 Bernardin F, Maheut-Bosser A, Paille F:
lized by the body? Alcohol Res Health 2006; 11 Panza F, Frisardi V, Seripa D, Logroscino G, Cognitive impairments in alcohol-dependent
29:245–254. Santamato A, Imbimbo BP, Scafato E, Pilotto subjects. Front Psychiatry 2014;5:78.
130.237.122.245 - 8/29/2018 1:19:10 PM
Karolinska Institutet, University Library