Linguistic Society of America Language
Linguistic Society of America Language
Linguistic Society of America Language
Moribund Dialects and the Endangerment Canon: The Case of the Ocracoke Brogue
Author(s): Walt Wolfram and Natalie Schilling-Estes
Source: Language, Vol. 71, No. 4 (Dec., 1995), pp. 696-721
Published by: Linguistic Society of America
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/415741
Accessed: 11-11-2015 23:22 UTC
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/
info/about/policies/terms.jsp
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content
in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship.
For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Linguistic Society of America is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Language.
http://www.jstor.org
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MORIBUND DIALECTS AND THE ENDANGERMENT CANON:
THE CASE OF THE OCRACOKE BROGUE
WALT WOLFRAM NATALIE SCHILLING-ESTES
North Carolina State University University of North Carolina at
Chapel Hill and North Carolina
State University
Moribund dialects threatened by the encroachment of healthy varieties of the same
language have been overlooked in establishing the language endangerment canon. Endan-
gered varieties of languages as safe as even English exhibit structures not found in main-
stream language varieties and so are an invaluable resource to scholars of language
variation-and, indeed, of language patterning in general. Further, the insights into lan-
guage variation and change that we gain from studying moribund dialects inform our
study of the types of changes that characterize endangered and dying languages. Our
arguments are based on the examination of Ocracoke English, a dialect of American
English which is spoken on Ocracoke Island, located off the coast of North Carolina
and inhabited by about 600 year-round residents. This dialect developed in relative isola-
tion from mainstream varieties of American English but is now threatened by encroach-
ment from mainland dialects as the island becomes more accessible to the outside world.
Using the case of the Ocracoke production of the /ay/ diphthong as [Ak'], we present
linguistic and sociolinguistic evidence that Ocracoke English is indeed an endangered
dialect. We also describe the development of a community-based preservation program
that parallels the type of proactive programs that have been implemented thus far only
for endangered language situations.*
*
The research reported here was supported by National Science Foundation Grant SBR-93-
19577 and by the William C. Friday Endowment at North Carolina State University. Thanks to
Nancy Dorian, Kirk Hazen, Brian Joseph, and Dennis Preston for their helpful comments on an
earlier draft. Special acknowledgement is due to Yancey R. Hall for his assistance in extracting
some of the /ay/ data and especially to Dennis Preston for conducting the trinomial VARBRUL
analysis.
696
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MORIBUND DIALECTS AND THE ENDANGERMENT CANON 697
connection between this area of study and the study of the language death
process itself.
There are a number of reasons for this neglect of dying dialects in the midst
of the escalating concern for languages on the brink of extinction. Linguists
may feel that there is little point in rushing into a dying dialect area if the core
language as a whole shows no signs of vanishing. At first glance, this seems
like a reasonable basis for omitting dying dialects from the canon of language
endangerment. On closer inspection, however, this exclusion is based on the
questionable assumption that interlanguage variation is of more value than intra-
language diversity, which in turn, is based on the assumption that the systemic
boundaries between language and dialect are obvious and discrete. Further-
more, it is inconsistent to admit endangered dialects of safe languages as worthy
objects of study only when they are situated in bilingual settings-not when
they are surrounded by safe varieties of the same language (e.g. King 1989,
Tsitsipis 1989).
The omission of dying dialects from the language-endangerment canon may
also be based on the descriptive problems encountered in sorting out the subtle
ways in which dialects often differ from each other. It can be difficult to deter-
mine which language features are characteristic of the receding variety, which
belong to the expanding dialect, and which features were already present in
both varieties before one began to encroach on the other. As Hoenigswald
(1989:348) observed:
So long as the differences between the two languages are gross, the distinction [between
healthy and dying languages] seems feasible and meaningful. But what about dialects'? Which
do you believe or expect: that dialect death is an unspectacular, endemic, everyday occurrence,
taking place pervasively and beneath the threshold of awareness; or contrariwise, that there
can be no such thing as dialect death by definition'? Or does it matter'?It is probably no accident
that none of the papers assembled here deals with such a situation however remotely.
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
698 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 71, NUMBER 4 (1995)
of Ocracoke English lead us to question the 'widely shared intuition' (Hill 1989:
149) that the processes of change in language death differ from ordinary change
processes primarily in the rapidity with which they occur. A number of investi-
gations of the patterning of linguistic variables with respect to internal linguistic
and external social factors in the course of language change indicate that vari-
ables display quite different-and often less linguistically and sociolinguistically
'natural'-patterning at the beginning and end points of a change than at other
points in its progression (Fasold 1973, Wolfram 1988, Labov 1994). Such un-
usual patterning may well characterize language changes we find in dying lan-
guages and language varieties, since, presumably, a language on the verge of
extinction will be characterized by a heavy concentration of incipient changes
to exterior linguistic norms and terminating changes in native language struc-
tures. A portion of the current analysis, for example, involves the detailed study
of the replacement of an obsolescing Ocracoke English vowel production-that
is, the pronunciation of the layl diphthong as [Ai']-with the Southern Ameri-
can English pronunciation of this diphthong as [a:]. Our investigation shows
that this replacement process does not parallel ordinary, natural change as
neatly as we might assume.
