Justice, 2017
Justice, 2017
Justice, 2017
Research Article
Purpose: Preschoolers with language impairment (LI) are Results: Results of hierarchical linear models examining
prime candidates for early-literacy interventions, given their children’s print knowledge at 1-year postintervention
susceptibility for future reading difficulties. To date, most showed that the effect size (d = 0.20) favoring the
studies of early-literacy interventions for this population treatment group was similar to that observed one year
has assessed short-term impacts, with limited attention to prior (d = 0.21) at the end of intervention, suggesting that
whether initial effects are sustained over time. This study results did not fade over time. Results also showed that
was designed to evaluate longitudinal impacts of print- children with LI and comorbid low nonverbal cognition
focused read-alouds implemented by early childhood special benefited the most from the intervention delivered 1 year
education teachers for a clinic sample of children with LI. earlier.
Method: Assessment data available for 172 children with Conclusion: The maintenance of short-term effects
LI were analyzed to examine their print knowledge 1-year to 1-year postintervention supports the value of early
postintervention. Measures examined children’s alphabet childhood teachers using print-focused read-alouds to
knowledge, print concepts, and name-writing skills, which improve the early-literacy skills of children with LI in their
were used to derive a print-knowledge composite. classrooms.
I
n recent years, researchers have shown considerable commercial storybooks to their students each week and
interest in identifying effective avenues for improving integrated systematic discussions about print form and fea-
the literacy skills of children with disabilities across tures into these read-alouds (Justice, Logan, Kaderavek, &
all grades of schooling (McClintock, Pesco, & Martin‐Chang, Dynia, 2015). Results showed that children with LI whose
2014; Wright, Mitchell, O’Donoghue, Cowhey, & Kearney, teachers implemented the program had significantly better
2015). Given the well-documented susceptibility of children print knowledge in the spring of the year (controlling for
with language impairment (LI) for reading difficulties, a fall scores) compared with children with LI whose teachers
significant volume of this work has focused specifically implemented a comparison read-aloud program. However,
on this subset of children with disabilities (Justice, Skibbe, as with many other experimental studies focused on improv-
McGinty, Piasta, & Petrill, 2011; Lovelace & Stewart, 2007; ing the literacy skills of children with LI (e.g., Lovelace &
McClintock et al., 2014; Wright et al., 2015). For instance, Stewart, 2007; McClintock et al., 2014; Wright et al., 2015),
one recent study reported results of a randomized controlled the study did not attend to whether the short-term positive
trial (RCT) involving 291 three- to five-year-old children effects were sustained past the period of intervention. To this
with LI enrolled in early childhood special education (ECSE) end, it is unclear whether the observed short-term improve-
classrooms in the United States. In this study, teachers ments in literacy skills lead to longer term sustained benefits
implemented a 30-week classroom-based intervention featur- for children with LI, or fade out with time.
ing print-focused read-alouds; specifically, teachers read The present study represents a follow-up to the initial
report of the RCT referenced previously, and was designed
to determine if the intervention impacts observed during the
a
The Ohio State University, Columbus intervention year were sustained at 1-year postintervention.
b
University of Toledo, Perrysburg, Ohio An additional focus was to determine whether treatment
Correspondence to Laura M. Justice: justice.57@osu.edu effects at 1-year follow-up were associated with children’s
Editor: Krista Wilkinson nonverbal cognition. The latter focus was of interest given
Associate Editor: Julie Wolter that the children with LI showed significant heterogeneity
Received December 28, 2015
Revision received June 23, 2016
Accepted October 19, 2016 Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the time
https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_AJSLP-15-0200 of publication.