The data from intralanguage variation may even prove valuable for examining
templates of universal language, given the interplay between universality and
diversity in language. Trudgill and Chambers (1991:294), citing an empirical
case of nonstandard syntax variation in Wessex English that challenges the
universal IP template (lhalainen 1991), observe that familiarity with dialect
variation 'could potentially sharpen theoretical arguments as much as do im-
ported "exotica" from Kinyarwanda or Ponapean.' Although the in-depth in-
vestigation of the /ay! variable we present in this analysis does not directly
challenge current assumptions about the nature of universal grammar, the po-
tential significance of intralanguage variation for universal grammar certainly
must be admitted.
Arguments for the inclusion of dialects in the endangerment canon are not
limited to linguistic considerations per se, but extend to the sociocultural role
of language as well (Hale, in Hale et al. 1992, Woodbury 1993). Hale (1992:1)
points out that 'It [language loss] is part of a much larger process of LOSS OF
CULTURAL AND INTELLECTUAL DIVERSITY in which politically dominant lan-
guages and cultures simply overwhelm indigenous local languages and cultures,
placing them in a condition which can only be described as embattled.'
While the link between dialect and (sub)culture may be more subtle than
the link between language and culture, it is certainly no less real. In fact, the
subtlety of the dialect-subculture connection probably heightens the level of
the threat to a dialect's maintenance. Dialects are typically disregarded when
considering nonmainstream subcultures. Even worse, they are often dispar-
aged, since they are typically held to be unworthy approximations of a standard
language variety (Labov 1969).
Our investigation of Ocracoke English reveals that nonmainstream dialects,
despite their negative valuation by those who esteem standard English, indeed
play a large role in the shaping of cultural identity. Further, the threat to cultural
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MORIBUNDDIALECTSAND THE ENDANGERMENTCANON 699
identity reflected in the loss of these dialects is just as real as the threat of
cultural loss symbolized by language death. For over two and a half centuries,
Ocracoke Island, located twenty miles off the coast of North Carolina and
accessible to this day only by ferry, existed in relative isolation from the main-
land U.S. The island is fourteen miles long and a little less than a mile wide at
its widest point. The village of Ocracoke is located within the southernmost
mile; the remainder of the island is now a National Seashore protected from
residential development. During an extended period of insulation, Ocracokers
developed not only a unique dialect but also a unique cultural identity which
became inextricably bound to their language variety. Shortly after World War
II, with the advent of a state-run ferry service and the construction of a paved
road on the island, Ocracoke began to host ever-increasing numbers of tourists
and new residents from the mainland. The unique culture of the island is now
threatened by mainland ways of life, and the traditional dialect is correspond-
ingly fading. During tourist season, newcomers to the island now outnumber
ANCESTRAL ISLANDERS-residents who can trace their island heritage at least
several generations back. Even more strikingly, a minority of the current gener-
ation of Ocracoke schoolchildren are ancestral islanders. Our conversations
with Ocracokers reveal that their traditional dialect has for many years played
a strong symbolic role in projecting island identity; correspondingly, a true
sense of cultural loss is felt as the dialect vanishes. As one of our key informants
in Ocracoke, a fifty-year-old male, told us:
(1) I got a little kid, see, he's four weeks old, [by the] time he gets grown,
his accent will be what they call 'dingbattish' [i.e. non-island]. But
I would like for him to keep the same accent and heritage that we've
had for years and years, but all this is gone now. The only way we
can preserve it is for you fellers to put it on tape.
The link between dialect and culture is even felt by younger members of the
Ocracoke community, as indicated in a thirteen-year-old girl's response to a
question about her feelings toward the disappearance of the traditional Ocra-
coke dialect.
(2) It's [i.e. the Ocracoke dialect] sacred, really, the way we talk; it's
something the island is special for.
Despite the cultural value embodied in their language varieties, speakers of
endangered dialects often relinquish their native speech in the face of pressure
from safer, encroaching varieties and from those who condemn vernacular vari-
eties in general, whether dying or thriving. Through the systematic and rigorous
study of endangered dialects-and, crucially, the dissemination of the results
to speakers of the dialects under study-linguists can show these speakers that
no language variety, no matter what negative social valuation it has acquired,
is linguistically inferior. The study of endangered dialects is thus important,
not only from an intellectual and scientific point of view but from a sociocultural
and humanistic perspective.
The threatened status of a number of small postinsular dialect communities
should be of great concern to dialectologists and language variationists who
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
700 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 71, NUMBER 4 (1995)
value the full spectrum of diversity within a language. In fact, the history of
dialectology shows a fastidious concern for documenting the forms found in
nonmainstream communities, and one of the motivations for initiating the Lin-
guistic Atlas of the United States and Canada was the concern for capturing
some of the relic forms found in such communities before they disappeared
(Kurath 1939). Thus, our study of obsolescing forms in Ocracokefits well within
the focus of traditional dialect study. At the same time, we extend our concern
beyond the mere documentation of such forms so that we examine the linguistic
and sociolinguistic dynamics of dialect erosion. We further relate the status of
moribund dialects to the current concern with language endangerment. In the
process, we argue that the social responsibility of linguists toward dying dialects
is every bit as great as the responsibility of linguists toward dying languages
and their speakers (Hale et al. 1992).
In this presentation, we first consider evidence for the designation of the
Ocracoke dialect as endangered, with special focus on the patterning of the
/ayl variable mentioned above. Following the presentation of our evidence, we
discuss the social responsibility that linguists and dialectologists should assume
with respect to documenting and disseminating information about the state of
moribund dialects. We conclude by describing a community-based, collabora-
tive model that involves the development of materials and programs designed
to foster knowledge of and pride in the local dialect. Although such programs
exist for selected endangered languages (e.g. Watahomigie and Yamamoto,
Craig, in Hale et al. 1992), we are not aware of similar programs focused on
endangered dialects of safe languages. We hope that the description of our
program will encourage other students of language variation to become involved
with communities in a way that benefits the local research communities
as well as the linguists who use these communities for field-initiated
research.
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MORIBUND DIALECTS AND THE ENDANGERMENT CANON 701
sons), claims Irish ancestry. The historically isolated islanders made their living
chiefly through such maritime-related trades as fishing and guiding ships
through the Outer Banks barrier islands. Ocracokers were isolated not only
geographically and economically from the mainland South but also socially.
Islanders tended to limit their social ties to the island, or at least to the Outer
Banks chain of islands, and marriages were confined almost exclusively to
island families, as evidenced in the relatively small number of surnames we
find on the island to this day. Many of these surnames can be traced back to
the island's original settlers.
Shortly after World War II, the longstanding isolation of Ocracoke Island
came to an end. A coastal highway was constructed along the Outer Banks
islands, and a state-run ferry service was instituted. These changes allowed far
greater access to Ocracoke than had been available in the pre-War years, when
boat transportation was sporadic and land travel was often over bare sand.
Ocracoke now began to host ever-increasing numbers of tourists and new resi-
dents from the mainland. Today, Ocracoke is home to only about 200-250
ancestral islanders. This represents less than half the year-round population
and less than one-tenth of the population on a typical day during the summer
tourist season. In addition, the island economy has been converted from one
largely independent of mainlanders to one almost wholly dependent on out-
siders. As with other islands along the Eastern seaboard, a vibrant tourist indus-
try has developed, and most ancestral islanders are employed in the tourist
service industry in one way or another. With the breakdown of economic and
geographic barriers separating Ocracoke from the mainland has come a corre-
sponding breakdown in social barriers. Marriage outside the island community
is becoming increasingly common, and the youngest islanders now interact
on a daily basis with classmates and teachers whose families are transplanted
mainlanders.
We summarize the social, cultural, and historical facts supporting our conten-
tion that the Ocracoke dialect is now endangered as follows:
1. Two and a half centuries of geographic isolation are brought to a sudden
end with the completion of highways and the implementation of a state-run
ferry service.
2. Ancestral islanders become a minority population on the island, as main-
landers establish permanent and vacation residences on the island.
3. The economic base shifts from a relatively self-sufficient marine-related
economy to one heavily dependent on the tourist trade.
4. Social networks extend beyond the confines of the island as Ocracokers
come into more contact with outsiders; marriage with mainlanders becomes
more commonplace, as do working and other social relationships.
The sociohistorical situation contextualizing the Ocracoke dialect neatly par-
allels what is found in a number of endangered languages, including East Suther-
land Gaelic, the focus of Dorian's classic study (1981) of the dying language of
a small Scottish fishing village, and the Newfoundland French spoken in Port-
au-Port, a Canadian fishing village community on which the outside world is
rapidly encroaching (King 1989).
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
702 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 71, NUMBER 4 (1995)
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MORIBUND DIALECTS AND THE ENDANGERMENT CANON 703
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
704 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 71, NUMBER 4 (1995)
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MORIBUND DIALECTS AND THE ENDANGERMENT CANON 705
VARBRUL Probabilities
[3y] [a:) [ay]
Social Factors
Linguistic Factors
First, consider the constraint ordering for the variable incidence of the [oy]
variant with respect to the following phonological environment, as indicated
by the VARBRUL analysis: voiced obstruent > nasal > lateral > voiceless
obstruent. That is, a following voiced obstruent is the most favoring environ-
ment for the occurrence of [oy] as opposed to lay] or [a:], and a following
voiceless obstruent is the least favoring, with nasals and laterals in between.