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • 1–14 • Copyright © 2017 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 1
Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/ajslp/0/ by a Univ Of Newcastle User on 04/07/2017
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
in their cognitive skills, with approximately half of the sam- consistently shows that even at very young ages, children
ple exhibiting nonverbal cognition scores more than 1 SD with LI show significant lags in their development of early-
below the mean (i.e., 85 or lower standard score points). literacy skills as compared with typical peers (Boudreau
The nature of the sample allowed us to consider whether & Hedberg, 1999; Cabell, Justice, Zucker, & McGinty,
children with LI with very low nonverbal cognition may 2009; Cabell et al., 2010). Early-literacy skills are considered
have responded differently to the intervention than children foundational antecedents for or precursors to reading
with average or better cognitive performance. Further, achievement, and acquisition of these antecedents is consid-
results of the initial RCT suggested that the intervention ered necessary for formal reading instruction to be successful.
was most influential for children with LI who had low For instance, children with SLI at 4 years of age know,
nonverbal cognition (Justice, Logan, Kaderavek, et al., on average, about seven letters of the alphabet, com-
2015)—namely, children with lower levels of cognition par- pared with age-matched peers who know an average
ticularly benefitted from being exposed to print-focused 20 letters (Cabell et al., 2009). Alphabet knowledge is
read-alouds. considered one of the strongest and most reliable predic-
tors of future reading achievement (Lonigan & Shanahan,
2009); consequently, this lag in letter-naming skills seen
Early Literacy Skills of Children With LI in children with LI during the preschool years seems to
The presence of LI is one of the most common reasons represent a significant risk factor in their overall literacy
for which young children qualify for participation in ECSE development.
programs. ECSE programs are early-education settings To attenuate such early risks, and to potentially
that serve children between 3 and 5 years of age who have improve future reading achievement, researchers have
a diagnosed disability. Oftentimes, these classrooms serve developed and tested targeted approaches to improving
both children with and without disabilities in an effort for the early-literacy skills of children with LI, particularly
children with disabilities to be educated alongside children their print knowledge and phonological awareness (Ezell,
who are typically developing. By far, the majority of children Justice, & Parsons, 2000; Gillon, 2002; Justice, Chow,
with disabilities served in ECSE settings exhibit LI, either Capellini, Flanigan, & Colton, 2003; Lovelace & Stewart,
as a primary condition or comorbid with/secondary to 2007; McNamara, Vervaeke, & Van Lankveld, 2008; van
other conditions, such as autism or intellectual disability Bysterveldt, Gillon, & Moran, 2006). In general, these
(e.g., Down syndrome). The former is typically referred to studies show that targeted interventions for developing
as specific or primary LI, whereas the latter is often referred children’s early-literacy skills can lead to significant improve-
to as nonspecific LI (Tomblin et al., 1997). Children with ments in these skills for children with LI. Targeted inter-
nonspecific LI typically have lower nonverbal cognition ventions are those that explicitly focus on improving
than those with specific LI (SLI), and low nonverbal cogni- children’s early-literacy skills, such as their knowledge of the
tion is usually an exclusionary factor when a diagnosis of letter names or their awareness of rhyming patterns; often,
SLI is made (see Tomblin et al., 1997). In fact, many stud- such interventions follow a scope and sequence delineating
ies of children with LI will exclude children with signifi- specific early-literacy targets to address over an intervention
cant deficits in nonverbal cognition (e.g., L.-Y. Guo & period (e.g., Gillon, 2002; Justice et al., 2003). These can
Schneider, 2016; Justice et al., 2011). be differentiated from the interventions typically delivered to
Of relevance to this study are reports of the heightened children with LI, in which oral-language goals are focused
susceptibility of children with LI for reading disability, to on most intensively so as to improve children’s vocabulary
include both children with specific and nonspecific variations and morphosyntactic skills (Fey, Cleave, Long, & Hughes,
of LI (Catts, Fey, Tomblin, & Zhang, 2002; Morgan, Farkas, 1993; Girolametto, Pearce, & Weitzman, 1996). A com-
& Wu, 2011; Skibbe et al., 2008). Morgan et al. (2011) con- parison of the effects of a 12-week early-literacy intervention
ducted growth-curve modeling to examine the trajectory that explicitly targeted print knowledge and phonological
of reading development for children with LI compared with awareness to 12 weeks of traditional language intervention,
typically developing peers. At kindergarten, children with focused only on oral-language goals, for 3- to 5-year-olds
LI performed about 0.4 SD lower than those without dis- with LI showed that only the former had significant impacts
abilities in reading achievement. Over time, however, the on children’s early-literacy development (McNamara et al.,
gap between the two groups widened: By fifth grade, children 2008). We can interpret such work to show that traditional
with LI performed 0.8 SD lower than those without reading language intervention focused exclusively on facilitating
disabilities. Such work has led to great interest in determining children’s oral language skills (e.g., vocabulary, grammar)
how reading disabilities among children with LI can be does not contribute to growth in early-literacy skills for
prevented, potentially by enhancing children’s early-literacy children with LI; thus, interventions explicitly designed to
skills prior to entrance to formal schooling and the rigors of lead to growth in early-literacy skills should accompany
reading instruction in kindergarten. the provision of traditional language intervention.