Interestingly, the ranking of voiced over voiceless obstruents with respect
to the raising and backing of the /ayl nucleus in Ocracoke is the opposite of
the constraint ordering associated with the more centralized raising of layl in
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
706 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 71, NUMBER 4 (1995)
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MORIBUND DIALECTS AND THE ENDANGERMENT CANON 707
[iy] [u']
[E "] /oy/
[a'] [: ]
\. /
/ayl
(adapted from Labov 1991:9)
FI;GURE i.
created by the lengthening of vowels in words with original short /a/; and the
new, highly peripheral phoneme began to front and raise, proceeding from /a/
to /ae/ to /e/, as we would expect under Labov's Principle I.2
The other alternative for /ayl as the nucleus bottoms out is to remain diph-
thongal and began to rise along a BACK peripheral path. Seemingly, this is the
path followed by Outer Banks [oy]. The different, yet equally peripheral loca-
tions of the nucleus of Outer Banks [oy] and the Southern [a:] vowel are illus-
trated in Figure 2, along with that of the nucleus of standard English unraised
[ay], which lies in peripheral position, and that of the nucleus of standard raised/
centralized [a'], which lies in nonperipheral space.
If the constraining effect of the following segment on /ay/ raising is ordered
in one direction along the voicing dimension in peripheral space and in the
opposite direction in nonperipheral space, we can readily explain the opposing
constraint orderings affecting Ocracoke [oy] vs standard English [ay], as well
as the parallel constraint rankings affecting the Ocracoke and Southern English
variants.
2.2.2. THE ENCROACHMENT
OF SOUTHERN[a:]. We need to examine the South-
ern unglided [a:] not merely as an external dialect norm but as a socially mean-
ingful dialect feature which is making inroads into the Ocracoke variety, as
islanders come into increasing contact with mainland Southerners.3 It is not
2 Labov's
principles for vowel system and subsystem movement are as follows:
Principle I: In chain shifts, tense nuclei rise along a peripheral track
Principle II: In chain shifts, lax nuclei fall along a nonperipheral track
Principle III: In chain shifts, back vowels move to the front
Principle III': In chain shifts, tense vowels move to the front along peripheral paths, and lax
vowels move to the back along nonperipheral paths
3 Note that at prior periods in the island's history, Ocracokers came into more contact with non-
Southerners, chiefly coastal residents, than with Southerners. For example, prior to World War
II, many Ocracoke men left the island for extended periods to work in such Northern port cities
as Philadelphia and Wilmington, Delaware. Today, islanders are much more likely to leave the
island for work in North Carolina and Virginia. Further, much of the sustained, service-related
contact Ocracokers now maintain with outsiders takes place with Southern mainlanders.
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
708 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 71, NUMBER 4 (1995)
FIGURE 2.
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MORIBUND DIALECTS AND THE ENDANGERMENT CANON 70(9
1.000 -
.750 -
. .500 -
L:9--- 474 ,
t ~~~~~~~.417
!L
.250 -??
=C-0.176
.000 I
Older Mid-Poker Mid-Non-Poker Mid-Women Younger
AgelSocial Group
FIGURE 3.
ing men and older speakers in our sample show no significant difference in the
use of [oy]. Does this indicate a reversal of, or at least resistance to, a change
in progress on the island toward reduced [3y] usage? A look at the incidence
of [oy] among middle-aged male speakers who are not members of the poker
game network, middle-aged women, and the youngest speakers shows that the
maintained incidence of the [3y] variant by the poker players is not a true
dialect reversal. The non-poker-playing age cohorts and younger speakers show
reduced frequency levels for the traditional hoi toider variant compared with
both older speakers and middle-aged poker players. In fact, if we restricted
our tabulations to speakers aged 16 and under, we would find only vestiges of
the [oy] production (less than 10% of all /ay! tokens are realized with [3y]),
showing how rapidly this form is fading.
The pattern of [Oy] production by the middle-aged poker players suggests
that the projection of the traditional vowel is a temporary maintenance of the
traditional island variant, apparently taking place before giving way to external
norms. This temporary rise in a declining variant is highly reminiscent of the
situation Labov observed for centralized /ay! and /aw! in his classic study of
Martha's Vineyard (1963). Labov, however, reported that the dramatic rise in
receding forms he observed among ancestral islanders who strongly identified
with traditional island ways of life represented a permanent reversal of a change
in progress. In the Ocracoke case, the maintenance of the traditional variant
represents a sort of dialectal last gasp before the [Oy] variant dies.