To this end, a growing body of work has sought to An important issue within the early-literacy inter-
improve our understanding of the early-literacy skills of vention literature is whether short-term treatment effects
children with LI, prior to the advent of formal schooling are sustained over time. This is an important avenue of
and exposure to formal reading instruction. Such work inquiry, given that the focus of early-literacy intervention
of a work. Therefore, for the present purposes, we acknowl- Even within the context of formal schooling and receipt
edge the result of the inferential test statistics but elect to of special-education services, children with LI significantly
not arbitrarily reject the tested hypothesis. lag behind their typical peers in the development of reading
To this end, we interpret the results of this 1-year across the primary grades (Morgan et al., 2011). Thus, at
intervention follow-up favorably, noting that the observed least in part, it may be that treatment effects are sustained
effect size suggests there to be continued impacts of print- over time due to the counterfactual, in that children with LI
focused read-alouds 1 year after the close of the intervention, are likely developing their print knowledge only slowly over
with those in the experimental condition performing one- time, outside of an explicit intervention context targeting
fifth of a standard deviation better than those in the control print-knowledge development.
group in their print knowledge. This modest yet sustained Considering the second point, it is worth considering
effect is coherent with the results of a separate study exam- how best to interpret the size of the effect observed, which
ining 1- and 2-year effects of print-focused read-alouds for corresponded to a 0.2 SD unit on the print-knowledge
children from low-income homes (Piasta et al., 2012). This composite. Interpretation of effect-size magnitude is often
study reported a postintervention effect at 1 year of 0.26 and made relative to Cohen’s benchmarks, such that 0.2 is small,
0.27 at 2 years, finding that preschool children who experi- 0.5 is medium, and 0.8 is large (Cohen, 1988). However,
enced print-focused read-alouds performed about one-fourth these benchmarks are generic and not tied to a given sub-
of a standard deviation better in reading than those in a stantive field. As Hill et al. (2008) pointed out, effect sizes
comparison condition in kindergarten and first grade. are best interpreted empirically and in relation to what is
The observation that treatment effects are maintained normative for a given aspect of development or, in this case,
over a year, resulting in a difference in the print knowledge a particular type of intervention. Hill et al. conducted a
between children who did and did not experience classroom- meta-analysis, in which they aggregated the mean effect sizes
based print-focused read-alouds in the prior year, seems to reported in 76 past meta-analyses of educational interventions
beg the question as why the children in the control condition (kindergarten through twelfth grade) employing the RCT
did not catch up with those in the treatment condition in design, which represents the most “rigorous impact design
their print knowledge. Catch-up by those in the control possible.” The mean effect extracted from 76 studies for
condition may be the culprit behind the fade-out effect children in the early elementary grades (Grades 1–3, the
commonly seen in studies of early-childhood interventions lowest grade range available and most comparable to the
(Barnett, 2011). That the children in the control condition present sample) was 0.23 (SD = 0.21). This benchmark
did not catch up may be indicative of the fact that children suggests that print-focused read-alouds yield effects that
with LI tend to have more difficulties developing print knowl- are comparable to those seen in the random-assignment
edge than typical children (Boudreau & Hedberg, 1999; literature. Therefore, we can interpret the use of print-
Gillam & Johnston, 1985) and that intensive experiences focused read-alouds as having a sustained effect on student
with print (such as that received by children in the treatment achievement that is comparable to that seen in RCTs more
condition in this study) are necessary for achieving catch-up. generally.