It is noteworthy that the traditional Outer Banks [oy] is maintained at a high
level in the speech of Ocracoke poker players even as the Southern variant is
added. The middle-aged poker players rank higher on the Southern [a:] index
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
710 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 71, NUMBER 4 (1995)
than both older speakers and their middle-aged non-poker-playing male cohorts,
although middle-aged females and younger speakers outrank them in their use
of La:]. In a somewhat ironic way, these men are projecting island identity by
mixing traditional vernacular variants with encroaching Southern vernacular
variants. The selective, additive pattern uncovered with /ay/ seems to be con-
firmed by other emerging patterns we have observed in Ocracoke speech which
show that speakers may add vernacular features from surrounding dialects to
their speech; at the same time they maintain a select group of some distinct
island traits, in order to maximize the projection of island identity via vernacu-
larity in speech (Schilling-Estes & Wolfram 1994). The adoption of selected
features of external varieties is commonplace in threatened languages and is,
according to Dorian (1994), one of the more viable strategies for preserving
endangered languages, albeit in an altered form, in the face of external language
encroachment.
The pattern of [oy] recession and the selective, additive pattern of [a:] expan-
sion in Ocracoke suggest that obsolescing dialect forms do not simply represent
'ordinary' language changes that proceed at an accelerated pace. We would
expect linguistic variables associated with such ordinary changes to display a
more orderly correlation to external social factors such as age and internal
linguistic factors such as following phonetic environment than the unexpected
patterning we observe in the obsolescing Ocracoke [3y] and the incipient South-
ern American [a:]. Not only do the patterns we observe for hOy]and [a:] in
Ocracoke inform our understanding of obsolescing forms in language-death
situations, but they also lead us to refine our notions about the structured nature
of variability in language change. In the next section, we reformulate conditions
on the orderly progression of change, based on insights garnered from Ocracoke
[3y] and [a:].
2.2.3. EXPLAINING PHONETICALLY IMPLAUSIBLE CONSTRAINT HIERARCHIES. In
Table 3, we summarize the constraint orderings for /ay! variability with respect
to the following phonological environment. Boldface numbers indicate phoneti-
cally implausible constraint orderings in terms of the following phonological
environment-that is, constraint effect rankings for the variable phonetic var-
iants of /ay! that do not match the natural, expected tendencies established
empirically in other studies (e.g. Labov 1963, Bernstein & Gregory 1994, Feagin
1994).
How do we explain the apparent phonetic implausibility of the constraint
ordering for Southern unglided [a:] as it begins to compete with the traditional
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MORIBUND DIALECTS AND THE ENDANGERMENT CANON 711
variant? We suggest that there are three bases for this phonetic implausibility.
One is a shift in the peripherality of the nucleus of lay/. When Southern unglid-
ing enters the Ocracoke dialect, it affects /ay/ in just those contexts in which
the diphthong is most likely to have been realized in traditional speech with a
nonperipheral, more centralized nucleus-that is, before voiceless consonants.
Before voiced consonants, a favored environment for [3y], the /ay/ diphthong
is almost never unglided to [a:], perhaps because the nucleus is too far along
on its rotational path backward and upward to be readily unglided. Ungliding
affects diphthongs when they are near low central position where they are still
vulnerable to a shift to peripherality-not in the mid back area where the shift to
peripherality has already taken place.4 As we have seen, the socially unmarked
standard English raising of [a'] to [a'], a nonperipheral vowel, shows a mirror
image constraint order with that for the traditional Ocracoke and Southern
peripheral nuclei. We hypothesize that the transition between peripheral and
nonperipheral status, with their mirror image constraint hierarchies, may result
in a temporary nonnatural ordering of phonological constraints. We presume
that over time, as the new change is carried to completion, the natural constraint
order will override this unnatural one, but not necessarily in the initial stages
of the change.
Our second explanation relates to the nature of variation during change. Most
variationists now assume that variation takes place in a way conveniently de-
scribed as an S-slope curve (Weinreich et al. 1968, Bailey 1973, Labov 1994).
At the beginning and end points of change, the change slope is relatively grad-
ual, and one variant predominates. At the middle stages of the change, however,
the curve is sharp and relatively rapid, as variation among competing variants
is maximized. We hypothesize that the beginning and end points of a change
are more likely to display upheaval in the natural ordering of constraint effects
than the midpoint of a change. During the midpoints, we expect the most natural
phonological constraint orderings between alternating variants. At the end
points, however, the constraint orders may be less phonologically natural, par-
ticularly in cases of dialect mixture. At the end points, lexical constraints and
rote processing of variants are likely to be maximized, a situation conducive
for overruling natural phonetic tendencies.5 In other words, the environment
in which a given change occurs first may not necessarily be the most natural
phonological environment for this change; similarly, as a change reaches its
4 Thomas (1995) offers a quite different interpretation geared more toward explanatory percep-
tually based principles of phonetic change.
5 There are also implications about the orderly progression of change and the role of the lexicon
in change that seem related to the progression slope. For example, the role of the lexicon in
phonological change is more prominent at the incipient and cessation stages of a change than at
its midpoint. Part of the resolution of the ongoing controversy over regularity in phonological
change (Labov 1981) may be related to the trajectory slope of the change. lrregularity and lexical
diffusion are maximized at the beginning and the end of the slope and phonological regularity is
maximized during the rapid expansion in the application of the rule change during the midcourse
of change.
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
712 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 71, NUMBER 4 (1995)
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MORIBUND DIALECTS AND THE ENDANGERMENT CANON 713
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
714 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 71, NUMBER 4(1995)
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MORIBUND DIALECTS AND THE ENDANGERMENT CANON 715
F2-F1
1900 1700 1500 1300 1100 900 700 500 300
I I t _
I I --t----+- -l------- --T300
- 400
f - 500
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
716 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 71, NUMBER 4 (1995)
According to Labov (1982), there are two primary principles that may moti-
vate (socio)linguists to take social action, namely, the PRINCIPLE OF ERROR COR-
RECTION and the PRINCIPLE OF DEBT INCURRED. These are articulated as follows:
(5) PRINCIPLE OF ERROR CORRECTION: A scientist who becomes aware of
a widespread idea or social practice with important consequences
that is invalidated by his own data is obligated to bring this error
to the attention of the widest possible audience (Labov 1982: 172).
(6) PRINCIPLE OF DEBT INCURRED: An investigator who has obtained lin-
guistic data from members of a speech community has an obligation
to use the knowledge based on that data for the benefit of the com-
munity, when it has need of it (Labov 1982: 173).
There are several outstanding instances in the history of social dialectology
where these principles have been applied. In the 1960s, sociolinguists took a
prominent prodifference stance in the so-called deficit-difference controversy
that was taking place within education and within speech and language pathol-
ogy (Baratz 1968, Labov 1969, Wolfram 1970). Consonant with the principle
of error correction, sociolinguists took a united stand against the classification
and treatment of normal, natural dialect differences as language deficits or disor-
ders. There is little doubt that sociolinguists played a major role in pushing the
definition of linguistic normalcy toward a dialect-sensitive one, although the
practical consequences of this definition are still being worked out in many
clinical and educational settings.
In keeping with the principle of debt incurred, social dialectologists also rose
to the occasion in the celebrated Ann Arbor Decision (1979). Linguistic testi-
mony was critical to the judge's ruling in favor of a group of African-American
children whose parents brought suit against the Ann Arbor, Michigan, Board
of Education for not taking their dialect into account in reading instruction
(Farr-Whiteman 1980, Smitherman 1981). In effect, the judge ruled that the
defendants had failed to take appropriate action to overcome language barriers,
in violation of Title 20 of the U.S. Code, Section 1703 (f).6 In compliance with
the judge's ruling, a series of workshops was conducted to upgrade awareness
and to apply sociolinguistic expertise in reading instruction.
These relatively well known cases of social involvement are not the only
ones in which the principles of social action articulated by Labov have been
applied. There are many other instances where linguists have been involved in
individual or corporate cases related to language equity in the workplace, the
educational system, and society at large. For the most part, however, the social
role assumed by linguists in these cases has been that of REACTIVE ADVOCACY,
where the linguist responds to a social inequity by providing sociolinguistic
evidence. In the typical scenario, a linguist who has conducted research in a
6
The relevant portions of this Code are as follows:
No state shall deny equal educational opportunity to an individual on account of his or her
race, color, sex, or national origin by. . .
(f) the failure by an educational agency to take appropriate action to overcome language
barriers that impede equal participation by its students in its instruction programs.
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MORIBUND DIALECTS AND THE ENDANGERMENT CANON 717
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
718 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 71, NUMBER 4 (1995)
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
MORIBUNDDIALECTSAND THE ENDANGERMENTCANON 719
tion efforts. Along with these institutional efforts, we have shown our documen-
tary several times at the local bar and grill, Howard's Pub, where both local
residents and tourists typically congregate for informal socializing. These show-
ings resulted in animated, positive discussions about the dialect by both ances-
tral islanders and tourists. The threatened status of the Ocracoke brogue has
also been the subject of several local and regional television and radio news
programs, and there were at least seven major feature articles in local and
regional newspapers from 1993 to 1995 that focused on the state of the dialect
and the threats to its survival. Several of these stories were accompanied by
sound bites; readers were invited to call an advertised telephone number and
listen to a recorded sample of the brogue firsthand. The Virginian Pilot news-
paper reported that more readers called in to hear the brogue than any other
sound bite they had yet made available to their readership.
We have attempted to engage students, teachers, and community members in
collecting and documenting dialect data. Several community members routinely
make observations and take notes for us about lexical items; and one school-
teacher, following one of our lessons, returned the following day with over two
pages of lexical items and phrases elicited from her elderly relatives. On another
occasion, she wrote a poem composed of many unique Outer Banks lexical
items in celebration of the dialect. The level of community support and coopera-
tion we have observed throughout the course of this effort, on an institutional
and personal level, is probably unprecedented in the history of American En-
glish dialect study.
The fate of the Ocracoke brogue is still undecided, and circumstances beyond
our and even the community's control probably will determine its ultimate
destiny. But at least the community is becoming informed as never before about
its dialect heritage and the linguistic-cultural stakes involved in its demise. And
if nothing else, we have documented for the historical record the status of the
once-vibrant brogue. We hope that our being there has made a difference and
that we have given something in return for our seemingly inane and intrusive
sociolinguistic probing. In the process of conducting our sociolinguistic study
for scientific purposes, we have become partners with the community in raising
awareness about the dialect in its traditional form and also in its current, mori-
bund state.
REFERENCES
BAILEY,CHARLES-JAMES N. 1973. Variation and linguistic theory. Washington, DC.:
Center for Applied Linguistics.
BARATZ,JOAN. 1968. Language and the economically deprived child: A perspective.
ASHA 10.143-45.
BERNSTEIN,CYNTHIA,and ELIZABETH GREGORY.1994. The social distribution of glide
shortened /ai/ in LAGS. Paper presented at SECOL 50, Memphis, TN.
BLANTON,PHYLLIS,and KARENWATERS(producers). 1994. The Ocracoke brogue: A
video documentary. Raleigh, NC: The North Carolina Language and Life Project.
CAMPBELL,LYLE, and MARTHAC. MUNTZEL.1989. The structural consequences of
languge death. Investigating obsolescence: Studies in language contraction and ob-
solescence, ed. by Nancy C. Dorian, 181-96. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
720 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 71, NUMBER 4 (1995)
SANKOFF. 1974
HENRIETTA, and DAVID SANKOFF
CEDERGREN, HENRIETTA
CEDERGREN 1974. Var ab e ru
Variable rules:
es Per
Performance
ormance as
a s ca re
a sstatistical ec on oof compe
reflection ence Language 50
competence. 50.333-55.
333 55
CHOMSKY MORRIS HALLE. 1968
NOAM, and MORRISHALLE
CHOMSKY, NOAM 1968. The sound pa ern oof Eng
pattern sh New York
English. York:
Harper and Row
Row.
WOLFRAM; and NANJO DUBE
DONNA; WALT WOLFRAM
CHRISTIAN, DONNA
CHRISTIAN DUBE. 1988
1988. Var
Variation
a on and change
in
n geograph
geographically so a ed commun
ca y isolated communities:
es ach an Eng
Appalachian
Appa sh and Ozark Eng
English English
sh
Pub ca on oof the
(Publication he Amer can D
American Dialect
a ec Soc 74). Tusca
e y 74
Society oosa AL
Tuscaloosa, AL: Un vers y oof
University
abama Press
Alabama
A Press.
DORIAN,NANCYC
DORIAN NANCYC. 1981 1981. Language dea death: cyclee oof a Sco
h The lifee cyc Scottish
sh Gae
Gaelicc d
dialect.
a ec
ade ph a PA
Philadelphia,
Ph PA: Un vers y oof Pennsyv
University an a Press
Pennsyvlania Press.
--. ed 1989
(ed.) 1989. Inves ga ng obso
Investigating escence S
obsolescence: Studies
ud es in
n language
anguage con rac on and obso
contraction obso-
lescence.
escence Cambr dge Cambr
Cambridge: Cambridgedge Un vers y Press
University Press.
1994. Pur
--. 1994 Purism
sm vs
vs. comprom
compromise se in
n language
anguage rev a za on and language
revitalization anguage rev
revival.
va LanLan-
guage in n Soc e y 23
Society 23.479-94.
479 94
FARR-WHITEMAN,
FARR WHITEMAN MARCIA
M ARCIAed 1980. Reac
(ed.) 1980 ons too Ann Arbor
Reactions Arbor: Vernacu
Vernacularar B ack En
Black En-
sh and educa
gglish education.
on Wash ng on DC
Washington, DC: Cen
Centerer for
or App
Applieded LLinguistics.
ngu s cs
FASOLD,RALPHW
FASOLDRALPH concept oof ear
1973. The concep
W. 1973 er a er More or less
earlier-later: ess correc
correct. New ways
oof ana yz ng var
analyzing a on in
variation n Eng sh ed
English, ed. by Char es James N
Charles-James N. Ba ey and Roger W
Bailey W.
Shuy, 183
Shuy 183-97.
97 Wash ng on D
Washington, D.C.:
C George
Georgetown
own Un vers y Press
University Press.
--. 1990
1990. The soc o ngu s cs oof language.
sociolinguistics anguage Cambr dge MA
Cambridge, MA: BasBasil B
Blackwell.
ackwe
FEAGIN,CRAWFORD
FEAGIN CRAWFORD. Long i' as a m
1994. 'Long
1994 crocosm oof Sou
microcosm Southern a es speech
hern sstates speech. Paper
ed aat NWAV 23
presented
presen 23, S an ord CA
Stanford, CA.
GUY, GREGORYR
GUY R. 1993
1993. The quan a ve ana
quantitative analysis ngu s c var
ys s oof linguistic variation.
a on Amer
Americancan
dialect
d research, ed
a ec research Denniss R
ed. by Denn R. Pres on 223
Preston, 49 Ph
223-49. ade ph a PA
Philadelphia, PA: John BenBen-
jamins.
am ns
KEN; MICHAEL KRAUSS
HALE, KEN
HALE YAMAMOTO;COLETTE
WATAHOMIGIE;AKIRA YAMAMOTO
KRAUSS; LUCILLE WATAHOMIGIE
ENGLAND. 1992
JEANNE; and NORA ENGLAND
CRAIG; LAVERNE MASAYESVA JEANNE
CRAIG 1992. Endangered
anguages Language 68
languages. 68.1-42.
1 42
HILL, JANE H
HILL H. 1989
1989. The soc unc ons oof re
a functions
social a v za on in
relativization n obso escen and non
obsolescent non-
obsolescent
obso escen languages.
anguages In Dor
Dorian,
an 149-66.
149 66
HOENIGSWALD,
HOENIGSWALD HENRY
H ENRYMM. 1989
1989. Obso
Obsolescence anguage h
escence and language s ory Ma
history: ers oof linear-
Matters near
eve ng loss,
y leveling,
ity, ke In Dor
he like.
oss and the an 347
Dorian, 347-54.
54
IHALAINEN, OssI
IHALAINEN 1991. Per
OssI. 1991 Periphrastic
phras c n aaffirmative
do in rma ve sentences
sen ences in he d
n the a ec oof Eas
dialect East
Somerset. D
Somerse a ec s oof Eng
Dialects sh S
English: ud es in
Studies n gramma ca var
grammatical a on ed
variation, ed. by Pe
Peter
er Trud
Trud-
ggill and JJ. K
K. Chambers
Chambers, 148 160 New York
148-160. York: Longman
Longman.
RUTH. 1989
KING, RUTH
KING 1989. On thehe soc a mean
social meaning ngu s c var
ng oof linguistic ab y in
variability anguage dea
n language death
h
ssituations:
ua ons VarVariation
a on in n New
Newfoundland French. In Dor
ound and French an 139
Dorian, 139-48.
48
KURATH, HANS
KURATH HANS. 1939
1939. Handbook oof the ngu s c geography oof New Eng
he linguistic and Prov
England. Provi-
dence, RI
dence RI: Brown Un University.
vers y
LABOV, WILLIAM
LABOV 1963. The soc
WILLIAM. 1963 a mo
social motivation change. Word 19
va on oof sound change 19.273-307.
273 307
1969. The logic
. 1969 og c oof nons andard Eng
nonstandard sh George
English. Georgetown anguage and
own monographs on language
ngu s cs 22
linguistics ed. by James A
22, ed 44 Wash
a s 11-44.
Alatis, Washington, DC: George
ng on DC own Un
Georgetown University
vers y
Press.
Press
. 1972
1972. Some pr
principles ngu s c me
nc p es oof linguistic hodo ogy Language in
methodology. n Soc e y 11.97-154.
Society 97 154
. 1981
1981. Reso v ng the
Resolving he Neogrammar
Neogrammarian an con roversy Language 57
controversy. 57.267-309.
267 309
. 1982
1982. Ob ec v y and comm
Objectivity men in
commitment ngu s c sc
n linguistic ence Language in
science. n Soc
Society
ey
11.165-201.
11 165 201
1991. The three
. 1991 hree d a ec s oof Eng
dialects sh Quan
English. a ve ana
Quantitative yses oof sound change in
analyses n
progress,
progress ed. by Pene
ed Penelope
ope Eckert,
Ecker York: Academ
44 New York
11-44. Academicc Press.
Press
1994. Pr
. 1994 nc p es oof linguistic
Principles change: In
ngu s c change Internal ac ors Cambr
erna factors. Cambridge, MA: Bas
dge MA Basil
Blackwell.
B ackwe
M1LROY,LESLEY.1980
M1LROYLESLEY 1980. Language and soc a ne
social works Ox
networks. Basil B
ord Bas
Oxford: Blackwell.
ackwe
ROBINS, ROBERTH
ROBINS M. UHLENBECK (eds.)
ROBERTH., and EUGENIUS M 1991. Endangered
eds 1991 languages.
anguages
Oxford:
Ox ord Berg
Berg.
DAVID, and WILLIAM LABOV
SANKOFF, DAVID
SANKOFF he uses oof var
1979. On the
LABOV. 1979 ab e ru
variable es Language
rules.
in
n Soc e y 88.189-222.
Society 189 222
Th s con en down oaded om 130 179 16 201 on Wed 11 Nov 2015 23 22 02 UTC
A use sub ec o JSTOR Te ms and Cond ons
MORIBUNDDIALECTSAND THE ENDANGERMENTCANON 721
Natalie Schilling-Estes
Department of Linguistics
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
Chapel Hill, NC 27599
This content downloaded from 130.179.16.201 on Wed, 11 Nov 2015 23:22:02 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions